You are on page 1of 12

Evolution of vocabulary in the

poetry of Sylvia Plath


............................................................................................................................................................

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/dsh/article/32/3/660/2669772 by Universidade de Bras�lia user on 09 September 2023


Fabian B. Wadsworth, Jérémie Vasseur and David E. Damby
Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, Germany
.......................................................................................................................................
Abstract
In this study, we provide a quantitative analysis of the temporal evolution of
word choice across the career of Sylvia Plath, a poet who suffered from mental
illness, which culminated in her suicide. Her work provides an excellent case
study for temporally indexed changes in vocabulary due to well-dated records of
her productivity. We use simple functions to discriminate bulk trends in her
vocabulary and classify words into those that accelerate, decelerate, or represent
consistency in usage. Overall, we find that Plath’s vocabulary was exceptionally
consistent throughout her career from 1956 to 1963; however, we observe relative
changes in the frequency of words used. Two examples of this evolution are in
her use of colours and personal pronouns. Namely, we note a consistently
Correspondence:
increasing preference towards black and white above other colour words from
Fabian B. Wadsworth, 1956 to 1963 and, in contrast, we observe a marked transition from dominant use
Ludwig-Maximilians- of third-person feminine to dominant use of first-person singular in her later
Universität, Theresienstr. work. We find that the most dynamic changes in word use occurred between
41/III, 80333 Munich, March and May 1959 and we tie these observations to events recorded in Plath’s
Germany.
E-mail:
journal entries. We propose that our function-discrimination approach offers a
fabian.wadsworth@gmail vital tool in the objective analysis of word-use for understanding style evolution
.com and for correlation with a poet’s life events that may influence their work.
.................................................................................................................................................................................

1 Introduction Hypothesis regarding the universal positivity of lan-


guage (Dodds et al. 2014) and how global samples of
The effect language has on our thinking and how texts can contain fundamental patterns of social,
our thinking shapes our language remain highly historical, and linguistic significance (Lieberman et
contentious topics (Pinker 1994; Chomsky 2002; al. 2007). While these efforts acknowledge that a
Deutscher 2011; Whorf et al. 2012). However, the quantitative approach surely smears nuances only
role of word choice and metaphor are known to found by detailed textual analysis, it has facilitated
reflect aspects of our internal motives, conceptua- the quest for the largest scale laws of evolving lan-
lizations, and confidence (Pennebaker et al. 2003; guage, which may shape our understanding of the
Lakoff and Johnson 2008). Therefore, in recent relationship between language and mind.
years, much effort has been made to explore what The written language of prominent creative
the quantification of (often very large) corpora of people has been postulated to differ with quality
textual information can tell us about the authors, or of mental health of the individual author (Silvia
society and humanity in general (Lieberman et al. and Kaufman 2010). Moreover, poets are thought
2007; Michel et al. 2011; Kloumann et al. 2012; to have a greater than average propensity for mental
Dodds et al. 2014). These efforts have yielded fruit- illness (Ludwig 1995; Kaufman 2001; Kaufman and
ful results, such as confirmation of the Pollyanna Baer 2002; Kaufman and Sexton 2006; Silvia and

Digital Scholarship in the Humanities, Vol. 32, No. 3, 2017. ß The Author 2016. Published by Oxford University 660
Press on behalf of EADH. All rights reserved. For Permissions, please email: journals.permissions@oup.com
doi:10.1093/llc/fqw026 Advance Access published on 25 July 2016
vocabulary of plath

Kaufman 2010). Often compared with the writings when Sylvia Plath diligently dated her work
of scientists, creative writers (as defined by Djikic et (Fig. 1). A Python script is composed to count the
al. 2006) are repeatedly found to express more occurrence of each word in each poem and to filter
words associated with negative emotional states and remove punctuation and numbers and to con-
than other groups of people (Djikic et al. 2006). vert upper case letters to lower case letters while

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/dsh/article/32/3/660/2669772 by Universidade de Bras�lia user on 09 September 2023


But such studies typically use a collected body of plurals are considered as words distinct from the
work by single authors (Andreasen 1987), ignoring singular equivalent. The resultant data are then pro-
the possibility that mental states and mental health cessed to quantitatively constrain the occurrence of
change drastically over a writer’s lifetime. Sylvia words throughout The Collected. We note that The
Plath is a well-recognized case study for exploring Collected includes unfinished poems detailed in the
the relationship between written work and emo- ‘Notes’ section. Additionally, there is a small add-
tional state as her work is a rich source of evidence itional archive of Juvenalia and poems that remain
for her feelings about her developing situation uncollected in various places worldwide. We do
(Newman 1970; Kroll 1976; Axelrod 1990; Lant consider any poems additional to those that
and Plath 1993; Hargrove 1994; Moses 2007; King appear in The Collected and those that appear in
2011). Especially coupled with the record of journal the analysis by Hargrove (1994) and otherwise we
entries, books, and stories, the written work of Plath do not consider these additional poems. This is, in
represents a valuable preservation of a writer’s life. part, justified by the fact that the detailed work of
We present a temporally indexed analysis of the time-indexing the possible range of composition
work of Sylvia Plath and provide a range of analyt- dates has only been applied to The Collected and a
ical tools to form a view of her changing word- few additional poems.
choices toward her suicide. Correct chronicling of First, Plath’s productivity is assessed by consider-
her poems (Hargrove 1994) permits a word-choice ing the word-count as a function of time for each
analysis of her work and subsequent interpretation poem in the period studied (1956–63; Fig. 1). The
of time-dependent changes in word use across her error bars represent the uncertainty in the chrono-
career in the context of a huge body of literary criti- logical precision quoted by Hargrove. In subsequent
cism on the poems of Plath. When considered analysis, we use only the chronology by Hargrove, as
alongside the detailed chronicling of her life events the objectivity of the Hughes chronology has been
of Kroll (1976) and others, we propose that trends questioned (Hargrove 1994). Hargrove’s (1994)
in bulk word usage can potentially be tied to the analysis only extends from 1956 to 1959 and so,
broadest changes of circumstance in Plath’s life, for Plath’s late poems, we have to rely on the
while admitting that this is a controversial link to Hughes chronology and the date-indexing of Plath
make (Lant 1993). We conclude that it is the herself, hence both chronologies are here presented
changes in the vocabulary of Sylvia Plath approach- (Fig. 1). We see that the productivity, manifest as a
ing her suicide, rather than the more-static obser- word-count rate, varies in time (Fig. 1). Therefore,
vations of her bulk vocabulary, which can best be we present subsequent temporal evolution data as a
used to assess her declining mental health. function of poem number x rather than absolute
time—which is equivalent to normalizing the
time-axis by the productivity, or poem-rate. Were
2 Materials and Methods we to analyse the poems using a non-normalized
time, all metrics of changing vocabulary are over-
The poems of Sylvia Plath that appear in The shadowed by the productivity signal.
Collected Poems (Plath 1981) (hereafter referred to Second, we take individual words and assess their
as The Collected) are organized into both the usage with normalized time. To do this we define
chronological sequence proposed by Hughes two different mathematical functions, a power law
(1981) and that of Hargrove (1994); the two se- of the form y ¼ ax n and a linear function of the
quences begin in 1956 and converge after 1959, form y ¼ mx þ b. The former case is equivalent to

Digital Scholarship in the Humanities, Vol. 32, No. 3, 2017 661


F. B. Wadsworth et al.

A B

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/dsh/article/32/3/660/2669772 by Universidade de Bras�lia user on 09 September 2023


Fig. 1 The poem-productivity of Sylvia Plath between 1956 and her death in 1963. A – The cumulative number of
poems as a function of time. Although similar here, the Hughes and Hargrove chronologies differ dramatically on the
order of the poems and in subsequent analysis the Hargrove chronology is used exclusively (see text). B – The cumu-
lative number of words written in the poems as a function of time. The histogram refers to the Hargrove chronology
and not the Hughes chronology

the latter case when n ¼ 1, a ¼ m, and b ¼ 0, such threshold-invariant value at a threshold of ¼7


that y ¼ ax or, equivalently, y ¼ mx. Reasonable words (Supplementary Fig. 1). We apply a second
fits to the two regression types yield fit-parameters threshold when considering the regression analysis
that inform the general usage of particular words, such that a word should appear in at least four
thereby allowing us to interpret style evolution poems to be included,  ¼ 4. This is important be-
across the chronology of Plath’s work used herein cause, while a word may occur many times in the
(see §3). In both cases, just two parameters can be corpus, if it appears all in one poem, then we cannot
used to characterize the normalized evolution of differentiate time-dependent behaviour from poem-
Plath’s vocabulary when x is the poem number in to-poem. This results in a subset of the corpus of
chronological order and y is the cumulative fre- 788 words which will be the set used in the subse-
quency of the word in question: the intercept b quent analysis. Nevertheless, when  ¼ 4, there are
and the power-law exponent n. Once the two regres- cases of the most-rare words for which four data
sion types are fit to the data, we can assess the fre- points are being fit, which should be noted when
quency distribution of the fitting parameters a, n, interpreting the distribution of fitting parameters.
m; and b (Fig. 2 and Supplementary Fig. 2). We
then identify individual words that yield best-fit
values of n that are very high, close to unity, or 3 Results and Interpretation
very low (Fig. 3) to group words into those that
accelerate in usage, remain consistent in usage, or 3.1 Trends in the filtered Plath corpus
decelerate in usage. By fitting both types of regression—the linear and
The total number of words in the unfiltered the power-law regressions (see §2)—to the normal-
corpus of Sylvia Plath’s 224 poems is 9,499. The ized evolution of cumulative frequency with nor-
threshold in the number of occurrences we choose malized poem number for these 788 words, we
before we include words in our analysis is defined by find that 0:13  n  17:07, 4:56  b  0:65, 0:29
the variation of the mean of the distribution of  m  5:57. Because n and a or m and b are allowed
fitted n and b, both of which converge to a to vary freely, this permits some limiting

662 Digital Scholarship in the Humanities, Vol. 32, No. 3, 2017


vocabulary of plath

A B

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/dsh/article/32/3/660/2669772 by Universidade de Bras�lia user on 09 September 2023


Fig. 2 The frequency distributions of fitted parameters b and n. A – The frequency distribution of the best-fit value of b
for all words in ¼7. B – The frequency distribution of the best-fit value for n for all words in ¼7. Both distributions
are fit with well by a non-central normal distribution (black curves)

Fig. 3 The temporal evolution of 18 individual words in the Sylvia Plath corpus. A – words that are most-consistent in
usage, B – words that decelerate with time and C – words that accelerate with time

Digital Scholarship in the Humanities, Vol. 32, No. 3, 2017 663


F. B. Wadsworth et al.

interpretations a priori for cases when a particular respectively, we can visualize the evolution of usage
regression is the best-fit to the cumulative frequency of words that are, by this analysis, constrained to be
of a given word as a function of poem number: most consistent over the corpus (Fig. 3A;
Supplementary Tables). Such words include
(1) For a linear regression that closely intercepts
‘great’, ‘lilies’, ‘window’ and ‘hill’, ‘legs’, ‘wings’.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/dsh/article/32/3/660/2669772 by Universidade de Bras�lia user on 09 September 2023


the origin (b  0) or a power-law regression
Similarly, if we take the words that have the
where n  1, the word is consistent in usage
lowest n and highest b, we find the words which
over normalizsed time.
are best described by a decelerating trend of usage
(2) For a linear regression where b  0,the word is
over the corpus (Fig. 3B; Supplementary Tables).
consistent in usage over normalizsed time
Words of such decelerating behaviour include
from a threshold time t0 ¼ –b/m, before
‘bride’, ‘loves’, ‘crying’ and ‘suns’, ‘fierce’, ‘said’.
which it was in less-consistent usage (see
Conversely, if we look for the words that have the
text).
highest n and lowest b, we can find those words that
(3) For a linear regression where b  1, the word
most accelerate in usage over the corpus (Fig. 3C;
is consistent in usage after an initial spike in
Supplementary Tables). For this last subset of accel-
usage at low value of normalized time.
erating words, we can qualitatively see that for some
(4) For a power-law regression where n  1, the
of them, such as ‘kill’, ‘husband’, and ‘veil’, the
word is not consistent in usage and exhibits
usage before a critical onset time is zero, which ex-
an acceleration in usage.
(5) For a power-law regression where n  1, the plains why the linear regression fits better than the
word is not consistent in usage and exhibits a power-law description. We use this observation to
deceleration in usage. introduce the concept of a word-onset time t0 ,
which refers to the date before which Plath does
While the fitted values of both a and m contain not make regular use of that particular word. This
information (see Supplementary Fig. 2), we confine critical onset behaviour contrasts with, for example,
our analysis to the b and n. ‘hurts’, ‘marry’, and ‘babies’ which are well
Fig. 2 shows the frequency distribution of fitted described by the power-law behaviour (Fig. 3C).
parameters n and b. We note that both show a good Note that the word-onset time t0 does not refer to
agreement with an off-centre normal distribution. the first occurrence of the word.
The frequency distribution of b values has a mean To look in more detail at the variance of b and n,
and a median of –0.033 and 0.015, respectively, we can plot them as a function of one another,
showing that the average best-fit linear regression whence we find a strong inverse relationship
goes through the origin such that words with an (Fig. 4A). This relationship tells us that our discrim-
onset time t06¼0 are rare (Fig. 2A; see below). The ination into simple categories of consistent (near the
frequency distribution of n values has a median at centre of Fig. 4A), accelerating (top-left quadrant of
1.03, which is equivalent to a linear regression, but a Fig. 4A), and decelerating (bottom right quadrant of
mean of 1.37, showing that the best-fit power laws Fig. 4A) is valid as the data are not spuriously dis-
predict an average accelerating usage of words (Fig. tributed elsewhere. There is significant spread of the
2B). These frequency distributions for best-fit b and data in the accelerating top-left quadrant, which we
n characterize the average evolution of vocabulary in explore further.
the poems of Plath and we find that, in general, her If we now further discriminate the words to
work shows consistency. However, we suggest that it select those that yield best-fit values of n  1 and b
could be at the end-members of these distributions  0 (words that fall in the upper left quadrant of
where the strongest deviations from consistency Fig. 4A), we can look for how variable is the word-
occur. onset time t0 . To do this, we look only at the subset
We show in Fig. 3 some examples from the end- of words for which the linear regression produced a
member and median cases. For example, if we take better agreement with the data than the power law
the words that have the n and b closest to 1 and 0, regression. This is akin to asking if the concept of t0

664 Digital Scholarship in the Humanities, Vol. 32, No. 3, 2017


vocabulary of plath

A B

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/dsh/article/32/3/660/2669772 by Universidade de Bras�lia user on 09 September 2023


Fig. 4 Detailed information in the fitted parameters. A – The variation of the best-fit n parameter with the linear
parameter b for which error bars result from the goodness of fit of each parameter to each word evolution, respectively.
B – The frequency distribution of the word onset time t0 computed from the linear regressions such that t0 ¼ b=m
using only regression parameters for words that accelerate in usage (n > 1; b < 0) and for which the linear regression
proved a better fit to the data than the power law regression. This has been converted back to the equivalent poem-
number at which t0 occurs

is a better model for the data. We find words that By this method we can distinguish consistent,
are typified by this behaviour are again ‘husband’, decelerating, and accelerating word families
‘veil’, and ‘kill’ (Fig. 3C). The frequency distribution (Fig. 3; Supplementary Tables), which we propose
of t0 for these words is given in Fig. 4B (bin size: is a unique and powerful tool for the quantitative
2.68; total number of bins: 60) and shows a bimodal analysis of literature.
distribution with a second peak centred on poem
105–108 (‘A winter ship’, ‘Old ladies home’,
‘Aftermath’, and ‘Magnolia shoals’), written be- 3.2 Trends in word classes: colours and
tween 25 March and 27 May 1959 (Hargrove personal pronouns
1994). This distribution tells us that when we look We can analyse words in an independently defined
at the end-member evolution of words we can subset, such as colour adjectives (Fig. 5) or personal
characterize as accelerating and those which are pronouns (Fig. 6). Usage of trends in such subsets
not significantly used before a critical onset time, can be informative when taken in the context of the
we find that there is a specific era in Plath’s work heritage of literary analysis on Plath’s work (see §4).
where a new group of words appeared in her vo- In these analyses, we take the cumulative frequency
cabulary which, prior to this onset time, were not in of a given word, such as ‘green’ in the case of the
use. This is a fascinating tool of discrimination, as it colour adjectives, relative to the mean usage of all
distinguishes words which represent a shift in style words in that class of words, for example all colour
(see §4) and could, potentially, be correlated with words. This permits us to look at how colour words
any external pressures or influences that might cata- and personal pronouns shift in dominance relative
lyse such a shift. to one another. For the case of colour words, we

Digital Scholarship in the Humanities, Vol. 32, No. 3, 2017 665


F. B. Wadsworth et al.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/dsh/article/32/3/660/2669772 by Universidade de Bras�lia user on 09 September 2023


Fig. 5 Information recorded in the colour adjectives. The Fig. 6. Information recorded in the personal pronouns.
cumulative of the mean usage of all colour words, which The cumulative of the mean usage of all personal pro-
is linear with poem number (dashed line), is subtracted nouns, which is linear with poem number (dashed line),
from the cumulative frequency of an individual colour is subtracted from the cumulative frequency of an indi-
word (labelled) showing the relative dominance of a vidual subset of the pronouns (labelled) showing the rela-
given word over the others in the group. Inset: the pos- tive dominance of a given word over the others in the
ition of these words in the n versus b plot (see Fig. 4) group. Inset: the position of these words in the n versus b
plot (see Fig. 4)
confine our analysis to the nine colours seen in
Fig. 5, as other colours are far rarer. third-person neutral, for example ‘it’, pronouns
The colour words display reasonably consistent (Fig. 6). This shift occurs around poem number
behaviour (n1 and b0; Fig. 5 inset) with ‘black’ 120–150, translating to dates of 22 November 1959
and ‘white’ remaining in high relative usage (Hargrove 1994) and 30 September 1961 (Hughes
throughout Plath’s work. ‘green’ is less consistent, 1970), respectively.
dropping out of dominant relative colour usage As with the analysis of the most accelerating and
toward the end of her work (Fig. 5). ‘Red’ and most decelerating end-members in Plath’s work, we
‘blue’ are close to the mean usage of colour adjec- put forward that quantitatively defined patterns such
tives, and less commonly used colours such as as this can guide the Plath reader to search for mean-
‘brown’ and ‘purple’ are far below the mean usage ing in, for example, the shift in voice from ‘she’ to ‘I’.
of colour adjectives. This may well prove to be an observation bolstered
In contrast with the gentle evolution of colour by detailed literary analysis of The Collected.
usage, there is a marked reorganization of the usage
of personal pronouns. Plath’s dominant pronoun
class at the start of her work is the third-person 4 A Discussion
feminine, typified by the words ‘her’ or ‘she’. In
contrast, toward the end of her work, there is a 4.1 A critique of our approach
dramatic shift to the dominant use of both first- Poetry is perhaps the most distilled and concen-
person singular, for example ‘I’ and ‘me’, and trated form of writing disciplines which undergoes

666 Digital Scholarship in the Humanities, Vol. 32, No. 3, 2017


vocabulary of plath

a variety of methods of composition, revision, and between intent (conscious) and (potentially less-
timings of finalization. A poem’s genesis is often not conscious) style is critical to make for the present
a point event, but rather is the result of influences analysis.
accumulated over, sometimes, many years and mul-
tiple revisions. This has the effect that the compos- 4.2 Broad associations: dynamism in

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/dsh/article/32/3/660/2669772 by Universidade de Bras�lia user on 09 September 2023


ition time (mental or actual) of a single poem may Plath’s poetry and life
be smeared over years with aspects of the construc-
Many scholars have investigated themes and
tion occurring at very different times, eventually all
changes in the work of Plath (Newman 1970;
juxtaposed in the final piece. This uncertainty in the
Perloff 1973). A demarcation is often made between
timing of poem-composition, however, lends cre-
(1) the pre-1956 poems, usually referred to as ‘ju-
dence to the use of blunt tools like the statistical venilia’ (Hughes 1981), (2) the early poems, most of
fits used here to look for word use trends. which appear in The Colossus, (3) the transition
Furthermore, in the case of Plath’s work, and in poems (Perloff 1973), and (4) the late poems,
particular the Ariel poems, we can be confident which appear in Ariel and Winter Trees. We would
from the rapid writing rate (sometimes two poems expect to find dynamism in the word choice
per day in 1962–63; Fig. 1), the proximity to her throughout these periods given that Plath’s poetic
death, and the exact date-indexing of her original career can be shown to transition from less sure-
manuscripts, that these poems were written quickly footed to confident. Evidence for this is explicit in
and with little revision. Therefore, we are confident the journals and letters of Plath wherein we read
that we are capturing an accurate and complete early accounts of anxiety, fearing that she would
overview of the temporal evolution of Plath’s be ‘accused of sentimentality or emotionalism or
word use in these late poems. The early poems, feminine tactics’ (Plath 2007) while later we find
however, clearly include uncertainty in composition that she is bolstered by interactions and successes
date, composition time duration, and the degree to such as she confesses to her mother in a letter
which revisions were important. In this regard, we (1962): ‘My voice is taking shape, coming strong. . .’
simply rely on those poems published in The (Plath 1975). Ted Hughes, in his controversial essay
Collected and the time-indexing provided by on the chronology of Plath’s work, reinforces the
Hargrove (1994) and acknowledge this shortcom- case for dynamism by suggesting that Plath aban-
ing. Wimsatt and Beardsley (1946, 1954) proposed doned a ‘highly disciplined’ and ‘highly intellectual’
that authorial intent as transmitted to a text is not style toward the end of her poetry, thus qualitatively
interpretable from the text alone. Called The inten- testifying to temporal evolution (Hughes 1970).
tional fallacy, it has received much attention and Furthermore, there is mounting evidence, not least
could be extended to criticism of this work because found in the Journals of Plath that these four peri-
we posit links between trends of word use in poems ods in her poetry are related to periods of her life, of
with the changing mental state of the poet. In his her relationship with Ted Hughes and of her mental
landmark 1977 essay The Death of the Author, state (Taussig 2001). Here we give a few examples of
Barthes (1977) tells us that the critical reader owes how what we track as dynamic changes or as repre-
more responsibility to the text than to the author sentations of consistency in Plath’s work compare
and that the mediation of intention by the text and, with previous textual analyses and discuss the rele-
perhaps worse, by the subjectivity of the reader, neg- vance of our methodology.
ates interpretation of intent at all. While we ac- Plath’s poetry is dominated by broad consistency
knowledge the important problem of interpreting in word-choices (Fig. 2). Coarsely, however, words
the intentions of Plath, our approach is independent in the class n  1, b  0 are more common than
of intent; we propose that the trends that we identify those that occur at n  1, b  0 (Figs. 2 and 4).
in word usage over her career are the manifestation This means that Plath’s poetry has more accelerating
of a changing style and overall vocabulary of refer- words than decelerating words. In detail, we have
ence from which Plath drew. This distinction distinguished two types of accelerating behaviour:

Digital Scholarship in the Humanities, Vol. 32, No. 3, 2017 667


F. B. Wadsworth et al.

(1) that best-characterized by the power-law behav- marked ‘onset’ of new words in 1959 (Fig. 4B). Most
iour and exemplified by the word ‘hurts’ and (2) telling, the shift at this time is consistent with the
that best-characterized by the linear behaviour change we observe in the use of personal pronouns.
with a word-onset time and exemplified by, for ex- Plath shifts to dominant use of first-person at the
ample, the word ‘husband’ or ‘kill’. We have also expense of most others in her work. This strikes as

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/dsh/article/32/3/660/2669772 by Universidade de Bras�lia user on 09 September 2023


observed that for this class of accelerating words for consistent with the emphasis of the personal in the
which the linear regression is a better description work of both Anne Sexton and Robert Lowell and
than the power law regression, the computed value with Burton’s (2013) suggestion that this period was
of t0 shows a bimodal frequency distribution (Fig. the most influential in shaping the later Ariel style.
4B). This means that Plath has a smaller subset of The affiliation of Plath with the confessional
accelerating words which came into usage at a poets is somewhat ambivalent (Kinzie 1970; Lant
common time later in her career: constrained to 1993), feels uncomfortable in suggesting Plath’s
25 March and 27 May 1959. poetry is grounded in life experiences. Some of
If we turn to Plath’s journal entries written be- this ambiguity may arise from her ‘. . .ambivalence
tween these dates and in the following months, we about her identity relations with her represented
can establish the broad life events that might be ‘‘I’’’ (Axelrod 1990). If the word ‘I’ is to be intim-
associated with the influx of new word choices ately coupled to an image of the Self, then the def-
into her otherwise consistent poetry. During this inition of Lant (1993) of confessional poetry is apt
period, Plath attended Robert Lowell’s poetry to repeat: ‘the central experience of shattering of the
course at Boston University, conceived Frieda self, and the labour of fitting it together again or
Hughes (June 1959), and from September to finding a new one’. Or, in Plath’s own words, ‘one
November 1959, lived at Yaddo artist’s colony makes of one’s own heavens and hells a few hunks of
(Plath 2007). On 20 May 1959, Plath writes that neatly typewritten paper’ (Plath 2007). Our analysis
she must ‘Go inward’ and ‘. . .write about the shows that the first-person pronouns come to dom-
things of the world with no glazing.’ (Plath 2007, inate the time-evolution landscape of the personal
pp. 484–5). This after she resolved to ‘start with pronouns toward the end of her poetry (Fig. 6).
real things: real emotions, and leave out the baby Perhaps, then, our analysis not only testifies that
gods, the old men of the sea, the thin people, the The Collected records dynamic evolution of word-
knights, the moon-mothers, the mad maudlins, the use but also a shift from an early non-confessional
lorelei, the hermits, and get into me.’ (25 February ‘highly intellectual’ (Hughes 1981) style to a late
1959; Plath 2007, p. 471). For Burton (2013), this more-confessional, immersive style.
period that began in early 1959 represents an inspir- The period in 1959, which is here cited as a quan-
ational moment in the trajectory of Sylvia Plath’s titative shift in style, includes the conception of
work. Burton (2013) further reminds us that Plath Plath’s first child (June 1959). In the journals of
would later describe these developments thus: ‘the this period Plath makes heavy reference to babies,
shift in tone is already history’ (from Plath, The conception, fertility, and birth (Plath 2007). Indeed,
Spoken Word 1963). Hargrove (1994) agrees with the word ‘babies’ is among our here-defined list of
this view and, like Burton (2013), suggests that the the 20 most accelerating words and is associated
influence of Robert Lowell, and to a lesser extent, with a t0 at 28 January 1959. Workers have sug-
Anne Sexton, on Plath at this time was large. Not gested that the poems appearing in the Ariel collec-
least giving her permission to harness in her poetry tion share a theme of fertility (Moses 2007; Wilkins
the personal issues with which she was actively enga- 2010; King 2011). Fertility, blood, and the colour-
ging with her therapist Ruth Beuscher. Hargrove word ‘red’ have been associated with the theme of
(1994) additionally implicates Roethke as a defining the Triple Goddess in the Ariel collection (Wilkins
influence which Plath took to be a proponent of the 2010). Wilkins (2010) refers heavily to the influence
personal in her work. These assertions and life of Robert Graves’s The White Goddess (Graves 1961)
events are in sync with our finding that there is a on the work of Ted Hughes and Sylvia Plath.

668 Digital Scholarship in the Humanities, Vol. 32, No. 3, 2017


vocabulary of plath

Indeed, some have claimed that the gravity of we show here, may be of powerful use as the debate
Hughes influence on the work of Plath is most surrounding words and our mind continues and, in
clearly manifest in their shared appreciation of the particular, to elucidate the link between language
Graves work (Lindop 2003). Graves states ‘. . .the and mental health.
New Moon is the white goddess. . .the Full Moon,

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/dsh/article/32/3/660/2669772 by Universidade de Bras�lia user on 09 September 2023


the red goddess. . .the Old Moon, the black goddess’
(Graves 1961), a delineation of concepts that would 5 Concluding Remarks
be familiar to Plath. Our analysis can be used to
confirm that ‘moon’, ‘white’, ‘black’, and ‘red’ are Approaching her suicide, Plath’s productivity accel-
among the most consistently used words in the erated until she wrote one to two poems per day
Plath corpus with n values close to 1 of 1.15, 1.06, (1962–63; Fig. 1). We use simple mathematical
0.88, and 1.16, respectively. Furthermore, the three functions to dissect Plath’s language into word-
colours ‘white’, ‘black’, and ‘red’ are among the classes of acceleration, consistency, or deceleration.
most-used colours (Fig. 5) and, indeed, the most- We find that in general Plath’s poetry is consistent
used adjectives in general. Wilkins (2010) goes on to in vocabulary (Fig. 2); however, there are end-
draw the investigative thread through associations member exceptions (Fig. 3). Words that accelerate
between these colours, the concept of the moon and in terms of normalized time are words that Plath
the triple goddess, fertility, motherhood, and the used more regularly in her poetry towards her
Graves work. This is a good example of how our death. These words are potentially interpretable in
coarse bulk trend analysis coupled with a detailed terms of her life events and changing influences. The
reading can provide a coherent picture of word sig- tools we develop and examples we provide, bol-
nificance and evolution. stered by detailed reference to the vast body of
Finally, the link between creativity and mental Plath scholarship, may serve to elucidate how
illness remains a controversial one (Weisberg poet’s word choices evolve through time and to
2006; Silvia and Kaufman 2010), with authors contribute to the debate as to a link between creative
claiming no relation (Schlesinger 2009), some rela- people and mental illness.
tion (Weisberg 2006; Sawyer 2011), or very strong
links (Nettle 2001; Kottler 2006; Power et al. 2015).
Runco (1998) explores this with specific focus on Acknowledgements
the work of Sylvia Plath and concludes that the
We are grateful for discussion with James
complexities of Plath’s life and work categorically
Pennebaker, Alexander Todaro, Sabine Kobayter,
preclude a strong objective understanding of her
Robert Parker, Sam Waterman, Ralph Kellas,
suicide, the final manifestation of a long period of
Deryn Rees-Jones, Ann Skea, Rebecca Hearne, and
mental illness. We hope that the tools we present
John Wheeler. And we acknowledge constructive
herein will be of use to those seeking to interpret
comments by anonymous reviewers and editorial
creative people’s mental states as it is surely always
handling by Edward Vanhoutte. The authors are
the case that this is a temporally evolving process,
members of Figure One: An Independent Science
often toward tragedy. As we began by saying, the
Collective and as their inaugural paper, they are
possibility of links between thought and the lan-
grateful to their supporters and friends.
guage we choose to use, or the link between lan-
guage and our mind has fuelled fierce debate
(Pinker 1994; Deutscher 2011; Whorf et al. 2012).
We show that a few tentative links can be made References
between word-use and the facts of Plath’s life Andreasen, N. C. (1987), Creativity and mental illness.
(such as the onset of the use of the word ‘babies’ American journal of Psychiatry, 144(10): 1288–92.
or the shift to the use of ‘I’ over ‘she’). However, we Axelrod, S. G. (1990), Sylvia Plath: The Wound and the Cure
would consider that tools-of-discrimination such as of Words. Baltimore, U.S.: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Digital Scholarship in the Humanities, Vol. 32, No. 3, 2017 669


F. B. Wadsworth et al.

Barthes, R. (1977). The Death of the Author (Heath S., Kottler, J. A. (2006). Divine Madness. San Francisco, CA:
Trans.). London: Fontana. Jossey-Bass.
Burton, S.-J. (2013). An introduction to ‘‘The Boston Kroll, J. (1976). Chapters in a Mythology: The Poetry of
Trio’’: Sylvia Plath with Robert Lowell and Anne Sylvia Plath. New York, NY: Harper & Row.
Sexton. Plath Profiles: An Interdisciplinary Journal for Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M. (2008). Metaphors We Live

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/dsh/article/32/3/660/2669772 by Universidade de Bras�lia user on 09 September 2023


Sylvia Plath Studies, 6: 75–84. By. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Chomsky, N. (2002). Syntactic Structures. Berlin, Lant, K. M. (1993). The big strip tease: female bodies and
Germany: Walter de Gruyter. male power in the poetry of Sylvia Plath. Contemporary
Deutscher, G. (2011). Through the Language Glass: Why Literature, 34(4): 620–69. doi:10.2307/1208804.
the World Looks Different in Other Languages. London, Lieberman, E., Michel, J. –B., Jackson, J., Tang, T., and
U.K: Random House. Nowak, M. A. (2007). Quantifying the evolutionary
Djikic, M, Oatley, K. and Peterson, J. B. (2006). The dynamics of language. Nature, 449(7163): 713–16.
bitter-sweet labor of emoting: the linguistic comparison doi: http://www.nature.com/nature/journal/v449/
of writers and physicists. Creativity Research Journal, n7163/suppinfo/nature06137_S1.html.
18(2): 191–7.
Lindop, G. (2003). Graves and the Goddess: Essays on
Dodds, P. S., Clark, E. M., Desu, S., Frank, M. R., Robert Graves’s The White Goddess. Cranbury, U.S.:
Reagan, A. J., Williams, J. R, Mitchell, L., Harris, K. Susquehanna University Press.
D., Kloumann, I. M., Bagrow, J. P., Megerdoomian,
Ludwig, A. M. (1995). The Price of Greatness: Resolving the
K., McMahon, M. T., Tivnan, B. F., and Danforth, C.
Creativity and Madness Controversy. New York, U.S.:
M. (2014). Human language reveals a universal positiv-
Guilford Press.
ity bias. arXiv Preprint arXiv:14063855.
Michel, J.-B., Shen, Y. K., Aiden, A. P., Veres, A., Gray,
Graves, R. (1961). The White Goddess: A Historical
M. K., Pickett, J. P., Hoiberg, D., Clancy, D., Norvig,
Grammar of Poetic Myth. London, UK: Faber, pp. 511.
P., Orwant, J., Pinker, S., Nowak, M. A., Aiden, E. L.
Hargrove, N. D. (1994). The Journey Toward Ariel: (2011). Quantitative analysis of culture using millions
Sylvia Plath’s Poems of 1956-1959. Lund, Sweden: of digitized books. Science, 331(6014): 176–82.
Lund University Press.
Moses, K. (2007). Sylvia Plath’s voice, In Helle, A. (ed.),
Hughes, T. (1970). Notes on the chronological order of The Unraveling Archive: Essays on Sylvia Plath. Ann
Sylvia Plath’s poems. The Art of Sylvia Plath, 192. Arbor, MI: Michigan UP, pp. 89–121.
Hughes, T. (ed.) (1981). The Collected Poems. Sylvia Nettle, D. (2001). Strong Imagination. New York, NY:
Plath. New York, U.S.: Harper & Row. Oxford University Press.
Kaufman, J. C. (2001). The Sylvia Plath effect: mental Newman, C. (ed.) (1970). The Art of Sylvia. Bloomington
illness in eminent creative writers. The Journal of and London: Indiana University Press, pp. 283–300.
Creative Behaviour, 35(1): 37–50.
Pennebaker, J. W., Mehl, M. R. and Niederhoffer, K. G.
Kaufman, J. C. and Baer, J. (2002). I bask in dreams of (2003). Psychological aspects of natural language use:
suicide: mental illness, poetry, and women. Review of our words, our selves. Annual Review of Psychology,
General Psychology, 6(3): 271.
54(1): 547–77.
Kaufman, J. C. and Sexton, J. D. (2006). Why doesn’t the
Perloff, M. G. (1973). On the road to ‘‘ariel’’: the ‘‘tran-
writing cure help poets? Review of General Psychology,
sitional’’ poetry of Sylvia Plath. The Iowa Review, 4(2):
10(3): 268.
94–110. doi: 10.2307/20158047.
King, J. L. (2011). Red earth, motherly blood: articulating
Plath, S. (ed.) (1975). Letters Home. New York, NY:
Sylvia plath’s anxieties of motherhood. Plath Profiles: An
Harper & Row.
Interdisciplinary Journal for Sylvia Plath Studies, 4: 77–87.
Plath, S. (1981). The Collected Poems. London: Faber &
Kinzie, M. (1970). An informal check list of criticism. In:
Faber.
Newman, C. (ed.), The Art of Sylvia. Bloomington and
London: Indiana University Press, pp. 283–300. Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct. New York, NY:
Harper Perennial Modern Classics, pp. 483.
Kloumann, I. M., Danforth, C. M., Harris, K. D., Bliss,
C. A., and Dodds, P. S. (2012). Positivity of the English Plath, S. (2007). The Unabridged Journals of Sylvia Plath.
language. PLoS One, 7(1): e29484. Random House LLC.

670 Digital Scholarship in the Humanities, Vol. 32, No. 3, 2017


vocabulary of plath

Power, R. A., Steinberg, S., Bjornsdottir, G., Rietveld, C. Silvia, P. J. and Kaufman, J. C. (2010). Creativity and
A., Abdellaoui, A., Nivard, M. M., Johannesson, M., mental illness. Cambridge Handbook of Creativity, 199:
Galesloot, T. E., Hottenga, J. J., Willemsen, G., 381–94.
Cesarini, D., Benjamin, D. J., Magnusson, P. K., Taussig, M. (2001). Dying is an art, like everything
Ullén, F., Tiemeier, H., Hofman, A., van Rooij, F. J., else. Critical Inquiry, 28(1): 305–16. doi: 10.2307/

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/dsh/article/32/3/660/2669772 by Universidade de Bras�lia user on 09 September 2023


Walters, G. B., Sigurdsson, E., Thorgeirsson, T. E., 1344270.
Ingason, A., Helgason, A., Kong, A., Kiemeney, L. Weisberg, R. W. (2006). Creativity: Understanding
A., Koellinger, P., Boomsma, D. I., Gudbjartsson, Innovation in Problem Solving, Science, Invention, and
D., Stefansson, H., and Stefansson, K. (2015). the Arts. Hoboken, New Jersey, U.S.: John Wiley &
Polygenic risk scores for schizophrenia and bipolar dis- Sons.
order predict creativity. Nature Neuroscience, 18(7):
Whorf, B. L., Carroll, J. B., Levinson, S. C., and Lee, P.
953–5. doi: 10.1038/nn.4040 http://www.nature.com/
(2012). Language, Thought, And Reality: Selected
neuro/journal/v18/n7/abs/nn.4040.html#supplemen-
Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. MIT Press.
tary-information.
Runco, M. A. (1998). Suicide and creativity: the case of Wilkins, A. (2010). ‘‘Through the beautiful red’’: the use
Sylvia Plath. Death Studies, 22(7): 637–54. of the color red as the triple-goddess in Sylvia Plath’s
Ariel. Plath Profiles: An Interdisciplinary Journal for
Sawyer, R. K. (2011). Explaining Creativity: The Science of
Sylvia Plath Studies, 3: 74–89.
Human Innovation. New York, U.S.: Oxford University
Press. Wimsatt, W. K., and Beardsley, M. C. (1946). The inten-
tional fallacy. The Sewanee Review, 54: 468–88.
Schlesinger, J. (2009). Creative mythconceptions: a closer
look at the evidence for the ‘‘mad genius’’ hypothesis. Wimsatt, W. K., and Beardsley, M. C. (1954). The Verbal
Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, Icon: Studies in the Meaning of Poetry. Lexington, KY:
3(2): 62. University Press of Kentucky.

Digital Scholarship in the Humanities, Vol. 32, No. 3, 2017 671

You might also like