You are on page 1of 3

British Journal of Anaesthesia, xxx (xxx): xxx (xxxx)

doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2023.04.016
Advance Access Publication Date: xxx
Editorial

EDITORIAL

Depth of anaesthesia monitoring: time to reject the index?


Timothy J. McCulloch1,2,* and Robert D. Sanders1,2,3,4
1
Central Clinical School, Faculty of Medicine and Health, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia, 2Department of
Anaesthetics, Royal Prince Alfred Hospital, Sydney Local Health District, Sydney, NSW, Australia, 3Institute of Academic
Surgery, Royal Prince Alfred Hospital, Sydney Local Health District, Sydney, NSW, Australia and 4NHMRC Clinical Trials
Centre, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia

*Corresponding author. E-mail: Tim.McCulloch@sydney.edu.au

Summary
Depth of anaesthesia monitors can fail to detect consciousness under anaesthesia, primarily because they rely on the
frontal EEG, which does not arise from a neural correlate of consciousness. A study published in a previous issue of the
British Journal of Anaesthesia showed that indices produced by the different commercial monitors can give highly
discordant results when analysing changes in the frontal EEG. Anaesthetists could benefit from routinely assessing the
raw EEG and its spectrogram, rather than relying solely on an index produced by a depth of anaesthesia monitor.

Keywords: anaesthesia; awareness; consciousness; depth of anaesthesia index; electroencephalography; monitoring

Anaesthetists currently have access to a variety of depth of accumulating data suggest this brain region is not central for
anaesthesia (DOA) monitors that process the EEG to produce a producing consciousness itself,5 although other evidence
numerical index intended to guide dosing of anaesthetic suggests it may be important for the conscious perception of
agents. Definitive evidence that these monitors reduce the risk external sensory stimuli.6 Casey and colleagues7 recently
of awareness under anaesthesia is lacking; however, clinical showed that during anaesthesia and sleep, correlates of
experience is that DOA monitor indices usually increase on consciousness more likely reside in deeper structures, such as
emergence from anaesthesia. It is therefore likely that, in any the anterior and posterior cingulate cortex. However, given the
individual patient, failure to deliver adequate anaesthetic current state of knowledge, features of the frontal EEG asso-
agents during surgery will lead to a rise in the index, poten- ciated with loss of responsiveness during general anaesthesia
tially alerting the anaesthetist in time to prevent a serious are just thatdassociations.8 Whilst there are features of the
episode of awareness. This prima facie evidence is the basis for frontal EEG typically seen with anaesthesia-induced loss of
recommendations that DOA monitoring be used when the risk responsiveness, those features are not present in all patients
of awareness is increased, such as TIVA with a neuromuscular and are not seen with all classes of anaesthetic agents.
blocking agent1 and lower-dose inhalational anaesthesia.2 In a previous issue of the British Journal of Anaesthesia, Hight
Currently available DOA monitors are known to have and colleagues9 reported the behaviour of DOA monitors when
serious limitations, despite their apparent successful imple- processing frontal EEG recordings collected previously from a
mentation into clinical practice. For pragmatic reasons, these group of patients undergoing cardiac surgery. They first
monitors are all based on the frontal EEG, which is problem- identified episodes of apparent lightening of anaesthesia,
atic, as there is no theoretical basis for using the frontal EEG to defined on the basis of spectral analysis revealing changes
monitor for (un)consciousness.3,4 The prefrontal cortex is similar to the spectral changes often seen just before emer-
important for a conscious brain’s executive functions, but gence from general anaesthesia. There was substantial varia-
tion in the behaviour of DOA monitors when presented with
these EEG recordings. In many cases, the various monitors
DOI of original article: 10.1016/j.bja.2022.12.026.

© 2023 British Journal of Anaesthesia. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
For Permissions, please email: permissions@elsevier.com

1
2 - Editorial

produced indices that changed in opposite directions (i.e. over the EEGs of those brave volunteers who allowed themselves to
the same time period within the same patient, some indices undergo awake paralysis.
increased, whilst others decreased), and there was poor This latest finding by Hight and colleagues adds to the
concordance in whether the indices were within their rec- existing evidence that available DOA monitors are not ideal.
ommended clinical range for general anaesthesia. It is con- This is not to say these monitors are not useful. The manu-
cerning that the same EEG signal can result in such disparate facturers’ claims that there is a very low risk of awareness
output from a group of devices all purporting to monitor the with recall if their index is within the recommended range are
same phenomenon. They cannot all be right! There was no borne out by decades of clinical use during which case reports
clear winner, as none of the individual monitors displayed an of complete failure have been uncommon. Nevertheless,
increased index for every EEG episode of apparent lightening there are convincing reports of intraoperative recall of surgical
of anaesthesia. procedures when a DOA index had been falsely
The EEG features used by Hight and colleagues9 to define reassuring.17e19 Whilst these concerning events appear to be
apparent lightening of anaesthesia were an increase in the uncommon, isolated forearm studies indicate awareness
dominant frequency within the alpha band and a simulta- without recall (during otherwise acceptable levels of general
neous decrease in delta power. This pattern has been re- anaesthesia) is not uncommon,20 and DOA monitors do not
ported during emergence from propofol anaesthesia,10 and reliably detect these episodes of consciousness.8,21 It appears
increased alpha frequency has also been shown with that modern anaesthesia practice is effective at minimising
decreasing volatile anaesthetic concentration.11 Analysing the risk of awareness with recall whilst still allowing aware-
the EEG spectrum using the methods of Hight and colleagues9 ness without recall. If this is considered a problem for our pa-
has not been extensively validated as a method for detecting tients, it is difficult to envisage how the problem will be
inadequate anaesthetic dosing. In a previous paper, based on overcome until monitors can be developed that more directly
the same set of EEG recordings, these authors cautiously respond to consciousness itself.
stated that these spectrogram patterns, ‘ … might sometimes It is remarkable that despite widespread clinical use, the
represent inadequate hypnotic effect of anaesthesia’.12 data behind the decisions to designate a clinical range (40e60
Therefore, it cannot be concluded with any certainty that a in the case of BIS) remain unclear. In the seminal study by
DOA monitor whose index did not rise was necessarily failing Glass and colleagues,22 the effective bispectral index for 95% of
to detect inadequate anaesthesia. In 12 of the 52 cases in the the population to be unresponsive to verbal command was 51
current study, all five DOA monitors produced an index (95% confidence interval [CI]: 47e52), whilst for explicit recall it
designated as either within the clinical range or deeper than was 64 (95% CI: 52e69). Neither of these results provide con-
the clinical range. This result could be interpreted as showing fidence in an upper limit of 60. Based on a case in the seminal
that all five monitors failed to detect inadequate anaesthesia, B-Aware trial, the authors suggested that an upper limit of
but the alternative must also be considered that at least some bispectral index monitoring of 55 be considered.23 Given the
of those events were not actually episodes of inadequate challenges from data provided by studies of intraoperative
anaesthesia. isolated forearm monitoring, awake but paralysed volunteers,
Another caveat when interpreting the results of Hight and and the B-Aware study, is it time to reject these indices or at
colleagues9 is that failure of the monitors to agree on whether least reconsider the relevant range?
the EEG should be scored as outside the recommended clinical We agree with the recommendations from professional
range for anaesthesia is not necessarily a significant discor- bodies that DOA monitoring be used when there is a presumed
dance. For example, if one monitor’s index is a single point increased risk of awareness.12 However, clinicians do need to
above the limit for the recommended clinical range and an- be aware that these monitors are not completely reliable, and
other’s index is a single point below, this could arguably be it is possible for patients to be aware despite a reassuring in-
considered a trivial disagreement. dex. We consider it critical that the index is interpreted in the
The EEG records used by Hight and colleagues had been context of the raw electroencephalogram and spectrogram,
recorded digitally for a previous study.12 These recordings although we acknowledge that trials showing this is a superior
were converted back to an analogue electrical signal and then approach are lacking. Anaesthetists do not use HR alone as a
fed to the DOA monitors being tested. A similar methodology metric of cardiac function, and the DOA index alone is not
was first reported by Eagleman and colleagues,13 who devised adequately informative. We concur with the suggestion of
a ‘playback’ system to test the performance of DOA monitors Hight and colleagues9 that clinicians familiarise themselves
using EEG recordings from elderly patients during induction, with the spectrogram signatures that frequently accompany
maintenance, and emergence from general anaesthesia. In emergence from anaesthesia. Unfortunately, not all monitors
contrast to the results of Hight and colleagues, Eagleman and currently display a raw EEG spectrogram.
colleagues13 found that the DOA monitors all performed Beyond the EEG spectrogram, there are other lines of
similarly well, despite the fact that elderly patients frequently enquiry that could lead to advances in EEG analysis to help
do not exhibit typical EEG spectrograph patterns.14 with titration of anaesthetics. Recent advances include the use
Storing EEGs and playing them back to DOA monitoring of deep learning algorithms to classify EEG correlates of
algorithms is an intriguing technique that has potential for different anaesthetic states,24 better phenotyping of general
wider application. We could significantly increase the knowl- anaesthesia to understand the neurophysiology underlying
edge gained from studies on EEG and DOA by routinely storing different states of consciousness and incorporating better
and sharing digital EEG recordings. For example, two studies spatial resolution of the surface EEG,7 and incorporating
have demonstrated a spectacular failure of the BIS® monitor evoked responses into EEG analysis.24,25 However, none of
to detect consciousness in awake volunteers who were given these technologies are, at present, ready for clinical use.
neuromuscular blocking agents.15,16 Whether the other DOA As a final caveat, any information derived from the frontal
monitors would also fail under these experimental conditions EEG has the inherent limitation that the signal might not arise
is not known, and regrettably, there are no digital recordings of from a true neural correlate of consciousness. Nonetheless,
Editorial - 3

the frontal EEG is what we have available at present, and it 11. Hight D, Voss LJ, Garcia PS, Sleigh J. Changes in alpha
behoves us to maximise its utility by interpreting all the frequency and power of the electroencephalogram during
available informationdnot just the index. volatile-based general anesthesia. Front Syst Neurosci 2017;
11: 36
12. Kaiser HA, Peus M, Luedi MM, et al. Frontal electroen-
Authors’ contribution cephalogram reveals emergence-like brain activity
Concept: RDS, TJM occurring during transition periods in cardiac surgery. Br J
Writing of the paper: TJM, RDS Anaesth 2020; 125: 291e7
13. Eagleman SL, Drover CM, Li X, MacIver MB, Drover DR.
Offline comparison of processed electroencephalogram
Declarations of interest monitors for anaesthetic-induced electroencephalogram
changes in older adults. Br J Anaesth 2021; 126: 975e84
RDS is an editor of the British Journal of Anaesthesia. TJM has no
14. Purdon PL, Pavone KJ, Akeju O, et al. The ageing brain:
conflicts to declare.
age-dependent changes in the electroencephalogram
during propofol and sevoflurane general anaesthesia. Br J
Anaesth 2015; 115: i46e57
References
15. Messner M, Beese U, Romsto € ck J, Dinkel M,
1. Checketts MR, Alladi R, Ferguson K, et al. Recommenda- Tschaikowsky K. The bispectral index declines during
tions for standards of monitoring during anaesthesia and neuromuscular block in fully awake persons. Anesth Analg
recovery. Anaesthesia 2016; 71: 85e93 2003; 97: 488e91
2. APSF Committee on Technology. APSF-endorsed statement 16. Schuller PJ, Newell S, Strickland PA, Barry JJ. Response of
on revising recommendations for patient monitoring during bispectral index to neuromuscular block in awake volun-
anesthesia 2022. Available from: https://www.apsf.org/wp- teers. Br J Anaesth 2015; 115: i95e103
content/uploads/newsletters/2022/3701/APSF3701-2022- 17. Vassiliadis M, Geros D, Maria K. Awareness despite low
02-a03-StatementPatientMonitoring.pdf. [Accessed 20 spectral entropy values. Anesth Analg 2007; 105: 535
March 2023] 18. Rampersad SE, Mulroy MF. A case of awareness despite an
3. Sanders RD, Gaskell A, Sleigh J. Can use of frontal EEG ‘adequate depth of anesthesia’ as indicated by a bispectral
monitoring increase intraoperative connected conscious- index monitor. Anesth Analg 2005; 100: 1363e4
ness? Br J Anaesth 2018; 121: 191e3 19. Avidan MS, Jacobsohn E, Glick D, et al. Prevention of
4. Sanders RD, Mostert N, Lindroth H, Tononi G, Sleigh J. Is intraoperative awareness in a high-risk surgical popula-
consciousness frontal? Two perioperative case reports tion. N Engl J Med 2011; 365: 591e600
that challenge that concept. Br J Anaesth 2018; 121: 330e2 20. Sanders RD, Tononi G, Laureys S, Sleigh JW. Unrespon-
5. Koch C, Massimini M, Boly M, Tononi G. Neural correlates siveness s unconsciousness. Anesthesiology 2012; 116:
of consciousness: progress and problems. Nat Rev Neurosci 946e59
2016; 17: 307e21 21. Sanders RD, Gaskell A, Raz A, et al. Incidence of connected
6. Sergent C, Corazzol M, Labouret G, et al. Bifurcation in consciousness after tracheal intubation: a prospective,
brain dynamics reveals a signature of conscious process- international, multicenter cohort study of the isolated
ing independent of report. Nat Commun 2021; 12: 1149 forearm technique. Anesthesiology 2017; 126: 214e22
7. Casey CP, Tanabe S, Farahbakhsh Z, et al. Distinct EEG 22. Glass PS, Bloom M, Kearse L, Rosow C, Sebel P, Manberg P.
signatures differentiate unconsciousness and disconnec- Bispectral analysis measures sedation and memory ef-
tion during anaesthesia and sleep. Br J Anaesth 2022; 128: fects of propofol, midazolam, isoflurane, and alfentanil in
1006e18 healthy volunteers. Anesthesiology 1997; 86: 836e47
8. Gaskell AL, Hight DF, Winders J, et al. Frontal alpha-delta 23. Myles PS, Leslie K, Mcneil J, Forbes A, Chan MTV. Bispec-
EEG does not preclude volitional response during anaes- tral index monitoring to prevent awareness during
thesia: prospective cohort study of the isolated forearm anaesthesia: the B-Aware randomised controlled trial.
technique. Br J Anaesth 2017; 119: 664e73 Lancet 2004; 363: 1757e63
9. Hight D, Kreuzer M, Ugen G, et al. Five commercial ‘depth 24. Lee M, Sanz LRD, Barra A, et al. Quantifying arousal and
of anaesthesia’ monitors provide discordant clinical rec- awareness in altered states of consciousness using inter-
ommendations in response to identical emergence-like pretable deep learning. Nat Commun 2022; 13: 1064
EEG signals. Br J Anaesth 2023; 130: 536e45 25. Casey CP, Tanabe S, Farahbakhsh Z, et al. Dynamic causal
10. Purdon PL, Pierce ET, Mukamel EA, et al. Electroencepha- modelling of auditory surprise during disconnected con-
logram signatures of loss and recovery of consciousness sciousness: the role of feedback connectivity. Neuroimage
from propofol. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2013; 110: E1142e51 2022; 263, 119657

You might also like