You are on page 1of 7

International Journal of Law and Psychiatry 46 (2016) 103–109

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

International Journal of Law and Psychiatry

Early parenting styles and sexual offending behavior: A comparative study


Vera Sigre-Leirós a,⁎, Joana Carvalho a,b, Pedro J. Nobre a
a
Faculdade de Psicologia e de Ciências da Educação da Universidade do Porto, Rua Alfredo Allen, 4200-135 Porto, Portugal
b
Universidade Lusófona de Humanidades e Tecnologias, Lisboa, Portugal

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Available online 25 March 2016 Sexual offenders, in general, report problematic rearing practices from their parents, lacking however more em-
pirical research on this topic regarding particular subtypes of offenders. The current study examined the relation-
Keywords: ship between early parenting styles and different types of sexual offending. A total of 113 sexual offenders
Early parenting styles (rapists, pedophilic and nonpedophilic child molesters), and 51 nonsexual offenders completed the EMBU (My
Distal factors Memories of Upbringing), the Brief Symptom Inventory, and the Socially Desirable Response Set Measure. Results
Sexual offending
showed that rapists were less likely to remember their fathers as being emotionally warm compared with non-
Sexual offenders
Pedophilic offenders
sexual offenders and pedophilic child molesters. In addition, compared with rapists, pedophilic offenders per-
ceived their mothers as having been less emotionally warm to them. Overall, results showed that certain
developmental experiences with parents were able to distinguish between subtypes of offenders supporting
an association between distal interpersonal factors and sexual offending. These findings may have important im-
plications for early intervention and prevention of sexual crimes. Further research using larger samples of pedo-
philic child molesters is recommended.
© 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction Ward & Siegert, 2002). Overall, the present study focused on the
examination of the role of distal factors, in particular, the parental
Sexual offending represents a significant and widespread problem rearing practices experienced by sex offenders during childhood and
within our society. The empirical examination of its potential causes adolescence.
and risk factors is crucial to the development of effective prevention
strategies as well as adequate treatment programs aiming to decrease 1.1. Early parent–child relationships and attachment style
the likelihood of sexual recidivism (e.g. Bumby & Hansen, 1997; Lee,
Jackson, Pattison, & Ward, 2002; Ward & Siegert, 2002). Bowlby (1969, 1973, 1980) has clearly influenced research and un-
The achievements in this field of knowledge have been significant, derstanding about the nature of human relationships across the life
evolving from the emphasis on single factors (e.g., cognitive distortions) span (Pietromonaco & Barrett, 2000). According to Bowlby (1969,
to the acknowledgement of the role of different types of factors includ- 1973), the attachment system is basic and biologically adaptive leading
ing biological, developmental, psychological, social, and situational (e.g. the child to seek and maintain proximity to the primary attachment fig-
Bumby & Hansen, 1997; Hall & Hirschman, 1991, 1992; Marshall & ure, particularly the mother. The quality of the responsiveness of the at-
Barbaree, 1990; Ward & Beech, 2006; Ward & Siegert, 2002). Indeed, a tachment figure is an important factor in the regulation of child's
comprehensive theory of sexual offending should be multifactorial, pro- emotional experience as well as on child's later interpersonal function-
posing how developmental experiences, individual features, sociocul- ing. Specifically, the same author proposed the concept of “internal
tural aspects, and subsequent mechanisms result in important working models” as internalized representations of the self and others
vulnerabilities (Ó Ciardha & Ward, 2013; Ward, Polaschek, & Beech, that result from the nature of early attachment bonds. Thus, a child
2006; Ward & Siegert, 2002). Therefore, the conceptualization of this whose parents are emotionally available and supportive will be able to
behavior presupposes the interaction between two general types of develop a self model as being lovable and competent. In turn, experi-
factors: distal (dispositional or vulnerability characteristics that result ences of emotional unavailability and lack of support will contribute to
from developmental history) and proximal (state or contextual factors the perception of the self as unlovable and incompetent (Bowlby, 1969,
that trigger the underlying vulnerabilities; Ward & Hudson, 1998; 1973). Indeed, in the first years of life, family is considered as primarily
responsible for child's care and development, and the parenting style ex-
perienced is proposed as a key precursor of the child's attachment pat-
⁎ Corresponding author. Tel.: +351 259 331 098; fax: +351 226 07 9 725. tern (Davids, Londt, & Wilson, 2015). Particularly, attachment security
E-mail address: vera.sigre.leiros@gmail.com (V. Sigre-Leirós). has been associated with parental attitudes of affection, emotional

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijlp.2016.02.042
0160-2527/© 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
104 V. Sigre-Leirós et al. / International Journal of Law and Psychiatry 46 (2016) 103–109

warmth and the promotion of the child's autonomy and independence. general, findings seem to indicate a differential relationship between
In turn, attachment insecurity has been related to a parental style charac- early maternal and paternal practices and later sexual offending sub-
terized by indifference, neglect, or overprotection, representing a signif- types (Simons, Wurtele, & Durham, 2008; Smallbone & Dadds, 1998;
icant pathogenic variable (Bogaerts, Vanheule, & Declercq, 2005; Smallbone & McCabe, 2003). Specifically, some studies have found
Giotakos, Vaidakis, Markianos, Spandoni, & Christodoulou, 2004; that rapists and child molesters reported more problematic relation-
Parker, Tupling, & Brown, 1979). ships with their fathers than with their mothers, although this seemed
The majority of the available studies aiming to investigate the to be more frequent in rapists (Lisak, 1994; McCormack, Hudson, &
individual's perception of early parenting behavior have been conduct- Ward, 2002). When compared with nonoffenders, rapists reported
ed with the Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI; Parker et al., 1979), which higher father abusiveness whereas child molesters reported higher
is comprised of two dimensions (care and overprotection/control), or mother abusiveness (Giotakos et al., 2004).
with the Egna Minnen Betraffande Uppfostra — My Memories of Upbring- Regarding attachment styles, contrary to child molesters who re-
ing (EMBU; Perris, Jacobsson, Linndstrom, Von Knorring, & Perris, 1980) ported more anxious attachment bonds to both mother and father, rap-
which is comprised of three dimensions (emotional warmth, rejection, ists reported more avoidant attachment bonds particularly to the father
and overprotection), separately for the mother and the father (Simons et al., 2008).
(Petrowski et al., 2009). In all, it has been found that parental rearing be- Following a more specific purpose, Smallbone and Dadds (1998)
havior, particularly lack of maternal care, is a significant factor for the found that intrafamiliar child molesters reported more problematic rela-
vulnerability of psychopathology in adulthood (Enns, Cox, & Clara, tionships with their mothers whereas rapists remembered their fathers
2002; Gao, Raine, Chan, Venables, & Mednick, 2010; Petrowski et al., as having been particularly uncaring, abusive, and violent to them. In a
2009). Additionally, Baker and Hoeger (2012) found that parental later work, both rapists and intrafamiliar child molesters were found to
warmth was associated with better intra and interpersonal adjustment, present higher rates of childhood insecure attachment with their fathers
self-regulation and academic performance. In turn, parental rejection compared with extrafamiliar child molesters (Smallbone & McCabe,
was related to internalizing clinical symptoms such as anxiety whereas 2003).
parental overcontrol was most associated with hypomanic activation Moreover, regarding the factors associated with recidivism
and psychoticism. Overall, the quality of parental rearing practices in among sex offenders, besides important indicators of pedophilia
the first years of life is proposed as a determinant factor of both child (e.g., phallometrically assessed sexual arousal to children, having a
and adult interpersonal and emotional functioning. boy victim, having extrafamiliar victims; Hanson & Bussière, 1998;
Hanson & Morton-Bourgon, 2004; Seto, Abramowitz, & Barbaree,
1.2. Early parenting styles in sexual offenders 2008), a negative relationship with mothers was the only developmen-
tal variable related to sexual recidivism (Hanson & Bussière, 1998).
Within the sexual offending field, the Marshall and Barbaree's Inte- Nonetheless, dysfunctional early relational experiences with parents
grated Theory (1990) was the first model to propose the influence of de- are characteristic of both sexual and nonsexual offenders (e.g. Maniglio,
velopmental adversities such as attachment problems and different 2012; McCormack et al., 2002). Accordingly, further research on this
forms of early abuse on this behavior. In a latter work, Marshall and topic is necessary to clarify the hypothesis of whether specific combina-
Marshall (2000) underlined this assumption, considering that poor tions of parent–child relationships are related to specific types of sexual
childhood relationships with parents constitute the first etiological offending behavior (e.g., higher frequency of problematic rearing prac-
step to sexual offending. Indeed, available literature has shown that sex- tices from father and mother, respectively in rapists and child mo-
ual offenders' developmental histories are characterized by high levels lesters; e.g., Giotakos et al., 2004; Smallbone & Dadds, 1998).
of family dysfunction, interpersonal problems and different types of Furthermore, sex offenders are a heterogeneous group and there are
abuse (sexual, physical, emotional and neglect; e.g., Burk & Burkhart, different subtypes and categories likely with specific features and ther-
2003; Connoly & Woollons, 2008; Lee et al., 2002; Maniglio, 2012). apeutic needs (e.g., Gannon, Collie, Ward, & Thakker, 2008; Groth, 1979;
Regarding recalled rearing behavior from parents, sexual offenders, Knight & Prentky, 1990; Sigre-Leirós, Carvalho, & Nobre, 2015a,b).
in general, report inconsistent and problematic practices including Given the higher risk of sexual recidivism associated with pedophilia
high levels of neglect, rejection, control, and low levels of care (Seto, 2009; Seto et al., 2008), for preventive and clinical purposes it
(Bogaerts et al., 2005; Craissati, McClurg, & Browne, 2002; Marsa et al., may be relevant to discriminate child molesters with and without pedo-
2004). Recent studies aiming to describe the early family environment philic interests. Specifically, pedophilia (or pedophilic disorder) consists
of adult sex offenders found that many of them had witnessed domestic of a paraphilic disorder characterized by “recurrent, intense sexually
violence situations, had a negative relationship with the mother, and arousing fantasies, sexual urges, or behaviors involving sexual activity
had been separated from their parents (Londt & Roman, 2014). Further- with a prepubescent child” (American Psychiatric Association, 2013,
more, using a sample of non-admitting sex offenders it was found that p. 697). Besides different assessment methods such as self-report, view-
many had been raised without a paternal figure, and when father was ing time, and phallometry, pedophilia can be also assessed on the basis
present, their relationship was frequently marked by physical and emo- of offender's sexual behavior history (Seto, 2009). In particular, child
tional abuse (Davids et al., 2015). These developmental experiences are sexual abusers with boy victims, multiple child victims, younger or un-
likely associated with the development of an insecure attachment style related victims are more likely to be pedophiles compared with of-
which is proposed as a risk factor for later sexual offending (e.g. Burk & fenders with only girl victims, a single child victim, older or related
Burkhart, 2003; Maniglio, 2012; Marshall, 1989, 1993; Marshall & victims (Seto, 2009). All this information can be obtained using the
Barbaree, 1990; Marshall & Marshall, 2000). Specifically, according to Screening Scale for Pedophilic Interests (Seto & Lalumière, 2001), which
Burk and Burkhart (2003), insecure attachment functions as a diathesis is proposed by the authors as a useful research instrument when
for this behavior given that it may inhibit the development of basic self- phallometric data are unavailable. Overall, empirical research on devel-
regulation skills, resulting in distorted self-perceptions and negative opmental experiences of subtypes of sex offenders, such as early parent-
emotional states. This difficulty to deal with emotional states can result ing styles, may have important implications regarding increasingly
in a lack of control, which in the presence of sexual desire, can be related effective early intervention and prevention strategies.
to the use of sex as a way to satisfy the individual's emotional and sexual
needs (Ward & Beech, 2006). 1.3. The present study
In the last two decades, independent studies have compared differ-
ent types of offenders on their perceptions of rearing behavior from The present study examined the relationship between early parenting
both mother and father. Despite some variability across studies, in styles and different types of sexual offending behavior. Specifically, three
V. Sigre-Leirós et al. / International Journal of Law and Psychiatry 46 (2016) 103–109 105

subtypes of sex offenders (pedophilic and nonpedophilic child molesters, interests among sex offenders with child victims. The scale is scored based
and rapists) and a non-sex offender group were compared on the basis of on file information related to offenders' sexual behavior history, particu-
perceived parental rearing behavior during childhood and adolescence. larly the characteristics of their victims (any male victim; more than
Overall, considering the findings of available studies on this topic one victim; any prepubescent victim; and any unrelated/extrafamilial vic-
(e.g. Giotakos et al., 2004; Lisak, 1994; McCormack et al., 2002; tim). The SSPI is significantly correlated with phallometric testing (Seto &
Smallbone & Dadds, 1998), it was hypothesized that both pedophilic Lalumière, 2001).The total score of the scale ranges from 0 to 5 and higher
and nonpedophilic child molesters would be more likely to report a scores are indicative of pedophilic sexual interests (Seto & Lalumière,
negative view of their mother's rearing behavior. In addition, it was ex- 2001). Accordingly, in this study, men convicted for child sexual abuse
pected that rapists would be characterized by a more negative percep- were distributed into two comparative subgroups based on low (i.e., 0–
tion of their father's attitudes toward them. 2) and high (i.e., 3–5) SSPI total score (i.e., nonpedophiles and pedophiles,
respectively).
2. Methods
2.2.2. Egna Minnen Betraffande Uppfostra — My Memories of Upbringing
2.1. Participants and procedures (EMBU)
The EMBU (Perris et al., 1980) is a self-report measure of the individ-
This study involved the participation of 113 male convicted sexual uals' perceptions of parental rearing practices during childhood and ad-
offenders, namely 63 child molesters and 50 rapists. Child molesters olescence. It is comprised of 23 items consisting of three dimensions
were convicted for sexual crimes against children (males or females) separately for father and mother: emotional warmth (refers to parental
under the age of 14. Rapists were convicted for sexual crimes against fe- behaviors of affection and support), rejection (refers to critical, punitive
male victims over the age of 14. A group of 51 male convicted nonsexual and abusive parental attitudes), and overprotection (refers to anxious,
offenders also participated in the study. Among their crimes were fraud, intrusive, and overinvolved parenting patterns; Arrindell et al., 1999).
burglary, robbery and homicide. Based on criminal files' information, The EMBU has demonstrated good indicators of reliability and validity,
nonsexual offenders did not commit sexual offenses. Within the group and structural invariance across different samples (e.g., Arrindell et al.,
of child molesters, two subgroups were identified according to the 1999). The Portuguese version of the scale also shows acceptable psy-
Screening Scale for Pedophilic Interests (Seto & Lalumière, 2001): pedo- chometric properties (global Cronbach's alpha values: .73 for father,
philic child molesters (n = 20; pedophiles), and nonpedophilic child and .74 for mother; Canavarro, 1999).
molesters (n = 43; nonpedophiles).
In all, participants were recruited from four Portuguese prisons. All 2.2.3. Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI)
participants were informed about the purposes of the study and signed The BSI (Derogatis & Spencer, 1982) assesses current/state psycho-
an informed consent form before completing the measures voluntarily pathology on the basis of nine dimensions of symptoms (somatization,
and individually, with the help of the primary investigator. In order to depression, hostility, anxiety, phobic anxiety, psychoticism, obsessive–
collect factual information about the characteristics of the crimes, the compulsive, paranoid ideation, and interpersonal sensitivity) and
criminal records of the participants were checked. Data were collected three global indexes (the General Severity Index [GSI], the Positive
between January 2012 and October 2013. The study's ethical approval Symptom Distress Index [PSDI], and the Positive Symptom Total
was given by Direcção Geral dos Serviços Prisionais in Portugal. [PST]). The GSI has been proposed as the best indicator of current psy-
The sociodemographic characteristics of the participants are pre- chological distress (Derogatis & Melisaratos, 1983). As with the original
sented in Table 1. The groups differed significantly with respect to age scale, the Portuguese version of the BSI presents acceptable psychomet-
(pedophiles and nonpedophiles were significantly older than rapists ric properties with Cronbach's alpha values ranging from .62 for
and nonsex offenders) and years of school education (nonpedophiles psychoticism to .80 for somatization (Canavarro, 2007).
and rapists were the less educated). The groups did not significantly dif-
fer in marital status (see Table 1). 2.2.4. Socially Desirable Response Set Measure (SDRS-5)
The SDRS-5 (Hays, Hayashy, & Stewart, 1989) is a five-item scale of
2.2. Measures the participant's social desirability levels which may influence the valid-
ity of self-reported data. Accordingly, it may be introduced as covariate
2.2.1. Screening Scale for Pedophilic Interests (SSPI) in multivariate analysis. Internal consistency of the scale is acceptable
The SSPI (Seto & Lalumière, 2001; translated to Portuguese by (Cronbach's alpha = .68; Hays et al., 1989). The Portuguese version of
Carvalho, 2011) is a four-item measure used to assess pedophilic sexual the scale presents a Cronbach's alpha of .70 (Carvalho, 2011).

Table 1
Participant sociodemographic characteristics.

Rapists Pedophiles Nonpedophiles Nonsex offenders


(n = 50) (n = 20) (n = 43) (n = 51)

Age (years)
M (SD) 37.96a (9.13) 45.70b (14.32) 44.42b (9.72) 40.63ab (12.17)
Range 24–60 23–76 27–73 25–78
F(3160) = 3.81*
Marital status (%)
Single 58 50 30.2 43.1
Married/cohabiting 34 25 41.9 27.5
Separated/divorced/Widowed 8 25 27.9 29.4
χ2 = 12.51; df = 6; p N .05
Education (years)
M (SD) 7.74ab (3.02) 9.20bc (3.83) 6.51a (3.04) 9.29c (3.74)
Range 4–12 4–15 4–15 4–15
F(3160) = 6.23**

Note. Means in the same row with different superscripts letters differ significantly from each other.
*p b .05, **p b .01.
106 V. Sigre-Leirós et al. / International Journal of Law and Psychiatry 46 (2016) 103–109

Table 2
Means and standard deviations of the EMBU's dimensions by group and for the total sample.

Group
EMBU's dimensions Rapists Pedophiles Nonpedophiles Nonsex offenders Total sample
(n = 50) (n = 20) (n = 43) (n = 51) (N = 164)

F_Emotional Warmth 17.00 (5.65) 16.95 (5.61) 17.84 (5.30) 19.35 (5.58) 17.95 (5.58)
F_Rejection 11.40 (4.06) 9.65 (2.01) 10.47 (3.00) 11.20 (4.28) 10.88 (3.70)
F_Overprotection 14.34 (4.07) 12.50 (3.69) 13.47 (3.57) 15.06 (3.82) 14.11 (3.88)
M_Emotional Warmth 20.12 (4.34) 17.65 (5.19) 19.33 (4.77) 19.73 (4.96) 19.49 (4.77)
M_Rejection 12.40 (3.83) 12.30 (3.96) 11.77 (3.18) 12.96 (4.58) 12.40 (3.94)
M_Overprotection 15.46 (3.80) 14.05 (4.39) 13.98 (3.70) 15.53 (3.31) 14.92 (3.74)

Note. F = father; M = mother.

2.3. Data analyses correlation matrix of all predictors is presented. Looking specifically to
parental rearing dimensions, all dimensions were significantly correlat-
Statistical analyses were carried out using the Statistical Package for ed. Negative correlations between father's and mother's emotional
the Social Sciences (SPSS, version 18.0). Univariate analyses of variance warmth and father's and mother's rejection were found.
were conducted to examine differences among the four offender groups
in age and years of school education. Chi-square analysis was conducted 3.2. Offender type as a function of perceived parental rearing behavior
to test group differences in marital status. Correlation analyses were
also performed between all predictors. Furthermore, the relationship A MLR was conducted entering the covariates [i.e., age, school educa-
between perceived parental rearing behavior (assessed by the EMBU) tion, social desirability (assessed by the SDRS-5) and GSI (assessed by
and different types of sexual offending was examined through sets of the BSI)], and the three dimensions of father's and mother's rearing be-
Multinomial Logistic Regression analyses (MLR; see Maroco, 2011; havior (respectively, emotional warmth, rejection, and overprotection;
Petrucci, 2009; Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). Significant group differences assessed by the EMBU) as predictors. Regarding assumption testing,
in age and school education were found, thus these variables were en- an examination of the standard errors of coefficients did not indicate
tered in the analyses as covariates. Additionally, social desirability and numerical problems (i.e., SEB b 2.0; see Table 4). Additionally, the corre-
current psychological distress (GSI) were also introduced as covariates lation matrix between the predictors did not show collinearity prob-
because of the participants' incarcerated condition and their potential lems (rs b 0.80; see Table 3). The full model was significantly reliable,
effects on self-report data. Indeed, given the heterogeneity of the χ2 (30, N = 164) = 69.591, p b .001, accounting for between 35%
study population, among other factors, the combination of these vari- (Cox and Snell R2) and 37% (Nagelkerke R2) of the variance in offender
ables could confound the findings. status. The classification accuracy rate for each category was the follow-
The MLR results are interpreted in terms of odds ratio with odds ratio ing: 56% for rapists; 20% for pedophiles; 48.8% for nonpedophiles; and,
greater than 1 indicating a greater likelihood for the event of interest and 54.9% for nonsex offenders. The model's overall classification accuracy
odds ratio less than 1 indicating a lower likelihood for the event of inter- rate was 49.4% which improves the proportional-by-chance accuracy
est. To identify whether particular parenting styles would be specific to a rate of 34.3%, thus showing adequate accuracy (cf. Petrucci, 2009).
subtype of offenders, comparisons between all groups were conducted. In Table 4, the parameter estimates for the different runs of the anal-
In particular, to compare pedophiles, nonpedophiles and rapists with ysis are presented.
nonsex offenders, the nonsex offenders were entered in the analysis as The odds ratio shown in the table indicated that, after controlling for
the reference category. To compare pedophiles and nonpedophiles group differences in covariates, rapists were less likely to perceive their
with rapists, rapists were then entered as the reference category. Finally, fathers as emotionally warm when compared with nonsex offenders
to compare pedophiles with nonpedophiles, the regression analyses (OR = .85, p ≤ .05). Similarly, compared with rapists, pedophiles were
were rerun with nonpedophiles as the reference category. more likely to report their fathers as emotionally warm (OR = 1.23,
p b .05). Additionally, pedophiles recalled their mothers as having been
3. Results less warm toward them than rapists (OR = .77, p b .05) (see Table 4).

3.1. Descriptive results 4. Discussion

Table 2 shows the Means and Standard Deviations of the EMBU's di- The present study compared three subtypes of sex offenders (rap-
mensions for each of the groups and for the total sample. In Table 3, the ists, pedophilic and nonpedophilic child molesters) and a nonsex

Table 3
Correlation matrix of predictor variables (N = 164).

Age School GSI Social F_Emotional F_Rejection F_Overprotection M_Emotional M_Rejection M_Overprotection
education desirability Warmth Warmth

Age –
School education −.19* –
GSI −.15 −.11 –
Social desirability .25** −.16* −.11 –
F_Emotional Warmth −.14 .17* −.03 .02 –
F_Rejection .03 −.19* .21** −.03 −.23** –
F_Overprotection −.02 −.16* .19* .04 .39*** .35*** –
M_Emotional Warmth −.17* .12 −.02 −.02 .64*** −.18* .23** –
M_Rejection .00 −.05 .26** −.05 −.19* .66*** .25** −.32*** –
M_Overprotection −.00 −.08 .29*** −.01 .20* .25** .79*** .32*** .31*** –

Note. F = father; M = mother.


*p b .05, **p b .01, ***p b .001.
V. Sigre-Leirós et al. / International Journal of Law and Psychiatry 46 (2016) 103–109 107

Table 4
Multinomial logistic regression analysis of offender type as a function of perceived parental rearing behavior.

Predictor B SEB Wald Odds 95% CI


statistic ratio

Pedophiles vs. nonsex offendersa Age .04 .03 2.14 1.04 .99–1.09
School education −.01 .10 .00 1.00 .83–1.20
GSI 1.73 .67 6.72** 5.66 1.53–21.01
Social desirability .23 .23 .99 1.25 .80–1.96
F_Emotional Warmth .04 .11 .16 1.04 .85–1.29
F_Rejection −.15 .16 .93 .86 .63–1.17
F_Overprotection −.30 .17 3.12 .74 .53–1.03
M_Emotional Warmth −.13 .12 1.13 .88 .70–1.11
M_Rejection −.06 .11 .29 .94 .75–1.18
M_Overprotection .12 .16 .60 1.13 .83–1.53
Nonpedophiles vs. nonsex offendersa Age .01 .02 .26 1.01 .97–1.06
School education −.27 .08 12.86*** .76 .66–.88
GSI .37 .57 .43 1.45 .48–4.44
Social desirability .19 .18 1.05 1.20 .85–1.71
F_Emotional Warmth −.07 .09 .61 .93 .78–1.11
F_Rejection −.06 .11 .31 .94 .76–1.17
F_Overprotection −.10 .16 .38 .90 .66–1.24
M_Emotional Warmth .09 .10 .88 1.10 .91–1.33
M_Rejection −.02 .11 .04 .98 .80–1.20
M_Overprotection −.07 .16 .18 .93 .68–1.29
Rapists vs. nonsex offendersa Age −.04 .02 2.37 .97 .92–1.01
School education −.14 .07 3.65 .87 .76–1.00
GSI 1.15 .51 5.13* 3.15 1.17–8.52
Social desirability .14 .17 .71 1.15 .83–1.60
F_Emotional Warmth −.16 .08 3.83* .85 .73–1.00
F_Rejection −.01 .10 .01 .99 .81–1.21
F_Overprotection −.04 .14 .06 .97 .73–1.28
M_Emotional Warmth .13 .09 2.21 1.14 .96–1.36
M_Rejection −.06 .10 .40 .94 .78–1.14
M_Overprotection −.01 .14 .01 .99 .75–1.31
Pedophiles vs. Rapistsa Age .07 .03 6.97** 1.08 1.02–1.14
School education .13 .10 1.82 1.14 .94–1.38
GSI .59 .65 .80 1.80 .50–6.47
Social desirability .09 .23 .13 1.09 .69–1.72
F_Emotional Warmth .20 .10 3.92* 1.23 1.00–1.50
F_Rejection −.14 .15 .88 .87 .65–1.17
F_Overprotection −.27 .17 2.51 .77 .55–1.07
M_Emotional Warmth −.26 .12 5.05* .77 .61–.97
M_Rejection .00 .12 .00 1.00 .80–1.26
M_Overprotection .13 .16 .73 1.14 .84–1.55
Nonpedophiles vs. rapistsa Age .05 .02 4.18* 1.05 1.00–1.10
School education −.14 .08 3.23 .87 .75–1.01
GSI −.77 .54 2.09 .46 .16–1.32
Social desirability .04 .18 .06 1.05 .74–1.48
F_Emotional Warmth .09 .07 1.62 1.09 .95–1.26
F_Rejection −.05 .10 .31 .95 .78–1.15
F_Overprotection −.07 .15 .19 .94 .70–1.25
M_Emotional Warmth −.04 .08 .27 .96 .82–1.12
M_Rejection .04 .10 .17 1.04 .86–1.27
M_Overprotection −.06 .15 .14 .94 .70–1.27
Pedophiles vs. nonpedophilesa Age .03 .03 1.05 1.03 .98–1.08
School education .27 .10 7.45** 1.31 1.08–1.58
GSI 1.36 .68 3.96* 3.90 1.02–14.87
Social desirability .04 .24 .03 1.04 .65–1.67
F_Emotional Warmth .11 .11 1.10 1.12 .91–1.38
F_Rejection −.09 .16 .32 .92 .68–1.24
F_Overprotection −.20 .18 1.21 .82 .57–1.17
M_Emotional Warmth −.22 .12 3.26 .80 .63–1.02
M_Rejection −.04 .12 .11 .96 .76–1.22
M_Overprotection .19 .17 1.22 1.21 .86–1.70

Note. N = 164; SEB = standard error of the regression coefficient; 95% CI = 95% confidence interval of odds ratio; GSI = General Severity Index; F = father; M = mother; *p ≤ .05, **p ≤ .01,
***p b .001.
a
Reference category.

offender group on the basis of perceived parental rearing behavior dur- Given the heterogeneity of the study population, some potential
ing childhood and adolescence. Guided by the hypothesis about a differ- confounding variables (i.e., age, school education, social desirability,
ential relationship between early maternal and paternal practices and and current psychological distress) were controlled. Overall, after ac-
later sexual offending subtypes (e.g. Smallbone & Dadds, 1998; counting for group differences in these covariates, our hypotheses
Smallbone & McCabe, 2003), we expected to give some new contribu- were partially supported. In particular, results showed that rapists
tion to the available literature on this topic by comparing pedophilic were less likely to remember their fathers as emotionally warm com-
and nonpedophilic offenders. pared with nonsex offenders and pedophiles. In addition, compared
108 V. Sigre-Leirós et al. / International Journal of Law and Psychiatry 46 (2016) 103–109

with rapists, pedophiles perceived their mothers as having been less role of differential patterns of developmental experiences on later sexu-
emotionally warm toward them. Thus, among the three factors assessed al offending. If more robust studies corroborate the present findings, it
by the EMBU (emotional warmth, rejection, and overprotection respec- would be possible to provide further empirical support for the concep-
tively for the father and the mother) only significant differences were tualization models of sexual offending and particularly of pedophilia.
found with respect to the emotional warmth dimension. Accordingly, Despite the quasi-experimental nature of the study design, given
we can speculate about the particular relevance of early emotional envi- that data are cross-sectional, it is important to underline that findings
ronment on rape and pedophilic behavior. cannot be interpreted as establishing causal relationships, and some im-
The emotional warmth dimension refers to parental attitudes of affec- portant limitations must be acknowledged. As already recognized, the
tion, acceptance, support, and valuing (Arrindell et al., 1999). In line with group of pedophilic child molesters is clearly small and further studies
the present findings, evidence for rapists' negative perception of paternal with larger samples are of utmost relevance. Likewise, the constitution
practices as well as poor attachment bonds with the father is relatively of the pedophilic group was only based in the SSPI data, and further rep-
consistent among available studies (e.g. Lisak, 1994; McCormack et al., lications including additional criteria for pedophilia (e.g., phallometry)
2002; Simons et al., 2008; Smallbone & Dadds, 1998). are required. Moreover, although sociodemographic data, social desir-
Regarding child molesters, findings have been less consistent, de- ability and current levels of psychopathological symptoms were statisti-
spite some trend for a more negative view of mothers' rearing behavior, cally controlled, because of the retrospective nature of the assessment of
particularly by intrafamiliar child molesters (e.g. Craissati et al., 2002; parenting styles, results may reflect current constructions of past events
Giotakos et al., 2004; Smallbone & Dadds, 1998). Nonetheless, it was rather than the true developmental experiences. Thus, the interpreta-
also previously found that rapists as well as intrafamiliar child molesters tion of the present results should be done having these limitations in
are more likely to report insecure attachment with fathers than mind.
extrafamiliar child molesters (Smallbone & McCabe, 2003). Given that Overall, despite the need for more robust studies on this topic, the
in this study rapists perceived their fathers as less emotionally warm findings may have important general implications for early intervention
than pedophiles, and considering that the SSPI criteria for the pedophilic and prevention of sexual offending. Specifically, preventive efforts
group includes the presence of unrelated/extrafamiliar victims, we may might consider early family dynamics, namely parental rearing behavior
conclude that results were partially consistent with Smallbone and in childhood. In particular, the importance of attending to the child's
McCabe's (2003) findings. Moreover, in the Hanson and Bussière’ early emotional/relational experiences is highlighted given that they
meta-analysis (1998), a negative relationship with mothers was the function as a prototype for later interpersonal behavior and emotional
only developmental variable related to sexual recidivism. Similarly, in self-regulation (e.g., Bowlby, 1969, 1973). Therefore, the early identifi-
this study pedophilic offenders reported lesser maternal emotional cation and intervention with families with dysfunctional dynamics (in-
warmth (i.e., mother inconsistent, unresponsive, or unavailable) and cluding, for instance, family psychoeducation and parent management
available evidence suggests that pedophiles are more likely to sexually training) may be implemented as a general prevention approach.
reoffend (Seto, 2009).
It may be also relevant to note that, according to Baker and Hoeger's
5. Conclusion
(2012) findings, the EMBU's parental warmth dimension was related to
three important areas of functioning in early adulthood, namely overall
In conclusion, the present study corroborated the association between
self-regulation, academic performance, and intra and interpersonal ad-
distal interpersonal factors and sexual offending behavior. Specifically,
justment (e.g., greater warmth toward, and trust in others). Therefore,
the findings indicated a relationship between specific developmental ex-
we may presuppose that the lack of parental emotional warmth will
periences, particularly the lack of parental emotional warmth, and differ-
have a negative and contrary impact on adult socio-emotional function-
ent manifestations of offensive sexual behavior. These findings may have
ing, likely resulting in important relational and intimacy deficits. In fact,
important implications for early intervention and prevention of sexual
available literature has shown that sex offenders, in general, lack the
offending focused on the family relational context. Further research on
basic social skills (e.g., assertiveness and self-confidence) necessary to
this topic using larger samples of pedophilic offenders is needed. We
develop adequate interpersonal relationships with peers (e.g. Bumby
hope that the present study motivates further empirical investigation
& Hansen, 1997; Geer, Estupinan, & Manguno-Mire, 2000; Maniglio,
on the role of developmental factors in pedophilia.
2012; Marshall, Barbaree, & Fernandez, 1995; Pervan & Hunter, 2007;
Sigre-Leirós et al., 2015b). Moreover, it has been proposed that individ-
uals who grow up in an emotionally poor environment are frequently Acknowledgments
unable to deal with their emotions adequately, reacting with aggression
in emotional situations (e.g. Simons et al., 2008; Ward & Beech, 2006). This work was supported by a grant from the Fundação para a
Regarding pedophilic offenders, the present results indicated a rela- Ciência e a Tecnologia in Portugal (SFRH/ BD/73032/2010).
tionship between maternal rearing style (namely the lack of mother's
emotional support) and pedophilic behavior. Consistent with this,
References
prior research has also shown the relatively greater impact of lack of
maternal care on adult psychological disorders (and indeed pedophilia American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
is a paraphilic disorder) compared with paternal care (Enns et al., ders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Arrindell, W. A., Sanavio, E., Aguilar, G., Sica, C., Hatzichristou, C., Eisemann, M., ... Ende, J.
2002; Gao et al., 2010). However, given the small size of the pedophilic (1999). The development of a short form of the EMBU: Its appraisal with students in
group, these results are only preliminary, and further research with Greece, Guatemala, Hungary and Italy. Personality and Individual Differences, 27,
larger samples is clearly necessary. 613–628.
Baker, C. N., & Hoeger, M. (2012). Parental child-rearing strategies influence self-
Overall, results showed that certain developmental experiences
regulation, socio-emotional adjustment, and psychopathology in early adulthood:
with parents were able to distinguish between subtypes of offenders, Evidence from a retrospective cohort study. Personality and Individual Differences,
indicating an association between distal interpersonal factors and sexu- 52, 800–805.
Bogaerts, S., Vanheule, S., & Declercq, F. (2005). Recalled parental bonding, adult attach-
al offending. Significant differences were found for rapists and pedo-
ment style, and personality disorders in child molesters: A comparative study. The
philic child molesters. Specifically, during childhood and adolescence, Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology, 16, 445–458.
while rapists perceived less emotional support by the father, pedophilic Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and Loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
offenders perceived less emotional support by the mother. Accordingly, Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and Loss: Vol. 2. Separation. New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and Loss: Vol. 3. Loss. New York: Basic Books.
as previously proposed (e.g. Smallbone & Dadds, 1998; Smallbone & Bumby, K. M., & Hansen, D. J. (1997). Intimacy deficits, fear of intimacy, and loneliness
McCabe, 2003), the findings seem to support the hypothesis about the among sexual offenders. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 24, 315–331.
V. Sigre-Leirós et al. / International Journal of Law and Psychiatry 46 (2016) 103–109 109

Burk, L. R., & Burkhart, B. R. (2003). Disorganized attachment as a diathesis for sexual de- Marshall, W. L., & Barbaree, H. E. (1990). An integrated theory of the etiology of sexual
viance developmental experience and the motivation for sexual offending. Aggression offending. In W. L. Marshall, D. R. Laws, & H. E. Barbaree (Eds.), Handbook of Sexual
and Violent Behavior, 8, 487–511. Assault: Issues, Theories, and Treatment of the Offender (pp. 257–275) New York
Canavarro, M. C. (1999). Relações afectivas e saúde mental. Coimbra: Quarteto Editora. Plenum.
Canavarro, M. C. (2007). Inventário de Sintomas Psicopatológicos: Uma revisão crítica dos Marshall, W. L., & Marshall, L. (2000). The origins of sexual offending. Trauma, Violence &
estudos realizados em Portugal. In M. Simões, C. Machado, M. M. Gonçalves, & E. L. Abuse, 1, 250–263. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1524838000001003003.
Almeida (Eds.), Avaliação Psicológica. Instrumentos Validados para a População Marshall, W. L., Barbaree, H. E., & Fernandez, Y. M. (1995). Some aspects of social compe-
Portuguesa, Vol. III, Coimbra: Quarteto Editora. tence in sexual offenders. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 7,
Carvalho, J. (2011). Vulnerability factors for sexual aggression. Doctoral Thesis Aveiro, 113–127.
Portugal: Universidade de Aveiro. Marshall, W. L. (1993). The role of attachments, intimacy and loneliness in the etiology
Connoly, M., & Woollons, R. (2008). Childhood sexual experience and adult offending: An and maintenance of sexual offending. Sexual and Marital Therapy, 8, 109–121.
exploratory comparison of three criminal groups. Child Abuse Review, 17, 119–132. McCormack, J., Hudson, S. M., & Ward, T. (2002). Sexual offender's perceptions of their
Craissati, J., McClurg, G., & Browne, K. (2002). The parental bonding experiences of sex of- early interpersonal relationships: an attachment perspective. The Journal of Sex
fenders: A comparison between child molesters and rapists. Child Abuse and Neglect, Research, 39, 85–93.
26, 909–921. Ó Ciardha, C., & Ward, T. (2013). Theories of cognitive distortions in sexual offending
Davids, O., Londt, M., & Wilson, L. (2015). Imprisoned sex offenders' chronic denial and what the current research tells us. Trauma, Violence & Abuse, 14, 5–21.
their childhood family environment. The Open Family Studies Journal, 7, 42–47. Parker, G., Tupling, H., & Brown, L. B. (1979). A parental bonding instrument. British
Derogatis, L. R., & Melisaratos, N. (1983). The brief symptom inventory: An introductory Journal of Medical Psychology, 52, 1–10.
report. Psychological Medicine, 13, 595–605. Perris, C., Jacobsson, L., Linndstrom, H., Von Knorring, L., & Perris, H. (1980). Development
Derogatis, L. R., & Spencer, P. M. (1982). The Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI): Administration, of a new inventory for assessing memories of parental rearing behaviour. Acta
and procedures manual — I. Baltimore, MD: Clinical Psychometric Research. Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 61, 265–274.
Enns, M. W., Cox, B. J., & Clara, I. (2002). Parental bonding and adult psychopathology: Re- Pervan, S., & Hunter, M. (2007). Cognitive distortions and social self-esteem in sexual of-
sults from the US National Comorbidity Survey. Psychological Medicine, 32, 997–1008. fenders. Applied Psychology in Criminal Justice, 3, 75–91.
Gannon, T. A., Collie, R. M., Ward, T., & Thakker, J. (2008). Rape: Psychopathology, theory Petrowski, K., Berth, H., Schmidt, S., Schumacher, J., Hinz, A., & Braehler, E. (2009). The as-
and treatment. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 982–1008. sessment of recalled parental rearing and its relationship to life satisfaction and inter-
Gao, Y., Raine, A., Chan, F., Venables, P. H., & Mednick, S. A. (2010). Early maternal and pa- personal problems: A general population study. BMC Medical Research Methodology,
ternal bonding, childhood physical abuse and adult psychopathic personality. 6, 17–22.
Psychological Medicine, 40, 1007–1016. Petrucci, C. J. (2009). A primer for social worker researchers on how to conduct a multi-
Geer, J. H., Estupinan, L. A., & Manguno-Mire, G. M. (2000). Empathy, social skills, and nomial logistic regression. Journal of Social Service Research, 35, 193–205.
other relevant cognitive processes in rapists and child molesters. Aggression and Pietromonaco, P. R., & Barrett, L. F. (2000). The Internal Working Models concept: What
Violent Behavior, 5, 99–126. do we really know about the self in relation to others? Review of General
Giotakos, O., Vaidakis, N., Markianos, M., Spandoni, P., & Christodoulou, G. N. (2004). Tem- Psychology, 4, 155–175.
perament and character dimensions of sex offenders in relation to their parental rear- Seto, M. C. (2009). Pedophilia. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 5, 391–407.
ing. Sexual and Relationship Therapy, 19, 141–150. Seto, M. C., & Lalumière, M. L. (2001). A brief screening scale to identify pedophilic inter-
Groth, A. N. (1979). Men who rape: The psychology of the offender. New York: Plenum ests among child molesters. Sexual abuse: A journal of research and treatment, 13,
Press. 15–25.
Hall, G. C. N., & Hirschman, R. (1991). Toward a theory of sexual aggression: A quadripar- Seto, M. C., Abramowitz, C. S., & Barbaree, H. E. (2008). Paraphilias. In J. Hunsley, & E. J.
tite model. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 662–669. Mash (Eds.), Assessments that work (pp. 488–514). New York: Oxford University
Hall, G. C. N., & Hirschman, R. (1992). Sexual aggression against children: A conceptual Press.
perspective of etiology. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 19, 8–23. Sigre-Leirós, V., Carvalho, J., & Nobre, P. (2015a). Cognitive schemas and sexual offending:
Hanson, R., & Bussière, M. (1998). Predicting relapse: A meta-analysis of sexual offender Differences between rapists, pedophilic and nonpedophilic child molesters, and non-
recidivism studies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 86, 348–362. sexual offenders. Child Abuse and Neglect, 40, 81–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
Hanson, R. K., & Morton-Bourgon, K. E. (2004). Predictors of sexual recidivism: An updated chiabu.2014.10.003.
meta-analysis. Ottawa, Canada: Public Safety and Emergency Preparedness Canada. Sigre-Leirós, V., Carvalho, J., & Nobre, P. (2015b). Adult interpersonal features of subtypes
Hays, R. D., Hayashy, T., & Stewart, A. L. (1989). A five-item measure of socially desirable of sexual offenders. Journal of Forensic and Legal Medicine, 34, 5–10. http://dx.doi.org/
response set. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 49, 629–636. 10.1016/j.jflm.2015.04.015.
Knight, R. A., & Prentky, R. A. (1990). Classifying sexual offenders: The development and Simons, D. A., Wurtele, S., & Durham, R. L. (2008). Developmental experiences of child
corroboration of taxonomic models. In W. L. Marshall, D. R. Laws, & H. E. Barbaree sexual abusers and rapists. Child Abuse & Neglect, 32, 549–560.
(Eds.), Handbook of sexual assault: Issues, theories, and treatment of the offender Smallbone, S. W., & Dadds, M. R. (1998). Childhood attachment and adult attachment in
(pp. 23–52). New York: Plenum Press. incarcerated adult male sex offenders. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 13, 555–573.
Lee, J. K., Jackson, H. J., Pattison, P., & Ward, T. (2002). Developmental risk factors for sex- Smallbone, S. W., & McCabe, B. (2003). Childhood attachment, childhood sexual abuse,
ual offending. Child Abuse and Neglect, 26, 73–92. and onset of masturbation among adult sexual offenders. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of
Lisak, D. (1994). Subjective assessment of relationships with parents by sexually aggres- Research and Treatment, 15, 1–9.
sive and nonaggressive men. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 9, 399–411. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston: Pear-
Londt, M., & Roman, N. (2014). The effect of child abuse on adult perpetrators of sex of- son Education, Inc.
fences: A developmental perspective. Child Abuse Research: A South African Journal, Ward, T., & Beech, A. (2006). An integrated theory of sexual offending. Aggression and
15, 1–6. Violent Behavior, 11, 44–63.
Maniglio, R. (2012). The role of parent–child bonding, attachment, and interpersonal Ward, T., & Hudson, S. M. (1998). The construction and development of theory in the sex-
problems in the development of deviant sexual fantasies in sexual offenders. ual offending area: A meta-theoretical framework. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research
Trauma, Violence & Abuse, 13, 83–96. and Treatment, 10, 47–63.
Maroco, J. P. (2011). Análise Estatística com o SPSS statistics (5th ed.). Pêro Pinheiro: Ward, T., & Siegert, R. J. (2002). Toward and comprehensive theory of child sexual abuse:
ReportNumber. A theory knitting perspective. Psychology, Crime & Law, 9, 319–351.
Marsa, F., O'Reilly, G., Carr, A., Murphy, P., O'Sullivan, M., Cotter, A., & Hevey, D. (2004). At- Ward, T., Polaschek, D. L. L., & Beech, A. (2006). Theories of sexual offending. Sussex, UK:
tachment styles and psychological profiles of child sex offenders in Ireland. Journal of Wiley.
Interpersonal Violence, 19, 228–251.
Marshall, W. L. (1989). Intimacy, loneliness, and sexual offenders. Behavior Research and
Therapy, 27, 491–503.

You might also like