You are on page 1of 11

Adult Daughters' Perceptions of the Mother-Daughter Relationship: A Cross-Cultural

Comparison
Author(s): Mudita Rastogi and Karen S. Wampler
Source: Family Relations, Vol. 48, No. 3 (Jul., 1999), pp. 327-336
Published by: National Council on Family Relations
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/585643 .
Accessed: 04/12/2014 09:35

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

National Council on Family Relations is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
Family Relations.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 192.231.202.205 on Thu, 4 Dec 2014 09:35:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Adult Daughters'Perceptionsof the Mother-Daughter
Relationship:A Cross-CulturalComparison*
Mudita Rastogi** and Karen S. Wampler

This study comparesthe perception of adult daughters' relationshipwith their mothersacross the EuropeanAmerican,Asian In-
dian American, and Mexican American cultures, using intergenerational,feminist object relations and attachment theories.
Threedimensionswere used to measure the relationships:closeness, reliability,and collectivism. Each dimensionwas measured
using two instruments:the AASand the MAD. The latter was developed to be sensitive to culturaldifferencesand includes a new
variable called trust in hierarchy.This variable representspositive beliefs about, and an acceptance of hierarchyin intergenera-
tional relationships.Theparticipantswere 91 womenfrom the threeethnicgroups. Cross-culturaldiferences in the adult daughter-
mother relationship were found, with the Asian Indian American group scoring higher than the EuropeanAmerican group on
many variables. Scoresfor the MexicanAmericangroup tended tofall between those of the other two groups.

T he mother-daughterrelationship is highly significant in and increasedcontentvalidityof the measure.The firsttwo dimen-


many cultures. The purpose of this study was to examine sions, closeness and reliability,(also see Collins & Read, 1990)
the mother-adultdaughter relationship across the Euro- were firstmeasuredusing the MAD. For this, new culturallysensi-
pean American, Mexican American,and Asian IndianAmerican tive variables"connectedness"and "interdependence" were devel-
cultures. This study is significant because: (a) no cross-cultural oped. The two correspondingMAD subscales are sensitive to the
researchon mother-daughterrelationshipsexists in the literature culturallydiversemannerin which daughtersreportconnectedness
and, (b) certainvariablesused in the study (connectedness,inter- and interdependencewith their mothers.Closeness and reliability
dependence, and trust in hierarchy)have not been measuredin were measuredagainusing two subscalesof the Adult Attachment
previous studies on family relationships.The three ethnic groups Scale (AAS, Collins & Read, 1990), close and depend. Thus the
were comparedon three key dimensions derived from intergen- variables,connectednessand close, and interdependenceand de-
erational family therapy, feminist object relations, and attach- pend, are linkedby distincttermssubsumedunderthe dimensions
ment theories. These dimensions of relationships (closeness, of closeness and reliability respectively. Another importantdi-
reliability,and collectivism) were each measuredin two different mensionof mother-daughter relationshipsinvolvescollectivism.A
ways using both an existing and a new measure.These six vari- subscale based on the new construct, trust in hierarchy, was
ables included connectednessand closeness (underthe closeness added to the MAD to measurethis dimension. Again, this was a
dimension), interdependenceand dependency (underthe dimen- culturallysensitive way to measurecollectivism. A second instru-
sion of reliability), and differentiation and trust in hierarchy ment used for this dimensionwas Level of Differentiationof Self
(underthe dimension of collectivism.). Scale (LDSS, Haber,1990) which measuresdifferentiation.
Family professionals utilize theories, techniques and other Second, open-endedquestions were employed to collate re-
empirically derived generalizations in their work. However, sponses regardingmother-daughterrelationships in the partici-
rarely do we know if the relationships between variables that pants' own words. These responses were coded and comparedto
hold true for a certaingroup of people in the United States (e.g., the quantitative results. Similar qualitative and quantitative
EuropeanAmericanwomen) are universallygeneralizable.With- responses were an indication that the MAD items producedre-
out testing them across various cultural groups (e.g., Mexican sults similarto whatthe participantssaid in theirown words.These
American and Asian Indian American women), the explanatory responseshelped check constructvalidity since the questionsin a
power of theories and other generalizationsis severely restricted standardizedtest may be interpreteddifferentlyby personsfrom a
and runs the risk of being ethnocentric.In utilizing the normsde- differentculture.
veloped in one context to measurethe behaviorin anotherculture Third, the questionnaireasked the participantsto talk about
we may draw erroneousconclusions if the two cultures are dis- their"actual"relationshipwith theirmothersas well as their"ideal"
similar (Straus, 1969). Thus, comparativeresearch helps shape relationship.Both sets of scores were obtainedfor each variable.A
our theories and tests the generalizabilityof knowledge derived description of the "ideal" relationship represented what each
from hypothesestested in one context to otherdissimilarcontexts woman desiredor valued.Further,the groupmeans of the "ideal"
(Lee & Haas, 1993). responsesrepresentedwhat thatculturalgroupvalued.This helped
ensurethatthe MAD itemswererelevantacrossdifferentcultures.
Methodological Challenges
This preliminary cross-cultural investigation into mother-
daughterrelationships came with its own methodological chal-
*This article is based on the doctoral dissertation of the first author, which was
lenges having to do with validity. These concerns are divided chaired by the second author.The authorswould like to acknowledge the valuable input of
into: (a) instrumentation,and (b) sampling. the dissertationcommittee membersand would also like to thankthe researchparticipantsin
this study. Portions of this research were presented at the AAMFT Annual Conference in
Instrumentation.To ensure conceptual equivalence (Straus, Baltimore,November 1995.
1969) of the constructsacross the three cultures certain precau-
**Address correspondenceto: Mudita Rastogi, Illinois School of Professional Psy-
tions were used. First, a new, culturallysensitive measurecalled chology, 20 South ClarkStreet,Chicago, IL 60603; e-mail: mudita@msn.com
the Mother-AdultDaughter(MAD) questionnairewas developed.
This allowed the researchersto measurethe threekey dimensions Key Words:attachment,cross-cultural,gender,intergenerational,mother-daughter
using culturallyrelevantitems. These items were developedusing
theory, cross-culturalliterature,and feedback from a pilot study (FamilyRelations, 1999, 48, 327-336)

1999, Vol. 48, No. 3 327

This content downloaded from 192.231.202.205 on Thu, 4 Dec 2014 09:35:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Sampling.The Asian IndianAmericans,as a group,are idio- and individuationin the mother-daughterrelationshiptheir argu-
syncraticin that the majorityof them were born in India, immi- ment is not backedup by quantitativeresearch.
gratedto the U.S. since the ImmigrationAct of 1965 was passed, Attachmenttheory(Bowlby, 1988) providesanotherapproach
and on averageare highly educated(Sheth, 1995). Most of these to viewing the mother-daughterrelationship (Collins & Read,
women's motherslive in India.The threeethnic groupswere pre- 1990; Goldberg,1991; Hazan& Shaver,1994b;Stevenson-Hinde,
dicted to be inherentlydifferent from each other on key demo- 1990). While in adulthoodone's mothermay not be the primary
graphic factors such as length of stay in the U.S., geographical attachmentfigure she continuesto be a significantattachmentfig-
distance from one's mother, education and income. Therefore, ure for adultchildren(Collins & Read, 1990). These authorsused
matchingall participantson the first two factorswas not possible the variablesunderlyingattachment,i.e., close, dependand anxiety
within the scope of this study. Further,as Lee and Haas (1993) to study attachment.However none of their researchwas cross-
point out, samples from differentculturesthat are similarto each cultural.
other may not be completely representativeof their own culture.
Due to the cross-culturalnatureof this study,concernsof external In part,the currentstudy tests the generalizabilityof the the-
validity had to be balanced with concerns for achieving some ories of Bowen (1972) and Williamson (1981) across different
comparabilitybetween the three ethnic groups. In orderto maxi- cultures. It does so by empirically testing whether the relations
mize the women's between groupdifferencesassociatedwith eth- between differentiation,interdependence,and trust in hierarchy
nicity, the researchers attempted to reduce variability of the predictedby the theoristsare upheldin othercultures.Intergener-
ational family therapywritings (Bowen, 1972; Kerr, 1981; Kerr
participantson the variableseducation and income. In addition,
in orderto minimizeany between groupdifferencesbased on lan- & Bowen, 1988) emphasize differentiation and autonomy as
being healthy, over dependence in family relationships.Bowen
guage comprehensiononly those women who were fluent in Eng-
argues that mother-daughterrelationshipsare significantbecause
lish were recruitedfor the study. Similarly,only women between
they serve as the mode of transmission of family patterns of
the ages of 25 and 35 were targeted.Findings of significantdif-
closeness, enmeshment,distance,and conflict from generationto
ference on dependentvariablescould then be reasonablyattrib-
generation. Bowen's theory has been criticized for overlooking
uted to cultureafter statisticallytesting for the influence of other
the impact of other social and economic systems on family func-
extraneous variables. Though sampling did not achieve perfect
tioning (Walters, 1988), and devaluingthe feminine relationship
equivalenceon all extraneousvariables,the researcherswere able
orientation(Knudson-Martin,1994). Williamson (1981) equates
to highlight significant differences in the mother-daughterrela-
healthy adultdevelopmentwith the stage of "terminationof hier-
tionship on the variablesstudied. Given the challenges relatedto archy"with one's parents.He arguesthat lower hierarchyand de-
sampling and conceptualequivalence,it is importantto note that pendence for an adult are relatedto greaterautonomy,closeness,
a numberof hypotheseswere supported. and intimacy. Both these theories pathologize cultures that em-
TheMother-DaughterRelationship phasize more dependenceover autonomy.
Closeness. This dimension refers to a sense of connection
There is a lack of research that examines adult daughters'
and intimacyin the relationshipbut is not restrictedto geographi-
perception of the mother-daughterrelationship across different
cal proximitybetween the women (Arcana,1979; Eichenbaum&
cultures(Boyd, 1989). Feministsidentifythis as a salientrelation-
Orbach, 1988; Silverstein, 1985). O'Connor (1990) studied a
ship (Chodorow, 1978) and one that can provide solidarity and
sample of 60 workingand lower middle class women in London,
supportto all women (La Sorsa & Fodor, 1990). Researchshows
aged 20 to 42. While only 16 women named their motheras the
that the intensity of the adult daughter-motherbond, emotional
personthey felt "veryclose" to, these relationshipswere found to
connection,and interdependenceare the highest in comparisonto include a great deal of support, attachment,positive feedback,
otherintergenerationalrelationships(Fischer,1991). some dependencyand practicalhelp, low level of confiding, and
In addition to the lack of theoreticallybased cross-cultural high visual contact. Geographicaldistance did not hinder close-
researchin this area, the mother-daughterrelationshiphas often ness. O'Connorconcludedthat closeness is not linked to tending,
been discussed in negative terms (see Eichenbaum & Orbach, a view sharedby Thompsonand Walker(1984) and Walkerand
1988; Friday, 1977). The currentstudy fills existing gaps by ex- Thompson (1983). However, O'Connor'sstudy did not compare
amining importantareas of strengthin this relationship.The hy- the resultsby social class or ethnic group.
potheses for the study were derived from theory and include The last criticism is important because, as Boyd (1989)
looking at both new variablesalong with those previouslyidenti- pointedout, many culturesfoster closer mother-daughter relation-
fied as being important. ships. To some extent, this depends on the mother'spracticalim-
Severaltheoriescan be used to view the mother-daughter re- portanceto the daughter,the degreeof segregationby genderin the
lationship.Object-relationsfeminist theoristsbelieve that patriar- society, and the living arrangementsin the family (Neisser, 1973).
chal influences lead to women being the primarycare-giversto This suggests that the very definition of closeness would also
young children.Thus young girls experiencea connectednessand varyby culture.This makesit importantto use culturallysensitive
lack of individuation in their relationship with their mothers measures.
whereas boys eventually renounce their primarylove-object in Reliability.This dimensionrefersto knowingthatthe mother
orderto identify with older males (Chodorow,1978). A woman's or daughterwill be therefor you to dependon. It includesthe give
own needs often go unrecognizedand unmet as she cares for oth- and take of relationships.Aid given and received in the mother-
ers. Over time, women will projecttheir neediness onto their in- daughter dyad has been a subject of research (Lang & Brody,
fant daughters.Thus girls grow up learningto decipherthe needs 1983; Thompson & Walker,1984; Walker& Thompson, 1983).
of others (Eichenbaum& Orbach, 1988). While these theorists These studies look at the link between dependencyand closeness.
identify significantvariablessuch as connectedness,dependence, The lattershowed that aid received and contactbetween mothers
328 Family Relations

This content downloaded from 192.231.202.205 on Thu, 4 Dec 2014 09:35:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
and adult daughters did not necessarily reflect closeness. But closeness, and exchange of resourcesexisted within the extended
these studies did not look at the role culturalfactors play and do family. No studies were found dealing with the relationship of
not tie the findings to a majortheoreticalperspective.Once again, mothersand their adult daughtersfor either Asian IndianAmeri-
one would expect significantdifferencesbetween culturesin the can or MexicanAmericanfamilies.
areaof reliabilityin this relationship. Reliability.High levels of conformity,hierarchy,and interde-
Collectivism.This dimension refers to the balance between pendence are fostered in the Asian Indian American family but
one's individualityand the needs of the group.It includesthe vari- especially in females (Ewing, 1991; Segal, 1991). Individualsare
ables of differentiationand hierarchy.Few studies were found in expected to be allocentric,that is make sacrifices for the good of
this literaturesearchwhich looked at mother-daughter issues from the family while maintaining strong family ties (Sprecher &
a combined perspective of Bowlby's attachment theory and Chandak,1992). Bumiller (1990) also noted that marriedIndian
Bowen or Williamson'sintergenerationaltheory.There seems to women were expected to shift some of their dependenceto their
be an obvious link in terms of an inverserelationbetween depen- husbandand his family, and rely on theirown parentsless.
dency and differentiation/individuation. Kerr and Bowen (1988) Falicov (1982), and Uribe et al. (1993) found that Mexican
definedhigh differentiationwithin a family system in termsof low Americans valued greater amounts of dependence and collec-
dependence. Similarly,Williamson (1981) linked low individua- tivism. However,Buriel (1993) and Delgado-Gaitan(1993) found
tion to high levels of dependencein a relationship.These theorists that autonomywas proportionalto acculturation.Diaz-Guerrero's
also argued for a connection between relatively low differentia- (1987) content analysis found that Mexican Americanvalues in-
tion/individuation, greater hierarchy, and greater dependence clude affiliative obedience, fear of authority,and family honor,
within a family system. Thus, low differentiation,high depen- which distinguishthem from the EuropeanAmericanculture.A
dency, and greaterhierarchyare all consideredunhealthy.How- study involving EuropeanAmerican,Hispanic,and Asian Ameri-
ever, as Knudson-Martin(1994) and Walters(1988) pointed out, can families by Julian, McKenry, and McKelvey (1994) found
women are expected to tend to relationships,but are considered differencesin parenting;EuropeanAmericanand Hispanicparents
"mature"only when seen as differentiatedwithin their families. placed more emphasis on "being independent"than did Asian
However,accordingto previouslycited literature,a strongmother- Americans.
daughterattachmentmay be correlatedwith higher levels of ad-
justment. Bowen and Williamson's theoretical views have No correspondingwork explicitly measures differentiation
significantimplicationsfor culturesthat encouragegreaterhierar- and hierarchy.Studies mentioned above suggest that adults of
chy and dependencyin the adult daughter-mother relationshipas Asian Indian American and Mexican American origin are ex-
compared to the middle-class European American population. pected to defer to, seek advice from, and otherwise continue in
This perspectiveis discussedat a laterpoint. hierarchicalrelationshipswith theirparents.However,no research
measuresthis. This relationshipquality,i.e., the construct"trustin
Davis and Jones (1992) used the Haber(1990) Level of Dif- hierarchy"(Rastogi, 1995) has not been studied cross-culturally.
ferentiationof Self Scale (LDSS) to comparemothersand daugh- (Thisconstructis describedin detaillater.)The valuingof hierarchy
ters on attachmentand differentiation.Findings showed that the also has implications for differentiationin the mother-daughter
variableswere independentof each other,that is, a lower level of relationship.It is possible that differentiationwould be less im-
attachmentwas not necessary in orderto develop a higher level portantfor a healthy and satisfying intergenerationalrelationship
of differentiation.However,this study did not classify individuals in a collectivistic culture.
by attachmentstyle (e.g., secure, insecure, etc.); instead it rated
them on a scale where a higher score representedgreater inti- Hypotheses
macy and attachment.Moreover,the sample was limited to Euro-
pean Americanwomen. Six sets of hypotheses were tested relating to the three key
dimensions.The variableswere: (a) connectedness;(b) closeness;
Cross-CulturalVariationin Closeness and Reliability (c) interdependence;(d) dependency;(e) trustin hierarchy;and (f)
differentiation.On all of the variables,except differentiation,it was
Spiegel (1982) uses the work of Kluckhohnto describehow hypothesizedthatfor both actualand ideal scores the Asian Indian
one culture may value self-reliance and individualismwhile an- Americangroup will score the highest, followed by the Mexican
other may encouragemore closeness, reliability,and collectivism Americangroup,and then the EuropeanAmericangroup.For ac-
between parentsand children.Due to differencesin family norms tual and ideal differentiation the opposite was hypothesized,
between culturalgroups, only througha comparisonof different namely,the scores for the EuropeanAmericangroupwould be the
culturesand efforts to determineconceptualequivalencecan one
highest, followed by the Mexican American,and then the Asian
ascertainif a theory holds true for all people (i.e., is universally
IndianAmericans.
applicable).
Closeness. Hazan and Shaver(1994a) proposethat since dif- Methods
ferent culturesare likely to encouragedifferentdegrees of close-
ness in a given relationship in a certain developmental phase
attachmentis culturally embedded. For example, Kakar (1981)
Recruitmentand Procedures
wrote aboutthe primacyof the mother-sonbond in extendedfami- The target was to elicit participation from at least thirty
lies in India and the emphasis of connectednessover separation. women each from EuropeanAmerican, Asian Indian American,
Otherexpectationsinclude a "collectivistic"orientationfor family andMexicanAmericanethnicgroups.The samplewas non-random
members, (Dhruvarajan,1993; Naidoo & Davis, 1988; Segal, and purposive.The women were recruitedthroughpersonalcon-
1991) and less emphasis on self-direction (Stopes-Roe & tacts, throughtheir place of employmentin two cities in Texas,
Cochrane,1990). In Hispanicfamilies,Uribe,LeVine,and LeVine and the Internet.The initial sample criteriaincluded women be-
(1993), and Vega (1990), found that strong ties, geographical tween the ages of 25 and 35 whose motherswere currentlyliving.
1999, Vol. 48, No. 3 329

This content downloaded from 192.231.202.205 on Thu, 4 Dec 2014 09:35:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
It was also decided to include only those women who had re- lationships.Itemswere adaptedto measurethe subjects'perceived,
ceived some college education.This was done to controlfor class actual, and ideal closeness and dependencyin their relationship
differences and also provided comparability since the entire with their mother. Alpha values for closeness and dependency
Asian Indian American sample was predictedto have a college were adequatefor both actualand ideal closeness and dependency
degree. Each participantwas initially contactedin person or over (0.65 to 0.91). Evidence for the validityfor the AAS is being ac-
the telephone in order to request her participation.A question- cumulated(Collins & Read, 1990). A factor analysis lent support
nairepacketwas then given/mailedto her.The packetcontaineda for the existence of three underlying factors and the scoring
demographicquestionnaireand four other instruments.Of the 91 scheme (Collins & Read, 1990). In addition,the AAS subscales
participants,15 were recruitedover the Internet.They responded were used to predictattachmentclassification(an indicatorof cri-
to the cover letter posted on a feminist usenet newsgroup and terion validity) in the Hazan and Shaver measure (Collins &
providedtheir mailing addressfor the researcher.The women re- Read, 1990) with 73% accuracy.A majordrawbackof the AAS
cruited met all the criteria for inclusion in the study. The three is that is was not developed to be sensitive to the manner in
ethnic groups were equally representedin the Internetsample. which otherculturesmay understandcloseness and dependency.
The construction and implementation of this survey followed Motherand AdultDaughter(MAD)questionnaire.The MAD
Dillman's (1978) "TotalDesign Method."Two weeks following was developed by the first authorto measurevarious aspects of
the initial distributionof the questionnaire,a follow-up letterwas the currentmother-adultdaughterrelationshipin a culturallysen-
mailed. A total of 140 questionnaireswere distributed.Ninety- sitive manner(Rastogi, 1998). Items were generatedin two ways.
one of these were returnedby the deadline, puttingthe response Theoreticaland empiricalwork on mother-daughter, intergenera-
rate at 65%. tional,andcross-culturalrelationshipswas used to definethe terms
Sample description.All but two of the researchparticipants and generatea preliminaryquestionnaire.This questionnairewas
were women between the ages of 25 and 35, having some college used in a pilot study.Responsesfrom a pilot studywere utilizedto
education.Of the exceptions,one woman was 23 and did not have rewordsome items and create new ones to include a wider range
a college educationand anotherwoman was 39 years of age. All of items reflecting connectedness, interdependence,and trust in
women were highly acculturatedbut there were still differences hierarchyin differentcultures.
between groups since the majority of the Indian immigrantsto For this study, an "actual"as well as an "ideal"form of the
the U.S. arrived after 1965 (Sheth, 1995). As expected, there MAD was developed and administered.The former referredto
were significantdifferencesbetween the ethnic groups in educa- the current relationship between the adult daughter and her
tion, X2(10,N = 91) = 25.54, p = 0.004 and income, X2(10,N = mother,and the latterto the kind of relationshipshe would like to
91) = 21.36, p = 0.02. Approximatelya thirdof each group had have. The contentof the items of the two forms, and their scoring
an undergraduatedegree. However,abouttwo-thirdsof the Euro- were similarfor each of the subscales;the differencessimply lay
pean Americanand the Asian IndianAmericangroups and only in the tense used and the instructionsgiven.
one-thirdof the Mexican Americanshad been to graduateschool. Nine items of the MAD measurethe constructof actualcon-
About two-thirds of the Asian Indian American group earned nectedness,and 8 measureideal connectedness.This discrepancy
more than $35,000 while only one-thirdof the EuropeanAmeri- in the numberof items resultedfromthe inabilityto convertone of
cans and Mexican Americans fell in the same income bracket. the items to the ideal closeness form. The subscale of connected-
Another expected difference was in distance (in miles) from the ness combines the ability to share feelings and opinions, as well
mother, with most Mexican American women living 201-800 as to make sacrifices within the context of the adult daughter-
miles and most Asian IndianAmericanwomen living 3,001 and motherrelationship.It includes,but is not limited to the construct
above miles from theirmothers,X2(10, N = 91) = 75.54, p = 0.00. of emotional closeness, support,high felt attachment,and inti-
Unexpectedly,the groups differedby age of respondentwith the macy. It is differentfrom Collins and Read's (1990) constructof
Mexican Americangroupolder (M = 31, SD = 3.6) thanthe Euro- closeness because it furthersubsumespracticalandbehavioralin-
pean American(M = 29, SD = 3) and the Asian IndianAmerican dicatorsof showing care. However,connectednessdoes not carry
(M = 28, SD = 2.8) women, F(2,88) = 7.30, p < 0.001. The ma- with it the necessity for frequentphysical contact or verbal ges-
jority of the participantswere married(56%),most had no children turesof closeness. Higherscores indicatemore comfortwith, and
(72.5%), and the mean age of their mothers was 55.77 (SD = greaterdisplay of connectedness.This definitionis culturallyfair
6.34). The majorityof the women (82.4%)had lived 15 to 25 years and attemptsto be sensitive to the issues of immigrantwomen.
with their mother.Variablesthat did not differ by ethnicity were Scores rangedfrom 9 to 45 for actualconnectednessand 8 to 40
age of mother,maritalstatus,numberof children,and years lived for ideal connectedness.The values of alphafor the two versions
with mother. These demographic differences led to additional of the subscalewere 0.92 and 0.87 respectively.
analyses in orderto check for confoundsand are reportedfurther Items measuringinterdependenceexaminedthe daughter'sex-
ahead. pectationthathermotherwill be available,emotionallyor otherwise,
when needed. The constructof interdependenceincludes mutual
Measures
dependencyin emotional and practicalways, within the mother-
Revised Adult AttachmentScale (AAS). This scale was de- daughterrelationship.The focus on practicalhelp distinguished
vised by Collins and Read (1990) based on Hazan and Shaver's this constructfrom dependence(Collins & Read, 1990). This in-
(1987) single item scale and attachmenttheory.The AAS has 18 cludes actuallyseekingmother'shelp and advice,or feeling free to
items and three subscales:close measuresthe comfortof the sub- do so. Furthermore,no negativeconnotationsare associatedwith
ject with intimacyand closeness, and dependcontainsitems indi- these behaviors.This constructis also sensitive to cross-cultural
cating the subject's extent of trust in others and the ability to variation.Scores rangedfrom 3 to 15 with higherscores reflecting
dependon them.The presentstudydid not use the anxietysubscale greaterinterdependence.The values of Cronbach'salpha for the
scores. This scale is not normallyapplied to mother-daughterre- actualandideal subscaleswere0.86 and0.74, respectively.
330 Family Relations

This content downloaded from 192.231.202.205 on Thu, 4 Dec 2014 09:35:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
The first authoridentified the construct"trustin hierarchy." Level of Differentiationof Self Scale. The LDSS was devel-
It reflects the positive aspects of the hierarchicalrelationshipbe- oped by Haber(1990) to assess the level of differentiationof in-
tween the mother and her adult daughterin some collectivistic dividualswithin a family system. It began with the re-analysisof
cultures.The MAD appearsto be the firstinstrumentto measureit. other measuresof differentiation,and Bowenian theory (Bowen,
This construct has a negative connotation associated with it in 1972; Kerr, 1981; Kerr& Bowen, 1988). The scale has 24 items
adult-to-adult relationshipsin Whitemiddle-classAmericanculture. and uses a four-pointLikert scale, with possible scores ranging
High trustin hierarchyincludes women deferringto their mother from 24 to 96. According to Bowenian theory, individualswho
regardless of the daughter's age. This is out of respect for the score higher are thoughtto have a strongersense of self, be less
mother's wisdom, and perhapsher higher status in the family. It reactive to emotional forces within a relationship system, and
is assumed that the older woman will know what is best for her have lower levels of dysfunction(Haber,1990). In this study,the
daughter.Even if the daughterdoes not believe this, deference is LDSS was administeredtwice to measurethe respondent'sactual
expected of her in social situations. Frequently,adult daughters and ideal level of differentiation.Alpha was found to be 0.89 for
accept the positive aspects of hierarchicalinteractionswith their actualdifferentiation,and 0.91 for ideal differentiation.
mother. Scores could range from 6 to 30, with higher scores
pointing to greatertrustin hierarchy.The alpha values were 0.87 Results
for actual,and 0.77 for ideal trustin hierarchy.
One-wayANOVAswere conductedto test differencesamong
Validityof the MAD was assessed in three ways. Pre-testing the three ethnic groups.These results are reportedin Table 1 (ac-
of this scale with a pilot sample confirmed face validity. The tual) and Table2 (ideal). Groupmeans were then comparedusing
feedbackwas used to rewordsome of the items. Second, theoreti- the Tukeytest. On most variables,it was hypothesizedthat Asian
cally derived items in the subscales were used to ensure content IndianAmericanswould fall on the high end on each variable,fol-
validity of the subscales. Third, construct validity was assessed lowed by the Mexican Americans,with the EuropeanAmerican
by correlatingscores of the closeness and dependencysubscales group the lowest. In the area of differentiation,the Asian Indian
of the MAD with those obtained for the same subscales of the American women were expected to score the lowest; with the
AAS since there exists some degree of overlap between the two Mexican American women next, and the European American
sets of constructs.Furtherquantitativescores obtainedusing the women the highest.Most hypotheseswere supported.
MAD were cross-checked with responses to open-ended ques- For the participants'perceptions of the currentrelationship
tions. Correlationsbetween several of the subscales were in the between themselves and their mothers,there were significantdif-
expected range. Scores on the MAD and the AAS correlated ferences between the ethnic groups on four of the six variables
highly and significantlyfor connectedness(r = .77, p = .001), and (see Table 2). In each case, the Asian Indian American group
interdependence(r = .73, p = .00 1). The threeethnic groupsshow showed more extremescores than the EuropeanAmericangroup.
some variation.For the trust in hierarchysubscale, only content Thus, Asian IndianAmericanwomen indicatedmore connected-
and face validity were available.The correlationsfor actual and ness, F(2,88) = 4.47, p = .01, more closeness, F(2,88) = 4.17, p =
ideal closeness were 0.77 and 0.58, respectively,and those for ac- .02, more dependency,F(2,88) = 4.05, p = .02, and highertrustin
tual and ideal dependencywere 0.73 and 0.41, respectively.The hierarchy,F(2,88) = 7.14, p = .001, thandid the EuropeanAmeri-
significanceof all of these correlationsis p < .001 (one-tailed). can women. In each case, the MexicanAmericanwomen's scores
Open-endedquestions. The open-endedquestions targetthe fell in the middle, but did not differ significantlyfrom the other
subjects'understandingof closeness and dependencyin the actual two groups. Treatmentmagnitudeor the amountof varianceex-
and ideal mother-daughter relationshipin her culture.The answers plained, (Omega Squared)for actual trust in hierarchywas .12
were used to see how the respondents(especially those from the (see Table 1). This is a medium effect size and was the second
Mexican American and Indian cultures) defined the terms and largestomega squaredvalue obtainedin the results.
whether they referred to the behaviors that make up closeness A slightly differentpatternemerged for the ideal scores (see
and dependenceas well as connectednessand interdependence. Table 2). Again, the hypotheses were the same as above. Results

Table 1
One-WayANOVAsfor Actual Variablesfor Each of the ThreeEthnic Groups
Ethnic Group
EA AIA MA F Value Omega Treatment
Variable (n = 31) (n = 30) (n = 30) (2,88) Tukey Sq. Magnitude
Connectedness M 32.84 38.50 34.20 4.47** AIA > EA 0.07 Medium
SD 9.05 4.41 8.69
Closeness M 21.16 26.00 23.67 4.17* AIA > EA 0.07 Medium
SD 8.18 4.78 6.15
Interdependence M 9.32 10.73 9.80 1.23 0.01 Small
SD 3.76 2.83 3.97
Dependency M 21.06 24.77 22.07 4.05* AIA > EA 0.06 Medium
SD 6.29 3.14 5.71
TinH M 14.48 19.70 16.77 7.14*** AIA > EA 0.12 Medium
SD 5.58 4.52 5.99
Differentiation M 78.77 77.63 77.50 0.21 -0.02 Small
SD 7.53 8.19 9.36
Note: EA = EuropeanAmerican;AIA = Asian IndianAmerican;MA = Mexican American;T in H = Trustin Hierarchy.
*p<0.05. **p<o.ol. ***p<o.ool.

1999, Vol. 48, No. 3 331

This content downloaded from 192.231.202.205 on Thu, 4 Dec 2014 09:35:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Table 2
One-WayANOVAsfor Ideal Variablesfor Each of the ThreeEthnic Groups
Ethnic Group
EA AIA MA F Value Omega Treatment
Variable (n = 31) (n = 30) (n = 30) (2,88) Tukey Sq. Magnitude
Connectedness M 33.81 33.73 32.67 0.44 -0.01 Small
SD 5.70 4.12 5.78
Closeness M 26.52 28.00 23.96 9.91**** EA, AIA > MA 0.16 Large
SD 4.07 2.44 3.90
Interdependence M 11.06 11.87 11.10 0.82 0.004 Small
SD 2.87 2.32 2.99
Dependency M 24.84 25.43 22.70 4.09* AIA > MA 0.06 Medium
SD 3.94 3.50 4.22
T in H M 16.74 19.87 17.67 3.92* AIA > EA 0.06 Medium
SD 3.95 4.83 5.27
Differentiation M 89.29 83.03 83.70 4.82** EA > AIA, MA 0.08 Medium
SD 7.00 9.29 9.54
Note: EA = EuropeanAmerican;AIA = Asian IndianAmerican;MA = Mexican American;T in H = Trustin Hierarchy.
*p<0.05. **p<O.Ol. ****p<O.OOOl.

showed significantdifferencesbetween the ethnic groups on four Two discriminantfunction analyses were conducted,one for
of the six variables. These were closeness, F(2,88) = 9.91, p = the actual(see Tables3a and 3b) and one for the ideal scores (see
.0001, dependency, F(2,88) = 4.09, p = .02, trust in hierarchy, Tables4a and 4b). Each analysis helped identify which of the six
F(2,88) = 3.92, p = .02, and differentiation,F(2,88) = 4.82, p = .01. variablesdifferentiatedthe most between the threeethnic groups.
However,the hypotheseswere only partlysupported,with a more For the actual variables, two discriminant functions were ex-
complex patternemerging than with the differences on the per- tractedwith a combinedX2(l2, N = 91) = 22.14, p = .03, but only
ceived actual levels of these variables.Overall, the hypothesized Function 1 was significant. The two functions accounted for
differencesemerged most clearly for the Asian IndianAmerican 89.91% and 10.09%respectively,of the between-groupvariabil-
group,i.e. it was most significantlydifferentin the expecteddirec- ity. Function 1 (actual) was high on trustin hierarchy,connected-
tion from the EuropeanAmericangroupon trustin hierarchy,and ness, closeness, dependency, and interdependence, i.e., all
ideal differentiation.Unexpectedly,both the Asian IndianAmeri- variablesbut differentiation.Function 1 was negativelyassociated
can and the EuropeanAmerican groups desired more closeness with the EuropeanAmericangroup,but positively with the Asian
thanthe MexicanAmericangroup.For ideal differentiation,signif- IndianAmericangroup.Since Function2 (actual) was not signif-
icant differencesemergedfor the Mexican Americangroupin the icant, this analysis was unableto separatethe Mexican American
expected direction.The effect size for ideal closeness was found groupfrom the EuropeanAmericanand the Asian IndianAmeri-
to be 0.16 which is considereda large effect size (see Table2). It can groups. Overall, the percent of subjects correctly classified
was also the largestomega squaredvalue obtainedin this study. by ethnic group was 45.05%. For the ideal variables, two dis-
It is worth noting that there were some differencesbetween criminantfunctions were extracted,of which both were signifi-
the results obtainedabove using the AAS (Collins & Read, 1990) cant with a combined X2(l2, N = 91) = 37.41, p = .000. For the
subscales (close and depend) comparedwith the MAD subscales second function,x2(5, N = 91) = 12.72,p = .03. The two functions
(connectednessand interdependence)for both the actual and the accountedfor 67.62% and 32.38% respectively,of the between-
ideal analyses. These differencesmay be the result of the cultur- groupvariability.Function 1 (ideal) includedhigh levels of close-
ally sensitivecontentof the MAD. ness, dependency, interdependence,and connectedness. Again,
differentiationwas separatedout from most other variablesbut

Table 3a Table4a
DiscriminantAnalysisfor the ThreeEthnic Groupsby Actual Variables DiscriminantAnalysisfor the ThreeEthnic Groupsby Ideal Variables
GroupCentroids GroupCentroids
European Asian Indian Mexican European Asian Indian Mexican
Discriminant % of Chi-Square American American American Discriminant % of Chi-Square American American American
Function Variance (df) (n = 31) (n = 30) (n = 30) Function Variance (df) (n = 31) (n = 30) (n = 30)
1 89.91 22.14 (12)* -0.56 0.66 -0.08 1 67.62 37.41*** (12) -0.01 0.70 -0.70
2 10.09 2.45 (5) 0.14 0.09 -0.24 2 32.38 12.72* (5) 0.55 -0.28 -0.29
*p<.05. *p < .05. ***p < .001.

Table 3b Table4b
CorrelationsBetween DiscriminatingVariablesand DiscriminantFunctions CorrelationsBetweenDiscriminatingVariablesand DiscriminantFunctions
Variables Function 1 Function2 Variables Function 1 Function2
Trustin Hierarchy .79* -.12 Closeness .81* .19
Connectedness .62* .31 Dependency .50* .25
Closeness .60* -.25 Connectedness .20* -.18
Dependency .59* .24 Interdependence .14* .14
Interdependence .33* .06 Differentiation -.06 .82*
Differentiation -.10 .27* Trustin Hierarchy .41 -.45*
*Denotes the largercorrelationbetween a variableand the two functions. *Denotes the largercorrelationbetween a variableand the two functions.

332 Family Relations

This content downloaded from 192.231.202.205 on Thu, 4 Dec 2014 09:35:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
this time trustin hierarchywas also excluded. Function 1 (ideal) spect for the other,"was by far the most common response and
was negativelyassociatedwith the Mexican Americangroup,and was endorsedby the women in all threegroupsin the actualcate-
separatedit from the Asian Indian American group, which was gories. Overall,the Asian IndianAmericanwomen used termslike
positively associatedwith it. Function2 (ideal) includedhigh dif- "trust"and "being dependable,"suggesting securityto be impor-
ferentiationand low trust in hierarchy.The EuropeanAmerican tant in the relationship.They also idealized their own relationship
group was highly positively associated with this function, while and valued communication and friendship. Mexican American
the Asian Indian American and Mexican groups had a mildly women's relationshipsalso included availability,but with a focus
negativeassociationwith it. Overall,the percentof cases correctly on honest communication,and doing activitiestogether.Thus they
classified by ethnic group was 58.24%. Both the actual and the emphasizedemotional and practicalintimacy.They too idealized
ideal discriminantfunctionswere able to classify the Asian Indian their own relationship with their mothers. European American
American group with greatest accuracy.These percentageswere women balancedautonomywith caring.They mentionedhonesty
66.7 and 63.3 respectively(comparedto chance being at 33.3%). and caring,but less dependabilityand respect.Althoughthe same
numberof responses emerged, there were fewer categories with
As expected, there were differencesin demographiccharac-
five or more responses in the ideal area.Most strikingis the lack
teristics between the three ethnic groups. As a check, two-way
of informationon the ideal, with many women assertingthattheir
ANOVAs were conducted, with the independentvariablesbeing
own relationshipwas ideal, or that they did not know an ideal re-
ethnicity and each of these categorical variables (education, in-
lationship.
come, and distance) for all the dependentvariables included in
the study. For the continuous variable (age), an ANCOVA was Twenty-twocategories emerged from responses to the ques-
conducted. The categorical variables were collapsed into fewer tion about actual dependency. Each of the groups mentioned
categoriesin orderto have adequatenumbersin each cell. In gen- making one's own decisions, and keeping dependenceto a mini-
eral, the independentvariablethat showed largersignificantmain mum in the mother-daughterrelationship.In addition,the Asian
effects was ethnic group. In other words, it appearsthat possible IndianAmericangroup'srelationshipsseemed more hierarchical,
confoundingfactors such as income and educationare not as im- and referredto the importance of mother's advice, and getting
portantas ethnicity in understandingconnectedness, closeness, emotional support.The Mexican Americanwomen thoughtit im-
interdependence,dependency,trust in hierarchy,and differentia- portant to "be dependable" but not be dependent. Thus they
tion. These findingsconfirmwhat was expected. sought to give ratherthan receive in their relationship.The Euro-
pean Americangroupfocused on autonomy,especiallyin financial
ContentAnalysis matters.In the case of ideal dependency,a total of 25 categories
were formed.Again, severalparticipantsdid not offer much infor-
The purpose of using content analysis in this study was: (a) mationfor this question.The most frequentanswerwas that their
to obtaininformationon culturaldifferencesin the definitionof the own relationshipwas ideal, or that they did not know an ideal re-
constructs of closeness and dependency,and (b) to cross-check lationship. All women felt that ideally, dependence should be
the validity of the quantitativefindings. Since standardizedin- minimized. The differences lay in the Asian Indian American
strumentsrely on linguisticconsensus,open-endedquestionswere women's mothersgiving advice, the Mexican Americanmothers
used to see how closeness and dependencyare defined across cul- giving emotional support, and the European American group
tures. In four open-endedquestions,participantswere asked their being dependable. To summarize, both similarities and differ-
opinion of what was actual closeness, ideal closeness, actual de- ences exist between the responses of the three ethnic groups that
pendency, and ideal dependency in the context of the adult correspondto quantitativeresults.
daughter-motherrelationshipin their culture.Insteadof using the
word "ideal" the questions on ideal closeness and dependency
Discussion
asked about the kind of relationshipthe person wished to have
with her own mother. The authors attemptedto study any differences associated
This study used manifest (surface)content, ratherthan inter- with culture in the mother-adultdaughterrelationshipalong the
pretation,to improvereliability(Babbie, 1989). Validityis ensured dimensions of closeness, reliability, and collectivism. Samples
by, first, the directnessof the open-endedquestions. Second, the were chosen based on the participants'ability to communicate
categoriesthatemergedfrom the responsesmatchedcertainitems fluently in English and homogeneity on variables such as age.
of the standardizedmeasuresused elsewhere in the questionnaire Attemptsto limit variabilityhad to be balancedwith inherentdif-
packet. ferences between the three populationson variablessuch as dis-
tance from mother.The groups expectedly differed on education
Categories were developed based on the participants' re- and income but the authorsattemptedto reduce between group
sponses to questions. All responses were tabulatedfor frequency differencesby selecting samples with high educationand income
of occurrenceof categorieswithin each ethnic group.(No outside for all groups.However,this led to the three groupsnot being en-
judges were used for these two steps.) Kerlinger(1986) termsthis tirelyrepresentativeof theirrespectivecultures.Thus these results
the "nominalmethod."To capturethe similaritiesand differences may not be generalizable to all members of a particularethnic
between the ethnic groups,responsesthatoverallwere mentioned group, especially those who are most dissimilar to the study's
by five or more women were included. (A complete set of re- participants.Second, demographicdifferencesbetween the three
sponsesis availablefrom the firstauthor.)Also calculatedwere the groups need to be further examined. The European American
rank orders of those responses in terms of frequencywithin that groupwas more educatedthanthe MexicanAmericanwomen but
ethnic group. The rank orderreflected all the possible responses both groupsearnedthe same amountof money.This might be be-
ratherthanjust the responsesshown in the tables. cause about 10% of the EuropeanAmericanwomen in the study
For actual and ideal closeness, a total of 27 categoriesof re- were also part-timestudents and may have been employed less
sponses emerged from the participants'responses. "Showingre- than full time. Additionally,the Mexican Americanwomen were
1999, Vol. 48, No. 3 333

This content downloaded from 192.231.202.205 on Thu, 4 Dec 2014 09:35:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
older and may thereforehold higherjob positions with more pay. Mexican American daughters were older than the other two
Asian Indian American women's higher reported income may groups.This may makethem less likely to dependon theirmother.
come from being marriedand in a two income household.While Second, based on the content analysis it may be speculatedthat
intra-groupvariabilityin income, education, and distance from the MexicanAmericanwomen have immigrantmotherswho may
mother were found to not have a confoundingeffect on the de- be dependenton theirmore acculturateddaughtersthanvice-versa.
pendent variables, these differences need further examination. Overall, these results suggest the importance of differences in
Third,the Asian IndianAmericangroupseemed to consist of im- interdependencein adultdaughter-mother relationshipsacross the
migrantswhose motherslived in India.Additionaldata aboutthe three cultures. While intergenerationalfamily therapy theories
immigrationstatus of all women and its impact on the mother- (Bowen, 1972; Harvey & Bray, 1991; Harvey, Curry, & Bray,
daughterrelationshipwould be of interest. 1991; Kerr, 1981; Kerr& Bowen, 1988; Williamson, 1981) em-
The strengthof this study lies in the fact that the hypotheses phasize autonomy over dependence in family relationshipsthe
were derivedfrom theory and previous literature.Challenges re- currentfindings support a more ethnic-focused examination of
lated to conceptualequivalencewere dealt with and the new in- interdependence. Also, the application of these findings may
strument(MAD) led to the use of constructsthat had not been need to take into account factors like level of acculturationof
previouslymeasured.In spite of complex samplingand conceptual motherand age of the daughter.
equivalenceissues, a numberof hypotheses were supported.Re- The results supportedthe hypotheses that each of these cul-
sults showed the Asian Indian American group's score was dif- tures encourages different degrees of hierarchical interactions
ferent from that of at least one other group in the expected (Julianet al., 1994). Though the Asian IndianAmericanwomen
direction.This matched the findings that showed that the Asian were higher on educationand income and thus hold the potential
IndianAmericangroupcould be classified most accuratelyon the for more autonomyand less hierarchythan the other two groups,
basis of the discriminantfunctions;this groupwas most different they appearto behave contrarily.Clearly,the Asian IndianAmer-
of the three. ican women believe in deferringto one's mother and accepting
For actual connectedness and closeness, the Asian Indian her wisdom, both in the current relationship and as a general
Americangroupwas higher,andfor ideal closeness,both the Euro- value more than European American women. The open-ended
pean American and the Asian Indian American groups scored answers also elicited similar responses. This demonstratesthe
significantlyhigher than the Mexican American women. This is close associationbetween dependencyand trustin hierarchyand
especially significant since the Mexican American group lived points to the common core of "trust"shared by both. Women
geographicallyclosest to theirmothers,followed by the European who are more comfortabledependingon their mothersalso seem
American and then the Asian Indian American group. These re- likely to be more comfortablewith, and acceptingof hierarchyin
sults suggest that geographicaldistance does not hinder mother- that relationship. The interconnections between the two con-
daughter connectedness for the Asian Indian American group. structs are a way to further explore intergenerational theory.
While the results for the EuropeanAmerican and the Mexican Given thatthe Indiancultureis high on trustin hierarchy,the role
Americangroupsarenot as clear-cut,furtherresearchis warranted. of individuationand terminationof hierarchyin healthy adultre-
Contentanalysisfindingsmatchquantitativeresults.The European lationships needs to be reexamined. These results demonstrate
Americangroupemphasizedboth autonomyand caringwhile the that,contraryto prevailingtheory(e.g., Bowen, 1972; Williamson,
Asian Indian American group emphasized a secure relationship. 1981), certainculturesvalue trust in hierarchyin the adult child
This relates to the differencesin actual connectednessand close- and parentrelationship.
ness between the two groups. The Mexican Americangroup ap- On actualdifferentiation,it appearsthatthe threeculturesare
peared to value practicaland emotional closeness. Results show more similarthan different.However,results of ideal differentia-
that such culturaldifferences were picked up by the MAD and tion show the European American culture to be significantly
not the AAS. Significantly,the qualitativeresults show that both higherthanthe others,and supportthe workby Julianet al. (1994)
the degree and the definitionsof closeness are culturebound. At and Spiegel (1982), who comparedthe threegroupson character-
a theoreticallevel, the resultsprovidesupportfor the feminist ob- istics such as individuality versus collective orientation.While
ject relationsargumentsof the importanceof the mother-daughter the groups do not differ in practicein the area of differentiation,
bond in the lives of adult women (Chodorow,1978; Eichenbaum the EuropeanAmericangroupendorsesit as a desirableideal.
& Orbach,1988; Walters,1988). The discriminantanalysis underscoredthis point. High dif-
For both actual and ideal dependency,results varied by the ferentiationloadeddifferentlyfrom most othervariables,(also see
measures used. For interdependence,the three groups were not Davis & Jones, 1992), as a separatefunction for actual, and in
differentfrom each other.Possibly,the MAD was sensitiveto dif- combination with low trust in hierarchy for ideal variables. In
ferentaspects of interdependenceacross culturesbut led to equiv- both instances, the relationshipcharacteristicsthat separatedthe
alent means. On the AAS, the Asian Indian American group Asian IndianAmericanand the EuropeanAmericangroup were
scored higher than the EuropeanAmericanwomen on actual de- diametricallyopposed to each other,with a more complex pattern
pendency,and the Mexican Americangroupon ideal. In the con- emerging for the Mexican American group. The results imply
tent analysis, the Asian Indian American women were most that within certain cultures it is entirely possible to have a high
likely to cite seeking advice and supportfrom their mother,mak- degree of connectednessand interdependenceand high differenti-
ing them higher on dependency, while the EuropeanAmerican ation within the mother-daughterrelationship.Second, only the
women stressed non-dependency,especially in financial matters. EuropeanAmericangroup saw high differentiationand low trust
The Mexican American women preferredbeing dependableto- in hierarchyas an ideal statebut these sentimentswere not shared
wardtheir mothers,but did not want to dependon them in return. by all cultures.A universalapplicationof the principlesof high
Thus, their results on the AAS might indicatethese women's low differentiation (Bowen, 1972) and low hierarchy (Williamson,
inclinationto be dependenton theirmother.Demographically,the 1981) to all families is contraindicatedby these results.
334 Family Relations

This content downloaded from 192.231.202.205 on Thu, 4 Dec 2014 09:35:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
The sample used in this study was small and non-random; chy" are unique to a culture? These issues are worth pursuing.
this may have affectedthe validity of the results. Some of the dif- Furtherrefinementof the instruments,particularlythe use of item
ferences in the results of the two measurescould come from lack analysis for the MAD, could result in the creationof scales that
of conceptualequivalence.Quantitativedifferences in scores are are culturallyspecific, somewhat differentfrom each other, and
less likely between the cultureswhere differencesare of a quali- achieve conceptualequivalence.
tative kind. For example, it is possible that the MAD tappedcul- In severalof the quantitativeanalyses,the means of the Mex-
turallyspecific and practicalways of denotingconnectednessand ican American group appearedto fall in between the other two
interdependencefor each of the three groups, so that the total groups. This group's membershipwas least accuratelypredicted
scores for the threegroupswere similar. in the discriminantanalysis. This shows that furtherresearchis
Clinical Implications warrantedas the level of acculturationor some other variable(s)
may mediate the nature of the mother-daughter relationship
The results are significant in that they supportthe idea that across cultures. It is also possible that the MAD and the AAS
the theories,namelyintergenerational, feministobjectrelationsand were less culturallysensitive to the Mexican Americangroup.To
attachment,apply differentlyto the three culturalgroups studied. remedy this, more items derivedfrom the categoriesemphasized
Despite the non-randomnatureof the sampling, the results lead by the Mexican American women in the content analysis could
to the conclusion thatthe theoriesmay not be universallyrelevant be addedto futureversionsof the MAD.
to all culturalgroups. The differences between cultures suggest
that researchersand practitionersworking with culturallydiverse
families need use caution when evaluatingrelationships.In this
References
sample, the European American women desired closeness, but Arcana,J. (1979). Our mother'sdaughters.London:The Women'sPress.
wanted less dependence while the Mexican American women Babbie, E. (1989). The practice of social research (5th ed.). Belmont, CA:
wanted some closeness and were comfortablewith their mother Wadsworth.
Bowen, M. (1972). Towardthe differentiationof a self in one's own family. In
depending on them. The Asian Indian American women were J. L. Framo (Ed.), Family interaction:A dialogue betweenfamily researchers
very high on connectedness,closeness, dependence,and trust in andfamily therapists(pp. 111-173). New York:Springer.
hierarchy.When evaluating non-EuropeanAmerican families, Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-childattachmentand healthy human
practitionersshould be awareof the negativecorrelationbetween development.New York:Basic.
the constructsof trustin hierarchyand differentiation.Practitioners Boyd, C. J. (1989). Mothers and daughters:A discussion of theory and research.
Journalof Marriageand the Family,51, 291-301.
need to explore alternative,culturallyspecific, healthy forms of Bumiller, E. (1990). May you be the motherof a hundredsons. New York:Ran-
adultdaughterand motherrelationships.This may include a will- dom House.
ingness to modify how one defines ideal relationships,and what Buriel, R. (1993). Childrearingorientationsin Mexican Americanfamilies: The
one considers functional levels of connectedness,hierarchy,etc. influence of generationand socioculturalfactors.Journal of Marriage and the
(Other intergenerationalrelationshipsin the family may follow Family,55, 987-1000.
Chodorow,N. (1978). Reproductionof mothering:Psychoanalysis and the soci-
these patternstoo.) Results also point to the importanceof family ology of gender. Berkeley:Universityof CaliforniaPress.
practitioners'and researchers'need to explore meanings of con- Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment,workingmodels and rela-
structs for the family, ratherthan assuming linguistic consensus tionship quality in dating couples. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychol-
across cultures. For example, practitioners should evaluate ogy, 58, 644-663.
whether or not their clients' notions of relationshipsatisfaction Davis, B., & Jones, L. C. (1992). Differentiationof self and attachmentamong
adultdaughters.Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 13, 321-331.
(as it is understoodwithin their culture)are the same as those of Delgado-Gaitan(1993). Parentingin two generationsof Mexican Americanfam-
the therapist's.This would ensure a more culturallysensitive de- ilies. InternationalJournalof BehavioralDevelopment,16, 409-427.
livery of services. Dhruvarajan,V. (1993). Ethnic cultural retention and transmission among first
generationHindu Asian Indians in a Canadianprairie city. Journal of Com-
The findings suggest thatthe therapistneeds to pay attention parative FamilyStudies,24, 63-79.
to the adultdaughter-motherrelationshipboth as a source of sup- Diaz-Guerrero,R. (1987). Historicalsocioculturalpremises and ethnic socializa-
port and difficulty. Women from all three cultures wrote about tion. In J. S. Phinney& M. J. Rotheram(Eds.), Children'sethnic socialization:
the high degree of felt attachmentand satisfactionthatthey derive Pluralismand development(pp. 239-250). NewburyPark:Sage.
from this relationship.This points to the enduringsignificanceof Dillman, D. A. (1978). Mail and telephonesurveys.New York:Wiley.
Eichenbaum,L., & Orbach,S. (1988). Between women.New York:Viking.
the mother-daughterintergenerationalbond.
Ewing, K. P. (1991). Can psychoanalytictheories explain the intrapsychicauton-
omy and interpersonalengagementin the extendedfamily.Ethos, 19, 131-160.
FutureResearch Falicov, C. J. (1982). Mexican families. In M. McGoldrick, J. K. Pearce, &
Follow-upresearchis needed using more representativesam- J. Giordano (Eds.), Ethnicity and family therapy (pp. 134-163). New York:
Guilford.
ples for each ethnic group.This would allow us to drawmore au- Fischer,L. R. (1991). Between mothersand daughters.Marriage and FamilyRe-
thoritativeconclusions regardinggroup differences. Also, more view, 16, 237-248.
informationabout the women, such as their immigrationhistory Friday,N. (1977). My mother/myself.New York:Delacorte Press.
and that of their mothers would strengthen the study. Further, Goldberg, S. (1991). Recent developments in attachmenttheory and research.
comparingthese women with their counterpartsin their country CanadianJournalof Psychiatry,36, 393-400.
of origin would add to our understandingof the mother-daughter Haber,J. (1990). The Haberlevel of differentiationof self scale. In 0. Strickland
& C. Waltz (Eds.), Measuring of nursing outcomes: Vol.4. Measuring client
relationship. self-care and coping skills (pp. 320-329). New York:Springer.
This study stirs up, but does not resolve, an importantissue. Harvey,D. M., & Bray, J. H. (1991). An evaluationof an intergenerationalthe-
Are constructslike closeness quantitativelydifferent,but compa- ory of personal development:Family process determinantsof psychological
and health distress.Journal of FamilyPsychology,4, 42-69.
rable, for each cultureor are they qualitativelydifferentfor each Harvey,D. M., Curry,C. J., & Bray, J. H. (1991). Individuation/intimacyin in-
culture, and hence incomparable? Do some constructs have a tergenerational relationships and health: Patterns across two generations.
"commoncore" across cultures,while otherslike "trustin hierar- Journalof FamilyPsychology,5, 204-236.

1999, Vol. 48, No. 3 335

This content downloaded from 192.231.202.205 on Thu, 4 Dec 2014 09:35:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Hazan, C., & Shaver,P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualizedas an attachment Stevenson-Hinde,J. (1990). Attachmentwithin family systems: An overview.In-
process. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology,52, 511-524. fant Mental Health Journal, 11, 218-227.
Hazan, C., & Shaver,P. R. (1994a). Attachmentas an organizationalframework Stopes-Roe, M., & Cochrane,R. (1990). The child-rearingvalues of Asian and
for researchon close relationships.Psychological Inquiry,5, 1-22. British parentsand young people: An inter-ethnicand inter-generationalcom-
Hazan,C., & Shaver,P. R. (1994b). Deeper into attachmenttheory.Psychological parisonin the evaluationof Kohn's 13 qualities.BritishJournalof Social Psy-
Inquiry,5, 68-79. chology, 29, 149-160.
Julian,T. W., McKenry,P. C., & McKelvey, M. W. (1994). Culturalvariationsin Straus, M. A. (1969). Phenomenalidentity and conceptual equivalence of mea-
parenting:Perceptionsof Caucasian,African American, Hispanic, and Asian surementin cross-nationalcomparativeresearch.Journal of Marriage and the
Americanparents.FamilyRelations, 43, 30-37. Family,31, 233-239.
Kakar,S. (1981). The inner world: A psychoanalytic study of childhood and so- Thompson,L., & Walker,A. J. (1984). Mothersand daughters:Aid patternsand
ciety in India. New Delhi: OxfordUniversityPress. attachment.Journalof Marriageand the Family,46, 313-322.
Kerlinger, F. N. (1986). Foundations of behavioral research (3rd ed.). Fort Uribe, F. M. T., LeVine, R. A., & LeVine, S. E. (1993). Maternaleducationand
Worth:Holt, Rinehartand Winston. maternalbehaviorin Mexico: Implicationsfor the changing characteristicsof
Kerr,M. E. (1981). Family systems theory and therapy.In A. S. Gurman& D. P. Mexican immigrantsto the United States. InternationalJournalof Behavioral
Kniskern (Eds.), Handbook of family therapy. (pp. 226-264). New York: Development,16, 395-408.
Brunner/Mazel. Vega, W. A. (1990). Hispanic families in the 1980s: A decade of research.Jour-
Kerr,M. E., & Bowen, M. (1988). Familyevaluation.New York:Norton. nal of Marriageand the Family,52, 1015-1024.
Knudson-Martin,C. (1994). The female voice: Applications of Bowen's family Walker,A. J., & Thompson,L. (1983). Intimacyand intergenerational aid and con-
systems theory.Journalof Marital and Family Therapy,20, 35-46. tact among mothers and daughters.Journal of Marriage and the Family,45,
Lang, A. M., & Brody,E. M. (1983). Characteristicsof middle aged daughtersand 841-849.
help to theirelderlymothers.Journalof Marriageand the Family,45, 193-202. Walters,M. (1988). Mothersand daughters.In M. Walters,B. Carter,P. Papp, &
La Sorsa, V. A., & Fodor, I. G. (1990). Adolescent daughter/mid-life mother 0. Silverstein(Eds.), The invisibleweb (pp. 35-5 1). New York:Guilford.
dyad. Psychology of WomenQuarterly,14, 593-606. Williamson, D. S. (1981). Personalauthorityvia terminationof the intergenera-
Lee, G. R., & Haas, L. (1993). Comparativemethods in family research.In P. G. tional hierarchicalboundary:A "new"stage in the family life cycle. Journal
Boss, W. J. Doherty, R. LaRossa, W. R. Schumm, & S. K. Steinmetz (Eds.), of Marital and Family Therapy,7, 441-452.
Sourcebookoffamily theories and methods:A contextualapproach (pp. 117-
131). New York:Plenum.
Mudita Rastogi, Ph.D., is Assistant Professor at the Illinois
Naidoo, J. C., & Davis, J. C. (1988). CanadianSouth Asian women in transition:
A dualistic view of life. Journal of ComparativeFamilyStudies, 19, 311-327. School of ProfessionalPsychology and Affiliate Therapistat the
Neisser, E. (1973). Mothersand daughters.New York:Harperand Row. Family Instituteat NorthwesternUniversity.Her research areas
O'Connor, P. (1990). The adult mother/daughterrelationship:A uniquely and include gender,intergenerational,and cross-culturalissues.
universallyclose relationship?Sociological Review,38, 293-323.
Rastogi, M. (1995). Adult daughters'perceptionof the mother-daughterrelation-
ship: A cross-cultural comparison. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,Texas
Karen S. Wampler,Ph.D., is Professor of Marriageand Family
Tech University,Lubbock,Texas. Therapyin the Departmentof HumanDevelopment and Family
Rastogi, M. (1998). Mother and Adult Daughter (MAD) questionnaire. Manu- Studies, TexasTech University.She currentlyserves as a member
script submittedfor publication. of the Commission on Accreditation for Marriage and Family
Segal, U. A. (1991). Culturalvariables in Asian Indian families. The Journal of TherapyEducation.
ContemporaryHumanServices, 72, 233-242.
Sheth, M. (1995). Asian IndianAmericans.In P. G. Min (Ed.), Asian Americans:
Contemporarytrendsand issues (pp. 169-198). ThousandOaks, CA: Sage. Received 10-31-1997
Silverstein,0. (1985). Mrs. Grayand Susan.Networker,72-74. Revised & Resubmitted9-25-1998
Spiegel, J. (1982). An ecological model of ethnic families. In M. McGoldrick, Second Revise & Resubmit4-12-1999
J. K. Pearce, & J. Giordano(Eds.), Ethnicityand family therapy (pp. 31-54).
New York: Guilford.
Accepted 4-16-1999
Sprecher,S., & Chandak,R. (1992). Attitudesabout arrangedmarriagesand dat-
ing among men and women from India. Free Inquiry in Creative Sociology,
20, 59-69.

Access organizations, practitioners,


professors, & students interested in
family issues!

Rent the Family Relations subscribermailing list for only


$110 per thousand names

rhi ContactNCFRtoll free at 888-781-9331 U 612-781-9331


NCFR * Fax: 612-781-9348 * E-mail:ncfr3989@ncfr.com
FR 799

336 Family Relations

This content downloaded from 192.231.202.205 on Thu, 4 Dec 2014 09:35:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like