You are on page 1of 103

Personality Neuroscience and Dark Values

Master Degree Project in Cognitive Neuroscience


One year 30 ECTS
Spring term 2014

Björn Persson

Supervisor: India Morrison


Supervisor: Petri Kajonius
Examiner: Judith Annett
Personality Neuroscience and Dark Values
Submitted by Björn Persson to the University of Skövde as a final year project to-

wards the degree of M.Sc. in the School of Bioscience. The project has been super-
vised by Petri Kajonius and India Morrison.

12 August, 2014

I hereby certify that all material in this final year project which is not my own work
has been identified and that no work is included for which a degree has already been
conferred on me.

Signature:

II
Acknowledgements

Many thanks to Petri, for your friendliness, patience, and invitation to this project. I also
extend my gratitude towards India Morrison, for your supervision. Most of all, thanks
to Gabriel Sjödin, for his friendship.

”...but there is no shelter”


— R, Watchmen

”There is no fate that cannot be surmounted by scorn.”


— A C, The Myth of Sisyphus

III
Table of Contents

Acknowledgements III

Table of Contents IV

Abstract VII

Introduction 1

Theoretical Foundation 2

The Dark Triad . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3

Machiavellianism . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4

Narcissism . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8

Psychopathy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13

Personality: Big Five, and HEXACO . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 18

The Biological Basis of Personality . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 20

Empathy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 22

Schwartz Value Orientation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 26

General Methods 28

Design . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29

Study I 31

Introduction 32

Methods 33

Participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33

IV
Instruments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 34

Results 36

Descriptive statistics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 36

Dark Triad and Schwartz’s Value Types . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 36

Discussion 39

Traits and Values in the Dark Triad Components . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 40

Dark Values and Morality . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 41

Conclusion and Future Studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 42

Study II 43

Introduction 44

Methods 45

Participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 45

Instruments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 46

Results 48

Descriptive Statistics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 48

Moderating Dark Traits . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49

Controlling for Big Six and Dark Triad . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 50

Principal Component Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 52

Discussion 52

General Discussion 54

V
Conclusions from Empirical Studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 54

Future Directions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 56

References 59

Appendix A 90

Appendix B 91

Appendix C 92

Appendix D 93

Appendix E 94

Appendix F 95

Appendix G 96

VI
Abstract

Personality neuroscience offers a new theory of the biological basis of personality traits. It

involves the use of neuroscientific methodologies to study individual differences in behav-

ior, motivation, emotion, and cognition. Personality psychology has contributed much

in identifying the important dimensions of personality, but relatively little to understand-

ing the biological sources of those dimensions. In recent years, personality psychology

has become the foundation for the study of personality disorders, and by extension, neu-

roscience. First, I provide a theoretical foundation for the neuroscience of normal and

abnormal personality traits. Second, I conduct two empirical studies on deviant person-

ality traits captured by the Dark Triad (i.e., Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopa-

thy) and relate them to universal human values. Study I shows that darker personalities

endorse values that are self-enhancing, and that justify self-serving behavior. Study II in-

vestigates the relationship between the aforementioned constructs and empathy based on

the idea that empathy is an important moderating factor of dark traits. In the discussion,

suggestions for future studies in neuroscience are presented, as well as some limitations

relating to the constructs.

Keywords: Personality neuroscience, Dark Triad, Schwartz Universal Values,

Empathy

VII
Running head: PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 1

Personality Neuroscience and Dark Values

Personality psychology describes how personality traits are expressed in different

behaviors. In recent years, the emerging field of personality neuroscience has offered a

new theory of the biological basis of personality traits. It involves the use of

neuroscientific methodologies to study individual differences in behavior, motivation,

emotion, and cognition. Branched together with the study of personality disorders –

which has previously been exclusively a matter for psychiatry – personality neuroscience

is becoming popular way of studying both normal and abnormal personality traits.

The integration of personality psychology and neuroscience is not coincidental.

Respectable psychiatrists have long argued that ”[b]y re-defining the foundation of

psychiatry as clinical neuroscience, we also accelerate the integration of psychiatry with

the rest of medicine” (Insel & Quirion, 2005, p. 5), thus acknowledging the

fundamental insight from contemporary neuroscience that mental illnesses are brain

disorders. Moreover, psychiatric journals are shifting towards a neurobiological

perspective (Widiger, 2013), and empirical evidence has shown that psychiatrists in

training have positive attitudes about the relevance of the brain (Fung, Akil, Widge,

Roberts, & Etkin, 2014). The circle is coming to its close with the emergence of

personality neuroscience (DeYoung & Gray, 2009), a field dedicated towards observing

individual differences in both brain structure and function.

This thesis consists of a theoretical framework and two empirical studies. The main

topic is the Dark Triad (i.e., Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy), which is

theory that describes exploitative, antisocial, and manipulative personality traits. The

first study is explorative, as no other studies have investigated the relationship between

dark personality traits and values. The research questions posed in the first study are:
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 2

what do individuals with dark personality traits value? Do individuals with higher

scores on dark traits value other things than low scorers? The result of the first study is

something we decided to call Dark Values, due to the strong relationship between the

dark traits, and values that justify self-serving behavior, even at the expense of others.

The second study is an expansion of the first study. Its purpose is to examine

whether empathy moderates the relationship between personality and values. A lack of

empathy is considered the core of many darker personality traits. Thus, we ask whether

more empathic individuals have more prosocial values? Is the relationship between

dark personality traits moderated by empathy? These questions are important to better

understand why people behave and feel the way they do. People with dark traits

unsurprisingly behave in various antisocial ways that ultimately hurt other people.

Understanding more about how and why such people function the way they do is a

fundamental step in preventing such behavior. This thesis is a small step towards that

goal. In the discussion the results are discussed and conclusions are drawn. Future

directions and study proposals are considered.

Theoretical Foundation

This section serves the purpose of providing a theoretical framework for the two

studies. Even though the two studies could stand by themselves, the coming subsections

provide a much broader picture than is available in the articles. The first section

describes the main topic, namely the three components of the Dark Triad. The second

section describes the most common measurement of normal personality traits: the

Five-Factor Model (FFM). The third section describes empathy focusing on the most

commonly used self-report instrument, the Interpersonal Reactivity Index. The final

section describes personal values, which is the dependent variable in this thesis.
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 3

Before presenting the contents of the constructs, the hypothesized relation between

them is important to acknowledge. The Big Five, and Dark Triad are both theories

pertaining to personality traits, the former referring to normal traits, and the latter to

deviant traits. Empathy is used as a potential moderator between traits and values, the

latter being the dependent variable selected for these two studies. The Big Five is

essentially used as a control in both studies: darker personality traits are both

theoretically and empirically used as an extension of normal traits. Thus, the inclusion

of the Big Five is to be sufficiently rigorous, but the main focus is on deviant, and

anti-social behaviors captured by the Dark Triad. Personal values are chosen as a

convenient dependent variable, as values have previously been used in research on

personality disorders (Locke, 2000).

The Dark Triad

The Dark Triad is a non-pathological personality construct consisting of three

different components: Machiavellianism, subclinical narcissism, and subclinical

psychopathy (Paulhus & Williams, 2002). The term subclinical refer to individuals in

the general population that share certain characteristics with the clinical

(institutionalized or imprisoned) population. There is often an implicit assumption that

subclinical refers to a less severe version of a disorder, but such claims are not

necessarily true, being that subclinical samples naturally cover wider ground and thus

also include extreme cases (Furnham, Richards, & Paulhus, 2013). It should be noted

that Machiavellianism is not a disorder, which is the case for narcissism and

psychopathy. As the reader will notice, the constructs comprising the Dark Triad have

all been investigated within neuroscience, but the Dark Triad itself has remained

outside: within work and personality psychology.


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 4

Machiavellianism

The concept of Machiavellianism is derived from the writings of the 16th-century

Italian political philosophers, Niccolò Machiavelli. In his treatises, The Prince

(1532/1992) and The Discourses (1531/2007), Machiavelli presented his view of people

as untrustworthy, self-serving, and malevolent, and argued that a ruler would better

maintain power by utilizing exploitative and deceitful tactics. About four centuries

later, during the 1950s and 60s, psychologist Richard Christie became interested in the

relation between personality and political ideology, and this led to an investigation of

Machiavellian attitudes such as having a cynical view of human nature, a low level of

interpersonal affect, and minimum concern for conventional morality. Ultimately, it led

Christie towards the development of a theory which proposed that the tendency to

accept Machiavelli’s world view was a measurable individual difference variable

(Christie & Geis, 1970).

In the landmark manuscript Studies in Machiavellianism, Christie and Geis (1970)

described how they collected statements from Machiavelli’s writings and started asking

people how much they agreed with them, and why. Eventually, after much item

discrimination and analysis, this became the questionnaire that has now been in use for

over 40 years. Christie and Geis developed five renditions of the MACH scale. The vast

majority of research has been performed using MACH-IV (Fehr, Samsom, & Paulhus,

1992), because the MACH-V, according to some critics, created more problems than it

resolved (Fehr et al., 1992; Hunter, Gerbing, & Boster, 1982; Ray, 1983). Both the

MACH-IV and MACH-V are available for use and have remained in the same

composition since 1970. However, the validity of the MACH-IV is controversial: some

regard it reliable and valid (Jones & Paulhus, 2009; Ramanaiah, Byravan, & Detwiler,
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 5

1994), while others have the opposite opinion (Panitz, 1989; Rauthmann, 2012, 2013;

Rauthmann & Will, 2011). For instance Rauthmann and Will (2011) criticized it for

alleged shortcomings concerning response style, varying reliability, varying factor

structures, insufficient content and construct validity.

Many years after the publication of Studies in Machiavellianism, many things are

clear about Machiavellians. They are characterized as focusing on winning at any cost

(Ryckman, Thornton, & Butler, 1994), being detached, cold, selfish, and act based on

pure instrumentality (Fehr et al., 1992). Machiavellians are interested in money, power,

and competition (Stewart & Stewart, 2006), while deprioritizing community building,

self-love, and family concerns (McHoskey, 1999). They are not more intelligent in

terms of IQ (Paulhus & Williams, 2002), but are nevertheless very good at reading

social situations (Gunnthorsdottir, McCabe, & Smith, 2002) and forming beneficial

alliances (Christie & Geis, 1970). Some researchers hypothesized that in order to

succeed in one’s manipulations, a precondition would have to be a refined use of

Theory of Mind (ToM; Premack & Woodruff, 1978). However, when empirically

tested, high scorers on Machiavellianism have been shown to be quite weak mind

readers (Ali & Chamorro-Premuzic, 2010; Lyons, Caldwell, & Shultz, 2010), and have

lower emotional intelligence (EI; Ali, Amorim, & Chamorro-Premuzic, 2009; Austin,

Farrelly, Black, & Moore, 2007). More expectedly, high scorers have lower levels of

empathy, and are worse at understanding emotions than low scorers (Bagozzi et al.,

2013; Wastell & Booth, 2003). For more elaborate reviews of the literature, see

(Christie & Geis, 1970; Fehr et al., 1992; Jones & Paulhus, 2009).

Gunnthorsdottir et al. (2002) hypothesized that the MACH-IV could be used as an

identifier of cooperativeness. However, they failed to confirm this hypothesis, but


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 6

nevertheless found that levels of Machiavellianism predict reciprocity. Using a

bargaining game in which participants are faced with a choice between trust and

distrust, and between reciprocation and defection, they found that high MACH-IV

scorers overwhelmingly defect, whereas low scorers are inclined to cooperate. Another

study found that schizophrenics have lower ”Machiavellian intelligence” when

compared to controls, on the basis that they were less able to understand Machiavellian

expressions when constructed in narratives. The authors interpret this finding as

showing that schizophrenics have poorer mentalizing abilities (Mazza, De Risio,

Tozzini, Roncone, & Casacchia, 2003).

Using a similar game called The Trust Game (Fehr & Rockenbach, 2003), one of

few fMRI studies on Machiavellianism was conducted (Bereczkei, Deak, Papp, Perlaki,

& Orsi, 2013). The Trust Game is played by an investor and a trustee. The investor

must decide how much of her initial capital (in this study, the equivalent of about $5)

she wants to transfer to the trustee. Once the money has been transferred, the sum is

tripled by the experimenter and then the trustee has the opportunity to return none,

some, or all of the money to the investor. Low scorers on Machiavellianism ended up

receiving less money than high scorers (the participants’ were paid the sum they ended

up with in the game), and offering more money to the other player both when playing

as an investor and trustee. Similar results have been reported in other studies (Czibor &

Bereczkei, 2012; Spitzer, Fischbacher, Herrnberger, Grön, & Fehr, 2007). When

comparing the brain activity in high scorers versus low scorers, the former showed

greater activity in certain brain regions. When playing as instructors, high scorers

showed greater activity in anterior cingulate cortex and thalamus. When playing as

trustees, greater activity was elicited in the dorsal anterior insula, and inferior frontal
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 7

gyrus. In other words, decisions in social dilemma situations evoked higher neural

response in Machiavellian individuals. The researchers suggest that Machiavellians may

conduct specific neural operations when faced with social dilemmas that allow them to

be more successful at exploiting others (Bereczkei et al., 2013).

In a study on the neurological correlates of Machiavellianism in patients with

Parkinson’s disease, McNamara, Durso, and Harris (2007) found that high scorers on

Machiavellianism were impaired on tests relating to prefrontal function in contrast to

low scorers. This study opens up the possibility that patients with Parkinson’s disease

with frontal lobe impairment are susceptible to personality change, and that an intact

prefrontal lobe is required in order to maintain prosocial (as opposed to Machiavellian)

personality traits (McNamara et al., 2007).

Verbeke et al. (2011) hypothesized that because Machiavellians are sensitive to

status seeking in social contexts, this would lead to neuroplasticity, and ultimately

structural changes in the basal ganglia, prefrontal cortex, insula, and hippocampus.

Using structural MRI and a technique called Voxel-Based Morphometry (VBM), the

results showed an array of brain structures varying in size across the two groups (high

versus low scorers). These areas were the caudate, pallidum, putamen, insula, inferior

opercular frontal gyrus, orbitofrontal gyrus, inferior triangular frontal gyrus, superior

medial frontal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, superior frontal gyrus, superior temporal

gyrus, the calcarine, the hippocampus and parahippocampal gyrus (Verbeke et al.,

2011). The researchers offer explanations for why these areas correlate with MACH-IV

scores, but ultimately the study is difficult to interpret, being that all listed brain areas

are also involved in many other things.

Another study looked more closely at the orbital frontal cortex (OFC). The OFC
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 8

was parcellated into three regions, the gyrus rectus, the middle orbital gyrus, and the

lateral orbital gyrus. The participants completed the MACH-IV, social comprehension

and memory tests, which were all used to gauge the features of social cognition in the

participants. The results were very interesting, as hierarchical regression analyses

revealed what may be a double dissociation between MACH-IV scores and left lateral

orbital gyrus on one hand, and social comprehension with right middle orbital gyrus,

on the other. The left lateral orbital gyrus had a partial correlation of 0.62, and the left

middle orbital gyrus 0.48 with MACH-IV scores. Further, the regression showed that

51.3 to 61.67% of the variance in MACH-IV scores could be accounted for by

individual differences in gray matter volumes in left lateral and middle orbital gyri

(Nestor et al., 2012).

Narcissism

The concept of narcissism comes from the legend of Narcissus in Greek mythology:

a dismally vain hero who fell in love with his own reflection. This classical account was

later utilized in clinical psychology at the end of the 19th century (Ellis, 1898), albeit

with some characteristics that have since been removed from the current definition of

narcissism (Levy, Ellison, & Reynoso, 2011). The term narcissism, as it was used

originally, and NPD can hardly be said to refer to the same phenomenon. A crucial

construct transformation has taken place, especially due to the publication of the

DSM-III (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1980), which transformed clinical

descriptions into a working definition, ultimately yielding assessable criteria

(Rhodewalt & Peterson, 2009). For a more detailed review of the history of narcissism,

the reader is directed to (Levy et al., 2011) and for a thorough analysis of the

development of the construct over time, see (Pincus & Lukowitsky, 2010).
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 9

The current edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders,

fifth edition, describes the typical features of narcissistic personality disorder (NPD) as:

”…variable and vulnerable self-esteem, with attempts at regulation through attention

and approval seeking, and either overt or covert grandiosity” (DSM-V; APA, 2013,

p. 767). Further, the diagnostic criteria include impairment in personality function

concerning identity, self-direction, empathy, and intimacy, along with the pathological

personality traits grandiosity and attention-seeking.

NPD is perhaps best described by way of elucidating the terminology listed above

in terms of more specific abnormal behaviors typical of narcissistic individuals. These

include: (1) exaggerated self-appraisal, either inflated or deflated, or a combination of

the two, possibly representing emotional regulation; (2) unreasonably high goals in

order to elevate one’s status above everyone else’s; (3) impaired empathy manifested as

disregard for others, while being excessively attuned to others’ reactions if perceived as

relevant to oneself; (4) largely superficial relationships maintained for personal gain; (5)

a general self-centeredness, believing oneself to be better than others, and often

expressing such beliefs, as well as (6) attempting to be in the center of attention in order

to seek admiration (APA, 2013).

Narcissism is a very complex construct due to its inconsistent definitions in

different fields (i.e., clinical psychology, psychiatry, personality psychology). With the

various turns the DSM has taken, it is difficult to disentangle the terms narcissism and

NPD. Many support the notion that some narcissistic tendencies are healthy and even

beneficial, whereas when they are endorsed in excess they become abnormal and pass

into the realm of abnormal and maladaptive behavior. The debate concerning

dimensionality versus taxonicity has not been fully resolved, as some researchers
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 10

maintain that there is a difference in kind between normal and abnormal narcissism

(Pincus et al., 2009).

In subclinical samples, NPD is most commonly studied with the Narcissistic

Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin & Hall, 1979; Raskin & Terry, 1988). In fact, the

frequency with which the NPI has been used has been criticized as being too

commonplace (e.g., Brown, Budzek, & Tamborski, 2009; Pincus & Lukowitsky, 2010).

Cain, Pincus, and Ansell (2008) noted that the NPI has been the main or only measure

of narcissistic traits in approximately 77% of social and personality psychology studies

since 1985. However, Miller and Campbell (2011) argued that the criticism of NPI has

been blown out of proportion, but nevertheless agree that a better assessment

instrument can and should be developed.

The underlying factor structure of the NPI has been investigated by several

research groups, ultimately yielding various unstable results with two (Corry, Merritt,

Mrug, & Pamp, 2008), three (Ackerman et al., 2011; Kubarych, Deary, & Austin,

2004), four (Emmons, 1984, 1987), and seven (Raskin & Terry, 1988) factors. Raskin

and Terry (1988) sought roughly eight components, because DSM-III had eight

behavioral dimensions under narcissistic personality disorder but ultimately proposed a

seven factor solution. However, neither the factor structure nor the issue of the relative

merits of the various scales has been resolved (Ackerman et al., 2011; Miller, Price, &

Campbell, 2012). Furthermore, with the difficulties of clear conceptualization and

operationalization in light of the DSM-V, more researchers have advocated

investigating lower order traits associated with narcissism (Brown et al., 2009;

Rosenthal & Hooley, 2010) and moving away from the NPI. In response, Miller et al.

(2012) compared a large number of instruments – and without being negative about the
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 11

possible benefits of alternative assessments – they concluded that the NPI is a

well-known and well validated measurement of grandiose narcissism that cannot be

pushed to the side without good statistical evidence for doing so.

The relationship between NPD and the FFM has been thoroughly studied. The

most consistent finding being that narcissists are high in extraversion and low in

agreeableness (Saulsman & Page, 2004). For the other three domains, results have been

quite inconsistent. Samuel and Widiger (2008) compared five measures of narcissism1

and found a large degree of variability across measures, concluding however, that all

five measures include narcissistic grandiosity but not narcissistic vulnerability.

Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism is a further distinction complicating the matter of

how to conceptualize and measure narcissism. While it is possible to derive two

separate factors illustrating the difference between these two categories, the

nomological connection between the two is not clear (Miller et al., 2011).

Furthermore, narcissism and deficits in empathy have long been thought to be

deeply interconnected, dating back at least to the writings of the psychoanalyst Wälder

(1925). Lack of empathy has remained a fundamental postulate, and was one of the

original NPD criteria in the publication of the DSM-III (APA, 1980) and has since been

highlighted in much narcissism research (e.g., Adler, 1986; Akhtar, 1989, 2000;

Baskin-Sommers, Krusemark, & Ronningstam, 2014; Cooper, 1998; Watson, Grisham,

Trotter, & Biderman, 1984; Watson & Morris, 1991).

A neuroscientific study has investigated stress levels in subclinical narcissists, on

the basis that narcissism has been associated with heightened sensitivity to social

evaluation, which should increase physiological and psychological stress responses. The

study found that a higher level of narcissism in men (but not in women) is related to
1
MMPI-2, MCMI-III, PDQ-4, NPI, and SNAP
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 12

heightened cortisol reactivity and negative affect (Edelstein, Yim, & Quas, 2010). In

another stress-related study using Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI),

researchers found that the level of subclinical narcissism is associated with activation in

an a priori anatomically defined social pain network (dorsal anterior cingulate cortex

[dACC], anterior insula [AI], and subgenual anterior cingulate cortex [subACC]). The

effect was found by investigating what happens in people’s brain when they are socially

excluded. Activation in the region of interest (dACC, AI, subACC) correlated

significantly (r = .42, CI=[.12, .60]) with NPI scores. This study suggests that

narcissistic individuals have heightened neural responses to exclusion (Cascio, Konrath,

& Falk, 2014).

In what may the first fMRI study on subclinical narcissism, researchers found that

decreased activation in the right anterior insula was associated with high levels of

narcissism in an empathy task (Fan et al., 2011a). In a related study on the structural

properties of the narcissist’s brain, researchers found that clinical narcissists have less

gray and white matter volume than healthy controls (HC), after controlling for IQ.

Gray matter volume in HC was 730.0 cubic centimeters (cc), and 691.4 cc in narcissists

(d = .57). Gray matter volume was 573.1 cc in HC and 537.3 in narcissists (d = .53).

Both results are ”marginally significant” (p = 0.1 for gray matter and 0.13 for white

matter). Moreover, these effects were especially large in the anterior insula (d = 1.23);

there was also an association between self-reported emotional empathy and size of left

anterior insula (r = .62) (Schulze et al., 2013).

Other studies have found relationships between narcissism and higher testosterone

levels (Lobbestael, Baumeister, Fiebig, & Eckel, 2014), diminished skin conductance

response reactivity, and enhanced cardiac deceleration in anticipation of aversive stimuli


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 13

(Kelsey, Ornduff, McCann, & Reiff, 2001). To what extent these latter findings are

generalizable to the rest of the Dark Triad is unknown, but resting heart rate and heart

rate during a stressor are associated with antisocial behavior, even after controlling for

gender, age, method of recording, use of psychiatric control group, recruitment source,

concurrent versus prospective nature of testing, and source of behavioral rating (Lorber,

2004; Ortiz & Raine, 2004). Low heart rate has been referred to as one of the most

important explanatory factors for violence (Farrington, 1997).

Psychopathy

Psychopathy is a severe personality disorder characterized by self-centeredness,

callousness, and a profound lack of empathy, which hinders the individual to form

warm, and healthy emotional relationships with others (Hare, 1999). In 1941, Hervey

Cleckley published the seminal work The Mask of Sanity, which is still used as a

reference for describing how psychopaths behave. Cleckley presented a description of

the typical psychopath on the basis of countless clinical observations of adult male

patients categorized as “psychopathic”. He introduced 16 characteristics typical of the

psychopath: superficial charm, absence of delusions, absence of nervousness or

psychoneuroticism, unreliability, insincerity, lack of remorse, antisocial behavior, poor

judgment, pathologic egocentricity and incapacity for love, lack of affect, specific loss

of insight, unresponsiveness in interpersonal relations, fantastic and uninviting

behavior, suicides rarely carried out, impersonal sex life, and failure to meet life plans.

These criteria were used as a starting point for Robert D. Hare during the creation of

the Psychopathy Checklist (PCL; Hare, 1985) and in the subsequent development of

the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R; Hare, 1991, 2003). The PCL-R has since

become the gold standard assessment instrument for psychopathy (Patrick, 2006).
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 14

This thesis is mainly concerned with subclinical psychopathy, or what is sometimes

referred to as successful psychopathy. The literature on subclinical psychopathy is

rapidly growing, especially with the rising interest in the Dark Triad. However, there is

little consensus regarding what subclinical psychopathy refers to. Hall and Benning

(2006) described the subclinical psychopath as an individual that embody most of the

psychopathic behaviors, but refrains from serious antisocial behavior, and is therefore

rarely imprisoned or institutionalized. Further, the subclinical psychopath is engaged in

behaviors that breach social norms – but are not necessarily illegal – such as achieving

personal or professional success at the expense of others. However, this description

aligns better with Machiavellianism, as psychopaths tend to be bad at carrying out long

term goals (Furnham et al., 2013). In a more recent publication, Glenn and Raine

(2014) provide four different criteria utilized in research:

1. Individuals scoring high in psychopathy who have never been convicted of a

crime.

2. Individuals scoring high in psychopathy who are not incarcerated.

3. Individuals with psychopathic traits and high social status.

4. Serial killers who have escaped detection for a significant period.

Evidently, there is a great deal of conceptual separation between these items that

may cause problems. Items (2) and (4) are especially problematic. Very few clinical

psychopaths are imprisoned at all times, thus with (2), the distinction between a clinical

and a subclinical psychopath turns into a matter of temporality. The possibility exists

that an individual is considered a clinical psychopath on Monday, and a subclinical

psychopath on Tuesday, upon release from an institution. This is merely a

terminological distinction, rather than a substantial distinction relating to any specific


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 15

etiology or nomological network. It does not offer any explanation as to why or how

the subclinical psychopath would be different, qualitatively or quantitatively, from a

clinical psychopath, because they are one and the same. Item (4) goes against the more

traditional definition (Hall & Benning, 2006) of refraining from serious antisocial

behavior. Item (3) is one of the notions that circulate in popular media (Skeem,

Polaschek, Patrick, & Lilienfeld, 2011), but this conception, too, has been criticized

because ”it is the corporate Machiavellian who successfully perpetrates white collar

crime” (Furnham et al., 2013, p. 208). Moreoever, a distinction between the two

groups should preferably be generalizable cross-culturally, and as such, it cannot rely

strictly on incarceration status, being that people are incarcerated very different things

in different cultures. Thus, additional theoretical work – and subsequent empirical

work – is needed in order to make non-arbitrary distinctions between psychopathic

categories, assuming that the clinical–subclinical distinction is a categorical one.

There are several difficulties with studying subclinical psychopathy. First, it has

been argued that the focus on antisocial behavior in DSM-III, and DSM-IV has led to

an under-representation of other behaviors and traits (Lilienfeld, 1998; Messick, 1995).

Second, identifying subclinical psychopaths has proven to be a difficult task. Third, the

screening of psychopaths in subclinical environments is not as rigorous as in the clinical

population (Hall & Benning, 2006). This problem has led to the creation of alternative

instruments, such as the Psychopathic Personality Inventory (PPI; Lilienfeld &

Andrews, 1996), with the purpose of deriving similar data from the general population.

In a cross-sectional analysis, Harpur and Hare (1994) showed that psychopathy

factors were differently related to age. Mean scores on Factor 2 – the antisocial

component – declined with age. Factor 1 – the affective and interpersonal component –
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 16

was shown to be unrelated to age. In fact, Factor 1 scores were very similar in different

age groups, which indicate that age is not a significant moderator of callous behavior.

This finding also serves the purpose of illustrating that psychopathy consists of two

distinct but correlated components. On the basis of this and other similar studies, Hare,

Neumann, and Widiger (2013) note that psychopaths seems to ”age out” of their

criminal activity over time. However, the traits associated with psychopathy continue

to cause problems throughout their lives. This phenomenon is consistent with a very

general picture of person–situation interaction. Despite situational changes over time,

the underlying traits continue to produce antisocial behavior.

In recent years there has been a rapid increase in fMRI studies in psychopathy, and

meta-analytic findings has highlighted reductions in the right orbitofrontal cortex, right

anterior cingulate cortex, and left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Yang & Raine, 2009).

In a very large recent study (N = 218), Ermer, Cope, Nyalakanti, Calhoun, and Kiehl

(2013) showed that similar structural deficits are found in adolescent with psychopathic

traits. Self-report psychopathy (Psychopathy Checklist–Youth Version; Forth, Kosson,

& Hare, 2003) correlated negatively with gray matter volume in paralimbic regions

and multiple regression showed that volume of various brain areas can be used as

predictors of self-reported psychopathy score. These findings show that psychopathic

traits are a neurodevelopmental disorder. Another very large study by the same group

(N = 298) found similar structural abnormalities in adult psychopaths from

medium/maximum correctional facilities. The orbitofrontal cortex, parahippocampal

cortex, the temporal poles, and posterior cingulate cortex showed especially large gray

matter volume reductions (Ermer, Cope, Nyalakanti, Calhoun, & Kiehl, 2012). These

findings were later replicated in a smaller sample (N = 39) of female adolescents with
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 17

psychopathic traits (Cope, Ermer, Nyalakanti, Calhoun, & Kiehl, 2014).

Other studies have found abnormal function and lower volume in the amygdala in

clinical psychopaths (Dolan & Fullam, 2009; Pardini, Raine, Erickson, & Loeber,

2014), but not subclinical psychopaths (Yang, Raine, Colletti, Toga, & Narr, 2010).

The same finding applies to the prefrontal cortex, where clinical psychopaths, but not

subclinical psychopaths, have reduced gray matter volume (Yang, Raine, Colletti, Toga,

& Narr, 2009, 2005). Raine et al. (2004) also reported an exaggerated anterior

hippocampal volume asymmetry (right > left) in clinical psychopaths, relative to both

control, and subclinical psychopaths. Functional studies have also revealed deficient

fear conditioning (Birbaumer et al., 2005), aberrant processing of moral decisions

(Harenski, Harenski, Shane, & Kiehl, 2010), the neural correlates social decision

making (Rilling et al., 2007), abnormal processing of language (Intrator et al., 1997;

Kiehl, Hare, McDonald, & Brink, 1999, 2004; Williamson, Harpur, & Hare, 1991),

and deficient emotional processing (Kiehl et al., 2001; Müller et al., 2003) in

psychopaths. The interested reader is referred to (Yang & Raine, 2009) for a review of

structural findings and to (Seara-Cardoso & Viding, 2014) for functional findings. The

latter article contrasts clinical findings with community samples, and concludes that

both groups are very similar, with a few exceptions.

Studies such as these have ultimately led to the possibility of better predicting

future rearrest via brain activity. The odds that an offender with relatively low anterior

cingulate activity would be rearrested were approximately double to that of an offender

with high activity in this region, holding constant other observed risk factors (Aharoni

et al., 2013).
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 18

Personality: Big Five, and HEXACO

Personality traits are defined as ”dimensions of individual differences in tendencies

to show consistent patterns of thoughts, feelings, and actions” (McCrae & Costa, 2003,

p. 25). Traits are considered enduring dispositions (e.g., Roccas, Sagiv, Schwartz, &

Knafo, 2002), due to studies revealing their relative stability over time, especially after

the age of 30 (Bleidorn et al., 2013; Ferguson, 2010; Terracciano, Costa, & McCrae,

2006).

The Five-Factor Model (FFM) also known as the ”Big Five” is the most popular

paradigm for representing personality structure and it has been claimed to be the most

scientifically rigorous taxonomy in behavioral science (McCrae, 2009). The FFM is

comprised of the factors Openness to Experience (O), Conscientiousness (C),

Extraversion (E), Agreeableness (A), and Neuroticism (N), creating the frequently used

mnemonic OCEAN. Typical characteristics of high scores on the respective factors are

listed below (from Matthews, Deary, & Whiteman, 2003).

• Openness: Inquiring intellect, appreciation of art and unusual ideas.

• Conscientiousness: Organized, dependable, disciplined, and willing achieve

• Extraversion: Energy, positive emotions, assertiveness, and sociability

• Agreeableness: Compassion, cooperation, helpfulness, and likability

• Neuroticism: Nervous, anxious, angry, depressed, and vulnerable.

Many traits defined in the FFM co-vary: a person who is sociable and assertive

also tends to be cheerful and energetic (DeYoung et al., 2010). Such an individual will

get a high score on the Extraversion factor. However, the same individual may or may

not score highly on the Openness factor, which denotes characteristics such as

intellectual curiosity, appreciation of art and unusual ideas. Neuroticism describes an


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 19

individual’s sensitivity to anxiety, stress, and negative emotions; Agreeableness includes

cooperation, social harmony, and trust; and Conscientiousness concerns the way we

direct and control our impulses, and includes domains like self-efficacy, dutifulness, and

achievement-striving.

There is an abundance of inventories currently in use for measuring the FFM, the

most common ones being the commercial Revised NEO Personality Inventory

(NEO-PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 1992) and the International Personality Item Pool

(IPIP-NEO; Goldberg, 1999), available for the public domain, but both inventories

exist in various versions varying in length. It should be noted that the NEO-PI-R and

the IPIP-NEO are not directly equivalent (Goldberg et al., 2006), albeit very similar.

The longer versions (>100 items) include measurements of second-order facets whereas

shorter versions (~5-60 items) only include the five first-order factors, without facets.

In recent years, a sixth factor has been proposed, known as the Honesty-Humility

(HH) factor (Ashton & Lee, 2007). It belongs to an alternative six factor

conceptualization of personality, known as HEXACO, in which both the factor and

facet structure is different from the traditional FFM. The relative merits of each model

is still being debated (Ashton & Lee, 2007; Saucier, 2002) and go beyond the scope of

this thesis. Honesty-Humility denotes personality features like being sincere, honest,

faithful, loyal, and unassuming, and is commonly measured with the HEXACO

Personality Inventory-Revised (HEXACO-PI-R; Ashton & Lee, 2009; Lee & Ashton,

2004). Recently, HH has been included in a 24-item IPIP-inventory known as the

Mini-IPIP6 (Sibley et al., 2011).


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 20

The Biological Basis of Personality

The biological approach to studying personality has its roots in Eysenck’s (1967)

seminal study, in which he argued that introverts differed from extroverts in arousal.

Specifically, in the regulation of the ascending reticular activation system, based on the

idea that introverts are more easily stimulated than extroverts and thus have lesser need

to explore the environment for new stimuli. Eysenck’s work was criticized by his

student, Jeffrey Alan Gray (1981), and interest in the neuroscience of personality

stagnated for decades to come. However, in the recent decade there has been increasing

interest which ultimately culminated in a field DeYoung and Gray (2009) calls

Personality Neuroscience. The relevance of a neuroscientific approach to the Big Five

comes from the fact that the FFM is descriptive, rather than explanatory. What this

means is that a causal theory of the Big Five is lacking, and it is believed that a

neuroscientific approach can make a significant advance towards this goal by

connecting traits to specific biological systems (DeYoung et al., 2010).

With this newfound approach arise several difficulties. Arguably, the most

important one being the serious methodological issues associated with functional

magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in research on social cognition. Specifically, studies

have been criticized for being under-powered, leading to an increased proportion of

false-positive results (Vul, Harris, Winkielman, & Pashler, 2009; Yarkoni, 2009).

Proposed solutions have been presented focusing on sample size and determining a

priori regions of interest (ROIs) (Mar, Spreng, & DeYoung, 2013). However, results in

many studies have been undermined due to the criticisms raised by Vul et al. (2009) and

Yarkoni (2009). Thus, evaluating the available evidence is difficult and conclusions

must be drawn carefully.


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 21

Adelstein et al. (2011) studied the Big Five in relation to resting-state functional

connectivity (RSFC) in order to determine whether the individual brain’s intrinsic

activation patterns are different in different types of personalities. RSFC represents

intrinsically organized functional brain networks that persist across tasks (Damoiseaux

et al., 2006), in other words, the researcher needn’t rely on any specific task. RSFC has

been successfully utilized in research on other individual difference risk-taking (Cox

et al., 2010) aggression (Hoptman et al., 2010) and cognitive efficiency

(Andrews-Hanna et al., 2007). These RSFC patterns are, like personality traits, stable

across time (Shehzad et al., 2009; Zuo et al., 2010). Adelstein et al. (2011) decided to

study the anterior cingulate cortex and precuneus, because they are thought to be

cortical hubs, connecting to the majority of the rest of the brain (Buckner et al., 2009).

Among many other results, the findings showed that neuroticism and extraversion

predicted connectivity between the two hubs and the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex and

lateral paralimbic regions, respectively. Furthermore, personality and functional

connections were inconsistently present across participants, which suggest that there is

a common core across individuals, and outside that core are inter-individual differences

(Adelstein et al., 2011).

In another fMRI study, DeYoung et al. (2010) tested a number of hypotheses on

the basis of previous neuroscientific studies pertaining to traits, and managed to

confirm their hypotheses for four out of five traits. Extraversion was associated with

the volume of medial orbitofrontal cortex, which is involved in reward values of stimuli.

Extraversion appears to reflect sensitivity to reward. This finding has since been

replicated (DeYoung, Hawes, Civai, & Rustichini, in press). Neuroticism was

associated with reduced volume in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, the left posterior
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 22

hippocampus, and with increased volume in the mid-cingulate gyrus, both in gray and

white matter. The authors argue that this finding is consistent with the theory that

neuroticism reflects sensitivity to threat and punishment. Agreeableness was associated

with reduced volume in posterior left superior temporal sulcus and with increased

volume in posterior cingulate cortex. The latter has been implicated as a component in

theory of mind (Saxe & Powell, 2006). Conscientiousness was positively associated

with a large region around the middle frontal gyrus volume in the left lateral prefrontal

cortex. The prefrontal cortex is involved in many executive functions, such as planning,

which aligns well with trait conscientiousness. Finally, Openness to Experience was not

associated with anything in specific, but other studies have connected it to the

prefrontal cortex and dopamine function (DeYoung, Peterson, & Higgins, 2005), and

the D4 receptor gene (DRD4) catechol-O-methyltransferase gene (COMT) has been

found to predict Openness (DeYoung et al., 2011). Ultimately, these studies are

impressive, because of the large sample sizes used (N > 100), but despite making a

priori hypothesis (as opposed to carrying out exploratory work), the various parts of

the brain is involved in so many things that some correlations may be spurious.

Empathy

In all three Dark Triad components, a marked lack of empathy has, at one point or

another, been a central concern. Empathy is a notoriously complicated construct and

researchers have substantial disagreements pertaining to theoretical and methodological

matters. The focus here is not to outline the many problems, but rather to provide

empirical evidence. Hence, I will not discuss the many theoretical problems

surrounding empathy. For detailed reviews of this matter, I direct the reader to (Batson,

2009; Bernhardt & Singer, 2012; Davis, 1996; Walter, 2012).


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 23

One definition states that: “Empathy is our ability to identify what someone else is

thinking or feeling, and to respond to their thoughts and feelings with an appropriate

emotion” (Baron-Cohen, 2012, p. 12). Recently, Zaki and Ochsner (2012) grouped

empathic processes into three classes: (I) experience sharing, the sharing of internal

states, (II) mentalizing, explicitly considering (and perhaps understanding) targets’

states and their sources, and (III) prosocial concern: expressing motivation to improve

targets’ experiences (for example, by reducing their suffering). Thus, empathy involves

recognizing, predicting, and responding appropriately to others’ emotions.

Empathy is commonly studied through simple observations, (Keysers et al., 2004),

imagination (Jackson, Brunet, Meltzoff, & Decety, 2006), or evaluation (Jackson,

Meltzoff, & Decety, 2005). Empathy is also studied across domains, meaning that

many different paradigms are applied to the study of empathy, such as pain (Coll,

Budell, Rainville, Decety, & Jackson, 2012), disgust (Jabbi, Swart, & Keysers, 2007),

touch (Keysers et al., 2004), anxiety (Morelli, Rameson, & Lieberman, 2012),

happiness, and anger (Dimberg & Thunberg, 2012). At least 13 brain regions have

been suggested to be part of the empathy network (Baron-Cohen, 2012). However,

given that ”brain areas” differ in levels of abstraction, this is not very valuable

information. However, an fMRI based meta-analysis aggregated results from 40 studies

(Fan, Duncan, de Greck, & Northoff, 2011b). The researchers found that the dACC,

anterior mid-cingulate cortex (aMCC), supplementary motor area (SMA), and bilateral

anterior insula were all consistently activated during empathy tasks. The authors

further distinguish between cognitive–evaluative and affective-perceptual empathy. The

dorsal aMCC was more frequently more activated during cognitive-evaluative empathy,

and the right anterior insula was more activated during affective-perceptual empathy.
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 24

The left anterior insula was active in both forms of empathy. Moreover, the researchers

argue that the difference between the empathy systems can be detected at the level of

regional activation. Others have argued that the link between the systems is provided

by the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (Walter, 2012).

The most commonly used self-report measurement of empathy is the Interpersonal

Reactivity Index (IRI; Davis, 1980, 1983). IRI is a 28 item questionnaire that measures

empathy on four sub-scales. Each sub-scale consists of seven items scored on a

five-point Likert scale. The sub-scales are: (1) Perspective Taking, the tendency to

spontaneously adopt the psychological point of view of others; (2) Fantasy, which taps

respondents’ tendencies to transpose themselves imaginatively into the feelings and

actions of fictitious characters in books, movies, and plays; (3) Empathic Concern,

assesses ”other-oriented” feelings of sympathy and concern for unfortunate others; and

(4) Personal Distress, which measures ”self-oriented” feelings of personal anxiety and

unease in tense interpersonal settings (Davis, 1983). Factor analysis has shown that the

IRI has a hierarchical structure of four first-order factors corresponding to the four

scales, with two second-order orthogonal factors, one general empathy factor and one

emotional control factor (Pulos, Elison, & Lennon, 2004). Furthermore, Pulos et al.

(2004) argue that if a higher order empathy scale is desired, the sub-scales empathic

concern, perspective taking, and fantasy may be useful to summarize.

A common question pertaining to all self-report methods is whether such reports

are trustworthy. To answer this question, one method is to triangulate or compare it to

other measures. Cliffordson (2001) compared agreement between parents’ judgments

of self and students’ judgments of their parents and concluded that the agreement was

both substantial and likely to be accurate. Another study found that individuals with
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 25

low levels of self-reported empathy were more aggressive than individuals with high

empathy while under the influence of alcohol (Giancola, 2003). An fMRI study found

that Empathic Concern scale covaried with activity in the ACC, and left AI, but not

right AI. Individuals with high scores on empathy scales showed stronger activations in

the aforementioned areas (Singer et al., 2004). This study is however, severely

underpowered (Vul et al., 2009).

However, a recent study investigated relative and global meta-accuracy in relation

to empathy in two different experimental paradigms (Kelly & Metcalfe, 2011). Global

meta-accuracy assesses whether people know how good they are with respect to other

people. Relative meta-accuracy assesses whether people know what they know. In this

case, the relative measures were carried out by showing participants successive sets of

eyes using the Reading the Mind in the Eyes task (Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill,

Raste, & Plumb, 2001) or the Emotional Expression Multimorph Task (in experiment

two; Blair, Colledge, Murray, & Mitchell, 2001). Having seen all sets of eyes, the

participants were asked to rate how confident they were that they would be able to

make correct emotion identification. This phase was prospective. There was also a

retrospective phase, in which participants watched the same sets of eyes, but this time

accompanied by the response alternatives. This design captures an important aspect,

because it is more difficult to make prospective judgments, due to the lack of

information about response alternatives.

In both experiments, using two different tasks, the results were similar. People are

generally quite good at relative meta-accuracy, especially of the retrospective kind, as

the gamma correlation2 between judgment and performance was 0.40 in experiment

one, and 0.45 in experiment two. The global accuracy level was measured using the
2
Gamma correlation is a nonparametric correlation, for details about this statistic, see (Nelson, 1990)
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 26

Perspective Taking and Empathic Concern scales from the IRI. Both experiments

yielded nonsignificant correlations. This indicates that people are quite good at

knowing whether they are right or wrong in their judgments when actually reading

faces. The study also indicates that the participants’ own estimation of empathic ability

did not correlate at all with actual task performance. There are inherent difficulties

with these two experiments, one of them being that the sample size was quite small (N

= 100), another being that the tasks are quite unrealistic, as you are required to make a

judgment based solely on a facial expression, without seeing the rest of the body

language.

There are additional reasons to not have complete trust in self-report

measurements of empathy, as a recent meta-analysis of aggression and (lack of)

empathy showed a surprising non-relation (Vachon, Lynam, & Johnson, 2014). 82%

of the studies included in the meta-analysis used self-report questionnaires, samples

included community, student, and criminals. One should not discard the possibility that

aggressiveness is uncorrelated with lack of empathy, but it goes against a very long

tradition of researchers and clinicians whom have drawn opposite conclusions (e.g.,

Cleckley, 1988). Thus, thorough studies using procedural tasks in combination with

self-report instruments – both in clinical and subclinical populations – are required to

better determine the role of empathy in human behavior.

Schwartz Value Orientation

Basic human values are empirically measured on the cultural or individual level.

Values have been neglected in psychology because of their inherently subjective nature,

measurement difficulty (Hitlin & Piliavin, 2004), complex theory, and definitional

inconsistency (Rohan, 2000). Despite these issues, values are important as they relate
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 27

both to behavior and how we feel about ourselves and others. Here the focus is solely

on the individual level, more specifically on personal values as conceptualized by

Shalom H. Schwartz, who is the founder of the dominant theory of personal values. In

Schwarz’s theory there are 10 value types: Security, Tradition, Conformity, Benevolence,

Universalism, Self-direction, Stimulation, Hedonism, Achievement, and Power (cf.

Figure 1).

Figure 1. Schwartz’s quasi-circumplex model (Schwartz, 1994)

According to Schwartz and Bilsky (1987, p. 551) values are ”(a) concepts or

beliefs, (b) about desirable end states or behaviors, (c) that transcend specific situations,

(d) guide selection or evaluation of behavior and events, and (e) are ordered by relative

importance.” Further, values are relatively stable cognitive representations of desirable,

abstract goals (Roccas et al., 2002); these cognitive structures are closely linked to
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 28

feelings (Schwartz, 2003). Values should not be conflated with (I) attitudes, (II) traits,

(III) norms, and (IV) needs because: (I) Values are more idealized and abstract than

attitudes, which is a more concrete expression of favor or disfavor toward a thing,

person, situation etc. (II) Traits describe what people are like whereas values describe

what people consider important (e.g., one may have a neurotic disposition without

valuing such a disposition;) Roccas et al., 2002). (III) Norms are situation based, values

are trans-situational. Norms can pressure one into behaving a certain way, but value

based behavior does not exert such pressure. (IV) Needs describe biological influences.

Values carry the function of articulating needs in a socially acceptable way (Hitlin &

Piliavin, 2004).

Values are higher order cognitive components that are difficult to link to behaviors.

Thus, very little empirical research has been conducted on the origins of values. One

behavior genetics study showed that all value types (except Achievement) have common

genetic components, with heritability estimates ranging from 10.8% (Power) to 38%

(Conformity). While more research is needed, this study indicates that values should

not be viewed as completely learned behaviors (Schermer, Feather, Zhu, & Martin,

2008). Furthermore, the importance of values has been stressed by Locke (2000), who

reasoned that values do not only influence behavior, but is also an appropriate target

for interventions. Previous studies have utilized similar circumplex models and applied

them to personality disorders (Locke, 2000).

General Methods

In both studies, measurements of the Dark Triad, general personality traits, and

personal values are collected. Further, in Study II, empathy measures are collected in

order to ascertain the relationship between dark personality traits and empathy. In both
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 29

studies, the overarching assumption is that personality traits and values are connected,

and that traits are antecedents of values. Olver and Mooradian (2003) presented an

integrative model with personality traits representing “nature”, and values as learned

behaviors representing “nurture”, based on the observation that values seem to be more

externally influenced and traits more internally influenced. Personality neuroscience

(DeYoung & Gray, 2009) provides additional support for this premise, demonstrating

biological correlates of the Big Five traits (DeYoung, 2010; DeYoung, Quilty, &

Peterson, 2007; Yarkoni, 2014). Considering the long history of research on traits and

values, these constructs have been infrequently studied together (Olver & Mooradian,

2003; Parks & Guay, 2009; Rokeach, 1973), but the relevance of interpersonal values

in relation to personality disorders has been highlighted previously (Locke, 2000).

Design

The general approach taken in the two studies is a correlational one. Both studies

use well-validated self-report questionnaires. Self-report questionnaires, even very brief

ones, have been found to be reliable predictors of behavioral outcomes (Gosling,

Rentfrow, & Swann Jr, 2003; Konrath, Meier, & Bushman, 2014; Meyer et al., 2001;

Milojev, Osborne, Greaves, Barlow, & Sibley, 2013). The results are investigated using

Pearson correlations (r), and hierarchical multiple regression. Pearson correlations, in

these circumstances, are considered large when they approach 0.3 or greater (Hemphill,

2003).

Multiple regressions are used in two ways. First, for predicting the increase in a

dependent variable when an independent variable increases; and second, for estimating

the relative contributions of individual independent variables on a dependent variable,

by controlling for the effects of other independent variables. In this case, Schwartz’s
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 30

value types are dependent variables and personality traits (Big Five and Dark Triad) are

independent variables. The Big Five is used a control for normal personality traits, or in

other words, to determine what the contribution of abnormal traits are on values, when

holding normal traits constant. All regression analyses met the relevant regression

assumptions.3

A few limitations deserve mentioning. First, Study I has a relatively low sample

size (N = 155), but Schönbrodt and Perugini (2013) found that (N = 161) is a critical

starting point for where correlations does not intolerably deviate from its true value.

Accordingly, we do not believe that sample size is a major issue. Furthermore, no causal

claims are intended in either study, because the correlational nature of the data

collection logically excludes the possibility of causal determination.

To restate the research questions: The first study is set out to answer what

individuals with dark personalities value. Do individuals with higher scores on dark

traits value other things than low scorers? The second study asks whether the

relationship between dark personalities and self-enhancing values are moderated by

empathy.

3
Tested with Shapiro-Wilk test on standardized residuals, tests indicated normality; there was no sig-
nificant autocorrelation in adjacent residuals (Durbin-Watson statistic); no perfect multicollinearity as
measured by variation inflation factors (VIFs) (Field, 2013).
Study I
Dark Values: The Dark Triad in Schwartz’s Value Types
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 32

Introduction

The Dark Triad is an increasingly popular construct representing malevolent and

anti-social personality traits in the general population (Paulhus & Williams, 2002).

Recent studies include investigations of the relation between Dark Triad traits and

morally related behaviors pertaining to Internet use (Buckels, Trapnell, & Paulhus,

2014), schadenfreude (James, Kavanagh, Jonason, Chonody, & Scrutton, 2014), and

work behavior (O’Boyle Jr, Forsyth, Banks, & McDaniel, 2012).

The Dark Triad traits are seen in interactions with others, a common feature being

the tendency to exclude others and promote oneself (Gurtman, 2009; Rauthmann &

Will, 2011). A possible unifier of Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy is the

disregard for conventional morality (Campbell et al., 2009; Furnham et al., 2013).

Values are intimately connected with morality in that they reflect what people believe to

be good or bad (Bardi, Lee, Hofmann-Towfigh, & Soutar, 2009; Schwartz, 2007).

Traits account for how people behave, whereas values describe what people

consider important. To the best of the author’s knowledge, no study has been

conducted on the Dark Triad traits and Schwartz’s basic human values (Schwartz,

1992). Thus, it is currently unknown whether high Dark Triad scorers endorse different

sets of values from low scorers. In this study the author investigated and coined the

term Dark Values, denoting the set of values held by high scorers on the Dark Triad.

The present study aims to identify which values are shared by dark personalities,

by ascertaining the relationships between the factors in the Dark Triad and Schwartz’s

10 value types. The main prediction is that the Dark Triad explains additional variance

on universal values, after controlling for the Big Five traits. On the assumption that the

main prediction holds, the second prediction is a positive relationship between the Dark
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 33

Triad and Self-enhancing values (Power and Achievement), on the basis that they

emphasize the pursuit of self-interest (Schwartz, 1992). From this it follows that a

negative relationship is to be expected with Self-transcendence values (Universalism and

Benevolence). An exploratory approach with no proposed predictions was used

regarding Openness to change (Self-direction, Stimulation, and Hedonism) and

Conservation values (Security, Conformity, and Tradition). No causality is implied in

the reporting of the study. The term Dark Values coined in this study refers to the

values adopted by high scorers on the Dark Triad.

Methods

Participants

The participants (N = 155) constituted two samples. The first sample was from

University West in Sweden and consisted of a class of 79 freshmen from a human

resource management program (63 female, 16 male; ranging from 18 to 56 years, M =

25.1, SD = 7.2).

The second sample was collected through Amazon’s international online polling

service, Mechanical Turk (MTurk). This method of adding online samples has been

used successfully in previous studies (Buckels et al., 2014; Jones & Paulhus, 2014).

MTurk has demonstrated reliability (Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011), and

provides a good spread of socio-economic backgrounds (Casler, Bickel, & Hackett,

2013). The sample consisted of 76 U.S. participants (46 female, 30 male; 18 to 82

years, M = 42.6, SD = 15.4), having completed at least 50 MTurk tasks with an

acceptance rate of 95% or more, according to recommendations. The participants were

compensated with $1. Five control questions were added to ensure that participants
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 34

were paying attention (e.g. “This questionnaire is about classical economics”). This

procedure led to the exclusion of 9 participants.

Cronbach alphas were calculated and compared with a meta-analysis

(Parks-Leduc, Feldman, & Bardi, 2014), yielding similar results. Samples differed

significantly (p < .01) on two traits; Extraversion which was higher in the Swedish

sample and Openness which was higher in the online US sample. These differences are

unlikely to matter, as they are both expected, and likely irrelevant for ascertaining the

general relationship between the Dark Triad and values. Appendix C demonstrates the

reliability of the data, comparing the Big Five factor Agreeableness from the on-site

student sample (S1) and the MTurk sample (S2), against meta-analytic results

(Parks-Leduc et al., 2014). Agreeableness is the most relevant trait, as some consider it

the core of the Dark Triad (Jakobwitz & Egan, 2006).

Instruments

The Big Five Inventory (BFI-44; John, Naumann, & Soto, 2008) is a 44-item

self-report personality inventory. Participants rated how much they agreed (1 =

Strongly disagree, 5 = Strongly agree) with statements such as: “I see myself as someone

who is full of energy” (Extraversion). Cronbach’s alphas for the 5 factors ranged

between .82-.91.

The Short Dark Triad (SD3; Jones & Paulhus, 2014) is a 27-item self-report

questionnaire measurement of Dark Triad traits. Participants rated how much they

agreed (1 = Strongly disagree, 5 = Strongly agree) with statements such as: “You should

wait for the right time to get back at people” (Machiavellianism). The SD3 has been

used successfully in other MTurk studies (e.g., Buckels et al., 2014; Jones & Paulhus,

2014). Cronbach’s alphas for the 3 factors ranged between .75-.84.


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 35

Table 1

Descriptive Analysis of Personal Value Types

M SD α S K

Security 4.11 0.76 0.64 0.23 -0.55


Tradition 3.22 0.93 0.54 0.33 -0.06
Conformity 4.14 0.82 0.72 -0.19 0.07
Benevolence 4.66 0.72 0.65 -0.66 0.72
Universalism 4.35 0.79 0.78 -0.06 0.84
Self-direction 4.61 0.73 0.64 0.38 -0.29
Stimulation 3.34 0.99 0.74 0.05 -0.78
Hedonism 4.19 1.01 0.83 -0.32 -0.57
Achievement 3.70 1.06 0.85 -0.05 -0.83
Power 3.14 1.07 0.69 0.25 -0.46
Self-enhancement 3.46 0.95 0.87 0.11 -0.57
Self-transcendence 4.48 0.62 0.80 -0.55 1.51
Openness 4.10 0.62 0.79 -0.13 -0.51
Conservation 3.85 0.63 0.79 0.32 0.14

Note. S = Skewness; K = Kurtosis. The scores ranges from 1-6.

The Portrait Value Questionnaire (PVQ-40; Schwartz et al., 2001) is a 40-item

self-report questionnaire that measures values by allowing the participant to identify

with short vignettes. These are scored on a six-point Likert scale ranging from “Not

like me at all” to “Very much like me”. The scale requires centering to control for

differences in individual response patterns. All questionnaires were administered in the

original English versions, both in class, and online (30 min). The mandatory high-level

English ability and practical use in Swedish universities is regarded to secure a similar

high level of understanding (Bolton & Kuteeva, 2012). Cronbach’s alphas in the 10

factors ranged between .54-.85, which is within the expected range (Parks-Leduc et al.,

2014).
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 36

Results

Descriptive statistics

BFI-44 means and standard deviations for Openness (M = 3.52, SD = 0.68),

Conscientiousness (M = 3.75, SD = 0.61), Extraversion (M = 3.42, SD = 0.86),

Agreeableness (M = 3.69, SD = 0.65), and Neuroticism (M = 2.85, SD = 0.91)

conformed to expectations. Cronbach alphas for BFI-44 were very good, ranging from

0.82-0.91. PVQ-40 descriptives are reported in Table 1. Being that the purpose of

using the Big Five is as a control, the correlations between the Big Five and the Dark

Triad, and the Big Five and PVQ have been omitted from this section. However,

omitted results are found in the Appendices.

Dark Triad and Schwartz’s Value Types

The relationship between the Dark Triad and Schwartz’s 10 value types are

summarized in Table 2. As expected, the correlations with Self-enhancing values such

as Power (PO) and Achievement (AC) were particularly strong. The opposite,

Self-transcending values such as Universalism (UN) and Benevolence (BE), showed

slightly smaller effects. The PVQ-40 and BFI instruments tend to yield particularly

strong relationships when combined (Parks-Leduc et al., 2014).

Furthermore, a sinusoid pattern can be seen in Figure 2, which is not as easily

derived from Table 2. The pattern describes how Machiavellianism, narcissism, and

psychopathy relate to the circumplex. A similar pattern has been found between

empathy and values (Myyry, Juujärvi, & Pesso, 2010). The correlations align with the

orthogonal structure of the circumplex: starting at Tradition (TR), the trend is a

negative relationship reaching a null-effect at Self-direction (SD), continuing with a


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 37

Table 2

Correlations between Dark Triad and Schwartz’s value types

Machiavellianism Narcissism Psychopathy

Security −.10 −.29** −.30**


Tradition −.41** −.47** −.35**
Conformity −.24* −.36** −.27*
Benevolence −.51** −.17 −.43**
Universalism −.44** −.30** −.31**
Self-direction .02 .06 .01
Stimulation .19 .32** .32**
Hedonism .37** .24* .38**
Achievement .59** .48** .46**
Power .65** .64** .60**

Note. N = 136. After Bonferroni correction, * p < .01. ** p <.0025. (two-tailed).

positive relationship peaking at Power (PO).

The research aim in this study was to explore Dark Values (the added explained

variance on values from the Dark Triad, after controlling for the Big Five). Two-step

hierarchical regression analyses on the ten value types are reported in Table 3. The first

three columns report percentages of accounted variance. The results indicate that the

Dark Triad accounts for added variance in seven of the ten value types, thus confirming

the main prediction. These effects are especially evident in the Self-transcendence-

Self-enhancement values, but also in the Conservation values. The Dark Triad does not

predict Openness values. Furthermore, the beta-coefficients in Table 3 report that

Machiavellianism explains five of the ten value types, narcissism three, and

psychopathy only two.

Figure 3 is the summary of the main research question, reporting the percentage of
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 38

Table 3

Accounted variance from the Big Five and the Dark Triad on Schwartz’s values

Value Type Big Five R2 Big Five + DT R2 ΔR2 β Mach β Narc β Psych

Security 31.4 36.6 5.2* -.01 -.03 -.30**


Tradition 29.8 43.1 13.3*** -.42*** -.13 -.02
Conformity 31.6 35.6 4.0 -.19 -.12 -.03
Benevolence 42.0 48.7 6.7*** -.19 -.11 -.15
Universalism 25.8 36.1 10.3*** -.26* -.22* -.07
Self-direction 37.5 37.6 0.01 .05 .00 .00
Stimulation 36.7 40.1 3.4 .10 .04 .17
Hedonism 17.8 25.1 7.3** .29* -.10 .15
Achievement 19.2 42.7 23.5*** .43*** .30** .10
Power 42.9 64.9 22.0*** .28*** .37*** .19*

Note. N = 136. All R2 values are reported in %. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p <.001.
(two-tailed).

0,8
0,6
Mach Narc Psych
0,4
0,2
0
-0,2
-0,4
-0,6
-0,8

Figure 2. Correlations between Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy and


Schwartz’s 10 values. Sinusoidal pattern clearly visible.

variance accounted for by the Dark Triad, in addition to the Big Five. The circumplex

has been slightly modified to represent the values axes, which has proven useful in

furthering research on values (Lindeman & Verkasalo, 2005). The Dark Triad

demonstrated the most predictive power on Self-enhancement values with over 20%
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 39

additional variance, followed by Tradition and Universalism with over 10%, and

Hedonism, Security and Benevolence showing significant results around 5%.

Discussion

The results aligned with previous studies and extend the knowledge of the Dark

Triad (Parks-Leduc et al., 2014; Paulhus & Williams, 2002). The Dark Triad correlates

with three out of four value dimensions (Self-enhancing, Self-transcending, and

Conservation) and adding explanatory variance (10-20%), in addition to the Big Five.

However, the fourth dimension (Openness to change) showed minimal or no relation to

the Dark Triad. Openness to change-items involves mostly non-moral content,

potentially explaining the lack of effect (Schwartz, 2007). Tradition, on the other hand,

SELF-
TRANSCENDENCE

Universalism Benevolence
10.3% 6.7%

Self-direction Conformity
0.01% 4.0%

Stimulation Tradition
OPENNESS 3.4% 13.3%
TO CHANGE CONSERVATION

Hedonism Security
7.3% 5.2%

Achievement Power
23.5% 22.0%

SELF-ENHANCEMENT

Figure 3. Dark Values. Based on R2 coefficients from hierarchical regressions, showing


the added variance from the Dark Triad on Schwartz’s 10 value types (Big Five is
controlled for).
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 40

showed a particular impact by the Dark Triad (cf. Figure 3). This effect exemplifies the

high scorer on the Dark Triad, showing disregard for conventions, bordering on

anti-social behavior. The study shows that individuals scoring high on the Dark Triad

share certain value types.

Traits and Values in the Dark Triad Components

Most of the additional variance in universal values was accounted for by

Machiavellianism which indicates that this disposition is not well captured by the Big

Five. As seen in Figure 2, a majority of the effects from Machiavellianism reaches the

medium threshold of r = .20 proposed by Hemphill (2003), while narcissism and

psychopathy shows medium effects in less than half of the value types. Behavioral

genetics report a strong shared-environment component for Machiavellianism, but not

for narcissism and psychopathy (Larsson, Andershed, & Lichtenstein, 2006; Vernon,

Villani, Vickers, & Harris, 2008). Olver and Mooradian (2003) proposed that traits

can be separated from values, through the endogenous characteristics of traits (nature)

versus the learned adaptations of values (nurture).

The results suggest that Machiavellianism is highly value-driven, while narcissism

and psychopathy are more trait-driven. On the item-level in the Machiavellianism

subscale – which includes hiding secrets, making the most of opportunities, using

information for one’s own benefit, and avoiding conflict with others for long-term

benefit – a learned world-view is implied. The narcissism scale, on the other hand,

measures a person’s perceived interaction with others and one’s own intrinsic self-image.

The items concern the influence of the presence of others, people’s opinions, and being

at the center of attention; all implying social interaction. Lastly, the psychopathy scale

is more based on temperaments – such as being out of control and harboring revenge –
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 41

which have demonstrated high heritability (Larsson et al., 2006).

The exploratory analysis implies that the Dark Triad is composed by both trait-like

and value-like measurements, thus illuminating the varying relationships with universal

values. This could have potential implications in the construct debate (Fossati, Pincus,

Borroni, Munteanu, & Maffei, 2014).

Dark Values and Morality

The title choice Dark Values suggests that Self-enhancing values such as Power and

Achievement should perhaps not be considered morally neutral (Arvan, 2013). People

generally consider Self-transcending values to contain more morally relevant content

than the other dimensions (Schwartz, 2007). Items in the Short Dark Triad are not very

flattering: “I’ll say anything to get what I want”, “I can be mean to others”, “I like to

get revenge on authorities”, or “I seek out danger” and carry moral implications. The

authors propose that values become a more important moral issue, having been

revealed by Dark Values.

The results that high scorers on the Dark Triad hold values that entail the

exclusion of others and the enhancement of oneself. This supports Rauthmann and

Will’s (2011) results, who found that persons scoring high on Machiavellianism have

low a propensity for including others in their work and tend to view others negatively.

An application of this could be the difficulty of distinguishing the value types of for

instance newly employed or business partners. Using personality tests such as the Big

Five to find people extraordinary low on, for instance, Agreeableness is a start in

prevention of anti-social work behaviors. Twenge, Baumeister, Tice, and Stucke (2001)

report effects such as increased aggression, less helpful behavior, less rational and

intelligent choices in subjects who are excluded socially. Knowing the consequences of
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 42

Dark Values helps in working with people.

Conclusion and Future Studies

The Dark Triad predicts universal values to a greater extent than the Big Five. The

opposite, a Bright Triad, the hypothesized measure of a socially inclusive, caring, and

extraordinarily agreeable individual, is not necessarily captured by low scores on the

Dark Triad. Being that values affect behavior when activated (Verplanken & Holland,

2002), a potentially fruitful line of experimental research opens up: it is unknown to

what extent it is possible to manipulate values in highly Machiavellian and callous

individuals. This is unchartered territory, as little is known about value activation in

non-normal distributions.

In conclusion, the authors propose that future studies pertaining to morality and

dark personalities utilize values, which may advance insights into what lies behind

human universal values. Dark Values might bear similarities to the dark matter in the

physical universe, hidden but constantly contributing, in this case to the universal

values of human beings.


Study II
The Role of Empathy and the Dark Triad in Predicting Universal Values
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 44

Introduction

The present study progresses from Study I. In this study the relationship between

Dark Triad and personal values are explored. The main hypothesis is that dark traits

are moderated by empathy. As in the previous study, the hypothesis is tested on the

dependent variable personal values. The importance of this question lies in the

well-established idea that individuals with dark personalities profoundly lack empathy

(Jonason & Krause, 2013; Wai & Tiliopoulos, 2012). In order to make potential

interventions, the relationship between empathy and dark personalities needs to be

thoroughly understood.

In attempts to uncover the core of the Dark Triad, candidates such as the

personality traits Agreeableness (Jakobwitz & Egan, 2006) and Honesty-Humility (Lee

& Ashton, 2005) have been proposed, while others have suggested that lack of

empathy is a better candidate (Jones & Paulhus, 2011).

Much research has shown that personality traits are intimately connected with

universal values (Parks & Guay, 2009; Parks-Leduc et al., 2014). Despite this known

relationship, little interest has been directed towards how empathy and callousness

relate to universal human values. To the best of the author’s knowledge, no other

studies on Dark Triad and universal values has been conducted. Thus, very little is

known about which universal values callous people endorse. There are ample reasons

for believing that values are relevant to both callous as well as empathic personalities.

Values are beliefs infused with affect (Schwartz, 2012), and have been shown to be

relevant in moral decision making (Lönnqvist, Walkowitz, Verkasalo, & Wichardt,

2011). Traits are an expression of what people mostly are like, while values are an

expression of what people like. The main question in this study is how these constructs
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 45

are linked, in order to further the knowledge about the common element underlying the

Dark Triad.

Like the first study, personality traits (the six-factor model and Dark Triad) are

considered antecedents to values, on the basis of Olver and Mooradian’s (2003)

nature–nurture distinction. Empathy is tested as a moderator between personality and

values. Furthermore, calls have been made suggesting that low Honesty–Humility

represents the core of callousness and can therefore replace the Dark Triad (Lee &

Ashton, 2014). This study will attempt to further the knowledge on personality traits

and its relation to empathy via universal values. Can empathy and the Dark Triad

predict universal values? If so, are empathy and callousness needed in addition to

personality traits to predict universal values, or can they be replaced?

Methods

Participants

An online sample made up of older international participants was deliberately

chosen, in order to get a spread of values in the sample. The sample consisted of 193

participants (105 female, 88 male). Participants ranged in age from 19 to 76 years (M =

38.6, SD = 12.6). Participants were recruited through Amazon’s Mechanical Turk

(MTurk), which is an online service that allows data collection against small financial

incentives. MTurk has proven to be a reliable way to acquire data (Buhrmester et al.,

2011). Further, this method of data gathering has been used successfully by previous

researchers on empathy and callousness and is gaining popularity (Buckels et al., 2014;

Jones & Paulhus, 2014). Studies have also shown that MTurk provides a spread of

socio-economic background which is desirable in values research (Casler et al., 2013).


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 46

Participants were compensated with $1 doing a task that, on average, lasted 16

minutes. The task was made available for participants from the United States that had

completed at least 5000 previous tasks with an acceptance rate 97% or more. Five

control questions were used in order to sort out disingenuous participants (e.g., “This

questionnaire is about classical economics”, “What is the name of the current president

of the United States of America?”). The collected data was also filtered in terms of

excluding participants that answered > 80% of the items with the same response and by

observing unnatural answering patterns (e.g., a consistent use of only two alternatives).

In total, 14 participants were excluded.

Tables in Appendix C illustrates the reliability of the sample, comparing

correlations from the Study I sample (S1) with the MTurk sample in this study (S2),

matched against a recent meta-analysis on the Big Five, and Schwartz’s value types

(Parks-Leduc et al., 2014).

Instruments

As in the previous study, SD3 was used to gauge levels of DT in MTurk

participants. A newer version of the Portrait Values Questionnaire was used, known as

the PVQ-RR (Cieciuch, Davidov, Vecchione, Beierlein, & Schwartz, 2014; Schwartz

et al., 2012), which consists of 57 items rated on a six-point Likert scale. The items

were rephrased to be gender neutral (e.g., “It is very important to this person to have a

good time”), in order to give all participants the same questionnaire without dividing it

by gender. This goes against Schwartz’s advice, who commented that the strength of

correlations drop when this type of language is used (S. Schwartz, 2014, personal

communication). In contrast to the previous instrument (PVQ-40), the PVQ-RR

measures 19 value categories in comparison to the original 10. Out of the 9 additional
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 47

categories, two are known as ”Face” and ”Humility”, the remaining 7 are new

divisions of previous categories. For instance, Universalism is now divided in three

sub-scales known as Nature, Concern, and Tolerance. There are good reasons for the

creation of these additonal distinctions, but for the purpose of this exploratory work on

DT, the four higher-order factors were utilized: Self-enhancement (Power and

Achievement), Self-transcendence (Universalism and Benevolence), Openness to change

(Self-direction, Stimulation, Hedonism), and Conservation (Security, Tradition,

Conformity, Humility, and Face).

The Mini-IPIP6 (Sibley et al., 2011) is a 24-item self-report inventory used to

measure a six-factor model (The Big Five plus the Honesty-Humility domain; Ashton &

Lee, 2009), without including facet scores. It is measured on a 1-7 Likert scale ranging

from “Strongly disagree” to “Strongly agree”. This scale is of particular interest since it

is an extension of the classical Big Five model with Honesty-Humility added, that has

not yet reached popularity. As far as the authors know, no study has shown the relation

between the HH factor and personal values.

Empathy was measured by the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI; Davis, 1983),

which consists of a total of 28 items. The scale is divisible into four subscales, two

measuring cognitive empathy (Perspective Taking and Fantasy) and two measuring

affective empathy (Personal Distress and Empathic Concern). The scale ranges from 0

(“Does not describe me well”) to 4 (“Describes me very well”). In this article, only

Perspective Taking (IRI-PT) and Empathic Concern (IRI-EC) are utilized, as Fantasy

and Personal Distress has been found to be quite unreliable (Cliffordson, 2002).

Factors are calculated by adding the items into a total score for each scale.

The Short Dark Triad 3 (SD3; Jones & Paulhus, 2014) is a 27 item self-report
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 48

questionnaire developed for measuring Dark Triad levels (i.e., Machiavellianism,

narcissism, and psychopathy) in the general population, and is used to gauge the

common core of callousness in this study. The items are measured on a 1-5 Likert-scale

with low scores indicating low levels of callousness and high scores indicating high

levels of callousness.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Four scales were used (Mini-IPIP6, IRI, SD3, and PVQ-RR). Cronbach’s alphas for

Mini-IPIP6 were good, ranging from 0.75-0.86. Descriptive statistics on the personality

traits report Openness (M = 5.47, SD = 1.31), Conscientiousness (M = 5.33, SD = 1.29),

Extraversion (M = 3.37, SD = 1.60), Agreeableness (M = 5.37, SD = 1.35), Neuroticism

(M = 3.01, SD = 1.39), and Honesty-Humility (M = 4.69, SD = 1.63). Reliabilities

varied from acceptable to excellent for the PVQ-RR value types (0.63-0.90), with the

exception of personal security, with a poor value of 0.58. Low alphas are expected on

some value types (cf. Schwartz et al., 2012). Alphas for empathy (IRI) ranged from

0.87-0.91 and callousness (SD3) ranged from 0.75-0.86. Descriptives for PVQ-RR, IRI,

and SD-3 are: Security (M = 4.55 SD = 0.67), Tradition (M = 3.21, SD = 1.35),

Conformity (M = 3.86, SD = 0.94), Humility (M = 4.08, SD = 1.01), Benevolence (M =

4.78, SD = 0.60), Universalism (M = 4.41, SD = 0.76), Self-direction (M = 4.82, SD =

0.71), Stimulation (M = 3.33, SD = 1.02), Hedonism (M = 3.92, SD = 0.89),

Achievement (M = 3.75, SD = 0.87), Power (M = 2.32, SD = 1.03), Face (M = 3.84, SD

= 0.96), IRI-PT (M = 19.24, SD = 5.58), IRI-EC (M = 20.98, SD = 5.71),

Machiavellianism (M = 2.89, SD = 0.72), Narcissism (M = 2.58, SD = 0.74), and


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 49

Psychopathy (M = 2.01, SD = 0.62). No anomalous results are to be reported.

Correlation tables have been omitted from the results section but are found in the

Appendices.

Moderating Dark Traits

Using the SPSS macro PROCESS (Hayes, 2008), the role of empathy as a

moderator of the Dark Triad was analyzed. The conceptual model is depicted in

Figure 4. A moderation effect is the combined effect of two variables on another, or

what is in statistical terms known as an interaction effect. PROCESS is a SPSS macro

that automatically calculates grand mean centering, and also multiplies the chosen

variables.

First global indices were calculated for empathy and Dark Triad. Empathy was

calculated by adding IRI-PT and IRI-EC to one variable. The Dark Triad index was

calculated by using the mean of Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy

variables, in accordance with the respective scale instructions. Next, two moderated

regressions were computed using PROCESS. The dependent variables were

Self-enhancement (Model 1), and Self-transcendence (Model 2) higher-order factors,

because the aggregate categories explain most variance of Dark Traits (cf. Study I). The

X Y

Figure 4. Conceptual model of moderation, from SPSS macro PROCESS (Model 1;


Hayes, 2008).
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 50

Table 4

Moderated Regression Analysis

β SE t p

Model 1 (Self-enhancement)
Constant 2.76 [2.67, 2.85] 0.05 58.86 p < .001
Empathy -0.02 [-0.05, 0.01] 0.01 -4.29 p < .001
Dark Triad -0.25 [-0.46, -0.04] 0.01 12.03 p < .001
Empathy × Dark Triad -0.01 [-0.02, 0.00] 0.01 -1.70 p = .090
Model 2 (Self-transcendence)
Constant 3.99 [3.92, 4.07] 0.04 106.73 p < .001
Empathy 0.00 [-0.01, 0.00] 0.00 -2.44 p = .015
Dark Triad -0.49 [-0.61, -0.37] 0.10 -7.67 p < .001
Empathy × Dark Triad 0.04 [-0.06, 0.14] 0.01 0.74 p = .460

Note. N = 182. R2 = .26 for Model 1, and .25 for Model 2.

independent variable was the global Dark Triad index, and the moderating variable

was the global empathy index. These analyses are reported in Table 4.

Based on these results, there are no significant interaction effects on either

Self-enhancement or Self-transcendence values. In Model 1, there is a tendency towards

an interaction effect, but it is small. Because of these results, additional efforts were

made to ascertain the relative contributions of the Big Six in the prediction of values.

The PROCESS analyses do not control for the Big Six, but in this case merely

investigates the relationship between three variables.

Controlling for Big Six and Dark Triad

To further explore the relationship between personality traits and values,

hierarchical multiple regression was utilized. Unlike moderation analysis, hierarchical

multiple regression can be used to see how much variance each variable explains, but it
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 51

is not used to detect interaction effects. Analyses were carried out in three steps and are

reported in Table 5. The first column report percentages of accounted variance from

the Big Six. The second column reports Big Six and DT contributions, and the third

column reports Big Six, DT, and empathy (IRI-PT and IRI-EC). The last column

contains R2 -change between steps two and three. The results indicate that IRI does not

significantly contribute to either enhancement or transcendence values. After

controlling for the effects of personality constructs, empathy does not add explanatory

power on the aforementioned values.

Examining the data further, there was a remarkable increase in multicollinearity in

Step 3 between IRI-EC, IRI-PT and Agreeableness, which means that one variable can

be predicted from the other. Specifically, the Variation Inflation Factors (VIFs)

approached four for Agreeableness, while other variables remained steadily around 1.5.

While this doesn’t necessarily make the regression model inaccurate, it does indicate

that Agreeableness and empathy variables are highly correlated. Accordingly, the

Table 5

Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis of Big Six, Dark Triad Components, and IRI
on Value Types

Block 1 Block 2 Block 3


Value Type Big Six DT IRI ΔR2

Self-enhancement 54.6 61.9 64.0 2.2


Self-transcendence 29.6 43.4 44.2 0.8
Openness to change 22.9 33.1 33.2 0.1
Conservation 46.6 48.4 53.1 4.7

Note. All reported R2 values are in %. DT refers to the Dark Triad components, not a
global Dark Triad index. IRI = Perspective Taking and Empathic Concern scales only.
Each step includes the variables from previous steps. ΔR2 refers to the increment of
change between step 2 and step 3. N = 193.
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 52

dimension reduction technique Principal Component Analysis (PCA) was used to

determine to what extent Agreeableness, IRI-EC, and IRI-PT overlap.

Principal Component Analysis

PCA was used to ascertain the underlying structure of Mini-IPIP6-Agreeableness

(MI6-A), IRI-PT, and IRI-EC; accordingly, a total of 18 items were used. The goal is to

reduce the 18 items to see how many components are formed, not to say anything

about latent variables, which is not what PCA is appropriate for (Borsboom, 2006).

First, the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy is .94, above the

minimum recommended value of .5, which indicates that factor analysis should provide

compact and reliable factors (Field, 2013). Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant

χ2 (153) = 2242.91, p < .001, which indicates that dimension reduction is possible.

Instead of using the traditional Kaiser criterion, which includes all components where

the Eigenvalue is larger than one, an attempt was made to derive just one component.

This component explained a massive 52.8% of all variance. No rotation was used, as

factors cannot be rotated upon extraction of one component. The factor loading

component matrix is presented in Appendix B, and the scree plot in Figure 5, which

shows how the latent factors contribute to a one-factor model.

Discussion

The results from the present study aligned with Study I and previous studies on

both the Dark Triad (Paulhus & Williams, 2002), and the Big Five–Values relationship

(Parks-Leduc et al., 2014). As seen in the Appendix, the results in the two studies were

very similar, despite using slightly different instruments. Thus, the present study serves

the purpose of validating the first study, while further expanding the knowledge on the
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 53

10

8
Eigenvalue

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18

Component Number

Figure 5. Scree plot from PCA depicting the strong influence of one component.

relationships between empathy, dark personalities, and values.

As the results clearly indicate, empathy does not moderate the relationship

between dark personalities and self-enhancing values, at least not in this data set.

Neither does it provide additional variance over and above the Big Six and Dark Triad.

Because these results are surprising, the PCA was conducted to ascertain whether

Agreeableness, and IRI measured the same thing. As the PCA reveals, the overlap

between these variables is very large, with one component explaining over 50% of the

total variance. Practically, this means that the Agreeableness and empathy instruments

measures more or less the same thing. A limitation of these results is that the

personality scale Mini-IPIP6 is a short scale, with the Agreeableness factor being

composed by just four items. To be able to confirm that these results have external

validity, analyses with broader instruments are necessary. To further explore the idea
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 54

that empathy moderates the relationship between dark traits and self-enhancing values,

Structural Equation Modelling (SEM) would be an optimal solution for doing so.

General Discussion

Conclusions from Empirical Studies

The first study revealed that some individuals endorse certain sets of values that

justify self-serving behavior, even at the expense of others. This finding is consistent

with the research literature – and appeals to common sense – insofar as disagreeable

people ”ought” to endorse questionable moral positions (cf. Arvan, 2013). Study I also

showed that it is possible to predict someone’s values with personality data. However,

the novel finding is that the Dark Triad also predicts values, especially self-enhancing

values, even when the Big Five is controlled for. The author theorizes that

Machiavellianism is especially value-driven, both on the basis that it was the best

predictor of values, and because other studies have found a strong shared-environment

component in Machiavellianism (Vernon et al., 2008). The connection between the

Dark Triad and self-enhancing values does not necessarily work in reverse. A low

scorer on the Dark Triad does not necessarily endorse self-transcending values. A low

scorer merely has the absence of Dark Triad traits. As such, there is room for a

theoretical ”Bright Triad”, potentially encompassing extraordinary kindness,

inclusiveness, and general agreeableness.

In the second study the attempt was to find a moderating relationship between the

Dark Triad and personal values. The hypothesized moderator was empathy. This

relationship was not uncovered, as there were no significant interaction effects between

neither self-enhancing nor self-transcending values and the Dark Triad. It was also
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 55

shown that empathy was not very useful as a predictor of values of any kind, at least

not when used in addition to personality measurements. The nonsuccess in finding a

significant moderation effect led to further analysis utilizing a PCA. It was hypothesized

that the failure to moderate between variables could be because agreeableness and

empathy was not sufficiently discriminant. This hypothesis was confirmed as the

analysis showed that one component explained over 50% variance, and that all 18

items loaded on the same component. Future studies should utilize instruments with

more questions that include facets, in order to allow for better discrimination between

constructs, if such discrimination is indeed possible.

Another limitation needs attention. Self-report questionnaires have proven

predictive power on a range of behavioral outcomes (e.g., Barrick & Mount, 1991;

Judge, Heller, & Mount, 2002, 1999; Poropat, 2009; Roberts, Kuncel, Shiner, Caspi, &

Goldberg, 2007; Steel, Schmidt, & Shultz, 2008). The same cannot necessarily be said

about self-measurement of empathy. Self-report always relies on that the participant

has reliable metacognitive accuracy. If this condition is not satisfied, the external

validity of the test decreases. Surprisingly, metacognitive accuracy in empathy is much

understudied (W. Ickes, personal communication). Using tasks that are as ecologically

valid as possible and correlating that data with self-report questionnaires is necessary in

order to achieve corroborating evidence that the accuracy assumption holds. In the

future, such tasks may be created using virtual reality equipment, but in the current

conditions, it is possible to conduct such studies using computer tasks. Remarkably, the

authors was able to find only one such study (i.e., Kelly & Metcalfe, 2011). In that

study, the correlation between meta-cognitive accuracy and the procedural task was

very low (and non-significant). Although this study potentially stands alone, a
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 56

non-significant correlation indicates that the accuracy assumption is unlikely to hold.

It is important to note that scoring high on a procedural empathy test is not

sufficient evidence for determining that someone acts empathic in everyday life, but I

believe that it is an important first step in being able to verify the legitimacy of asking

persons about their empathic capacities. In much the same way that psychologists do

not generally measure IQ by asking participants whether they believe they have high or

low IQs, psychologists and neuroscientists should not ask participants whether they are

empathic. At least not before the relationship has been sufficiently studied and verified.

As Paul Meehl once remarked in relation to the problems of ”soft” psychology: ”A

theory is corroborated to the extent that we have subjected it to such risky tests; the

more dangerous tests it has survived, the better corroborated it is.” (1978, p. 817). In

the current state of affairs, the self-reporting of empathy has not been subjected to

sufficiently risky tests.

Future Directions

The two studies conducted in this thesis were exploratory in nature, as no previous

study had investigated the relation between the Dark Triad and Schwartz’s values. It

should be viewed as a stepping stone towards integrating personality psychology more

closely with cognitive neuroscience. However, at this stage, it is difficult to extrapolate

from the collected data to a neuroscientific framework. Thus, rather than applying

post-hoc theory to collected data a few alternatives for how to integrate these empirical

studies using neuroscientific methodologies in future studies are provided.

To the best of the author’s knowledge, no study has been conducted on personal

values using neuroscientific instruments. However, the absence of such studies may be

caused by the degrees of separation between personality psychology and cognitive


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 57

neuroscience, rather than that values are not susceptible to neuroscientific research.

One may reason that values are a higher order construct, much like morality, or some

decision making paradigms. Higher order constructs have been manipulated in

neuroscience. For instance, Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) has been used to

manipulate decision making in behavioral economics research (Knoch, Pascual-Leone,

Meyer, Treyer, & Fehr, 2006), in deception and moral cognition (Karim et al., 2010),

and to study the effect of biases on information processing (Franken, Muris, Nijs, &

van Strien, 2008). TMS has also been used to correlate motor cortex excitability and

neuroticism (Wassermann, Greenberg, Nguyen, & Murphy, 2001), and induce

temporary psychopathy by creating virtual lesions (Dutton, 2012).

A question that arises is whether it is possible to change someone’s values by using

TMS? Changing interpersonal values has been argued to be an important step towards

treating personality disorders (Locke, 2000). Currently, the constraints on value

manipulation are unknown, but headway in this area may provide new insights into the

Dark Triad components. For instance, are psychopaths more or less likely to change

values than a non-psychopath? A potential starting point is to use repetitive TMS

(rTMS) on the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, which has been found to increase

utilitarian moral judgments (Tassy et al., 2011). Utilitarian moral reasoning is also

featured in psychopathy (Koenigs, Kruepke, Zeier, & Newman, 2012), and moral

judgments are intimately connected with self-transcending values (Schwartz, 2007).

Finally, there are meta-scientific concerns pertaining to individual differences to

take into account. Every psychological study ultimately boils down to its participants,

and in some cases it may be more important to know what those participants are like

than is currently acknowledged. The concern being that eventually neuroscience will
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 58

progress to a point where it will be necessary to control for various personality traits in

order to properly delineate different experimental groups, or the results will be

confounded. Personality neuroscience is a start in answering that call.


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 59

References

Ackerman, R. A., Witt, E. A., Donnellan, M. B., Trzesniewski, K. H., Robins, R. W., &

Kashy, D. A. (2011). What does the narcissistic personality inventory really mea-

sure? Assessment, 18(1), 67–87. doi:10.1177/1073191110382845

Adelstein, J. S., Shehzad, Z., Mennes, M., DeYoung, C. G., Zuo, X.-N., Kelly, C., …

Castellanos, F. X. (2011). Personality is reflected in the brain’s intrinsic functional

architecture. PloS One, 6(11), e27633. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0027633

Adler, G. (1986). Psychotherapy of the narcissistic personality disorder patient: two con-

trasting approaches. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 143(4), 430–436.

Aharoni, E., Vincent, G. M., Harenski, C. L., Calhoun, V. D., Sinnott-Armstrong, W.,

Gazzaniga, M. S., & Kiehl, K. A. (2013). Neuroprediction of future rearrest. Pro-

ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(15), 6223–6228. doi:10.1073

/pnas.1219302110

Akhtar, S. (1989). Narcissistic personality disorder: descriptive features and differential

diagnosis. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 12(3), 505–529.

Akhtar, S. (2000). The shy narcissist. In P. Fonagy, R. Michels, & J. Sandler (Eds.), Chang-

ing ideas in a changing world: the revolution in psychoanalysis (pp. 111–119). Lon-

don: Karnac Books.

Ali, F., Amorim, I. S., & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2009). Empathy deficits and trait emo-

tional intelligence in psychopathy and machiavellianism. Personality and Individual

Differences, 47(7), 758–762. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.016

Ali, F. & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2010). Investigating theory of mind deficits in non-

clinical psychopathy and machiavellianism. Personality and Individual Differences,

49(3), 169–174. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.027


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 60

American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental

disorders. (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental

disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Lustig, C., Head, D., Raichle, M. E.,

& Buckner, R. L. (2007). Disruption of large-scale brain systems in advanced aging.

Neuron, 56(5), 924–935. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.038

Arvan, M. (2013). Bad news for conservatives? moral judgments and the dark triad per-

sonality traits: a correlational study. Neuroethics, 6(2), 307–318. doi:10.1007/s12

152-011-9140-6

Ashton, M. C. & Lee, K. (2007). Empirical, theoretical, and practical advantages of

the HEXACO model of personality structure. Personality and Social Psychology

Review, 11(2), 150–166. doi:10.1177/1088868306294907

Ashton, M. C. & Lee, K. (2009). The HEXACO–60: a short measure of the major di-

mensions of personality. Journal of Personality Assessment, 91(4), 340–345. doi:1

0.1080/00223890902935878

Austin, E. J., Farrelly, D., Black, C., & Moore, H. (2007). Emotional intelligence, machi-

avellianism and emotional manipulation: does ei have a dark side? Personality and

Individual Differences, 43(1), 179–189. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.11.019

Bagozzi, R. P., Verbeke, W. J., Dietvorst, R. C., Belschak, F. D., van den Berg, W. E.,

& Rietdijk, W. J. (2013). Theory of mind and empathic explanations of machi-

avellianism a neuroscience perspective. Journal of Management, 39(7), 1760–1798.

doi:10.1177/0149206312471393
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 61

Bardi, A., Lee, J. A., Hofmann-Towfigh, N., & Soutar, G. (2009). The structure of in-

traindividual value change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(5),

913–929. doi:10.1037/a0016617

Baron-Cohen, S. (2012). Zero degrees of empathy: a new theory of human cruelty and

kindness. Penguin Books Ltd.

Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “reading

the mind in the eyes” test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults

with asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology

and Psychiatry, 42(2), 241–251. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00715

Barrick, M. R. & Mount, M. K. (1991). The Big Five personality dimensions and job

performance: a meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 44(1), 1–26. doi:10.1111/j.1

744-6570.1991.tb00688.x

Baskin-Sommers, A., Krusemark, E., & Ronningstam, E. (2014). Empathy in narcissistic

personality disorder: from clinical and empirical perspectives. Personality Disor-

ders: Theory, Research, and Treatment. doi:10.1037/per0000061

Batson, C. D. (2009). These things called empathy: eight related but distinct phenomena.

In J. Decety & W. Ickes (Eds.), The social neuroscience of empathy (pp. 14–34).

Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Bereczkei, T., Deak, A., Papp, P., Perlaki, G., & Orsi, G. (2013). Neural correlates of

machiavellian strategies in a social dilemma task. Brain and Cognition, 82(1), 108–

116. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2013.02.012

Bernhardt, B. C. & Singer, T. (2012). The neural basis of empathy. Annual Review of

Neuroscience, 35, 1–23. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150536


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 62

Birbaumer, N., Veit, R., Lotze, M., Erb, M., Hermann, C., Grodd, W., & Flor, H. (2005).

Deficient fear conditioning in psychopathy: a functional magnetic resonance imag-

ing study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(7), 799–805. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.6

2.7.799

Blair, R. J. R., Colledge, E., Murray, L., & Mitchell, D. (2001). A selective impairment

in the processing of sad and fearful expressions in children with psychopathic ten-

dencies. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29(6), 491–498. doi:10.1023/A:1

012225108281

Bleidorn, W., Klimstra, T. A., Denissen, J. J. A., Rentfrow, P. J., Potter, J., & Gosling, S. D.

(2013). Personality maturation around the world a cross-cultural examination of

social-investment theory. Psychological Science, 24(12), 2530–2540. doi:10.1177

/0956797613498396

Bolton, K. & Kuteeva, M. (2012). English as an academic language at a swedish univer-

sity: parallel language use and the ‘threat’of english. Journal of Multilingual and

Multicultural Development, 33(5), 429–447. doi:10.1080/01434632.2012.67024

Borsboom, D. (2006). The attack of the psychometricians. Psychometrika, 71(3), 425–

440. doi:10.1007/s11336-006-1447-6

Brown, R. P., Budzek, K., & Tamborski, M. (2009). On the meaning and measure of

narcissism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(7), 951–964. doi:10.11

77/0146167209335461

Buckels, E. E., Trapnell, P. D., & Paulhus, D. L. (2014). Trolls just want to have fun.

Personality and Individual Differences, 67, 97–102. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.0

16
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 63

Buckner, R. L., Sepulcre, J., Talukdar, T., Krienen, F. M., Liu, H., Hedden, T., … John-

son, K. A. (2009). Cortical hubs revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity: map-

ping, assessment of stability, and relation to alzheimer’s disease. The Journal of

Neuroscience, 29(6), 1860–1873. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5062-08.2009

Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s mechanical turk a new

source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science,

6(1), 3–5. doi:10.1177/1745691610393980

Cain, N. M., Pincus, A. L., & Ansell, E. B. (2008). Narcissism at the crossroads: pheno-

typic description of pathological narcissism across clinical theory, social/personality

psychology, and psychiatric diagnosis. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(4), 638–

656. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.09.006

Campbell, J., Schermer, J. A., Villani, V. C., Nguyen, B., Vickers, L., & Vernon, P. A.

(2009). A behavioral genetic study of the dark triad of personality and moral devel-

opment. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 12(2), 132–136. doi:10.1375/twin.1

2.2.132

Cascio, C. N., Konrath, S., & Falk, E. B. (2014). Narcissists’ social pain seen only in the

brain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. doi:10.1093/scan/nsu072

Casler, K., Bickel, L., & Hackett, E. (2013). Separate but equal? a comparison of par-

ticipants and data gathered via amazon’s mturk, social media, and face-to-face be-

havioral testing. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(6), 2156–2160. doi:10.1016

/j.chb.2013.05.009

Christie, R. & Geis, F. L. (1970). Studies in machiavellianism. New York: Academic Press.

Cieciuch, J., Davidov, E., Vecchione, M., Beierlein, C., & Schwartz, S. H. (2014). The

cross-national invariance properties of a new scale to measure 19 basic human


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 64

values: a test across eight countries. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45(5),

764–776. doi:10.1177/0022022114527348

Cleckley, H. M. (1988). The mask of sanity. (Original work published 1941). New Amer-

ican Library.

Cliffordson, C. (2001). Parents’ judgments and students’ self-judgments of empathy: the

structure of empathy and agreement of judgments based on the interpersonal re-

activity index (IRI). European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 17(1), 36–47.

doi:10.1027//1015-5759.17.1.36

Cliffordson, C. (2002). The hierarchical structure of empathy: dimensional organization

and relations to social functioning. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 43(1), 49–

59. doi:10.1111/1467-9450.00268

Coll, M., Budell, L., Rainville, P., Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2012). The role of gender in

the interaction between self-pain and the perception of pain in others. The Journal

of Pain, 13(7), 695–703. doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2012.04.009

Cooper, A. M. (1998). Further developments in the clinical diagnosis of narcissistic per-

sonality disorder. In E. Ronningstam (Ed.), Disorders of narcissism: diagnostic,

clinical, and empirical implications (pp. 53–74). Washington, DC: American Psy-

chiatric Press.

Cope, L. M., Ermer, E., Nyalakanti, P. K., Calhoun, V. D., & Kiehl, K. A. (2014). Paral-

imbic gray matter reductions in incarcerated adolescent females with psychopathic

traits. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42(4), 659–668. doi:10.1007/s108

02-013-9810-4
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 65

Corry, N., Merritt, R. D., Mrug, S., & Pamp, B. (2008). The factor structure of the

narcissistic personality inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 90(6), 593–

600. doi:10.1080/00223890802388590

Costa, P. T. & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO personality inventory (NEO PI-R).

Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Cox, C. L., Gotimer, K., Roy, A. K., Castellanos, F. X., Milham, M. P., & Kelly, C. (2010).

Your resting brain cares about your risky behavior. PLoS One, 5(8), e12296. doi:1

0.1371/journal.pone.0012296

Czibor, A. & Bereczkei, T. (2012). Machiavellian people’s success results from monitoring

their partners. Personality and Individual Differences, 53(3), 202–206. doi:10.101

6/j.paid.2012.03.005

Damoiseaux, J., Rombouts, S., Barkhof, F., Scheltens, P., Stam, C., Smith, S. M., &

Beckmann, C. (2006). Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(37), 13848–13853. doi:10

.1073/pnas.0601417103

Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy.

JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10(85).

Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a mul-

tidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(1), 113–

126. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113

Davis, M. H. (1996). Empathy: a social psychological approach. Boulder, Colorado:

Westview Press, Inc.


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 66

DeYoung, C., Hawes, D., Civai, C., & Rustichini, A. (in press). Extraversion predicts

neural sensitivity to reward in large fmri studies. Personality and Individual Differ-

ences, 60, S14. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.07.361

DeYoung, C. G. (2010). Personality neuroscience and the biology of traits. Social and

Personality Psychology Compass, 4(12), 1165–1180. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.20

10.00327.x

DeYoung, C. G., Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., Gray, J. R., Eastman, M., & Grigorenko,

E. L. (2011). Sources of cognitive exploration: genetic variation in the prefrontal

dopamine system predicts openness/intellect. Journal of research in personality,

45(4), 364–371. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2011.04.002

DeYoung, C. G. & Gray, J. R. (2009). Personality neuroscience: explaining individual

differences in affect, behavior, and cognition. In P. J. Corr & G. Matthews (Eds.),

The cambridge handbook of personality psychology (pp. 323–346). New York, NY:

Cambridge University Press.

DeYoung, C. G., Hirsh, J. B., Shane, M. S., Papademetris, X., Rajeevan, N., & Gray, J. R.

(2010). Testing predictions from personality neuroscience brain structure and the

Big Five. Psychological Science, 21(6), 820–828. doi:10.1177/0956797610370159

DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2005). Sources of openness/intellect:

cognitive and neuropsychological correlates of the fifth factor of personality. Jour-

nal of Personality, 73(4), 825–858. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00330.x

DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between facets and domains: 10

aspects of the big five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(5), 880–

896. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.880
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 67

Dimberg, U. & Thunberg, M. (2012). Empathy, emotional contagion, and rapid facial

reactions to angry and happy facial expressions. PsyCh Journal, 1(2), 118–127.

doi:10.1002/pchj.4

Dolan, M. C. & Fullam, R. S. (2009). Psychopathy and functional magnetic resonance

imaging blood oxygenation level-dependent responses to emotional faces in violent

patients with schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 66(6), 570–577. doi:10.1016

/j.biopsych.2009.03.019

Dutton, K. (2012). The wisdom of psychopaths: what saints, spies, and serial killers can

teach us about success. Macmillan.

Edelstein, R. S., Yim, I. S., & Quas, J. A. (2010). Narcissism predicts heightened cortisol

reactivity to a psychosocial stressor in men. Journal of Research in Personality,

44(5), 565–572. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.06.008

Ellis, H. (1898). Auto-erotism. Alienist and Neurologist, 260–299.

Emmons, R. A. (1984). Factor analysis and construct validity of the narcissistic personal-

ity inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48(3), 291–300. doi:10.1207/s1

5327752jpa4803_11

Emmons, R. A. (1987). Narcissism: theory and measurement. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 52(1), 11–17. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.1.11

Ermer, E., Cope, L. M., Nyalakanti, P. K., Calhoun, V. D., & Kiehl, K. A. (2012). Aber-

rant paralimbic gray matter in criminal psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-

ogy, 121(3), 649–658. doi:10.1037/a0026371

Ermer, E., Cope, L. M., Nyalakanti, P. K., Calhoun, V. D., & Kiehl, K. A. (2013). Aber-

rant paralimbic gray matter in incarcerated male adolescents with psychopathic


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 68

traits. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(1),

94–103. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2012.10.013

Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, Ill: Charles C.

Thomas.

Fan, Y., Wonneberger, C., Enzi, B., De Greck, M., Ulrich, C., Tempelmann, C., …

Northoff, G. (2011a). The narcissistic self and its psychological and neural corre-

lates: an exploratory fmri study. Psychological Medicine, 41(08), 1641–1650. doi:1

0.1017/S003329171000228X

Fan, Y., Duncan, N. W., de Greck, M., & Northoff, G. (2011b). Is there a core neural

network in empathy? an fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis. Neuroscience &

Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(3), 903–911. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.009

Farrington, D. P. (1997). The relationship between low resting heart rate and violence.

In A. Raine, P. Brennan, D. P. Farrington, & S. A. Mednick (Eds.), Biosocial bases

of violence: conceptual and theoretical issues (Vol. 292, pp. 89–105). New York,

NY: Plenum Press.

Fehr, B., Samsom, D., & Paulhus, D. L. (1992). The construct of machiavellianism: twenty

years later. In C. D. Spielberger & J. N. Butcher (Eds.), Advances in personality

assessment (Vol. 9, pp. 77–116). New York: Routledge.

Fehr, E. & Rockenbach, B. (2003). Detrimental effects of sanctions on human altruism.

Nature, 422(6928), 137–140. doi:10.1038/nature01474

Ferguson, C. J. (2010). A meta-analysis of normal and disordered personality across the

life span. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(4), 659–667. doi:10.10

37/a0018770
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 69

Field, A. P. (2013). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics (4th). London, UK:

Sage Publications.

Forth, A. E., Kosson, D. S., & Hare, R. D. (2003). Hare psychopathy checklist: youth

version (PCL: YV). Toronto, ON, Canada: Multi-Health Systems.

Fossati, A., Pincus, A. L., Borroni, S., Munteanu, A. F., & Maffei, C. (2014). Are patho-

logical narcissism and psychopathy different constructs or different names for the

same thing? a study based on italian nonclinical adult participants. Journal of Per-

sonality Disorders, 28(3), 394–418. doi:10.1521/pedi_2014_28_127

Franken, I. H., Muris, P., Nijs, I., & van Strien, J. W. (2008). Processing of pleasant

information can be as fast and strong as unpleasant information: implications for

the negativity bias. Netherlands Journal of Psychology, 64(4), 168–176. doi:10.10

07/BF03076419

Fung, L. K., Akil, M., Widge, A., Roberts, L. W., & Etkin, A. (2014). Attitudes toward

neuroscience education among psychiatry residents and fellows. Academic Psychi-

atry, 1–8. doi:10.1007/s40596-014-0034-x

Furnham, A., Richards, S. C., & Paulhus, D. L. (2013). The dark triad of personality: a

10 year review. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7(3), 199–216. doi:1

0.1111/spc3.12018

Giancola, P. R. (2003). The moderating effects of dispositional empathy on alcohol-

related aggression in men and women. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(2),

275–281. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.112.2.275

Glenn, A. L. & Raine, A. (2014). Psychopathy: an introduction to biological findings

and their implications. New York, NY: New York University Press.
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 70

Goldberg, L. R. (1999). A broad-bandwidth, public domain, personality inventory mea-

suring the lower-level facets of several five-factor models. Personality Psychology

in Europe, 7, 7–28.

Goldberg, L. R., Johnson, J. A., Eber, H. W., Hogan, R., Ashton, M. C., Cloninger, C. R.,

& Gough, H. G. (2006). The international personality item pool and the future of

public-domain personality measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 40(1), 84–

96. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2005.08.007

Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann Jr, W. B. (2003). A very brief measure of the

big-five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37(6), 504–528.

doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00046-1

Gray, J. A. (1981). A critique of eysenck’s theory of personality. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.),

A model for personality (pp. 246–276). Springer.

Gunnthorsdottir, A., McCabe, K., & Smith, V. (2002). Using the machiavellianism in-

strument to predict trustworthiness in a bargaining game. Journal of Economic

Psychology, 23(1), 49–66. doi:10.1016/S0167-4870(01)00067-8

Gurtman, M. B. (2009). Exploring personality with the interpersonal circumplex. Social

and Personality Psychology Compass, 3(4), 601–619. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2

009.00172.x

Hall, J. R. & Benning, S. D. (2006). The “successful” psychopath: adaptive and subclin-

ical manifestations of psychopathy in the general population. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.),

The handbook of psychopathy (pp. 459–478). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Hare, R. D. (1985). The Psychopathy Checklist. Unpublished manuscript, University of

British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada.


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 71

Hare, R. D. (1991). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist–Revised. Toronto, ON, Canada:

Multi-Health Systems.

Hare, R. D. (1999). Without conscience: the disturbing world of the psychopaths among

us. New York, NY: New York: Guilford Press.

Hare, R. D. (2003). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist–Revised, 2nd edition. Toronto, ON,

Canada: Multi-Health Systems.

Hare, R. D., Neumann, C. S., & Widiger, T. A. (2013). Psychopathy. In T. A. Widi-

ger (Ed.), The oxford handbook of personality disorders (pp. 478–504). Oxford

University Press.

Harenski, C. L., Harenski, K. A., Shane, M. S., & Kiehl, K. A. (2010). Aberrant neu-

ral processing of moral violations in criminal psychopaths. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology, 119(4), 863–874. doi:10.1037/a0020979

Harpur, T. J. & Hare, R. D. (1994). Assessment of psychopathy as a function of age.

Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103(4), 604–609. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.103

.4.604

Hayes, A. F. (2008). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process

analysis: a regression-based approach. Guilford Press.

Hemphill, J. F. (2003). Interpreting the magnitudes of correlation coefficients. American

Psychologist, 58(1), 78–79. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.58.1.78

Hitlin, S. & Piliavin, J. A. (2004). Values: reviving a dormant concept. Annual review of

sociology, 30, 359–393. doi:10.1146/annurev.soc.30.012703.110640

Hoptman, M. J., D’Angelo, D., Catalano, D., Mauro, C. J., Shehzad, Z. E., Kelly, A. C., …

Milham, M. P. (2010). Amygdalofrontal functional disconnectivity and aggression


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 72

in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36(5), 1020–1028. doi:10.1093/schbul/

sbp012

Hunter, J. E., Gerbing, D. W., & Boster, F. J. (1982). Machiavellian beliefs and personality:

construct invalidity of the machiavellianism dimension. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 43(6), 1293–1305. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.43.6.1293

Insel, T. R. & Quirion, R. (2005). Psychiatry as a clinical neuroscience discipline. Jama,

294(17), 2221–2224.

Intrator, J., Hare, R., Stritzke, P., Brichtswein, K., Dorfman, D., Harpur, T., …, Keilp, J.,

et al. (1997). A brain imaging (single photon emission computerized tomography)

study of semantic and affective processing in psychopaths. Biological Psychiatry,

42(2), 96–103. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00290-9

Jabbi, M., Swart, M., & Keysers, C. (2007). Empathy for positive and negative emotions

in the gustatory cortex. Neuroimage, 34(4), 1744–1753. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.

2006.10.032

Jackson, P. L., Brunet, E., Meltzoff, A. N., & Decety, J. (2006). Empathy examined

through the neural mechanisms involved in imagining how i feel versus how you

feel pain. Neuropsychologia, 44(5), 752–761. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.20

05.07.015

Jackson, P. L., Meltzoff, A. N., & Decety, J. (2005). How do we perceive the pain of oth-

ers? a window into the neural processes involved in empathy. Neuroimage, 24(3),

771–779. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.006

Jakobwitz, S. & Egan, V. (2006). The dark triad and normal personality traits. Personality

and Individual Differences, 40(2), 331–339. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.07.006


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 73

James, S., Kavanagh, P. S., Jonason, P. K., Chonody, J. M., & Scrutton, H. E. (2014).

The dark triad, schadenfreude, and sensational interests: dark personalities, dark

emotions, and dark behaviors. Personality and Individual Differences, 68, 211–

216. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.04.020

John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., & Soto, C. J. (2008). Paradigm shift to the integrative Big

Five trait taxonomy. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook

of personality: theory and research (Vol. 3, pp. 114–158). New York, NY: Guilford

Press.

Jonason, P. K. & Krause, L. (2013). The emotional deficits associated with the dark

triad traits: cognitive empathy, affective empathy, and alexithymia. Personality and

Individual Differences, 55(5), 532–537. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.04.027

Jones, D. N. & Paulhus, D. L. (2009). Machiavellianism. In M. R. Leary & R. H. Hoyle

(Eds.), Handbook of individual differences in social behavior (pp. 93–108). New

York: Guilford Press.

Jones, D. N. & Paulhus, D. L. (2011). Differentiating the dark triad within the interper-

sonal circumplex. In M. Horowitz & S. Strack (Eds.), Handbook of interpersonal

theory and research (pp. 249–267). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Jones, D. N. & Paulhus, D. L. (2014). Introducing the short dark triad (sd3): a brief

measure of dark personality traits. Assessment, 21(1), 28–41. doi:10.1177/10731

91113514105

Judge, T. A., Heller, D., & Mount, M. K. (2002). Five-factor model of personality and

job satisfaction: a meta-analysis. Journal of applied psychology, 87(3), 530–541.

doi:10.1037/0021-9010.87.3.530
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 74

Judge, T. A., Higgins, C. A., Thoresen, C. J., & Barrick, M. R. (1999). The Big Five

personality traits, general mental ability, and career success across the life span.

Personnel Psychology, 52(3), 621–652. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.1999.tb00174.x

Karim, A. A., Schneider, M., Lotze, M., Veit, R., Sauseng, P., Braun, C., & Birbaumer, N.

(2010). The truth about lying: inhibition of the anterior prefrontal cortex improves

deceptive behavior. Cerebral Cortex, 20(1), 205–213. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp090

Kelly, K. J. & Metcalfe, J. (2011). Metacognition of emotional face recognition. Emotion,

11(4), 896. doi:10.1037/a0023746

Kelsey, R. M., Ornduff, S. R., McCann, C. M., & Reiff, S. (2001). Psychophysiological

characteristics of narcissism during active and passive coping. Psychophysiology,

38(2), 292–303. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.3820292

Keysers, C., Wicker, B., Gazzola, V., Anton, J.-L., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2004). A

touching sight: SII/PV activation during the observation and experience of touch.

Neuron, 42(2), 335–346. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00156-4

Kiehl, K. A., Hare, R. D., McDonald, J. J., & Brink, J. (1999). Semantic and affective

processing in psychopaths: an event-related potential (erp) study. Psychophysiology,

36(6), 765–774. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.3660765

Kiehl, K. A., Smith, A. M., Hare, R. D., Mendrek, A., Forster, B. B., Brink, J., & Liddle,

P. F. (2001). Limbic abnormalities in affective processing by criminal psychopaths

as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 50(9),

677–684. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01222-7

Kiehl, K. A., Smith, A. M., Mendrek, A., Forster, B. B., Hare, R. D., & Liddle,

P. F. (2004). Temporal lobe abnormalities in semantic processing by criminal psy-


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 75

chopaths as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Psychiatry Re-

search: Neuroimaging, 130(1), 27–42. doi:10.1016/S0925-4927(03)00106-9

Knoch, D., Pascual-Leone, A., Meyer, K., Treyer, V., & Fehr, E. (2006). Diminishing

reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right prefrontal cortex. Science, 314(5800),

829–832. doi:10.1126/science.1129156

Koenigs, M., Kruepke, M., Zeier, J., & Newman, J. P. (2012). Utilitarian moral judgment

in psychopathy. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(6), 708–714. doi:1

0.1093/scan/nsr048

Konrath, S., Meier, B. P., & Bushman, B. J. (2014). Development and validation of the

single item narcissism scale (SINS). PloS One, 9(8), e103469. doi:10.1371/journal.

pone.0103469

Kubarych, T. S., Deary, I. J., & Austin, E. J. (2004). The narcissistic personality inventory:

factor structure in a non-clinical sample. Personality and Individual Differences,

36(4), 857–872. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00158-2

Larsson, H., Andershed, H., & Lichtenstein, P. (2006). A genetic factor explains most

of the variation in the psychopathic personality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,

115(2), 221. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.115.2.221

Lee, K. & Ashton, M. C. (2004). Psychometric properties of the HEXACO personality

inventory. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39(2), 329–358. doi:10.1207/s1532

7906mbr3902_8

Lee, K. & Ashton, M. C. (2005). Psychopathy, machiavellianism, and narcissism in the

five-factor model and the hexaco model of personality structure. Personality and

Individual Differences, 38(7), 1571–1582. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2004.09.016


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 76

Lee, K. & Ashton, M. C. (2014). The dark triad, the big five, and the hexaco model.

Personality and Individual Differences, 67, 2–5. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.048

Levy, K. N., Ellison, W. D., & Reynoso, J. S. (2011). A historical review of narcissism

and narcissistic personality. In W. K. Campbell & J. D. Miller (Eds.), The handbook

of narcissism and narcissistic personality disorder (pp. 3–13). Hobokoen, NJ: John

Wiley & Sons.

Lilienfeld, S. O. (1998). Methodological advances and developments in the assessment

of psychopathy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(1), 99–125. doi:10.1016/S0

005-7967(97)10021-3

Lilienfeld, S. O. & Andrews, B. P. (1996). Development and preliminary validation of

a self-report measure of psychopathic personality traits in noncriminal population.

Journal of Personality Assessment, 66(3), 488–524. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa66

03_3

Lindeman, M. & Verkasalo, M. (2005). Measuring values with the short schwartz’s value

survey. Journal of Personality Assessment, 85(2), 170–178. doi:10.1207/s1532775

2jpa8502_09

Lobbestael, J., Baumeister, R. F., Fiebig, T., & Eckel, L. A. (2014). The role of grandiose

and vulnerable narcissism in self-reported and laboratory aggression and testos-

terone reactivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 69, 22–27. doi:10.1016

/j.paid.2014.05.007

Locke, K. D. (2000). Circumplex scales of interpersonal values: reliability, validity, and

applicability to interpersonal problems and personality disorders. Journal of Per-

sonality Assessment, 75(2), 249–267. doi:10.1207/S15327752JPA7502_6


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 77

Lönnqvist, J.-E., Walkowitz, G., Verkasalo, M., & Wichardt, P. C. (2011). Situational

power moderates the influence of self-transcendence vs. self-enhancement values

on behavior in ultimatum bargaining. Journal of Research in Personality, 45(3),

336–339. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2011.03.003

Lorber, M. F. (2004). Psychophysiology of aggression, psychopathy, and conduct prob-

lems: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 130(4), 531. doi:10.1037/0033-290

9.130.4.531

Lyons, M., Caldwell, T., & Shultz, S. (2010). Mind-reading and manipulation—is machi-

avellianism related to theory of mind? Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 8(3),

261–274. doi:10.1556/JEP.8.2010.3.7

Machiavelli, N. (1531/2007). The discourses. New York, NY: Dover Publications Inc.

Machiavelli, N. (1532/1992). The prince. New York, NY: Dover Publications Inc.

Mar, R. A., Spreng, R. N., & DeYoung, C. G. (2013). How to produce personality neu-

roscience research with high statistical power and low additional cost. Cognitive,

Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 13(3), 674–685. doi:10.3758/s13415-013-

0202-6

Matthews, G., Deary, I. J., & Whiteman, M. C. (2003). Personality traits (2nd ed.). Cam-

bridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Mazza, M., De Risio, A., Tozzini, C., Roncone, R., & Casacchia, M. (2003). Machiavel-

lianism and theory of mind in people affected by schizophrenia. Brain and Cogni-

tion, 51(3), 262–269. doi:10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00018-6

McCrae, R. R. (2009). The five-factor model of personality traits: consensus and contro-

versy. In P. J. Corr & G. Matthews (Eds.), The cambridge handbook of personality

psychology (pp. 148–161). Cambridge University Press.


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 78

McCrae, R. R. & Costa, P. T. (2003). Personality in adulthood: a five-factor theory per-

spective. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

McHoskey, J. W. (1999). Machiavellianism, intrinsic versus extrinsic goals, and social

interest: a self-determination theory analysis. Motivation and Emotion, 23(4), 267–

283. doi:10.1023/A:1021338809469

McNamara, P., Durso, R., & Harris, E. (2007). “machiavellianism” and frontal dysfunc-

tion: evidence from parkinson’s disease. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 12(4), 285–

300. doi:10.1080/13546800701202316

Meehl, P. E. (1978). Theoretical risks and tabular asterisks: sir karl, sir ronald, and the

slow progress of soft psychology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,

46(4), 806–834. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.46.4.806

Messick, S. (1995). Validity of psychological assessment: validation of inferences from

persons’ responses and performances as scientific inquiry into score meaning. Amer-

ican Psychologist, 50(9), 741–749.

Meyer, G. J., Finn, S. E., Eyde, L. D., Kay, G. G., Moreland, K. L., Dies, R. R., … Reed,

G. M. (2001). Psychological testing and psychological assessment: a review of evi-

dence and issues. American psychologist, 56(2), 128–165. doi:10.1037/0003-066

X.56.2.128

Miller, J. D. & Campbell, W. K. (2011). Addressing criticisms of the narcissistic per-

sonality inventory (npi). In W. K. Campbell & J. D. Miller (Eds.), The handbook

of narcissism and narcissistic personality disorder (pp. 146–152). Hobokoen, NJ:

John Wiley & Sons.


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 79

Miller, J. D., Hoffman, B. J., Gaughan, E. T., Gentile, B., Maples, J., & Keith Campbell,

W. (2011). Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism: a nomological network analysis.

Journal of Personality, 79(5), 1013–1042. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00711.x

Miller, J. D., Price, J., & Campbell, W. K. (2012). Is the narcissistic personality inventory

still relevant? a test of independent grandiosity and entitlement scales in the assess-

ment of narcissism. Assessment, 19(1), 8–13. doi:10.1177/1073191111429390

Milojev, P., Osborne, D., Greaves, L. M., Barlow, F. K., & Sibley, C. G. (2013). The mini-

ipip6: tiny yet highly stable markers of big six personality. Journal of Research in

Personality, 47(6), 936–944. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2013.09.004

Morelli, S. A., Rameson, L. T., & Lieberman, M. D. (2012). The neural components of

empathy: predicting daily prosocial behavior. Social Cognitive and Affective Neu-

roscience. doi:10.1093/scan/nss088

Müller, J. L., Sommer, M., Wagner, V., Lange, K., Taschler, H., Röder, C. H., … Hajak, G.

(2003). Abnormalities in emotion processing within cortical and subcortical regions

in criminal psychopaths: evidence from a functional magnetic resonance imaging

study using pictures with emotional content. Biological Psychiatry, 54(2), 152–162.

doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01749-3

Myyry, L., Juujärvi, S., & Pesso, K. (2010). Empathy, perspective taking and personal

values as predictors of moral schemas. Journal of Moral Education, 39(2), 213–

233. doi:10.1080/03057241003754955

Nelson, T. O. (1990). Metamemory: a theoretical framework and new findings. Psychol-

ogy of Learning and Motivation, 26, 125–173. doi:10.1016/S0079-7421(08)6005

3-5
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 80

Nestor, P. G., Nakamura, M., Niznikiewicz, M., Thompson, E., Levitt, J. J., Choate, V.,

… McCarley, R. W. (2012). In search of the functional neuroanatomy of sociality:

mri subdivisions of orbital frontal cortex and social cognition. Social Cognitive and

Affective Neuroscience, nss018. doi:10.1093/scan/nss018

O’Boyle Jr, E. H., Forsyth, D. R., Banks, G. C., & McDaniel, M. A. (2012). A meta-

analysis of the dark triad and work behavior: a social exchange perspective. Journal

of Applied Psychology, 97(3), 557–579. doi:10.1037/a0025679

Olver, J. M. & Mooradian, T. A. (2003). Personality traits and personal values: a concep-

tual and empirical integration. Personality and Individual Differences, 35(1), 109–

125. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00145-9

Ortiz, J. & Raine, A. (2004). Heart rate level and antisocial behavior in children and ado-

lescents: a meta-analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent

Psychiatry, 43(2), 154–162. doi:10.1097/00004583-200402000-00010

Panitz, E. (1989). Psychometric investigation of the MACH–IV scale measuring Machi-

avellianism. Psychological Reports, 64(3), 963–968. doi:10.2466/pr0.1989.64.3.9

63

Pardini, D. A., Raine, A., Erickson, K., & Loeber, R. (2014). Lower amygdala volume in

men is associated with childhood aggression, early psychopathic traits, and future

violence. Biological Psychiatry, 75(1), 73–80. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.04.003

Parks, L. & Guay, R. P. (2009). Personality, values, and motivation. Personality and

Individual Differences, 47(7), 675–684. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.002

Parks-Leduc, L., Feldman, G., & Bardi, A. (2014). Personality traits and personal values:

a meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review. doi:10.1177/1088868

314538548
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 81

Patrick, C. J. (2006). Preface. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), The handbook of psychopathy

(pp. xiii–xvi). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Paulhus, D. L. & Williams, K. M. (2002). The dark triad of personality: narcissism,

machiavellianism, and psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36(6), 556–

563. doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00505-6

Pincus, A. L., Ansell, E. B., Pimentel, C. A., Cain, N. M., Wright, A. G., & Levy, K. N.

(2009). Initial construction and validation of the pathological narcissism inventory.

Psychological Assessment, 21(3), 365. doi:10.1037/a0016530

Pincus, A. L. & Lukowitsky, M. R. (2010). Pathological narcissism and narcissistic per-

sonality disorder. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 421–446. doi:10.114

6/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131215

Poropat, A. E. (2009). A meta-analysis of the five-factor model of personality and aca-

demic performance. Psychological bulletin, 135(2), 322–338. doi:10.1037/a0014

996

Premack, D. & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Be-

havioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515–526. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00076512

Pulos, S., Elison, J., & Lennon, R. (2004). The hierarchical structure of the interpersonal

reactivity index. Social Behavior & Personality: An International Journal, 32(4),

355–360.

Raine, A., Ishikawa, S. S., Arce, E., Lencz, T., Knuth, K. H., Bihrle, S., … Colletti, P.

(2004). Hippocampal structural asymmetry in unsuccessful psychopaths. Biologi-

cal Psychiatry, 55(2), 185–191. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00727-3


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 82

Ramanaiah, N. V., Byravan, A., & Detwiler, F. R. J. (1994). Revised NEO personality

inventory profiles of machiavellian and non-machiavellian people. Psychological

Reports, 75(2), 937–938. doi:10.2466/pr0.1994.75.2.937

Raskin, R. N. & Hall, C. S. (1979). A narcissistic personality inventory. Psychological

Reports, 45(2), 590–590. doi:10.2466/pr0.1979.45.2.590

Raskin, R. & Terry, H. (1988). A principal-components analysis of the narcissistic per-

sonality inventory and further evidence of its construct validity. Journal of Person-

ality and Social Psychology, 54(5), 890–902. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.890

Rauthmann, J. F. (2012). Towards multifaceted machiavellianism: content, factorial, and

construct validity of a german machiavellianism scale. Personality and Individual

Differences, 52(3), 345–351. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.10.038

Rauthmann, J. F. (2013). Investigating the MACH–IV with item response theory and

proposing the Trimmed Mach*. Journal of Personality Assessment, 95(4), 388–

397. doi:10.1080/00223891.2012.742905

Rauthmann, J. F. & Will, T. (2011). Proposing a multidimensional machiavellianism

conceptualization. Social Behavior & Personality: An International Journal, 39(3),

391–404. doi:10.2224/sbp.2011.39.3.391

Ray, J. J. (1983). Defective validity of the machiavellianism scale. Journal of Social Psy-

chology, 119(2), 291–292. doi:10.1080/00224545.1983.9922836

Rhodewalt, F. & Peterson, B. (2009). Narcissism. In M. R. Leary & R. H. Hoyle (Eds.),

Handbook of individual differences in social behavior (pp. 547–560). New York:

Guilford Press.

Rilling, J. K., Glenn, A. L., Jairam, M. R., Pagnoni, G., Goldsmith, D. R., Elfenbein,

H. A., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2007). Neural correlates of social cooperation and non-
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 83

cooperation as a function of psychopathy. Biological Psychiatry, 61(11), 1260–

1271. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.021

Roberts, B. W., Kuncel, N. R., Shiner, R., Caspi, A., & Goldberg, L. R. (2007). The

power of personality: the comparative validity of personality traits, socioeconomic

status, and cognitive ability for predicting important life outcomes. Perspectives on

Psychological Science, 2(4), 313–345. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00047.x

Roccas, S., Sagiv, L., Schwartz, S. H., & Knafo, A. (2002). The Big Five personality

factors and personal values. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(6), 789–

801. doi:10.1177/0146167202289008

Rohan, M. J. (2000). A rose by any name? the values construct. Personality and social

psychology review, 4(3), 255–277. doi:10.1207/S15327957PSPR0403_4

Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. New York, NY: Free Press.

Rosenthal, S. A. & Hooley, J. M. (2010). Narcissism assessment in social–personality

research: does the association between narcissism and psychological health result

from a confound with self-esteem? Journal of Research in Personality, 44(4), 453–

465. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.05.008

Ryckman, R. M., Thornton, B., & Butler, J. C. (1994). Personality correlates of the hy-

percompetitive attitude scale: validity tests of horney’s theory of neurosis. Journal

of Personality Assessment, 62(1), 84–94. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa6201_8

Samuel, D. B. & Widiger, T. A. (2008). A meta-analytic review of the relationships be-

tween the five-factor model and< i> dsm-iv-tr</i> personality disorders: a facet level

analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(8), 1326–1342. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2008

.07.002
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 84

Saucier, G. (2002). Gone too far—or not far enough? comments on the article by ashton

and lee (2001). European Journal of Personality, 16(1), 55–62. doi:10.1002/per.4

32

Saulsman, L. M. & Page, A. C. (2004). The five-factor model and personality disorder em-

pirical literature: a meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 23(8), 1055–

1085. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2002.09.001

Saxe, R. & Powell, L. J. (2006). It’s the thought that counts specific brain regions for one

component of theory of mind. Psychological Science, 17(8), 692–699. doi:10.111

1/j.1467-9280.2006.01768.x

Schermer, J. A., Feather, N. T., Zhu, G., & Martin, N. G. (2008). Phenotypic, genetic,

and environmental properties of the portrait values questionnaire. Twin Research

and Human Genetics, 11(5), 531–537. doi:10.1375/twin.11.5.531

Schönbrodt, F. D. & Perugini, M. (2013). At what sample size do correlations stabilize?

Journal of Research in Personality, 47(5), 609–612. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2013.05.009

Schulze, L., Dziobek, I., Vater, A., Heekeren, H. R., Bajbouj, M., Renneberg, B., …

Roepke, S. (2013). Gray matter abnormalities in patients with narcissistic person-

ality disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47(10), 1363–1369. doi:10.1016

/j.jpsychires.2013.05.017

Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: theoretical

advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in ex-

perimental social psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 1–65). Orlando, FL: Academic.

Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Are there universal aspects in the structure and contents of human

values? Journal of Social Issues, 50(4), 19–45. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.1994.tb0

1196.x
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 85

Schwartz, S. H. (2003). A proposal for measuring value orientations across nations. Ques-

tionnaire Package of the European Social Survey, 259–290.

Schwartz, S. H. (2007). Universalism values and the inclusiveness of our moral universe.

Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(6), 711–728. doi:10.1177/0022022107

308992

Schwartz, S. H. (2012). An overview of the schwartz theory of basic values. Online Read-

ings in Psychology and Culture, 2(1), 1–20. doi:10.9707/2307-0919.1116

Schwartz, S. H. & Bilsky, W. (1987). Toward a Universal Psychological Structure of

Human Values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(3), 550–562. doi:1

0.1037//0022-3514.53.3.550

Schwartz, S. H., Cieciuch, J., Vecchione, M., Davidov, E., Fischer, R., Beierlein, C., …,

Demirutku, K., et al. (2012). Refining the theory of basic individual values. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(4), 663–688. doi:10.1037/a0029393

Schwartz, S. H., Melech, G., Lehmann, A., Burgess, S., Harris, M., & Owens, V. (2001).

Extending the cross-cultural validity of the theory of basic human values with a dif-

ferent method of measurement. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 32(5), 519–

542. doi:10.1177/0022022101032005001

Seara-Cardoso, A. & Viding, E. (2014). Functional neuroscience of psychopathic person-

ality in adults. Journal of Personality. doi:10.1111/jopy.12113

Shehzad, Z., Kelly, A. C., Reiss, P. T., Gee, D. G., Gotimer, K., Uddin, L. Q., …, Biswal,

B. B., et al. (2009). The resting brain: unconstrained yet reliable. Cerebral Cortex,

19(10), 2209–2229. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn256

Sibley, C. G., Luyten, N., Purnomo, M., Mobberley, A., Wootton, L. W., Hammond,

M. D., … Wilson, M. S. (2011). The Mini-IPIP6: validation and extension of a


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 86

short measure of the big-six factors of personality in new zealand. New Zealand

Journal of Psychology, 40(3), 142–159.

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2004).

Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Sci-

ence, 303(5661), 1157–1162. doi:10.1126/science.1093535

Skeem, J. L., Polaschek, D. L. L., Patrick, C. J., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2011). Psychopathic

personality bridging the gap between scientific evidence and public policy. Psycho-

logical Science in the Public Interest, 12(3), 95–162. doi:10.1177/152910061142

6706

Spitzer, M., Fischbacher, U., Herrnberger, B., Grön, G., & Fehr, E. (2007). The neu-

ral signature of social norm compliance. Neuron, 56(1), 185–196. doi:10 . 1016

/j.neuron.2007.09.011

Steel, P., Schmidt, J., & Shultz, J. (2008). Refining the relationship between personality

and subjective well-being. Psychological bulletin, 134(1), 138–161. doi:10.1037/0

033-2909.134.1.138

Stewart, A. E. & Stewart, E. A. (2006). The preference to excel and its relationship to

selected personality variables. Journal of Individual Psychology, 62(3), 270–284.

Tassy, S., Oullier, O., Duclos, Y., Coulon, O., Mancini, J., Deruelle, C., … Wicker, B.

(2011). Disrupting the right prefrontal cortex alters moral judgement. Social Cog-

nitive and Affective Neuroscience, nsr008. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr008

Terracciano, A., Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (2006). Personality plasticity after age 30.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(8), 999–1009. doi:10.1177/01461

67206288599
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 87

Twenge, J. M., Baumeister, R. F., Tice, D. M., & Stucke, T. S. (2001). If you can’t join

them, beat them: effects of social exclusion on aggressive behavior. Journal of Per-

sonality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 1058–1069. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.6

.1058

Vachon, D. D., Lynam, D. R., & Johnson, J. A. (2014). The (non)relation between empa-

thy and aggression: surprising results from a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin,

140(3), 751–773. doi:10.1037/a0035236

Verbeke, W. J., Rietdijk, W. J., van den Berg, W. E., Dietvorst, R. C., Worm, L., &

Bagozzi, R. P. (2011). The making of the machiavellian brain: a structural mri anal-

ysis. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 4(4), 205–216. doi:10

.1037/a0025802

Vernon, P. A., Villani, V. C., Vickers, L. C., & Harris, J. A. (2008). A behavioral genetic

investigation of the dark triad and the big 5. Personality and Individual Differences,

44(2), 445–452. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.09.007

Verplanken, B. & Holland, R. W. (2002). Motivated decision making: effects of activa-

tion and self-centrality of values on choices and behavior. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 82(3), 434–447. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.3.434

Vul, E., Harris, C., Winkielman, P., & Pashler, H. (2009). Puzzlingly high correlations

in fMRI studies of emotion, personality, and social cognition. Perspectives on Psy-

chological Science, 4(3), 274–290. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01125.x

Wai, M. & Tiliopoulos, N. (2012). The affective and cognitive empathic nature of the

dark triad of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 52(7), 794–799.

doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.01.008
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 88

Wälder, R. (1925). The psychoses: their mechanisms and accessibility to influence. Inter-

national Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 6, 259–281.

Walter, H. (2012). Social cognitive neuroscience of empathy: concepts, circuits, and genes.

Emotion Review, 4(1), 9–17. doi:10.1177/1754073911421379

Wassermann, E. M., Greenberg, B. D., Nguyen, M. B., & Murphy, D. L. (2001). Motor

cortex excitability correlates with an anxiety-related personality trait. Biological

psychiatry, 50(5), 377–382. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01210-0

Wastell, C. & Booth, A. (2003). Machiavellianism: an alexithymic perspective. Journal of

Social and Clinical Psychology, 22(6), 730–744. doi:10.1521/jscp.22.6.730.22931

Watson, P. J., Grisham, S. O., Trotter, M. V., & Biderman, M. D. (1984). Narcissism

and empathy: validity evidence for the narcissistic personality inventory. Journal of

Personality Assessment, 48(3), 301–305. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4803_12

Watson, P. J. & Morris, R. J. (1991). Narcissism, empathy and social desirability. Per-

sonality and Individual Differences, 12(6), 575–579. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(91

)90253-8

Widiger, T. A. (2013). Historical developments and current issues. In T. A. Widiger (Ed.),

The oxford handbook of personality disorders (pp. 13–34). New York, NY: Oxford

University Press.

Williamson, S., Harpur, T. J., & Hare, R. D. (1991). Abnormal processing of affective

words by psychopaths. Psychophysiology, 28(3), 260–273. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8

986.1991.tb02192.x

Yang, Y. & Raine, A. (2009). Prefrontal structural and functional brain imaging findings

in antisocial, violent, and psychopathic individuals: a meta-analysis. Psychiatry Re-

search: Neuroimaging, 174(2), 81–88. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.03.012


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 89

Yang, Y., Raine, A., Colletti, P., Toga, A. W., & Narr, K. L. (2009). Abnormal temporal

and prefrontal cortical gray matter thinning in psychopaths. Molecular Psychiatry,

14(6), 561–562. doi:10.1038/mp.2009.12

Yang, Y., Raine, A., Colletti, P., Toga, A. W., & Narr, K. L. (2010). Morphological al-

terations in the prefrontal cortex and the amygdala in unsuccessful psychopaths.

Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(3), 546–554. doi:10.1037/a0019611

Yang, Y., Raine, A., Lencz, T., Bihrle, S., LaCasse, L., & Colletti, P. (2005). Volume re-

duction in prefrontal gray matter in unsuccessful criminal psychopaths. Biological

Psychiatry, 57(10), 1103–1108. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.021

Yarkoni, T. (2009). Big correlations in little studies: inflated fMRI correlations reflect low

statistical power—commentary on Vul et al.(2009). Perspectives on Psychological

Science, 4(3), 294–298. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01127.x

Yarkoni, T. (2014). Neurobiological substrates of personality: a critical overview. In M. L.

Cooper & R. J. Larsen (Eds.), Apa handbook of personality and social psychology

(Vol. 4). American Psychological Association.

Zaki, J. & Ochsner, K. N. (2012). The neuroscience of empathy: progress, pitfalls and

promise. Nature Neuroscience, 15(5), 675–680. doi:10.1038/nn.3085

Zuo, X.-N., Kelly, C., Adelstein, J. S., Klein, D. F., Castellanos, F. X., & Milham, M. P.

(2010). Reliable intrinsic connectivity networks: test–retest evaluation using ica

and dual regression approach. Neuroimage, 49(3), 2163–2177. doi:10 . 1016 / j .

neuroimage.2009.10.080
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 90

Appendix A

Table 1

Partial Correlations between Big Five/Six and Dark Triad

Machiavellianism Narcissism Psychopathy

Extraversion1 -.14 .64 .01


Agreeableness1 -.43 .29 -.23
Conscientiousness1 -.02 .05 -.33
Neuroticism1 .21 -.41 -.04
Openness1 -.23 .18 .03

Extraversion2 -.20 .64 .05


Agreeableness2 -.21 .22 -.28
Conscientiousness2 .09 .22 -.27
Neuroticism2 -.13 -.25 .25
Openness2 -.31 .28 .00
Honesty-Humility2 -.31 -.41 -.10

Note. 1 denotes Study I, and 2 denotes Study II. Correlations larger than 0.2 are
significant at p < 0.05 in both studies.
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES 91

Appendix B

Table 2

Factor Loading Component Matrix based on a Principal Components Analysis for 18


items from MI6-A, IRI-EC, and IRI-PT

Items Component 1

Sympathize with others’ feelings (MI6-A) .856


I often have tender, concerned feelings for people less fortunate than me. .832
(EC)
Feel others’ emotions (MI6-A) .817
Other people’s misfortunes do not usually disturb me a great deal. (EC) .802
Am not really interested in others (MI6-A) .772
I am often quite touched by things that I see happen. (EC) .751
When I see someone being taken advantage of, I feel kind of protective .738
towards them. (EC)
I sometimes try to understand my friends better by imagining how things .733
look from their perspective. (PT)
When I see someone being treated unfairly, I sometimes don’t feel very .733
much pity for them. (EC)
I sometimes find it difficult to see things from the ”other guy’s” point of .721
view. (PT)
Am not interested in other people’s problems. (MI6-A) .710
I would describe myself as a pretty soft-hearted person. (EC) .704
Before criticizing somebody, I try to imagine how I would feel if I were in .701
their place. (PT)
Sometimes I don’t feel very sorry for other people when they are having .699
problems. (EC)
When I’m upset at someone, I usually try to ”put myself in his shoes” for .698
a while. (PT)
I believe that there are two sides to every question and try to look at them .653
both. (PT)
I try to look at everybody’s side of a disagreement before I make a decision. .615
(PT)
If I’m sure I’m right about something, I don’t waste much time listening .460
to other people’s arguments. (PT)

Note. Factor loadings < .3 are suppressed. N = 180.


PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES
Appendix C

Extraversion Neuroticism
0.8 0.8
0.6 0.6
Study Study
0.4 0.4
Correlation

Correlation
0.2 Meta 0.2 Meta
0.0 0.0
S1 S1
−0.2 −0.2
−0.4 S2 −0.4 S2
−0.6 −0.6
−0.8 −0.8
SE TR CO BE UN SD ST HE AC PO SE TR CO BE UN SD ST HE AC PO
Value Type Value Type

Agreeableness Openness to Experience


0.8 0.8
0.6 0.6
Study Study
0.4 0.4
Correlation

Correlation
0.2 Meta 0.2 Meta
0.0 0.0
S1 S1
−0.2 −0.2
−0.4 S2 −0.4 S2
−0.6 −0.6
−0.8 −0.8
SE TR CO BE UN SD ST HE AC PO SE TR CO BE UN SD ST HE AC PO
Value Type Value Type
Figure 1. Comparison between the two studies conducted
Conscientiousness
0.8 in this thesis and a meta-analysis of personality and values
0.6
Study
0.4
(Parks-Leduc et al., 2014). Study I, and Study II use Pear-
Correlation

0.2 Meta
0.0
S1
−0.2 son’s r and the adopted numbers are corrected correlations
−0.4 S2
−0.6
(ρ). Note that the two additional values from Study II (Face
−0.8

92
SE TR CO BE UN SD ST HE AC PO
Value Type and Humility) are not included because they are not included

in the meta-analysis.
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES
Appendix D

Correlations between DT and Values Study 1 Correlations between DT and Values Study 2
1.00 1.00

0.75 0.75

0.50 0.50

0.25 DT 0.25 DT
Correlation

Correlation
Mach Mach
0.00 0.00
Narc Narc
−0.25 −0.25
Psych Psych

−0.50 −0.50

−0.75 −0.75

−1.00 −1.00
SE TR CO BE UN SD ST HE AC PO FA SE TR CO HU BE UN SD ST HE AC PO
Value Type Value Type

Figure 2. The graph on the left depicts correlations between DT and PQV-40 (Study I), and

the graph on the right depicts correlations between DT and PVQ-RR (Study II). Note that the

two studies include ten and twelve value types, respectively. The order of the PVQ categories

illustrate that the orthogonal properties derived from the circumplex hold very well in relation

to DT, with the exception of the Benevolence category in Study II.

93
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES
Appendix E

Table 3

Intercorrelations of Personal Values (Study I)

SE TR CO BE UN SD ST HE AC PO

Security 1
Tradition .44 1
Conformity .34 .51 1
Benevolence -.12 .24 .14 1
Universalism .02 .11 -.07 .36 1
Self-direction -.09 -.20 -.37 -.03 .28 1
Stimulation -.62 -.46 -.54 -.12 -.08 .14 1
Hedonism -.34 -.52 -.31 -.34 -.37 -.07 .43 1
Achievement -.25 -.46 -.29 -.42 -.46 -.23 .08 .11 1
Power -.22 -.50 -.30 -.44 -.49 -.20 .05 .20 .57 1

Note. N = 155.

94
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES
Appendix F

Table 4

Pearson correlations between personal values (PVQ-RR) and Big Six (Study II)

SE TR CO HU BE UN SD ST HE AC PO FA

Extraversion -.12 -.02 -.32 -.33 .11 -.03 -.12 .31 .25 .30 .22 -.03
Agreeableness -.15 .04 .12 .24 .35 .50 -.19 -.08 -.10 -.21 -.49 -.24
Conscientiousness .35 .15 .09 .01 .04 -.12 -.04 -.27 -.20 .02 -.06 .04
Neuroticism -.08 -.06 .02 -.08 -.12 -.12 -.03 .11 .07 .06 .15 .14
Openness -.18 -.28 -.20 -.15 .20 .19 .24 .18 .14 .09 -.11 .02
Honesty–Humility .03 .12 .34 .55 .18 .37 .01 -.26 -.29 -.45 -.61 -.33

Note. N = 193. Correlations equal to, or larger than, 0.12 are significant at p = 0.05 level. Bold denotes correlations larger
than 0.3 in accordance with what Hemphill (2003) refers to as a large effect. SE = Security, TR = Tradition, CO = Confor-
mity, HU = Humility, BE = Benevolence, UN = Universalism, SD = Self-direction, ST = Stimulation, HE = Hedonism, AC =
Achievement, PO = Power, FA = Face.

95
PERSONALITY NEUROSCIENCE AND DARK VALUES
Appendix G

Table 5

Pearson correlations between Big Six, Dark Triad, and IRI-EC/PT (Study II)

O C E A N HH MA NA PS EC PT

Openness 1
Conscientiousness .03 1
Extraversion .17 .11 1
Agreeableness .29 .03 .13 1
Neuroticism -.22 -.30 -.20 -.17 1
Honesty–Humility -.11 -.05 -.29 .18 -.12 1
Machiavellianism -.05 -.01 .08 -.36 .08 -.51 1
Narcissism .25 .14 .65 .00 -.15 -.54 .36 1
Psychopathy .03 -.18 .22 -.39 .19 -.45 .60 .41 1
Empathic Concern .16 .10 .10 .81 -.16 .16 -.32 .01 -.34 1
Perspective Taking .35 .14 .18 .69 -.24 .08 -.29 .13 -.30 .61 1

Note. N = 193. Correlations equal to, or larger than, 0.15 are significant at p = 0.05 level. Bold denotes correlations larger
than 0.3 in accordance with what Hemphill (2003) refers to as a large effect.

96

You might also like