You are on page 1of 15

The Pictorial Scale of Perceived Competence and Social Acceptance for Young Children

Author(s): Susan Harter and Robin Pike


Source: Child Development, Vol. 55, No. 6 (Dec., 1984), pp. 1969-1982
Published by: Wiley on behalf of the Society for Research in Child Development
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1129772 .
Accessed: 09/02/2015 11:52

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Wiley and Society for Research in Child Development are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and
extend access to Child Development.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 193.255.248.150 on Mon, 9 Feb 2015 11:52:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
The Pictorial Scale of Perceived Competence
and Social Acceptance for Young Children

Susan Harter and Robin Pike


University of Denver

HARTER,SUSAN,and PIKE,ROBIN.The Pictorial Scale of Perceived Competence and Social Accep-


tance for Young Children. CHILDDEVELOPMENT,1984, 55, 1969-1982. A new pictorial scale of per-
ceived competence and social acceptance for young children, a downwardextension of the Per-
ceived Competence Scale for Children,is described. There-are2 versions of this instrument,1 for
preschoolers and kindergartnersand a second for first and second graders,each tapping 4 do-
mains: cognitive competence, physical competence, peer acceptance, and maternal acceptance.
Factor analyses reveal a 2-factorsolution. The first factor,general competence, is defined by the
cognitive and physical competence subscales. The second factor,social acceptance,comprises the
peer and maternalacceptancesubscales.The psychometricpropertieswere found to be acceptable.
Weak correlationsbetween children'sand teachers'judgmentsare discussed in terms of the young
child's tendency to confuse the wish to be competentor accepted with reality.It is urged that this
instrumentnot be viewed as a general self-conceptscale but be treatedas a measure of 2 separate
constructs,perceived competence and social acceptance.

Introduction perceptions across these domains. This ap-


Constructs such as self-concept and self- proach is based on the assumption that chil-
esteem have had a long history within the dren do not view themselves as equally ade-
field of psychology, although in recent years quate in all domains; rather, we have assumed
there has been a revival of interest in topics that they are capable of making meaningful
distinctions between different domains.
involving the self and self-description (see
Harter, 1983; Harter, in press). Despite this Support for this assumption has been ob-
interest, relatively little attention has been de- tained in measurement efforts with children
voted to the sensitive measurement of such and adolescents between the ages of 8 and 18.
constructs, particularly in the young child (see In the construction of the Self-Perception
also Wylie, 1979). The present article de- Profile for Children (Harter, Note 1), we dem-
scribes the construction of a new scale de- onstrated that children differentiate the fol-
signed to assess perceived competence and lowing five domains as revealed through
social acceptance in young children, ages 4-7. factor-analytic procedures: scholastic com-
Our conceptual approach to the assess- petence, athletic competence, ,'eer accep-
ment of self-judgments has been domain- tance, physical appearance, and conduct or
behavior. In addition to judgments in these
specific, unlike the frameworks adopted by
other test constructors (e.g., Coopersmith, specific domains, children aged 8 and older
can also make reliable judgments about their
1967; Piers & Harris, 1969), who have sought
to assess self-concept or self-esteem primar- general worth as a person. Thus, the current
structure of the Self-Perception Profile for
ily through the calculation of a single score, older children contains five separate domain-
summing items across diverse domains. In
specific subscales as well as a sixth subscale
contrast, we have sought to assess children's
tapping self-worth.
self-judgments separately within specific
domains in order to provide a profile of self- In more recent efforts to devise a self-

The researchnecessary for the development of this scale was supportedby grantHD-09613
fromthe NationalInstituteof Child Health and HumanDevelopmentof the Departmentof Health,
Education,and Welfare.The authorswould like to acknowledgethe extensive cooperationof both
the school personnel and pupils from the following school systems, without whose assistancethis
scale could not have been constructed:the Cherry Creek, Denver, and JeffersonCounty public
school systems, the Jewish CommunityCenter of Denver, the Evergreen Children's Center, St.
Timothy'sPreschool,WellshirePreschool,and ParkerPreschool.The authorsalso thankthe research
personnel in our group,who workedextensively on the development of the scale, including Carole
Efron,ChristineChao,and Beth Ann Bierer.Reprintsand informationon obtainingmaterialscan be
obtained by writing to Susan Harter, Department of Psychology, 2040 S. York Street, Denver,
Colorado80208.
[ChildDevelopment,1984,55, 1969-1982. ? 1984by the SocietyforResearchin Child Development,Inc.
All rightsreserved.0009-3920/84/5506-0020$01.00]

This content downloaded from 193.255.248.150 on Mon, 9 Feb 2015 11:52:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
1970 Child Development
report instrument for young children, we but more scholastically oriented skills such as
adopted a similar approach in that we sought being able to spell, read, or add are better
to identify meaningful domains in the child's measures of cognitive competence in the first
life and to construct separate subscales for and second grades.
each. We also opted for utilization of a similar
The younger children's instrument also
type of question format that (a) provides a differs from the older version in that it con-
greater range of responses for each item (four tains no self-worth subscale. Both theory (see
choices rather than the more typical two-
choice true/false format) and (b) reduces chil- Harter, 1983) and empirical findings have led
to the conclusion that children are not capable
dren's tendency to give the socially desirable of making judgments about their worth as per-
response (see Harter [1982] for a more com- sons until approximately the age of 8. The
plete description).
very concept of "personness" is not yet firmly
However, in devising a developmentally established among younger children, nor is
appropriate downward extension of the scale the notion that the self, so defined, can be
for 4-7-year-olds, several aspects of our proce- evaluated as a global entity.
dure were different from those used with the
There is another developmental contrast
older children. First, a pictorial format was de-
that involves the degree to which we can ex-
vised rather than a written questionnaire. Ex-
pect children's self-judgments to be accurate.
perience shows that, on questionnaires, young
children's inability to read as well as to under- Developmental frameworks such as those of-
fered by Piagetians or the proponents of
stand the items, coupled with related atten-
tional problems, attenuates both the reliability psychoanalytic theory would alert us that the
and validity of such instruments. In contrast, judgments of the young child may not be real-
istic. That is, young children confuse the wish
the pictorial format engages the young child's
to be competent with reality; they blur the
interest, is understandable, sustains the distinction between their ideal self-image and
child's attention, and leads to more meaning- the real self (Stipek, 1981). Related findings
ful responses.
by Ruble and her colleagues (see Ruble, 1983)
The pictorial format also allows us to de- indicate that it is not until approximately 9
pict skills and specific activities concretely. years of age that children make use of social
Whereas, at older ages, trait labels and general comparison for the purposes of judging their
descriptions of skill or adequacy can be em- own competence. Thus, certain cognitive lim-
ployed-such as terms like smart, popular, itations appear to interfere with the young
athletic, and good-looking-the young child child's ability to make realistic judgments
has not yet acquired these forms of self- about the self.
description (see Harter [1983] for a theoretical Given that young children may not be
discussion of these developmental shifts).
Rather, the young child's self-judgments in- very accurate judges of their competence or
volve the behavioral description of their social acceptance, comparisons of their scores
with objective indexes should not be exam-
specific abilities, such as completing puzzles, ined as an index of the validity of the instru-
running fast, and playing with friends. There- ment. This lack of convergence is an inter-
fore, the graphic presentation of these actions
and activities facilitates the young child's esting finding, in and of itself, one that bears
on the self-descriptive capabilities during this
understanding of the task since these forms of
self-description are developmentally appro- developmental period. Other forms of valid-
ity-such as discriminant, convergent, and
priate.
predictive validity-would appear to be more
Another difference involves the number appropriate, as will be demonstrated.
of versions of the scale required. At older
ages, one version can be utilized across a wide The specific content of the scale to be
range of ages. For the younger ages, however, described involves two general constructs,
it was necessary to devise one version for pre- perceived competence and perceived social
schoolers and kindergartners (4- and 5-year- acceptance. The measure contains two
olds) and a separate version for first and sec- subscales within each of these domains. Per-
ond graders (6- and 7-year-olds). This was ceived competence is divided into two sub-
necessitated by the fact that the specific skills scales, cognitive competence and physical
that define or connote competence and social competence. Social acceptance is divided into
acceptance change rather dramatically within two subscales, peer acceptance and maternal
this 4-year age range. For example, puzzles acceptance. While these particular subscales
may be indicative of cognitive competence appear to define salient domains in the life of
during the preschool and kindergarten years, the young child, obviously there are others,

This content downloaded from 193.255.248.150 on Mon, 9 Feb 2015 11:52:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Harter and Pike 1971
three of which (paternalacceptance, teacher dren were white, with the remaining4% His-
acceptance, and conduct) will appear in sub- panic, black, and Oriental.
sequent versions of this scale. Scale Description
Given the structure of this scale, we Scale structure.-The scale contains four
stronglyurge that the scale not be viewed as separate subscales-cognitive competence,
an index of self-concept or self-esteem per se. physical competence, peer acceptance, and
That is, certainjudgments such as the percep- maternalacceptance. Each subscale contains
tion of one's cognitive or physical competence six items. There are two versions of the scale,
or one's behavior may well reflectjudgments one for preschool-kindergartenand one for
about the self's capabilities. However, per- first and second grades. These two versions
ceptions concerning the degree to which one are not completely unique. Rather,for certain
has friends or obtains supportfromparentsor subscales, there are overlapping or common
teachers do not necessarily imply judgments items across the two versions. Table 1 pro-
about the adequacy of the self. For example, vides a masterlist of all items foreach version,
one may conclude that something about the grouped accordingto subscale. Asterisksnext
self is responsible forone's lack of friends.On to the item number indicated which items are
the other hand, the cause may reside in cer- common to both forms.
tain characteristicsof one's peers; that is, they
As can be seen in Table 1, none of the
are not nice or are not friendly. Similarly,lack
of parentalsupportmight be because one per- items that define the cognitive subscales at
the two developmental levels overlap. The
ceives the self as unlovable, yet, on the other
hand, one may perceive one's parents as un- preschool-kindergartenform contains a num-
ber of rudimentaryreadiness skills (knowing
loving. Thus, the very basis on which chil- colors, the alphabet, being able to count) in
dren make such judgments is an interesting
addition to performanceon puzzles and ob-
empirical issue itself. However, until further
researchhas clarifiedthis issue, we would do taining starson papers.The first-secondgrade
version includes those scholastic skills ini-
well not to assume that all of these seeming
tially encountered in the early primarygrades
self-judgments are based on characteristics (reading,writing, and arithmetic).
that reside in the self. For this reasonwe have
urged that the scale be treatedas a measureof For the domain of physical skills, four
what the title indicates, perceived compe- items occur on both versions (swinging,
tence and perceived social acceptance, rather climbing, skipping, and running).Two of the
than treatingit as a singularmeasure of "self- preschool-kindergartenskills (tying shoes and
concept" or "self-esteem." hopping), however, are replaced by more ad-
vanced physical skills for the first-second
Method grade version (bouncing a ball and jumping
This particularinstrumenthas undergone rope).
numerous revisions in terms of scale struc- Within the domain of peer acceptance,
ture, item content, and question format,based four of the items involving friends are com-
on extensive piloting with large numbers of mon acrossthe two versions. Two of the items
subjects. In this article, we will restrict our on the preschool-kindergartenversion (stay-
description to the final version of the four- ing overnight and eating at friends' houses)
subscale instrument. are replaced at the first-secondgrade level by
others sharing toys and others sitting next to
Subjects you. These particularsocial overtures in the
Subjectswere 90 preschoolers(mean age early primarygrades would appear to be im-
= 4.45), 56 kindergartners (mean age = 5.54),
65 firstgraders(mean age = 6.32), and 44 sec- portantindexes of popularity.
ond graders(meanage = 7.41), approximately For the domain of maternalacceptance,
equally divided by gender within each group. there are four activities or maternalbehaviors
These samples provided the primarydata for in common across the two age-graded ver-
the factor analyses, means, standard devia- sions (Mom takes you places you like, cooks
tions, internalconsistency reliabilitydata,and "yourfavoritefoods, reads to you, and talks to
subscales intercorrelationsreported. For an you). Two preschool-kindergartenitems drop
additional sample of 77 preschoolers,28 kin- out (Momsmiles and Momtalksto you) forthe
dergartners,and 38 first and second graders, first-secondgradeversion and are replacedby
scores for both self-reportand teacher ratings Mom lets you eat at friends' and stay over-
were available. All subjects were drawn from night. These maternalacceptance items were
schools in middle-class neighborhoods. In generated from a list of the most commonly
terms of ethnic composition, 96%of the chil- mentioned behaviorsby young childrenin re-

This content downloaded from 193.255.248.150 on Mon, 9 Feb 2015 11:52:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
1972 Child Development
TABLE 1
ITEMS GROUPED ACCORDINGTO SUBSCALEFOR EACH FORM

Subscale and Item No. Preschool-Kindergarten First-Second Grades


Cognitive competence:
1 ................... Good at puzzles Good at numbers
5 .................... Gets stars on paper Knowsa lot in school
9 .................... Knows names of colors Can read alone
13 .................... Good at counting Can write words
17 ..................... Knows alphabet Good at spelling
21 ................ ..... Knows first letter of name Good at adding
Physical competence:
3 ..................... Good at swinging Good at swinging
7* ..................... Good at climbing Good at climbing
11 ................... Can tie shoes Good at bouncing ball
15* .................. Good at skipping Good at skipping
19* ..................... Good at running Good at running
23 ................... Good at hopping Good at jump-roping
Peer acceptance:
2* .................. Has lots of friends Has lots of friends
6 ................... Stays overnight at friends' Others share their toys
10* .................. .. Has friends to play with Has friends to play with
14* ................. .. Has friends on playground Has friends on playground
18* ..................... Gets asked to play with others Gets asked to play with others
22 ..................... Eats dinner at friends' house Others sit next to you
Maternalacceptance:
4 ................... Mom smiles Mom lets you eat at friends'
8* .................. Mom takes you places you like Mom takes you places you like
12*"........................ Mom cooks favorite foods Mom cooks favoritefoods
16* ....................... Mom reads to you Mom reads to you
20 .................... Mom plays with you Mom plays with you
24* .................. Mom talks to you Mom talks to you

NOTE.-Item number refers to position of the item in the order administered to the child. Asterisk designates
items common to both forms.

sponse to the question, "Tell me the things ple item, the female subjectwould be told that
your mother does that let you know that she the girl on the child's left is good at puzzles
likes or loves you." but the child on the right is not very good at
Picture plates.-The pictures accom- puzzles. The child's first task is to indicate
which of the two girls she is most like. After
panying each version are bound separately,as making that decision, the child is then asked
are sets forboys and girls. Thus, there are four to think only about the picture on that side
books of plates, both a boys' and girls' set for and indicate whether she is a lot like that girl
the preschool-kindergartenand first-second
(the big circle) or just a little bit like that girl
grade versions. The activities depicted in (the smaller circle). For each item there are
each item are identical for girls and boys. more specific descriptive questions that ac-
Only the gender of the target child is differ- company each circle, such as "Are you just
ent, so that a subject can respond to pictures pretty good at puzzles [small circle] or really
depicting a same-genderchild. good [large circle]?" The book of plates is
Items occurin the orderof cognitive com- constructedso that, as the picture for a given
petence, social acceptance, physical compe- item is presented to the child, the item de-
tence, and maternalacceptance,and continue scription to be read by the examiner sitting
to repeat themselves in that order. Within opposite the child is printedon the back of the
each subscale, items are counterbalancedso preceding picture(see manual[Harter& Pike,
thatthree of the pictures depict the most com- Note 2] for more specific instructions).
petent or accepted child on the left and three Scoring.-Each item is scored on a four-
of them depict the more competent or ac-
point scale, where a score of 4 would be the
cepted child on the right. most competent or accepted and a score of 1
Sample item.-The scale is individually would designate the least competent or ac-
administered.A sample item is presented in cepted. Thus, for the sample item, the child
Figure 1. The child is first read a brief state- who indicates that she is a lot like the girl on
ment about each child depicted. For the sam- the left who is good at puzzles would receive

This content downloaded from 193.255.248.150 on Mon, 9 Feb 2015 11:52:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Harter and Pike 1973

FIG.1.-Sample item

a score of 4. If she chose the smaller circle on Subscale reliabilities, in the form of internal
the left, she would get a 3. If she indicates that consistency coefficients, will then be pre-
she is a little like the girl on the right who is sented, followed by subscale means and stan-
not very good at puzzles, she would receive a dard deviations. Intercorrelations among sub-
2. And if she is a lot like that girl, she would scales, as well as correlations between child
get a score of 1. (These scores are designated and teacher ratings, will then be described.
on a scoring key under the verbal descriptions
provided for the examiner for each item in the Factor Pattern
picture plates.) Item scores are averaged Tables 2 and 3 present the factor pattern
across the six items for a given subscale, and based on an oblique (promax) rotation, a solu-
these four means provide the child's profile of tion that allows the factors to intercorrelate.
perceived competence and social acceptance. This solution was considered the most appro-
Teacher rating scale.-A teacher rating priate given our expectation, based on previ-
ous findings, that there would be moderate
scale parallels the child's instrument.
Teachers rate the child in three of the four and meaningful correlations among self-
areas tapped on the child's version: cognitive judgments in these domains. Cattell's "scree"
text, based on the magnitude of the eigen-
competence, physical competence, and peer values, as well as interpretability, indicated
acceptance. (We did not feel that it was appro- that a two-factor solution best described the
priate to have teachers rate the maternal ac- data from both the combined preschool-
ceptance of the child.) On this scale, teachers
are given a brief verbal description of each kindergarten samples as well as the combined
first-second grade samples.'
item (e.g., good at puzzles, has lots of friends,
good at swinging) and then rate how true that As can be seen in Tables 2 and 3, for both
statement is on a four-point scale (really true,
groups, items generally have moderate to high
pretty true, only sort of true, and not very loadings on their designated factor, and with
true). Thus, these scores can be compared two exceptions for the preschool-kindergarten
with the child's scores, depending on the pur-
sample, items do not cross-load on the other
poses of the study. factor. Loadings are somewhat higher for the
first-second grade samples. (Loadings less
Results than .19 are not presented, for the sake of clar-
The primary results bear upon the ity.) Factor 1 is defined by the two compe-
psychometric properties of the scale. To de- tence subscales, cognitive and physical; thus
termine the factorial validity of the scale, the it is considered to reflect perceptions of gen-
factor pattern will first be presented, along eral competence. Factor 2 is defined by the
with item means and standard deviations. peer acceptance and maternal acceptance sub-

1 Initially we performedthe more traditionalorthogonalrotation,which also revealed a two-


factor solution. However, the oblique rotationnot only seemed more appropriateconceptuallybut
provided a somewhatbetter fit.

This content downloaded from 193.255.248.150 on Mon, 9 Feb 2015 11:52:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
1974 Child Development
TABLE 2
FACTOR PATTERNAND ITEM MEANS AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS FOR THE PRESCHOOLAND
KINDERGARTENSAMPLESCOMBINED

Subscale, Item No., and Item Factor 1 Factor2 Mean SD


Cognitive competence:
1. Good at puzzles .................... .39 3.2 .77
5. Gets stars on paper ................. .37 3.1 .95
9. Knows names of colors .............. .57 3.6 .60
13. Good at counting .................. .43 3.6 .61
17. Knows alphabet ................... .48 3.6 .67
21. Knows first letter of name ........... .58 -.33 3.6 .62
Physical competence:
3. Good at swinging .................. .19 3.6 .84
7. Good at climbing .................. .33 3.4 .77
11. Can tie shoes ..................... .42 2.8 1.12
15. Good at skipping ................ .34 3.4 .84
19. Good at running .................... .23 3.4 .76
23. Good at hopping ................... .22 .30 3.4 .75
Peer acceptance:
2. Has lots of friends .................. .36 3.2 .79
6. Stays overnight at friends'. .......... .47 3.1 .92
10. Has friends to play games with ...... .23 3.1 .86
14. Has friends on the playground....... .36 3.2 .79
18. Gets asked to play with others....... .44 3.1 .81
22. Eats dinner at friends' house ........ .61 2.7 1.01
Maternalacceptance:
4. Mom smiles .................. .... .52 3.3 .67
8. Mom takes you places you like ...... .52 3.1 .80
12. Mom cooks favorite foods ........... .53 3.0 .75
16. Mom reads to you ................ .61 3.0 .96
20. Mom plays with you ................ .70 2.5 1.04
24. Mom talks to you ................... .62 3.1 .91
NOTE.-N = 145.

scales; it is considered to reflect perceptions It should be noted that, since the item
of social acceptance. means for the competence subscales, in par-
Item Means a'ndStandard Deviations ticular, were skewed toward the upper end of
the scale, the range of scores was restricted,
As can be seen in Tables 2 and 3, the
which in turn attenuated the magnitude of
majority of means are in the range of 3.0-3.6, these reliability estimates. That is, the over-
indicating that young children tend to report
whelming majority of children's item scores
relatively positive feelings of competence and were either 3 or 4. Paradoxically, therefore,
acceptance. Standard deviations indicate that although children responded consistently to
there is still considerable variability, even these items in terms of scores at the upper end
though judgments are being made in the up- of the scale, the restricted range necessarily
per ranges of the scale. The use of the upper leads to lower statistical estimates of reliability.
ranges is not thought to reflect social desirabil-
ity response tendencies so much as the young Subscale Means and Standard Deviations
child's blurring of the boundaries between re- The subscale means and standard devia-
ality and the wish to be competent or ac- tions are presented in Table 5. These subscale
cepted, as anticipated. means, like the item means, are skewed in the
direction of positive judgments, reflecting the
Reliability tendency for young children to report rela-
Subscale reliabilities, presented in Table
tively positive feelings of competence and so-
4, were assessed by employing coefficient oa cial acceptance. Scores are somewhat higher
that provides an index of internal consistency.
for the two competence subscales, compared
If one looks at individual subscales, it can be
to the two social acceptance subscales. Con-
seen that these values range from .50 to .85.
sistent with this pattern, the standard devia-
When one combines subscales according to
tions are somewhat lower for the competence
their designated factors, these reliabilities in-
than the social acceptance subscales.
crease substantially, falling within a range of
.75-.89. The reliability of the total scale, all 24 A 4 x 4 (group x subscale) analysis of
items, is in the mid- to high .80s. variance, with subscale as a repeated mea-

This content downloaded from 193.255.248.150 on Mon, 9 Feb 2015 11:52:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Harter and Pike 1975
TABLE 3
FACTOR PATTERN AND ITEM MEANS AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS FOR THE FIRST- AND
SECOND-GRADE SAMPLESCOMBINED

Subscale, Item No., and Item Factor 1 Factor2 Mean SD

Cognitive competence:
1. Good at numbers ................ .51 3.2 .73
5. Knows a lot in school............. .63 3.5 .64
9. Can read alone .................. .50 3.4 .73
15. Can write words ................. .65 3.6 .58
17. Good at spelling ................. .51 3.4 .65
21. Good at adding .................. .40 3.5 .62
Physical competence:
3. Good at swinging ................ .22 3.7 .60
7. Good at climbing ................ .48 3.4 .80
11. Good at bouncing ball............ .43 3.5 .71
15. Good at skipping................. .33 3.7 .63
19. Good at running ................ .50 3.4 .70
23. Good at jumping rope ............ .40 3.1 1.02
Peer acceptance:
2. Has lots of friends ............... .67 3.1 .85
6. Others share their toys ........... .27 3.3 .78
10. Has friends to play games with ... .60 3.0 .90
14. Has friends on the playground.... .67 3.2 .89
18. Gets asked to play with others .... .72 3.1 .85
22. Others sit next to you ............ .67 3.1 .81
Maternalacceptance:
4. Mom lets you eat at friends' ...... .44 2.8 .89
8. Mom takes you places you like ... .58 3.1 .95
12. Mom cooks favorite foods......... .63 3.1 .77
16. Mom reads to you................ .61 2.7 1.13
20. Mom lets you stay overnight...... .51 2.9 1.01
24. Mom talks to you ................ .50 3.0 .94
NOTE.-N = 104.

sure, revealed a significant effect for subscale, cal competence and the two social acceptance
F(3,693) = 92.02, p < .001. As can be seen in subscales for the second graders, however,
Table 5, this primarily indicates that scores did not fit this pattern.
were significantly higher for the two compe-
tence subscales, where most means were at or Correlationsbetween Child and Teacher
close to 3.4, than for the two social acceptance Ratings
subscales where the means ranged from 2.8 to The intercorrelations between child and
3.1. While there was no group effect, there teacher judgments were calculated across all
was a group x subscale interaction that ap- subjects since differences between age groups
pears primarily to be because, for the first and were small. These values were .37 (p < .001)
second graders only, peer acceptance scores for cognitive competence, .30 (p < .005) for
were higher than maternal acceptance scores, physical competence, and .06 for social accep-
which was not the case for the younger tance. While these correlations are moder-
groups, F(9,693) = 5.85, p < .01. ately weak, the pattern indicates that agree-
ment between pupil and teacher is highest in
Intercorrelationsamong Subscales the cognitive competence domain, next high-
Table 6 presents the intercorrelations est in the physical domain, and virtually negli-
among the four subscales for each of the four gible for peer acceptance.
groups. The clearest patterns obtained, consis-
tent with the factor analysis, are for the two Validity Data
competence scales to intercorrelate moder- Convergent validity.-As one index of
ately for each group and for the two social the validity of children's judgments, we con-
acceptance subscales to intercorrelate some- ducted an inquiry after the measure had been
what more highly. Among preschoolers, kin- administered asking children the bases for
dergartners, and first graders, peer acceptance their responses in the two competence do-
also correlates moderately with both cognitive mains. Children were asked, "How do you
and physical competence, as does maternal know you are good at/not good at [depending
acceptance. The correlations between physi- on the child's initial response] this [activity

This content downloaded from 193.255.248.150 on Mon, 9 Feb 2015 11:52:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
[ e

1J

Sert - c
z oO C~CoO
fx 4

SC

[-,

C/2

UV
0

U
z "0
o / )"a
C1 1
EzJ
4 Uz 0

a, rr"0..~c
*3 * V.

,<
p;az

U "0

cc o b"

?r o Wu0 V~a" c~

ZL~Z

.e . (

This content downloaded from 193.255.248.150 on Mon, 9 Feb 2015 11:52:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Harter and Pike 1977
TABLE 5
SUBSCALE MEANS AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS FOR EACH GROUP

COMPETENCE SUBSCALES ACCEPTANCESUBSCALES

Cognitive Physical Peer Maternal


Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
Preschool (N = 90) ........ 3.4 .45 3.2 .49 3.0 .56 3.1 .59
Kindergarten(N = 56) ..... 3.6 .41 3.4 .35 2.9 ,56 2,9 ,58
Preschool and
kindergartencombined .. 3.5 ,43 3.3 .46 2.9 .56 3.0 .59
First grade (N = 65) ....... 3.4 .37 3.4 .38 3.1 .55 2.8 .60
Second grade (N = 44) .... 3.5 .31 3.4 .40 3.1 .55 2.8 .56
First and second
grades combined ...... 3.4 .35 3.4 .39 3.1 .55 2.8 .58

TABLE 6
AMONGSUBSCALESFOR EACH GROUP
INTERCORRELATIONS

Cognitive Physical Peer


Competence Competence Acceptance
Physical competence:
Preschool .............. .56***
Kindergarten........... .43***
First grade ............. .55***
Second grade............ .43**
Peer acceptance:
Preschool ............... .56*** .48***
Kindergarten........ .... .45*** .42***
First grade ............. .59*** ,50***
Second grade........... .32* 08
Maternalacceptance:
Preschool ...........,. .48*** .43*** .64***
Kindergarten........... .27* .50*** .62***
First grade ............. .51"** ,48*** .66***
Second grade........... .32* .00 .80***
* p < .025,
** < .01.
p
*** p < .001.

specified]? How can you tell?" The purpose words right on a test," "the teacher tells me";
of this procedure was to determine (a) (b) possess/performcomponent skills (11%):
whether children could give reasons, and if "I sound out the letters," "I memorize the
so, (b) whether they were compelling in the words," "I draw straightletters"; (c) specific
sense that they bolstered or supported the par- demonstrations (32%): "I started reading
ticular self-judgment they had given previ- when I was 3," "I can write words like 'cat'
ously. and 'dog,' " "I can read two whole books,""I
can write in handwriting,""I don't have to
Systematic data (Chao, Harter, Adams, & read out loud, I can think it up in my mind";
Strop, Note 3) were available for a sample of 43
first graders and 48 second graders who were (d) routes to developing skills (20%):"I prac-
asked about three cognitive skills (reading,
tice a lot," "I can spell 'cause I read a lot," "I
practice on my flash cards," "My mom and
spelling, and writing) and two physical skills
dad helped me learn how"; and (e) habitual
(climbing and running). For the cognitive
skills, 96% of the children readily gave activity (14%):"I read a lot at home," "I do
specific reasons for why they felt that they
writingevery day,""I've spelled a lot before."
were competent or not competent. These fell In the physical domain, the categories
into the following five categories for which differedsomewhatfromthe cognitive domain,
sample responses are provided: (a) perfor- and there were also within-domain differ-
mance feedback (19%): "I get the hardest ences in the percentage of responses for

This content downloaded from 193.255.248.150 on Mon, 9 Feb 2015 11:52:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
1978 Child Development

climbing and running. The categories, includ- significantly lower than the scores (mean =
ing sample responses, were as follows: (a) so- 3.3) of those who were promoted, t(22) = 3.5,
cial comparison (51% for running, 2% for p < .005.
climbing): "I got to race a lot and win," "I was For the social domain, we examined the
first place in running in gym," "The boys say
perceived peer acceptance scores for kinder-
they can run faster but at races I can beat gartners and first and second graders who re-
them," "I'm the best in climbing races"; (b)
habitual activity (18% for running; 31% for cently moved and who had attended this par-
ticular school for less than 2 months. We
climbing): "I run a lot," "I practice jogging," hypothesized that these children would have
"I do a lot of feet work in the gym," "I climb a
lower peer acceptance scores than children
lot on bars," "I've been climbing for a long
who had been in the school for a minimum of
time"; (c) specific demonstrations (26% and 1 year. The scores of the 10 "new" children
52%, respectively): "I can run around the were significantly lower (mean = 2.9) than a
block a couple of times," "I run a lot in foot-
ball and tag 'em down," "I climb up to my comparison group of children, matched for
age and gender, whose scores (mean = 3.3)
treehouse," "I can do chin-ups"; (d) some- indicated greater peer acceptance, t(18) = 2.7,
body teaches (1% and 40%, respectively):
"Mom taught me," "Somebody taught me to p< .01.
climb the jungle gym"; and (e) don't injure In the physical domain, we have exam-
self (4% and 3%, respectively): "I hardly ever ined the validity issue with regard to the
trip and fall," "I don't fall off and scratch my- scores of children who were preterm infants.
self." (A total of 96% of the responses about Prematurity is frequently associated with de-
running and 97% of the responses about velopmental lags in gross motor skills. From
climbing could be coded in these categories.) preschools that had participated in our stud-
ies, we were able to obtain information from
Therefore, for both the cognitive and teachers as to which of the children they were
physical domains, the findings demonstrate certain had been born preterm. These were
that children can provide very definite rea-
sons for their alleged competencies; more- compared with a sample of preschoolers who
were known to have been full-term infants.
over, they volunteer these readily. Although Group differences in teacher ratings for the
we do not have systematic data for the youn-
physical domain were considerably lower for
ger ages, many of these children spontane- the preterm group (mean = 2.3) compared
ously elaborated on (and sometimes demon- with the full-term group (mean = 3.1), t(14) =
strated) their prowess during the course of the 3.4, p < .005. Correspondingly, the physical
normal administration, and these comments
competence scores of the eight children who
reveal that they too have specific reasons for had been preterm infants were found to be
their judgments. Furthermore, although the lower (mean = 2.8) than the scores (mean =
sample responses presented were those of- 3.3) of children who had been full-term in-
fered by children judging themselves to be
fants, t(14) = 2.9, p < .01.
competent, children rating themselves as in-
competent also gave plausible responses (e.g., We have begun to examine the validity of
"I can't spell words on tests," "I draw crooked the maternal acceptance subscale in one study
letters," "I watch too much TV," "I can't do of childhood depression (Harter & Wright,
twirls on the jungle gym," "I'm the last when Note 4). Our prediction was that depression in
we run"). Therefore, the overall pattern is one young children (defined in terms of dysphoric
of convergence between the initial perceived mood and lack of energy or interest) would be
competence judgments and the reasons chil- directly related to lack of maternal acceptance.
dren offered for these perceptions. In this study, we did not have a group of se-
verely depressed children. However, within
Discriminant validity.-As one test of the normal range of scores for kindergartners
validity in the cognitive domain, we made the and first and second graders, we found the
prediction that children held back in first correlation between our depression/cheer-
grade for academic reasons should score lower fulness measure and maternal acceptance to
on the cognitive competence subscale than be .48, p< .001.
those who were promoted to the second
grade. Over a 2-year period we identified 12 Finally, although our new paternal accep-
children who had been held back, and we tance scale has not yet been integrated into
compared these children with a sample of 12 the versions reported on in this paper, an in-
children, matched on age and gender, from teresting study has just been completed on
the pool of those who had been promoted. young children with abusive fathers (Kelty,
The cognitive competence scores of those Note 5). Examining 11 such children, it was
held back (mean = 2.4) were found to be found that their fathers' acceptance scores

This content downloaded from 193.255.248.150 on Mon, 9 Feb 2015 11:52:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Harter and Pike 1979
(mean = 2.6) were significantly lower than quartiles (3.6 and 2.3, respectively, for the
the scores (mean = 3.2) of a group of 13 cognitive domain), differences in the per-
nonabused children from the same preschool, ceived cognitive competence of these two
t(22) = 3.4, p < .005. subgroups are highly significant (3.8 vs. 2.6),
Predictive validity.-In one study, Bierer t(102) = 5.9, p < .001. Thus, for children who
fall at either end of the competence con-
(1981) examined the relationship between tinuum, there is much more convergence be-
first and second graders' perceived cognitive
tween teacher and child ratings than for those
competence and their preference for chal-
falling within the mid-ranges of the distribu-
lenge on a behavioral task involving subjects' tion.
choice of puzzles, varying in difficulty level.
It was initially hypothesized that per-
Discussion
ceived cognitive competence would predict
difficulty-level preferences. This correlation The attempt to devise a pictorial self-
(r = .42) was significant, p < .005. However, report measure of young children's percep-
further examination indicated that it was at- tions of their competence and social accep-
tenuated because a subgroup of children was tance would appear to have been successful.
present whose perceptions of their compe- Children eagerly respond to the pictorial for-
tence appeared to be inflated-that is, whose mat, they comprehend the items, and the
scores were at least 1.2 higher (on a four-point psychometric properties of the scale seem
scale) than their teacher's ratings of their cog- sound. The item scores and standard devia-
nitive competence on the same items. This tions revealed reasonable variability, indicat-
subgroup tended to select puzzles that were ing that the scale is sensitive to individual
much easier than one would expect, based on differences in perceived competence and ac-
their perceived competence, although their ceptance among young children.
choices were consistent with their actual The reliability, as assessed through in-
competence, as judged by the teacher. In dexes of internal consistency, was found to be
terms of the validity question, these findings
revealed ti.-+ for pupils whose ratings are acceptable. Several forms of validity were also
examined. In normative samples, the reasons
either congruent or lower than the teachers, children gave for their self-perceptions were
their perceived cognitive competence is pre- consistent with their judgments on the items
dictive of their actual behavior. That is, these themselves and were quite plausible. This
perceptions appear to mediate their behav- suggests that the ratings are valid, in the sense
ioral preference for challenge. However, the that young children's self-perceptions of their
presence of overraters in the sample at- competencies appear to be based on specific
tenuates the predictive validity of this sub- behavioral referents.
scale.
Correlations between child and teacher The findings also indicated that scores on
the introduction, it was sug- the various subscales do discriminate be-
ratings.-In
tween groups of children predicted to differ in
gested that the tendency of young children to
be somewhat inaccurate observers of their each domain. For example, children new to a
own competencies does not necessarily indict school setting reported lower peer acceptance
the validity of the instrument. The findings than those who have attended the school for a
presented indicate that the correlations be- year or more. Children who have been held
tween self- and teacher ratings in the two back a grade for academic reasons reported
lower perceived cognitive competence than
competence domains are significant, although
those experiencing normal promotion. Chil-
they are moderately weak, at best, consistent
with our expectation. Nevertheless, we did dren who were preterm infants, with related
find that, for the competence domains, teacher delays in motor development, had lower
and child ratings were more highly correlated physical competence scores than children
within the same domain (cognitive = .37, who had been born full-term. Thus, the vari-
ous subscales would appear to discriminate
physical = .30) than they were across the two
domains (teacher-cognitive/pupil-physical = clearly between a given subgroup for whom
.11; teacher-physical/pupil-cognitive = .16). there is reason to expect relatively low scores
Thus, while we have not relied heavily on this and children, matched for age and gender,
type of external validity, the pattern suggests from the normative sample. In addition, chil-
that children's competence judgments are re- dren judged by teachers to be very competent
lated to their actual competence. scored considerably higher than those whom
teachers judged to be low in competence.
Moreover, when one examines the per-
ceived competence scores of children whom At a more theoretical level, the factor pat-
the teachers rate as in the top and bottom tern obtained with this instrument is of inter-

This content downloaded from 193.255.248.150 on Mon, 9 Feb 2015 11:52:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
1980 Child Development
est, since it provides certain clues with regard more critical of others than of the self (Gesell
to the structure of the young child's self- & Ilg, 1946; Stipek, 1981).
descriptions. The two-factor solution revealed
one factor comprised of the cognitive and With regard to competence judgments,
the verbal interview data revealed that self-
physical competence subscales and a second
factor comprised of the peer and maternal ac- perceptions of skill were directly tied to
ceptance subscales. We have labeled these specific behaviors emitted by the self. We
factors "general competence" and "social ac- have yet to inquire about the bases for judg-
ments concerning social acceptance. How-
ceptance." The single competence factor im-
ever, we have collected interview data on the
plies that young children do not make a clear
distinction between what we identified as perceived routes to both competence and so-
cial acceptance. We have systematically asked
cognitive and physical domains. Competence
children what the child depicted as incompe-
at one type of skill is associated with compe- tent or socially unaccepted would have to do
tence at the other. One is either "good at do-
to become like the child pictured as compe-
ing things" or one is not. These skill domains, tent or accepted. The responses to compe-
however, are distinguished from social accep- tence involve self-improvement, primarily
tance by peers and by mother.
through instruction (I learn from the teacher),
The structure of young children's self- or personal effort (I practice a lot, try harder,
perceptions across these domains is less dif- etc.), with a gradual shift toward high levels of
ferentiated than the structure obtained for personal effort over the age span of 4 to 7
older children (Harter, 1982) where we find (Chao, Harter, Adams, & Strop, Note 3).
that cognitive and physical skills clearly
In contrast, the routes to peer acceptance
define separate factors.2 This developmental
involve behaviors designed to influence
difference is consistent with findings indicat-
others. Here we find a gradual shift from rela-
ing that the structure of the self becomes more tively naive solutions (find friends, just ask
differentiated with age (see Harter, Note 1). In
people to be your friends) to social strategies
addition, the self-structure would appear to be such as being nice, helpful, polite, and kind.
more highly related to mental age than The spontaneous mention of these strategies
chronological age. In one study we employed increases from a low of 18% among 4-5-year-
retarded children whose mental ages ranged olds to 46% in first graders and 65% in second
from 5 to approximately 8, the same range one
would expect for the young normal-IQ chil- graders.
dren in the present study. Consistent with the These findings are interesting in light of
findings reported here, the retarded pupils the recent emphasis on social skills (see
did not make a distinction between the cogni- Asher, Oden, & Gottman, 1977; Bash &
tive and physical competence domains, Camp, 1980; Gottman, Gonso, & Rasmussen,
whereas social acceptance defined a separate 1975; Hartup, 1979, 1983; Ladd & Oden,
factor (Silon & Harter, in press). 1979; Spivack & Shure, 1974). Across these
studies it has been revealed that a variety of
Inspection of the subscale means indi- social skills are associated with peer popular-
cates a general tendency for scores to be
skewed toward positive self-evaluations, al- ity and social acceptance. Moreover, they re-
veal that elementary school children possess
though this tendency was greater for the two an awareness of how social skills influence
competence subscales than for the two social their acceptance by peers. Our own findings
acceptance subscales. This pattern appears suggest that, at the youngest ages, children
plausible since judgments about one's com- have not yet acquired the knowledge concern-
petencies may be more intimately related to ing this relationship in the social domain, al-
one's appraisal of the self, in contrast to judg-
ments about social acceptance, which may be though they do seem to appreciate the need
for skill development in the cognitive and
influenced by one's view of the characteristics
physical domains. Gradually, over the early
of these particular others. Since fantasies
grades, they come to appreciate the need to
about the ideal self intrude upon judgments of
employ social strategies in order to obtain
the real self at this age level, the competence friends.
scores are likely to be somewhat inflated. To
the extent that social acceptance items pull for This pattern raises numerous questions
judgments of others, these scores would be for further study. For example, in what ways
expected to be lower since findings have dem- are social skills different from those in the cog-
onstrated that young children are likely to be nitive and physical domains? Why should the
2 In the revision of the originalperceived competence scale, we have determinedthatchildren
age 8 and older make distinctionsamongfive domains(scholastic,athletic,appearance,social accep-
tance, and conduct),as revealed by a clean five-factorsolution (Harter,1983).

This content downloaded from 193.255.248.150 on Mon, 9 Feb 2015 11:52:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Harter and Pike 1981

understanding of these two types of skills pur- and interest for age-appropriate activities. To
sue a different developmental course? In ad- the extent that the child's mood and energy
dition, how do the perceived characteristics of level may be critical mediators of behaviors
others influence one's judgments of social ac- leading to the development of new skills, it
ceptance? In light of these considerations, we would be important to assess these predictors
would urge that our instrument not be treated as early as possible. Furthermore, a domain-
solely as a measure of "self-concept" since the specific measure allows one to determine
social acceptance construct tapped may in- which domains best predict the mediators and
volve a number of dimensions extending be- behaviors of interest.
yond one's perceptions of the self per se. Second, the degree to which a young
Differences between the two compe- child's judgments are inaccurate might be im-
tence domains and the social domain of peer portant to examine. Normative or age-
acceptance were also revealed in the examina- appropriate distortions may not be cause for
tion of the accuracy of children's judgments. concern. However, it may be important to de-
The correlations between teacher and child tect either extreme inflation of one's abilities
ratings for the two competence domains were or the unrealistic portrayal of oneself as ex-
significant, though moderately weak; how- tremely incompetent. Furthermore, a child
ever, they were negligible in the social do- may show these inaccuracies in some domains
main. The greater congruence in the compe- more than others, and these particular distor-
tence domain may be because of clearer tions may well have behavioral correlates.
sources of information on which to base one's Findings with somewhat older children indi-
judgments. Our findings suggest that, in the cate that, by third and fourth grades, there are
cognitive domain, performance feedback is behavioral patterns associated with extreme
beginning to emerge as a criterion for perfor- tendencies to overrate or underrate one's cog-
mance, whereas in the physical domain, social nitive competence. For example, both of these
comparison is becoming the basis for judg- inaccurate subgroups tend to avoid behavioral
ments of competence. Of particular interest is preference for challenge compared with those
the finding that social comparison is used children who accurately rate their compe-
more frequently for the activity of running, tence (Bierer, 1981).
which appears to be a more competitive activ-
Third, there would appear to be a need
ity, than for climbing. for an instrument to assess the self-
Our data are consistent with Ruble and perceptions among special subgroups of chil-
Frey's (Note 6) findings that, in the domain of dren who may be under particular types of
academic achievement, social comparison is stress. Children of divorce, of abusive parents,
not consistently employed in the early grades. and with learning disabilities or physical
Our findings also indicate that social compari- handicaps are all special groups that have
son effects may be somewhat domain-specific come to the recent attention of basic research-
since social comparison does form the basis ers, clinicians, and those engaged in social
for judgments of certain physical skills. We policy. However, as has been pointed out, not
have yet to examine the bases on which chil- all children necessarily suffer from events that
dren make judgments in the social domain. have been categorically identified as stressful
However, the lack of congruence between (Garmezy & Rutter, 1983). A variety of indi-
child and teacher ratings of social acceptance vidual difference variables, including self-
may result from several factors: performance concept, have been implicated as factors in-
feedback may be less salient, children may be fluencing the child's ability to withstand stress
less able to employ social comparison in this and cope adaptively. Thus, a domain-specific
domain, and/or children and adults may em- measure might well be useful in predicting
ploy different criteria. Further research in this children's reactions with an eye toward deter-
area would be fruitful. mining which type of profile is associated
with resiliency and adaptation, or its
Finally, to what uses might such an in- counterpart. In conclusion, although there are
strument be put, particularly given the several theoretical issues requiring further re-
qualification that young children's judgments search, we believe that there are a number of
are not very accurate? First, among normative uses to which this instrument might well be
samples, scores may be useful in predicting
put in order to illuminate our understanding
behaviors, motivations, and/or emotional reac- of the young child.
tions of interest. Our own findings (Harter &
Wright, Note 4) indicate that the social accep- Reference Notes
tance subscales, particularly the maternal sub-
scale, are significantly correlated with the 1. Harter, S. Supplementary description of the
child's self-reported mood as well as energy Self-PerceptionProfilefor Children: Revision

This content downloaded from 193.255.248.150 on Mon, 9 Feb 2015 11:52:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
1982 Child Development

of the Perceived Competence Scale for Chil- in children.Child Development,1975,46, 709-


dren. Unpublished manuscript,University of 718.
Denver, 1983. Harter,S. The perceived competence scale forchil-
2. Harter,S., & Pike, R. Proceduralmanual to ac- dren. Child Development, 1982, 53, 87-97.
companythe PictorialScale of PerceivedCom- Harter,S. Developmental perspectives on the self-
petence and SocialAcceptancefor YoungChil- system. In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), P. H.
dren. Unpublished manuscript,University of Mussen (Series Ed.), Handbook of child psy-
Denver, 1983. chology. (Vol. 4): Socialization, personality,
3. Chao, C., Harter,S., Adams,P., & Strop,J. Di- and social development. New York: Wiley,
mensions underlyingchildren'sperceptionsof 1983.
competence. Unpublished data, University of Harter, S. Competence as a dimension of self-
Denver, 1983. evaluation:Towarda comprehensivemodel of
4. Harter,S., & Wright, K. The relationship be- self-worth.In R. Leahy (Ed.),The development
tween social support, depression, and young of the self. New York: Academic Press, in
children's perceptions of competence and so- press.
cial acceptance.Unpublishedmanuscript,Uni- Hartup,W. W. Peer relationsand the growthof so-
versity of Denver, 1984. cial competence. In M. W. Kent & J. E. Rolf
5. Kelty, E. Perceivedcompetenceand social ac- (Eds.), The primary prevention of
ceptancein youngchildrenof abusiveand non- psychopathology. (Vol. 3): Promoting social
abusive parents. Unpublished manuscript, competence and coping in children. Hanover,
Universityof Colorado,1983. N.H.: UniversityPress of New England, 1979.
6. Ruble, D. N., & Frey, K. S. Self-evaluationand Hartup,W. W. Peer relations.In E. M. Hethering-
social comparisonin children:Developmental ton (Ed.),P. H. Mussen (Series Ed.),Handbook
changes in knowledgeandfunction. Paperpre- of child psychology.(Vol.4): Socialization,per-
sented at the biennial meeting of the Society sonality, and social development. New York:
for Research in Child Development, Detroit, Wiley, 1983.
1983. Ladd, G. W., & Oden, S. The relationshipbetween
peer acceptance and children's ideas about
helpfulness. Child Development, 1979, 50,
402-408.
References Piers, E., & Harris,D. The Piers-HarrisChildren's
Asher, S.R., Oden, S. L., & Gottman,J. M. Chil- Self-Concept Scale. Nashville, Tenn.: Coun-
dren's friendships in school setting. In L. G. selor Recordingsand Tests, 1969.
Katz (Ed.), Current topics in early childhood Ruble, D. N. The developmentof social comparison
education (Vol. 1). Norwood,N.J.:Ablex, 1977. processesand their role in achievement-related
Bash, M. A. S., & Camp, B. W. Teacher trainingin self-socialization. In E. T. Higgins, D. N.
the Think Aloud classroom program. In G. Ruble, & W. W. Hartup(Eds.), Social cognition
Cartledge& J. F. Milburn(Eds.), Teachingso- and social development:New York:Cambridge
cial skills to children. New York:Pergamon, UniversityPress, 1983.
1980. Silon, E., & Harter,S. The assessmentof perceived
Bierer,B. Preferencefor challenge among children competence, motivationalorientation,and anx-
who over-rate,under-rate,and accuratelyrate iety in segregatedand mainstreamededucable
their cognitive competence. Unpublished mentally retardedchildren.Journal of Educa-
Ph.D. dissertation,Universityof Denver, 1981. tional Psychology,in press.
Coopersmith, S. The antecedents of self-esteem. Spivack, G., & Shure, M. B. Social adjustment of
San Francisco:W. H. Freeman, 1967. young children. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass,
Garmezy, N. & Rutter, M. (Eds.). Stress, coping, 1974.
and development in children. New York: Stipek, D. J. Children's perceptions of their own
McGraw-Hill,1983. and their classmates'ability.Journal of Educa-
Gesell, A., & Ilg, F. The childfrom five to ten. New tinal Psychology, 1981, 73, 404-410.
York:Harper& Row, 1946. Wylie, R. Theory and research on selected topics.
Gottman,J. M., Gonso, J., & Rasmussen,B. Social (Vol. 2): The self concept. Lincoln: University
interaction,social competence, and friendship of NebraskaPress, 1979.

This content downloaded from 193.255.248.150 on Mon, 9 Feb 2015 11:52:42 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like