You are on page 1of 8

Art Therapy

Journal of the American Art Therapy Association

ISSN: 0742-1656 (Print) 2159-9394 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/uart20

Recognizing Emotions: Testing an Intervention for


Children With Autism Spectrum Disorders

Donna Abely Richard, William More & Stephen P. Joy

To cite this article: Donna Abely Richard, William More & Stephen P. Joy (2015) Recognizing
Emotions: Testing an Intervention for Children With Autism Spectrum Disorders, Art Therapy, 32:1,
13-19, DOI: 10.1080/07421656.2014.994163

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/07421656.2014.994163

Published online: 24 Mar 2015.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 5049

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 11 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=uart20
Art Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy Association, 32(1) pp. 13–19, © AATA, Inc. 2015

Recognizing Emotions: Testing an Intervention


for Children With Autism Spectrum Disorders

Donna Abely Richard, William More, and Stephen P. Joy

Abstract
In recent years art therapy practice with individuals
A severely impaired capacity for social interaction is one of with autism has emerged as an area of interest possibly due
the characteristics of individuals with autism spectrum disorder to the higher rates of autism diagnoses, the responsiveness
(ASD). Deficits in facial emotional recognition processing may of those diagnosed with ASD to art, and the expansion of
be associated with this limitation. The Build-a-Face (BAF) art art therapy into new clinical settings. In a recent study
therapy intervention was developed to assist with emotional research on autism was identified as the third most impor-
recognition through the viewing and manipulating of three- tant population needing attention according to art therapy
dimensional facial features on a mannequin-like bust. This researchers (Kaiser & Deaver, 2013). This study also indi-
cated the need for more outcome research studies, research
pilot study tested the abilities of 19 children with ASD to
on interventions that produce specific outcomes with cer-
identify emotions depicted in facial photographs before and tain populations or specific disorders, and experimental and
after a single BAF intervention session. The results indicated quasi-experimental studies. Recent years have seen a drive
no statistically significant difference between the treatment and toward evidence-based research that supports and encour-
control groups; however, the treatment group had greater ages the use of art therapy as an effective form of therapeutic
improvement than the control. Future research involving intervention. Several articles have focused on the need to
multiple BAF sessions is needed to test the effectiveness of BAF conduct scientific research on this subject (Kapitan, 2006;
for promoting emotion recognition. Martin, 2009; Slayton, D’Archer, & Kaplan, 2010). These
publications have ranged from descriptions of research
approaches and suggestions for topics (Deaver, 2002) to the
benefits of conducting these procedures (Anderson, 2001).
Introduction The Build-a-Face (BAF) art therapy intervention was
created by the first author for the purpose of this research
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD), also known as perva-
and was designed to address the needs of children with
sive developmental disorder, is a diagnostic category consist-
ASD. It was specifically designed to assist with facial emo-
ing of four separate disorders: autistic disorder, Asperger’s
tional recognition that would reinforce identification of
syndrome, childhood disintegrative disorder, and pervasive
emotions through the use of three-dimensional materials.
development disorder not otherwise specified (American
ASD research presents several hypotheses regarding
Psychiatric Association, 2013). ASD is an early onset disor-
impairments in social interaction through studies of the
der; delays or abnormal functioning must be noted from
identification of facial expression and facial recognition
early childhood, even if those symptoms are unrecognized
in both adults and children with autism. Other research
until later childhood years. These disorders are largely char-
has focused on emotion recognition. Hobson, Ouston,
acterized by impairments in communication, social interac-
and Lee (1988) compared the abilities of adolescents
tions, and imagination. Impairments in social interactions
and young adults with autism to recognize emotions in
consist of deficits in the usage or comprehension of eye con-
photographs of full and partial human faces with the
tact, facial expression, and body postures. Also, individuals
recognition capacities of non-autistic adolescents with
with ASD exhibit a lack of social and emotional responsive-
mental retardation. The researchers directed the partici-
ness. In 2014 the Centers for Disease Control reported that
pants to match individual expressions of emotions across
one out of every 68 children in the United States received
individuals and subsequently in photographs featuring
an autism spectrum disorder diagnosis.
inverted faces. Individuals with autism experienced
greater difficulty identifying emotions in photographs of
Editor’s Note: Donna Abely Richard is a graduate of Alber- faces with visible eyes and noses but with the mouths
tus Magnus College, New Haven, CT. William More is Adjunct
Assistant Professor in the Masters of Arts in Art Therapy Program
omitted. However, participants with autism were signifi-
and Stephen P. Joy is Professor of Psychology at the same institu- cantly more successful with matching identity and emo-
tion. Correspondence concerning this article may be addressed to tion in upside-down faces than the participants with
the first author at donna.abely@snet.net mental retardation. Although Hobson et al. concluded
Color versions of one or more figures in this article can be that autistic adolescents and adults are proficient in rec-
found online at www.tandfonline.com/uart. ognizing identity and emotion in upside-down faces; the
13
14 INTERVENTION FOR CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

researchers expressed doubt that individuals with autism The children showing typical development focused
fully understand the meaning of these expressions and instead on the most socially relevant information, such as
emotions. human movement.
Deruelle, Rondan, Gepner, and Fagot (2006) studied Although numerous studies have examined ASD as it
impairments in visual processing in children with autism relates to facial recognition and expression, all hoping to
and Asperger’s syndrome. The researchers based their stud- provide a clearer understanding of the deficits in communi-
ies on the findings of Hobson et al. (1988), in that they cation and social interaction, only a few in art therapy have
claimed that children with autism tend to process the parts considered the artistic development of those with ASD.
of a complex object rather than the object as a whole. Der- Selfe (1977) stated that artistically gifted children with
uelle et al. sought to characterize the processing of visual autism follow the same developmental drawing patterns as
stimuli (facial and geometrical patterns) by children with typical children, but at an earlier age. Charman and Baron-
ASD and to determine whether they exhibit a global Cohen (1993) examined drawing development in children
(whole) preference or a local (parts) preference. In their with autism who lack particular artistic talent and con-
study, children with autism and Asperger’s syndrome had cluded that their formation of drawing skills is related to
difficulty reproducing the facial and geometrical patterns nonverbal mental age and appears to follow normal devel-
from the stimuli. The findings indicated that the clinical opmental stages. Results from Charman and Baron-
group, in comparison to the control group, was deficient in Cohen’s study also indicate that individuals with ASD can
configural processing (the ability to perceive relationships create intellectually realistic drawings from memory. Sacks
among facial features), but expressed a preference for parts (as cited in Furniss, 2008) also noted that some children
of the geometric patterns. with ASD exhibit high levels of visual memory.
ASD research has attempted to identify the causes of The art of some young children with ASD demonstrates
the deficits in recognizing and producing facial affects, as self-expression, imagination, and creativity (Selfe, 1977) and
these deficits contribute to social impairments. Wallace, also represents a method of understanding beyond speech
Coleman, and Bailey (2008) tested the facial expression rec- and language (Lowenfeld & Brittain, 1987). According to
ognition profile of adults with ASD. Their main goals were Betts (2003), the communication inherent in drawings of
to measure participants’ abilities to recognize facial expres- children with ASD could potentially provide information
sion while evaluating the following competing hypotheses: about perception, memory, and cognitive abilities. Evans
(a) impaired configural processing (e.g., two eyes above a and Dubowski (2001) noted that children with ASD could
nose and mouth), or disabled processing of facial structure benefit from early therapeutic intervention and learning to
and spatial distancing of features (e.g., nose-mouth dis- communicate through art during the developmental pro-
tance); (b) the “theory of mind” or the inability to recognize cess. Betts (2003) developed the Face Stimulus Assessment
other people’s mental states; and (c) atypical amygdala func- (FSA), a projective drawing that can be used with children
tions. The amygdala is the section of the brain that is pri- and adolescents with disabilities such as ASD. The FSA
marily responsible for the processing and memory of consists of a series of three stimulus images: The first is a
emotional reactions, including fear recognition. The results standardized image of a human face, the second is an out-
of the study by Wallace et al. support prior evidence that line of a face, and the third is a blank page. Betts intended
suggested that individuals with ASD are significantly less for the FSA to provide clients with two consecutive oppor-
successful than individuals without ASD at recognizing tunities to draw a face that would assess their memory and
upright facial expressions of fear, sadness, and disgust, indi- visual retention capacity. She speculated that the FSA might
cating impairment in configural processing. Individuals be beneficial to the evaluation of psychosocial and cognitive
with ASD were able to more accurately recognize sadness in development, creative potential, and graphic ability.
the inverted facial expressions than the control group. The In a separate but similar line of research, Martin (2008)
amygdala functioning hypothesis could explain the ten- conducted a pilot study of the Portrait Drawing Assessment
dency for these individuals to misidentify fearful expressions that examined face processing by asking participants to cre-
as angry ones. The researchers concluded that there might ate portrait drawings. The study participants, who were
be multiple explanations for deficits in facial recognition. diagnosed with ASD, used the researcher’s face to render
Recent ASD research has used eye-tracking technol- their portraits. The researcher concluded that this assess-
ogy to measure visual fixation patterns of individuals with ment might be used therapeutically for the development of
autism. Klin, Jones, Schultz, Volkmar, and Cohen (2002) facial processing and facial recognition skills.
conducted visual scan path studies, which provided evi- Because research suggests that children with ASD
dence that individuals with ASD focus on the lower por- struggle with facial and emotional recognition (Hobson
tion of the face (the mouth) rather than the upper portion et al., 1988; Klin et al., 2002) and art therapists have begun
(the eyes). More recently, Klin, Lin, Gorrindo, Ramsey, to use the rendering of a face or portrait to help with visual
and Jones (2009) suggested that children with autism retention and facial processing, we reasoned that the manip-
focus on mouths because of audiovisual synchrony (or “lip ulation of facial features on a three-dimensional head would
sync”), which is the exact match of lip motion and speech be useful for eliciting and reinforcing emotion-based expres-
sound. The study compared toddlers with autism to tod- sions for children with ASD. Three-dimensional figures
dlers with typical development and found that the 2-year- more accurately represent the three-dimensional quality of
olds with autism preferred the audiovisual synchronies. the human face and their realistic nature may expedite the
RICHARD / MORE / JOY 15

process of emotion identification. The Expressive Therapies emotions in facial expressions. This test was designed to
Continuum (ETC; Kagin & Lusebrink, 1978), a frame- measure individual differences in the accurate sending and
work for the use of art in therapy, may provide support for receiving of nonverbal social information. The DANVA
the use of three-dimensional objects to achieve the goal of 2-CF also determines a person’s ability to identify the non-
emotional recognition. Information processing occurs at verbal communication of emotion, differing in high and
each of the first three levels on the ETC: Kinesthetic/Sen- low intensity. The four emotions measured include happi-
sory, Perceptual/Affective, and Cognitive/Symbolic. By ness, anger, sadness, and fear. The emotions are represented
using three-dimensional materials to recreate emotions with in 24 photographs of children’s facial expressions (6 photo-
facial features, first the Kinesthetic/Sensory level is engaged graphs for each emotion), and the participants are
through touch, next the Perceptual/Affective level is acti- instructed to identify each one. The four emotion terms are
vated as the face is directly constructed with the materials, printed on a piece of paper and placed in front of the chil-
and then possibly the Cognitive/Symbolic level can be dren to help them remember the emotions while identify-
mobilized to reinforce the identification of emotions. ing the facial expressions in the photographs.
Build-a-Face (BAF), an art-making task developed by The DANVA 2-CF manual (Nowicki & Duke,
the first author specifically for this study, was tested as an 1994b) summarizes the results of a number of studies. The
intervention to assist in facial processing and recognition of measure has been found to demonstrate reasonable internal
emotions among children with ASD in an elementary consistency and test–retest reliability. Internal consistency
school setting. We sought to evaluate whether the stimulus has ranged from .69 to .81 with a mean of .76 across 10
of building faces using various features with different emo- studies with children aged 4 to 16. Test–retest reliability
tional expressions would be helpful to children with ASD. was r D .74. A lower test–retest reliability of r D .66 was
According to Hobson et al. (1988), individuals with ASD reported for preschool children. In support of the measure’s
rely on parts of the face rather than the whole for facial validity, lower DANVA 2-CF scores were significantly cor-
recognition. The BAF intervention provides a tactile experi- related with lower social competence as rated by teachers of
ence so the child can touch and feel each of the facial fea- preschool children and elementary school-aged children.
tures before bringing them together to create each facial Lower DANVA 2-CF scores in elementary school-aged
expression. The BAF intervention uses preconstructed facial children correlated with greater external control expectan-
features to build a face rather than requiring the children to cies, difficulties with teachers, lower self-esteem in girls, and
create their own. We postulated that the children’s use of greater depression and higher levels of psychopathology in
the preconstructed features would reinforce the integration boys. Academically, lower DANVA 2-CF scores are associ-
of the parts into a whole. Our hypothesis was that children ated with lower achievement in elementary and preschool-
with ASD would improve their ability to recognize emo- aged children.
tions in facial expressions with the reinforcement of the
BAF three-dimensional stimulus activity. Materials

Method The intervention created for this study (Build-a-Face)


includes four sets of facial features (eyes, noses, mouths,
Participants and brows) for representing four different emotions (happi-
ness, sadness, anger, and fear), as well as a mannequin head.
Nineteen children between the ages of 8 and 14, all Facial features were molded with Super Sculpey, using
recruited from a private elementary school in Connecticut, DANVA 2-CF photographs as a guide. The color brown
participated in the study. Each child was previously diag- was chosen for the eyes to correspond with the eye color in
nosed with ASD. Participants were randomly assigned to the photographs, and the model irises were molded with
either the treatment group or the control group using a Sculpey III–Hazelnut. The sclera, or the white portion of
table of random digits. The treatment group consisted of the eye, was molded with Original Sculpey–White. A sec-
10 males, and the control group was comprised of 6 males ond set of facial features was created to account for break-
and 3 females. The sample originally consisted of 25 chil- ages. Scotch Adhesive Putty was used to attach the facial
dren with ASD; however, 6 participants were removed features to the Styro Full Blank Head. On the head Benja-
from the study. Two children were unable to participate min Moore & Co. Cafe Royal paint was applied in a flat
due to their inability to understand the instructions, 3 chil- finish. AquaZAR Semi-Gloss, a water-based polyurethane,
dren were omitted due to lack of responses or repetitive was used to seal the head and prevent the paint from peel-
answers on the Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy ing. Black line markings were drawn on the head to indicate
questionnaire described below, and the last withdrawn par- the placement of brows, eyes, nose, and mouth (see
ticipant was unable to participate due to illness. Figure 1).
Magneatos, a three-dimensional construction set con-
Measures taining 72 primary-colored plastic magnetic balls and rods,
was used for the control group’s activity. This construction
The Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy set was designed to promote hand and eye coordination,
2–Child Facial Expressions (DANVA 2-CF; Nowicki & spatial awareness and judgment skills, and imagination.
Duke, 1994a) measures a child’s ability to recognize Magneatos won the Oppenheim Toy Portfolio Award and
16 INTERVENTION FOR CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

emotion in each. The administration of the test followed


the general directions from the DANVA 2 manual.
After administration of the DANVA 2-CF, each partic-
ipant in the treatment group completed the art therapy
intervention of building a face using individual facial fea-
tures (mouth, nose, eyes, and brows) to represent the cor-
rect emotion. The participant was asked to create four
different faces, first representing happiness, then sadness,
then anger, and, finally, fear. The young ages of some par-
ticipants prevented them from understanding the meaning
of “fear” so the word “afraid” was substituted, which proved
easier to comprehend. The participant was directed to
choose a mouth, nose, eyes, and brows (in that order) that
represented the correct emotion. For example, the
researcher asked, “Which one of these mouths do you think
would be a happy mouth?” and the participant received two
attempts at choosing the correct feature. The participant
was first allowed one attempt to select the correct feature
independently. If the correct feature was chosen, the
researcher responded with “That’s right; that is a happy
mouth.” If an incorrect feature was selected, the participant
was redirected with a descriptive statement about the fea-
ture, such as “I don’t think that’s a happy mouth. A happy
mouth has ends that turn upward.” After this feedback
from the researcher, the participant made a second (cued)
Figure 1 Build-a-Face Models
attempt at selecting the correct feature. If this attempt also
failed, the researcher directed the participant to the correct
feature. Next, the participant was instructed to place the
the Oppenheim Special Needs Adaptable Product Award facial feature onto the Styrofoam head, using the pre-
(Oppenheim Toy Portfolio, 2013). Pediatric therapists use marked lines as a guide. After the activity’s completion, the
Magneatos as a therapy tool to build skills and self-esteem DANVA 2-CF was readministered as a posttest.
for children on the autism spectrum. The rationale for the After the participants in the control group took the
use of Magneatos in this study was to provide a control DANVA 2-CF pretest, they participated in an art activity
activity that would also be tactile and building-related. using Magneatos. They were asked to use the materials to
build their own three-dimensional design. Then the
DANVA 2-CF was readministered as a posttest.
Procedures The participants were asked to respond verbally, but
because four participants were unable to communicate with
The Institutional Review Board of Albertus Magnus spoken language, they utilized alternate methods. Three
College approved the study procedures; there was no Insti- used the printed word guide to select the chosen emotion,
tutional Review Board approval process at the elementary and the fourth used a nonverbal word device, which elec-
school. The principal of the school granted permission to tronically speaks the selected word.
conduct the study after reviewing the proposed procedures.
The elementary school sent informed consent forms home
with the potential participants for their parents to review. Results
Parents were also provided with a description of the tasks
their children would be asked to perform. The first author Student t tests were conducted to determine the extent
then arranged individual sessions that lasted no more than of change on the DANVA 2-CF pretest versus posttest and
1 hour with students whose parents had consented to their the extent of differences between the two groups. The
participation. In addition, verbal assent from the partici- mean DANVA 2-CF score for the treatment group on the
pants themselves was requested at the beginning of the ses- pretest was 11.00 (SD D 4.29). The mean DANVA 2-CF
sions. Study sessions were scheduled in consultation with score for the control group was 9.67 (SD D 5.87). Results
participants’ teachers. Participants were tested in a quiet showed no statistically significant difference between the
room or in a partitioned section of the classroom. two groups, t(17) D .57, p D .58.
First the DANVA 2-CF was administered as a pretest For the DANVA 2-CF posttest, the mean score for the
for participants in both the treatment group and the control treatment group was 12.50 (SD D 4.17). The mean score
group. Participants were shown 24 different photographs of for the control group was 10.11 (SD D 4.54). The two
children’s faces expressing the emotions of happiness, sad- groups did not differ to a statistically significant degree, t
ness, anger, and fear, and were asked to identify the (17) D 1.20, p D.25.
RICHARD / MORE / JOY 17

The mean amount of change between the pretest and The BAF intervention was developed to explore the use
posttest DANVA 2-CF scores for the treatment group was of a three-dimensional activity to promote emotional recog-
1.50 (SD D 2.80). For the control group, the mean change nition skills for children with ASD. There have been previ-
in DANVA 2-CF score was 0.44 (SD D 2.13). Although ous studies on the effects of two-dimensional art tasks
the treatment group improved more than the control group involving face processing and recognition skills (Betts,
by almost half a standard deviation, there was no statisti- 2003; Martin, 2008) and the benefits of sensory stimula-
cally significant difference in the amount of change across tion (Kearns, 2004) as they pertain to children with ASD.
the two groups, t(17) D .92, p D .37. Although no empirical studies of children with ASD exist,
One-sample t tests showed change within the treatment Waters and Silberg (2002) found that art activities involv-
group, t(9) D 1.70, p D .12, and the control condition, ing tactile experiences helped dissociative children connect
t(8) D .63, p D .55. The results were also examined sepa- through the ability to touch and create.
rately for each of the four emotions (happy, sad, angry, and The tactile experience of the BAF intervention relates to
fearful). However, no significant difference was found for the processes described in the Expressive Therapies Contin-
any of the emotions. uum (Hinz, 2008; Lusebrink, 2004, 2010). The ETC
The results indicate that improvement was made in the explores the use of different art media and a patient’s ability
expected direction. In the treatment group, 7 participants to process information as images are created. The processes
improved and 3 exhibited no change. By comparison, only engaged with the BAF intervention relate to the kinesthetic
4 participants improved in the control group, with 5 component through the building of the face and with the
remaining the same. However, the proportions of x2(1, N sensory component through tactile engagement with the
D 19) D 1.27, p D .26 did not differ significantly. three-dimensional facial features, and to the Perceptual/
Finally, Cohen’s d was used in order to determine the Affective level through the process of face formulation. On
effect size within each group. The result for the treatment the Cognitive/Symbolic level, BAF may affect the informa-
group was d D .35, whereas the control group result was tion-processing element of the cognitive component, which
d D .07. This indicates that a small but real effect presented consists of cognitive operations. The placement of the three-
itself in the treatment group but failed to emerge in the dimensional facial features to form an emotion-based expres-
control group. sion involves the use of spatial differentiation and integra-
tion, along with the cognitive integration of forms (BAF
facial features) to create concept formation (BAF emotion-
Discussion based face as constructed on the mannequin head). The ulti-
mate goal of the BAF intervention would be improving the
This pilot study tested the hypothesis that children child’s ability to maneuver the parts to form not only a
with autism spectrum disorder would better recognize emo- human face but also, more importantly, to form the correct
tions in facial expressions following the use of the Build-a- positions to create a symbolic representation of the requested
Face intervention. The results showed no statistically signif- emotion. Based on these observations, the ETC offers sup-
icant differences between the treatment and control groups; port for the three-dimensional, tactile experience of BAF.
however, the findings suggest that use of the BAF interven- The ETC provides a valuable and standardized way of evalu-
tion may be helpful. In the treatment group, 7 of 10 partici- ating the child’s information processing and movement at
pants improved, compared to 4 of 9 in the control group. the different levels. The scope of this research did not allow
The effect sizes showed that there was a small but real effect for such a depth of evaluation, but future research may be
in the treatment group that did not emerge for the control indicated to achieve a deeper level of understanding of the
group. factors affecting children’s ability to recognize emotions.
It was observed that many participants in the treatment The limitations of this study included problems with
group demonstrated excitement upon being introduced to the administration of the test. Participants found it difficult
BAF. The children were eager to touch the individual facial to remain focused during the lengthy session of testing and
features and the head. Several appeared more focused dur- intervention. As per the school’s policy, incentives such as
ing the intervention than did members of the control snacks, drinks, and computer time were offered to several
group, who seemed less interested in the Magneatos. After participants to sustain their motivation and urge them to
completion of the facial expressions, several treatment par- complete the session. Due to their unfamiliarity with the
ticipants mimicked the expressions spontaneously. More researcher, teachers or school aides accompanied some par-
than any other expression, participants recognized the ticipants to help them stay on task using verbal prompting
“happy” example, possibly due to the familiarity of the during the session. If the participants had been familiar
smile. As found by Wallace et al. (2008), fearful expressions with the researcher, the study sessions may have run
were least recognized, which may be related to the hypothe- smoother, resulting in a more successful intervention.
sis that states that the amygdala portion of the brain, which The small sample size presented another limitation; it
performs a primary role in fear recognition, does not func- restricted the statistical power of the results and created a
tion normally for people with ASD. Because typically devel- gender imbalance between the groups. The treatment group
oping children did not participate in this study, it is difficult consisted of 10 males, whereas the control group consisted
to ascertain whether developmental issues cause the recog- of 6 males and 3 females. The imbalance of gender in the
nition lapse, but it is a provocative possibility. groups was an artifact of randomization in a small sample.
18 INTERVENTION FOR CHILDREN WITH AUTISM

Future studies should include a larger and more gender-bal- Asperger syndrome. International Journal of Psychology, 41(2),
anced sample or separate studies should be conducted to 97–106. doi:10.1080/00207590500184610
determine the effectiveness of the BAF intervention for
boys and for girls. Evans, K., & Dubowski, J. (2001). Art therapy with children on the
Based on one session of treatment and a small sample, autistic spectrum: Beyond words. London, England: Jessica Kingsley.
the results were not significant. However, future research
Furniss, G. (2008). Celebrating the artmaking of children with
should consider providing multiple interventions to deter- autism. Art Education, 9, 8–12.
mine if this would help children with ASD improve their
emotional recognition skills. Humans learn through repeti- Hinz, L. D. (2008). Walking the line between passion and cau-
tion of tasks; therefore, children with ASD could potentially tion in art therapy: Using the Expressive Therapies Continuum
benefit from consecutive interventions using BAF. to avoid therapist errors. Art Therapy: Journal of the American
Future researchers may also want to focus on using the Art Therapy Association, 25(1), 38–40. doi:10.1080/
BAF intervention with only one emotion or only one per 07421656.2008.10129352
session. This would allow for a shorter session length, which
may help participants remain more focused. Sessions focus- Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988). What’s in a face?
ing on one emotion over a period of time may also help to The case of autism. British Journal of Psychology, 79, 441–453.
doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1988.tb02745.x
solidly reinforce that particular emotion before moving on
to the next one. The children in this study demonstrated Kagin, S. L., & Lusebrink, V. B. (1978). The Expressive Thera-
excitement over the introduction of BAF; further research pies Continuum. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 5, 171–180.
could determine whether the participants’ enthusiasm
would continue over several BAF sessions. Kaiser, D., & Deaver, S. (2013). Establishing a research agenda
Although the results of our study were equivocal, it is for art therapy: A Delphi study. Art Therapy: Journal of the
hoped that future research on validating the Build-a-Face American Art Therapy Association, 30(3), 112–121.
intervention for improving recognition of emotions among doi:10.1080/07421656.2013.819281
children with autism spectrum disorder will be conducted.
Research projects on interventions such as BAF, and studies Kapitan, L. (2006). The “Multipler Effect”: Art therapy research that
like the one discussed in this article, will help the art ther- benefits all. Art Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy Associ-
ation, 23(4), 154–155. doi:10.1080/07421656.2006.10129336
apy profession contribute both to the demystification of
ASD and to the quality-of-life improvement among those Kearns, D. (2004). Art therapy with a child experiencing sensory
affected by autism. integration difficulty. Art Therapy: Journal of the American Art
Therapy Association, 21(2), 95–101. doi:10.1080/
07421656.2004.10129551
References
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic
manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. social situations as predictors of social competence in individu-
als with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 809–816.
Anderson, F. E. (2001). Benefits of conducting research. Art
Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy Association, 18(3), Klin, A., Lin, D. J., Gorrindo, P., Ramsey, G., Jones, W.
134–141. doi:10.1080/07421656.2001.10129733 (2009). Two-year-olds with autism orient to non-social
physical contingencies rather than biological motion.
Betts, D. (2003). Developing a projective drawing test: Experien- Nature, 459, 257–261. Retrieved from http://www.nature
ces with the Face Stimulus Assessment (FSA). Art Therapy: .com/nature/journal/v459/n7244/full/nature07868.html
Journal of the American Art Therapy Association, 20(2), 77–82.
doi:10.1080/07421656.2003.10129393 Lowenfeld, V., & Brittain, W. I. (1987). Creative and mental
growth. New York, NY: Macmillian.
Centers for Disease Control. (2014). CDC estimates 1 in 68 children
has been identified with autism spectrum disorder. Retrieved from Lusebrink, V. B. (2004). Art therapy and the brain: An
http://www.cdc.gov/media/releases/2014/p0327-autism-spec attempt to understand the underlying processes of art
trum-disorder.html expression in therapy. Art Therapy: Journal of the American
Art Therapy Association, 21(3), 125–135. doi:10.108/
Charman, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1993). Drawing development 07421656.2004.10129496
in autism: The intellectual to visual realism shift. British Journal
of Developmental Psychology, 11, 171–185. Lusebrink, V. B. (2010). Assessment and therapeutic applications
of the Expressive Therapies Continuum: Implications for brain
Deaver, S. (2002). What constitutes art therapy research? Art structures and functions. Art Therapy: Journal of the American
Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy Association, 19(1), Art Therapy Association, 27(4), 168–177. doi:10.1080/
23–27. doi:10.1080/07421656.2002.1012972 07421656.2010.10129380

Deruelle, C., Rondan, C., Gepner, B., & Fagot, J. (2006). Proc- Martin, N. (2008). Assessing portrait drawings created by chil-
essing of compound visual stimuli by children with autism and dren and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Art
RICHARD / MORE / JOY 19

Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy Association, 25(1), Selfe, L. (1977). Nadia: A case of extraordinary drawing ability in
15–23. doi:10.1080/07421656.2008.10129348 an autistic child. London, England: Academic Press.

Martin, N. (2009). Art therapy and autism: Overview and Slayton, S. C., D’Archer, J., & Kaplan, F. (2010). Outcome
recommendations. Art Therapy: Journal of the American Art studies of the efficacy of art therapy: A review of the find-
Association, 26(4), 187–190. doi:10.1080/07421656.2009. ings. Art Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy Associ-
10129616 ation, 27(3), 108–118. doi:10.1080/07421656.2010.10129
660
Nowicki, S., Jr., & Duke, M. P. (1994a). Individual differences in the
nonverbal communication of affect: The Diagnostic Analysis of Wallace, S., Coleman, M., & Bailey, A. (2008). An investigation
Nonverbal Accuracy Scale. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18, 9–35. of basic facial expressions recognition in autism spectrum disor-
ders. Cognition and Emotion, 22(7), 1353–1380. doi:10.1080/
Nowicki, S., Jr., & Duke, M. P. (1994b). Manual for the receptive 026993070782153
tests of the Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy 2. Unpub-
lished manuscript. Waters, F., & Silberg, J. (2002). Promoting integration in disso-
ciative children. In J. Silberg (Ed.), The dissociative child: Diag-
Oppenheim Toy Portfolio. (2013). Magneatos. Retrieved from http: nosis, treatment, and management (2nd ed.). Lutherville, MD:
//www.toyportfolio.com/SingleProduct.php?ProductIDD4208. Sidran Press.

You might also like