You are on page 1of 8

Neuropsychologia 49 (2011) 1843–1850

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Neuropsychologia
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia

The effect of hippocampal damage in children on recalling the past and


imagining new experiences
Janine M. Cooper a , Faraneh Vargha-Khadem a , David G. Gadian b , Eleanor A. Maguire c,∗
a
Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience Unit, Institute of Child Health, University College London, 30 Guilford Street, London WC1N 1EH, United Kingdom
b
Radiology and Physics Unit, Institute of Child Health, University College London, 30 Guilford Street, London WC1N 1EH, United Kingdom
c
Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, Institute of Neurology, University College London, 12 Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, United Kingdom

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Compared to adults, relatively little is known about autobiographical memory and the ability to imag-
Received 10 October 2010 ine fictitious and future scenarios in school-aged children, despite the importance of these functions for
Received in revised form 16 February 2011 development and subsequent independent living. Even less is understood about the effect of early hip-
Accepted 7 March 2011
pocampal damage on children’s memory and imagination abilities. To bridge this gap, we devised a novel
Available online 15 March 2011
naturalistic autobiographical memory task that enabled us to formally assess the memory for recent auto-
biographical experiences in healthy school-aged children. Contemporaneous with the autobiographical
Keywords:
memories being formed, the children also imagined and described fictitious scenarios. Having established
Autobiographical memory
Scene construction
the performance of healthy school-aged children on these tasks, we proceeded to make comparisons with
Episodic memory children (n = 21) who had experienced neonatal hypoxia/ischaemia, and consequent bilateral hippocam-
Hippocampus pal damage. Our results showed that healthy children could recall autobiographical events, including
Children spatiotemporal information and specific episodic details. By contrast, children who had experienced
Developmental amnesia neonatal hypoxia/ischaemia had impaired recall, with the specific details of episodes being lost. Despite
this significant memory deficit they were able to construct fictitious scenarios. This is in clear contrast to
adults with hippocampal damage, who typically have impaired autobiographical memory and deficits in
the construction of fictitious and future scenarios. We speculate that the paediatric patients’ relatively
intact semantic memory and/or some functionality in their residual hippocampi may underpin their
scene construction ability.
© 2011 Elsevier Ltd. Open access under CC BY license.

1. Introduction 2002; Rosenbaum et al., 2005; but see Squire et al., 2010, and
Maguire & Hassabis, 2011 for a response) who tested patients
Being able to recall the past and imagine and plan for the future with bilateral hippocampal damage that was acquired in adult-
are prerequisites for independent living. The hippocampus is part hood, rendering them amnesic. They were asked to imagine and
of a network of brain regions that supports the retrieval of autobi- describe fictitious scenarios and also possible plausible future
ographical memories (Cabeza & St. Jacques, 2007; Maguire, 2001; episodes. The patient group was significantly impaired relative to
Spreng, Mar, & Kim, 2009; Svoboda, McKinnon, & Levine, 2006). control participants on both tasks, and a possible source for their
Functional MRI (fMRI) findings have indicated the hippocam- deficit was identified. While patients were able to produce rele-
pus is also involved in imagining fictitious episodes (Hassabis, vant details when asked to imagine, their scene descriptions lacked
Kumaran, & Maguire, 2007a; Summerfield, Hassabis, & Maguire, spatial coherence and were fragmented. It was concluded that the
2009, 2010), and the simulation of personal future events (e.g. hippocampus may play a critical role in imagination by binding
Addis & Schacter, 2008; Addis, Wong, & Schacter, 2007; Addis, together the disparate elements of an event or scene in a process
Pan, Vu, Laiser, & Schacter, 2009; Botzung, Denkova, & Manning, called scene construction (Cohen and Eichenbaum, 1993; Hassabis
2008; Okuda et al., 2003; Szpunar, Watson, & McDermott, 2007). & Maguire, 2007, 2009; Hassabis et al., 2007b; O’Keefe & Nadel,
Further neuropsychological evidence for this comes from Hassabis, 1978; see Buckner & Carroll, 2007; Schacter & Addis, 2007 for other
Kumaran, Vann, and Maguire (2007b), (see also Andelman, Hoofien, views).
Goldberg, Aizenstein, & Neufeld, 2010; Klein, Loftus, & Kihlstrom, Whilst Hassabis et al. (2007b) documented impairment of scene
construction associated with adult-onset amnesia and bilateral hip-
pocampal damage, it was recently reported that another patient
∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +44 20 78337457; fax: +44 20 78131445. with selective hippocampal lesions and developmental amnesia
E-mail address: e.maguire@fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk (E.A. Maguire). (DA; Vargha-Khadem et al., 1997) was unimpaired on the scene

0028-3932/© 2011 Elsevier Ltd. Open access under CC BY license.


doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.03.008
1844 J.M. Cooper et al. / Neuropsychologia 49 (2011) 1843–1850

construction task (Maguire, Vargha-Khadem, & Hassabis, 2010). aged children and adolescents to construct imagined scenes and
Jon, now an adult, sustained his hippocampal pathology perinatally, scenarios.
and showed the symptoms of DA from early childhood. His ability We therefore set out to examine a number of key issues. First,
to describe detailed fictitious and future scenarios stands in marked we devised a novel autobiographical memory task that enabled us
contrast to the patients whose hippocampal damage and amnesia to formally assess memory for recent autobiographical experiences
were acquired in adulthood (Hassabis et al., 2007b). Maguire et al. in a group (n = 12) of healthy school-aged children. This ecologically
(2010) noted that Jon’s intact scene construction could be due to valid task involved spending the day with each child during which
the effortful strategies he had developed over the years to assist a series of (unbeknownst to them) pre-planned ‘events’ occurred,
with imagination, perhaps relying on his well-developed semantic memory for which was assessed on the subsequent day in a surprise
memory store, reflecting plastic changes in response to the neona- recall test. In this way, each child underwent the same controlled
tal hypoxic/ischaemic insults which may have ameliorated some yet naturalistic protocol to examine autobiographical recall, and
of the effects of early hippocampal damage. Also, Jon’s preserved our novel analysis method enabled different aspects of autobio-
scene construction could be associated with residual hippocampal graphical recollection to be documented. Contemporaneous with
function, given that his remaining hippocampal tissue activated the autobiographical memories being formed, the children also
during fMRI when he recalled the few autobiographical memo- imagined and described fictitious scenarios in an adaptation of the
ries that he retains (Maguire, Vargha-Khadem, & Mishkin, 2001). Hassabis et al. (2007b) paradigm. Having recorded performance on
By contrast another, now adult, DA patient HC (Case E6 in Vargha- the autobiographical memory recall and scene construction tests in
Khadem et al., 2003; DA6 in Adlam, Vargha Khadem, Mishkin, & healthy school-aged children, and adding to the literature thereon,
de Haan, 2005; Adlam, Malloy, Mishkin, & Vargha-Khadem, 2009) we proceeded to compare the performance of the young healthy
was recently reported to be impaired at simulating personal future controls to that of a group of children who had been exposed
events (Kwan et al., 2010). The test used differed somewhat from to neonatal hypoxia/ischaemia with concomitant selective bilat-
that employed by Hassabis et al. (2007b) and Maguire et al. (2010), eral hippocampal volume loss, the majority of whom had memory
in that there were fewer trials, there was no spatial coherence problems. We report data from the largest group of such patients
measure, and the focus was on internal and external details – a yet identified (n = 21) allowing us to establish with confidence the
legacy of the test’s origins in the autobiographical memory litera- boundaries within which their autobiographical memory recall and
ture. We suggest that this test may not be the most appropriate scene construction abilities operate.
means of assessing the subtleties of imagination/simulation in We hypothesised that the healthy control school-aged chil-
patients. To examine this assertion directly, a study is currently dren would perform with high accuracy on the autobiographical
underway in which patient HC’s performance on the Hassabis memory retrieval test, and that they would provide rich descrip-
et al. (2007b) test is being investigated (E.A. Maguire personal tions of imagined scenarios. Given their hippocampal pathology,
communication). impairments on standard memory tests, and reports from par-
Given the uncertain status of scene construction/simulation in ents of everyday memory problems, we expected the patient
the context of single case studies of patients with DA, a large sam- group to be impaired in autobiographical memory recall, similar to
ple of children with neonatal hypoxia/ischaemia and associated patients with adult-acquired selective hippocampal damage hav-
hippocampal damage and memory impairment is required to eval- ing resulted in amnesia. Regarding scene construction, the question
uate the contributions of age and experience to processes involved was whether they would show the same intact profile as DA patient
in scene construction. In order to assess how children with hip- Jon, or resemble the impaired adult patients with amnesia?
pocampal damage perform on autobiographical memory and scene
construction tasks, it is necessary to compare such a patient group
to healthy control children. 2. Materials and methods
Surprisingly, studies of autobiographical memory in school-
2.1. Participants
aged children and adolescents are fewer than those in adults. Bauer,
Burch, Scholin, and Güler (2007) suggest that children between the Twenty-one patients (13 male; mean age 13.29 years; SD 2.49; range 9–18 years;
ages of 7 and 10 years give mainly gist or fact-based information 3 left handed) participated, each of whom had suffered neonatal hypoxia/ischaemia
when recalling personal events and often do not provide details due to cardiorespiratory disease. They were all born at term, had no neurodevel-
opmental abnormalities, were free of genetic syndromes, and were native English
pertaining to who was present, or the spatiotemporal context of speakers. Twelve healthy control participants (6 male; mean age 12.33 years; SD
an event. Developmental changes to regions of the brain such as 2.06; range 9–15 years; 2 left handed) also took part. All participants and their
the frontal lobes, areas associated with retrieval abilities and exec- parents gave informed written consent to participate in the study in accordance
utive functions in adolescence (Huizinga, Dolan, & van der Molen, with the regulations of the local research ethics committees. There was no signif-
icant difference between the groups in terms of age (t = −1.12, P = 0.27). Parental
2006; Lehto, Juujärvi, Kooistra, & Pulkkinen, 2003), may account
occupation was classified according to the International Standard Classification of
for the increase in contextual information observed (Pasupathi & Occupation (ISCO-88) and then transformed into an International Socio-Economic
Wainryb, 2010; Piolino et al., 2007) in older children’s narratives Index of Occupational Status, which provides socio-economic values for parental
of autobiographical events, an ability that has been found to con- occupations. There was no significant difference between the patients and controls
tinue to develop along with self-awareness through adolescence for socioeconomic status (t = 1.27, P = 0.22).
All participants were assessed for verbal, nonverbal and overall intelligence
and into early adulthood (McLean, 2005; Pasupathi & Wainryb, (Verbal IQ, Performance IQ, Full Scale IQ; Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children
2010; for a review see Piolino, Desgranges, & Eustache, 2009). 4th Ed.), verbal fluency (subtest of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System),
Picard, Reffuveille, Eustache, and Piolino (2010) also suggested the and every day memory, working memory and attention (General Memory, Work-
development of episodic event memory is linked to an increased ing Memory, Attention/Concentration; Children’s Memory Scale) (see Fig. 1). The
scores of the control group were at the high end of the average range (i.e. from 8
efficiency of cognitive abilities, although little is known about other
to 13 points above the standard mean of 100), and consequently higher than the
processes, such as scene construction, that may be involved. Thus, scores of the patients which were within the average range and only 3–5 points
the relatively limited literature suggests that school-aged children below the standard population mean. When FSIQ was controlled for, there were no
and adolescents are able to recall everyday memories. While work differences between the two groups on any measure except on every day memory
is starting to emerge on future-oriented thinking in pre-schoolers (see Fig. 1); this was also the case when verbal and performance IQs were examined
separately. Of note, neither age nor IQ correlated significantly with any of the exper-
(e.g. Atance & Jackson, 2009; Atance & Meltoff, 2006; Grant & imental measures of recall and scene construction. This accords with previous use
Suddendorf, 2010; Suddendorf, Nielsen, & von Gehlen, 2011) much of the scene construction test (Hassabis et al., 2007), where background participant
less has been documented formally about the ability of school- characteristics such as age and IQ did not affect scores on this task.
J.M. Cooper et al. / Neuropsychologia 49 (2011) 1843–1850 1845

examined by an experienced paediatric neuroradiologist to screen for detectable sig-


nal abnormality, space occupying lesions, or volume loss (other than hippocampal)
across the whole brain. No consistent abnormalities outside of the medial tempo-
ral lobe were found. For the measurement of hippocampal volumes, the data were
reformatted into 1 mm-thick contiguous slices in a tilted coronal plane perpendic-
ular to the long axis of the hippocampus. Hippocampal cross-sectional areas were
measured in all slices along the entire length of the hippocampus, using MEDx 3.43
(Medical Numerics, Inc., Maryland, USA) software. The volumes were calculated by
summing the cross-sectional areas and multiplying by the distance between the
slices. Corrections, derived from the regression line of control hippocampal volume
versus intracranial volume, were then made for intracranial volume. Bilateral dam-
age to the hippocampi was confirmed for the patient group (see Fig. 2), with a mean
hippocampal volume bilaterally of 2550 mm3 (SD 386), compared to 3339 mm3 (SD
213) in the control group, a 21% reduction (t = 5.50, P = 0.001).

2.2. Tasks and procedures

Testing took place over two days. On day 1, each participant took part in events
that (unbeknownst to them) followed a scripted protocol. There were 13 events in
Fig. 1. Mean scores (±1 standard error) for the controls and patients on baseline total, 10 that were used for the main experiment, and 3 for practice trials during
neuropsychological measures. Comparisons of mean scores between the controls recall. The events (e.g. choosing a snack, drawing a complicated picture) were found
and patients showed a significant difference, with the controls consistently scor- during pilot testing to fit seamlessly into a day of neuropsychological assessments.
ing higher than the patients, although the magnitude of this difference was far Piloting also revealed through participant ratings that all events were similar in
greater in the domain of every day memory. Of note, the patients did not differ their level of difficulty and memorability, as well as ensuring they were not emo-
significantly from the population mean (100) of these standardised tests on any tive or obscure. Using discrete but thorough notes taken during the events, the
measure except every day memory (P < 0.0001). FSIQ = full scale IQ, VIQ = verbal experimenter chose the 10 events that contained full, clear and detailed actions
IQ; PIQ = performance IQ; WM = working memory; Attn = attention; MQ = memory and interactions to use in the memory test on day 2. All participants were unaware
quotient (see text for details of the test instruments). they would be asked to recall the events on the following day, or of the experimenter
taking notes.
During this first day, each participant also performed the scene construction task
Whole brain structural MRI scans were obtained for each participant using a 1.5T modelled on that of Hassabis et al. (2007b); see also Maguire et al., 2010). Each par-
Siemens Avanto Scanner, with a T1-weighted 3D FLASH sequence with the follow- ticipant was tested individually and sat facing the experimenter. The session was
ing parameters: repetition time 11 ms, echo time 4.94 ms, flip angle 15◦ ; matrix size digitally recorded. The instructions were explained to the participant and exam-
224 × 256; in-plane resolution 1 mm × 1 mm; partition thickness 1 mm; 176 sagittal ples were given. During this practice session, the experimenter continued until they
partitions in the third dimension; acquisition time 5.34 min; no gap. The scans were were satisfied that the participant had fully understood what was required of them.

Fig. 2. The top panels show MRI T1 images from an example control participant (C) and an age and gender matched patient (P), with the hippocampi outlined in green. The
lower panel shows the mean (±standard error) left and right hippocampal volumes for control and patient groups.
1846 J.M. Cooper et al. / Neuropsychologia 49 (2011) 1843–1850

Scenarios were chosen that would be suitable for school-aged children (e.g. camp- Table 1
ing, kite-flying); they were neither fantasy-based nor too similar to events that had Recall of autobiographical events.
happened to them in the past. When participants constructed an imagined scene,
Measure Mean (SD)
they rated how similar it was to actual memory or part of a memory on a scale of 1
(nothing like a memory) . . . 5 (very like a memory). Only those scenes that scored Patients (n = 20) Controls (n = 12)
1 or 2 on this scale were considered for inclusion (mean of final 10 scenarios: con-
trols 1.54; patients 1.19). For each scenario a short description was read aloud by Event description 9.05 (1.64) 9.58 (0.67)
the experimenter from a prepared script (e.g. “Imagine you are by a campfire in Location/time 6.10 (1.94)* 7.67 (2.71)
the mountains”) and the participant was to close their eyes and vividly imagine the Episodic details 3.70 (2.66)* 5.83 (2.66)
situation from the cue and describe it out loud in as much detail as possible. Par- Scorer accuracy rating 5.95 (2.10)* 7.77 (1.03)
ticipants were told not to recall personal memories but create something that was *
Significantly different to controls.
completely new. They were told to verbalise their imagined experience until it came
to a natural end or they felt no more details could be given. The experimenter could
use general probes to encourage the participants (e.g. “can you see anything else?”), occurred in all cases). Finally, a scorer rated how well they felt
however it was strictly prohibited to introduce any idea, concept, entity or detail the details provided by the participant reflected the actual event
that had not already been mentioned by the participant. After each scenario was
recorded, participants were asked to rate it according to a number of different cri-
that had taken place on the previous day. This accuracy rating was
teria (see Section 3). At various points during the trial and before the post-scenario between 0 (indicating the construction was completely devoid of
ratings, the experimenter confirmed that the participant was still familiar with the details pertaining to the event with no sense of recollection) and 10
task instructions, the scenario in question, and the scenario that had been created. (indicating an extremely rich and vivid recollection). Table 1 shows
On day 2, each participant was asked to recall out loud, one at a time, and in as
the mean scores on the autobiographical recall measures.
much detail as possible, each of the ten events from the previous day in response to
short verbal cues (e.g. “recall choosing a snack”). Also on day 2 they were required to Patients and controls provided similar event descriptions
recall out loud in as much detail as possible the ten imagined scenarios from the day (F = 0.76; P = 0.39). By contrast, controls were significantly bet-
before. All descriptions of imagined events on day 1 and real and imagined events on ter at recalling the specific information that located an event in
day 2 were recorded, anonymised and then transcribed (blindly) by a professional time and place (F = 4.32; P = 0.047). Moreover, patients were sig-
transcription company.
nificantly poorer than controls at recalling the detailed episodic
2.3. Scoring information that made an event unique (F = 5.16; P = 0.031). Com-
bined with the significantly poorer accuracy ratings ascribed to
Scoring is described in the Section 3. As some scores were based on the experi- their recall (F = 6.64; P = 0.015), the patients’ impaired recollec-
menter’s ratings, and the primary experimenter was not blind to participant status, tion of spatiotemporal and episodic details confirms that the
four subjects were selected at random (2 patients and 2 controls; 12% of the data)
and their data scored by a second experimenter who was blind to participant iden-
children exposed to neonatal hypoxia/ischaemia and concomitant
tity. Cronbach’s Alpha was used to assess inter-rater reliability. To summarise, all hippocampal damage had a clear deficit in autobiographical event
experimental measures were above 0.7 (range 0.855–0.921) which is considered to recall.
indicate reliability (de Vaus, 2002; Gliem & Gliem, 2003). There were no significant
differences in reliability for subjective ratings and content measures.
3.2. Scene construction
2.4. Statistical analysis
Having confirmed that autobiographical event memory recall
The two groups [patients (n = 21) and controls (n = 12)] were compared using was significantly impaired in the patients, we then examined their
a range of statistical tests: t-tests, analysis of covariance with age as a continu-
ability to imagine fictitious scenarios. Their descriptions of newly
ous variable, where appropriate non-parametric Mann–Whitney U test, and partial
correlations controlling for participant age; a significance threshold of P < 0.05 was constructed experiences created on day 1 were analysed in terms of
applied throughout. As described above, there were unequal numbers in the two three components: content, participant ratings and spatial coher-
groups. In order to verify that this did not influence the results, we selected 12 ence. These components had previously been identified by Hassabis
patients at random and compared them with the 12 control participants on the et al. (2007b) when assessing scene construction in healthy and
experimental recall measures and the scene construction task. The same pattern of
amnesic adults (see also Maguire et al., 2010). A similar scoring
results that we report in the Section 3 was found, and at the same statistical thresh-
old as the main analyses, including the significant correlations with hippocampal protocol was employed here with some adjustments to suit paedi-
volume. atric participants (see Section 2.2). See Table 2 for the mean scores
on the scene construction measures.
3. Results
3.2.1. Content
3.1. Recall of autobiographical events Each scenario description was segmented into a set of state-
ments. Every statement was then classified as belonging to one of
Although the patients’ selective memory problems had been four main categories: the number of entities present (EP), sensory
identified on baseline neuropsychological assessments, we first descriptions (SD), thoughts, emotions and actions (TEA) and spatial
wanted to establish how they would perform on a novel natural- references (SPA). Hassabis et al. (2007b) found that the production
istic autobiographical memory recall task. Having been exposed of 7 details per category was an optimal reflection of performance
to events on day 1, recall for these autobiographical episodes was
assessed on day 2. Note that one patient, while following the pro- Table 2
tocol of day 1, was not available to test on day 2 and so recall on Performance on the scene construction task.
day 2 was examined in 20 patients.
Measure Mean (SD)
Recall of each of the ten autobiographical events was assessed
using three criteria (each scoring 1 or 0 points). First, we assessed Patients (n = 21) Controls (n = 12)
whether a participant could give an overall description of the event Content
in response to a retrieval cue (e.g. “recall choosing a snack”). In Spatial references 3.35 (1.41) 4.01 (1.44)
order to assess specific episodic details, two other criteria were Entities present 5.92 (0.92) 6.40 (0.50)
Sensory descriptions 5.34 (1.36) 5.97 (1.46)
considered. Spatiotemporal information about the location and/or Thoughts, emotions, actions 4.53 (1.34) 5.56 (1.28)
time of the event, and specific details of the episode that could Participant ratings
not be inferred from the retrieval cue (this included details such Sense of presence 1.31 (0.28) 1.16 (0.17)
as thoughts, actions or dialogues that occurred during the event, Perceived salience 4.18 (0.64) 4.00 (0.85)
Spatial coherence index 3.64 (1.44) 4.31 (1.33)
which were noted covertly by the experimenter at the time, and
J.M. Cooper et al. / Neuropsychologia 49 (2011) 1843–1850 1847

Fig. 3. Example transcripts from the scene construction task on day 1.

while ensuring that those with more circuitous descriptions were Finally, we also assessed the difficulty of scene construction.
not unfairly advantaged. Thus, the score for each detail category Each participant was asked to give a rating between 1 and 5 (1
was capped at a maximum of 7 (of note, the results were highly – ‘no difficulty’ . . . 5 – ‘it was very difficult’) on the completion of
similar when the uncapped data were used). Compared to controls, each scenario. Controls and patients did not differ (means: controls
patients produced a similar amount of each statement type and 1.88; patients 1.93; F = 0.25, P = 0.62), with both groups finding the
there were no significant group differences (EP F = 2.63, P = 0.114; task easy to perform.
SD F = 2.41, P = 0.131; TEA F = 4.04, P = 0.054; SPA F = 1.82, P = 0.187).
See Fig. 3 for example scenario descriptions. 3.3. Recall of imagined scenarios

3.2.2. Participant ratings Having observed a clear difference in the patients between their
On completion of each imagined construction, participants were impaired autobiographical memory recall and their intact ability
asked to rate their perceived salience from 1 to 5 (1 – ‘couldn’t to imagine fictitious scenarios, we then wondered how they would
really see anything’ . . . 5 – ‘extremely vivid’) to determine how perform when asked to recall the imagined scenarios. Therefore, as
vivid a scene had been in their mind while they were imagining with the autobiographical events experienced on day 1 and tested
it. They were also asked to rate their sense of presence from 0 to on day 2, the imagined scenarios constructed on day 1 were also
2 (0 – ‘did not feel like I was there at all’ . . . 2 – ‘felt like I was tested on day 2 (see Table 3).
really there’). There was no difference between the controls and In the first instance, the recalled scenarios were scored for con-
the patients on either self-rated measure, with both groups feeling tent in the same way as during their initial creation, i.e., in terms of
they were present within the scenario and that the scene was vivid entities present (EP), sensory descriptions (SD), thoughts, emotions
in their mind (salience F = 0.65, P = 0.43; presence F = 2.69, P = 0.11). and actions (TEA), and spatial references (SPA). The groups did not
differ significantly on any category (EP F = 2.69, P = 0.11; SD F = 0.18,
P = 0.18; TEA F = 0.16, P = 0.15; SPA F = 1.88, P = 0.18). On the face of
3.2.3. Spatial coherence index
it, it appeared that perhaps the memories of these imagined sce-
As part of the feedback on each scenario, participants were
narios were preserved while the autobiographical memories were
presented with a set of 12 statements each providing a possi-
not. However, our next two measures revealed that this was not
ble qualitative description of the newly constructed experience.
the case.
Participants were instructed to indicate the statements they felt
The first measure was a reproducibility rating which was
accurately described their construction. They were free to iden-
designed to examine the extent to which a recalled scenario
tify as many or as few statements as they thought appropriate. Of
reflected that which was imagined in the original construction.
the 12 statements, 4 of them directly suggested a scene was frag-
For each scenario, the scorer gave a rating between 0 and 10 (0
mented, 4 directly suggested a scene was coherent, and 4 spoke
– ‘no similarity to original construction’ . . . 10 – ‘virtually iden-
to other aspects of the scene not necessarily related to its coher-
ence – but which were more likely to be chosen if the scene
was coherent. Thus, the number of direct statements relating to Table 3
coherence/fragmentation was the same. No participant chose all 12 Recall of imagined scenarios.

statements, nor did they select directly contradictory statements. Measure Mean (SD)
One point was awarded for each integrated statement selected and
Patients (n = 20) Controls (n = 12)
one point taken away for each fragmented statement. This yielded
a score between −4 and +8 that was then normalised around zero Content
Spatial references 2.12 (0.95) 2.60 (1.13)
to give final Spatial Coherence Index score ranging between −6
Entities present 4.82 (1.45) 5.57 (1.09)
(totally fragmented) and +6 (completely integrated). Any construc- Sensory descriptions 3.29 (1.73) 3.93 (1.14)
tion with a negative Spatial Coherence Index was considered to be Thoughts, emotions, actions 3.1 (1.37) 3.93 (1.74)
incoherent and fragmented. Both groups produced coherent con- Scorer reproducibility rating 5.59 (1.68)* 6.81 (1.06)
New information 0.56 (0.56)* 0.26 (0.24)
structions, and there was no significant difference between the
*
groups (F = 1.23, P = 0.28). Significantly different to controls.
1848 J.M. Cooper et al. / Neuropsychologia 49 (2011) 1843–1850

tical to the original construction’). The reproducibility rating for rich, vivid and spatially coherent. Interestingly, despite having
the patients was significantly lower (F = 4.73, P = 0.038) than that a significant autobiographical memory deficit, the children with
for the controls. This suggests that during recall, the patients were hippocampal damage were unimpaired at constructing imagined
not necessarily recalling the original imagined scenario, but were experiences. This is in clear contrast to patients with adult-onset
creating a variant of the original or a new scenario. This finding hippocampal damage and amnesia, who are typically impaired at
was supported by another measure – a count of the number of scene construction (Hassabis et al., 2007b).
elements that truly deviated from the original constructed scene. Our novel test, permitting the naturalistic experiencing of real-
This showed that the patient group included more new informa- world events in an incidental fashion, and their subsequent probing
tion in the ‘recalled’ scenarios than controls (F = 4.05, P = 0.05). As to in a surprise memory test allowed us to confirm that by (mean) age
the introduction of new material when recalling autobiographical 12, recall of recent autobiographical memories is well developed.
events, the difference between the group scores only approached While severe hypoxia/ischaemia and concomitant hippocampal
statistical significance (F = 3.48; P = 0.072), suggesting that genera- damage in children still permits the generation of an overall
tion of new material was specific to scene construction. description of an autobiographical event, this lacks a spatiotempo-
ral context and the specific details that make it truly episodic and
precisely accurate. This is additional evidence that the hippocam-
3.4. Relationships between the variables
pus plays a crucial role in episodic recollection (Maguire, 2001;
Mishkin, Vargha-Khadem, & Gadian, 1998; Nadel & Moscovitch,
In the patient group we also examined whether there were any
1997; Tulving, 2002; Vargha-Khadem et al., 2003). Our finding of
correlations between performance on our autobiographical mem-
significant correlations between hippocampal volume and mea-
ory test and scores on our scene construction task, and hippocampal
sures from our autobiographical memory task, with poorer memory
volumes and the standardized memory quotient (MQ, Children’s
associated with decreased hippocampal volume, underscores this
Memory Scale). Mean hippocampal volumes in the patients corre-
further. The performance of the children with hypoxia/ischaemia
lated positively with measures from our autobiographical memory
was in fact similar to younger healthy children reported by Bauer
recall task – event description scores (right hippocampus r = 0.65,
et al. (2007). They found that between the ages of 7 and 10 years
P = 0.002; left hippocampus r = 0.55, P = 0.012), and accuracy ratings
gist or fact-based information predominated during recall of per-
(right hippocampus r = 0.52, P = 0.018; left hippocampus r = 0.46,
sonal events, with few details pertaining to the spatiotemporal
P = 0.043). Thus, the poorer the autobiographical memory, the
context. This suggests that if episodic recollection emerges along
smaller the hippocampal volume. There was no significant relation-
a trajectory, then early hippocampal damage interferes with this
ship between scene construction scores and hippocampal volumes,
development and leads to a plateauing of this ability before the
which is not surprising, given that patients were unimpaired on this
onset of adolescence.
task and variance was low.
It is all the more surprising, therefore, that the children with
Measures from our autobiographical memory test correlated
hippocampal damage and impaired memory for personal episodes
positively with the MQ (location/time r = 0.47, P < 0.045; accuracy
were able to imagine fictitious experiences. Moreover, when asked
rating r = 0.55, P < 0.014). Similarly, the reproducibility score from
to recall previously created scenarios, they in fact created essen-
the ‘recall’ of imagined scenarios on day 2 also correlated with the
tially new constructions, thus eschewing their memory recall
MQ score (r = 0.53, P < 0.021). Thus the higher the MQ, the better
problems, instead relying on their intact ability to construct. This
the score for recall of real-world events and for recall of imagined
ability could help the children to ‘fill-in’ the gaps in their memory
scenarios, demonstrating consistency across the different memory
and may make it more difficult to detect the extent of their memory
measures.
problems in daily life. Thus in a large group of children who had
There were no significant correlations between our autobi-
experienced neonatal hypoxia/ischaemia with concomitant hip-
ographical memory measures and performance on the scene
pocampal damage, scene construction appears unimpaired. This
construction task. Again, this is not surprising, given that patients
accords with the previous case report of patient Jon, with develop-
were significantly impaired on the former, but were unimpaired on
mental amnesia, who had a similar profile of memory impairment
the latter.
and intact scene construction (Maguire et al., 2010). We therefore
conclude that intact scene construction is a consistent and reliable
4. Discussion feature of bilateral hippocampal damage sustained in childhood,
and developmental amnesia. This is clearly different to patients
While there has been a good deal of research on autobiograph- with hippocampal damage and amnesia acquired in adulthood,
ical memory in adults (Cabeza & St. Jacques, 2007; Maguire, 2001; where in the main they are significantly impaired at imagining fic-
Spreng et al., 2009; Svoboda et al., 2006), much less attention has titious experiences. As noted earlier, findings in one DA patient, HC
been focused on autobiographical memory in school-aged children (Kwan et al., 2010) seem to run contrary to the other 22 patients (Jon
(Bauer et al., 2007; Peterson, Grant, & Boland, 2005; Picard et al., and the current paediatric patients), and work is currently under-
2010; Piolino et al., 2007; Van Abbema & Bauer, 2005), despite its way to examine the anomalous finding in this patient further (E.A.
importance for development and subsequent independent living. Maguire personal communication).
Here we devised a novel naturalistic autobiographical memory task The question that naturally arises is what underpins the
and found that school-aged children (mean age of 12 years) could patients’ intact scene construction? Jon described his ability to
recall the events in detail, including spatiotemporal information construct scenes as something that he had worked on over the
and specific episodic details. This was in contrast to a group of years (Maguire et al., 2010). Thus it could be that the patients were
children with neonatal hypoxia/ischaemia and bilateral hippocam- using strategies to enhance their ability, or compensatory strate-
pal volume reduction in whom recall was impaired, with specific gies to circumvent their problem. We think it unlikely, however,
details of the episodes lost. that the children (some of whom were as young as 9 years old)
While there is a relatively limited literature on autobiographi- were engaged in active or conscious strategies, as none of them
cal memory in school-aged children, even less is known formally declared this when probed.
about their ability to imagine fictitious experiences, despite its The children, as in previously documented cases of developmen-
clear importance in planning future actions. We found that tal amnesia, had developed IQs in the normal range. This shows that
healthy school-aged children could construct scenarios which were their semantic knowledge base was intact, and perhaps this was
J.M. Cooper et al. / Neuropsychologia 49 (2011) 1843–1850 1849

sufficient to permit successful scene construction. The fact that the Addis, D. R., Pan, L., Vu, M. A., Laiser, N., & Schacter, D. L. (2009). Constructive episodic
children were able to acquire semantic knowledge in the face of sig- simulation of the future and the past: Distinct subsystems of a core brain net-
work mediate imagining and remembering. Neuropsycholgia, 47, 2222–2238.
nificant hippocampal damage sustained very early in life suggests Adlam, A. L., Vargha Khadem, F., Mishkin, M., & de Haan, M. (2005). Deferred
there is a mechanism for such learning that is not solely dependent imitation of action sequences in developmental amnesia. Journal of Cognitive
on episodic/autobiographical memory. It may be that this mecha- Neuroscience, 17, 240–248.
Adlam, A. L., Malloy, M., Mishkin, M., & Vargha-Khadem, F. (2009). Dissociation
nism is also able to sustain scene construction, although the nature between recognition and recall in developmental amnesia. Neuropsychologia,
of such a mechanism is still debated (Baddeley, Vargha-Khadem, & 47, 2207–2210.
Mishkin, 2001; Mishkin et al., 1998; Squire & Zola, 1998; Tulving Aggleton, J. P., Vann, S. D., Denby, C., Dix, S., Mayes, A. R., Roberts, N., & Yonelinas,
A. P. (2005). Sparing of the familiarity component of recognition memory in a
& Markowitsch, 1998). If this is the case, then it will be impor-
patient with hippocampal pathology. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1810–1823.
tant in future studies to ascertain how more semantically based Andelman, F., Hoofien, D., Goldberg, I., Aizenstein, O., & Neufeld, M. Y. (2010). Bilat-
scene construction can be differentiated from scene construction eral hippocampal lesion and a selective impairment of the ability for mental
time travel. Neurocase, 16, 426–435.
that involves true visualisation. Interestingly, this distinction can
Atance, C. M., & Jackson, L. K. (2009). The development and coherence of future
and has been readily made by adult patients with amnesia, who oriented behaviors during the preschool years. Journal of Experimental Child
are very clear that they cannot visualise a scene, even though they Psychology, 102, 379–391.
are able to produce semantically relevant content (Hassabis et al., Atance, C. M., & Meltoff, A. N. (2006). Preschoolers’ current desires warp their choices
for the future. Psychological Science, 17, 583–587.
2007b). Given that DA patients sustained such early hippocampal Baddeley, A., Vargha-Khadem, F., & Mishkin, M. (2001). Preserved recognition in
damage, and may never have known what it is like to truly imagine a case of developmental amnesia: Implications for the acquisition of semantic
a scene or event, then in the absence of any comparator, it is per- memory? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 357–369.
Bauer, P. J., Burch, M. M., Scholin, S. E., & Güler, O. E. (2007). Using cue words to inves-
haps not surprising that they consider their scene constructions to tigate the distribution of autobiographical memories in childhood. Psychological
be coherent. Science, 18, 910–916.
While semantic memory may be the basis of intact scene con- Botzung, A., Denkova, E., & Manning, L. (2008). Experiencing past and future personal
events: Functional neuroimaging evidence on the neural bases of mental time
struction in our paediatric patients, Maguire et al. (2010) suggested travel. Brain and Cognition, 66, 202–212.
that Jon’s ability to construct imagined scenarios may be supported Buckner, R. L., & Carroll, D. C. (2007). Self-projection and the brain. Trends in the
by some functioning of his residual hippocampal tissue. Although Cognitive Sciences, 11, 49–57.
Cabeza, R., & St. Jacques, P. (2007). Functional neuroimaging of autobiograpical
his hippocampi are reduced in volume by 50%, they were never-
memory. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11, 219–227.
theless activated during fMRI when he recalled some of the few Cohen, N. J., & Eichenbaum, H. (1993). Memory, amnesia and the hippocampal system.
autobiographical memories that he retains. Interestingly, the one Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
de Vaus, D. A. (2002). Surveys in social research. St. Leonards, N.S.W: Allen and Unwin.
patient with adult-acquired hippocampal damage and amnesia that
Gliem, J. A., & Gliem, R. R. (2003). Calculating, interpreting, and reporting Cron-
has intact scene construction (P01 – Hassabis et al., 2007b, also bach’s Alpha reliability coefficient for likert-type scales. In Midwest Research to
known as KN – Aggleton et al., 2005; McKenna and Gerhand, 2002), Practice Conference in Adult, Continugin, and Community Education Ohio, USA,
showed fMRI activity in the residual tissue of his right hippocam- (pp. 82–88).
Grant, J. B., & Suddendorf, T. (2010). Young children’s ability to distinguish past
pus during a memory task. It is therefore possible that the children and future changes in physical and mental states. British Journal of Experimental
with hippocampal damage in the present study retained some func- Psychology, 28, 853–870.
tionality in their residual hippocampal tissue which was sufficient Hassabis, D., & Maguire, E. A. (2007). Deconstructing episodic memory with con-
struction. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11, 299–306.
to support scene construction, but not enough for accurate and Hassabis, D., & Maguire, E. A. (2009). The construction system of the brain. Philo-
detailed autobiographical memory. sophical Transaction of the Royal Society London B Series: Biological Science, 364,
In this study we have described the effect of early hippocam- 1263–1271.
Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D., & Maguire, E. A. (2007). Using imagination to understand
pal damage on autobiographical memory and scene construction the neural basis of episodic memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 14365–14374.
in children. It is clear that future studies assessing patients such Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D., Vann, S. D., & Maguire, E. A. (2007). Patients with hip-
as these [and indeed Jon (Maguire et al., 2010) and P01 (Hassabis pocampal amnesia cannot imagine new experiences. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences USA, 104, 1726–1731.
et al., 2007b)] should involve fMRI during scene construction in
Huizinga, M., Dolan, C. V., & van der Molen, M. W. (2006). Age-related change in
order to establish if residual hippocampal tissue is active, whether executive function: Developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neu-
additional brain areas are recruited or up-regulated, or a com- ropsycholgia, 44, 2017–2036.
Klein, S. B., Loftus, J., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (2002). Memory and temporal experience: The
pletely different set of brain regions is engaged compared with
effects of episodic memory loss on an amnesic patient’s ability to remember the
controls. To our knowledge, there are no published fMRI studies of past and the future. Social Cognition, 20, 353–379.
autobiographical memory/imagining fictitious experiences in chil- Kwan, D., Carson, N., Addis, D. R., & Rosenbaum, R. S. (2010). Deficits in past
dren with hippocampal pathology, or in fact any involving healthy remembering extend to future imagining in a case of developmental amnesia.
Neuropsycholgia, 48, 3179–3186.
children. This is surprising, given that understanding how such Lehto, J. E., Juujärvi, P., Kooistra, L., & Pulkkinen, L. (2003). Dimensions of executive
critical cognitive functions develop in the first place may provide functioning: Evidence from children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
key insights into their functional neuroanatomy, and we urge the 21, 59–80.
Maguire, E. A. (2001). Neuroimaging studies of autobiographical event memory.
deployment of fMRI in this developmental context. Philosophical Transaction of the Royal Society London B Series: Biological Science,
356, 1441–1451.
Maguire, E. A., Vargha-Khadem, F., & Mishkin, M. (2001). The effects of bilateral hip-
Acknowledgements pocampal damage on fMRI regional activations and interactions during memory
retrieval. Brain, 124, 1156–1170.
We thank the participants and their families for giving their time Maguire, E. A., Vargha-Khadem, F., & Hassabis, D. (2010). Imagining fictitious and
future experiences: Evidence from developmental amnesia. Neuropsychologia,
so generously.
48, 3187–3192.
Funding: This work was supported by the Wellcome Trust (EAM), Maguire, E.A. & Hassabis, D. (2011). Role of the hippocampus in imagination and
and MRC programme grant number G0300117 (FVK, DGG, JMC). future thinking. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 108, E39.
McKenna, P., & Gerhand, S. (2002). Preserved semantic learning in an amnesic
patient. Cortex, 38, 37–58.
References McLean, K. C. (2005). Late adolescent identity development: Narrative meaning
making and memory telling. Developmental Psychology, 41, 683–691.
Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). Remembering the past and imagin- Mishkin, M., Vargha-Khadem, F., & Gadian, D. G. (1998). Amnesia and the organiza-
ing the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction tion of the hippocampal system. Hippocampus, 8, 212–216.
and elaboration. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1363–1377. Nadel, L., & Moscovitch, M. (1997). Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and
Addis, D. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). Constructive episodic simulation: Temporal dis- the hippocampal complex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 7, 217–227.
tance and detail of past and future events modulate hippocampal engagement. O’Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. (1978). The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: Clarendon
Hippocampus, 18, 227–237. Press.
1850 J.M. Cooper et al. / Neuropsychologia 49 (2011) 1843–1850

Okuda, J., Fujii, T., Ohtake, H., Tsukiura, T., Tanji, K., Suzuki, K., Kawashima, R., Fukuda, Squire, L. R., van der Horst, A. S., McDuff, S. G., Frascino, J. C., Hopkins, R. O., &
H., Itoh, M., & Yamadori, A. (2003). Thinking of the future and past: The roles of Mauldin, K. N. (2010). Role of the hippocampus in remembering the past and
the frontal pole and the medial temporal lobes. Neuroimage, 19, 1369–1380. imagining the future. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 107,
Pasupathi, M., & Wainryb, C. (2010). On telling the whole story: Facts and inter- 19044–19048.
pretations in autobiographical memory narratives from childhood through Suddendorf, T., Nielsen, M., & von Gehlen, R. (2011). Children’s capacity to remember
midadolescence. Developmental Psychology, 46, 735–746. a novel problem and to secure its future solution. Developmental Science, 14,
Peterson, C., Grant, V. V., & Boland, L. D. (2005). Childhood amnesia in children and 26–33.
adolescents: Their earliest memories. Memory, 13, 622–637. Summerfield, J. J., Hassabis, D., & Maguire, E. A. (2009). Cortical midline involvement
Picard, L., Reffuveille, I., Eustache, F., & Piolino, P. (2010). Development of autonoetic in autobiographical memory. Neuroimage, 44, 1188–1200.
autobiographical memory in school-age children: Genuine age effect or devel- Summerfield, J. J., Hassabis, D., & Maguire, E. A. (2010). Differential engagement of
opment of basic cognitive abilities? Consciousness and Cognition, 18(4), 864–876, brain regions within a ‘core’ network during scene construction. Neuropsycholo-
864–876. gia, 48, 1501–1509.
Piolino, P., Hisland, M., Ruffeveille, I., Matuszewski, V., Jambaqué, I., & Eustache, F. Svoboda, E., McKinnon, M. C., & Levine, B. (2006). The functional neuroanatomy of
(2007). Do school-age children remember or know the personal past? Conscious- autobiographical memory: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2189–2208.
ness and Cognition, 16, 84–101. Szpunar, K. K., Watson, J. M., & McDermott, K. B. (2007). Neural substrates of envi-
Piolino, P., Desgranges, B., & Eustache, F. (2009). Episodic autobiographical memo- sioning the future. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 104,
ries over the course of time: Cognitive, neuropsychological and neuroimaging 642–647.
findings. Neuropsycholgia, 47, 2314–2329. Tulving, E., & Markowitsch, H. J. (1998). Episodic and declarative memory: Role of
Rosenbaum, R. S., Köhler, S., Schacter, D. L., Moscovitch, M., Westmacott, R., Black, the hippocampus. Hippocampus, 8, 198–204.
S. E., Gao, F., & Tulving, E. (2005). The case of K.C.: Contributions of a memory- Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychol-
impaired person to memory theory. Neuropsycholgia, 43, 989–1021. ogy, 53, 25.
Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2007). The cognitive neuroscience of constructive Van Abbema, D. L., & Bauer, P. J. (2005). Autobiographical memory in mid-
memory: Remembering the past and imagining the future. Philosophical Trans- dle childhood: Recollections of the recent and distant past. Memory, 13,
actions of the Royal Society London B Biological Sciences, 362, 773–786. 829–845.
Spreng, R. N., Mar, R. A., & Kim, A. S. (2009). The common neural basis of autobi- Vargha-Khadem, F., Gadian, D. G., Watkins, K. E., Connelly, A., Van Paesschen, W.,
ographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind and the default & Mishkin, M. (1997). Differential effects of early hippocampal pathology on
mode: A quantitative meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, episodic and semantic memory. Science, 277, 376–380.
489–510. Vargha-Khadem, F., Salmond, C. H., Watkins, K. E., Friston, K. J., Gadian, D. G., &
Squire, L. R., & Zola, S. M. (1998). Episodic memory, semantic memory, and amnesia. Mishkin, M. (2003). Developmental amnesia: Effect of age at injury. Proceedings
Hippocampus, 8, 205–211. of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 100, 10055–10060.

You might also like