You are on page 1of 12

The Perceived Competence Scale for Children

Author(s): Susan Harter


Source: Child Development, Vol. 53, No. 1 (Feb., 1982), pp. 87-97
Published by: Wiley on behalf of the Society for Research in Child Development
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1129640 .
Accessed: 05/10/2013 03:21

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Wiley and Society for Research in Child Development are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and
extend access to Child Development.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 131.252.96.28 on Sat, 5 Oct 2013 03:21:06 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
The Perceived Competence Scale for Children

Susan Harter
University of Denver

HARTER,SUSAN. The Perceived Competence Scale for Children. CmILD DEVELOPMENT, 1982,
53, 87-97. A new self-report instrument, the Perceived Competence Scale for Children, is
described.Emphasisis placed on the assessmentof a child's sense of competenceacrossdifferent
domains, instead of viewing perceived competence as a unitary construct. 3 domains of compe-
tence, each constituting a separate subscale, were identified: (a) cognitive, (b) social, and (c)
physical. A fourth subscale, general self-worth, independentof any particularskill domain, was
included. A new question format was devised which provides a broaderrange of responsesand
reduces the tendency to give socially desirable responses. The psychometricpropertiesof the
scale are presented for third through ninth grades. Emphasisis placed on its factorial validity.
Each subscale defines a separate factor, indicating that children make clear differentiations
among these domains.The factor structureis extremelystable acrossthis grade range. The scale
is viewed as an alternative to those existing self-concept measures of questionable validity
and reliability.

Concepts such as "self-esteem," "self-con- at the conceptual level and therefore do not
cept," and "perceived competence" have be- point to any clear operational definition. Nor
come central to formulations emerging from is any rationale provided for the potpourri of
social learning theory (Bandura 1977), self- empirical referents selected. Items on such pop-
perception theory (Bem 1972), social cognition ular scales as the Coopersmith Self-Esteem In-
(Lepper & Greene 1978), and theories of com- ventory (1967) and the Piers-Harris Self-Con-
petence and intrinsic motivation (de Charms cept Scale (Piers 1969; Piers & Harris 1964)
1968; Deci 1975; Harter 1978, 1981a, 1981b; tap a range of diverse content including cog-
White 1959, 1963). At a more applied level, nitive competencies, physical skills, popularity,
the assessment and enhancement of an indi- acceptance by parent, morality, personality
vidual's self-esteem is critical to clinicians, edu- traits, physical characteristics, and affective re-
cators and program-evaluation researchers. actions. Responses to these heterogeneous items
are then summed, and the total score is inter-
However, there are serious problems with
those instruments designed to tap these self- preted as an index of global self-regard. In em-
evaluative processes (see critiques by Harter, ploying such a procedure, Coopersmith has
assumed that children do not make distinctions
in press a; Robinson & Shaver 1973; Wylie
among the domains in their lives. This assump-
1974, 1979). Typically, constructs such as self- tion was seriously questioned in the scale-con-
concept and self-esteem are vaguely defined struction efforts reported here.
This researchwas supportedby grant no. HD-09613, from the National Institute of Child
Health and Human Development, Department of Health and Human Services, U.S. Public
Health Service. I would like to acknowledgethe extensive cooperationof both the school
per-
sonnel and pupils from the following school systems, without whose assistancethis scale could
not have been constructed:Cherry Creek Public School
System, Denver Public School System,
Jefferson County Public School System; and Colorado Academy, all in Colorado; Ventura School
District in Ventura, California; Killingworth Public School System and
Wightwood School, in
Connecticut; Rochester Public Schools, in conjunctionwith the research team led by Edward
Deci at the University of Rochester.I would also like to thank the many research
our group who worked extensively on the development of the scale. personnelin
These included Carole
Efron, Jim Connell, Karen John, Jeff Baarstad, Barbara Minton, Duane Buhrmester, and Cris
Chandler, each of whom contributed valuable input into the various revisions of the scale and
assisted in administering the instrument to numerous children.
Special thanks go to Claire Kot-
tenstette for her many hours of coding and keypunching the data.
debted to Bob Engstrom,whose application of his statistical Finally, we are deeply in-
expertise to the data analysis was
ultimately responsible for making the scale both empirically and conceptually sound. A manual
is availablefor $9.95. Author'saddress: Departmentof Psychology,
University of Denver, Uni-
versity Park, Denver, Colorado80208.
[Child Development, 1982, 53, 87-97. @ 1982 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
All rights reserved. 0009-3920/82/5301-0008$01.00]

This content downloaded from 131.252.96.28 on Sat, 5 Oct 2013 03:21:06 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
88 Child Development
One dimension of self-evaluation, per- According to such a model, judgments
ceived competence, was selected, given my concerning one's overall self-worth are not in-
theoretical interest in developing a model of ferred from the summation of responses to
competence motivation (see Harter 1978, items tapping a wide array of specific abilities
1981a). The emphasis was on the child's per- and attributes; rather, they are tapped by
ception of his or her competence. The relation- items which directly inquire into how much
ship between this perception and one's actual the individual likes himself or herself as a per-
competence was viewed as an empirically in- son. Thus, in order to assess these perceptions
teresting question in and of itself. in children, we included a fourth subscale,
The model underlying the construction of general self-worth. Items on this subscale make
reference to being sure of oneself, being happy
the Perceived Competence Scale for Children
with the way one is, feeling good about the
hypothesizes that children do not feel equally
way one acts, and thinking that one is a good
competent in every skill domain. We first
person.
sought to specify the major competence do-
mains which are relevant to elementary school In addition to these theoretical consider-
children. The skills represented by items on ations, there are certain psychometric problems
existing scales included scholastic performance, which have plagued existing measures. Many
peer relationships, and a variety of physical scale constructors initially include items or sub-
skills. These competencies were consistent with scales designed to tap different domains of the
our own observations of the mastery behaviors child's life; however, the emerging factor pat-
most salient for elementary school children, tern does not provide clear support for the
and thus they provided an initial framework. subscale structure built into the measure. Some
Interviews with children were then designed
investigators have continued to treat the sub-
to determine which activities within these do- scales as psychologically meaningful. Others
mains were particularly important to children have concluded that elementary school children
in making judgments of competence.
simply do not make evaluative distinctions
Three separate competence subscales among domains. The former strategy is not
emerged: (a) cognitive competence, with an justified on psychometric grounds. Nor is the
latter conclusion warranted, since an inappro-
emphasis on academic performance (doing well
at schoolwork, being smart, feeling good about priate designation of domains and/or poor item
one's classroom performance); (b) social com- selection may be responsible for the failure to
demonstrate that children do indeed make dif-
petence, vis-A-vis one's peers (having a lot of
friends, being easy to like, and being an im- ferentiations. The scale construction efforts re-
portant member of one's class); and (c) physi- ported here were initially directed toward es-
cal competence, with a focus on sports and tablishing an instrument which met the criteria
outdoor games (doing well at sports, learning for the factorial validity of the subscale struc-
new outdoor games readily, and preferring to ture and for acceptable subscale reliabilities.
play sports rather than merely watch others Furthermore, it was essential to demonstrate
that the factor structure remained stable across
play).
the grade levels for which it was initially in-
It was also hypothesized that children
tended, third through sixth grades.. Subsequent-
(age 8 and older) not only make discrete judg- ly, we began to examine the convergent con-
ments about their competence in different do- struct and discriminant validity of the instru-
mains, but that by this age they have also con- ment, as well as its appropriateness for an
structed a view of their general self-worth as older sample of junior high school pupils.
a person, over and above these specific com-
petence judgments. This assumption highlights Finally, the typical response format on
the hierarchical nature of the self-evaluative extant measures (e.g., true-false, like me-unlike
process whereby self-esteem or self-worth is me) also presents problems. Our exploratory
viewed as a superordinate construct and com- work with Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory
petence judgments represent one type of lower- reveals that this type of two-choice format
order evaluative dimension. (See Epstein pulls for socially desirable responding. Specifi-
[1973]; Harter [in press]; Rosenberg [1979], cally, the self-esteem score from this instru-
each of whom supports this general position; ment correlates positively and significantly with
also see Harter [in press] for the developmen- both the lie scale embedded in this measure
tal rationale which places the emergence of as well as with scores on the Children's Social
this type of self-theory around the age of 8.) Desirability Scale (Crandall, Crandall, & Kat-

This content downloaded from 131.252.96.28 on Sat, 5 Oct 2013 03:21:06 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Susan Harter 89
kovsky 1965). Thus a major purpose in con- Experimenters
structing the present scale was to design a new Scale administration in Colorado and Cali-
question format which would offset the ten- fornia has been conducted by me and a team
dency for children to give socially desirable of research staff and graduate students, com-
responses. prised of three men and six women. Data
from Connecticut and New York were collected
The primary goal, then, was to devise an
instrument which (a) provided a profile of the by colleagues and their research staff.
child's perceived competence in the cognitive, QuestionFormat
social, and physical domains; (b) tapped the A "structure alternative format" was de-
child's sense of general self-worth; (c) revealed signed to offset the tendency to give socially
a sound factor structure indicating that these desirable responses. The child is presented with
dimensions were psychologically meaningful; the type of question shown at the bottom of
and (d) minimized the influence of social de- this page.
sirability response tendencies. The child is first asked to decide which
A secondary goal was to examine the re- kind of kid he or she is most like--the kids
lationship between pupils' perceived compe- described on the right or the left. Once having
tence and their teachers' ratings of their actual made this decision, the child decides whether
competence. Thus a parallel teacher-rating the description on that side is sort of true or
scale was also constructed. This was an ex- really true for him or her. Each item is scored
ploratory attempt to determine the extent -to from 1 to 4, where a score of 1 indicates low
which the factor pattern and subscale inter- perceived competence and a score of 4 reflects
correlations for teacher ratings were compara- high perceived competence. Scores are summed
ble to the pupil ratings. Since teachers are and then averaged for each subscale, resulting
most familiar with the child's scholastic per- in four separate subscale means.
formance, their ratings also represented a pos-
sible test of the convergent validity of the The effectiveness of this question format
measure in the cognitive domain. lies in the implication that half of the children
in the world (or in one's reference group)
view themselves in one way, whereas the other
Method half view themselves in the opposite manner.
Subjects That is, this type of question legitimizes either
Earlier versions of the scale were individ- choice. The option of checking either "sort of
ually administered to approximately 300 third- true for me" or "really true for me" also broad-
through sixth-grade school children in Colo- ens the range of choices over the typical two-
rado. The factorial validity of the scale was choice format. In addition, none of the choices
then demonstrated with a sample of' 133 9- involves the response "false." Rather, the child
12-year-old children from California, to whom must decide which option is most true for him
the scale was group administered. Replications or her. Several sources of evidence bear on the
were then conducted on the following sam- effectiveness of this format. The correlation
ples: (a) a combined Connecticut-California between perceived competence ratings and
sample of 341 third through sixth graders, (b) scores on the Children's Social Desirability
a New York sample of 714 third through sixth Scale (Crandall et al. 1965) is .09, whereas
graders, (c) three separate Colorado samples the Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory corre-
in this same age range totaling 470 subjects, lates .33 with the Crandall scale. Inspection of
and (d) a California sample of 746 subjects individual protocols reveals that children utilize
from third through ninth grade. These samples the entire range of scores, including the ex-
have been drawn from primarily middle- and treme designating the lowest perceived com-
upper-middle-class populations. For every sam- petence. The item standard deviations, pre-
ple, there was approximately the same number sented in table 1, also reflect this variability.
of boys and girls at each grade level. Furthermore, although the rating scale is ordi-

Really Sort of Sort of Really


true true true true
for me for me for me for me
Some kids often Other kids can
' '-1 forget what they
learn
but remember things
easily
O D

This content downloaded from 131.252.96.28 on Sat, 5 Oct 2013 03:21:06 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
90 Child Development
nal, both item and subscale score distributions and in the remaining 14, low competence is
are extremely normal, making parametric sta- described first. Within each subscale, three
tistical analyses appropriate. items are keyed in one direction, four in the
Item Constructionand Scale Revision other. With regard to item order, no two con-
From the outset, four domains--cognitive, secutive items are from the same subscale, and
no more than two consecutive items are keyed
social, physical, and general-were designated.
Face validity and meaningfulness to children, in the same direction.
as determined from individual interviews, Teacher-RatingScale
served as initial guidelines. Some items were A 28-item teacher-rating form parallels
adapted from existing scales. The cognitive- the child scale. Items were reworded to obtain
competence domain included school as well as the teacher's best judgment of the child's
nonschool, social domain referred primarily to actual competence. Thus a teacher item would
peers but also included items referring to read, "This kid often forgets what he or she
"people," and the physical domain included learns, but this kid remembers things easily."
sports as well as skills at making and building The same four-choice question format and
things. After initial item revisions, based on scoring procedure was emiployed.
feedback from individual children, a 40-item
version, 10 items per subscale, was group ad- Results
ministered to a sample of 215 third through
sixth graders. Factor analysis indicated that a FactorialValidity
four-factor solution was the most appropriate, Both orthogonal and oblique solutions
in terms of both statistical criteria (Cattell's were obtained, each revealing the same stable
scree test) and interpretability. However, only factor structure. The factor pattern1 from the
six to seven items on each subscale met all of oblique (Promax) rotation is presented. Al-
the criteria: (a) moderate to high loadings on though it was assumed that individuals would
the designated factor, (b) no cross loadings of show differences across the four subscales, it
the same magnitude, (c) mean value near the was also anticipated that there would be some
midpoint, (d) sufficient variability (SD at or correlation among subscale scores. Thus an
near 1), and (e) contribution to the internal oblique solution, which allows the factors to
consistency of the subscale. intercorrelate, was considered to be the most
The content of items not meeting these appropriate. Cattell's scree test, which employs
criteria included liking to talk in front of class, criteria based on the magnitude of the eigen-
values, indicated that four factors should be
thinking one's ideas were good, worrying about extracted.
what people think, knowing what to say to
people, liking to build things, being good at The factor pattern for the combined Con-
fixing things, thinking one was good looking, necticut-California sample of 341 pupils is pre-
and feeling that one would grow up to be im- sented in table 1. There it can be seen that
portant. These items seemed to be overly gen- items have moderate to high loadings on their
eral and therefore vague, or they tapped skills designated factor and that with one exception
that went beyond the content of the majority in this sample they do not cross-load on other
of items that defined a given factor. For exam- factors. This factor pattern has been found
ple, they tapped nonschool cognitive compe- to be extremely stable and has been replicated
tence, social interaction with adults, and physi- in five additional samples. In any given sample,
cal skills involving crafts and fine-motor skills. between one and four moderately low cross-
Thus it was decided to define cognitive as loadings have been obtained; however, par-
school competence, social as peer related, and ticular items do not systematically cross-load.
physical as skill at sports and outdoor games. To give an example of the replicability of
Several new items were written, and an eight-
the factor pattern, for the New York sample
item per subscale revision was then adminis-
the average loadings of items on their desig-
tered to a new sample of 133 children. Anal- nated factors were .67, .61, .64, and .50 for
yses revealed that seven items on each sub- the cognitive, social, physical, and general sub-
scale met the criteria outlined above, resulting
in the final 28-item scale. scales, respectively. (Three items, one physi-
cal and two general, cross-loaded on other fac-
In 14 of the items, the first part of the tors with loadings of .32, .36, and .37. How-
statement reflects high perceived competence, ever, since items which cross-load vary from
1 For those more familiar with orthogonalrotations,these are available on request.

This content downloaded from 131.252.96.28 on Sat, 5 Oct 2013 03:21:06 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Susan Harter 91

sample to sample, they have not been inter- slightly above the midpoint of 2.5, indicating
preted.) no ceiling or floor effects. Standard deviations
fluctuate around the value of 1, revealing ade-
Separate analyses by grade have revealed
that the factor pattern is very stable2 across quate item variability.
grades 3-6. While initially designed for ele- Reliability
mentary school children, it has since been suc- Subscale reliability was assessed by em-
cessfully employed with 293 junior high school ploying coefficient a which provides an index
students in the seventh, eighth, and ninth of internal consistency. For the sample in table
grades. The same factor pattern emerged with 1, these values were .76, .78, .83, and .73, for
average loadings of .57, .45, .53, and .38 for the cognitive, social, physical, and general sub-
items on their designated factors on the cog- scales. Across all samples, reliabilities range
nitive, social, physical, and general subscales, from .75 to .83, .75 to .84, .77 to .86, and .73
respectively. Only one item cross-loaded with to .82, for the four subscales, respectively.
a value of .31.
Test-retest reliability data have been col-
Item Meansand StandardDeviations lected from a sample of 208 Colorado pupils
As can be seen in table 1, item means fall retested after 3 months, and the New York

TABLE 1
FACTOR PATTERN,a ITEM MEANS, AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS FOR A SAMPLE OF
341 PUPILS IN THE THIRD THROUGH SIXTH GRADES

Cognitive Social Physical General Item Item


Item Abbreviation I II III IV X SD

Cognitive competence:
Good at schoolwork........ .64 2.7 .93
Like school, doing well...... .63 2.8 1.02
Just as smart as others...... .67 2.9 1.00
Can figure out answers...... .45 2.8 .93
Finish schoolwork quickly... .50 3.1 .80
Remember things easily..... .42 2.9 1.01
Understand what read...... .69 2.8 .92
Social competence:
Have a lot of friends ....... .61 3.0 .93
Popular with kids .......... .66 3.2 1.00
Easy to like................ .40 2.5 .99
Do things with kids. ....... .60 2.9 1.06
Easy to make friends....... .42 (.43) 2.7 1.07
Important to classmates .... .43 2.8 .98
Most kids like me .......... .49 2.9 .89
Physical competence:
Do well at all sports ........ .79 2.9 1.06
Better at sports ............ .62 2.5 1.20
Do well at new activity..... .45 2.8 .95
Good enough at sports...... .75 2.7 .98
First chosen for games...... .55 3.2 1.03
Play rather than watch ..... .60 2.8 .91
Good at new games ........ .46 2.5 1.06
General self-worth:
Sure of myself ............. .64 2.6 1.12
Happy the way I am ....... .41 3.0 .89
Feel good/way I act........ .55 2.8 .99
Sure am doing right thing... .39 3.0 .85
Am a good person .......... .69 3.0 1.00
Want to stay the same ..... .48 2.9 .93
Do things fine............. .35 2.7 .95
"a
Loadingsless than .27 are not presentedfor the sake of clarity.

2 Congruence coefficients (see Cattell 1966) to assess the correlation between factor load-
ings across samples were calculated between each pair of grades for the California sample, for
each factor separately. For the fourth through ninth grades, these values ranged from .69 to .90.
Third-grade values were slightly lower, from .61 to .83. Over all grade comparisons, the average
congruency coefficients were .83 for cognitive, .74 for social, .80 for physical, and .76 for general.

This content downloaded from 131.252.96.28 on Sat, 5 Oct 2013 03:21:06 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
92 Child Development

sample of 810 pupils retested after 9 months. The pattern of subscale relationships is
These correlations, corrected for attenuation, slightly different from that of the pupils.
were .78, .80, .87, and .70 for the Colorado Teachers view general self-worth as related to
sample, and .78, .75, .80, and .69 for the New both cognitive (r's = .40 and .54 for the two
York sample, for the four subscales. samples) and social (r's = .63 and .50) com-
Subscale Means petence, as do pupils. However, teachers con-
sider general self-worth to be significantly (p
Subscale means for four samples are pre- < .05) less related to physical competence (r's
sented in table 2. These values are highly
= .26 and .17) than do pupils. Teachers are
stable across subscale as well as grade, and
consistent with pupils in viewing social and
this pattern is replicable, with slight fluctu-
ations, across the four samples. Subscale stan- physical as highly related (.33 and .38 for
dard deviations are also very consistent across teachers). However, teachers tend to see cog-
nitive and social competence as more highly
grade and sample, ranging from .55 to .79. related (.43 and .45) than do pupils.
Grade x gender analyses of variance revealed
no significant differences for the cognitive, so- Convergent Validity
cial, and general subscales. However, on the Cognitive domain.-Teachers have consis-
physical subscale males consistently have shown tently said that they felt most confident about
significantly (p < .05) higher scores than have their judgments of cognitive competence. Across
females (e.g., 3.0 vs. 2.5 for the sample in samples, the correlation between pupil ratings
table 1). and teacher ratings is in the .40's. For the Cali-
fornia sample, extending into ninth grade, these
Intercorrelations among Subscales
correlations document a definite age trend. For
Table 3 reveals that while there are sam-
third, fourth, fifth, and sixth grades, they were
ple variations, there are several common pat- .28, .32, .50, and .55, in that order, showing
terns. The correlations of the general self-
a steady increase in magnitude. In the seventh
worth subscale with each of the three compe-
tence subscales consistently tend to be among grade the correlation drops to .31 and then
rises to .66 in the eighth grade and to .73 in
the highest, in the range between .40 and .58.
the ninth grade.
The social and physical subscales are highly
related across samples, within the range of .46 It is interesting that this same pattern is
and .58. The relationship between the cogni- obtained when we examine the relationship
tive subscale and both the social and physical between perceived cognitive competence and
subscales tends to be lower, with some sample standardized achievement test scores from this
variation. Correlations among factors are of a same sample, taken from the Iowa Test of
similar magnitude, for example, general self- Basic Skills. Given the high correlation among
worth correlates with the three competence the percentile scores for reading comprehen-
subscales .42-.53, social and physical correlate sion, language skills, and math skills, the mean
.56, and social and cognitive correlate .43. of these three scores was correlated with the
Teacher Ratings perceived cognitive competence score. For the
Teacher ratings were obtained from 28 third, fourth, fifth, and sixth grades, these
teachers for the California sample and 16 teach- values were .27, .40, .45, and .45, in that order.
ers for the Colorado sample, which represented For the seventh grade this correlation dropped
four teachers per grade. A factor pattern, vir- to .29 and then increased to .44 for the eighth
tually identical to the pupils', resulted with the grade and to .54 in the ninth grade.
following average loadings on the designated Social domain.-In the domain of peer
factor: .84, .74, .83, and .66. One general item friendship, we have examined the relationship
(this child is fine the way he [she] is) cross- between the social subscale score and socio-
loaded substantially on cognitive, and one so- metric standing as defined by the Roster and
cial item (most kids like him [her]) also had Rating Scale developed by Roitascher (1974).
a moderate loading on the general subscale.3 On this latter measure, each child is given
Internal consistency reliabilities for the teach- a class roster and asked to rate each classmate
ers' ratings were .96, .93, .94, and .93 for the in terms of how good a friend that person is,
cognitive, social, physical, and general sub- on a scale from 1 to 5. A child's sociometric
scales. score is derived by averaging the same-sex
3 To examine the
comparabilityin factor patterns,coefficientsrepresentingthe congruence
between teacher and pupil factor loadings were calculated for each factor. These values ranged
from .90 to .97 for elementaryschool and .72 to .88 for the juniorhigh.

This content downloaded from 131.252.96.28 on Sat, 5 Oct 2013 03:21:06 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
oo .. t to

"S0"G : 00

C31
c~ ; : : e' i : : cC; cM

S00 C

.... .
S. . . . . . . .. . .. . . .. .

000

10 10Z: : ' :- : : : ..z


::
o,
E-4 P1V-1 o

z X00 ON
00 0..0.0 . 0w0. 0 . Vw 0 w

0-4

E 0
Cl) . . . . . . . .
Cl)
PC

...
S00 1, 0 00 00 0 0 II
C5 a

~ 00
0l?;s I
? S\000to0?aO tC
t into tot
,v

c7 Cl) *I I I I I I I I

cn . . . . . . .
~0

Cd
0
w 0
c0
o1 o
UI \ ):\ 0,d
Cl ~ $ O~ vv

. . . . . . . . . I.

This content downloaded from 131.252.96.28 on Sat, 5 Oct 2013 03:21:06 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
94 Child Development
TABLE 3 motivational subscales on our measure of in-
SUBSCALE INTERCORRELATIONS
OF PUPILS' RATINGS trinsic versus extrinsic orientation in the class-
FOR THREE SAMPLES OF THIRD THROUGH SIXTH room (Harter 1981b). Perceived cognitive
GRADES AND ONE SAMPLE OF THIRD THROUGH competence is strongly related to preference
NINTHGRADES for challenge (r = .57) and to independent
mastery (r = .54), and it is moderately re-
Cognitive Social Physical lated to curiosity (r = .33). Higher-order fac-
toring reveals that perceived cognitive compe-
California-Connecticut tence, challenge, independent mastery, and
(N = 341):
Social.............. ...26 .. curiosity form a distinct factor with very high
Physical............ .35 .48 loadings of .76, .87, .80, and .79, respectively.
General............ .46 .48 .50
New York (N = 714): In a related behavioral study on prefer-
Social.............. .45 ence for challenge (Harter, Note 2) sixth-grade
Physical............ .35 .58 children were divided into high (top third)
General............ .58 .58 .40 and low (bottom third) perceived cognitive
Colorado(N = 209):
Social.............. .36 competence subgroups and given their choice
Physical............ .36 .57 of anagrams varying in difficulty level, that is,
General............ .46 .52 .52 three-, four-, five-, and six-letter anagrams.
California(N = 746): Those with relatively high perceived compe-
Social.................36 tence scores chose significantly, t(48) = 3.6,
Physical.............29 .46
General............ .49 .53 .44 p < .001, more difficult anagrams to solve (X
number of letters = 5.1) than did those with
NOTE.-All correlationssignificantat p < .001. low perceived competence scores (X = 3.8).
Discriminant Validity
peers' ratings of the child. In a study with 85
fourth, fifth, and sixth graders, Thompson Cognitive domain.-In one study (Baar-
stad, Note 3) with learning disabled children,
(Note 1) has found the correlation between
this sociometric index and the social subscale support was found for the prediction that while
to be .59. such children should rate their competence
lower than do normal children of the same age
Physical domain.-Although ratings of and grade, this difference will be most pro-
physical competence from regular classroom nounced in the cognitive area. Results indi-
teachers consistently correlate with pupil rat- cated a significant difference, t (38) = 2.9, p
ings between the narrow range of .41 and .43 < .005, for the perceived cognitive competence
across samples, these ratings have not been ratings (X = 1.9 for the 20 learning-disabled
used as a primary index of validity since teach- children compared with 2.8 for the 20 normal
ers report very little confidence in these judg- children). While social, physical, and general
ments. Thus, for one sample of 209 third self-worth ratings were also lower for the learn-
through sixth graders in Colorado, the gym ing disabled, these differences did not reach
teacher rated each pupil on his or her physical acceptable levels of significance.
competence. These ratings were made in Feb- Social and physical domain.-For one
ruary, after the teacher had at least 6 months sixth-grade sample in which athletic achieve-
to observe the children's skills. The correlations ment was a prominent school value, the hy-
between the gym teacher's rating and pupils'
pothesis was tested that those pupils selected for
ratings was .62 and showed negligible vari- the sports teams (N = 23) would score higher
ations across the four grades sampled. on perceived physical and social competence
Construct Validity than would their classmates (N = 57). Physi-
One purpose for devising this instrument cal and social scores for the sports group were
was to test certain hypotheses in my model of 3.4 and 3.2, compared with the means of their
classmates, 2.5 and 2.7, respectively, t(78) =
competence motivation. One such hypothesis 3.4 and 2.5, p < .001 and p < .01, respec-
is that perceived competence should be posi-
tively related to ones intrinsic motivational tively.
orientation to prefer challenge, to be curious, Discussion
and to engage in independent mastery attempts
(Harter 1978). Clear support for this predic- The findings indicate that the goal of con-
tion comes from the correlations between per- structing a self-report measure which provides
ceived cognitive competence and the three a profile of the child's perceived competence

This content downloaded from 131.252.96.28 on Sat, 5 Oct 2013 03:21:06 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Susan Harter 95
and general self-worth has been adequately ing suggests that at this particular IQ and
achieved. The very clear and stable factor mental age level, a child does not make the
structure reveals that children as young as 8 type of abstract evaluation of self as is tapped
make meaningful differentiations among the by our general self-worth items. In an inde-
four areas tapped by the scale: cognitive com- pendent effort (Harter & Pike, Note 5) to con-
petence in school, social competence with struct a downward extension of perceived com-
peers, physical competence in sports, and gen- petence scale for 4-7-year-olds, a similar pat-
eral self-worth. The group data, along with tern emerged on this pictorial version in the
the individual profiles, many of which show form of a two-factor solution, competence and
dramatic subscale differences, suggest that those social acceptance. This finding raises the possi-
instruments (e.g., the Coopersmith Self-Esteem bility that the perceived competence construct
Inventory) which yield a single score are is qualitatively different at different develop-
masking important distinctions which children mental levels.
can make about their competence in different
For normal samples, subscale intercorre-
domains. In addition, the new question format
lations among the three competence domains
not only provides a four-point ordinal index
of a child's feelings of competence and self- reveaY that the largest correlation is between
the social and physical subscales. This pattern
worth but also functions effectively to reduce
the child's tendency to give socially desirable suggests that one's popularity during the ele-
mentary and junior high school years, as tapped
responses.
by the social subscale, is dependent to a con-
siderable degree on one's prowess at athletics.
Although the perceived competence scale
was originally designed for use with elemen- One's academic performance bears less of a rela-
tary school children, our experience with junior tionship to one's popularity or one's physical
high school pupils has demonstrated that this skills. The teachers' ratings are in agreement
scale can be employed at these grade levels. with the pupils' in recognizing that one's peer
The factor pattern and subscale reliabilities popularity is very directly related to the pupil's
are highly stable across the third through ninth skill in sports. We focus on "popularity" here
grades. The items seem to tap concerns which since we have begun to question whether the
are central throughout this broad age range, social scale actually assesses competence in the
sense of social skills or whether it simply taps
namely, doing well at school, having friends,
competence in sports, and liking the kind of a peer friendship dimension which may be
person one is. It is quite likely, however, that more highly related to factors such as athletic
junior high school pupils make additional dis- prowess than social competence per se. We
tinctions, for example, between same-gender are currently researching this issue (see Thomp-
and opposite-gender friendships. This would son, Note 1).
argue for the construction of a scale specifi- Another issue being pursued is precisely
cally designed to tap critical new domains how the general self-worth scale is interpreted
which emerge during adolescence.
by children. Minton (Note 6) has discovered
Caution should also be taken in the use that the reasons children give for their judg-
of this scale with very different populations or ments on the self-worth items include refer-
ences to personal characteristics such as being
special groups. For example, in a recent dis-
sertation (Silon, Note 4) the factor structure nice, friendly, helpful, "morally" good; to the
of this scale for a group of 126 mentally re- control of anger; to one's relationship with
tarded pupils between the ages of 9 and 12 parents; and to expressed feelings of self-ac-
was examined. The findings revealed a very ceptance, being happy with the way one is.
Children make few references to competence.
interpretable two-factor solution. The first fac- In future work, we will need to address the
tor, which was labeled "competence," drew
issue of whether this subscale actually taps a
primarily from items which were on the orig-
inal cognitive and physical subscales, suggest- superordinate sense of self-worth or whether
it is merely tapping second-order self-evalu-
ing that educable retarded children do not ative dimensions other than competence (see
make a distinction between these two skill
domains. The second factor, which we labeled Harter, in press).
"popularity," was defined by the more concrete While validity data have been reported
social items making reference to the number for the competence subscales, the interpreta-
of friends one had. Finally, there was no evi- tion of concurrent measures such as teacher
dence for a general self-worth factor. This find- ratings and achievement test scores should be

This content downloaded from 131.252.96.28 on Sat, 5 Oct 2013 03:21:06 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
96 Child Development

approached judiciously. This scale is directed It has also been demonstrated that dis-
toward an assessment of the child's perception crepancy scores of normal IQ pupils are pre-
of his or her competence. We view the degree dictive of behavior on cognitive tasks. Bierer
of convergence or discrepancy between a child's (Note 8) found that two groups of children,
perceived competence and an index of actual those who overestimate and those who under-
competence as an interesting empirical ques- estimate their cognitive competence, are more
tion in and of itself. Low correlations between unrealistic than accurate raters in their diffi-
these two indices do not necessarily cast doubt culty level choices.
on the validity of the scale.
These findings may have implications for
The grade differences in the relationship
between perceived competence and both teach- program-evaluation efforts, suggesting that a
er ratings and achievement scores may be in- goal should be to foster a realistic sense of com-
petence rather than enhancement per se. That
terpreted within this context. The relationship is, reduction in the magnitude of discrepancy
between perceived and actual competence rises
scores may be as critical an outcome variable
systematically through the elementary school as a mean increase in the competence score.
years, shows a dramatic drop in the seventh
grade, and then rises again in the eighth and Finally, it should be noted that the present
ninth grades. (Subscale reliabilities, however, scale is restricted to cognitive competence in
show no variation with age.) This cross-sec- school, peer social relationships, and physical
tional pattern suggests that children become competence in sports. It does not tap cognitive
increasingly better able to make realistic judg- competence outside of school, social relation-
ments about their competence during the ele- ships with adults, or the type of physical skills
mentary school years. However, with the ad- required to do crafts, build and fix things, and
vent of junior high-bringing with it a new so on. These are the focuses of a supplemen-
school structure, several different teachers, and tary scale in progress, which may be useful
a new social hierarchy-it may well be that in the evaluation of competencies outside the
those seventh grade adolescents lose their abil- more traditional domains. This type of exten-
ity to make realistic judgments about their cog- sion also guards against the temptation to treat
nitive competence. This interpretation is con- the existing scale as synonymous with the per-
sistent with findings by Connell (Note 7) who ceived construct itself. Like any such construct,
administered his Perceived Control Scale to this it becomes increasingly more differentiated, if
same sample. One subscale on the instrument, one allows it the conceptual and empirical room
unknown source of control, taps the degree to to do so.
which the child indicates that he or she simply
does not know what is responsible for the out-
comes in the cognitive domain. Connell finds Reference Notes
a steady decline in this score from third to
1. Thompson, R. Children's self-evaluation of
sixth grade and then a dramatic increase in
seventh grade, reflecting the fact that seventh peer relations. Unpublished doctoral disserta-
tion, University of Denver, 1981.
graders acknowledge difficulty in understand- 2. Harter, S. Perceived cognitive competence as
ing why things happen to them. They then a determinantof children'spreferencefor chal-
acquire an understanding of these contingen- lenge. Unpublished manuscript,University of
cies during the eighth- and ninth-grade years.
Denver, 1979.
In another study (Silon, Note 4) com- 3. Baarstad, J. Perceived competence and moti-
paring educable retarded children who were vational orientation in normal and learning
mainstreamed with those in self-contained class- disabled children. Unpublishedmaster'sthesis,
rooms, it was found that for the mainstreamed University of Denver, 1978.
group only there was virtually no relationship 4. Silon, E. Perceived competence, anxiety, and
between their perceived and their actual com- motivational orientation in educable retarded
petence, as measured by teacher ratings and children who are mainstreamedcompared to
achievement test scores. One possible interpre- those in self-contained classrooms. Unpub-
tation is that the constant movement back and lished doctoral dissertation,Universityof Den-
forth between classes for retarded pupils and ver, 1980.
classrooms of normal IQ pupils makes it diffi- 5. Harter, S., & Pike, R. The pictorial scale of
cult for mainstreamed children to develop any perceived competence and acceptance for
realistic basis for evaluating their competence, young children. Unpublishedmanuscript,Uni-
given the shifting comparison groups. versity of Denver, 1981.

This content downloaded from 131.252.96.28 on Sat, 5 Oct 2013 03:21:06 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Susan Harter 97
6. Minton, B. Dimensions of informationunder- in children: individual differences and devel-
lying children's judgments of their compe- opmentalchange. Minnesotasymposiaon child
tence. Unpublished master'sthesis, University psychology. Vol. 14. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum,
of Denver, 1979. 1981. (a)
7. Connell, J. P. A multidimensionalmeasure of Harter,S. A new self-reportscale of intrinsicversus
children's perceptionsof control. Unpublished extrinsic orientation in the classroom: moti-
manuscript,University of Denver, 1980. vational and informationalcomponents.Devel-
8. Bierer, B. The relationshipbetween perceived opmental Psychology, 1981, 17, 300-312. (b)
competence and children's difficulty level Harter, S. Developmentalperspectives on the self-
choices. Unpublished master's thesis, Univer- system. In M. Hetherington-(Ed.), Social de-
sity of Denver, 1977. velopment: Carmichael'smanual of child psy-
chology. New York: Wiley, in press.
References Lepper, M. R., & Greene, D. Overjustificationre-
search and beyond: towarda means-endsanal-
Bandura,A. Self-efficacy:toward a unifying theory
of behavioral change. Psychological Review, ysis. In M. R. Lepper & D. Greene (Eds),
The hidden cost of reward. Hillsdale, N.J.:
1977, 84, 191-215.
Bem, D. J. Self-perceptiontheory. In L. Berkowitz Erlbaum, 1978.
(Ed.), Advances in experimentalsocial psy- Piers, E. V. Manual for the Piers-Harrischildren's
chology. Vol. 6. New York: Academic Press, self-concept scale. Nashville, Tenn.: Counselor
1972. Recordings& Tests, 1969.
Cattell, R. B. The meaning and strategicuse of fac- Piers, E. V., & Harris, D. B. Age and other cor-
tor analysis.In R. B. Cattell (Ed.), Handbook relates of self-concept in children. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 1964, 55, 91-95.
of multivariateexperimentalpsychology. Chi-
cago: Rand McNally, 1966. Robinson,J. P., & Shaver, P. R. Measuresof social
Coopersmith, S. The antecedents of self-esteem. psychologicalattitudes. Ann Arbor,Mich.: In-
San Francisco: W. H. Freeman, 1967. stitute for Social Research, 1973.
Crandall, V. C.; Crandall, V. J.; & Katkovsky,W. Roitascher,R. C. A microeconomicmodel of socio-
A children's social desirability questionnaire. metric choice. Sociometry,1974, 37, 219-238.
Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1965, 29, Rosenberg, M. Conceiving the self. New York:
27-36. Basic, 1979.
de Charms,R. Personal causation:the internal af- White, R. Motivationreconsidered:the concept of
fective determinantsof behavior. New York: competence. Psychological Review, 1959, 66,
Academic Press, 1968. 297-323.
Deci, E. L. Intrinsicmotivation.New York:Plenum, White, R. Ego and reality in psychoanalytictheory.
1975. PsychologicalIssues, 1963, 3(3). (Monograph)
Epstein, S. The self-concept revisited or a theory Wylie, R. C. The self-concept. Vol. 1. A review of
of a theory. AmericanPsychologist, 1973, 28, methodological considerationsand measuring
405-416. instruments.Lincoln: University of Nebraska
Harter, S. Effectance motivation reconsidered: Press, 1974.
toward a developmental model. Human De- Wylie, R. C. The self-concept. Vol. 2. Theory and
velopment, 1978, 1, 34-64. research on selected topics. Lincoln: Univer-
Harter,S. A model of intrinsic mastery motivation sity of Nebraska Press, 1979.

This content downloaded from 131.252.96.28 on Sat, 5 Oct 2013 03:21:06 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like