You are on page 1of 9

Perspective

Autonomic dysregulation and the Window


of Tolerance model of the effects of complex
emotional trauma Journal of Psychopharmacology
25(1) 17–25
! The Author(s) 2011
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0269881109354930
jop.sagepub.com

FM Corrigan1, JJ Fisher2 and DJ Nutt3

Abstract
This paper reviews the Window of Tolerance model of the long-term effects of the severe emotional trauma associated with childhood abuse, a model
which can also be applied to adult trauma of sufficient severity to cause post-traumatic stress disorder, chronic dysthymic disorders and chronic anxiety
disorders. Dysfunctional behaviours such as deliberate self-harm and substance abuse are seen as efforts to regulate an autonomic nervous system
which is readily triggered into extreme states by reminders of the original traumatic events. While midbrain areas such as the periaqueductal gray
mediate instant defence responses to traumatic events and their memory triggers it is proposed that ascending monoaminergic tracts are implicated in
longer-term changes in mood and arousal. An imbalance of ascending dopaminergic tracts may drive rapid fluctuations in level of arousal and in the
associated mood, drive and motivation. Animal models of depression frequently use traumatic experiences of pain, isolation or social defeat to induce
changes in mesolimbic and mesocortical dopamine systems which may alter prefrontal cortical control of midbrain defence responses. A focus on the
pharmacology of the Window of Tolerance could provide advances in drug treatments for promoting emotional regulation in those who are suffering
from the chronic sequelae of traumatic experiences.

Keywords
Ascending dopamine systems, autonomic dysregulation, dysfunctional behaviours, trauma, window of tolerance

The psychophysiology of traumatic experience parasympathetic non-responsiveness or submission–compli-


ance responses.
It has been established that exposure to threat or trauma
stimulates the autonomic nervous system (ANS), resulting Physiological arousal and the Window of
in sympathetic hyperarousal and parasympathetic Tolerance
(dorsal-vagal-mediated) hypoarousal states accompanying
animal defence survival responses such as fight, flight, sub- One model for understanding and explaining the fluctuations
mission and freeze (LeDoux, 2002; Ogden et al., 2006; Porges, in clinical features that can occur unpredictably and rapidly in
2003; Van der Kolk, 1996a, 1996b). Following cessation of the disorders that arise from the effects of severe trauma
the threat, many victims continue to suffer from autonomic (Ogden et al., 2006) is the ‘Window of Tolerance’ model of
sensitivity to stimuli directly or indirectly related to the trau- autonomic arousal (Siegel, 1999). Siegel (1999) proposes that
matic events. Thus, threatening and traumatic experiences between the extremes of sympathetic hyperarousal and para-
result in a bewildering array of cognitive, emotional and phys- sympathetic hypoarousal is a ‘window’ or range of optimal
iological symptoms: emotions of fear, shame and rage; numb- arousal states in which emotions can be experienced as toler-
ing of feelings and body sensations; overactivity of the stress able and experience can be integrated (Figure 1). Although it
response system; and painful, negative beliefs about the self has not yet been validated experimentally, the pleomorphic
that serve to intensify the distressing feelings and body complex trauma disorders that are the result of childhood
responses. With a dysregulated nervous system that cannot abuse become more comprehensible within this framework
modulate either heightened emotional states or states of and there is evidence that different emotions are accompanied
depression and numbing, patients often report difficulty in by distinct patterns of ANS response with sympathetic
tolerating emotional and physiological arousal without
becoming overwhelmed, as well as problems in recovering 1
Argyll & Bute Hospital, Lochgilphead, Argyll, UK.
from experiences of intense activation or depression (Ogden 2
Center for Integrative Healing, Watertown, Massachusetts, USA.
et al., 2006; Van der Kolk, 1996a). In addition, they report 3
Department of Neuropsychopharmacology and Molecular Imaging,
strong somatic responses in which the body tends to become Imperial College, London, UK.
frozen, collapsed or driven: action becomes either impossible
or impulsive. Non-threatening situational cues often activate Corresponding author:
sympathetic nervous system (SNS) activity and fight– FM Corrigan, Argyll & Bute Hospital, Lochgilphead, Argyll PA31 8LD, UK
flight responses, while dangerous situations instead elicit Email: frank.corrigan@nhs.net

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MONTANA on April 6, 2015


18 Journal of Psychopharmacology 25(1)

Sympathetic-dominant Hyperarousal: Freeze


Emotionally flooded, reactive, impulsive, hypervigilant, fearful, angry. Mute, terrified, frozen
Intrusive imagery and affects, racing thoughts defence responses.
Flashbacks, nightmares, high-risk behaviour High arousal coupled
Efforts to reduce this state may include suicide planning, self harm, with physical immobility*
compulsive cleaning, abuse of alcohol or opiates

Window of Tolerance
Optimal arousal zone, encompassing both intense emotion
and states of calm or relaxation, in which emotions can be
tolerated and information integrated

Parasympathetic-dominant Hypoarousal:
Flat affect, numb, “empty” or “dead”
Cognitively dissociated, inability to think
Collapsed, disabled defensive responses
Helpless and hopeless
Efforts to reduce may include suicide planning, self-harm, compulsive

Figure 1. Autonomic arousal in the wake of trauma: sympathetic hyperarousal and parasympathetic hypoarousal states drive emotional and autonomic
dysregulation. Also shown is the frozen fear state in which both extremes may be present. States of optimal arousal and emotional regulation are
relatively rare or difficult to maintain. Some examples are included of how dysfunctional behaviours can be utilised in the service of emotional
regulation: both to reduce the intensity of the high arousal states and the depth of the low arousal states. Adapted from Ogden et al (2006) and Siegel
(1999).

activation accompanied by varying degrees of parasympa- dissociation or even ‘feigned death’ (Porges, 2003), a state
thetic deactivation (Rainville et al., 2006). Other studies from which the prospect of dying can seem a welcome release.
have observed that trauma-related symptoms and emotions The PNS-dominant state of near death is accompanied by
may be accompanied by more extreme autonomic effects flaccidity of the muscles and is therefore distinguishable
(Lanius, 2005; Perry et al., 1995). In a study of individuals from the tonic immobilization state (‘scared stiff’ or cata-
with borderline personality disorder, a diagnosis that has tonic) (Moskowitz, 2004), in which high physiological arousal
been correlated with childhood trauma (Herman et al., is combined with an inability to engage in voluntary move-
1989; Ogata et al., 1990; Zanarini et al., 1989) subjects ment of the body, a peritraumatic orienting and defence
demonstrated sympathetically-driven active defence responses response postulated to be secondary to simultaneous SNS
to social interaction with a stranger, in contrast with controls and PNS co-activation (Ogden et al., 2006). Peritraumatic
who were more able to readily engage the ‘vagal brake’ (the tonic immobility has been associated with severe psychologi-
ventral vagal complex) for rapid attunement with the other cal sequelae of depression and anxiety years after the child-
person (Austin et al., 2007). While emotional dysregulation hood sexual abuse in which it was experienced, when fear and
driving maladaptive efforts to diminish distress has provided helplessness were combined with an inability to escape from
a useful model for treating borderline personality disorder the horrifying situation (Bovin et al., 2008; Heidt et al., 2005).
with dialectical behaviour therapy (Linehan, 1993), and can More recently the occurrence of tonic immobility in male
be adapted to many situations in which harmful actions occur victims of urban violence has been associated with a poor
in response to distress, the autonomic dysregulation model response to drug therapy (Fiszman et al., 2008).
allows additional hypotheses about the associated psychophy- This Window of Tolerance model (Ogden et al., 2006;
siology of involuntary somatic responses to triggers that Siegel, 1999) describes all these animal defensive states
evoke some component of a trauma memory. (Figure 1): the hyperarousal connected to sympathetic activa-
tion, the autonomic hypoarousal characteristic of parasym-
pathetically mediated dorsal vagal responses, and the ‘deer in
Trauma, negative cognitions and affective the headlights’ frozen flight response connected to high SNS
states outside the Window of Tolerance and PNS co-activation. In addition, the Window of Tolerance
as an explanatory conceptualization also prescribes a treat-
When a traumatic event is overwhelming and inescapable, the ment approach: that of regulating autonomic arousal within a
associated high arousal emotional state may not be easily Window of Tolerance in which affect and cognition can be
modulated, leading to inhibition of cortical activity tolerated so that patients can both think and feel (Fisher and
(LeDoux, 2002; Ogden et al., 2006), a loss of cognitive Ogden, 2007). When patients with trauma-related disorders,
witnessing, and perhaps even to states of speechless rage or such as post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and borderline
blind terror (Van der Kolk, 1996b). Alternatively, the dorsal personality disorder, develop greater ability to self-regulate
vagal branch of the parasympathetic nervous system (PNS) autonomic arousal, symptoms tend to diminish or ameliorate,
may become activated (Porges, 1995) leading to a state of and they are able to engage more effectively in
total submission characterized by numbing, collapse, well-established treatments for trauma. In addition, use of a

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MONTANA on April 6, 2015


Corrigan et al. 19

model that emphasizes the role of the ANS in perpetuating McHaffie et al. (2005). The behaviours associated with fight
post-traumatic symptoms addresses both physiological hyper- or flight have sufficient similarity in different people to suggest
arousal symptoms (high-risk behaviour, suicidal impulses, that there are intrinsic tendencies to activate particular muscle
self-injury) and hypoarousal symptoms (shame, dysthymia, groups when the situation requires specific actions to ensure
depression, loss of energy, self-loathing) (Figure 1). survival. Grillner et al. (2005) describe a toolbox of motor
programs which is kept under tonic inhibition by gamma
aminobutyric acid (GABA)ergic pallidal neurons (including
Emotions, defence responses and the mid- those of the substantia nigra pars reticulata) until a selection
brain periaqueductal grey is made in the striatum. A failure of selection from two dis-
inhibited programs would lead to the state of frozen flight or
One hypothesis offering an explanatory model for these frozen fight accompanying the ANS co-activation.
effects of traumatic experiences on arousal levels involves The neurochemistry of PAG activation is complex, invol-
the midbrain periaqueductal grey (PAG), which is a key struc- ving, to select a few examples, 5-hydroxytryptamine (5HT)-
ture for mediating the physiological effects of defence activated inputs from the basolateral amygdala (Martinez
responses. Dorsolateral columns are involved in active, sym- et al., 2007), cholinergic stimulation of the central amygdala
pathetically driven fight or flight behaviours while ventrolat- (Leite-Panissi et al., 2003), GABA-A modulated 5HT activa-
eral areas are engaged in parasympathetically dominant tion of the ventrolateral orbital prefrontal cortex (Huo et al.,
freeze reactions (Bandler and Shipley, 1994; Watt, 2000). 2008) and intrinsic acetylcholine (Kroes et al., 2007), GABA
Recently it has been proposed that the dorsomedial column (De Menezes et al., 2006; Reimer et al., 2008), cytokines
has specificity for the modulation of aversive, avoidant states (Bhatt et al., 2008), opioids (Kishimoto et al., 2001), canna-
with corticotrophin-releasing factor (CRF) exerting an exci- binoids (Terzian et al., 2008), CRF (Carvalho-Netto et al.,
tatory influence on this region (Borelli and Brandao, 2007). It 2007), glutamate and 5HT (Moraes et al., 2008).
will be of interest to see whether more precise techniques for
imaging the brainstem reveal an association of the dorsome-
dial column with trauma-related avoidant responses of dis- Affect regulation and the Window of Tolerance
gust and shame as activation of the dorsomedial column leads
to conditioned place aversion (Zanoveli et al., 2007). The During multiple or prolonged exposures to childhood trau-
Window of Tolerance model displayed in Figure 1 sees matic events (including sexual abuse, physical abuse, expo-
freeze responses as the result of co-activation of sympathetic sure to domestic violence, and trauma related to medical
and parasympathetic components of the ANS and this, in treatments or accidents), hyper- and hypoarousal states
PAG terms, would mean co-activation of dorsolateral and occur not only in response to threatening events but also to
ventrolateral columns. However, dorsomedial PAG stimula- their anticipation. After repeated SNS/PNS activation during
tion for conditioned place aversion is associated with formative years, the adult in later life may have difficulties
increased open field freezing, possibly of the attentive, in regulating affect (Ogden et al., 2006; Schore, 2003;
information-gathering type (Zanoveli et al., 2007), and there Van der Kolk, 1996a), resulting in prolonged states of fear
are two other types of freezing (stimulus bound and and anger (SNS-dominance) or despair and depression
non-stimulus bound) produced by activation of dorsal aspects (PNS-dominance). In the affectively flattened hypoaroused
of PAG (Brandao et al., 2008). Thus it is possible that there state, the trauma survivor may feel that life is not worth
are several different types of freeze response depending on living, that nothing matters, that death would be a relief.
which dorsal PAG columns are co-activated with the There is a lack of energy and enthusiasm, and a reported
low-arousal, opioid-mediated ventrolateral area responsible absence of distress may coexist with the matter-of-fact
for conditioned freeze with its bradycardia, hypotension suicidal thinking. This presentation may mimic, or coexist
and energy-conservation functions. The PAG activation of with, a major depressive episode, and prescription of antide-
other brainstem structures mediates the autonomic changes pressants is the obvious choice for the clinician. If antidepres-
required by emotions and defence responses and the resulting sant use leads to an increase in anxiety and arousal, there can
changes in the physiological state of the body are fed back be a switch to a hyperaroused state (in which the energy for
through the thalamus to insular and anterior cingulate corti- a suicide attempt is available) or a rapid alteration between
ces (Craig, 2005). Distressing emotions associated with the the extremes. If suicidal thinking is initially a way of soothing
sympathetic arousal of trauma, such as fear, anger and dis- the empty nihilism and despair of the hypoaroused state
gust, are also dependent on activation of the right anterior (Herman, 1992), antidepressant-related autonomic activation
insular cortex. The concomitant opponent deactivation of the may result in the ideas taking on fresh urgency as a way to
left insular cortex (Craig, 2005) may lead to loss of feelings of end intense suffering. Similarly, a temporary intensification of
comfort and safety and difficulties in articulating the experi- the hypoarousal state would increase suicidal thinking, which,
ence. During treatment with a safe, empathic therapist if followed by increased energy, could result in impulsive
encouraging non-judgemental mindfulness, and through acting out. The converse may also occur when suicidal think-
training in skills for soothing self-regulation (e.g. Fay, ing is initially a way of soothing intense activation, unbear-
2007), the balance of insula activation could move to able impulses and overwhelming emotions. Increased focus
left-dominance. and autonomic stability as a result of increased serotonin
Motor aspects of defence responses will require subcortical reuptake inhibition may result in more ability to plan and
circuits through the PAG similar to those described by implement actions.

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MONTANA on April 6, 2015


20 Journal of Psychopharmacology 25(1)

Ascending tracts and levels of arousal and other dysfunctional behaviours in patients with a history
of trauma is that these substances and the associated compul-
Although the acute physiological effects of traumatic events sive activities seek to regulate low- and high-arousal states.
and post-traumatic triggers can be attributed to the SNS/PNS The use of alcohol and cannabis to reduce high activation
accompaniments of defence responses, the sustained effects on states is more difficult to explain if the only relevant neuro-
cognition and emotion require an alternative explanation for biology relates to the rewarding stimulation in the nucleus
the alteration of the ‘torque’ (Watt, 2000) of the thalamocor- accumbens shell. However, alcohol and benzodiazepines
tical mantle. Key areas are the intralaminar nuclei, the would reduce arousal through a GABAergic inhibition of
nucleus reticularis thalami, the superior colliculi, the core the ML-DA system, and GABA interneurons are responsible
monoamine nuclei in the brainstem, and the midbrain retic- for different degrees of activation of the PAG columns
ular formation, with PAG outputs having modulatory influ- (Watt, 2000).
ences on these significant structures (Watt, 2000). Ascending Suicide planning is used by trauma patients to both acti-
dopaminergic tracts such as the mesocortical, nigrostriatal vate and deactivate arousal, depending on the baseline state
and mesolimbic systems are good candidates for this role. (Perry and Szalavitz, 2007). This soothing or activating
The innervation from the midbrain ventral tegmental area scenario-construction is likely to require imagery circuitry in
(VTA) reaches not only the nucleus accumbens and ventral the visual cortex, precuneus and posterior cingulate cortex
pallidum but also areas involved in the memory of emotional but also important may be the dopaminergic projections to
experiences and learning of fear responses, such as the baso- the prefrontal cortex which, according to Alcaro et al. (2007),
lateral nucleus of the amygdala (McGaugh, 2004). Also, the facilitate information processing without the affective aspects
mesodiencephalic areas involved in the generation of emo- of Seeking, potentially an increase in arousal without positive
tions and defence responses have bilateral projections to the affect. Given that these mesocortical projections can inhibit
thalamus that are separate from the nigrostriatal, mesolimbic dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens (Alcaro et al.,
and mesocortical dopamine systems (Sanchez-Gonzalez et al., 2007) so the prefrontal areas will be critical to the direction
2005) and have more diverse origins. The PAG projections to of the modulation.
the mediodorsal nucleus, the principal thalamic input to the Bathing, washing and cleaning are often used by complex
orbitomedial prefrontal cortex, are likely to be significant for trauma survivors to avoid the dysphoria associated with
affectively loaded mood states and behaviours but a circuit hyper- or hypoarousal states. The association between child-
involving the laterodorsal nucleus of the thalamus, the baso- hood trauma and adult obsessive-compulsive symptoms
lateral amygdala and the dorsal PAG has also been proposed has recently been documented from a large study of college
for panic disorder (Zanoveli et al., 2007). students (Mathews et al., 2008) and an affective orbitofron-
Long-term administration of antidepressant drugs attenu- tal–striatal circuit has been implicated in obsessive-
ates the footshock stress response of mesocortical dopamine compulsive disorder (Menzies et al., 2008). A wide range of
neurons in the rat (Dazzi et al., 2001). The atypical antipsy- obsessive-compulsive behaviour functions to either increase
chotics olanzapine and clozapine have a similar effect (Dazzi cortical activity (as in counting and checking) or regulate arou-
et al., 2004), which may help to explain why drugs such as sal (as in bathing, washing and cleaning rituals).
quetiapine and olanzapine are often clinically useful for The valence of the hyperarousal state is also subject to
widening the Window of Tolerance in complex trauma fluctuations and dysfunctional behaviours may be attempts
patients. The persistent state of behavioural depression fol- to convert a fearful, negatively valenced hyperarousal to a
lowing a prolonged period of repeated immersion stress which more positive appetitive state. Reynolds and Berridge (2008)
is accompanied by reduced dopamine transmission in the rat have demonstrated that the shell of the rat nucleus accumbens
prefrontal cortex (Mizoguchi et al., 2008) may serve as a is highly sensitive to environmental influences such as bright
model for the hypoarousal state. light and loud music, and the behaviours generated by dis-
Stimulation of dopamine D2 receptors in the prefrontal ruption of glutamate transmission there are readily converted
cortex reduces, whereas N-methyl D-aspartate (NMDA) from appetitive to fearful. There is preliminary evidence from
antagonism in the prefrontal cortex increases, the release of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies of
dopamine in the nucleus accumbens and the spontaneous altered processing of rewards in the nucleus accumbens and
motor activity of the rat (Del Arco and Mora, 2008), support- prefrontal cortex in PTSD (Sailer et al., 2008).
ing the hypothesis that an imbalance of dopamine transmis- Rapid oscillation between extremes of arousal state and
sion in mesocortical and mesolimbic systems would have frantic efforts to achieve regulation, even by way of dysfunc-
effects on motor activation and arousal. tional behaviours, can lead to a ‘biphasic rollercoaster’ of
experience which is chaotic and disturbing to the sufferer
(Figure 2).
Dysfunctional behaviours and the Window
of Tolerance Trauma and animal models of depression
Illicit drugs that activate the mesolimbic dopamine (ML-DA) Animal models of depression that have highlighted the impor-
system and provide reward through activation of the shell of tance of dopaminergic tracts have relied on the traumas of
the nucleus accumbens include cocaine and amphetamine pain, isolation or defeat. Rearing rat pups in social isolation
(Alcaro et al., 2007). One explanatory hypothesis for the induces hyperfunction of ML-DA systems and hypofunction
development of substance abuse, self-destructive behaviour of mesocortical dopamine (Fone and Porkess, 2008). Similar

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MONTANA on April 6, 2015


Corrigan et al. 21

observations are made when tail pressure is used as the stres-


sor: dopamine depletion in the medial prefrontal cortex Prefrontal cortex and the autonomic nervous
potentiates the stress-induced increase in extracellular dopa- system
mine in the nucleus accumbens shell (King et al., 1997).
Rats that repeatedly demonstrated defensive or submissive The orbitomedial prefrontal cortex has projections, presum-
behaviour when placed in the cage of an aggressive resident ably regulatory, to the mesodiencephalic areas necessary for
rat were found to have increased dopamine levels in the the generation of core emotions and defence responses
nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex when they were (Ongur and Price, 2000). Animal studies suggest that in
again exposed to the threat of social defeat (Tidey and some species parasympathetic responses, with bradycardia
Miczek, 1996), providing support for the hypothesis that and respiratory inhibition, are elicited from the anterior cin-
the hypervigilant, hyperaroused state is associated with gulate cortex while sympathetic responses are elicited from
increased extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens more ventral regions of subcallosal and orbital prefrontal
and prefrontal cortex. However, rats that were returned to cortex (Buchanan and Powell, 1993). Sympathetic responses
the safety of their home cage (not in sight of the aggressor) can also be elicited by stimulation of midline and mediodorsal
were found to have higher dopamine concentrations while thalamic nuclei and these structures may therefore be able to
being behaviourally predominantly inactive, suggesting that counteract parasympathetic changes mediated by the anterior
they were in a state of aroused vigilance rather than in a cingulate and orbital prefrontal cortex (Buchanan and
post-traumatic hypoarousal condition. The aversion to Powell, 1993). In the rat there are direct projections from
social contact of socially defeated mice is normalized by the anterior cingulate and orbitofrontal cortex to the dorsal
chronic administration of antidepressant (Berton et al., motor nucleus of the vagus, the solitary nucleus and the
2006), supporting the use of this animal trauma response nucleus ambiguus of the medulla. In the rabbit there are
model in modelling depression and in the discovery of new also projections to the mediodorsal, midline and intralaminar
antidepressants. nuclei of the thalamus. Given that a direct pathway exists
Thus, trauma can lead to increased mesolimbic dopamine from areas 32 and 25 to the autonomic regulatory nuclei in
and reduced or increased mesocortical dopamine. Also, the the medulla it has been proposed that prefrontal cortex and
dopaminergic tracts from PAG through the dorsomedial tha- thalamic activity is associated not with reflex homeostatic
lamus to prefrontal cortex which have been demonstrated in changes in autonomic activity but with the more complex
the macaque monkey (Sanchez-Gonzalez et al., 2005) would changes involved in associative learning. The anterior cingu-
warrant further study in the traumatized human. late cortex, the orbitofrontal cortex and the mediodorsal
nucleus of the thalamus all have reciprocal connections with
the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (Buchanan and
Powell, 1993).
Functional brain imaging in the human confirms the role
of both the anterior cingulate cortex and the ventral prefron-
tal cortex in control of autonomic responses. Stimulation of
Flashback the sympathetic system with exercise or arithmetic is accom-
terror Eating Flashback
rage Self-directed panied by activation in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex
for rage Withdrawal (Critchley et al., 2000), while parasympathetic modulation is
comfort nightmares accompanied by activation in the ventromedial prefrontal
cortex (Nagai et al., 2004; Wong et al., 2007). The vagal
brake (Porges, 1995) may have its hardwiring in the human
brain in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex rather than the
anterior cingulate cortex as would be expected from the
animal studies referred to above. It is not yet possible to attri-
bute these prefrontal activations unequivocally to specific
I’m fat Isolation Shame Alcohol autonomic effects or to identified neurotransmitter systems.
disgusting predisposes intoxication
a failure to memory However, Bergmann (2008), echoing and developing the
BIPHASIC work of Schore (1994), has proposed that post-traumatic
ROLLERCOASTER
hyperarousal states are mediated by a dopaminergic circuit
involving a ventral sympathetic area of orbitofrontal cortex
Figure 2. An example of how dysfunctional behaviours can be efforts to that has reciprocal dopaminergic connections with the ventral
regulate distress by a person striving to be within the Window of tegmental area and projections to the nucleus accumbens.
Tolerance. Eating for comfort to soothe the terror of a flashback is ini- This system is often involved in positively valenced states of
tially helpful but then leads to feelings of shame, self-loathing and appetitive behaviour, motivational reward and active coping,
worthlessness. This gives rise to suicidal thinking but also to withdrawal but could be negatively valenced under conditions of threat
and social isolation which make intrusive memories more disturbing. The (Reynolds and Berridge, 2008). The post-traumatic hypoar-
next flashback is accompanied by rage then by shame at the rage, leading ousal state, in contrast, is considered by Bergmann (2008) to
to alcohol intoxication to promote oblivion. Withdrawal from the alcohol be mediated by a noradrenergic circuit involving lateral
leads to a high arousal state in which triggers to further flashbacks with regions of orbitofrontal cortex that have reciprocal connec-
dominant fear or rage will occur, continuing the cycle. tions with, amongst others, parasympathetic autonomic areas

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MONTANA on April 6, 2015


22 Journal of Psychopharmacology 25(1)

of the lateral hypothalamus and arousal-regulating neurons in experience of incomplete and/or obstructed fight responses
brainstem vagal centres. This lateral tegmental limbic fore- automatically followed by parasympathetically mediated
brain–midbrain circuit down-regulates negative affect by acti- ‘total submission’ (Porges, 1995) responses. Energy conser-
vating the onset of a parasympathetic inhibitory state which vation allows individuals to sustain such states for lengthy
can be positively valenced with soothing and calming or neg- periods of time. Subsequently, the patient’s subjective expe-
atively valenced with apathy and shame. rience is one of depression, hopelessness and worthlessness,
which is often exacerbated in the context of anger or
self-assertion.
Brain imaging in complex trauma and in Energy conservation can be seen in the DSM-IV (APA,
depression 1994) criteria for major depressive episode of reduced energy
and fatigue. Low mood is described as feeling sad or empty,
Preliminary findings suggest that patients with complex and feelings of worthlessness or inappropriate guilt may be
trauma and dissociative disorders have smaller hippocampal present. Feelings of hopelessness feature in the DSM-IV cri-
volumes than controls and that the degree of reduction in teria for dysthymic disorder, along with other features of a
hippocampal volume is correlated both with the clinical sever- low arousal state such as low energy and low self-esteem.
ity of the disorder and the extent of early trauma (Ehling Concurrent experience of anxiety and depression then
et al., 2008). Similar findings were reported in a study of would not be a question of co-morbidity but of activated
women with borderline personality disorder and a history fear simultaneous with the incomplete fight or flight
of early life trauma (Driessen et al., 2000). Despite these find- responses. When these are constantly present, rather than
ings, imaging studies of hippocampal volume in depression available briefly for situations of danger, the short-term per-
are often conducted with no recording of the trauma history spective remains and obviates the possibility of optimism.
of the participants and this is acknowledged to be a possible How can there be enjoyment, interest, energy or pleasure
confounding variable in efforts to link volume changes with when the outlook rises from a state of frozen submission?
depressive subtype (Greenberg et al., 2008). Brain research by This result of social defeat stress, as in the rat model of
Lanius et al. (cited in Ogden et al., 2006) demonstrated Berton et al. (2006), is a chronic depressive condition reflect-
reduced thalamic activation in subjects with histories of ing the survival need for dorsal vagal and parasympathetic
trauma relative to controls but this has not yet been studied dominance.
in relation to arousal level. Because trauma-related stimuli continue to evoke sympa-
thetically mediated hyperarousal responses and the ‘vagal
brake’ to inhibit or truncate fight or flight responses,
The immediacy of hopelessness and chronic trauma patients often experience characteristic fluctuations
depression of state described in the ‘distress dipole’ model. If fight or
flight responses chronically coexist with a hypoarousal state
In some cases, depressive episodes appear to be linked to that down-regulates to truncate active defensive responses,
years of bullying, neglect or other abuse. Psychodynamic the- patients may become trapped in the ‘distress dipole’ and
orists considered internalized anger to be capable of transfor- become unable to access any positive affective state. Some
mation into depressed mood but they provided no account of examples are given in Figure 3.
how this might be mediated physiologically. If there are
repeated situations in which a fight response is obstructed,
thoughts of hopelessness, helplessness and worthlessness Summary: psychopharmacology and the
accompanying the parasympathetic down-regulation of the Window of Tolerance
fight response would be associated with low mood – and a
mental state that is biologically determined for a brief and We are arguing that while arousal states outside the window
immediate response to attack can then become chronic. of tolerance are initially associated with SNS or PNS activa-
During an attack by a predator there is a need for a focus tion, the psychological effects that persist beyond the acute
on immediate response rather than on long-term planning. change in heart rate and respiratory rate appear to be
The function of hopelessness, the belief that ‘this is never mediated – at least in part – by ascending dopaminergic
going to get better’, may be the disconnection of the situa- tracts from the midbrain to the thalamus and the mesocorti-
tionally redundant fight and flight responses. This outlook cal, nigrostriatal and mesolimbic dopaminergic systems.
conserves energy, through avoidance, hiding or other appro- Evidence from animal studies supports the view that trauma
priate behavioural responses, and also facilitates submission induces excessive dopamine release in mesolimbic systems,
if there is a clear imbalance of power. but the role of reduced dopaminergic activity in mesocortical
The physiological experience of helplessness (loss of tracts is less clear. It may be an imbalance between mesocor-
energy and tone, collapse, slowed cognitive processing), tical and mesolimbic dopamine transmission that facilitates
accompanied by the cognition ‘there is nothing I can do: rapid switching between high and low arousal states in those
no action of mine will be helpful’ promotes resignation with a history of severe trauma. The nucleus accumbens is
and energy conservation. Like hopelessness and worthless- sensitive to environmental factors (Reynolds and Berridge,
ness (‘I deserve this because I’m bad’) it reinforces submis- 2008) and the experience of the arousal state as positively
sion responses and passive compliance or acceptance. Thus, or negatively valenced may relate to whether it is in fearful/
symptoms of depression and hopelessness reflect the defensive or appetitive mode.

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MONTANA on April 6, 2015


Corrigan et al. 23

DISTRESS DIPOLES
Sympathetic
hyperarousal
FLIGHT
Fear and hypervigilance: ‘I
can’t be trapped. I can’t get
too close or committed. I
have to be ready to run.’ ‘If
I can’t run, I can escape FIGHT
with drink or drugs or Anger and self-blame:
eating disorders.’ ‘I shouldn’t have let it
happen. It will not happen
again. I won’t trust. I won’t
be controlled.’
FIGHT
Anger and hypervigilance: ‘It will
happen again, but now I’m
ready. I will push people away –
I’ll test their credibility. And if
necessary, I’ll kill myself, or I’ll
kill them.’

WINDOW OF TOLERANCE

ATTACH
‘I can’t take care of myself
– I’m in danger, and I need
help. Please love me and SUBMIT SUBMIT
care for me and protect Safety lies in compliance. . Shame and depression increase
me.’ ‘Please don’t leave . ‘I can’t say No. I always compliance and thus safety: ‘I deserved
..‘ have to give in.’ ‘If I go it, or it wouldn’t have happened.’
numb and compliant, I ‘I’m giving up because I don’t deserve
won’t be hurt again.’ ‘It more.’ ‘I’m too tired to fight.’ ‘There’s no
isn’t safe to assert myself point – it’s hopeless.’ ‘I’m weak and
or ask for help – it’s worthless.’
dangerous to fight.’

Parasympathetic hypoarousal

Figure 3. Some examples of states in which the ‘rollercoastering’ is so rapid that the high and low arousal states effectively coexist. The negative
cognitions are in matching pairs, each with an opposite arousal state, giving rise to an extremely unpleasant dynamic dysphoric tension.

High arousal states that are aversive can be modulated by activities requiring focused attention. Low arousal benefits
suicide planning, starvation, abuse of alcohol and cannabis, from gentle activity, movement, humour and increased corti-
bathing, grooming and compulsive cleaning, and by cal activity. In suicidal patients with both low and high arou-
self-harm. Low arousal states can be modified by compulsive sal patterns, treatment with antidepressants and atypical
activities involving grooming and cleaning, suicide planning, antipsychotics needs to be monitored carefully from the per-
risk-taking (such as driving too fast), self-harm, and abuse of spective of autonomic arousal and the ‘Window of Tolerance’
alcohol, amphetamine, ecstasy or cocaine. Treatment to maximize the psychopharmacological benefits in this pop-
approaches need to involve patient education about these ulation and reduce the possibility of an increase in suicidal
states and their role in symptom perpetuation and exacerba- behaviour.
tion. In addition to pharmacological approaches, the substi-
tution of more effective and less harmful behaviours to
regulate autonomic states must be emphasized. For both
hyperarousal and hypoarousal, mindfull curiosity is regulat- References
ing, as is psychoeducation. In high arousal, soothing strate- Alcaro A, Huber R, and Panksepp J (2007) Behavioral functions
gies may be useful, as well as exercise, yoga and other physical of the mesolimbic dopaminergic system: an affective

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MONTANA on April 6, 2015


24 Journal of Psychopharmacology 25(1)

neuroethological perspective. Brain Res Rev doi: 10.1016/j.brain- with borderline personality disorder and early traumatization.
resrev.2007.07.014. Arch Gen Psychiatry 57: 1115–1122.
American Psychiatric Association (APA). (1994) Diagnostic Criteria Ehling T, Nijenhuis ERS, Krikke AP (2008) Volume of discrete brain
from DSM-IV – Quick Reference. Washington DC: American structures in complex dissociative disorders: preliminary findings.
Psychiatric Association. Prog Brain Res 167: 307–310.
Austin MA, Riniolo TC, Porges SW (2007) Borderline personality Fay D (2007) Becoming Safely Embodied. Somerville, MA: Heart Full
disorder and emotion regulation: insights from the polyvagal Life Publishing.
theory. Brain Cogn 65: 69–76. Fisher J, Ogden P (2007) Sensorimotor psychotherapy. In: Courtois
Bandler R, Shipley MT (1994) Columnar organization in the mid- CA, Ford JD (eds) Treating Complex Post-traumatic Disorders.
brain periaqueductal gray: modules for emotional expression? New York: Guilford Press, 312–328.
Trends Neurol Sci 17: 379–389. Fiszman A, Mendlowicz MV, Marques-Portella C, et al. (2008)
Bergmann U (2008) She’s come undone: a neurobiological explora- Peritraumatic tonic immobility predicts a poor response to phar-
tion of dissociative disorders. In: Forgash C, Copeley M (eds) macological treatment in victims of urban violence with PTSD.
Healing the Heart of Trauma and Dissociation: with EMDR and J Affect Disord 107: 193–197.
Ego State Therapy. New York: Springer, 61–90. Fone KCF, Porkess MV (2008) Behavioural and neurochemical
Berton O, McClung CA, DiLeone RJ, et al. (2006) Essential role of effects of post-weaning social isolation in rodents – relevance to
BDNF in the mesolimbic dopamine pathway in social defeat developmental neuropsychiatric disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev
stress. Science 311: 864–868. 37: 1087–1102.
Bhatt S, Bhatt R, Zalcman SS, and Siegel A (2008) Role of IL-1beta Greenberg DL, Payne ME, MacFall JR, Steffens DC, Krishnan RR
and 5-HT2 receptors in midbrain periaqueductal gray (PAG) in (2008) Hippocampal volumes and depression subtypes. Psych
potentiating defensive rage behavior in cats. Brain Behav Immun Res: Neuroimag 163: 126–132.
22: 224–233. Grillner S, Hellgren J, Menard A, Saitoh K, Wilkstrom MA (2005)
Borelli KG and Brandao ML (2007) Effects of ovine injections into Mechanisms for selection of basoc motor programs – roles for the
the dorsomedial, dorsolateral and lateral columns of the peria- striatum and pallidum. Trends Neurosci 28: 364–370.
queductal gray: a functional role for the dorsomedial column. Heidt JM, Marx BP, Forsyth JP (2005) Tonic immobility and chid-
Horm Behav, doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.08.013. hood sexual abuse: a preliminary report evaluating the sequela of
Bovin MJ, Jager-Hyman S, Gold SD, Marx BP, Sloan DM (2008) rape-induced paralysis. Behav Res Therap 43: 1157–1171.
Herman JL (1992) Trauma and Recovery. New York: W.W. Norton
Tonic immobility mediates the influence of peritraumatic fear and
& Sons.
perceived inescapability on posttraumatic stress symptom severity
Herman JL, Perry JC, van der Kolk BA (1989) Childhood trauma in
among sexual assault survivors. J Traum Stress 21: 402–409.
borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry 146: 490–495.
Brandao ML, Zanoveli JM, Ruiz-Martinez RC, Oliveira LC,
Huo F-Q, Qu C-L, Li Y-Q, Tang J-S, Jia H (2008) GABAergic mod-
Landeira-Fernandez J (2008) Different patterns of freezing beha-
ulation is involved in the ventrolateral orbital cortex 5-HT1A
viour organized in the periaqueductal grey of rats: association
receptor activation-induced antinociception in the rat. Pain 139:
with different types of anxiety. Behav Brain Res 188: 1–13.
398–405.
Buchanan SL, Powell DA (1993) Cingulothalamic and prefrontal
King D, Zigmond MJ, Finlay JM (1997) Effects of dopamine deple-
control of autonomic function. In: Vogt BA, Gabriel M (eds)
tion in the medial prefrontal cortex on the stress-induced increase
Neurobiology of Cingulate Cortex and Limbic Thalamus. Boston:
in extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens core and
Birkhauser, 381–414.
shell. Neuroscience 77: 141–153.
Carvalho-Netto EF, Litvin Y, Nunes-de-Souza RL, Blanchard DC,
Kishimoto K, Koyama S, Akaike N (2001) Synergistic mu-opioid
Blanchard RJ (2007) Effects of intra-PAG infusion of ovine CRF and 5-HT1A presynaptic inhibition of GABA release in rat peri-
on defensive behaviours in Swiss-Webster mice. Behav Brain Res aqueductal gray neurons. Neuropharmacology 41: 529–538.
176: 222–229. Kroes RA, Burgdorf J, Otto NJ, Panksepp J, Moskal JR (2007)
Craig AD (2005) Forebrain emotional asymmetry: a neuroanatomi- Social defeat, a paradigm of depression in rats that elicits
cal basis? Trends Cogn Sci 9: 566–571. 22-kHz vocalizations, preferentially activates the cholinergic sig-
Critchley HD, Corfield DR, Chandler MP, Mathias CJ, Dolan RJ nalling pathway in the periaqueductal gray. Behav Brain Res 182:
(2000) Cerebral correlates of autonomic cardiovascular arousal: a 290–300.
functional neuroimaging investigation in humans. J Physiol 523: Lanius UF (2005) EMDR processing with dissociative clients:
259–270. adjunctive use of opioid antagonists. In: Shapiro R (ed.) EMDR
Dazzi L, Serra M, Spiga F, Pisu MG, Jentsch JD, Biggio G (2001) Solutions: Pathways to Healing. New York: Norton.
Prevention of the stress-induced increase in frontal cortical dopa- LeDoux J (2002) The Synaptic Self: How our Brains Become Who
mine efflux of freely moving rats by long-term treatment with We Are. New York: Guilford Press.
antidepressant drugs. Eur Neuropsychopharm 11: 343–349. Leite-Panissi CRA, Coimbra NC, Menescal-de-Oliveira L (2003) The
Dazzi L, Seu E, Cherchi G, Biggio G (2004) Inhibition of cholinergic stimulation of the central amygdala modifying the
stress-induced dopamine output in the rat prefrontal cortex by tonic immobility response and antinociception in guinea pigs
chronic treatment with olanzapine. Biol Psychiatry 55: 477–483. depends on the ventrolateral periaqueductal gray. Brain Res
Del Arco A, Mora F (2008) Prefrontal cortex-nucleus accumbens Bull 60: 167–178.
interaction: in vivo modulation by dopamine and glutamate in Linehan MM (1993) Skills Training Manual for Treating Borderline
the prefrontal cortex. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 90: 226–235. Personality Disorder. New York: Guilford Press.
De Menezes RCA, Zaretsky DV, Fontes MAP, DiMicco JA (2006) Martinez RCR, de Oliveira AR, and Brandao ML (2007)
Microinjection of muscimol into caudal periaqueductal gray Serotonergic mechanisms in the basolateral amygdala differen-
lowers body temperature and attenuates increases in temperature tially regulate the conditioned and unconditioned fear organized
and activity evoked from the dorsomedial hypothalamus. Brain in the periaqueductal gray. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol, doi:
Res 1092: 129–137. 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2007.02.015.
Driessen M, Herrmann J, Stahl K, et al. (2000) Magnetic resonance Mathews CA, Kaur N, Stein MB (2008) Childhood trauma and
imaging volumes of the hippocampus and the amygdala in women obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Depress Anxiety 25: 742–751.

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MONTANA on April 6, 2015


Corrigan et al. 25

McGaugh JL (2004) The amygdala modulates the consolidation of Reimer AE, Oliveira AR, Brandao ML (2008) Selective involvement
memories of emotionally arousing experiences. Ann Rev Neurosci of GABAergic mechanisms of the dorsal periaqueductal gray and
27: 1–28. inferior colliculus on the memory of the contextual fear as
McHaffie JG, Stanford TR, Stein BE, Coizet V, Redgrave P (2005) assessed by the fear potentiated startle test. Brain Res Bull 76:
Subcortical loops through the basal ganglia. Trends Neurosci 28: 545–550.
401–407. Reynolds SM, Berridge KC (2008) Emotional environments retune
Menzies L, Chamberlain SR, Laird AR, Thelen SM, Sahakian BJ, the valence of appetitive versus fearful functions in nucleus
Bullmore ET (2008) Integrating evidence from neuroimaging and accumbens. Nature Neurosci 11: 423–425.
neuropsychological studies of obsessive-compulsive disorder: the Sailer U, Robinson S, Fischmeister FPS, et al. (2008) Altered reward
orbitofrontal-striatal model revisited. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32: processing in the nucleus accumbens and mesial prefrontal cortex
525–549. of patients with posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychologia
Mizoguchi K, Shoji H, Ikeda R, Tanaka Y, Tabira T (2008) 46: 2836–2844.
Persistent depressive state after chronic stress in rats is accompa- Sanchez-Gonzalez MA, Garcia-Cabezas MA, Rico B, Cavada C
nied by HPA axis dysregulation and reduced prefrontal dopami- (2005) The primate thalamus is a key target for brain dopamine.
nergic neurotransmission. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 91: 170–175. J Neurosci 25: 6076–6083.
Moraes CLK, Bertoglio LJ, Carobrez AP (2008) Interplay between Schore AN (1994) Affect regulation and the origin of the self.
glutamate and serotonin within the dorsal periaqueductal gray New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
modulates anxiety-related behaviour of rats exposed to the ele- Schore AN (2003) Affect Dysregulation and Disorders of the Self.
vated plus-maze. Behav Brain Res 194: 181–186. New York: WW Norton.
Moskowitz AK (2004) ‘‘Scared stiff’’: catatonia as an Siegel DJ (1999) The Developing Mind. New York: Guilford.
evolutionary-based fear response. Psychol Rev 111: 984–1002. Terzian ALB, Aguiar DC, Guimaraes FS, and Moreira FA (2008)
Nagai Y, Critchley HD, Featherstone E, Trimble MR, Dolan RJ Modulation of anxiety-like behaviour by Transient Receptor
(2004) Activity in ventromedial prefrontal cortex covaries with Potential Vanilloid Type 1 (TRPV1) channels located in the dor-
sympathetic skin conductance level: a physiological account of a solateral periaqueductal gray. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol, doi:
‘‘default mode’’ of brain function. NeuroImage 22: 243–251. 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2008.11.004.
Ogata SN, Silk KR, Goodrich S, Lohr NE, Westen D, Hill EM Tidey JW, Miczek KA (1996) Social defeat stress selectively alters
(1990) Childhood sexual and physical abuse in adult patients mesocorticolimbic dopamine release: an in vivo microdialysis
with borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry 147: study. Brain Res 721: 140–149.
1008–1013. Van der Kolk BA (1996a) The complexity of adaptation to trauma:
Ogden P, Minton K, Pain C (2006) Trauma and the body: a sensor- self-regulation, stimulus discrimination, and characterological
imotor approach to psychotherapy. New York: Norton. development. Traumatic Stress: the Effects of Overwhelming
Ongur D, Price JL (2000) The organization of networks within the Experience on Mind, Body, and Society. New York: Guilford
orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and Press, 182–213.
humans. Cereb Cortex 10: 206–219. Van der Kolk BA (1996b) The body keeps the score: approaches to
Perry B and Szalavitz M (2007) The boy who was raised as a dog: and the psychobiology of posttraumatic stress disorder. In: Traumatic
other stories from a child psychiatrist’s notebook – what trauma- stress: the Effects of Overwhelming Experience on Mind, Body, and
tized children can teach us about life, loss and healing. New York: Society. New York: Guilford Press, 214–241.
Basic Books. Watt DF (2000) The centrencephalon and thalamocortical integra-
Perry BD, Pollard RA, Baker WI, Sturges C, Vigilante D, and tion: neglected contributions of periaqueductal gray. Conscious
Blakley TL (1995) Continuous heartrate monitoring in maltreated Emotion 1: 91–114.
children. In: Annual meeting of the American Academy of Child Wong SW, Masse N, Kimmerly DS, Menon RS, Shoemaker JK
and Adolescent Psychiatry, New Research (abstract). (2007) Ventral medial prefrontal cortex and cardiovagal control
Porges S (1995) Orienting in a defensive world: mammalian modifi- in conscious humans. NeuroImage 35: 698–708.
cations of our evolutionary heritage. A polyvagal theory. Zanarini MC, Gunderson JG, Marino MF, Schwartz EO,
Psychophysiology 32: 301–318. Frankenburg FR (1989) Childhood experiences of borderline
Porges S (2003) The polyvagal theory: phylogenetic contributions to patients. Compr Psychiatry 30: 18–25.
social behaviour. Physiol Behav 79: 503–513. Zanoveli JM, Ferreira-Netto C, Brandao ML (2007) Conditioned
Rainville P, Bechara A, Naqvi N, Damasio AR (2006) Basic emo- place aversion organized in the dorsal periaqueductal gray
tions are associated with distinct patterns of cardiorespiratory recruits the laterodorsal nucleus of the thalamus and the basolat-
activity. Int J Psychophysiol 61: 5–18. eral amygdala. Exp Neurol 208: 127–136.

Downloaded from jop.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MONTANA on April 6, 2015

You might also like