You are on page 1of 10

University of Nebraska - Lincoln

DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln


Educational Psychology Papers and Publications Educational Psychology, Department of

2015

Shy Children in the Classroom: From Research to


Educational Practice
Irina Kalutskaya
University of Nebraska-Lincoln, irina@huskers.unl.edu

Kristen A. Archbell
Carleton University

Kathleen Moritz Rudasill


University of Nebraska-Lincoln, kmrudasill@vcu.edu

Robert J. Coplan
Carleton University

Follow this and additional works at: http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/edpsychpapers


Part of the Child Psychology Commons, Cognitive Psychology Commons, Developmental
Psychology Commons, and the School Psychology Commons

Kalutskaya, Irina; Archbell, Kristen A.; Rudasill, Kathleen Moritz; and Coplan, Robert J., "Shy Children in the Classroom: From
Research to Educational Practice" (2015). Educational Psychology Papers and Publications. 222.
http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/edpsychpapers/222

This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Educational Psychology, Department of at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska -
Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Educational Psychology Papers and Publications by an authorized administrator of
DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln.
Published in Translational Issues in Psychological Science 1.2 (2015), pp 149–157.
doi 10.1037/tps0000024 “This article may not exactly replicate the final version published
in the APA journal. It is not the copy of record.” digitalcommons.unl.edu
digitalcommons.unl.edu
Copyright © 2015 American Psychological Association. Used by permission.
Submitted April 14, 2014; revised January 29, 2015; accepted February 13, 2015.

Shy Children in the Classroom:


From Research to Educational Practice

Irina N. Kalutskaya,1 Kristen A. Archbell,2


Kathleen Moritz Rudasill,1 and Robert J. Coplan2
1 Department of Educational Psychology, University of Nebraska–Lincoln
2 Department of Psychology, Carleton University
Irina N. Kalutskaya and Kristen A. Archbell are graduate student authors.
Corresponding author — Irina N. Kalutskaya, 229 Teachers College Hall, Department of Educational Psychology,
University of Nebraska–Lincoln, Lincoln, NE 68588; email irina@huskers.unl.edu

Abstract
Shyness is a temperamental trait characterized by wariness, fear, and self-consciousness in social situations.
In elementary school, child shyness is associated with a wide range of socioemotional difficulties, including
poor peer relationships (e.g., exclusion, victimization), internalizing problems (e.g., low self-esteem, anxi-
ety, depression), and academic adjustment problems (e.g., lack of engagement, poor academic performance).
In the current article we particularly review recent research examining the implication of shyness in edu-
cational contexts. Topics covered include the development of shyness, why shy students might perceive the
classroom as a potential threat, and the unique challenges faced by shy children at school. Further, we con-
sider research pertaining to shy children and their teachers, including teachers’ attitudes and beliefs toward
childhood shyness, and the critical role of teacher– child relationships for shy children’s school adjustment.
Thereafter, we briefly summarize the findings from teacher-focused and child-focused intervention programs
aimed to improve academic and social performance of shy children. We conclude with a short description of
implications of shyness for educational practitioners and posit some directions for future research.

Keywords: classroom, shyness, social adjustment, teachers

Researchers have long been interested in the ized by wariness, fear, and self-consciousness
links between children’s socioemotional function- in social situations (Rubin, Coplan, & Bowker,
ing at school and their academic success (McKin- 2009). Goldsmith et al. (1987) provided a semi-
ney, Mason, Perkerson, & Clifford, 1975). Histor- nal definition of temperament as consisting of
ically, much of this work has focused on children “relatively consistent, basic dispositions inher-
who either display prosocial and other positive ent in the person that underlie and modulate the
behaviors (e.g., Caprara, Barbaranelli, Pastorelli, expression of activity, reactivity, emotionality,
Bandura, & Zimbardo, 2000), or children prone to and sociability” (p. 524). Contemporary views of
externalizing problems, such as aggression and temperament acknowledge that it results from
inattention (e.g., Hinshaw, 1992). However, in re- interactions between biological and environmen-
cent years, researchers have begun to examine tal factors (e.g., Shiner et al., 2012). Tempera-
the unique academic and social challenges faced mental traits, including shyness, tend to be rel-
by shy children at school (Evans, 2010). atively stable across time—particularly from the
Shyness is a temperamental trait character- preschool years and onward (Karevold, Ystrøm,

149
150 Kalutskaya et al. in Translational Issues in Psychological Science 1 (2015)

Coplan, Sanson, & Mathiesen, 2012; Roberts & Why Might We Worry About Shy Children
DelVecchio, 2000). Among younger children, shy- at School?
ness manifests primarily as fear and hesitancy
when encountering new people (i.e., behavioral With the advent of formal schooling, the school
inhibition, Kagan, 1997). In older children shy- setting provides a major additional context (af-
ness also tends to encompass embarrassment ter the home environment) for children’s social
and self-consciousness in situations of perceived interaction and development. The classroom is a
social evaluation (Crozier, 1995). From a moti- social context—and the mere presence of peers
vational perspective, shy children are thought may elevate stress among shy children (Coplan
to be experience an approach–avoidance con- & Arbeau, 2008) which may affect both their so-
flict, whereby their eagerness to join peer activ- cial competence and academic adjustment. For ex-
ities (high social approach motivation) is sup- ample, heightened social-evaluative concerns are
pressed by underlying fear and anxiety (high a common marker of shyness in childhood (e.g.,
social avoidance motivation) (Coplan, Prakash, Crozier, 1995). This may cause shy children to ru-
O’Neil, & Armer, 2004). minate on the social impression they give others,
When the demands of the environment are not thus distracting them from class lessons. More-
conducive with the needs of child’s temperament over, children displaying shy, socially awkward,
(e.g., the lack of the “goodness of fit”), adjustment and anxious behaviors are more likely to be per-
difficulties may arise. For shy children, who are ceived by their peers as unattractive playmates
already predisposed to Negative Affectivity (e.g., and “easy marks,” and tend to be excluded from
anger, sadness, fear, physical discomfort, and re- social activities (Chen, DeSouza, Chen, & Wang,
covery from distress) the school environment of- 2006; Gazelle & Ladd, 2003). Children who com-
ten appears to represent a “poor fit” (Coplan & Ar- paratively spend more time alone in the presence
beau, 2008). In support of this notion, a growing of available playmates (for various reasons) tend
body of empirical research suggests that shy chil- to evoke more negative responses from peers (Co-
dren are at increased risk for a range of concur- plan et al., 2013).
rent and subsequent academic and socioemotional Shy children are prone to rate themselves as
difficulties, including internalizing problems (e.g., less physically attractive, less socially skilled,
anxiety, depression), and negative experiences and less positively in general (Leary, 2001). Neg-
with peers (e.g., exclusion, victimization) (see Ru- ative peer experiences for shy children may fur-
bin et al., 2009 for an extensive review). In this ther evoke negative self-perceptions and lower
review, we examine the theoretical and empiri- self-esteem, as well as heightened symptoms of
cal literature pertaining to shyness in elementary depression (e.g., Gazelle & Ladd, 2003). For ex-
school classrooms. The major purpose of this re- tremely shy children, performing everyday class-
view is to describe how classroom environments room activities can present additional stress due
are related to the adjustment of shy children in to their negative self-perceptions (Leary & Kow-
elementary grades. This review adds to the pre- alski, 1995).
vious studies on shyness in the classroom (e.g., In terms of academic skills, Coplan and Ev-
Evans, 2001, 2010) by considering both the char- ans (2009) suggested that shy children’s academic
acteristics of shy children, as well as teacher and performance might be affected by their lack of
classroom contributions to shy children’s school participation, specific use of language, perfor-
adjustment. This review is unique because it ad- mance anxiety, or lower academic engagement.
dresses the needs of shy children, and identifies Indeed, shy children tend to talk less in class, and
the characteristics that make these children dif- when they do speak they make fewer comments
ferent from their non-shy peers. We also briefly and typically take more time to respond (Evans,
summarize findings from intervention programs 2001). Shy children’s peers may perceive this re-
aimed to improve shy children’s functioning at luctance to talk as a lack of preparedness or lack
school. Finally, we discuss implications for edu- of knowledge, which may contribute to further so-
cational practitioners and impart directions for cial exclusion. Consequently, shy children often
future research. “participate” in group activities by simply watch-
ing others (Coplan et al., 2004), which in turn
Shy Children in the Classroom: From Research to Educational Practice 151

limits their opportunities for learning and mas- interactions with students and classroom man-
tering academic tasks. agement style (Brophy & Good, 1974). The influ-
Elevated levels of anxiety and self-conscious- ence that teachers have on shy children may de-
ness might also contribute to the test performance pend on their awareness of shyness as a problem,
of shy children, particularly in testing situations as well as their classroom management demands.
requiring demonstration of expressive vocabu- Teachers work constantly to manage multi-
lary skills (e.g., Coplan & Evans, 2009, for a re- ple demands in the classroom, and they may not
view). Curby, Rudasill, Edwards, and Perez-Ed- always have the resources or time to intervene
gar (2011) suggest that shy children typically in every situation that arises. As a result, many
need more instructional feedback and teacher teachers feel unprepared to effectively manage
encouragement, to maintain social and academic the classroom environment, and this deficit is
engagement in a classroom. Of note, language likely to affect their ability to assist all children
skills may serve a protective function in the so- (Aloe et al., 2014). Rather, the children receiving
cioemotional functioning of shy children (Coplan primacy in teacher attention are those displaying
& Weeks, 2009). For example, Coplan and Armer behavior that is disruptive or noticeable and hin-
(2005) found out that shy children with greater dering others’ learning (Dobbs & Arnold, 2009).
expressive vocabulary skills had higher self-per- Indeed, the current literature provides mixed
ception and lower risk for psychological malad- findings on teachers’ awareness of shyness in the
justment. However, it has also been suggested classroom. Some research suggests shy children
that better language skills may actually increase tend to go unnoticed by teachers, perhaps because
shy children’s dependence on teachers, by en- of their quiet nature and decreased likelihood of
abling their ability to communicate with teachers being disruptive during class activities (Rimm-
in ways that may seem needy or unnecessary (Ru- Kaufman & Kagan, 2005). Coplan and Prakash
dasill, Rimm-Kaufman, Justice, & Pence, 2006). (2003) observed preschool teachers and children
Notwithstanding, there is at least some evi- during teacher-supervised free play and found
dence to suggest that teacher ratings of shy chil- that children who most frequently received initi-
dren’s academic performance and skills are more ations from teachers were also the most shy and
negative compared with children’s performance anxious (as compared with children who initiated
on standardized tests’ results (e.g., Hughes & interaction with teachers or spent less time with
Coplan, 2010). In this regard, it has been sug- teachers). Similarly, kindergarten teachers report
gested that teachers might attribute poorer aca- that they would anticipate serious social and ac-
demic performance in shy children to their lower ademic costs for shyness and would be likely to
behavioral engagement in class and perceive less intervene in responses to such behaviors (Arbeau
participating children as less academically suc- & Coplan, 2007). Most recently, Coplan, Bullock,
cessful. The testing environment may also be an Archbell, and Bosacki (2015) reported that pre-
important factor to consider (Crozier, & Perkins, school teachers anticipated quite negative social
2002). For example, Crozier and Hostettler (2003) and academic outcomes for shy young children in
found that shy children performed significantly their classrooms (as compared to other common
worse than non-shy children when tests were ad- types of “misbehaviors” in the classroom). How-
ministered in face-to-face situations, as compared ever, among teachers of older children, Rudasill
to standard administration. Awareness of these and colleagues have reported that shy children
issues may assist teachers in determining optimal tend to receive fewer teacher-initiated interac-
testing environments for shy children to “show tions (Rudasill, 2011; Rudasill & Rimm-Kaufman,
what they know.” 2009).
Teachers’ perceptions of students have been
Shy Children and Teachers connected to students’ interactions with peers
(e.g., Rudasill, Niehaus, Buhs, & White, 2013),
There is some evidence pointing to the important suggesting that students seem to be aware of
role teachers can play in shy children’s behavior teachers’ beliefs toward other students, and
and development in school. It has been suggested that they may behave in accordance with
that teachers establish the classroom ecology via these beliefs. With this in mind, perhaps most
152 Kalutskaya et al. in Translational Issues in Psychological Science 1 (2015)

concerning is elementary school teachers’ appar- Implications for Educational Practice


ent tendency to attribute lower intelligence to
shy children (Coplan et al., 2013). Indeed, it has In terms of implications for educational prac-
been suggested that children’s verbal participa- tice, there are a myriad of specific strategies that
tion may influence teachers’ perceptions of in- have been forwarded and evaluated in previous
telligence (Gordon & Thomas, 1967), which may research that may aid the social and academic
make it difficult for shy children to demonstrate development of shy children, including practices
knowledge. This negative academic attribution related to increasing teachers’ awareness about
puts shy children of all ages at risk for creat- shyness, described above. For example, when
ing a self-fulfilling prophecy (Jussim & Harber, teachers ask fewer direct questions, and instead
2005), whereby the teachers’ expectations nega- make personal comments, shy children tend to
tively affect shy children’s views of themselves, increase their verbal participation (Evans & Bie-
which could perpetuate a downward spiral in ac- nert, 1992). Moreover, when shy children are
ademic performance. gradually exposed to potentially intimidating task
There is some evidence that children’s shyness (such as making a class presentation), and teach-
is linked to the quality of their relationships with ers scaffold and praised with each successive step,
teachers. A close teacher–child relationship con- shy children show improved academic and social
sists of warm and open interactions between the performance (O’Connor et al., in press). Such ap-
teacher and the child, and is predictive of positive proaches are reminiscent of graduated exposure
school outcomes among all children (Hamre & Pi- techniques, which can reduce stress of shy chil-
anta, 2001). Although shy children typically have dren (see Cappe & Alden, 1986). Further, Hen-
low levels of conflict with their teachers (e.g., Ru- derson and Fox (1998) recommend that teachers
dasill & Rimm- Kaufman, 2009), they also tend provide shy children with more activity choices,
to form less close and more dependent relation- and suggest discussing upcoming changes to rou-
ships with their teachers (e.g., Rudasill & Rimm- tine in advance to allow shy children to mentally
Kaufman, 2009; Rudasill et al., 2006). Dependent prepare. Finally, Evans (2001) interviewed Grade
teacher–child relationships have been associated 1 teachers regarding the strategies used in their
with teacher-rated child anxiety, asocial behav- classroom that seem to increase comfort levels of
ior, and peer exclusion (Arbeau et al., 2010). Of shy children and promote verbal participation.
note, there is some evidence to suggest that close Teachers indicated that they would ask shy chil-
teacher–child relationships can serve to buffer dren easy questions that they could answer (to in-
a shy child from negative outcomes in schools, crease child confidence), ask questions to shy chil-
whereas dependent teacher-child relationships dren first (to ensure they would not be “cut off”
appear to play an exacerbating role (Arbeau, Co- by vocal students), and establishing a personal
plan, & Weeks, 2010). relationship with shy children to increase a pos-
Shy children may also be particularly sensi- itive and trusting relationship. Improving teach-
tive to the emotional climate of the classroom ing strategies takes effort, but applying learner-
(Gazelle, 2006). Classrooms high in quality centered techniques can maximize learning for
and emotional climate might help shy children shy children, while minimizing their anxiety and
to maintain their focus and improve cognitive tension.
thinking. In contrast, classrooms with low qual- Implications for practice can also be drawn
ity might be detrimental, because they lack the from previous intervention programs designed
potential to compensate for temperamental vul- to assist shy and anxious children. Child-focused
nerabilities of shy children (Gazelle, 2006). Thus, intervention studies specifically designed for shy
it is particularly important for teachers to under- children are surprisingly rare. However, there is
stand how shyness might be manifested in the some evidence that social skills training (SST)
classroom to create classroom environments that may be a promising approach. For example, Co-
can diminish negative adjustment outcomes for plan, Schneider, Matheson, and Graham (2010)
shy students (e.g., O’Connor, Cappella, McCor- evaluated a social skills based early intervention,
mick, & McClowry, in press). designed to assist extremely inhibited preschool-
ers. SST provided initial free play, circle time
Shy Children in the Classroom: From Research to Educational Practice 153

(where social behavior and relaxation techniques of INSIGHTS (e.g., a randomized trial), the find-
were discussed), and songs or games to convey so- ings from this study can be interpreted causally,
cial skills instruction. Leaders prompted model- thus providing additional evidence for the efficacy
ing, and reinforced specific social skills such as of temperament- based intervention programs on
initiating conversations and approaching another students’ achievement through improving their
child. In this study, children in the SST sessions behavioral engagement.
demonstrated significant post-intervention de- Finally, educating teachers about child shy-
crease in observed socially wary behaviors and in- ness, associated adjustment issues, as well as
creased socially competent behaviors at preschool, special techniques to help shy children (SST/
compared with children in the control or waitlist Friends for Children) could prove to be a pow-
condition. It may be possible for teachers to incor- erful way to provide teachers with the tools to
porate SST during circle time or other daily activ- help shy children (Coplan & Arbeau, 2008). For
ities, and use strategies during free play to model example, Rapee et al. (2005) reported that par-
and reinforce social skills. ents who participated in techniques and interven-
Friends for Children is a cognitive–behavioral tions to assist their shy child found a significant
intervention program designed and validated reduction in their child’s anxiety one year later.
as a group-based treatment for clinically anx- Therefore, if teachers incorporate teaching strat-
ious children (Shortt, Barrett, & Fox, 2001). This egies into the classroom that are geared toward
program assists children in learning important shy children, teachers may be able to improve the
skills and techniques to help manage anxiety. As well-being and future adjustment of these chil-
shy children often experience anxiety, these pro- dren. Indeed, shy children in classrooms where
grams may prove beneficial to them. Techniques teachers were trained to identify shyness in chil-
in these programs include relaxation, cognitive dren and mitigate anxiety-provoking experiences
restructuring (turning “negative red thoughts” for shy children evidenced more behavioral en-
into “positive green thoughts”), attention train- gagement and better critical thinking and math
ing, parent-assisted exposure, and peer support. skills than their shy peers in other classrooms
Barrett and Turner (2001) examined the integra- (O’Connor et al., in press).
tion of the Friends for Children program into a
school setting for children ages 10–12 (where the Emphases for Educational Practitioners
teachers were trained as leaders), and found sig-
nificantly reduced self-rated anxiety of children Collectively, evidence highlights important impli-
post-program, compared with children in a stan- cations for educational practice. First, research
dard curriculum control group. This study dem- on shyness in the classroom demonstrates that
onstrates that intervention programs aimed at shyness is indeed a risk factor for children’s aca-
reducing anxiety can be effectively delivered in demic and social adjustment at kindergarten and
a school-based population and integrated into a elementary school. Most traditional school-readi-
school curriculum. ness models emphasize development of academic
Recently, O’Connor, Cappella, McCormick, and abilities and skills in children, and tend to un-
McClowry (in press) tested the efficacy of a tem- derestimate the importance of social skills and
perament-based social-emotional learning skills competences (Rimm-Kaufman & Pianta, 2000).
program (INSIGHTS into Children’s Temper- Therefore, it seems particularly important to de-
ament or INSIGHTS) where teachers and chil- vote time and effort to helping shy children to de-
dren learn about individual differences in tem- velop social skills to facilitate their successful ad-
perament, and teachers are given strategies to justment in school.
potentiate interactions with shy children for opti- In addition, shy students need more support
mal outcomes. Results indicated that shy children during transitions to new school environments.
performed better on assessments of critical think- The transition to formal school and from ele-
ing and math skills when in INSIGHTS class- mentary to middle school can be very stressful
rooms, compared with control classrooms using for all students (e.g., Rimm-Kaufman & Pianta,
a supplemental reading program. Because of the 2000). The kindergarten transition is laden with
strong research design used in the test of efficacy new experiences and new people, rendering it a
154 Kalutskaya et al. in Translational Issues in Psychological Science 1 (2015)

particularly taxing time for shy children (Arbeau (Lengua, Honorado, & Bush, 2007). It also should
& Coplan, 2007). The transition to middle school be noted that shyness might develop along with
often means an adjustment to a large, more im- learning disabilities or language delays. Future
personal environment where there are fewer close research should consider how broader environ-
connections with important adults (Niehaus, Ru- mental factors and personal characteristics con-
dasill, & Rakes, 2012). At both of these transition tribute to social and academic adjustment of shy
points it is important to bring together parents, children. The studies with increased complexity
teachers, and school personnel to create familiar- in designs that assess individual (shyness, anx-
ity and small school communities that facilitate ious withdrawal), relationship (mutual friend-
learning and adjustment. Current research in- ships, perceived support), and group characteris-
dicates that school connectedness (e.g., “feeling tics (rejection, victimization) reflect this emerging
close to people at school”) decreases the risk of ad- trend (Oh et al., 2008; Booth-LaForce & Oxford,
justment problems in middle school students over 2008).
and above perceived teacher and peer support Finally, there is a growing literature exam-
(e.g., Niehaus et al., 2012). Thus, further exami- ining the construct of shyness across cultures
nation of this effect for shy students is necessary. (Chen, 2010). For example, in traditional Chinese
Several directions for future research with rel- society, wariness and behavioral restraint may be
evant implications for educational practice are more positively evaluated and encouraged, viewed
also evident. For example, current research on as indicators of social maturity, mastery, and un-
shyness is moving toward examining the role of derstanding. However, as a result of the rapid
various exacerbating and protective factors that ongoing change in Chinese society (e.g., toward
may moderate relations between shyness and in- a market-oriented economy), the adaptive value
dices of school adjustment. For example, child of shy behavior in China appears to be declining
characteristics that serve protective roles for shy (Chen, Cen, Li, & He, 2005). It will be important
children include sociocommunicative skills (e.g., for future researchers to directly examine the role
Coplan & Weeks, 2009), social competence (e.g., of culture in the display and implications of shy
Markovic & Bowker, 2014), and temperamental behaviors in the North American classroom.
differences including emotional regulation and at- In conclusion, previous research on shyness
tention focusing (Rudasill & Konold, 2008). Fu- has contributed to our knowledge on concomitants
ture work should examine how improving self- and consequences on development of shyness in
regulation may contribute to better behavioral childhood. However, currently there are relatively
and emotional adjustment for shy children. Also, few studies on intervention of shyness. Develop-
future research can draw from Bronfenbrenner’s ment of intervention programs tailored to the
ecological systems theory to evaluate the effects needs of shy children can significantly improve
of classroom microsystems on social and academic their well-being at school. Raising awareness of
adjustment of shy children (e.g., Bronfenbrenner, shyness among parents, teachers, and educa-
1997). For example, a shy child may have less tional practitioners can result in better services
positive academic adjustment in a classroom and eventually better outcomes for shy children.
with low levels of emotional support (such as low
teacher warmth and responsiveness to children’s
needs), whereas a classroom with a high level of References
emotional support may allow a shy child to thrive.
Certain limitations also must be taken into Aloe, A. M., Amo, L. C., & Shanahan, M. E. (2014).
account with future research. First, few studies Classroom management self-efficacy and burn-
have considered how shyness and adjustment are out: A multivariate meta-analysis. Educational
associated in non-European American or low SES Psychology Review, 26, 101–126. doi 10.1007/
samples (see O’Connor et al., in press for an ex- s10648-013-9244-0
ception). It has been noted that negative environ- Arbeau, K. A., & Coplan, R. J. (2007). Kindergarten
mental factors including family income, parent teachers’ beliefs and responses to hypothetical
prosocial, asocial, and antisocial children. Mer-
education, neighborhood, and quality of parenting
rill-Palmer Quarterly, 53, 291–318. doi 10.1353/
are related to lower effortful control in children
mpq.2007.0007
Shy Children in the Classroom: From Research to Educational Practice 155

Arbeau, K. A., Coplan, R. J., & Weeks, M. (2010). Quarterly, 51, 20–41. doi 10.1353/mpq.2005.0004
Shyness, teacher-child relationships, and socio- Coplan, R. J., Bullock, A., Archbell, K., & Bosacki,
emotional adjustment in grade 1. International S. (2015). Preschool teachers’ attitudes, be-
Journal of Behavioral Development, 34, 259–269. liefs, and emotional reactions to young chil-
doi 10.1177/0165025409350959 dren’s peer group behaviors. Early Childhood
Barrett, P., & Turner, C. (2001). Prevention of anxi- Research Quarterly, 30, 117–127. doi 10.1016/j.
ety symptoms in primary school children: Prelim- ecresq.2014.09.005
inary results from a universal school-based trial. Coplan, R. J., & Evans, M. A. (2009). At loss for
British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 40, 399– words? Introduction to the special issue on shy-
410. doi 10.1348/014466501163887 ness and language in childhood. Infant and Child
Booth-LaForce, C., & Oxford, M. (2008). Trajectories Development, 18, 211–215. doi 10.1002/icd.620
of social withdrawal from grades 1 to 6: Predic- Coplan, R. J., & Prakash, K. (2003). Spending time
tion from early parenting, attachment, and tem- with teachers: Characteristics of preschool-
perament. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1298– ers who frequently elicit versus initiate inter-
1313. doi 10.1037/a0012954 actions with teachers. Early Childhood Re-
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1997). Ecological models of hu- search Quarterly, 18, 143–158. doi 10.1016/
man development. In M. Gauvain & M. Cole, S0885-2006(03)00009-7
eds., Readings on the development of children. Coplan, R. J., Prakash, K., O’Neil, K., & Armer, M.
New York, NY: Macmillan. (2004). Do you “want” to play? Distinguishing be-
Brophy, J. E., & Good, T. L. (1974). Teacher-student tween conflicted shyness and social disinterest in
relationships. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 40,
Winston. 244–258. doi 10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.244
Cappe, R. F., & Alden, L. E. (1986). A comparison of Coplan, R. J., Rose-Krasnor, L., Weeks, M., Kings-
treatment strategies for clients functionally im- bury, A., Kingsbury, M., & Bullock, A. (2013).
paired by extreme shyness and social avoidance. Alone is a crowd: Social motivations, social with-
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, drawal, and socio-emotional functioning in later
54, 796 – 801. doi 10.1037/0022-006X.54.6.796 childhood. Developmental Psychology, 49, 861–
Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Pastorelli, C., 875. doi 10.1037/a0028861
Bandura, A., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2000). Proso- Coplan, R. J., Schneider, B. H., Matheson, A., & Gra-
cial foundations of children’s academic achieve- ham, A. A. (2010). “Play skills” for shy children:
ment. Psychological Science, 11, 302–306. doi Development of a social skills-facilitated play
10.1111/1467-9280.00260 early intervention program for extremely inhib-
Chen, X. (2010). Shyness-inhibition in childhood and ited preschoolers. Infant and Child Development,
adolescence: A cross-cultural perspective. In K. 19, 223–237.
H. Rubin & R. J. Coplan, eds., The development Coplan, R. J., & Weeks, M. (2009). Shy and soft-
of shyness and social withdrawal (pp. 213–235). spoken? Shyness, pragmatic language, and so-
New York, NY: Guilford Press. cioemotional adjustment in early childhood. In-
Chen, X., Cen, G., Li, D., & He, Y. (2005). Social func- fant and Child Development, 18, 238–254. doi
tioning and adjustment in Chinese children: The 10.1002/icd.622
imprint of historical time. Child Development, 76, Crozier, W. R. (1995). Shyness and self-esteem in
182–195. doi 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00838.x middle childhood. British Journal of Educa-
Chen, X., DeSouza, A., Chen, H., & Wang, L. (2006). tional Psychology, 65, 85–95. doi 10.1111/j.2044-
Reticent behavior and experiences in peer in- 8279.1995.tb01133.x
teractions in Chinese and Canadian children. Crozier, W. R., & Hostettler, K. (2003). The influence
Developmental Psychology, 42, 656–665. doi of shyness on children’s test performance. Brit-
10.1037/0012-1649.42.4.656 ish Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 317–
Coplan, R. J., & Arbeau, K. A. (2008). The stresses 328. doi 10.1348/000709903322275858
of a “brave new world”: Shyness and school ad- Crozier, W. R., & Perkins, P. (2002). Shyness
justment in kindergarten. Journal of Research as a factor when assessing children. Educa-
in Childhood Education, 22, 377–389. doi tional Psychology in Practice, 18, 239–244. doi
10.1080/02568540809594634 10.1080/0266736022000010267
Coplan, R. J., & Armer, M. (2005). ‘Talking yourself Curby, T. W., Rudasill, K., Edwards, T., & Pérez-
out of being shy’: Shyness, expressive vocabulary, Edgar, K. (2011). The role of classroom quality
and adjustment in preschool. Merrill-Palmer in ameliorating the academic and social risks
156 Kalutskaya et al. in Translational Issues in Psychological Science 1 (2015)

associated with difficult temperament. School of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 893–
Psychology Quarterly, 26, 175–188. doi 10.1037/ 903. doi 10.1037/0022-006X.60.6.893
a0023042 Hughes, K., & Coplan, R. J. (2010). Exploring pro-
Dobbs, J., & Arnold, D. H. (2009). Relationship be- cesses linking shyness and academic achieve-
tween preschool teachers’ reports of children’s ment in childhood. School Psychology Quarterly,
behavior and their behavior toward those chil- 25, 213–222. doi 10.1037/a0022070
dren. School Psychology Quarterly, 24, 95–105. Jussim, L., & Harber, K. D. (2005). Teacher expec-
doi 10.1037/a0016157 tations and self-fulfilling prophecies: Knows and
Evans, M. A. (2001). Shyness in the classroom and unknowns, resolved and unresolved controver-
home. In W. R. Crozier & L. E. Alden, eds., Inter- sies. Personality and Social Psychology Review,
national handbook of social anxiety: Concepts, re- 9, 131–155. doi 10.1207/s15327957pspr0902_3
search, and interventions relating to the self and Kagan, J. (1997). Temperament and the reactions to
shyness (pp. 159–183). Westport, CT: Wiley. the unfamiliarity. Child Development, 68, 139–
Evans, M. A. (2010). Language performance, aca- 143. doi 10.2307/1131931
demic performance and signs of shyness: A com- Karevold, E., Ystrøm, E., Coplan, R. J., Sanson, A.,
prehensive review. In K. H. Rubin & R. J. Co- & Mathiesen, K. S. (2012). A prospective longi-
plan, eds., The development of shyness and social tudinal study of shyness from infancy to adoles-
withdrawal (pp. 179–212). New York, NY: Guil- cence: Stability, age-related changes, and pre-
ford Press. diction of socio-emotional functioning. Journal
Evans, M. A., & Bienert, H. (1992). Control and par- of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 1167–1177.
adox in teacher conversations with shy children. doi 10.1007/s10802-012-9635-6
Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Re- Leary, M. A. (2001). Shyness and the self: Atten-
vue canadienne des sciences du comportement, tional, motivational, and cognitive self-processes
24, 502–516. doi 10.1037/h0078758 in social anxiety and inhibition. In W. R. Crozier
Gazelle, H. (2006). Class climate moderates the peer & L. E. Alden, eds., International handbook of so-
relations and emotional adjustment of children cial anxiety. Concepts, research and interventions
with an early history of anxious solitude: A child relating to the self and shyness (pp. 217–234).
! environment model. Developmental Psychology, New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
42, 1179 –1192. doi 10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1179 Leary, M. R., & Kowalski, R. M. (1995). Social anx-
Gazelle, H. H., & Ladd, G. W. (2003). Anxious sol- iety. New York, NY: Guilford.
itude and peer exclusion: A diathesis–stress Lengua, L. J., Honorado, E., & Bush, N. R. (2007).
model of internalizing trajectories in child- Contextual risk and parenting as predictors of
hood. Child Development, 74, 257–278. doi effortful control and social competence in pre-
10.1111/1467-8624.00534 school children. Journal of Applied Develop-
Goldsmith, H. H., Buss, A. H., Plomin, R., Rothbart, mental Psychology, 28(1), 40–55. doi 10.1016/j.
M. K., Thomas, A., Chess, S., … McCall, R. B. appdev.2006.10.001
(1987). Roundtable: What is temperament? Four Markovic, A., & Bowker, J. C. (2014). Shy but funny?
approaches. Child Development, 58, 505–529. doi Examining peer-valued characteristics as moder-
10.2307/1130527 ators between anxious-withdrawal and peer out-
Gordon, E. M., & Thomas, A. (1967). Children’s be- comes during early adolescence. Journal of Youth
havioral style and the teacher’s appraisal of and Adolescence. Advance online publication. doi
their intelligence. Journal of School Psychology, 10.1007/s10964-014-0113-z
5, 292–300. doi 10.1016/0022-4405(67)90006-4 McKinney, J., Mason, J., Perkerson, K., & Clifford,
Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2001). Early M. (1975). Relationship between classroom be-
teacher–child relationships and the trajectory havior and academic achievement. Journal
of children’s school outcomes through eighth of Educational Psychology, 67, 198–203. doi
grade. Child Development, 72, 625– 638. doi 10.1037/h0077012
10.1111/1467-8624.00301 Niehaus, K., Rudasill, K. M., & Rakes, C. R. (2012).
Henderson, H. A., & Fox, N. A. (1998). Inhibited and A longitudinal study of school connectedness and
uninhibited children: Challenges in school set- academic outcomes across sixth grade. Journal
tings. School Psychology Review, 27, 492–505. of School Psychology, 50, 443– 460. doi 10.1016/j.
Hinshaw, S. P. (1992). Academic underachieve- jsp.2012.03.002
ment, attention deficits, and aggression: Comor- O’Connor, E. E., Cappella, E., McCormick, M. P.,
bidity and implications for intervention. Journal & McClowry, S. G. (in press). Enhancing the
Shy Children in the Classroom: From Research to Educational Practice 157

academic development of shy children: A test Rudasill, K. M., & Konold, T. R. (2008). Contri-
of the efficacy of INSIGHTS. School Psychology butions of children’s temperament to teach-
Review. ers’ judgments of social competence from
Oh, W., Rubin, K. H., Bowker, J. C., Booth-LaForce, kindergarten through second grade. Early Ed-
C., Rose-Krasnor, L., & Laursen, B. (2008). Tra- ucation and Development, 19, 643–666. doi
jectories of social withdrawal from middle child- 10.1080/10409280802231096
hood to early adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Rudasill, K. M., Niehaus, K., Buhs, E. S., & White,
Child Psychology, 36, 553–566. doi 10.1007/ J. M. (2013). Temperament in early childhood
s10802-007-9199-z and peer interactions in third grade: The role of
Rapee, R. M., Kennedy, S., Ingram, M., Edwards, teacher-child relationships in early elementary
S., & Sweeny, L. (2005). Prevention and early in- grades. Journal of School Psychology, 51, 701–
tervention of anxiety disorders in inhibited pre- 716. doi 10.1016/j.jsp.2013.08.002
school children. Journal of Consulting and Clin- Rudasill, K. M., Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Justice, L.
ical Psychology, 73, 488–497. M., & Pence, K. (2006). Temperament and lan-
Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., & Kagan, J. (2005). Infant guage skills as predictors of teacher-child rela-
predictors of kindergarten behavior: The contri- tionship quality in preschool. Early Education
bution of inhibited and uninhibited temperament and Development, 17, 271–291. doi 10.1207/
types. Behavioral Disorders, 30, 331–347. s15566935eed1702_4
Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., & Pianta, R. C. (2000). An Rudasill, K. M., & Rimm-Kaufman, S. E. (2009).
ecological perspective on the transition to kin- Teacher-child relationships quality: The role of
dergarten: A theoretical framework to guide child temperament and teacher-child interac-
empirical research. Journal of Applied Devel- tions. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 24,
opmental Psychology, 21, 491–511. doi 10.1016/ 107–120. doi 10.1016/j.ecresq.2008.12 .003
S0193-3973(00)00051-4 Shiner, R. L., Buss, K. A., McClowry, S. G., Putnam,
Roberts, B. W., & DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The S. P., Saudino, K. J., & Zentner, M. (2012). What
rank-order consistency of personality traits is temperament now? Assessing progress in tem-
from childhood to old age: A quantitative review perament research on the twenty-fifth anniver-
of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, sary of goldsmith et al. Child Development Per-
126, 3–25. doi 10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.3 spectives, 6, 436–444.
Rubin, K. H., Coplan, R. J., & Bowker, J. (2009). Shortt, A. L., Barrett, P. M., & Fox, T. L. (2001).
Social withdrawal in childhood. Annual Review Evaluating the FRIENDS program: A cogni-
of Psychology, 60, 11.1.133–11. doi 10.1146/an- tive-behavioral group treatment for anxious
nurev.psych.60.110707.163642 children and their parents. Journal of Clini-
Rudasill, K. M. (2011). Child temperament, teacher- cal Child Psychology, 30, 525–535. doi 10.1207/
child interactions, and teacher-child relation- S15374424JCCP3004_09
ships: A longitudinal investigation from first to
third grade. Early Childhood Research Quarterly,
26, 147–156. doi 10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.07.002

You might also like