You are on page 1of 18

European Spine Journal (2020) 29:480–496

https://doi.org/10.1007/s00586-019-06278-6

REVIEW ARTICLE

Chronic physical illnesses, mental health disorders, and psychological


features as potential risk factors for back pain from childhood
to young adulthood: a systematic review with meta‑analysis
Amber M. Beynon1   · Jeffrey J. Hebert1,2 · Christopher J. Hodgetts1 · Leah M. Boulos3 · Bruce F. Walker1

Received: 27 August 2019 / Revised: 27 August 2019 / Accepted: 29 December 2019 / Published online: 6 January 2020
© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2020

Abstract
Purpose  To report evidence of chronic physical illnesses, mental health disorders, and psychological features as potential
risk factors for back pain in children, adolescents, and young adults.
Methods  This systematic review and meta-analysis included cohort and inception cohort studies that investigated potential
risk factors for back pain in young people. Potential risk factors of interest were chronic physical illnesses, mental health
disorders (e.g. depression, anxiety), and other psychological features (e.g. coping, resistance). Searches were conducted in
MEDLINE, Embase, CINAHL, and Scopus from inception to July 2019.
Results  Nineteen of 2167 screened articles were included in the qualitative synthesis, and data from 12 articles were included
in the meta-analysis. Evidence from inception cohort studies demonstrated psychological distress, emotional coping problems,
and somatosensory amplification to be likely risk factors for back pain. Evidence from non-inception cohort studies cannot
distinguish between risk factors or back pain triggers. However, we identified several additional factors that were associated
with back pain. Specifically, asthma, headaches, abdominal pain, depression, anxiety, conduct problems, somatization, and
‘feeling tense’ are potential risk factors or triggers for back pain. Results from the meta-analyses demonstrated the most
likely risk factors for back pain in young people are psychological distress and emotional coping problems.
Conclusion  Psychological features are the most likely risk factors for back pain in young people. Several other factors were
associated with back pain, but their potential as risk factors was unclear due to risk of bias. Additional high-quality research
is needed to better elucidate these relationships.
Graphic abstract
These slides can be retrieved under Electronic Supplementary Material.

Key points Take Home Messages

1. The purpose of this systematic review was to report evidence of childhood 1. Based on this systematic review childhood psychological features are
chronic physical illnesses, mental health disorders, and psychological the most likely risk factors for back pain in young people.
features as potential risk factors for back pain in children, adolescents, and
young adults. 2. Several other factors were associated with later back pain, but their
potential as risk factors was unclear due to risk of bias.
2. The most likely childhood risk factors identified for back pain are
psychological distress, as well as psychological features including emotional
3. The identification of early life risk factors and development of future
coping problems and somatosensory amplification.
prevention strategies could lead to a reduction in the economic burden
3. Due to the limitations of the literature the role of some physical illnesses that back pain places on society.
and mental health disorders as potential comorbidities, triggers, or risk
factors of back pain remains unclear.

Beynon A, Hebert J, Hodgetts C, Boulos L, Walker B (2019) Chronic physical illnesses, Beynon A, Hebert J, Hodgetts C, Boulos L, Walker B (2019) Chronic physical illnesses, Beynon A, Hebert J, Hodgetts C, Boulos L, Walker B (2019) Chronic physical illnesses,
mental health disorders, and psychological features as potential risk factors for back pain mental health disorders, and psychological features as potential risk factors for back pain mental health disorders, and psychological features as potential risk factors for back pain
from childhood to young adulthood: a systematic review with meta -analysis. Eur Spine J; from childhood to young adulthood: a systematic review with meta -analysis. Eur Spine J; from childhood to young adulthood: a systematic review with meta -analysis. Eur Spine J;

Keywords  Back pain · Systematic review · Meta-analysis · Risk factors · Children · Young adult

Abbreviations
Electronic supplementary material  The online version of this
article (https​://doi.org/10.1007/s0058​6-019-06278​-6) contains QUIPS Quality In Prognostic Studies tool
supplementary material, which is available to authorized users. OR Odds ratio
RR Risk ratio
Extended author information available on the last page of the article

13
Vol:.(1234567890)
European Spine Journal (2020) 29:480–496 481

CI Confidence intervals Methods


N Number of participants
SD Standard deviation The study protocol was prospectively registered with Pros-
NR Not reported pero (CRD42019119226). This systematic review and meta-
NA Not applicable analysis adhere to the Preferred Reporting Items for System-
BP Back pain atic Review and Meta-Analysis [14] and to the Meta-analyses
LBP Low back pain Of Observational Studies in Epidemiology guidelines [15].
MBP Mid-back pain
β Beta
Eligibility criteria
BMI Body mass index
We included original peer-reviewed cohort or inception
cohort studies that investigated potential risk factors for pain
in the thoracic and/or lumbar spine (i.e. back pain) in chil-
Introduction
dren, adolescents, and young adults. Potential risk factors
of interest were chronic physical illnesses such as cardio-
Low back pain is the leading cause of years lived with
vascular disease, respiratory tract disease, digestive system
disability worldwide [1] and affects people of all ages,
disease, endocrine disease, or immune system disease, as
including children [2, 3]. Low back pain etiology is com-
well as mental health disorders (e.g. depression, anxiety), or
plex and has many contributors, including social factors,
other psychological features (e.g. coping, resistance). Back
physical factors, psychological factors, and certain comor-
pain outcomes were either self-reported or clinically evalu-
bidities [4].
ated. We excluded studies when the mean baseline age was
Asthma, allergies, and depression are reportedly asso-
greater than 24 years of age. We did not exclude studies
ciated with low back pain from adolescence to adulthood
based on the language of publication.
[5, 6]. A systematic review and meta-analysis of cross-sec-
tional twin studies found that young people and adults with
chronic conditions such as asthma, diabetes, and headaches Search strategies
were more likely to report low back pain (pooled OR range
1.6–4.2) [7]. Respiratory and digestive disorders also dem- Systematic searches were conducted in MEDLINE (Ovid
onstrate cross-sectional and longitudinal associations with MEDLINE and Epub Ahead of Print, In-Process & Other
back pain in adults [8, 9]. Similarly, a history of cardiovas- Non-Indexed Citations, Daily and Versions), Embase,
cular disease is associated with increased risk of chronic low CINAHL with Full Text, and Scopus from inception to 30
back pain in adults [10]. A large Canadian National Popu- July 2019. Reference lists of included papers were searched
lation Health Survey reported that adolescents and adults to identify other potentially suitable studies.
living with major depression were almost three times more The search was conducted by a health librarian (LB),
likely to report back pain 2 years later [11]. using a back pain filter developed for the Cochrane Back and
Some etiological studies report risk factors for back pain, Neck Group. Due to the large recall of the search, scope was
while others report factors that are associated with back pain. limited to risk factors by employing the causation (etiology)
Risk factors are variables that are causally related to a change best balance filter developed by the McMaster Hedges Team
in the risk of a health process, outcome, or condition [12]. [16]. We applied a modified age filter to identify paediatric
In back pain research, it is important to distinguish between studies [17]. A study-type filter was adapted from the SIGN
potential risk factors that cause the initial onset of pain and Observational Studies filter; cross-sectional study queries
triggers that may precipitate an episode of pain [13]. Studies were removed [18]. The full search strategies for each data-
investigating potential risk factors and triggers of back pain base are listed in Online Resource 1.
have often included adult populations in which it is difficult Search results were imported into bibliographic manage-
to identify disease-free (i.e. no history of back pain) cohorts. ment software (EndNote X9.2) and duplicates discarded.
It would, therefore, be of value to consider young popula-
tions, ideally before the onset of back pain, to determine Study selection, data extraction, and risk of bias
whether chronic illnesses and mental health conditions are
in fact risk factors for back pain rather than comorbidities. Titles and abstracts were screened by two researchers (AB
The purpose of this systematic review was to investigate and CD or CH) against the eligibility criteria. The full text
whether chronic physical illnesses, mental health disorders, of possibly relevant papers was obtained and again assessed
and psychological features are potential triggers or risk fac- against the same criteria (AB and CH). Disagreements were
tors for back pain in children, adolescents, and young adults.

13

482 European Spine Journal (2020) 29:480–496

resolved through consensus between the reviewers and con- unsuitable purely due to high I2 values, given that the indi-
sultation with a third review author when necessary. vidual study estimates were within a reasonable range [26,
Data from included studies were extracted by two review 27].
authors (AB and CH) using a data collection form based Potential risk factors were included in the meta-analysis
on the Checklist for critical Appraisal and data extraction when data were available from two or more studies. Physi-
for systematic Reviews of prediction Modelling Studies cal illness and mental health risk factors were considered
(CHARMS) guideline [19]. Data collection forms were individually. However, when studies reported on many dif-
piloted on four studies prior to full data extraction. The pilot- ferent psychological features, we further categorized these
ing process did not result in changes to the form. Disagree- factors into seven domains: peer problems, emotional coping
ments were resolved through consensus or consultation with problems, conduct problems, hyperactivity, somatosensory
a third review author. amplification, feeling tense or stressed, and fatigue/tiredness.
Included articles were assessed for risk of bias by two
review authors (AB and CH) with the Quality In Prognos-
tic Studies (QUIPS) tool [20, 21] (Online Resource 2). We Results
made minor modifications to the QUIPS tool by adopting
risk factor language, rather than prognostic factor language, Study selection
by changing the word prognostic to risk. Six domains were
assessed for risk of bias including study participation, study We identified 3097 articles through searching databases and
attrition, risk factor measurement, outcome measurement, one additional record through searching the relevant refer-
study confounding, and statistical analysis and reporting. A ences lists. After duplications were removed and records
study with a low overall risk of bias required the majority screened, 19 articles (N = 34,279 participants) met the
of the six domains to be scored as a low risk. A study with selection criteria and were included in the qualitative syn-
moderate risk of bias overall would have some items rated as thesis. From these, 12 articles (N = 25,372 participants) were
moderate risk of bias within the study. A study with overall included in the meta-analysis (Fig. 1). Seven studies were
high risk of bias would have significant flaws with an associ- excluded from the meta-analysis owing to methodological
ated risk of bias in relation to some of the six domains [20]. or clinical heterogeneity.

Summary of findings and synthesis of results


Study characteristics
The characteristics of included studies were tabulated for
comparison. Potential risk factors were assessed by the num- Of the 19 included studies, 16 were prospective cohort
ber of times investigated, number of times the factor was studies [5, 28–42] and three were inception cohort studies
found to be associated with the outcome of interest, and the [43–45]. Study populations were from Australia [30, 33, 41],
strength of the associations. Canada [32, 42, 44], England [35, 36], Finland [34, 39], UK
Meta-analysis of reported odds ratios (OR) and risk [40, 45], China [29], Denmark [5], Germany [43], Norway
ratios (RR) with 95% confidence intervals [95% CI] were [38], Sweden [28], Thailand [37], and USA [31]. The char-
performed using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis v3 software acteristics of all included studies are reported in Table 1.
(Biostat, Inc., USA). Data from studies reporting multiple
potential risk factors within the same domain (e.g. ‘emo- Risk of bias assessment
tional coping problems’ and ‘feeling tense or stressed’) were
first combined with fixed effect models to account for the Risk of bias results are presented in Table 2. Two studies
lack of independence between study outcomes [22]. We were rated as low risk of bias, 12 studies were at moderate
then applied random effects models to pool data between risk of bias, and five studies were at high risk of bias. Com-
studies [22]. Where possible, odds ratios were converted to mon sources of bias included study attrition, study partici-
risk ratios as RR = OR/(1 − P0 + (P0 × OR), where P0 is the pation, and outcome measurement. Study attrition concerns
baseline risk or the incidence of the outcome of interest in comprised either a high attrition rate or when reasons for
the non-exposed group [23, 24]. Studies were assessed for drop out were not reported. Regarding study participation,
statistical heterogeneity using I2 [22, 25]. Although there is the source population and/or the inclusion and exclusion
no agreement on I2 interpretation, we applied the follow- criteria were often poorly described. The back pain meas-
ing criteria: 0–40% represented low heterogeneity, 30–60% ures were often unclear; many studies did not use validated
represented moderate heterogeneity, 50–90% represented questions and/or did not clearly specify the type of back pain
substantial heterogeneity, and 75–100% represented consid- (e.g. ongoing, episodic or first time). Many studies did not
erable heterogeneity [25]. Meta-analysis was not considered

13
European Spine Journal (2020) 29:480–496 483

Fig. 1  Study selection flow diagram

report a clear delineation of the area of the back under study, Physical illnesses
such as with pain diagrams.
Four cohort studies reported on headaches as a potential
Summary of findings risk factor for back pain [5, 29, 35, 36], with two studies
reporting no association [35, 36], and two studies report-
Five prospective cohort studies [5, 29, 35, 36, 40] and one ing increased odds of back pain (OR [95% CI] = 2.5 [1.6,
inception cohort study [44] investigated physical illnesses, 4.1] (n = 6554) [5], OR [95% CI) = 4.5 [1.8, 11.5] (n = 212)
eight prospective cohort studies investigated mental health [29]). Abdominal pain was evaluated in three cohort stud-
disorders [29–33, 38, 41, 42], and 15 studies considered ies [35, 36, 40]; two studies reported an increased risk of
different psychological features as potential risk factors for back pain (RR [95% CI] = 1.8 [1.1, 3.0] (n = 933) [36], RR
back pain [28, 29, 31, 33–37, 39–45], of which three were [95% CI] = 1.8 [1.3, 2.4] (n = 3271) [40]) among participants
inception cohort studies [43–45]. An overview of the results with previous abdominal pain, and one study reported no
from all included studies is reported in Table 3. association [35]. One inception cohort study [44] and one

13

Table 1  Study characteristics of the included studies


484

Reference, (year), Study design, set- Baseline: sex, age Sample size: base- Follow-up: no. of Back pain defini- Risk factors meas- Confounders Analysis approach
country ting/sample mean (SD)a line and completed times, length of tion ures

13
time

[43] Barke, (2014), Inception cohort, 46% female, 11.25 Baseline: 3524 1, 12 months Self-reported, first- Dysfunctional Age and sex Logistic regression
Germany representative (2.28) Completed: 2040 time BP stress coping,
community pain catastro-
sample phizing, anxiety
sensitivity, and
somatosensory
amplification
[28] Brattberg, Prospective cohort, Both % female NR, Baseline: 591 1, 2 years Self-reported BP Fear of school- Age and gender NR
(1994), Sweden school 8–13 Completed: 471 ‘often’ mates, loneli-
ness, difficulties
making friends,
feelings of being
outsider, been
bullied, passive
reactive to bully-
ing, nervousness,
difficulties verbal-
izing feelings,
diffuse feelings of
anger
[29] Cheung, Prospective cohort, 78% female, 19.4 Baseline: 265 3, 2, 12 and Self-reported LBP: Constant tiredness, Age, gender and Multivariate logistic
(2010), China university (1.12) Completed: 212 26 months past week, month low mood, feel- height regression
and year ing tense, under
stress, headaches,
fatigue and anxi-
ety
[30] Coenen, Prospective cohort, 53% female, 17 Baseline: 2868 2, 3 years, 2 years LBP, past month Mental HRQOL Sex, waist circum- Linear and logistic
(2016),Australia birth cohort (NR) Completed: 1249 (health-related ference, comorbid regression, latent
quality of life) pain class analysis
[31] Dunn, (2011) Prospective cohort, 53% female, 11 Baseline: 1996 11, 3 months Presence of BP, Depression and NR Latent growth curve
USA representative (NR) Completed: 1333 pain persistence somatization analysis, ANOVA
community
sample
[32] Feldman, Prospective cohort, 47% female, 13.8 Baseline: 810 2, 6 months Substantial LBP in Mental health Age, gender, smok- Logistic regression,
(2001), Canada high school (1.2) Completed: 502 last 6 months status ing generalized esti-
mating equations
[33] Gill, (2014), Prospective cohort, 49% female, 14 Baseline: 1596 1, 3 years LBP, MBP. No BP Internalizing Sex Logistic regression
Australia birth cohort (NR) Completed: 1291 last month at 14 and external-
years (first time izing behaviour,
or episodic) depression
[34] Gustafsson, Prospective cohort, 54% female, 10 Baseline: 1097 2, 2 years, 3 years BP preceding Daytime sleepiness, Age and sex Ordinal logistic
European Spine Journal (2020) 29:480–496

(2018), Finland elementary (NR) Completed: 568 6 months psychological regression


school symptoms
Table 1  (continued)
Reference, (year), Study design, set- Baseline: sex, age Sample size: base- Follow-up: no. of Back pain defini- Risk factors meas- Confounders Analysis approach
country ting/sample mean (SD)a line and completed times, length of tion ures
time

[5] Hestbaek, Prospective cohort, 52% female, 17.27 Baseline: 9600 1, 8 years later LBP, persistent or Asthma, atopic dis- Gender and age Multivariate logistic
(2006), Denmark twin register (NR) Completed: 6554 recurrent BP, past ease, headaches regression
year
[35] Jones, (2009), Prospective cohort, Both  % female Baseline: 1496 2, 1 year, 3 years LBP, persistent or Headache, Age and sex Poisson regression
England secondary school NR, 11–14 Completed: 178 recurrent past abdominal pain,
(54% with LBP month daytime tiredness,
at baseline) behavioural and
emotional charac-
European Spine Journal (2020) 29:480–496

teristics
[36] Jones, (2003), Prospective cohort, Both  % female Baseline: 1446 1, 1 year LBP, past month Psychosocial Age and gender Poisson regression
England secondary school NR, 11–14 Completed: 933 factors, somatic model
complaints: head-
aches, abdominal
pain, sore throats
[37] Kanchanomai, Prospective cohort, 74% female, 19.4 Baseline: 2511 4, 3 months LBP during previ- Psychosocial fac- NR Univariate and mul-
(2015), Thailand university (1.1) Completed: 524 ous 3 months tors tivariate regression
models
[38] Lien, (2011), Prospective cohort, 57% female, 15–16 Baseline: 5750 1, 3 years Self-reported BP Mental distress Ethnicity, fam- Multivariate logistic
Norway tenth-grade pri- Completed: 3316 last 12 months ily structure, regressions
mary school self-perceived
socioeconomic
status, stratified
by gender.
[39] Mikkonen, Prospective cohort, 56% female 16 Baseline: 7344 1, 2 years LBP during the Emotional and Socioeconomic Latent class analysis,
(2016), Finland birth cohort (NR) Completed: 1625 past 6 months behavioural status, stratified log-binomial
(55% eligible problems by gender regression
cohort)
[44] Mustard, Inception cohort, Both  % female Baseline: 2867 2, 4 years, 14 years First episode of BP Psychological Age, sex, childhood Logistic regression
(2005), Canada representative NR, 4–16 Completed: 1928 status, emotional conditions, health
community and behavioural status, behaviour,
sample disorders, chronic socioeconomic
medical condi- status, work
tions environment
[40] Muthuri Prospective cohort, 50% female, From Baseline: 5362 24, 2 yrs during BP previous Illnesses and BMI, psychiatric Longitudinal latent
(2018), Eng- birth cohort birth Completed: 3271 childhood, 5–10 12 months injury early life, disorders, educa- class analysis,
land, Wales, and years in adult- abdominal pain, tion level, occu- logistic regression
Scotland hood classroom behav- pation, smoking, models
iour (conduct parental BP, sex
and emotional
problems)

13
485

486 European Spine Journal (2020) 29:480–496

cohort study [40] reported no association between any ill-

Multinomial logistic

structural equation
Logistic regression
Analysis approach ness or medical condition and back pain [40, 44]. In one of

Latent variable
these studies, all chronic medical conditions were combined

regression

modelling
including: arthritis, asthma, missing fingers, blindness, and
speech problems [44]. The other study included any ‘serious
illnesses’ from 0 to 15 years of age [40]. One cohort study
reported no association between atopic disease and subse-
Sex and back pain quent back pain [5]; however, children and young adults with
characteristics.
stress, psycho-
Sex, ergonomic

asthma demonstrated increased odds of back pain (OR [95%


social work
Confounders

at 14 years
CI] = 1.3 [1.1, 1.6] (n = 6554) [5]).

Sex
Mental health disorders
Risk factors meas-

depression, self-
emotional prob-
behavioural and

Anxiety and/or

Five cohort studies considered overall mental health status


Psychological

Self-efficacy,
depression,

as a potential risk factor for back pain [30, 32, 38, 41, 42],
distress

esteem

three of these studies found positive associations [30, 38, 42]


lems
ures

with increased odds in one study (OR [95% CI] = 1.6 [1.1,


2.4] (boys), OR [95% CI] = 1.5 [1.1, 1.9] (girls) (n = 3316)
(between 32 and

[38]). Two studies reported no association between over-


Back pain defini-

LBP, last month

Backache, past

all mental health status and back pain [32, 41]. Regarding
Incident LBP

6 months
33 years)

depression, two cohort studies found positive associations


SD standard deviation, NR not reported, NA not applicable, BP back pain, LBP low back pain, MBP mid-back pain

[31, 33] with increased odds of back pain in one study (OR
tion

[95% CI] = 1.05 [1.02, 1.08] (n = 1291) [33]); however, this


study only reported an association between depression and
Sample size: base- Follow-up: no. of

mid-back pain, but not with low back pain [33]. Only one
line and completed times, length of

cohort study considered anxiety as a potential risk factor and


1, 10 years

1, 3 years

5, 2 years

found increased odds of back pain (OR [95% CI] = 4.6 [1.9,


11.1] (n = 212) [29]).
time

Psychological features
Completed: 1415
Completed: 5781

Completed: 1088
Baseline: 11,407

Baseline: 1608

Baseline: 2488

Many different psychological features have been consid-


ered as potential risk factors for back pain. Six studies
considered overall psychological state [34–37, 44, 45], of
which three found no association [34, 35, 37], two incep-
tion cohorts reported increased odds of back pain (OR
Prospective cohort, 49% female, 10.50
Baseline: sex, age

51% female, 23

Prospective cohort, 52% female, 14

[95% CI] = 1.8 [1.1, 3.2] (n = 1928) [44], OR [95% CI] = 2.5


mean (SD)a

[1.6, 3.9] (n = 5781) [45]), and one cohort study found an


(0.50)

increased risk of back pain (RR [95% CI] = 1.6 [1.1, 2.3]


 Age range reported when mean (SD) not reported
(NR)

(NR)

(n = 933) [36]). Individual psychological features reported


to be associated with back pain from cohort studies include
conduct problems (RR [95% CI] = 2.5 [1.7, 3.7] (n = 933)
Study design, set-

[45] Power, (2001), Inception cohort,

representative

[36], RR [95% CI] = 2.1 [1.3, 3.4] (n = 3271) [40]), soma-


birth cohort

birth cohort

community
ting/sample

tization (OR [95% CI] = 1.3 [1.1, 1.5] (n = 1088) [41]), and


sample

peer problems (RR [95% CI] 2.3 [1.2, 4.2] (n = 178) [35]).
Individual psychological features reported as risk factors
with back pain from inception cohort studies include emo-
Table 1  (continued)

[41] Smith (2017),


England, Wales,

tional or behavioural disorders (OR [95% CI] = 1.87 [1.02,


Reference, (year),

(2008), Canada
and Scotland

3.41] (n = 1928) [44]), dysfunctional coping (OR [95%


[42] Stanford,
Australia,

CI] = 1.4 [1.1, 2.0] (boys) (n = 2040) [43]), anxiety sensi-


country

tivity (OR [95% CI] = 1.5 [1.1, 2.0] (boys) (n = 2040) [43],


a

13
European Spine Journal (2020) 29:480–496 487

Table 2  Risk of bias assessment: modified QUIPS


Study Study participation Study attrition Risk factor Outcome Study confounding Statistical analy- Overall rating
measure- measure- sis and reporting
ment ment

Barke (2014) High High High High Moderate Low High


Brattberg (1994) Moderate High High Moderate High High High
Cheung (2010) Low High Moderate Low Low Low Moderate
Coenen (2016) Moderate High Low Low Low Low Moderate
Dunn (2011) Low Moderate Low High High Moderate High
Feldman (2001) Low Low Low Moderate Low Low Low
Gill (2014) Moderate Moderate Low Moderate Moderate Low Moderate
Gustafsson (2018) Moderate Moderate Low Moderate Low Moderate Moderate
Hestbaek (2006) Moderate Moderate Moderate Low Low Low Moderate
Jones (2009) Moderate High Moderate Moderate Low Low Moderate
Jones (2003) Moderate Moderate Moderate Moderate Low Low Moderate
Kanchanomai (2015) Low High Low Moderate High Moderate High
Lien (2011) Low High Low High Low Low High
Mikkonen (2016) Moderate High Low Moderate Low Low Moderate
Mustard (2005) Moderate High Low Low Low Low Moderate
Muthuri (2018) Low Moderate Low Moderate Low Low Moderate
Power (2001) Moderate High Low Moderate Low Low Moderate
Smith (2017) Low High Low Low Low Low Low
Stanford (2008) Low Low Low Moderate Low Low Moderate

and somatosensory amplification (OR [95% CI] = 1.8 [1.0, Mental health disorders
3.1] (girls) (n = 2040) [43]).
Mental health disorders including depression, and mental
Meta‑analysis health status overall could not be included in a meta-anal-
ysis due to the large methodological and statistical het-
Physical illnesses erogeneity between the studies. Anxiety was investigated
as a potential risk factor for back pain in only one study.
Only one study reported on asthma and atopic disease
[5]; therefore, these factors were not included in a meta- Psychological features
analysis. Two studies considered any illness or medical
condition as a potential risk factor for back pain [40, 44]; Eleven studies considering psychological features as poten-
however, the conditions were too varied to combine in tial risk factors for back pain were included in the meta-
a meaningful way. We synthesized data from four stud- analysis [28, 29, 33, 35, 36, 39–41, 43–45]. Of the included
ies investigating two types of physical illness [5, 35, 36, studies, four studies reported risk ratios [35, 36, 39, 40], and
40]. Overall, there was a positive association between seven studies reported odds ratios [28, 29, 33, 41, 43–45],
headaches at 11–22 years of age and experiencing back odds ratios could not be converted to risk ratios from the
pain 1–8 years later (pooled RR [95% CI] = 1.9 [1.4, 2.6]; information provided within these studies. Two inception
n = 7665; I 2 = 0.00) (Fig. 2) [5, 35, 36]. Similarly, data cohort studies evaluated the role of psychological distress
combined from three studies [35, 36, 40] demonstrated overall [44, 45]. Participants with psychological distress
a positive association between abdominal pain at ages had increased odds of back pain (pooled OR [95% CI] = 2.2
11–14 years and back pain 1–3 years later (pooled RR [1.6, 3.1]; n = 7709; I2 = 0.00) (Fig. 3). Based on two cohort
[95% CI] = 1.7 [1.3, 2.2]; n = 4382; I2 = 0.00) (Fig. 2). studies, psychosocial difficulties were not associated with
increased risk of back pain (pooled RR [95% CI] = 1.4 [0.96,
1.97]; n = 1111; I2 = 28.56) (Fig. 3) [35, 36].
Emotional coping problems were associated with
increased odds of back pain (pooled OR [95% CI] = 1.4 [1.1,
1.8]; 3968; I2 = 0.00) based on two inception cohorts [43, 44]

13

Table 3  Results from included studies


488

Study Potential risk factor for back pain Results (95% CI) Overall findings/alternative presentation of results

13
Barke (2014) Dysfunctional coping OR 1.44 (1.06, 1.97) (boys) (adjusted for age) Positive association
OR 1.36 (0.68, 2.72) (girls) (adjusted for age) Boys: dysfunctional coping, anxiety sensitivity
Pain catastrophizing OR 1.07 (0.66, 1.72) (boys) Girls: somatosensory amplification
OR 0.51 (0.30, 0.88) (girls) (adjusted for age) Protective association
Girls: pain catastrophizing
Anxiety sensitivity OR 1.47 (1.09, 1.99) (boys) (adjusted for age)
No association
OR 0.86 (0.65, 1.14) (girls) (adjusted for age)
Boys: pain catastrophizing, somatosensory amplification.
Somatosensory amplification OR: 1.22 (0.91, 1.62) (boys) Girls: dysfunctional coping, anxiety sensitivity
OR: 1.78 (1.04, 3.05) (girls) (adjusted for age)
Brattberg (1994) Fear of schoolmates OR 2.44 (1.13, 5.27) Positive association to psychosocial features
Loneliness OR 3.64 (1.24, 11.09) Fear of schoolmates, loneliness, difficulties to make friends,
feelings of being an outsider, has been bullied, passive
Difficulties to make friends OR 2.11 (1.11, 4.01)
reactive to bullying, nervousness, difficulties verbalizing
Feelings of being an outsider OR 2.08 (1.14, 3.81) feelings
Has been bullied OR 2.11 (1.14, 3.60) No association
Passive reactive to bullying OR 3.39 (1.59, 7.30) Diffuse feelings of anger
Nervousness OR 2.10 (1.27, 3.47)
Difficulties verbalizing feelings OR 1.63 (1.01, 2.65)
Diffuse feelings of anger Not significant (results not reported)
Cheung (2010) Low mood OR 3.12 (1.31, 7.41) (adjusted age, gender, height) Positive association
Anxiety OR 4.61 (1.92, 11.08) (adjusted age, gender, height) Low mood, anxiety, feeling tense, under stress, fatigue,
constant tiredness, headaches
Feeling tense OR 3.97 (1.65, 9.55) (adjusted age, gender, height)
Under stress OR 4.52 (1.76, 11.62) (adjusted age, gender, height)
Fatigue OR 4.10 (1.55, 10.85) (adjusted age, gender, height)
Constant tiredness OR 5.09 (1.06, 24.43) (adjusted age, gender, height)
Headaches OR 4.52 (1.78, 11.51) (adjusted age, gender, height)
Coenen (2016) Mental-health-related quality of life Low pain and impact cluster as reference Higher back pain cluster lower score on mental-health-
High pain and impact cluster: β − 2.84 (− 5.60, − 0.07) related quality of life (poorer health-related quality of life)
(adjusted for sex)
Dunn (2011) Depression Higher back pain cluster high mean depression score Somatization and depression scores lowest in cluster 1,
Somatization Higher back pain cluster high mean somatization score the ‘no pain problem’ cluster. Largest improvements in
depression in back pain cluster 2: decreasing probability
of pain during follow-up
Feldman (2001) Mental health score OR 0.98 (0.96–1.00) No association Mental health score
Gill (2014) Externalizing and Internalizing OR 1.003 (0.995, 1.012) (LBP) No association
OR 1.020 (1.010, 1.030) (MBP) (adjusted for sex) LBP: Psychosocial variables
Depression OR 1.015 (0.989, 1.041) (LBP) Positive association
OR 1.051 (1.022, 1.081) (MBP) (adjusted for sex) MBP: Internalizing and externalizing score, Depression
index
Gustafsson (2018) Daytime sleepiness (F4 = 14.62, p < 0.0001) No association Daytime sleepiness and psychological
Psychological symptoms (F1 = 149.1, p < 0.0001) symptoms
European Spine Journal (2020) 29:480–496
Table 3  (continued)
Study Potential risk factor for back pain Results (95% CI) Overall findings/alternative presentation of results

Hestbaek (2006) Asthma OR 1.34 (1.10, 1.62) (adjusted for age and sex) Positive association
Asthma, headache
Atopic disease OR 1.08 (0.82, 1.42) (adjusted for age and sex) No association Atopic disease
Headaches OR 2.52 (1.56, 4.07) (adjusted for age and sex)
Jones (2009) Headaches RR 1.3 (0.5, 3.3) Positive association
Abdominal pain RR 1.5 (0.7, 3.6) High level of peer problems
No association
High level of psychosocial difficulties overall RR 1.1 (0.7, 1.9) (adjusted for age and sex)
Headaches, abdominal pain, high level of psychosocial dif-
High level of peer problems RR 2.3 (1.2, 4.2) (adjusted for age and sex) ficulties overall, emotional problems, conduct problems,
Emotional problems RR: 1.5 (0.9, 2.6) (adjusted for age and sex) hyperactivity, prosocial behaviour, and daytime tiredness
European Spine Journal (2020) 29:480–496

Conduct problems RR: 1.1 (0.7, 1.8) (adjusted for age and sex)
Hyperactivity RR 1.0 (0.6, 1.6) (adjusted for age and sex)
Daytime tiredness RR 1.1 (0.7, 1.9) (adjusted for age and sex)
Prosocial behaviour RR 1.5 (0.9, 2.4) (Adjusted for age and sex)
Jones (2003) Headaches RR 1.6 (0.97, 2.8) (adjusted for age and gender) Positive association
Abdominal pain RR 1.8 (1.1, 3.0) (adjusted for age and gender) Abdominal pain, high level of adverse psychosocial expo-
sure, conduct problems
High level of adverse psychosocial exposure RR: 1.6 (1.1, 2.3) (adjusted for age and sex)
No association
High level of peer problems RR 1.3 (0.9, 1.9) (adjusted for age and sex) Headaches, peer problems, emotional problems, hyperac-
Emotional problems RR: 1.2 (0.8, 1.8) (adjusted for age and sex) tivity, prosocial behaviour, and daytime tiredness
Conduct problems RR: 2.5 (1.7, 3.7) (adjusted for age and sex)
Hyperactivity RR 1.4 (0.98, 2.1) (adjusted for age and sex)
Prosocial behaviour RR 0.9 (0.6, 1.3) (adjusted for age and sex)
Kanchanomai (2015) Psychosocial factors Not significant (results not reported) No association
Psychosocial factors
Lien (2011) Mental distress OR 1.6 (1.1, 2.4) (boys) (adjusted for ethnicity, family Positive association
structure, socioeconomic) Mental distress
OR 1.5 (1.1, 1.9) (girls) (adjusted for ethnicity, family
structure, socioeconomic)
Mikkonen (2016) Externalizing behaviour RR 3.62 (1.54, 8.50) (girls), Girls: externalizing behaviour cluster associated signifi-
RR 1.12 (0.26, 4.76) (boys) cantly with ‘Consultation for LBP’ at 18 years. Boy: none
of the clusters associated with new LBP at 18 years

13
489

Table 3  (continued)
490

Study Potential risk factor for back pain Results (95% CI) Overall findings/alternative presentation of results

13
Mustard (2005) Chronic medical ­conditionsa OR 1.01 (0.56, 1.82) (adjusted for age, sex, childhood Positive association
conditions, childhood health status, and measures early Psychological distress, emotional or behavioural disorders
adult health, behaviour, socioeconomic status, work in childhood
environment) No association
Chronic medical conditions
Psychological distress (moderate/high) OR 1.85 (1.07, 3.20) (adjusted for conduct problems age,
sex, childhood conditions, childhood health status, and
measures early adult health, behaviour, socioeconomic
status, work environment)
Emotional or behavioural disorders in childhood OR 1.87 (1.02, 3.41) (adjusted for age, sex, childhood
conditions, childhood health status, and measures early
adult health, behaviour, socioeconomic status, work
environment)
Muthuri (2018) Serious illness 0–15 years RR 1.10 (0.79–1.52) Positive association
Abdominal pain RR 1.76 (1.28, 2.43) (adjusted for BMI, psychiatric Abdominal pain and conduct problems
disorders, education level, occupation, smoking status, No association
parental back pain, sex) Serious illness in childhood and emotional problems
Emotional problems RR 0.84 (0.56, 1.27)
Conduct problems RR 2.07 (1.28, 3.35) (adjusted for BMI, psychiatric
disorders, education level, occupation, smoking status,
parental back pain, sex)
Power (2001) Psychological distress high OR 2.52 (1.65, 3.86) (adjusted for sex, ergonomic stress, Positive association
and psychosocial work characteristics) Psychological distress
Smith (2017) Depression/Anxiety Not significant (results not reported) Positive association
Somatic complaints LBP with impact Vs no LBP: Somatic complaints, aggressive behaviour
OR 1.30 (1.10, 1.54) (adjusted for sex and baseline BP) No association
Depression/anxiety
Aggressive behaviour LBP with impact versus no LBP:
OR 1.37 (1.16, 1.62) (adjusted for sex and baseline BP)
Stanford (2008) Anxiety and/or depression Start-point intercept: β = 0.32, p < 0.001, end-point inter- Positive association
cept: β = 0.16, p < 0.001 Anxiety/depression and self-esteem with both the start- and
Self-esteem Start-point intercept: β = 0.22, p < 0.001, end-point inter- end-point trajectories
cept: β = 0.16, p < 0.01).

Italics No statistically significant difference


LBP low back pain, MBP mid-back pain, BP back pain, OR odds ratio (95% confidence intervals), β beta (95% confidence intervals), RR risk ratio (95% confidence intervals), BMI body mass
index
Chronic medical ­conditionsa: include asthma; heart problems; epilepsy or convulsions without fever; kidney disease; arthritis or rheumatism; cerebral palsy; diabetes; cancer; spina bifida; mus-
cular dystrophy or another muscle disease; cystic fibrosis; missing fingers, hands, arms, toes, feet, or legs; deformity of the feet, legs, fingers, arms, or back; club foot or cleft palate; paralysis or
weakness; blindness or chronic sight problems; deafness or chronic hearing problems; muteness or chronic speech problems; chronic pain or discomfort; or any other medical problem or condi-
tion
European Spine Journal (2020) 29:480–496
European Spine Journal (2020) 29:480–496 491

(Fig. 3) and (pooled OR [95% CI] = 1.4 [1.0, 1.8]; n = 3062; Fig. 3  Forest plot of psychological features as potential triggers or ◂

I2 = 86.99) based on four cohort studies (Fig. 3) [28, 29, 33, risk factors for back pain. a Psychological distress overall (incep-
tion cohorts), b psychological difficulties overall, c emotional coping
41]. There was no association between emotional coping problems (inception cohorts), d emotional coping problems (OR), e
problems and back pain (pooled RR [95% CI] = 1.0 [0.8, emotional coping problems (RR) [outcome/subgroup results com-
1.3]; n = 600; I2 = 31.45) (Fig. 3) based on data from four bined: studies reporting multiple outcomes or subgroups were first
other cohort studies [35, 36, 39, 40]. Having a high level of combined (through fixed effect models), accounting for lack of inde-
pendence within study]
‘peer problems’ was not associated with back pain (pooled
RR [95% CI] = 1.6 [0.9, 2.8]; n = 1111; I2 = 57.46) (Fig. 4)
[35, 36]. Conduct problems were associated with increased additional factors that are associated with back pain. Specifi-
risk of back pain (pooled RR [95% CI] = 1.8 [1.1, 2.9]; cally, asthma, headaches, abdominal pain, depression, anxi-
n = 4382; I2 = 72.08) (Fig. 4) [35, 36, 40]. Both studies that ety, conduct problems, somatization, and ‘feeling tense’ are
considered feeling ‘tense’ or ‘stressed’ or ‘nervous’ found potential risk factors or triggers for back pain. Results from
increased odds of back pain (pooled OR [95% CI] = 2.7 [1.4, the meta-analyses demonstrated the most likely risk factors
5.2]; n = 682; I2 = 42.74) (Fig. 4) [28, 29]. Hyperactivity was for back pain in young people are psychological distress
not associated with increased risk of back pain (pooled RR and emotional coping problems. Other factors identified in
[95% CI] = 1.2 [0.9,1.7]; n = 1111; 11.31) (Fig. 4) [35, 36]. the meta-analyses as potential risk factors or triggers are
headaches, abdominal pain, conduct problems, and ‘feeling
tense’.
Discussion The current results accord with related systematic
reviews. A previous systematic review and meta-analysis
Summary of evidence reported negative emotional symptoms to be a potential
risk factor for musculoskeletal pain during childhood and
This systematic review identified evidence from inception adolescence (pooled OR [95% CI] 1.54 (1.06, 2.24) [46].
cohort studies that demonstrated the most likely risk factors Another systematic review found good evidence that psy-
for back pain are psychological distress, as well as psycho- chological distress and psychosocial factors increase the risk
logical features including emotional coping problems and of back pain in children [3]. Regarding physical illnesses, a
somatosensory amplification. Evidence from non-inception systematic review and meta-analysis of cross-sectional twin
cohort studies cannot distinguish between potential risk fac- studies reported that young people and adults with chronic
tors or triggers for back pain. However, we identified several conditions such as asthma, diabetes, and headaches were

a Headaches
Study name Statistics for each study Risk ratio and 95% CI
Risk Lower Upper Relative
ratio limit limit Z-Value p-Value weight
Jones 2009 1.300 0.506 3.340 0.545 0.586 10.33
Jones 2003 1.600 0.942 2.718 1.738 0.082 32.72
Hestbaek 2006 2.280 1.526 3.407 4.021 0.000 56.95
1.916 1.415 2.595 4.204 0.000
2 0.1 0.2 0.5 1 2 5 10
N=7665 I =0.00 Negative Association Positive Association

Hestbaek 2006: odds ratio converted to risk ratio


b Abdominal pain
Statistics for each study Risk ratio and 95% CI
Risk Lower Upper Relative R
ratio limit limit Z-Value p-Value weight
Jones 2009 1.500 0.661 3.402 0.971 0.332 9.81
Jones 2003 1.800 1.090 2.973 2.296 0.022 26.14
Muthuri 2018 1.760 1.277 2.425 3.457 0.001 64.04
1.743 1.349 2.252 4.245 0.000
2
0.1 0.2 0.5 1 2 5 10
N= 4382 I =0.00 Negative Association Positive Association

Fig. 2  Forest plots of physical illnesses as potential triggers or risk factors for back pain. a Headaches and b abdominal pain

13

492 European Spine Journal (2020) 29:480–496

a Psychological distress overall (inception cohorts)


Study name Statistics for each study Odds ratio and 95% CI
Odds Lower Upper Relative R
ratio limit limit Z-Value p-Value weight
Mustard 2005 1.850 1.101 3.108 2.324 0.020 40.15
Power 2001 2.520 1.648 3.854 4.263 0.000 59.85
2.226 1.602 3.092 4.771 0.000
0.1 0.2 0.5 1 2 5 10
2
N= 7709 I =0.00 Negative Association Positive Association

b Psychological difficulties overall


Study name Statistics for each study Risk ratio and 95% CI
Risk Lower Upper Relative
ratio limit limit Z-Value p-Value weight
Jones 2009 1.100 0.668 1.812 0.374 0.708 39.50
Jones 2003 1.600 1.107 2.314 2.498 0.012 60.50
1.380 0.964 1.976 1.758 0.079
0.1 0.2 0.5 1 2 5 10
2
N= 1111 I =28.56 Negative Association Positive Association

c Emotional coping problems (inception cohorts)


Study name Statistics for each study Odds ratio and 95% CI
Odds Lower Upper Relative R
ratio limit limit Z-Valuep-Value weight
Barke 2014 1.351 1.037 1.760 2.229 0.026 83.89
Mustard 2005 1.870 1.023 3.419 2.033 0.042 16.11
1.424 1.117 1.814 2.858 0.004
0.1 0.2 0.5 1 2 5 10
2
N= 3968 I =0.00 Negative Association Positive Association

Outcome and subgroup results combined: Barke 2014

d Emotional coping problems


Study name Statistics for each study Odds ratio and 95% CI

Odds Lower Upper Relative R


ratio limit limit Z-Valuep-Value weight
Brattberg 1994 1.630 1.006 2.640 1.985 0.047 19.24
Cheung 2010 3.120 1.312 7.420 2.574 0.010 9.20
Gill 2014 1.010 1.004 1.017 3.032 0.002 37.72
Smith 2017 1.370 1.159 1.619 3.695 0.000 33.83
1.362 1.007 1.843 2.004 0.045
N= 3062 I =86.99
2 0.1 0.2 0.5 1 2 5 10
Negative Association Positive Association

Subgroup results combined: Gill 2014

e Emotional coping problems


Study name Statistics for each study Risk ratio and 95% CI
Risk Lower Upper Relative
ratio limit limit Z-Value p-Value weight
Jones 2003 1.200 0.800 1.800 0.881 0.378 27.35
Jones 2009 1.500 0.883 2.550 1.498 0.134 18.64
Mikkonen 2016 0.840 0.558 1.265 -0.835 0.404 27.01
Muthuri 2018 0.840 0.558 1.265 -0.835 0.404 27.00
1.032 0.794 1.340 0.234 0.815
2 0.1 0.2 0.5 1 2 5 10
N= 600 I =31.45 Negative Association Positive Association

Subgroup results combined: Mikkonen 2016

13
European Spine Journal (2020) 29:480–496 493

f High level of peer problems

Study name Statistics for each study Risk ratio and 95% CI
Risk Lower Upper Relative
ratio limit limit Z-Value p-Value weight
Jones 2009 2.300 1.229 4.303 2.606 0.009 39.89
Jones 2003 1.300 0.895 1.889 1.376 0.169 60.11
1.632 0.944 2.822 1.754 0.079
0.1 0.2 0.5 1 2 5 10
2 Negative Association Positive Association
N= 1111 I =57.46

g Conduct problems

Study name Statistics for each study Risk ratio and 95% CI
Risk Lower Upper Relative R
ratio limit limit Z-Valuep-Value weight
Jones 2009 1.100 0.686 1.764 0.396 0.692 32.29
Jones 2003 2.500 1.695 3.688 4.618 0.000 35.78
Muthuri 2018 2.070 1.280 3.349 2.964 0.003 31.93
1.806 1.109 2.940 2.376 0.018
2 0.1 0.2 0.5 1 2 5 10
N= 4382 I =72.08 Negative Association Positive Association

h Tense or stressed or nervousness

Study name Statistics for each study Odds ratio and 95% CI

Odds Lower Upper Relative


ratio limit limit Z-Value p-Value weight
Brattberg 1994 2.100 1.270 3.471 2.893 0.004 65.28
Cheung 2010 4.236 1.703 10.537 3.105 0.002 34.72
2.679 1.392 5.157 2.950 0.003
0.1 0.2 0.5 1 2 5 10
2 Negative Association Positive Association
N= 683 I =42.74

Outcome results combined: Cheung 2010

i Hyperactivity

Study name Statistics for each study Risk ratio and 95% CI
Risk Lower Upper Relative
ratio limit limit Z-Value p-Value weight
Jones 2009 1.000 0.612 1.633 0.000 1.000 39.04
Jones 2003 1.400 0.956 2.049 1.731 0.084 60.96
1.228 0.890 1.694 1.249 0.211
0.1 0.2 0.5 1 2 5 10
2
N= 1111 I =11.31 Negative Association Positive Association

Fig. 4  Forest plot of psychological features as a potential predictor of subgroup results combined: studies reporting multiple outcomes
back pain continued. f High level of peer problems, g conduct prob- or subgroups were first combined (through fixed effect models),
lems, h tense or stressed or nervousness, i hyperactivity [outcome/ accounting for the lack of independence within study]

13

494 European Spine Journal (2020) 29:480–496

more likely to report low back pain (pooled OR range = 1.6 Conclusion


to 4.2) [7].
The most likely risk factors for back pain are psychological
Methodological considerations for included studies distress, as well as psychological features including emo-
tional coping problems and somatosensory amplification.
The majority of the included studies were not inception Due to the limitations of the literature, the role of some
cohorts, meaning that back pain and potential risk factor physical illnesses, mental health disorders, and psychologi-
data were collected concurrently, making the temporal rela- cal features as potential comorbidities, triggers, or risk fac-
tionship difficult to establish [13]. Many studies reported tors for back pain remains unclear. Additional high-quality
high attrition rates, and reasons for loss to follow-up were research is needed to better elucidate these relationships.
often unreported. Other common limitations were that the
source population and/or the selection criteria were not Acknowledgements  We would like to thank Cody Davenport for his
well described. The outcome measurement (back pain) was assistance with the study screening process.
often unclear. Many studies did not specify the temporal
Authors’ contribution  AB, JH, and BW were involved with the concept
nature of the back pain (ongoing, episodic, or first time). and design of the study. LB conducted the searches. AB and CH con-
While some studies applied a clear identification of the ducted study selection and data extraction. AB analysed and interpreted
area of the back under study, such as through the use of the data with the assistance of BW, JH, and CH. AB drafted the manu-
pain diagrams, many other studies did not. We intended script and performed revisions with substantial feedback and editing
from all authors. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
to convert odds ratios to risk ratios; however, this was not
always possible owing to insufficient reporting (e.g. lack Funding  This study was funded by a scholarship from Murdoch
of information on back pain incidence or prevalence in the University, Western Australia and funding provided by Chiropractic
non-exposed participants). Australia Research Foundation. JH receives salary support from the
Canadian Chiropractic Research Foundation and the New Brunswick
Health Research Foundation. The funding sources had no involvement
in study design, analysis, interpretation, or manuscript preparation.
Methodological considerations for this review
Data availability  The data sets generated during and/or analysed dur-
There are no established risk of bias tools for risk factor ing the current study are available from the corresponding author on
reasonable request.
studies. We assessed risk of bias with a modified QUIPS
tool that was originally designed for studies of prognosis.
Compliance with ethical standards 
Due to methodological heterogeneity and that there were
many different potential risk factors studied, few studies Conflict of interest  The authors declare that they have no conflict of
could be included together in the respective meta-analy- interest.
ses. Therefore, the syntheses are based on small numbers
of studies and this lends itself to problems estimating
between-studies variance [22]. Future studies are likely to
change these estimates. Furthermore, we were unable to References
undertake sensitivity analyses or consider study subgroups
(e.g. low risk of bias studies). Three meta-analyses demon- 1. Global Burden of Disease II, Prevalence Collaborators (2018)
strated substantial-to-considerable statistical heterogeneity. Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years
lived with disability for 354 diseases and injuries for 195 countries
and territories, 1990–2017: a systematic analysis for the Global
Burden of Disease Study 2017. Lancet 392(10159):1789–1858
Recommendations for future research 2. Hoy D, Bain C, Williams G, March L, Brooks P, Blyth F, Woolf
A, Vos T, Buchbinder R (2012) A systematic review of the global
prevalence of low back pain. Arthritis Rheum 64(6):2028–2037.
Additional research is needed to understand the etiology of https​://doi.org/10.1002/art.34347​
back pain. Future studies should better delineate between 3. Kamper SJ, Yamato TP, Williams CM (2016) The prevalence,
risk factors responsible for incident back pain cases and risk factors, prognosis and treatment for back pain in children
and adolescents: an overview of systematic reviews. Best Pract
other factors that may be triggers for back pain episodes. Res Clin Rheumatol 30(6):1021–1036. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Future research should also apply validated questionnaires berh.2017.04.003
and clearly identify and define the type of back pain under 4. Hartvigsen J, Hancock MJ, Kongsted A, Louw Q, Ferreira ML,
study (e.g. ongoing, episodic, or first time). Genevay S, Hoy D, Karppinen J, Pransky G, Sieper J (2018)
What low back pain is and why we need to pay attention. Lancet
391(10137):2356–2367

13
European Spine Journal (2020) 29:480–496 495

5. Hestbaek L, Leboeuf-Yde C, Kyvik KO (2006) Is comorbidity in 24. Grant RL (2014) Converting an odds ratio to a range of plausible
adolescence a predictor for adult low back pain? A prospective relative risks for better communication of research findings. BMJ
study of a young population. BMC Musculoskelet Disord 7(1):29 348:f7450
6. Hurwitz EL, Morgenstern H (1999) Cross-sectional associations 25. Higgins JP, Green S (2008) Cochrane handbook for systematic
of asthma, hay fever, and other allergies with major depression reviews of interventions. Wiley, Hoboken
and low-back pain among adults aged 20–39 years in the United 26. Higgins JP (2008) Commentary: heterogeneity in meta-analysis
States. Am J Epidemiol 150(10):1107–1116 should be expected and appropriately quantified. Int J Epidemiol
7. Ferreira PH, Beckenkamp P, Maher CG, Hopper JL, Ferreira 37(5):1158–1160
ML (2013) Nature or nurture in low back pain? Results of a 27. Rücker G, Schwarzer G, Carpenter JR, Schumacher M (2008)
systematic review of studies based on twin samples. Eur J Pain Undue reliance on I 2 in assessing heterogeneity may mislead.
17(7):957–971 BMC Med Res Methodol 8(1):79
8. Holmberg S, Thelin A, Stiernstrom E, Svardsudd K (2005) Low 28. Brattberg G (1994) The incidence of back pain and head-
back pain comorbidity among male farmers and rural referents: a ache among Swedish school children. Qual Life Res 3(Suppl
population-based study. Ann Agric Environ Med 12(2):261–268 1):S27–S31
9. Smith MD, Russell A, Hodges PW (2009) Do incontinence, 29. Cheung K (2010) The incidence of low back problems among
breathing difficulties, and gastrointestinal symptoms increase nursing students in Hong Kong. J Clin Nurs 19(15–16):2355–2362
the risk of future back pain? J Pain 10(8):876–886. https​://doi. 30. Coenen P, Smith A, Paananen M, O’Sullivan P, Beales D, Straker
org/10.1016/j.jpain​.2009.03.003 L (2017) Trajectories of low back pain from adolescence to young
10. Ha IH, Lee J, Kim MR, Kim H, Shin JS (2014) The association adulthood. Arthr Care Res 69(3):403–412
between the history of cardiovascular diseases and chronic low 31. Dunn KM, Jordan KP, Mancl L, Drangsholt MT, Le Resche L
back pain in South Koreans: a cross-sectional study. PLoS ONE (2011) Trajectories of pain in adolescents: a prospective cohort
9(4):e93671. https​://doi.org/10.1371/journ​al.pone.00936​71 study. Pain 152(1):66–73
11. Currie SR, Wang J (2005) More data on major depression as 32. Feldman DE, Shrier I, Rossignol M, Abenhaim L (2001) Risk
an antecedent risk factor for first onset of chronic back pain. factors for the development of low back pain in adolescence. Am
Psychol Med 35(9):1275–1282 J Epidemiol 154(1):30–36
12. Porta M (2014) A dictionary of epidemiology. Oxford Univer- 33. Gill DK, Davis MC, Smith AJ, Straker LM (2014) Bidirectional
sity Press, Oxford relationships between cigarette use and spinal pain in adolescents
13. Ardakani EM, Leboeuf-Yde C, Walker BF (2018) Failure to accounting for psychosocial functioning. Br J Health Psychol
define low back pain as a disease or an episode renders research 19(1):113–131. https​://doi.org/10.1111/bjhp.12039​
on causality unsuitable: results of a systematic review. Chi- 34. Gustafsson M-L, Laaksonen C, Aromaa M, Löyttyniemi E, Sal-
ropr Man Therap 26(1):1. https ​ : //doi.org/10.1186/s1299​ anterä S (2018) The prevalence of neck-shoulder pain, back pain
8-017-0172-9 and psychological symptoms in association with daytime sleepi-
14. Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, Mulrow C, Gøtzsche PC, Ioan- ness–a prospective follow-up study of school children aged 10 to
nidis JP, Clarke M, Devereaux PJ, Kleijnen J, Moher D (2009) 15. Scand J Pain 18(3):389–397
The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and 35. Jones GT, Macfarlane GJ (2009) Predicting persistent low back
meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: pain in schoolchildren: a prospective cohort study. Arthr Rheum
explanation and elaboration. PLoS Med 6(7):e1000100 61(10):1359–1366. https​://doi.org/10.1002/art.24696​
15. Stroup DF, Berlin JA, Morton SC, Olkin I, Williamson GD, Ren- 36. Jones GT, Watson KD, Silman AJ, Symmons DP, Macfarlane GJ
nie D, Moher D, Becker BJ, Sipe TA, Thacker SB (2000) Meta- (2003) Predictors of low back pain in British schoolchildren: a
analysis of observational studies in epidemiology: a proposal for population-based prospective cohort study. Pediatrics 111(4 Pt
reporting. JAMA 283(15):2008–2012 1):822–828
16. Wilczynski NL, Haynes RB (2003) Developing optimal search 37. Kanchanomai S, Janwantanakul P, Pensri P, Jiamjarasrangsi W
strategies for detecting clinically sound causation studies in MED- (2015) A prospective study of incidence and risk factors for the
LINE. In: Proceedings of the AMIA symposium, pp 719–723 onset and persistence of low back pain in Thai university stu-
17. Leclercq E, Leeflang MM, van Dalen EC, Kremer LC (2013) Vali- dents. Asia Pac J Public Health 27(2):Np106–115. https​://doi.
dation of search filters for identifying pediatric studies in PubMed. org/10.1177/10105​39511​42757​9
J Pediatr 162(3):629–634.e622 38. Lien L, Green K, Thoresen M, Bjertness E (2011) Pain complaints
18. Scottish Intercollegiate Guidelines Unit SIGN Observational Stud- as risk factor for mental distress: a three-year follow-up study. Eur
ies filter. https​://www.sign.ac.uk/searc​h-filte​rs.html. Accessed 28 Child Adolesc Psychiatry 20(10):509
June 2018 39. Mikkonen P, Heikkala E, Paananen M, Remes J, Taimela S,
19. Moons KG, de Groot JA, Bouwmeester W, Vergouwe Y, Mallett Auvinen J, Karppinen J (2016) Accumulation of psychosocial
S, Altman DG, Reitsma JB, Collins GS (2014) Critical appraisal and lifestyle factors and risk of low back pain in adolescence: a
and data extraction for systematic reviews of prediction modelling cohort study. Eur Spine J 25(2):635–642. https​://doi.org/10.1007/
studies: the CHARMS checklist. PLoS Med 11(10):e1001744 s0058​6-015-4065-0
20. Hayden JA, van der Windt DA, Cartwright JL, Côté P, Bombardier 40. Muthuri SG, Kuh D, Cooper R (2018) Longitudinal profiles of
C (2013) Assessing bias in studies of prognostic factors. Ann back pain across adulthood and their relationship with child-
Intern Med 158(4):280–286 hood factors: evidence from the 1946 British birth cohort. Pain
21. Hayden JA, Côté P, Bombardier C (2006) Evaluation of the qual- 159(4):764
ity of prognosis studies in systematic reviews. Ann Intern Med 41. Smith A, Beales D, O’Sullivan P, Bear N, Straker L (2017) Low
144(6):427–437 back pain with impact at 17 years of age is predicted by early
22. Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JP, Rothstein HR (2011) Intro- adolescent risk factors from multiple domains: analysis of the
duction to meta-analysis. Wiley, Hoboken Western Australian Pregnancy Cohort (Raine) Study. J Orthop
23. Zhang J, Kai FY (1998) What’s the relative risk? A method of Sports Phys Ther 47(10):752–762. https​://doi.org/10.2519/jospt​
correcting the odds ratio in cohort studies of common outcomes. .2017.7464
JAMA 280(19):1690–1691

13

496 European Spine Journal (2020) 29:480–496

42. Stanford EA, Chambers CT, Biesanz JC, Chen E (2008) The fre- 45. Power C, Frank J, Hertzman C, Schierhout G, Li L (2001) Predic-
quency, trajectories and predictors of adolescent recurrent pain: tors of low back pain onset in a prospective British study. Am J
a population-based approach. Pain 138(1):11–21 Public Health 91(10):1671–1678
43. Barke A, Gaßmann J, Kröner-Herwig B (2014) Cognitive process- 46. Huguet A, Tougas ME, Hayden J, McGrath PJ, Stinson JN, Cham-
ing styles of children and adolescents with headache and back bers CT (2016) Systematic review with meta-analysis of child-
pain: a longitudinal epidemiological study. J Pain Res 7:405 hood and adolescent risk and prognostic factors for musculoskel-
44. Mustard CA, Kalcevich C, Frank JW, Boyle M (2005) Childhood etal pain. Pain 157(12):2640–2656
and early adult predictors of risk of incident back pain: Ontario
Child Health Study 2001 follow-up. Am J Epidemiol 162(8):779– Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
786. https​://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwi27​1 jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Affiliations

Amber M. Beynon1   · Jeffrey J. Hebert1,2 · Christopher J. Hodgetts1 · Leah M. Boulos3 · Bruce F. Walker1

2
* Amber M. Beynon Faculty of Kinesiology, University of New Brunswick, 3
amber.beynon@murdoch.edu.au Bailey Drive, Fredericton, NB E3B 5A3, Canada
3
1 Maritime SPOR SUPPORT Unit, 5790 University Avenue,
College of Science, Health, Engineering and Education,
Halifax, NS B3H 1V7, Canada
Murdoch University, 90 South Street, Murdoch, WA 6150,
Australia

13
Reproduced with permission of copyright owner. Further reproduction
prohibited without permission.

You might also like