You are on page 1of 11

Sex Roles

https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-020-01178-2

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Sexual Consent: How Relationships, Gender, and Sexual


Self-Disclosure Affect Signaling and Interpreting Cues for Sexual
Consent in a Hypothetical Heterosexual Sexual Situation
Nicholas P. Newstrom 1 & Steven M. Harris 2 & Michael H. Miner 1

# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2020

Abstract
Sexual communication is critical to establishing sexual encounters. Sexual script theory has been used to explore how individuals
communicate sexual consent and perceive sexual consent cues. Gender differences appear to dictate how consent cues are
expressed and interpreted. Using a sample of 309 U.S. heterosexual participants (Mage = 34.6 years, range = 19.3–72.2), we
explore how single and partnered women and men interpret and perceive cues for consenting to sexual behaviors in a hypothetical
situation. Results revealed that relationship length and sexual self-disclosure did not affect how individuals would communicate
sexual consent; gender differences were identified as women reported being more likely to interpret their use of direct forms of
communication as consent indications whereas men reported they were more likely to interpret their use of indirect communi-
cation strategies as consent indications. Within-subjects analyses suggest that some forms of communication would be
interpreted differently depending on if they were to be used by the participant or used by the participant’s partner. Although
there is considerable agreement across genders on what classes of behaviors are indicative of consent, differences in the sexual
scripts of men and women may contribute to gender differences in consent. Given these gender and within-subjects’ differences,
further research is needed to explore the nuances of sexual communication.

Keywords Gender differences . Sexual consent . Sexual script theory

Sexual health is an important aspect of the human experi- (Byers 2005; Dosch et al. 2016). Positive or negative change
ences. Researchers have found that sexual satisfaction is asso- in sexual satisfaction level is associated with positive or neg-
ciated with positive mental health and improved relationships ative changes in relationship satisfaction levels (Byers 2005).
Broadly, researchers have found that couples who are able to
communicate positively about conflict and about physical af-
The present study used original data collected as part of the dissertation
completed by Nicholas Newstrom in partial completion of a doctoral fection report greater levels of relationship satisfaction over
degree from the Department of Family Social Science, University of the course of their relationships (Mackey et al. 2000, 2004).
Minnesota. Failure to communicate about sexual desires conversely have
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article been shown to have negative impacts on couples’ satisfaction
(https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-020-01178-2) contains supplementary and may lead to conflict.
material, which is available to authorized users. However, few studies have explored what forms of com-
munication are typically used during conversations related to
* Nicholas P. Newstrom sexual behavior. Furthermore, sexual behaviors unfold differ-
newst038@umn.edu
ently at different points in time. For example, the behaviors
and communication used to engage in sexual behaviors are
1
Program in Human Sexuality, Department of Family Medicine and different from those used during the act of sex itself. The
Community Health, University of Minnesota, 1300 South Second
present paper explores the different communication patterns
Street, Suite 180, Minneapolis, MN 55454, USA
2
expected to be used by U.S. heterosexual women and men
Couple and Family Therapy (CFT) Program, Department of Family
when they are consenting to engage in sexual intercourse.
Social Science, College of Education and Human Development,
University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, USA We investigate what different behaviors individuals use when
Sex Roles

in a hypothetical sexual situation to indicate consent (i.e., con- sexual consent (consent perceptions). Through factor analysis,
sent cues) as well as how these behaviors are interpreted when they found that their scales measured five patterns for consent
observed in others (i.e., consent perceptions). cues: (a) direct verbal cues (“Will you have sex with me?”),
(b) direct nonverbal cues (“You [your partner] begins to take
off your clothes”), (c) indirect verbal cues (“You talk about
Consent and Sexual Script Theory your positive feelings about having sex with her/him”; “She/
He talks about her/his positive feelings about having sex with
Sexual script theory has often been used as one way to con- you”), (d) indirect nonverbal cues (“You rub, fondle, and
ceptualize sexual behavior by providing a framework for touch her/him sexually”; “She/He rubs, fondles, and touches
studying the behaviors that precede and occur during and after you sexually”) and (e) statements about intoxication levels
sexual intercourse (Gagnon and Simon 1973). This theory (“You say, ‘I’m really drunk’”; “She/He says, ‘I’m really
suggests that sexual behaviors are informed by social con- drunk’”).
structs such as culture, interpersonal relationships and intra- Hickman and Muehlenhard (1999) found that both women
psychic scripts (e.g., sexual fantasy; memory) (Wiederman and men used indirect cues to communicate consent.
2015). Consistent with a social constructionist perspective, Specifically, men were more likely than women were to use
sexual script theory posits that men and women create mean- indirect nonverbal cues (e.g., touching, kissing or caressing
ing and interpret sexual behaviors through shared beliefs with the other person) whereas women were more likely than
others and interactions with their social world. Sexual scripts men were to use indirect verbal consent cues (e.g., asking if
are a form of social scripts that are representations individuals the other person has a condom) and to give “no response.”
create to organize sexual behaviors and are developed from Hickman and Muehlenhard then asked participants to rate
culture, media, family members, sexual fantasy, and past sex- which cue types from their partners were most likely to indi-
ual encounters with others. cate sexual consent (consent perceptions). They found that
Although both men and women are more likely to use men rated direct verbal cues, direct nonverbal cues, indirect
nonverbal communication as sexual consent cues, gender dif- verbal statements, indirect nonverbal statements, and state-
ferences exist (Hall 1998; Hickman and Muehlenhard 1999). ments about intoxication as more indicative consent cues than
Traditionally, the sexual scripts of men socialize them to be women did. Hickman and Muehlenhard concluded that the
the aggressor and pursuer during sexual encounters whereas chance for miscommunication is high, given that they found
women are socialized to remain passive and to communicate gender differences across all five categories of behavior cues.
indirectly (Wiederman 2005). In this manner, sexual scripts When the sexual scripts of women and men are shared and
influence the way men’s and women’s behaviors are used to align (e.g., both have traditional sexual scripts), sexual en-
signal consent to sexual behavior and how men and women counters operate smoothly (Wiederman 2005). However,
interpret a prospective partners’ behavior as a signal of their when these scripts do not align or a partner deviates from their
consent to such behavior. A concern is that traditional scripts assigned script, problems can occur (Klein et al. 2019;
may lead to actual or perceived token resistance, overt resis- Wiederman 2005). Klein et al. (2019) found that women
tance cues, and indirect consent cues (Muehlenhard and who deviated from traditional sexual scripts were regarded
Hollabaugh 1988; Sprecher et al. 1994). Research has also more negatively by their male partners. These differences in
found that men are more likely to use deception and manipu- behaviors and their interpretation are influenced by the atti-
lation when trying to gain consent of their female partners and tudes and beliefs (sexual script) each individual brings to the
that social factors (e.g., being at a bar) are more likely to be encounter, and sexual scripts influence how behaviors are ex-
interpreted as automatic signs for consent cues (Jozkowski hibited and interpreted. If behaviors exhibited are viewed by
et al. 2018; Jozkowski and Peterson 2013). one as an indication of consent, while the other does not be-
Men and women also appear to value different consent lieve they are cueing consent, misinterpretation can occur and
cues and to perceive cues differently. Women are more likely the incident may be experienced differently by each
to rate verbal strategies as more important consent cues than participant.
men (Humphreys and Herold 2007; Jozkowski et al. 2013).
Jozkowski et al. (2013) found, in a sample of college students,
that men were more likely to attend to their female partner’s Couples and Sexual Self-Disclosure
nonverbal consent cues whereas women’s consent perceptions
were more likely focused on their partners’ use of verbal cues. Consent cues are likely to change over time as individuals in
In one of the most thorough studies of sexual consent cues relationships get to know their partners better and develop a
and consent perceptions, Hickman and Muehlenhard (1999) more intimate, ongoing relationship. Relationship status likely
created two scales to measure how college-aged heterosexual affects consent cues and perceptions in that the on-going in-
participants communicate (consent cues) and interpret cues for teractions that occur within committed sexual relationships
Sex Roles

may change one’s conceptualization of what consent is and (2019a, b) identified 41 studies exploring sexual consent and
how consent is communicated. Researchers have found that found a mean age for participants of 20.45 years. The fact that
individuals in long-term relationships are more likely to use sexual consent research has relied on college-aged individuals
nonverbal and indirect cues to indicate consent (Curtis and limits the generalizability of findings, but may also distort
Burnett 2017; Humphreys 2007; Marcantonio et al. 2018; some results given that relationships mature over time and
Willis et al. 2019a, b). Willis and Jozkowski (2019) used a change with the demands of family, work, and aging. Thus
diary study to investigate how relationship status affected con- this body of research may not adequately reflect how relation-
sent cues. They found that individuals in relationships were ship length and context affect consent cues and perceptions.
less likely to rely on communication cues and more likely to The present study expands upon the existing research and
use tacit knowledge or do nothing in response to their part- investigated three research questions to explore sexual scripts
ners’ consent cues to indicate that they would like to have sex in a sample including individuals older than college aged: (a)
with their partners. How does gender affect the extent to which behavior engaged
Whereas the accumulated data indicate that women and in by the individual or by their partner is interpreted as indi-
men are likely to cue consent using non-verbal and indirect cating consent in a hypothetical sexual situation?; (b) How do
behaviors, current sexual violence prevention programs focus relationship satisfaction and sexual self-disclosure affect how
on direct verbal expressions of consent. Direct verbal consent individuals in relationships would interpret behavior cues as
involves the ability to explicitly communicate one’s sexual consent to sex in a hypothetical sexual situation?; and (c) How
intentions. This process is similar to sexual self-disclosure do behaviors used by participants affect their interpretation of
(SSD), which may serve as a predictor or moderator of the these same behaviors used by their prospective sexual partners
manner in which individuals express consent. SSD is the pro- in a hypothetical sexual situation?
cess of sharing one’s sexual likes and dislikes with another
(Greene and Faulkner 2005; MacNeil and Byers 2009).
Researchers have found that higher levels of SSD are associ-
ated with greater relationship and sexual satisfaction Method
(Jozkowski 2013). Satinsky and Jozkowski (2015) explored
sexual self-disclosure and sexual consent among cisgender, Participants
heterosexual women who wanted to have oral sex with their
partners. Using structural equation modeling, they found that Participants were recruited from Amazon Mechanical Turk
entitlement to sexual pleasure and sexual consent communi- (MTurk) during an 8-h period on May 19, 2017. MTurk is
cation was mediated by self-efficacy, suggesting that commu- an Internet marketplace that allows companies and researchers
nication about sexual behavior (oral sex) is related to per- to pay workers to complete surveys for monetary compensa-
ceived ability to talk about sexual consent. tion. As a host, Amazon takes a 10% commission from every
This ability to share one’s sexual likes and dislikes, which survey that is completed. Considering the amount of time
indicates a certain level of comfort with sexual communica- required for the survey (45 min) and the sensitive nature of
tion, may have implications for the types of behaviors that one the questions, participants were reimbursed $1.00 U.S. for
would use to cue interest or lack of interest in engaging in completing the survey.
sexual behavior. That is, using direct verbal cues requires A total of 368 participants started the survey. However,
comfort with sexual communication that appears consistent only 350 participants completed the entire survey.
with SSD. Theoretically, gaining consent for sex may be Participants who did not successfully answer the validity
deemed an attractive sexual quality. Gaining explicit verbal check were eliminated from the sample, leaving a final sample
consent may help sexual partners to assert their sexual needs of 309 U.S. heterosexual participants. The average age of
and desires clearly (Troost 2008). When partners are enthusi- participants was 34.6 (SD = 12.45, range = 19.27–72.17)
astic about gaining consent for sexual behaviors they have yet years; and there was no significant differences between men
to try with their respective partners, it forces the requester to (M = 34.86, SD = 12.97, range = 19.27–72.17) and women
focus on the needs and desires of their partner. This may (M = 34.21, SD = 11.75, range = 21.33–67.52), t(304) = .406,
increase the level of emotional intimacy required to both make p = .406. The sample included 72.2% (n = 223) who identified
requests for sexual behaviors and be turned down by a partner as Caucasian, 17.5% (n = 54) identified as Asian American,
(Troost 2008). 3.6% (n = 11) identified as African American, 3.6% (n = 11)
In the present study we investigate how single and identified as South Asian or Pacific Islander, and 3.2% (n =
partnered individuals project they would indicate and 10) identified as Other. Thirty-two percent of the sample (n =
interpret cues for sexual behavior. Much of the research to 100) reported income greater than $50,000. A majority
date has used samples of undergraduate college students. (79.3%, n = 246) of participants reported that they were in
Conducting a systemic view of the literature, Willis et al. relationships (Women = 116, 47.3%; Men = 130, 52.7%).
Sex Roles

Materials and Procedures Direct Refusal item (“You say, ‘No.’” and “She/He says,
‘No’”) because in our study, very few participants (n = 55)
An advertisement asked subjects to participate in a survey indicated that their use of this behavior or their partner’s use
examining how heterosexual, cisgender (when one’s gender would indicate consent. The subscales we analyzed included:
matches their biological sex) individuals convey and interpret (a) Direct Verbal cues (e.g., “I want you”) comprising five
verbal and nonverbal signals to engage in sexual intercourse. items (αmen = .81; αwomen = .75), (b) Direct Nonverbal
The procedure for our study was approved by the University’s cues composed of one item (“You don’t say anything—you
Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects (IRB) and just start having intercourse with her/him”), (c) Indirect Verbal
participants read an informed consent statement prior to com- cues (e.g., “You ask if she/he has a condom”) comprising four
pleting the survey. Once participants selected the survey to items (αmen = .54; αwomen = .83), (d) Indirect Nonverbal
complete from Amazon MTurk, they were directed to click cues (e.g., “You help her/him undress you”) composed of 10
on the survey’s link and then directed to a website where the items (α = .94; αwomen = .83), (e) Nonresponse cues com-
instruments were presented using REDCap software. prising four items (e.g., “You do not say, ‘No’”)
REDCap is a system for data storage and sharing designed (αmen = .85; α = .89), and (f) Statements about Intoxication
initially for multi-site trials. This software administers and (e.g., “I’m really drunk”) combining two items (αmen = .94;
collects completed questionnaires and allows for the data to αwomen = .91). A 7-point Likert-type scale was used with
be stored confidentially and safely (Harris et al. 2009). Upon ratings from 0 (does not show your consent to sexual
completing the study, participants were given a unique ID intercourse) to 6 (definitely shows your consent to sexual
code, which they entered in Amazon MTurk to verify that they intercourse). For all multi-item subscales, ratings across items
completed the survey before they received payment. were averaged such that higher scores indicate stronger levels
Demographic information was collected including date of of communicating sexual consent.
birth, gender, income, and relationship status. Interpreting sexual consent cues. Hickman and
Signaling and interpreting consent. Hickman and Muehlenhard’s (1999) interpreting consent scale used gender
Muehlenhard’s (1999) scales were used to measure how indi- appropriate language for a hypothetical dating scenario:
viduals cued and interpreted consent. These measures includ- You are very attracted to your date and would like to have
ed gender-specific versions for men and for women. Their sexual intercourse with him [her]. You have been out several
original scales had 33 items; however, after conducting a fac- times but the two of you have not had sexual intercourse
tor analysis, they dropped three items for a total of 30 items in (penile-vaginal intercourse) together before. The two of you
order to improve conceptual clarity. Based on the recommen- are finally alone in a private place. You make a sexual advance
dations of the authors, the final 27 items were used in the by sitting close to him [her], kissing him [her], and then
current study (S.E. Hickman, personal communication, April starting to undress him [her]. In response to your sexual ad-
5, 2017). Due to a production error, four items were dropped vance in which you sit close to him [her], kiss him [her], and
from the consent cue scales for men: one validity check was then start to undress him [her]. . .
dropped, two items were dropped for the Indirect Verbal con- The same 27 communication cues were used and adjusted
sent cue scale (“You suggest you should get a condom out”; to measure the extent that these behaviors would be
“You ask if she has a condom”) and one item from the non- interpreted as consent cues in a hypothetical situation.
response consent cue scale (“You do not resist her sexual The subscales were the same as those for communicating
advances”). sexual consent: (a) Direct Verbal (e.g., “She/He says, ‘I want
Communicating sexual consent. In accordance with to have sex with you’”) (αmen = .75; αwomen = .71), (b)
Hickman and Muehlenhard’s (1999) procedures, participants Direct Nonverbal (e.g., “She/He doesn’t say anything—she/
were asked how they would communicate sexual consent if he just starts having intercourse with you”), (c) Indirect Verbal
they were in a hypothetical situation with an partner of the (e.g., “She/He asks if you have a condom”) (αmen = .83;
other gender: αwomen = .88), (d) Indirect Nonverbal (e.g., “She/He rubs,
You and your date have been out several times but the two fondles, and touches you sexually”) (αmen = .95;
of you have not had sexual intercourse (penile-vaginal αwomen = .95), (e) Nonresponse cues (e.g., “She/He does
intercourse) together before. The two of you are finally alone not say, ‘No’”) (αmen = .92; αwomen = .90), and (f)
in a private place. She [he] sits close to you, kisses you, and Statements About Intoxication (e.g., “I’m really drunk”)
then starts to undress you. In response to her [him] sitting (αmen = .95; αwomen = .95). The same 7-point, Likert-type
close, kissing you, and then starting to undress you. . . rating scale. For all multi-item subscales, ratings across items
They were then asked their level of agreement on 27 items, were averaged such that higher scores indicate stronger levels
describing various ways that they could signal sexual consent. of consent perceptions. Hickman and Muehlenhard’s (1999)
We used six of the seven subscales identified by Hickman and scales were chosen because they were not open-ended items,
Muehlenhard (1999) for our study. We omitted analyzing the meaning participants had to choose from a list of behaviors.
Sex Roles

Sexual self-disclosure. To measure the extent to which in- whereas only one of the accuracy checks was included in the
dividuals in relationships shared their sexual likes and dislikes interpretation scale. After surveys were eliminated using the
with their partners, we used the 12-item Sexual Self- accuracy checks, descriptive statistics and histograms were
Disclosure (SSD) scale (Byers and Demmons 1999). This used to examine the normality of all scale items. Then, log-
measure was only administered to those who indicated they transformations were used to address positive skewness.
were currently in a relationship. Participants were asked to Although data analysis was conducted using the log-
think about their sexual relationship with their current partner transformed scores, untransformed means are presented in
and indicate how often they communicated with them about both tables and in the Results section for clarity and ease of
their sexual likes and dislikes. Participants were prompted by interpretation. To control for experiment-wise error, we ap-
the stem, “How much have you told your partner about…,” plied a Bonferroni correction (p < .008).
and then were asked to respond to a variety of sexual behav- To examine the effect of gender and relationship status
iors (e.g., “…the way(s) you like/don’t like to be kissed?”; “… would have on consent behaviors, five two-way factorial anal-
the way(s) you like/don’t like to have intercourse?”). Six items yses of variances (ANOVAs) were used. The outcome cues
of the scale measured the extent to which participants have (Direct Verbal, Indirect Verbal, Direct Nonverbal, Indirect
told their partners about how much they liked sexual behav- Nonverbal, Statements about Intoxication, Nonresponse) were
iors (e.g., kissing, oral sex) and six items were used to measure entered as dependent variables and gender consisted of two
the extent to which participants told their partners how much levels (male and female.) ANOVAs were used instead of lin-
they disliked the same sexual behaviors. A 7-point, Likert- ear regression in order to test the three levels of relationship
type scale was used with anchors from 1 (Nothing at all) to length. An additional six two-way factorial analyses of vari-
7 (Everything). Higher averaged scores indicated that partici- ances (ANOVA) were conducted by using the combined sub-
pants were more likely to sexually disclose to their partner. scales for interpreting sexual consent cues across the same
Cronbach’s alphas were .90 for men and .93 for women. behavior categories.
Relationship satisfaction. Relationship satisfaction was We used 12 regression equations to test the relationship
measured using the Revised Dyadic Adjustment Scale among relationship satisfaction, sexual self-disclosure, and
(RDAS) (Busby et al. 1995). The RDAS is a 14-item scale the interaction between relationship satisfaction and sexual
that assesses the consensus, cohesion, and satisfaction of dy- self-disclosure. Six equations were applied to the men’s sub-
adic couples, and it has been used in a variety of studies to scales of Direct Verbal, Indirect Verbal, Direct Nonverbal,
measure individuals’ attitudes on couple satisfaction. Items Indirect Nonverbal cues, Nonresponse cues, Statements about
included: “How often do you and your partner quarrel?” and Intoxication, and Nonresponse cues and five were applied to
“Do you ever regret that you married (or lived together)?” the women’s subscales.
Items were rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale from 0 (All In order to identify differences between behaviors partici-
of the time) to 5 (Never). Ratings were averaged across items pants would choose to cue consent and how participants
so that higher scores indicates stronger relationship satisfac- would interpret consent cues in a hypothetical situation, we
tion. Cronbach’s alphas were .87 for men and .84 for women. used a one-way, repeated measures ANOVA. This analysis
had one between-subjects factor with two levels (female and
Data Analysis male) and one within-subjects factor with two levels (actor
and receiver). ANOVAs were conducted for each class of
IBM’s v22 SPSS was used for the analysis of the data. behaviors (Direct Verbal, Indirect Verbal, Direct Nonverbal,
Descriptive statistics, boxplots, correlations and scatterplots Indirect Nonverbal, Statements about Intoxication, and
were calculated to test assumptions of normality. Little’s Nonresponse cues). Because both the men’s and women’s
Missing Completely at Random Test was used to assess the scales were scored the same, the two versions of each subscale
patterns of missing data. This test indicated that 2% of the data were combined. To control for familywise error, we applied
were missing in a random pattern and thus were not the result Bonferroni correction within each set of analyses (p < .008).
of a systematic problem. Expectation maximization was used
to impute missing values. This technique uses an algorithm to
re-estimate new parameters of the missing data frame. Then, a Results
second iteration is used to re-estimate missing values based on
this parameter frame (Little and Rubin 1989). Indicating Consent
Hickman and Muehlenhard (1999) incorporated two accu-
racy check questions into their scale (e.g., “He/she yawns— A 2 × 3 analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted on the
answer this question with a nine” and “You scratch your influence of gender (male or female), relationship length (less
arm—answer this question with an eight”). Both of these ac- than one year, one to 5 years, greater than 5 years), and the
curacy checks were included in the self-initiation scale interaction between gender and relationship length on the type
Sex Roles

of communication participants reported they would use. The situation. When relationship satisfaction was high, there is
main effect of gender was statistically significant for three no impact of sexual self-disclosure.
types of communication: Indirect Verbal cues, F(1, 227) = Interpreting Partner’s Consent.
41.14, p < .001, ηp2 = .15; Indirect Nonverbal cues, F(1, With regard to interpreting consent, the main effect of
239) = 13.08, p < .001, ηp2 = .05; and Statements about Gender was significant for Indirect Verbal cues, F(1, 239) =
Intoxication, F(1,239) = 18.14, p < .001, ηp2 = .07 (see 12.35, p < .001, ηp2 = .05, and for Indirect Nonverbal cues,
Table 1). F(1, 239) = 8.23, p = .004, ηp2 = .03. These analyses indicate
Correlations were calculated between the independent var- that men in our sample would be more likely to interpret
iables (Relationship Satisfaction, Sexual Self-Disclosure). A Indirect Verbal cues (e.g., “She asks if you have a condom”)
nonsignificant correlation was identified between Sexual Self- and Indirect Nonverbal cues (e.g., “She hugs and caresses
Disclosure and Relationship Satisfaction (r = .02, p = .760). you”) by their female partners as indicative of consent than
We ran multiple regression equations to test the relationship women would interpret such cues as indicative of their male
between relationship satisfaction, sexual self-disclosure, and partners’ consent (see Table 1). Additionally, men would also
the interaction between relationship satisfaction and sexual be more likely to interpret their partner’s statements about
self-disclosure (see Tables 2 s and 3 s in the online supple- their intoxication level as indicative of consent, although the
ment). The interaction between relationship satisfaction and magnitude of their ratings indicate that men do not interpret
sexual self-disclosure was significant only for Indirect statements about intoxication as strong indications of consent
Nonverbal cues, F(3, 126) = 2.39, p < .009, with an R2 of for sexual intercourse, F(1, 239) = 9.37, p = .002, ηp2 = .04.
.054 for men (see Table 1s in the online supplement). When The interaction between relationship satisfaction and sexu-
relationship satisfaction was low, the magnitude of sexual al self-disclosure was also significant for interpreting Indirect
self-disclosure was related to the degree to which Indirect Nonverbal cues, F(3, 126) = 2.38, p < .010, with an R2 of .054
Nonverbal cues signaled sexual consent in a hypothetical (see Table 4 s in the online supplement). None of the other
behavior types were significant for relationship satisfaction,
sexual self-disclosure, or the interaction between relationship
Table 1 Descriptive statistics for combined cues indicating and satisfaction and self-disclosure (See Tables 4 s and 5 s in the
interpreting consent by participants’ gender online supplement). As previously for signaling consent,
when relationship satisfaction was rated low, men would be
Men Women Total
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
more likely to interpret their partner’s Indirect Nonverbal cues
as indicative of consent when their level of sexual self-
Direct Verbal disclosure was high in a hypothetical situation. Again, with
Indicated Consent 5.10 (1.10) 5.17 (.89) 5.12 (1.00) higher relationship satisfaction, there is no impact of SSD.
Interpreted Consent 5.10 (.96) 5.16 (.88) 5.13 (.93)
Indirect Verbal
Indicated Consent 3.88 (1.39)a,c 2.85 (1.62)b 3.44 (1.58) Differences between Indicated and Interpreted
Interpreted Consent 4.26 (1.26)a,d 3.38 (1.51)b 3.89 (1.44) Consent
Direct Nonverbal
Indicated Consent 4.34 (2.03) 4.88 (1.64)c 4.57 (1.89) Turning to our repeated measures ANOVAs which sepa-
Interpreted Consent 4.80 (1.74) 4.99 (1.61)d 4.88 (1.67) rately compared men’s and women’s projections of their
Indirect Nonverbal own indicated consent with their interpreted consent of
Indicated Consent 3.94 (1.41)a 3.14 (1.57)b 3.60 (1.53) their partner, only two comparisons were statistically sig-
Interpreted Consent 3.79 (1.48)a 3.17 (1.55)b 3.52 (1.54) nificant. There was a significant effect of Indirect Verbal
Nonresponse behaviors, Wilks’ λ = .89, F(1, 307) = 42.65, p = .001,
Indicated Consent 3.67 (1.63) 2.92 (1.69) 3.36 (1.69) ηp2 = .12, finding that men in our sample indicated that
Interpreted Consent 3.42 (1.68) 3.03 (1.72) 3.26 (1.70) their use of Indirect Verbal behaviors were less indicative
Statements About Intoxication of sexual consent than the Indirect Verbal behaviors of
Indicated Consent 1.38 (1.63)a .58 (1.16)b 1.04 (1.50)
their partners in a hypothetical sexual situation (see
Interpreted Consent 1.30 (1.68)a .64 (1.23)b 1.02 (1.54)
Table 5 s in the online supplement). There was also a
significant effect for Direct Nonverbal behaviors, Wilks’
Note. All analyses were conducted with log transformed means whereas λ = .97, F(1, 307) = .13, p = .003, ηp2 = .03, suggesting
the statistics reported here as not transformed. Means with different sub- that women reported they would be more likely to inter-
scripts (a,b) across a row indicate a significant gender difference; means
with different subscripts (c, d) down a column indicate a significant dif-
pret their Direct Nonverbal behaviors as indicative of con-
ference between indicated and interpreted consent (at p < .008 using a sent while rating their prospective male partner’s direct
Bonferroni-correction) non-verbal behaviors as less indicative of consent.
Sex Roles

Discussion Nonverbal cues as indicative of consent while rating their


hypothetical male partner’s Direct Nonverbal cues as less in-
Overall, our findings indicate discrepancies that could lead to dicative of consent. Given the tenants of sexual script theory,
misunderstanding of consent in three areas. First, our results our within-subjects findings support the idea that sexual
indicate gender differences related to sexual consent cues. scripts that occur prior to sexual intercourse are influenced
Second, we also detected gender differences regarding the by social interactions with other-gender partners (Gagnon
consent perceptions of both men and women. Third, we found and Simon 1973). Recall that intrapsychic scripts, such as
within-subjects differences that affect sexual consent commu- memories of previous sexual encounters, contribute to the
nication. The following sections describe and interpret the formation of current sexual scripts (Sternberg 1996).
results of how individuals communicate and interpret sexual Women and men in our study may have been socialized to
consent. indicate and interpret communication differently based on
their previous sexual encounters. Women may have found
Consent Cues success in their previous use of Direct Nonverbal communi-
cation and were not looking for men to use this strategy with
Our results revealed that women and men would use similar them. Conversely, men may have been socialized to be attune
consent cues in a hypothetical sexual situation with their part- to the Indirect Verbal cues of women.
ner. However, we did find some differences. Women indicat-
ed their Direct Verbal and Direct Nonverbal consent cues Perception Cues
were more indicative of sexual consent whereas men indicated
that their Indirect Verbal cues were more indicative of consent Men in our study were more likely to indicate that Indirect
in a hypothetical situation. Although neither men nor women Verbal and Nonverbal perception cues, as well as statements
rated potential use of statements about their intoxication as about intoxication, are indicative of consent by their female
particularly strong indicators of consent, we did find that partners. These findings are similar to those of Hickman and
men’s ratings of statements about intoxication were higher Muehlenhard (1999) who also found that men in their sample
than women’s, consistent with the gender differences identi- were more likely than women to interpret Indirect Verbal and
fied by Hickman and Muehlenhard (1999). Nonverbal cues and statements about intoxication as more
It is important to note that the partial eta squared statistics, indicative of consent. However, in their sample, they found
used to indicate effect size, were small for Direct Verbal and that men rated Direct Verbal cues as more indicative of con-
Direct Nonverbal cues. The largest effect size was for the sent than women. In our sample, this was not the case.
Indirect Verbal cues used by men (.15) suggesting that these Our results indicate that, for the most part, men attuned to
group differences are more robust and may be indicative of the same behaviors that women report would indicate consent
men being less direct and possibly more manipulative in their if they used them. The exception is that whereas women re-
approaches to sexual behavior. Women may be socialized ported that using Indirect, Nonverbal cues would be less likely
more to be more direct in their sexual communication in order to signal consent, men reported that they interpret such cues as
to prevent sexual miscommunication (Muehlenhard and indications of consent. The sexual scripts of men in our sam-
Hollabaugh 1988; Sprecher et al. 1994). However, the idea ple appear to lead them to use indirect communication during
that sexual miscommunication can be prevented by women negotiation of sexual behaviors. Sexual script theory posits
only forces the responsibility of consent solely on women that society and cultural influences contribute to the sexual
(O’Byrne et al. 2008). It is possible that because traditional behaviors of women and men (Gagnon and Simon 1973).
sexual scripts require men to initiate sexual behavior, indirect Traditional heterosexual dating scripts dictate that the man is
communication allows them to save face if their potential in a proactive role whereas the woman assumes a reactive one
partner is uninterested or it may be indicative of discomfort (Laner and Ventrone 2000; Rose and Frieze 1993). This tra-
with or prohibitions against direct sexual advances. However, ditional script, where women are more passive in their dating
our measure was not about frequency of use because we did communication, may have socialized men to be attuned to the
not ask participants which consent cues they use the most. indirect signals of women (Muehlenhard and Hollabaugh
Our within-subjects analyses indicated that other behavior 1988; Sprecher et al. 1994). In response to these indirect cues,
classes, Indirect Verbal, and Direct Nonverbal cues have dif- they in turn would use them with women.
ferent meanings with respect to whether they would indicate Relationship satisfaction and sexual self-disclosure do not
consent when used by men and women in hypothetical situa- appear to affect the consent or perception cues in our study.
tions. Men indicated that their hypothetical use of Indirect However, the interaction between sexual self-disclosure and
Verbal cues was less indicative of consent than their hypothet- relationship satisfaction was significant for Indirect Nonverbal
ical female partners’ use of Indirect Verbal cues. In contrast, cues that men would use in a hypothetical situation and how
women would be more likely to interpret their Direct men would interpret Indirect Nonverbal cues in a fictional
Sex Roles

situation. Men rated Indirect Nonverbal cues and interpreta- Actor and Perceiver Differences
tions of these cues as high when sexual self-disclosure was
high and relationship satisfaction was low. This interaction Our data indicate that, for the most part, both women and men
effect is consistent with sexual script theory (Wiederman perceive behaviors in others as indications of consent that they
2015) in that it indicates that an internal factor (sexual self- themselves would use to cue consent. However, in both men
disclosure) interacts with an experiential factor (a negative and women, we found that Indirect Verbal cues were rated
relationship experience as measured by the RDAS) to influ- more highly as an indication of consent when used by a per-
ence how men communicate and perceive consent. spective sexual partner than our participants rated such behav-
Specifically, for those men in unsatisfying relation- iors when used by themselves. That difference would appear
ships, their level of sexual self-disclosure, which indi- to indicate that, in both women and men, although they do not
cates comfort communicating with their partners about believe that their Indirect Verbal cues are indicative of con-
sex, is related to increased perceptions that both their sent, they do perceive their partner’s Indirect Verbal cues as
and their partners’ use of indirect non-verbal cues are indicating consent. This perceptual disparity sets up an inter-
indicative of consent for sexual intercourse in the pre- esting situation in that for both men and women, they appear
sented fictional dating situations. For those men in rela- to be drawing inferences about consent from behaviors they
tively satisfying relationships, sexual self-disclosure did themselves would see as less indicative of their interest in
not influence the ratings of indirect, non-verbal behav- engaging in sexual intercourse. For Direct Nonverbal cues
iors. There is an indication that unstable relationships there are gender differences in this mismatch between what I
influence the types of behaviors used to negotiate sexual would do to communicate consent and what I perceive you are
situations, such that indirect behavior is more likely communicating with the same behaviors. In this case, men
among both men and women when there is relationship rated their hypothetical use of Direct Nonverbal cues as less
uncertainty (Theiss 2011). Thus, our findings may indi- indicative of consent than when their perspective partner used
cate that learning from experience within their relation- these cues. Women rated both their use and their hypothetical
ships that sexual communication is indirect would lead partner’s use as equally indicative of consent. Thus, male sex-
men with sexual comfort to define such indirect behavior ual scripts appear to include the expectation that their potential
as sexual consent. However, these findings should be female partners will use direct nonverbal cues to tell them of
interpreted with caution because this interaction effect their interest in engaging in sexual behavior, but that such
only explained 5% of the variance in both ratings of behavior is not part of the consent script for men. As we noted
consent cues and consent perceptions. previously, men believe that their indirect behavior, whether
Further, our analyses of relationship factors are limited in verbal or nonverbal, is more indicative of their consent for
that rather than querying how individuals within relationships sexual intercourse.
would signal or interpret consent of their partners, we asked
them to put themselves into a fictitious scenario that involved Limitations and Future Research Directions
the initial instance of sexual intercourse. Thus, we may have
lost the influence of time spent in relationship because it does The present study had several limitations in addition to those
not change how one may view consent for an initial sexual we described previously for our relationship analyses. First,
encounter. Additionally, our rating anchors asked how likely a the cross-sectional design limits statements about causality,
particular behavior would be an indication of consent if used and self-report is also subject to biases and contamination.
rather than how often one might use a particular behavior to The various sexual scripts that individuals use to consent to
signal consent. sex may be difficult to measure in a survey given the variety of
With regard to our study, we had assumed that being different ways consent can be shown. Human communication
in a relationship would have changed the sexual scripts and sexual behaviors are complex, and there may be addition-
of partnered individuals, but, except for the interaction al forms of communication that were not accounted for, spe-
effect found in men for indirect non-verbal behavior, cifically among the indirect, nonverbal and intoxication sub-
this was not the case. The vignette we used was a scales. For example, pouring a glass of alcohol while smiling
hypothetical situation, asking that participants think seductively is another indirect, nonverbal behavior pattern that
about a date they are on but it did not specify that this may be indicative of sexual consent that was not measured in
person was their partner. Thus, some participants could our study.
have responded with their partner in mind or responded Additionally, it is likely that different forms of communi-
as if they would be having sex with an acquaintance for cation interact with each other to lead to perceived consent.
the first time. This may explain why we were unable to Hickman and Muehlenhard’s (1999) scales limit the ability to
find an association between relationship satisfaction and make statements about the number of times a particular cue
a particular consent behavior. type was used or if combinations of behaviors were used. We
Sex Roles

also do not have data on the context of relationships or sexual partner is not signaling consent and the other partner believes
orientation. For example, there may have been individuals in that they are. These programs may benefit from highlighting
relationships that for religious reasons were not having sex the complex nature of communication and encouraging indi-
until marriage or individuals who participated in our study viduals to ask clarifying questions about their partner’s inten-
who were in same-sex relationships even though recruitment tions. Another is that existing sexual scripts influence men and
materials asked for sexually active, heterosexual, cisgender women to both perceive their own sexual cues in different
participants. The vignettes we used were the original ways and their potential partners’ sexual cues differently.
Hickman and Muehlenhard (1999) vignettes that did not ask These differences appear to conform to traditional sexual
participants who are in relationships already what consent scripts and may further reinforce gender stereotypes and pow-
cues they would use with their partner or ask how they would er imbalances within heterosexual sexual relationships.
perceive cues from their partner. Finally, Hickman and
Muehlenhard’s scales were also used to investigate the con-
sent cues and perceptions of college-aged individuals. It is Conclusions
possible that the vignettes and behaviors used in the original
study resonated better with college-aged individuals than the Our study extends the available research and indicates
older adult sample recruited for our study. that different strategies are used by men and women not
The Indirect Verbal subscale for men only included two only in college students, but also in a more general sam-
items whereas the female Indirect Verbal subscale contained ple of U.S. adults. Not only did women and men differ in
four items. This difference may explain the poor reliability for how they would perceive their own behavior to be indic-
the male version of this scale. The male version of the ative of consent, but in at least two cases, there were
Nonresponse subscale did not include one of the items from differences in whether they felt a behavior was a cue of
the original scale, however, this did not appear to affect the consent depending on whether it was their behavior or the
reliability of the scale for men or women. behavior of their perspective partner in a hypothetical
Although participants in our study indicated whether their sexual situation. Our findings suggest that both women
use of cues would cue consent, the frequency that they are and men may have been socialized to use indirect forms
used is still unknown. Thus, it is unclear which behavioral of communication and that men may have been keyed to
cues are more likely to be used in negotiating and consenting pay attention to the nonverbal, indirect strategies of wom-
to engage in sexual intercourse. To address this issue, future en. The within-subject differences suggest that when men
studies should include self-report measures asking about the are presented with these scripts, men respond with non-
consent cues that individuals actually use with their partners. verbal, indirect cues as well. Further exploring these
Although our study used a sample that was older than college- within-subject differences using different consent cues
aged samples, our sample was still predominantly Caucasian. may further explicate sexual scripts. Given that sexual
Exploring the consent cues and perceptions of same-sex indi- scripts are nuanced, exploring within-subjects differences
viduals and ethnic minority populations is required to increase and how different behaviors are used in combination may
external validity of findings. Longitudinal and experimental further an understanding of sexual interactions and com-
designs are also promising ways of exploring sexual consent. munication of consent.
Diary studies could be employed to track how cues and per- The results of our study suggest that gender differences
ceptions change over time and experimental studies using vir- influence how women and men indicate and interpret commu-
tual reality technology would allow researchers to control en- nication for sexual consent. It appears that both genders infer
vironmental elements in hypothetical situations (e.g., gender consent from non-verbal and indirect behaviors. Although in
of the proposed sexual partner). past studies sexual self-disclosure has been found to be asso-
ciated with indirect forms of communication, we found that
Practice Implications this was only true for men with lower relationship satisfaction.
Future studies should examine the complex interactions
Our study found gender differences between how women and among relationship variables and different combinations of
men would signal and interpret consent. A key issue is that we statements and behaviors that men and women use to indicate
have found that men will imply consent when women display sexual consent and explore the mechanisms involved in
behaviors that they indicate are less indicative of their willing- changing sexual norms. Finally, it should be noted that, for
ness to engage in sexual intercourse. Thus, sexual intent can the most part, the gender differences we found were rather
be misinterpreted and a potential exists for a coercive or ma- small. Thus, there appears to be more agreement among men
nipulative sexual encounter. One implication is that sexual and women as to what behaviors communicate consent to
assault prevention programs should address how consent is sexual intercourse than there are differences, at least in a hy-
signaled and interpreted, especially in situations where one pothetical context.
Sex Roles

Acknowledgements We would also like to acknowledge Cynthia Meyer Harris, P. A., Taylor, R., Thielke, R., Payne, J., Gonzalez, N., & Conde, J.
and Tai Mendenhall’s critiques and comments during the writing process G. (2009). Research electronic data capture (REDCap)—A
as well as Heidi Fall’s help editing and formatting. metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing
Thanks to the University of Minnesota Department of Family translational research informatics support. Journal of Biomedical
Medicine and Community Health, especially Heidi Fall for her editing Informatics, 42(2), 377–381. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbi.2008.08.
and formatting as well as the critiques and comments from Cynthia Meyer 010.
and Tai Mendenhall during the writing process. Hickman, S. E., & Muehlenhard, C. L. (1999). “By the semi-mystical
appearance of a condom”: How young women and men communi-
Funding This research was funded by a dissertation award from the cate sexual consent in heterosexual situations. Journal of Sex
University of Minnesota Family Social Science Department and by the Research, 36(3), 258–272. https://doi.org/10.1080/
National Institutes of Health’s National Center for Advancing 00224499909551996.
Translational Sciences, grant UL1TR002494. The content is solely the Humphreys, T. (2007). Perceptions of sexual consent: The impact of
responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official relationship history and gender. Journal of Sex Research, 44(4),
views of the National Institutes of Health’s National Center for 307–315. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224490701586706.
Advancing Translational Sciences. Humphreys, T., & Herold, E. (2007). Sexual consent in heterosexual
relationships: Development of a new measure. Sex Roles, 57, 305–
315. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-007-9264-7.
Compliance with Ethical Standards Jozkowski, K. N. (2013). The influence of consent on college students’
perceptions of the quality of sexual intercourse at last event.
Conflict of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of International Journal of Sexual Health, 25(4), 260–272. https://
interest. doi.org/10.1080/19317611.2013.799626.
Ethical Approval: All procedures performed in studies involving hu- Jozkowski, K. N., Manning, J., & Hunt, M. (2018). Sexual consent in and
man participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the out of the bedroom: Disjunctive views of heterosexual college stu-
University of Minnesota Institutional Review Board (FWA number dents. Women’s Studies in Communication, 41(2), 117–139. https://
00000312) and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amend- doi.org/10.1080/07491409.2018.1470121.
ments or comparable ethical standards. Jozkowski, K. N., & Peterson, Z. D. (2013). College students and sexual
consent: Unique insights. Journal of Sex Research, 50(6), 517–523.
Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2012.700739.
participants included in the study. Jozkowski, K. N., Peterson, Z. D., Sanders, S. A., Dennis, B., & Reece,
M. (2013). Gender differences in heterosexual college students' con-
ceptualizations and indicators of sexual consent: Implications for
contemporary sexual assault prevention education. The Journal of
Sex Research, 51(8), 904–916. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.
References 2013.792326.
Klein, V., Imhoff, R., Reininger, K. M., & Briken, P. (2019). Perceptions
Busby, D. M., Christensen, C., Crane, D. R., & Larson, J. H. (1995). A of sexual script deviation in women and men. Archives of Sexual
revision of the Dyadic Adjustment Scale for use with distressed and Behavior, 48, 631–644. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-018-1280-
nondistressed couples: Construct hierarchy and multidimensional x.
scales. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 21(3), 289–308. Laner, M. R., & Ventrone, N. A. (2000). Dating scripts revisited. Journal
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1752-0606.1995.tb00163.x. of Family Issues, 21(4), 488–500. https://doi.org/10.1177/
019251300021004004.
Byers, E. S. (2005). Relationship satisfaction and sexual satisfaction: A
longitudinal study of individuals in long-term relationships. Journal Little, R. J., & Rubin, D. B. (1989). The analysis of social science data
of Sex Research, 42(2), 113–118. https://doi.org/10.1080/ with missing values. Sociological Methods & Research, 18(2–3),
00224490509552264. 292–326. https://doi.org/10.1177/0049124189018002004.
Mackey, R. A., Diemer, M. A., & O’Brien, B. A. (2000). Psychological
Byers, E. S., & Demmons, S. (1999). Sexual satisfaction and sexual self-
intimacy in the lasting relationships of heterosexual and same-
disclosure within dating relationships. Journal of Sex Research,
gender couples. Sex Roles, 43, 201–225. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:
36(2), 180–189. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499909551983.
1007028930658.
Curtis, J. N., & Burnett, S. (2017). Affirmative consent: What do college Mackey, R. A., Diemer, M. A., & O’Brien, B. A. (2004). Relational
student leaders think about “yes means yes” as the standard for factors in understanding satisfaction in the lasting relationships of
sexual behavior? American Journal of Sexuality Education, 12(3), same-sex and heterosexual couples. Journal of Homosexuality,
201–214. https://doi.org/10.1080/15546128.2017.1328322. 47(1), 111–136. https://doi.org/10.1300/J082v47n01_07.
Dosch, A., Rochat, L., Ghisletta, P., Favez, N., & Van der Linden, M. MacNeil, S., & Byers, E. S. (2009). Role of sexual self-disclosure in the
(2016). Psychological factors involved in sexual desire, sexual ac- sexual satisfaction of long-term heterosexual couples. The Journal
tivity, and sexual satisfaction: A multi-factorial perspective. of Sex Research, 46(1), 3–14. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 45, 2029–2045. https://doi.org/10. 00224490802398399.
1007/s10508-014-0467-z. Marcantonio, T., Jozkowski, K. N., & Wiersma-Mosley, J. (2018). The
Gagnon, J., & Simon, W. (1973). Sexual conduct: The social sources of influence of partner status and sexual behavior on college women’s
human sexuality. Chicago: Aldine. consent communication and feelings. Journal of Sex & Marital
Greene, K., & Faulkner, S. L. (2005). Gender, belief in the sexual double Therapy, 44(8), 776–786. https://doi.org/10.1080/0092623X.2018.
standard, and sexual talk in heterosexual dating relationships. Sex 1474410.
Roles, 53(3/4), 239–251. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-005-5682- Muehlenhard, C. L., & Hollabaugh, L. C. (1988). Do women sometimes
6. say no when they mean yes? The prevalence and correlates of
Hall, D. S. (1998). Consent for sexual behavior in a college student women’s token resistance to sex. Journal of Personality and
population. Electronic Journal of Human Sexuality, 1, 1–16 http:// Social Psychology, 54(5), 872–879. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-
www.ejhs.org/volume1/consent1.htm. 3514.54.5.872.
Sex Roles

O’Byrne, R., Hansen, S., & Rapley, M. (2008). “If a girl doesn't say Wiederman, M. W. (2005). The gendered nature of sexual scripts. The
‘no’…”: Young men, rape and claims of ‘insufficient knowledge. Family Journal, 13(4), 496–502. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 18(3), 168– 1066480705278729.
193. https://doi.org/. https://doi.org/10.1002/casp.922. Wiederman, M. W. (2015). Sexual script theory: Past, present, and future.
Rose, S., & Frieze, I. H. (1993). Young singles' contemporary dating In J. DeLamater & R. F. Plante (Eds.), Handbook of the sociology of
scripts. Sex Roles, 28(9–10), 499–509. https://doi.org/10.1007/ sexualities (pp. 7–22). Madison: Springer.
BF00289677. Willis, M., Blunt-Vinti, H. D., & Jozkowski, K. N. (2019a). Associations
Satinsky, S., & Jozkowski, K. N. (2015). Female sexual subjectivity and between internal and external sexual consent in a diverse national
verbal consent to receiving oral sex. Journal of Sex & Marital sample of women. Personality and Individual Differences, 149, 37–
Therapy, 41(4), 413–426. https://doi.org/10.1080/0092623X.2014. 45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.05.029.
918065. Willis, M., Hunt, M., Wodika, A., Rhodes, D. L., Goodman, J., &
Sprecher, S., Hatfield, E., Cortese, A., Potapova, E., & Levitskaya, A. Jozkowski, K. N. (2019b). Explicit verbal sexual consent commu-
(1994). Token resistance to sexual intercourse and consent to un- nication: Effects of gender, relationship status, and type of sexual
wanted sexual intercourse: College students’ dating experiences in behavior. International Journal of Sexual Health, 31(1), 60–70.
three countries. The Journal of Sex Research, 31(2), 125–132. https://doi.org/10.1080/19317611.2019.1565793.
https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499409551739. Willis, M., & Jozkowski, K. N. (2019). Sexual precedent’s effect on
Sternberg, R. J. (1996). Love stories. Personal relationships, 3(1), 59–79. sexual consent communication. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 48,
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.1996.tb00104.x. 1723–1734. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-018-1348-7.
Theiss, J. A. (2011). Modeling dyadic effects of the associations between
relational uncertainty, sexual communication, and sexual satisfac-
tion for husbands and wives. Communication Research, 38(4), 565– Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdic-
584. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650211402186. tional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Troost, H./. C. (2008). Reclaiming touch: Rape culture, explicit verbal
consent, and body sovereignty. In J. Friedman & J. Valenti (Eds.),
Yes means Yes! (pp. 171–178). Berkley: Seal Press.

You might also like