You are on page 1of 11

REVIEW

Universal Newborn Hearing Screening


Summary of Evidence
Diane C. Thompson, MS Context Each year, approximately 5000 infants are born in the United States with
Heather McPhillips, MD, MPH moderate-to-profound, bilateral permanent hearing loss (PHL). Universal newborn hear-
ing screening (UNHS) has been proposed as a means to speed diagnosis and treat-
Robert L. Davis, MD, MPH ment and thereby improve language outcomes in these children.
Tracy A. Lieu, MD, MPH Objectives To identify strengths, weaknesses, and gaps in the evidence supporting
Charles J. Homer, MD, MPH UNHS and to compare the additional benefits and harms of UNHS with those of se-
lective screening of high-risk newborns.
Mark Helfand, MD, MS
Data Sources We searched the MEDLINE, CINAHL, and PsychINFO databases for rel-

E
ACH YEAR APPROXIMATELY 5000 evant articles published from 1994 to August 2001, using terms for hearing disorders,
infants are born in the United infants or newborns, screening, and relevant treatments. We contacted experts and re-
States with moderate-to- viewed reference lists to identify additional articles, including those published before 1994.
profound, bilateral permanent Study Selection We included controlled and observational studies of (1) the accu-
hearing loss (PHL). Estimates of the racy, yield, and harms of screening using otoacoustic emissions (OAEs), auditory brain-
prevalence of moderate, severe, and pro- stem response (ABR), or both in the general newborn population and (2) the effects of
screening or early identification and treatment on language outcomes. Of an original 340
found congenital PHL among new-
articles identified, 19 articles, including 1 controlled trial, met these inclusion criteria.
borns range from 1 in 900 to 1 in 2500.1-8
Moderate, severe, and profound congen- Data Extraction Data on population, test performance, outcomes, and method-
ological quality were extracted by 2 authors (D.C.T., H.M.) using prespecified criteria
ital PHL is associated with delayed lan-
developed by the US Preventive Services Task Force. We queried authors when in-
guage, learning, and speech develop- formation needed to assess study quality was missing.
ment.9-13 This delay is measurable before
age 3 years14,15 and has consequences Data Synthesis Good-quality studies show that from 2041 to 2794 low-risk and
86 to 208 high-risk newborns were screened to find 1 case of moderate-to-profound
throughout life. On average, deaf stu- PHL. The best estimate of positive predictive value was 6.7%. Six percent to 15% of
dents graduate from high school with infants who are missed by the screening tests are subsequently diagnosed with bilat-
language and academic achievement lev- eral PHL. In a trial of UNHS vs clinical screening at age 8 months, UNHS increased the
els below those of fourth-grade stu- proportion of infants with moderate-to-severe hearing loss diagnosed by age 10 months
dents with normal hearing.16,17 (57% vs 14%) but did not reduce the rate of diagnosis after age 18 months. No good-
Diagnosis and treatment are often de- quality controlled study has compared UNHS with selective screening of high-risk new-
layed until ages 1 or 2 years in children borns. In fair- to poor-quality cohort studies, intervention before age 6 months was
with congenital PHL, particularly among associated with improved language and communication skills by ages 2 to 5 years.
These studies had unclear criteria for selecting subjects, and none compared an in-
those at low risk for PHL.18-22 Current
ception cohort of low-risk newborns identified by screening with those identified in
theory holds that auditory stimuli dur- usual care, making it impossible to exclude selection bias as an explanation for the
ing the first 6 months of life are critical results. In a mathematical model based on the literature review, we estimated that
to the development of speech and lan- extending screening to low-risk infants would detect 1 additional case before age 10
guage skills.23-25 Advocates of universal months for every 1441 low-risk infants screened, and result in treatment before 10
newborn hearing screening (UNHS) be- months of 1 additional case for every 2401 low-risk infants screened. With UNHS,
lieve that earlier application of avail- 254 newborns would be referred for audiological evaluation because of false-positive
able therapies, such as speech and lan- second-stage screening test results vs 48 for selective screening.
guage therapy, amplification, and family Conclusions Modern screening tests for hearing impairment can improve identifi-
support, could reduce or eliminate the cation of newborns with PHL, but the efficacy of UNHS to improve long-term lan-
gap in language skills between deaf and guage outcomes remains uncertain.
hearing children.26,27 Screening also iden- JAMA. 2001;286:2000-2010 www.jama.com
tifies infants who have mild and unilat-
Author Affiliations are listed at the end of this Thompson, MS, Harborview Injury Prevention and Re-
eral hearing impairment, but the conse- article. search Center, 325 Ninth Ave, Box 359960, Seattle,
quences of delay are not well established Corresponding Author and Reprints: Diane C. WA 98104 (e-mail: dct@u.washington.edu).

2000 JAMA, October 24/31, 2001—Vol 286, No. 16 (Reprinted with Corrections) ©2001 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://jama.jamanetwork.com/ by a UNIVERSITAETSBIBLIOTHEK GIESSEN User on 05/13/2015


NEWBORN HEARING SCREENING

in these infants, and most are not can- ematical model of the benefits and versal screening programs,26,43-51 1 study
didates for hearing aids and other thera- harms of UNHS. of the accuracy of OAEs and ABR in high-
pies associated with early identifica- risk infants,52 and 8 studies of language
tion.7,20,28,29 Search Strategy outcomes14,15,53-58 met these inclusion cri-
Selective screening of high-risk new- We searched MEDLINE, CINAHL, and teria. An additional 110 articles pro-
borns is an alternative to UNHS. Among PsychINFO for relevant articles pub- vided supplemental data about the in-
infants in a neonatal intensive care unit lished in English from 1994 to Septem- cluded screening studies, results of
(NICU), the risk of moderate-to-severe ber 2000, using the keywords hearing selective screening programs, and other
PHL is 10 to 20 times higher than in the disorders and infant or newborn com- background information.
general population.30 In addition to bined with terms for screening and rel- Two authors abstracted data for
NICU admission, the Joint Committee evant treatments, such as early inter- population, test performance, out-
on Infant Hearing high risk guidelines vention, amplification, and American Sign comes, and methodological quality from
specify 4 other risk factors (BOX).31,32 Language. The search was updated each included study. We classified each
From 10% to 30% of newborns meet quarterly through August 2001. We also study as “good,” “fair,” or “poor” us-
these criteria, which can identify 50% to examined reference lists of review ar- ing prespecified criteria developed by
75% of all cases of moderate-to- ticles7,35-42 and queried experts. To iden-
profound bilateral hearing loss.2 tify articles published before 1994, we
In 1995 the US Preventive Services relied on systematic reviews pub- Box. Risk Factors for
Task Force (USPSTF) found insuffi- lished in 1996 and 1997.20,33 Sensorineural Hearing Loss
cient evidence to recommend UNHS,33 in Newborns
based on the low prevalence of PHL and Study Selection, Data Abstraction, 1. Neonatal intensive care unit ad-
the risk for substantial misclassifica- Validity Assessment, and Synthesis mission for 2 or more days
tion. While the evidence for the effi- Two authors reviewed titles and ab- 2. Usher syndrome, Waardenburg
syndrome, or findings associ-
cacy of early intervention for patients stracts of 864 articles from the original
ated with other syndromes known
diagnosed by screening was incom- searches and selected 340 articles as pos- to include hearing loss
plete, the Task Force endorsed selec- sibly relevant to 1 of the key questions. 3. Family history of hereditary child-
tive screening of high-risk newborns be- We then selected the following to in- hood sensorineural hearing loss
cause of their higher prevalence of PHL. clude in evidence tables: (1) controlled 4. Congenital infections such as
Since 1995, many health care pro- trials; (2) reports on the accuracy, yield, toxoplasmosis, bacterial menin-
fessionals and federal health care or harms of screening using otoacous- gitis, syphilis, rubella, cytomega-
agencies have advocated for UNHS, tic emissions (OAEs) or auditory brain- lovirus, and herpes
which is now mandated by law in 32 stem response (ABR); or (3) reports of 5. Craniofacial anomalies, includ-
states.18,31,34 Is this widespread sup- the effects of screening, or of early iden- ing morphologic abnormalities of
port for UNHS now justified? To up- tification and treatment, on language out- the pinna and ear canal
date the USPSTF recommendations, we comes. Ten studies of the yield of uni-
critically reviewed recent evidence to
identify strengths, weaknesses, and gaps
in the evidence supporting UNHS. Figure. Newborn Hearing Screening Analytic Framework

METHODS Can UNHS accurately diagnose moderate-to-


profound sensorineural hearing impairment?
We focused our literature search on key In UNHS programs, how many children are Does identification and treatment prior to age
identified and treated early? 6 months improve language and communication?
questions underlying the clinical logic
of screening for hearing impairment in
Earlier Diagnosis Improved Mental
newborns (F IGURE ). The logic as- High-Risk ?
and Treatment of
? Improved
Language and Health, Psychosocial
and Low-Risk SCREEN EARLY
sumes that screening tests are accu- Newborns Permanent, Bilateral, INTERVENTION Communication and Cognitive Function,
Moderate-to-Profound (Preschool Age) School and Occupational
rate; that screening reduces delays in Hearing Loss Performance
diagnosis and treatment; that earlier ? ? Throughout Life

treatment results in better language What are the


potential adverse
function within the preschool period; effects of
and that improvement in early lan- Adverse Effects of screening and of
Screening (Labeling, early treatment? Adverse Effects of
guage function will improve educa- Anxiety, Overdiagnosis) Early Treatment
tional, occupational, and social func-
tion later in life. We reviewed the Early intervention indicates hearing aids or other amplification, American Sign Language and/or English in-
literature about each assumption, and struction, speech and language therapy, and family education and support. UNHS indicates universal new-
born hearing screening.
used the results to construct a math-
©2001 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. (Reprinted with Corrections) JAMA, October 24/31, 2001—Vol 286, No. 16 2001

Downloaded From: http://jama.jamanetwork.com/ by a UNIVERSITAETSBIBLIOTHEK GIESSEN User on 05/13/2015


NEWBORN HEARING SCREENING

the USPSTF for grading the internal va- estimating prevalence, sensitivity and 5 hospital-based programs, provided in-
lidity of studies.59 When necessary, we specificity, compliance, treatment effect formation about the yield of UNHS in
sought from authors additional infor- size, and other model parameters from actual screening programs (TABLE 1).
mation needed to apply the criteria. The the included studies. Excel 97 (Micro- In the 2 good-quality studies, screen-
entire 13-member Task Force dis- soft Corp, Redmond, Wash) was used ing detected 1 case of bilateral, mod-
cussed the review, examined and rated for all analyses. erate-to-profound PHL for every 925 to
the quality of 4 key studies of early in- 1422 newborns screened. The yield was
tervention,54,58,56,57 and provided over- RESULTS 2041 to 2794 low-risk and 86 to 208
all guidance. Can UNHS Accurately Diagnose high-risk newborns screened to find 1
We constructed a mathematical Moderate-to-Profound case.29,43,44,61 Fair-quality and poor-
model of the likely benefits and harms Sensorineural Hearing Impairment? quality studies had higher yields due to
of UNHS vs selective screening of a hy- Ten publications, including 1 con- inclusion of infants who had mild, uni-
pothetical cohort of 10 000 newborns, trolled trial, 4 state-based programs, and lateral, or unconfirmed hearing loss.

Table 1. Studies of Universal Newborn Hearing Screening*


No. Screened/ Yield (NNS)
Description (Quality No. Available to Identify 1 Case Positive
Study Rating) Screening Tests (% Screened) of Bilateral PHL Screen, No. (%)
Wessex UNHS Trial Controlled, TEOAE followed by ABR 21 279/25 609 (83) 23/21 279 (925) 342 (1.6)
Group,43 1998 nonrandomized trial at
4 hospitals from
10/93 to 10/96 (good)
Prieve et al,60 2000 Statewide demonstration TEOAE followed by 69 766/71 922 (97) 49/69 736 (1422) 4699 (6.5) (stage 1)
project at 7 perinatal TEOAE or ABR at
centers, 8 hospitals in birth admission;
New York (good) TEOAE, ABR at 4-6
wk (stage 2)
Vohr et al,26 1998 Cohort from 8 maternity TEOAE followed by ABR 52 659/53 121 (99) 79/52 659 (666)‡ 5397 (10.2) (stage 1);
hospitals in Rhode (high-risk infants); 677 (1.3) (stage 2)
Island from 1/93 to TEOAE and ABR in
12/96 (fair) 2-6 wk (low-risk)
Finitzo et al,45 1998 Cohort from 9 Texas ABR or TEOAE at birth 52 508/54 228 (97) 20/17 105 (855)‡㛳 1787 (3.4)
hospitals from 1/94 to admission followed
6/97 (fair) by either ABR or
TEOAE at 1-8 wk
Barsky-Firsker and Hospital-based series at ABR by audiologists 15 749/16 229 (97) NR 485 (3.1)
Sun,47 1997 St Barnabas Medical (stage 1)
Center, New Jersey,
from 1/93 to 12/95
(fair)
Watkin,48 1996 Hospital-based series at TEOAE followed by 11 606/14 353 (81) 19/11 606 (755) 337 (2.9)
Whipps Cross TEOAE and ABR
Hospital, England (fair) within 4 wk
Mehl and Cohort from 26 Colorado 19 ABR, 1 TEOAE, 6 41 796 (NR) NR 2709 (6.5)
Thompson,46 hospitals from 1992 ABR (follow-up
1998 to 1996 (poor) screen not reported)
Aidan et al,49 1999 Hospital-based series in TEOAE in 48 h; TEOAE 1421/1727 (82) 2/1421 (711) 238 (16.7)
Paris, France, of within 4 wk
infants in normal
newborn nursery
(poor)
Clemens et al,50 2000 Hospital-based series at ABR followed by retest 5010/5034 (99.5) NR 103/5054
Women’s Hospital of for failure (stage 1a)
Greensboro, NC, from or ABR (stage 1b);
7/98 to 6/99 (poor) outpatient ABR and
diagnostic ABR
(stage 2)
Mason and Series of infants born at ABR 10 372/10 773 (96) 12/10 372 (864) 415 (4.0)
Herrmann,51 1998 Kaiser, Honolulu,
Hawaii, from 3/92 to
2/97 (poor)
*NNS indicates number needed to screen; PHL, permanent hearing loss; UNHS, universal newborn hearing screening; TEOAE, transient evoked otoacoustic emissions; ABR,
automated auditory brainstem response; NR, not reported; NIH, National Institutes of Health; NICU, neonatal intensive care unit; and FH, family history.
†Reported different rates for misses and fails.
‡Includes mild, bilateral hearing loss.
§Includes unilateral hearing loss.
㛳Data reported for 1996 only.

2002 JAMA, October 24/31, 2001—Vol 286, No. 16 (Reprinted with Corrections) ©2001 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://jama.jamanetwork.com/ by a UNIVERSITAETSBIBLIOTHEK GIESSEN User on 05/13/2015


NEWBORN HEARING SCREENING

Most programs in Table 1 used a screening test was 6.7%.43 In the well- A behavioral test is the appropriate
2-stage screening protocol, in which an baby nursery, the PPV was 2.2%, mean- gold standard determination for perma-
infant who fails the initial test (OAE or ing that 1 of every 45 infants referred for nent hearing impairment,62 but cannot
ABR) is retested in the hospital or as an outpatient audiological evaluation even- be performed reliably before 8 to 9
outpatient within 12 weeks of dis- tually proved to have moderate-to- months of age.63,64 One large, good-
charge, and is referred for audiological profound bilateral PHL. None of the quality study of test performance mea-
evaluation if he or she fails the second screening programs measured the sen- sured the sensitivity and specificity of
test. Among infants with positive screen- sitivity and specificity of neonatal screen- OAE and ABR using an independent gold
ing tests, the likelihood that the infant ing against an independent gold stan- standard, visual reinforcement audiom-
has hearing loss (ie, the positive predic- dard, although 3 reported the percentage etry, performed at ages 8 to 12 months.52
tive value [PPV] of the screening test) of cases (6%-15%) missed by screening The study found that these screening tests
varied by study. In 1 good-quality study, but eventually diagnosed by other are not sensitive enough to rule out sig-
the overall PPV for the second-stage means.26,28,43 nificant hearing loss. The sensitivity of
OAE ranged from 80% for moderate hear-
ing loss to 98% for profound hearing loss.
No. Low-Risk No. High-Risk
For ABR, sensitivity and specificity were
No. Followed Up Definition of Identified/No. Identified/No. 84% and 90%, respectively. The 2-stage
(% Lost to Follow-up) High Risk Screened (NNS) Screened (NNS) protocol missed 11% of affected ears.
NR (NR) NIH criteria 7/19 555 (2794) 20/1724 (86) Overall, neonatal testing resulted in a final
diagnosis of bilateral moderate-to-
profound PHL among 1 in 230 high-
† (43.4) NICU infants 33/NR (2041)‡ 52/NR (208)‡ risk and 1 in 2348 low-risk infants.
These estimates of accuracy and yield
are lower than expected, but are prob-
ably more reliable than those from ac-
4575 (15.2) NICU infants 61/47 529 (779)§ 50/5130 (103)§
tual screening programs (Table 1). In
those programs, decisions about diag-
nosis and treatment are made on the ba-
1224 (31.5) NR NR NR
sis of a diagnostic ABR performed when
the infant is 1 to 6 months old. The use
of this intermediate diagnostic stan-
NR (NR) NICU infants 29/14 014 (483) 23/1735 (75) dard facilitates earlier intervention, but
may overestimate the number of cases of
PHL. In the Wessex trial, the first audio-
logical examination was done when the
290 (14) Risk factors 7 (NR) 13 (NR)
and/or NICU infants infants were between ages 8 and 12
weeks. Of 158 infants who screened posi-
1296 (52.2) NR NR NR tive, 27 were diagnosed as having PHL;
in 2 of these cases (7.4%), however, the
123 (48.3) Hypoxemia, 2/1421 (711) NR diagnosis was wrong, and the infants had
hyperbilirubinemia, normal hearing when reexamined at 4
FH
months or 10 months of age.61 In an-
other study, 5 (29%) of 17 infants ini-
85 (17.5) NICU infants NR 4/454 (114) tially diagnosed to have moderate PHL
were later found to have only mild hear-
ing loss.28 No other studies listed in Table
1 described follow-up procedures to de-
362 (12.8) NICU infants 5/8971 (1794) 7/1401 (200)
termine how often the intermediate di-
agnosis of PHL was incorrect.

In UNHS Programs, How Many


Children Are Identified
and Treated Early?
The most important indicators of the
benefit of UNHS are the number of ad-
©2001 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. (Reprinted with Corrections) JAMA, October 24/31, 2001—Vol 286, No. 16 2003

Downloaded From: http://jama.jamanetwork.com/ by a UNIVERSITAETSBIBLIOTHEK GIESSEN User on 05/13/2015


NEWBORN HEARING SCREENING

ditional cases of significant hearing im- low-risk infants screened, 1 addi- 56% of the screened group compared
pairment that are diagnosed and treated tional case of PHL was diagnosed be- with 24% of the unscreened group.
early.18,29 In a British trial comparing fore age 10 months. While this study used relevant, vali-
UNHS with no newborn screening, dated measures of language outcomes
UNHS increased rates of referral to an Does Identification and Treatment and controlled for several important po-
audiologist by age 6 months for in- Prior to Age 6 Months Improve tential confounders, study design is-
fants with moderate-to-profound PHL Language and Communication? sues limited the conclusions that could
(an increase of 51 per 100 000 infants; No prospective, controlled study di- be drawn. Eligibility for the screened
95% confidence interval, 7.4-94.0 per rectly examined whether newborn hear- group was determined by the availabil-
100 000; P = .03), but not confirma- ing screening results in improved ity of an assessment of language out-
tion of diagnosis (P = .22) or initiation speech, language, or educational de- comes at ages 2 to 4 years. Because the
of management within 10 months velopment. None of the state-based pro- groups were drawn from different hos-
(P = .08).43 Among those infants with grams described in Table 1 reported the pitals and time periods, factors other
moderate or severe hearing loss, how- outcomes of treatment for infants iden- than exposure to UNHS might have in-
ever, screening led to highly signifi- tified as having hearing impairment. fluenced outcomes. Selection of sub-
cant increases in confirmation and man- TABLE 2 summarizes methodologi- jects and assessment of outcome were
agement by age 10 months. With cal aspects and results of 8 recent co- unblinded, and neither the number of
UNHS, 13 of 23 (57%) children with hort studies from 3 intervention pro- excluded subjects, nor the reasons for
moderate or severe impairment were di- grams that compared early- and late- exclusion, are reported.
agnosed by 10 months, compared with identified children with impaired The most widely cited CHIP study
2 of 13 (14%) without UNHS.43 Use of hearing. All of these studies used stan- compared 72 hearing-impaired chil-
UNHS did not reduce the rate of diag- dardized receptive and expressive tests dren identified prior to age 6 months
nosis after 18 months, either overall to evaluate speech and language skills with 78 hearing-impaired children iden-
(5/27 for UNHS vs 6/26 for the con- in preschool children,69,70 and all re- tified after 6 months.54 After adjust-
trol group) or in the moderate-to- ported statistically significant associa- ment for cognitive function, children
severe subgroup. tions between the age at the time of di- identified before age 6 months had lan-
In the best-quality US study,29 ages agnosis and language development at guage scores at or near their cognitive
of diagnosis for mild-to-moderate hear- ages 2 to 5 years. test scores, whereas children identi-
ing losses were 3.5 months (mean) and Six of these 8 studies reported speech fied after age 6 months performed, on
2.5 months (median), and those for se- and language results for children en- average, 20 points lower on language
vere PHL were 6.3 months (mean) and rolled in the Colorado Home Interven- scores than on cognitive scores. Chil-
3.8 months (median). In about 40% of tion Program (CHIP).14,15,53-56,71,72 One dren with low cognitive abilities (cog-
cases, the diagnosis of PHL was de- of these compared language perfor- nitive quotient ⬍80) experienced a
layed until ages 1 to 2 years because of mance of hearing-impaired children smaller improvement in total lan-
infant illness, developmental delays, born in hospitals with UNHS pro- guage, but no statistically significant im-
noncompliant parents,29 or transient grams to that for children born in hos- provement in receptive and expres-
conductive hearing losses.28,52 pitals with no UNHS program (Table sive language abilities.
How much of the overall effect of 2).55 It found that mean scores for ex- This study had limitations as well.
UNHS can be attributed to screening pressive, receptive, and total language Late-identified children were more likely
low-risk infants? Uncertainty about the were within normal ranges for the to be cognitively impaired, to have se-
proficiency of selective screening makes screened group and 18 to 21 points vere or worse hearing loss, to use sign
it difficult to determine how much higher (P⬍.001) than for the un- language, and to have mothers with
UNHS can add. The most frequently screened group. A 20-point gap is more lower educational achievement. The sta-
cited studies of selective screening were than 1 SD lower than normal for age, tistical method used in the analysis did
performed over 10 years ago,65-68 and which would indicate that a child with not simultaneously adjust for more than
they did not report the number of in- average intellect would have the lan- 2 factors and may not have removed the
fants diagnosed before ages 6 or 10 guage abilities of a child who had an IQ influence of these differences. Addition-
months. In recent good-quality and fair- of 80. Children identified as having ally, the study did not provide data on
quality studies of UNHS, 19% to 42% hearing impairment prior to age 6 dropout rates in the 2 groups, and out-
of infants with PHL had no risk fac- months (whether in the screened or un- come assessments were not masked.
tors.29,43,51,67 Only 1 study reported re- screened group) had a smaller gap be- All of the studies in Table 2 had sev-
sults in sufficient detail to calculate how tween language development and cog- eral important limitations. The study
often low-risk infants with bilateral nitive ability than children identified populations were composed of conve-
moderate-to-profound PHL are diag- after 6 months. Language develop- nience samples. None of the studies had
nosed early by UNHS. For every 7692 ment was within the normal range for clear criteria for inclusion, none had
2004 JAMA, October 24/31, 2001—Vol 286, No. 16 (Reprinted with Corrections) ©2001 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://jama.jamanetwork.com/ by a UNIVERSITAETSBIBLIOTHEK GIESSEN User on 05/13/2015


NEWBORN HEARING SCREENING

Table 2. Cohort Studies Reporting Language Outcomes*


Comparability and
Maintenance of Early vs Late Adjustment for
Study (Quality) Selection of Subjects Groups Confounders Results
Studies From Colorado Home Intervention Program (CHIP)
Apuzzo and Convenience sample of 69 Late-identified group was more One-way ANOVA did not At age 40 mo, infants identified
Yoshinaga- high-risk infants diagnosed likely to have adjust for SES, family before age 2 mo had higher
Itano,53 1995 between ages 2 and 25 mo. severe-to-profound hearing involvement, or other mean MCDI scores for
(fair) Those with severe cognitive loss (65% vs 50%). No potential confounders. expressive language (P⬍.01).
delay were excluded. report of attrition or
follow-up rates.
Yoshinaga-Itano Convenience sample of 40 Late-identified group was more Child’s sex, severity of At age 40 mo, infants identified
and Apuzzo,14 high-risk infants, divided into likely to have hearing loss, cognitive before age 6 mo had better
1998 (poor) those identified and treated severe-to-profound hearing function, and other adjusted mean MCDI scores
before age 6 mo (n = 15) and loss (52% vs 47%). No disabilities examined in for expressive language (81.1
those treated after age 18 report of attrition or 2-way ANCOVAs, not vs 64.3, P⬍.05) and
mo (n = 25). Those with follow-up rates. multiple regression (no receptive language (84.4 vs
severe cognitive delay were simultaneous adjustment 70.1, P⬍.05).
excluded (DQ ⬍60). for multiple confounders).
Yoshinaga-Itano Convenience sample of 82 Late-identified group was more Child’s sex, severity of At age 26 mo, infants identified
and Apuzzo,15 infants, ages 19 to 36 mo, likely to have hearing loss, cognitive before age 6 mo had better
1998 (poor) with mild-to-profound PHL, severe-to-profound hearing function, and other adjusted mean MCDI scores
divided into those identified loss (77% vs 42%). No disabilities examined in for expressive language (76.2
before age 6 mo (n = 34) and report of attrition or 2-way ANCOVAs, not vs 56.6, P = .001), receptive
between ages 7 and 18 mo follow-up rates. multiple regression (no language (82.1 vs 58.3,
(n = 48). Early group simultaneous adjustment P = .002), MacArthur CDI
identified by high-risk registry; for multiple confounders). adjusted mean receptive
late group by usual care. vocabulary (200 vs 86.4,
Those with severe cognitive P⬍.001), and expressive
delay were excluded (DQ vocabulary (117 vs 54,
⬍60). P⬍.03).
Yoshinaga-Itano Convenience sample of 150 At baseline, compared groups There was stratification by At ages 13 to 36 mo, adjusted
et al,54 1998 children ages 13 to 36 mo differed in some CQ (⬍80 vs ⱖ80). Other mean MCDI receptive LQ
(poor) with mild-to-profound PHL, demographic covariates (sex, minority was higher for those
divided into those identified characteristics and in the status, maternal identified before age 6 mo
before (n = 72) or after proportion of subjects with education level, Medicaid (79.6 vs 64.6, P⬍.001). Mean
(n = 78) age 6 mo. The cognitive impairment and status, severity, mode of MCDI expressive LQ was
number of low-risk infants severe-to-profound hearing communication, other higher (78.3 vs 63.1, P⬍.001)
and the role of UNHS in loss (CQ ⬍80, 29% early disabilities) were and total language (79 vs 64,
identifying subjects are not group vs 56% late group; examined singly in 2-way P⬍.001) was higher in early
described. Selection bias is severe-to-profound hearing ANCOVAs. identified group. No
likely because the design loss, 34% early group vs differences in LQ among 4
probably excluded infants 46% late group). No report age-of-identification levels in
who were diagnosed to have of attrition or follow-up late-identified group.
hearing loss but did not enter rates.
the program, or who entered,
but were lost to follow-up.
Yoshinaga-Itano Children born in a hospital with a The exposure was birth at a Pairs matched on age of Mean (SE) scores for expressive,
et al,55 2000 UNHS program in effect at hospital with a UNHS testing (9-59 mo), degree receptive, and total language
(poor) time of birth (n = 25) were program, not age of of hearing loss (mild, were within normal range for
compared with children born identification. Because the moderate, moderately the screened group and 18
in a hospital without a UNHS groups were drawn from severe, profound), and to 21 points higher (P⬍.001)
program (n = 25). All subjects different hospitals and time CQ. than the unscreened group
had been enrolled in the periods, factors other than (expressive language, 82.9
CHIP program. Eligibility for exposure to UNHS might [3.7] vs 62.1 [4.3]; receptive
the screened group was have influenced outcomes. language, 81.5 [3.7] vs 66.8
determined by the availability Selection of subjects and [4.0]; total language, 82.2
of an assessment of assessment of outcome [3.3] vs 64.4 [3.9]). Language
language outcomes. The were unblinded, and neither development was within
creation of the study groups the number of excluded normal range for 56% of the
and description of the subjects nor the reasons for screened group compared
patients limited the exclusion are reported. with 24% of the unscreened
conclusions that could be group.
drawn.
Mayne et al,56 Convenience sample of 113 Demographic comparisons of Regression analysis adjusted At ages 24 to 36 mo, age at
2000 (poor) children ages 24 to 73 mo, the groups were not for degree of hearing diagnosis explained 23% of
divided into those diagnosed reported. No report of loss, mode of the variance in expressive
before and after age 6 mo. attrition or follow-up rates. communication, other language scores.
The number of low-risk disabilities, parents’
infants and the role of UNHS hearing, CQ, mother’s
in identifying subjects are not education, ethnicity, SES.
described. Overlap of sample
with previous CHIP studies
was not reported.
(continued)

©2001 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. (Reprinted with Corrections) JAMA, October 24/31, 2001—Vol 286, No. 16 2005

Downloaded From: http://jama.jamanetwork.com/ by a UNIVERSITAETSBIBLIOTHEK GIESSEN User on 05/13/2015


NEWBORN HEARING SCREENING

Table 2. Cohort Studies Reporting Language Outcomes (cont)*


Comparability and
Maintenance of Early vs Late Adjustment for
Study (Quality) Selection of Subjects Groups Confounders Results
Studies From Other Programs
Moeller,57 2000 Convenience sample of Not reported. No report of Multiple regression analysis At age 5 y, family involvement
(fair) 5-year-olds (n = 112) who attrition or follow-up rates. adjusted for family accounted for 57% of
completed the Diagnostic Early identified children may involvement, degree of variance in vocabulary and
Early Intervention Program in have more opportunity to hearing loss, and age of enrollment accounted
Lincoln, Neb. Children with drop out, although nonverbal IQ. for 11.5%. Adjusted mean
nonverbal IQ ⬍70 and those differential dropout may be vocabulary and reasoning
who did not participate in less of a problem at 5 y scores were within normal
program through age 5 y than in studies assessing range among children
were excluded. The number closer to enrollment. enrolled prior to age 11 mo
of low-risk infants and the but were lower for
role of UNHS in identifying later-identified children.
subjects are not described.
Outcome assessments were
made preintervention and
postintervention.
Calderon and Cohort of 80 children with Not reported. Late-diagnosed Controlled for degree of At age 3 y, age at entry to
Naidu,58 2000 profound hearing loss group had less hearing loss, degree of program explained 43.5% of
(fair) enrolled in ECHI in Seattle, severe-to-profound loss outcome impairment that the variance in receptive
Wash. Children with (36% vs 66%). Overall loss was present upon entry language and 49% of the
developmental delay were to follow-up not reported. into program (baseline variance in expressive
excluded. Cohort grouped by Because the early test levels). language. Children treated
3 levels by age of entry into diagnosed group was in the before age 2 y had better
program: ⬍12 mo (n = 9), program longer, they had outcomes than those treated
12-24 mo (n = 39), ⬎24 mo more opportunity to drop after age 2 y. Only 3 children
(n = 32). The method of out, so a differential loss to entered the program prior to
sampling is not described, follow-up is likely. age 6 mo.
but the design excluded
patients who entered the
program but did not
graduate.
*ANOVA indicates analysis of variance; SES, socioeconomic status; MCDI, Minnesota Child Development Inventory; DQ, developmental quotient; PHL, permanent hearing loss;
ANCOVA, analysis of covariance; CDI, child development inventory; UNHS, universal newborn hearing screening; CQ, cognitive quotient; LQ, language quotient; and ECHI, Early
Child Hearing Intervention.

blinded assessments, and all selected tion “inconclusive” and the quality of ditionally, 8% of mothers said they
children for inclusion based on the avail- evidence as “fair/poor.” treated their child differently (eg, spoke
ability of a language assessment be- louder or clapped their hands).50 No
tween ages 2 to 5 years. This could in- What Are the Potential study attempted to assess the effect of pa-
troduce bias, because early identified Adverse Effects of Screening rental anxiety or changes in parental be-
children who remained in the program and of Early Treatment? havior on infants’ development or on the
may have had better results than early Screening. Potential adverse effects of parent-infant relationship.
identified children who were not avail- false-positive screening tests include Treatment. The harms of early inter-
able for follow-up. None of the studies misdiagnosis, parental misunderstand- vention have not been adequately stud-
provide information on attrition or fol- ing and anxiety, and unfavorable label- ied, and differing ethical and philosophi-
low-up rates. All but 1 compared chil- ing. As noted earlier, the intermediate cal attitudes about deaf awareness and
dren who were identified early and late diagnostic standard determining PHL culture have led to controversy about the
by means other than UNHS, rather than is imperfect; in expert hands, as many content of early interventions.33 Treat-
children whose age at identification and as 7% of infants diagnosed as having ment strategies for hearing loss in chil-
enrollment was determined primarily by permanent PHL may eventually prove dren include hearing aids or other am-
whether or not they were screened. to have normal hearing. The fre- plification, American Sign Language
Other factors, such as family involve- quency of misdiagnosis in everyday and/or English instruction, speech and
ment (an important contributor to lan- practice settings has not been studied. language therapy, and family education
guage development),57 the degree of In the only controlled trial,43 parents and support. Different experts advocate
other disability, or the quality of pedi- whose infants were screened had anxi- substantially different approaches based
atric care, might have influenced the ety and attitudes similar to parents in the on competing theories of language ac-
time of identification and the language unscreened group. Three other small quisition and communication. Treat-
outcome. The task force rated the studies found false-positive screening re- ment strategies vary widely, even among
strength of evidence linking early treat- sults produced significant or lasting anxi- programs in states or hospitals with es-
ment with improved language func- ety in 3.5% to 14% of parents.50,73,74 Ad- tablished screening programs.75 This
2006 JAMA, October 24/31, 2001—Vol 286, No. 16 (Reprinted with Corrections) ©2001 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://jama.jamanetwork.com/ by a UNIVERSITAETSBIBLIOTHEK GIESSEN User on 05/13/2015


NEWBORN HEARING SCREENING

variation reflects uncertainty about number needed to screen (NNS) to de- come with and without early treat-
the efficacy of the interventions, which tect 1 additional case before age 10 ment—are not known. To use a hypo-
have not been evaluated in randomized months would be 1441 and the NNS thetical example, if 50% of newborns
trials or in population-based cohort to treat 1 additional case before 10 with PHL would have poor language
studies.13,20,33 months would be 2401. With UNHS, ability if diagnosed after age 10 months,
The argument for early intervention 254 newborns would be referred for au- and early intervention reduced this by
is based on the prevailing theory of lan- diological evaluation because of false- 50%, then the NNS to prevent 1 addi-
guage development, which holds that positive second-stage screening test re- tional case of delayed language acqui-
early auditory input is an important pre- sults (vs 48 for selective screening), and sition would be 6771.
cursor of language development. An op- 1 of these would also be falsely diag-
posing viewpoint suggests that, during nosed to have PHL at the first posthos- COMMENT
infancy, nonverbal communication, joint pital visit to an audiologist. TABLE 4 summarizes the evidence for
attention, shared experiences, and mu- Of the 6 additional early diagnosed, each of the major assumptions underly-
tual understanding are more important low-risk newborns, how many would ing the case for UNHS. Several gaps in
precursors of language development than actually benefit from early treatment? information about UNHS effectiveness
are hearing speech and forming sounds. The data needed to estimate this—the remain. Modern screening tests for hear-
Proponents of this view theorize that probabilities of a poor language out- ing impairment can improve identifica-
early intervention could harm infants be-
cause it leads parents to focus on “means Table 3. Benefits of Screening a Hypothetical Cohort of 10 000 Newborns for
of communication the child has the least Moderate-to-Profound PHL*
prerequisites for” and on the child’s dis- Probability or High-Risk
ability instead of his or her competen- Benefit and Relevant Factors Effect Size UNHS Screening
cies.76 Because there are no randomized Assumptions, %†
trials of different management strate- Proportion at high risk 0.2
gies, it is impossible to assess the merits Prevalence
High-risk group 0.008
of these concerns.
Low-risk group 0.0008
Miss rate (proportion not screened in hospital) for UNHS
Summary of Benefits and Harms High-risk group 0.1
TABLE 3 summarizes the benefits and Low-risk group 0.05
harms of UNHS and selective screen- Follow-up rate for misses 0.9
ing in a hypothetical cohort of 10 000 Miss rate for high-risk screening
newborns. We used our literature re- High-risk group 0.2
view to estimate prevalence, sensitiv- Follow-up rate for misses 0.75
ity and specificity, compliance, and the Sensitivity of 2-stage screening 0.85
likelihood of being diagnosed and Specificity of 2-stage screening 0.97
treated before age 10 months. There are Compliance with follow-up 0.9
no reliable data to estimate how often Accuracy of diagnostic ABR
Sensitivity 1.0
selective screening misses patients Specificity 0.995
whose risk factors were not detected Proportion of low-risk infants diagnosed before 0.35
during hospitalization. We assumed 10 mo without screening
that in a selective screening program, Treated before 1 y 0.6
20% of high-risk infants are never tested Results, No.
in the hospital, vs 10% for UNHS. There Infants screened 9400 1600
are also no reliable data by which to es- Cases diagnosed before 10 mo 17 12
timate the probability that a low-risk in- Cases treated before 10 mo 10 7
fant will be diagnosed by 10 months Out of total 22 22
without newborn screening; we esti- False-positive screening tests 254 48
mate this to be 35% in our base case. Normal infants incorrectly diagnosed to have PHL 1 0
With UNHS, an additional 7800 at first posthospital audiological examination
screening tests would be done, result- NNS to diagnose 1 case 584 173
ing in the diagnosis of 6 additional cases NNS to diagnose 1 additional case before 10 mo 1441 ...
of moderate-to-profound hearing loss NNS to treat 1 additional case before 10 mo 2401 ...
*PHL indicates permanent hearing loss; UNHS, universal newborn hearing screening; ABR, automated auditory brain-
diagnosed before age 10 months. Of stem response; NNS, number needed to screen; and ellipses, not applicable.
these, 3 additional cases would be †Base case assumptions are derived from the studies in Table 2, except for miss rate for high-risk screening (high-risk
group), accuracy of diagnostic ABR (sensitivity), and proportion of low-risk infants diagnosed without screening.
treated before age 10 months. Thus, the
©2001 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. (Reprinted with Corrections) JAMA, October 24/31, 2001—Vol 286, No. 16 2007

Downloaded From: http://jama.jamanetwork.com/ by a UNIVERSITAETSBIBLIOTHEK GIESSEN User on 05/13/2015


NEWBORN HEARING SCREENING

Table 4. Strength of Evidence for Universal Newborn Hearing Screening*


Key Question Evidence Evidence Type Quality of Evidence
Can UNHS accurately OAEs and ABR are highly Cohort or case-control analytic Good. One controlled trial measured the
diagnose accurate screening tests for study predictive value (6.7%) of a positive
moderate-to-profound congenital PHL (sensitivity, test result, and 1 good-quality cohort
sensorineural hearing 84%; specificity, 90%). study measured sensitivity and
impairment? specificity against an independent
gold standard.
In UNHS programs, how many UNHS increases the chance Controlled trial without Good. One cohort study in the United
children are identified and that diagnosis and randomization; cohort or States and 1 controlled study in the
treated early? treatment will occur before case-control analytic study United Kingdom reported the
age 6 mo. UNHS increases frequency of treatment before ages 6
early identification between and 10 mo, respectively. Other
19% and 42% over studies did not provide sufficient
selective screening in information, and none included
high-risk children. patients who, although screened,
were diagnosed and treated late
because of loss to follow-up.
However, no controlled trials of
UNHS vs selective screening have
been done.
Does identification and Evidence is inconclusive. Cohort or case-control analytic Fair/poor. Studies have selection bias
treatment prior to age 6 mo study; multiple time series, and baseline differences between
improve language and dramatic uncontrolled compared groups. These studies did
communication in infants experiments not specifically describe outcomes in
who would not be the subgroup of children who would
diagnosed that early in a be identified by UNHS but not by
selective, high-risk selective screening.
screening program?
What are the potential adverse Evidence is inconclusive. Opinions of respected Poor. Most postulated adverse effects
effects of screening and authorities have not been evaluated in studies.
early treatment?
*UNHS indicates universal newborn hearing screening; OAEs, otoacoustic emissions; ABR, automated auditory brainstem response; and PHL, permanent hearing loss.

tion of newborns with PHL, but as many tal hearing impairment, the risk of important factor. None of the studies
as 10% of newborns with normal hear- delayed language development in oth- attempted to link short-term improve-
ing will require a second screening test. erwise healthy infants diagnosed at ages ments to better function later in life.
From 1% to 3% of newborns will be 1 or 2 years is not known. Because As use of UNHS rapidly increases, it
referred for audiological assessment; the frequency and severity of poor lan- is important to conduct longitudinal
over 90% of those referred are false- guage outcomes in this group is uncer- studies of UNHS to address these
positives. The consequences of false- tain, only adequately controlled trials knowledge gaps. Further randomized
positives have not been adequately evalu- or cohort studies can establish the ef- trials of UNHS seem unlikely to be con-
ated, nor has the reliability of audiological ficacy of early intervention. ducted in the United States. Although
and behavioral assessment64—the stan- Several cohort studies show that, by it would be possible to compare states
dard used to make decisions about treat- ages 2 to 4 years, children with hear- with and without UNHS, such studies
ment—been established. Moreover, the ing aids and other therapy in the first would be prone to uncontrollable con-
false-negative rate is higher than previ- 6 months of life had better language founding due to differences among
ously thought, probably 20% to 30% in skills than those treated later. None of states. However, better evidence about
most programs. This new finding calls these studies compared an inception co- the effectiveness of UNHS is needed and
into question the assumption that a new- hort of newborns offered UNHS with could be obtained via population-
born who passes a screening test has infants managed by usual care (includ- based studies that begin with incep-
normal hearing. It also suggests that ing selective screening). Additionally, tion cohorts and carefully report out-
stricter pass criteria, which have been these studies had unclear criteria for se- comes in all possible patients, as well
promoted as a way to reduce false- lecting subjects, making it impossible as rates of loss to follow-up. Speech, lan-
positives, also reduce the effectiveness of to exclude baseline differences in the guage, and scholastic achievement of
screening. compared groups as a cause for the as- deaf and hard-of-hearing children
A clearer picture of the conse- sociation of early identification with should be followed over time. States that
quences of delayed diagnosis in low- language development. The hypoth- have UNHS should conduct such popu-
risk newborns would strengthen the esis that early intervention is a predic- lation-based studies to evaluate whether
case for universal screening. While di- tor of language acquisition is plau- the long-term language outcomes of
agnosis of hearing impairment is of- sible, but the studies do not establish deaf children improve as the age of
ten delayed in children with congeni- that screening low-risk newborns is the identification decreases.
2008 JAMA, October 24/31, 2001—Vol 286, No. 16 (Reprinted with Corrections) ©2001 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://jama.jamanetwork.com/ by a UNIVERSITAETSBIBLIOTHEK GIESSEN User on 05/13/2015


NEWBORN HEARING SCREENING

Author Affiliations: University of Washington, Se- 9. Ruben RJ. Effectiveness and efficacy of early de- tification, hearing aid fitting, and enrollment in early
attle (Ms Thompson and Drs McPhillips and Davis); tection of hearing impairment in children. Acta Oto- intervention. Ear Hear. 2000;21:118-130.
Harvard Pilgrim Health Care, Boston, Mass (Dr Lieu), laryngol. 1991;482:127-131. 30. White KR, Vohr BR, Behrens TR. Universal new-
National Initiative for Children’s Healthcare Quality, 10. Rach GH, Zielhuis GA, van den Broek P. The in- born hearing screening using transient evoked oto-
Boston, Mass (Dr Homer), Oregon Health & Science fluence of chronic persistent otitis media with effu- acoustic emissions: results of the Rhode Island Hearing
University, Portland VA Medical Center, Portland (Dr sion on language development of 2- to 4-year-olds. Assessment Project. Semin Hear. 1993;14:18-29.
Helfand). Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 1988;15:253-261. 31. Joint Committee on Infant Hearing. Joint Com-
Author Contributions: Study concept and design: 11. Zinkus PW, Gottlieb MI. Patterns of perceptual mittee on Infant Hearing Year 2000 Position State-
Thompson, McPhillips, Davis, Helfand. and academic deficits related to early chronic otitis me- ment: Principles and Guidelines for Early Hearing De-
Acquisition of data: Thompson, McPhillips, Davis, dia. Pediatrics. 1980;66:246-253. tection and Intervention Programs. Available at: http://
Helfand. 12. Moeller MP, Osberger MJ, Eccarius M. Recep- www.infanthearing.org/jcih/. Accessibility verified
Analysis and interpretation of data: Thompson, tive Language Skills: Language and Learning Skills of October 1, 2001.
McPhillips, Davis, Lieu, Homer, Helfand. Hearing-Impaired Children. Bethesda, Md: Ameri- 32. Green GE, Scott DA, McDonald JM, Wood-
Drafting of the manuscript: Thompson, Helfand. can Speech, Language and Hearing Association; 1986. worth GG, Sheffield VC, Smith RJ. Carrier rates in the
Critical revision of the manuscript for important in- Monographs of the American Speech, Language and midwestern United States for GJB2 mutations caus-
tellectual content: Thompson, McPhillips, Davis, Lieu, Hearing Association, 23:41-53. ing inherited deafness. JAMA. 1999;281:2211-
Homer, Helfand. 13. Carney AE, Moeller MP. Treatment efficacy: hear- 2216.
Statistical expertise: Lieu. ing loss in children. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1998; 33. US Preventive Services Task Force. Screening for
Obtained funding: Helfand. 41:S61-S84. hearing impairment. In: US Preventive Services Task
Administrative, technical, or material support: McPhil- 14. Yoshinaga-Itano C, Apuzzo ML. Identification of Force Guide to Clinical Preventive Services. 2nd ed.
lips, Davis, Lieu, Homer. hearing loss after age 18 months is not early enough. Baltimore, Md: Williams & Wilkins; 1996:393-405.
Study supervision: Thompson, Davis, Helfand. Am Ann Deaf. 1998;143:380-387. 34. Grandori F, Lutman ME. European Consensus
Funding/Support: The study on which this article is 15. Yoshinaga-Itano C, Apuzzo ML. The develop- Statement on Neonatal Hearing Screening: Finalized
based was conducted by the Oregon Health & Sci- ment of deaf and hard of hearing children identified at the European Consensus Development Confer-
ence University Evidence-Based Practice Center, un- early through the high-risk registry. Am Ann Deaf. ence on Neonatal Hearing Screening, Milan, Italy,
der contract 290-97-0018 to the Agency for Health- 1998;143:416-424. 15-16 May 1998. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 1998;
care Research and Quality. 16. Holt JA. Stanford Achievement Test, 8th edition: 44:309-310.
Also Available: A longer and more detailed system- reading comprehension subgroup results. Am Ann Deaf 35. Grandori F, Lutman M. The European Consen-
atic evidence review of UNHS will be available at http:// Ref Iss. 1993;138:172-175. sus Development Conference on Neonatal Hearing
www.ahrq.gov/clinic/serfiles.htm. 17. Allen TE. Patterns of academic achievement among Screening (Milan, May 15-16, 1998). Am J Audiol.
Disclaimer: The authors of this article are respon- hearing impaired students: 1974 and 1983. In: Schil- 1999;8:19-20.
sible for its contents, including any clinical or treat- droth A, Karchmer AM, eds. Deaf Children in America. 36. Brookhouser PE. Sensorineural hearing loss in chil-
ment recommendations. No statement in this article Boston, Mass: College-Hill Press; 1986:161-206. dren. Pediatr Clin North Am. 1996;43:1195-1216.
should be construed as an official position of the 18. National Institutes of Health. Early identification 37. Eilers RE, Berlin C. Advances in early detection of
Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality or the of hearing impairment in infants and younger chil- hearing loss in infants. Curr Probl Pediatr. 1995;25:
US Department of Health and Human Services. dren. Rockville, Md: National Institutes of Health; 1993. 60-66.
Acknowledgment: We thank Jennifer Sundheim, 38. Finitzo T, Crumley WG. The role of the pediatri-
19. Fonseca S, Forsyth H, Grigor J, et al. Identifica-
MLIS, and Mitchelle Honore, BS, at the University of cian in hearing loss: from detection to connection. Pe-
tion of permanent hearing loss in children: are the tar-
Washington and Kathryn Pyle Krages, AMLS, MA, Su- diatr Clin North Am. 1999;46:ix-x, 15-34.
gets for outcome measures attainable? Br J Audiol.
san Carson, MPH, and Susan Wingenfeld, BA, at Or- 39. Martin WH, Schwegler JW, Gleeson AL, Shi YB.
1999;33:135-143.
egon Health & Science University for their assistance New techniques of hearing assessment. Otolaryngol
20. Davis A, Bamford J, Wilson I, Ramkalawan T, For-
on this project. Clin North Am. 1994;27:487-510.
shaw M, Wright S. A critical review of the role of neo-
40. Oudesluys-Murphy AM, van Straaten HL, Bho-
natal hearing screening in the detection of congeni-
lasingh R, van Zanten GA. Neonatal hearing screen-
tal hearing impairment. Health Technol Assess. 1997;
REFERENCES ing. Eur J Pediatr. 1996;155:429-435.
1:i-iv, 1-176.
41. Silverman CA. Audiologic assessment and ampli-
1. Maki-Torkko EM, Lindholm PK, Vayrynen MRH, Leisti 21. Meadow-Orlans KP, Mertens DM, Sass-Lehrer
fication. Prim Care. 1998;25:545-581.
JT, Sorri MJ. Epidemiology of moderate to profound MA, Scott-Olson K. Support services for parents and 42. Erenberg A, Lemons J, Sia C, Trunkel D, Ziring P,
childhood hearing impairments in northern Finland: any their children who are deaf or hard of hearing: a na- for the American Academy of Pediatrics Task Force
changes in ten years? Scand Audiol. 1998;27:95-103. tional survey. Am Ann Deaf. 1997;142:278-293. on Newborn and Infant Hearing, 1998-1999. New-
2. Fortnum H, Davis A. Epidemiology of permanent 22. Harrison M, Roush J. Age of suspicion, identifi- born and infant hearing loss: detection and interven-
childhood hearing impairment in Trent Region, 1985- cation, and intervention for infants and young chil- tion. Pediatrics. 1999;103:527-530.
1993. Br J Audiol. 1997;31:409-446. dren with hearing loss: a national study. Ear Hear. 1996; 43. Wessex Universal Neonatal Hearing Screening Trial
3. Karikoski JO, Marttila TI. Prevalence of childhood 17:55-62. Group. Controlled trial of universal neonatal screening
hearing impairment in southern Finland. Scand Au- 23. Kuhl PK, Williams KA, Lacerda F, Stephens KN, for early identification of permanent childhood hear-
diol. 1995;24:237-241. Lindbloom B. Linguistic experience alters phonetics per- ing impairment: Wessex Universal Neonatal Hearing
4. Parving A, Jensen JH. Prevalence of permanent ception in infants by six months of age. Science. 1992; Screening Trial Group. Lancet. 1998;352:1957-1964.
childhood hearing impairment: its role in audit of lo- 255:606-608. 44. Prieve B, Dalzell L, Berg A, et al. The New York
cal paediatric hearing health services. J Audiol Med. 24. Gopnik A, Meltzoff AN, Kuhl PK. The Scientist State universal newborn hearing screening demon-
1998;7:100-108. in the Crib: Minds, Brains, and How Children Learn. stration project: outpatient outcome measures. Ear
5. Boyle CA, Yeargin-Allsopp M, Doernberg NS, Holm- New York, NY: William Morrow & Co; 1999. Hear. 2000;21:104-117.
green MS, Murphy CC, Schendel DE. Prevalence of 25. Sininger YS, Doyle KJ, Moore JK. The case for early 45. Finitzo T, Albright K, O’Neal J. The newborn with
selected developmental disabilities in children 3-10 identification of hearing loss in children: auditory sys- hearing loss: dectection in the nursery. Pediatrics. 1998;
years of age: the metropolitan Atlanta developmen- tem development, experimental auditory depriva- 102:1452-1460.
tal disabilities surveillance program, 1991. Mor Mor- tion, and development of speech perception and hear- 46. Mehl AL, Thomson V. Newborn hearing screen-
tal Wkly Rep CDC Surveill Summ. 1996;45:1-14. ing. Pediatr Clin North Am. 1999;46:1-14. ing: the great omission. Pediatrics. 1998;101:E4.
6. Van Naarden K, Decoufle P, Caldwell K. Preva- 26. Vohr BR, Carty LM, Moore PE, Letourneau K. The 47. Barsky-Firkser L, Sun S. Universal newborn hear-
lence and characteristics of children with serious hear- Rhode Island Hearing Assessment Program: experi- ing screenings: a three-year experience. Pediatrics.
ing impairment in metropolitan Atlanta, 1991-1993. ence with statewide hearing screening (1993-1996). 1997;99:E4.
Pediatrics. 1999;103:570-575. J Pediatr. 1998;133:353-357. 48. Watkin PM. Neonatal otoacoustic emission screen-
7. Stein LK. Factors influencing the efficacy of uni- 27. Finitzo T, Diefendorf AO. The state of the infor- ing and the identification of deafness. Arch Dis Child
versal newborn hearing screening. Pediatr Clin North mation: evidence gathering in infant hearing pro- Fetal Neonatal Ed. 1996;74:F16-F25.
Am. 1999;46:95-105. grams. Am J Audiol. 1997;6:91-94. 49. Aidan D, Avan P, Bonfils P. Auditory screening
8. Fortnum H, Summerfield Q, Marshall D, Davis A, 28. Watkin PM. Outcomes of neonatal screening for in neonates by means of transient evoked otoacous-
Bamford J. Prevalence of permanent childhood hear- hearing loss by otoacoustic emission. Arch Dis Child tic emissions: a report of 2,842 recordings. Ann Otol
ing impairment in the United Kingdom and implica- Fetal Neonatal Ed. 1996;75:F158-F168. Rhinol Laryngol. 1999;108:525-531.
tions for universal neonatal hearing screening: ques- 29. Dalzell L, Orlando M, MacDonald M, et al. The 50. Clemens CJ, Davis SA, Bailey AR. The “false posi-
tionnaire based ascertainment study. BMJ. 2001;323: New York State universal newborn hearing screen- tive” in universal newborn hearing screening. Pedi-
536. ing demonstration project: ages of hearing loss iden- atrics. 2000;106:7-11.

©2001 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. (Reprinted with Corrections) JAMA, October 24/31, 2001—Vol 286, No. 16 2009

Downloaded From: http://jama.jamanetwork.com/ by a UNIVERSITAETSBIBLIOTHEK GIESSEN User on 05/13/2015


NEWBORN HEARING SCREENING

51. Mason JA, Herrmann KR. Universal infant hearing 59. Harris R, Helfand M, Woolf S, et al. Current meth- 67. Watkin PM, Baldwin M, McEnery G. Neonatal at
screening by automated auditory brainstem response ods of the U.S. preventive services task force. Am J risk screening and the identification of deafness. Arch
measurement. Pediatrics. 1998;101:221-228. Prev Med. 2001;20:21-35. Dis Child. 1991;66:1130-1135.
52. Norton SJ, Gorga MP, Widen JE, et al. Identifi- 60. Prieve BA, Stevens F. The New York State Univer- 68. Gerber S. Review of a high risk register for con-
cation of neonatal hearing impairment: evaluation of sal Newborn Hearing Screening Demonstration Project: genital and early onset deafness. Br J Audiol. 1990;
transient evoked otoacoustic emission, distortion prod- introduction and overview. Ear Hear. 2000;21:85-91. 24:347-356.
uct otoacoustic emission, and auditory brain stem re- 61. Kennedy CR, for the Wessex Universal Neonatal 69. Ireton H, Thwing E. The Minnesota Child Devel-
sponse test performance. Ear Hear. 2000;21:508- Screening Trial Group. Controlled trial of universal neo- opment Inventory. Minneapolis: University of Min-
528. natal screening for early identification of permanent nesota; 1974.
53. Apuzzo M-RL, Yoshinaga-Itano C. Early identi- childhood hearing impairment: coverage, positive pre- 70. Fenson L, Dale P, Reznick J, et al. MacArthur Com-
fication of infants with significant hearing loss and the dictive value, effect on mothers and incremental yield. municative Development Inventories: User’s Guide &
Minnesota child development inventory. Semin Hear. Acta Paediatr Suppl. 1999;88:73-75. Technical Manual. San Diego, Calif: Singular Publi-
1995;16:124-139. 62. Widen JE, Folsom RC, Cone-Wesson B, et al. Iden- cations; 1993.
54. Yoshinaga-Itano C, Sedey AL, Coulter DK, Mehl tification of neonatal hearing impairment: hearing sta- 71. Thomson V. The Colorado Newborn Hearing
AL. Language of early- and later-identified children tus at 8 to 12 months corrected age using a visual re- Screening Project. Am J Audiol. 1997;6:74-77.
with hearing loss. Pediatrics. 1998;102:1161-1171. inforcement audiometry protocol. Ear Hear. 2000;21: 72. Thomson V, Rose LB, O’Neal J, Finitzo T. State-
55. Yoshinaga-Itano C, Coulter D, Thomson V. The 471-487. wide implementation of universal newborn hearing
Colorado Newborn Hearing Screening Project: ef- 63. Moore JM, Thompson G, Folsom RC. Auditory screening. Semin Hear. 1998;19:287-300.
fects on speech and language development for chil- responsiveness of premature infants utilizing visual re- 73. Watkin PM, Baldwin M, Dixon R, Beckman A. Ma-
dren with hearing loss. J Perinatol. 2000;20(suppl 8): inforcement audiometry (VRA). Ear Hear. 1992;13: ternal anxiety and attitudes to universal neonatal hear-
S132-S137. 187-194. ing screening. Br J Audiol. 1998;32:27-37.
56. Mayne AM, Yoshinaga-Itano C, Sedey AL, Carey 64. Folsom RC, Diefendorf AO. Physiologic and be- 74. Barringer DG, Mauk GW. Survey of parents per-
A. Expressive vocabulary development of infants and havioral approaches to pediatric hearing assessment. ceptions regarding hospital-based newborn hearing
toddlers who are deaf or hard of hearing. Volta Rev. Pediatr Clin North Am. 1999;46:107-120. screening. Audiol Today. 1997;9:18-19.
2000;100:1-28. 65. Mauk GW, White KR, Mortenson LB, Behrens TR. 75. Stredler-Brown A, Arehart KH. Universal new-
57. Moeller MP. Early intervention and language de- The effectiveness of screening programs based on high- born hearing screening: impact on early intervention
velopment in children who are deaf and hard of hear- risk characteristics in early identification of hearing im- services. Volta Rev. In press.
ing. Pediatrics. 2000;106:E43. pairment. Ear Hear. 1991;12:312-319. 76. Priesler G. The development of communication
58. Calderon R, Naidu S. Further support of the ben- 66. Hyde M, Riko K, Malizia K. Audiometric accu- and language in deaf and severely hard of hearing chil-
efits of early identification and intervention with chil- racy of the click of ABR in infants at risk for hearing dren: implications for the future. Int J Pediatr Oto-
dren with hearing loss. Volta Rev. 2000;100:53-84. loss. J Am Acad Audiol. 1990;1:59-66. rhinolaryngol. 1999;49(suppl 1):S39-S43.

Words are chameleons, which reflect the colour of their


environment.
—Learned Hand (1872-1961)

2010 JAMA, October 24/31, 2001—Vol 286, No. 16 (Reprinted with Corrections) ©2001 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://jama.jamanetwork.com/ by a UNIVERSITAETSBIBLIOTHEK GIESSEN User on 05/13/2015

You might also like