You are on page 1of 6

Please cite this article in press as: Bonnefond and Jensen, Alpha Oscillations Serve to Protect Working Memory

Maintenance against
Anticipated Distracters, Current Biology (2012), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.029
Current Biology 22, 1–6, October 23, 2012 ª2012 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.029

Report
Alpha Oscillations Serve to Protect
Working Memory Maintenance
against Anticipated Distracters
Mathilde Bonnefond1,* and Ole Jensen1,* revealed that the reaction times for strong distracters
1Radboud University Nijmegen, Donders Institute for Brain, (725 6 35 ms) was significantly longer than for weak dis-
Cognition and Behaviour, Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, tracters (699 6 31 ms; p < 0.05; Figure 1B). The findings
Kapittelweg 29, 6525 EN Nijmegen, The Netherlands suggest that strong distracters are incorporated into WM in
some trials.

Summary Occipitotemporal Alpha Power Increases Prior to the


Anticipation of Strong Distracters
When operating in a complex world, it is essential to have To characterize the oscillatory activity reflecting the antici-
mechanisms that can suppress distracting information pation of distracters, we quantified the properties of the
[1, 2]. Such mechanisms might be related to neuronal MEG data in the interval before the strong versus the weak
oscillations, which are known to be involved in gating of distracters (0 < t < 1.1 s). The time-frequency representation
incoming information [3]. We here apply a working memory (TFR) of power revealed larger 8–12 Hz alpha activity over
(WM) task to investigate how neuronal oscillations are posterior sensors prior to the distracter onset (Figures 2A
involved in the suppression of distracting information that and 2B). A cluster-based randomization test (see Supple-
can be predicted in time. We used a modified Sternberg mental Information available online) showed that the effect
WM task in which distracters were presented in the retention was significant in the alpha band in the 0.6–1.1 s interval
interval, while we recorded the ongoing brain activity over left-lateralized parieto-occipital sensors (p < 0.05;
using magnetoencephalography. The data revealed a robust Figure 2B). The sources reflecting the alpha power increase
adjustment of the phase of alpha oscillations in anticipation were located in the occipital cortex extending into the left
of the distracter. In trials with strong phase adjustment, temporal cortex (Figure 2C).
response times to the memory probe were reduced. Further,
the power of alpha oscillations increased prior to the dis- An Increase in Alpha Activity Protects against Anticipated
tracter and predicted performance. Our findings demon- Distracters
strate that the doors of perception close when a distracter We set out to determine whether alpha power is associated
is expected. The phase adjustment of the alpha rhythm with distracter suppression as assessed by the reaction
adds to the computational versatility of brain oscillations, times to the probe. We computed the average alpha
because such a mechanism allows for modulating neuronal power (8 Hz < f < 12 Hz; 0.6 < t < 1.1 s) for the cluster of sensors
processing on a fine temporal scale. shown in Figure 2B for fast and slow reaction times. This was
done by sorting the trials according to reaction times in five
Results bins. Trials from the second and fourth bin were considered
fast and slow in order to ignore trials with either too long or
To investigate the role of oscillatory activity in distracter too short reaction times because they are likely to be domi-
suppression in working memory (WM) tasks, we designed nated by premature button presses or lapses in attention
a modified version of the Sternberg paradigm. A memory set (see also Supplemental Information). A two-way repeated-
of four consonants was presented to the subject followed by measures ANOVA with factors ‘‘distracter type’’ and ‘‘reaction
a probe. In the retention interval, we presented either weak time’’ revealed a significant interaction between reaction time
(a symbol) or strong (a letter) distracters in different blocks and distracter type (p < 0.05). A post hoc tests showed that
such that the subjects could anticipate the nature of the dis- alpha power was significantly higher for fast than for slow
tracter (Figure 1A). To optimize the chances for identifying reaction times trials in the strong distracter condition (p <
a phase effect in the 10 Hz band, we flashed the distracters 0.05; Figure 2D). These results suggest that stronger alpha
for 33 ms. Importantly, the onset of the distracter at t = 1.1 s activity in anticipation of a strong distracter leads to a better
was predictable across trials. The ongoing brain activity was distracter suppression.
recorded using magnetoencephalography (MEG) from 18
subjects performing this task. Alpha Power Predicts Individual Differences in
Performance
Reaction Times Longer for Strong than Weak Distracters We performed an analysis comparing individual abilities to
Trials suppress distracters in relation to the ability to adjust the alpha
It is well established that the reaction time to the probe activity prior to a distracter. Over subjects we correlated the
increases with memory load in the Sternberg task [4]. Thus difference in reaction time and the difference in alpha power
the potential incorporation of a distracter in WM should be (data derived from left occipital-temporal cortex using a beam-
reflected in longer reaction times. A two-way repeated- former spatial filter; see Supplemental Information) between
measures ANOVA analysis with the factors ‘‘distracter’’ weak and strong distracters. We observed a negative correla-
(weak versus strong) and ‘‘probe type’’ (old versus new) tion: the stronger the difference in power between anticipated
distracter types, the smaller the difference in reaction times
(r = 20.56; p = 0.02; Figure 2E). These data suggest that indi-
*Correspondence: mathilde.bonnefond@donders.ru.nl (M.B.), ole.jensen@ viduals with a better ability to modulate their alpha activity
donders.ru.nl (O.J.) are less impaired by strong distracters.
Please cite this article in press as: Bonnefond and Jensen, Alpha Oscillations Serve to Protect Working Memory Maintenance against
Anticipated Distracters, Current Biology (2012), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.029
Current Biology Vol 22 No 20
2

Figure 1. The Modified Sternberg Paradigm


(A) A set of four consonants was presented sequentially at a rate of 1,133 ms
per letter. As in the conventional Sternberg paradigm, subjects indicated
by button press whether the probe was part of the memory set (‘‘old’’ or
‘‘new’’). In the retention interval, either a ‘‘weak’’ (a symbol) or ‘‘strong’’ (a Figure 2. The Power of Alpha Band Activity Prior to the Distracter Onset
consonant) distracter was presented. The distracter strength could be (A) Time-frequency representation of power when comparing strong and
anticipated by the subjects because weak and strong distracters were weak distracters. The alpha power was higher for strong compared to
grouped in blocks of trials. weak distracters. The arrow indicates the onset of the distracter.
(B) The hit rate and reactions times in response to the memory probe (error (B) The topography of the alpha power (8–12 Hz; 0.6–1.1 s; combined planar
bars represent the SEM). Although the hit rate was independent of distracter gradient) when comparing strong to weak distracters. The cluster random-
type, the response times were significantly longer for the strong compared ization analysis revealed a significant cluster (marked by asterisks; p < 0.05)
to weak distracters. This indicates that strong distracters are encoded in in posterior sensors.
working memory in some trials and interferes with retrieval. (C) The source analysis of the alpha activity (8–12 Hz; 0.6–1.1 s) for strong
compared to weak distracters revealed the strongest contributions from
left occipitotemporal cortices (MNI coordinates: [252 262 26]; t values
Alpha Oscillations Align in Phase Prior to Anticipated are masked corresponding to a p value of 0.05). To further support
Strong Distracters this result, we also compared predistracter period (0.6 s < t < 1.1 s) to prep-
Anticipatory phase adjustment could provide a mechanism for robe period (1.833 < t < 2.233 s). We found overlapping sources is occipito-
parietal cortex.
gain regulation in association with temporal expectation [3].
(D) The difference in alpha power (0.6 < t < 1.1s; 8 < f < 12 Hz) when
We aimed to determine whether alpha oscillations can adjust comparing trials with fast and slow reaction times for, respectively, weak
in phase to optimally suppress the distracter. To this end, we and strong distracters (error bars represent the SEM). The log-ratio of alpha
computed the phase-locking factor (PLF) over trials for each power was significantly larger for strong compared to weak distracters.
condition. The TFR of the PLF (thresholded at 0.087; see (E) Differences in alpha power (strong compared to weak distracters) corre-
Supplemental Information) over the cluster of sensors shown lated negatively with differences in reaction times (strong compared to weak
distracters) over subjects, i.e., subjects in which the alpha power increased
Figure 2B revealed phase locking in the alpha band prior to
in anticipation of a strong distracter were also subjects that were less
the distracter onset (Figure 3A). The topography of the PLF slowed down during memory probing.
(8 Hz < f < 12 Hz; 0.6 < t < 1.1 s) considering all sensors revealed
a left-lateralized distribution that matched the power differ-
ence between conditions (cf. Figures 2B and Figures 3B). that the phase locking was explained by sustained alpha
Further analyses revealed that the phase effects are not oscillations elicited by the last memory item. The difference
trivially explained by systematic variations in power (see was significant when averaged over the 0.6–1.1 s interval
Supplemental Information). Next, we considered the differ- (p < 0.01; Figure 3D). These findings demonstrate that alpha
ence in PLF for strong versus weak distracters (Figure 3C) phase is adjusted in anticipation of a predictable strong
(the PLF was transformed to Rayleigh Z values to control for distracter.
biases due to differences in trial numbers; see Supplemental To further substantiate our findings, we investigated the
Information). The difference in phase locking increased in phase adjustment using a spatial filter aimed at extracting
anticipation of the strong distracter (Figure 3C). This excludes the activity from the occipital-temporal source. Using a linearly
Please cite this article in press as: Bonnefond and Jensen, Alpha Oscillations Serve to Protect Working Memory Maintenance against
Anticipated Distracters, Current Biology (2012), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.029
Alpha Oscillations Protect Working Memory
3

Figure 4. Assessing Difference in Predistracter Phase for Slow and Fast


Reaction Times
(A) The difference in phase between fast and slow trials was quantified using
a phase-difference index (error bars represent the SEM). We found a signif-
icant phase difference index for the strong but not the weak distracter trials.
(B) The time-locked responses (event-related fields) prior to the distracter
according to trials with fast (in red) and slow (in blue) reaction times at
source level. A clear lag can be observed for three cycles of alpha
activity prior to the distracter (as indicated by small green arrows). a.u. =
Figure 3. Phase Adjustment of Alpha Activity Prior to Distracter Onset
arbitrary units.
(A) The time-frequency representation of the phase-locking factor (PLF) in See also Figure S2.
the retention interval over sensors of interest (see Figure 2B for sensors
selection; PLF thresholded at 0.087 corresponding to p < 0.01 in the Ray-
leigh test) reveals that alpha oscillations are adjusted in phase prior to the alpha activity prior to the distracter was clearly apparent in
onset of the distracter. The strong PLF values observed in the theta band
this averaged trace (marked by green arrows). Note that the
(5–8 Hz) a t = w0.4 s and t = w1.1 s reflect activity evoked by, respectively,
the last memory item and the distracter. Note that the acausal temporal phase-adjusted alpha activity occurred just prior to the dis-
smoothing associated with the phase estimation was approximately three tracter and cannot be explained by sustained oscillations eli-
cycles long, i.e., 300 ms for 10 Hz and 500 ms for 6 Hz. The arrow indicates cited by the last memory item at t = 0 s. We also found
the onset of the distracter. We further observed a significant PLF in 14 of 18 evidence for frontal sources in which the phase adjustment
subjects (0.6 < t < 1.1 s; 8 < f < 12 Hz; Rayleigh test: p < 0.01). of alpha activity was more pronounced just prior to strong
(B) The topography of the PLF (0.6 < t < 1.1 s; 8 < f < 12 Hz) reveals the stron-
compared to weak distracters (see Supplemental Information).
gest values in posterior sensors.
(C) Time-frequency representation of the difference in PLF (transformed to In future research, it would be of interest to investigate whether
Rayleigh Z values where Z = number of trials 3 PLF2) comparing strong these frontal regions are involved in exercising a top-down
and weak distracters (for sensors marked by asterisks in Figure 2B). The drive that adjusts the alpha activity in posterior regions ([6,
difference is most pronounced in the alpha band just prior to the onset of 7]; see Supplemental Information; Figure S1A).
the distracter; i.e., the phase adjustment is not explained by sustained oscil-
lations elicited by the last memory item.
Alpha Phase Predicts the Efficiency of the Suppression
(D) The PLF is significantly higher in the strong than in the weak distracter
condition over the sensors of interest (p< 0.01; error bars represent of the Processing of the Distracter
the SEM). We further tested whether the phase-locking values for slow
(E) The time-locked responses (event-related fields) prior to the distracter. and fast trials were associated with a difference in absolute
The single trials from the left occipital-temporal source were derived using alpha phases. This was done using a phase-difference index
a beam forming approach. The time-locked responses were averaged over [8, 9]. The phase difference index in Figure 4A shows the differ-
subjects. A clear modulation in the alpha band is seen prior to the distracter
ence in PLFs when comparing true versus randomized trials
(as indicated by small green arrows). a.u., arbitrary units.
See also Figure S1. for fast versus slow reaction times trials. For the strong
distracter, the significant phase difference index suggests
that fast and slow distracter trials are associated with different
constrained minimum variance beamformer [5], we obtained phases of the alpha activity at the onset of the distracter.
the time course for each trial from seven grid points in the Figure 4B shows the averaged traces for fast and slow reaction
left occipital-temporal cortex (see Figure 2C). The phase times trials for data from the occipitotemporal source in the
adjustment should as well be observable in the trial average strong distracter condition. This analysis confirms that fast
just prior to distracter onset. The grand average over trials and slow reaction time trials are associated with different
and subjects is shown in Figure 3E (baseline: 20.233 < t < alpha phases. Our key interpretation is that if the distracter
20.033 s, low-pass filtered at 30 Hz). The phase-adjusted arrives at a certain alpha phase, it will be more intrusive
Please cite this article in press as: Bonnefond and Jensen, Alpha Oscillations Serve to Protect Working Memory Maintenance against
Anticipated Distracters, Current Biology (2012), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.029
Current Biology Vol 22 No 20
4

as compared to the reverse phase. Further, the results increase during the retention period in the Sternberg task
demonstrate that the anticipatory phase adjustment has also perform faster [28].
functional implications, and that the phase adjustment is not The finding that alpha phase adjusts prior to the distracter
trivially a consequence of the power increase prior to the is highly novel. Clearly, the alpha activity is rhythmic and
distracter. the phase of the oscillatory activity has been shown to
Also note the slow modulation of the ERFs in Figure 3E. This modulate processing [8, 29–33]. We now show that partici-
modulation is reflected in the delta band but is similar for high pants can adjust alpha phase when the timing of a distracter
and low distracter trials (see Figure S2). This could suggest is predictable. Such an adjustment of oscillatory activity has
that the phase of the delta rhythm is involved in controlling been shown in attentional tasks in monkeys with respect to
the temporal modulation, but not magnitude, of the alpha delta oscillations [3]. In this study, the monkeys had to detect
oscillations. We conclude that anticipatory phase adjustments a target either in the visual or the auditory domain both
mainly in the alpha band serve to protect WM against presented in alternation at 1.5 Hz. The phase of the delta
distracters. activity (w1.5 Hz) was top-down adjusted in the visual cortex
for the attended visual stimulus (visual-attention condition)
Unpredictable Compared to Predictable Distracter Onsets to appear at a high-excitability phase and the unattended
Result in Longer Reaction Times visual stimulus (auditory-attention condition) to appear at
If the temporal predictability of the distracter allows for a low-excitability phase. Our findings extend this concept:
suppression by a phase-adjustment of the alpha oscillations, in the Lakatos et al. study [3], the frequency of the oscillations
then a predistracter delay varying by w100 ms (matching the was determined by the cadence of the task. We show that
duration of an alpha cycle) should reduce performance. In an intrinsically generated cortical rhythm can adjust in
a behavioral study conducted outside the MEG system (see phase. Further, this alpha phase adjustment is consistent
Supplemental Information), we compared fixed (1.1.s) to vari- with the behavioral experiment showing that distracters
able delays (from 1.03 to 1.16 s; 33 ms steps) between the predictable in time are associated with better performance
last memory item and the distracter. The total duration of the compared to distracters presented with a w100 ms jitter.
retention interval was kept identical for both conditions. One might suspect that the phase adjustment just prior to
Fixed intervals resulted in significantly shorter reaction times the distracter is explained by alpha activity evoked by the
(664 6 30 ms versus 687 6 33 ms; 12 participants; p < 0.05). last memory, which sustained in phase until the appearance
These data suggest that when subjects can precisely antici- of the distracter. This concern is ruled out by the observation
pate the distracter they are better at suppressing it. that the behaviorally relevant alpha adjustment increases just
prior to the distracter onset (see Figures 3E and Figures 4B;
Discussion Figure S1B).
Our findings demonstrating alpha phase adjustment imply
The present experiment was designed to identify the mecha- that the human brain is able to predict events with a temporal
nism responsible for suppressing anticipated intruding infor- precision on the order of 20–30 ms or less. This is consistent
mation. In a task where subjects could anticipate the strength with various findings demonstrating that humans are able
and exact timing of distracters, we observed both a stronger to discriminate fine temporal durations [34–41]. We further
power increase and phase adjustment of alpha activity in showed that alpha phase adjustment is predictive of fast and
occipitotemporal areas prior to strong compared to weak dis- slow reaction times to the probe. We propose that a trial in
tracter onsets. Importantly, we demonstrated that trials with which performance is poor is a result of an insufficient adjust-
fast and slow reaction times were associated with different ment of alpha phase possibly due to lapses in attention.
absolute predistracter alpha phases and a stronger alpha Although the duration of the distracter was 33 ms, we expect
power in the faster trials. These findings suggest that a failure that the results would be reproduced when using longer
to adjust the alpha phase and power just prior to a distracter lasting distracters because it is the stimulus onset that defines
will disturb WM processes. the critical moment for when our attention is drawn.
These results underscore that alpha activity is responsible From a mechanistic point of view, one might ask why an
for active functional inhibition of sensory regions [10, 11]. For adjustment of both alpha magnitude and phase is required
instance, previous studies have shown that when attention is for optimal performance. It has been proposed that the alpha
allocated to the right hemifield, the alpha activity decreases rhythm is a consequence of pulses of inhibition that can vary
over the left visual system while it increases relatively in the in magnitude [10, 11, 42, 43]. This notion has recently received
right hemisphere. The same holds for the somatosensory support from monkey recordings showing that alpha oscilla-
system [12–21]. These modulations in alpha activity are pre- tions inhibit neuronal firing selectively at specific phases [44].
dictive of performance [15, 18]. Decreases in alpha activity An adjustment in alpha magnitude only would provide a
have also been shown to correlate with temporal expectations suboptimal mechanism for functional inhibition, because
[22, 23]. We here show for the first time that alpha is modulated information appearing between the inhibitory pulses will not
according to the strength of distracters and that alpha power is be suppressed; thus, there is a need for alpha phase adjust-
linked to the suppression of distracting information (see also ment as well. Our findings are consistent with the notion that
[24]). The source of the alpha activity included the left fusiform a key function of the central nervous system is to predict
gyrus, an area known to be engaged in letter processing upcoming events [45]. We here provide insight to the physio-
[25–27]. Moreover, the activity in this area predicted individual logical substrate of temporal predictions.
differences in reaction time between the strong and the weak In this study, we demonstrated that alpha phase can be
distracter conditions. This result indicates that distracter adjusted in anticipation of an event in order to close the doors
suppression in particular relies on the inhibition of representa- of perception when intruding information might disturb
tional specific areas. Further it extends results from a previous ongoing processes. This new principle adds considerable to
study showing that subjects with a stronger posterior alpha the versatility of the alpha rhythm in the context neuronal
Please cite this article in press as: Bonnefond and Jensen, Alpha Oscillations Serve to Protect Working Memory Maintenance against
Anticipated Distracters, Current Biology (2012), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.029
Alpha Oscillations Protect Working Memory
5

processing. The stage is now set for investigating how general motor goal during delayed prosaccades and antisaccades.
this principle is [46]. J. Neurosci. 28, 8397–8405.
17. Worden, M.S., Foxe, J.J., Wang, N., and Simpson, G.V. (2000).
Anticipatory biasing of visuospatial attention indexed by retinotopically
Supplemental Information specific alpha-band electroencephalography increases over occipital
cortex. J. Neurosci. 20, RC63.
Supplemental Information includes two figures, Supplemental Results and 18. Haegens, S., Händel, B.F., and Jensen, O. (2011). Top-down controlled
Discussion, and Supplemental Experimental Procedures and can be found alpha band activity in somatosensory areas determines behavioral
with this article online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.029. performance in a discrimination task. J. Neurosci. 31, 5197–5204.
19. Hari, R., and Salmelin, R. (1997). Human cortical oscillations: a neuro-
Acknowledgments magnetic view through the skull. Trends Neurosci. 20, 44–49.
20. van Ede, F., Jensen, O., and Maris, E. (2010). Tactile expectation modu-
The authors gratefully acknowledge The Netherlands Organization for lates pre-stimulus beta-band oscillations in human sensorimotor
Scientific Research (NWO) VICI grant number 453-09-002, ‘‘The healthy cortex. Neuroimage 51, 867–876.
brain’’ funded by the Netherlands Initiative Brain and Cognition, a part of 21. van Ede, F., de Lange, F., Jensen, O., and Maris, E. (2011). Orienting
the Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) under grant number attention to an upcoming tactile event involves a spatially and tempo-
056-14-011 and the Fyssen funding scheme. We thank Paul Gaalman for rally specific modulation of sensorimotor alpha- and beta-band oscilla-
assistance with structural MRI collection and Sander Berends for assis- tions. J. Neurosci. 31, 2016–2024.
tance with MEG recordings. 22. Rohenkohl, G., and Nobre, A.C. (2011). a oscillations related to anticipa-
tory attention follow temporal expectations. J. Neurosci. 31, 14076–
Received: January 31, 2012 14084.
Revised: July 21, 2012 23. Lima, B., Singer, W., and Neuenschwander, S. (2011). Gamma
Accepted: August 15, 2012 responses correlate with temporal expectation in monkey primary
Published online: October 4, 2012 visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 31, 15919–15931.
24. Haegens, S., Luther, L., and Jensen, O. (2011). Somatosensory anticipa-
References tory alpha activity increases to suppress distracting input. J. Cogn.
Neurosci. 24, 677–685.
1. Zanto, T.P., and Gazzaley, A. (2009). Neural suppression of irrelevant 25. James, K.H., James, T.W., Jobard, G., Wong, A.C., and Gauthier, I.
information underlies optimal working memory performance. (2005). Letter processing in the visual system: different activation
J. Neurosci. 29, 3059–3066. patterns for single letters and strings. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci.
2. Chadick, J.Z., and Gazzaley, A. (2011). Differential coupling of visual 5, 452–466.
cortex with default or frontal-parietal network based on goals. Nat. 26. Joseph, J.E., Gathers, A.D., and Piper, G.A. (2003). Shared and dissoci-
Neurosci. 14, 830–832. ated cortical regions for object and letter processing. Brain Res. Cogn.
3. Lakatos, P., Karmos, G., Mehta, A.D., Ulbert, I., and Schroeder, C.E. Brain Res. 17, 56–67.
(2008). Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of atten- 27. Longcamp, M., Anton, J.L., Roth, M., and Velay, J.L. (2003). Visual
tional selection. Science 320, 110–113. presentation of single letters activates a premotor area involved in
4. Sternberg, S. (1966). High-speed scanning in human memory. Science writing. Neuroimage 19, 1492–1500.
153, 652–654. 28. Scheeringa, R., Petersson, K.M., Oostenveld, R., Norris, D.G., Hagoort,
5. Van Veen, B.D., van Drongelen, W., Yuchtman, M., and Suzuki, A. (1997). P., and Bastiaansen, M.C. (2009). Trial-by-trial coupling between EEG
Localization of brain electrical activity via linearly constrained minimum and BOLD identifies networks related to alpha and theta EEG power
variance spatial filtering. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 44, 867–880. increases during working memory maintenance. Neuroimage 44,
6. Siegel, M., Donner, T.H., and Engel, A.K. (2012). Spectral fingerprints of 1224–1238.
large-scale neuronal interactions. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 121–134. 29. Busch, N.A., Dubois, J., and VanRullen, R. (2009). The phase of ongoing
7. Zanto, T.P., Rubens, M.T., Thangavel, A., and Gazzaley, A. (2011). EEG oscillations predicts visual perception. J. Neurosci. 29, 7869–7876.
Causal role of the prefrontal cortex in top-down modulation of visual 30. Mathewson, K.E., Gratton, G., Fabiani, M., Beck, D.M., and Ro, T. (2009).
processing and working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 656–661. To see or not to see: prestimulus alpha phase predicts visual aware-
8. Dugué, L., Marque, P., and VanRullen, R. (2011). The phase of ongoing ness. J. Neurosci. 29, 2725–2732.
oscillations mediates the causal relation between brain excitation and 31. Scheeringa, R., Mazaheri, A., Bojak, I., Norris, D.G., and Kleinschmidt, A.
visual perception. J. Neurosci. 31, 11889–11893. (2011). Modulation of visually evoked cortical FMRI responses by phase
9. Vanrullen, R., Busch, N.A., Drewes, J., and Dubois, J. (2011). Ongoing of ongoing occipital alpha oscillations. J. Neurosci. 31, 3813–3820.
EEG phase as a trial-by-trial predictor of perceptual and attentional vari- 32. Vanrullen, R., and Dubois, J. (2011). The psychophysics of brain
ability. Front Psychol 2, 60. rhythms. Front. Psychol. 2, 203.
10. Jensen, O., and Mazaheri, A. (2010). Shaping functional architecture 33. Romei, V., Gross, J., and Thut, G. (2012). Sounds reset rhythms of
by oscillatory alpha activity: gating by inhibition. Front Hum. Neurosci. visual cortex and corresponding human visual perception. Curr. Biol.
4, 186. 22, 807–813.
11. Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P., and Hanslmayr, S. (2007). EEG alpha oscilla- 34. Nobre, A.C., and O’Reilly, J. (2004). Time is of the essence. Trends Cogn.
tions: the inhibition-timing hypothesis. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 53, Sci. 8, 387–389.
63–88. 35. Nobre, A., Correa, A., and Coull, J. (2007). The hazards of time. Curr.
12. Rihs, T.A., Michel, C.M., and Thut, G. (2007). Mechanisms of selective Opin. Neurobiol. 17, 465–470.
inhibition in visual spatial attention are indexed by alpha-band EEG 36. McAuley, J.D., and Jones, M.R. (2003). Modeling effects of rhythmic
synchronization. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 603–610. context on perceived duration: a comparison of interval and entrain-
13. Rihs, T.A., Michel, C.M., and Thut, G. (2009). A bias for posterior alpha- ment approaches to short-interval timing. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum.
band power suppression versus enhancement during shifting versus Percept. Perform. 29, 1102–1125.
maintenance of spatial attention. Neuroimage 44, 190–199. 37. Barnes, R., and Jones, M.R. (2000). Expectancy, attention, and time.
14. Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Heise, K.F., Gruber, W.R., Holz, E., Karim, Cognit. Psychol. 41, 254–311.
A.A., Glennon, M., Gerloff, C., Birbaumer, N., and Hummel, F.C. 38. Allan, L.G., and Kristofferson, A. (1974). Judgments about duration of
(2009). Brain oscillatory substrates of visual short-term memory brief stimuli. Percept. Psychophys. 15, 434–440.
capacity. Curr. Biol. 19, 1846–1852. 39. Friberg, A., and Sundberg, J. (1995). Time discrimination in a monotonic,
15. Thut, G., Nietzel, A., Brandt, S.A., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2006). Alpha- isochronous sequence. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 98, 2524–2531.
band electroencephalographic activity over occipital cortex indexes 40. Repp, B.H. (2000). Compensation for subliminal timing perturbations in
visuospatial attention bias and predicts visual target detection. perceptual-motor synchronization. Psychol. Res. 63, 106–128.
J. Neurosci. 26, 9494–9502. 41. Praamstra, P., Turgeon, M., Hesse, C.W., Wing, A.M., and Perryer, L.
16. Van Der Werf, J., Jensen, O., Fries, P., and Medendorp, W.P. (2008). (2003). Neurophysiological correlates of error correction in sensori-
Gamma-band activity in human posterior parietal cortex encodes the motor-synchronization. Neuroimage 20, 1283–1297.
Please cite this article in press as: Bonnefond and Jensen, Alpha Oscillations Serve to Protect Working Memory Maintenance against
Anticipated Distracters, Current Biology (2012), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.029
Current Biology Vol 22 No 20
6

42. Jensen, O., van Dijk, H., and Mazaheri, A. (2010). Amplitude asymmetry
as a mechanism for the generation of slow evoked responses. Clin.
Neurophysiol. 121, 1148–1149, author reply 1149–1150.
43. Jensen, O., Bonnefond, M., and VanRullen, R. (2012). An oscillatory
mechanism for prioritizing salient unattended stimuli. Trends Cogn.
Sci. 16, 200–206.
44. Haegens, S., Nácher, V., Luna, R., Romo, R., and Jensen, O. (2011).
a-Oscillations in the monkey sensorimotor network influence discrimi-
nation performance by rhythmical inhibition of neuronal spiking. Proc.
Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 108, 19377–19382.
45. Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory?
Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 127–138.
46. Fell, J., and Axmacher, N. (2011). The role of phase synchronization in
memory processes. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 105–118.

You might also like