You are on page 1of 20

Health & Place 46 (2017) 155–174

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Health & Place


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/healthplace

Review Essay

A review of neighborhood effects and early child development: How, where, MARK
and for whom, do neighborhoods matter?

Anita Minha, , Nazeem Muhajarineb, Magdalena Janusc, Marni Brownelld, Martin Guhna
a
Human Early Learning Partnership, School of Population and Public Health, 2206 E Mall, Vancouver, BC, Canada V6T 1Z3
b
Saskatchewan Population Health and Evaluation Research Unit, University of Saskatchewan, 3333 E-Wing, Health Sciences Building, 104 Clinic Place,
Saskatoon, SK, Canada S7N 2Z4
c
The Offord Centre for Child Studies, McMaster University, 1280 Main Street West, Hamilton, ON, Canada L8S 4K1
d
Manitoba Centre for Health Policy, Community Health Sciences, Max Rady College of Medicine, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Manitoba, 408-
727 McDermot Ave., Winnipeg, MB, Canada R3E 3P5

A R T I C L E I N F O A BS T RAC T

Keywords: This paper describes a scoping review of 42 studies of neighborhood effects on developmental health for
Early child development children ages 0–6, published between 2009 and 2014. It focuses on three themes: (1) theoretical mechanisms
Neighborhood effects that drive early childhood development, i.e. how neighborhoods matter for early childhood development; (2)
Literature review dependence of such mechanisms on place-based characteristics i.e. where neighborhood effects occur; (3)
Mediation
dependence of such mechanisms on child characteristics, i.e. for whom is development most affected. Given that
Moderation
ecological systems theories postulate diverse mechanisms via which neighborhood characteristics affect early
child development, we specifically examine evidence on mediation and/or moderation effects. We conclude by
discussing future challenges, and proposing recommendations for analyses that utilize ecological longitudinal
population-based databases.

1. Introduction systematically test sophisticated hypotheses about the contextual


influences on human development (Brownell et al., 2016; Nickel
The 1990s saw the emergence of a series of empirical papers et al., 2014; Jutte et al., 2011; Roos et al., 2010; Mountain et al., 2016).
demonstrating that the neighborhoods in which children live, play, and Despite these significant developments, previous reviews
go to school, particularly those rife with poverty, matter to their health (cf. Sampson et al. (2002), van Vuuren et al. (2014)) have criticized
and wellbeing. Important theoretical contributions such as W.J. the existing research for having limited capacity to inform effective
Wilson's The Truly Disadvantaged (Wilson, 1987) and policies and interventions. Sampson et al. (2002) argued that confusion
Bronfenbrenner's social ecological model of human development over the direction of causation due to selection bias (the ability for
(Bronfenbrenner, 1979) foreshadowed this research. These scholars people to self-select into neighborhoods) is one of the more important
postulated a concept that was relatively new at the time — that the methodological limitations for interpreting statistical associations
neighborhood context, in conjunction with other factors, such as child between neighborhood factors and health. Broader criticisms of the
characteristics, parenting, and family resources, matters to the well- literature harken back to Jencks and Mayer (1990) argument that
being of developing children. neighborhood research is a “black-box” (Galster, 2012, van Ham and
Now well into the third decade of this research, there is strong Manley, 2012), showing that neighborhoods are important, but reveal-
evidence to suggest that the social, economic, cultural, and built ing little about the processes that produce inequalities (Jackson et al.,
characteristics of children's neighborhoods lay down important, some- 2009). From an empirical standpoint, this gap may be understood as a
times life-long foundations for their development. Research in this field dearth of evidence about underlying processes, mechanisms, or path-
has simultaneously benefitted from methodological and technological ways through which neighborhoods may affect children's developmen-
advances in the last few years. The development of infrastructures for tal health (Sharkey and Faber, 2014).
population-based longitudinal data capture and linkage, geospatial To advance a research agenda that will fill existing gaps in the
mapping, and statistical software to test complex multilevel and literature and avoid the pitfalls of past neighborhood effects studies, we
longitudinal mediation and moderation models make it possible to pause to review this burgeoning literature. We focus on what is known


Corresponding author.
E-mail address: anita.minh@gmail.com (A. Minh).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.healthplace.2017.04.012
Received 2 November 2016; Received in revised form 18 March 2017; Accepted 27 April 2017
Available online 18 May 2017
1353-8292/ © 2017 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/BY-NC-ND/4.0/).
A. Minh et al. Health & Place 46 (2017) 155–174

(i.e. where we are) and especially on what we need to know to advance their developmental health is made in at least three bodies of theory.
this research further (i.e. where do we go from here). This paper begins First, Urie Bronfenbrenner's bio-ecological, or person-process-context-
with an overview of the theoretical frameworks underpinning the time framework for human development (Bronfenbrenner and Morris,
literature on neigbourhoods and their effects on developmental health. 2007; Bronfenbrenner, 1979, Bronfenbrenner, 1999), suggests that
Next, using scoping review methodology (Arksey and O'Malley, 2005), developmental change must be understood as the product of the
we synthesize recent empirical evidence of this relationship along three sustained interaction between a child, meaning their characteristics,
dimensions: ‘how’ (the structural and mediating pathways), ‘where’, and the features of their immediate and distal environments over time.
and ‘for whom’ (the neighborhood- and child-level moderating factors The person characteristics refer to the developmental, biological, and
respectively). Given the empirical and technological progress in this psychological aspects of the developing child. Processes that lead to
field over the last few decades, we argue that it is not only possible but developmental change may include parent-child or child-child activ-
also imperative for future research to examine these questions using ities, group or solitary play, reading, learning new skills, studying,
mediation and moderation analyses. In our discussion, we describe athletic activities and performing complex tasks. The environments
how a Canadian intersectoral coalition of researchers and governments that constitute the bio-ecological systems framework refer to a series of
aims to meet this challenge with a national databank that monitors nested contexts ranging from intimate to distal, or what
early child development and neighborhood characteristics (Guhn et al., Bronfenbrenner refers to as “micro” to “macro”. The intimate contexts
2016). We close by drawing from the evidence summarized in this refer to the child's family, childcare facility, and neighborhood of
paper to make recommendations for future research in the neighbor- residence – those within which the processes leading to developmental
hood effects on child development at this important juncture in the change play out constantly through direct and reciprocal interactions
field. between a child and objects or other people. The more distal contexts
include interactions that occur between systems or environments –
1.1. Defining developmental health in early childhood some which the child interacts with directly (e.g., the family and the
school) and others that indirectly influences their developmental health
The early years of a child's life – the time between birth and age 5 – (e.g., local or national policies). For example, policies about childcare
represent sensitive periods for development (Knudsen, 2004). The provision may influence childcare availability and quality, which in
developing brain is influenced from the time the fetus is in utero and it turn determines the social and environmental stimuli that children
continues to change through neuro-synaptic pruning over the first encounter. In our view, the bio-ecological approach offers a useful
months and years of a child's life (Aylward, 1997; Levitt, 2003; Monk framework to conceptualize a wide array of contexts that may influence
et al., 2001). Neuropsychological research suggests that rapid growth of children's developmental health. Given its breadth, however, it is
children's brains during this time makes them particularly susceptible difficult to operationalize and test it as a whole. Instead, as
to environmental stimuli both positive and detrimental to development Bronfenbrenner (1999) has suggested, the research literature is rich
(Fox et al., 1994; Ursache and Noble, 2016; Noble et al., 2015). with “latent paradigms” or frameworks that implicitly offer empirical
Through a process called biological embedding, social and environ- support for the model.
mental experiences in a child's early years are theorized to shape A second theoretical body of literature casts a sociological lens on
physiological changes that have lifelong protective or detrimental the relationship between children's early development and the effects
effects on children's learning, behavior, health and wellbeing (Boyce of poverty in their residential environments. Research of this kind may
and Ellis, 2005; Hertzman and Boyce, 2010; Vimpani, 2000; Mitchell be traced to work from the US in the 1980s that began in response to
et al., 2014). Recent advances in neuroscience, allowing for direct concerns about a growing underclass of people living in ghettos. Poor
measurement of the brain, confirm the strong association between neighborhoods were perceived as disadvantages to their residents,
poverty and brain growth in the first three years, resulting in isolating them from opportunities (Wilson, 1987). This perspective
differences in gray matter volume between children in families of suggests that neighborhoods may indicate place-based inequalities in
varying socioeconomic status (Hanson et al., 2013). the distribution of social and environmental risks and opportunities for
The concept that children's developmental health is both an developmental health. In a recent review of the neighborhood effects on
outcome of their early experiences and a predictor of future life success health literature, Galster (2012) mapped out the social, environmental,
has profound implications for its measurement. While definitions of geographic, and institutional mechanisms that are theorized to link
developmental health in early childhood have historically focused on neighborhoods to individual health. In neighborhoods where residents
academic and cognitive qualities such as literacy and numeracy (Kagan, work towards common goals (Sampson et al., 1999, 2002), or in areas
1999, 1992), contemporary conceptions extend this definition to where people share similar behavioral and attitudinal norms (Brooks-
include physical changes (including health-related factors) as well as Gunn et al., 1993; Kohen et al., 1998), residents may be more likely to
social and emotional factors that are related to early life (Heim et al., share resources, and to monitor and support their local children
2004; Bartley et al., 1994; Stern et al., 2000; Wadsworth and Kuh, (Froiland et al., 2014; Leventhal and Brooks-Gunn, 2000; Sampson
1997; Scott-Little et al., 2006; Shonkoff and Phillips, 2000). This et al., 1999). Contact among residents may mean that children are
broadened conception is accompanied by a growing interest in more likely to share behaviors or attitudes towards problem behavior
measuring developmental health in association with communities (Froiland et al., 2014; Leventhal and Brooks-Gunn, 2000). Parents’
where children live. Love et al. (1994), for example, proposed a perceptions of their neighborhoods may also be influenced by their
methodology for assessing children's development in a way that is social environment, which may in turn affect the parenting strategies
sensitive to the influence of local, cultural, and social issues. Where that they use within their homes (Roosa et al., 2003). There may be
concepts of developmental health have increasingly focused on under- damaging developmental consequences as a result of exposure to
standing the complex interactions between properties of individuals environmental hazards, such as lead in soil and paint; asthma-inducing
and systems, however, new challenges have emerged with regards to air pollutants may be more prevalent among disadvantaged areas
measuring and modeling these concepts, which we discuss later in this (Earls and Buka, 2000; Litt et al., 2002; Ash and Fetter, 2004). Some
paper. neighborhoods may be located within political jurisdictions that have
fewer resources for childhood or family services, or have operational
1.2. Situating the neighborhood effects literature in the bio-ecological challenges which lead to services that are inferior in quality (Galster,
model 2012). Finally, vital societal resources (e.g., schools, healthcare, social
services, etc.) in neighborhoods may employ people who do not
The claim that a child's place of residence makes a difference to necessarily live in the neighborhood but who may be role-models for

156
A. Minh et al. Health & Place 46 (2017) 155–174

Fig. 1. Framework for interrogating neighborhood effects on early childhood development: How, Where, and for Whom?

children (Brooks-Gunn et al., 1993). This strand of work is significant effects on health offers little that may be translated to policy action
because it not only describes how neighborhoods come to differ from (Galster, 2012, van Vuuren et al., 2014). Neighborhood poverty (or
one another, but also operationalizes the importance of specific socioeconomic status) and its association with developmental health
characteristics on health and development relative to other influences. continue to be the most studied neighborhood level characteristics
Third, social epidemiologists have theorized about structural (Jackson et al., 2009). Statistical analyses of the association between
determinants of health, providing a framework for understanding the neighborhood poverty and health involve multilevel modeling to
pervasive influence of context in shaping individual wellbeing, includ- disentangle neighborhood impact from those of individual character-
ing how it may vary by neighborhood. This framework emerged from istics on health. However, these models have been criticized for
theories about the social causation of health which gained traction in oversimplifying contextual effects by estimating the residual left over
the 1990s, suggesting that socioeconomic status, social networks, and when multilevel regression methods have supposedly eliminated
power divisions, are the “fundamental causes” of disease (Link and compositional effects (Tunstall et al., 2004; Oakes, 2006). Macintyre
Phelan, 1995). The structural determinants of health are the mechan- et al. (2002) refer to this problem as a false dichotomy between context
isms – arising from the arrangement of labour markets, collective and composition. The conceptual interplay between person and their
beliefs, government policies – that “generate stratification and social places that is central to the bio-ecological model of development is
class divisions in society, and that define individual socioeconomic artificially separated in these statistical models, leading to confusion
position within hierarchies of power, prestige, and access to resources” about the direction of causation and of the mechanisms involved.
(Solar and Irwin, 2010). They are theorized to influence developmental Statistical analyses using theoretical frameworks that adequately
health in early childhood because in part, they drive the differential account for the complex interplay between person and place, especially
distribution of factors that promote or undermine development (e.g., the bio-ecological framework, are markedly absent from the literature
social resources, institutional resources, exposure to environmental on developmental health. A recent review found that fewer than one
risks) across geographies (Solar and Irwin, 2010). For example, local third of studies on the neighborhood effects on children ages 0–18,
zoning laws may determine how opportunities for healthy food choices published between 2003 and 2013, provided a theoretical rationale to
or green space are distributed between and within neighborhoods explain their statistical model or even why the neighborhood socio-
(Chum, 2011). The political debates surrounding gentrification and economic indicators that they used were chosen (van Vuuren et al.,
mixed-income development are other examples of the social hierar- 2014). Sharkey and Faber (2014) express this problem as an imbalance
chies created at a level upstream of the neighborhood, which may come between the large number of studies whose aims are to test whether
into play at a neighborhood level to promote or mitigate developmental neighborhood factors are associated with health outcomes and the
inequalities (Berube, 2010; Darcy, 2010; Joseph, 2010). To date, relatively fewer number that specifically examine why (or how), where,
however, the majority of evidence in the social epidemiological and for whom these matter.
literature concerns the impacts of inequality in neighborhood condi- The recent emergence of population-level databases linking data on
tions, income, and social position without addressing the structures place and children's developmental outcomes is a signal that future
that create hierarchies in the first place. Amidst this growing chorus of research may be able to shift towards an agenda that prioritizes policy-
calls for social epidemiology to pay greater attention to political and friendly questions about how, where, and for whom neighborhoods
economic systems that produce and distributed wealth and opportu- matter, rather than dwell on the question of whether they do
nity, there have also been appeals for more policy-relevant evidence (Mountain et al., 2016; Guhn et al., 2016). From a methodological
linking neighborhood inequalities in developmental health to their perspective, we argue that these questions translate into empirical
“macrosocial” determinants (Shankardass and Dunn, 2012, Blas et al., examination of (i) structural and mediating mechanisms of effect; and
2008, Putnam and Galea, 2008, O'Campo, 2012). (ii) two types of moderating effects: i.e., first, involving the (neighbor-
hood) context, and second, child characteristics, which may moderate
the degree to which structural pathways and neighborhood mechan-
2. Gaps in understanding neighborhood effects isms affect children's developmental health (Fig. 1; Wu and Zumbo,
2008). We thus review current research on the structural determinants,
Despite the many theories about how neighborhoods may affect mediators, and moderators of the relationship between neighborhoods
developmental health, the empirical evidence on the neighborhood

157
A. Minh et al. Health & Place 46 (2017) 155–174

and developmental health, to summarize current evidence on how, raised in the introduction as our organizing framework for integrating
where, and for whom neighborhoods matter in early childhood, and to the findings conceptually. Specifically, we summarized (i) in what ways
suggest gaps that future research may fill. did studies address ‘how’ neighborhood characteristics are related to
developmental outcomes (that is, via which structural and/or mediat-
3. Methodology ing neighborhood processes); (ii) in what ways did studies examine
whether the association between structural and/or mediating processes
3.1. Search strategy and child development outcomes were moderated by place or setting,
i.e. ‘where’ (that is, in which contexts), or by child characteristics i.e.
We conducted a scoping review using the methodology proposed by ‘for whom’ (that is, for which children).
Arksey and O'Malley (2005). Unlike with systematic reviews, authors of
scoping reviews will not typically assess the quality of included studies; 4. Results
scoping reviews also differ from narrative or literature reviews in that
authors will analyze and reinterpret the literature (Levac et al., 2010). 4.1. Descriptive summary of studies
Because recent reviews have already provided comprehensive accounts
of the literature (van Vuuren et al., 2014, Leventhal and Brooks-Gunn, Forty-two research articles were identified from our search. Table 2
2000, Sellström and Bremberg, 2006), we undertook a scoping review presents study characteristics (study population, sample size, develop-
to contribute a descriptive account of current research on mediation mental outcome examined, neighborhood definition and characteristics
and moderation in the field. We therefore included for review only used, and whether the analyses examined mediation or moderation/
those papers that (1) were published in English in peer-reviewed interaction effects) and a summary of the main findings from each of
journals after 2009, (2) studied children ages 0–6, and (3) statistically the reviewed studies. Reviewed studies pertained to populations in the
modeled the relationship between children's neighborhood residence United States (n=26), Canada (n=7), the United Kingdom (n=7),
and their developmental health, as broadly defined by validated Norway (n=1), and Australia (n=1). Studies looked at a range of
assessments of children's development (Janus and Offord, 2007; developmental outcomes, from specific outcomes (e.g., reading
Kagan, 1992; Doherty, 1997) (Table 1). Gray literature was not achievement) to overall developmental health as measured across a
included in our analyses. As well, because we suspected that the range of domains (e.g., school readiness). Samples sizes ranged from
neighborhood effects on early childhood development may not be only 26-59531 children and/or families, in as few as two neighbor-
comparable between low-, middle-, and high-income countries, we hoods to as many as 2113 neighborhoods. Thirty-one of the studies
limited our review to evidence from OECD countries. The study employed multilevel analyses in which the nesting of children within
selection process is depicted in Fig. 2. neighborhoods was statistically accounted for.
We aimed to describe quantitative evidence on the structural None of the reviewed studies were experimental. Thirteen used
determinants, mediators, and moderators in the relationship between longitudinal analytical methods and the other 29 used a cross-sectional
neighborhoods and early childhood development. We defined early design. All studies controlled for family-level variables that may be
childhood as the period between birth and school entry (i.e. up to about related to self-selection into neighborhoods, including family socio-
the age of 6). We identified articles for review based on keyword economic status (SES), residential instability, and maternal depression
searches in the following databases: EconLit, PsycINFO, ERIC, in their regression models. One study used a natural experiment
Academic Search Complete, Education Source, MEDLINE, Urban design, comparing data from families in social housing with data from
Studies Abstracts and Google Scholar. Combinations of the keywords a control group of families from the general population (Martens et al.,
neighborhood(s) or neighborhood(s), child, children were used to 2014). The authors reasoned that using data from the social housing
identify relevant articles. Specific terms relating to developmental cohort would minimize selection bias because residents of social
outcomes that may have been included in the articles’ full text, such housing presumably have limited opportunities to choose where to
as development, reading, writing, mathematics, math, physical, live due to housing allocation methods.
emotion, social, emotional, cognitive, cognition, communication,
literacy, learning, education, outcome, influence, and effect were also 4.2. Theoretical integration
included in the search.
Table 3 summarizes the results of our theoretical integration of the
3.2. Theoretical integration of literature review reviewed studies according their findings on (i) how, (ii) where, and
(iii) for whom neighborhood residence may influence developmental
We conducted two analytical steps to summarize and then con- health in early childhood. The table summarizes theories and relevant
ceptually integrate the evidence from the studies identified in this constructs that the reviewed studies examined; the indicators that were
review. In step 1, we characterized studies according to the study used; and cites a selection of the reviewed studies from which this
population and sample size, the neighborhood characteristic(s) and information was gathered. The section on “how” describes theories
child development domain(s) examined, and whether the study exam- about the structural mechanisms that appear to be related to neighbor-
ined mediation and/or moderation (interaction effects), and the main hood-level inequalities in children's developmental health. This section
finding(s). In step 2, we used the conceptual-methodological questions also summarizes theories about the neighborhood and family char-

Table 1
Developmental domains in early childhood used for the search.

Domains Developmental constructs

Physical health and well-being (PHWB) Physical readiness for the school day, physical independence, and gross and fine motor skills
Social competence (SC) Overall social skills, understanding of responsibility and respect, approaches to learning, and readiness to explore
new things
Emotional maturity (EM) Prosocial and helping behavior, anxious and fearful behavior, overall happiness, and hyperactivity and inattention
Language and cognitive development (LCD) Basic literacy and numeracy, advanced literacy, and interest in literacy/numeracy
Communication skills and general knowledge (CSGK) Skills with communicating needs and wants, the symbolic use of language, story-telling, and interest in knowledge
about life and the world

158
A. Minh et al. Health & Place 46 (2017) 155–174

Fig. 2. Study selection process.

acteristics that mediate the association between neighborhood resi- concluded that neighborhoods that are socioeconomically heteroge-
dence and child outcomes. The section on “where” summarizes theories neous may be protective for children's development compared with
of moderation of this association by place-based characteristics. neighborhoods of either concentrated poverty or concentrated wealth.
Finally, the section on “for whom” summarizes theories that were Six of the reviewed studies examined gender or cultural dimensions
explored using cross-level interactions i.e. moderation of this associa- of neighborhoods, implicating structural determinants that reinforce
tion by child-level characteristics. gender and ethnic divisions between spaces in the unequal distribution
of developmental health (Kershaw and Forer, 2010; Leventhal and
4.3. How: Class, gender, and ethnic dimensions of place Shuey, 2014; Lloyd and Hertzman, 2010; Vaden-Kiernan et al., 2010;
Hanson et al., 2011; Rydland et al., 2014). Intersectionality is the
We found that neighborhood poverty (or SES) was the most studied theory that social stratification by gender, race, and class and, most
structural determinant of children's developmental outcomes, appear- importantly, intersections among these variables, may impact how
ing in 34 out of the 42 studies reviewed. With the exception of 12 power and opportunities for children's development are distributed
studies that were reviewed (Flouri et al., 2010b, 2010a, O'Campo et al., across society (Choo and Ferree, 2010). Guided by this theory, one of
2010; De Marco and Vernon-Feagans, 2013; Benson and Borman, these studies by found that variables indicating place-based differences
2010, Fedor et al., 2010, Bubier et al., 2009, Brown and Ackerman, in the gendered dimensions of care-giving (e.g., neighborhoods where
2011, Callahan et al., 2011, Fite et al., 2011, Singh and Ghandour, most of the caregiving responsibilities were allocated to women), could
2012, Froiland et al., 2014), all studies described an association better predict the development of British Columbian children at school
between neighborhood poverty or disadvantage and poorer develop- entry when considered with neighborhood SES than SES alone
mental health (Cushon et al., 2011; Daly et al., 2009; Edwards and (Kershaw and Forer, 2010).
Bromfield, 2009; Flouri et al., 2013; Flouri et al., 2012; Hanson et al., The other five examined the relationship between ethnic or cultural
2011; Jeon et al., 2014; Kershaw and Forer, 2010; Kohen et al., 2009; dimensions of children's neighborhoods and developmental health but
Odgers et al., 2009; Vaden-Kiernan et al., 2010; Vanfossen et al., 2010; findings were mixed. Two studies in this review—one in Canada and
Riina et al., 2014; Sastry and Pebley, 2010) and/or an association two in the United States—observed that a higher concentration of
between neighborhood wealth or advantage and better developmental ethnic, immigrant and language minority individuals within the
health (Anderson et al., 2014; Benson and Faas, 2014; Dupere et al., neighborhood was associated with positive child development out-
2010; Kershaw and Forer, 2010; Leventhal and Shuey, 2014), with comes after controlling for neighborhood SES (Leventhal and Shuey,
some evidence of diminishing returns in developmental health as 2014; Lloyd and Hertzman, 2010; Vaden-Kiernan et al., 2010). Two
wealth increases (Carpiano et al., 2009; Dupere et al., 2010). Using other studies, from the United States and Norway, observed the
the index of concentration at the extremes, a measure of inequality, opposite: that a higher concentration of ethnic majority children was
Carpiano et al. (2009) observed that children's developmental out- associated with positive developmental outcomes (Hanson et al., 2011;
comes at school entry in a Canadian province improved as neighbor- Rydland et al., 2014).
hood SES increased but only until a certain point. The authors

159
Table 2
The reviewed studies and their results.

Citation Main model parameters Mediator (s) Moderator (s) Results


A. Minh et al.

Study population (setting, sample size) • Developmental outcome


• Neighborhood definition
• Neighborhood-level covariates (indicators)
Andersen et al. (2014) • Reading and math achievement, Behavior NA NA Neighborhood affluence in early childhood was
Children from the National Institute of Child problems associated with higher achievement and fewer
Health and Human Development Study of • Census block groups internalizing behaviors contemporaneously. These
Early Child Care and Youth Development (US, • Affluence, Poverty associations endured until adolescence for reading
n=1364) achievement.

Benson and Borman (2010) • Reading achievement NA NA Neighborhood social context mattered for reading
Children proceeding through kindergarten and • Postal code achievement at school entry and for reading
the first grade (Early Childhood Longitudinal • Social context (income, education); Race/ethnic achievement growth during the summer.
Study – Kindergarten Cohort, US, n=4180) composition The proportion of neighborhood residents from
minority race/ethnic groups was not associated
with reading achievement at school entry or during
summer.

Brown and Ackerman (2011) • Emotional regulation Maternal negative emotionality NA Maternal negative emotionality partially explained
Children enrolled in an urban Head Start pre- • NA the link between family and neighborhood
school as well as their mothers and teacher • Neighborhood adversity (physical conditions, adversity and children's emotional dysregulation.
(US, n=113) safety, and facilities)

Bubier et al. (2009) • Externalizing behaviors NA Sympathetic functioning Baseline sympathetic functioning moderated the
Ethnic minority, inner-city first through fourth • NA relationship between neighborhood cohesion and
grade children (US, n=57) • Neighborhood cohesion externalizing behaviors. Higher levels of

160
neighborhood cohesion prospectively predicted
higher levels of externalizing behaviors among
children with heightened baseline sympathetic
functioning, and lower levels of externalizing
behaviors among children with attenuated baseline
sympathetic functioning.

Callahan et al. (2011) • Problem behaviors NA Harsh parenting After controlling for externalizing problems, the
African American mother and 2 year old • NA positive association between harsh parenting and
children enrolled in a Head Start program • Neighborhood danger, and belonging internalizing problems became stronger as
(New Orleans, US, n=55) neighborhood danger increased.

Carpiano et al. (2009) • School readiness NA NA Increasing heterogeneity in neighborhood


Children in kindergarten ages 4 and 5 • Stakeholder-informed neighborhoods socioeconomic composition was related to
(Canada, BC, n=37,798) • Disadvantage/advantage (Index of concentration improved physical, social, emotional,
at the extremes) communications, and overall development.

Caughy et al. (2013) • Behavioral problems NA NA Poor conditions of the physical environment of the
Families living in a low-income, primarily • Circles of increasing radius surrounding the neighborhood were related to more behavioral
African American neighborhood, with over child's place of residence. problems, and the geographic extent of the physical
selection of families with children aged 5–13 • Physical conditions environment that mattered was an area with a
(US, City in the Southwest, n=261) • Behavior of children's peers radius between 400 and 800 m surrounding the
child's home. In addition, the average level of
behavior problems of neighborhood peers within
255 m of the child's home was also positively
associated with child behavior problems. These
effects were independent of one another.

Cushon et al. (2011) • School readiness NA NA Poverty was significantly associated with declining
(continued on next page)
Health & Place 46 (2017) 155–174
Table 2 (continued)

Citation Main model parameters Mediator (s) Moderator (s) Results


Study population (setting, sample size) outcome
A. Minh et al.

• Developmental definition
• Neighborhood
• Neighborhood-level covariates (indicators)
Three populations of kindergarteners (Canada, • “Planned neighborhoods” scores in physical health and well-being.
SK, n=1458/ 2159/ 2218) • Poverty (%Aboriginal, single parents, low income,
residential stability, low education,
unemployment)

• Verbal/Language development Child care quality Geographic Isolation, Collective Neighborhood disadvantage did not predict
De Marco and Vernon-Feagans, (2013) • Census block group socialization children's development or child care quality.
36 month-old children from families, from • Disadvantage (Income, female-headed Neighborhood safety predicted children's receptive
three counties in eastern North Carolina, and households, social assistance, unemployment), language, with child care quality a partial mediator
central Pennsylvania (US, n=1292) Safety of this relationship.
Collective socialization but not geographic isolation
moderated the relationship between neighborhood
safety and child care quality.

Delany-Brumsey et al. (2014) • Behavioral problems NA Caregiver depression Disadvantage was associated with more
Children from the age of 5–17, whose primary • Census tract internalizing and externalizing behavior problems.
caregiver was female (US, Los Angeles, n=741) • Residential stability, Concentrated disadvantage, Neither social capital nor residential stability was a
Social capital significant predictor. The interaction between
caregiver depression and social capital was not
significant.

Dupere et al. (2010) • Verbal language development, Reading skills, Maternal depression, Quality of NA The presence of educated, affluent professionals in
Children in grade 1 from 10 sites, in which the Math skills, Vocabulary home learning environment, Child the neighborhood had a favorable association with

161
mother is over 18, speaks English and does not • Census block group care quality, Classroom instructional children's achievement until it plateaued at
have substance abuse or serious health • Advantage (income, education, occupation) quality, School advantage moderate levels of advantage.
problems (US, n=1364) Child care and school contexts explained more of
the neighborhood effects than the home
environment.

• Behavioral problems, Prosocial behavior Parental perceptions of NA Neighborhood facilities, cleanliness, safety and
Edwards and Bromfield, (2009) • Post-codes (12,000–15,000 residents/unit) neighborhood facilities, belonging mediated the relationship between
Nationally representative sample of 4–5 year • SES (Socioeconomic Indices for Area's Index of neighborhood safety, neighborhood neighborhood socio-economic status and conduct
olds (Australia, n=4983) Advantage/ Disadvantage), Residential stability, cleanliness, neighborhood belonging problems.
Ethnic homogeneity, Remoteness (Accessibility/ The relationship between neighborhood safety and
Remoteness Index for Australia), Physical conduct problems was in turn, mediated by
disorder neighborhood belonging.
Neighborhood socio-economic status was not
associated with pro-social behavior.
Neighborhood cleanliness and neighborhood
belonging were the only neighborhood variables
that were significantly associated with pro-social
behavior

Fedor et al. (2010) • Behavior concerns NA Gender Children in low-violence communities exhibited
Children attending Head Start programs in • MI greater parent-reported behavior concerns than
high- and low-violence communities (US, • Violence children in high-violence communities and no
n=388) significant differences were found on levels of
protective factors.
There was not a significant interaction effect
between gender and community type for any of the
scales measured.

(continued on next page)


Health & Place 46 (2017) 155–174
Table 2 (continued)

Citation Main model parameters Mediator (s) Moderator (s) Results


Study population (setting, sample size) outcome
A. Minh et al.

• Developmental definition
• Neighborhood
• Neighborhood-level covariates (indicators)
Fite et al. (2010) • Delinquency and depressive symptoms NA Attendance in a community-based A marginally significant trend (p < 0.06) for
Children between 5 and 13 years of age • NA program program attendance to buffer the effects of
recruited to a mid-sized southeastern • Neighborhood problems neighborhood problems on child depressive
community-based care program (US, n=147) symptoms was found.

Flouri et al. (2010a) • Internalizing and externalizing problems, Family adversity, Family SES, Parenting, Child cognitive ability, The deprivation-psychopathology association
Children ages 2–5, and their older siblings Psychopathology, Prosocial behavior Parents' psychological distress, Child's developmental delay, Child's remained significant even when family adversity
ages 4–15 (UK, n=9630) • LSOA (Lower-layer super output areas) Parenting temperament was adjusted for, but it became non-significant
• Deprivation (Index of Multiple Deprivation) once adjustment was made for family-level SES.
None of the interactions were statistically
significant. When parental qualifications were
excluded from the family-level contextual controls
the effect of area risk remained significant on both
externalizing and internalizing psychopathology.

Flouri et al. (2010b) • Internalizing and externalizing problems, Parenting, Parents’ depression Family adversity The area risk/broad psychopathology association
3-year old children (UK, n=4748) Psychopathology, Prosocial behavior remained significant even after family risk was
• LSOA (Lower-layer super output areas) controlled but not after family level socioeconomic
• Deprivation (Index of Multiple Deprivation) disadvantage was controlled.

Flouri et al. (2012) • Emotional and behavioral problems NA Child's cognitive ability Neighborhood deprivation was significant, even
Children aged 9 months (UK, Millennium • “Small areas" after adjustment for covariates, significantly
Cohort Study, n=9736) • Deprivation (Index of Multiple Deprivation) associated with children's peer problems. Verbal

162
and nonverbal cognitive ability moderated this
association.

Flouri et al. (2013) • Emotional and behavioral problems NA NA Being in a lower deprivation neighborhood at Wave
Children aged 3–16 in the first two waves of • LSOA (Lower-layer super output areas) 1 was related to lower scores of both emotional and
Millennium Cohort Study (UK, n=23,162) • Neighborhood disadvantage (Index of Multiple behavioral problems 2 years later, even after
Deprivation), Moved to neighborhood adjustment for child's age and sex, family adversity,
family structure and maternal psychological
distress. Adjusting for family socio-economic
disadvantage at Wave 1 partially explained the
association between neighborhood SES with
emotional and behavioral problems.
Children whose families subsequently moved—even
within or between lower deprivation
neighborhoods—were at higher risk of emotional
and behavioral problems.

Froiland et al. (2013) • Early literacy Home literacy (parent–child shared NA Neighborhood socioeconomic well-being predicted
Children enrolled in the Head Start preschool • Census block reading, number of books at home) home literacy, which in turn predicted early literacy
program (US, Midwestern State, n=551) • SES (education, housing occupancy, receipt of
social assistance), urbanicity

Froiland et al. (2014) • Expressive vocabulary Home literacy NA Neighborhood social networks predicted home
Parents and children from a Head Start centre • NA literacy, which in turn predicted expressive
(Suburban area of Midwestern city, US, n=89) • Neighborhood social networks vocabulary.

Grinstein-Weiss et al. (2012) • Positive behaviors NA Homeownership status of parent Homeownership was found to have a stronger
Children between 4 and 18 years old whose • (Missing) positive effect on children's positive behavior as
families participated in the Community • Poverty, Residential stability, Population density neighborhood population density increases.
(continued on next page)
Health & Place 46 (2017) 155–174
Table 2 (continued)

Citation Main model parameters Mediator (s) Moderator (s) Results


Study population (setting, sample size) outcome
A. Minh et al.

• Developmental definition
• Neighborhood
• Neighborhood-level covariates (indicators)
Advantage Program (US, NC, n=812)

Hanson et al. (2011) • Language and vocabulary, Literacy, Mathematics, NA NA Neighborhood economic hardship predicted
4-year-old children living in poverty, living Social skills children's lower mathematics and letter knowledge
with disabilities, or who were English • Census block academic outcomes and one social skills outcome.
Language Learners (US, n=1006) • SES (poverty, female-head of household, Children's residence in primarily English-speaking
education, unemployment), Linguistic isolation neighborhoods was associated with higher levels of
social participation.

Heberle et al. (2014) • Behavior problems Family disadvantage, Violence or NA Neighborhood disadvantage was associated with
12–42 month old children living (US, New- • Census tracts conflict exposure, Parental disruptive behavior, but the effect was negligible
Haven Meriden Standard Metropolitan Area, • Disadvantage (Sawhill Index) depression, Parenting behavior when all of the more proximal factors (i.e. family
n=1204) disadvantage, violence or conflict exposure, parent
depressive symptoms, and parenting behavior)
were accounted for.

Jeon et al. (2014) • Cognitive skills, Social and emotional competence Home learning environment, NA Neighborhood disadvantage was associated with
Children in early childcare programs (US, • Census tracts Parental depression parental depressive symptoms above and beyond
Midwestern State, n=420) • Disadvantage (poverty, unemployment, female- family SES, which, in turn, was associated with
headed households, public assistance, %African children's greater probability of social-emotional
American) problems.

Kershaw and Forer (2010) • School readiness NA NA Poverty rates, the share of local wealthy families,

163
Kindergarten children (Canada, BC, n=40,772) • Stakeholder-informed neighborhoods the share of residents receiving social assistance,
• Poverty, Wealth, Social assistance, Employment, employment rates, and lone-parent households
Lone-parent households, Child-care services significantly predicted various domains of
development.

Kingston et al. (2013) • Behavioral problems, Cognitive-academic skills, NA Parent involvement in education Increased parent involvement in education was
Children enrolled in the 8 pre-kindergarten Social and emotional behavior problems related to lower rates of behavior problems among
classes in public elementary schools in lower • Census tracts children from neighborhoods with higher levels of
and middle-income urban neighborhoods • Affluence, Occupation, Childcare burden childcare burden.
whose primary caregiver spoke English
(n=171)

Kohen et al. (2009) • Verbal and Language development, Behavior, NA NA Low income and education were associated with
Children attending Kindergarten classes in 7 School readiness, Hyperactivity, Aggression, poorer verbal and behavioral outcomes. The
cities across Canada (n=2743) Anxiety number of immigrants was associated with lower
• Urban areas: Census tracts (between 2500 and verbal, and higher aggression and anxiety scores,
8000 residents) but also with higher developmental scores.
• Rural areas: Census subdivisions (municipalities The school environment had a larger impact on
so have greater variability in population size) Kindergarten children's standardized, parent, and
• Low income, Youth unemployment, Low teacher reported outcomes than neighborhoods.
education, % Aboriginal status, Immigrants,
Residential stability

Leventhal and Shuey (2014) • Behavioral functioning and achievement, verbal NA Neighborhood immigrant Immigrant concentration and immigrant status
Children from three age cohorts in the Project ability, literacy skills concentration; were moderators of associations between
on Human Development in Chicago • Neighborhood clusters of census tracts Immigrant status neighborhood processes and children's
Neighborhoods (Chicago, US, n=3209) • Concentrated poverty and affluence, residential development, but the nature of these associations
mobility, cohesion, friends/kin, youth services, depended on the outcome and racial/ethnic group
organizational involvement considered.
(continued on next page)
Health & Place 46 (2017) 155–174
Table 2 (continued)

Citation Main model parameters Mediator (s) Moderator (s) Results


Study population (setting, sample size) outcome
A. Minh et al.

• Developmental definition
• Neighborhood
• Neighborhood-level covariates (indicators)
Lima et al. (2010) • Externalizing and Internalizing problems NA Family risk Perceived negative social climate moderated the
Children entering first grade (US, Baltimore, • Census block groups effect of family risks on behavior problems such
n=405) • Community Involvement with Children that more risk was associated with a larger
(Willingness to intervene in acts of delinquency increment in both externalizing behavior problems
and misbehavior, to assist children in need, and and psychological problems for children living in
level of social interaction), Social climate high versus low risk neighborhoods.
(Physical/social disorder, fear of retaliation, fear
of victimization)

Lloyd and Hertzman (2010) • Verbal and Language development, Cognitive NA Rural/urban In rural neighborhoods, increased concentrated
Children between the ages 5–6 (Canada, BC, development immigration predicted increased kindergarten
n=5022) • Stakeholder-informed neighborhoods language scores. In urban neighborhoods,
• Disadvantage/ advantage (Index of concentration increased concentrated immigration predicted
at the extremes), Concentrated immigration, increased kindergarten language and
Residential stability, Presence of caregivers, % communication scores.
Aboriginal status An increased proportion of young children in a
neighborhood predicted decreases in rural
children's language scores.
Increased concentrations of Aboriginal persons
predicted decreased urban children's kindergarten
language scores.

164
Lovasi et al. (2014) • Child intelligence and Neurodevelopment NA Child's prenatal exposure to Neighborhood-level low English proficiency was
Children of African American and Dominican 1 km network buffers around prenatal home polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons independently associated with lower overall
women (US, New York City, n=326)
• address (PAH) intelligence scores and lower verbal and
• Poverty, Education, English-proficiency, performance scores. Low neighborhood-level
Inadequate plumbing educational attainment was also associated with
lower performance scores.
Neighborhood associations were similar or
diminished among the high PAH exposure group,
as compared with the low PAH exposure group.

Martens et al. (2014) • School readiness NA NA No statistically significant relationship between


Children ages 0–19 living in social housing in • Community Centre Areas (CCAs, average income quintile and school readiness among
Winnipeg (Canada, MB, n=712) population appr. 10,000) children living in social housing.
• Neighborhood income quintiles
Midouhas et al. (2014) • Emotional and behavioral problems Parental involvement, Quality of Child's nonverbal ability Low neighborhood human capital was related to
Children in the Millennium Cohort Study (UK, • Administrative boundary: “Lower layer Super home environment (home hyperactivity and peer problems, but effects were
n=9850) Output Area” organization), School environment fully attenuated by the achievement level of
• Human capital (education, occupation) (school achievement) children's schools.
Low neighborhood human capital was associated
with greater conduct problems, and change in
conduct problems over time.
Higher nonverbal ability did not dampen the
adverse impact of low neighborhood human capital
on the trajectory of conduct problems.

• Behavioral competence Parenting behavior Intimate partner violence (IPV) in While positive neighborhood characteristics such as
O'Campo et al. (2010)Children entering • Census block group the family high levels of community involvement with
first grade (US, Baltimore, n=393) • Disadvantage (poverty, public assistance, children—based upon four scales capturing
(continued on next page)
Health & Place 46 (2017) 155–174
Table 2 (continued)

Citation Main model parameters Mediator (s) Moderator (s) Results


Study population (setting, sample size) outcome
A. Minh et al.

• Developmental definition
• Neighborhood
• Neighborhood-level covariates (indicators)
unemployment, female-headed households), neighborhood levels of social interaction and
Community involvement with children, Negative collective socialization of children—were protective
social climate (Physical/social disorder, fear of for high levels of behavioral problems among
retaliation, fear of victimization) families not reporting IPV, this protective effect was
not seen among families who did report IPV.
These interactions between neighborhood factors
and IPV were not explained by parental factors.

• Antisocial behavior NA Collective efficacy Deprivation was associated with higher levels of
Odgers et al. (2009)Nationally • Postal code antisocial behavior at school entry and a slower rate
representative sample of twins (E- • Deprivation of decline from involvement in antisocial behavior
Longitudinal Twin Study, UK, n=2232) between the ages of 5 and 10.
Even after controlling for neighborhood problems
and family-level factors, collective efficacy was
negatively associated with levels of antisocial
behavior at school entry but only in deprived
neighborhoods.

Puchala et al. (2010) • School readiness NA Child speaks English as a second High residential stability and low employment was
Children attending kindergarten in Saskatoon • Stakeholder-informed neighborhoods language (ESL) associated with lower scores on the communication
(Canada, SK, n=5833) • Income, Ethnic diversity, Education, skills and general knowledge domains only.
Employment, Residential The difference in scores between non-ESL and ESL
• Stability. Single parents, % Aboriginal status children decreased as neighborhood diversity

165
increased.

Riina et al. (2014) • Internalizing and externalizing problems NA Parent-to-child physical aggression The difference in internalizing problems between
Caregivers and their children in the Project on • Neighborhood clusters of census tracts low and high cohesion neighborhoods among
Human Development in Chicago • Disadvantage, cohesion, immigrant children exposed to parent-to-child physical
Neighborhoods (PHDCN) (Chicago, US, concentration, residential stability aggression was statistically significant only at ages
n=2810) 11 and older. The difference in internalizing
problems between low and high cohesion
neighborhoods among children not exposed to
PCPA was not statistically significant

Rydland et al. (2014) • Vocabulary skills NA NA Children in the neighborhood with fewer co-ethnics
Turkish immigrant children (Norway, n=26) • NA (only two neighborhoods compared) had better vocabulary skills at age 5 than children
• Education, Co-ethnic concentration in the neighborhood with more co-ethnics. This
difference was maintained up to age 10.

Sastry and Pebley (2010) • Reading and math skills NA NA There were large effects of average neighborhood
Children ages 3–17 participating the Los • Census tract income on children's reading and mathematics
Angeles Family and Neighborhood Survey (LA, • Neighborhood median income achievement.
US, n=2350)

• Behavioral problems Family cohesion; Social Age Higher levels of behavioral problems were
Singh and Ghandour, (2012) Children • MI participation; Geographic mobility; Gender associated with socially disadvantaged
aged 6–17 years, 2007 National Survey of • Neighborhood social conditions (Safety, presence Sleep behavior; Television watching Race/ethnicity neighborhoods and lower household SES. The
Children's Health (US, n=59,531) of litter, poor/dilapidated housing, vandalism) neighborhood and household socioeconomic effects
were reduced but remained highly significant after
adjusting for potentially intervening variables
Interactions of neighborhood conditions and
household SES with age and gender were not
(continued on next page)
Health & Place 46 (2017) 155–174
A. Minh et al. Health & Place 46 (2017) 155–174

The effects of neighborhood violence, employment,


neighborhood conditions and household SES varied

There were no mediation effects found for family or


Low neighborhood SES was associated with poorer

social variables between neighborhood factors and

female-headed households do not manifest in first


4.3.1. How: Evidence of neighborhood mediators

grade, but affect the trajectory of child aggression


cognitive functioning and high neighborhood ESL

A large number of moderation effects were found


statistically significant. However, the impacts of
Structural factors exert their influence on children's development

although there was not a clear pattern to the

income, and percentages of single males and


by determining the distribution of resources or risks that may alter
development (i.e., via neighborhood-level mediators). This review

with children's behavioral functioning.


found the most evidence of mediation for neighborhood social-inter-

Note: PHWB = Physical Health and wellbeing, SC = Social competence, EM = Emotional maturity, LCD = Language and cognitive development, CSGK = Communication skills and general knowledge, NA = Not applicable.
between first and seventh grades.
active mechanisms. Social-interactive resources or the lack thereof
significantly by race/ethnicity
were examined with indicators such as parents’ perceived social and
physical disorder, fear of retaliation or victimization (O'Campo et al.,
2010), and researchers’ ratings of noise and safety (De Marco and
Vernon-Feagans, 2013). Though one study found no relationship

child outcomes.
between neighborhood social capital and behavioral outcomes
(Delany-Brumsey et al., 2014), and another found that behavioral
Results

results.
problems were more common within neighborhoods with less violence
(Fedor et al., 2010), the majority of studies found evidence to support
theories that social-interactive mechanisms mediate the relationship
Family social risk (depression, locus
of control, social support, violence);

between neighborhood SES and developmental health. Studies found


that a lack of safety, increased neighborhood social disorder, less
Family involvement in child's

belonging, less social cohesion, and a lower potential for community


involvement with children (or collective efficacy), was directly related
to worse language, emotional, and behavioral outcomes for children
(Bubier et al., 2009; Brown and Ackerman, 2011; Callahan et al., 2011;
Moderator (s)

Fite et al., 2010; Singh and Ghandour, 2012; Froiland et al., 2014;
education

Riina et al., 2014); and in many cases, partially explained the


association between early developmental health and neighborhood
NA

disadvantage (Edwards and Bromfield, 2009, O'Campo et al., 2010,


De Marco and Vernon-Feagans, 2013).
Family social risk (depression, locus
of control, social support, violence);

By contrast, the reviewed studies provided relatively less evidence


Neighborhood family structure

supporting theories of institutional or geographic mechanisms; none of


Family involvement in child's

the studies provided evidence on environmental mechanisms that link


Neighborhood violence;

neighborhood residence with developmental health. One study exam-


Neighborhood income;

ined remoteness, a geographical mechanism, but found that it failed to


explain the relationship between neighborhood SES and Australian
Mediator (s)

children's conduct problems (Edwards and Bromfield, 2009).


education

Institutional processes that were examined included school-advantage,


child-care quality, and neighborhood facilities and services. Greater
school advantage (measured as the proportion of students not eligible
to receive free lunch or achievement levels) (Dupere et al., 2010,
SES; English-Second-Language status; High
SES; Mobile, young, and diverse; Family density

Midouhas et al., 2014), higher quality child-care institutions (measured


• Neighborhood-level covariates (indicators)

according to the quality of stimulation and support given to children in


the child care setting) (Dupere et al., 2010, De Marco and Vernon-
Feagans, 2013), and a stronger relationship between caregivers and
development, Behavior

children at childcare institutions (Dupere et al., 2010), partially


explained how neighborhood advantage may be related to better
definition
outcome

language and cognitive development in early childhood. Edwards and


Main model parameters

Bromfield (2009) found, however, that the relationship between


neighborhood SES and children's behavioral outcomes was not sig-
aggression
• Developmental
• Neighborhood

tracts

tracts

nificantly explained by the quality of neighborhood facilities such as


• Employment
• Cognitive

parks, play-spaces, roads, and access to shopping, services and


• Census

• Census

transportation. Leventhal and Shuey (2014) found no relationship


• Child
• Low

between the presence of youth services in a neighborhood and


children's behavioral functioning, when controlling for other neighbor-
hood and individual-level variables.
Children aged 3 or older who are participating

All of the above neighborhood-level mechanisms may exert their


Urban girls and boys between the first and
in the Head Start program (US, n=1785)

seventh grades (US, Baltimore, n=1409)

influence on developmental health by subsequently altering aspects of


children's family lives, including their parents’ access to resources to
Study population (setting, sample size)

support children's development, parental wellbeing, and parenting


Vaden-Kiernan et al. (2010)

behaviors. Studies in this review found that the quality, structure,


and nature of the family environment explained at least part or all of
Vanfossen et al. (2010)

the relationship between neighborhood SES and developmental health


(Froiland et al., 2014; Brown and Ackerman, 2011 Dupere et al., 2010;
Table 2 (continued)

Flouri et al., 2010b, Heberle et al., 2014, Jeon et al., 2014, Kingston
et al., 2013, O'Campo et al., 2010, Singh and Ghandour, 2012).
Children who lived in more advantaged neighborhoods had better
Citation

early language and cognitive development, as measured by their


vocabulary and reading scores in Grade 1; this association was partially
explained by more learning materials at home, heightened parental

166
Table 3
Theoretical processes, conceptual linkages to early child development, and the empirical constructs and indicators used.

Theory Conceptual Linkage Constructs Indicators Notable studies


A. Minh et al.

How: Structural mechanisms and mediators (neighborhood and family-level)

Structural mechanisms

Neighborhood Institutions and social and economic patterns inherent in Disadvantage/ Poverty Andersen et al. (2014); Carpiano et al. (2009); Cushon et al.
disadvantage/ neighborhood life influence child outcomes by creating advantage Income (inequality) (2011); Flouri et al. (2013); Hanson et al. (2011); Martens et al.
advantage opportunities, providing resources, and encouraging behavior. Deprivation Employment (2014); Vanfossen et al. (2010); De Marco and Vernon-Feagans
Relevant neighborhood institutions include the neighborhood's Opportunity structure Occupation (2013); Delany-Brumsey et al. (2014); Dupere et al. (2010);
employment patterns, economic vitality, educational and religious Female-headed households Edwards and Bromfield (2009); Fite et al. (2011); Flouri et al.
institutions, family structure, and the political institution. Family structure (2010b, 2012); Froiland et al. 2013; Grinstein-Weiss et al. (2012);
Change in deprivation Heberle et al. (2014); Jeon et al., (2014); Kershaw and Forer
(2010); Kingston et al. (2013); Kohen et al. (2009); Leventhal and
Shuey (2014); Lloyd and Hertzman (2010); Lovasi et al. (2014);
O'Campo et al. (2010); Odgers et al. (2009); Puchala et al. (2010);
Riina et al. (2014); Sastry and Pebley (2010); Vaden-Kiernan et al.
(2010)
Intersectionality Multiple systems of oppression can simultaneously, influence Gendered dimensions of Social assistance Benson and Borman (2010); Hanson et al. (2013); Kershaw and
children (e.g., race, class, gender, citizenship, etc.) Instead of a care Child-care services Forer (2010); Puchala et al. (2010); Rydland et al. (2014); Hanson
single, universal neighborhood effect on development, there may Racial inequalities Race/ethnic composition et al. (2011)
be some effects on some outcomes, in some population groups,
and in some types of neighborhoods

Social-interactive mechanisms

Collective efficacy/Social In neighborhoods with high levels of “mutual trust or willingness Collective efficacy Community involvement with children Delany-Brumsey et al. (2014); Edwards and Bromfield (2009);

167
disorganization to intervene or the common good” of the community, residents Social capital Social interaction between neighbours O’Campo et al. (2010); Odgers et al. (2009); Bubier et al. (2009);
trust one another and work together to: fight cuts to services such Social cohesion Psychological sense of community De Marco and Vernon-Feagans (2013); Callahan (2011); Singh
as recreational facilities or healthcare; maintain their Collective socialization Trust and Ghandour (2012); Riina et al. (2014)
neighborhoods against neglect, destruction, or abandonment, Collective efficacy
which can directly impact child emotional well-being; and provide Social climate
social support to one another. Disorder Negative social climate Caughy et al. (2013); Edwards and Bromfield (2009); O’Campo
Concentrated economic disadvantage and population instability Social disorder Residential stability et al. (2010); Singh and Ghandour (2012); Brown and Ackerman
can erode trust and the willingness of residents to act for the Social disorganization Crime (2011); De Marco & Vernon-Feagans (2013); Callahan et al.
common good, which may compromise parenting and family Poor physical conditions Abandoned cars (2011); Fedor et al. (2010); Fite et al. (2010); Lima et al. (2010)
management processes, such as child monitoring. Parents may Neglected buildings
have more difficulty accessing resources in the community, such Poor lighting
as after school programs or extracurricular activities that might Trash Violence
foster child development. Safety concerns can also limit children's
opportunities to play.
Norms and Collective When many families in a community share similar practices Social capital Collective efficacy De Marco and Vernon-Feagans (2013); Delany-Brumsey et al.
socialization regarding children, strong collective norms may develop. In this Social cohesion Mutual assistance between neighbours (2014); Leventhal and Shuey (2014)
way, adults in a neighborhood can influence young people who are Collective socialization Informal social control
not their children, acting as role models for young children; as
enforcers, monitoring children's behaviors, maintaining order,
and intervening when necessary.
Social networks Relationships between early childhood well-being and Social networks Frequency of informal contact with De Marco and Vernon-Feagans (2013); Edwards and Bromfield
neighborhood characteristics may be mediated through support Social networks neighbours (2009); Leventhal and Shuey (2014); Froiland et al. (2014)
networks available to parents, parental behavior, and the home Relations/kin Number of family members and friends
environment.
Epidemic or Contagion Residents in the neighborhood can influence one another. For Peer influences % English speaking residents Caughy et al. (2013)
example, children living in affluent neighborhoods may benefit
developmentally by interacting with peers who are more
developmentally advanced.
Transactional Neighborhood structural features influence individuals' Parental perceptions of Quality of facilities Edwards and Bromfield (2009)
(continued on next page)
Health & Place 46 (2017) 155–174
Table 3 (continued)

Theory Conceptual Linkage Constructs Indicators Notable studies


A. Minh et al.

How: Structural mechanisms and mediators (neighborhood and family-level)

Structural mechanisms

perceptions of the neighborhoods which in turn are related to the neighborhood Safety
neighborhood social processes. Neighborhood social processes Cleanliness
subsequently affect children's outcomes Belonging

Family-level The neighborhood may affect (through any of the mechanisms Family adversity Parental depression Brown and Ackerman (2011); Delany-Brumsey et al. (2014);
listed) parents’ physical and mental health, stress, coping skills, Family risk Adverse life events (e.g., family financial Dupere et al. (2011); Flouri et al. (2010a, 2010b)
sense of efficacy, behaviors, and material resources. All of these, crisis, family death, injury of family Gristein-Weiss et al. (2012); Heberle et al. (2014); Jeon et al.
in turn, may affect the home environment in which children are member, etc.) (2014); Kingston et al. (2013); Lima et al. (2010); Midouhas et al.
raised. Intimate partner violence (2014);
Renter O’Campo (2010), Singh and Ghandour (2012); Vaden-Kiernan
Parent-child relationship Quality of home learning environment et al. (2010); Froiland et al. (2014)
Parenting practices Cognitive stimulation at home
Supportive parenting Parenting behavior (e.g., authoritarian
Hostility vs. permissive)
Parent involvement with education
Connectedness
Maternal warmth

Institutional mechanisms

Institutional resources Affluent neighborhoods may have higher-quality public and Care/service context Child care quality De Marco and Vernon-Feagans (2013); Dupere et al. (2010);
private services. Adults who live outside the community but work Child care Presence of youth services Leventhal and Shuey (2014)

168
in these institutions (e.g., school teachers, community centre Youth services
workers, and librarians) can influence children's development. School environment School SES Dupere et al. (2014); Midouhas et al. (2014)
Classroom instructional quality

Geographical mechanisms
Geographic isolation Geographic isolation may limit residents’ access to jobs, shopping Remoteness Distance to good, services, and Edwards and Bromfield, 2009
outlets, and public institutions putting children's development at opportunities for social participation
risk, but may also be a protective factor given less exposure to
drugs, violence, and other social problems.

Environmental mechanisms
Toxin exposure Children may be exposed to unhealthy levels of air-, soil-, and/or Toxin exposure Air-pollution levels Lovasi et al., (2014)
water-borne pollutants because of the current and historical land
uses and other ecological conditions in their neighborhoods

Where: Dependence on place characteristics


We did not find evidence of theoretical frameworks that describe moderation of the Rural vs. urban Rural/Urban Lloyd and Hertzman, 2010
neighborhood-developmental health relationship by place.

For whom? Dependence on child characteristics

Biological sensitivity Nervous system functioning may buffer the effect of stressors Cognitive ability School readiness Bubier et al. (2009); Callahan et al. (2011); Flouri et al. (2010a);
associated with neighborhood deprivation because it is related to Verbal/non-verbal ability Flouri et al. (2012); Midouhas et al. (2014); Lovasi et al. (2014)
greater self-control and strengthened control over emotions. Autonomic functioning Sympathetic functioning
Exposure to stressful environments may heighten stress reactivity. Developmental delay Developmental screening
Children with high stress reactivity are hypothesized to (a) be at Parental emotional Harsh parenting
risk for negative outcomes under conditions of adversity, and (b) regulation
experience positive outcomes under conditions of support
Parents who are harsh with their young children may also be less
(continued on next page)
Health & Place 46 (2017) 155–174
Table 3 (continued)

Theory Conceptual Linkage Constructs Indicators Notable studies


A. Minh et al.

How: Structural mechanisms and mediators (neighborhood and family-level)

Structural mechanisms

able to regulate their own emotional reactivity in response to


stress from neighborhood conditions. The effects of harsh
parenting may thus be exacerbated.
Attachment For children whose security in the home is threatened violence or Parent-to-child physical Conflict tactics Riina et al., 2014
neglect, relationships with and between adults in the aggression
neighborhood may be compensatory sources of emotional
support. The extent to which an abused child views adult
neighbours as trustworthy may reduce chronic feelings of fear and
distress.
Programs and services High levels of program attendance may buffer the impact of Program attendance Weekly program attendance Fite et al. (2010)
neighborhood problems on child depressive symptoms.
Other We observed a lack of theoretical frameworks describing race or Race/ethnicity Puchala et al. (2010); Singh and Ghandour (2012); Leventhal and
ethnicity as a moderator of neighborhood effects amongst the English as a second language Shuey (2014)
reviewed studies.

Integrative models
Integrated process model Neighborhood structure and quality affect parental perceptions, Neighborhood structural Poverty O’Campo et al. (2010); Leventhal and Brooks-Gunn (2013); Bubier
behavior, experiences and child behavioral and cognitive characteristics Community involvement with children et al. (2009)
outcomes directly, but neighborhood characteristics can also serve Neighborhood social Intimate partner violence
as moderators of specified associations between parental behavior climate Parenting behavior
and perceptions. Neighborhood processes, such as social climate, Family environment
also serve to attenuate or enhance associations between parenting Parent/child relationship

169
and child outcomes.
Ecological models Included in this model is the concept of person–context
interactions: Individual characteristics (e.g., immigrant status)
influence contexts (e.g., neighborhood) in which children
develop; the bidirectionality of influence is such that contexts
(e.g., neighborhood social cohesion) also affect individuals (e.g.,
children's behavior). In addition, characteristics of contexts may
interact (e.g., concentration of immigrants and social ties) to
influence development.
Health & Place 46 (2017) 155–174
A. Minh et al. Health & Place 46 (2017) 155–174

responsiveness, and a higher level of stimulation at home compared Cross-level interactions involve a variable indicating neighborhood-
with children in less advantaged neighborhoods (Dupere et al., 2010). level risks or resources (such as income or employment, or social
By contrast, children in deprived neighborhoods had worse emotional climate and community involvement with children) interacting with a
development, an association that was partially explained by higher variable indicating risk or resources at the child or family level (such as
maternal or paternal depression or distress (Jeon et al., 2014; Flouri family income or the quality of the parent-child relationship (Lima
et al., 2010a; Heberle et al., 2014). Children living in neighborhoods et al., 2010), or adverse events like a death in the family (Flouri et al.,
with more negative social climates were found to have greater risk of 2010b)).
behavioral problems, an association that was statistically explained by Some significant cross-level interactions were observed in this
parenting practices (O'Campo et al., 2010). Only two studies that review. First, three studies found evidence that the characteristics of
examined characteristics of the family environment as a mediator children – their experiences of environmental exposure, their cognitive
failed to find evidence to support this claim. Vaden-Kiernan et al. abilities, and their nervous system functioning – moderate the relation-
(2010) found that neither family-level SES or family social factors ship between neighborhood adversity and developmental health
(including family involvement with the child's education) significantly (Lovasi et al., 2014; Flouri et al., 2012; Bubier et al., 2009). Lovasi
explained any of the relationship between neighborhood SES and child et al. (2014) looked at pre-natal toxin exposure as a moderator of the
outcomes. Midouhas et al. (2014) found that neither reading to the effect of neighborhood structural features – including indicators of
child nor home organization explained the relationship between poverty, education, and cultural isolation – on cognitive development
neighborhood SES and children's emotional and behavioral problems. at age 5, among African American children from New York City. They
found that for children with high prenatal exposure to the environ-
4.4. Where: Neighborhood-level moderators mental toxin, polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbon (PAH), neighborhood
disadvantage was associated with worse performance on an intelligence
The mechanisms that alter early childhood development, whether test at age 5 than among children with low prenatal PAH exposure.
they originate from the neighborhood or family level, may have a Flouri et al. (2012) found that verbal and non-verbal cognitive ability
stronger or weaker impact depending on where they occur. Six of the moderated the association between neighborhood deprivation and
reviewed studies provide insight into the place-based factors that may emotional and behavioral problems. In a study of 57 children from
moderate the relationship between neighborhood characteristics and high-poverty neighborhoods, Bubier et al. (2009) observed that chil-
child development outcomes (De Marco and Vernon-Feagans, 2013, dren whose nervous system function indicated higher risk of behavioral
Leventhal and Shuey, 2014, Odgers et al., 2009, Kingston et al., 2013, problems had fewer externalizing behaviors in highly cohesive neigh-
Puchala et al., 2010, Lloyd and Hertzman, 2010). However, studies borhoods. Contrary to their hypothesis, however, they also observed
seemed to lack common conceptual frameworks or methodological that for children whose nervous system functioning indicates greater
guidelines—in some instances, it appeared that moderation effects were sensitivity to contextual influence, there was a greater risk of externa-
not tested based on theoretical hypotheses, but were reported because lizing behaviors associated with more neighborhood cohesion.
the statistical analyses were conducted in a way that generically tested Four studies also found evidence that the neighborhood effects on
main effects and interaction terms. development depend on children's familial life (Lima et al., 2010;
Two studies found that the concentration of immigrants in a O'Campo et al., 2010; Callahan et al., 2011, Kingston et al., 2013). Two
neighborhood had a stronger association with children's development, of these found that stressful family environments, including the
but that this association was dependent on the outcome considered, the experience of family adversity and harsh parenting, augmented the
racial/ethnic group in question, and the urbanicity of the neighborhood negative effect of neighborhood adversity on their emotional develop-
(Lloyd and Hertzman, 2010; Leventhal and Shuey, 2014). Odgers et al. ment and behavioral outcomes (Lima et al., 2010; Callahan et al.,
(2009) found that improvement in children's emotional development 2011). Two other found that, children who reported violence in their
was related to collective efficacy in the neighborhood – residents’ family through intimate partner violence (O;Campo et al., 2010) or
ratings of shared norms in the neighborhood, trust, and the likelihood physical aggression from their parents (Riina et al., 2014) failed to
of their neighbours to intervene (Sampson et al., 1997) – but only if experience the benefits to their social and emotional development that
these children lived in deprived neighborhoods and not if they lived in might have come from living in neighborhoods with high levels of
affluent ones. De Marco (2013) found that collective socialization community involvement and social cohesion. Riina et al. (2014)
moderated the relationship between neighborhood safety and child additionally found that age was a factor in this moderating effect. In
care quality which was related to children's receptive language skills. their study, while social cohesion buffered the effect of violence at home
One study found that children were less likely to engage in externaliz- for children above the age of 11, the same protective effect was not
ing behaviors despite having less-involved parents, but only if they observed for young children. By contrast, one study found that children
lived in neighborhoods that had many adults, compared to neighbor- with supportive family environments were found to experience aug-
hoods with fewer adults (Kingston et al., 2013). The authors concluded mented benefits from living in advantaged neighborhoods (Kingston
that results pointed to the importance of interventions that addressed et al., 2013). In a study conducted in an urban area in the United
community resources, rather than focus solely on interventions that States, children who had parents that were highly involved with their
primarily promoted parental engagement. Finally, one study found that children's education were found to have better social development than
the extent to which developmental inequalities existed between groups children whose parents were less involved, particularly if they lived in
of children differed between places. Specifically, children who spoke affluent neighborhoods (Kingston et al., 2013).
English as a second language (ESL) had lower communication and One study found a protective effect of accessing community-based
general knowledge scores compared to native English speakers, but the programs. From a sample of 147 children between the ages of 5–13
gap between ESL and non-ESL children was greater in ethnically years, Fite et al. (2011) observed that children had fewer behavioral
homogeneous neighborhoods than it was in neighborhoods with problems and depressive symptoms if they lived in a neighborhood
greater diversity (Puchala et al., 2010). with greater social and economic problems, but more frequently
attended the community-based care program.
4.5. For whom: Child-level moderators The findings from this review, however, do not yet provide
conclusive evidence about which combinations of neighborhood and
Studies examined cross-level interactions to determine whether child-family-level risks or resources are most impactful for predicting
children's developmental status, health, or social background shapes specific developmental outcomes. For example, studies found incon-
the extent to which neighborhood mechanisms alter their development. sistent results for the moderating effects of gender, race/ethnicity, and

170
A. Minh et al. Health & Place 46 (2017) 155–174

immigrant status (Leventhal and Shuey, 2014; Fedor et al., 2010; Singh neighborhoods, their findings were mixed, suggesting little in the way
and Ghandour, 2012) in the relationship between neighborhood of conclusions about how ethnicity and place come together to
conditions and developmental health. Two studies failed to find an influence developmental health.
association for the interaction between family adversity and neighbor- Given prominent early conceptualizations of racial segregation as
hood SES on children's psychopathology (Flouri et al., 2010a, 2010b). key mechanisms through which social disparities are reinforced
One study failed to find an interaction between caregiver depression (Wilson, 1987; Williams and Collins, 2001; Charles, 2003), the failure
and neighborhood social capital on children's behavioral problems for studies to find conclusive evidence of these theories signals a need
(Delany-Brumsey et al., 2014). While Vaden-Kiernan et al. (2010) for more research. To adequately investigate such aspects of social
observed a large number of moderation effects, there was no clear stratification of opportunities and resources by place, it may be
pattern to their results. The characteristics of families and children that important to develop data sources that would allow one to operatio-
exacerbate or dampen the neighborhood effect on developmental nalize concepts of race and ethnicity. Fully characterizing the social
health may be specific to both the developmental domain and to the inequalities that racialized and ethnic minorities experience in associa-
neighborhood mechanism at work. tion with their residential environments may not only require assess-
ment of self-reported race and ethnicity but also multiple aspects of
5. Discussion racism, or acculturation that contribute to health and disease. At the
institutional level, residential segregation is a mechanism for racism
The last few decades have seen an increase in quantitative evidence that can restrict children and families from accessing needed resources
to show that children's residential environments are related to their such as quality schools and services. The subjective experiences of
early developmental health. Using research from 2009, we attempted discrimination can negatively affect mental and physical health
to show not only the importance of residential environments, but also (Paradies et al., 2015). The inconsistency seen in the current quanti-
what the evidence says about how, where, and for whom neighbor- tative evidence would suggest that there may be a need for ongoing
hoods matter. In terms of ‘how’ neighborhoods influence developmen- theoretical development about how these concepts underlie area-based
tal health, the evidence from our review would suggest that social deprivation and developmental inequality and how they can be
inequalities between neighborhoods are related to variability in early measured.
childhood development. These effects are mediated through social Perhaps more importantly, there remains a gap in evidence about
interactive mechanisms such as safety or the likelihood that the the social structures or policies that produce or reduce social inequal-
community would intervene on children. However, less is known about ities by neighborhood. None of the studies in this review provided
the how environmental, geographical, and institutional mechanisms evidence of the structural macro-social factors that shape residential
link neighborhood residence to developmental health. segregation (e.g., economic restructuring, migration, discrimination,
The reviewed evidence also showed that neighborhood mechanisms and public policies). These upstream forces drive social differences
are related to the differences in children's family environments. In the across areas or neighborhoods (Diez Roux, 2001), and must therefore
majority of studies that examined family-level variables, such as the be linked to neighborhood differences and mechanisms.
learning environment at home or parenting practices, family-level
variables were found to explain all or part of the association between 5.2. Knowledge gaps about how: Improving inference about
neighborhoods and children's development. These studies underscore mediation via qualitative studies and natural experiments
the importance of the environment that is most proximal to the child,
the family, while recognizing that families exist in relation to the Future research must also more deeply explore how the way
people, resources, and opportunities within their residential environ- children and their families interact with their neighborhood is shaped
ments. by the variability in neighborhood characteristics, and how it may
In terms of ‘where’ and ‘for whom’ the reviewed research demon- contribute developmental inequalities. Though our review found much
strates that developmental inequalities between neighborhoods may be evidence to support social-interactive mediators of the relationship
magnified or buffered in some places but not others, and for some between neighborhoods and developmental health, we also observed
individuals but not others. The same mechanism that is important to that mediation effects remain a challenge for statistical studies on
developmental health in some areas may not be relevant in others. neighborhood effects on child development. Unlike models in which
Similarly, while there is a social gradient in developmental health in there are clear etiological pathways or temporal precedence of ex-
relation to neighborhood SES, characteristics and experiences of the posure, mediator, and outcome, the statistical examination of neigh-
child with regard to their health, social position, and family environ- borhood impacts reviewed in this study was continually hampered by a
ment, may make some more vulnerable to poor developmental health lack of distinction in the temporal ordering of indicators of neighbor-
than others. Most of the reviewed studies did not refer to a cohesive hood exposure, neighborhood or family-level mediators, and develop-
theory for driving research about which place- or child-level character- mental outcome. This is especially true of family-level variables. While
istics interact with one another to impact which developmental out- they strongly explain the statistical association between neighborhood
comes and why. The field may therefore benefit from striving toward SES and developmental health, it is unclear whether they do so because
greater conceptual-methodological integration. they in fact mediate the relationship or because there is a tendency of
families with certain characteristics to self-select into neighborhoods of
5.1. Knowledge gaps about how: gender, ethnicity, and other certain kinds. These debates recall questions about whether it is
structural determinants theoretically important and/or appropriate to distinguish between
composition (family and individual-level) and context (neighborhood)
A number of knowledge gaps remain. First, there is less clarity (Macintyre et al., 2002).
about the developmental impact of social divisions between neighbor- Rather than attempt to statistically separate context and composi-
hoods by gender, ethnicity, and race, perhaps because compared with tion, we therefore suggest two approaches to help future research
SES (or poverty) less data exists to operationalize such stratifications. elucidate mediation. First, neighborhood effects researchers may
Available data may also fail to adequately capture the complexity of incorporate qualitative methods into their studies, which have histori-
such concepts. Only one of the reviewed studies attempted to identify cally complemented and strengthened quantitative evidence about the
gendered dimensions of children's residential environments that may mechanisms linking neighborhoods with developmental health in early
be related to their developmental health (Kershaw and Forer, 2010). childhood (Galster, 2012). Such methods may allow researchers to
While a number of the reviewed studies looked at ethnic dimensions of meaningfully engage in discussions of mediation that extend its

171
A. Minh et al. Health & Place 46 (2017) 155–174

definition beyond traditional cause and effect models towards the more already linked with customized sociodemographic data, offers oppor-
dynamic models of people and the places that they live in, that is tunity to study the association of child development and neighborhood
suggested by the bio-ecological theory of developmental health. across jurisdictions (e.g., Webb et al., 2017, Guhn et al., 2016), and
Second, the challenge of understanding mediation may be ad- may be further linked with other data sources (e.g., economic data,
dressed in studies using natural experimental designs, which take a policy data) to systematically test hypotheses about the structural
more pragmatic, intervention-based approach to understanding the processes that create social hierarchies within and between neighbor-
modifiable aspects of neighborhood effects on developmental health. hoods (i.e. macro-social determinants). One may, for example, com-
For example, neighborhoods periodically change as a result of policies pare early childhood development across many time points and
or due to grass-roots efforts by residents. They also may change due to jurisdictions across Canada to investigate the impact of different policy
rise in property prices or industrial events, such as closure of sources of environments as they change. These data may also help researchers to
employment (Formoso et al., 2010). The study of developmental identify key neighborhoods into which one might conduct qualitative
consequences of these changes may be thought of as natural experi- studies that add depth to existing theories about gender and ethnic
ments involving variation to characteristics in the environment. Such divisions of place; or to further explore mechanisms that link neigh-
studies have the benefit of not only examining mediators by examining borhood residence with inequalities in early childhood development.
modifiable pathways and interventions, but also generating evidence Together, such studies fill gaps in knowledge about how neighborhoods
about the actions that will be most effective and efficient under influence early childhood development. Finally, because this monitor-
different conditions (Braveman et al., 2011). Within the counterfactual ing system collects representative data, it has the statistical power to
framework, these studies also have the benefit of reducing selection support a research agenda that aims to understand the features of place
biases that have hindered the inference of causal relationships in and individual children which moderate the relationship between
observational studies. For these reasons, we join others in recommend- neighborhood residence and developmental health i.e. where and for
ing that neighborhood effects research brings together data that may be whom. Given a unique population-level database, projects such as
used in the analysis of natural experiment designs (Diez Roux, 2008; CanNECD present opportunities to examine questions about how,
Galster and Santiago, 2015). where and for whom certain neighborhood characteristics are most
salient in a way that was not previously possible.
5.3. Knowledge gaps about where and for whom
6. Conclusion
While the reviewed evidence as yet do not lead us to draw
conclusions about where and for whom neighborhood effects are From Urie Bronfenbrenner's first iteration of an ecological systems
important, they do reinforce the notion that the neighborhood effects theory of human development (Bronfenbrenner, 1979) to his bio-
on developmental health are not generalizable across all places or all ecological theory of human development, as presented in 2006
people (Sharkey and Faber, 2014). These findings may have profound (Bronfenbrenner and Morris, 2007), the person-process-context-time
implications for ensuring that research evidence informs policies with model presented challenges with to operationalization. Its complexity
greater specificity. The unintended consequences of failing to account is in its portrayal of the multiple processes taking place across a
for differences by population or place have been demonstrated in number of ecological levels and over time that jointly affect child
studies of the Moving to Opportunity project – a housing intervention development outcomes. The recent development of benchmark data on
giving low-income residents the opportunity to move to more affluent neighborhood and developmental indicators would be a starting point
neighborhoods – which was found to effectively improve the mental for this research agenda. This review suggests that from within the
health of young girls but was deleterious to the delinquent behavior of ongoing challenges of researching the how, where, and for whom
young boys (Osypuk et al., 2012). Similarly, previous findings that neighborhoods are important to developmental health, there emerge
family factors interact differently with children's outcomes in rural and opportunities to advance the evidence in this field.
urban areas (Janus, 2011) suggest that the constraints or opportunities
afforded by the characteristics of the settings may be necessary References
considerations for developing effective policies. Researchers may
therefore look across groupsand settings to understand how neighbor- Andersen, T.G., Dobrev, D., Schaumburg, E., 2014. A robust neighborhood truncation
hood characteristics (such as social climate, or urbanicity) or individual approach to estimation of integrated quarticity. Econom. Theory 30, 3–59.
Anderson, S., Leventhal, T., Dupéré, V., 2014. Exposure to neighborhood affluence and
characteristics (such as ethnicity, and gender) may shape children's poverty in childhood and adolescence and academic achievement and behavior.
experiences of policy changes and interventions. Such efforts should Appl. Dev. Sci. 18, 123–138.
also aim to strengthen underdeveloped theories about where and for Arksey, H., O'malley, L., 2005. Scoping studies: Towards a methodological framework.
37-41.
whom neighborhoods are important to children's developmental Ash, M., Fetter, T.R., 2004. Who lives on the wrong side of the environmental tracks?
health. Evidence from the EPA's risk-screening environmental indicators model. Social. Sci.
Q. 85, 441–462.
Aylward, G.P., 1997. Infant and early childhood neuropsychology. Clinical child
5.4. Emerging possibilities for studying neighborhood effects on early psychology library. Plenum Press, New York.
childhood development Bartley, M., Power, C., Blane, D., Smith, G.D., Shipley, M., 1994. Birth-weight and later
socioeconomic disadvantage - evidence from the 1958 British cohort study. Br. Med.
J. 309, 1475–1478.
In the past, researchers faced limitations with describing how,
Benson, J., Borman, G.D., 2010. Family, neighborhood, and school settings across
where, and for whom neighborhoods matter, due to logistical chal- seasons: when do socioeconomic context and racial composition matter for the
lenges with capturing data at multiple ecological levels, over time, and reading achievement growth of young children? Teach. Coll. Rec. 112, 1338–1390.
for large enough populations that allow test of effects at neighborhood, Benson, M.J., Faas, C., 2014. Assessing adolescent contexts: measures of families, peers,
schools and neighborhoods. Int. J. Child Adolesc. Health 7, 53–69.
family, and child levels. Today, such barriers are being broken down. In Berube, A., 2010. Comment on Mark Joseph's “Is mixed ‐ income development an
Canada, for example, a coalition of researchers and governments are antidote to urban poverty?”. Hous. Policy Debate 17, 235–247.
collaborating Canadian Neighborhoods and Early Childhood Blas, E., Gilson, L., Kelly, M.P., Labonte, R., Lapitan, J., Muntaner, C., Ostlin, P., Popay,
J., Sadana, R., Sen, G., Schrecker, T., Vaghri, Z., 2008. Addressing social
Development project (CanNECD; Guhn et al., 2016). CanNECD has a determinants of health inequities: what can the state and civil society do? Lancet
research agenda that aims to monitor children's development and their 372, 1684–1689.
neighborhoods across the country and over multiple time points. The Boyce, W.T., Ellis, B.J., 2005. Biological sensitivity to context: i. An evolutionary-
developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Dev.
developmental data, representing almost 90% of children from over Psychopathol. 17, 271–301.
2000 neighborhoods across the country and over multiple time points,

172
A. Minh et al. Health & Place 46 (2017) 155–174

Braveman, P., Egerter, S., Williams, D.R., 2011. The social determinants of health: Formoso, D., N Weber, R., Atkins, M, S., 2010. Gentrification and urban children's well-
coming of age. Annu. Rev. Public Health 32, 381–398. being: tipping the scales from problems to promise. Am. J. Community Psychol. 46,
Bronfenbrenner, U., 1979. The Ecology of Human Development: Experiments by Nature 395–412.
and Design. Fox, N.A., Calkins, S.D., Bell, M.A., 1994. Neural plasticity and development in the 1st 2
Bronfenbrenner, U., 1999. Environments in developmental perspective: theoretical and years of life - evidence from cognitive and Socioemotional domains of research. Dev.
operational models. In: Friedman, S.L., Wachs, T.D., Friedman, S.L., Wachs, T.D. Psychopathol. 6, 677–696.
(Eds.), Measuring Environment Across the Life Span: Emerging Methods and Froiland, J.M., Powell, D.R., Diamond, K.E., Son, S.-H.C., 2013. Neighborhood
Concepts.. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, US. socioeconomic well-being, home literacy, and early literacy skills of at-risk
Bronfenbrenner, U., Morris, P.A., 2007. The Bioecological Model of Human preschoolers. Psychol. Sch. 50 (8), 755–769. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pits.21711.
Development. Handbook of Child Psychology. John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Froiland, J.M., Powell, D.R., Diamond, K.E., 2014. Relations among neighborhood social
Brooks-Gunn, J., Duncan, G.J., Klebanov, P.K., Sealand, N., 1993. Do neighborhoods networks, home literacy environments, and children's expressive vocabulary in
influence child and adolescent development? Am. J. Sociol. 99, 353–395. suburban at-risk families. Sch. Psychol. Int. 35, 429–444.
Brown, E.D., Ackerman, B.P., 2011. Contextual risk, maternal negative emotionality, and Galster, G.C., 2012. The Mechanism(s) of Neighbourhood Effects: Theory, Evidence, and
the negative emotion dysregulation of preschool children from economically Policy Implications. In: Van ham, M., Manley, D., Bailey, N., Simpson, L. &
disadvantaged families. Early Educ. Dev. 22, 931–944. Maclennan, D. (eds.) Neighbourhood Effects Research: New Perspectives.
Brownell, M.D., Ekuma, O., Nickel, N.C., Chartier, M., Koseva, I., Santos, R.G., 2016. A Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands.
population-based analysis of factors that predict early language and cognitive Galster, G.C., Santiago, A.M., 2015. Evaluating the potential of a natural experiment to
development. Early Child. Res. Q. 35, 6–18. provide unbiased evidence of neighborhood effects on health. Health Serv. Outcomes
Bubier, J.L., Drabick, D.A.G., Breiner, T., 2009. Autonomic functioning moderates the Res. Methodol. 15, 99–135.
relations between contextual factors and externalizing behaviors among inner-city Grinstein-Weiss, M., Key, C., Yeo, Y.H., Yoo, J., Holub, K., Taylor, A., Tucker, J., 2012.
children. J. Fam. Psychol. 23, 500–510. Homeownership, Neighbourhood Characteristics and Children’s Positive Behaviours
Callahan, K.L., Scaramella, L.V., Laird, R.D., Sohr-Preston, S.L., 2011. Neighborhood Among Low- and Moderate-income Households 49. Urban Studies (Sage
disadvantage as a moderator of the association between harsh parenting and toddler- Publications, Ltd.), 3545–3563. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0042098012443861.
aged children's internalizing and externalizing problems. J. Fam. Psychol. 25, 68–76. Guhn, M., Janus, M., Enns, J., Brownell, M., Forer, B., Duku, E., Muhajarine, N., Raos,
Carpiano, R.M., Lloyd, J.E.V., Hertzman, C., 2009. Concentrated affluence, concentrated R., 2016. Examining the social determinants of children's developmental health:
disadvantage, and children's readiness for school: a population-based, multi-level protocol for building a pan-Canadian population-based monitoring system for early
investigation. Social. Sci. Med. 69, 420–432. childhood development. BMJ Open 6, e012020.
Caughy, M.O.B., Leonard, T., Beron, K., Murdoch, J., 2013. Defining neighborhood Hanson, J.L., Hair, N., Shen, D.G., SHI, F., Gilmore, J.H., Wolfe, B.L., Pollak, S.D., 2013.
boundaries in studies of spatial dependence in child behavior problems. Int. J. Family poverty affects the rate of human infant brain growth. PLoS One 8, e80954.
Health Geogr. 12, 24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1476-072X-12-24. Hanson, M.J., Miller, A.D., Diamond, K., Odom, S., Lieber, J., Butera, G., Horn, E.,
Charles, C.Z., 2003. The dynamics of racial residential segregation. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 29, Palmer, S., Fleming, K., 2011. Neighborhood community risk influences on
167–207. preschool children's development and school readiness. Infants Young-. Child. 24,
Choo, H.Y., Ferree, M.M., 2010. Practicing Intersectionality in Sociological Research: a 87–100.
Critical Analysis of Inclusions, Interactions, and Institutions in the Study of Heberle, A.E., Thomas, Y.M., Wagmiller, R.L., Briggs-Gowan, M.J., Carter, A.S., 2014.
Inequalities. Sociol. Theory 28, 129–149. The impact of neighborhood, family, and individual risk factors on toddlers'
Chum, A., 2011. Policy implications of neighbourhood effects on health research: disruptive behavior. Child Dev. 85, 2046–2061.
towards an alternative to poverty deconcentration. Health Place 17, 1098–1104. Heim, C., Plotsky, P.M., Nemeroff, C.B., 2004. Importance of studying the contributions
Cushon, J.A., Vu, L.T.H., Janzen, B.L., Muhajarine, N., 2011. Neighborhood poverty of early adverse experience to neurobiological findings in depression.
impacts children's physical health and well-being over time: Evidence from the Early Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 641–648.
Development Instrument. Early Educ. Dev. 22, 183–205. Hertzman, C., Boyce, T., 2010. How experience gets under the skin to create gradients in
Daly, B.P., Shin, R.Q., Thakral, C., Selders, M., Vera, E., 2009. School engagement among developmental health. Annu. Rev. Public Health 31, 329–347, (3p following 347).
urban adolescents of color: does perception of social support and neighborhood Jackson, L., Langille, L., Lyons, R., Hughes, J., Martin, D., Winstanley, V., 2009. Does
safety really matter? J. Youth Adolesc. 38, 63–74. moving from a high-poverty to lower-poverty neighborhood improve mental health?
Darcy, M., 2010. De-concentration of disadvantage and mixed income Housing: a critical A realist review of 'Moving to Opportunity'. Health Place 15, 961–970.
discourse approach. Hous., Theory Soc. 27, 1–22. Janus, M., 2011. Transition to school. In: Laverick, D.M., Jalongo, M.R. (Eds.),
De Marco, A., Vernon-Feagans, L., 2013. Rural neighborhood context, child care quality, Transitions to Early Care and Education. International Perspectives on Making
and relationship to early language development. Early Educ. Dev. 24, 792–812. Schools Ready for Young Children.. Springer, Netherlands.
Delany-Brumsey, A., Mays, V.M., Cochran, S.D., 2014. Does neighborhood social capital Janus, M., Offord, D., 2007. Development and psychometric properties of the Early
buffer the effects of maternal depression on adolescent behavior problems? Am. J. Development Instrument (EDI): a measure of children's school readiness. Can. J.
Community Psychol. 53, 275–285. Behav. Sci. 39, 1–22.
Diez Roux, A.V., 2001. Investigating neighborhood and area effects on health. Am. J. Jencks, C., Mayer, S.E., 1990. The social consequences of growing up in a poor
Public Health 91, 1783–1789. neighborhood. In: Lynn, L.E., Mcgeary, M.G.H. (Eds.), Inner-City Poverty in the
Diez Roux, A.V., 2008. Next steps in understanding the multilevel determinants of United States.. National Academy Press, Washington, DC.
health. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 62, 957–959. Jeon, L., Buettner, C.K., Hur, E., 2014. Family and neighborhood disadvantage, home
Doherty, G., 1997. Zero to six: The basis for school readiness. Human Resources environment, and children's school readiness. J. Fam. Psychol.: JFP: J. Div. Fam.
Development Canada. Psychol. Am. Psychol. Assoc. (Div. 43) 28, 718–727.
Dupere, V., Leventhal, T., Crosnoe, R., Dion, E., 2010. Understanding the positive role of Joseph, M.L., 2010. Is mixed‐income development an antidote to urban poverty? Hous.
neighborhood socioeconomic advantage in achievement: the contribution of the Policy Debate 17, 209–234.
home, child care, and school environments. Dev. Psychol. 46, 1227–1244. Jutte, D.P., Roos, L.L., Brownell, M.D., 2011. Administrative record linkage as a tool for
Earls, F., Buka, S., 2000. Measurement of community characteristics. In: Meisels, public health research. Annu. Rev. Public Health 32 (32), 91–108.
J.P.S.S.J. (Ed.), Handbook of Early Childhood Intervention2nd ed.. Cambridge Kagan, S.L., 1992. Readiness pPast, present, and future - shaping the agenda. Young-.
University Press, New York, NY, US. Child. 48, 48–53.
Edwards, B., Bromfield, L.M., 2009. Neighborhood influences on young children's Kagan, S.L., 1999. Cracking the readiness mystique. Young-. Child. 54, 2–3.
conduct problems and pro-social behavior: evidence from an Australian national Kershaw, P., Forer, B., 2010. Selection of area-level variables from administrative data:
sample. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 31, 317–324. an intersectional approach to the study of place and child development. Health Place
Fedor, M.C., Bender, S.L., Carlson, J.S., 2010. Examining risk and protective factors in 16, 500–511.
head start populations located in high- and low-violence communities. Infants Kingston, S., Huang, K.Y., Calzada, E., Brotman, L., 2013. Parent involvement in
Young-. Child. 23, 209–217. education as a moderator of family and neighbourhood socioeconomic context on
Fite, P.J., Vitulano, M., Wynn, P., Wimsatt, A., Gaertner, A., Rathert, J., 2010. Influence school readiness among young children. J. Community Psychol. 41, 265–276.
of perceived neighborhood safety on proactive and reactive aggression. J. Knudsen, E.I., 2004. Sensitive periods in the development of the brain and behavior. J.
Community Psychol. 38, 757–768. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1412–1425.
Fite, P.J., Vitulano, M.L., Preddy, T.M., 2011. The positive impact of attending a Kohen, D., Oliver, L., Pierre, F., 2009. Examining the effects of schools and
community-based youth program on child depressive symptoms. J. Community neighbourhoods on the outcomes of Kindergarten children in Canada. Int. J. Speech-
Psychol. 39, 804–814. Lang. Pathol. 11, 404–418.
Flouri, E., Tzavidis, N., Kallis, C., 2010b. Area and family effects on the psychopathology Kohen, D.E., Hertzman, C., Brooks-Gunn, J., 1998. Neighbourhood influences on
of the Millennium Cohort Study children and their older siblings. J. Child Psychol. children’s school readiness. Hull, Quebec.
Psychiatry 51, 152–161. Levac, D., Colquhoun, H., O'brien, K.K., 2010. Scoping studies: advancing the
Flouri, E., Tzavidis, N., Kallis, C., 2010a. Adverse life events, area socioeconomic methodology. Implement. Sci., 5.
disadvantage, and psychopathology and resilience in young children: the importance Leventhal, T., Brooks-Gunn, J., 2000. The neighborhoods they live in: the effects of
of risk factors' accumulation and protective factors' specificity. Eur. Child Adolesc. neighborhood residence on child and adolescent outcomes. Psychol. Bull. 126,
Psychiatry 19, 535–546. 309–337.
Flouri, E., Mavroveli, S., Tzavidis, N., 2012. Cognitive ability, neighborhood deprivation, Leventhal, T., Shuey, E.A., 2014. Neighborhood context and immigrant young children's
and young children's emotional and behavioral problems. Social. Psychiatry development. Dev. Psychol. 50, 1771–1787.
Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 47, 985–992. Levitt, P., 2003. Structural and functional maturation of the developing primate brain. J.
Flouri, E., Mavroveli, S., Midouhas, E., 2013. Residential mobility, neighbourhood Pediatr. 143, S35–S45.
deprivation and children's behaviour in the UK. Health Place 20, 25–31. Lima, J., Caughy, M., Nettles, S.M., O'Campo, P.J., 2010. Effects of cumulative risk on

173
A. Minh et al. Health & Place 46 (2017) 155–174

behavioral and psychological well-being in first grade: Moderation by neighborhood influences on low-income children's development: theoretical and methodological
context. Social. Sci. Med. 71, 1447–1454. issues. Am. J. Community Psychol. 31, 55–72.
Link, B.G., Phelan, J.O., 1995. Social conditions as fundamental causes of disease. J. Rydland, V., Grover, V., Lawrence, J., 2014. The second-language vocabulary trajectories
Health Social. Behav. 35, 80–94. of Turkish immigrant children in Norway from ages five to ten: the role of preschool
Litt, J.S., Tran, N.L., Burke, T.A., 2002. Examining urban brownfields through the public talk exposure, maternal education, and co-ethnic concentration in the neighborhood.
health "macroscope". Environ. Health Perspect. 110 (Suppl 2), 183–193. J. Child Lang. 41, 352–381.
Lloyd, J.E.V., Hertzman, C., 2010. How neighborhoods matter for rural and urban Sampson, R.J., Raudenbush, S.W., Earls, F., 1997. Neighborhoods and violent crime: a
children's language and cognitive development at kindergarten and Grade 4. J. multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science 277, 918–924.
Community Psychol. 38, 293–313. Sampson, R.J., Morenoff, J.D., Earls, F., 1999. Beyond social capital: spatial dynamics of
Lovasi, G.S., Eldred-Skemp, N., Quinn, J.W., Chang, H.-W., Rauh, V.A., Rundle, A., collective efficacy for children. Am. Sociol. Rev. 64, 633–660.
Orjuela, M.A., Perera, F.P., 2014. Neighborhood social context and individual Sampson, R.J., Morenoff, J.D., Gannon-Rowley, T., 2002. Assessing "neighbourhood
polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbon exposures associated with child cognitive test effects": social processes and new directions in research. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 28,
scores. J. Child Fam. Stud. 23, 785–799. 443–478.
Love, J.M., Aber, J.L., Brooks-Gunn, J., Mathematica Policy Research, I., 1994. Sastry, N., Pebley, A.R., 2010. Family and neighborhood sources of socioeconomic
Strategies for Assessing Community Progress Toward Achieving the First National inequality in children's achievement. Demography 47, 777–800.
Educational Goal. Mathematic Policy Research. Scott-Little, C., Kagan, S.L., Frelow, V.S., 2006. Conceptualization of readiness and the
Macintyre, S., Ellaway, A., Cummins, S., 2002. Place effects on health: how can we content of early learning standards: the intersection of policy and research? Early
conceptualise, operationalise and measure them? Social. Sci. Med. 55, 125–139. Child. Res. Q. 21, 153–173.
Martens, P.J., Chateau, D.G., Burland, E.M.J., Finlayson, G.S., Smith, M.J., Taylor, C.R., Sellström, E., Bremberg, S., 2006. The significance of neighbourhood context to child and
Brownell, M.D., Nickel, N.C., Katz, A., Bolton, J.M., 2014. The effect of neighborhood adolescent health and well-being: a systematic review of multilevel studies. Scand. J.
socioeconomic status on education and health outcomes for children living in social Public Health 34, 544–554.
housing. Am. J. Public Health 104, 2103–2113. Shankardass, K., Dunn, J.R., 2012. How Goes the Neighbourhood? Rethinking
Midouhas, E., Kuang, Y., Flouri, E., 2014. Neighbourhood human capital and the Neighbourhoods and Health Research in Social epidemiology. In: O’Campo, P.,
development of childrens emotional and behavioural problems: the mediating role of Dunn, J.R. (Eds.), Rethinking Social Epidemiology. Springer Netherlands,
parenting and schools. Health Place 27, 155–161. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ Dordrecht.
j.healthplace.2014.02.004. Sharkey, P., Faber, J.W., 2014. Where, when, why, and for whom do residential contexts
Mitchell, C., Hobcraft, J., Mclanahan, S.S., Siegel, S.R., Berg, A., Brooks-Gunn, J., matter? moving away from the dichotomous understanding of neighborhood effects.
Garfinkel, I., Notterman, D., 2014. Social disadvantage, genetic sensitivity, and Annu. Rev. Sociol. 40, 559–579.
children's telomere length. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 111, 5944–5949. Shonkoff, J.P., Phillips, D.A., 2000. From Neurons to Neighborhoods: The Science of
Monk, C.S., Webb, S.J., Nelson, C.A., 2001. Prenatal neurobiological development: Early Childhood Development. National Academy Press, Washington.
molecular mechanisms and anatomical change. Dev. Neuropsychol. 19, 211–236. Singh, G.K., Ghandour, R.M., 2012. Impact of neighborhood social conditions and
Mountain, J.A., Nyaradi, A., Oddy, W.H., Glauert, R.A., DE Klerk, N.H., Straker, L.M., household socioeconomic status on behavioral problems among US children.
Stanley, F.J., 2016. Data linkage in an established longitudinal cohort: the Western Matern. Child Health J. 16, 158–169.
Australian Pregnancy Cohort (Raine) Study. Public Health Res Pract., 26. Solar, O., Irwin, A., 2010. A Conceptual Framework for Action on the Social
Nickel, N.C., Chateau, D.G., Martens, P.J., Brownell, M.D., Katz, A., Burland, E.M.J., Determinants of Health.
Walld, R., Hu, M.M., Taylor, C.R., Sarkar, J., Goh, C.Y., Team, P.E., 2014. Data Stern, M.P., Bartley, M., Duggirala, R., Bradshaw, B., 2000. Birth weight and the
Resource Profile: Pathways to Health and Social Equity for Children (PATHS Equity Metabolic Syndrome: thrifty phenotype or thrifty genotype? Diabetes-Metab. Res.
for Children). Int. J. Epidemiol. 43, 1438–1449. Rev. 16, 88–93.
Noble, K.G., Engelhardt, L.E., Brito, N.H., Mack, L.J., Nail, E.J., Angal, J., Barr, R., Fifer, Tunstall, H.V., Shaw, M., Dorling, D., 2004. Places and health. J. Epidemiol. Community
W.P., Elliott, A.J., Network, P., 2015. Socioeconomic disparities in neurocognitive Health 58, 6–10.
development in the first two years of life. Dev. Psychobiol. 57, 535–551. Ursache, A., Noble, K.G., 2016. Neurocognitive development in socioeconomic context:
Oakes, J.M., 2006. Commentary: advancing neighbourhood-effects research–selection, multiple mechanisms and implications for measuring socioeconomic status.
inferential support, and structural confounding. Int J. Epidemiol. 35, 643–647. Psychophysiology 53, 71–82.
O'Campo, P., 2012. Are we producing the right kind of actionable evidence for the social Vaden-Kiernan, M., D'elio, M.A., O'brien, R.W., Tarullo, L.B., Zill, N., Hubbell-Mckey, R.,
determinants of health? J. Urban Health-Bull. New Y. Acad. Med. 89, 881–893. 2010. Neighborhoods as a developmental context: a multilevel analysis of
O'Campo, P., Caughy, M.O., Nettles, S.M., 2010. Partner abuse or violence, parenting and neighborhood effects on Head Start families and children. Am. J. Community
neighborhood influences on children's behavioral problems. Social. Sci. Med. 70, Psychol. 45, 49–67.
1404–1415. Van Ham, M., Manley, D., 2012. Neighbourhood effects research at a crossroads. Ten
Odgers, C.L., Moffitt, T.E., Tach, L.M., Sampson, R.J., Taylor, A., Matthews, C.L., Caspi, challenges for future research Introduction. Environ. Plan. A 44, 2787–2793.
A., 2009. The protective effects of neighborhood collective efficacy on british children van Vuuren, C.L., Reijneveld, S.A., Van Der Wal, M.F., Verhoeff, A.P., 2014.
growing up in deprivation: a developmental analysis. Dev. Psychol. 45, 942–957. Neighborhood socioeconomic deprivation characteristics in child (0-18 years) health
Osypuk, T.L., Schmidt, N., Nguyen, Q., Lincoln, A.L., 2012. Effects of housing mobility studies: a review. Health Place 29, 34–42.
and lower poverty neighborhoods on adolescent asthma: the moving to opportunity Vanfossen, B., Brown, C.H., Kellam, S., Sokoloff, N., Doering, S., 2010. Neighborhood
experiment. Am. J. Epidemiol. 175, (S48-S48). context and the development of aggression in boys and girls. J. Community Psychol.
Paradies, Y., Ben, J., Denson, N., Elias, A., Priest, N., Pieterse, A., Gupta, A., Kelaher, M., 38, 329–349.
Gee, G., 2015. Racism as a determinant of health: a systematic review and meta- Vimpani, G., 2000. Developmental health and the wealth of nations. Health Promot. Int.
analysis. PLoS One 10, e0138511. 15, 181–182.
Puchala, C., Vu, L.T.H., Muhajarine, N., 2010. Neighbourhood ethnic diversity buffers Wadsworth, M.E.J., Kuh, D.J.L., 1997. Childhood influences on adult health: a review of
school readiness impact in ESL children. Can. J. Public Health Rev. Can. De. St. resent work from the British1946 national birth cohort study, the MRC National
Publique 101, Suppl, S13–8. survey of Health and development. Paediatr. Perinat. Epidemiol. 11, 2–20.
Putnam, S., Galea, S., 2008. Epidemiology and the macrosocial determinants of health. J. Webb, S., Janus, M., Duku, E., Raos, R., Brownell, M., Forer, B., Guhn, M., Muhajarine,
Public Health Policy 29, 275–289. N., 2017. Neighbourhood socioeconomic status indices and early childhood
Riina, E.M., Martin, A., Brooks-Gunn, J., 2014. Parent-to-child physical aggression, development. SSM - Popul. Health 3, 48–56.
neighborhood cohesion, and development of children's internalizing and Williams, D.R., Collins, C., 2001. Racial residential segregation: a fundamental cause of
externalizing. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 35, 468–477. racial disparities in health. Public Health Rep. (Wash., D. C.: 1974) 116, 404–416.
Roos, N.P., Roos, L.L., Brownell, M., Fuller, E.L., 2010. Enhancing policymakers' Wilson, W.J., 1987. The truly disadvantaged: the inner city, the underclass, and public
understanding of disparities: relevant data from an information-rich environment. policy.
Milbank Q. 88, 382–403. Wu, A.D., Zumbo, B.D., 2008. Understanding and using mediators and moderators.
Roosa, M.W., Jones, S., Tein, J.-Y., Cree, W., 2003. Prevention science and neighborhood Social. Indic. Res. 87, 367–392.

174

You might also like