You are on page 1of 9

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/321813131

An Examination of Aversive Heterosexism in the Courtroom: Effects of


Defendants' Sexual Orientation and Attractiveness, and Juror Gender on Legal
Decision Making

Article  in  Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity · December 2017


DOI: 10.1037/sgd0000253

CITATIONS READS
6 1,141

2 authors:

Jennifer Coons Russ Espinoza


California State University, Fullerton California State University, Fullerton
11 PUBLICATIONS   41 CITATIONS    23 PUBLICATIONS   136 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

NSF Grant? View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Russ Espinoza on 28 May 2019.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity © 2017 American Psychological Association
2018, Vol. 5, No. 1, 36 – 43 2329-0382/18/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/sgd0000253

An Examination of Aversive Heterosexism in the Courtroom: Effects of


Defendants’ Sexual Orientation and Attractiveness, and Juror Gender on
Legal Decision Making

Jennifer V. Coons Russ K. E. Espinoza


University of California, Riverside California State University, Fullerton

The current study addressed whether defendant sexual orientation, defendant attractiveness, and juror
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

gender influences juror decisions. Two competing theories were assessed as the primary mechanism for
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

any potential negative bias sexual minorities may experience in the legal system: heterosexism and
aversive heterosexism. Heterosexism is a form of bias toward gays/lesbians that appears more blatant in
displays of discrimination, whereas aversive heterosexism appears as more of an implicit form of bias,
only appearing when sexual orientation is paired with another perceived negative characteristic. Partic-
ipants were randomly assigned to receive a trial transcript of either a heterosexual or gay/lesbian and
attractive/unattractive defendant. Participants were asked to render a verdict, recommend a sentence, and
provide guilt and culpability ratings. The theory of aversive heterosexism gained partial support from the
results. When the defendant was gay/lesbian and coupled with other another negative attribute (unat-
tractive) male jurors demonstrated bias.

Keywords: aversive heterosexism, heterosexism, juror decision making, gays and lesbians

Prejudice and discrimination toward gay men and lesbians has a male defendants are found guilty less often, receive lighter sen-
long history in the United States (Lehavot & Lambert, 2007). tences, and are less culpable compared to unattractive or male
However, given the changing dynamics in social behavior and defendants (Abwender & Hough, 2001). One area that has yet to
demand characteristics, blatant displays of prejudice toward this be thoroughly examined is the relationship among defendant sex-
group has decreased over the last few decades (Nadal, 2013). One ual orientation, defendant attractiveness, and juror gender to influ-
area of research regarding gay men and lesbians that has been ence juror decisions on legal outcomes.
given little attention is within the legal system. Specifically, the Given the current state of debate regarding gay and lesbian
effects of one particular defendant characteristic, defendant sexual rights and views toward gays and lesbians, defendant sexual ori-
orientation, on legal outcomes has been significantly understudied. entation may play a significant role in juror decision-making.
Defendant characteristics are well researched in the psychology However, there has yet to be an examination if this bias toward gay
and law literature because of their influence on juror decisions in male or lesbian defendants is best explained through explicit and
the U.S. criminal court process and the disproportionate numbers blatant prejudices or implicit and subtle prejudices. In addition, if
of incarcerated minority inmates (Espinoza, 2010; Espinoza & bias toward gay male or lesbian defendants is based on the com-
Willis-Esqueda, 2008). Among the more researched areas of de- bined effects of sexual orientation, attractiveness, and gender, what
fendant characteristics contributing to juror bias have been race, theoretical explanation would best describe such biases? The pur-
gender, socioeconomic status, and attractiveness (Mazzella & Fe- pose of this study is to explore the theories of heterosexism and
ingold, 1994). Regarding defendant attractiveness, more attractive aversive heterosexism to explain potential juror biases when de-
defendants receive lighter sentences and are less culpable com- fendant sexual orientation, defendant attractiveness, and juror gen-
pared to unattractive defendants (Patry, 2008). These findings also der varies.
hold when attractiveness and gender are combined: attractive fe-

Heterosexism
Heterosexism states that society’s sexual orientation default is
This article was published Online First December 14, 2017. heteronormativity (Neisen, 1990). Heterosexism also builds upon a
Jennifer V. Coons, Department of Psychology, University of California, hierarchy that heterosexism is superior to homosexuality or bisex-
Riverside; Russ K. E. Espinoza, Department of Psychology, California uality. Heterosexism is applied in everyday life and impacts indi-
State University, Fullerton.
viduals’ attitudes toward sexual minorities. Heterosexism does not
For more information on trial transcripts and manipulations please
contact Jennifer V. Coons.
just allow for discrimination toward sexual minorities but pro-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jennifer motes this discrimination. Heterosexism also assumes that every
V. Coons, Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside, individual is heterosexual until told otherwise and expects indi-
900 University Avenue, Riverside, CA, 92521, United States. E-mail: viduals who are a sexual minority to explain and defend their
jennifer.duncan.coons@gmail.com intimate relationships. In addition, since heterosexuality is as-

36
DEFENDANT SEXUAL ORIENTATION IN THE COURTROOM 37

sumed and extremely prevalent in society many gay males, lesbi- bias) and a subtler (implicit) bias. Little research has focused on
ans, and bisexual individuals internalize this phenomenon and may this second aspect of cognitive functioning, implicit bias, as it
experience inner conflict such as depression or alcoholism (Ama- pertains to homosexual defendants. One such theory that may help
dio, 2006). to explain the subtle/implicit bias LGBQ students reported expe-
Heterosexism is seen through the argumentation and support of riencing is aversive heterosexism.
laws as well. One can look at the legalization of gay marriage and
employment status laws by state to see that views toward homo-
Aversive Heterosexism
sexual individuals can be highly discriminatory. In the United
States of America an individual cannot be hired or fired based on As our society leans toward acceptance of gays and lesbians
gender, age, ethnicity, pregnancy, national origin, disability, mil- perhaps there is still an undercurrent of implicit bias that is
itary service, genetic information, or religious affiliation, however, occurring. One that exists out of conscious awareness of the
this does not include sexual orientation or gender identity at the perpetrator of this bias. What occurs when people hold generally
federal law level. Even when companies possess policies which egalitarian values and are explicitly not biased toward gays and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

protect sexual minorities, discrimination continues to occur. In lesbians? A modified version of the aversive racism theory may be
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

addition, some states have enacted laws regarding serving homo- appropriate here. Aversive racism states that because blatant rac-
sexual customers ensuring the rights of employers to refuse service ism is unacceptable by today’s society individuals internalize their
to homosexual patrons. Research on discrimination in the work- racism. Aversive racists act outwardly accepting, hold egalitarian
place has found that sexual minorities are discriminated against values, and go out of their way to not appear racist. Those who fall
regarding suitability for a job. Particularly, heterosexual males in this category often feel uncomfortable finding fault with racial
indicate a lower level of job suitability for potential employees minorities and subsequently will only show bias to these persons
who are gay males (Pichler, Varma, & Bruce, 2010). when another perceived negative variable is apparent (Dovidio &
Heterosexism may therefore be extended to understand how Gaertner, 1986).
gays and lesbians may be blatantly discriminated against as de- Because Dovidio and Gaertner’s (1986) conception of contem-
fendants in a criminal court case. Because heterosexuality is the porary prejudice toward African Americans by egalitarian Euro-
default and accepted sexual orientation, heterosexism would indi- pean Americans the theory of aversive racism has been used to
cate that no matter what other circumstances exist, gays and explain prejudice toward Latinos (Mastro, Behm-Morawitz, &
lesbians would be judged more harshly in a criminal court case Kopacz, 2008), toward the disabled (Deal, 2007), and in applied
than heterosexual defendants. Because this form of bias would be settings such as employment (Gilbert & Lownes-Jackson, 2005)
based on the aspect of sexual orientation alone, heterosexism may and the legal system (Espinoza & Willis-Esqueda, 2008; Minero &
only account for explicit or blatant bias toward gays and lesbians Espinoza, 2016; Willis-Esqueda, Espinoza, & Culhane, 2008).
as this way of thinking openly accepts and champions individuals However, little research has examined if aversive prejudice is
who exclude gay men and lesbians as a part of the “normal” applicable to gay men and lesbians in the legal system.
society. The current study aims to expand the theory of aversive preju-
One study examined differences between subtle biases and dice to gay men and lesbians in the legal setting. Sexual orientation
blatant biases toward gay men and lesbians. Hong, Woodford, has been a popular topic in recent years in both the media and legal
Long, and Renn (2016) examined subtle and blatant heterosexist settings (e.g., gay marriage). This popularity has led to increased
discrimination experienced by lesbian, gay, bisexual, and queer acceptance of gay males and lesbians within society and less
(LGBQ) college students in the United States. LGBQ students acceptability of blatant bias and discrimination toward gays and
reported experiencing both subtle, microaggressions and blatant lesbians (although it still occurs frequently). Because aversive
forms of discrimination on college campuses, demonstrating a racism concerns the internalization of racism due to the unaccept-
culture of heteronormativity by college students. Though these ability of outward racism by society it is proposed that aversive
researchers did not specifically examine the holders of the bias heterosexism is the implicit bias toward gays and lesbians because
(they examined the recipients of the bias), they did distinguish explicit bias is unacceptable by the more recent (and future)
between two different types of bias the recipients perceived the leaders who create and uphold society’s standards. Following
holders were displaying—subtle, microaggressions and blatant Dovidio and Gaertner’s (2000) protocol, Aberson (2003) assessed
displays of discrimination and prejudice. The interesting notion of the effects of sexual orientation during the hiring process. Aberson
these findings was that Hong et al. (2016), described these subtle, used gay males or heterosexual males as job candidates rather than
microaggressions as somewhat unconscious acts by perpetrators, Black or Caucasian males (and did not include the racism scales).
and blatant displays of discrimination as conscious acts toward Participants were asked how appropriate a job candidate was for an
LGBQ students. The authors did not examine the implicit nature of HIV/AIDS awareness program (who had been tested for but did
these biases by the perpetrators, nor compare the cognitive differ- not contract HIV) with the job candidate’s sexual orientation and
ences of these two biases. However, if the microaggressions, as qualifications manipulated. Results showed the predicted main
Hong et al. (2016) explained, are sometimes subtle and even effect of job qualification on job appropriateness, however, there
unconscious manifestations of bias, then there appears to be two was no interaction of job qualification and candidate sexual ori-
types of cognitive functioning going on— one for subtle displays entation. Results also indicated no significant differences between
(or implicit) and one for blatant displays (or explicit) of bias. heterosexual males and gay males on appropriateness of fit for the
Results of the Hong et al. (2016) study suggests that at the very current job (Aberson, 2003).
least, LGBQ students report that they are the recipients of two Although these results seem positive for gay males there is one
types of discrimination: heterosexism guided bias (overt/explicit flaw in the design: the job position created was for an HIV/AIDS
38 COONS AND ESPINOZA

awareness educator which may have significantly confounded the secondary characteristic to assess if heterosexism or aversive het-
study. As Aberson (2003) stated, the gay male candidates may erosexism was the driving force behind juror decision making. In
have received more positive job recommendations due to the addition, because gender plays a role in juror decision making it
historical tie between gay men and HIV/AIDS. Therefore, it is was included in the current study.
important to evaluate other scenarios that may be influenced by an
individuals’ sexual orientation in relation to life changing out-
Defendant Sexual Orientation
comes. Of interest in the current study are the effects of sexual
orientation in the courtroom. It is assumed, through the lens of To understand the implicit and/or subtle link between specific
aversive heterosexism, that gay and lesbian defendants will not be stereotypes, Deaux and Lewis (1984) examined how one stereo-
blatantly viewed negatively by individuals when they adhere to type can connect to others and how one piece of information can
other acceptable standards of society (e.g., attractive). Only when overrule gender identification. In a series of studies, they found
sexual orientation is coupled with other negative characteristics participants were more likely to ascribe gay male or lesbian labels
will this bias emerge. to persons who were described as having traits that contradicted
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

the typical masculine and feminine traits. They also found that
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

judgments of male sexuality are more harshly adhered to. If traits


Attractiveness and Gender
are inconsistent for males, there is a higher likelihood they are
No secondary negative characteristics are needed to evaluate labeled as a gay male. Because of these findings, the authors
heterosexism in the legal system, however, to evaluate aversive stipulate that once people are given one piece of information they
heterosexism in the legal system jurors need other perceived will develop/create other gender related information and attribute
negative variables to justify any biases toward gay men and it to that person, specifically toward higher likelihoods of certain
lesbians. One such variable that has been documented as leading to occupation and sexual orientation.
positive or negative perceptions in the legal system is defendant To examine the different views toward differing types of gay
attractiveness. Attractive defendants are treated less punitively in males, Laner and Laner (1979) conducted a study which included
juror decisions than unattractive defendants, and attractive victims three different types of gay males: effeminate (hypomasculine),
are given more credibility compared with unattractive victims noneffeminate (masculine), and hypermasculine who were rated
(Mazzella & Feingold, 1994). For example, Vrij and Firmin (2001) on general likability. The highest overall likability rating was for
found that attractive victims of rape are given higher credibility the gay male who had a “standard masculine” appearance, indi-
ratings and lower responsibility ratings. Furthermore, when the cating that acceptability for gay males is most likely when they
defendant was attractive, the perceived evidence was considered appear like any other man rather than the extreme effeminate or
weaker for unattractive female victims. In addition, attractive hypermasculine. This study shows that when gay males try to
defendants are given more lenient sentences compared to their blend in they would be viewed more favorably by heterosexuals
counterparts (Desantts & Kayson, 1997). DeSantis and Kayson and less likely to be judged harshly.
proposed that this effect may have been observed because attrac- People tend to use consistency among information to render
tive individuals are assessed to have more socially desirable traits judgments of people; therefore, inconsistency creates a problem. If
and that they may have more to lose than unattractive individuals. females and their sexual orientation is judged differently (i.e., less
Evaluating the combined effects of attractiveness and gender for static) than male’s sexuality it may indicate less conflict in fe-
juror decisions Ahola, Hellstrom, and Christianson (2010) as- male’s gender roles. Without this conflict females may be more
sessed the extent to which written and/or visual information of a favorably viewed in the legal system. However, male sexual ori-
crime scene, defendant gender, and defendant appearance im- entation is more rigid and conflict may lead to harsher outcomes in
pacted criminal court outcomes. In those cases where a photograph the legal system if they appear to be gay.
was included in the crime description harsher bias was elicited to Some individuals believe that homosexuality is a learned be-
the crime itself and to the defendant. Furthermore, harshest pun- havior and therefore may be judged more harshly because they
ishment was given for male defendants overall and attractive believe that gay males and lesbians can change their sexual ori-
female defendants were given the shortest sentencing. entation. Aguero, Bloch, and Byrne (1984) examined if there were
Juror gender may also play a key role in the juror decision different attitudes and behaviors exhibited toward gay males de-
making process when defendant attractiveness is varied. To assess pendent on whether people believed that homosexuality is learned
the interaction of juror gender, defendant attractiveness, juror race, or physiological in nature. Participants completed the Sexual Opin-
and defendant race on verdict and sentencing decisions, Abwender ion Survey, which measures restrictions of sex roles, participants’
and Hough (2001) conducted a study in which an intoxicated sexual behavior, attitudes and beliefs about homosexuality, and
woman (Black or Caucasian and attractive or unattractive) hit and assessment of reaction to research on sexuality. Surprisingly, re-
killed a pedestrian while driving. Male participants gave a longer sults showed that males reported a more liberal view of sexuality
sentence for attractive defendants whereas women gave longer (although this may be a sign of the times in which these female
sentences to unattractive defendants. Pozzulo, Dempsey, Maeder, participants were unable to express their potential openness toward
and Allen (2010) found that male defendants receive higher guilt sexuality) but that males had more dislike toward gay males and
ratings but additionally found that female jurors gave higher guilt lesbians. In general, however, those with higher negative affect
ratings for all defendants than male jurors in a sexual assault of a scores and believed homosexuality was a learned behavior had the
child case indicating that juror gender interacts with crime com- highest scores of dislike toward gay males and lesbians. Further-
mitted on decision making. Based on the somewhat inconsistent more, males who believed homosexuality is a learned behavior
results from these studies, defendant attractiveness was used as a reported lower levels of acceptance of a friend who was a gay
DEFENDANT SEXUAL ORIENTATION IN THE COURTROOM 39

male. This study indicates that males have a higher disapproval of Materials and Procedure
homosexuality, especially within males who believe that homo-
sexuality is a learned behavior and can therefore be unlearned. Participants were instructed to read a criminal court case, render
Though the Aguero et al. (1984) article was over 30 years ago, a verdict, provide sentencing length, and answer other questions
the results are still applicable today. Joslyn and Haider-Markel regarding the case and the defendant. Participants were randomly
(2016) examined genetics as an explanation for homosexuality and assigned to receive a trial transcript of either an attractive/unat-
found that when homosexuality is explained through genetics, and tractive and heterosexual or gay/lesbian defendant who was being
tried for the murder of their significant other.
that this sexual orientation cannot be changed, persons felt less
The trial transcript’s front page included a 2” ⫻ 3” black and
prejudice toward homosexuals. However, if persons did not attri-
white photograph to garner attention to the defendants’ attractive-
bute homosexuality to genetics, then prejudice toward this group
ness level. The pictures were piloted (n ⫽ 31 unique participants
increased. What these two articles demonstrate is that prejudice
from the same university as those who completed the study) and
toward gay men and lesbians has not changed for a large percent-
chosen out of 50 total photographs for the most attractive and
age of society, regardless of any scientific, genetic evidence ex-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

unattractive photographs. The photographs were rated on a 1–9


plaining the orientation as unchangeable.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

point Likert-scale, ranging from 1 (being unattractive) to 9 (being


What has changed may be the outward response to gay men and
attractive). The photograph chosen for the attractive male defen-
lesbians. As acceptability of gay and lesbians increases in our
dant had a mean attractiveness rating of 7.94 (SD ⫽ 1.12) and the
society blatant prejudice should be decreasing. If blatant prejudice
photograph chosen for the unattractive male defendant had a mean
is decreasing it can either be disappearing altogether or hidden
attractiveness rating of 1.15 (SD ⫽ 1.51) in the pilot study. The
under the surface of acceptance. If prejudice toward gay men and
photograph chosen for the attractive female defendant had a mean
lesbians is not disappearing altogether but is in hiding at an
attractiveness rating of 7.77 (SD ⫽ 1.12) and the photograph
implicit level this may lend support to aversive heterosexism. This
chosen for the unattractive female defendant had a mean attrac-
study attempts to compare two theories (heterosexism and aversive
tiveness rating of 1.61 (SD ⫽ 1.02) in the pilot study. Although
heterosexism) to explain potential discriminatory practices toward
these photographs were used to manipulate attractiveness partici-
gays and lesbians in the legal system.
pants perceived attractiveness was used for the analyses. In the
current study, the overall attractiveness rating for the attractive
Research Questions male defendants was 5.25 (SD ⫽ 2.21, n ⫽ 129), the unattrac-
tive male defendant was 3.70 (SD ⫽ 1.77, n ⫽ 125), the attractive
The effect of stigma, or these observed negative biases, on female defendant was 5.76 (SD ⫽ 2.021, n ⫽ 121), and the
defendants radically affects their guilt outcomes and sexual mi- unattractive female defendant was 2.61 (SD ⫽ 1.59, n ⫽ 119).
norities face a strong reaction by individuals who may practice The trial transcript included a description of the charges brought
sexual prejudice, which may still be prevalent today (Herek, 2000). to the defendant (murder of their significant other), summaries of
If heterosexism is the primary explanation for bias toward gay the prosecution and defense, defendant background information,
males and lesbians in the courtroom, then we would expect only eye-witness testimony, and the California state capital case infor-
sexual orientation of the defendant to predict harsher punishment mation regarding verdict and sentencing process. The only differ-
(more guilty verdicts, longer sentencing lengths, greater culpabil- ences between the trial transcripts were the photo of the defendant
ity, and more negative characteristics assessments). However, if (to manipulate attractiveness and gender), the name of the defen-
aversive heterosexism is the primary explanation for this bias then dant and victim, the pronouns used throughout (to manipulate
we would expect these harsher punishments to occur only when gender and sexual orientation) and defendant background infor-
defendant sexual orientation is coupled with other negative, but mation (to ensure that the manipulation of sexual orientation
socially acceptable variables (unattractiveness), or influenced by would not be ignored); all other information was identical.
social roles of gender. For example, jurors are expected to rate the After rendering a verdict, participants were asked to recommend
unattractive gay/lesbian defendant more harshly than all other sentencing length, if applicable, of life in prison with the possi-
conditions (i.e., attractive gay/lesbian defendants). The purpose of bility of parole after 20 years, life in prison without parole, or
the current study is to assess the unique effects of these stigmata, death by lethal injection. Regardless of the participant’s dichoto-
individually as well as collectively, on defendant outcomes. mous Guilty/Not Guilty verdict and the three provided sentencing
length options, participants were asked to indicate on a 1 to 7
Likert-scale how guilty they thought the defendant was, ranging
Method from 1 (not guilt at all) to 7 (completely guilty) and how culpable
for the crime (responsibility for crime, belief in the defendant’s
Participants version of the crime [reverse-scored], blame, and intentionality of
the crime committed) the defendant was on a 7-point Likert-type
Using a format similar to previous research (Espinoza & Willis- rating scale. In addition, participants were asked to indicate how
Esqueda, 2008; Willis-Esqueda et al. 2008), a total of 494 Cali- attractive the defendant was on a 7-point Likert-type rating scale.
fornia State University, Fullerton students acted as mock jurors Attractiveness was included here to serve two purposes: as a
and read a trial transcript (219 males and 275 females; mean age ⫽ manipulation check and to include participant rated attractiveness
19.20 years, SD ⫽ 2.12). Participants identified themselves as in the statistical analyses rather than ascribe perceived defendant
European American (30%), Latino American (29.1%), or Asian attractiveness based off pilot data. Only participants who passed
American (28.5%). the manipulation check (correctly identifying defendant sexual
40 COONS AND ESPINOZA

orientation) were included in the analyses. The trial transcript of Sexual Orientation, Perceived Attractiveness, and
the study was 12 pages and took no longer than 30 min to Defendant Outcomes
complete.
The first step assessed how sexual orientation and perceived
attractiveness were related to verdict, recommended sentencing
Results
length, perceived guilt, and perceived culpability. Sexual orienta-
The current study expected that less attractive gay/lesbian de- tion was not related to verdict, ␹2(1) ⫽ 2.56, p ⫽ .110, ␸ ⫽ .072
fendants would be given harsher outcomes as compared to their nor to recommended sentencing length, ␹2(2) ⫽ 2.82, p ⫽ .244.
counterparts and that this would be different for male and female Heterosexual defendants were perceived as significantly less guilty
mock jurors. If only the main effects of sexual orientation and/or (M ⫽ 4.03, SD ⫽ 1.89) than gay/lesbian defendants (M ⫽ 4.45,
attractiveness were significant, and there was an absence of an SD ⫽ 1.70), t(492) ⫽ 2.60, p ⫽ .010, d ⫽ .233. Heterosexual
interaction, this would lend support to heterosexism as the best defendants were also perceived as significantly less culpable (M ⫽
explanation for juror bias toward homosexual defendants. How- 4.21, SD ⫽ 1.78) than gay/lesbian defendants (M ⫽ 4.60, SD ⫽
1.64), t(492) ⫽ 2.52, p ⫽ .012, d ⫽ .228, for the crime.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ever, if a significant interaction is found this would signify support


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

for aversive heterosexism. The outcome variables of interest were Perceived attractiveness was significantly different for those
overall verdict, recommended sentencing length, perceived guilt, found guilty (M ⫽ 4.76, SD ⫽ 2.17) versus not guilty (M ⫽ 4.02,
and perceived culpability ratings. The four culpability measures SD ⫽ 2.31), t(492) ⫽ 3.63, p ⬍ .001, d ⫽ .330, but not regarding
(responsibility, belief in defendant’s version of the crime [reverse- sentencing length, F(2, 272) ⫽ .649, p ⫽ .523, partial ␩2 ⫽ .005.
scored], blame, and intentionality) were combined to create a Perceived attractiveness was significantly correlated with per-
single item to measure culpability (alpha ⫽ .90). Sexual orienta- ceived guilt, r(492) ⫽ ⫺.210, p ⬍ .001, and perceived culpability,
tion was effect coded (⫺1 ⫽ heterosexual defendants) and per- r(492) ⫽ ⫺.239, p ⬍ .001. Finally, perceived attractiveness did
ceived attractiveness was mean centered. The interaction term was not differ significantly between heterosexual (M ⫽ 4.30, SD ⫽
created by multiplying the effect coded sexual orientation variable 2.24) and gay/lesbian defendants (M ⫽ 4.39, SD ⫽ 2.32), t(492) ⫽
by the mean centered perceived attractiveness variable. .452, p ⫽ .651, d ⫽ 039.
The total sample consisted of 494 participants who were ran-
domly assigned to one of four conditions: heterosexual and attrac-
Guilt Ratings
tive, heterosexual and unattractive, gay/lesbian and attractive, and
gay/lesbian and unattractive. Two-hundred forty-four mock jurors The next set of analyses assessed how sexual orientation and
received a trial packet with a heterosexual defendant and 250 mock perceived attractiveness predicted perceived guilt ratings for the
jurors received a trial packet with an attractive defendant. Mock full sample and then by mock juror gender. Sexual orientation was
jurors, regardless of trial packet received, rated the defendants as effect coded (⫺1 reflecting heterosexual defendants) and per-
moderately attractive (M ⫽ 4.35, SD ⫽ 2.78, range ⫽ 1–9). ceived attractiveness was mean centered before creating the inter-
Two-hundred and 75 mock jurors found the defendant guilty; of action term (the product of sexual orientation and perceived at-
these, 152 recommended a sentencing length of parole after 20 tractiveness). The multiple regression analysis included sexual
years (109 recommended life without parole). Mock jurors per- orientation, perceived attractiveness, and the interaction between
ceived all defendants as moderately guilty (M ⫽ 4.24, SD ⫽ 1.79, sexual orientation and perceived attractiveness. Table 1 summa-
range ⫽ 1–7) and culpable (M ⫽ 4.41, SD ⫽ 1.72, range ⫽ 1–7) rizes the regression analysis results which indicates that there is a
for the crime regardless of the verdict or sentencing length. main effect of sexual orientation and perceived attractiveness,

Table 1
Multiple Regression Predicting Perceived Guilt

Variable b (SE) ␤ t p

Constant 4.277 (.078) 54.635 ⬍.001


Sexual orientation .197 (.078) .111 2.519 .012
Perceived attractiveness ⫺.170 (.034) ⫺.217 ⫺4.961 ⬍.001
SO ⫻ Perceived attractiveness .075 (.034) .096 2.154 .029
Male jurors (n ⫽ 219)
Constant 4.143 (.120) 34.479 ⬍.001
Sexual orientation .331 (.120) .180 2.758 .006
Perceived attractiveness ⫺.176 (.056) ⫺.207 ⫺3.129 .002
SO ⫻ Perceived attractiveness .121 (.056) .142 2.145 .033
Female jurors (n ⫽ 275)
Constant 4.380 (.104) 42.271 ⬍.001
Sexual orientation .106 (.144) .061 1.021 .308
Perceived attractiveness ⫺.180 (.043) ⫺.243 ⫺4.154 ⬍.001
SO ⫻ Perceived attractiveness .054 (.043) .073 1.232 .219
Note. SO ⫽ Sexual orientation and is effect coded (heterosexual ⫽ ⫺1, gay/lesbian ⫽ 1). Perceived
Attractiveness is mean centered, N ⫽ 494. Average guilt rating for the full sample was 4.24 (SD ⫽ 1.79), for
male jurors it was 4.17 (SD ⫽ 1.84), and for female jurors it was 4.30 (SD ⫽ 1.74).
DEFENDANT SEXUAL ORIENTATION IN THE COURTROOM 41

specifically, heterosexual and attractive defendants are perceived conducted. The multiple regression model with sexual orientation,
as less guilty. However, there was also a significant interaction perceived attractiveness, and the interaction term for male mock
term, indicating that attractive, heterosexual defendants are per- jurors predicting culpability was significant, R ⫽ .315, F(3,
ceived as the least guilty, R ⫽ .260, F(3, 490) ⫽ 11.853, p ⬍ .001 215) ⫽ 7.890, p ⬍ .001 (see Table 2). The multiple regression
(see Table 1). model with sexual orientation, perceived attractiveness, and the
Next, we ran two additional multiple regression models to interaction term for female mock jurors was also significant, R ⫽
determine if this model was different for male and female mock .309, F(3, 271) ⫽ 9.529, p ⬍ .001 (see Table 2). Again, results
jurors. The multiple regression model with sexual orientation, from the male mock jurors were like the full sample model and the
perceived attractiveness, and the interaction between sexual orien- results from the female mock jurors were different from the overall
tation and perceived attractiveness for male mock jurors predicting and male mock juror models. Specifically, sexual orientation and
guilt ratings was significant, R ⫽ .288, F(3, 215) ⫽ 6.483, p ⬍ the interaction between sexual orientation and perceived attrac-
.001 (see Table 1). The multiple regression model with sexual tiveness are no longer significant predictors of sentencing ratings
orientation, perceived attractiveness, and the interaction term for for female mock jurors.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

female mock jurors was also significant, R ⫽ .266, F(3, 271) ⫽


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

6.860, p ⬍ .001 (see Table 1). Results from the male mock jurors Discussion
were like the full sample model, however, results from the female
mock jurors are rather different from the overall and male mock The current study was conducted to determine how bias toward
juror models. Specifically, sexual orientation and the interaction gay men and lesbians occurs in the legal system. Minority defen-
between sexual orientation and perceived attractiveness are no dants experience bias in the legal system regarding verdict and
longer significant predictors of sentencing ratings for female mock culpability (Espinoza & Willis-Esqueda, 2008); however, sexual
jurors. minority defendants is a minority group that has not been well
researched in the literature. Previous literature that evaluated the
role of sexual orientation focused on only gay men or on chal-
Culpability Ratings
lenging topics related specifically to sexual orientation, for exam-
The final set of analyses assessed how sexual orientation and ple, gay men assaulting heterosexual men or gay men sexually
perceived attractiveness predicted perceived culpability ratings for assaulting a child (Hill, 2000; Wiley & Bottoms, 2009). Although
the full sample and then by mock juror gender. The multiple bias toward gay men in the legal system has been somewhat
regression analysis included sexual orientation, perceived attrac- documented, bias toward lesbians in the legal system has been
tiveness, and the interaction between sexual orientation and per- sparse. Most of the previous literature indicates a strong bias
ceived attractiveness. Table 2 summarizes the regression analysis toward gay men by male participants and about an equal amount of
results which indicates that there is a main effect of sexual orien- bias toward both gay men and lesbians by female participants. The
tation and perceived attractiveness, specifically, heterosexual and results of this study adds to this area of research by offering results
attractive defendants are perceived as less culpable. However, important to both gay and lesbian members of society.
there is also a significant interaction term, indicating that attrac- Two competing theories were examined to explain bias toward
tive, heterosexual defendants are perceived as the least culpable, gay men and lesbians within the legal system; heterosexism and
R ⫽ .288, F(3, 490) ⫽ 14.735, p ⬍ .001 (see Table 2). aversive heterosexism. Heterosexism holds that society’s view of
To determine if this model was different for male and female the world is heterosexual in nature; heterosexuality is the norm for
mock jurors two additional multiple regression analyses were individuals or is the default in how individuals think about the

Table 2
Multiple Regression Predicting Perceived Culpability

Variable b (SE) ␤ t p

Constant 4.447 (.075) 59.439 ⬍.001


Sexual orientation .182 (.075) .106 2.439 .015
Perceived attractiveness ⫺.186 (.033) ⫺.249 ⫺5.690 ⬍.001
SO ⫻ Perceived attractiveness .082 (.053) .109 2.504 .013
Male jurors (n ⫽ 219)
Constant 4.312 (.114) 37.919 ⬍.001
Sexual orientation .330 (.114) .188 2.900 .004
Perceived attractiveness ⫺.174 (.053) ⫺.214 ⫺3.255 .001
SO ⫻ Perceived attractiveness .151 (.053) .186 2.829 .005
Female jurors (n ⫽ 275)
Constant 4.556 (.099) 45.887 ⬍.001
Sexual orientation .087 (.009) .052 .879 .380
Perceived attractiveness ⫺.213 (.042) ⫺.295 ⫺5.108 ⬍.001
SO ⫻ Perceived attractiveness .044 (.042) .062 1.050 .295
Note. Sexual orientation is effect coded (heterosexual ⫽ ⫺1, gay/lesbian ⫽ 1). Perceived attractiveness is
mean centered, N ⫽ 494. Average culpability rating for the full sample was 4.41 (SD ⫽ 1.72), for male jurors
it was 4.34 (SD ⫽ 1.76), and for female jurors it was 4.46 (SD ⫽ 1.69).
42 COONS AND ESPINOZA

world and that people who do not conform to heterosexuality are exists today even with greater acceptance toward homosexuality
engaging in deviant behaviors and deserve to be punished. If (Andersen & Fetner, 2008). Although the end result, bias and
heterosexism best explained biases toward gay men and lesbian discrimination, is similar for both theories (either explicit— het-
defendants then sexual orientation would predict overt, negative erosexism— or implicit—aversive heterosexism), these results
bias toward gay men and lesbians as it would be considered suggest that we may want to try different intervention strategies to
acceptable by society. The current study did find support for reduce both the explicit and implicit negative views toward gays/
heterosexism; mock jurors did find the sexual minority defendants lesbians such as increased contact between heterosexual and ho-
(gay male or lesbian) guilty more often than heterosexual defen- mosexual individuals.
dants. However, the current study also found that there was a
significant interaction between sexual orientation and perceived Limitations and Future Research
attractiveness and that this relationship is different for male and
female mock jurors. Female jurors did not perceive gay/lesbian One major limitation of the current study is that the participants
defendants guiltier or more culpable for the crime in question. came from a liberal college population. Attitudes toward gay men
and lesbians are correlated with age and educational level; older
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Based on these results, the theory of heterosexism does not appear


and less educated individuals possess more negative attitudes
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

to be a sufficient explanation for the biases that may exist for gay
men and lesbian defendants. toward gay men and lesbians (Herek, 1994). Therefore, because of
the sample used in the current study, future research should com-
pare these findings with a general population. A second limitation
Evidence for Aversive Heterosexism was the lack of overall guilty ratings. To truly determine the
The theory of aversive heterosexism, on the other hand, postu- differences in guilt and culpability ratings between gay men,
lates that society’s views on acceptance of negative bias toward lesbian, and heterosexual defendants the defendant should have
gay men and lesbians are diminishing and people generally hold been found guilty more often than seen in this study. Had the
egalitarian views toward gay men and lesbians. This is especially defendant been found overwhelmingly guilty, detecting the driving
true for younger and educated individuals (Andersen & Fetner, force (i.e., heterosexism or aversive heterosexism) for bias toward
2008). As applied to the legal system, this theory holds that jurors gay men and lesbians may have been more fully understood. One
will not demonstrate blatant bias toward minority defendants based way to ensure that most participants would have rendered a guilty
on sexual orientation alone. Only when sexual orientation is cou- decision would have been to create a criminal case where the
pled with another perceived negative variable (e.g., unattractive) evidence was staggering against the defendant (e.g., surveillance
will jurors show bias because the implicit aversiveness they may videotape, DNA evidence, etc.).
feel will no longer be based solely on sexual orientation (Espinoza In conclusion, it may be that neither heterosexism nor aversive
& Willis-Esqueda, 2008; Minero & Espinoza, 2016). heterosexism adequately explains bias toward gay men and lesbian
Evidence in support of aversive heterosexism was found in the defendants in the U.S. legal system. Perhaps the shifting standards
current study. Attractiveness was included to act as catalyst for and views toward gay men and lesbian individuals have yet to be
aversive heterosexism. For the full sample sexual orientation, clearly articulated— especially in the legal decision-making liter-
perceived attractiveness, and the interaction between sexual orien- ature. It was the attempt of this research to explore these shifting
tation and perceived attractiveness significantly predicted per- standards as they apply in the legal system and how the intertwin-
ceived guilt and culpability ratings. However, perceived guilt and ing effects of sexual orientation, attractiveness, and gender effect
culpability models were different for male and female mock jurors. juror decision making. Results showed that explicit and implicit
Only for male mock jurors did the interaction between sexual biases toward gays and lesbians in the legal system do exist, and
orientation and perceived attractiveness predict greater guilt and that given the combined effects of sexual orientation, gender and
culpability ratings (evidence for aversive heterosexism) along with attractiveness, aversive heterosexism may best explain these im-
significant main effects for both (evidence for heterosexism). Only plicit biases. Only future examination will help clarify the potential
perceived attractiveness predicted greater perceived guilt and cul- biases gay men and lesbians may face in the United States criminal
pability ratings for female mock jurors. This suggests that males justice system.
may be experiencing both heterosexism and aversive heterosexism
as they had a greater issue with the gay/lesbian defendants than References
females did which agrees with prior work (Deaux & Lewis, 1984). Aberson, C. L. (2003). Aversive bias toward gay men? Current Research
One reason this may have occurred is that females may be more in Social Psychology, 8, 266 –274.
accepting of gay/lesbians since they have less rigid gender roles in Abwender, D. A., & Hough, K. (2001). Interactive effects of characteristics
our changing society than males do (Kray, Howland, Russell, & of defendant and mock juror on U.S. participants’ judgment and sen-
Jackman, 2017). tencing recommendations. The Journal of Social Psychology, 141, 603–
These results partly discredit heterosexism as the sole explana- 615. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224540109600574
tory theory for bias toward gay men and lesbians within the legal Ahola, A. S., Hellstrom, A., & Christianson, S. A. (2010). Is justice really
blind? Effects of crime descriptions, defendant gender, and appearance,
system and partially support, at least for males, aversive hetero-
and legal practitioner gender on sentences and defendants evaluations in
sexism. Results of the guilt ratings and culpability ratings indicate a mock trial. Psychiatry, Psychology and Law, 17, 304 –324. http://dx
that, regardless of verdict, gay male and lesbian defendants are .doi.org/10.1080/13218710903566896
receiving greater perceived guilt and culpability when coupled Amadio, D. M. (2006). Internalized heterosexism, alcohol use, and
with other negative characteristics for male mock jurors. These alcohol-related problems among lesbians and gay men. Addictive Be-
results indicate that negative bias toward gay men and lesbians haviors, 31, 1153–1162. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.08.013
DEFENDANT SEXUAL ORIENTATION IN THE COURTROOM 43

Andersen, R., & Fetner, T. (2008). Cohort differences in tolerance of Laner, M. R., & Laner, R. H. (1979). Personal style or sexual preference:
homosexuality: Attitudinal change in Canada and the United States, Why gay men are disliked. International Review of Modern Sociology,
1981–2000. Public Opinion Quarterly, 72, 311–330. http://dx.doi.org/ 9, 215–228.
10.1093/poq/nfn017 Lehavot, K., & Lambert, A. J. (2007). Toward a greater understanding of
Aguero, J. E., Bloch, L., & Byrne, D. (1984). The relationships among anti-gay prejudice: On the role of sexual orientation and gender role
sexual beliefs, attitudes, experience, and homophobia. Journal of Ho- violation. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 29, 279 –292. http://dx
mosexuality, 10(1-2), 95–107. .doi.org/10.1080/01973530701503390
Deal, M. (2007). Aversive disablism: Subtle prejudice toward disabled Mastro, D. E., Behm-Morawitz, E., & Kopacz, M. (2008). Exposure to
people. Disability & Society, 22, 1–14. television portrayals of Latinos: The implications of aversive racism and
Deaux, K., & Lewis, L. L. (1984). Structure of gender stereotypes: Inter- social identity theory. Human Communication Research, 34, 1–27.
relationships among components and gender label. Journal of Person- http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2007.00311.x
ality and Social Psychology, 46, 991–1004. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ Mazzella, R., & Feingold, A. (1994). The effects of physical attractiveness,
0022-3514.46.5.991 race, socioeconomic status, and gender of defendants and victims on
Desantts, A., & Kayson, W. A. (1997). Defendants’ characteristics of judgments of mock jurors: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Social
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

attractiveness, race, and sex and sentencing decisions. Psychological Psychology, 24, 1315–1338. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1994
Reports, 81, 679 – 683. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1997.81.2.679
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

.tb01552.x
Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (1986). Prejudice, discrimination and Minero, L. P., & Espinoza, R. K. E. (2016). The influence of defendant
racism. Orlando, FL: Academic. immigration status, country of origin, and ethnicity on juror decisions:
Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (2000). Aversive racism and selection An aversive racism explanation for juror bias. Hispanic Journal of
decisions: 1989 and 1999. Psychological Science, 11, 315–319. http:// Behavioral Sciences, 38, 55–74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/073998
dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00262 6315620374
Espinoza, R. K. E. (2010). Juror bias in the U.S. legal system: Examining Nadal, K. (2013). That’s so gay! Microaggressions and the lesbian, gay,
problems and solutions. Saarbrücken, Germany: Lambert Academic bisexual and transgender community. Washington, DC: American Psy-
Publishing.
chological Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14093-000
Espinoza, R. K. E., & Willis-Esqueda, C. (2008). Defendant and defense
Neisen, J. H. (1990). Heterosexism: Redefining homophobia for the 1990s.
attorney characteristics and their effects on juror decision making and
Journal of Gay & Lesbian Psychotherapy, 3, 21–35. http://dx.doi.org/
prejudice against Mexican Americans. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic
10.1300/J236v01n03_02
Minority Psychology, 14, 364 –371. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0012767
Patry, M. W. (2008). Attractive but guilty: Deliberation and the physical
Gilbert, J. A., & Lownes-Jackson, M. (2005). Blacks, whites, and the new
attractiveness bias. Psychological Reports, 102, 727–733. http://dx.doi
prejudice: Does aversive racism impact employee assessment? Journal
.org/10.2466/pr0.102.3.727-733
of Applied Social Psychology, 35, 1540 –1553. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
Pichler, S., Varma, A., & Bruce, T. (2010). Heterosexism in employment
j.1559-1816.2005.tb02183.x
decisions: The role of job misfit. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,
Herek, G. M. (1994). Assessing heterosexuals’ attitudes toward lesbians
40, 2527–2555. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2010.00669.x
and gay men: A review of empirical research with the ATLG scale. In
Pozzulo, J. D., Dempsey, J., Maeder, E., & Allen, L. (2010). The effects of
B. Greene, G. M. Herek, B. Greene, & G. M. Herek (Eds.), Lesbian and
gay psychology: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 206 – victim gender, defendant gender, and defendant age on juror decision
228). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/ making. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 37, 47– 63. http://dx.doi.org/10
9781483326757.n11 .1177/0093854809344173
Herek, G. M. (2000). The psychology of sexual prejudice. Current Direc- Vrij, A., & Firmin, H. R. (2001). Beautiful thus innocent? The impact of
tions in Psychological Science, 9, 19 –22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ defendants’ and victims’ physical attractiveness and participants’ rape
1467-8721.00051 beliefs on impression formation in alleged rape cases. International
Hill, J. M. (2000). The effects of sexual orientation in the courtroom: A Review of Victimology, 8, 245–255. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/02697
double standard. Journal of Homosexuality, 39, 93–111. http://dx.doi 5800100800301
.org/10.1300/J082v39n02_05 Wiley, T. R., & Bottoms, B. L. (2009). Effects of defendant sexual
Hong, J. S., Woodford, M. R., Long, L. D., & Renn, K. A. (2016). orientation on jurors’ perceptions of child sexual assault. Law and
Ecological covariates of subtle and blatant heterosexist discrimination Human Behavior, 33, 46 – 60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10979-008-
among LGBQ college students. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 45, 9131-2
117–131. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-015-0362-5 Willis-Esqueda, C., Espinoza, R. K. E., & Culhane, S. (2008). The effects
Joslyn, M. R., & Haider-Markel, D. P. (2016). Genetic attributions, im- of ethnicity, SES and crime status on juror decision making: A cross-
mutability, and stereotypic judgments: An analysis of homosexuality. cultural examination of European American and Mexican American
Social Science Quarterly, 97, 376 –390. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ssqu mock jurors. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 30, 181–199.
.12263 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0739986308315319
Kray, L. J., Howland, L., Russell, A. G., & Jackman, L. M. (2017). The
effects of implicit gender role theories on gender system justification:
Fixed beliefs strengthen masculinity to preserve the status quo. Journal Received June 2, 2017
of Personality and Social Psychology, 112, 98 –115. http://dx.doi.org/10 Revision received September 24, 2017
.1037/pspp0000124 Accepted September 26, 2017 䡲

View publication stats

You might also like