You are on page 1of 7

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

2002, Vol. 70, No. 4, 1022–1028 0022-006X/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0022-006X.70.4.1022

Does Imaginal Exposure Exacerbate PTSD Symptoms?


Edna B. Foa, Lori A. Zoellner, Norah C. Feeny, Elizabeth A. Hembree, and Jennifer Alvarez-Conrad
University of Pennsylvania

Symptom exacerbation (i.e., treatment side effects) has often been neglected in the psychotherapy
literature. Although prolonged exposure has gained empirical support for the treatment of chronic
posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), some have expressed concern that imaginal exposure, a component
of this therapy, may cause symptom exacerbation, leading to inferior outcome or dropout. In the present
study, symptom exacerbation was examined in 76 women with chronic PTSD. To define a “reliable”
exacerbation, we used a method incorporating the standard deviation and test–retest reliability of each
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

outcome measure. Only a minority of participants exhibited reliable symptom exacerbation. Individuals
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

who reported symptom exacerbation benefited comparably from treatment. Further, symptom exacerba-
tion was unrelated to dropout. Thus, although a minority of individuals experienced a temporary
symptom exacerbation, this exacerbation was unrelated to outcome.

Over the past two decades, imaginal exposure has gained in- ences by victims, who may, as a result, develop an aversion to
creasing attention as a treatment for chronic posttraumatic stress coming to therapy. They further posited that after treatment, sexual
disorder (PTSD). Imaginal exposure, or reliving, involves repeated assault victims may exhibit higher levels of distress than they did
recounting of the traumatic experience, an intervention that has before treatment and may be less likely to enter treatment in the
been shown to promote habituation of pathological anxiety (e.g., future.
Foa & Chambless, 1978; Levin, Cook, & Lang, 1982). Imaginal Kilpatrick and Best’s (1984) assertions were not supported by
exposure was first used to treat PTSD in combat veterans (e.g., studies that evaluated the efficacy of prolonged exposure therapy,
Cooper & Clum, 1989; Keane, Fairbank, Caddell, & Zimering, in which imaginal exposure occupies a central role, with assault-
1989). Veterans with PTSD treated with imaginal exposure, then related PTSD. Indeed, among female victims of sexual and non-
termed imaginal flooding, improved relative to wait-list control sexual assault with chronic PTSD, prolonged exposure has con-
and standard treatment, respectively, although the magnitude of sistently yielded significant reductions of PTSD, depression, and
their improvement was moderate. Several other studies in veterans anxiety symptoms compared with control conditions (Foa, Roth-
have reported similar results (e.g., Boudewyns & Hyer, 1990; baum, Riggs, & Murdock, 1991; Foa et al., 1999). Furthermore,
Boudewyns, Hyer, Woods, Harrison, & McCranie, 1990). comparable results have been found using prolonged exposure
Despite this success, trauma experts have been reluctant to use with victims of various traumas (e.g., Devilly & Spence, 1999;
imaginal exposure with victims of traumatic events other than Marks, Lovell, Noshirvani, Livanou, & Thrasher, 1998).
combat, in particular sexual assault victims, for fear of “retrauma- Despite the accumulating evidence for the efficacy of prolonged
tizing” them and increasing their suffering. Specifically, Kilpatrick exposure therapy, concerns about the possible exacerbation of
and Best (1984) suggested that high levels of anxiety induced PTSD and anxiety symptoms during or following this treatment
through imaginal exposure may be construed as negative experi- have persisted. Recently, Tarrier et al. (1999) found both cognitive
therapy and imaginal exposure equally effective interventions for
chronic PTSD. In their discussion, however, they asserted that “a
significantly greater number of patients receiving IE worsened
Edna B. Foa, Lori A. Zoellner, Norah C. Feeny, Elizabeth A. Hembree,
over treatment. However, this effect disappeared by follow-up”
and Jennifer Alvarez-Conrad, Center for the Treatment and Study of
Anxiety, Department of Psychiatry, University of Pennsylvania.
(Tarrier et al., 1999, p. 17). For further discussion of Tarrier et al.’s
Lori A. Zoellner is now at the Department of Psychology, University of claim regarding worsening of symptoms following exposure, see
Washington. Norah C. Feeny is now at the Department of Psychiatry, Case Devilly and Foa (2001).
Western Reserve University. Imaginal exposure has been hypothesized to increase not only
Preparation of this article was supported by Grant MH42173 to Edna B. PTSD and anxiety but also other pathological signs. In a descrip-
Foa from the National Institute of Mental Health. We thank Lisa Jaycox, tion of six cases, Pitman et al. (1991) proposed that imaginal
Elizabeth Meadows, Constance Dancu, Lee Fitzgibbons, Jane Folk, and exposure can exacerbate feelings of guilt, self-blame, and failure.
Elna Yadin, who recruited, entered, or treated some of the clients in this They further suggested that although imaginal exposure may
study. We also express our appreciation to Grant Devilly for his thoughtful
heighten awareness of these emotions and cognitions, it may not be
comments on an earlier version of this article, particularly in relation to
defining reliable exacerbation.
effective in resolving these posttrauma negative appraisals. In
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Edna B. another article, Pitman, Orr, Altman, et al. (1996) asserted that in
Foa, Center for the Treatment and Study of Anxiety, University of Penn- comparison with flooding (i.e., exposure), eye movement desen-
sylvania, 3535 Market Street, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19104. Email: sitization and reprocessing (EMDR) was “less anxiety provoking
foa@mail.med.upenn.edu for patients (as well as therapists), better tolerated, and preductive

1022
SYMPTOM EXACERBATION AND EXPOSURE 1023

of fewer adverse consequences” (Pitman, Orr, Altman, & Longpre, conducted by experienced clinicians, which included the PTSD Symptom
1996, p. 428). These conclusions were drawn despite the fact that Scale—Interview Version (PSS–I; Foa, Riggs, Dancu, & Rothbaum, 1993)
although examined in separate studies, EMDR and flooding pro- and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Axis I Disorders—
duced comparable outcomes. For further discussion of these is- Patient Edition (SCID–I/P, Version 2; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams,
1995). Women who were assaulted by an intimate partner with whom they
sues, see Cahill and Frueh (1997).
had an ongoing relationship and those with primary diagnoses of organic
In summary, many well-controlled studies have documented the
mental disorder, schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, or current alcohol or drug
efficacy and efficiency of prolonged exposure therapy in reducing dependence were excluded from the study. All participants were inter-
PTSD and related pathology, such as depression and anxiety (see viewed at the Center for the Treatment and Study of Anxiety or at Women
Foa & Rothbaum, 1998, for a review). However, the question Organized Against Rape (WOAR) in urban Philadelphia, Pennsylvania. Of
remains to what extent, if at all, this therapy is associated with all women who were seen for an intake evaluation, about 16% were not
symptom exacerbation. In the present study, we sought to examine eligible for the study, most often because either PTSD was not the primary
this question by analyzing biweekly assessments of PTSD, depres- diagnosis or they did not meet PTSD diagnostic criteria. An additional 11%
sion, and anxiety conducted over the course of treatment. In this were eligible but declined participation or did not follow through with
entering the program. The remaining women entered the study. The present
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

way, symptoms were monitored prior to the onset of imaginal


investigation included all available participants (N ⫽ 76) from the larger,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

exposure and during several weeks of imaginal exposure. We


ongoing outcome study, who were randomized to an active treatment
addressed three main questions: (a) Is the onset of imaginal expo-
condition (i.e., were not randomized to wait list) and whose data had been
sure associated with an exacerbation of PTSD or anxiety symp- entered and verified for accuracy.
toms? (b) Is the onset of imaginal exposure associated with an Participants ranged in age from 17 to 57 years (M ⫽ 32.78, SD ⫽ 10.46).
increase in depression symptoms? and (c) Do patients who show A total of 35.5% were African American, 2.6% Asian, 56.6% Caucasian,
an exacerbation in these symptoms fail to improve in treatment or and 5.3% Hispanic. Of the women, 69.7% were victims of a sexual
drop out of treatment? assault, 19.7% of nonsexual assault, and 10.5% of childhood sexual assault.
In exploring these questions, a key issue becomes how to The average time since assault was almost 7 years (6.86 years), with a
operationally define clinically significant exacerbation. Notably, range of 3 months to 39 years. Sixty-three percent of the women were
the issue of how to define clinically significant improvement has single, 23.2% married or cohabiting, and 13.7% separated or divorced. A
total of 26.1% had a high school education or less, 46.6% had some
received much attention (e.g., Jacobson, Roberts, Berns, &
technical school or college education, and 27.3% had a college education.
McGlinchey, 1999; Jacobson & Truax, 1991), whereas less con-
Of the participants, 32.4% reported a total income of over $30,000 per
sideration has been given to the issue of how to determine symp- year, 46.5% between $10,000 and $30,000, and 21.1% below $10,000 per
tom worsening or exacerbation. To carefully assess possible symp- year.
tom exacerbation, we used multiple measures (i.e., PTSD severity,
depression, anxiety) and conservative criteria for exacerbation. Measures
Clinically, symptoms fluctuate from one assessment to another,
and this fluctuation reflects both “true” and error variance. How- PSS–I. The PSS–I is a 17-item interview assessing the severity of each
of the DSM–IV PTSD symptoms during the past 2 weeks and ascertaining
ever, clinically significant symptom exacerbation implies deterio-
PTSD diagnostic status. Each symptom is rated on a 4-point scale ranging
ration beyond error variability. To measure reliable, clinically from 0 (not at all) to 3 (very much). Subscale scores are calculated by
meaningful deterioration, we took into account both the standard summing items in each of the PTSD symptom clusters: reexperiencing,
deviation and the test–retest reliability of each of our measures, avoidance, and arousal. Foa et al. (1993) found high internal consistency
using a method based on the standard error of measurement (see (␣ ⫽ .85), moderate to high correlations with other measures of psycho-
Devilly & Foa, 2001). To determine a cutoff score defining reli- pathology, high test–retest reliability (r ⫽ .80), and high interrater reli-
able exacerbation, we considered two types of samples: psychiatric ability (k ⫽ .91). For the present study, internal consistency based on
and nonclinical samples. The former samples typically yield larger Cronbach’s alpha was .80. A randomly selected 10% sample of the inter-
standard deviations than the latter and thus would produce higher views conducted with the participants in this investigation were indepen-
cutoff scores, which in turn would lead to the classification of a dently rated by another evaluator. The interrater reliability was high (k ⫽
.94). The data for treatment outcome were derived from the PSS–I.
smaller number of participants as “significantly deteriorated.” In
Standardized Assault Interview (SAI; Rothbaum, Foa, Riggs, Murdock,
the present study, we opted to use the more conservative alterna- & Walsh, 1992). The SAI is a 136-item semistructured interview, which
tive (i.e., a higher number of people classified as deteriorated) by gathers information regarding demographic variables, previous victimiza-
using values derived from normal samples. tion history, assault characteristics, and interactions with the legal system.
In regard to previous victimization history, the SAI assesses the presence
Method of other Criterion A traumas, in either adulthood or childhood, on the basis
of the DSM–IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). In regard to the
Participants index assault-related characteristics, the SAI specifically assesses the time
since assault, perception of life threat during the assault, and injury during
Participants were female victims of sexual and nonsexual assault with a the assault. An earlier version of this interview measure reported an
primary diagnosis of chronic PTSD, with minimum symptom duration of 3 interrater reliability of .90. Demographic information, time since assault,
months. This cutoff point for chronic PTSD was derived from the Diag- and assault type were derived from this interview.
nostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed., DSM–IV; PTSD Symptom Scale—Self-Report (PSS–SR; Foa et al., 1993). The
American Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria. Participants were re- PSS–SR consists of 17 questions that correspond to PTSD symptoms from
cruited for a larger PTSD treatment outcome study through referrals from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd ed., rev.,
medical professionals, local victim assistance agencies, and media adver- DSM–III–R; American Psychiatric Association, 1987), each rated on a
tisements. Eligibility for the study was determined in the initial evaluation scale ranging from 0 to 3 for frequency and severity. The PSS–SR has been
1024 FOA, ZOELLNER, FEENY, HEMBREE, AND ALVAREZ-CONRAD

found to be internally consistent (␣ ⫽ .91) and stable over a period of one to the traumatic memory was begun in Session 3 and consisted of reliving
month (r ⫽ .74). Subscales assessing reexperiencing, avoidance, and the traumatic event in imagination and describing the memory in the
arousal are also internally consistent and stable. Higher scores on this present tense. The assault memory was repeated if necessary to allow total
measure indicate more severe symptoms. The data regarding symptom reliving of 45– 60 min in duration. Imaginal exposure was repeated in all
change following introduction of imaginal exposure were derived from the subsequent treatment sessions for 30 – 45 min. The exposure was tape-
PSS–SR. recorded, and participants were instructed to listen to the tape daily at
Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI; Beck, Epstein, Brown, & Steer, 1988). home. Additional homework included in vivo exposure to objectively safe
The BAI is a 21-item inventory measure for trait anxiety. It has good situations that caused anxiety or were avoided. The procedure in the PE/CR
internal consistency, acceptable reliability, and acceptable convergent and treatment was identical, except that cognitive restructuring was introduced
discriminant validity (Fydrich, Dowdall, & Chambless, 1992; Hewitt & in Session 3, and imaginal exposure in Session 4. All subsequent sessions
Norton, 1993). included 30 – 45 min of imaginal exposure and 15–25 min of cognitive
Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & restructuring. For a detailed description of treatments, see Foa and Roth-
Erbaugh, 1961). The BDI is a 21-item inventory used to assess depres- baum (1998). Because imaginal exposure in the PE/CR condition was not
sion, with items rated on a 0 to 3 scale. This instrument has moderate to initiated until the fourth treatment session, any symptom exacerbation in
good internal consistency, with alpha ranging between .58 to .93. the PE/CR condition between Week 2 and Week 4 would not be related to
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

the onset of imaginal exposure. Thus, PE/CR treatment provided a control


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

condition to PE treatment, where imaginal exposure was introduced at


Procedure Week 3, and consequently, the Week 4 assessment would reflect the effects
of this treatment component.
Independent evaluations were conducted by interviewers blind to treat-
ment assignment. Evaluations were conducted before and after treatment,
which consisted of either 9 or 12 therapy sessions. Number of sessions was Results
determined on the basis of a participant’s response to treatment by Session
8: For those who obtained 70% or greater reduction in self-reported PTSD The Impact of Initial Imaginal Exposure on Symptom
symptoms, treatment terminated at Session 9; for the remainder, therapy Exacerbation: A Comparison of PE and PE/CR
was extended to 12 sessions.
Independent evaluations. As part of a treatment study for chronic For our initial analysis, we operationally defined a symptom
PTSD, trained female evaluators that had either a master’s or a PhD degree exacerbation (i.e., reliable deterioration) from Week 2 to Week 4,
in psychology and had extensive training in instrument administration using data obtained from a normal sample on 2-week test–retest
interviewed participants. The pretreatment interview included the PSS–I. reliability and standard error of measurement for each of the three
After their eligibility was ascertained, participants were given a series of measures (Devilly & Foa, 2001). The 2-week test–retest reliability
self-report measures. These included the BDI, the PSS–SR, and the BAI. of the PSS–SR is .83 and the standard deviation is 10.54 in a
Participants returned the self-report measures at their first treatment ses- non-PTSD sample (Foa, Cashman, Jaycox, & Perry, 1997). There-
sion, within 2 weeks of the initial evaluation. Participants were reimbursed fore, the standard error of measurement at any time point for the
$50 for the initial interview and completion of the self-report question- scale would be SE ⫽10.54公(1 ⫺ .83), yielding 4.35. The standard
naires. During the first treatment session, the therapist administered the
error of the difference between two administrations of a scale
assault interview (SAI). Posttreatment evaluations were conducted within 2
weeks of treatment completion and included the PSS–I, PSS–SR, BDI,
would be calculated using 公2(SE2). Thus, for the PSS–SR, the
and BAI. Participants were reimbursed a total of $100 for completion standard error of the differences between two administrations
of treatment, posttreatment evaluation, and posttreatment self-report would be calculated as 公2(4.322). Thus, reliable exacerbation for
questionnaires. the PSS–SR would be defined as an increase of greater than 6.15.
Biweekly assessments. In addition to the pre- and posttreatment eval- For the BAI, the standard deviation for a normal sample is 9.6 and
uations, participants completed self-report measures prior to treatment test–retest is .62 (Creamer, Foran, & Bell, 1995). Thus, reliable
sessions for every 2 weeks during the active treatment phase. All self- exacerbation would be defined as an increase of greater than 8.37.
report questionnaires (i.e., PSS–SR, BDI, and BAI) were completed im- For the BDI, the standard deviation for a normal sample is 6.82
mediately before Sessions 2, 4, 6, and 8 (and 10, if treatment consisted and test–retest reliability is .78 (Oliver & Simmons, 1985). Thus,
of 12 sessions). These self-report measures were rated for the previous
reliable exacerbation would be defined as an increase of greater
2-week period and provided the indexes of change in PTSD, anxiety, and
depression symptoms that were used to determine presence or absence of
than 4.53 for depression (BDI). For our main chi-square analyses,
exacerbation. power to detect medium effects with an alpha of .05 ranged from
Treatment. Participants were randomly assigned to weekly sessions of .64 to .71 depending on sample size, and power to detect large
treatment by prolonged imaginal and in vivo exposure alone (PE; n ⫽ 41, effects with an alpha of .05 ranged from .97 to .99, depending on
53.9%) or by prolonged imaginal and in vivo exposure combined with sample size (Cohen, 1988).
cognitive restructuring (PE/CR; n ⫽ 35, 46.1%), or were assigned to a As described earlier, for the PE condition, the first imaginal
9-week wait list followed by treatment (not included in the present inves- exposure occurred at Week 3, and therefore symptom exacerbation
tigation). Individual treatment was conducted by female master’s- and at Week 4 could be attributable to the initiation of imaginal
PhD-level therapists who received ongoing supervision and followed man- exposure; for the PE/CR condition, the first imaginal exposure
uals that specified precise treatment guidelines for each session. Treatment
occurred at Week 4, and therefore symptom exacerbation at
sessions were 90 –120 min in length. Data from both treatment completers
(n ⫽ 64, 84.2%) and treatment dropouts (n ⫽ 12, 15.8%) were included in
Week 4 would be attributable to other sources. To examine
the analyses. whether undergoing the first imaginal exposure was associated
For the PE condition, Sessions 1 and 2 were devoted to information with an increase in symptoms from Week 2 to Week 4, we
gathering, presentation of treatment rationale, construction of in vivo conducted chi-square analyses, comparing the PE and PE/CR
exposure hierarchy, and initiation of in vivo homework. Imaginal exposure conditions. The majority of individuals in the PE condition
SYMPTOM EXACERBATION AND EXPOSURE 1025

(84.6%, n ⫽ 33) and in the PE/CR condition (97.1%, n ⫽ 33) did We next examined the hypothesis that individuals who experi-
not exhibit an exacerbation of PTSD symptoms (PSS–SR) upon enced an exacerbation would fail to improve in treatment. To do
initiation of imaginal exposure. However, for those who did ex- this, we compared posttreatment scores for individuals who expe-
hibit an exacerbation in PTSD symptoms at Week 4, 85.7% (n ⫽ rienced an exacerbation with those who did not experience an
6) were in the PE condition and 14.3% (n ⫽ 1) were in the PE/CR exacerbation. Means and standard deviations can be seen in Ta-
condition, ␹2(N ⫽ 73) ⫽ 3.24, p ⫽ .07, C ⫽ .21.1 Thus, if an ble 1. Posttreatment scores did not differ between individuals who
exacerbation did occur, it tended to be associated with initiation of experienced an exacerbation and those who did not, on either
imaginal exposure, though this difference did not reach PTSD severity or general anxiety: for PSS–SR, t(57) ⫽ 1.41, p ⫽
significance. .17; for BAI, t(59) ⫽ 1.42, p ⫽ .16. There was a trend toward
For general anxiety (BAI), the majority of individuals in the PE posttreatment scores differing on depression (BDI), t(59) ⫽ 1.83,
condition (71.8%, n ⫽ 28) and in the PE/CR condition (91.2%, p ⫽ .07, partial ␩2 ⫽ .05, with the exacerbation group reporting
n ⫽ 31) did not exhibit an exacerbation upon the initiation of
slightly higher depression scores. Finally, the presence of PTSD
imaginal exposure. However, of the individuals who reported an
diagnosis, measured through clinician interview with the PSS–I, at
exacerbation in anxiety symptoms, 78.6% (n ⫽ 11) were in the PE
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

posttreatment was compared between individuals who experienced


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

condition and 21.4% (n ⫽ 3) were in the PE/CR condition, ␹2(N ⫽


an exacerbation and those who did not. Overall, 68.3% of all
73) ⫽ 4.40, p ⬍ .05, C ⫽ .24. Thus, there was a relationship
participants no longer met PTSD diagnostic criteria. There was no
between initiation of imaginal exposure and exacerbation in anx-
iety symptoms. reliable difference between groups in rate of PTSD diagnosis at
Finally, for depression (BDI), a similar pattern emerged. The posttreatment, ␹2(N ⫽ 60) ⫽ 0.34, p ⫽ 0.55.
majority of individuals in the PE condition (87.2%, n ⫽ 34) and in
the PE/CR condition (97.1%, n ⫽ 33) did not exhibit an exacer- Exacerbation of Symptoms and Posttreatment End-State
bation of depressive symptoms upon initiation of imaginal expo-
Functioning
sure. However, for those who did exhibit exacerbation in depres-
sive symptoms at Week 4, 83.3% (n ⫽ 5) were in the PE condition To further examine the relationship between symptom exacer-
and 16.7% (n ⫽ 1) were in the PE/CR condition, ␹2(N ⫽
bation and response to treatment, we generated a composite index
73) ⫽ 2.35, p ⫽ .13. Thus, if an exacerbation did occur, it tended
for clinically significant improvement (i.e., good end-state func-
to be associated with initiation of imaginal exposure, though this
tioning) and compared symptom exacerbation in those with good
difference did not reach significance.
and poor end-state functioning (for rationale, see Foa et al., 1999).
Participants were categorized as attaining good end-state function-
The Relationship Between Symptom Exacerbation and ing if their severity score was 15 or below on the PSS–I and 10 or
Improvement: Posttreatment Comparison below on both the BDI and the BAI.
Overall, 57.4% of all participants obtained good end-state func-
To examine the impact of initial imaginal exposure on symptom tioning. A total of 52.9% (n ⫽ 9) of those with symptom exacer-
exacerbation across both the PE/CR and the PE conditions, we bation and 59.1% (n ⫽ 26) of those without exacerbation obtained
calculated symptom change from Week 2 to Week 6 for all good end-state functioning, ␹2(N ⫽ 61) ⫽ 0.19, p ⫽ .66. Thus,
participants. Extending the analyses to Week 6 was necessary in individuals who experienced an exacerbation of symptoms did not
order to include the PE/CR condition, as imaginal exposure was benefit from treatment less than those without such an
instituted at Week 4, after the assessment had been conducted. exacerbation.
Thus, assessment of exacerbation was examined over three ses-
sions of imaginal exposure for the PE group and over two sessions
of imaginal exposure for the PE/CR group. Overall, 10.5% (n ⫽ 8) Symptom Exacerbation and Treatment Dropout
reported an exacerbation in PTSD symptoms. For individuals who
reported an exacerbation in PTSD symptoms (PSS–SR), the in- Twelve participants (15.8%) dropped out of treatment before
crease ranged from 7 to 17 points (M ⫽ 9.50, SD ⫽ 4.07). A total Session 9, with 1 participant dropping out after Week 2, 6 partic-
of 21.1% (n ⫽ 16) reported an exacerbation in anxiety symptoms ipants dropping out between Weeks 4 and 6, and 5 participants
(BAI), ranging from 9 to 34 (M ⫽ 17.46, SD ⫽ 8.41). Fi- dropping out between Weeks 6 and 8. To examine whether par-
nally, 9.2% (n ⫽ 7) reported an exacerbation in depression symp- ticipants who showed symptom exacerbation were more likely to
toms (BDI), ranging from 5 to 16 (M ⫽ 8.58, SD ⫽ 3.98). Thus, drop out of treatment, we conducted a chi-square analysis. For
only a minority of women reported an exacerbation in symptoms; individuals who showed an exacerbation in symptoms, 15% (n ⫽
an increase in general anxiety was the most common form of 3) dropped out of treatment compared with 16.7% (n ⫽ 9) among
exacerbation. individuals who did not show such exacerbation, ␹2(N ⫽
Overall, 20 participants out of 76 reported a reliable exacerba- 74) ⫽ 0.03, p ⫽ .86. Thus, individuals who dropped out of
tion of PTSD, anxiety, or depression symptoms. Because of the treatment were not more likely to show symptom exacerbation.
overlap in participants reporting an exacerbation, we conducted
subsequent analyses combining exacerbation groups, in which
reliable exacerbation was defined as a reliable exacerbation on any 1
Pearson’s coefficient of contingency, C, was used as a measure of
of the three measures. effect size for chi-square analyses (Cohen, 1988).
1026 FOA, ZOELLNER, FEENY, HEMBREE, AND ALVAREZ-CONRAD

Table 1 apy often leads to exacerbation of trauma-related symptoms, a high


Means and Standard Deviations Across Treatment Sessions rate of dropout, and reduced benefits from treatment. Our results
for Individuals Reporting No Exacerbation or suggest that a minority of participants receiving treatment for
Exacerbation of Symptoms chronic PTSD do show reliable, albeit on average small, increases
in symptoms after the introduction of imaginal exposure: 10.5%
PSS–SR BAI BDI reported an increase in PTSD symptoms, 21.1% in anxiety,
Time M SD M SD M SD
and 9.2% in depression. One week after the first imaginal exposure
in the PE condition (at the Week 4 assessment), more individuals
Pretreatment in that condition reported exacerbation in anxiety symptoms than
No increase 33.21 8.85 24.18 11.35 24.55 8.27 in the PE/CR condition, where the first imaginal exposure occurred
Increase 29.90 7.00 22.52 11.21 23.64 9.70
after the Week 4 assessment. This comparison suggests that the
Week 2
No increase 28.94 10.45 21.86 11.79 22.56 9.18 introduction of imaginal exposure may be associated with a tem-
Increase 24.76 7.86 15.40 11.16 17.99 7.94 porary increase in general anxiety symptoms.
Week 4
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

The suggestion that symptom exacerbation may impede re-


No increase 24.13 10.16 18.05 11.93 17.55 8.90
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sponse to treatment (e.g., Kilpatrick & Best, 1984) did not receive
Increase 28.55 8.00 26.08 14.33 18.83 6.49
Week 6 support in the present study. Individuals reporting initial symptom
No increase 19.58 12.16 14.19 13.30 13.36 8.73 exacerbation on PTSD, anxiety, or depression benefited from
Increase 22.44 8.04 21.78 15.55 15.22 7.26 treatment as much as those who did not report such an exacerba-
Week 8 tion. It is important to note that even individuals who occupied the
No increase 16.76 11.89 13.75 13.65 12.71 10.19
upper range of symptom exacerbation benefited from treatment.
Increase 20.29 12.51 15.28 10.74 15.82 10.49
Posttreatment The contention that imaginal exposure encourages premature treat-
No increase 9.09 8.28 6.84 9.35 7.11 7.31 ment termination did not gain support from our data either, as 6 of
Increase 14.13 14.52 11.18 9.57 11.85 9.60 the noncompleters (50%) terminated therapy between Weeks 4
and 6, and 5 (42%) did so between Weeks 6 and 8, well after the
Note. PSS–SR ⫽ PTSD Symptom Scale—Self-Report; BAI ⫽ Beck
Anxiety Inventory; BDI ⫽ Beck Depression Inventory; PTSD ⫽ posttrau- introduction of imaginal exposure. Moreover, the number of treat-
matic stress disorder. ment completers (those who completed 9 or 12 sessions) who
reported symptom exacerbation did not differ from that of non-
completers. Overall, the present results do not support the concern
Post Hoc Analyses: Individuals With High Exacerbation (e.g., Kilpatrick & Best, 1984; Pitman et al., 1991) that prolonged
of Symptoms exposure therapy leads to general symptom exacerbation, which in
Although no differences in response to treatment and dropout turn encourages dropouts and hampers response to treatment for
rates were obtained between those with and those without symp- PTSD.
tom exacerbation, it is possible that only extreme symptom exac- In summary, prolonged exposure therapy has gained more em-
erbation has an impact on response to treatment. To explore this pirical support for its efficacy than any other treatment for PTSD
post hoc hypothesis, we examined the symptom reduction from (see Foa & Meadows, 1997, for a review), and some studies even
pre- to posttreatment in individuals who reported an exacerbation suggest that it is the most efficient treatment for this disorder (as
of two times greater than the established cutoff on either PTSD cited in Hembree & Foa, 2000). However, enthusiasm for this
severity (⬎ 12.3), anxiety (⬎ 16.74), or depression (⬎ 9.06). Six treatment has been tempered by concerns about its safety. The
participants met this criterion: 1 for PTSD severity and for anxi- results of the present study are reassuring about the tolerability of
ety, 4 for anxiety only, and 1 for depression only. Of those 6, 1 exposure treatment for clients with chronic PTSD. Indeed, our
(16.7%) dropped out of treatment. Four out of the 5 treatment results indicate that clinicians need not abandon the use of expo-
completers exhibited symptom reduction: Case A reported a de- sure when clients report temporary symptom worsening. As noted
crease in PTSD symptoms (PSS–SR) from 38 to 11, in anxiety above, although exposure therapy does not jeopardize patients’
symptoms (BAI) from 28 to 2, and in depressive symptoms (BDI) welfare, the introduction of imaginal exposure may cause a tem-
from 29 to 14; Case B reported a decrease in PTSD symptoms porary increase in anxiety symptoms for a small minority of
from 25 to 8, in anxiety from 11 to 4, and in depression from 16 patients. Given this finding, clinicians would be well advised to
to 5; Case C reported a decrease in PTSD symptoms from 47 to 40, inform patients about this possibility at the outset of treatment and
in anxiety symptoms from 40 to 28, and in depression from 47 to to reassure them about the transient nature of such increases.
22; Case D reported a decrease in PTSD symptoms from 26 to 11 Several caveats regarding these findings should be noted. First,
and reported increases in anxiety symptoms from 11 to 14 and in the sample in the present study was limited to female assault
depression from 14 to 23; and finally, Case E reported a decrease victims, and so the generalizability to other trauma populations is
in PTSD symptoms from 30 to 6, in anxiety symptoms from 33 unknown. Second, the therapists in this study were highly experi-
to 7, and in depression from 22 to 6. enced in treating female assault victims and in managing anxiety in
the context of imaginal exposure. Indeed, some researchers have
suggested (e.g., Dreessen & Arntz, 1998) that many therapeutic
Discussion
complications may be overcome by a skilled therapist and that,
In this article, we examined the assertion that imaginal exposure therefore, such complications do not necessarily influence treat-
to traumatic memories in the context of prolonged exposure ther- ment outcome. Thus, the generalizability of the present results to
SYMPTOM EXACERBATION AND EXPOSURE 1027

therapists less experienced with the treatment of chronic PTSD is Dreessen, L., & Arntz, A. (1998). The impact of personality disorders on
also unknown. Third, because the cell sizes were small for some of treatment outcome of anxiety disorders: Best-evidence synthesis. Behav-
our comparisons, there may not have been enough power to detect iour Research & Therapy, 36, 483–504.
small to medium-size effects on individual self-report measures. First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. (1995).
However, in this study, we were primarily interested in examining Structured clinical interview for DSM–IV Axis I disorders—patient
edition (SCID–I/P, Version 2). New York: New York State Psychiatric
whether large differences (i.e., clients worsening significantly and
Institute, Biometrics Research Department.
showing evidence of an inability to tolerate or benefit from expo- Foa, E. B., Cashman, L., Jaycox, L., & Perry, K. (1997). The validation of
sure) existed between those who did and those who did not show a self-report measure of PTSD: The Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale.
symptom exacerbation. Across analyses, power was sufficient to Psychological Assessment, 9, 445– 451.
detect such large, clinically significant effects. Foa, E. B., & Chambless, D. L. (1978). Habituation of subjective anxiety
The method by which we operationalized reliable exacerbation during flooding in imagery. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 16,
is novel and thus has not been validated in comparison with other 391–399.
methods for determining clinically significant deterioration. Al- Foa, E. B., Dancu, C. V., Hembree, E. A., Jaycox, L. H., Meadows, E. A.,
& Street, G. P. (1999). A comparison of exposure therapy, stress
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

though we selected a relatively sensitive, or conservative, method


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

of detecting deterioration, other definitions may yield different inoculation training, and their combination for reducing posttraumatic
results. The study of treatment deterioration warrants additional stress disorder in female assault victims. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 67, 194 –200.
attention. Future research should include a weekly measure of
Foa, E. B., & Meadows, E. A. (1997). Psychosocial treatments for post-
symptoms of interest, which would allow for a higher resolution of
traumatic stress disorder: A critical review. In J. Spence, J. M. Darley,
symptom patterns over time. Although we failed to find a rela- & D. J. Foss (Eds.), Annual review of psychology (Vol. 48, pp. 449 –
tionship between symptom exacerbation and benefit from treat- 480). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.
ment, the examination of predictors of symptom increase may shed Foa, E. B., Riggs, D. S., Dancu, C. V., & Rothbaum, B. O. (1993).
light on individual variables and response to treatment. Reliability and validity of a brief instrument for assessing post-traumatic
stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 6, 459 – 473.
Foa, E. B., & Rothbaum, B. O. (1998). Treating the trauma of rape. New
References York: Guilford Press.
Foa, E. B., Rothbaum, B. O., Riggs, D., & Murdock, T. (1991). Treatment
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical man-
ual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author. of post-traumatic stress disorder in rape victims: A comparison between
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical man- cognitive– behavioral procedures and counseling. Journal of Consulting
ual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. and Clinical Psychology, 59, 715–723.
Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., & Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory Fydrich, T., Dowdall, D., & Chambless, D. L. (1992). Reliability and
for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. Journal of validity of the Beck Anxiety Inventory. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 6,
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 893– 897. 55– 61.
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). Hembree, E., & Foa, E. B. (2000). PTSD: Psychological factors and
An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychia- psychosocial interventions. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 61(Suppl. 7),
try, 4, 561–571. 33–39.
Boudewyns, P. A., & Hyer, L. (1990). Physiological response to combat Hewitt, P. L., & Norton, R. (1993). The Beck Anxiety Inventory: A
memories and preliminary treatment outcome in Vietnam veterans: psychometric analysis. Psychological Assessment, 5, 408 – 412.
PTSD patients treated with direct therapeutic exposure. Behavior Ther- Jacobson, N. S., Roberts, L. J., Berns, S. B., & McGlinchey, J. B. (1999).
apy, 21, 63– 87. Methods for defining and determining the clinical significance of treat-
Boudewyns, P. A., Hyer, L., Woods, M. G., Harrison, W. R., & McCranie, ment effects: Description, application, and alternatives. Journal of Con-
E. (1990). PTSD among Vietnam veterans: An early look at treatment sulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 300 –307.
outcome using direct therapeutic exposure. Journal of Traumatic Jacobson, N. S., & Truax, P. (1991). Clinical significance: A statistical
Stress, 3, 359 –368. approach to defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research.
Cahill, S. P., & Frueh, B. C. (1997). Flooding versus eye movement Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 12–19.
desensitization and reprocessing therapy: Relative efficacy has yet to be Keane, T. M., Fairbank, J. A., Caddell, J. M., & Zimering, R. T. (1989).
investigated—Comment on Pitman et al. (1996). Comprehensive Psy- Implosive (flooding) therapy reduces symptoms of PTSD in Vietnam
chiatry, 38, 300 –303. combat veterans. Behavior Therapy, 20, 245–260.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analyses for the behavioral sciences Kilpatrick, D. G., & Best, C. L. (1984). Some cautionary remarks on
(2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. treating sexual assault victims with implosion. Behavior Therapy, 15,
Cooper, N. A., & Clum, G. A. (1989). Imaginal flooding as a supplemen- 421– 423.
tary treatment for PTSD in combat veterans: A controlled study. Behav- Levin, D. N., Cook, E. W., & Lang, P. J. (1982). Fear imagery and fear
ior Therapy, 3, 381–391. behavior: Psychophysiological analysis of clients receiving treatment for
Creamer, M., Foran, J., & Bell, R. (1995). The Beck Anxiety Inventory in anxiety disorder. Psychophysiology, 19, 571–572.
a non-clinical sample. Behavior Research & Therapy, 33, 477– 485. Marks, I., Lovell, K., Noshirvani, H., Livanou, M., & Thrasher, S. (1998).
Devilly, G. J., & Foa, E. B. (2001). Comments on Tarrier et al.’s study and Treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder by exposure and/or cognitive
the investigation of exposure and cognitive therapy. Journal of Consult- restructuring: A controlled study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55,
ing and Clinical Psychology, 69, 114 –116. 317–325.
Devilly, G. J., & Spence, S. H. (1999). The relative efficacy and treatment Oliver, J. M., & Simmons, M. E. (1985). Affective disorders and depres-
distress of EMDR and a cognitive– behavior trauma protocol in the sion as measured by the diagnostic interview schedule and the Beck
amelioration of posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disor- Depression Inventory in an unselected adult population. Journal of
ders, 13, 131–157. Clinical Psychology, 41, 469 – 477.
1028 FOA, ZOELLNER, FEENY, HEMBREE, AND ALVAREZ-CONRAD

Pitman, R. K., Altman, B., Greenwald, E., Longpre, R. E., Macklin, M. L., Rothbaum, B. O., Foa, E. B., Riggs, D. S., Murdock, T., & Walsh, W.
Poire, R. E., & Steketee, G. S. (1991). Psychiatric complication during (1992). A prospective examination of post-traumatic stress disorder in
flooding therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Clinical rape victims. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 5, 455– 475.
Psychiatry, 52, 17–20. Tarrier, N., Pilgrim, H., Sommerfield, C., Fragher, B., Reynolds, M.,
Pitman, R. K., Orr, S. P., Altman, B., & Longpre, R. E. (1996). Emotional Graham, E., & Barrowclough, C. (1999). A randomized trial of cognitive
processing during eye movement desensitization and reprocessing ther- therapy and imaginal exposure in the treatment of chronic posttraumatic
apy of Vietnam veterans with chronic posttraumatic stress. Comprehen- stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67,
sive Psychiatry, 37, 419 – 429. 13–18.
Pitman, R. K., Orr, S. P., Altman, B., Longpre, R. E., Poire, R. E., &
Macklin, M. L. (1996). Emotional processing and outcome of imaginal Received October 2, 2000
flooding therapy in Vietnam veterans with chronic posttraumatic stress Revision received December 29, 2001
disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 37, 409 – 418. Accepted January 7, 2002 䡲
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

You might also like