You are on page 1of 8

Different Treatment Thresholds in Non-Western

Children With Behavioral Problems


BARBARA W.C. ZWIRS, M.A., HUIBERT BURGER, M.D., TOM W.J. SCHULPEN, M.D.,
AND JAN K. BUITELAAR, M.D.

ABSTRACT
Objective: First, to investigate whether non-Western children in the Netherlands are less likely to be treated for behavioral
problems than Western children; second, to examine whether discrepancies in treatment status are related to differences
in level of problem behavior and impairment. Method: The study included 2,185 children of the four largest ethnic groups
in the Netherlands, namely, 684 Dutch, 702 Moroccan, 434 Turkish, and 365 Surinamese children from grades three to
five of elementary school. Teachers completed the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire and five DSM-IV items on
externalizing problems. In addition, they provided information on the treatment status of the child. Results: Moroccan
boys displayed more problem behavior, Turkish boys less problem behavior, and Surinamese boys similar rates of
problem behavior compared with Dutch boys. No difference in problem behavior was found between Western and non-
Western girls. Adjusted for age, level of problem behavior, and impairment, Moroccan and Turkish children and
Surinamese girls were less likely to receive treatment for problem behavior. Conclusions: The higher treatment
thresholds of non-Western children compared with Western children in the Netherlands could not be explained by
differences in level of problem behavior or impairment. Detection of behavioral problems in non-Western children should
receive more attention. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 2006;45(4):476Y483. Key Words: behavioral problems,
ethnicity, impairment, treatment.

With an estimated prevalence of 3%Y5%, attention- and conduct disorder (CD), mood disorders, and an-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the xiety disorders (Gillberg et al., 2004).
most common psychiatric disorders among school-age Because most research on ADHD has been done
children (Buitelaar, 2002a). In both clinical and epi- among Western children, much less is known about the
demiological samples, ADHD is highly comorbid, in disorder in non-Western children. Nevertheless, there is
particular with oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) some evidence that the prevalence of ADHD may vary
within different ethnic groups. For instance, differences
in prevalence of behavioral problems have been ob-
Accepted September 15, 2005.
served across countries (Crijnen et al., 1999; Verhulst
Ms. Zwirs and Dr. Schulpen are with the Department of Pediatrics,
University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands; Dr. Burger is et al., 2003) and also within countries across different
with the University Medical Center Utrecht, Julius Center for Health Sciences ethnic groups (Reid et al., 1998; Stevens et al., 2003).
and Primary Care, Utrecht; Dr. Buitelaar is with the Department of Psychiatry, Moreover, although comorbidity diagnoses of ADHD
Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Center and Academic Center for Child
and Adolescent Psychiatry, Nijmegen, The Netherlands. such as ODD, CD, and anxiety disorders have been
This study was financially supported by The Netherlands Organization for found both in Western and non-Western children
Scientific Research (ZON-MW) grant number 99-9.1-64, by the Foundation (Samuel et al., 1998; Souza et al., 2004), the rates of
for Children’s Welfare Stamps Netherlands, and by the Province of Utrecht, The
Netherlands.
these comorbid diagnoses seem to vary (Samuel et al.,
Reprint requests to Barbara W.C. Zwirs, Department of Pediatrics, 1998).
University Medical Center, KE04.133.1, PO Box 85090, 3508 AB Utrecht, Furthermore, not only does the prevalence of be-
The Netherlands; e-mail: b.zwirs@wkz.azu.nl.
havioral disorders seem to be influenced by ethnicity
0890-8567/06/4504Y0476Ó2006 by the American Academy of Child
and Adolescent Psychiatry. but also treatment rates appear to differ according to
DOI: 10.1097/01.chi.0000192251.46023.5a ethnicity. For instance, Safer and Malever (2000) found

476 J. AM . ACAD. CH ILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIAT RY, 45:4, APRIL 2006

Copyright @ 2006 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
DIFFERENT TREATMENT THRESHOLDS

that black, Hispanic, Asian, and Native American population. A total of 174 teachers from 45 of 87 (52%) schools
participated in the study. In 37 of 45 schools (82%), all teachers
students received less ADHD medication than white participated. Most teachers were of Dutch origin (77%). The
students (Safer and Malever, 2000). Likewise, Rowland reasons for nonparticipation among schools and teachers were
et al. (2002) reported that the prevalence of ADHD mainly logisticVthey already participated in other studies or they
were too occupied with other activities.
medication treatment was much lower among African Because both participating and nonparticipating schools are from
American children and Hispanic American children the same neighborhoods, which are characterized by a low SES level
than among white children (Rowland et al., 2002). This and a large minority population, we assumed that the socio-
pattern of lower treatment rates among nonwhite demographic characteristics of the children in the 45 participating
schools do not differ from those in the 42 schools that did not
compared with white is confirmed by other studies participate. Therefore, it is unlikely that selection bias would have
(Angold et al., 2002; Kataoka et al., 2002; Olfson et al., distorted our results. Parents of all 2,802 children in these three
2003) except for a study among Native Americans grades received a letter with information on the screening
procedures. Children of an ethnic origin other than Dutch,
(Angold et al., 2000). In the latter study, no difference Moroccan, Turkish, or Surinamese were excluded from the study
in the rate of stimulant use was found between Native (N = 336) because we were particularly interested in the four largest
Americans with ADHD and others with ADHD. ethnic groups and the number of children with another ethnic
origin was too small to reach an acceptable power. Because a total of
The aim of this study was to examine the treatment 281 parents refused to give permission, teachers completed 2,185 of
rates of behavioral problems in the four largest ethnic 2,466 questionnaires (89%), with no significant variation in this
groups in the Netherlands: Dutch, Moroccan, Turkish, proportion across ethnicity (Table 1). A child was classified as
and Surinamese. Numbering 1.6 million, immigrants Moroccan, Turkish, or Surinamese when the child himself or herself
or at least one parent had been born in Morocco, Turkey, or
constitute about 10% of the total Dutch population. Surinam, respectively. When both parents were of non-Dutch
The largest group of immigrants has come to the origin, we used the mother’s country of birth to determine the
Netherlands as labor migrants since the 1960s and child’s ethnicity.
The study protocol was approved by the Medical Ethical
early 1970s. They came from Mediterranean countries, Committee of the University Medical Center Utrecht. Parents of
mainly Turkey and Morocco. Surinamese migrated all participating children gave informed consent.
from South America to the Netherlands during the
process of decolonization because Surinam was a Dutch Instrumentation
colony until 1975.
Teachers completed the Dutch version of the Strengths and
Given the high rate of comorbid diagnoses such as Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ; Goodman, 1997; for the Dutch
ODD and CD in children with ADHD and possible version, see Van Widenfelt et al., 2003). The SDQ includes 25
differences in the presentation of ADHD among chil- items describing positive and negative attributes of children. These
25 items are distributed among five scales of five items each,
dren of different ethnic origins, we examined not only generating scores for emotional symptoms, conduct problems,
symptoms of ADHD but also ODD and CD, ad- hyperactivity, peer problems, and social behavior. A factor analysis
dressing three questions: (1) What is the occurrence of of the Dutch SDQ for parents and youths yielded a five-factor
solution that was nearly identical with the five factors proposed by
behavioral problems (ADHD, ODD, and CD) among Goodman (Muris et al., 2003). Van Widenfelt et al. (2003)
Dutch, Moroccan, Turkish, and Surinamese children translated and validated the teacher SDQ for Dutch children and
according to teacher ratings? (2) Do these behavioral reported Cronbach " coefficients 9.70 for all subscales and higher
problems impair these ethnic groups equally? (3) Are interinformant correlations than for the corresponding Achenbach
scales (Achenbach, 1991).
Dutch, Moroccan, Turkish, and Surinamese children Given our specific interest in ADHD and related externalizing
with a given level of behavioral problems and im- behavior problems, we added five DSM-IV items (American
pairment equally likely to be treated? Psychiatric Association, 1994) to the SDQ: two ADHD items on
inattention (Boften has trouble organizing activities[; Boften avoids,
dislikes, or does not want to do things that take a lot of mental effort
METHOD for a long period of time (such as schoolwork or homework[); one
ADHD item about impulsivity (Boften has trouble waiting one_s
turn[); one ADHD item about hyperactivity (Boften talks
Subjects
excessively[), and an item regarding oppositional behavior (Boften
A total of 2,802 children enrolled in grades three through five of argues with adults[).
mainstream elementary schools in multicultural neighborhoods in Each item was rated on a 3-point scale from 1 (not true) to 3
two large cities (Amsterdam and Utrecht) in the Netherlands were (definitely true). The items on the Hyperactivity scale (5 items) and
sampled in 2002 and 2003. To obtain a sample with all four the Conduct Problem scale (5 items) of the SDQ and the 5 items
ethnicities represented with similar socioeconomic status (SES), we directly adapted from the diagnostic criteria for ADHD and ODD
sampled schools from areas with low SES and a large immigrant in the DSM-IV, 15 items in total, were summed to determine a

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 45:4, APRIL 2006 477

Copyright @ 2006 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
ZWIRS ET AL.

problem score (minimum 15, maximum 45). A score above the risk of a Turkish, Surinamese, or Moroccan ethnic background for
90th percentile is generally considered high and is the recom- problem behavior, impairment, and treatment. Dutch ethnicity
mended cutoff score (Reid et al., 2000). In the present study, we served as the reference group. These relative risks were expressed
therefore dichotomized the problem score at this cutoff in the total as ORs with 95% CIs. To examine whether any differences in
group. Scores higher than the cutoff were defined as problem risk could be explained by age or SES, these factors were included
behavior. as covariates. In addition, to find out whether the observed re-
Furthermore, the SDQ includes a short impact supplement that lationships could be explained by the child being born in the
inquires whether the child has a problem according to the teacher country of origin, we incorporated the proportion of foreign-born
and, if so, asks about overall distress (Bdo the difficulties upset or children among the non-Dutch groups as a covariate. Adjusting
distress the child?[), social impairment (Bdo the difficulties interfere for the proportion foreign born children barely affected the re-
with the child’s everyday life in peer relationships?[), educational sults, however.
impairment (Bdo the difficulties interfere with the child’s everyday In all analyses we accounted for dependency of the observations at
life in classroom learning?[), the burden on the teacher (Bdo the the class level by calculating robust standard errors using the
difficulties put a burden on you or the class as a whole?[), and clustering option of STATA 7.0. The analyses were performed in
chronicity (Bhow long have these difficulties been present?[). All boys and girls separately.
5 impact items were scored on a 4-point scale from 0 (not at all) to
3 (very much) and were aggregated to generate an impairment score
(minimum 0, maximum 12). The item on chronicity, which was RESULTS
delineated on a 4-point scale from 0 (shorter than a month) to
3 (91 year) was not incorporated in this score because many Table 1 shows demographic characteristics of the
teachers knew their pupils for only 6 months. Because approxi- study population. Irrespective of ethnic group, the re-
mately 80% of the children scored zero on the impairment score, sponse rate was high, half of the children were male, the
we decided to dichotomize the score at this value. Accordingly, a
score 90 was considered as impairment.
mean age was almost 8 years, and the SES was low. In
Finally, five items regarding the treatment status of the child were Table 2, the relationship between problem behavior
added. Examples of these items are Bis this child being treated for and ethnicity is shown. Compared with Dutch boys,
behavioral disorders?[ and Bdoes this child use medicines because of Moroccan boys were about 70% more likely to display
behavioral disorders?[ These items were scored as follows: 1 (does
not know), 2 (no), and 3 (yes). Teachers in the Netherlands are well problem behavior. Turkish boys were about 60% less
informed about the treatment status of the child because the mental likely to show problem behavior. There was no signifi-
health care system cooperates with them during the treatment cant difference between Dutch and Surinamese boys. In
process (e.g., teachers are asked to fill in questionnaires about the
behavior of the children in school). Therefore, we consider teachers girls, no marked differences were observed.
to be valid informants on the treatment status of the children. In addition, the risk of impairment was examined
Importantly, the percentage of the teachers’ answer on Bdoes not (Table 3). Overall, 409 (19%) children were impaired.
know[ was not significantly higher for non-Dutch children com-
pared with Dutch children (odds ratio [OR] = 1.02; 95% con-
Compared with Dutch boys, Turkish boys were 40%
fidence interval [CI] = 0.71Y1.45). less likely to be impaired. Moroccan and Surinamese
Information about SES was obtained by an area-based method. boys did not differ much from Dutch boys in im-
Because the postal code may be considered as a proxy measure for pairment, as is shown in Table 3. Girls did not differ
income, level of education, and rate of unemployment (Knol,
1998), SES was estimated by using the postal code of schools and significantly in scores on impairment.
scored on a 5-point scale from 1 (high) to 5 (low). These codes were Problem behavior was strongly related to impairment
obtained from Statistics Netherlands. with an overall OR of 29.6 (95% CI: 16.5Y53.3), which
was similar in all ethnicities. Therefore, we repeated the
Statistical Analysis analysis controlling for problem behavior (Table 3). It
Proportions were tested for statistically significant differences appeared that the lower frequency of impairment in
using the 22 test. Logistic regression was used to estimate the relative Turkish boys could be largely explained by the lower

TABLE 1
Demographic Characteristics of the Study Population

Ethnicity Eligible n Included n Response Rate, % Age (yr) Mean (Min, Max) Boys, % SES Median (Min, Max)
Dutch 768 684 89 7.6 (5, 10) 52 5 (1, 5)
Moroccan 792 702 88 7.9 (6, 11) 54 5 (1, 5)
Turkish 497 434 87 8.0 (5, 10) 49 5 (1, 5)
Surinamese 409 365 89 7.7 (6, 11) 46 5 (1, 5)
Note: SES = socioeconomic status.

478 J. AM . ACAD. CH ILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIAT RY, 45:4, APRIL 2006

Copyright @ 2006 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
DIFFERENT TREATMENT THRESHOLDS

TABLE 2
Relative Risk of Problem Behavior for Different Ethnicities, Adjusted for Age and SES
Scoring Above
Gender Ethnicity n Median (Min, Max) Clinical Cutoff, % OR (95% CI)
Boys Dutch 343 22 (15, 42) 13.6 Reference
Moroccan 366 23 (15, 45) 21.8 1.61 (1.07Y2.42)*
Turkish 205 20 (15, 43) 7.2 0.39 (0.20Y0.75)
Surinamese 163 21 (15, 43) 17.6 1.27 (0.75Y2.17)
Girls Dutch 324 17 (15, 37) 4.9 Reference
Moroccan 311 19 (15, 44) 6.7 1.25 (0.65Y2.38)
Turkish 215 19 (15, 41) 6.0 1.11 (0.52Y2.37)
Surinamese 192 19 (15, 41) 6.7 1.41 (0.66Y3.02)
Note: Median refers to the median of the problem score. OR = odds ratio; CI = confidence interval.
* p G .05.

level of problem behavior in this group because the difference between Dutch and Surinamese boys was no
difference in impairment almost disappeared. longer statistically significant.
Finally, treatment rates were assessed (Table 4).
Overall, 101 (5%) children were treated for problem
DISCUSSION
behavior, with large differences between Dutch and
non-Dutch boys and girls. In comparison with Dutch This study is the first to compare the level of problem
boys, Turkish boys were almost 90% less likely to be behavior, associated impairment of functioning, and
treated for behavioral problems, whereas this per- the likelihood of treatment between Western and non-
centage was about 65 for Moroccan and Surinamese Western children in Europe.
boys. Moroccan, Turkish, and Surinamese girls all The present study indicates that according to
were about 70% less likely to be treated for behav- teachers of grades three through five, Moroccan boys
ioral problems. display more problem behavior and Turkish boys show
Problem behavior and impairment were strongly less problem behavior than Dutch boys, which could
associated with being treated: the ORs were 7.5 (95% not be explained by differences in the proportion of
CI: 4.8Y11.8) and 27.6 (95% CI: 13.7Y55.6), respec- foreign-born children. The relatively high score on
tively. These ORs were similar in the different teacher-reported problem behavior for Moroccan boys
ethnicities. After adjusting for problem behavior, non- is in accordance with a recent study in the Netherlands
Dutch children were still less likely to be treated for in which teachers reported more problem behavior
behavioral problems (Table 4). When we additionally among Moroccan children than among Dutch and
adjusted for the level of impairment, the results were Turkish children (Stevens et al., 2003). Recent studies
essentially similar to the unadjusted analysis, but the have offered contradictory results regarding differences

TABLE 3
Relative Risk of Impairment for Different Ethnicities
After Adjusting for Age After Adjusting for Age, SES,
Gender Ethnicity n Impaired, % and SES OR (95% CI) and Problem Level OR (95%CI)
Boys Dutch 337 24.6 Reference Reference
Moroccan 362 28.6 1.08 (0.78Y1.50) 0.74 (0.47Y1.15)
Turkish 202 16.3 0.58 (0.40Y0.83)* 0.81 (0.51Y1.30)*
Surinamese 161 22.9 0.80 (0.53Y1.20) 0.66 (0.38Y1.17)
Girls Dutch 321 12.5 Reference Reference
Moroccan 306 16.3 1.28 (0.82Y2.00) 0.84 (0.52Y1.35)
Turkish 210 12.4 1.07 (0.69Y1.65) 0.87 (0.54Y1.42)
Surinamese 188 12.9 1.18 (0.73Y1.90) 0.83 (0.49Y1.41)
* p G .05.

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 45:4, APRIL 2006 479

Copyright @ 2006 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
ZWIRS ET AL.

TABLE 4
Relative Probability of Treatment for Behavioral Problems for Different Ethnicities
After Adjusting for Age, After Adjusting for Age,
After Adjusting for Age SES, and Problem SES, Problem Level, and
Gender Ethnicity n Treated, % and SES OR (95% CI) Level OR (95%CI) Impairment Level OR (95% CI)
Boys Dutch 332 11.7 Reference Reference Reference
Moroccan 359 4.7 0.33 (0.18Y0.61)* 0.26 (0.13Y0.53)* 0.33 (0.15Y0.70)*
Turkish 202 3.0 0.13 (0.04Y0.39)* 0.17 (0.06Y0.53)* 0.20 (0.06Y0.70)*
Surinamese 150 5.3 0.37 (0.16Y0.85)* 0.36 (0.16Y0.81)* 0.40 (0.15Y1.04)
Girls Dutch 310 6.5 Reference Reference Reference
Moroccan 306 2.3 0.35 (0.13Y0.99)* 0.23 (0.08Y0.71)* 0.18 (0.06Y0.58)*
Turkish 210 1.9 0.28 (0.09Y0.85)* 0.18 (0.05Y0.60)* 0.14 (0.03Y0.65)*
Surinamese 188 1.7 0.26 (0.08Y0.88)* 0.17 (0.05Y0.56)* 0.19 (0.05Y0.77)*

* p G .05.

in problem behavior between Turkish boys and Dutch The relatively low score on problem behavior among
boys. Crijnen et al. (2000) found no differences in Turkish boys may be explained by cultural differences
teacher-reported problem behavior between Turkish in parenting because obedience, conformism, and re-
immigrant and Dutch children (Crijnen et al., 2000), spect are highly valued in Turkish culture (Bengi-Arslan
whereas in self-reports on problem behavior, Turkish et al., 1997). Moreover, Turkish culture is character-
boys scored lower on delinquent behavior than Dutch ized by strong internal social cohesion and effective
boys (Murad et al., 2003). social control mechanisms, which are stronger than in
An explanation for the observed higher problem Moroccan culture (Dagevos, 2001). These mechanisms
scores among Moroccan boys may be the wider cul- may serve as protective factors in the development of
tural gap that Moroccans have to bridge compared problem behavior.
with Surinamese and Turks. Being colonial migrants, Despite the observed differences in problem behavior
Surinamese possessed Dutch nationality, spoke Dutch, among boys from different ethnic origins, we found no
and were more familiar with Dutch culture, which may differences among girls. This finding is also consistent
explain the similar scores on problem behavior be- with the study mentioned previously (Stevens et al.,
tween Surinamese and Dutch children. The migration 2003). Explanations for the absence of differences be-
history of Turks and Moroccans differs from that of tween girls may be related to the explanations for the
Surinamese in that Turks and Moroccans came as labor presence of differences among boys. For instance, in
migrants from Islamic countries. Despite the simi- contrast to Moroccan boys, Moroccan girls are not
larities, some important differences do exist between confronted with a bad image in the Netherlands. On the
Turkey and Morocco. For instance, Morocco is less contrary, they are said to be more integrated and to
secularized, democratized, and industrialized than do much better than Moroccan boys. Furthermore,
Turkey, and in Morocco, women have fewer political Moroccan girls experience less freedom of movement
rights than in Turkey. Thereby, the literacy rate is much and find themselves confronted much more with social
lower and the level of education is lower in Morocco control than boys (Buitelaar, 2002b). Moreover, be-
than in Turkey. Therefore, Moroccans must deal with a cause the rate of hyperactivity and impulsivity symp-
wider culture gap than do Surinamese and Turks. toms and comorbid disruptive behavior problems have
Another explanation may be the negative stigma been reported to be lower in girls with ADHD com-
from which Moroccan boys seem to suffer. This nega- pared with boys with ADHD (Abikoff et al., 2002;
tive stigma on the one hand and (presumed) problem Biederman et al., 2004) and the expression of aggression
behavior on the other may reinforce each other. Fur- in girls in general has found to be more covert compared
thermore, the gender-specific parenting in Moroccan with boys, the identification of behavioral problems in
culture does confront Moroccan boys with a harsh girls may be problematic. As a result, observing possible
authoritarian socialization in the home but with a lack differences in the level of behavioral problems between
of supervision outside the home (Pels, 2003). girls of different ethnic origin may be even more difficult.

480 J. AM . ACAD. CH ILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIAT RY, 45:4, APRIL 2006

Copyright @ 2006 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
DIFFERENT TREATMENT THRESHOLDS

We found no differences in impairment scores be- psychiatric care and the psychiatrist’s decision to refer
tween children of different ethnic origins. Thus, accord- the child from outpatient to inpatient psychiatric
ing to teachers, problem behavior does impair Dutch, care correspond with the third and the fourth filters,
Moroccan, Turkish, and Surinamese children equally. respectively.
This finding is consistent with the only other com- Several barriers at the first filter may contribute to the
parable study in which African American youth in observed difference in treatment rate between Dutch
the United States have been found to be as impaired and non-Dutch children. The first barrier may be
as white youth (Angold et al., 2002). The present differences in parental perceptions, meaning that non-
findings may suggest that children of different ethnic Dutch parents may apply other definitions of normal
origin differ in the number of symptoms but not in and abnormal behavior. The second barrier, a differ-
impairment; however, we reported only on impairment ence in attribution style, may cause non-Dutch parents
according to teachers. Therefore, impairment may be to not regard behavioral problems as something that
determined by ethnicity in other situations than the could be treated (Bussing et al., 1998). The third
school. barrier, a difference in social desirability, may cause
Despite our observation that problem behavior im- non-Dutch parents to be less willing to discuss mental
paired all ethnic groups equally, non-Dutch girls were problems with outsiders. The fourth barrier, differ-
about 80% less likely to receive psychiatric help than ences in caregiver strain, may cause non-Dutch parents
Dutch girls. Moroccan and Turkish boys were 70% to experience fewer demands, responsibilities, difficul-
and 80% less likely to be treated for their problem ties, and negative psychic consequences of caring for a
behavior, respectively. The probability of treatment child with behavioral problems than Dutch parents
among Surinamese boys was similar to that found in (McCabe et al., 2003). Fifth, the Dutch mental health
Dutch boys. This finding is in accordance with a study services system may not meet the services needs of non-
in which the use of health care was found to be rela- Dutch parents. Sixth, a discrepancy in behavior at
tively low in first-generation Turkish and Moroccan school and at home may cause non-Dutch children to
immigrants, whereas the Surinamese did not differ show relatively less problem behavior at home than at
from the Dutch population in service use (Stronks school. Finally, because teachers have been found to
et al., 2001). Importantly, because citizen status in play an important role in referring children with
itself is not related to healthcare access in the Neth- behavioral problems (Schneider and Grimes, 1993;
erlands, it not likely that a difference in citizen status Wisniewski et al., 1995), a potential teacher bias in
between Surinamese on the one hand and Turks and referrals may be another barrier at this stage.
Moroccans on the other hand explains the observed A potential barrier at the second stage is a difference
differences in treatment status. The present findings in a GP’s perceptions (i.e., GPs may apply other
are also in line with results of studies in the United definitions of normal and abnormal behavior in
States in which treatment rates have found to be lower children of different ethnic origins); however, minority
among nonwhite children compared with white chil- children have been found to be identified by clinicians
dren (Angold et al., 2002; Kataoka et al., 2002; Olfson as having symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity at
et al., 2003). the same rate as other children (Wasserman et al.,
Because the discrepancy in treatment status be- 1999). Another barrier at this filter may be a differ-
tween Dutch and non-Dutch could not be explained ence in attribution styleVGPs may consider behav-
by differences in problem level or impairment, ex- ioral problems in non-Dutch children to be a cultural
planations may be related to barriers in the help- problem instead of a treatable disorder. A barrier that
seeking process. Goldberg and Huxley (1980) describe may serve at both the third and fourth filters is dis-
this process as consisting of five levels separated by crimination, in that GPs or psychiatrists may be less
four filters. The first filter concerns expression and willing to refer non-Dutch children to specialized men-
recognition of behavioral problems and the decision tal health care (Kendall and Hatton, 2002; Williams
to consult a general practitioner (GP). The second and Rucker, 2000). Finally, a barrier possibly serving
filter represents the recognition of the child_s problem at all filters is that ethnicity may determine the expres-
by the GP. The GP’s decision to refer the child to sion of behavioral problems. Consequently, behavioral

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 45:4, APRIL 2006 481

Copyright @ 2006 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
ZWIRS ET AL.

problems in children of some ethnic origins may be ment, or treatment status, their role was not investi-
more difficult to identify than in children of other ethnic gated in the present study.
origins.
Clinical Implications and Future Research
Limitations Our finding that non-Dutch boys and girls are less
We explored only the teachers’ perspective. As a likely to be treated for their behavioral problems than
result, perceptual bias may be responsible for the Dutch children is worrisome given the evidence that
observed higher problem scores among Moroccan boys. behavioral disorders increase the risks of peer prob-
A perceptual bias in teacher ratings has been reported lems, school problems, substance abuse, criminality,
previously. Sonuga-Barke and Minocha (1993) and psychiatric disorders later in life (Biederman et al.,
found that teachers rated Asian immigrant children 1996; Mannuzza et al., 1989). In addition to clinical
higher on hyperactivity symptoms than English native implications, the present results have political implica-
children, whereas the scores on more objective measures tions, indicating that non-Westerners have less access
of hyperactivity were similar for both groups (Sonuga- to services than Westerners.
Barke and Minocha, 1993). Nevertheless, it is not likely With the increasing number of immigrants world-
that a strong perceptual bias influenced our data wide, our findings do not have relevance only for the
because lower problem scores were observed among Netherlands but also for the United States and other
Turkish boys. Moreover, as teachers have been found to multiethnic societies because treatment thresholds of
play an important role in initiating the help-seeking Western and non-Western children in these countries
process, their perceptions are of great interest, whether may parallel the currently observed treatment thresh-
or not they are biased. Finally, because the treatment olds. Therefore, detection of behavioral disorders in
status of the child is not determined by a teacher’s non-Western children should receive more attention
perceptions, we still observe an enormous discrepancy from policymakers in child and adolescent mental
in treatment status between Dutch and non-Dutch health, teachers, and clinicians in multiethnic societies
children. in general. Possibly, additional means of accessing the
In addition, our samples were from inner-city healthcare systems should be developed for immigrant
neighborhoods with a low SES, and therefore our children to reduce inequities. Other strategies may in-
findings cannot be generalized to other neighborhoods clude placing much more emphasis on various training
with middle and higher SES levels without additional curricula on the detection and presentation of behav-
study. Nevertheless, because most migrants in the ioral disorders in non-Western children, but first more
Netherlands live in urban areas and are from the lowest research is needed to clarify the mechanisms underlying
SES, generalizing to middle and higher SES neighbor- disparities in service use between Western and non-
hoods seems of limited relevance. Western children.
Moreover, adjustment for SES by an area-based
method is a rather crude way to control for differences Disclosure: Dr. Buitelaar has served as a consultant to and has been on
in SES. We think, however, the SES indicated by the advisory boards for Eli Lilly, Janssen Cilag BV, UCB, and Shire in the
past 2 years. The other authors have no financial relationships to
school would closely resemble that of the household disclose.
because most children attend a school in their own
neighborhood. In addition, because a substantial REFERENCES
number of minorities in the Netherlands have had no Abikoff HB, Jensen PS, Arnold LL et al. (2002), Observed classroom
formal education and no comparable Dutch group can behavior of children with ADHD: relationship to gender and
be found (Uniken Venema et al., 1995), we do not comorbidity. J Abnorm Child Psychol 30:349Y359
American Psychiatric Association (1994), Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
think it is possible to have a more suitable sample than of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM-IV) , Washington, DC: American
our sample from urban areas with a low SES and a large Psychiatric Association
Achenbach TM (1991), Integrative Guide to the 1991 CBCL/4-18, YSR, and
immigrant population. TRF Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of
Finally, although religion and perhaps other socio- Psychiatry
Angold A, Erkanli A, Egger HL, Costello EJ (2000), Stimulant treatment for
demographic factors may play an important role in children: a community perspective. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
explaining differences in problem behavior, impair- 39:975Y984

482 J. AM . ACAD. CH ILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIAT RY, 45:4, APRIL 2006

Copyright @ 2006 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
DIFFERENT TREATMENT THRESHOLDS

Angold A, Erkanli A, Farmer EM, Fairbank JA, Burns BJ, Keeler G, Costello and Dutch adolescents in the Netherlands. J Child Psychol Psychiatry
EJ (2002), Psychiatric disorder, impairment, and service use in rural 44:412Y423
African American and white youth. Arch Gen Psychiatry 59:893Y901 Muris P, Meesters C, Van Den BF (2003), The Strengths and Difficulties
Bengi-Arslan L, Verhulst FC, Van der Ende J, Erol N (1997), Under- Questionnaire (SDQ)Vfurther evidence for its reliability and validity in
standing childhood (problem) behaviors from a cultural perspective: a community sample of Dutch children and adolescents. Eur Child
comparison of problem behaviors and competencies in Turkish im- Adolesc Psychiatry 12:1Y8
migrant, Turkish and Dutch children. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol Olfson M, Gameroff MJ, Marcus SC, Jensen PS (2003), National trends in
32:477Y484 the treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry
Biederman J, Faraone S, Milberger S et al. (1996), A prospective 4-year 160:1071Y1077
follow-up study of attention-deficit hyperactivity and related disorders. Pels T (2003), Respect van twee kanten; Een studie over last van Marokkaanse
Arch Gen Psychiatry 53:437Y446 jongerent (The Question of Respect; A Study on Misconduct of Moroccan
Biederman J, Mick E, Faraone S, Braaten E (2004), Influenceof gender on Boys). Assen, The Netherlands: Koninklijke van Gorcum
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children referred to a Reid R, DuPaul GJ, Power TJ et al. (1998), Assessing culturally different
psychiatric clinic. Am J Psychiatry 159:42 students for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder using behavior rating
Buitelaar JK (2002a), Epidemiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity scales. J Abnorm Child Psychol 26:187Y198
disorder: what have we learned over the last decade? In: Hyperactivity Reid R, Riccio CA, Kessler RH et al. (2000), Gender and ethnic differences
and Attention Disorders of Childhood, Sandberg S, ed. Cambridge, UK: in ADHD as assessed by behavior ratings. J Emot Behav Disord 8:38Y53
Cambridge University Press, pp 1Y39 Rowland AS, Umbach DM, Stallone L, Naftel AJ, Bohlig EM, Sandler DP
Buitelaar MW (2002b), Negotiating the rules of chaste behaviour: re- (2002), Prevalence of medication treatment for attention deficit-
interpretations of the symbolic complex of virginity by young women of hyperactivity disorder among elementary school children in Johnston
Moroccan descent in The Netherlands. Ethnic Racial Studies 25:462Y489 County, North Carolina. Am J Public Health 92:231Y234
Bussing R, Schoenberg NE, Perwien AR (1998), Knowledge and infor- Safer DJ, Malever M (2000), Stimulant treatment in Maryland public
mation about ADHD: evidence of cultural differences among African- schools. Pediatrics 106:533Y539
American and white parents. Soc Sci Med 46:919Y928 Samuel VJ, Biederman J, Faraone SV et al. (1998), Clinical characteristics of
Crijnen AA, Achenbach TM, Verhulst FC (1999), Problems reported by attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in African American children. Am
parents of children in multiple cultures: the Child Behavior Checklist J Psychiatry 155:696Y698
syndrome constructs. Am J Psychiatry 156:569Y574 Schneider PL, Grimes RM (1993), Potential bias in teacher referrals to the
Crijnen AA, Bengi-Arslan L, Verhulst FC (2000), Teacher-reported problem school nurse. J Sch Health 63:426Y428
behaviour in Turkish immigrant and Dutch children: a cross-cultural Sonuga-Barke EJS, Minocha K (1993), Inter-ethnic bias in teachers’ ratings
comparison. Acta Psychiatr Scand 102:439Y444 of childhood hyperactivity. Br J Dev Psychol 11:187Y200
Dagevos J (2001), Perspectief op integratie; Over de sociaal-culturele en Souza I, Pinheiro MA, Denardin D, Mattos P, Rohde LA (2004), Attention-
structurele integratie van etnische minderheden in Nederland (Perspec- deficit/hyperactivity disorder and comorbidity in Brazil: comparisons
tive on Integration; On the Social-Cultural Position of Ethnic Minorities between two referred samples. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 13:243Y248
in The Netherlands). The Hague: Wetenschappelijke raad voor het Stevens GW, Pels T, Bengi-Arslan L, Verhulst FC, Vollebergh WA, Crijnen
regeringsbeleid AA (2003), Parent, teacher and self-reported problem behavior in The
Gillberg C, Gillberg IC, Rasmussen P et al. (2004), Co-existing disorders in Netherlands: comparing Moroccan immigrant with Dutch and with
ADHDVimplications for diagnosis and intervention. Eur Child Adolesc Turkish immigrant children and adolescents. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr
Psychiatry 13(Suppl 1):I80YI92 Epidemiol 38:576Y585
Goldberg D, Huxley P (1980), Mental Illness in the Community: The Stronks K, Ravelli AC, Reijneveld SA (2001), Immigrants in the Nether-
Pathway to Psychiatric Care. London: Tavistock lands: equal access for equal needs? J Epidemiol Community Health
Goodman R (1997), The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a 55:701Y707
research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 38:581Y586 Uniken Venema HP, Garretsen HF, van der Maas PJ (1995), Health of
Kataoka SH, Zhang L, Wells KB (2002), Unmet need for mental health care migrants and migrant health policy, The Netherlands as an example. Soc
among U.S. children: variation by ethnicity and insurance status. Am J Sci Med 41:809Y818
Psychiatry 159:1548Y1555 Van Widenfelt BM, Goedhart AW, Treffers PD, Goodman R (2003),
Kendall J, Hatton D (2002), Racism as a source of health disparity in Dutch version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ).
families with children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. ANS Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 12:281Y289
Adv Nurs Sci 25:22Y39 Verhulst FC, Achenbach TM, van der EJ et al. (2003), Comparisons of
Knol FA (1998), Van hoog naar laag; van laag naar hoog (From High to Low; problems reported by youths from seven countries. Am J Psychiatry
From Low to High). The Hague: Sociaal Cultureel Planbureau 160:1479Y1485
Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Konig PH, Giampino TL (1989), Hyperactive boys Wasserman RC, Kelleher KJ, Bocian A et al. (1999), Identification of
almost grown up. IV. Criminality and its relationship to psychiatric attentional and hyperactivity problems in primary care: a report from
status. Arch Gen Psychiatry 46:1073Y1079 pediatric research in office settings and the ambulatory sentinel practice
McCabe KM, Yeh M, Lau A, Garland A, Hough R (2003), Racial/ethnic network. Pediatrics 103:E38
differences in caregiver strain and perceived social support among Williams DR, Rucker TD (2000), Understanding and addressing racial
parents of youth with emotional and behavioral problems. Ment Health disparities in health care. Health Care Financ Rev 21:75Y90
Serv Res 5:137Y147 Wisniewski JJ, Andrews TJ, Mulick JA (1995), Objective and subjective
Murad SD, Joung IM, van Lenthe FJ, Bengi-Arslan L, Crijnen AA (2003), factors in the disproportionate referral of children for academic
Predictors of self-reported problem behaviours in Turkish immigrant problems. J Consult Clin Psychol 63:1032Y1036

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 45:4, APRIL 2006 483

Copyright @ 2006 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

You might also like