You are on page 1of 9

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/269874783

The Art in Fiction: From Indirect Communication to Changes of the Self

Article in Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity and the Arts · November 2014


DOI: 10.1037/a0037999

CITATIONS READS
62 3,973

2 authors:

Maja Djikic Keith Oatley


University of Toronto University of Toronto
33 PUBLICATIONS 1,228 CITATIONS 182 PUBLICATIONS 10,410 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

On Truth and Fiction: Dialogues between Literature and Cognition View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Maja Djikic on 22 December 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts © 2014 American Psychological Association
2014, Vol. 8, No. 4, 498 –505 1931-3896/14/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0037999

The Art in Fiction: From Indirect Communication


to Changes of the Self

Maja Djikic and Keith Oatley


University of Toronto

Recent studies have shown that reading literary fiction can prompt personality changes that include
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

improvements in abilities in empathy and theory-of-mind. We review these studies and propose a
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

psychological conception of artistic literature as having 3 aspects that contribute to such changes. These
are that literary fiction is simulation of selves with others in the social world; that taking part in this type
of simulation can produce fluctuations that are precursors to personality changes; and that the changes
occur in readers’ own ways, being based not on persuasion but on indirect communication.

Keywords: art, fiction, empathy, personality, self-change

The indirect mode of communication makes communication an art in In this paper we begin by reviewing recent empirical findings
quite a different sense than when it is conceived in the usual manner. which indicate that reading literary prose helped improve empathy
. . . To stop a man on the street and stand still while talking to him, and the ability to understand others, and thus to change personal-
is not so difficult as to say something to a passer-by in passing, ity. We then propose three psychological aspects of artistic liter-
without standing still and without delaying the other, without attempt-
ature that made this change possible: that such literature mainly
ing to persuade him to go the same way, but giving him instead an
takes the form of simulation rather than description, that it can
impulse to go precisely his own way.
—Søren Kierkegaard (1846/1968, p. 246 –247) produce fluctuations in personality systems, and that its influence
is indirect and exploratory.
Fiction is an art form, but of what does it consist? During the last
15 years, psychologists have made progress on understanding the Reading Fiction Improves Empathy and the Ability to
art in fiction. One important property has come to the fore: Understand Others
literature can facilitate self-change. This is impressive, given the
stability of the personality system (Costa & McCrae, 1994; Mc- Traditionally, teachers of literature have argued that reading
Crae & Costa, 1990, 1996) and difficulties people encounter in novels by writers such as Jane Austen, Gustave Flaubert, and
attempting to change it (Baumeister, Heatherton, & Tice, 1994). Leo Tolstoy, or short stories by writers such as Anton Chekhov
Although there have been anecdotal reports of self-changing in and Alice Munro, invites us to understand others better. The
encounters with literary works (Sabine & Sabine, 1983), aspects of literary scholar Keen (2007), for instance, argued that increas-
self that have recently attracted attention in empirical research are ing empathy by means of literature made for improvements in
empathy and the ability to understand others. A striking feature of the self and society. Some philosophers, however, have been
self-change through literature is that the effects are not direct, as skeptical. Radford (1975), for instance, argued that the idea of
occurs with persuasion, where an author intends the reader or feeling moved by fictional characters is incoherent, and Currie
listener to think, feel, or be disposed to act, in a way he or she (2011) wrote, “when we engage with great literature we do not
desires. The art in fiction is a social influence, but one that helps come away with more knowledge, clarified emotions, or deeper
people to understand and feel, and even change their selfhood, in human sympathies” (p. 15). The question, then, is what is the
their own ways. The influence is what Kierkegaard (1846/1968) evidence?
called “indirect communication” (pp. 246 –247). Hakemulder (2000) proposed that fictional narrative can be a
“moral laboratory.” He asked people to read pieces of fiction, and
found that these could help people to imagine themselves into the
shoes of others, and this could affect beliefs about what it must be
like to be someone else (see also Hakemulder, 2008). Mar, Oatley,
This article was published Online First October 20, 2014. Hirsh, dela Paz, and Peterson (2006) found (in a study that used
Maja Djikic, Rotman School of Management, University of Toronto,
measures of a different kind than those of Hakemulder) that the
Canada; Keith Oatley, Department of Human Development and Applied
Psychology, University of Toronto.
more fiction people read the better were their empathy and their
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Maja ability to understand others. To measure the amount of fiction and
Djikic, Desautels Centre for Integrative Thinking, Rotman School of nonfiction people read, Mar et al. (2006) adapted the Author
Management, University of Toronto, 105 St. George Street, Toronto, Recognition Test developed by Stanovich, West, and Harrison
Ontario, Canada, M5S 3E6. E-mail: maja.djikic@rotman.utoronto.ca (1995) in which people are given a list of names, and they were to

498
ART IN FICTION 499

check those that they recognize as authors. People who read a lot Two criticisms of this set of studies were that they were corre-
are likely to know, for instance, that Toni Morrison and J. R. R. lational and that they did not measure effects of increased empa-
Tolkien are authors, whereas Lauren Adamson and Eric Amsel are thy. Johnson (2012) solved these problems. In an experiment, he
not. Stanovich et al. found that the number of correctly recognized found not only increased empathy for the protagonist among those
names of authors on this test gave a close proxy to the amount who were mentally transported into a story they read, but also an
people read as measured, for instance, by daily activity diaries. increase in participants’ altruistic behavior: Those whose empathy
They also found that the better their scores on author recognition, increased were more likely to help a stranger who had dropped
the better was their vocabulary and their general knowledge, even some pens on the floor. A reservation about Johnson’s (2012)
when age, IQ, and level of education were controlled for. Rain and study is that it did not use literary fiction, but a story written
Mar (2014) presented fresh evidence of the predictive validity of specially for this experiment. This problem was solved however by
author recognition checklists. Johnson (2013) when he found that transportation into literary
Mar et al. (2006) had the idea of modifying the original Author fiction decreased prejudice against Arab Muslims. Moreover,
Recognition Test so that it consisted of names of writers of fiction, Johnson, Cushman, Borden, and McCune (2013) found, in an
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

names of writers of nonfiction, and names of people who were not experiment, that instructions to generate imagery while reading
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

writers. With colleagues (Mar et al., 2006) he gave this modified fiction increased empathy and prosocial behavior, and Johnson
test to participants along with two outcome measures of social (2014) found that reading narrative fiction decreased bias of peo-
understanding. One outcome measure was Baron–Cohen Wheel- ple as they thought about Arab faces seen in photographs.
wright, Hill, Raste, and Plumb’s (2001) Mind-in-the-Eyes test, a In terms of conventional measures of personality change with
36-item task to measure empathy and theory-of-mind (the ability more direct focus on literary reading, Djikic, Oatley, and
to attribute mental states to oneself and others, and to understand Moldoveanu (2013b) demonstrated in an experiment that people
that others can have intentions and desires that are different from who were low on the Big Five personality trait of Openness to
one’s own; Premack & Woodruff, 1978). In the Mind-in-the-Eyes Experience (John, Donahue, & Kentle, 1991) increased their em-
test, participants look at photographs of the eyes of people seen as pathy as measured on the perspective taking scale of Davis’s
if through a letter box so that the rest of the face is not visible. Each (1983) Interpersonal Reactivity Index when they read one of eight
fictional short stories, as compared with those who read one of
person whose eyes are seen in the test has been photographed
eight nonfictional essays. Both the stories and the essays were
while making a readable facial expression. Participants have to
chosen from literary anthologies.
choose one of four adjectives to indicate what they think the
Further experiments also have been conducted to focus on
person was feeling and thinking, for instance: “joking,” “flus-
literary reading. Kidd and Castano (2013) randomly assigned
tered,” “desire,” or “convinced.” The second outcome measure
participants to read one of three literary short stories (one of which
was an Interpersonal Perception Task ⫺15 (Costanzo & Archer,
was Anton Chekhov’s Chameleon) (1884/1979) or one of three
1989), in which participants watch 15 video clips of naturally
nonfictional essays (one of which was Charles Mann’s (2011)
occurring social interaction, and for each clip answer a question
“How the Potato Changed the World”). As compared with people
about what was going on. Mar et al., 2006 found that the more
who read one of the essays, those who read a fictional piece
fiction people read (as measured by their ability to recognize
significantly improved their scores on the Mind-in-the-Eyes test.
names of fiction writers), the better were their scores on the In their second experiment Kidd and Castano (2013) used a
Mind-in-the Eyes test. Results on the Interpersonal Perception Test different measure of theory-of-mind, and compared a piece of
were smaller but in the same direction. By contrast, people who literary fiction, a piece of fiction that was popular (as gauged by
were predominant readers of nonfiction (as measured by their Amazon), and no reading at all. Reading literary fiction produced
ability to recognize names of authors of nonfiction) scored lower better theory-of-mind than reading popular fiction, but the effect
on these tests. was only marginally significant. In three further experiments, the
In a replication, Mar, Oatley, and Peterson (2009) found that researchers compared different pieces of literary and popular fic-
higher scores on the Mind-in-the-Eyes test associated with reading tion and found in each case that, as compared with those who read
fiction occurred even when a set of controls had been applied. The the popular fiction, those who read the literary fiction were sig-
effect was not, for instance, explained by preference to read fiction nificantly better on the Mind-in-the-Eyes test.
among people who had better empathy and theory-of-mind, or by Missing, currently, from studies of this type are experiments on
any other trait of personality. Mar et al. (2009)found, too, that the longer term effects. For instance, people may be randomly as-
myth that avid fiction readers are socially isolated is untrue; their signed to read for, say, 10 hr a week over a period of several
social networks were found to be better than those of people who months, either fiction or explanatory nonfiction of their choice.
read less fiction. Fong, Mullin, and Mar (2013) demonstrated that Another limitation is that most work of this type has so far focused
the genre of fiction most closely associated with improved empa- on fiction as compared with nonfiction. Influences of forms such
thy and theory-of-mind as measured by the Mind-in-the-Eyes test as biography and narrative history have yet to be examined.
was romance. Family stories and adventure stories also showed Another problem with current research on this issue is that it has
positive associations, but science fiction did not. In preschool focused predominantly on reading prose. In a study on poetry used
children, Mar, Tackett, and Moore (2010) found that the more functional MRI (fMRI), Zeman, Milton, Smith, and Rylance
stories they had read to them, and the more movies they watched, (2013) found that as well as activating brain areas concerned with
the better they were at five tests of theory-of-mind. By contrast, reading, pieces that were more stylistically concentrated (such as
hours of watching TV did not correlate with any measures of the poetry of Keats, 1959) activated areas that are usually activated
theory-of-mind. by music, those concerned with introspection, and also those
500 DJIKIC AND OATLEY

concerned with memory. Poetry (and, by extension what we might develop. In acquiring social expertise, readers of fiction change
call the “poetry of prose”), then, is not restricted to narrative somewhat in themselves. As the experimental studies by Johnson
effects. As to theater, Goldstein (2009) discussed how actors are (2013); Djikic, Oatley, and Moldoveanu (2013b); and Kidd and
able to improve theory-of-mind, and Goldstein and Winner (2012) Castano (2013) found, reading fiction that is literary made people
found that children in elementary school and adolescents in high more empathetic, that is to say better able to experience something
school improved their empathy and theory-of-mind with training of the emotional states of others in an inward way. It is likely that
in acting as compared with training in other arts. As to work on this process diminishes the actor-observer bias (Jones & Nisbett,
film, we mentioned Mar et al.’s (2010) finding that preschool 1971), in which people tend to see themselves as acting in relation
children who watched more films had better theory-of-mind. There to events while they tend to see others as acting out fixed person-
is also a body of work on psychological effects of film in engaging ality traits.
adult spectators (see for instance, Green et al., 2008; Oatley, 2013; A serious problem in studies of reading literature is how to
Oliver & Bartsch, 2010; Schramm & Wirth, 2010), although this choose control groups. In many experiments on psychological
work has not yet examined changes of empathy or other aspects of effects of literary narrative as compared with expository prose,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

personality. there is a large confound. It is that narrative prose is generally


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Despite these limitations, recent research has pointed to the easier to read than expository prose. A measure that can be used
conclusion that fiction, and particularly artistic fiction, is an agent to avoid the most troublesome influences of this confound is the
of self-change. In the following section, we suggest three aspects Flesch–Kincaid measure of reading difficulty (Kincaid, Fish-
of literary fiction that can facilitate change in facets of personality. burne, Rogers, & Chissom, 1975), which is based on the aver-
age length of words and the average length of sentences in a
text. Although the Flesch–Kincaid score does not measure more
Three Aspects of Art in Literature sophisticated aspects of the text (such as literary features, style,
etc.), it does adequately measure how easy the text is to read.
Literary Fiction as Simulation of Other Selves and Using this measure, Mar, Oatley, and Eng (2003) built on the
Other Minds studies of Larsen and Seilman (1988) and Seilman and Larsen
(1989) who found that reading narrative prose elicited more
Oatley (1992, 1999) proposed that rather than conceptualizing actor-based memories than did reading expository prose. The
fiction as flawed empirical description of the world, we should passages studied by Mar and colleagues (2003) were arranged
think of it as simulation. It is a simulation with subject matter of to have exactly the same semantic content, the same level of
the type that Bruner (1986) suggested, of human (or human-like) reading difficulty on the Flesch–Kincaid measure, and the same
agents, their intentions, and the vicissitudes these intentions meet. length. Participants were randomly assigned to read the narrative or
It is a type of simulation that runs not on computers but on minds. expository text. As they read, they were asked to mark the
Stories told orally were probably the very first simulations, with margin whenever a memory occurred. After reading they wrote
subject matter based on the closest interests of our ultrasocial summaries of these memories. As compared with those who
species. These simulations are of what we and others are up to with read the expository piece, those who read the narrative had
each other, and of how to understand such matters. If you learn to memories that were more vivid and more likely to involve the
fly a plane you might do well to spend time in a flight simulator. reader as an actor or observer in a detailed scene. Narrative text,
The studies discussed earlier, of fictional reading’s association in other words, involves readers more deeply and personally in
with better empathy and abilities of theory-of-mind, were what they are reading. It invites what Green, Chatham, and
prompted by wondering if fiction might be the mind’s flight Sestir (2012) called “transportation” into the narrative world.
simulator.
In terms of effects on the brain, Speer, Reynolds, Swallow, and
Fluctuations of Personality Prompted by Literature
Zacks (2009) studied people who read a short story while they
were in a functional MRI (fMRI) machine. When they read of a How is it possible that literature can promote change in a system
character doing the action of grasping something, the part of their as stable as personality? We hypothesize that the art of literature
brain concerned with grasping with a hand was activated. When (its style, figurative expressions, and invitations to involve the
the character moved to a new scene, the part of the reader’s brain reader) can temporarily destabilize the personality system. Djikic,
concerned with analyzing visual scenes was activated. Speer et al. Oatley, Zoeterman, and Peterson (2009) conducted an experiment
discussed their findings in terms of readers running a simulation of on the short story of Chekhov (1899/1917): The Lady With the
events they read about. Little Dog. People were assigned to read either this story or a
A principal reason for the superiority of fiction over nonfiction control text. Chekhov’s story is about two people who start an
for promoting understanding of others is, as Zunshine (2006) affair at the seaside resort of Yalta. The control text, written by
argued, that theory-of-mind is the typical subject of fiction. The Zoeterman and Djikic, was a nonfiction-style courtroom report of
main difference between psychological effects of fiction and non- a divorce case based directly on the short story. It contained the
fiction is a matter of expertise (Oatley, 2011, 2012). People who same information, and had some of the same conversations. Even
read a lot of fiction get better at its subject matter: understanding more important, participants found it just as interesting as Chek-
other minds and what people are up to with each other in the social hov’s story, though not so artistic. In effect, the literary quality of
world. By contrast, people who read about geometry or genetics the text (that relies on rhythm, stylistic factors, literary features)
become more expert at understanding how diagrams can concep- was diminished, while all the narrative components of the text
tualize space or how DNA is important in how plants and animals were maintained. As with Mar et al.’s (2003) study, the control text
ART IN FICTION 501

was arranged with the same reading difficulty and length as instability induced in the system to change into a different config-
Chekhov’s story. uration of personality (see Sabine & Sabine, 1983). Art can there-
In the experiment, people were randomly assigned to read fore be a facilitator, though not a dictator, of self-change.
Chekhov’s story or the control piece. Before they started reading The study by Djikic et al. (2009) was replicated by Djikic,
Djikic et al. (2009) measured their personality traits by means of Oatley, and Carland (2012). Based on the data collection in which
the Big Five personality inventory, and asked them, also, to indi- people were randomly assigned to read one of eight literary short
cate on 0 to 10 scale the intensity of a list 10 emotions they may stories or one of eight literary nonfiction essays (in the study by
have been feeling at the time, with 0 being least intense, and 10 Djikic, Oatley & Moldoveanu, 2013a, mentioned above), they
most intense. After reading we again administered the personality found that people who read a text they judged to be artistic
inventory, and again measured the intensity of participants’ emo- experienced greater fluctuation of personality than did those who
tions. We found that those who read Chekhov’s story, but not those read a text they judged not to be artistic. The genre (fiction vs.
who read the control text, experienced fluctuations, or changes, in nonfiction) did not matter as much as the artistic quality, in
their personality. A participant would, for example, report an producing these fluctuations or short-term changes in personality.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

increase in openness and a decrease in conscientiousness, while These results answer a question that arises in relation to studies
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

another participant would have a completely different pattern of such as those of Kidd and Castano (2013): How is it possible that
changes. a single exposure to a piece of literature can change something as
The main point was that individuals who read Chekhov’s story stable as the personality trait of empathy? The results imply a
temporarily changed (fluctuated in their personality) more on process in which the artistic component of literature temporarily
average than those who read the less artistic version. Furthermore, unfreezes one’s personality system, as its narrative components
for the readers of Chekhov’s story, the changes were all in differ- allow the person to incorporate others’ experience in their own
ent directions. In other words, in contrast to effects of persuasion, personality system and restabilize it.
changes of individuals in their personality were idiosyncratic. Miall and Kuiken (2002) found that literariness of a narrative is
Everyone had a different type of change. The changes were me- closely related to its ability to prompt emotions that are the
diated by the intensity of emotions that readers experienced while readers’ own. The induction of personality instability is helped by
reading, and these emotions too, were idiosyncratic of different this emotional instability (Djikic et al., 2009). As well as referring
types and directions. They indicated what people felt, individually, semantically to people, actions, and objects, as narrative must do,
about the story. artistic narrative draws on tropes such as metaphor and metonymy.
Popular literature often moves our emotions, but at the end of an It arranges words in syntactical orders that may not be of the most
emotional roller coaster of the type provided by the typical horror common kind; it has its own rhythms, alliterations, and asso-
story or thriller, we remain much as we were before we opened the nances. It achieves what researchers on literature have called
book. Emotion is important to personality change, but not emotion “foregrounding” (Miall & Kuiken, 1994; van Peer, 1986) that is to
as programmed by writers who have decided in advance that they say it brings phrases to attention and makes them more likely to be
want their readers to be anxious (in a thriller), or horrified (in a emotionally moving.
horror story), and suchlike. We think that the intensity of the One difficulty with artistic, that is, literary narratives, is that we
different emotions people felt as they read Chekhov’s story indi- often focus so much on the effects of narratives, that we underes-
cated the strength and importance they attached to the story’s timate effects of artistic style. As Mithen (1996) argued, art is a
characters and events, that is to say by how touched they were by relatively late arrival in human mentality. It is perhaps about
the story. The readers’ emotions were not prespecified. They were 50,000 years old. It is signaled in the archaeological record not just
the readers’ own. by cave paintings but by burial sites that imply the existence of
Personality is a stable system. Research has shown that a change funerals at which stories would be told about the dead person.
in any stable system (biological, physiological, or psychological) Mithen proposed that the central component is metaphor, in which
needs to be preceded by a fluctuation that is strong enough to move a something can be a something else. Marks on a cave wall can
the system to a different level (Bak & Chen, 1991; Schiepek, also be a bison. At a funeral someone dead is also alive on another
Fricke, & Kaimer, 1992). The fluctuation necessary for change in plane. As Dissanayake (1992) argued, the effect of art is to make
personality occurs both for negative changes, such as trauma (van something special.
der Kolk, 1987/2003), and positive ones, such as growth in psy- Art in general, including literary effects of foregrounding, has
chotherapeutic settings (Bonanno, 2004; Linley & Joseph, 2004). effects that Flaubert referred to when he constructed what seems to
Furthermore, this type of fluctuation, or variability in personality, be the first theory (perhaps still the best theory, see Oatley &
is known to happen during developmentally active life periods Djikic, 2008) of how to write prose fiction. This type of writing,
(Fleeson & Jolley, 2006). Djikic et al.’s (2009) study indicated said Flaubert,
that, in regard to Chekhov’s literary story, it was not the narrative
content that caused this fluctuation, but the artistic qualities. would be as rhythmical as verse, as precise as the language of science,
and with the undulations, the humming of a cello, the plumes of fire,
Fluctuation prompted by literary style is temporary. This means
a style that would enter your mind like a rapier thrust, and on which
that for many individuals exposed to literature (and other arts), the finally your thoughts would slide as if over a smooth surface. (as cited
personality system temporarily may open and then revert to its in Williams, 2004, p. 167)
former configuration. The implication is that an experience of this
type can be merely of passing interest. By contrast, those who The effect of art in fictional narratives is shared with art in other
resonate more strongly with a work of art, as indicated by strong domains: with instrumental music, with visual arts, or with dance.
emotions of their own while reading, may be helped by the Fluctuations in personality comparable to those that occurred in
502 DJIKIC AND OATLEY

reading artistic literature have been found when people listened to Oatley, and Peterson (2006) found that in interviews aimed to elicit
music (Djikic, 2011) and looked at pieces of visual art (Djikic, autobiographical information, writers were far more preoccupied
Oatley, & Peterson, 2012). These results support the hypothesis with their emotions than physicists were with theirs. The writing,
that literature shares with other arts an effect of introducing a then, becomes for the author an exploration of how the emotional
perturbation to personality, which can sometimes be a precursor to self interacts with self and the world. A comparable effect occurs
a more permanent personality change. for readers.

Artistic Literature as Indirect Communication Putting the Aspects Together


There is a large field of research on persuasion, in which social The three aspects of literary art that we have discussed above do
psychologists have shown how by words and images people can not provide a complete characterization of the art in fiction.
cause others to have beliefs, emotions, and dispositions of partic- Among other important aspects are: art’s relation to the world as
ular kinds. Green and Brock (2005) found that narrative can indicated by the concept of mimesis as discussed by Aristotle
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

increase persuasiveness of a message. Scientific writing, political (trans. 1970), art’s ability, as Longinus (trans. 1965) put it, to be
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

communication, advertising, and propaganda, all seek to persuade. sublime, art’s ability to build bridges between the seen and the
But, as we propose here, art does not try to persuade people to unseen (Kemp, 2006), and the idea that art is, as Hyde (1983) put
believe or act in any particular way. Rather, writers offer cues, and it, not part of a commercial transaction but a gift that can set up a
invite readers to draw their own inferences. certain type of relationship with the person who engages in it.
Kidd and Castano (2013) argued that literary fiction is more The three aspects of art we propose here fit together psycho-
open-ended than popular fiction. People are invited to think more. logically and, as we have shown earlier, beginnings have been
A good explanation for the effect had been offered by Kotovych, taken in exploring them empirically. In this closing section we
Dixon, Bortolussi, and Holden (2011). They reported three exper- integrate these aspects in relation to Chekhov’s story, The Lady
iments, two of which were on Alice Munro’s The Office, a literary With the Little Dog. Not only is Chekhov generally considered the
first-person short-story about a woman who rents an office in greatest artist of the short story, but this story is regarded by many
which to write. In Munro’s stories one has to make inferences. For as his best. Empirically, as Djikic et al. (2009) demonstrated, not
example, near the beginning of The Office, one reads: “But here only did the participants of their experiment regard this story as
comes the disclosure which is not easy for me. I am a writer. That artistic, but empirically it was found that reading it produced
does not sound right. Too presumptuous, phony, or at least uncon- change in how they perceived their own personalities, the change
vincing.” (as cited in Kotovych et al., 2011, p.270) The woman that was unique to each participant.
narrator talks to the reader, who has to infer what she feels. In In relation to the first aspect—that of simulation—we suggest
Kotovych et al.’s first experiment some participants were given that we engage in the story, first of all, by identification with the
Munro’s story to read, and others were given a version that instead main character, Gurov. As we start up the simulation, we set aside
of the lines quoted above says, “I’m embarrassed telling people our own goals and concerns, somewhat as a person does who is
that I am a writer.” Readers of the original Munro story attained a starting a session of meditation, and we take on the concerns and
deeper understanding, and a closer identification with the narrator intentions of Gurov. In this way, as Trabasso and Chung (2004)
than those who were told explicitly how she felt. In their 2003 demonstrated, we empathize with the protagonist. At the same
book, Psychonarratology, Bortolussi and Dixon (2003) proposed time, as Kuiken, Miall, and Sikora (2004) stated, reading a story of
that literary fiction is like conversation in which we make infer- this kind implicates the self, connecting us to memories and
ences about other people. It is not that literary fiction makes us deepening our self-understanding.
work harder than we may want. Its secret is that, as compared with The relation of this to the second aspect is that because as
some types of popular fiction that are explicit in what the reader is readers we have taken on the concerns of the protagonist, it is we
expected to think and feel; it comes closer to conversation, an ourselves—not any fictional character—who experience the emo-
activity we enjoy and spend an enormous amount of time in. The tions of the story. In artistic works not only can we experience our
inferences of conversation are everyday means by which we come own emotions, but we can reflect on them. Emotions are critical
to know the minds of others. psychological mediators between outer and inner, and they give us
The manner in which artistic literature differs from nonartistic a sense of urgency and importance (Oatley & Johnson-Laird,
narratives such as explanatory essays or some popular fiction is 2014). It is not just that works of art have, as Mithen (1996)
that it does not give us ready-made answers to our questions. explained, qualities of metaphor in which one thing can be some-
Collingwood (1938) proposed that art that is properly so-called thing else, but as Oatley (in press) proposed, we can become
does not aim to produce a specified effect. He explained that there metaphorical. By identification we can become a literary character.
are many crafts that have this attribute. If a carpenter makes a In Chekhov’s story we can remain ourselves, and become Gurov or
chair, he or she has a plan to produce an object with attributes that Anna. We thereby put ourselves in the position of experiencing
are specified in advance. By contrast, said, Collingwood, art is not perturbations in our usually fixed schemas of personality. The
based on any such plan. Instead, in a language such as words, process is one that Kaufman and Libby (2012) called “experience
painting, or music, it is an expression of an emotion that is not yet taking” in which, as they demonstrated in six studies, aspects of
fully understood. The outcome is unknown. Although the concen- our emotions, our beliefs, our behavior, and our self can change.
tration on expression of emotions is too restrictive, Collingwood’s The third aspect of artistic literature is that the writer’s commu-
hypothesis did usefully separate art from craft. It has also been a nication to the reader is indirect. In The Lady With the Little Dog,
useful starting point for psychological studies. For instance Djikic, we may feel, with Gurov, that life has a husk and a kernel, so that
ART IN FICTION 503

although some parts of our life are public, other parts are hidden. stability of the adult personality. In T. F. Heatherton & J. L. Weinberger
Or we may feel sympathy for Anna, who finds herself in a (Eds.), Can personality change? (pp. 21– 40). Washington, DC: Amer-
marriage that convention seems to have provided for her, but ican Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/10143-002
which no longer engages her. Or we may find ourselves disap- Costanzo, M., & Archer, D. (1989). Interpreting the expressive behaviour
proving of Gurov and Anna, who are both married but are now of others: The Interpersonal Perception Task. Journal of Nonverbal
Behavior, 13, 225–245.
having an affair. Or, with Malcolm (2002) who based her book on
Currie, G. (2011, September 2). Let’s pretend. Times Literary Supplement,
a pilgrimage she made to Russia to walk in Chekhov’s footsteps, pp. 14 –15.
we may feel that the deep essentials that Chekhov suggested in this Davis, M. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence
story are of the privacy of inwardness and, at the same time, of for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social
leaves of autumn, of growing old. Psychology, 44, 113–126.
Chekhov put the issue of the indirectness of his stories like this. Dissanayake, E. (1992). Homo Aestheticus: Where art come from and why.
In a letter of October 27, 1888 to Suvorin, he wrote that there are Seattle: University of Washington Press.
two things one must not confuse, “answering the questions and Djikic, M. (2011). The effect of music and lyrics on personality. Psychol-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

formulating them correctly. Only the latter is required of an au- ogy of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 5, 237–240. doi:10.1037/
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

thor” (as cited in Heim & Karlinsky, 1997, p. 117). A few lines a0022313
later he suggested that artistic writing compares with presentation Djikic, M. (2014). Art of mindfulness: Integrating Eastern and Western
approaches. In A. Ie, C. Ngnoumen, & E. J. Langer (Eds.), The Wiley–
in a court of law. “It is the duty of the court to formulate the
Blackwell handbook of mindfulness (pp. 139 –148). Oxford, England:
questions correctly, but it is up to each member of the jury to
Wiley.
answer them according to his own preference” (as cited in Heim & Djikic, M., Oatley, K., & Carland, M. (2012). Genre or artistic merit: The
Karlinsky, 1997, p. 117). effect of literature on personality. Scientific Study of Literature, 2,
In his elegy to Yeats, Auden (1977) wrote, “poetry makes 25–36. doi:10.1075/ssol.2.1.02dji
nothing happen” (p. 242). He was right; art is not like a toaster that Djikic, M., Oatley, K., & Moldoveanu, M. C. (2013a). Opening the closed
makes bread turn into toast. But recent evidence has shown that mind: The effect of exposure to literature on the need for closure.
artistic literature can allow things to happen in the minds and Creativity Research Journal, 25, 149 –154. doi:10.1080/10400419.2013
personalities of readers. Psychologically, this is a nondirective .783735
social influence: indirect communication. Djikic, M., Oatley, K., & Moldoveanu, M. (2013b). Reading other minds:
Art involves the nondirective property of inviting those who Effects of literature on empathy. Scientific Study of Literature, 3, 28 – 47.
engage with it to experience their own emotions and thoughts. doi:10.1075/ssol.3.1.06dji
Djikic, M., Oatley, K., & Peterson, J. (2006). The bitter-sweet labor of
Literature can help us navigate our self-development by transcend-
emoting: The linguistic comparison of writers and physicists. Creativity
ing our current self while at the same time making available to us
Research Journal, 18, 191–197. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1802_5
a multitude of potential future selves. Djikic, M., Oatley, K., & Peterson, J. B. (2012). Serene arts: The effect of
personal unsettledness and of paintings’ narrative structure on person-
References ality. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 30, 183–193. doi:10.2190/EM.30.2.e
Auden, W. H. (1977). The English Auden: Poems, essays, and dramatic Djikic, M., Oatley, K., Zoeterman, S., & Peterson, J. (2009a). On being
writings 1927–1939. London, England: Faber and Faber. moved by art: How reading fiction transforms the self. Creativity Re-
Bak, P., & Chen, K. (1991). Self-organized criticality. Scientific American, search Journal, 21, 24 –29. doi:10.1080/10400410802633392
264, 46–53. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0191-46 Fleeson, W., & Jolley, S. (2006). A proposed theory of the adult develop-
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). ment of intraindividual variability in trait-manifesting behavior. In D. K.
The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: A study with Mroczek & T. D. Little (Eds.), Handbook of personality development
normal adults, and adults with Asperger’s syndrome or high-functioning (pp. 41–59). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 241–251. Fong, K., Mullin, J., & Mar, R. (2013). What you read matters: The role of
doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00715 fiction genres in predicting interpersonal sensitivity. Psychology of Aes-
Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, T., & Tice, D. (1994). Losing control: How thetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7, 370 –376. doi:10.1037/a0034084
and why people fail at self-regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Goldstein, T. (2009). Psychological perspectives on acting. Psychology of
Bonanno, G. A. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience: Have we Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 6 –9. doi:10.1037/a0014644
underestimated the human capacity to thrive after extremely aversive Goldstein, T., & Winner, E. (2012). Enhancing empathy and theory of
events? American Psychologist, 59, 20 –28. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.59 mind. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13, 19 –37. doi:10.1080/
.1.20 15248372.2011.573514
Bortolussi, M., & Dixon, P. (2003). Psychonarratology: Foundations for Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2005). Persuasiveness of narratives. In T. C.
the empirical study of literary response. Cambridge: England: Cam- Brock & M. C. Green (Eds.), Persuasion: Psychological insights and
bridge University Press. perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 117–142). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Green, M. C., Chatham, C., & Sestir, M. (2012). Emotion and transporta-
University Press. tion in fact and fiction. Scientific Study of Literature, 2, 37–59. doi:
Chekhov, A. C. (1884/1979). In R. E. Matlow (Ed.), Anton Chekhov’s 10.1075/ssol.2.1.03gre
Short Stories (pp. 1–3). New York: Norton. Green, M. C., Kass, S., Carrey, J., Herzig, B., Feeney, R., & Sabini, J.
Chekhov, A. C. (1899/1917). The lady with the toy dog. In S. S. Kote- (2008). Transportation across media: Repeated exposure to text and film.
liansky & G. Cannan (Eds.), The house with the mezzanine and other Media Psychology, 11, 512–539. doi:10.1080/15213260802492000
stories (pp. 69 –94). New York: C. Scribner’s Sons. Hakemulder, F. J. (2000). The moral laboratory: Experiments examining
Collingwood, R. G. (1938). The principles of art. Oxford, England: Oxford the effects of reading literature on social perception and moral self-
University Press. concept. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Benjamins. doi:10.1075/upal.34
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1994). Set like plaster: Evidence for the Hakemulder, F. J. (2008). Imagining what could happen: Taking the role of
504 DJIKIC AND OATLEY

a character on social cognition. In S. Zyngier, M. Bortolussi, A. Ches- Mar, R. A. (2011). The neural bases of social cognition and story com-
nokova, & J. Auracher (Eds.), Directions in empirical literary studies: In prehension. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 103–134. doi:10.1146/
honor of Willie van Peer (pp. 139 –153). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: annurev-psych-120709-145406
Benjamins. doi:10.1075/lal.5.13hak Mar, R. A., Oatley, K., & Eng, A. (2003, August). Abstraction and the
Heim, M. H., & Karlinsky, S. (1997). Anton Chekhov’s life and thought: vividness of details in fiction. Paper presented at the 111th annual
Selected letters and commentary. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University convention of the American Psychological Association, Toronto, Can-
Press. ada.
Hyde, L. (1983). The gift: Imagination and the erotic life of property. New Mar, R. A., Oatley, K., Hirsh, J., dela Paz, J., & Peterson, J. B. (2006).
York, NY: Vintage. Bookworms versus nerds: Exposure to fiction versus non-fiction, diver-
John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., & Kentle, R. (1991). The Big Five Inventory. gent associations with social ability, and the simulation of fictional
Technical Report, University of California, Berkeley. social worlds. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 694 –712. doi:
Johnson, D. R. (2012). Transportation into a story increases empathy, 10.1016/j.jrp.2005.08.002
prosocial behavior, and perceptual bias toward fearful expressions. Per- Mar, R. A., Oatley, K., & Peterson, J. B. (2009). Exploring the link
sonality and Individual Differences, 52, 150 –155. doi:10.1016/j.paid between reading fiction and empathy: Ruling out individual differences
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

.2011.10.005 and examining outcomes. Communications: The European Journal of


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Johnson, D. R. (2013). Transportation into literary fiction reduces preju- Communication, 34, 407– 428.
dice against, and increases empathy for Arab Muslims. Scientific Study Mar, R. A., Tackett, J. L., & Moore, C. (2010). Exposure to media and
of Literature, 3, 77–92. doi:10.1075/ssol.3.1.08joh theory-of-mind development in preschoolers. Cognitive Development,
Johnson, D. R. (2014). Changing race boundary perception by reading 25, 69 –78. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.11.002
narrative fiction. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 36, 83–90. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1990). Personality in adulthood. New
doi:10.1080/01973533.2013.856791 York, NY: Guilford Press.
Johnson, D. R., Cushman, G. C., Borden, L. A., & McCune, M. S. (2013). McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1996). Toward a new generation of
Potentiating empathic growth: Generating imagery while reading fiction personality theories: Theoretical context for the five-factor model. In
increases empathy and prosocial behavior. Psychology of Aesthetics, J. S. Wiggins (Ed.), The five-factor model of personality: Theoretical
perspectives (pp. 51– 87). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Creativity, and the Arts, 7, 306 –312. doi:10.1037/a0033261
Miall, D. S., & Kuiken, D. (1994). Foregrounding, defamiliarization, and
Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1971). The actor and the observer:
affect: Response to literary stories. Poetics, 22, 389 – 407. doi:10.1016/
Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. New York, NY: Gen-
0304-422X(94)00011-5
eral Learning Press.
Miall, D. S., & Kuiken, D. (2002). A feeling for fiction: Becoming what we
Kaufman, G. F., & Libby, L. K. (2012). Changing beliefs and behavior
behold. Poetics, 30, 221–241. doi:10.1016/S0304-422X(02)00011-6
through experience-taking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Mithen, S. (1996). The prehistory of the mind: The cognitive origins of art
ogy, 103, 1–19. doi:10.1037/a0027525
and science. London, England: Thames and Hudson.
Keats, J. (1959). Selected poems and letters of Keats (Edited by D. Bush).
Oatley, K. (1992). Best laid schemes: The psychology of emotions. New
New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin.
York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Keen, S. (2007). Empathy and the novel. New York, NY: Oxford Univer-
Oatley, K. (1999). Why fiction may be twice as true as fact: Fiction as
sity Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195175769.001.0001
cognitive and emotional simulation. Review of General Psychology, 3,
Kemp, M. (2006). Seen/unseen: Art, science, and intuition from Leonardo
101–117. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.3.2.101
to the Hubble telescope. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Oatley, K. (2011). Such stuff as dreams: The psychology of fiction. Oxford,
Kidd, D. C., & Castano, E. (2013). Reading literary fiction improves theory
England: Wiley-Blackwell. doi:10.1002/9781119970910
of mind. Science, 342, 377–380. doi:10.1126/science.1239918 Oatley, K. (2012). The cognitive science of fiction. WIREs Cognitive
Kierkegaard, S. (1968). Concluding unscientific postscript (D. F. Swenson Science, 3, 425– 430. doi:10.1002/wcs.1185
& W. Lowrie, Trans.). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. (Orig- Oatley, K. (2013). How cues on the screen prompt emotions in the mind.
inal work published 1846) In A. P. Shimamura (Ed.), Psychocinematics (pp. 269 –284). New York,
Kincaid, J. P., Fishburne, R. P., Rogers, R. L., & Chissom, B. S. (1975). NY: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199862139
Derivation of new readability formulas (Automated Readability Index, .003.0014
Fog Count, and Flesch Reading Ease formula) for Navy enlisted per- Oatley, K. (in press). Worlds of the possible: Abstraction, imagination,
sonnel. Research Branch Report 8 –75. Naval Air Station Memphis: consciousness. Pragmatics and Cognition.
Chief of Naval Technical Training. Oatley, K., & Djikic, M. (2008). Writing as thinking. Review of General
Kotovych, M., Dixon, P., Bortolussi, M., & Holden, M. (2011). Textual Psychology, 12, 9 –27. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.12.1.9
determinants of a component of literary identification. Scientific Study of Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2014). Cognitive approaches to emo-
Literature, 1, 260 –291. doi:10.1075/ssol.1.2.05kot tions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 134 –140. doi:10.1016/j.tics
Kuiken, D., Miall, D. S., & Sikora, S. (2004). Forms of self-implication in .2013.12.004
literary reading. Poetics Today, 25, 171–203. doi:10.1215/03335372-25- Oliver, M. B., & Bartsch, A. (2010). Appreciation as audience response:
2-171 Exploring entertainment gratifications beyond hedonism. Human Com-
Larsen, S. F., & Seilman, U. (1988). Personal remindings while reading munication Research, 36, 53– 81. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2009
literature. Text, 8, 411– 429. doi:10.1515/text.1.1988.8.4.411 .01368.x
Linley, P. A., & Joseph, S. (2004). Positive change following trauma and Premack, D. G., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a
adversity: A review. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 17, 11–21. doi: theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515–526. doi:
10.1023/B:JOTS.0000014671.27856.7e 10.1017/S0140525X00076512
Malcolm, J. (2002). Reading Chekhov: A critical journey. New York, NY: Radford, C. (1975). How can we be moved by the fate of Anna Karenina?
Random. Proceedings of the Aristotelican Society, 49, 67– 80.
Mann, C. (2011, November). How the potato changed the world. Smith- Rain, M., & Mar, R. A. (2014). Measuring reading behavior: Examining
sonian Magazine. Retrieved from http://www.smithsonianmag.com/ the predictive validity of print-exposure checklists. Empirical Studies of
history-archeology/How-the-Potato-Changed-the-World.html the Arts, 32, 93–108. doi:10.2190/EM.32.1f
ART IN FICTION 505

Sabine, G., & Sabine, P. (1983). Books that made the difference. Hamden, Van der Kolk, B. A. (2003). Psychological trauma (2nd ed.). Arlington,
CT: Library Professional. VA: American Psychiatric Publishing. (Original work published 1987)
Schiepek, G., Fricke, B., & Kaimer, P. (1992). Synergetics of psychother- Van Peer, W. (1986). Stylistics and psychology: Investigations of fore-
apy. In W. Tschacher, G. Schiepek, & E. J. Brunner (Eds.), Self- grounding. London, England: Croom Helm.
organization and clinical psychology (pp. 239 –267). Berlin, Germany: Williams, T. (2004). The writing process: Scenarios, sketches and rough
Springer-Verlag. doi:10.1007/978-3-642-77534-5_13 drafts. In T. Unwin (Ed.), The Cambridge companion to Flaubert (pp.
Schramm, H., & Wirth, W. (2010). Exploring the paradox of sad-film 165–179). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. doi:
enjoyment: The role of multiple appraisals and meta-appraisals. Poetics, 10.1017/CCOL0521815517.010
38, 319 –335. doi:10.1016/j.poetic.2010.03.002 Zeman, A., Milton, F., Smith, A., & Rylance, R. (2013). By heart: An
Seilman, U., & Larsen, S. (1989). Personal resonance to literature. Poetics,
fMRI study of brain activation by poetry and prose. Journal of Con-
18, 165–177. doi:10.1016/0304-422X(89)90027-2
sciousness Studies, 20(9 –10), 132–158.
Speer, N. K., Reynolds, J., Swallow, K., & Zacks, J. M. (2009). Reading
Zunshine, L. (2006). Why we read fiction: Theory of mind and the novel.
stories activates neural representations of visual and motor experience.
Columbus: Ohio State University Press.
Psychological Science, 20, 989 –999. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

.02397.x
Stanovich, K. E., West, R. F., & Harrison, M. R. (1995). Knowledge
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

growth and maintenance across the life span: The role of print exposure. Received January 21, 2014
Developmental Psychology, 31, 811– 826. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.31.5 Revision received July 28, 2014
.811 Accepted July 30, 2014 䡲

View publication stats

You might also like