You are on page 1of 14

PsyCh Journal 10 (2021): 141–154

DOI: 10.1002/pchj.402

An Indonesian model of well-being: The integration of universal


and cultural factors

Herdiyan Maulana ,1 Nigar Khawaja,2 and Patricia Obst2


1
Faculty of Psychology, Universitas Negeri Jakarta, Jakarta Selatan, Indonesia, 2School of
Psychology & Counselling, Faculty of Health, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane,
Queensland, Australia

Abstract: The current study examined a predictive model of both universal factors (socioeconomic, neuroticism, and extraversion) and cultur-
ally specific factors (sense of community, trust, and self-construal) towards the well-being of Indonesians. A nationally representative sample
(N = 929) was recruited across the nation to complete a survey in either online or offline format. The survey contained a measure of well-being
as well as demographic, psychosocial, and cultural variables. Analysis via hierarchical multiple regression showed that both universal and cul-
tural factors were associated with well-being in this Indonesian sample. In particular, once the cultural factors were added to the model, there
were changes in universal factor effects to well-being. The finding of this study recognized the multidimensional model of well-being and sup-
ports the importance of evaluating well-being at the dimensional level to allow for an exploration of the nuanced relationship between distinct
indicators and facets of well-being. The theoretical implications of these results and future directions were also discussed in this study.

Keywords: cultural factors; Indonesia; social factors; well-being


Correspondence Dr. Herdiyan Maulana, Faculty of Psychology, Universitas Negeri Jakarta, Kampus D, Fakultas Pendidikan Psikologi
UNJ, Jalan Halimun No.2 Jakarta Selatan, Indonesia. Email: herdiyan-maulana@unj.ac.id
Received 29 January 2020. Accepted 9 August 2020.

Well-being is an important concept and one that all humans rather than individualist. Much of the well-being research
strive for; it is critically important for an individual’s physi- conducted from a Western perspective has also overlooked
cal and psychosocial health (Diener, Seligman, Choi, & potential culturally specific factors that may be of impor-
Oishi, 2018). The well-being of a nation’s population is tance in non-Western populations (Lu, 2005; Lu &
considered as an indicator of progress and development of Gilmour, 2004). Recent theorists have postulated that uni-
the society (Veenhoven, 1984). Consensus has emerged in versal and culturally specific factors are both important in
the literature that well-being is a multidimensional concept, gaining a full understanding of the experience of well-being
and studies have identified a multitude of factors associated in distinct cultures (Lomas, 2015). The current research
with the dimensions of well-being (Knoop & Delle explores the influence of universal and culturally specific
Fave, 2013; Lomas, 2015). These studies have highlighted factors on the well-being of Indonesians. This study would
a number of individual sociodemographic and personality potentially contribute to the cross-cultural psychological
characteristics as factors associated with well-being. Inter- theory and understanding of well-being in one of the
estingly, although the majority of these studies have been world’s largest non-individualist populations.
conducted in Western countries, they are considered uni-
versal in nature (Knoop & Delle Fave, 2013). Universal and cultural factors
There remain few studies that have explored the rele-
vance of these universal factors to the well-being of people Well-being is a multidimensional concept (Lomas, 2015),
in non-Western countries with strong collectivist cultures with physical, cognitive, social, and behavioral dimensions

© 2020 Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences and John Wiley & Sons Australia, Ltd
142 An Indonesian model of well-being

(Diener, 2009). At the physical level, it involves the pres- These universal factors emphasize the importance of
ence of physical health, not only the absence of disease individual personal characteristics and economic status.
(Diener et al., 2015; Diener, Pressman, Hunter, & Del- However, well-being is also a product of the interaction
gadillo-Chase, 2017). At the cognitive level, it consists of between people and their sociocultural context (Knoop &
thoughts and ideas associated with happiness, life satisfac- Delle Fave, 2013; Lomas, 2015). These cultural factors
tion, and self-worth (Marcinko, 2015). The social dimen- encompass values and attitudes that reflect sociocultural
sion consists of positive interactions and the quality of preferences and ideals (Berry et al., 2002). These cultural
relationships with others (Lu & Gilmour, 2004). While the factors may play an even more important role than univer-
behavioral level encompasses actively working towards sal individual factors in the well-being of non-Western col-
self-fulfillment (Ryan & Deci, 2001). lectivist populations (Jetten, Postmes, & McAuliffe, 2002;
According to Berry, Poortinga, Segall, and Dasen (2002), Lu & Gilmour, 2004). Cultural values, emphasizing social
there are a number of universal factors that contribute to responsibility, heighten the importance of the community
these aspects of well-being regardless of the cultural context. and family in these cultures (Lu, 2005). A sense of commu-
Researchers have identified these universal factors in psycho- nity is developed through an individual’s interaction with
logical states (e.g., personality and coping strategy) and their community (Obst & White, 2004) and has been
sociodemographic aspects, such as income, occupation sta- shown to be a protective factor for well-being, acting as a
tus, education background, and physical health (De Neve, buffer against risk factors and challenges and enabling the
Christakis, Fowler, & Frey, 2012; Michalos, 2017). Fulfill- building of trust. In contrast, social trust has been found to
ment of these essential needs is associated with the experi- be associated with well-being (Jovanovic, 2016) by foster-
ence of higher levels of well-being across cultures ing reciprocal social support (Li, Lan, & Ju, 2015; Schwarz
(Glozlah, 2015; Gori-Maia, 2013; Khodarahimi, 2015; et al., 2010; Wakefield et al., 2017).
Khumalo, Temane, & Wissing, 2012). A regular income and Indeed, self-construal, our definition of self, either being
financial security are required to meet the basic needs and independent or dependent on others, is deeply based in these
the demands of everyday life (Ferrer-i-Carbonell, 2005; cultural values. Western populations’ tendency towards an
Oshio, Nozaki, & Kobayashi, 2011). In addition, education independent self-construal emphasizes the value of autonomy
provides an individual with skills that help secure a good and individuality. Collective cultures have a tendency of lean-
position in the job market (Tan, Tambyah, & Kau, 2006), ing towards a more dependent or interdependent self-construal
and a good marital relationship provides positive emotional with an emphasis on social connectedness (Cross, Hardin, &
and social support (Khodarahimi, 2015). Gercek-Swing, 2011; Kitayama, Markus, & Kurokawa,
Being free from mental and physical illnesses also 2000). Self-construal can guide the way in which people place
clearly contributes to well-being for all individuals value on different aspects of their life and how they define
(Steptoe, Deaton, & Stone, 2015). Mental and physical well-being (Pilarska, 2014). The Indonesian people place a
health can be influenced by psychological strengths, such strong emphasis on community, social trust, and group mem-
as positive coping, resilience, and an ability to engage in bership as key factors contributing to a meaningful life experi-
self-care (Marcinko, 2015; Ryan & Deci, 2001). Personal- ence (Landiyanto, Ling, Puspitasari, & Irianti, 2011;
ity factors, in particular extraversion and neuroticism, have Sujarwoto & Tampubolon, 2014). Hence, this current research
also consistently been found to be associated with well- explores the influence of the potentially important culturally
being across cultures (Jensen, Kirkegaard Thomsen, relevant factors on the well-being of Indonesians.
O’Connor, & Mehlsen, 2020; Schimmack, Radhakrishnan,
Oishi, Dzokoto, & Ahadi, 2002; van den Berg &
Pitariu, 2005). Extraversion is commonly regarded as a Indonesian perspective of well-being
positive emotional disposition (Kim, Schimmack, Oishi, &
Tsutsui, 2017; Lucas, Diener, Grob, Suh, & Shao, 2000) Indonesia is the fourth most populated country in the world. It
that helps people to openly socialize and interact with is a nation with rich cultural diversity characterized by a
others. In contrast, neuroticism has been negatively associ- strong collectivist culture emphasizing social cohesion over
ated with positive emotions, leading to low levels of well- personal benefits and pursuits (Jetten et al., 2002). Religious
being (Røysamb, Nes, & Czajkowski, 2018). practices and values are placed as a medium through which

© 2020 Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences and John Wiley & Sons Australia, Ltd
PsyCh Journal 143

quality of life can be achieved. Previous studies indicated that identified in previous studies and the well-being in a sam-
religious practices were positively associated with Indone- ple of Indonesians. Well-being was measured using the
sians’ quality of life (Yeo, 2014) as well as with stronger IWS, the newly developed valid measure of well-being for
social cohesion (Yuniarti, 2006). Previous studies with the the Indonesian population (Maulana et al., 2019). The
Indonesian population have shown that socioeconomic research first explores the relationship of universal and cul-
factors, such as income (Eggleston, Hardee, Irwanto, tural factors with overall well-being and then for each of
Poerwandari, & Severy, 2001), employment status (Rahayu, the four dimensions—Basic Needs, Social Relations, Self-
2016), health (Eggleston et al., 2001), and gender differences acceptance, and Spirituality—measured by the IWS. Based
(Landiyanto et al., 2011), are associated with well-being. on the research reviewed above, it was hypothesized that
However, other studies have not fully supported these associa- the universal factors of marital status, health, education
tions (e.g., Safarina, Mawarpury, & Sari, 2014; Sohn, 2013) level, income, employment, and the personality factors of
and have highlighted the importance of other factors, such as neuroticism and extraversion would be associated with
religious values and principles, which influence attitudes, well-being. Additionally, it was hypothesized that culturally
values, and people’s interactions with each other (Primasari & specific variables, such as trust, sense of community, and
Yuniarti, 2012; Yeo, 2014; Yuniarti, 2006). self-construal, would be significantly associated with well-
A recent qualitative investigation by Maulana, Obst, and being after taking into account demographics and the uni-
Khawaja (2018) identified material needs, social relations, versal factors.
self-acceptance, and spirituality as salient factors contribut-
ing to the well-being of Indonesians. Based on this earlier
Method
study, the researchers developed the Indonesian Well-Being
Scale (IWS; Maulana, Khawaja, & Obst, 2019). A nation-
Participants
ally representative sample (N = 1,028) from 27 cities across
Participants (N = 929) were Indonesian adults (68%
Indonesia participated in the scale’s development study. The
females, 32% males) from a range of ethnic groups across
scale was subjected to a combination of exploratory factor
Indonesia. Most of the participants (83%) lived in the cen-
analysis, confirmatory factor analysis, and standard reliabil-
tral region of the country (Java island), while the rest came
ity analyses (test–retest, convergent, and divergent validity).
from Indonesia’s western (Sumatra) and eastern parts (Bali,
These statistical techniques together promise a rigorous
Kalimantan, Maluku, Sulawesi, and Papua). The majority
empirical test of the respective scale. The scale yields a mul-
of participants were young. They were either married or
tidimensional measure of well-being specific to the Indone-
single. They primarily identified as Muslims (>90%). Most
sian context. This culturally appropriate scale encapsulates
of the participants were in paid occupations with varied
aspects most valued by this population and addresses prob-
educational levels. Nearly half of the participants were from
lems caused by adaptation and translation of English-
the medium income level, while others reported high or no
language scales into other languages. The IWS measures the
income. Demographic details are provided in Table 1.
dimensions of basic needs and social support, identified in
the research as universal factors. It also measures the psy-
chological traits of self-acceptance, social cohesion, and Data collection
spirituality as they apply specifically to the Indonesian cul- Health, safety, and ethical clearances (approval:16000
ture (Maulana et al., 2019). By using an Indonesian-cul- 00635) were obtained from the Queensland University of
tural-derived measure from an Indonesian perspective, we Technology, Australia, and Universitas Negeri Jakarta,
expect that a valid and reliable measure of well-being can be Indonesia. The study is part of a larger study conducted as
conducted in the most culturally appropriate way. part of the first author’s PhD dissertation over a period of
3 years where data were collected across Indonesia. Data
were collected online and through hard copy. Participants
Research aim completed a questionnaire battery available in the Indone-
sian language. Two scales that were not available in the
The current research aimed to explore the relative contribu- Indonesian language (the Brief Sense of Community Scale
tion of key universal and culturally specific factors and the Interpersonal Trust Scale) were back translated.

© 2020 Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences and John Wiley & Sons Australia, Ltd
144 An Indonesian model of well-being

Table 1 surveys were recruited through local universities, local reli-


Demographic Data of Study’s Participants
gious communities, and health community centres in
Distribution, Jakarta, Indonesia. Once the participant had signed the con-
Characteristic n (%)
sent form, they were invited to complete the paper-based
Gender
Female 632 (68%) questionnaire. The participants were debriefed after they
Male 297 (32%) had completed the questionnaires and were offered a small
Age group
gift as a token of appreciation (e.g., a notebook/pen worth
Early adult (18–34 years old) 670 (71%)
Middle adult (35–54 years old) 250 (27%) $2 A.U.D.).
Late adult (>55 years old) 9 (2%)
Marital Status
Married 372 (40%) Measures
Not Married 539 (58%)
Demographic information
Widowed 18 (2%)
Location A form was provided that covered information about age,
Western region of Indonesia 92 (10%) religion, gender, physical health, education level, occupa-
Central region of Indonesia 775 (83%)
Eastern region of Indonesia 59 (7%) tion status, income level, and marital status. Physical health
Religion was rated with a single item (“How was your physical
Islam 853 (92%)
health condition for the last 2 weeks?”).
Christianity 57 (6%)
Hinduism 7 (<1%)
Buddhism 2 (<1%)
Unspecified 13 (1%) Indonesian Well-Being Scale
Highest education The IWS (Maulana et al., 2019) consists of 20 items.
Postgraduate 260 (28%)
Undergraduate 372 (40%)
Respondents respond to each item using a 5-point Likert
Primary education 288 (31%) scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree). The scale
Did not attend school 9 (1%) measures four dimensions of well-being: basic needs fulfill-
Occupation status
Paid occupation 712 (77%) ment, social needs fulfillment, self-acceptance, and spiritu-
Unpaid occupation 189 (20%) ality. There are four items measuring the fulfillment of
Unemployed 28 (3%)
Income (per month)
basic needs (e.g. “I can pay for my/children’s education”),
High standard income (above 7 million 167 (18%) six items addressing social relations (e.g. “I spend time
rupiah) with the family I live with”), five items related to self-
Medium standard income (1.5 to 7 million 418 (45%)
rupiah) acceptance (e.g. “I avoid comparing my life to others”),
Low standard income (below 1.5 million 188 (20%) and five items measuring spirituality (e.g. “God has
rupiah)
No regular income 156 (17%)
decided what is best for me”). The scale has excellent inter-
nal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .89), is positively corre-
lated with other well-being scales (the Satisfaction With
Life Scale and the Warwick–Edinburgh Mental Well-Being
All participants were informed about the confidential
Scale (Stewart-Brown et al., 2011) = .44–.78), composite
nature of the study and their right to withdraw at any time
reliability (CR = .79–.94), and maximum shared variance
without penalty. Participants who were recruited online
(MSV = .20–.30) indices was satisfactory (Maulana et al.,
responded to an email advertisement and social media post
2019). The IWS is available both in the Indonesian and
inviting them to take part in the study. Participant submis-
English languages.
sion of the online survey was regarded as consent to the
research. All online participants who completed the ques-
tionnaires were offered entry into a draw to win 100,000 Personality factors
rupiah (equivalent to $10 A.U.D.) worth of phone credit Two items for each personality type were used from the
vouchers. Big Five Inventory-10 (Rammstedt & John, 2007), which
The paper-based method was used to accommodate those comprises 10 items, with two items each for five personal-
who lacked Internet access or a computer and lived in ity traits. The scale uses a 5-point Likert-type response
Jakarta. The principal author recruited two assistants to col- ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).
lect the data. Participants who were offered paper-based The scale is available in the Indonesian language and has

© 2020 Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences and John Wiley & Sons Australia, Ltd
PsyCh Journal 145

sound test–retest reliability with Pearson coefficient corre- Interpersonal Trust Scale
lation range from .895 to .977 (Lesmana & Santoso, 2019). Interpersonal trust was assessed with two items: “How
The scale maintains its validity and reliability when it is much do you trust people in your hometown?” and “How
translated into a different language (Rammstedt, Kemper, & much do you trust people in general?” The scale was devel-
Borg, 2013). oped by Jovanovic (2016) and both items have been
reported to have a significant correlation (r = .86) and are
significantly associated with subjective well-being. Both
items were rated on an 11-point Likert scale, ranging from
Adult Sources of Self-Esteem Inventory
0 (do not trust at all) to 10 (trust completely). This scale
The Adult Sources of Self-Esteem Inventory (Elovson &
has been translated and adapted into the Indonesian lan-
Fleming, 1989) consists of 20 items assessing the salience
guage and cultural context. The scale has acceptable inter-
of both independent and dependent self-construal orienta-
nal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .88).
tions with two subscales. A 10-point Likert scale ranges
from 0 (low importance of the statement for the respon-
dent) to 10 (higher importance of the statement for the Results
respondent). Sample item statements for the Independent
subscale are “Looks and physical attractiveness” and Preliminary analyses
“Earning a great amount of money and acquiring valuable All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS Version
possessions,” while examples for the Dependent subscale 25.00. There were no missing values in the data. Data were
are “Having influence over the events or people in your subjected to standard statistical assumption checks, includ-
life” and “Relationships with your family, being on good ing normality, linearity, and homogeneity. The White and
terms with your family.” The original English-language Breusch–Pagan tests on the total well-being score indicated
scale was translated and adapted to Indonesian by Pekerti that heteroscedasticity was present (p < .001; Astivia &
and Arli (2017). The Indonesian version of the Adult Zumbo, 2019). To correct the data for the heteroscedasticity
Sources of Self-Esteem Inventory has sound internal con- breach, a robust standard error using a heteroscedasticity-
sistency (Cronbach’s αs for the two factors were .80 and consistent covariance matrix approach with HC4-type esti-
.86, respectively) and meets the construct validity test (min- mator was used. All scales were checked for internal consis-
imum factor loading, .60; Pekerti & Kwantes, 2011). tency by using Cronbach’s α. All scales have sound internal
consistency. The intercorrelations among all variables were
tested using the bivariate Pearson correlation and are pro-
Brief Sense of Community Scale vided in Table 2. The correlation analysis revealed small to
The Brief Sense of Community Scale (BSCS) developed by moderate relationships between universal and cultural fac-
Peterson et al., (2008) was used to measure the participants’ tors. Low to moderate correlations between universal factors
sense of community. This scale consists of eight items that and well-being were identified. Marital status, health, and
examine the sense of community based on four underlying extraversion were positively and strongly correlated with
dimensions (McMillan & Chavis, 1986). These dimensions well-being. As expected, neuroticism was negatively corre-
are Need Fulfillment, Membership, Influence, and Emo- lated with well-being and its indicators. Similar patterns
tional Connection. Respondents were asked to respond to applied to the remaining well-being indicators with the
questions about their community/neighborhood (e.g., “I can exception of spirituality, whereas most of the universal fac-
get what I need in this neighbourhood”). The scale uses a tors (gender, education level, age group, marital status,
5-point Likert-type response ranging from strongly disagree income, and occupation) had weak or no correlation to well-
(0) to strongly agree (5). The scale had satisfactory validity being.
and reliability levels in the original version (Cronbach’s
α = .92; Peterson et al., 2008) as well as in a different lan- Hierarchical multiple regression on well-being
guage (e.g., German; comparative fit index = .994; Tucker– total score
Lewis index = .988; root-mean-square error of approxima- A hierarchical multiple regression was run to first examine
tion = .042; Wombacher, Tagg, Bürgi, & MacBryde, 2010). the relationship between universal and cultural factors

© 2020 Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences and John Wiley & Sons Australia, Ltd
146 An Indonesian model of well-being

(independent variables) and well-being (dependent variable) Basic Needs


(see Table 3). At Step 1, universal factors (gender, educa- The overall model, including all universal and cultural vari-
tion level, age group, marital status, health, income, occu- ables, accounted for 27% of the variability in the Basic
pation status, extraversion, and neuroticism) were entered. Needs dimension. The universal factors entered at Step
At Step 2, cultural factors (trust, sense of community, inde- 1 accounted for 23%, F(9, 919) = 30.52, of the variation in
pendent self-construal, and dependent self-construal) were the aspects of basic needs. As seen in Table 4, as expected,
added to examine the additional variance explained by income was the strongest factor associated with basic needs
these factors. The overall model accounted for 48% of the in Step 1, while neuroticism was negatively associated with
well-being variance and a number of factors emerged as basic needs. When trust, sense of community, and indepen-
significant contributors of that variance. However, their dent and dependent self-construal were added at Step 2, all
contributions indicated by beta weight varied. of these variables remained significant. The addition of
The universal factors (gender, education level, age group, these variables in Step 2 accounted for an additional 4%, F
marital status, health, income, occupation status, extraver- (13, 915) = 26.74, p < .001, of the variation in basic needs.
sion, and neuroticism) accounted for 32% of the variance As seen in Table 4, sense of community was the strongest
in well-being, F(9, 919) = 49.78, p < .001. As seen in cultural factor correlating with basic needs.
Table 3, marital status, health, income, and extraversion
were positively associated with well-being, while neuroti- Social Relations
cism was negatively associated with well-being. Universal The total model (including all universal and cultural vari-
factors remained significantly associated with well-being ables) accounted for 34% of the Social Relations varia-
even when the cultural factors were added. The addition of tion, while the universal factors entered in Step
trust, sense of community, independent self-construal, and 1 accounted for 20%, F(9, 919) = 25.90, of the variation
dependent self-construal at Step 2 accounted for an addi- in Social Relations. In Step 1, marital status, health, and
tional 16%, F(13, 915) = 68.93, p < .001, of the variation extraversion were positively correlated with higher
in well-being. As expected, independent self-construal was Social Relations scores. The addition of trust, sense of
negatively associated with well-being, while sense of com-
community, and self-construal in Step 2 accounted for a
munity and dependent self-construal were the greatest fac-
further 14%, F(13, 915) = 37.45, p < .001, of the vari-
tors associated with well-being.
ance in Social Relations. Once culturally specific factors
were considered in this step, the universal factors
remained significant although there was a noticeable
Hierarchical multiple regression predicting well- decrease on the effect of these factors on Social Rela-
being indicators tions. Among the culturally specific factors, both sense
A set of dummy variables—gender (1 = male, 0 = female), of community and dependent self-construal took the
occupation status (1 = no occupation, 2 = unpaid occupa- largest variant in the Social Relation domain.
tion, and 3 = paid occupation), and marital status (1 = mar-
ried, 0 = not married)—were constructed to address the
Self-acceptance
issue of whether different responses for gender, occupation,
The total model (including all universal and cultural vari-
and marital status would have an impact on well-being. A
ables) accounted for 35% of the Self-acceptance domain.
second set of multiple regression analyses was conducted
The universal factors entered in Step 1 accounted for 28%,
on the four separate dimensions of the IWS (Basic Needs,
F(13, 923) = 28.47, of the variation in Self-acceptance. In
Social Needs, Self-acceptance and Spirituality; see
this step, marital status, health, and extraversion were posi-
Table 4). Similar to above, the universal factors (gender,
tively associated with Self-acceptance, while neuroticism
education level, age group, marital status, health, income,
was negatively correlated. When culturally specific factors
occupation status, extraversion, and neuroticism) were
were entered at Step 2, all of these variables remained sig-
entered at Step 1 and the culturally specific factors (trust,
nificant on Self-acceptance except for extraversion. The
sense of community, independent self-construal, and depen-
addition of trust, sense of community, and self-construal at
dent self-construal) at Step 2.
Step 2 increases the variance in Self-acceptance by 7%,

© 2020 Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences and John Wiley & Sons Australia, Ltd
PsyCh Journal

Table 2
Descriptive Statistics and Pearson Correlations for All Variables
Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Independent variables
1 Gender 0.32 0.46 —
2 Education level 4.61 1.21 .04 —
3 Age group 1.94 1.06 .10** .43*** —
4 Marital status 0.40 0.48 .11*** .41*** .65*** —
5 Health 3.62 0.87 .01 .08** .07* .08** —
6 Income 2.19 1.53 .22*** .59*** .47*** .36*** .08** —
7 Occupation status 2.75 0.49 .07** .42*** .28*** .22*** .07* .57*** —
8 Extraversion 3.46 0.81 .05* .03 .13*** .13*** .20*** .06* .04 —
9 Neuroticism 2.77 0.78 −.07* −.11*** −.17*** −.16*** −.22*** −.15*** −.11*** −.33*** —
10 Trust 37.28 11.20 .05 .02 .12*** .08** .22*** .08** .03 .20*** −.15*** —
11 Sense of community 28.03 4.79 .09*** .07* .18*** .18*** .21*** .14*** .10*** .35*** −.25*** .37*** —
12 Independent 92.00 14.58 −.02 −.00 −.07* −.08** .08** .02 .02 .24*** −.07** .26*** .20*** —
13 Dependent 67.64 8.49 −.00 .09** .08** .12*** .14*** .10*** .11*** .31*** −.20*** .25*** .30*** .61*** —
Dependent variables
14 Well-being 80.25 9.09 .01 .19*** .26*** .30*** .34*** .26*** .14*** .37*** −.36*** .30*** .50*** .19*** .46*** —
15 Basic needs 14.39 2.81 .05 .30*** .23*** .24*** .17*** .40*** .21*** .18*** −.25*** .21*** .28** .17*** .24*** .59*** —
16 Social relations 24.07 3.51 −.01 .09** .15*** .20*** .28** .12** .08** .35*** −.22*** .23*** .46*** .19*** .39*** .76*** .28*** —
17 Self-acceptance 18.74 3.32 .06* .13*** .27*** .28*** .33*** .20*** .13* .27*** −.38*** .23*** .36*** .05* .31*** .77*** .35*** .41*** —
18 Spirituality 23.04 .03 .09** .14*** .16 .03 .00 .25*** .18*** .30*** .12*** .35***

© 2020 Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences and John Wiley & Sons Australia, Ltd
3.04 −.08** −.17*** .69*** .15*** .41*** .42*** —
N = 929.
* p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001.
147
148 An Indonesian model of well-being

Table 3
Results from Final Model of Hierarchical Multiple Regression of Universal and Cultural Factors Overall to Well-Being
B SEB β 95% CI sr ΔR2
Step 1 Universal factors .32
Gender −1.50 0.54 −.07** −2.57 −0.43 −.07
Education level −0.14 0.26 −.02 −0.66 0.37 −.01
Age group −0.11 0.32 −.01 −0.52 0.75 .01
Marital status 3.29 0.68 .17*** 1.95 4.63 .13
Physical health 2.31 0.29 .22*** 1.74 2.88 .21
Income 0.99 0.23 .16*** 0.52 1.46 .13
Occupation status −0.49 0.61 −.02 −1.70 0.71 −.02
Extraversion 2.67 0.32 .24*** 2.03 3.31 .22
Neuroticism −2.16 0.34 −.18*** −2.83 −1.49 −.17
Step 2 Universal factors .16
Gender −1.58 0.50 −.08** −2.52 −0.64 −.07
Education level −0.05 0.22 −.00 −0.49 0.39 −.00
Age group −0.05 0.34 −.00 −0.73 0.62 .00
Marital status 2.28 0.66 .12*** 0.97 3.59 .09
Physical health 1.68 0.28 .16*** 1.13 2.23 .15
Income 0.91 0.21 .15*** 0.48 1.33 .10
Occupation status −1.06 0.52 −.05 −2.09 −0.02 −.04
Extraversion 1.11 0.34 .10*** 0.44 1.78 .08
Neuroticism −1.46 0.30 −.12*** −2.06 −0.85 −.11
Culturally specific factors
Trust 0.04 0.02 .06* 0.00 0.09 .05
Sense of community 0.52 0.05 .27*** 0.40 0.63 .23
Independent −0.08 0.02 −.13*** −0.12 −0.04 −.09
Dependent 0.36 0.04 .34*** 0.28 0.44 .25
Note. Parameter estimates with robust standard errors using HC4 method. N = 929.
* p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001.

F (13, 915) = 39.06, p < .001. Overall, all cultural factors Discussion
in Step 2 were positively associated with Self-acceptance,
except for trust. The present study first aimed to investigate the relationship
between universal and culturally specific factors with well-
being and its dimensions using the IWS (Maulana et al.,
Spirituality 2019). The addition of the cultural factors added signifi-
The total model (including universal and culturally specific cantly to the prediction of variance in well-being over the
variables) accounted for 20% of the variance in the Spiritu- demographic and universal factors. These findings align with
ality dimension. The universal factors entered in Step the universal and relativism model of well-being (Lomas,
1 accounted for 10%, F(9, 919) = 12.09, of the variation 2015) and provide evidence for the distinct influence of fac-
in Spirituality. In this step, extraversion provides the larg- tors across different dimensions of well-being in the Indone-
est variant to Spirituality, while gender and marital status sian context (Sohn, 2013; Sujarwoto & Tampubolon, 2014).
were negatively correlated. When cultural factors were Findings suggest that level of income, marital status,
entered at Step 2, there was a significant decrease in each health, and extraversion are important variables to consider
extraversion and marital status on Spirituality, while gender when evaluating general well-being. As expected, to some
differences remained significant on Spirituality (see extent, cultural factors played an important role in such
Table 4). The addition of trust, sense of community, and relationships. These findings so far are in line with previous
self-construal added another 10%, F(13, 915) = 19.19, studies conducted in Indonesia (e.g. Rahayu, 2016) and
p < .001, of the variance in Spirituality. Overall, all cul- other Asian countries (e.g. Singapore; Ng, 2015).
tural factors in Step 2 were positively associated with Spir- Indonesia’s collectivistic culture is noted for its strong
ituality, except for trust. social support systems and group cohesion (Jetten et al.,

© 2020 Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences and John Wiley & Sons Australia, Ltd
PsyCh Journal

Table 4
Hierarchical Multiple Regression Predicting Well-Being Indicators
Basic needs Social relations Self-acceptance Spirituality
Dependent variables B [95% CI] β ΔR2 B [95% CI] β ΔR2 B [95% CI] β ΔR2 B [95% CI] β ΔR2
Step 1
Universal factors .23 .20 .28 .10
Gender −0.28 [−0.64, 0.07] −.04 −0.46 [−0.91, −0.01] −.06** 0.00 [−0.40, 0.41] .00 −0.75 [−1.17, −0.34] −.11***
Education level 0.17 [0.00, 0.35] .07* −0.06 [−0.28, 0.15] −.02 −0.16 [−0.35, 0.03] −.05 −0.10 [−0.30, 0.09] −.04
Age group −0.10 [−0.31, 0.10] −.04 −0.01 [−0.28, 0.25] −.00 0.30 [0.06, 0.54] .09** −0.06 [−0.31, 0.18] −.02
Marital status 0.45 [0.01, 0.90] .08 0.98 [0.42, 1.53] .13** 0.96 [0.45, 1.47] .14*** 0.88 [0.36, 1.39] .14***
Health 0.25 [0.07, 0.44] .08*** 0.82 [0.57, 1.06] .20*** 0.90 [0.68, 1.11] .23*** 0.33 [0.11, 0.55] .09**
Income 0.64 [0.49, 0.80] .35*** 0.14 [−0.05, 0.34] .06 0.13 [−0.04, 0.31] .06 0.06 [−0.11, 0.24] .03
Occupation type −0.31 [−0.71, 0.09] −.05 −0.05 [−0.56, 0.45] −.00 0.09 [−0.36, 0.55] .01 −0.22 [−0.69, 0.24] −.03
Extraversion 0.32 [−0.11, 0.53] .09* 1.18 [0.91, 1.45] .27*** 0.41 [0.17, 0.65] .10*** 0.74 [0.50, .99] .20***
Neuroticism −0.51 [−0.73, −0.29] −.14*** −0.27 [−0.56, 0.00] −.06 −1.08 [−1.34, −0.83] −.25*** −0.28 [−0.54, 0.02] −.07*
Step 2
Universal factors .04 .14 .07 .10
Gender −0.30 [−0.64, 0.04] −.05 −0.50 [−0.92, −0.09] −.06** −0.02 [−0.40, 0.36] −.00 −0.75 [−1.14, −0.36] −.11***
Education level 0.20 [0.03, 0.36] .08* −0.02 [−0.22, 0.17] −.00 −0.13 [−0.32, 0.05] −.05 −0.08 [−0.27, 0.10] .03
Age group −0.12 [−0.33, 0.08] −.04 −0.06 [−0.31, 0.18] −.02 0.24 [0.01, 0.47] .07* −0.10 [−0.33, 0.13] −.03
Marital status 0.41 [−0.02, 0.84] .07 0.64 [0.12, 1.16] .08** 0.65 [0.17, 1.14] .09** 0.57 [0.08, 1.07] .09**
Health 0.14 [−0.04, 0.33] .04 0.60 [0.38, 0.83] .15*** 0.75 [0.54, 0.96] .19*** 0.18 [−0.03, 0.39] .05
Income 0.61 [0.46, 0.76] .33*** 0.11 [−0.06, 0.29] .05 0.12 [−0.04, 0.29] .05 0.05 [−0.11, 0.22] .02
Occupation type −0.36 [−0.75, 0.02] −.06 −0.26 [−0.72, 0.20] −.03 −0.05 [−0.48, 0.38] −.00 −0.38 [−0.82, 0.05] −.06
Extraversion 0.04 [−0.17, 0.26] .01 0.60 [0.33, 0.86] .13*** 0.10 [−0.14, 0.34] .02 0.36 [0.11, 0.61] .09***
Neuroticism −0.43 [−0.64, −0.21] −.12*** −0.03 [−0.29, 0.23] −.00 −0.90 [−1.15, −0.66] −.21*** −0.09 [−0.34, 0.15] −.02

© 2020 Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences and John Wiley & Sons Australia, Ltd
Cultural factors
Trust 0.01 [0.00, 0.03] .07* 0.00 [−0.01, 0.02] .01 0.01 [−0.00, 0.03] .05 0.01 [−0.00, 0.02] .04
Sense of community 0.07 [0.03, 0.11] .13*** 0.22 [0.17, 0.26] .30*** 0.11 [0.07, 0.15] .16*** 0.10 [0.06, 0.15] .17***
Independent 0.01 [−0.00, 0.02] .05 −0.02 [−0.03, −0.00] −.08** −0.04 [−0.05, −0.02] −.17*** −0.03 [−0.04, −0.01] −.15***
Dependent 0.02 [0.00, 0.05] .07* 0.11 [0.08, 0.14] .27*** 0.10 [0.07, 0.13] .26*** 0.12 [0.09, 0.14] .33***
N = 929.
* p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001.
149
150 An Indonesian model of well-being

2002; Sujarwoto & Tampubolon, 2014). Indonesians tends context, but also in the broader community sense (Jetten
to put the interests of family members and others at the et al., 2002; Yuniarti, 2006). This is consistent with the
core when assessing quality of life (Jaafar et al., 2012). A cultural-relativism perspective asserting sociocultural fac-
slight increase of effect was found on universal factors in tors that underpin the mechanism of how well-being is
relation to well-being when cultural factors were accounted evaluated and shapes what is considered important by an
for. This might suggest that for Indonesians, both social individual in their life (Lomas, 2015).
and cultural norms explain more of the variance in well- Furthermore, findings of this study indicate that self-
being compared to universal factors alone. acceptance was strongly associated with having good health
Further, it is also interesting to note that this study and less neuroticism. However, adding cultural factors into
showed slightly different patterns between different factors the model assisted in understanding the role of self-
and well-being dimensions. The findings elucidated that the acceptance as part of the well-being dimension. This find-
role of income was stronger for Basic Needs compared to ing is consistent with previous studies by Maulana
other well-being dimensions. Parallel to existing literature et al. (2018) and Yuniarti (2006) showing that self-
(Ferrer-i-Carbonell, 2005; Hagerty & Veenhoven, 2003), acceptance in Indonesians is likely to be associated with
higher income does increase the perception of well-being self-awareness and how they perceive life, which is
because people have a better chance of meeting their basic strongly associated with their sociocultural context. It is
needs. With the high number of people living just above interesting to note that, contrary to Ryff’s (1989) concep-
the poverty line (40%) in Indonesia, and a wide social gap tions, which attribute self-acceptance to be more
(Statistics Indonesia, 2016), it is unsurprising that income individual-oriented, this study’s findings suggest that the
becomes one of the most important resources in achieving strengthening of an individual’s acceptance is developed
a satisfactory life in such society (Frey & Stutzer, 2000). from a will to abide by social norms through engaging
As expected, once cultural factors were considered, the more with the community.
effect of income to basic needs remained stable. This study’s findings indicate that the spirituality dimen-
Compared to other well-being dimensions, Social Rela- sion of well-being was not contingent upon any universal
tions appeared to be the most influenced dimension when factors, except for gender, marital status, and extraversion.
cultural factors were taken into account. The findings indi- Cultural factors of sense of community and dependent self-
cate that the effect of universal factors on social relations construal provide positive associations with spirituality,
decreased when the cultural factors, which focused on which means that becoming increasingly involved with the
sense of community and dependent self-construal, were community and being considerate of others led to a greater
added in the model. Sense of community and self-construal sense of spirituality in the Indonesian setting. As a collec-
appear to play important roles in people’s life experience tivist society with a strong religious tradition, spirituality is
(Cross, Gore, & Morris, 2003; Pilarska, 2014), including greatly linked with an individual’s social function (Jaafar
how they evaluate the quality of their social relations. This et al., 2012). Contrary to most Western societies, in which
suggests that Indonesians are likely to provide stronger spirituality is often placed in a personal domain, Indone-
evaluations of their well-being if, in addition to the univer- sians practice religious values to improve social connected-
sal factors, they are attached to their community, are mar- ness and to fulfill the expectations of others (Yeo, 2014).
ried, have good health, and are identified as extraverts. This This finding is parallel with that of Kwon (2008), who indi-
finding indicated the strong association between the social cates that spirituality is expressed in different ways in the
dimension of well-being and cultural factors. Although it United States compared to Korea due to different cultural
has been marked as fundamental to well-being across cul- perspectives. All of these cultural traits are likely to have
tures (e.g. Li et al., 2015; Wakefield et al., 2017), having an influence on the way people perceive well-being as
positive social relations in the Indonesian context was including factors associated with it. Overall, cultural factors
found to be very culturally specific. As a strong collectivis- have been found to be important in clarifying the associa-
tic society, the social connection was very much expressed tion between universal variables and well-being.
as a two-way process, both as a feeling of being supported Figure 1 summarizes the key findings of this study.
by others (e.g. community) and a strong feeling of being Given the intersection of the universal and culturally spe-
responsible for others’ needs as well, not only in the family cific factors, we can distinguish the value of the

© 2020 Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences and John Wiley & Sons Australia, Ltd
PsyCh Journal 151

Figure 1. Diagram showing the interaction between well-being with universal and cultural factors.

multidimensional approach of well-being. This model multidimensional nature of well-being. This current study
would have important ramifications in the future. In partic- is unique in examining the contribution of both universal
ular, this model offers an alternative multidimensional and cultural factors to distinct dimensions of well-being rel-
framework of well-being in a non-Western social context evant to the Indonesian population. In practice, these find-
that is characterized by a combination of common universal ings support the use of multidimensional measures of well-
factors and sociocultural context variables that are not being as they are applied in the IWS (Maulana et al.,
available in the existing well-being model. 2019). Further, it would facilitate the development of more
effective programs to enhance well-being in Indonesia
Theoretical and practical implications based on a combination of universal and cultural aspects.
The concept of well-being is complex and requires further
refinement, especially in the cross-cultural context Limitations and future research
(Knoop & Delle Fave, 2013). The current findings support It must be noted that the current study used a cross-
the application of Lomas’s (2015) universal relativism sectional design. Therefore, no causal relationship can be
model in extending understanding about the underlying inferred. A large longitudinal study would be an important
associations between universal factors, cultural factors, and next step to build research evidence for the factors that pre-
the distinct dimensions of well-being in the Indonesian dict well-being in an Indonesian context. Although a large
context. In addition, this current study affirms the convenience sample was used in this study, it must be noted

© 2020 Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences and John Wiley & Sons Australia, Ltd
152 An Indonesian model of well-being

that most of the participants (83%) were educated and from Cross, S. E., Hardin, E. E., & Gercek-Swing, B. (2011). The
the central part of Indonesia (Java island). A more stratified what, how, why, and where of self-construal. Personality and
Social Psychology Review, 15(2), 142–179. https://doi.org/10.
sample technique can be applied in the future to achieve a 1037/0022-3514.85.5.933
better representation of Indonesia’s diverse population. De Neve, J. E., Christakis, N. A., Fowler, J. H., & Frey, B. S.
(2012). Genes, economics, and happiness. Journal of Neurosci-
ence, Psychology, and Economics, 5(4), 193–211. https://doi.
Conclusion org/10.1037/a0030292
This study sheds light on universal and cultural factors Diener, E. Berlin, Germany: Springer; (2009). Social Indicators
associated with well-being in the Indonesian context. These Research Series. Vol. 37. The science of well-being: The col-
lected works of Ed Diener. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-
links were examined in depth using the IWS, which aims 2350-6
to ensure that a culturally sensitive approach to well-being Diener, E., & Tay, L. (2015). Subjective well-being and human
is present. Two distinct groups of variables were identified welfare around the world as reflected in the Gallup World Poll.
International Journal of Psychology, 50(2), 135–149. https://
reflecting the idea of universal and cultural determinants of
doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12136.
well-being. A number of universal factors, such as gender, Diener, E., Pressman, S. D., Hunter, J., & Delgadillo-Chase, D.
marital status, health, and income, were linked to well- (2017). If, why, and when subjective well-being influences
being in various ways. The findings showed that the addi- health, and future needed research. Applied Psychology: Health
and Well-Being, 9(2), 133–167. https://doi.org/10.1111/aphw.
tion of cultural factors, including sense of community, trust,
12090
and dependent self-construal, impacted the way universal Diener, E., Seligman, M. E., Choi, H., & Oishi, S. (2018). Happi-
factors contribute to well-being. Some associations of these est people revisited. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13
universal factors remained important, while others became (2), 176–184. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617697077
Eggleston, E. W., Hardee, E. L., Irwanto, K.,
less significant. Overall, this study recognizes the idea that Poerwandari, E. K., & Severy, L. J. (2001). Measuring
cultural factors determine how people judge their life and women’s psychological well-being in Indonesia. Women &
further how they may attenuate other variables that are Health, 32(4), 17–32. https://doi.org/10.1300/J013v32n04_02
Elovson, A. & Fleming, J. (1989). The Adult Sources of Self-
linked to well-being. It is important to note that this study
Esteem Inventory. Unpublished manuscript, California State
also supports the importance of measuring well-being at University, Los Angeles, CA.
the dimensional level to allow for an exploration of the Ferrer-i-Carbonell, A. (2005). Income and well-being: An empiri-
nuanced relationship between distinct indicators and facets cal analysis of the comparison income effect. Journal of Public
Economics, 89(5–6), 997–1019. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
of well-being as well as to help clarify factors that might be
jpubeco.2004.06.003
underrepresented in Western studies. Frey, B. S., & Stutzer, A. (2000). Happiness, economy and institu-
tions. Economic Journal, 110(466), 918–938. https://doi.org/
10.1111/1468-0297.00570
Disclosure of conflict of interest Glozlah, F. N. (2015). Exploring Ghanaian adolescents’ meaning
of health and well-being: A psychosocial perspective. Interna-
tional Journal of Qualitative Studies on Health and Well-Being,
The authors declare that there are no conflicts of interest
10(1), 263–270. https://doi.org/10.3402/qhw.v10.26370
related to this manuscript. Gori-Maia, A. (2013). Relative income, inequality and subjective
well-being: Evidence for Brazil. Social Indicators Research,
113(3), 1193–1204. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-012-0135-4
References Hagerty, M. R., & Veenhoven, R. (2003). Wealth and happiness
revisited: Growing national income does go with greater happi-
Astivia, O. L. O., & Zumbo, B. D. (2019). Heteroskedasticity in ness. Social Indicators Research, 64(1), 1–27. https://doi.org/
multiple regression analysis: What it is, how to detect it and 10.1023/A:1024790530822
how to solve it with applications in R and SPSS. Practical Jaafar, J. L., Idris, M. A., Ismuni, J., Fei, Y., Jaafar, S.,
Assessment, Research & Evaluation, 24(1), 2. https://doi.org/ Ahmad, Z., … Sugandi, Y. S. (2012). The sources of happiness
10.7275/q5xr-fr95 to the Malaysians and Indonesians: Data from a smaller nation.
Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., Segall, M. H., & Dasen, P. R. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 65, 549–556. https://
(2002). Cross-cultural psychology: Research and applications doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.11.164
(2nd ed.). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Jensen, R. A., Kirkegaard Thomsen, D., O’Connor, M., &
Cross, S. E., Gore, J. S., & Morris, M. L. (2003). The relational- Mehlsen, M. Y. (2020). Age differences in life stories and neu-
interdependent self-construal, self-concept consistency, and roticism mediate age differences in subjective well-being.
well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85 Applied Cognitive Psychology, 34(1), 3–15. https://doi.org/10.
(5), 933–944. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.5.933 1002/acp.3580

© 2020 Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences and John Wiley & Sons Australia, Ltd
PsyCh Journal 153

Jetten, J., Postmes, T., & McAuliffe, B. J. (2002). ‘We’re all indi- Maulana, H., Khawaja, N., & Obst, P. (2019). Development and
viduals’: Group norms of individualism and collectivism, levels validation of the Indonesian Well-Being Scale. Asian Journal
of identification and identity threat. European Journal of Social of Social Psychology, 22(3), 268–280. https://doi.org/10.1111/
Psychology, 32(2), 189–207. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.65 ajsp.12366
Jovanovic, V. (2016). Trust and subjective well-being: The case of Maulana, H., Obst, P., & Khawaja, N. (2018). Indonesian perspec-
Serbia. Personality and Individual Differences, 98, 284–288. tive of well-being: A qualitative study. Qualitative Report, 23
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.061 (12), 3136–3152. Retrieved from https://nsuworks.nova.edu/tqr/
Khodarahimi, S. (2015). The role of marital status in emotional vol23/iss12/18/
intelligence, happiness, optimism and hope. Journal of Com- Marcinko, I. (2015). The moderating role of autonomous motiva-
parative Family Studies, 46, 351–371. https://doi.org/10.3138/ tion on the relationship between subjective well-being and
jcfs.46.3.351 physical health. PLoS One, 10(5), e0126399. https://doi.org/10.
Khumalo, I., Temane, Q., & Wissing, M. (2012). Socio- 1371/journal.pone.0126399
demographic variables, general psychological well-being and McMillan, D. W., & Chavis, D. M. (1986). Sense of community:
the mental health continuum in an African context. Social Indi- A definition and theory. Journal of Community Psychology, 14
cators Research, 105(3), 419–442. https://doi.org/10.1007/ (1), 6–23. https://doi.org/10.1002/1520-6629
s11205-010-9777-2 Michalos, A. C. Berlin: Springer; (2017). Connecting the Quality
Kim, H., Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., & Tsutsui, Y. (2017). Extra- of Life Theory to Health, Well-being and Education: The
version and life satisfaction: A cross-cultural examination of Selected Works of Alex C. Michalos. https://doi.org/10.1007/
student and nationally representative samples. Journal of Per- 978-3-319-51161-0
sonality, 86(4), 604–618. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12339 Ng, W. (2015). Processes underlying links to subjective well-
Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., & Kurokawa, M. (2000). Culture, being: Material concerns, autonomy, and personality. Journal
emotion, and well-being: Good feelings in Japan and the of Happiness Studies, 16, 1575–1591. https://doi.org/10.1007/
United States. Cognition & Emotion, 14(1), 93–124. https:// s10902-014-9580-x
doi.org/10.1080/026999300379003 Obst, P. L., & White, K. M. (2004). Revisiting the Sense of Com-
Knoop, H. H. & Delle Fave, A. Dordrecht: Springer; (Eds.). munity Index: A confirmatory factor analysis. Journal of Com-
(2013). Cross-Cultural Advancements in Positive Psychology. munity Psychology, 32(6), 691–705. https://doi.org/10.1002/
Vol. 3. Well-being and cultures: Perspectives from positive psy- jcop.20027
chology. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-4611-4 Oshio, T., Nozaki, K., & Kobayashi, M. (2011). Relative income
Kwon, S. Y. (2008). Well-being and spirituality from a Korean and happiness in Asia: Evidence from nationwide surveys in
perspective: Based on the study of culture and subjective well- China, Japan, and Korea. Social Indicators Research, 104(3),
being. Pastoral Psychology, 56, 573–584. https://doi.org/10. 351–367. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-010-9754-9
1007/s11089-008-0134-1 Pekerti, A. A., & Arli, D. (2017). Do cultural and generational
Landiyanto, E. A., Ling, J., Puspitasari, M., & Irianti, S. E. cohorts matter to ideologies and consumer ethics? A compara-
(2011). Wealth and happiness: Empirical evidence from Indo- tive study of Australians, Indonesians, and Indonesian migrants
nesia. Chulalongkorn Journal of Economics, 23, 1–17. in Australia. Journal of Business Ethics, 143(2), 387–404.
Retrieved from https://mpra.ub.uni-muenchen.de/50012/ https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-015-2777-z
Lesmana, T., & Santoso, R. (2019). Karakteristik Kepribadian, Harga Pekerti, A. A., & Kwantes, C. T. (2011). The effect of self-
Diri dan Gaya Hidup Hedonisme Pada Mahasiswa Konsumen construals on perceptions of organizational events. Interna-
Starbucks [Personality characteristics, self-esteem and lifestyle tional Journal of Cross-Cultural Management, 11(3), 303–323.
hedonism in Starbucks-consuming students]. Jurnal Ilmu Perilaku, https://doi.org/10.1177/1470595811413101
3(1), 59–71. https://doi.org/10.25077/jip.3.1.59-71.2019 Peterson, N. A., Speer, P. W., & McMillan, D. W. (2008). Valida-
Li, Y., Lan, J., & Ju, C. (2015). Self-esteem, gender, and the rela- tion of a Brief Sense of Community Scale: Confirmation of the
tionship between extraversion and subjective well-being. Social principal theory of sense of community. Journal of Community
Behavior and Personality, 43(8), 1243–1254. https://doi.org/10. Psychology, 36(1), 61–73. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcop.20217
2224/sbp.2015.43.8.1243 Pilarska, A. (2014). Self-construal as a mediator between identity
Lomas, T. (2015). Positive cross-cultural psychology: Exploring structure and subjective well-being. Current Psychology, 33(2),
similarity and difference in constructions and experiences of 130–154. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-013-9202-5
well-being. International Journal of Well-Being, 5(4), 60–77. Primasari, A., & Yuniarti, K. W. (2012). What make teenagers
https://doi.org/10.5502/ijw.v5i4.437 happy? An exploratory study using indigenous psychology
Lu, L. (2005). In pursuit of happiness: The cultural psychological approach. International Journal of Research Studies in Psy-
study of SWB. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 47, 99–112. chology, 1(2):53–61. https://doi.org/10.5861/ijrsp.2012.v1i2.80
Lu, L., & Gilmour, R. (2004). Culture and conceptions of happi- Rahayu, T. P. (2016). The determinants of happiness in Indonesia.
ness: Individual oriented and social oriented SWB. Journal of Mediterranean Journal of Social Sciences, 7(2), 393. https://
Happiness Studies, 5(3), 269–291. https://doi.org/10.1007/ doi.org/10.5901/mjss.2016.v7n2p393
s10902-004-8789-5 Rammstedt, B., & John, O. P. (2007). Measuring personality in
Lucas, R. E., Diener, E., Grob, A., Suh, E. M., & Shao, L. one minute or less: A 10-item short version of the Big Five
(2000). Cross-cultural evidence for the fundamental features of Inventory in English and German. Journal of Research in Per-
extraversion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology., 79 sonality, 41(1), 203–212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2006.
(3), 452–468. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.3.452 02.00

© 2020 Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences and John Wiley & Sons Australia, Ltd
154 An Indonesian model of well-being

Rammstedt, B., Kemper, C. J., & Borg, I. (2013). Correcting Big Stewart-Brown, S. L., Platt, S., Tennant, A., Maheswaran, H.,
Five personality measurements for acquiescence: An Parkinson, J., Weich, S., … Clarke, A. (2011). The warwick-
18-country cross-cultural study. European Journal of Personal- edinburghh mental well-being scale (WEMWBS): a valid and
ity, 27(1), 71–81. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1894 reliable tool for measuring mental well-being in diverse
Røysamb, E., Nes, R. B., & Czajkowski, N. O. (2018). Genetics, populations and projects. Journal of Epidemiology and Com-
personality and wellbeing. A twin study of traits, facets and life munity Health, 65(Suppl 2), A38–A39, 198. https://doi.org/10.
satisfaction. Scientific Reports, 8, 12298. https://doi.org/10. 1136/jech.2011.143586.86
1038/s41598-018-29881-x Sujarwoto, S., & Tampubolon, G. (2014). Decentralisation and
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human citizen happiness: A multilevel analysis of self-rated happiness
potentials: A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonia in Indonesia. Journal of Happiness Studies, 16(2), 455–475.
well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1), 141–166. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-014-9518-3
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141 Tan, S. J., Tambyah, S. K., & Kau, A. K. (2006). The influence
Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations of value orientations and demographics on quality-of-life per-
on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Person- ceptions: Evidence from a national survey of Singaporeans.
ality and Social Psychology, 57(6), 1069–1081. https://doi.org/ Social Indicators Research, 78(1), 33–59. https://doi.org/10.
10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069 1007/s11205-005-7158-z
Safarina, N. A., Mawarpury, M., & Sari, K. (2014). Kesejahteraan van den Berg, P. T., & Pitariu, H. (2005). The relationships
Subjektif Pada Penderita Diabetes Mellitus Tipe II Berdasarkan between personality and well-being during societal change.
Tingkat Pendidikan [Subjective well-being of type II diabetes Personality and Individual Differences, 39(1), 229–234. https://
mellitus patients based on education level]. Jurnal Psikologi doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.01.007
Integratif, 2(1). (in Indonesian). 62–70. Veenhoven, R. The Netherlands: Springer Science & Business
Selvam, S. G., & Collicutt, J. (2013). The ubiquity of the charac- Media; (1984). Conditions of happiness. https://doi.org/10.
ter strengths in African traditional religion: A thematic analysis. 1007/978-94-009-6432-7
In Well-being and cultures (pp. 83–102). Springer, Dordrecht. Wakefield, J. R. H., Sani, F., Madhok, V., Norbury, M.,
Schimmack, U., Radhakrishnan, P., Oishi, S., Dzokoto, V., & Dugard, P., Gabbanelli, C., … Poggesi, F. (2017). The relation-
Ahadi, S. (2002). Culture, personality, and subjective well- ship between group identification and satisfaction with life in a
being: Integrating process models of life satisfaction. Journal cross-cultural community sample. Journal of Happiness Stud-
of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(4), 582–593. https:// ies, 18(3), 785–807. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-016-9735-z
doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.4.582 Wombacher, J., Tagg, S. K., Bürgi, T., & MacBryde, J. (2010).
Schwarz, B., Albert, I., Trommsdorff, G., Zheng, G., Shi, S., & Measuring sense of community in the military: Cross-cultural
Nelwan, P. R. (2010). Intergenerational support and life satis- evidence for the validity of the Brief Sense of Community
faction: A comparison of Chinese, Indonesian, and German Scale and its underlying theory. Journal of Community Psychol-
elderly mothers. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41, ogy, 38(6), 671–687. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcop.20388
706–722. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022110372197 Yeo, P. L. (2014). Religiosity, personality, and subjective well-
Sohn, K. (2013). Sources of happiness in Indonesia. Singapore being among Muslim adults in Indonesia (Doctoral dissertation,
Economic Review, 58(2), 1350014. https://doi.org/10.1142/ Regent University, VA). Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations
S0217590813500148 and Theses Global. Retrieved from https://search.proquest.com/
Statistics Indonesia (2016). Statistical yearbook of Indonesia 2016. openview/754f9077e0f9083aca990815aa9c8628/1?pqorigsite=
Central Jakarta, Indonesia: BPS Republik Indonesia (Statistics gscholar&cbl=18750&diss=y
Indonesia). Retrieved from https://www.bps.go.id/publication/ Yuniarti, K. W. (2006). The magical strength of Nrimo and
2016/06/29/7aa1e8f93b4148234a9b4bc3/ statistikindonesia-2016 Gotong-royong: A quick response report following the May
Steptoe, A., Deaton, A., & Stone, A. A. (2015). Subjective well- 27, 2006 earthquake in Yogyakarta. Anima Journal of Psychol-
being, health, and ageing. Lancet, 385(9968), 640–648. https:// ogy, 24(3), 201–206. Retrieved from http://anima.ubaya.ac.id/
doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61489-0 class/openpdf.php?file=1371801925.pdf

© 2020 Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences and John Wiley & Sons Australia, Ltd

You might also like