You are on page 1of 10

Community Dent Oral Epidemiol 2006; 34: 310–19 Copyright Ó Blackwell Munksgaard 2006

All rights reserved

Anne E. Sanders1, Gary D. Slade1,


The shape of the socioeconomic– Gavin Turrell2, A. John Spencer1 and
Wagner Marcenes3

oral health gradient: implications


1
Australian Research Centre for Population
Oral Health (ARCPOH), The University of
Adelaide, Adelaide, SA, Australia, 2School of
Public Health, Queensland University of
for theoretical explanations Technology, Brisbane, Qld, Australia,
3
Center for Oral Biometrics, Queen Mary’s
School of Medicine and Dentistry, University
of London, London, UK
Sanders AE, Slade GD, Turrell G, John Spencer A, Marcenes W. The shape of the
socioeconomic–oral health gradient: implications for theoretical explanations.
Community Dent Oral Epidemiol 2006; 34: 310–19. Ó Blackwell Munksgaard,
2006

Abstract – Objectives: The nature of the relationship between status and health
has theoretical and applied significance. To compare the shape of the
socioeconomic -oral health relationship using a measure of relative social status
(MacArthur Scale of Subjective Social Status) and a measure of absolute material
resource (equivalised household income); to investigate the contribution of
behaviour in attenuating the socioeconomic gradient in oral health status; and
to comment on three hypothesised explanatory mechanisms for this
relationship (material, psychosocial, behavioural). Methods: In 2003, cross-
sectional self-report data were collected from 2,915 adults aged 43–57 years in
Adelaide, Australia using a stratified cluster design. Oral conditions were (1)
<24 teeth, (2) 1+ impact/s reported fairly often or very often on the 14-item Oral
Health Impact Profile; (3) fair or poor self-rated oral health, and (4) low
satisfaction with chewing ability. Prevalence ratios and 95% confidence
intervals (PR, 95%CI) were calculated from a logistic regression model.
Covariates were age, sex, country of birth, smoking, alcohol use, body mass
index, frequencies of toothbrushing and interdental cleaning. Results: There
was an approximately linear relationship of decreasing prevalence for each oral
condition across quintiles of increasing relative social status. In the fully
adjusted model the gradient was steepest for low satisfaction with chewing
(PR ¼ 4.1, 95%CI ¼ 3.0–5.4). Using equivalised household income, the shape
more closely resembled a threshold effect, with an approximate halving of the Key words: dental health surveys; income;
prevalence ratio between the first and second social status quintiles for the inequalities; middle aged; social hierarchy
adverse impact of oral conditions and fair or poor self-rated oral health. Anne Sanders, Australian Research Centre
Adjustment for covariates did not attenuate the magnitude of PRs. Conclusion: for Population Oral Health, Dental School,
The University of Adelaide, Adelaide 5005,
The nature of the relationship between social status and oral conditions differed
SA, Australia
according to the measure used to index social status. Perception of relative social Tel: +61 8 8303 4171
standing followed an approximately straight-line relationship. In contrast, there Fax: +61 8 8303 3070
was a discrete threshold of income below which oral health deteriorated, e-mail: anne.sanders@adelaide.edu.au
suggesting that the benefit to oral health of material resources occurs mostly at Submitted 15 August 2005;
the lower end of the across the full socioeconomic distribution. accepted 1 December 2005

The health of populations is better at higher levels (2–11). Findings are varied as to whether health
of socioeconomic position (1). Despite evidence of a deteriorates only below a critical level of socioeco-
graded relationship, there is a lack of empirical nomic resource (a threshold effect) or whether it
clarity about the shape of the gradient. Does health deteriorates across the full spectrum of decreasing
improve in a linear fashion across the entire socioeconomic resource (a linear or curvilinear
socioeconomic spectrum, for instance, or are there effect).
diminishing gains above a certain threshold? Very Analysis of the shape of socioeconomic-health
few studies have explicitly investigated the func- gradient can lead to greater theoretical clarity about
tional form of the socioeconomic gradient in health the production of health inequalities. Hypotheses

310
16000528, 2006, 4, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1600-0528.2006.00286.x by Cochrane Colombia, Wiley Online Library on [15/09/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Shape of the socioeconomic–oral health relationship

contend that the relationship is fundamentally status. In the oral health literature, no study has
based on absolute material conditions, or psycho- examined the shape of the relation using either
social response to relative social status, or relative status or absolute material resource.
status-related patterns of health behaviour (Fig. 1 The first aim of this study was to compare the
illustrates these mechanisms as depicted by shape of the socioeconomic gradient in oral condi-
Brunner and Marmot (12)). These postulated mech- tions using an indicator of relative social status and
anisms might operate simultaneously. an indicator of absolute material resource. A
Apart from its theoretical contribution, there is second aim was to determine the extent to which
applied value to allocative decision-making from selected health behaviours attenuated the socioe-
understanding the health benefit conveyed by conomic gradient in oral conditions. The third aim
additional socioeconomic units. As Blakely et al was to comment on three prominent hypothesized
(7) discussed if the shape is linear such that an mechanisms linking socioeconomic position to oral
additional unit of 44socioeconomic resource results health: the direct impact of absolute material
in an additional unit health gain, regardless of resource; the psychosocial impact of relative com-
affluence, then income redistribution from rich to parison in a social hierarchy; and the impact of
poor would not affect average rates of disease in status-related patterns of behaviour.
the population. Health gains for the poor would be Previous studies of the shape of the socioeco-
equivalent to the health loss for the rich. Alternat- nomic gradient have examined mortality, clinician-
ively if the gradient were threshold or curvilinear, assessed conditions and self-assessed health (2–11),
such that the slope flattened at higher levels of but not oral conditions. Oral conditions are highly
income, then the increased risk in health for the prevalent in economically developed countries and
rich resulting from an income transfer would be as such, have substantial importance to population
smaller than the decreased risk to health for the health.
poor. Not only would inequality in health reduce,
but also overall disease rates would reduce.
To date studies examining the shape of the
Methods
socioeconomic gradient have investigated indica-
tors such as income or occupation that describe Study and sampling designs
access to and control over material resources. Data were from the Adelaide Small Area Dental
Education has also been investigated, because it is Study, a cross-sectional study conducted in the
seen to reflect acquired levels of capital, knowledge Adelaide Statistical Division. This 1,826.9 km2 area
and skills. To date, no study of the shape of the had an estimated resident population of 1,066,103
socioeconomic-health gradient in the general in 2001 and contains the capital city of South
health literature has investigated relative social Australia and its metropolitan area. The study used

Social Material
structure factors

Work

Psychological
Social
environment Brain

Neuroendocrine
and immune
Health response
behaviours

Pathophysiological
Fig. 1. The social determinants of changes
health conceptual model showing a
Organ impairment
hypothesized direct pathway via Early Culture
material factors and an indirect pat- Genes
life
hway cognitve interpretation (Source: Well-being
12). By permission of Oxford Uni- Mortality
morbidity
versity Press.

311
16000528, 2006, 4, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1600-0528.2006.00286.x by Cochrane Colombia, Wiley Online Library on [15/09/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Sanders et al.

a stratified two-stage cluster design based on Best off


postcodes. To address research hypotheses, 40 Most education
persons were required from 60 small areas to yield Most money
Best jobs
an overall sample of 2,400 persons Adults in
Australia aged 43–57 years form the numerically
large post-war cohort and because they preceded
the introduction of water fluoridation, this cohort
has highly restored dentitions requiring ongoing
dental care maintenance.
After omitting nine postcodes with small popu-
lations (n < 600) the remaining 113 postcodes were
ranked according to their Index of Relative Socio-
economic Disadvantage (IRSD) score (13) into
deciles; from each decile six postcodes were
randomly selected creating a total of 60 postcodes.
Then, using electoral voting list, 70 individuals
aged 43 to 57 years were selected through simple
random sampling from each postcode for a total of
4,200 potential participants.
Data were collected between September and Worst off
December 2003 using a self-completed mailed Least education
questionnaire to sampled adults following recom- Least money
Worst jobs or no jobs
mended methods for mail surveys (14).
Fig. 2. The 10-rung ladder on which respondents indi-
Socioeconomic measures cated their relative social status relative to other people.
Relative social status was evaluated using the
MacArthur Scale of Subjective Social Status (15). household income was divided by the total
This scale has been used in health research (16–21) summed points to arrive at the ‘‘equalised income’’
and its test-retest reliability is established (21). In per household member. A midpoint estimate was
this study, participants were asked to place a cross arbitrarily assigned to the lowest and highest
on the rung of a 10-rung ladder and that best income ranges.
represented their perceived position relative to Quintiles were constructed for both status indi-
other people (see Fig. 2). The drawing was accom- cators. Quintiles of relative social status were
panied by the text of Singh-Manoux et al (19) derived by collapsing rungs one to four to form
‘‘Think of this ladder as representing where people the ‘low’ category and rungs 8–10 to form the ‘high’
stand in our society. At the top of the ladder are the category. Rungs five, six and seven formed categ-
people who are the best off, those who have the ories of ‘low-moderate’, ‘moderate’ and ‘moderate-
most money, most education, and best jobs. At the high’ respectively. Similarly, equivalised income
bottom are the people who are the worst off, those was divided into quintiles ranging from low to
who have the least money, least education, and high where low represented the most financially
worst jobs or no jobs. The higher up you are on this disadvantaged 20% of the distribution. Prevalence
ladder, the closer you are to people at the very top ratios (PRs) were calculated that compared each of
and the lower you are, the closer you are to the the first four socioeconomic quintiles with the
bottom’’ (p1323). highest quintile. Categories of both status indica-
The absolute material resource indicator was tors were grouped to create quintiles because we
equivalised household income. Respondents were wished to compare relative degrees of socioeco-
asked to indicate their total household income from nomic position that contrasted approximately
nine categories taken from the 2001 Census in equal numbers of subjects using both indicators.
Australia. To adjust for the size and composition of
households, an equivalence factor was computed Measures of oral morbidity
using the Modified Organisation for Economic Co- Four self-reported oral conditions were selected.
operation and Development equivalence scale (22). These were number of remaining teeth, the social
The midpoint value of each category of total impact of oral conditions evaluated with 14-item

312
16000528, 2006, 4, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1600-0528.2006.00286.x by Cochrane Colombia, Wiley Online Library on [15/09/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Shape of the socioeconomic–oral health relationship

Oral Health Impact Profile (23), global self-rated Measures of health behaviour
oral health (5-point ordinal scale), and global One hypothesis for the socioeconomic-health rela-
satisfaction with chewing ability (6-point ordinal tionship is that the poor health of disadvantaged
scale). Missing teeth was evaluated by first advi- groups is explained by a greater propensity for risk
sing participants that ‘‘There are 16 teeth including behaviour. In advocating a collaborative approach
wisdom teeth in the UPPER (upper case in text) to oral health promotion, Sheiham and Watt (27)
jaw.’’ Participants were then asked, ‘‘How many of list smoking, alcohol use and hygiene among their
these 16 do you have remaining in the upper jaw?’’ list of six common risk factors for many chronic
Similar information was provided to determine a conditions. We selected five behaviours with
count of remaining mandibular teeth and the plausible associations with oral morbidity. These
number of teeth in both arches was summed were smoking status, alcohol consumption, body
during analysis. Previous research has shown that mass index, frequency of toothbrushing and fre-
tooth count information obtained by self-assess- quency of interdental cleaning. Previously we have
ment yields valid data (24–25). Self-rated oral reported that risk behaviours for general and oral
health was evaluated with the question ‘‘Overall, health including the ones examined in this study
how would you rate your oral health?’’ to which tend to cluster together (28). Safe guidelines in
response categories were excellent, very good, Australia suggest consuming alcohol on fewer than
good, fair, poor. And chewing satisfaction was seven days each week and observing limits for
evaluated by asking, ‘‘How satisfied are you at males and females of no more than two and four
present with your ability to chew?’’ to which standard drinks per drinking session respectively.
response categories were completely satisfied, sat- Body mass index in the overweight and obese
isfied, reasonably satisfied, a little dissatisfied, categories reveals an energy imbalance where
dissatisfied, completely dissatisfied’’. energy intake exceeds energy expenditure. It is
Each of these measures was dichotomised to likely that frequent consumption of food and
produce a group with poor oral health that com- beverage is an important contributing factor and
prised approximately 20% of the sample. Preval- this same behaviour may also be implicated in
ence was estimated for (1) <24 teeth, (2) one or tooth loss among these individuals. BMI is also
more impact(s) reported fairly often or very often, associated with periodontitis, an additional cause
(3) fair or poor self-rated oral health and (4) low of tooth loss. We did not adjust for use of dental
satisfaction with chewing ability. services. Unlike these health behaviours, the use of
Several factors influenced the selection of cut dental services is limited by factors at a policy
points. A prevalence of approximately 20% was level, and are not seem to be within the realm of
one factor. This represents the poorest quintile of individual choice in the same way as these
oral health status and thus matches the quintile personal behaviours.
ranges in social status. Quintiles are commonly The University of Adelaide Human Ethics and
used in public health research. It was also Research Committee approved the study (#H80-
considered beneficial to compare oral conditions 2002).
affecting a similar proportion of the sample so
that any apparent differences in socioeconomic- Hypothesised mechanisms
oral health relationships could not be attributed Three prominent hypothesized mechanisms link
to an artefact of varying prevalence. Fewer than socioeconomic position to health status viz. the
24 remaining teeth represents less than two-thirds direct impact of absolute material resource; the
of the permanent dentition, and exceeds the psychosocial impact of relative comparison in a
number of missing teeth that a person might social hierarchy; and the impact of status-related
have with missing third molars and four ortho- behaviours. The conceptual diagram of Brunner
dontic extractions. We followed the precedent for and Marmot (Fig. 1) shows each of these pathways
prevalence of social impact set by Slade et al. labelled ‘‘Material factors’’, ‘‘Psychological’’ and
(26). Similarly convention determined the cut- ‘‘Health behaviours’’ respectively. Ultimately, the
point for self-rated oral health. The response behavioural and psychosocial mechanisms are
distribution determined the cut point for chewing thought to affect health indirectly via biological
satisfaction. A more restricted cut-point of a little process, whereas absolute material resource is
satisfied or worse included only 9.8% of the shown in the conceptual model to impact health
sample. directly. The two socioeconomic indicators pertain

313
16000528, 2006, 4, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1600-0528.2006.00286.x by Cochrane Colombia, Wiley Online Library on [15/09/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Sanders et al.

to two of these hypotheses. Equivalised household household income, 9.6% (n ¼ 281). Data for 2,221
income is an indicator of absolute material resource individuals were analysed after omitting cases with
while relative social status is an indicator of missing values for any of the variables examined.
psychosocial processes. These pathways are illus- The four dichotomised oral conditions had
trated in Fig. 1 along with the behavioural path- moderate to strong, bivariate associations, as
way. indexed by prevalence ratios between each pair of
conditions. Correlations ranged from 3.8 for the
Statistical analysis association between <24 teeth and fair or poor self-
To test the first aim, we computed prevalence ratios rated oral health to 9.8 for the association between
(PRs) as the primary indicator of socioeconomic one or more impact(s) reported fairly often or very
inequality across quintiles of social status. PRs and often and low satisfaction with chewing ability.
their 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) were Equivalised income and relative social status were
computed using parameter estimates and their moderately correlated, as evidenced by Pearson’s
variances/covariances obtained from binary logis- correlation coefficient of 0.41.
tic regression models (29). We used PRs rather than A graphic presentation (Fig. 3) revealed a con-
odds ratios as the latter are a poor approximation sistent shape in the relationship between relative
of proportional disease frequency and produce social status and each oral condition of a linear,
biased estimates when the condition is frequently monotonic relationship with prevalence decreasing
occurring (30). To test the second aim, three models at higher levels of perceived social status. Linearity
were constructed. Model 1 was unadjusted, and was most evident for dissatisfaction with chewing.
therefore contained only four dummy variables For this condition, prevalence decreased from 40.0
(one for the four lowest quintiles of socioeconomic percent in the lowest quintile to 11.3 percent in the
position) as predictor variables. To reduce potential highest quintile.
confounding, age in years, sex and country of birth A different, but consistent, shape was observed
(Australia or other), were entered in Model 2. for absolute material resource. It was characterised
These demographic factors are associated with as a threshold effect with a steep decrease in
socioeconomic resource but are not believed to be prevalence between the low and the low-moderate
in the causal pathway. Model 3 additionally quintiles, followed by flatter, less pronounced
adjusted for health related behaviours. decrease, across the remaining three quintiles. For
Data were weighted to correct for different three oral conditions (impacts experienced fairly/
probabilities in both sampling and in response. very often, fair or poor self-rated oral health and
These weights produced estimates of prevalence low satisfaction with chewing ability), prevalence
and PR that were representative of the population did not differ significantly between the moderate,
of electors in this age group in this geographic area. moderate-high and high quintiles. When the pre-
Analysis was conducted using SUDAAN to adjust ceding relationships were investigated separately
for the clustered sampling design and was limited for each sex, there was similar socioeconomic
to cases with non-missing values for all the vari- patterning in the oral conditions for males and
ables analysed in the study. females (not reported).
There were monotonic decreases in unadjusted
prevalence ratios across quintiles of relative social
status for all four oral conditions (Table 1, Model 1)
Results Unadjusted PRs for persons in the lowest com-
A response rate of 69.4% was achieved pared with the highest quintile ranged from 3.3
(n ¼ 2,915). Males comprised 45.7%, the mean (95% CI: 2.6, 4.3) for fair or poor self-rated oral
age was 50.1 years and 70% were born in Australia. health to 4.5 (95% CI: 3.3, 6.2) for oral health
Of the behaviours examined, 18% were current impacts. Adjustment for sex, age in years and
smokers, 18% exceeded Australian guidelines for country of birth in Model 2 did not noticeably
safe alcohol consumption and 60% were over- affect the magnitude of those PRs. Further adjust-
weight or obese. The range of responses for weekly ment for behaviour in Model 3 tended to attenuate
toothbrushing frequency was 0–55 (mean ¼ 11.5) the magnitude of PRs marginally, consistent with a
and for interdental cleaning was 0–30 flatter gradient, although PRs for most quintiles of
(mean ¼ 2.9). Non-response to the subjective relative social status remained statistically signifi-
social status question was 2.7% (n ¼ 80) and to cant, as evidenced by 95% CIs that excluded one.

314
16000528, 2006, 4, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1600-0528.2006.00286.x by Cochrane Colombia, Wiley Online Library on [15/09/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Shape of the socioeconomic–oral health relationship

Fewer than 24 teeth One or more impact(s) fairly/very often


50 Relative social status 50 Relative social status
Absolute material resource Absolute material resource

40 40
Per cent (± 95% CI)

Per cent (± 95% CI)


30 30

20 20

10 10

0 0
Low Mod High Low Mod High Low Mod High Low Mod High

Fair or poor self-rated oral health Low satisfaction with ability to chew
50 50
Relative social status
Relative social status
Absolute material resource Absolute material resource

40 40
Per cent (± 95% CI)

Per cent (± 95% CI)

30 30

20 20

10
10

0
Low Mod High Low Mod High 0
Low Mod High Low Mod High

Fig. 3. Prevalence of oral morbidity according to relative social status and absolute material resource (weighted data).

The linear shape to the gradient persisted in the adjusting for demographic and behavioural vari-
adjusted models for all four oral conditions. ables (Model 2). PRs for the other two oral
Compared with the preceding findings, there conditions were slightly attenuated in Model 3
was a weaker, unadjusted association between compared with Model 1, but not significantly so.
absolute material resource and each oral condition
as evidenced by smaller PRs for the lowest quintile
(Table 1). For example, the lowest quintile of
Discussion
equivalised income had a 2.5-fold increase in
unadjusted prevalence of tooth loss compared with When measured using relative social status, the
the highest quintile, whereas the corresponding PR shape of the socioeconomic gradient was approxi-
using relative status was 3.4. Furthermore, at levels mately linear for all four oral conditions. The shape
of material resource above the lowest quintile, resembled more of a threshold effect for all condi-
unadjusted PRs were close to one, and in many tions characterised by a substantial decrease in
instances they were not statistically significant. PRs prevalence from the first to the second quintile. For
for two oral conditions (<24 teeth and dissatisfac- two conditions the prevalence ratios approximately
tion with ability to chew) remained unaltered after halved between the first and second quintiles in the

315
16000528, 2006, 4, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1600-0528.2006.00286.x by Cochrane Colombia, Wiley Online Library on [15/09/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Sanders et al.

Table 1. Prevalence ratios (PR) with 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) for the association between oral morbidity and
socioeconomic position (relative social status and absolute material resource)
Relative social status Absolute material resource
a b c
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 1a Model 2b Model 3c
Oral health condition Quintile PR 95% CI PR 95% CI PR 95% CI PR 95% CI PR 95% CI PR 95% CI
Fewer than 24 teeth Low 3.4 2.6, 4.6 3.9 2.8, 5.4 2.9 2.0, 4.1 2.3 1.7, 3.1 2.7 1.9, 3.82.1 1.5, 3.0
Low-mod 2.4 1.8, 3.2 2.6 1.9, 3.6 2.1 1.5, 3.0 1.7 1.2, 2.2 1.9 1.4, 2.61.7 1.2, 2.4
Moderate 1.7 1.2, 2.5 1.8 1.3, 2.6 1.5 1.0, 2.2 1.4 1.1, 1.9 1.7 1.2, 2.41.6 1.1, 2.3
Mod-high 1.5 1.1, 2.0 1.6 1.2, 2.3 1.4 1.0, 2.0 0.7 0.5, 1.2 1.0 0.6, 1.60.9 0.6, 1.5
High (ref) 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
1+ impact(s) fairly often Low 4.5 3.3, 6.2 4.2 3.0, 5.8 3.4 2.5, 4.7 3.7 2.7, 5.1 3.5 2.6, 4.8 3.1 2.2, 4.3
or very often Low-mod 2.5 1.7, 3.7 2.4 1.6, 3.5 2.1 1.4, 3.1 1.9 1.3, 2.8 1.8 1.2, 2.7) 1.7 1.1, 2.5
Moderate 1.9 1.3, 2.9 1.8 1.2, 2.7 1.6 1.1, 2.4 1.7 1.3, 2.4 1.7 1.3, 2.4 1.7 1.2, 2.3
Mod-high 1.5 1.0, 2.2 1.5 1.0, 2.1 1.3 0.9, 1.9 0.9 0.6, 1.5 0.9 0.6, 1.5 0.9 0.6, 1.5
High (ref) 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
Fair or poor self-rated Low 3.3 2.6, 4.3 3.8 2.9, 5.0 3.0 2.3, 4.0 2.2 1.8, 2.8 2.5 1.9, 3.2 2.1 1.6, 2.7
oral health Low-mod 1.8 1.3, 2.4 1.9 1.4, 2.6 1.6 1.1, 2.3 1.3 1.0, 1.7 1.3 1.0, 1.7 1.1 0.8, 1.5
Moderate 1.5 1.2, 2.0 1.6 1.3, 2.1 1.4 1.1, 1.9 1.2 0.9, 1.5 1.2 0.9, 1.6 1.1 0.8, 1.5
Mod-high 1.4 1.0, 2.0 1.4 1.0, 2.1 1.3 0.9, 1.9 0.8 0.6, 1.2 0.8 0.6, 1.2 0.8 0.6, 1.2
High (ref) 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
Low satisfaction with Low 3.9 3.1, 5.1 4.3 3.2, 5.8 4.1 3.0, 5.4 2.2 1.7, 2.8 2.3 1.8, 3.0 2.2 1.7, 2.9
chewing ability Low-mod 2.6 1.9, 3.5 2.7 2.0, 3.8 2.6 1.9, 3.6 1.4 1.1, 1.8 1.5 1.2, 1.9 1.5 1.1, 1.9
Moderate 2.1 1.6, 2.8 2.2 1.6, 2.9 2.1 1.6, 2.8 1.2 1.0, 1.6 1.3 1.0, 1.7 1.3 1.0, 1.7
Mod-high 1.7 1.3, 2.3 1.8 1.3, 2.4 1.7 1.3, 2.4 0.8 0.5, 1.2 0.9 0.6, 1.3 0.9 0.6, 1.3
High (ref) 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
a
Model 1: unadjusted.
b
Model 2: adjusted for sex; age in years; country of birth (Australia or not Australia).
c
Model 3: adjusted for sex; age in years; country of birth (Australia or not Australia); smoking status; body mass index;
frequency of toothbrushing; frequency of interdental cleaning.

fully adjusted model. For impacts fairly often or magnitude that either shape is possible. We deci-
very often, the PR estimate decreased from 3.1 ded against the expression ‘‘curvilinear’’ since
(95%CI: 2.2, 4.3) to 1.7 (95%CI: 1.1, 2.5) between the decrease in prevalence for all four oral conditions
low and low-mod quintiles and for fair or poor self- did not diminish at each successive quintile.’’
rated oral health the PR estimate decreased from One explanation for socioeconomic inequality in
2.1 95%CI: to 1.1 (95% CI: 2.2, 4.3). For all four oral health is that risk behaviours lie in the causal
conditions, the slope of the socioeconomic gradient pathway between socioeconomic position and oral
was flatter using equivalised household income health and are more prevalent among socioeco-
than the relative social status indicator. nomically disadvantaged groups. We thought it
There is some uncertainty whether the shape of instructive to examine this explanation, and our
the relationship across relative social status quin- results demonstrate an insignificant role played by
tiles is linear or curvilinear. Examination of the these behaviours in explaining inequalities in these
ratio of estimated prevalence between successive oral conditions Adjustment for demographic and
quintiles in the fully adjusted model does not behavioural risk factors did not significantly
reveal a flattening of the relationship at higher attenuate the gradient for any oral health condition.
levels of status that would be consistent with a In other research we found evidence that while use
curvilinear relationship. For example, for fewer of dental services flattened the slope of gradient in
than 24 remaining teeth, the prevalence ratio self-reported oral health, dental self-care behaviour
between successive relative status quintiles was did not significantly attenuate the gradient (31). It
1.4, 1.4, 1.1 and 1.4 respectively. For the other three is possible that while risk behaviours are important
oral conditions the prevalence ratio between the determinants of health, they do not account for
mod-high and high quintiles was greater than it relationships between social status and oral health
was between the moderate and mod-high quintiles, observed here. In one intervention study that
also non-consistent with curvilinearity. Yet disper- provided oral hygiene instruction to children, oral
sion around the point estimates is of sufficient hygiene practices were found to produce a steeper

316
16000528, 2006, 4, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1600-0528.2006.00286.x by Cochrane Colombia, Wiley Online Library on [15/09/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Shape of the socioeconomic–oral health relationship

socioeconomic gradient in oral health as the proportions had been sampled from each decile of
desired behaviour was adopted more quickly area disadvantage. Response tended to be lower in
among the more advantaged individuals (32). more disadvantaged deciles. Hence the 10 sam-
A strength of this study was the comparison of pling deciles individually represented between
two theoretically different socioeconomic indica- 7.3% and 13.1% of the total sample yield. It was
tors. Findings of a different functional form for less than 10% in three of the five most disadvantage
each will be useful in debate over possible mech- deciles, but in only one of the five least disadvan-
anisms underlying the reasons for socioeconomic taged deciles.
inequalities. Such debate is beyond the scope of It is possible that a characteristic such as positive
this paper, but findings support the view that or negative affect may influence perceptions of
beliefs about relative socioeconomic position may both relative social status and perceptions of oral
be more strongly associated with health status than conditions in one direction. Nevertheless these
objective indicators of absolute material resource perceptions of status and health are real regardless
(16) among more advantaged population groups, of whether or not they are subject to psychological
while absolute income may matter more at the impact. We point out that missing teeth, while self-
disadvantaged tail of the distribution. reported, is still an objective condition as are
A potential limitation is non-reporting of house- household income and family composition. The
hold income. If non-reporting is socioeconomically fact that the shape of the relationships was consis-
patterned, missing data may adversely affect the tent irrespective of whether the oral condition
estimates for low socioeconomic groups if these measure was subjective or objective and irrespect-
have a greater proportion of missing responses. ive of whether the socioeconomic measure was
However Turrell (33) found that respondents on subjective or objective indicates that any bias that
high incomes were most likely to not report their might arise by such mechanisms is not evident in
income and that income non-reporting was lowest these results.
among the unemployed and those receiving gov- Previous studies that examined the MacArthur
ernment support. Another limitation is the cluster- Scale of Subjective Social Status have not reported
ing in the responses around the central rungs of the the shape of the socioeconomic gradient and hence
ladder used here to measure relative social status. comparisons with this study are not possible.
This necessitated collapsing rungs 1–4 and rungs Several studies have examined the shape using
8–10 which compromised the sensitivity of the material resource indicators (2–11), including
scale and our ability to present what was otherwise equivalised income. For example, a comparison of
a finely graduated monotonic linear distribution health survey data collected from 10 European
across the 10 rungs for all four oral health countries found the gradient between quintiles of
outcomes. The use of quintiles was an empirically equivalised household income and fair/poor self-
defensible method of creating approximately rated general health was linear (11). Yet a threshold
numerically equivalent groups. We recommend effect separated the lowest income quintile from
that methodological work be conducted on the the remainder of the population in Britain, while
psychophysical properties of the ladder that could the Nordic countries of Finland, Sweden, Norway
enable its use as an interval scale rather than as and Denmark had flatter gradients. A near linear
ordinal level of measurement. relationship was also observed between net equiv-
We compared participants with missing and alised household income and mortality in a pros-
non-missing values for the variables examined in pective study in Finland (9). Ecob and Davey Smith
this study. Participants with missing values were (4) investigated the gradient between equivalised
significantly more likely (p < 0.05, Chi-square) to household income and both self-assessed and
be female, to have lower relative social status examiner-assessed health using national survey
scores and fewer than 24 remaining teeth. We data in the United Kingdom. They found the
found no significance difference (p > 0.05, Chi- gradient for each health measure was approxi-
square) for country of birth, equivalised income mately linear between the 10th and 90th percentile,
and the three other oral health conditions. Two but differed significantly from linearity at the
variables that stood out as having substantially extreme ends of the distribution.
higher frequencies of missing values were equiv- A curvilinear gradient characterised by a steep
alised income and body mass index. We also slope at lower levels of the socioeconomic distri-
examined variation in study participation as equal bution and a gradual attenuation at higher levels

317
16000528, 2006, 4, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1600-0528.2006.00286.x by Cochrane Colombia, Wiley Online Library on [15/09/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Sanders et al.

has also been observed (2, 3, 6, 10). Such a gradient indicator used. We suggest that the two indicators
implies that health at the low end of social status is are tapping different mechanisms that contribute to
more sensitive to changes in income than is health observe social inequalities in oral conditions. The
at the upper end of social status. Although a introduction of relative social status into studies of
threshold effect is less commonly reported, the shape of the socioeconomic gradient in health
Backlund et al (2) found a threshold effect in the highlights the importance of perceived social rela-
shape of gradient between income and mortality in tivities. Results supported the role of psychosocial
the United States in some age-sex groups where factors in explaining variation in health at the
differences in mortality ceased above a particular advantaged tail of the distribution while absolute
level of income. material resource was more sensitive at the most
If poor health precedes socioeconomic position disadvantaged tail of the distribution. To maximise
in the causal pathway, then there would be little the value of the MacArthur Scale of Subjective
point in addressing socioeconomic factors to Social Status we encourage more work into its
reduce socioeconomic inequalities in health. How- psychophysical properties. Results add further
ever selection or reverse causation plays only a doubt to the importance of the behavioural hypo-
minor role and most evidence shows that socio- thesis for explaining health inequalities.
economic conditions precede health outcomes (34,
35). In 1998 the World Health Organization in
Europe (36) listed the social gradient first among
Acknowledgements
ten factors identified as the key social determinants
of health and a major contributor to unequal health The study was supported by a project grant from the
National Health and Medical Research Council (no.
outcomes in populations. 250315). Dr. Turrell is supported by a National Health
A problem that continues to baffles theorists and and Medical Research Council/National Heart Founda-
policy makers alike is how socioeconomic condi- tion Career Development Award (CR 01B 0502).
tions translate into health outcomes. Various the-
oretical perspectives have been put forward and of
these, two commonly contended explanations are
References
the materialist and the psychosocial arguments.
1. Adler NE, Boyce T, Chesney MA, Cohen S, Folkman
The former asserts health is responsive to absolute
S, Kahn RL et al. Socioeconomic status and health.
levels of material resource. Not only does income The challenge of the gradient. Am Psychol
permit access to timely and comprehensive health 1994;49:15–24.
care, it also provides opportunities for a whole 2. Backlund E, Sorlie PD, Johnson NJ. The shape of the
relationship between income and mortality in the
constellation of choices that affect health. In this
United States. Evidence from the National Longitud-
study, equivalised household income was an indi- inal Mortality Study. Ann Epidemiol 1996;6:12–20.
cator of absolute material resource that might be 3. Backlund E, Sorlie PD, Johnson NJ. A comparison of
used to explore this explanation. The psychological the relationships of education and income with
explanation places much less emphasis on material mortality: the National Longitudinal Mortality
Study. Soc Sci Med 1999;49:1373–84.
resource per se, but rather asserts that the meaning 4. Ecob R, Davey Smith G. Income and health: what is
that people assign to their relative social standing is the nature of the relationship? Soc Sci Med
critical. The relative social status indicator in this 1999;48:693–705.
study specifically is consistent with this explan- 5. Martikainen P, Makela P, Koskinen S, Valkonen T.
Income differences in mortality: a register-based
ation. The findings suggest that perceptions of follow-up study of three million men and women.
relative position influenced oral health over and Int J Epidemiol 2001;30:1397–405.
above the influence of absolute levels of material 6. Finch BK. Socioeconomic gradients and low birth-
resource. However at the lower tail of the socioe- weight: empirical and policy considerations. Health
Serv Res 2003;38:1819–41.
conomic distribution, absolute material resources 7. Blakely T, Kawachi I, Atkinson J, Fawcett J. Income
are associated with greatest gains to oral health. and mortality: the shape of the association and
confounding New Zealand Census-Mortality Study,
1981–1999. Int J Epidemiol 2004;33:874–83.
8. Schnittker J. Education and the changing shape of the
Conclusion income gradient in health. J Health Soc Behav
2004;45:286–305.
The shape of the socioeconomic-oral health rela- 9. Martikainen P, Adda J, Ferrie JE, Davey Smith G,
tionship differs according to the socioeconomic Marmot M. Effects of income and wealth on GHQ

318
16000528, 2006, 4, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1600-0528.2006.00286.x by Cochrane Colombia, Wiley Online Library on [15/09/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Shape of the socioeconomic–oral health relationship

depression and poor self rated health in white 21. Operario D, Adler NE, William DR. Subjective social
collar women and men in the Whitehall II study. J status: reliability and predictive utility for global
Epidemiol Community Health 2003;57:718–23. health. Psychol Health 2004;19:237–46.
10. Mackenbach JP, Martikainen P, Looman CW, Dal- 22. Australian Bureau of Statistics. Household Income
stra JA, Kunst AE, Lahelma E. The shape of the and Income Distribution, 2000–01. Cat. no. 6523.0.
relationship between income and self-assessed Australia: Australian Bureau of Statistics; 2003. p. 40–
health: an international study. Int J Epidemiol 1.
2005;34:286–93. 23. Slade GD. Derivation and validation of a short-form
11. Kunst AE, Bos V, Lahelma E, Bartley M, Lissau I, oral health impact profile. Community Dent Oral
Regidor E et al. Trends in socioeconomic inequalities Epidemiol 1997;25:284–90.
in self-assessed health in 10 European countries. Int J 24. Pitiphat W, Garcia RI, Douglass CW, Joshipura KJ.
Epidemiol 2005;34:295–305. Validation of self-reported oral health measures. J
12. Brunner E, Marmot M. Chapter 2. In: Marmot M, Public Health Dent 2002;62:122–8.
Wilkinson RG, editors. Social organization, stress, 25. Gilbert GH, Duncan RP, Kulley AM. Validity of self-
and health in social determinants of health. Oxford: reported tooth counts during a telephone screening
Oxford University Press; 1999. p. 21. interview. J Public Health Dent 1997;57:176–80.
13. Australian Bureau of Statistics. Census of Population 26. Slade GD, Nuttall N, Sanders AE, Steele JG, Allen PF,
and Housing: Socio-Economic Indexes for Area’s Lahti S. Impacts of oral disorders in the United
(SEIFA), Technical Paper Catalogue number. Kingdom and Australia. Br Dent J 2005;198:489–93.
2039.0.55.001. Australia: Australian Bureau of Statis- 27. Sheiham A, Watt RG. The common risk factor
tics; 2001. approach: a rational basis for promoting oral health.
14. Dillman DA. Mail and Internet surveys – the tailored Community Dent Oral Epidemiol 2000;28:399–406.
design method. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; 28. Sanders AE, Spencer AJ, Stewart JF. Clustering of
2000. risk behaviours for oral and general health. Com-
15. Adler NE, Epel ES, Castellazzo G, Ickovics JR. munity Dent Health 2005;22:133–40.
Relationship of subjective and objective social status 29. Slade GD, Gansky SA, Spencer AJ. Two-year inci-
with psychological and physiological functioning: dence of tooth loss among South Australians aged
preliminary data in healthy white women. Health 60+ years. Community Dent Oral Epidemiol
Psychol 2000;19:586–92. 1997;25:429–37.
16. Ostrove JM, Adler NE, Kuppermann M, Washington 30. Kleinbaum D, Kupper L, Morgenstern H. Epidemi-
AE. Objective and subjective assessments of socioe- ologic research: principles and quantitative methods.
conomic status and their relationship to self-rated New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold; 1982. p. 184–5.
health in an ethnically diverse sample of pregnant 31. Sanders AE, Spencer AJ, Slade GD. Evaluating the
women. Health Psychol 2000;19:613–8. role of dental behaviour in oral health inequalities.
17. Goodman E, Adler NE, Daniels SR, Morrison JA, Community Dent Oral Epidemiol 2006;34:71–9.
Slap GB, Dolan LM. Impact of objective and subject- 32. Schou L, Wight C. Does dental health education
ive social status on obesity in a biracial cohort of affect inequalities in dental health? Community Dent
adolescents. Obes Res 2003;11:1018–26. Health 1994;11:97–100.
18. Goodman E, Adler NE, Kawachi I, Frazier AL, 33. Turrell G. Income non-reporting: implications for
Huang B, Colditz GA. Adolescents’ perceptions of health inequalities research. J Epidemiol Community
social status: development and evaluation of a new Health 2000;54:207–14.
indicator. Pediatrics 2001;108:E31. 34. Fein O. The influence of social class on health status:
19. Singh-Manoux A, Adler NE, Marmot MG. Subjective American and British research on health inequalities.
social status: its determinants and its association J Gen Intern Med 1995;10:577–86.
with measures of ill-health in the Whitehall II study. 35. Blane D, Davey Smith G, Bartley M. Social selection:
Soc Sci Med 2003;56:1321–33. what does it contribute to social class differences in
20. Kopp M, Skrabski A, Rethelyi J, Kawachi I, Adler health? Sociol Health Illn 1993;15:1–15.
NE. Self-rated health, subjective social status, and 36. Wilkinson R, Marmot M, editors. Social determinants
middle-aged mortality in a changing society. Behav of health: the solid facts. Geneva: World Health
Med 2004;30:65–70. Organization; 1998. p. 10–1.

319

You might also like