You are on page 1of 16

Roeper Review

ISSN: 0278-3193 (Print) 1940-865X (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/uror20

After Six Decades of Systematic Study of


Creativity: What Do Teachers Need to Know About
What It Is and How It Is Measured?

Ahmed M. Abdulla & Bonnie Cramond

To cite this article: Ahmed M. Abdulla & Bonnie Cramond (2017) After Six Decades of
Systematic Study of Creativity: What Do Teachers Need to Know About What It Is and How It Is
Measured?, Roeper Review, 39:1, 9-23, DOI: 10.1080/02783193.2016.1247398

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/02783193.2016.1247398

Published online: 07 Nov 2016.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 1801

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 9 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=uror20
Roeper Review, 39:9–23, 2017
Copyright © The Roeper Institute
ISSN: 0278-3193 print / 1940-865X online
DOI: 10.1080/02783193.2016.1247398

EXAMINING CREATIVITY

After Six Decades of Systematic Study of Creativity:


What Do Teachers Need to Know About What It Is and
How It Is Measured?
Ahmed M. Abdulla and Bonnie Cramond

With creativity now being emphasized in schools, it is important for teachers to understand
what it is and how it is measured. This review of the literature is an attempt to make sense of
the many definitions and measures of creativity. As a result, this comprehensive review shows
that most definitions agree that there are at least two criteria to judge whether a person or a
product is creative or not, originality and usefulness. Organized according to Rhodes’s 3 P
conceptualization of creativity—person, process, product, and press—more than 40 assess-
ments used in creativity research were reviewed. Familiarity with the definitions, views of, and
measurement of creativity can help teachers recognize and foster creativity in their students.
Keywords: creative person, creative press, creative process, creative product, creativity,
creativity definitions, creativity measures

More than 60 years ago, Guilford (1950) talked about the employment. Indeed, several states have begun to develop
lack of interest in studying one of the most important aspects creativity indexes to assess the creativity taught in schools
of human intellectual ability—creativity. He called for more (Robelen, 2012). Yet, most U.S. teachers came through an
efforts in order to understand this psychological construct education system that did not emphasize creativity, so how
(Guilford, 1950). Since that time, many things have changed can they be expected to teach it? In fact, the Adobe (2012)
in the creativity field, and creativity now has become one study found that of the young professionals they surveyed,
of the most studied phenomena in psychology and other education majors were the most likely to say that creativity
disciplines, including economics (Florida, 2012), medicine is an innate skill, whereas biological science and engineering
(Devi, 2015; Natrielli, Silva, & Natrielli, 2013), engineering majors were more likely to say that creativity can be learned.
(D. H. Cropley, 2015), and mathematics (Sriraman & Lee, What should teachers know about the nature and assessment
2011). Thus, it should not be surprising that creativity is now of creativity as a basis for developing it?
being pushed into the K–12 curriculum. For example, cre- Creativity is viewed as a syndrome or complex (Runco,
ativity is one of the components of the 21st century’s 4 C’s 2014); however, there is not complete agreement on what
skills, which also include critical thinking, communication, creativity is. One important area of emphasis in the last
and collaboration (Trilling & Fadel, 2009). 6 decades has been on two constructs related in one way or
With recent reports from IBM (2012) and Adobe (2012) another to creativity: divergent thinking and problem solving
showing that employers are looking for creative think- (Guilford, 1967; Runco, 2014). This is a workable defini-
ing in their new hires, it is natural that schools should tion for educators, because divergent thinking and problem
begin to examine how well they prepare students for future solving are processes that can be taught. However, there is
much evidence that creativity is more than divergent think-
ing (Runco, 2008) and problem solving (Runco, 2014). Thus,
Accepted 30 April 2016. this review of literature explores how creativity is defined
Address correspondence to Ahmed M. Abdulla, Road 2904, Building and how it is measured in the creativity research in ways that
293, Manama 329, Bahrain-Arabian Gulf University, Bahrain. E-mail: are additionally useful to educators.
ahmedcreatv@gmail.com
10 A. M. ABDULLA AND B. CRAMOND

This review answers three primary questions of import to and undefined so that part of the task was to formulate the
educators: What is creativity? How does the view of creativ- problem itself. (p. 328)
ity relate to the way it is developed in schools? How does the
measurement of creative components relate to the fostering
of creativity? Finally, Han et al. (2013) defined problem finding as “a kind
of intellectual trait or ability that is demonstrated in the pro-
cess of producing and expressing new-found questions in a
WHAT IS CREATIVITY? unique, novel and useful and purposeful way, using existing
contexts and experience” (p. 249).
A review of studies on creativity revealed that there is
a consistency between theory and practice regarding what
creativity is. Most of the studies reviewed agree that cre- Summary of Creativity Definitions
ativity requires both originality and appropriateness (Acar & It can be concluded that most of the definitions found
Runco, 2015; Brophy, 1998; Charyton & Snelbecker, 2007; in empirical studies confirmed Runco and Jaeger’s (2012)
Davidovitch & Milgram, 2006; Dietrich, 2004; Fasko, 2001; standard definition of creativity as bipartite, requiring both
Kharkhurin & Samadpour Motalleebi, 2008; Milgram & originality and effectiveness. Yet, some suggested a third
Hong, 1999; Mumford & Simonton, 1997; Pohlman, 1996; criterion, such as Simonton’s (2012) surprise, a criterion
Rudowicz, 2003; Runco, Illies, & Eisenman, 2005; Runco used by the U.S. Patent Office in order to evaluate creative
et al., 2011; Scott, Leritz, & Mumford, 2004; Simonton, products. One might ask whether these theoretical criteria
2012; Sowden, Pringle, & Gabora, 2015; West, Tateishi, are consistent with how creativity is viewed and fostered in
Wright, & Fonoimoana, 2012; Zha, Walczyk, Griffith-Ross, schools. In order to make creativity applicable in schools, the
Tobacyk, & Walczyk, 2006). However, in addition to orig- view of creativity as a cognitive process involved in problem
inality, other divergent thinking abilities were used in cre- solving has been used. This view allows some of these cogni-
ativity research in order to define creativity, such as flu- tive processes to be measured; thus, it is also a psychometric
ency, flexibility, and elaboration (e.g., Benedek, Fink, & view. However, this process view encapsulates just one of the
Neubauer, 2006; Fontenot, 1993; Runco, 1986a, 1986b; 4 Ps of creativity described by Rhodes (1961). By includ-
Runco & Albert, 1986; Runco et al., 2011; West et al., 2012). ing the other 3 Ps, process, product, and press, educators can
In addition, there is a considerable amount of research that envision a more complete approach to fostering creativity.
treated creativity as a kind of problem finding and solving
(Basadur, Runco, & Vega, 2000; Chand & Runco, 1993;
Csikszentmihalyi & Getzels, 1970, 1971; Dow & Mayer,
HOW DOES THE VIEW OF CREATIVITY
2004; Han, Hu, Liu, Jia, & Adey, 2013).
RELATE TO THE WAY IT IS FOSTERED IN
For example, Runco and Chand (1995) stated, “Thinking
SCHOOLS?
is creative if it leads to original and adaptive ideas, solutions,
or insights” (p. 244). Scott et al. (2004) stated, “Creativity
Several theories and definitions have emerged since the
ultimately involves the production of original, potentially
1960s. One that is applicable to education is considered
workable, solutions to novel, ill-defined problems of rela-
a problem-solving or psychometric view (Guilford, 1968;
tively high complexity” (p. 362). Davidovitch and Milgram
Torrance, 1962a; Wallach & Kogan, 1965) because it asserts
(2006) defined creative thinking as “a cognitive process of
that creative abilities can be described as cognitive processes
original problem solving by means of which original prod-
distinct from intelligence quotient (IQ; Kozbelt, Beghetto,
ucts are generated” (p. 385). Zha et al. (2006) concluded
& Runco, 2010) that can be measured. Guilford (1967), for
that “intellectual creativity is the ability to view what is
example, distinguished between two kinds of intellectual
ordinary in a novel or atypical way; the ability to detect
abilities in his Structure of Intellect (SOI) model: diver-
problems that others may not recognize; or the ability to gen-
gent thinking versus convergent thinking. Guilford (1968)
erate original, exceptional, adaptive, or effective solutions
asserted that divergent thinking was an essential component
to problems” (p. 355). Runco (1996) stated, “Creativity is
of creativity. Thus, he and other psychologists developed
manifested in the intentions and motivation to transform the
a number of divergent thinking tests in order to assess
objective world into original interpretations, coupled with
divergent thinking abilities.
the ability to decide when this is useful and when it is not”
However, before assessing creativity, Guilford, Torrance,
(p. 4). Getzels (1975) suggested,
and others had to define what creativity is. Guilford defined
creativity in terms of problem solving. According to Guilford
Thinking may be called creative if: 1) the product has novelty
(1967), “There is something creative about all genuine prob-
and value either for the thinker or the culture, 2) the think-
ing is unconventional, 3) it is highly motivated and persistent lem solving, and creative production is typically carried out
or of great intensity, and 4) the problem was initially vague as a means to the end of solving some problems” (p. 312).
CREATIVITY: WHAT IT IS AND HOW IT IS MEASURED 11

Torrance also considered the role that problem solving plays product” (p. 309). However, it should be noted that judg-
in creativity. According to Torrance (1988), his briefest defi- ing whether a product is creative or not is not as objective
nition is the one he called his survival definition: “When a as measuring height or gravity, which can be measured
person has no learned or practiced solution to a problem, accurately with appropriate instruments (D. H. Cropley &
some degree of creativity is required.” (p. 57). However, Cropley, 2011). Thus, teachers who wish to encourage cre-
Torrance (1988) offered a kind of comprehensive definition ative products should consider the methods reviewed in the
of creativity that takes into consideration other cognitive second part of this article in evaluating them for creativity.
and noncognitive abilities in what he called his research The fourth P, creative press, refers to “the relationship
definition: between human beings and their environment. Creative pro-
duction is the outcome of certain kinds of forces playing
I tried to describe creative thinking as the process of sens- upon certain kinds of individuals as they grow up and
ing difficulties, problems, gaps in information, missing ele- as they function” (Rhodes, 1961, p. 308). It is important
ments, something askew; making guesses and formulating for educators to understand the role of the environment in
hypotheses about these deficiencies; evaluating and testing encouraging creativity and the ways in which they can affect
these guesses and hypotheses; possibly revising and retesting the environment to nurture creativity in their students.
them; and finally communicating the results. (p. 47)

In sum, this problem-solving/psychometric theory of cre- Summary of the Views of Creativity


ativity focuses on what Rhodes (1961) called the creative
Creativity is a multifaceted and complex phenomenon that
process, and such a theory defined creativity as kind of prob-
can be studied and fostered through different angles. In addi-
lem solving that requires using divergent thinking abilities
tion, it can be concluded that there is no exact agreement on
such as fluency, flexibility, originality, and elaboration.
what creativity is; the literature suggests that there are at least
However, Rhodes (1961) included four other Ps in his
two criteria that distinguish something as creative: original-
description of creativity in addition to process—creative per-
ity and usefulness. The four main approaches for measuring
sonality, creative product, and creative press. According to
creativity are based on the Rhodes (1961) framework of
Rhodes (1961), the term creative person “covers information
4 Ps—person, process, product, and press.
about personality, intellect, temperament, physique, traits,
habits, attitudes, self-concept, value systems, defense mech-
anisms, and behavior” (p. 307). A number of studies, both
implicit and explicit (e.g., Chan, 2011; Runco, 2007, 2011; HOW DOES THE MEASUREMENT OF
Sternberg, 2011), have been conducted on the creative per- CREATIVE COMPONENTS RELATE TO THE
son and found common characteristics (see Table 1) that FOSTERING OF CREATIVITY?
distinguish creative individuals. Knowing that these char-
acteristics, both positive and negative, are indicative of the If creativity measures correspond to the definitions found,
creative personality can help teachers recognize creativity in they should assess both originality and usefulness. Because
their students. Such knowledge can help them develop their there are different approaches by which creativity is mea-
own qualitative understandings of creative individuals and sured, this part consists of four sections reflecting Rhodes’
their needs. (1961) 4 Ps framework: creative process, creative person,
The third aspect of creativity emphasizes creative prod- creative product, and creative press. Educators interested
ucts, which are original, useful, and even surprising in identifying creative students, encouraging creative pro-
(Simonton, 2012). According to Rhodes (1961), “When an cesses, evaluating creative products, and establishing cre-
idea becomes embodied into tangible form it is called a ative environments would be well served to know about these
assessments and how to use them.
TABLE 1
Examples of Characteristics That Distinguish Creative Individuals

Active Dreamy Intrinsic Creative Process


Adaptable Energetic Intelligent
Adventurous Enterprising Inventive It can be said that creativity research started by assessing
Aesthetic Enthusiastic Motivated the creative process or potential. Through his SOI model,
Alert Flexible Original Guilford (1967) distinguished between divergent and con-
Ambitious Imaginative Progressive vergent production and created some measures that aimed to
Autonomous Impulsive Questioning
assess four main components of divergent production called
Artistic Independent Resourceful
Capable Individualistic Risk-taking fluency, flexibility, originality, and elaboration. This section
Clever Industrious Self-confident reviews the main measures that assess divergent thinking
Curious Innovative Humorous with a brief description of each. Table 2 lists additional
Daring Inquisitive Unorthodox assessments in each category.
12 A. M. ABDULLA AND B. CRAMOND

TABLE 2
Examples of Creativity Measures According to Rhodes’s (1961) 4 Ps Framework

4 Ps Definition Examples

Person “The term creative person covers information about The Khatena-Torrance Creative Perception Inventory (Khatena & Torrance,
personality, intellect, temperament, physique, 1976)
traits, habits, attitudes, self-concept, value Gough’s CPS (Gough, 1979)
systems, defense mechanisms, and behavior.” Openness Scale of the NEO Personality Inventory (Costa, McCrae, Stanton,
(Rhodes, 1961, p. 307) Jonassaint, Williams, & James, 2010)
Creativity Scale of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Indicator (Nassif &
Quevillon, 2008)
SRBCSS (Renzulli, Siegle, Reis, Gavin, & Sytsma Reed, 2009)
CBI (Hocevar, 1979)
RIBS (Runco, Plucker, & Lim, 2001)
Creative Behavior Disposition Scale (Taylor & Fish, 1979)
KAI (Kirton, 1976)
CSQ-R (Pollick & Kumar, 1997)
Style of Learning and Thinking (Torrance, McCarthy, Kolesinski, & Smith,
1988)
Creative Problem Solving Profile (Basadur, Graen, & Wakabayashi, 1990)
HDYT (Davis, 1975)
Group Inventory for Finding Interests (Davis & Rimm, 1982)
How Creative Are You? (Gattis & Raudsepp, 1969)

Process “The term process applies to motivation, perception, Guilford’s Tests of Creativity: Sketches, Figure Production, Making Objects,
learning, thinking, and communicating.” (Rhodes, Consequence, Alternate Uses, Match Problems (Berger & Guilford, 1965;
1961, p. 308) Christensen, Guilford, Merrifield, & Wilson, 1965a; S. Gardner et al., 1965)
Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking: Torrance Test of Creative
Thinking–Verbal Form, Torrance Test of Creative Thinking–Figural Form,
Torrance’s Abbreviated Test for Adults, Thinking Creatively in Action and
Movement, Thinking Creatively with Sounds and Words (Goff & Torrance,
2000; Khatena & Torrance, 1988; Torrance, 1966, 1981)
Wallach and Kogan Creativity Tests: Instances, Similarities, Pattern Meanings,
Line Meanings (Wallach & Kogan, 1965)
Mednick’s Remote Association Test (Mednick, 1968)
Creativity Assessment Packet (Williams, 1993)
Runco’s Creativity Assessment Battery (Runco & Jaeger, 2011)

Product “When an idea becomes embodied into tangible form Creative Product Semantic Scale (O’Quin & Besemer, 2006)
it is called a product.” (Rhodes, 1961, p. 309) SPAF (Reis & Renzulli, 1991)
Creative Product Inventory (Taylor & Sandler, 1972)
Test of Creative Thinking–Drawing Production (Urban & Jellen, 1996)
CAT (Amabile, 1983)

Press “The term creative press refers to the relationship Assessing the Climate for Creativity (KEYS; Amabile, 1995)
between human beings and their environment. Creative Climate Questionnaire (Ekvall, 1996)
Creative production is the outcome of certain Situational Outlook Questionnaire (Isaksen, Lauer, & Ekvall, 1999)
kinds of forces playing upon certain kinds of Team Climate Inventory (Anderson & West, 1994)
individuals as they grow up and as they function.” Siegel Scale of Support for Innovation (Siegel & Kaemmerer, 1978)
(Rhodes, 1961, p. 308) Creative Environment Perceptions Scale (Mayfield & Mayfield, 2010)
Work Environment Inventory (Amabile & Gryskiewicz, 1989)

Guilford’s Tests of Creativity Guilford, 1965), in which subjects are asked to construct
objects using a given set of shapes, such as a circle, rectangle,
A pioneer of creativity research, Guilford developed a
and square.
number of divergent thinking tests that are still used to date.
In addition, Guilford developed several verbal creativity
Guilford developed many divergent thinking (DT) tests that
tests such as the Consequence test (Christensen, Merrifield,
assess different dimensions of his SOI model. Some of these
& Guilford, 1965), which asks participants about the con-
tests are figural, such as Sketches and Figure Production,
sequences of hypothetical events such as, “What would be
which ask the participants to draw many objects or add lines
the results if people no longer needed or wanted sleep?”
to create new figures (Kaufman, Plucker, & Baer, 2008),
The Alternate Uses test (Christensen et al., 1965) requires
and Making Objects (S. Gardner, Gershon, Merrifield, &
CREATIVITY: WHAT IT IS AND HOW IT IS MEASURED 13

test-takers to list nonobvious uses for a common object, and their creative potential. But first I had to measure that poten-
the Match Problems test (Berger & Guilford, 1965) instructs tial. So I have a reputation as a psychometrician, but all along
individuals to remove a specific number of matches to create I have worked with the development of creativity” (cited in
a new configuration. Four creative indices are scored in those Cramond, 1994, p. 231).
tests: fluency (the number of responses produced), flexibility Influenced by some creativity tests, such as Guilford’s
(the ability to go beyond tradition, habits, and the obvious), Structure of Intellect (Guilford, 1965), Torrance created his
originality (uncommon, remote, and clever ideas), and elab- own creativity test in 1966. However, Torrance had his own
oration (the ability to work out the details of an idea or vision of the definition of creativity, which reflected the way
solution; Christensen, Guilford, & Wilson, 1957; Guilford, or method he measured creativity. What distinguished the
1973; Wilson, Guilford, & Christensen, 1953). TTCT from other creativity or divergent thinking tests was
Guilford’s creativity tests have been used in many stud- not only how Torrance perceived or defined creativity but
ies since the 1960s (e.g., Batey & Furnham, 2009; Benedek also how he made it fun; easy to use, score, and interpret;
& Neubauer, 2013; Bowers, 1967; A. J. Cropley, 1972; and applicable for diverse populations and cultures. Torrance
Eisenstadt, 1966; Furnham, Crump, Batey, & Chamorro- emphasized principles that creativity tests should meet. He
Premuzic, 2009; González & Gilbert, 1979; Hocevar, 1979, stated:
1980; Khaleefa, Erdos, & Ashria, 1996; Piers, Daniels, &
Quackenbush, 1960; Runco & Dow, 2004; Silvia, 2011). It had to be (a) a natural, everyday process; (b) suitable for
However, Guilford’s tests are not used as much in schools all ages and educational levels, kindergarten through gradu-
because he did not develop extensive guidelines for the ate and professional school; (c) easy enough for the young or
scoring of the tests beyond the descriptions of the creative disabled to make a creative response, yet difficult enough to
indices. challenge the most able; (d) unbiased with regard to gender
For example, Guilford (1957) distinguished four kinds and race, and open-ended to allow for responses from differ-
of fluency: word, associational, ideational, and expressional, ent experiential backgrounds; and (e) fun. (cited in Hébert,
and two kinds of flexibility: spontaneous and adaptive. Cramond, Neumeister, Millar, & Silvian, 2002, p. 14)
Regarding originality, although Guilford (1957) mentioned
one general factor of originality, he and his colleagues Another aspect that distinguished the TTCT from other cre-
(Wilson et al., 1953) defined originality in different ways: ativity tests was the administration processes for the tests—
(a) The uncommonness of response, which is defined opera- more specifically, the test instructions. Unlike Guilford’s
tionally as “the ability to produce ideas that are statistically SOI tests (Guilford, 1965), Torrance believed that students
infrequent for the population of which the individual is a should receive clear instructions regarding any tasks they
member” (p. 363), (b) the remoteness of associations, which would face in order to show their best performance.
is defined as “the ability to make remote or indirect asso- In addition, the TTCT contains both verbal and figural
ciations” (p. 366), and (c) cleverness, which is defined as activities, with two parts (A and B) for each form in order
“the ability to produce responses that are rated as clever by to create equivalent forms of the tests. In the Figural form
judges” (p. 367). Because the definitions provide the only of the TTCT, examinees face three figural activities: (a)
guidelines for scoring and there are not the norms and statis- Picture Construction, (b) Picture Completion, and (c) Lines
tical qualities that are required for school decisions, many (form A)/Circles (form B). The Figural battery assesses stu-
educators have opted for the Torrance Tests of Creative dents’ fluency, originality, abstractness of titles, elaboration,
Thinking instead. and resistance to premature closure abilities in addition to
13 creative strengths, including expressions of emotion, sto-
rytelling, motion, colorful or rich imagery, humor, fantasy,
Torrance’s Tests
synthesis, and unusual or internal visualization. The Verbal
Torrance was influenced by Guilford’s extensive work form of the TTCT consists of six different types of activities:
with divergent thinking (Makel & Plucker, 2008; Plucker, (a) asking, (b) guessing causes, (c) guessing consequences,
Waitman, & Hartley, 2011). Prior to publishing the official (d) product improvement, (e) unusual uses, and (f) just sup-
and first version of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking pose. The TTCT Verbal Battery assesses students’ fluency,
(TTCT; known as the Minnesota Test of Creative Thinking flexibility, and originality abilities.
in 1966), Torrance had already published many articles that Over 50 years of research, including studies of reliability
were concerned with measuring creativity (Torrance, 1962a, and validity, have made the TTCT the most used and famous
1962b, 1962c, 1964). However, it is important to mention creativity measure in the world (Baer, 2010; Plucker et al.,
that Torrance’s interest in measuring creativity was not his 2011; Runco, 2010). According to Millar (2002), the TTCT
ultimate goal. Rather, the TTCT was designed as a means has been proved for both components for the last 6 decades
to an end, not an end in itself. Torrance stated, “I have through application in over 2,000 studies throughout the
always been interested in empowering children, releasing world in more than 35 languages.
14 A. M. ABDULLA AND B. CRAMOND

Wallach and Kogan Creativity Tests on them. However, there are other tests that are con-
cerned with measuring the creative process. Examples
In their book, Modes of Thinking in Young Children,
of those tests are Mednick’s Remote Association Test
Wallach and Kogan (1965) reviewed some of their research
(Mednick, 1968), Torrance’s Abbreviated Test for Adults
on creativity in which they utilized some divergent thinking
(Goff & Torrance, 2000), Thinking Creatively in Action
tests. In chapter 2, they described their research concerning
and Movement (Torrance, 1981), Thinking Creatively With
the relationship between creativity and intelligence in which
Sounds and Words (Khatena & Torrance, 1988), Creativity
they have used the Instances test or game as they described
Assessment Packet (Williams, 1993), and Runco’s Creativity
it. An example of the Instances test is, “Name all the round
Assessment Battery (Runco & Jaeger, 2011).
things you can think of,” and “Name all the things you can
think of that will make a noise” (Wallach & Kogan, 1965,
p. 29). Another test developed by Wallach and Kogan is the Creative Person
Similarities test. This test asks participants to generate possi-
Due to the large number of scales, inventories, and ques-
ble similarities between two verbally specified objects, such
tionnaires that fall under the creative person umbrella, the
as “Tell me all the ways in which a potato and a carrot are
measures of a creative person were classified into four main
alike,” and “Tell me all the ways in which a cat and a mouse
categories: creative personality measures, creative behavior
are alike.” Such a test is even used in some IQ tests such
measures, creative styles measures, and attitude and values
as the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (Wechsler,
measures. This section will shed light on studies that targeted
2003). In addition to the verbal divergent thinking tests,
creative individuals and offer some examples of the measures
Wallach and Kogan developed some figural DT tests, such as
used in order to assess individuals’ creativity.
Pattern Meanings and the Line Meanings (Wallach & Kogan,
1965). In the Pattern Meanings test, the participant is asked
Creative Personality
to generate possible meanings or interpretations for each of
a number of abstract visual designs, and in the Line Meaning The Khatena-Torrance Creative Perception Inventory
test the participant is confronted with one or another kind of (Khatena & Torrance, 1976) consists of two separate tests
line and asked to generate meanings or interpretations rele- of creative self-perception: What Kind of Person Are You?
vant to the form of line in question (Wallach & Kogan, 1965). (WKOPAY) and Something About Myself (SAM; Khatena
In all of those tests, each participant is scored on the number & Torrance, 1976). According to Khatena and Torrance
and uniqueness of ideas. According to Wallach and Kogan (1976), WKOPAY and SAM measure different dimensions
(1965), of the creative personality and thus they could be used inde-
pendently. WKOPAY and SAM can be used individually
The variable number is defined as the total number of or in groups with children and adolescents from Grades
responses given by a child to a particular item, and the vari- 4–12 and with adults above high school. WKOPAY con-
able uniqueness is defined as follows: For each item in a sists of 50 statements that come in pairs and require each
procedure, a frequency distribution is constructed indicating examinee to choose one of each pair that best describes
the number of children in the total sample who give a partic- him or herself. WKOPAY assesses five personality fac-
ular response to that item. Any response to a given item that
tors: (a) acceptance of authority, (b) self-confidence, (c)
is offered by only 1 of the total sample is defined as a unique
inquisitiveness, (d) awareness of others, and (e) disciplined
response. (p. 30)
imagination. SAM consists of 50 single statements that ask
the examinee to determine whether or not the statements
The problems with using the Wallach and Kogan tests in edu-
describe him or her. If the statement does not describe the
cation are the same as with the Guilford tests—there are no
examinee, he or she will leave it blank. SAM assesses six
clear directions for scoring the tests and no national norms
personality factors: (a) environmental sensitivity, (b) initia-
to compare scores. Therefore, they are useful for research
tive, (c) self-strength, (d) intellectuality, (e) individuality,
and exploration purposes but not for placement decisions that
and (f) artistry (Khatena & Torrance, 1976). The Khatena-
require high reliability and validity.
Torrance Creative Perception Inventory has been employed
in several studies (e.g., Gallucci, Middleton, & Kline, 2000;
Other Tests That Measure the Creative Process Houtz et al., 2003; Kaltsounis, 1979; Montgomery, Hodges,
& Kaufman, 2004; Palaniappan, 1996).
The divergent thinking tests of Guilford, Torrance, and
Another example of creative personality measures is
Wallach and Kogan were reviewed in detail because those
Gough’s Creative Personality Scale (CPS; Gough, 1979),
tests dominate creativity research, especially research that
which was developed based on the Adjective Check List
deals with the creative process or potential. Another rea-
(Gough & Heilbrun, 1965). The CPS consists of 30 items or
son for the weight given to those tests is that many
adjectives: 18 items indicative of creativity: capable, clever,
other divergent thinking tests developed later were based
CREATIVITY: WHAT IT IS AND HOW IT IS MEASURED 15

confident, egotistical, humorous, individualistic, informal, relatively new measure of creative behavior, it has been used
insightful, intelligent, interests–wide, inventive, original, in many studies in the last 15 years (e.g., Kim & Hull, 2012;
reflective, resourceful, self-confident, sexy, snobbish, uncon- Kim & VanTassel-Baska, 2010; Kuan Chen, 2015; Pannells
ventional, and 12 contradictive items: affected, cautious, & Claxton, 2008; Plucker et al., 2006).
commonplace, conservative, conventional, dissatisfied, hon- Of these measures, teachers are most likely to find the
est, interests–narrow, mannerly, sincere, submissive, and sus- Renzulli SCRBSS to be the most user friendly because it was
picious (Kaduson & Schaefer, 1991). The CPS has also been specifically designed to be used by teachers observing stu-
used in several studies (e.g., Carson, Peterson, & Higgins, dents in classroom situations. It is also helpful for reminding
2003; Dollinger, Urban, & James, 2004; Ee, Seng, & Kwang, teachers of the characteristics of creative students.
2007; Meneely & Portillo, 2005; Patnoe, 1985; Sheldon,
1995; Sung Eun Chung & Meneely, 2012).
Creative Style, Attitudes, Interests, and Values
Educators and researchers are not only concerned with
Creative Behavior
identifying creative individuals according to their unique
Based on Renzulli’s (1977) three-ring conception of gift- personality characteristics and behaviors, but they may also
edness, Renzulli and Hartman (1971) developed the first ver- be concerned with looking at how creative individuals pro-
sion of the Scales for Rating the Behavior Characteristics of cess information and deal with different kinds of tasks.
Superior Students (SRBCSS), which consisted of 10 scales, According to Zhang (2015), intellectual styles refers to peo-
one of which measures creativity. The newest version of the ple’s preferred ways of processing information and dealing
SRBCSS consists of 14 scales (Renzulli et al., 2009). with tasks. Thus, in order to assess individuals’ creative
The Creativity Scale includes 10 items with a 6-point styles, a number of measures have been developed and used
Likert scale. Unlike other creative behavior scales, the since the 1960s. In addition, according to Clapham (2011),
SRBCSS is considered as an assessment by others, which is “Attitude and interest inventories are based on the belief
filled out by teachers, not the students or participants them- that creative individuals tend to have attitudes and inter-
selves. The SRBCSS has been used in many creativity and ests that distinguish them from less creative individuals”
giftedness studies (e.g., Chan, 2000; Chan & Chan, 2007; (p. 461). This section reviews four measures that assess
Jarosewich, Pfeiffer, & Morris, 2002; Kim & Hull, 2012; creative individuals’ styles, attitudes, interests, and values.
Kim & VanTassel-Baska, 2010). One of the famous creativity style measures used in
Hocevar (1979) developed the Creativity Behavior research and practice is the Kirton Adaption–Innovation
Inventory (CBI), which asks participants to rate their engage- Inventory (KAI; Kirton, 1976). The Kirton Adaption–
ment in various creative activities on a scale from 0 (never Innovation Inventory scale was developed to assess the
did this) to 3 (did this more than five times). Different forms two distinct cognitive styles that people exhibit in cre-
of the CBI were found in the literature; some used the ative endeavors. Kirton (1976) argued that “everyone can be
original form, which consists of 90 items and others used located on a continuum ranging from an ability to ‘do things
short forms that differ in the number of items used. Several better’ to an ability to ‘do things differently’ and the ends of
studies have used some form of the CBI (Dollinger, 2007; this continuum are labeled adaptive and innovative, respec-
Lee & Kemple, 2014; Nassif & Quevillon, 2008; Plucker tively” (p. 622). The KAI consists of 32 items self-report
& Dana, 1998; Silvia, Wigert, Reiter-Palmon, & Kaufman, assessing three components or factors: originality, efficiency,
2012; Wakefield, 1989). and rule/group conforming (Kirton, 1976). There are sev-
Another scale that assesses creative behavior is the Runco eral examples of creativity studies that used the KAI (Dew,
Ideational Behavioral Scale (RIBS; Runco et al., 2001). The 2009; Engle, Mah, & Sadri, 1997; Fischer & Freund, 2014;
RIBS is a self-report instrument designed to assess DT with Goldsmith, 1984; Isaksen & Puccio, 1988; Jacobson, 1993;
a Likert format (Runco et al., 2001). The RIBS is based on Loo & Shiomi, 1997; Skinner, 1996). Because Kirton’s
the belief that ideas can be treated as the products of original, (1976) research has indicated that successful teams need
divergent, and creative thinking (Runco et al., 2001). There both kinds of creativity, teachers might find this scale useful
are two versions of the RIBS: the short form, which con- with students who are old enough to complete it in assigning
sists of 19 items, and long form, which consists of 74 items students to working groups.
(Runco et al., 2014). The RIBS uses a 0–4 Likert scale as Another example of a creativity style measure is the
follows: 0 (never), 1 (approximately once a year), 2 (once Creativity Style Questionnaire–Revised (CSQ-R; Pollick &
or twice each month, approximately), 3 (once or twice each Kumar, 1997). The CSQ-R contains 76 items that measure
week, approximately), and 4 (just about every day, sometimes beliefs and strategies for going about being creative in every-
more than once each day; Runco et al., 2014). A number of day life using self-report items. The CSQ-R consists of seven
studies have demonstrated that the RIBS is useful as a cri- subscales: (a) Belief in the Unconscious Process, (b) Use
terion of original and divergent thinking (Ames & Runco, of Techniques, (c) Use of Other People, (d) Final Product
2005; Plucker, Runco, & Lim, 2006). Although the RIBS is a Orientation, (e) Superstition, (f) Environmental Control, and
16 A. M. ABDULLA AND B. CRAMOND

(g) Uses of the Senses (Pollick & Kumar, 1997). The Just as in the creative process and creative person, there is
CSQ was also used in a number of creativity studies (e.g., more than one way to assess the creative product. Searching
DeMoss, Milich, & DeMers, 1993; Keller, Lavish, & Brown, creativity studies that defined creativity in terms of a product
2007; Lack, Kumar, & Arevalo, 2003; Pollick & Kumar, revealed two main approaches: (a) studies employing quanti-
1997). tative measures (e.g., inventories, questionnaires and scales)
Davis (1975) developed the How Do You Think (HDTY) and (b) the consensual assessment technique.
Test in order to assess creative attitudes and interests. The
items of the HDYT, presented in a 5-point Likert-type
scale format, cover a variety of areas, including energy Quantitative Assessments
level, originality, interests, activities, self-confidence, sense A number of instruments have been developed to eval-
of humor, flexibility, risk taking, and playfulness (Davis, uate the creativity of a product. For example, the Creative
1975). According to Runco (2014), the HDYT contains many Product Semantic Scale, which is based on the creative
good examples of both supportive and inhibitive attitudes. product analysis model (Besemer & Treffinger, 1981), was
Several studies have used the HDYT (Al-Sabaty & Davis, developed to assess three dimensions in products: (a) nov-
1989; Bender, Nibbelink, Towner-Thyrum, & Vredenburg, elty, (b) resolution, and (c) elaboration and synthesis. The
2013; Clapham, 2004; Davis, Subkoviak, Runco, Ebersole, novelty dimension includes “consideration of new materials,
& Mraz, 1991; K. G. Gardner & Moran, 1990; Runco, new processes, new concepts and other elements of newness
Ebersole, & Mraz, 1991; Smith & Tegano, 1992; Zysberg in the product or the idea.” The resolution refers to “how well
& Schenk, 2013). the product does what it is supposed to do,” and elaboration
Basadur developed three scales to measure divergent and synthesis refers to “how the product presents itself to the
thinking attitudes related to creative problem solving: (a) customer” (O’Quin & Besemer, 2006, p. 35). Many studies
Valuing New Ideas, (b) Belief that Creativity Is Not Only employed the Creative Product Semantic Scale in order to
for a Select Few, and (c) Not Feeling Too Busy for New assess creativity in products, especially in business and mar-
Ideas. These scales use a 5-point Likert-type rating scale keting (Besemer, 1998; Besemer & O’Quin, 1986; Storme &
with responses ranging from strongly agree to strongly dis- Lubart, 2012; White, Shen, & Smith, 2002; White & Smith,
agree (Basadur & Finkbeiner, 1985; Basadur & Hausdorf, 2001).
1996; Basadur, Taggar, & Pringle, 1999). These scales have Reis and Renzulli (1991) developed the Student Product
been used to assess divergent thinking attitudes in a number Assessment Form (SPAF), which was designed to eval-
of studies (e.g., Baer & Oldham, 2006; Basadur, Runco, & uate students’ creative products in gifted programs. The
Vega, 2000; Basadur, Wakabayashi, & Takai, 1992). SPAF provides ratings of nine creative product traits: (a)
early statement of purpose; (b) problem focusing; (c) level
Creative Product of resources; (d) diversity of resources; (e) appropriate-
ness of resources; (f) logic, sequence, and transition; (g)
We might not need to use divergent thinking or personality action orientation; (h) audience; and (i) overall assess-
tests when a person demonstrates his or her creative abili- ment (originality of the idea; achieved objectives stated
ties through a novel and useful product. However, reaching in the plan; advanced familiarity with the subject; qual-
the “productivity” level, in addition to talent, needs time. ity beyond age/grade level; care and attention to detail;
Renzulli (2005), for example, differentiated between two time, effort, and energy; and original contribution; Reis &
kinds of giftedness, schoolhouse giftedness, a kind most Renzulli, 1991). The SPAF has been used in several studies
easily measured by IQ or other cognitive ability tests, and designed to assess students’ products (e.g., Newman, 2005;
creative–productive giftedness, a kind of giftedness shown Reis & Renzulli, 1991; Renzulli & Reis, 2007). This mea-
when individuals compose music, do works of art, write sure, which is available free in the article or for download
novels, design scientific experiments, come up with new from this link: http://gifted.uconn.edu/wp-content/uploads/
advertising campaigns, create new management structures, sites/961/2015/02/spaf.pdf, would be most useful to educa-
and so on (Renzulli, 2005; Sternberg, Jarvin, & Grigorenko, tors because it is designed for school use.
2010). Renzulli (2005) emphasized the importance of the
latter kind, which is more observable with older students:
The Consensual Assessment Technique
History tells us it has been the creative and productive people
The consensual assessment technique (CAT) is a tech-
of the world, the producers rather than consumers of knowl-
edge, the reconstructionists of thought in all areas of human nique used to evaluate creative products through the con-
endeavor, who have become recognized as “truly gifted” sensual agreement of judges. The CAT was proposed by
individuals. History does not remember persons who merely Amabile in 1983 and is used in a large number of studies
scored well on IQ tests or those who learned their lessons in the creativity field. According to Hennessey, Amabile,
well but did not apply their knowledge in innovative and and Mueller (2011), “Consensual Assessment is a tech-
action-oriented ways. (p. 256) nique used for the assessment of creativity and other aspects
CREATIVITY: WHAT IT IS AND HOW IT IS MEASURED 17

of products, relying on the independent subjective judg- Creative Press


ments of individuals familiar with the domain in which
the products were made” (p. 253). The main idea is that The research on places or “press” is especially useful in
the best individuals to judge whether the product (e.g., defining such interactions between persons and environ-
composing music, writing a story or novel, performing a ments. There are individual differences in terms of preferred
dance) are experts who are familiar with such a prod- environments, but again also general tendencies: Creativity
uct. Amabile (1983) proposed a consensual definition of tends to flourish when there are opportunities for exploration
creativity: and independent work, and when originality is supported and
valued. (Kozbelt et al., 2010, p. 25)
A product or response is creative to the extent that appropri-
ate observers independently agree it is creative. Appropriate Regardless of how creative you are according to your scores
observers are those familiar with the domain in which the on a divergent thinking test or a personality scale, you will
product was created or the response articulated. Thus, cre- always need an environment that is encouraging and psycho-
ativity can be regarded as the quality of products or responses
logically safe, an environment that offers you opportunities,
judged to be creative by appropriate observers, and it can also
be regarded as the process by which something so judged is
and, most important, an environment that values creativ-
produced. (p. 1001) ity and creative thinking. Indeed, there are many creative
individuals who did not have these characteristics in their
Unlike the majority of creative process measures, which treat environments and still created original and useful ideas and
creativity as domain general, the CAT considers the domain- products. However, creativity research designed to examine
specific aspect of creativity, which emphasizes that being the influence of the creative environment demonstrated that
creative in one domain does not necessarily predict creativ- environment plays a major role in the lives and productivity
ity in other unrelated domains (Baer, 2015). According to of creative individuals.
Baer (2011), “The domain specificity theory argues that the Although teachers do not always have control over the
skills, traits, or knowledge that underlie creative performance size of, and resources in, a classroom, they have a strong
in a given domain are largely unrelated to the skills, traits, influence on the psychological environment they create
or knowledge that underlie or creative performance in other (Cramond, 2005). Although the majority of research on and
domains” (p. 404). However, this (i.e., domain general vs. development of creative environment measures has been
domain specificity) is beyond the scope of this article and in business organizations, most of the same principles of
more about this can be found in other sources (e.g., Baer, creative environment apply to any organizational situation.
2010, 2011, 2015). Knowledge about establishing a creative environment may
A number of studies employed such a technique, even enable teachers to promote creativity more effectively than
before Amabile proposed the CAT. In their seminal study using specific creative strategies.
on problem finding, Csikszentmihalyi and Getzels (1971), The question that this section answers is, What kind
for example, examined the significance of the problem for- of environment enables creativity? This question has been
mulation stage of creativity. In order to evaluate the value, addressed by Mathisen and Einarsen (2004), who reviewed
originality, and craftsmanship of the artworks performed by a number of instruments used in assessing the cre-
31 advanced art students, they assigned a panel, which con- ative environment within organizations. In their review,
sisted of art experts. The CAT is used in evaluating different five instruments were assessed: (a) KEYS: Assessing the
kinds of products such as art (Chen et al., 2002; Hennessey, Climate for Creativity, (b) Creative Climate Questionnaire,
1994), music (Barbot & Lubart, 2012), design (Lu & Luh, (c) Situational Outlook Questionnaire, (d) Team Climate
2012), and poetry (Cheng, Wang, Liu, & Chen, 2010). Inventory, and (e) Siegel Scale of Support for Innovation.
This is an effective method because it emulates what hap- Each instrument was reviewed and examined in terms of the
pens in real life; when one completes a creative product instrument aim, the psychometric characteristics, the avail-
or performance, it is judges from the field who determine ability for research and commercial use, and whether or not
its worth. However, this method is less useful for educators the instrument has been described in an international journal
because it requires a large number of expert judges to eval- (Mathisen & Einarsen, 2004). They concluded, “Work envi-
uate the products in order to get good interrater reliability ronment quality does seem to affect the level of creativity
(e.g., 8–10 to judge collages; Hennessy & Amabile, 1999), and innovation in groups and organizations” (p. 138). They
and most schools would have difficulty assembling such a also concluded, “The instruments may be used to serve sev-
panel on a regular basis. Schools that use judges with prod- eral purposes, not only to assess the degree to which a work
ucts, such as with science fairs, social studies fairs, invention environment promotes creativity and innovation, but also to
conventions, and the like, usually find that the use of a cri- measure the effects of environmental improvement efforts”
terion rating sheet, a method proscribed with the CAT, helps (p. 138).
ensure that judges focus on relevant factors and can reach Since these five measures have been described in
agreement. detail by Mathisen and Einarsen (2004), they will only
18 A. M. ABDULLA AND B. CRAMOND

be briefly described here. Mayfield and Mayfield (2010) the needs of creative students in their classes. Through deep
developed the Creative Environment Perceptions Scale, understanding of what creativity is, what creative people are
which consists of nine items assessing three compo- like, how creative products can be evaluated, and how cre-
nents: creativity resource, work characteristics, and creativ- ative environments can be created, educators may be better
ity blocks. The Creative Environment Perceptions Scale enabled to understand and serve the creativity in all of their
asks each participant to evaluate to what degree the students.
environment he or she is in supports creativity using
a 5-point Likert scale (Mayfield & Mayfield, 2010).
Another example of a creative press measure is the Work
Environment Inventory (Amabile & Gryskiewicz, 1989), REFERENCES
which consists of eight scales assessing Organizational
Encouragement, Supervisory Encouragement, Work Group Acar, S., & Runco, M. A. (2015). Thinking in multiple directions:
Supports, Challenging Work, Workload Pressure, Freedom, Hyperspace categories in divergent thinking. Psychology of Aesthetics,
Organizational Impediments, and Sufficient Resources Creativity, and the Arts, 9, 41–53. doi:10.1037/a0038501
Adobe. (2012, November 7). Creativity and education: Why it mat-
(Amabile & Gryskiewicz, 1989).
ters. Retrieved from https://www.adobe.com/aboutadobe/pressroom/
For teachers, it is probably most important to know that pdfs/Adobe_Creativity_and_Education_Why_It_Matters_study.pdf
promoting creativity in the classroom means fostering psy- Al-Sabaty, I., & Davis, G. A. (1989). Relationship between creativity and
chological safety (it is okay to have a strange idea or be right, left, and integrated thinking styles. Creativity Research Journal, 2,
wrong), encouraging intrinsic motivation (students can pur- 111–117. doi:10.1080/10400418909534304
Amabile, T. M. (1983). The social psychology of creativity. New York, NY:
sue some of their own interests), and providing opportunities
Springer-Verlag.
for both stimulation and quiet reflection (times to take in Amabile, T. M., & Gryskiewicz, N. D. (1989). The Creative Environment
information and times to reflect upon it; Cramond, 2005). Scales: Work Environment Inventory. Creativity Research Journal, 2,
Ideally, there are also comfortable places to work, a variety 231–253. doi:10.1080/10400418909534321
of materials, and an attractive, well-lit space, but the physical Ames, M., & Runco, M. A. (2005). Predicting entrepreneurship from
ideation and divergent thinking. Creativity & Innovation Management,
environment is less important than the psychological one.
14, 311–315. doi:10.1111/caim.2005.14.issue-3
Anderson, N. R., & West, M. A. (1994). The Team Climate Inventory:
Manual and user’s guide. Windsor, England: ASE/NFER-Nelson Press.
HOW CREATIVITY IS MEASURED: Baer, J. (2010). Is creativity domain-specific? In J. C. Kaufman & R. J.
A SUMMARY Sternberg (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 321–341). New
York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Baer, J. (2011). Domains of creativity. In M. A. Runco & S. R. Pritzker
In order to examine the consistency between how creativ- (Eds.), Encyclopedia of creativity (pp. 404–408). San Diego, CA:
ity is defined and how it is assessed, more than 40 measures Academic Press/Elsevier.
were reviewed. It could be concluded that most of these Baer, J. (2015). The importance of domain-specific expertise in creativity.
measures assess the two criteria of creativity: originality Roeper Review, 37, 165–178. doi:10.1080/02783193.2015.1047480
Baer, M., & Oldham, G. R. (2006). The curvilinear relation between
and usefulness, but some terms were used interchangeably
experienced creative time pressure and creativity: Moderating effects of
in the creativity research. For example, some researchers openness to experience and support for creativity. Journal of Applied
use the term novelty to refer to originality, and others Psychology, 91, 963–970. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.91.4.963
use appropriateness and effectiveness to refer to usefulness. Barbot, B., & Lubart, T. (2012). Creative thinking in music: Its nature
Others have added other descriptors that are related, such and assessment through musical exploratory behaviors. Psychology of
Aesthetics Creativity and the Arts, 6, 231–242. doi:10.1037/a0027307
as Simonton’s surprising (Simonton, 2012) and Clapham’s
Basadur, M., & Finkbeiner, C. T. (1985). Measuring preference for ideation
purposeful (Clapham, 2011). in creative problem-solving training. Journal of Applied Behavioral
In addition, it should be noted that this review of litera- Science, 21(1), 37–49. doi:10.1177/002188638502100104
ture focused on how creativity is defined and measured in Basadur, M., Graen, G., & Wakabayashi, M. (1990). Identifying individ-
creativity research, with an eye to what is most applicable to ual differences in creative problem solving style. The Journal of Creative
Behavior, 24, 111–131. doi:10.1002/jocb.1990.24.issue-2
educators. It does not cover all of the issues related to creativ-
Basadur, M., & Hausdorf, P. A. (1996). Measuring divergent thinking atti-
ity definitions and assessments such as DT and time-on-task, tudes related to creative problem solving and innovation management.
online testing, the reliability and validity issues, and the pros Creativity Research Journal, 9, 21–32. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj0901_3
and cons of each approach of assessment discussed (e.g., cre- Basadur, M., Runco, M. A., & Vega, L. A. (2000). Understanding how cre-
ative process, person, product, and press). More about these ative thinking skills, attitudes and behaviors work together: A causal pro-
cess model. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 34, 77–100. doi:10.1002/
issues can be found in Beaty & Silvia (2012), Hass (2015),
j.2162-6057.2000.tb01203.x
Kaufman et al. (2008), Preckel, Wermer, and Spinath (2011), Basadur, M., Taggar, S., & Pringle, P. (1999). Improving the measurement
and Runco and Pritzker (2011). of divergent thinking attitudes in organizations. The Journal of Creative
In conclusion, this review of the literature attempted to Behavior, 33, 75–111. doi:10.1002/jocb.1999.33.issue-2
address the complexity of creativity definitions, views, and Basadur, M. S., Wakabayashi, M., & Takai, J. (1992). Training effects
on the divergent thinking attitudes of Japanese managers. International
assessment in an intellectually honest way but in a way
Journal of Intercultural Relations, 16, 329–345. doi:10.1016/
that can be used by educators to understand and address 0147-1767(92)90056-Z
CREATIVITY: WHAT IT IS AND HOW IT IS MEASURED 19

Batey, M., & Furnham, A. (2009). The relationship between creativity, Christensen, P. R., Guilford, J. P., & Wilson, R. C. (1957). Relations of cre-
schizotypy and intelligence. Individual Differences Research, 7, 272–284. ative responses to working time and instructions. Journal of Experimental
Beaty, R. E., & Silvia, P. J. (2012). Why do ideas get more creative across Psychology, 53(2), 82–88. doi:10.1037/h0045461
time? An executive interpretation of the serial order effect in divergent Christensen, P. R., Merrifield, P. R., & Guilford, J. P. (1965b). Consequence.
thinking tasks. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6, Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
309–319. doi:10.1037/a0029171 Clapham, M. (2011). Testing/measurement/assessment. In M. A. Runco
Bender, S. W., Nibbelink, B., Towner-Thyrum, E., & Vredenburg, D. & S. R. Pritzker (Eds.), Encyclopedia of creativity (pp. 458–464). San
(2013). Defining characteristics of creative women. Creativity Research Diego, CA: Academic Press/Elsevier.
Journal, 25, 38–47. doi:10.1080/10400419.2013.752190 Clapham, M. M. (2004). The convergent validity of the Torrance
Benedek, M., Fink, A., & Neubauer, A. C. (2006). Enhancement of Tests of Creative Thinking and creativity interest inventories.
ideational fluency by means of computer-based training. Creativity Educational and Psychological Measurement, 64, 828–841. doi:10.1177/
Research Journal, 18, 317–328. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1803_7 0013164404263883
Benedek, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2013). Revisiting Mednick’s model on Costa, P. J., McCrae, R. R., Stanton, M. V., Jonassaint, C. R., Williams,
creativity-related differences in associative hierarchies. Evidence for a R. B., & James, S. A. (2010). NEO Personality Inventory—Revised.
common path to uncommon thought. The Journal of Creative Behavior, Psychosomatic Medicine, 72, 141–147.
47, 273–289. doi:10.1002/jocb.35 Cramond, B. (1994). The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking: From cre-
Berger, R. M., & Guilford, J. P. (1965). Match problems. Lincoln: University ation through establishment of predictive validity. In R. F. Subotnik & K.
of Nebraska Press. D. Arnold (Eds.), Beyond Terman: Contemporary longitudinal studies of
Besemer, S. P. (1998). Creative product analysis matrix: Testing giftedness and talent (pp. 229–254). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
the model structure and a comparison among products—Three Cramond, B. (2005). Fostering creativity in gifted students. Waco, TX:
novel chairs. Creativity Research Journal, 11, 333–346. doi:10.1207/ Prufrock Press.
s15326934crj1104_7 Cropley, A. J. (1972). A five-year longitudinal study of the validity of
Besemer, S. P., & O’Quin, K. (1986). Analyzing creative products: creativity tests. Developmental Psychology, 6, 119–124. doi:10.1037/
Refinement and test of a judging instrument. The Journal of Creative h0032228
Behavior, 20, 115–126. doi:10.1002/j.2162-6057.1986.tb00426.x Cropley, D. H. (2015). Creativity in engineering: Novel solutions to complex
Besemer, S. P., & Treffinger, D. J. (1981). Analysis of creative products: problems. San Diego, CA: Academic Press/Elsevier.
Review and synthesis. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 15, 158–178. Cropley, D. H., & Cropley, A. J. (2011). Aesthetics and creativity. In M.
doi:10.1002/jocb.1981.15.issue-3 A. Runco & S. R. Pritzker (Eds.), Encyclopedia of creativity (pp. 24–28).
Bowers, P. G. (1967). Effect of hypnosis and suggestions of reduced San Diego, CA: Academic Press/Elsevier.
defensiveness on creativity test performance. Journal of Personality, 35, Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Getzels, J. W. (1970). Concern for discovery: An
311–322. doi:10.1111/jopy.1967.35.issue-2 attitudinal component of creative production. Journal of Personality, 38,
Brophy, D. R. (1998). Understanding, measuring, and enhancing individ- 91–105. doi:10.1111/jopy.1970.38.issue-1
ual creative problem-solving efforts. Creativity Research Journal, 11, Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Getzels, J. W. (1971). Discovery-oriented behav-
123–150. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1102_4 ior and the originality of creative products: A study with artists. Journal
Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2003). Decreased latent of Personality and Social Psychology, 19, 47–52. doi:10.1037/h003
inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement in high- 1106
functioning individuals. Journal of Personality Processes And Individual Davidovitch, N., & Milgram, R. M. (2006). Creative thinking as a predictor
Differences, 85, 499–506. of teacher effectiveness in higher education. Creativity Research Journal,
Chan, D. W. (2000). Exploring identification procedures of gifted students 18, 385–390. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1803_12
by teacher ratings: Parent ratings and student self-reports in Hong Kong. Davis, G. A. (1975). In frumious pursuit of the creative person. The Journal
High Ability Studies, 11(1), 69–82. doi:10.1080/713669176 of Creative Behavior, 9, 75–87. doi:10.1002/jocb.1975.9.issue-2
Chan, D. W. (2011). East vs. west. In M. A. Runco & S. R. Pritzker (Eds.), Davis, G. A., & Rimm, S. (1982). Group Inventory for Finding Interests
Encyclopedia of creativity (pp. 415–421). San Diego, CA: Academic (GIFFI) I and II: Instruments for identifying creative potential in the
Press/Elsevier. junior and senior high school. The Journal Of Creative Behavior, 16(1),
Chan, D. W., & Chan, L. (2007). Creativity and drawing abilities of 50–57.
Chinese students in Hong Kong: Is there a connection? New Horizons Davis, G. A., Subkoviak, M. J., Runco, M. A., Ebersole, P., & Mraz,
in Education, 55(3), 77–95. W. (1991). How Do You Think Test. Journal of Social Behavior and
Chand, I., & Runco, M. A. (1993). Problem finding skills as components in Personality, 6, 161–167.
the creative process. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 155–162. DeMoss, K., Milich, R., & DeMers, S. (1993). Gender, creativity, depres-
doi:10.1016/0191-8869(93)90185-6 sion, and attributional style in adolescents with high academic abil-
Charyton, C., & Snelbecker, G. E. (2007). Engineers’ and musicians’ ity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 21, 455–467. doi:10.1007/
choices of self-descriptive adjectives as potential indicators of creativ- BF01261604
ity by gender and domain. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Devi, G. (2015). Creativity in medicine. Neurology, 84(8), e53–e54.
Arts, 1, 91–99. doi:10.1037/1931-3896.1.2.91 doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000001298
Chen, C., Kasof, J., Himsel, A. J., Greenberger, E., Dong, Q., & Xue, Dew, R. (2009). Cognitive style, creativity framing and effects. The Journal
G. (2002). Creativity in drawings of geometric shapes: A cross-cultural of Creative Behavior, 43, 234–261. doi:10.1002/(ISSN)2162-6057
examination with the consensual assessment technique. Journal of Cross- Dietrich, A. (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of creativity. Psychonomic
Cultural Psychology, 33, 171–187. doi:10.1177/0022022102033002004 Bulletin & Review, 11, 1011–1026. doi:10.3758/BF03196731
Cheng, Y., Wang, W., Liu, K., & Chen, Y. (2010). Effects of associa- Dollinger, S. J. (2007). Creativity and conservatism. Personality and
tion instruction on fourth graders’ poetic creativity in Taiwan. Creativity Individual Differences, 43, 1025–1035. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.02.023
Research Journal, 22(2), 228–235. Dollinger, S. J., Urban, K. K., & James, T. A. (2004). Creativity and open-
Christensen, P. R., Guilford, J. P., Merrifield, P. R., & Wilson, R. C. (1965a). ness: Further validation of two creative product measures. Creativity
Alternate Uses—Form A. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Research Journal, 16, 35–47. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1601_4
20 A. M. ABDULLA AND B. CRAMOND

Dow, G. T., & Mayer, R. E. (2004). Teaching students to solve Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York, NY:
insight problems: Evidence for domain specificity in creativity training. McGraw-Hill.
Creativity Research Journal, 16, 389–398. doi:10.1080/1040041040953 Guilford, J. P. (1968). Creativity, intelligence, and their educational impli-
4550 cations. San Diego, CA: Knapp.
Ee, J., Seng, T. O., & Kwang, N. A. (2007). Styles of creativity: Adaptors Guilford, J. P. (1973). Characteristics of creativity. Springfield: Illinois
and innovators in a Singapore context. Asia Pacific Education Review, 8, State Office of the Superintendent of Public Instruction, Gifted Children
364–373. doi:10.1007/BF03026466 Section.
Eisenstadt, J. M. (1966). Problem-solving ability of creative and non- Han, Q., Hu, W., Liu, J., Jia, X., & Adey, P. (2013). The influence of
creative college students. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 30, 81–83. peer interaction on students’ creative problem-finding ability. Creativity
doi:10.1037/h0022902 Research Journal, 25, 248–258. doi:10.1080/10400419.2013.813754
Ekvall, G. (1996). Organizational climate for creativity and innovation. Hass, R. W. (2015). Feasibility of online divergent thinking assess-
European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 5, 105–123. ment. Computers in Human Behavior, 46, 85–93. doi:10.1016/
doi:10.1080/13594329608414845 j.chb.2014.12.056
Engle, D. E., Mah, J. J., & Sadri, G. (1997). An empirical comparison Hébert, T. P., Cramond, B., Neumeister, K. L. S., Millar, G., & Silvian,
of entrepreneurs and employees: Implications for innovation. Creativity A. F. (2002). E. Paul Torrance: His life, accomplishments, and legacy.
Research Journal, 10, 45–49. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1001_5 Storrs: The University of Connecticut, The National Research Center on
Fasko, D. (2001). Education and creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 13, the Gifted and Talented (NRC/GT).
317–327. doi:10.1207/S15326934CRJ1334_09 Hennessey, B. A. (1994). The consensual assessment technique: An
Fischer, S., & Freund, P. A. (2014). The professional context as a pre- examination of the relationship between ratings of product and pro-
dictor for response distortion in the Adaption–Innovation Inventory— cess creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 7, 193–208. doi:10.1080/
An investigation using mixture distribution item response theory 10400419409534524
models. International Journal of Testing, 14, 218–244. doi:10.1080/ Hennessy, B. A., & Amabile, T. M. (1999). Consensual assessment tech-
15305058.2013.862250 nique. In M. A. Runco & S. R. Prtizker (Eds.), Encyclopedia of creativity
Florida, R. L. (2012). The rise of the creative class: Revisited. New York, (Vol. 1, pp. 347–359). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
NY: Basic Books. Hennessey, B. A., Amabile, T., & Mueller, J. (2011). Consensual assess-
Fontenot, N. A. (1993). Effects of training in creativity and creative problem ment. In M. A. Runco & S. R. Pritzker (Eds.), Encyclopedia of creativity
finding upon business people. The Journal of Social Psychology, 133(1), (pp. 253–260). San Diego, CA: Academic Press/Elsevier.
11–22. doi:10.1080/00224545.1993.9712114 Hocevar, D. (1979). Ideational fluency as a confounding factor in the mea-
Furnham, A., Crump, J., Batey, M., & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2009). surement of originality. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 191–196.
Personality and ability predictors of the “Consequences” Test of doi:10.1037/0022-0663.71.2.191
Divergent Thinking in a large non-student sample. Personality and Hocevar, D. (1980). Intelligence, divergent thinking, and creativity.
Individual Differences, 46, 536–540. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.12.007 Intelligence, 4(1), 25–40. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(80)90004-5
Gallucci, N. T., Middleton, G., & Kline, A. (2000). Perfectionism and Houtz, J. C., Selby, E., Esquivel, G. B., Okoye, R. A., Peters, K. M., &
creative strivings. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 34, 135–141. Treffinger, D. J. (2003). Creativity styles and personal type. Creativity
doi:10.1002/jocb.2000.34.issue-2 Research Journal, 15, 321–330. doi:10.1207/S15326934CRJ1504_2
Gardner, K. G., & Moran, J. D. (1990). Family adaptability, cohesion, IBM. (2012). Leading through connections: Highlights of the Global Chief
and creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 3, 281–286. doi:10.1080/ Executive Officer Study. Somers, NY: IBM Global Business Services.
10400419009534361 Isaksen, S. G., Lauer, K. J., & Ekvall, G. (1999). Situational Outlook
Gardner, S., Gershon, A., Merrifield, P. R., & Guilford, J. P. (1965). Making Questionnaire: A measure of the climate for creativity and change.
objects. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Psychological Reports, 85, 665–674. doi:10.2466/pr0.1999.85.2.665
Gattis, M., & Raudsepp, E. (1969). How creative are you? Writer’s Digest, Isaksen, S. G., & Puccio, G. J. (1988). Adaption–innovation and the
4961. Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking: The level-style issue revisited.
Getzels, J. W. (1975). Creativity: Prospects and issues. In I. A. Taylor & J. Psychological Reports, 63, 659–670. doi:10.2466/pr0.1988.63.2.659
W. Getzels (Eds.), Perspectives in creativity (pp. 326–344). Chicago, IL: Jacobson, C. M. (1993). Cognitive styles of creativity: Relations of scores
Aldine. on the Kirton Adaption–Innovation Inventory and the Myers-Briggs
Goff, K., & Torrance, E. P. (2000). Abbreviated Torrance Test for adults. Type Indicator among managers in USA. Psychological Reports, 72,
Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service. 1131–1138. doi:10.2466/pr0.1993.72.3c.1131
Goldsmith, R. E. (1984). Personality characteristics associated with Jarosewich, T., Pfeiffer, S. I., & Morris, J. (2002). Identifying gifted stu-
adaption–innovation. The Journal of Psychology, 117, 159–165. dents using teacher rating scales: A review of existing instruments.
doi:10.1080/00223980.1984.9923672 Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 20, 322–336. doi:10.1177/
González, J. M., & Gilbert, O. (1979). Measurement of creativity: 073428290202000401
Development of an instrument and study of its reliability. Revista Kaduson, H. G., & Schaefer, C. E. (1991). Concurrent validity of the
Latinoamericana de Psicología, 11, 363–381. Creative Personality Scale of the Adjective Check List. Psychological
Gough, H. G. (1979). A creative personality scale for the Adjective Check Reports, 69, 601–602. doi:10.2466/pr0.1991.69.2.601
List. Journal of Personality And Social Psychology, 37, 1398–1405. Kaltsounis, B. (1979). Black students’ personality traits associated
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.37.8.1398 with originality and elaboration. Psychological Reports, 44, 83–87.
Gough, H. G., & Heilbrun, A. B. (1965). The adjective check list manual. doi:10.2466/pr0.1979.44.1.83
Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Kaufman, J. C., Plucker, J. A., & Baer, J. (2008). Essentials of creativity
Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5, 444–454. assessment. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
doi:10.1037/h0063487 Keller, C. J., Lavish, L. A., & Brown, C. (2007). Creative styles and gen-
Guilford, J. P. (1957). Creative abilities in the arts. Psychological Review, der roles in undergraduates students. Creativity Research Journal, 19,
64, 110–118. doi:10.1037/h0048280 273–280. doi:10.1080/10400410701397396
Guilford, J. P. (1965, August/September). Frames of reference for cre- Khaleefa, O. H., Erdos, G., & Ashria, I. H. (1996). Gender and creativity in
ative behavior in the arts. Paper presented at the Conference on Creative an Afro-Arab Islamic culture: The case of Sudan. The Journal of Creative
Behavior in the Arts, Los Angeles, CA. Behavior, 30, 52–60. doi:10.1002/jocb.1996.30.issue-1
CREATIVITY: WHAT IT IS AND HOW IT IS MEASURED 21

Kharkhurin, A. V., & Samadpour Motalleebi, S. N. (2008). The impact of Mumford, M. D., & Simonton, D. K. (1997). Creativity in the workplace:
culture on the creative potential of American, Russian, and Iranian col- People, problems, and structures. Journal of Creative Behavior, 31, 1–6.
lege students. Creativity Research Journal, 20, 404–411. doi:10.1080/ doi:10.1002/j.2162-6057.1997.tb00776.x
10400410802391835 Nassif, C., & Quevillon, R. (2008). The development of a preliminary cre-
Khatena, J. J., & Torrance, E. P. (1976). Khatena-Torrance Creative ativity scale for the MMPI-2: The C Scale. Creativity Research Journal,
Perception Inventory. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service. 20, 13–20. doi:10.1080/10400410701839918
Khatena, J. J., & Torrance, E. P. (1988). Thinking creatively with sounds and Natrielli, D. F., Silva, M. E., & Natrielli, D. G. (2013). Medicine and cre-
words. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service. ativity in medical psychology. São Paulo Medical Journal, 131, 281–282.
Kim, K. H., & Hull, M. F. (2012). Creative personality and anticreative doi:10.1590/1516-3180.2013.1314638
environment for high school dropouts. Creativity Research Journal, 24, Newman, J. L. (2005). Talents and type ills: The effects of the Talents
169–176. doi:10.1080/10400419.2012.677318 Unlimited model on creative productivity in gifted youngsters. Roeper
Kim, K. H., & VanTassel-Baska, J. (2010). The relationship between creativ- Review, 27, 84–90. doi:10.1080/02783190509554295
ity and behavior problems among underachieving elementary and high O’Quin, K., & Besemer, S. P. (2006). Using the Creative Product Semantic
school students. Creativity Research Journal, 22, 185–193. doi:10.1080/ Scale as a metric for results-oriented business. Creativity & Innovation
10400419.2010.481518 Management, 15(1), 34–44. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8691.2006.00367.x
Kirton, M. (1976). Adaptors and innovators: A description and mea- Palaniappan, A. K. (1996). A cross-cultural study of creative
sure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 61, 622–629. doi:10.1037/ perceptions. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82(1), 96–98. doi:10.2466/
0021-9010.61.5.622 pms.1996.82.1.96
Kozbelt, A., Beghetto, R. A., & Runco, M. A. (2010). Theories of creativity. Pannells, T. C., & Claxton, A. F. (2008). Happiness, creative ideation, and
In J. C. Kaufman & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of locus of control. Creativity Research Journal, 20, 67–71. doi:10.1080/
creativity (pp. 20–47). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. 10400410701842029
Kuan Chen, T. (2015). Assessing a Chinese version of the Runco Ideational Patnoe, S. (1985). The relationship between selected scales of the
Behavior Scale. Social Behavior & Personality: An International Journal, Adjective Check List and musical creativity: A validation study.
43, 1111–1122. doi:10.2224/sbp.2015.43.7.1111 Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 23(1), 64–66. doi:10.3758/BF0332
Lack, S. A., Kumar, V. K., & Arevalo, S. (2003). Fantasy proneness, cre- 9780
ative capacity, and styles of creativity. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 96(1), Piers, E. V., Daniels, J. M., & Quackenbush, J. F. (1960). The identifica-
19–24. doi:10.2466/pms.2003.96.1.19 tion of creativity in adolescents. Journal of Educational Psychology, 51,
Lee, I. R., & Kemple, K. (2014). Preservice teachers’ personality traits and 346–351. doi:10.1037/h0042669
engagement in creative activities as predictors of their support for chil- Plucker, J. A., & Dana, R. Q. (1998). Alcohol, tobacco, and marijuana use:
dren’s creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 26, 82–94. doi:10.1080/ Relationships to undergraduate students’ creative achievement. Journal
10400419.2014.873668 of College Student Development, 39, 472–483.
Loo, R., & Shiomi, K. (1997). A cross-cultural examination of the Kirton Plucker, J. A., Runco, M. A., & Lim, W. (2006). Predicting ideational behav-
Adaption–Innovation Inventory. Personality and Individual Differences, ior from divergent thinking and discretionary time on task. Creativity
22, 55–60. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(96)00178-X Research Journal, 18, 55–63. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1801_7
Lu, C., & Luh, D. (2012). A comparison of assessment methods and Plucker, J. A., Waitman, G. R., & Hartley, K. A. (2011). Education and cre-
raters in product creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 24, 331–337. ativity. In M. A. Runco & S. R. Pritzker (Eds.), Encyclopedia of creativity
doi:10.1080/10400419.2012.730327 (pp. 435–440). San Diego, CA: Academic Press/Elsevier.
Makel, M. C., & Plucker, J. A. (2008). Creativity. In S. I. Pfeiffer Pohlman, L. (1996). Creativity, gender and the family: A study of cre-
(Ed.), Handbook of giftedness in children: Psychoeducational the- ative writers. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 30, 1–24. doi:10.1002/
ory, research, and best practices (pp. 247–270). New York, NY: jocb.1996.30.issue-1
Springer. Pollick, M. F., & Kumar, V. K. (1997). Creativity styles of supervising
Mathisen, G. E., & Einarsen, S. (2004). A review of instruments managers. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 31, 260–270. doi:10.1002/
assessing creative and innovative environments within organizations. jocb.1997.31.issue-4
Creativity Research Journal, 16, 119–140. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj16 Preckel, F., Wermer, C., & Spinath, F. M. (2011). The interrelation-
01_12 ship between speeded and unspeeded divergent thinking and reasoning,
Mayfield, M., & Mayfield, J. (2010). Developing a scale to measure the cre- and the role of mental speed. Intelligence, 39, 378–388. doi:10.1016/
ative environment perceptions: A questionnaire for investigating garden j.intell.2011.06.007
variety creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 22, 162–169. doi:10.1080/ Reis, S. M., & Renzulli, J. S. (1991). The assessment of creative products
10400419.2010.481511 in programs for gifted and talented students. Gifted Child Quarterly, 35,
Mednick, S. A. (1968). Remote Associates test. Journal of Creative 128–134. doi:10.1177/001698629103500304
Behavior, 2, 213–214. doi:10.1002/j.2162-6057.1968.tb00104.x Renzulli, J. S. (1977). The enrichment triad model: A guide for developing
Meneely, J., & Portillo, M. (2005). The adaptable mind in design: defensible programs for the gifted and talented. Mansfield Center, CT:
Relating personality, cognitive style, and creative performance. Creativity Creative Learning Press.
Research Journal, 17, 155–166. doi:10.1080/10400419.2005.9651476 Renzulli, J. S. (2005). The three-ring conception of giftedness: A develop-
Milgram, R. M., & Hong, E. (1999). Creative out-of-school activities in mental model for promoting creative productivity. In R. J. Sternberg &
intellectually gifted adolescents as predictors of their life accomplishment J. E. Davidson (Eds.), Conceptions of giftedness (2nd ed., pp. 246–279).
in young adults: A longitudinal study. Creativity Research Journal, 12, New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
77–87. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1202_1 Renzulli, J. S., & Hartman, R. K. (1971). Scale for rating behav-
Millar, G. W. (2002). The Torrance kids at mid-life. Connecticut, NJ: Ablex. ioral characteristics of superior students. Exceptional Children, 38,
Montgomery, D., Hodges, P. A., & Kaufman, J. S. (2004). An exploratory 243–248.
study of the relationship between mood states and creativity self- Renzulli, J. S., & Reis, S. M. (2007). A technology based program
perceptions. Creativity Research Journal, 16, 341–344. doi:10.1080/ that matches enrichment resources with student strengths. International
10400419.2004.9651463 Journal of Emerging Technologies in Learning, 2(3), 57–64.
22 A. M. ABDULLA AND B. CRAMOND

Renzulli, J. S., Siegle, D., Reis, S. M., Gavin, M. K., & Sytsma Reed, Runco, M. A., Walczyk, J. J., Acar, S., Cowger, E. L., Simundson, M.,
R. E. (2009). An investigation of the reliability and factor structure & Tripp, S. (2014). The incremental validity of a short form of the
of four new scales for rating the behavioral characteristics of supe- Ideational Behavior Scale and usefulness of distractor, contraindicative,
rior students. Journal of Advanced Academics, 21, 84–108. doi:10.1177/ and lie scales. Journal of Creative Behavior, 48, 185–197. doi:10.1002/
1932202X0902100105 jocb.47
Rhodes, M. (1961). An analysis of creativity. Phi Delta Kappan, 42, Scott, G., Leritz, L. E., & Mumford, M. D. (2004). The effectiveness of
305–310. creativity training: A quantitative review. Creativity Research Journal,
Robelen, E. W. (2012). States mulling creativity indexes for schools. 16, 361–388. doi:10.1080/10400410409534549
Education Week, 31(19), 1–2. Retrieved from http://www.edweek.org/ew/ Sheldon, K. M. (1995). Creativity and self-determination in personality.
articles/2012/02/02/19creativity_ep.h31.html Creativity Research Journal, 8, 25–36. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj0801_3
Rudowicz, E. (2003). Creativity and culture: A two way interac- Siegel, S. M., & Kaemmerer, W. F. (1978). Measuring the perceived sup-
tion. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 47, 273–290. port for innovation in organizations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 63,
doi:10.1080/00313830308602 553–562. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.63.5.553
Runco, M. A. (1986a). The discriminant validity of gifted children’s diver- Silvia, P. J. (2011). Subjective scoring of divergent thinking: Examining
gent thinking test scores. Gifted Child Quarterly, 30, 78–82. doi:10.1177/ the reliability of unusual uses, instances, and consequences tasks.
001698628603000207 Thinking Skills and Creativity, 6(1), 24–30. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2010.
Runco, M. A. (1986b). Predicting children’s creative performance. 06.001
Psychological Reports, 59, 1247–1254. doi:10.2466/pr0.1986.59.3.1247 Silvia, P. J., Wigert, B., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Kaufman, J. C. (2012).
Runco, M. A. (1996). Personal creativity: Definition and developmental Assessing creativity with self-report scales: A review and empirical eval-
issues. New Directions for Child & Adolescent Development, 1996, 3–30. uation. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6, 19–34.
doi:10.1002/(ISSN)1534-8687 doi:10.1037/a0024071
Runco, M. A. (2007). Creativity theories and themes: Research, develop- Simonton, D. K. (2012). Taking the U.S. Patent Office criteria seri-
ment, and practice. Boston, MA: Elsevier Academic Press. ously: A quantitative three-criterion creativity definition and its
Runco, M. A. (2008). Commentary: Divergent thinking is not synonymous implications. Creativity Research Journal, 24, 97–106. doi:10.1080/
with creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2, 10400419.2012.676974
93–96. doi:10.1037/1931-3896.2.2.93 Skinner, N. F. (1996). Behavioral implications of adaption–innovation: II.
Runco, M. A. (2010). Divergent thinking, creativity, and ideation. In J. C. Adaption–innovation and motivation for uniqueness. Social Behavior and
Kaufman & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity Personality, 24, 231–234. doi:10.2224/sbp.1996.24.3.231
(pp. 413–446). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Smith, D. E., & Tegano, D. W. (1992). Relationship of scores on two person-
Runco, M. A. (2011). Implicit theories. In M. A. Runco & S. Pritzker ality measures: Creativity and self-image. Psychological Reports, 71(1),
(Eds.), Encyclopedia of creativity (2nd ed., pp. 641–646). San Diego, CA: 43–49. doi:10.2466/pr0.1992.71.1.43
Elsevier. Sowden, P., Pringle, A., & Gabora, L. (2015). The shifting sands of cre-
Runco, M. A. (2014). Creativity: Theories and themes: Research, develop- ative thinking: Connections to dual process theory and implications for
ment, and practice. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Academic Press. creativity training. Thinking and Reasoning, 21(1), 40–60. doi:10.1080/
Runco, M. A., & Albert, R. S. (1986). The threshold theory regarding 13546783.2014.885464
creativity and intelligence: An empirical test with gifted and nongifted Sriraman, B., & Lee, K. (2011). The elements of creativity and giftedness in
children. Creative Child & Adult Quarterly, 11, 212–218. mathematics. Rotterdam, The Netherlands: Sense.
Runco, M. A., & Chand, I. (1995). Cognition and creativity. Educational Sternberg, R. J. (2011). Componential models of creativity. In M. A. Runco
Psychology Review, 7, 243–267. doi:10.1007/BF02213373 & S. R. Pritzker (Eds.), Encyclopedia of creativity (pp. 415–421). San
Runco, M. A., & Dow, G. T. (2004). Assessing the accuracy of judgments of Diego, CA: Academic Press/Elsevier.
originality on three divergent thinking tests. Korean Journal of Thinking Sternberg, R. J., Jarvin, L., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2010). Explorations in
& Problem Solving, 14(2), 5–14. giftedness. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Runco, M. A., Ebersole, P., & Mraz, W. (1991). Creativity and self- Storme, M., & Lubart, T. (2012). Conceptions of creativity and rela-
actualization. Journal of Social Behavior & Personality, 6, 161–167. tions with judges’ intelligence and personality. The Journal of Creative
Runco, M. A., Illies, J. J., & Eisenman, R. (2005). Creativity, original- Behavior, 46, 138–149. doi:10.1002/jocb.10
ity, and appropriateness: What do explicit instructions tell us about their Sung Eun Chung, S., & Meneely, J. (2012). Profiling group dynamics
relationships? Journal of Creative Behavior, 39, 137–148. doi:10.1002/ within business and design student teams: Relationships among person-
j.2162-6057.2005.tb01255.x ality traits, problem-solving styles, and creative performance. Journal of
Runco, M. A., & Jaeger, G. (2011). Products. Advantages of the Interior Design, 37(3), 23–45. doi:10.1111/joid.2012.37.issue-3
rCAB. Athens, GA: Creativity Test Services. Retrieved from http:// Taylor, I. A., & Fish, T. A. (1979). The Creative Behavior Disposition Scale.
creativitytestingservices.com/ Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 11(1), 95–97. doi:10.1037/
Runco, M. A., & Jaeger, G. (2012). The standard definition of h0081577
creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 24, 92–96. doi:10.1080/ Taylor, I. A., & Sandler, B. E. (1972, September). Use of a creative product
10400419.2012.650092 inventory for evaluating products of chemists. Proceedings of the 80th
Runco, M. A., Noble, E. P., Reiter-Palmon, R., Acar, S., Ritchie, T., Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, 7(Pt. 1),
& Yurkovich, J. (2011). The genetic basis of creativity and ideational 311–312.
fluency. Creativity Research Journal, 23, 376–380. doi:10.1080/ Torrance, E. P. (1962a). Assessing the creative thinking abilities. In E. P.
10400419.2011.621859 Torrance (Ed.), Guiding creative talent (pp. 16–43). Englewood Cliffs,
Runco, M. A., Plucker, J. A., & Lim, W. (2001). Development and psycho- NJ: Prentice-Hall.
metric integrity of a measure of ideational behavior. Creativity Research Torrance, E. P. (1962b). Non-test ways of identifying the creatively gifted.
Journal, 13, 393–400. doi:10.1207/S15326934CRJ1334_16 Gifted Child Quarterly, 6, 71–75.
Runco, M. A., & Pritzker, S. R. (2011). Encyclopedia of creativity. Torrance, E. P. (1962c). Testing and creative talent. Educational Leadership,
Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Academic Press/Elsevier. 20(1), 7–72.
CREATIVITY: WHAT IT IS AND HOW IT IS MEASURED 23

Torrance, E. P. (1964). Guide for administrating and scoring Non-verbal West, R. E., Tateishi, I., Wright, G. A., & Fonoimoana, M. (2012).
Form B: Minnesota Test of Creative Thinking. Minneapolis: University of Innovation 101: Promoting undergraduate innovation through a two-
Minnesota. day boot camp. Creativity Research Journal, 24, 243–251. doi:10.1080/
Torrance, E. P. (1966). Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking: Norms technical 10400419.2012.677364
manual (Research ed.). Princeton, NJ: Personnel Press. White, A., Shen, F., & Smith, B. L. (2002). Judging advertising creativ-
Torrance, E. P. (1981). Thinking creatively in action and movement. ity using the Creative Product Semantic Scale. The Journal of Creative
Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service. Behavior, 36, 241–253. doi:10.1002/jocb.2002.36.issue-4
Torrance, E. P. (1988). The nature of creativity as manifest in its testing. White, A., & Smith, B. L. (2001). Assessing advertising creativity using the
In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of creativity: Contemporary psycho- Creative Product Semantic Scale. Journal of Advertising Research, 41(6),
logical perspectives (pp. 43–75). New York, NY: Cambridge University 27–34. doi:10.2501/JAR-41-6-27-34
Press. Williams, F. E. (1993). Creativity assessment packet examiner’s manual.
Torrance, E. P., McCarthy, B., Kolesinski, M., & Smith, J. (1988). Style Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
of learning and thinking—Grades 6–12—Youth form. Bensenville, IL: Wilson, R. C., Guilford, J. P., & Christensen, P. R. (1953). The measure-
Scholastic Testing Service. ment of individual differences in originality. Psychological Bulletin, 50,
Trilling, B., & Fadel, C. (2009). 21st Century skills: Learning for life in our 362–370. doi:10.1037/h0060857
times. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Zha, P., Walczyk, J. J., Griffith-Ross, D. A., Tobacyk, J. J., &
Urban, K. K., & Jellen, H. G. (1996). Test for creative thinking—Drawing Walczyk, D. F. (2006). The impact of culture and individualism–
production (TCT-DP) manual. Frankfurt, Germany: Swets Test Services. collectivism on the creative potential and achievement of American and
Wakefield, J. F. (1989). Creativity and cognition: Some implications for Chinese adults. Creativity Research Journal, 18, 355–366. doi:10.1207/
arts education. Creativity Research Journal, 2, 51–63. doi:10.1080/ s15326934crj1803_10
10400418909534300 Zhang, L. (2015). Fostering successful intellectual styles for creativity. Asia
Wallach, M. A., & Kogan, N. (1965). Modes of thinking in young children. Pacific Education Review, 16, 183–192. doi:10.1007/s12564-015-9378-5
New York, NY: Holt, Reinhart, & Winston. Zysberg, L., & Schenk, T. (2013). Creativity, religiosity, and politi-
Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fourth edi- cal attitudes. Creativity Research Journal, 25, 228–231. doi:10.1080/
tion technical and interpretive manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological 10400419.2013.783761
Corporation.

AUTHOR BIOS

Ahmed M. Abdulla is a faculty member in the Gifted Education Program at the Arabian Gulf University, Manama,
Bahrain. His research interests include creativity, divergent thinking, problem finding, and emotional intelligence. He
has presented a number of workshops on giftedness and creativity in Bahrain, Saudi Arabia, Oman, and the United
States and has presented papers at national and international conferences. E-mail: ahmedcreatv@gmail.com

Bonnie Cramond, PhD, is a professor of educational psychology at the University of Georgia. She has been the
director of the Torrance Center for Creativity and Talent Development, a board member of the National Association
for Gifted Children, and editor of the Journal of Secondary Education. She is on the advisory board for the American
Creativity Association, the Future Problem Solving Program International, and the Global Center for Gifted and
Talented Children; is a member of the International Creativity Society; is on the review board for several journals;
and has published numerous articles, chapters, and a book (another is in the works). A national and international
speaker, she has visited over 35 countries working toward infusing creativity into classrooms at all levels. She was
honored in 2011 to be invited to give a TEDx Talk on creativity and has gratefully received other international and
national honors. E-mail: bcramond@uga.edu

You might also like