You are on page 1of 17

DATE DOWNLOADED: Wed May 17 03:01:02 2023

SOURCE: Content Downloaded from HeinOnline

Citations:
Please note: citations are provided as a general guideline. Users should consult their preferred
citation format's style manual for proper citation formatting.

Bluebook 21st ed.


Esther Nir & Siyu Liu, What Do the Gatekeepers See? Perceptions and Evaluations of
Scientific Evidence among State Court Judges, 22 CRIMINOLOGY, CRIM. Just. L & Soc'y 1
(2021).

ALWD 7th ed.


Esther Nir & Siyu Liu, What Do the Gatekeepers See? Perceptions and Evaluations of
Scientific Evidence among State Court Judges, 22 Criminology, Crim. Just. L & Soc'y 1
(2021).

APA 7th ed.


Nir, E., & Liu, S. (2021). What do the gatekeepers see? perceptions and evaluations
of scientific evidence among state court judges. Criminology, Criminal Justice, Law &
Society, 22(1), 1-16.

Chicago 17th ed.


Esther Nir; Siyu Liu, "What Do the Gatekeepers See? Perceptions and Evaluations of
Scientific Evidence among State Court Judges," Criminology, Criminal Justice, Law &
Society 22, no. 1 (April 2021): 1-16

McGill Guide 9th ed.


Esther Nir & Siyu Liu, "What Do the Gatekeepers See? Perceptions and Evaluations of
Scientific Evidence among State Court Judges" (2021) 22:1 Criminology, Crim Just L &
Soc'y 1.

AGLC 4th ed.


Esther Nir and Siyu Liu, 'What Do the Gatekeepers See? Perceptions and Evaluations of
Scientific Evidence among State Court Judges' (2021) 22(1) Criminology, Criminal
Justice, Law & Society 1

MLA 9th ed.


Nir, Esther, and Siyu Liu. "What Do the Gatekeepers See? Perceptions and Evaluations
of Scientific Evidence among State Court Judges." Criminology, Criminal Justice, Law
& Society, vol. 22, no. 1, April 2021, pp. 1-16. HeinOnline.

OSCOLA 4th ed.


Esther Nir & Siyu Liu, 'What Do the Gatekeepers See? Perceptions and Evaluations of
Scientific Evidence among State Court Judges' (2021) 22 Criminology, Crim Just L &
Soc'y 1 Please note: citations are provided as a general guideline.
Users should consult their preferred citation format's style manual for proper
citation formatting.

Provided by:
International Islamic University Malaysia
VOLUME 22, ISSUE 1, PAGES 20-35 (2021)

Criminology, Criminal Justice, Law & Society

CRIMINOLOGY, CRIMINAL JUSTICE, LAW & SOCIETY


E-ISSN 2332-886X
Available online at
https://scholasticah.com/criminology-criminal-iustice-law-societv/

What Do the Gatekeepers See? Perceptions and Evaluations of Scientific


Evidence Among State Court Judges

Esther Nir,a Siyu Liub


a New Jersey City University
b Penn State Harrisburg

ABSTRACT AND ARTICLE INFORMATION

Judicial gatekeeping decisions impact criminal case processing (e.g., verdicts, pleas) in significant ways. Despite this
influential role thatjudges play, little is known about the key factors that contribute to judicial perceptions of evidence and
the evaluative processes thatjudges employ in making evidentiary rulings. Using qualitative interviews with 41 state court
judges presiding in a U.S. state employing the Frye standard, we explore judicial perceptions of a variety of scientific
evidentiary forms and the processes by which judges reach reliability and credibility determinations in performing their
gatekeeping duties. Our analyses reveal that judges are highly influenced by the general reputation of forensic evidence,
often lack understanding of the scientific methodologies involved, and tend to focus on factors external to reliability (e.g.,
expert's communication skills and showmanship, weight of the evidence) in their evaluative processes. Further, credibility
assessments of witnesses are fraught with subjective interpretations, potentially leading to disparate evidentiary rulings.

Article History: Keywords:

Received January 1 5th, 2021 evidence, judicial discretion, forensic, law, procedural justice, qualitative methods,
Received in revised form March 16 th, 2021 courts
Accepted March 16 th 2021

© 2021 Criminology, Criminal Justice, Law & Society and The Western Society of Criminology
Hosting by Scholastica. All rights reserved.

Correspondingauthor: Esther Nir, Department of Criminal Justice, New Jersey City University - Jersey City, Professional
Studies Building, Room 226, 2039 John F. Kennedy Boulevard, Jersey City, NJ, 07305, USA.
Email: enir@njcu.edu
2 NIR & Liu

"Evidence is a currency in which legal cases are determinative force of admissible evidence in criminal
transacted" (Cooney, 1994, p. 851). case processing. Among the various types of evidence,
judges are uniquely challenged by the task of
The judicial role as "gatekeeper of the evaluating scientific evidence. As Beecher-Monas
(1999) stated, "judges, traditionally triers of law,
evidence" is a prominent and critical decision-making
responsibility that is highly consequential to case occasionally pressed into service as triers of fact, now
must also be triers of science in cases where experts
outcomes (Daubertv. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals,
proffer scientific evidence" (p. 1047). These
1993; Schweitzer & Saks, 2009). Indeed, judges'
challenges prompt important questions: Do judges
decisions to admit incriminating pieces of evidence at
have sufficient knowledge and guidance to perform
trial may contribute to the establishment of the
their gatekeeping function effectively? How do judges
defendant's guilt, while rulings to allow favorable
perceive the strength and reliability of different types
defense evidence or exclude damaging prosecution
of scientific evidence? What evaluative processes do
evidence may lead to an acquittal. Judicial
judges employ in assessing the reliability of various
gatekeeping decisions may also influence plea offers
forms of scientific evidence? Gaining insight on
by the prosecution, as well as defense decisions to
judicial perceptions of evidence and evaluative
accept or reject offers (Kramer et al., 2007; Rossman
processes in determining admissibility is a crucial step
et al., 1980); specifically, plea negotiations are often
to better understanding criminal case processing as a
guided by prosecution and defense perceptions of the
likelihood of prevailing at trial (based, in part, on their whole. Yet, despite the imperative role that judicial
perceptions and evaluations of evidence play, only a
perceptions of the strength of the admissible
handful of existing empirical studies specifically focus
evidence), sometimes referred to as "bargaining in the
on this topic (Brodsky et al., 2010; Jurs, 2009;
shadow of the trial" (Bushway et al., 2014; Dezember
O'Brien, 2018; for studies using non-American data,
& Redlich, 2019; Kramer & Ulmer 2002; Landes,
see Canela et al., 2019; Connolly & Gordon, 2011;
1971; Redlich et al., 2016; Yan, 2019). In fact, mere
Moulin et al., 2018; Porter& Brinke, 2009). Using in-
projections that a particular judge may exclude or
depth qualitative interviews of 41 state court judges
admit a specific piece of evidence (e.g., due to
presiding in a state in the United States that employs
reliability or constitutional challenges) could
profoundly impact the manner by which prosecutors the Frye standard, we seek to help fill this gap by
exploring judicial knowledge, perceptions, and
and defense attorneys exercise their discretion in
resolving cases within the court community (Nardulli, opinions of the most common forms of scientific
1986; Nir & Liu, in press; Wilson, 2016). Lastly, the evidence used in criminal cases and the processes by
which judges analyze evidentiary reliability,
admission of faulty evidence has been linked with the
occurrence of wrongful convictions (Derwin, 2018).
credibility, and value in performing their gatekeeping
functions.
Beyond gatekeeping functions, judicial
perceptions of evidence may influence other stages of
criminal case processing. For example, in bench trials,
judges play the role of triers of fact and determine
Literature Review
whether the defendant is guilty based on their
perceptions of the reliability, credibility, and overall Types of Evidence
strength of the admissible evidence (Blanck, 1992). A broad range and scope of evidence may be
Further, despite the theoretical understanding that introduced during criminal prosecutions (Grimm,
evidence used to establish guilt should have little 2018) and can be classified in a variety of ways
bearing in sentencing (since questions of guilt have (Findley, 2013; Greenstein, 2009; Heller, 2006;
been resolved by this point of case processing), recent Thompson, 2012). In this article, we discuss specific
empirical studies establish that judicial perceptions of scientific evidentiary forms as points of focus to gain
evidentiary strength (as manifested by type and insight into the manner by which judges approach the
quantity) are significantly related to the length of task of evaluating the admissibility of scientific
custodial sentences judges impose (Nir & Griffiths, evidence, as well as their general perceptions of the
2018, 2019; Peterson et al., 2010, 2013) and that evidence evaluated. We discuss DNA, latent prints,
judicial confidence level in guilt (based on perceptions ballistic evidence, and chemist evidence, commonly
of evidentiary strength) partially guides sentencing introduced in criminal cases (for details, see Table 1).
decisions (Nir & Griffiths, 2019). Our respondents most commonly referenced these
The documented influence of evidentiary types of evidence during our interviews, and they have
considerations in driving case outcomes underlines the been tasked with reaching admissibility decisions on

Criminology, CriminalJustice, Law & Society - Volume 22, Issue 1


NIR & Li 3

Table 1: Evidentiary Types in the Current Study

By comparing the analytical result


Considered "the most Forensic evidence
from the evidence sample to reference
discriminating" evidence, garners a great deal
samples collected from individuals
deoxyribonucleic acid (DNA) of confidence from
with known identities through
DNA Expert testing facilitates identifications the general public
polymerase chain reaction (PCR)
of individuals; it is contained in and the courts. In
Short Tandem repeat (STR)
body fluids, skin samples, bones, fact, "the trust that is
technology on matching 20 loci (FBI,
and hair (FBI, 2019). laid upon the forensic
2020).
sciences generally
falls somewhere
between uncritical
faith and
manufactured myth"
(Saks & Faigman,
2008, p. 150; see also
Edmond & Roque,
2012).

Judges are generally


very receptive to
expert opinions
presented by the
prosecution on behalf
of the state (Edmond
"The observed physical & Roque, 2012).
characteristics of the questioned items
are compared to reference firearms or
other information sources to
This refers to any evidence determine if they are consistent"
related to the functionality and (FBI, 2019, p. 47). Often, using "a
Ballistic
Expert physical characteristics of traditional microscopic comparison of
evidence
suspect firearm or firearm- the actual evidence items," the digital
related items (FBI, 2019). image of a test-fired cartridge
case/shotshell casing or evidence
cartridge case/shotshell casing can be
compared with records in the
database (FBI, 2019, p. 70).

these forms of scientific evidence. We further explore Judicial Gatekeeping Decisions


judicial perceptions/approaches to credibility
determinations applicable to scientific expert The Standards and Processes of Gatekeeping
witnesses. Below, we describe the processes and legal
While jurors consider evidentiary weight in a
standards that guide and direct judges in their
case, judges are responsible for deciding the
gatekeeping tasks. We then review challenges drawn
admissibility of evidence (Faigman et al., 2016);
from the literature on these evaluative processes.
evidence determined to be inadmissible by the trial

Criminology, CriminalJustice, Law & Society - Volume 22, Issue 1


4 NIR & Liu

judge is not heard by the jury.1 During hearings or many types of "junk" science evidence were admitted
trials, expert witnesses may provide explanations on under Frye (e.g., hair comparison, bitemark analysis,
procedures and scientific contexts for forensic and voice spectrography) even though there is a lack
evidence. Before an expert witness is permitted to of sufficient support to establish that these evidentiary
present their opinion in court, the judge decides types could be of identification value. On the other
whether or not the witness possesses sufficient hand, many commentators highlight that the Daubert
expertise on the subject matter in question. In contrast standard shifted the authority of gatekeeping decisions
to lay witnesses, whose admissibility could rely from the relevant scientific community under Frye
largely on judicial common sense (Burns, 2016) and (i.e., fingerprint examiners in cases involving
legal training, expert witnesses must possess the fingerprint evidence) to trial judges, who do not
knowledge, experience, education, and skill set to routinely receive scientific training and thus may not
reliably inform the factfinder about the evidence in the be the most appropriate gatekeepers (Allen & Nafisi,
relevant and currently established field of scientific or 2010; Cheng & Yoon, 2005; Grimm, 2018; Shelton,
professional practice. Since judges are in the 2009). Nevertheless, some evidence show that judges
"gatekeeping" position, criteria have been established have a heightened awareness of "junk" science and
to help them reach determinations on the admissibility have adopted a more careful approach to evaluating
of expert witness testimony (Shelton, 2009). expert testimonies since Daubert (Cheng & Yoon,
Currently, two legal standards are widely 2005; Krafka et al., 2002; see Dickson, 2018, for a
practiced in the United States. The Frye Rule, also critique that Daubert falls short in guiding judges to
known as the "general acceptance" test, was first rule out "junk" science, using future dangerousness
established in Frye v. United States (1923, D.C. evidence as an example). Further, legal scholars have
Circuit) and requires trial judges to determine whether also pointed out instances where judges' tolerated lab
the scientific methods employed are generally reports involving misconduct (Giannelli, 2007, on
accepted in the scientific community. The newer Joyce Gilchrist, forensic chemist in Oklahoma City
standard is based on Daubert v. Merrell Dow Police Department and her abuse of scientific
Pharmaceuticals (1993) where the Supreme Court evidence).
solidified the "gatekeeper" role of the trial courts and In addition to reliability determinations,
recommended four factors to facilitate the evaluative evidentiary gatekeeping requires credibility
process: whether the methods used are generally evaluations by judges. In evaluating credibility, judges
accepted within the scientific community, whether make a host of assessments including whether the
they have been published and subjected to peer review, testimony sounds credible, whether the witness has a
whether the methods have been or can be tested (i.e., motive to lie, the witness's demeanor as nonverbal
falsifiability), and whether there is a known error rate. "leakage cues" (Ekman & Friesen, 1969; O'Regan,
The Daubertstandard replaces the prior Frye standard 2017), surrounding circumstances, the witness's facial
in federal courts and is aligned with the amended expressions (Brodsky et al., 2010; Connolly

&
Federal Rules of Evidence 702 (i.e., expert's Gordon, 2011; Porter & Brinke, 2009; Wessel et al.,
specialized knowledge, reliance on sufficient facts and 2006), and the use of "uptalk"-rising final intonation
data, application of "reliable principles and methods," (lower confidence rating by listeners in the expert
and reliable application of the methods to case facts; witness context, Levon & Ye, 2020).
FederalRules of Evidence 702, 2021; Grimm, 2018).
The EvaluativeDifficulties in Gatekeeping
Over the past two decades, a majority of state
jurisdictions became followers of the Daubert test or The ability of judges to ably perform their
similar model, and only a handful of states remain in gatekeeping function is fraught with hardships
the Frye camp (Cheng & Yoon, 2005; DeMatteo et al., (Derwin, 2018). First, judges must make evidentiary
2019; see also the Frye-Mack standard in Minnesota rulings on subjects that are outside of their legal
discussed by Alter, 2017).2 Fradella and colleagues expertise and general knowledge base. For example,
(2004) reported that judges perform well under judges have little training in medicine, methodology,
Daubert in excluding unqualified experts yet and statistics, yet gatekeeping decisions often rely on
demonstrate inconsistencies in allowing or excluding the application of knowledge from these fields (Canela
certain types of evidence (e.g., those of forensic et al., 2019; McQuiston-Surrett & Saks, 2009; Saks
&

identification). Faigman, 2008; Wojcikiewicz, 2013). Even guiding


Both the Frye and Daubert standards are not standards (i.e., Daubert or Frye) can only provide
without flaws. The Frye test is a single- prong criterion limited assistance to judges; in fact, scholars have
that lacks the flexibility and comprehensiveness to found that it remains questionable whether the
allow for constant scientific advancement (Allen implementation of either the Frye or Daubertstandard
&

Nafisi, 2010). Beecher-Monas (1998) argued that results in any practical difference in how judges reach

Criminology, CriminalJustice, Law & Society - Volume 22, Issue 1


NIR & Liu

determinations (Cheng & Yoon, 2005; Damon-Moore, (2010), Damon-Moore (2017) provides a deep and
2017; Groscup et al., 2002; Jensen, 2003; Moreno, scathing explanation for this judicial failure, focusing
2003). on the lack of scientific training (see also Kovera

&
Further, instead of following the applicable McAuliff, 2000), the lack of defense challenges to
standard of review, prior scholarship suggests that prosecutorial evidence (see also Neal et al., 2019),
when the scientific processes are difficult to reliance on heuristics (i.e., mental shortcuts relying on
understand, judges resort to what they do understand the fallacy of tradition; see also Liden et al., 2019),
(Beecher-Monas, 1998). For example, Gatowski and judicial instincts about the defense and prosecution,
her colleagues (2001) found that judges have trouble and the unspoken alliance between the bench and the
understanding the concepts of falsifiability and error prosecution (see Nir & Liu, in press), all of which
rates, and attach more weight to "general acceptance." disadvantage the criminal defendant's case.
Saks and Faigman (2008) found that in jurisdictions An additional challenge to judicial
that apply the Daubert standard, judges have "bent gatekeeping decisions involves the subjectivity
over backwards to evade the application of Daubert inherent in these determinations (Walsh, 1999). Given
when conscientious application would lead to the the differences in attitudes and philosophies among
exclusion of any of the nonscience forensic sciences" judges toward this role, Judge Walsh (1999) raised
(p. 163). Other factors employed by judges to evaluate doubt that the result of evidence earning entry in one
expert evidence include relevance and qualification of courtroom would be able to do so in another. Despite
the expert, or factors external to their expertise- Judge Walsh's concern, little empirical evidence on
including likability, believability, trustworthiness, and judicial disparity in evidentiary rulings is available.
intelligence (Brodsky et al., 2010).
The shortfalls in judicial scientific Method
knowledge are exacerbated by several factors. First,
Data for this study were drawn from
criminal defense attorneys are generally unable to
qualitative interviews with 41 state court judges
identify methodological weaknesses or to access
presiding in a Frye state. Interviewees preside over
expert witnesses to testify on behalf of their clients
felony cases; among other functions, they make
(Edmond & Roque 2012; Saks & Faigman, 2008).
evidentiary rulings on a myriad of pre-trial motions
Second, there is a vulnerability to bias in the
and hearings, determine the admissibility of expert
production of scientific evidence (Butt, 2013; Dror,
testimony at trial, preside over jury trials, conduct
2013; Dror & Cole 2010; Elwell, 2020; Kassin et al.,
bench trials, and sentence convicted defendants.
2013; National Academy of Science [NAS], 2009;
Judicial experience among our respondents ranges
Smalarz et al., 2016; Stevenage & Bennett, 2017),
from less than 2 years and over 30 years (average =
even though forensic evidence is considered to be the
17.6 years), with 36 judges having at minimum of 9
most influential in guilt determinations (Smith et al.,
years of experience on the bench. Moreover, the vast
2011).
majority of our subjects have prior legal experience in
Another major challenge to gatekeeping is
criminal law (e.g., about 95% of our sample are former
the troubling vulnerability of perception to judicial
prosecutors, defense attorneys, or both). Seven judges
bias (Burns, 2016). Using a sample of civil rights cases
in our sample are female, 3 and judges preside in urban
in U. S. district courts, O'Brien (2018) found that male
(53.7%), suburban (19.5%), or rural (26.8%) locations
judges are more likely than their female counterparts
and are either elected (over 60%) or appointed to the
to exclude expert evidence. Other factors, such as
bench.
judicial experience, may also be influential (Moulin et
After receiving approval from the Internal
al., 2018). Judicial bias is also observed unevenly in
Review Board (IRB), data collection took place during
civil and criminal cases. While defendants in civil
2014 and 2015,4 and include judges from counties all
cases are often able to successfully challenge
over the state. Efforts to recruit judges to participate in
plaintiffs' evidence, criminal defendants face
our study involved various methods (Nir, 2018),
insurmountable challenges (Dioso-Villa, 2015;
including reaching out to personal contacts (n = 8),
Groscup et al., 2002; Risinger, 2000; Rozelle, 2007;
referrals from other respondents (n 13), cold-calling
Shniderman, 2017; see Merlino et al., 2007 for a study
methods (n 15), and cold calling with reference to a
of admissibility decisions under Daubert, comparing
general email that one of our interviewees sent to
those of criminal and civil cases). Commentators like
fellow judges in his courthouse introducing our study
Judge Donald E. Shelton (2010) attribute the failure of
(n 5). Our interviews were conducted in judges'
judges as gatekeepers in criminal cases to precedent-
chambers and ranged from 45 minutes to 3 hours (90
based rationalizations and pro-prosecution bias,
minutes on average). An in-depth semi-structured
resulting in the admission of sub-par science into the
interview design was used to collect the data to
courtroom, regardless of validity. Echoing Shelton

Criminology, CriminalJustice, Law & Society - Volume 22, Issue 1


6 NIR & Liu

"ensure that the basic lines of inquiry [were] pursued forensic chemistry (4). In this section, we discuss these
with each person" (Patton, 2002, p. 343). As this study general impressions, followed by an analysis of the
was part of a larger project evaluating the influence of evaluative processes that judges employ in assessing
evidentiary weight on sentencing determinations, our reliability. We then discuss prevalent judicial concerns
interview guide included questions related to a myriad regarding scientific evidence and judicial credibility
of topics surrounding judicial decision-making, with assessments. We conclude the results with a
an emphasis on judicial perceptions of different discussion of how judges' personal experiences
evidentiary types and processes. Other areas of focus influence their evaluative processes.
include judicial perceptions of sentencing disparities,
General Impressions, By Evidentiary Type
sentencing factors, juries, mandatory minimums, and
perceptions of judicial discretion, among others. DNA Evidence
Regarding judicial perceptions of evidence, we asked
The vast majority of our respondents
judges to discuss their perceptions of different forms
perceive DNA evidence as the strongest, most
of evidence, including DNA, fingerprints, chemists,
objective form of evidence available: "DNA is 100%
ballistic evidence, and other non-scientific evidence
reliable . . . . It comes back so many billion to one"
(eyewitnesses, character witnesses, alibi witnesses),
(Judge 9). Among a host of positive terms used by
among others. Judges were probed to discuss the
judges to portray their perceptions of DNA evidence,
strengths and weaknesses of each evidentiary form and
descriptors include "a rock crusher" (Judge 26),
to compare different evidentiary types to one another;
"infallible" (Judge 11), "bulletproof' (Judge 34), and
they were also asked to discuss their evaluative
"the golden child" (Judge 7). Given its perceived
processes in assessing reliability and credibility of
value, most judges expressed a strong preference to
evidence in their own courtrooms. We utilized an
have DNA as part of the evidentiary package, when it
inductive grounded theory approach during data
is relevant and available: "I always prefer to have
collection and analysis to develop themes that "place
certainty, and you have that with DNA evidence. For
the data into a more general or abstract framework"
truth seeking courts, it is always best to be sure"
(Maxwell, 2005, p. 97). Observed themes include
(Judge 11). Anotherjudge described the value of DNA
judicial confidence in various forms of forensic
in criminal case processing this way: "When you get
evidence, judicial focus on the presentation skills of
an expert up there who says that 'with 99.7% certainty
expert witnesses, and concerns regarding the
they can rule out A, B, and C,' you have to give it
reliability and credibility of witnesses, among others.
weight" (Judge 18). Consistent with existing
We compared data from each new interview with prior
scholarship, the majority of our respondents not only
interviews and refined developing themes to
praise DNA for its evidentiary strength, but also accept
accurately reflect the complete body of data collected
the applicable science and testing procedures without
(Maxwell, 2005). In this paper, we focus on the
question (see Saks & Faigman, 2008), a few citing its
gatekeeping of scientific evidence widely used in
strong reputation.
criminal cases.
FingerprintEvidence
Results Though generally ranked right below DNA in
evidentiary strength, most judges view fingerprint
Overall, our respondents value scientific evidence as a highly probative and reliable form of
evidence and perceive it to be stronger and more evidence (see Smith et al., 2011):
reliable than non-scientific evidence. As one judge
noted: "Science doesn't lie. People make mistakes. Fingerprint evidence, when presented by
People lie. People can deceive. The science doesn't" responsible witnesses who are experienced
(Judge 34). A second judge echoed a similar and it is something where the jury can
sentiment: "Well, scientific evidence, most of it, either actually see how the prints match up and the
it is or it isn't whereas witness testimony, no matter number of points of comparison and all of
what they say, there is a possibility that they are that, is very strong evidence. (Judge 26)
wrong, right, half right, half wrong" (Judge 40). As a In describing their perceptions of its strength, many
result, judges generally feel more confident judges noted that fingerprint evidence is highly
adjudicating cases when the evidentiary package instrumental in resolving identification issues
includes scientific evidence (Nir & Griffiths, 2019). (particularly in circumstantial cases), is visually
Yet, judicial perceptions of reliability vary approachable and understandable (to juries and
considerably among scientific evidentiary types, with judges), and usually generates considerable juror
DNA ranking the most reliable, followed by interest.
fingerprint evidence (2), ballistics evidence (3), and

Criminology, CriminalJustice, Law & Society - Volume 22, Issue 1


NIR & Li 7

Ballistics Evidence on their own experiences, judges routinely referenced


the strong reputation of DNA evidence in resolving
Ranked third of all forensic evidence, most
identification issues, external to their experiences on
judges perceive ballistics evidence to be a relatively
the bench. Likewise, judges connected the discussion
reliable evidentiary form. Judges tend to divide expert
of chemist evidence with the dated scandal-tainted
ballistic testimony into two general categories-gun
reputation of the work in the jurisdiction. In fact, while
operability and ammunitions/weapons matching.
judges expressed outrage at past mistakes made by
While the vast majority of judges perceive gun
chemists, virtually all of these comments were based
operability evidence to be highly reliable and objective
on information judges received outside of their
(a couple describing it as "kosher" [Judge 2] or "based
courtrooms, even though they were reminded to
on some physical reality as opposed to statistical
comment on their direct experience.
analysis" [Judge 36]), judges expressed far less
Second, evidentiary rankings were
confidence in the scientific procedures involved in
influenced by the (perceived) degree that the evidence
matching shell casings to guns: "In terms of gun
relies on human evaluations, with more required
operability, ballistics evidence is purely scientific and
human assessments leading to a decrease in judicial
reliable. But if you are trying to match gyrations on
confidence in the evidentiary form. For example,
shells, it is somewhat subjective" (Judge 11).
DNA was touted as the most reliable evidentiary type
Forensic Chemistry due partially to judicial perceptions that DNA
evidence is largely based on computer generated
Judicial opinions regarding the reliability of
results and, therefore, relatively free of the perils of
chemist testimony run a wide gamut. On one end of
human assessments. In contrast, most judges
the spectrum, forensic chemistry is subject to far more
expressed less confidence in fingerprint evidence than
criticism than DNA, fingerprint, or ballistic evidence,
DNA, partially because it requires subjective
with about a quarter of the judges arguing that it is
assessments by human experts (i.e., regarding whether
unreliable. Interestingly, perceptions of the general
there is a match; see Dror, 2013, for a related
reliability of forensic chemistry evidence are more
discussion). As one judge stated,
focused on the work quality of the particular expert
than the other three evidentiary forms: the DNA goes in the computer and it's a
match by the computer and it makes the
So many of the chemists are screwy. They
match very accurately. With fingerprints, the
have had issues in a lot of the labs. They
computer says it is a match and a human
haven't met standards. Some of the chemists
being has to look with comparison
have fudged the numbers. It gets a little hairy.
microscopes and make a determination so
You have no way of knowing as you sit here
there is more room for human error. (Judge
that this is reliable or unreliable. I have seen
9)
them effectively impeached. (Judge 5)
Notably, while several judges explained that a
Presenting a very different perspective, several judges
computer's ability to make accurate matches far
in the jurisdiction expressed confidence in the overall
exceeds that of human experts, these same judges
scientific procedures employed by chemists appearing
conceded that they do not understand the computer
in their courtrooms:
processes involved.
The chemist comes in and they talk about the
Evaluative Processes
presumptive test and then about the gas test
and how they confirm that it is cocaine or it's In assessing the reliability of different
heroin or whatever. That is very routine stuff evidentiary forms, judges were encouraged and
and I have never seen anybody effectively afforded the opportunity to freely discuss their
challenge one of the police lab chemists on a evaluative processes. As our analyses reveal, certain
simple drug analysis. (Judge 27) evaluative patterns emerged among judges. First, we
observed that increased understanding of the scientific
Common Themes methods commonly utilized for a given type of
Two prevalent themes (regarding these evidence was associated with judges being more
general perceptions) warrant mention. First, judicial critical of these processes, whereas a lack of
notions of evidentiary reliability are heavily dependent understanding was often coupled with a higher degree
on judges' perceptions of the general reputation of the of acceptance. For example, judges were more focused
evidence, as opposed to their own courtroom on the scientific procedures employed in fingerprint
experiences. For example, despite our request that analyses than DNA; judges explained the various
judges discuss their evaluation of the evidence based points of comparison and quality of the prints, were

Criminology, CriminalJustice, Law & Society - Volume 22, Issue 1


8 NIR & Liu

aware of the comparative methods employed by There is skill in the laboratory and there is
experts in the field and noted the susceptibility of the skill in testifying. I can only judge their skill
evidence to subjective and potentially unreliable in testifying. There are some people who can
assessments. Likewise, judges were somewhat communicate more effectively than others,
focused on the methodologies (e.g., visual but I don't know if that means they're less
comparisons) used to match shell casings to guns in skilled at achieving the right result. (Judge
evaluating ballistics evidence; this increased 24)
understanding was accompanied by greater critiques Likewise, conversations pertaining to ballistics
regarding the methodologies and conclusions rendered evidence focused on the testifying skill of the expert.
by ballistics experts. In contrast, despite their high In general, judges were highly complementary of
regard for DNA evidence, almost a quarter of the ballistics experts, several noting less variation in
judges openly admitted that they did not fully quality among ballistics experts than fingerprint
understand how DNA evidence works or the specific experts: "The same witnesses come in all of the time.
methodologies involved (for similar discussions, see It's almost like you press play and they say what they
McQuiston-Surrett & Saks, 2009; Saks & Faigman, say" (Judge 5). A second judge stated: "They testify
2008; Wojcikiewicz, 2013); while discussions very well because that's all that they do. So, I think
regarding the reliability of DNA evidence were filled that their testimony is clean and succinct and
with praise, they involved virtually no critical analysis persuasive" (Judge 28). This focus on presentation
or even reference to scientific methods. skills was also prevalent in analyses of the reliability
Second, in responding to questions regarding of forensic chemistry, where judges were focused on
the reliability of different forms of evidence, the inability of expert chemists to effectively
assessments often centered around the showmanship, communicate with the jury.
communication skills, and likeability of testifying We observed a third pattern in judges'
experts rather than on the scientific basis of the evaluative processes. Our respondents often focused
testimony (see Brodsky et al., 2010). For example, in on the weight of the evidence (a task for the factfinder)
response to our question "what is your perception of rather than its admissibility (a task for the gatekeeper).
DNA evidence?" one judge noted: "Some explain it For example, some judges assessed the substantive
better than others. Some just have a little bit more flair limitations of DNA in criminal trials, extrinsic to the
to their testimony because it is pretty dry stuff' (Judge reliability of the scientific evidence itself. Specifically,
34). Another judge described the importance of about one third of the judges explained that DNA
reaching the jury: "I think that if the expert is hyper evidence is often only one piece of the puzzle and
technical it goes right over everyone's head. The best cannot establish all of the elements of the crime:
experts are those that make it comprehensible to the
ordinary juror" (Judge 9). A third judge assessed the DNA doesn't necessarily determine the case;
overall communication skills and protocols of most it only determines defendant's presence.
DNA experts appearing in his courtroom: What happened and everything else is still up
for grabs. Sometimes you get DNA from a
Most of them that I have seen have been from woman who says that she was raped; you still
the Office of the Chief Medical Examiner. have to show that it's not consensual. That's
They have very clear English-speaking not the end of the case. (Judge 9)
witnesses that can articulate what this is, how
it works and what it means. . . . They've Similarly, judges noted the evidentiary limitations of
developed a script that is capable of fingerprint analyses in establishing the required
explaining this very complicated stuff to the elements of the charged crime. In particular, while
jury in a very simple way. (Judge 19) judges perceive that fingerprint evidence is highly
probative for identifying perpetrators in circumstantial
Notably, none of our questions prompted our cases (e.g., it can place a defendant at the scene of the
respondents to comment on the expert's crime), it doesn't necessarily determine guilt: "Just
communication skills or showmanship; our inquiries because you find somebody's fingerprints, that in and
were focused on reliability and credibility of itself doesn't necessarily mean anything unless it
determinations applicable to admissibility decisions. ties in with other evidence" (Judge 40; i.e., there may
Similarly, assessments regarding fingerprint be another explanation for defendant's presence at the
testimony often led with critiques about the scene). Likewise, judges emphasized that ballistics
presentation skills of the expert witness; a few judges evidence is only a small piece of the evidentiary
openly explained that this focus is due to their lack of puzzle. Specifically, while evidence relating to gun
knowledge about the scientific processes: operability is usually highly reliable, weapon
operability is rarely an issue at trial: "I have never

Criminology, CriminalJustice, Law & Society - Volume 22, Issue 1


NIR & Liu 9

found it to determine guilt or innocence" (Judge 7). Variations in Quality of Experts


Finally, judges emphasized that chemist testimony is
With heightened concern regarding the
usually not dispositive of case outcome.
subjective aspects of fingerprint evidence, judges
Finally, a fourth area of emphasis in
focused on the quality of the fingerprint expert: "It all
evaluating the reliability of scientific evidence
depends on the quality of the expert, which varies. I
centered on chain of custody issues. For example, one
really think that there's a strong bearing in who
judge noted how chain of custody concerns can
collected it, how they collected it, what their
decrease the reliability of DNA evidence:
background and training is, and how they come across
You cannot challenge the science of DNA; as a witness" (Judge 30). Several judges criticized
everyone knows how reliable DNA is. I have certain police technicians who present fingerprint
seen cases where the evidence was evidence in the jurisdiction due to lack of qualified
mishandled or mixed with other fluids and experience:
then that type of issue comes up and then you
I have seen such slip shot police officers that
get a mixed sample. (Judge 9)
should never have been police officers let
Similarly, discussions about properly preserving the alone technicians taking evidence and then
chain of custody of fingerprints, ballistics evidence, trying to present it as matching or something.
and samples of drugs (e.g., by police and scientists) When they have only done 5 or 6 or 7, that
were noted during discussions about evidentiary doesn't mean that they are an expert; that just
reliability. means they happened to be there and
somebody said, 'here, you do this.' (Judge
Reference to the Frye Standard
39)
Only a minority of judges referenced the Frye
In contrast, about a third of the judges perceive that the
standard when discussing how they evaluate the
quality of fingerprint experts appearing in their
reliability of scientific evidence. Even among those
courtrooms as relatively uniform: "Distinctions in the
judges who acknowledged that Frye is the applicable
quality of these experts are minimal" (Judge 8).
standard which guides reliability determinations of
scientific evidence in the jurisdiction, their discussions Inability to Communicate Effectively
regarding reliability were dominated by other areas of
The overwhelming area of focused concern
concern.
in response to questions regarding the reliability of
Prevalent Areas of Concern chemist evidence is the inability of chemists to
effectively communicate with the jury in English: "As
Areas of expressed concerns varied by
far as the testimony is concerned, sometimes they have
evidentiary type. As discussed below, judges focused
trouble being understood because English isn't their
on the inability of attorneys to challenge DNA experts,
first language" (Judge 24). Apart from language
the variations in quality among fingerprint experts,
barriers, judges were critical of the overall ability of
and the poor communication skills of expert chemists.
chemists to grasp the attention of jurors: "There is an
These concerns are expressed below.
art to testifying that some people do not get, like the
Incompetency ofAttorneys ability to hold the jury's attention and being up on the
field" (Judge 17). Another judge simply stated:
Judges were highly critical of the inability of
"Chemists don't make the best witnesses" (Judge 6).
attorneys to challenge the reliability of DNA experts
and lamented that this widespread deficiency limits the Credibility Assessments
power of the court system to achieve its truth finding
Judges also discussed the factors they
mission: "The adversarial system fails its purpose
consider in evaluating witness credibility; consistent
when the attorney is not skilled enough to challenge
with the scholarly literature (see Brodsky et al., 2010;
the expert" (Judge 14; see Damon-Moore, 2017;
Connolly & Gordon, 2011; Porter & Brinke, 2009;
Edmond & Roque, 2012; Kovera & McAuliff, 2000;
Wessel et al., 2006), assessment tools and
Saks & Faigman, 2008). Many judges attributed this
determinants vary widely and similar facts were
inability to attorneys' lack of scientific knowledge (see
interpreted differently by various judges. In fact, a
Edmond & Roque, 2012; Saks & Faigman, 2008):
high degree of subjectivity is evident at the core of
"DNA people almost always get a free pass because
judicial credibility assessments, highlighting the
lawyers are intimidated by DNA and do not know how
potential for vastly disparate conclusions. Factors
to attack it" (Judge 16).
considered to determine credibility include motive,
body language, attitude, witness background ("you

Criminology, CriminalJustice, Law & Society - Volume 22, Issue 1


10 NIR & Liu

don't get your players from central casting" [Judge Discussion & Conclusions
17]), relationship between the parties, and consistency
in testimony, among other considerations. In assessing Given the importance of judicial gatekeeping
credibility, judges use a host of observational decisions, reliable and streamlined evaluative
techniques and similar facts may lead different judges processes are critical to ensure that evidentiary
to opposite conclusions. For example, one judge decision-making proceeds fairly and that evidence
explained how he evaluates credibility based on the admitted "through the gate" is reliable, relevant, and
witness's demeanor: "As a rule of thumb, if somebody trustworthy. Despite the importance of fair and
is too calm, I feel they were probably rehearsed and consistent gatekeeping processes, our study reveals
maybe they weren't telling the truth" (Judge 21). In that evidentiary rulings and evaluations are sometimes
contrast, other judges perceive nervousness negatively fraught with subjectivity and bias and made by
and calmness as a sign of truthfulness. A few judges uninformed judges using inadequate criteria.
described how body language can reveal dishonesty: First, consistent with existing scholarship
"I look at how they are moving their eyes. Which way (Allen & Nafisi, 2010; Canela et al., 2019;
are their eyes going as they are thinking? There is a McQuiston-Surrett & Saks, 2009; Neal et al., 2019;
whole science behind left bending eyes; which is recall Saks & Faigman, 2008), our data reveal that judges
and which is thinking up a lie" (Judge 4). Several often do not understand the scientific processes
judges found witnesses to lack credibility based on the involved in analyzing forensic evidence. While it is
vagueness of their answers to attorney questions: understandable that judicial expertise lies in legal
"When witnesses start being vague, instead of just matters and not science, those tasked to evaluate the
answering the question, when they beat around the admissibility of scientific evidence must be able to
bush like politicians do, that's a red flag to me" (Judge understand the processes that they are evaluating in
14). While some judges are confident in their own order to effectively carry out their gatekeeping
ability to assess credibility, others are far less mission. This issue is exacerbated by current
confident: "It's a very difficult area, it's a very documented validity and reliability concerns regarding
subjective area and I don't claim to have any superior main feature-comparison methods in forensic science
ability to determine credibility in a close case. And I (e.g., fallibility of DNA evidence, lack of foundational
have been wrong in the past" (Judge 37). validity of ballistic evidence, and the higher-than-
believed false positive rates of fingerprint analysis;
Influence of Personal Experiences
President's Council of Advisors on Science and
At times, judges referred to their own out-of- Technology, 2016). In Frye jurisdictions, how can
court encounters in discussing their perceptions of judges possibly evaluate whether or not the scientific
evidence. For some, their personal life experiences processes used are generally accepted within the
might have influenced the manner in which they scientific community if they cannot understand the
evaluated case evidence. One judge considered his methodologies employed by the testifying expert?
own experience as a witness in evaluating the This could be more alarming in Daubertjurisdictions
credibility of an "anxious looking" witness appearing where judges are further tasked with determining
in his courtroom: "If they were nervous, they probably reliability using evaluative criteria foreign to their area
had a good reason to be. Not because they were of expertise. This lack of knowledge would be
lying...because you should be uncomfortable, I was problematic on its own, even if defense attorneys were
once called as a witness, I was uncomfortable" (Judge fully schooled in applicable scientific processes and
21). A second judge was not disturbed by the able to competently challenge testifying experts.
prosecution's failure to produce fingerprint evidence However, prior studies show-and our subjects
in a weapons case, based on his own experiences with confirm-that this is not the case (Edmond & Roque,
guns: "People don't understand that certain surfaces 2012; Saks & Faigman, 2008). In fact, while the stakes
do not leave fingerprints so that's a difficult hurdle for involved in criminal cases (life, liberty) are greater
prosecutors... I... have handled guns so I know than those in civil cases (predominantly financial),
sometimes you leave one, sometimes you don't" criminal cases are far less likely to have competing
(Judge 1). Finally, a third judge was unconvinced by expert testimony, and defense attorneys are often less
the defense argument that a boxcutter could not have proficient in their ability to challenge scientific experts
caused an injury, based on his own past injury: (Dioso-Villa, 2015; Groscup et al., 2002; Risinger,
"Having been cut by one in the past I know it can cause 2000; Shniderman, 2017) than civil court actors.
serious damage" (Judge 2). These examples Further, the ability of criminal defendants to obtain
demonstrate how judicial life experiences sometimes competing experts may depend on their
influence judicial assessments of evidence. socioeconomic positions; variances in financial
resources may increase racial disparities in

Criminology, CriminalJustice, Law & Society - Volume 22, Issue 1


NIR & Li 1
11

gatekeeping decisions. In the end, the combination of that lead to stark differences in the application of
judges' lack of scientific knowledge, the inability of gatekeeping considerations. These differences
many defense attorneys to challenge expert witnesses, highlight monumental challenges to equitable
and financial constraints preventing some defendants gatekeeping processes similar to those echoed by
from securing competing experts, may lead judges to Judge Marvin Frankel and others in discussing
one-sidedly rely on prosecution experts in reaching sentencing disparities (Frankel, 1972; Lynch, 2009).
gatekeeping decisions. Existing scholarship is relatively silent regarding
While legislative attempts and (jurisdiction- unwarranted disparities in evidentiary rulings (in
dependent) case precedents strive to provide guidance contrast to the abundance of scholarship on sentencing
by delineating criteria and standards (such as the Frye disparities). Nevertheless, evidentiary rulings may
and Daubert standards) for judges to follow, lead to unfair outcomes that not only influence the
scholarship reveals that this guidance does not severity of punishment imposed post-conviction, but
effectively direct judicial gatekeeping processes also affect plea offers, verdicts, and case strategies at
(Cheng & Yoon, 2005; Damon-Moore, 2017; Groscup earlier influential stages. Without effective advocates
et al., 2002; Jensen, 2003; Moreno, 2003; Neal et al., or knowledgeable gatekeepers, criminal defendants
2019; see Beecher-Monas, 1999 for a combined use of are vulnerable to damaging unreliable evidence that
multiple mechanisms to facilitate judges' decision unfairly weaken their position during the adversarial
making). In fact, only a few of our respondents even process.
referenced the Frye standard in discussing reliability This study has several methodological
determinations, reinforcing prior scholarship that the limitations. First, our study directly explores judicial
applicable gatekeeping standards (i.e. Frye or perceptions of four types of forensic evidence (DNA,
Daubert, or other adapted variations) are not fingerprints, ballistics, and forensic chemistry) and
particularly influential in reliability assessments. judicial credibility assessments of witnesses. Future
Indeed, our respondents discussed many studies should expand the types of forensic evidence
considerations they employ in evaluating the and also study non-scientific evidence including
reliability of forensic evidence extrinsic to relevant confessions, eyewitnesses, and other physical and
factors, including the showmanship and documentary evidence. Second, our respondents all
communication skills of the testifying expert (see preside in a single state that employs the Frye standard
Brodsky et al., 2010; O'Regan, 2017). A few judges for admissibility of expert scientific evidence. Since
clearly expressed the reason for this focus: "There is the "general acceptance" test in Frye is considerably
skill in the laboratory and there is skill in testifying. I more general than the criteria established in Daubert,
can only judge their skill in testifying" (Judge 24). it would be helpful to sample judges from Daubert(or
While evaluations regarding showmanship and its variant) jurisdictions to further explore judicial
communication skills may touch on the witness's understanding and application of the relevant
effectiveness on the stand, it speaks little to the standards in their jurisdictions. However, this
reliability of the substance of that testimony limitation is mitigated based on existing literature that
(O'Regan, 2017). establishes that, regardless of the adoption of Frye or
Extending beyond reliability assessments, Daubert standards, "the practical results are
credibility determinations play a role in gatekeeping essentially the same" (Cheng & Yoon, 2005: 474; see
decisions. While credibility assessments do not also Faigman et al., 2002; Giannelli, 2003). Finally,
generally require specific expertise beyond judicial our study sample is limited to judges; future studies
common sense, our data reveal that the determination should explore the perceptions of other court actors
process follows a highly subjective path and that regarding judicial gatekeeping decisions.
different judges viewing similar witness aspects (e.g.,
body language) sometimes reach opposing
conclusions regarding trustworthiness. Further, our
data reveal that judges draw on their personal
experiences in assessing credibility and reliability. References
Indeed, our respondents described a host of influential
Allen, R. J., & Nafisi, E. (2010). Daubert and its
life experiences that guide their determinations. The
discontents. Brooklyn Law Review, 76(1), 131-
influence of a judge's life experiences on their
166.
perceptions and interpretations is natural; one might
even argue that judges are elected or appointed to the Alter, Z. (2017). Unpacking Frye-Mack: A critical
bench because of their rich backgrounds that influence analysis of Minnesota's Frye-Mack Standard for
their world and courtroom views. Yet, distinct life admitting scientific evidence. Mitchell Hamline
experiences can influence judicial decisions in ways Law Review, 43, 626-667.

Criminology, CriminalJustice, Law & Society - Volume 22, Issue 1


12 NIR & Liu

Beecher-Monas, E. (1998). Blinded by science: How Damon-Moore, S. L. (2017). Trial judges and the
judges avoid the science in scientific evidence. forensic science problem. New York University
Temple Law Review, 71(1), 55-102. Law Review, 92(5), 1532-1570.
Beecher-Monas, E. (1999). A ray of light for judges Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, 509 U.S.
blinded by science: Triers of science and 579 (1993).
intellectual due process. Georgia Law Review,
DeMatteo, D., Fishel, S., & Tansey, A. (2019). Expert
33(4), 1047-1112.
evidence: The (unfulfilled) promise of Daubert.
Blanck, P. D. (1992). Calibrating the scales of justice: Psychological Science in the Public Interest,
Studying judges' behavior in bench trials. Indiana 20(3), 129-134.
Law Journal, 68(4), 1119-1198. htps://doi.org/10. 1177/1529100619894336
Brodsky, S. L., Griffin, M. P., & Cramer, R. J. (2010). Derwin, A. (2018). The judicial admission of flawed
The witness credibility scale: An outcome expert opinion evidence informing wrongful
measure for expert witness research. Behavioral convictions. Western Journal of Legal Studies,
Sciences and the Law, 28, 892-907. 8(2), 1-19.
https://doi.org/0. 1002/bsl.9 17
Dezember, A., & Redlich, A. D. (2019). Plea
Burns, K. (2016). Judges, 'common sense' and judicial bargaining in the shadow of the trial. In C. Spohn
cognition. Griffith Law Review, 25(3), 319-351. & P. Brennan (Eds.), Handbook on Sentencing
https://doi.org/10.108010383441.2016.1259536 policies and practices in the 21st century (pp.
168-187). Routledge.
Bushway, S. D., Redlich, A. D., & Norris, R. J. (2014).
An explicit test of plea bargaining in the "shadow Dickson, J. (2019). Daubert won't do: Why expert
of the trial". Criminology, 52(4), 723-754. testimony regarding future dangerousness
https://doi.org/10.1111/1745-9125.12054 requires new rule of evidence. Georgetown Law
Journal, 107(2), 481-499.
Butt, L. (2013). The forensic confirmation bias:
Problems, perspectives, and proposed solutions- Dioso-Villa, R. (2016). Is the expert admissibility
Commentary by a forensic examiner. Journal of game fixed? Judicial gatekeeping of fire and arson
Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 2, evidence. Law & Policy, 38(1), 54-80.
59-60. https://doi.org/10. 111 1/lapo.12047
https://doi.org/10.1016/i.ia c.2013.01.012
Dror, I. (2013). The ambition to be scientific: Human
Canela, C., Buadze, A., Dube, A., Jackowski, C., expert performance and objectivity. Science and
Pude, I., Nellen, R., Signorini, P., & Liebrenz, M. Justice, 53, 81-82.
(2019). How do legal experts cope with medical littps://doi.org/10. 1016/j scijus 20 13.03.002
reports and forensic evidence? The experiences,
Dror, I. E., & Cole, S. A. (2010). The vision in "blind"
perceptions, and narratives of Swiss judges and
justice: Expert perception, judgment, and visual
other legal experts. Frontiers in Psychiatry,
cognition in forensic pattern recognition.
10(18), 1-13.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(2), 161-167.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsat.2019.00018
https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.172.161
Cheng, E. K., & Yoon, A. H. (2005). Does Frye or
Dror, I. E., Champod, C., Langenburg, G., Charlton,
Daubert matter? A study of scientific
D., Hunt, H., & Rosenthal, R. (2011). Cognitive
admissibility standards. Virginia Law Review,
issues in fingerprint analysis: Inter-and intra-
91(2), 471-513.
expert consistency and the effect of a 'target'
Cole, S. A. (2009). Forensics without uniqueness, comparison. ForensicScience International,208,
conclusions without individualization: The new 10-17.
epistemology of forensic identification. Law, https://doi.org/10. 1016/j.forsciint.2010. 10.013
Probability and Risk, 8(3), 233- 255.
Edmond, G., & Roque, M. S. (2012-2013). The cool
https:H/doi.org/10. 1093/lpr/mgpo 16
crucible: Forensic science and the frailty of the
Connolly, D. A., & Gordon, H. M. (2011). He-said- criminal trial. CurrentIssues in CriminalJustice,
she-said: Contrast effects in credibility 24, 51-68.
assessments and possible threats to fundamental https://doi.org/10.1080/10345329.2012.1203594
principles of criminal law. Legal and 4
Criminological Psychology, 16, 227-241.
https://doi.org/10.1348/135532510X498185

Criminology, CriminalJustice, Law & Society - Volume 22, Issue 1


NIR & Liu 13

Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V (1969). Nonverbal Giannelli, P. C. (2003). Admissibility of scientific
leakage and clues to deception. Psychiatry: evidence. Oklahoma City University Law
Journalfor the Study ofInterpersonalProcesses, Review, 28, 1-16.
32(1), 88-106.
Giannelli, P. C. (2007). Wrongful convictions and
https://doi.org/1.1080/0033274, 1969.1102357
forensic science: The need to regulate crime labs.
5
North CarolinaLaw Review, 86(1), 163-236.
Elwell, D. A. (2020). Industry-influenced evidence:
Greenstein, R. K. (2009). Determining facts: The myth
Bias, conflict, and manipulation in scientific
of diect evidence. Houston Law Review, 45(5),
evidence. Boston College Law Review, 61(6),
1801-1830.
2155-2190.
Grimm, P. W. (2018). Challenges facing judges
Faigman, D. L., Kaye, D. H., Saks, M. J., & Sanders,
regarding expert evidence in criminal cases.
J. (2002). Modern scientific evidence: The law
Fordham Law Review, 86(4), 1601-1616.
and science of expert testimony (Vol. 1). West
Group. Groscup, J. L., Penrod, S. D., Studebaker, C. A., Huss,
M. T., & O'Neil, K. M. (2002). The effects of
Faigman, D. L., Slobogin, C., & Monahan, J. (2016).
Daubert on the admissibility of expert testimony
Gatekeeping science: using the structure of
in state and federal criminal cases. Psychology,
scientific research to distinguish between
Public Policy, and Law, 8(4), 339-372.
admissibility and weight in expert testimony.
httips:H/doi.org/10. 1037//1076-8971.8.4.339
Northwestern University Law Review, 110(4),
859-904. Heller, K. (2006). The cognitive psychology of
circumstantial evidence. Michigan Law Review,
Federal Bureau of Investigation (FBI). (2019).
105(2), 241-306.
Handbook of forensic services.
http://www.f bi.gov/about-us/lab/a ndbook-of- Jensen, P. J. (2003). Frye versus Daubert: Practically
forensic-services-pdf the same. Minnesota Law Review, 87(5), 1579-
1620.
Federal Bureau of Investigation (FBI). (2020).
Frequently asked questions on CODIS and NDIS. Jurs, A. (2009). Judicial Analysis of Complex

&
httips://www .fbi.pov/services/laboratory/biometri Cutting-Edge Science in the Daubert Era:
c-analysis/codis/codis-and-ndis-fact-shect Epidemiologic Risk Assessment as a Test for
Case for Reform Strategies. Connecticut Law
Federal Rules of Evidence (2021). Rule 702
-

Review, 42(1), 49-100.


Testimony by Expert Witnesses.
htts://www.rulesofevidence.org/article-vii/rule- Kassin, S. M., Dror, I. E., & Kukucka, J. (2013). The
702/ forensic confirmation bias: Problems,
perspectives, and proposed solutions. Journal of
Findley, K. A. (2013). Judicial gatekeeping of suspect
Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 2(1),
evidence: Due process and evidentiary rules in the
42-52.
age of innocence. GeorgiaLaw Review, 47, 723-
https://doi.org/10.1016/i.iarmac.2013.01.001
774.
Kovera, M. B., & McAuliff, B. D. (2000). The effects
Fradella, H. F., O'Neill, L., & Fogary, A. (2004). The
of peer review and evidence quality on judge
impact of Daubert on forensic science.
evaluations of psychological science: Are judges
PepperdineLaw Review, 31(2), 323-362.
effective gatekeepers? Journal of Applied
Frankel, M. E. (1972). Lawlessness in sentencing. Psychology, 85(4), 574-586.
University of CincinnatiLaw Review, 41(1), 1-54. https:doi.org/10. 1037/002-9010.85.4.574
Frye v. United States, 293 F. 1013 (D.C. Cir, 1923). Krafka, C., Dunn, M. A., Johnson, M. T., Cecil, J. S.,
& Miletich, D. (2002). Judge and attorney
Gatowski, S. I., Dobbin, S. A., Richardson, J. T.,
experiences, practices, and concerns regarding
Ginsburg, G. P., Merlino, M. L., & Dahir, V.
expert testimony in federal civil trials.
(2001). Asking the gatekeepers: A national survey
Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 8(3), 309-
of judges on judging expert evidence in a post-
332. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8971.8.3.309
Daubertworld. Law and Human Behavior, 25(5),
433-458.
https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1012899030937
General Electric Co. v. Joiner, 522 U.S. 136 (1997).

Criminology, CriminalJustice, Law & Society - Volume 22, Issue 1


14 NIR & Liu

Kramer, J. H., & Ulmer, J. T. (2002). Downward Nardulli, P. F. (1986). Insider justice: Defense
departures for serious violent offenders: Local attorneys and the handling of felony cases.
court "corrections" to Pennsylvania's sentencing Journal of Criminal Law & Criminology, 77,
guidelines. Criminology, 40(4), 897-932. 379-417. https://doi.org/10.2307/1143338
https://doi.org/10.1111/i.1745-
National Academy of Sciences (NAS). (2009).
9125.2002.tb00977.x
Strengthening forensic science in the United
Kramer, G., Wolbransky, M., & Heilbrun, K. (2007). States: A path forward.
Plea bargaining recommendations by criminal https://doi.org/10. 17226/12589
defense attorneys: Evidence strength, potential
Neal, T. M., Slobogin, C., Saks, M. J., Faigman, D. L.,
sentence and defendant preferences. Behavioral
& Geisinger, K. F. (2019). Psychological
Sciences and the Law, 25, 573-585.
assessments in legal contexts: Are courts keeping
https://doi.org/10.1002/bsl.759
"junk science" out of the courtroom?
Kumho Tire Co. v. Carmichael, 526 U.S. 137 (1999). Psychological Science in the Public Interest,
20(3), 135-164.
Landes, W. (1971). An economic analysis of the
https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100619888860
courts. Journal of Labor Economics, 14(1), 61-
107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/466704 Nir, E. (2018). Approaching the bench: accessing
elites on the judiciary for qualitative
Levon, E., & Ye, Y. (2020). Language, indexicality
interviews. International Journal of Social
and gender ideologies: contextual effects on the
Research Methodology, 21(1), 77-
perceived credibility of women. Gender and
89. https://doi.org/10.1080/136455792017.1324
Language, 14(2), 123-151.
669
https://doi.org/10.1558/gen1.39235
Nir, E., & Griffiths, E. (2018). Sentencing on the
Liden, M., Grans, M., & Juslin, P. (2019). 'Guilty, no
evidence. CriminalJustice Policy Review,
doubt': detention provoking confirmation bias in
judges' guilt assessments and debiasing 29(4), 365-390.
techniques. Psychology, Crime & Law, 25(3), https: //doi. org/ 10. 1177/088740341663 5248
2 19-247.
Nir, E., & Griffiths, E. (2019). The thirteenth
juror. The British Journalof Criminology, 59(2),
Lynch, G. E. (2009). Marvin Frankel: A reformer
315-333. https:doiorg/10.1093/bic/azyO29
reassessed. Federal Sentencing Reporter, 21(4),
235-241. Nir, E., & Liu, S. (in press). Defending constitutional
https://doi.org/10.1525/fsr.2009.21.4.235 rights in imbalanced courtrooms. Journal of
CriminalLaw and Criminology.
Maxwell, J. A. (2005). Qualitative research design:
An interactive approach (2 "a ed.). Sage O'Brien, T. L. (2018). Gender, expert advice, and
Publications, Inc. judicial gatekeeping in the United States. Social
Science Research, 72, 134-145.
McQuiston-Surrett, D., & Saks, M. J. (2009). The
https:H/doi.org/10. 10 16/j. ssresearch.20 18.02.011I
testimony of forensic identification science: What
expert witnesses say and what factfinders hear. O'Regan, D. (2017). Eying the body: The impact of
Law and Human Behavior, 33, 436-453. classical rules for demeanor credibility, bias, and
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-008-9169-1 the need to blind legal decision makers. PaceLaw
Review, 37(2), 379-454.
Moreno, J. (2003). Einstein on the bench: Exposing
what judges do not know about science and using Patton, M. Q. (2002). Qualitative research and
child abuse cases to improve how courts evaluate evaluation methods (3 rd ed.). Sage Publications.
scientific evidence. Ohio State Law Journal,
Peterson, J. L., Sommers, I., Baskin, D., & Johnson,
64(2), 531-584.
D. (2010). The role and impact of forensic
Moulin, V., Mouchet, C., Pillonel, T., Gkotsi, G. M., evidence in the criminal justice process. U.S.
Baertschi, B., Gasser, J., & Teste, B. (2018). Department of Justice.
Judges' perceptions of expert reports: The effect https://www.ojp.gov/pdffilesl/rdj/grants/231977.
of neuroscience evidence. InternationalJournal pdf
of Law and Psychiatry, 61, 22-29.
https://doi.org/10.1016/i.ijlp.2018.09.008

Criminology, CriminalJustice, Law & Society - Volume 22, Issue 1


NIR & Liu 15

Peterson, J. L., Hickman, M. J., Strom, K. J., Shelton, D. E. (2009). Twenty-First Century forensic

&
Johnson, D. J. (2013). Effect of forensic evidence science challenges for trial judges in criminal
on criminal justice case processing. Journal of cases: Where the polybutadiene meets the
Forensic Science, 58, S78-S90. bitumen. Widener Law Journal, 18, 309-396.
https://doi.org/10. 1111/1556-4029.12020
Shelton, D. E. (2010). Forensic science evidence and
Porter, S., & Brinke, L. T. (2009). Dangerous judicial bias in criminal cases. The Judges'
decisions: A theoretical framework for Journal,49(3), 18-24.
understanding how judges assess credibility in the
Shniderman, A. B. (2017). Prosecutors respond to
courtroom. Legal and Criminological
calls for forensic science reform: More sharks in
Psychology, 14, 119-134.
dirty water. Yale Law Journal Forum, 126, 348-
https://doi.org/10.1348/135532508X281520
361.
President's Council of Advisors on Science and
Smalarz, L., Madon, S., Yang, Y., Guyll, M., & Buck,
Technology. (2016). Report to the President,
S. (2016). The perfect match: Do criminal
Forensic Science in Criminal Courts: Ensuring
stereotypes bias forensic evidence analysis? Law
Scientific Validity of Feature-comparison
and Human Behavior, 40(4), 420-429.
Methods. Executive Office of the President of the
https://doi.org/10.1037/lhb0000190
United States.
https://obamawhitehouse.archives. gov/sites/defa Smith, S. M., Stinson, V., & Patry, M. W. (2011). Fact
ult/files/rnicrosites/ost2/PCAST/pcast forensic s of fiction? The myth and reality of the CSI effect.
cience report finalgpdf CourtReview, 47, 4-7.
Redlich, A. D., Bushway, S. D., & Norris, R. J. (2016). Stevenage, S. V., & Bennett, A. (2017). A biased
Plea decision-making by attorneys and judges. opinion: Demonstration of cognitive bias on a
JournalofExperimentalCriminology, 12(4), 537- fingerprint matching task through knowledge of
561. https://doi.org/10.1007/si1292-016-9264-0 DNA test results. ForensicScience International,
276, 93-106.
Risinger, D. (2000). Navigating expert reliability: Are
https://doi.org/10. 1016/jforsciint.2017.04.009
criminal standards of certainty being left on the
dock. Albany Law Review, 64(1), 99-152. Thompson, S. G. (2012). Judicial gatekeeping of
police-generated witness testimony. Journal of
Rossman, H. H., McDonald, W. F., & Cramer, J. A.
CriminalLaw & Criminology, 102(2), 329-396.
(1980). Some patterns and determinants of plea-
bargaining decisions: A simulation and quasi- Trozzi, T. A., Schwartz, R. L., Hollars, M. L.,
experiment. In W. F. McDonald & J. A. Cramer Leighton, L. D., Trozzi, Y. E., & Wade, C. (2001).
(Eds.), Plea bargaining(pp. 77-114). Lexington Processing guide for developing latent prints.
Books. U.S. Department of Justice, Federal Bureau of
Investigation.
Rozelle, S. D. (2007). Daubert, Schmaubert: Criminal
hittps ://archives fbi. gov/archives/aboutt-
defendants and the short end of the science stick.
us/lab/forensic-science-
Tulsa Law Review, 43(2), 597-608.
cominunications/fsc/ an2001/lpu.pdf
Saks, M. J., & Faigman, D. L. (2008). Failed forensics:
Yan, S. (2019) Estimating the size of plea discounts:
How forensic science lost its way and how it
Why does it matter? In C. Spohn & P. Brennan
might yet find it. Annual Review of Law and
(Eds.), Handbook on sentencing policies and
Social Science, 4, 149-171.
practices in the 21st century (pp. 188-207).
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.lawsocsci.4.110
Routledge.
707.172303
U. S. Congress Committee on the Judiciary. (2014).
Schweitzer, N. J., & Saks, M. J. (2009). The
Federal Rules of Evidence (Rule 702, p. 15).
gatekeeper effect: The impact of judges'
URL:
admissibility decisions on the persuasiveness of
https://www.uscourts.gov/sites/default/files/Rule
expert testimony. Psychology, PublicPolicy, and
sO 2Oof o20Evidence. Retrieved on Mar. 24,
Law, 15(1), 1-18.
2021.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.lawsocsci.4.110
707172303 Walsh, J. T. (1999). Keeping the gate: The evolving
role of the judiciary in admitting scientific
evidence. Judicature, 83(3), 140-143.

Criminology, CriminalJustice, Law & Society - Volume 22, Issue 1


16 NIR & Liu

Wessel, E., Drevland, G. C., Eilertsen, D. E., About the Authors

&
Magnussen, S. (2006). Credibility of the
emotional witness: A study of ratings by court Esther Nir is an Associate Professor in the
judges. Law Human Behavior, 30, 221-230. Department of Criminal Justice at New Jersey
https:Hdoi.org/10. 1007/s1O979-006-9024- City University. She received her doctoral degree
from Rutgers University School of Criminal
Wilson, M. J. W. (2016). Defense attorney bias and the Justice and her law degree from Fordham Law
rush to the plea. University of Kansas Law School. She previously served as a prosecutor in
Review, 65(2), 271-326. Queens County, New York. Her research interests
https://doiorg/1O. 1716 1/1808.25553 include sentencing disparities, police misconduct,
Wojcikiewicz, J. (2013). Judges' attitude towards qualitative methods, and community engaged
scientific evidence. Revija za Kriminalistiko in learning initiatives. Her research can be found in
Kriminologijo, 64, 249-255. various criminal justice and research
methodology journals including The British
Journal of Criminology, CriminalJustice Policy
Review, The International Journal of Social
Research Methodology, International Journal of
Police Science and Management, and others.
Siyu Liu is an Assistant Professor of Criminal Justice
in the School of Public Affairs at Penn State
Harrisburg. Her research is currently focusing on
police legitimacy, juvenile delinquency, and the
Chinese death penalty. Her work has appeared in
Journal of Quantitative Criminology, Criminal
Justice and Behavior, Psychology, Crime and
Law, Asian Journal of Criminology, and others.
She received her doctoral degree in 2014 from the
School of Criminal Justice, State University of
New York at Albany.

Endnotes

In contrast to jury trials, judges in bench trials are both gatekeepers of the evidence and triers of the fact.
2 Subsequently, the Supreme Court underlined the limited scope of appellate review in judicial gatekeeping
decisions and emphasized the wide discretionary power of trialjudges in the evaluation of methodology, as well
as the expert's reasoning (General Electric Co. v. Joiner, 1997); the Court further expanded the trial judge's
authority based on Daubert beyond scientific testimonies to all expert testimonies (Kumho Tire Co v. Carmichael,
1999).
3 While only 7 of our 41 respondents are female, this number roughly reflects the percentage of female judges
practicing in the state at that time, by district.
4 Since our data were collected, the applicable rules and laws of evidence admission are substantially the same.

Criminology, CriminalJustice, Law & Society - Volume 22, Issue 1

You might also like