You are on page 1of 14

Asia Pacific Education Review

https://doi.org/10.1007/s12564-020-09633-1

Exploring the relationship between perfectionism and academic


burnout through achievement goals: a mediation model
Eunbi Chang1 · Hyunmo Seong2 · Sang Min Lee2

Received: 16 January 2019 / Revised: 6 May 2020 / Accepted: 19 May 2020


© Education Research Institute, Seoul National University, Seoul, Korea 2020

Abstract
This study investigated associations among perfectionism (i.e., perfectionistic strivings and perfectionistic concerns), achieve-
ment goals, and academic burnout among college students in South Korea, focusing on the mediating role of achievement
goals. A total of 281 students enrolled in a non-major psychology course voluntarily completed an online questionnaire.
Analyses were performed using structural equation modeling. The results indicated that achievement goals significantly
mediated the relation between perfectionism and academic burnout. Perfectionistic strivings had a negative effect on aca-
demic burnout, through mastery goals (i.e., mastery-approach and mastery-avoidance goals). Specifically, high levels of
perfectionistic strivings were linked to high levels of mastery-approach and low levels of mastery-avoidance goals, which
were sequentially linked to a low level of academic burnout. Contrastingly, perfectionistic concerns had a positive effect on
academic burnout, through mastery-avoidance goals. That is, higher perfectionistic concerns were associated with higher
mastery-avoidance goals, which in turn were linked to higher academic burnout. Implications and limitations are discussed.

Keywords Academic burnout · Perfectionism · Achievement goals · Korean undergraduates

Introduction reduce various negative outcomes for college students by


understanding academic burnout and its antecedents.
In South Korea, college students experience a high level of Perfectionism is one antecedent factor of academic burn-
academic stress, due to the effects an individual’s college out (e.g., Chang and Lee 2020). Several researchers (e.g.,
GPA can have on his or her future career and social status Chang et al. 2016; Newman et al. 2019) have focused on
(Chang and Lee 2020; Kim 2019). Prolonged and excessive perfectionism in college students, as college life empha-
academic stress results in various kinds of behavioral and sizes autonomy and responsibility and can strengthen one’s
psychological problems, including academic burnout (e.g., perfectionistic beliefs. Previous research (e.g., Chang et al.
Jung et al. 2015). 2016; Luo et al. 2016) has reported significant associations
Studies on academic burnout in college students have between perfectionism and academic burnout; however, the
been conducted (e.g., Chang and Lee 2020; Kim 2019; direction of the relationship differed, depending on perfec-
Vizoso et al. 2019), as it is something college students com- tionism dimensions (i.e., perfectionistic strivings and perfec-
monly experience (Chang and Lee 2020; May et al. 2015) tionistic concerns). Given that perfectionistic strivings and
and can lead to various detrimental outcomes. Specifically, perfectionistic concerns show a high correlation (Hill and
it has been shown to be negatively associated with academic Curran 2016), it is necessary to investigate the mechanisms
adjustment (Kim 2019) and academic performance (May underlying the different associations between perfectionism
et al. 2015; Vizoso et al. 2019). Thus, we can prevent or dimensions and academic burnout. However, only recently
did researchers begin to identify the process through which
* Sang Min Lee perfectionism influences academic burnout (e.g., Chang and
leesang@korea.ac.kr Lee 2020; Chang et al. 2016).
Thus, the present study aimed to identify a psychological
1
Department of Education, College of Education, Kangwon variable that may influence the degree to which perfection-
National University, Chuncheon, South Korea
ism dimensions influence academic burnout. Specifically,
2
Department of Education, College of Education, Korea we investigated the mediating role of achievement goals,
University, Anam‐dong, Seongbuk‐gu, Seoul, South Korea

13
Vol.:(0123456789)
E. Chang et al.

which play an important role in academic settings (e.g., internal locus of control and self-encouragement, which can
Honicke et al. 2019), in the relation between perfectionism lead to low levels of academic burnout. A meta-analysis on
(i.e., perfectionistic strivings and perfectionistic concerns) perfectionism and academic burnout (Cho et al. 2017) also
and academic burnout. confirmed the negative association between perfectionis-
tic strivings and academic burnout, and Hill and Curran’s
(2016) meta-analysis on perfectionism and burnout sup-
Literature review ported a similar pattern for this relationship.
Perfectionistic concerns have been shown to be posi-
Association between perfectionism and academic tively associated with academic burnout (e.g., Chang and
burnout Lee 2020; Chang et al. 2016; Lee and Yang 2019; Luo
et al. 2016). Many studies, including meta-analyses (Cho
Academic burnout is a negative consequence resulting from et al. 2017; Hill and Curran 2016), have supported these
chronic academic stress and extreme academic demands associations. For example, Chang and Lee (2020) reported
(McCarthy et al. 1990), and includes symptoms such as that perfectionistic concerns predicted low levels of goal
emotional exhaustion, cynicism, and inefficacy (Schaufeli reengagement, which was linked to high levels of academic
et al. 2002). Emotional exhaustion refers to feeling extremely burnout. Garratt-Reed et al. (2018) found a positive associa-
fatigued due to excessive academic demands. Cynicism tion between perfectionistic concerns and academic burn-
refers to a skeptical attitude toward academic tasks, which out through high levels of repetitive negative thoughts. Lee
leads to depersonalization from learning. Lastly, inefficacy is and Yang (2019) showed that perfectionistic concerns have
the feeling that one cannot reach the academic achievements a positive association with academic burnout through low
he or she wants (Shin et al. 2011). levels of internal locus of control and self-encouragement.
Previous research has examined various antecedent fac- In the association between perfectionism and academic
tors of academic burnout in college students. For example, burnout, achievement goals may play a mediating role.
passion (Saville et al. 2018), coping strategies (Alarcon et al. Achievement goals are reasons to engage in tasks (Elliot
2011; Vizoso et al. 2019), motivation (Chang et al. 2016), 1999; Elliot and Hulleman 2017; Pintrich 2000) and are
optimism (Vizoso et al. 2019), goal adjustment capacity important in academic settings. They are associated with
(Chang and Lee 2020), and perfectionism (Chang and Lee not only academic performance (e.g., Honicke et al. 2019)
2020; Chang et al. 2016) have been suggested as antecedent but also academic burnout among college students (e.g.,
factors. Naidoo et al. 2012). Students may pursue different types
Of those factors, perfectionism is characterized as a per- of achievement goals depending on their degree or type of
sonality trait involving striving for perfection, setting exces- perfectionism (e.g., Bong et al. 2014; Madjar et al. 2015),
sively high standards for personal performance, and provid- and the achievement goals students pursue may have dif-
ing critical evaluation of one’s own behavior (Stoeber and ferent links to academic burnout (e.g., Naidoo et al. 2012).
Otto 2006). While there have been many attempts to con-
ceptualize perfectionism (e.g., Frost et al. 1990; Hewitt and Association between perfectionism
Flett 1991), a recent conceptualization suggests there are two and achievement goals
major dimensions of perfectionism: perfectionistic strivings
and perfectionistic concerns (Hill and Curran 2016). Perfec- Perfectionism affects individual goal setting and mainte-
tionistic strivings, an adaptive form of perfectionism, repre- nance (Bong et al. 2014). Goal orientation (i.e., the reason
sent striving toward perfection and setting high standards for to approach a goal) may also differ depending on perfec-
one’s own performance (Hill and Curran 2016). Conversely, tionism type (Bong et al. 2014; Madjar et al. 2015). Elliot
perfectionistic concerns, a maladaptive form of perfection- and McGregor (2001) classified goal orientation using a
ism, represent a self-critical tendency toward imperfection 2 × 2 framework by combining valence (i.e., approach vs.
and excessive concerns over mistakes and failure (Hill and avoidance) and definition (i.e., mastery vs. performance)
Curran 2016). dimensions. Specifically, performance goals, which focus on
Previous research (e.g., Chang et al. 2016; Garratt-Reed demonstrating one’s competence over others (Ames 1992;
et al. 2018; Luo et al. 2016) has reported a negative associa- Pintrich 2000), are divided into performance-approach and
tion between perfectionistic strivings and academic burnout. performance-avoidance goals. Mastery goals, which focus
For example, Chang et al. (2016) found that perfectionistic on self-improvement (Ames 1992; Elliot and McGregor
strivings were negatively associated with academic burnout 2001; Pintrich 2000), are classified as mastery-approach
through high levels of intrinsic and identified motivation. and mastery-avoidance goals (Elliot and MacGregor 2001).
In a recent study by Lee and Yang (2019), perfectionistic Approach focus refers to moving toward positive outcomes
strivings were found to be associated with high levels of or success, whereas avoidance focus represents avoiding

13
Exploring the relationship between perfectionism and academic burnout through achievement…

negative outcomes or failure (Elliot and MacGregor 2001). Association between achievement goals
Thus, individuals with performance-approach goals try to and academic burnout
surpass others and prove their competence (Elliot 1999;
Elliot and Harackiewicz 1996); however, individuals with Whether one is motivated to approach or avoid may be
performance-avoidance goals try to avoid looking incom- closely related to the occurrence of burnout. Hobfoll’s
petent compared to others (Elliot and Harackiewicz 1996). (1989, 2001) conservation of resources theory suggests
Individuals with mastery-approach goals focus on develop- that people experience stress leading to burnout when their
ing personal competence by learning tasks and acquiring valuable resources are threatened or lost, or when they do
skills, whereas individuals with mastery-avoidance goals try not regain enough resources following resource investment.
to avoid loss of skills and competence (Elliot 1999; Elliot According to Hobfoll (2001), because people are more sen-
and Harachiewicz 1996). sitive to loss than gain, the impact of loss is stronger than
Throughout the literature, studies have suggested that that of gain if the amount of resources is equal. Thus, the
perfectionism and achievement goal orientations are sig- harmful effects of losing resources related to academic burn-
nificantly associated (e.g., Lee and Anderman 2020; Speirs out may be stronger than the mitigating effects of gaining
Neumeister et al. 2015). A dual process model of perfec- resources related to academic burnout (Naidoo et al. 2012).
tionism (Slade and Owens 1998) indicated that the positive The effects of achievement goals on academic burnout may
aspects of perfectionism (i.e., perfectionistic strivings) are differ, depending on achievement goal subtype.
associated with an approach focus. Theoretically, because To date, the association between achievement goals and
perfectionistic strivings are associated with hope of suc- academic burnout has not been clarified. A limited number
cess (Stoeber and Otto 2006), they may lead one to become of studies empirically investigated this relation, and they
involved in challenging tasks (i.e., approach focus). How- reported slight variations in the patterns of association.
ever, previous empirical studies (e.g., Speirs Neumeister Naidoo et al. (2012) reported that avoidance goals have a
et al. 2015) indicated that perfectionistic strivings are posi- positive association, whereas approach goals have a nega-
tively associated with not only approach goals (i.e., per- tive or insignificant association, with academic burnout. Yu
formance-approach and mastery-approach goals) but also and Chae (2016) showed that academic burnout is positively
avoidance goals (i.e., performance-avoidance and mastery- associated with avoidance goals and negatively associated
avoidance goals). Stoeber, Damian, and Madigan (2018) with approach goals.
analyzed 22 previous studies from 2006 to 2015, including However, relevant studies for other populations (e.g.,
the above-mentioned studies, that examined the association nurses, firefighters, and athletes) showed different patterns
between perfectionism and achievement goals. They also of achievement goals and burnout from the aforementioned
found that perfectionistic strivings had positive correlations studies (Naidoo et al. 2012; Yu and Chae 2016). In a study
with the four types of achievement goals. conducted with nurses (Adriaenssens et al. 2015), mastery-
Negative aspects of perfectionism (i.e., perfectionistic approach goals negatively predicted, while performance-
concerns) are associated with avoidance focus (Slade and avoidance goals positively predicted, burnout; moreover,
Owens 1998), and related to fear of mistakes of failure (Hill mastery-avoidance and performance-approach goals did not
and Curran 2016), which may lead one to avoid engaging significantly predict burnout. In a study conducted with fire-
in challenging tasks (i.e., avoidance focus). However, per- fighters (Vaulerin et al. 2016), only mastery-avoidance goals
fectionistic concerns are associated with not only avoidance showed a positive association with burnout. Additionally,
goals (i.e., performance-avoidance and mastery-avoidance in a study conducted with athletes (Isoard-Gautheur et al.
goals) but also performance-approach goals (Lee and Ander- 2016), mastery-approach goals were negatively associated
man 2020). Individuals with perfectionistic concerns are the- and mastery-avoidance goals were positively associated with
oretically motivated by social criteria or evaluations by oth- burnout, whereas there was no significant association found
ers (Frost et al. 1990; Hewitt and Flett 1991). Placing value between performance goals and burnout.
on social criteria and evaluations by others is associated with Some studies reported that the associations found between
performance goals that focus on demonstrating competence achievement goals and each factor of academic burnout
(Lee and Anderman 2020). Stoeber et al.’s (2018) analysis were slightly different. Kim, Lim, and Noh (2016) identi-
of previous studies supported these associations; perfection- fied negative associations for mastery-approach goals with
istic concerns had positive correlations with performance- both cynicism and inefficacy, a positive association between
approach, performance-avoidance, and mastery-avoidance performance goals and emotional exhaustion, and a negative
goals in most studies (e.g., Madjar et al. 2015). association between performance goals and inefficacy.
In summary, there are significant associations between
achievement goals and academic burnout; however, the spe-
cific directions of associations between achievement goal

13
E. Chang et al.

subtypes and academic burnout remain unclear. Addition- amount of course extra credit, as permitted by the course
ally, some studies (e.g., Kim et al. 2016) did not utilize the instructor. Participants completed the survey on their own
2 × 2 framework of achievement goals (Elliot and McGregor time, at a location available to them, using an online survey
2001); thus, they did not examine the association between tool. To reduce the likelihood of common method bias on
mastery-avoidance goals and academic burnout. Therefore, participants’ responses (Tourangeau et al. 2003), measures
more studies are needed to clarify the associations between were presented in a randomized order. The survey was con-
achievement goals and academic burnout. ducted in May 2017, during a period between midterm and
final examinations, to reduce the effects of exam-related
stressors on students’ responses. A total of 281 students were
Research question and hypotheses recruited as participants. There were no missing data, as it
was impossible to skip an item in the online survey without
In the present study, we aimed to address the following providing a response. Of the 281 students (154 males and
research question: Do achievement goals mediate the rela- 127 females), 21.4% were freshmen, 48.8% were sopho-
tion between perfectionism and academic burnout? Based mores, 20.3% were juniors, and 9.6% were seniors. Partici-
on the results of previous studies (see Stoeber et al. 2018), pants’ mean age was 20.68 years (SD = 2.00; range = 17 − 27
we hypothesized the association between perfectionism and years). Of the participants, 118 (42.0%) majored in STEM,
achievement goals; however, we did not hypothesize the spe- 115 (41.0%) majored in arts, humanities, or social sciences,
cific effect of achievement goals on academic burnout. The andays 48 (17.1%) did not report their majors.
study’s hypotheses are as follows:
Measures
Hypothesis 1.1 Perfectionistic strivings have significant
associations with four types of achievement goals. Academic burnout

Hypothesis 1.2 Four types of achievement goals mediate the The Korean version of the Maslach Burnout Inventory-Stu-
negative association between perfectionistic strivings and dent Survey (MBI-SS; Shin et al. 2011) was used to meas-
academic burnout. ure participants’ levels of academic burnout. The MBI-SS
consists of five items measuring emotional exhaustion (e.g.,
Hypothesis 2.1 Perfectionistic concerns have significant “I feel emotionally drained by my studies”), four items for
associations with avoidance goals (i.e., performance-avoid- cynicism (e.g., “I have become more cynical about the
ance goals and mastery-avoidance goals) and performance- potential usefulness of my studies”), and six items for aca-
approach goals. demic efficacy (e.g., “During class I feel confident that I am
effective in getting things done”).
Hypothesis 2.2 Avoidance goals and performance-approach The academic efficacy subscale is usually reverse-coded
goals mediate the positive association between perfectionis- to measure academic inefficacy. Several studies have sug-
tic concerns and academic burnout. gested this subscale to be more closely related with aca-
demic engagement, leaving emotional exhaustion and cyni-
As previous empirical evidence was insufficient, the cism as the “core” factors of burnout (e.g., Schaufeli et al.
association between perfectionistic concerns and mastery- 2000). Zhang, Gan, and Chan (2007) also found that the
approach goals was not hypothesized. academic efficacy subscale loaded better on a latent aca-
demic engagement variable, instead of burnout, in a Chinese
sample. Following this reasoning, without the academic effi-
Method cacy subscale, the emotional exhaustion and cynicism sub-
scales served as indicators of academic burnout as a latent
Participants and procedure construct in the analyses in the present study. Each item
is rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree to
Data were collected from a group of undergraduate stu- 5 = strongly agree), with a higher score indicating a higher
dents enrolled in a non-major, elective psychology course level of academic burnout. Shin et al. (2011) calculated the
at a comprehensive university, considered one of the most internal consistency of the MBI-SS to be .86 for emotional
prestigious in Seoul, South Korea. This course was selected exhaustion and .82 for cynicism. Cronbach’s alpha coeffi-
for data collection because students enrolled in the course cients of the MBI-SS in the current study were .84 and .88
had diverse majors, including art and design, social sci- for emotional exhaustion and cynicism, respectively.
ences, engineering, and education. Participants voluntarily
completed a series of questionnaires in exchange for a small

13
Exploring the relationship between perfectionism and academic burnout through achievement…

Perfectionistic strivings and perfectionistic concerns Achievement goals

Measures of perfectionistic strivings and perfectionistic con- The 2 × 2 achievement goal orientations scale developed by
cerns were selected from the Multidimensional Perfection- Park and Lee (2005) embraces two conceptual dimensions
ism Scale (MPS; Hewitt and Flett 1991), Frost Multidimen- of achievement goal orientations: definition (mastery vs.
sional Perfectionism Scale (FMPS; Frost et al. 1990), and performance) and valence (approach vs. avoidance). This
Almost Perfect Scale-Revised (APS-R; Slaney et al. 2001). scale has been implemented in a number of studies with
The Korean version of the MPS (Han 1993) contains 15 Korean college students (e.g., Cho and Lee 2011; Seo 2009)
items assessing self-oriented perfectionism (e.g., “I must and measures mastery-approach (9 items; e.g., “The reason
work to my full potential at all times”) and another 15 items why I study is to understand the content as thoroughly as
measuring socially prescribed perfectionism (e.g., “Anything possible”), mastery-avoidance (5 items; e.g., “It is satisfac-
that I do that is less than excellent will be seen as poor work tory for me to understand the content to a certain extent”),
by those around me”). Each item is rated on a 7-point Lik- performance-approach (7 items; e.g., “I want to be recog-
ert scale (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree). The nized as someone who performs better than others”), and
Korean version of the FMPS (Chung 1999), employing a performance-avoidance goals (5 items; e.g., “I don’t voice
5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly my opinion during class because I’m afraid of being embar-
agree), was used to measure concern over mistakes (9 items; rassed when I’m wrong”). Each item is rated on a 6-point
e.g., “People will probably think less of me if I make a mis- Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree to 6 = strongly agree). The
take”), doubts about actions (4 items; “I usually have doubts internal consistency coefficients of the subscales in Park and
about the simple everyday things that I do”), and personal Lee’s (2005) study were .83 for mastery-approach, .69 for
standards (7 items; “I expect higher performance in my daily mastery-avoidance, .88 for performance-approach, and .78
tasks than most people”). These FMPS subscales were used for performance-avoidance goals. In the current study, Cron-
in previous studies assessing perfectionistic strivings and bach’s alpha coefficients were .87 for mastery-approach, .75
perfectionistic concerns (e.g., Blankstein et al. 2008). From for mastery-avoidance, .90 for performance-approach, and
the Korean validated version of the APS-R (Park 2009), .83 for performance-avoidance goals.
discrepancy (10 items; e.g., “My performance rarely meas- To represent latent constructs in the four subdimensions
ures up to my standards”) and high standards (5 items; “I of achievement goals, items for each subscale were parceled
have high standards for my performance at work or school”) into three indicators. Following recommendations from Ban-
were also employed to comprehensively assess perfectionis- dalos (2002), before parceling items, unidimensionality of
tic concerns and perfectionistic strivings, respectively. The the subscales was tested, with Kaiser−Mayer−Olkin values
APS-R uses a 7-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree to (> .70; Field 2013) and unrotated factor loadings for each
7 = strongly agree). The reliability and validity of the MPS item (> .40; Hair et al. 2009) in exploratory factor analysis
(Chang et al. 2016; Han 1993), FMPS (Huh et al. 2015), and as criteria. All dimensions of achievement goals showed
APS-R (Jeong and Lee 2013; Park 2009) have been estab- Kaiser−Mayer−Olkin values greater than .78 and factor
lished in populations of Korean college students. The reli- loadings greater than .52, indicating substantial unidimen-
ability coefficients of the perfectionism measures used in the sionality for each subscale. As the unidimensionality of each
present study were .90 for self-oriented perfectionism, .78 subscale was assured, paired items with the highest and low-
for socially prescribed perfectionism, .80 for personal stand- est factor loadings were assigned successively to each parcel
ards, .87 for concern over mistakes, .65 for doubts about (Bandalos 2008) (Table 1).
actions, .82 for high standards, and .89 for discrepancy.
Self-oriented perfectionism from the MPS, personal Data analysis
standards from the FMPS, and high standards from the
APS-R similarly focus on individuals’ strivings to achieve Prior to testing our hypotheses, means, standard deviations,
perfection (see Hill and Curran 2016; Stoeber and Otto and reliabilities for the measures were examined. Then, the
2006); thus, these three subscales were used to represent per- measurement model and hypothesized structural model were
fectionistic strivings. Socially prescribed perfectionism from tested using Mplus 7.4 (Muthén and Muthén 1998−2017).
the MPS, concern over mistakes and doubts about actions The fit of the measurement model was tested before test-
from the FMPS, and discrepancy from the APS-R involve ing the structural model. For correlation coefficients among
fear of failure to live up to one’s own and others’ high expec- latent variables, factor loadings between each latent vari-
tations (Hill and Curran 2016; Stoeber and Otto 2006). Thus, able and its indicators were also examined. Next, the fit of
these facets of perfectionism served as indicators represent- the hypothesized structural model (partial-mediation model)
ing perfectionistic concerns as a latent construct. was tested and compared to alternative models (full-medi-
ation models), using chi-squared tests. Then, each indirect

13
E. Chang et al.

Table 1  Description of the scales


Construct Measure Example

Academic burnout MBI-SS (Shin et al. 2011; Schaufelit et al. 2000)


Emotional exhaustion 5 items, 1–5 scale “I feel emotionally drained by my studies”
Cynicism 4 items, 1–5 scale “I have become less enthusiastic about my studies”
Perfectionistic strivings MPS (Han 1993; Hewitt and Flett 1991), FMPS (Chung 1999; Frost et al.1990), APS-R (Park 2009;
Slaney et al. 2001)
Self-oriented perfectionism MPS, 15 items, 1–7 scale “I must work to my full potential at all times”
Personal standards FMPS, 7 items, 1–5 scale “I set higher goals for myself”
High standards APS-R, 5 items, 1–7 scale “I have high standards for my performance at work
or school”
Perfectionistic concerns MPS, FMPS, APS-R
Socially prescribed perfectionism MPS, 15 items, 1–7 scale “Anything that I do that is less than excellent will be
seen as poor work by those around me”
Concern over mistakes FMPS, 9 items, 1–5 scale “People will probably think less of me if I make a
mistake”
Doubts about actions FMPS, 4 items, 1–5 scale “It takes me a long time to do something right”
Discrepancy APS-R, 10 items, 1–7 scale “My performance rarely measures up to my stand-
ards”
Achievement goals 2 × 2 Achievement Goal Orientation Scale (Park and Lee 2005)
Mastery-approach 9 items, 1–6 scale “The reason why I study is to understand the content
as thoroughly as possible”
Mastery-avoidance 5 items, 1–6 scale “It is satisfactory for me to understand the content to
a certain extent”
Performance-approach 7 items, 1–6 scale “I want to be recognized as someone who performs
better than others”
Performance-avoidance 5 items, 1–6 scale “I don’t voice my opinion during class because I’m
afraid of being embarrassed when I’m wrong”

effect was tested using the bootstrap approach. Since the than .90 are regarded as a reasonable fit (Hu and Bentler
hypothesized mediation model included more than one 1999).
mediating variable, it was necessary to investigate the indi- Using these model fit indices, we compared three nested
vidual indirect effect of each mediation path, as well as the models. The initial model, Model A, includes all hypoth-
total indirect effects (Cheung 2007). To estimate and com- esized paths between latent variables. The second model,
pare the strengths of each mediation effect, phantom varia- Model B, constrained a direct path from perfectionistic striv-
bles were used. The variances of the phantom variables were ings to academic burnout. The last model, Model C, addi-
zero; thus, they exerted no influence on model fit, implied tionally constrained another direct path from perfectionistic
covariance matrix, or parameter estimates (Cheung 2007). concerns to academic burnout. By comparing these models,
By using phantom variables, bias-corrected bootstrap confi- we aimed to test whether the relationships from perfection-
dence intervals can be produced for specific indirect effects ism to academic burnout through achievement goals were
(see Lau and Cheung 2012). Thus, specific indirect effects fully or partially mediated.
within a complex mediation model with several mediation
paths can be estimated.
Several fit indices were used to evaluate model fit, includ- Results
ing chi-squared (χ2) statistics, Tucker−Lewis index (TLI),
the comparative fit index (CFI), and the root mean square Descriptive statistics
error of approximation (RMSEA). Although χ2 statistics test
whether the actual data fits the model, it is sensitive to sam- Means, standard deviations, and correlations among meas-
ple size (McDonald and Ho 2002). Thus, other fit indices ured variables are presented in Table 2. Perfectionistic
were also considered. The cut-off value for RMSEA is less strivings and perfectionistic concerns were significantly
than .08 (Browne and Cudeck 1993). TLI and CFI values positively correlated. Additionally, the four dimensions
greater than .95 are regarded as a good fit, and values greater of achievement goals were significantly correlated, except
for mastery-avoidance and performance-approach goals.

13
Exploring the relationship between perfectionism and academic burnout through achievement…

Mastery-approach goals and mastery-avoidance goals were strivings to academic burnout and from perfectionistic
correlated with perfectionistic strivings, but not perfection- concerns to academic burnout), indicated reasonable fit
istic concerns. Performance-approach goals showed a sig- and served as baseline. Next, to test whether achievement
nificant correlation with both perfectionistic strivings and goal variables fully or partially mediated the relationships
perfectionistic concerns, except for discrepancy, while per- between perfectionism and academic burnout, alternative
formance-avoidance goals displayed a significant correlation models, in which direct paths from perfectionism to aca-
with only perfectionistic concerns. Notably, the exhaustion demic burnout were constrained to be zero, were tested.
measure of academic burnout showed a significant positive Model fit comparisons were made using the DIFFTEST
correlation with perfectionistic concerns, whereas cynicism option in Mplus, because the difference between two S-B χ2
was primarily negatively correlated with perfectionistic is not distributed as χ2 (Satorra and Bentler 2010).
strivings. As all measurements met the criteria of normality First, the direct path from perfectionistic strivings to aca-
(skewness< 2.0, kurtosis < 7.0; West et al. 1995), further demic burnout was constrained to be zero (Model B). An
analysis was conducted. S-B χ2 difference test showed that this constrained model
(Model B) showed better data fit than Model A (ΔS-B
Measurement model χ2(1) = .250, p > .05).
Next, in Model C, a direct path from perfectionistic con-
Prior to testing our hypotheses, we evaluated multivari- cerns to academic burnout was also constrained to be zero,
ate normality of the variables. Mardia’s (1970) coefficient from Model B. Model C displayed significantly poorer fit
was greater than 5, indicating that measured variables were to the data than Model B (ΔS-B χ2(1) = 21.305, p < .001).
significantly non-normal. Maximum likelihood estimation Therefore, Model B, in which achievement goal dimen-
requires that the assumption of multivariate normality be sions fully mediated the relationship between perfectionistic
met (Hu et al. 1992). Therefore, we used robust-maximum strivings and academic burnout, and partially mediated the
likelihood estimation procedures and model fit was tested relationship between perfectionistic concerns and academic
using the Satorra−Bentler scaled χ2 (S-B χ2; Satorra and burnout, was chosen as the final model.
Bentler 2001), corrected for non-normality, along with TLI, Path coefficients of the final structural model are pre-
CFI, and RMSEA. sented in Fig. 1. In the final model, perfectionistic strivings
Confirmatory factor analysis was performed for a full significantly predicted all four dimensions of achievement
measurement model, including all latent variables. The goals (β = .53, − .91, .26, and − .43 for mastery-approach,
measurement model, which was used to test a structural mastery-avoidance, performance-approach, and perfor-
model, displayed an acceptable fit to the data: S-B χ 2 mance-avoidance goals, respectively, ps < .01). These results
(168, N = 281) = 260.217, TLI = .944, CFI = .955, and indicated that Hypothesis 1–1, which predicted the relation-
RMSEA = .044. Each subscale of perfectionism and aca- ship between perfectionistic strivings and the four goal ori-
demic burnout, as well as each parcel of achievement goal entation subscales, was supported. The direct path from per-
subscales, showed significant factor loadings to the respec- fectionistic strivings to academic burnout was constrained to
tive latent variables, with loadings ranging from .57 to .94. be equal to zero in the final structural model, which implied
Correlations among latent variables are shown in Table 3. that this path became non-significant by introducing indirect
Although perfectionistic strivings and perfectionistic con- paths.
cerns were rather strongly correlated, there was no indica- Perfectionistic concerns significantly predicted mastery-
tion of problems regarding multicollinearity (variance infla- avoidance, performance-approach, and performance-avoid-
tion factor = 2.00). ance goals (β = .78, .44, and .73, respectively, ps < .001),
indicating Hypothesis 2–1was supported. Perfectionistic
concerns also significantly predicted academic burnout
Structural model (β = .47, p < .001). Both mastery-approach and mastery-
avoidance goals displayed significant effects on academic
An initial structural model (Model A), with perfectionistic burnout (β = − .63 and .44, respectively, ps < .001); however,
strivings and perfectionistic concerns as predictor variables, performance-approach and performance-avoidance goals
academic burnout as a dependent variable, and the four showed non-significant effects on academic burnout.
dimensions of achievement goals as simultaneous media-
tors, was tested. In this model, all paths among latent vari- Indirect effects
ables, except for the path from perfectionistic concerns to
mastery-approach goals, were allowed. As shown in Table 4, In the final model, three indirect effects were found: (a)
Model A, which included two direct paths from perfection- a path from perfectionistic strivings to academic burn-
ism to academic burnout (i.e., the path from perfectionistic out through mastery-approach goals; (b) a path from

13
E. Chang et al.

perfectionistic strivings to academic burnout through mas-

(.83)
13
tery-avoidance goals; and (c) a path from perfectionistic con-
cerns to academic burnout through mastery-avoidance goals.

.32**
As paths from both performance-approach and performance-

(.90)
12

avoidance goals to academic burnout were not significant,


these variables were not considered to be significant media-

.30**
− .05
(.75)
tors in the model. To test the significance of indirect effects,
11

a bootstrap method with 1,000 bootstrapping samples and


95% bias-corrected confidence intervals (CI) was performed.

− .56**

-.21**
.22**
(.87)
The total indirect effects of both perfectionistic striv-
10

ings and perfectionistic concerns on academic burnout


were significant (β = − .724, 95% CI [− .989, − .474] for

.19**
(.89)
perfectionistic strivings; β = .341, 95% CI [.148, .561] for

.31
.02
.08
9

perfectionistic concerns). As there were two mediators in


the link between perfectionistic strivings and academic burn-
.25**
.35**
.53**
(.65)

.08
.11

out (i.e., mastery-approach and mastery-avoidance goals),


8

each indirect effect should be examined beyond the total


.36**
.55**
.56**
.59**

indirect effect. To estimate and test specific indirect effects,


(.87)

.10
.09
7

phantom variables were adopted. Results of total, direct, and


indirect effect analysis are presented in Table 5. Hypothesis
.27**
.39**
.60**
.44**
.42**
(.78)

1–2 was partially supported: negative indirect effects linking


.09
.07
6

perfectionistic strivings and academic burnout were found


for both mastery-approach (β = − .333) and mastery-avoid-
− .34**
.43**
.39**
.43**
.26**
.46**
.32**
(.82)

ance goals (β = − .398). Hypothesis 2–2 was also partially


.05
Table 2  Means, standard deviations, correlation coefficients, and internal consistency of measured variable
5

supported: after excluding non-significant paths, a positive


indirect effect was found from perfectionistic concerns to
− .24**
.53**
.44**
.41**
.78**
.40**
.63**
.43**

academic burnout via mastery-avoidance goals (β = .341).


(.80)

.10

Bootstrapped 95% CIs for these indirect effects did not con-
4

tain zero, indicating these indirect paths were statistically


− .26**

significant. Notably, the direct effect of perfectionistic con-


.43**
.32**
.40**
.50**
.52**
.31**
.71**
.62**
(.90)

.08

cerns on academic burnout remained significant after intro-


3

ducing indirect paths through achievement goals.


− .16**
− .21**
− .13*

.21**
.49**
− .03
− .56
(.88)

.13*
.07
.08
.09
2

Discussion
− .29**

.25**
.40**
.28**
.29**
.26**
.18**
.42**

− .06
− .01
(.84)

The present study examined the relationship between per-


.08
.05
1

fectionism (i.e., perfectionistic strivings and perfectionistic


concerns) and academic burnout, focusing on the mediating
1.00
.99
.76
.70
.99
.74
.68
.74
.72
.74
.90
.98
.94
SD

roles that 2 × 2 achievement goals play for college students.


First, Hypothesis 1–1 was supported, as perfectionistic
3.28
3.70
3.11
2.95
3.63
3.98
2.79
3.77
2.79
2.76
2.61
4.73
3.22

strivings were significantly associated with all types of


M

achievement goals. Specifically, when considering the cor-


Cronbach’s alphas are in parentheses

relation between the two perfectionism dimensions, perfec-


6. Socially prescribed perfectionism

tionistic strivings had a positive association with approach


13. Performance-avoidance goal
12. Performance-approach goal
3. Self-oriented perfectionism

goals and a negative association with avoidance goals. These


11. Mastery-avoidance goal
10. Mastery-approach goal

results implied that individuals with perfectionistic striv-


7. Concern over mistakes
8. Doubts about actions

ings tend to approach positive outcomes and do not avoid


4. Personal standards

*p < .05, **p < .01

negative outcomes. Previous studies have reported positive


5. High standards

9. Discrepancy

bivariate correlations between perfectionistic strivings and


1. Exhaustion
2. Cynicism

all types of achievement goals (e.g., Kim et al. 2015; Kim


and Choi 2017); however, our findings showed different pat-
terns of association between perfectionistic strivings and

13
Exploring the relationship between perfectionism and academic burnout through achievement…

Table 3  Correlations among Latent variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7


latent variables
1. Perfectionistic strivings –
2. Perfectionistic concerns .71*** –
3. Mastery-approach goal .50*** .12 –
4. Mastery-avoidance goal − .32*** .14 − .69*** –
5. Performance-approach goal .57*** .60*** .24*** − .05 –
6. Performance-avoidance goal .10 .41*** − .23** .32*** .35*** –
7. Academic burnout − .15 .31*** − .51*** .77*** .02 .33*** –

**p < .01, ***p < .001

Table 4  Model fit indices for Model S-B χ2 df Δχ2 TLI CFI RMSEA (90% CI)
measurement and structural
models Cutoff values – – – > .90 > .90 ≤ .08
Measurement model 260.217 168 – .944 .955 .044 (.033–.054)
Model A 330.261 175 – .909 .924 .056 (.047–.065)
Model B 330.890 176 .250 .910 .924 .056 (.047–.065)
Model C 342.942 177 21.305*** .904 .919 .058 (.049–.067)

***p < .001

avoidance goals, when accounting for the overlap between perfectionistic strivings and perfectionistic concerns. As

Fig. 1  Final structural model


Perfectionistic
(Model B). Note Solid paths Strivings
.53 Mastery-
are statistically significant at approach goal
p < .01. Non-significant paths
-.63
are dotted. Indicators and error -.91
variables are not represented for
clarity .26 Mastery-
avoidance goal .44

.70 -.43 Academic


-.07 Burnout
Performance-
approach goal

.47
-.06
.78 .44

Perfectionistic Performance-
Concerns avoidance goal
.73

Table 5  Direct and indirect β B SE Bootstrap CI (95%)


effect of perfectionism on
academic burnout PS→Academic burnout
Direct effect (PS→Academic burnout) .0 .0 N/A N/A
Indirect effect (PS→MAP→Academic burnout) − .333 − .218 .049 (− .491, − .197)
Indirect effect (PS→MAV→Academic burnout) − .398 − .261 .085 (− .640, − .179)
PC→Academic burnou
Direct effect (PC→Academic burnout) .467 .358 .103 (.257, .734)
Indirect effect (PC→MAV→Academic burnout) .341 .262 .088 (.148, .561)

PS perfectionistic strivings, PC perfectionistic concerns, MAP mastery-approach goals, MAV mastery-


avoidance goals

13
E. Chang et al.

Stoeber et al. (2018) noted, it could be argued that the and task performance for younger adults (Senko and Freud
associations between perfectionism and achievement goals 2015). Therefore, it seems that even though the trait of mas-
would become clearer when the positive association between tery goals (i.e., focusing on intrapersonal standards) is the
the two perfectionism dimensions is considered. same in mastery-approach and mastery-avoidance goals, the
Second, Hypothesis 1–2 was partially supported, as only avoidance element of mastery-avoidance goals (i.e., focus-
mastery goals (i.e., mastery-approach and mastery-avoid- ing on avoiding failure or negative outcomes) led to nega-
ance goals) mediated the negative association between tive consequences (i.e., high levels of academic burnout) for
perfectionistic strivings and academic burnout. Specifi- college students in the present study. According to Schnelle,
cally, perfectionistic strivings had a positive association Brandstätter, and Knöpfel (2010), adoption of approach or
with mastery-approach goals, which negatively predicted avoidance goals is affected by the level of goal-relevant
academic burnout. Further, perfectionistic strivings had a resources. People adopt approach goals if they have many
negative association with mastery-avoidance goals which, goal-relevant resources, whereas they adopt avoidance goals
in turn, positively predicted academic burnout. The negative if they have fewer goal-relevant resources. Thus, it can be
association between mastery-approach goals and academic considered that individuals who perceive academic goal-
burnout in the present study was consistent with previ- relevant resources (e.g., social support, time, and energy)
ous research that showed the positive effects of mastery- as limited adopt mastery-avoidance goals, and perceiving
approach goals in academic settings (e.g., Putwain et al. few resources is linked to academic burnout (Hobfoll 1989).
2018; Wang et al. 2017). For example, in Putwain et al.’s Performance goals (i.e., performance-approach and
(2018) study, mastery-approach goals predicted behavio- performance-avoidance goals) did not significantly pre-
ral engagement, which is positively linked to achievement. dict academic burnout in the present study. This result was
In Ranellucci et al. (2015) study, mastery-approach goals inconsistent with previous studies that showed significant
predicted greater enjoyment of learning, which is linked to associations between those factors (e.g., Naidoo et al. 2012),
effective self-regulated learning (i.e., cognitive elaboration). and some studies that reported a significant effect of per-
Third, Hypothesis 2–1 was supported, as perfectionistic formance goals on academic settings (e.g., Chen and Wong
concerns had significant associations with performance- 2015; Shih 2018; Zhou and Wang 2019). This inconsistency
approach, performance-avoidance, and mastery-avoidance may be due to the effects of performance goals, which could
goals, consistent with previous studies (e.g., Lee and Ander- vary depending on the learning context. King et al. (2017)
man 2020). The positive association between perfectionistic reported that performance goals have significant positive
concerns and avoidance goals reflected the avoidance ten- effects on educational outcomes in Hong Kong, unlike in
dency of perfectionistic concerns, as described by the dual some other countries, because performance goals are more
process model of perfectionism (Slade and Owens 1998). dominant in Hong Kong, which has a competitive learn-
In addition, the positive association between perfectionistic ing atmosphere. Similarly, Confucianism and social norms
concerns and performance goals supported previous findings emphasize educational success in a Korean cultural context
(e.g., Lee and Anderman 2020). For individuals with per- (Lee and Ju 2019). South Korean undergraduate students
fectionistic concerns, others (rather than themselves) serve are familiar with a competitive educational context, as they
as reference points, which is theoretically linked to perfor- take many high-stakes exams, including the College Scho-
mance goals that focus on the demonstration of competence lastic Ability Test (Kwon et al. 2017). Therefore, embracing
or not incompetence (Lee and Anderman 2020). performance goals may not be a “big deal” for them, thus
Fourth, Hypothesis2–2 was partially supported, as only reducing the effects of performance goals on academic burn-
mastery-avoidance goals partially mediated the positive out. Other learning contexts, such as grade level, may also
association between perfectionistic concerns and academic create this inconsistency. For example, Lee (2010) reported
burnout. This deleterious effect of mastery-avoidance a positive association between performance-avoidance goals
goals was consistent with the previous literature (Mih and academic burnout for South Korean middle and high
et al. 2015; Senko and Freund 2015). In Senko and Fre- school students, whereas the association was non-significant
und’s (2015) study, young adults (18 − 32 years) perceived in the present study. The different learning context between
mastery-avoidance goals to be more difficult to attain than secondary and post-secondary school may be the reason for
mastery-approach goals, and the perceived difficulty of this inconsistency. Further research is needed to analyze how
mastery-avoidance goals was linked to lower levels of inter- different cultures or educational contexts affect the relation-
est, perceived competence, and performance. While older ship between performance goals and academic outcomes.
adults generally experience resource loss, younger adults The present study can provide some suggestions for
generally experience resource gain (e.g., gaining knowledge, college faculty members and support staff to prevent aca-
social networks, and money); relative inexperience in pur- demic burnout in students. First, students can be screened
suing avoidance goals may be linked to lower enjoyment for high levels of perfectionistic concerns, and counseling

13
Exploring the relationship between perfectionism and academic burnout through achievement…

or consulting services to prevent academic burnout can be academic burnout in perfectionists. Previous relevant stud-
provided. Our findings indicated that higher-level mastery- ies have partially reported the associations among these
approach goals and lower-level mastery-avoidance goals variables; however, this study demonstrated the integrated
were related to lower levels of academic burnout for perfec- relationships among the variables using structural equation
tionistic students. Thus, practitioners may promote interven- modeling. Furthermore, by adopting a 2 × 2 goal orienta-
tion programs that help perfectionistic students adopt mas- tion framework, this study broadened the understanding
tery-approach goals, rather than mastery-avoidance goals of achievement goals and their effects on psychological
(e.g., encouraging self-enhancement). health, including academic burnout. Inconsistent with
Further, because students are likely to adopt achieve- prior studies that showed positive or negative effect of
ment goals that are in line with perceived classroom goal performance goals on academic settings, this study did
structure (Peng et al. 2018), educators should create a not show a significant effect of performance goals on
mastery-approach classroom goal structure by emphasizing academic burnout. Related to this result, the possibility
self-improvement and effort, rather than comparisons and that the effects of performance goals may differ depend-
competition with peers (Gertsakis et al. 2020). Addition- ing on educational context (e.g., culture or grade level)
ally, educators should provide a mastery goal structure for should be considered. Through this study, students with
students under a low-performance goal structure (Skaalvik perfectionistic traits themselves may better understand
and Federici 2016). According to Skaalvik and Federici the reasons why they experience academic burnout and
(2016), the meaning of a mastery goal structure within a make efforts to prevent it. Furthermore, educators will be
specific learning context can be perceived differently by able to create an educational environment that emphasizes
students, depending on the level of performance goal struc- mastery-approach goals to prevent academic burnout in
ture. That is, under strong-performance goal structures, stu- perfectionists.
dents perceive the importance of mastery goals as related
to performance goals. For example, they could interpret the Acknowledgements This work was supported by the National
Research Foundation of Korea Grant funded by the Korean Govern-
importance of self-improvement in terms of proving their ment (NRF-2017S1A5A2A01025536).
competence to others. However, under low-performance goal
structures, a strong-mastery goal structure facilitates stu-
dents’ abilities to perceive mastery goals (e.g., self-improve- References
ment) themselves as meaningful.
There were some limitations to the present study. First, Adriaenssens, J., De Gucht, V., & Maes, S. (2015). Association of
all theoretical variables were measured at once; therefore, goal orientation with work engagement and burnout in emer-
it is difficult to infer causal relationships among the vari- gency nurses. Journal of Occupational Health, 57(2), 151–160.
https​://doi.org/10.1539/joh.14-0069-OA.
ables. To more accurately examine the causal relationships Alarcon, G. M., Edwards, J. M., & Menke, L. E. (2011). Student
among variables, a longitudinal study should be conducted. burnout and engagement: A test of the conservation of resources
Second, this study relied on self-report questionnaires. Even theory. The Journal of psychology, 145(3), 211–227. https​://doi.
though self-report questionnaires are commonly used to org/10.1080/00223​980.2011.55543​2.
Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structure, and student motiva-
measure perfectionism and academic burnout, achievement tion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 261–271.
goal orientations can be situationally operationalized in an Bandalos, D. L. (2002). The effects of item parceling on goodness-
experimental setting (e.g., Senko and Freund 2015). Hence, of-fit and parameter estimate bias in structural equation mod-
it is recommended to replicate this study by implementing eling. Structural Equation Modeling, 9(1), 78–102. https​://doi.
org/10.1207/S1532​8007S​EM090​1_5.
an experimental research design. Additionally, the sample Bandalos, D. L. (2008). Is parceling really necessary? A comparison
in the present study was recruited from a single university. of results from item parceling and categorical variable method-
Follow-up research should include multiple institutions and ology. Structural Equation Modeling, 15(2), 211–240. https​://
more varied populations. doi.org/10.1080/10705​51080​19223​40.
Blankstein, K. R., Dunkley, D. M., & Wilson, J. (2008). Evaluative
concerns and personal standards perfectionism: Self-esteem as a
mediator and moderator of relations with personal and academic
needs and estimated GPA. Current Psychology, 27(1), 29–61.
Conclusions https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1214​4-008-9022-1.
Bong, M., Hwang, A., Noh, A., & Kim, S. I. (2014). Perfectionism
and motivation of adolescents in academic contexts. Journal of
The present study confirmed the mediating role of mastery Educational Psychology, 106, 711–729. https​://doi.org/10.1037/
goal orientations in the association between perfectionism a0035​836.
and academic burnout. Specifically, the results indicated Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing
model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural
that higher levels of mastery-approach goals and lower equation models (pp. 136–162). Beverley Hills, CA: Sage.
levels of mastery-avoidance goals are associated with

13
E. Chang et al.

Chang, E., & Lee, S. M. (2020). Mediating effect of goal adjustment Hill, A. P., & Curran, T. (2016). Multidimensional perfectionism and
on the relationship between socially prescribed perfectionism burnout: A meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology
and academic burnout. Psychology in the Schools, 57, 284–295. Review, 20(3), 269–288. https​://doi.org/10.1177/10888​68315​
https​://doi.org/10.1002/pits.22327​. 59628​6.
Chang, E., Lee, A., Byeon, E., Seong, H., & Lee, S. M. (2016). Hobfoll, S. E. (1989). Conservation of resources: A new attempt at
The mediating effect of motivational types in the relationship conceptualizing stress. American Psychologist, 44(3), 513–524.
between perfectionism and academic burnout. Personality and https​://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.44.3.513.
Individual Differences, 89, 202–210. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j. Hobfoll, S. E. (2001). The influence of culture, community, and the
paid.2015.10.010. nested-self in the stress process: Advancing conservation of
Chen, W. W., & Wong, Y. L. (2015). The relationship between goal resources theory. Applied psychology, 50(3), 337–421. https​://
orientation and academic achievement in Hong Kong: The role doi.org/10.1111/1464-0597.00062​.
of context. The Asia-Pacific Education Researcher, 24(1), 169– Honicke, T., Broadbent, J., & Fuller-Tyszkiewicz, M. (2019). Learner
176. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s4029​9-013-0169-7. self-efficacy, goal orientation, and academic achievement:
Cheung, M. W. L. (2007). Comparison of approaches to constructing Exploring mediating and moderating relationships. Higher Edu-
confidence intervals for mediating effects using structural equa- cation Research & Development, https​://doi.org/10.1080/07294​
tion models. Structural Equation Modeling, 14(2), 227–246. 360.2019.16859​41.
https​://doi.org/10.1080/10705​51070​93367​45. Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cut-off criteria for fit indexes in
Cho, E., & Lee, H. (2011). The effect of optimism and pessimism covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new
on college students’ adaptation and positive/negative affect: alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55. https​://doi.
Achievement goal orientation as a mediating variable. Korean org/10.1080/10705​51990​95401​18.
Journal of Youth Studies, 18(12), 351–374. Hu, L., Bentler, P. M., & Kano, Y. (1992). Can test statistics in covari-
Cho, S., Chung, J., Shin, H. (2017). A meta-analysis on the rela- ance structure analysis be trusted? Psychological Bulletin, 112(2),
tionship between multidimensional perfectionism and aca- 351–362. https​://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.2.351.
demic burnout. The Korean Journal of School Psychology, Huh, H. S., Lim, S. Y., & Kwon, S. M. (2015). The effect of perfection-
14(3):349–379. ism and impulsiveness on academic initiation- and completion-
Chung, S. J. (1999). A study of the development of cognitive-behav- procrastination. The Korean Journal of Clinical Psychology,
ioral group counseling program for reducing the perfection- 34(1), 147–172.
ism. (Doctoral dissertation, Yonsei University, Seoul, Korea). Isoard-Gautheur, S., Trouilloud, D., Gustafsson, H., & Guillet-Descas,
Retrieved from https​://dcoll​ectio​n.yonse​i.ac.kr/publi​c_resou​rce/ E. (2016). Associations between the perceived quality of the
pdf/00000​01107​98_20200​20311​0820.pdf coach–athlete relationship and athlete burnout: An examination
Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation and achieve- of the mediating role of achievement goals. Psychology of Sport
ment goals. Educational Psychologist, 34(3), 169–189. https​:// and Exercise, 22, 210–217. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.psych​sport​
doi.org/10.1207/s1532​6985e​p3403​_3. .2015.08.003.
Elliot, A. J., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). Approach and avoidance Jeong, S. M., & Lee, D. G. (2013). Differences in passive/active pro-
achievement goals and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality crastination, happiness, and depression by the clusters of perfec-
and Social Psychology, 73, 171–185. tionism in college students. Korean Journal of Counseling, 14(2),
Elliot, A. J., & Hulleman, C. S. (2017). Achievement goals. In A. J. 857–875.
Elliot, C. S. Dweck, & D. S. Yeager (Eds.), Handbook of com- Jung, I., Kim, J. H., Ma, Y., & Seo, C. (2015). Mediating effect of
petence and motivation: Theory and application (2nd ed., pp. academic self-efficacy on the relationship between academic
43–60). New York: Guildford Press. stress and academic burnout in chinese adolescents. International
Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2 _ 2 achievement goal Journal of Human Ecology, 16(2), 63–77. https:​ //doi.org/10.6115/
framework. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, ijhe.2015.16.2.63.
501–519. https​://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.3.501. Kim, E. Y., Lim, E. J., & Noh, J. H. (2016). Academic stress, major
Field, A. (2013). Discovering statistics using SPSS statistics (5th ed.). satisfaction, and academic achievement according to type of
London: Sage Publications. achievement goal orientation in nursing Students. Information,
Frost, R. O., Marten, P., Lahart, C., & Rosenblate, R. (1990). The 19(10A), 4557–4562.
dimensions of perfectionism. Cognitive Therapy and Research, Kim, H. S. (2019). The influence of academic emotional burnout on
14, 449–468. https​://doi.org/10.1007/BF011​72967​. academic adjustment among college students: The mediating
Garratt-Reed, D., Howell, J., Hayes, L., & Boyes, M. (2018). Is perfec- effect of a grateful disposition. Journal of Asia Pacific Coun-
tionism associated with academic burnout through repetitive nega- seling, 9(2), 57–72. https​://doi.org/10.18401​/2019.9.2.4.
tive thinking? PeerJ, 6, e5004. https:​ //doi.org/10.7717/peerj.​ 5004. Kim, L. E., Chen, L., MacCann, C., Karlov, L., & Kleitman, S.
Gertsakis, N., Kroustallaki, D., & Sideridis, G. D. (2020). How do (2015). Evidence for three factors of perfectionism: Perfection-
classroom goal structures matter? The impact on grammar istic strivings, order, and perfectionistic concerns. Personality
achievement, perceived autonomy support, flow, and affect. Inter- and Individual Differences, 84, 16–22. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.
national Journal of School & Educational Psychology. https:​ //doi. paid.2015.01.033.
org/10.1080/21683​603.2019.16941​11. Kim, N., & Choi, W. (2017). The influence of perfectionism, anxiety
Hair, J. F., Jr., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., & Anderson, R. E. (2009). and achievement goal orientation on academic procrastination.
Multivariate data analysis (7th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Journal of the Korea Institute of Youth Facility and Environment,
Pearson Prentice Hall. 15(1), 157–164.
Han, K. Y. (1993). Multidimensional perfectionism: Concept, meas- King, R. B., McInerney, D. M., & Nasser, R. (2017). Different goals for
urement and relevance to maladjustment (Doctoral dissertation). different folks: A cross-cultural study of achievement goals across
Korea University, Seoul, Korea. nine cultures. Social Psychology of Education, 20(3), 619–642.
Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1991). Perfectionism in the self and social https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1121​8-017-9381-2.
contexts: conceptualization, assessment, and association with Kwon, S. K., Lee, M., & Shin, D. (2017). Educational assessment in the
psychopathology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Republic of Korea: Lights and shadows of high-stake exam-based
60(3), 456–470. https​://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.60.3.456.

13
Exploring the relationship between perfectionism and academic burnout through achievement…

education system. Assessment in Education, 24(1), 60–77. https:​ // Park, H. J. (2009). Validation of the almost perfect scale-revised.
doi.org/10.1080/09695​94X.2015.10745​40. Korean Journal of Counseling and Psychotherapy, 21(1),
Lau, R. S., & Cheung, Q. W. (2012). Estimating and comparing 131–149.
specific mediation effects in complex latent variable models. Peng, S. L., Cherng, B. L., Lin, Y. Y., & Kuo, C. W. (2018). Four-
Organizational Research Methods, 15(1), 3–16. https​: //doi. dimensional classroom goal structure model: Validation and inves-
org/10.1177/10944​28110​39167​3. tigation of its effect on students’ adoption of personal achieve-
Lee, J. (2010). The mediating effects of achievement goal on the ment goals and approach/avoidance behaviors. Learning and
relationship between academic demand and academic burnout. Individual Differences, 61, 228–238. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Korean Journal of Counseling, 11, 1551–1565. lindi​f.2017.12.004.
Lee, S., & Ju, Y. (2019). The effects of parental study achievement Pintrich, P. R. (2000). Multiple goals, multiple pathways: The
pressure on academic achievement and psychological well-being role of goal orientation in learning and achievement. Jour-
of middle school student: focusing on the moderating effect of nal of Educational Psychology, 92(3), 544–555. https​://doi.
social support. The Journal of Humanities and Social Sciences, org/10.1037/0022-0663.92.3.544.
10(2), 331–346. Putwain, D. W., Symes, W., Nicholson, L. J., & Becker, S. (2018).
Lee, S., & Yang, S. (2019). A study on the relationship between adap- Achievement goals, behavioural engagement, and mathemat-
tive, maladaptive perfectionism and academic burnout among ics achievement: A mediational analysis. Learning and Indi-
university students: The mediating effects of internal locus of vidual Differences, 68, 12–19. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindi​
control and self-encouragement. Korean Journal of Youth Stud- f.2018.09.006.
ies, 26(4), 85–111. Ranellucci, J., Hall, N. C., & Goetz, T. (2015). Achievement goals,
Lee, Y. J., & Anderman, E. M. (2020). Profiles of perfectionism emotions, learning, and performance: A process model. Motiva-
and their relations to educational outcomes in college students: tion Science, 1(2), 98–120. https​://doi.org/10.1037/mot00​00014​.
The moderating role of achievement goals. Learning and Indi- Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (2001). A scaled difference chi-square
vidual Differences, 77, 101813. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindi​ test statistic for moment structure analysis. Psychometrika, 66(4),
f.2019.10181​3. 507–514. https​://doi.org/10.1007/BF022​96192​.
Luo, Y., Wang, Z., Zhang, H., Chen, A., & Quan, S. (2016). The Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (2010). Ensuring positiveness of the
effect of perfectionism on school burnout among adolescence: scaled difference chi-square test statistic. Psychometrika, 75(2),
The mediator of self-esteem and coping style. Personality and 243–248. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1133​6-009-9135-y.
Individual Differences, 88, 202–208. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j. Saville, B. K., Bureau, A., Eckenrode, C., & Maley, M. (2018). Pas-
paid.2015.08.056. sion and burnout in college students. College Student Journal,
Madjar, N., Voltsis, M., & Weinstock, M. P. (2015). The roles of per- 52(1), 105–117.
ceived parental expectation and criticism in adolescents’ multi- Schaufeli, W. B., Martinez, I. M., Pinto, A., Salanova, M., & Bakker,
dimensional perfectionism and achievement goals. Educational A. B. (2002). Burnout and engagement in university students: A
Psychology, 35(6), 765–778. https​: //doi.org/10.1080/01443​ cross-national study. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33,
410.2013.86475​6. 464–481. https​://doi.org/10.1177/00220​22102​03300​5003.
Mardia, K. V. (1970). Measures of multivariate skewness and kur- Schaufeli, W. B., Salanova, M., González-Romá, V., & Bakker, A. B.
tosis with applications. Biometrika, 57(3), 519–530. https​://doi. (2000). The measurement of burnout and engagement: A con-
org/10.1093/biome​t/57.3.519. firmative analytic approach. Journal of Happiness Studies, 5,
May, R. W., Bauer, K. N., & Fincham, F. D. (2015). School burnout: 464–481.
Diminished academic and cognitive performance. Learning and Schnelle, J., Brandstätter, V., & Knöpfel, A. (2010). The adoption
Individual Differences, 42, 126–131. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j. of approach versus avoidance goals: The role of goal-relevant
lindi​f.2015.07.015. resources. Motivation and Emotion, 34(3), 215–229. https​://doi.
McCarthy, M. P., Pretty, G. M., & Catano, V. (1990). Psychological org/10.1007/s1103​1-010-9173-x.
sense of community and student burnout. Journal of College Stu- Senko, C., & Freund, A. M. (2015). Are mastery-avoidance achieve-
dent Development, 31(3), 211–216. ment goals always detrimental? An adult development per-
McDonald, R. P., & Ho, M. H. R. (2002). Principles and practice in spective. Motivation and Emotion, 39(4), 477–488. https​://doi.
reporting structural equation analyses. Psychological Methods, org/10.1007/s1103​1-015-9474-1.
7(1), 64–82. https​://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.7.1.64. Seo, E. H. (2009). The relationship of procrastination with a mastery
Mih, V., Mih, C., & Dragos, V. (2015). Achievement goals and behav- goal versus an avoidance goal. Social Behavior and Personality,
ioral and emotional engagement as precursors of academic adjust- 37(7), 911–919. https​://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2009.37.7.911.
ing. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 209, 329–336. Shih, S. S. (2018). Examining relationships of Taiwanese adolescents’
https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspr​o.2015.11.243. achievement goals to academic engagement and coping. Journal
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2017). Mplus User’s guide. of Education and Human Development, 7(1), 153–165. https​://
(8th edn.). Los Angeles, CA: Author. doi.org/10.15640​/jehd.v7n1a​18.
Naidoo, L. J., DeCriscio, A., Bily, H., Manipella, A., Ryan, M., & You- Shin, H., Puig, A., Lee, J., Lee, J. H., & Lee, S. M. (2011). Cultural
dim, J. (2012). The 2× 2 model of goal orientation and burnout: validation of the maslach burnout inventory for Korean students.
The role of approach–avoidance dimensions in predicting burnout. Asia Pacific Education Review, 12(4), 633–639. https​://doi.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 42(10), 2541–2563. https​ org/10.1007/s1256​4-011-9164-y.
://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2012.00952​.x. Skaalvik, E. M., & Federici, R. A. (2016). Relations between class-
Newman, B. N., Strickler, J. G., O’Brien, C., Lui, T., & Lynch, M. room goal structures and students’ goal orientations in mathemat-
(2019). Deconstructing perfectionism in college students: Pat- ics classes: When is a mastery goal structure adaptive? Social
terns of behavior, emotion, and cognition. Personality and Psychology of Education, 19(1), 135–150. https:​ //doi.org/10.1007/
Individual Differences, 145, 106–111. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j. s1121​8-015-9323-9.
paid.2019.03.030. Slade, P. D., & Owens, R. G. (1998). A dual process model of perfec-
Park, B. G., & Lee, J. U. (2005). Development and validation of a 2 tionism based on reinforcement theory. Behavior Modification,
× 2 achievement goal orientation scale. The Korean Journal of 22(3), 372–390. https​://doi.org/10.1177/01454​45598​02230​10.
Educational Psychology, 19(1), 327–352.

13
E. Chang et al.

Slaney, R. B., Rice, K. G., Mobley, M., Trippi, J., & Ashby, J. S. performance among university students. Educational Psychology,
(2001). The revised almost perfect scale. Measurement and Evalu- 39(6), 768–783. https:​ //doi.org/10.1080/014434​ 10.2018.154599​ 6.
ation in Counseling and Development, 34(3), 130–145. Wang, C., Shim, S. S., & Wolters, C. A. (2017). Achievement goals,
Speirs Neumeister, K. L., Fletcher, K. L., & Burney, V. H. (2015). motivational self-talk, and academic engagement among Chinese
Perfectionism and achievement motivation in high-ability stu- students. Asia Pacific education review, 18(3), 295–307. https​://
dents: An examination of the 2× 2 model of perfectionism. Jour- doi.org/10.1007/s1256​4-017-9495-4.
nal for the Education of the Gifted, 38(3), 215–232. https​://doi. West, S. G., Finch, J. F., & Curran, P. J. (1995). Structural equation
org/10.1177/01623​53215​59250​2. models with nonnormal variables: Problems and remedies. In R.
Stoeber, J., & Otto, K. (2006). Positive conceptions of perfectionism: H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues,
approaches, evidence, challenges. Personality and Social Psychol- and applications (pp. 56–75). Thousand Oaks: Sage.
ogy Review, 10(4), 295–319. https​://doi.org/10.1207/s1532​7957p​ Yu, J. H., & Chae, S. J. (2016). Differences in academic burnout and
spr10​04_2. achievement goal orientation by perfectionism of medical stu-
Stoeber, J., Damian, L. E., & Madigan, D. J. (2018). Perfectionism: A dents. Journal of Health Informatics and Statistics, 41(1), 88–95.
motivational perspective. In J. Stoeber (Ed.), The Psychology of Zhang, Y., Gan, Y., & Chan, H. (2007). Perfectionism, academic burn-
perfectionism: Theory, research, applications (pp. 19–43). Lon- out and engagement among Chinese college students: A structural
don: Routledge. equation modeling analysis. Personality and Individual Differ-
Tourangeau, R., Singer, E., & Presser, S. (2003). Context effects in ences, 43, 1529–1540. https:​ //doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.04.010.
attitude surveys: Effects on remote items and impact on predic- Zhou, Y., & Wang, J. (2019). Goal orientation, learning strategies, and
tive validity. Sociological Methods & Research, 31(4), 486–513. academic performance in adult distance learning. Social Behavior
Vaulerin, J., Colson, S. S., Emile, M., Scoffier-Mériaux, S., & d’Arripe- and Personality, 47(7), 1–20. https​://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.819.
Longueville, F. (2016). The big five personality traits and French
firefighter burnout: The mediating role of achievement goals. Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 58(4), jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
e128–e132. https​://doi.org/10.1097/JOM.00000​00000​00067​9.
Vizoso, C., Arias-Gundín, O., & Rodríguez, C. (2019). Exploring
coping and optimism as predictors of academic burnout and

13

You might also like