You are on page 1of 10

This article was downloaded by: [The University of British Columbia]

On: 09 December 2014, At: 17:28


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Cognitive Neuroscience
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pcns20

Effects of transcranial direct-current stimulation


(tDCS) of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC)
during a mixed-gambling risky decision-making task
a b c b d e c
Ludovico Minati , Camila Campanhã , Hugo D. Critchley & Paulo Sérgio Boggio
a
Scientific Department , Fondazione IRCCS Istituto Neurologico Carlo Besta , Milano , Italy
b
Department of Psychiatry , Brighton and Sussex Medical School (BSMS) , Falmer , UK
c
Social and Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory and Developmental Disorders Programme ,
Centre for Health and Biological Sciences, Mackenzie Presbyterian University , São Paulo ,
Brazil
d
Neurobehavioural Clinic, Sussex Partnership NHS Foundation Trust , Brighton and Hove ,
UK
e
Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex , Falmer , UK
Accepted author version posted online: 03 Oct 2011.Published online: 11 Jan 2012.

To cite this article: Ludovico Minati , Camila Campanhã , Hugo D. Critchley & Paulo Sérgio Boggio (2012) Effects of
transcranial direct-current stimulation (tDCS) of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) during a mixed-gambling risky
decision-making task, Cognitive Neuroscience, 3:2, 80-88, DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2011.628382

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2011.628382

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and
are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of
the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE, 2012, 3 (2), 80–88

Effects of transcranial direct-current stimulation (tDCS)


of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) during
a mixed-gambling risky decision-making task

Ludovico Minati1,2, Camila Campanhã3, Hugo D. Critchley2,4,5, and Paulo Sérgio Boggio3
Downloaded by [The University of British Columbia] at 17:28 09 December 2014

1
Scientific Department, Fondazione IRCCS Istituto Neurologico Carlo Besta, Milano, Italy
2
Department of Psychiatry, Brighton and Sussex Medical School (BSMS), Falmer, UK
3
Social and Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory and Developmental Disorders Programme, Centre for
Health and Biological Sciences, Mackenzie Presbyterian University, São Paulo, Brazil
4
Neurobehavioural Clinic, Sussex Partnership NHS Foundation Trust, Brighton and Hove, UK
5
Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex, Falmer, UK

Previous studies have led to hypothesizing that right DLPFC activity is related to risk propensity, but the generality
of this relationship remains unclear. Here, we experimentally modulated DLPFC activity in 47 healthy, female
volunteers during a risky decision-making task, where monetary gambles defined by potential win, loss, and
outcome probability were evaluated in the absence of feedback and uncertainty. Three participant groups,
receiving left anodal/right cathodal, right anodal/left cathodal, and sham tDCS, were compared. Overall,
participants performed the task well above chance level and were significantly risk averse, but tDCS did not
affect task performance or risk propensity. However, right anodal/left cathodal tDCS significantly elevated response
confidence, independently of accept/reject response. Our results suggest that the relationship between DLPFC
activity and risk propensity may not be universally valid for all task types and requires further characterization.
Enhancing right and attenuating left DLPFC activity boosts subjective confidence, with potential implications for
understanding pathological gambling.

Keywords: Decision-making; Prospect theory; Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS); Dorsolateral prefrontal
cortex (DLPFC).

Many times every day we need to choose between systematically select the one with the highest EV, but
options carrying risks and potential rewards. An obser- living beings display specific adaptive biases
ver optimizing long-term gain would simply calculate (Bernoulli, 1954; von Neumann & Morgenstern,
the expected value (EV) of each option and 1953). An influential account of decisional behavior

Correspondence should be addressed to: Ludovico Minati, Scientific Department, Fondazione IRCCS Istituto Neurologico Carlo Besta, Via
Celoria 11, 20133 Milano MI, Italy. E-mail: lminati@ieee.org
L.M. was wholly funded and employed by Fondazione IRCCS Istituto Neurologico Carlo Besta (FINCB) during the core period of this study.
This research also forms part of a BSMS doctoral dissertation (10/056/MIN) and is based on preparatory work conducted while L.M. was
previously employed by BSMS. C.C. was supported by Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo (2009/04694-1). H.D.C. was
supported by a program grant from the Wellcome Trust. P.S.B. was supported by Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico
(305718/2009-6) and by MackPesquisa. The authors are grateful to Karina Di Siervi, Ana Alem, and Nathalia Baptista for excellent operational
assistance in the application of tDCS during data acquisition, to Sylvie Piacentini for useful discussions, and to three anonymous reviewers for
useful comments on an earlier draft. All authors declare under their own responsibility that they do not have any real or perceived conflicts of
interest pertaining to the present study.

© 2012 Psychology Press, an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an Informa business
www.psypress.com/cognitiveneuroscience http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2011.628382
DLPFC STIMULATION DURING DECISION-MAKING 81

is prospect theory, proposed by Kahneman and evidence for a complex relationship between DLPFC
Tversky (1979), according to which risky choice activity and risk-taking comes from tDCS studies of
options initially undergo an “editing” phase, integrat- drug addicts, who display a markedly different rela-
ing the available decisional parameters (e.g., potential tionship between stimulation pattern and risk propen-
wins and losses) into more abstract variables such as sity (Boggio, Zaghi, et al., 2010).
EV and outcome variance, followed by evaluation with Unfortunately, the two most widely used decision-
respect to a set “reference value.” Prospect theory can making tasks (Iowa Gambling Task and the Risk Task)
account for pervasive behavioral phenomena such as do not provide information relevant to understanding
risk and loss aversion by introducing nonlinear value the role of the DLPFC in the framework of prospect
functions that are evolutionarily plausible––for exam- theory. In the Iowa Gambling Task, participants pick
ple, overweighing potentially fatal losses with respect cards from four decks characterized by initially
to equivalent gains (Wilkinson, 2008). unknown and differing proportions of winning and
Human decision-making engages multiple neocorti- losing cards, and need to learn from outcomes which
Downloaded by [The University of British Columbia] at 17:28 09 December 2014

cal regions, principally in the prefrontal cortex, as well decks are overall advantageous and which ones are not.
as phylogenetically older structures (e.g., Krueger, This task has good ecological validity but limited func-
Grafman, & McCabe, 2008; Trepel, Fox, & Poldrack, tional specificity: As it involves feedback and uncer-
2005). In particular, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex tainty/ambiguity, performance depends heavily on
(DLPFC) is heavily implicated in action representation, intact learning and executive functions (Bechara,
executive function, behavioral control and motor plan- Damasio, Damasio, & Anderson, 1994). During the
ning (e.g., Hoshi, 2006), subserving decision-making Risk Task, on the other hand, participants are presented
principally through multi-attribute integration during with six boxes, colored pink or blue, and are requested
EV computation (Kahnt, Heinzle, Park, & Haynes, to guess the color of the box containing a “winning
2010). Patients with DLPFC lesions perform poorly on token.” The proportion of pink/blue boxes varies
the Iowa Gambling Task, and right-lateralized damage among trials and determines the available reward,
is especially linked to elevated risk-taking behavior and such that the riskiest choices are always associated
poor information integration (Clark, Manes, Antoun, with the largest potential rewards. This task does not
Sahakian, & Robbins, 2003). Experimental disruption involve ambiguity yet fails to isolate EV computation
of right, but not left, DLPFC function by low-frequency from risk sensitivity, since large rewards are preferen-
repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) also tially associated with unlikely outcomes (Rogers et al.,
increases risk propensity (Knoch et al., 2006). 1999).
Recently, the role of the DLPFC in decision- The aim of this study was to apply tDCS to explore
making has also been investigated by transcranial the role of the DLPFC in a risky decision-making task
direct current stimulation (tDCS) (Boggio, Campanhã, designed to index choice-option evaluation only, and
Valasek, Fecteau, & Pascual-Leone, 2010; Fecteau, involving the presentation of simple monetary gambles,
Knoch, et al., 2007). tDCS involves applying a weak where all information was explicitly provided and out-
direct current delivered by a battery-driven, constant- come feedback was not revealed (Minati et al., 2011). In
current stimulator to the scalp through two saline- light of the hypothesis of a straightforward general
soaked, surface sponge electrodes (35 cm2). The effect relationship between right DLPFC activity and risk-
depends on the current orientation, whereby anodal taking, predicated on converging results from lesion
stimulation typically increases cortical excitability studies (Clark et al., 2003), rTMS (Knoch et al.,
while cathodal stimulation reduces it, and tDCS has 2006), and tDCS in healthy young participants
well-established value in exploring working memory, (Fecteau, Knoch, et al., 2007), we hypothesized that
language, and emotional processing (Nitsche & Paulus, enhancing right and inhibiting left DLPFC activity
2000). with right anodal/left cathodal stimulation would shift
Interestingly, findings from tDCS experiments chal- responses toward an overall more conservative
lenge the view that right DLPFC activity mitigates risk- approach, whereas right cathodal/left anodal stimulation
taking in general, independently of the population would have the opposite effect. A negative finding, on
under study. In early experiments on young partici- the other hand, would go along with previous tDCS
pants, enhancing right DLPFC activity by anodal sti- results on elderly controls (Boggio, Campanhã, et al.,
mulation promoted the selection of safer choices 2010) and drug addicts (Boggio, Zaghi, et al., 2010) and
(Fecteau, Knoch, et al., 2007). Later investigations, challenge the view that the relationship between right
however, demonstrated that this pattern reverses with DLPFC activity and risk-taking reflects a fundamental
age, arguably reflecting transcallosal functional reor- feature of the brain that can be generalized to diverse
ganization (Boggio, Campanhã, et al., 2010). Further populations and tasks.
82 MINATI ET AL.

METHODS also administered prior to stimulation (as in Boggio,


Campanhã, et al., 2010). Upon completion of the experi-
Participants ment, all participants rated their perception of the stimu-
lation they had received on a 10-point scale, ranging
Forty-seven female participants were recruited by con- from –5 (“I am sure it was sham stimulation”) to 5 (“I
venience sampling among regular university students am sure it was active stimulation”). The characteristics
and randomly assigned to three groups: sham tDCS, of the participants are summarized in Table 1.
left anodal/right cathodal tDCS, and right anodal/left
cathodal tDCS. The experiment was undertaken with
the understanding and written, informed consent of all tDCS
participants, and approved by the institutional ethics
committee of the Mackenzie Presbyterian University, The device used, developed in-house, is well suited for
registered with the National Ethics Committee of blind stimulation, as a switch enables interruption of
Downloaded by [The University of British Columbia] at 17:28 09 December 2014

Brazil (SISNEP), and by the Research Governance and the current flow while maintaining the “ON” display
Ethics Committee (RGEC) of the Brighton and Sussex and showing the stimulation parameters unaltered. The
Medical School (BSMS, PhD project no. 10/056/MIN). electrode montage was the same as in previous studies
Participants did not receive any money but were awarded (Boggio, Campanhã, et al., 2010; Fecteau, Knoch,
university credits in line with local guidelines. et al., 2007), and the sponge electrodes were applied
All participants were right-handed according to the over the F3 and F4 sites (international EEG 10/20
Edinburgh Handedness Inventory. They were naive to system). While stimulating the left and right DLPFC
tDCS and the task under study, were free from neurolo- simultaneously with opposite polarities prevents separ-
gical and psychiatric disorders, and were not taking any ating the effects of enhancing activity on one side and
medication acting on the central nervous system inhibiting it on the other, the choice of this setup here
or illicit drugs. They were assessed with the Beck was motivated for comparability with previous results
Anxiety Inventory (BAI) (Beck & Steer, 1993), Beck obtained on the Risk Task (in Boggio, Campanhã, et al.,
Depression Inventory BDI) (Beck, Steer, & Brown, 2010; Boggio, Zaghi, et al., 2010; Fecteau, Knoch, et al.,
1996), and the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS) 2007). During active stimulation, participants received a
(Patton, Stanford, & Barratt, 1995). A visual-analog constant current of 2 mA. Stimulation was enabled
scale (VAS) evaluating mood state on 14 axes was approximately 3 min before starting the task and disabled

TABLE 1
Participant data for the three groups. Values are given as mean  SD. p value refers to ANOVA across the groups. See text for details

Left anodal/right Right anodal/left


Sham tDCS (n ¼ 16) cathodal tDCS (n ¼ 16) cathodal tDCS (n ¼ 15) ANOVA

Age (years) 21.8  2.5 22.3  3.2 20.9  1.0 p ¼ .3


Education (years) 15.8  2.5 16.3  3.2 14.9  1.0 p ¼ .3
Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) 8.2  5.0 6.1  5.3 7.7  4.8 p ¼ .3
Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) 7.7  5.5 5.2  4.5 6.1  3.9 p ¼ .4
Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS) 68.6  7.5 68.5  4.8 70.1  5.9 p ¼ .7
Visual analog scales (VAS)
Alert / drowsy 5.4  2.2 6.1  2.4 5.4  2.8 p ¼ .7
Calm / restless 4.5  2.3 4.2  1.5 4.2  1.9 p ¼ .9
Strong / weak 4.6  2.2 5.4  1.9 4.6  2.3 p ¼ .4
Confused / lucid 7.5  1.8 7.3  1.8 6.8  2.3 p ¼ .6
Sharp / blunt 4.9  2.4 4.7  1.5 4.8  2.0 p ¼ 1
Apathetic / dynamic 7.6  2.1 6.8  1.8 7.3  1.8 p ¼ .5
Satisfied / unfulfilled 5.2  2.5 4.2  2.3 4.4  2.0 p ¼ .4
Worried / unconcerned 5.8  2.8 5.5  3.0 5.0  2.2 p ¼ .7
Slow-minded / fast-thinking 6.8  1.5 7.2  1.4 6.5  1.3 p ¼ .4
Tense / relaxed 6.8  2.4 5.4  2.7 5.8  2.2 p ¼ .3
Attentive / neglectful 4.1  2.1 4.4  1.7 4.6  2.2 p ¼ .8
Inept / competent 8.1  1.4 7.5  1.7 8.0  1.8 p ¼ .6
Happy / sad 4.4  2.1 3.9  2.2 3.9  2.2 p ¼ .8
Hostile / friendly 7.8  1.5 8.0  1.5 7.8  1.9 p ¼ .9
Stimulation perception 1.1  3.3 0.8  3.3 0.9  2.8 p ¼ 1
DLPFC STIMULATION DURING DECISION-MAKING 83

immediately upon completion; the current was ramped convenience of each gamble by means of their “gut
up and down with slopes of 10 s. For the sham condition, feeling” without any mathematical computation, and
the current was slowly ramped down after 30 s of stimu- decide whether to accept or reject as rapidly as possi-
lation with random polarity, preserving the initial tingling ble, within a maximum of 6 s. In other words, choice
sensation often associated with active tDCS. For safety was between a zero-expected value option (EV ¼ 0)
reasons, the experimenter remained in the room for the and an option with EV ¼ p  kWIN–(1–p)  kLOSE.
whole duration of the experiment, away from the parti- They were informed that no feedback would be
cipant’s visual field. The overall effective duration of provided after each gamble, but the computer would
stimulation was 20.5  4.1 min across participants. The keep track of their responses in the form of a virtual
stimulation protocol and parameters complied with bank account, starting from a balance of zero and
accepted safety guidelines (Nitsche et al., 2008). updated with the amount won or lost each time.
Participants were also informed that the outcomes of
the gambles were not predetermined, but generated
Downloaded by [The University of British Columbia] at 17:28 09 December 2014

Gambling task by the computer only at the time of pressing the


response button. Further, they were told that their
The study task has been previously employed to assess overall aim was to earn as much money as possible,
decision-making in patients with Parkinson’s disease imagining they were playing with real money, that
and Huntington’s disease (Minati et al., 2011). As they would be able to see their final balance at the
shown in Figure 1a, each gamble consisted of three end of the experiment, and that the usefulness of their
parameters: potential loss (thereafter referred to as participation would depend on how much money they
kLOSE), potential win (kWIN), and probability of winning earned.
(p, not to be confused with statistical test probability). Across all gambles, the EV was nearly normally
These were presented simultaneously, in white Arial distributed in the range R$ [–300, 300] (Brazilian
font (size 36 points) over a black background, on a 17- national currency), having mean R$ –1.2  46.7,
inch screen positioned at about 1 m from the head. The Kolmogorov–Smirnov Z ¼ 1.4, p ¼ .04, skewness 0
task was written using the Cogent 2000 Toolkit and kurtosis 1.3 (i.e., symmetric and mildly leptokur-
(Wellcome Laboratory of Neurobiology, London, UK). tic). The range for kWIN and kLOSE was R$ [(–300,
Within each session, 240 gambles were shown, 300], while p was in [.1, .9]. The linear correlation
interspersed with three evenly spaced pauses of arbi- coefficient with respect to EV was similar across the
trary duration determined by the participant (<2 min three parameters displayed, namely r ¼ .29 for kWIN,
each). The task was self-paced; i.e., a new gamble r ¼ .32 for kLOSE and r ¼ .32 for p. There was no
appeared immediately after each response. The first significant difference between the distributions of kWIN
60 gambles (corresponding to approximately 4 min) and kLOSE, Mann–Whitney Z ¼ –0.3, p ¼ .7. This
were discarded from analysis, allowing for practice and balancing is relevant to ensure that, if decisional beha-
stabilization of the neuromodulation; the first five gam- vior is purely driven by EV, no preferential effects
bles after each pause were also ignored. related to kWIN, kLOSE or p will emerge. Scatter plots
Participants were instructed to give equal weight to and histograms of the gamble characteristics are shown
the three parameters, carefully consider the in Figure 1b–h.

Figure 1. Gamble pool characteristics. (a) Presentation format. (b–d) Scatter plots for EV as a function of kWIN, kLOSE and p. (e–h) Cumulative
distributions of EV, kWIN, kLOSE and p. See Methods section for description.
84 MINATI ET AL.

Participants responded according to a four-level All the parameters above were entered in between-
scale––i.e., “confident reject,” “unsure reject,” “unsure subject ANOVAs, having one factor with three levels
accept,” and “confident accept” They were informed that (sham, left anodal/right cathodal, and right anodal/left
there was no difference between the consequences of cathodal tDCS). To avoid potential bias, Welch’s cor-
confident and unsure responses, but that they should rection for the number of degrees of freedom was
attempt to make use of all four levels throughout the applied, adjusting the F value to account for unequal
task. This four-level scale suggested in Tom, Fox, variance across the three stimulation conditions. Post-
Trepel, and Poldrack (2007) was utilized to promote dee- hoc comparisons were performed with Games–Howell
per processing––i.e., to prompt participants to reflect on tests, which are moderately conservative and do not
subjective advantageousness instead of using a fixed rule. assume homoscedasticity. Accounting for multiple
comparisons across nine parameters (see below), we
set α ¼ .006 according to Bonferroni’s criterion. One-
Statistical analysis sample t-tests were also performed to demonstrate
Downloaded by [The University of British Columbia] at 17:28 09 December 2014

overall features of the behavioral responses.


Data analyses were performed with custom scripts
written under MATLAB 7.4 (Math Works, Inc.,
Natick, MA, USA) and SPSS 17.0 (SPSS, Inc., Power analysis
Somers, NY, USA). The reaction times (RT)s were
z-normalized at individual level, and gambles for We estimated effective power from the effect size
which these fell outside 3 SD (1.2% overall) from the of previous tDCS studies on the Risk Task––i.e.,
average were removed. f , 1.7 between Boggio, Campanhã, et al. (2010) and
For each participant, the accuracy, i.e., the proportion Boggio, Zaghi, et al. (2010). Using G*Power 3 soft-
of positive EV gambles accepted and negative EV gam- ware (University of Kiel, Kiel, Germany), assuming
bles rejected, was computed. The overall proportion of α ¼.006 and sample size n ¼ 47 with three groups, we
gambles accepted (subsequently referred to as accep- found the effective power (1 – β) to be 0.99.
tance) and the proportion of confident responses (sub-
sequently referred to as confidence) were calculated.
The equivalent amount earned was also computed, RESULTS
assuming a gain/loss equal to EV (positive or negative)
for each gamble taken and zero for each gamble rejected. The effects of stimulation on behavioral responses and
In order to probe the relative weight of each gamble corresponding statistics are reported in Table 2, and
parameter on decisional behavior, four logistic regres- scatter plots are shown in Figure 2. As indicated in
sions were performed, separately modeling gamble Table 1, participants in the three groups were matched
acceptance in terms of kWIN, kLOSE, p, and EV. on all scales (BAI, BDI, BIS, and VAS), and could not

TABLE 2
Effects of stimulation on behavioral responses. Values are given as mean  SD. ANOVAs included Welch’s correction, and
post-hoc comparisons were performed with Games–Howell tests (see Methods). RT: reaction time; Acceptance: proportion of
accepted gambles; Accuracy: proportion of negative EV gambles rejected and positive EV gambles accepted; Confidence: overall
proportion of confident responses; β(kWIN), β(kLOSE), β(p), and β(EV): logistic regression coefficients for reject/accept response.
See also Figure 2

Sham tDCS Left anodal/right cathodal Right anodal/left cathodal


(n ¼ 16) tDCS (n ¼ 16) tDCS (n ¼ 15) ANOVA

Mean RT 3780  760 ms 3660  880 ms 3200  870 ms p ¼ .2


Acceptance 0.41  0.14 0.43  0.14 0.41  0.10 p ¼ .9
Accuracy 0.78  0.07 0.76  0.10 0.76  0.07 p ¼ .9
Confidence 0.40  0.19 0.50  0.27 0.64  0.17 F(2, 28.7) ¼ 6.9, p ¼ .004, η2p ¼ .18
Right anodal vs. sham p ¼ .002
Amount earned R$1660  1000 R$1380  510 R$1410  410 p ¼ .6
β(kWIN) 0.01  0.009 0.009  0.004 0.013  0.004 p ¼ .1
β(kLOSE) -0.012  0.009 -0.009  0.006 -0.013  0.005 p ¼ .06
β(p) -0.4  3.2 0.4  2.1 -1.6  3.2 p ¼ .1
β(EV) 0.037  0.01 0.037  0.017 0.032  0.014 p ¼ .5
DLPFC STIMULATION DURING DECISION-MAKING 85
Downloaded by [The University of British Columbia] at 17:28 09 December 2014

Figure 2. Group comparison scatter plots. Acceptance: Proportion of accepted gambles. Accuracy: Proportion of negative-EV gambles rejected
and positive-EV gambles accepted. Confidence: Overall proportion of confident responses. β(kWIN), β(kLOSE), β(p) and β(EV): Logistic regression
coefficients for reject/accept response. See Methods section for description and Table 2 for statistical results.

distinguish between active and sham stimulation. No (0.64  0.17 vs. 0.40  0.19, p ¼ .002); these effects
participant reported any adverse effect. survived correction for multiple comparisons, for which
Overall, participants performed the task significantly α ¼ .006. Additional analyses performed separating
above chance level (0.77  0.08 vs. 0.50, t(46) ¼ 22.5, confidence for accept and reject responses revealed no
p < .001), earned significantly more than zero (R$1410 significant interaction (p ¼ .3). Further, inserting the
 470 vs. R$0, t(46) ¼ 20.7, p < .001), and were BAI, BDI, BIS, and VAS scores as covariates did not
significantly risk-averse; i.e., they accepted less than reveal any significant main effect or interaction.
50% of gambles (0.42  0.13 vs. 0.50, t(46) ¼ 4.2, p Right anodal/left cathodal tDCS was also associated
< .001). Accuracy, equivalent amount earned, and over- with slightly reduced RTs (3200  870 ms vs. 3780 
all risk propensity were clearly not modulated by stimu- 760 ms), but the effect was clearly not significant
lation (p  .6). There was a strong main effect on (p ¼ .2). Throughout the task, the RTs decreased on
response confidence, F(2, 28.7) ¼ 6.9, p ¼ .004, average by 18  16% between the first and the last 30
η2p ¼ .18, and post-hoc comparisons indicated that par- gambles taken into consideration for analysis.
ticipants receiving right anodal/left cathodal tDCS were Stimulation did not significantly affect the logistic
more confident than those receiving inactive stimulation regression coefficients.
86 MINATI ET AL.

DISCUSSION EV Þ 0 options (as in our task), influence the effect of


stimulating the DLPFC.
Irrespective of polarity, tDCS of the DLPFC did not Right anodal/left cathodal tDCS boosted response
affect overall response accuracy, equivalent amount confidence, while the opposite polarity had no signifi-
earned, RTs, and logistic regression coefficients. In cant effect. While we are unable to definitely conclude
particular, there was clearly no effect on risk propensity to what extent this effect was due to enhanced right
(p ¼ .9): As the attained power was 0.99, this is unlikely rather than attenuated left DLPFC activity, this finding
to be a false negative. However, right anodal/left cath- is in agreement with and extends previous work suggest-
odal tDCS significantly increased response confidence, ing a link between DLPFC activity and subjective con-
and the effect survived stringent Bonferroni correction. fidence. In an early object recognition study, right
Importantly, the stimulation protocol was analogous to DLPFC activity correlated positively with decision con-
that employed in previous studies on the Risk Task, in fidence independently of recognition judgment itself
which clearly significant modulation of risk propensity (Henson, Rugg, Shallice, & Dolan, 2000). This associa-
Downloaded by [The University of British Columbia] at 17:28 09 December 2014

was engendered, and the number of participants (47) was tion was confirmed in another investigation, where early
larger than all previous studies in this area (i.e., 27 in (i.e., 100 ms post-stimulus) magnetoencephalographic
Knoch et al., 2006; 36 in Fecteau, Knoch, et al., 2007; 28 responses in the right DLPFC were enhanced for trials
in Boggio, Campanhã, et al., 2010). receiving confident responses during a task involving the
Our results are at odds with a mechanistic view that presentation of trivia questions (Camchong, Goodie,
attenuating right with respect to left DLPFC activity McDowell, Gilmore, & Clementz, 2007). Recent work
generally leads to increased risk-taking, whereas indicates DLPFC involvement in determining the posi-
enhancing it promotes the selection of safer options. tive emotional bias associated with effective problem-
This hypothesis was advanced on the basis of conver- solving, as when assembling sentences from scrambled
ging results from lesion studies based on the Iowa words (Viviani, Lo, Sim, Beschoner, & Stingl, 2010).
Gambling Task (Clark et al., 2003) and brain stimula- Interestingly, there is evidence that gambling-related
tion experiments using the Risk Task (Boggio, cues elicit larger right DLPFC activity in problematic
Campanhã, et al., 2010; Fecteau, Knoch, et al., gamblers with respect to controls, potentially reflecting
2007). Existing literature, however, already included the high level of subjective confidence and illusion
findings casting doubt on the hypothesis of a straight- of control typically observed in these individuals
forward relationship. For instance, tDCS in older (e.g., Crockford, Goodyear, Edwards, Quickfall, & el-
adults and marijuana users appears to yield a “paradox Guebaly, 2005; Delfabbro, 2004). Our results suggest a
effect”, enhancing risk-taking irrespective of stimula- causative role; i.e., augmenting right and attenuating left
tion polarity (Boggio, Campanhã, et al., 2010; DLPFC activity through neuromodulation exaggerated
Boggio, Zaghi, et al., 2010). In another study, using confidence in decision-making. Unfortunately, we are
the Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART), tDCS unable to exclude a general motivational effect, since
decreased risk propensity, again irrespective of polar- mood questionnaires were not administered after stimu-
ity, but not when unilateral stimulation was applied lation. There was no significant interaction between con-
(Fecteau, Pascual-Leone, et al., 2007). fidence and decision (accept or reject) and no
The influence of task type draws attention to defini- corresponding change in risk propensity, suggesting that
tions of risk propensity, which is not a unitary construct it is unlikely that the effect represents a nonspecific
but depends on specific real-life situations and experi- “optimism” bias related to the perceived likelihood of
mental settings, such as go/no-go versus multiple-choice gamble outcome. Further, given that task performance
tasks (e.g., Botella, Narvaez, Martinez-Molina, Rubio, & was clearly unaffected, increased confidence does not
Santacreu, 2008). Risk propensity emerges from com- seem to be related to an objective boost in cognitive
plex, nonlinear interactions that are task-dependent–– performance. Rather, our data point to an effect primarily
hence, the observation that modulating DLPFC activity related to the subjective perception of the quality of one’s
influences risk propensity in some tasks does not imply decisions, paralleling observations in pathological gam-
that this role is universal. From an anatomical perspec- blers who are deluded that their decisions will eventually
tive, decision-making under uncertainty and risk overlap enable them to beat the odds and earn or recoup money
only partially, as individual regions assume different (Delfabbro, 2004). Interestingly, spiking models of sen-
roles depending on the type of decision (Krain, Wilson, sory discrimination and subsequent choice predict faster
Arbuckle, Castellanos, & Milham, 2006). It is therefore and larger synaptic responses on easy versus difficult
not surprising that task-specific differences in the “edit- choices, offering a framework to explore the effects of
ing” phase, such as whether choice is between two EV Þ neuromodulation and the relationship between neuronal
0 options (e.g., in the Risk Task) or between EV ¼ 0 and excitability and subjective confidence (Rolls et al., 2010).
DLPFC STIMULATION DURING DECISION-MAKING 87

An issue with this and previous studies (e.g., Boggio, different types of decision-making tasks. Enhancing
Campanhã, et al., 2010; Boggio, Zaghi, et al., 2010; right and attenuating left DLPFC activity leads to
Fecteau, Knoch, et al., 2007; Knoch et al., 2006) is more confident responses, corroborating previous evi-
that participants did not play with real money. While dence of an association between this region and sub-
we cannot make inferences on how financial remunera- jective confidence. This effect warrants further
tion could have modified the effect of tDCS, a recent confirmation and investigation, especially with regard
investigation has found that the effect of endowment on to its potential relationship to biases observed in patho-
task performance is not straightforward––i.e., small logical gamblers.
rewards may decrease performance and introduce con- Original manuscript received 15 June 2011
founds related to the relative importance of financial Revised manuscript accepted 27 September 2011
compensation (Gneezy & Rustichini, 2000). Further First published online 11 January 2012
work is also needed to address potential effects of
response instructions, as expressing the degree of sub-
Downloaded by [The University of British Columbia] at 17:28 09 December 2014

jective confidence could have altered the decisional


behavior with respect to a simple accept/reject situation.
REFERENCES
It appears motivated to investigate whether this task is Barasinska, N. (2010). Does gender affect funding success at
sensitive to the modality of presentation of the economic the peer-to-peer credit markets? Evidence from the largest
parameters––e.g., for example, whether they are exactly German lending platform. DIW Discussion Paper, 1094.
provided as numbers instead of visual analogs. While Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W.
the RTs changed only marginally throughout the experi- (2009). Insensitivity to future consequences following
damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7–15.
ment, we cannot exclude a difference in motivational Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1993). Beck Anxiety Inventory
status related to the absence of feedback, motivating manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
further exploration introducing outcome feedback. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck
Further, we cannot exclude outright that different results Depression Inventory–II manual (2nd ed.). San Antonio,
would have been obtained with male participants. Yet, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Bernoulli, D. (1954). Exposition of a new theory on the
in previous studies using the Risk Task, the effect of measurement of risk (republished). Econometrica, 22,
tDCS was independent of gender (Boggio, Campanhã, 22–36.
et al., 2010; Fecteau, Knoch, et al., 2007), and recent Boggio, P. S., Campanhã, C., Valasek, C. A., Fecteau, S.,
large-scale studies of real financial markets found no Pascual-Leone, A., & Fregni, F. (2010). Modulation of
difference in the investing behavior of males and decision-making in a gambling task in older adults with
transcranial direct current stimulation. European Journal
females (Barasinska, 2010). of Neuroscience, 31, 593–597.
It is important to recognize that the anatomical Boggio, P. S., Zaghi, S., Villani, A. B., Fecteau, S., Pascual-
specificity of neuromodulation is potentially limited. Leone, A., & Fregni, F. (2010). Modulation of risk-taking
First, the prefrontal cortices are strongly interconnected in marijuana users by transcranial direct current stimula-
and interdependent, and transcallosal connections typi- tion (tDCS) of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
(DLPFC). Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 112, 220–225.
cally inhibit the contralateral homolog when a region is Botella, J., Narvaez, M., Martinez-Molina, A., Rubio, V. J., &
stimulated (e.g., Ongür & Price, 2000; Voineskos et al., Santacreu, J. (2008). A dilemmas task for eliciting risk
2010). Second, even though recent imaging studies propensity. Psychological Record, 58, 529–546.
have demonstrated the local effects of tDCS on cortical Camchong, J., Goodie, A. S., McDowell, J. E., Gilmore, C. S,
excitability, complex current distributions may be gen- & Clementz, B. A. (2007). A cognitive neuroscience
erated that extend beyond the regions immediately approach to studying the role of overconfidence in problem
gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 23, 185–199.
underneath the electrodes (e.g., Paquette, Sidel, Clark, L., Manes, F., Antoun, N., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins,
Radinska, Soucy, & Thiel, 2011; Sadleir, Vannorsdall, T. W. (2003). The contributions of lesion laterality and
Schretlen, & Gordon, 2010). lesion volume to decision-making impairment following
In conclusion, our study shows that tDCS of the frontal lobe damage. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1474–1483.
DLPFC does not significantly affect the evaluation of Crockford, D. N., Goodyear, B., Edwards, J., Quickfall, J., &
el-Guebaly, N. (2005). Cue-induced brain activity in patho-
risky choice options characterized by EV determined logical gamblers. Biological Psychology, 58, 787–795.
by explicitly indicated potential loss, gain, and out- Delfabbro, P. (2004). The stubborn logic of regular gamblers:
come probability. This extends previous work with Obstacles and dilemmas in cognitive gambling research.
the Iowa Gambling Task and the Risk Task, challen- Journal of Gambling Studies, 20, 1–21.
ging the mechanistic view that risk propensity can be Fecteau, S., Knoch, D., Fregni, F., Sultani, N., Boggio, P. S., &
Pascual-Leone, A. (2007). Diminishing risk-taking beha-
universally modulated through DLPFC stimulation, vior by modulating activity in the prefrontal cortex: A
and motivates further research aimed at better charac- direct current stimulation study. Journal of Neuroscience,
terizing the exact involvement of this region across 27, 12500–12505.
88 MINATI ET AL.

Fecteau, S., Pascual-Leone, A., Zald, D. H., Liguori, P., current stimulation: State of the art 2008. Brain
Théoret, H., Boggio, P. S., et al. (2007). Activation of Stimulation, 1, 206–223.
prefrontal cortex by transcranial direct current stimulation Ongür, D., & Price, J. L. (2000). The organization of networks
reduces appetite for risk during ambiguous decision mak- within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats,
ing. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 6212–6218. monkeys and humans. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 206–219.
Gneezy, U., & Rustichini, A. (2000). Pay enough or don’t pay Paquette, C., Sidel, M., Radinska, B. A., Soucy, J. P., & Thiel, A.
at all. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 115, 791–810. (2011). Bilateral transcranial direct current stimulation mod-
Henson, R. N. A., Rugg, M. D., Shallice, T., & Dolan, ulates activation-induced regional blood flow changes dur-
R. J. (2000). Confidence in recognition memory for ing voluntary movement. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow
words: Dissociating right prefrontal roles in episodic and Metabolism, 31(10), 2086–2095.
retrieval. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, Patton, J. M., Stanford, M. S., & Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor
913–923. structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. Journal of
Hoshi, E. (2006). Functional specialization within the dorso- Clinical Psychology, 51, 768–774.
lateral prefrontal cortex: A review of anatomical and phy- Rogers, R. D., Owen, A. M., Middleton, H. C., Williams, E.
siological studies of non-human primates. Neuroscience J., Pickard, J. D., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W.
Downloaded by [The University of British Columbia] at 17:28 09 December 2014

Research, 54, 73–84. (1999). Choosing between small, likely rewards and
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: large, unlikely rewards activates inferior and orbital pre-
An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, frontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 9029–9038.
263–291. Rolls, E. T., Grabenhorst, F., & Deco, G. (2010). Decision-
Kahnt, T., Heinzle, J., Park, S. Q., & Haynes, J. D. (2010). making, errors, and confidence in the brain. Journal of
Decoding different roles for vmPFC and dlPFC in multi- Neurophysiology, 104, 2359–2374.
attribute decision making. NeuroImage, 56, 709–715. Sadleir, R. J., Vannorsdall, T. D., Schretlen, D. J., &
Knoch, D., Gianotti, L. R., Pascual-Leone, A., Treyer, V., Gordon, B. (2010). Transcranial direct current stimula-
Regard, M., Hohmann, M., et al. (2006). Disruption of tion (tDCS) in a realistic head model. NeuroImage, 51,
right prefrontal cortex by low-frequency repetitive tran- 1310–1318.
scranial magnetic stimulation induces risk-taking beha- Tom, S. M., Fox, C. R., Trepel, C., & Poldrack, R. A. (2007).
vior. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 6469–6472. The neural basis of loss aversion in decision-making
Krain, A. L., Wilson, A. M., Arbuckle, R., Castellanos, F. X., under risk. Science, 315, 515–518.
& Milham, M. P. (2006). Distinct neural mechanisms of Trepel, C., Fox, C. R., & Poldrack, R. A. (2005). Prospect
risk and ambiguity: A meta-analysis of decision-making. theory on the brain? Toward a cognitive neuroscience of
NeuroImage, 32, 477–484. decision under risk. Cognitive Brain Research, 23, 34–50.
Krueger, F., Grafman, J., & McCabe, K. (2008). Neural Viviani, R., Lo, H., Sim, E.-J., Beschoner, P., & Stingl, J. C.
correlates of economic game playing. Philosophical (2010). The neural substrate of positive bias
Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, in spontaneous emotional processing. PLoS ONE, 5,
Biological Sciences, 363, 3859–3874. e15454.
Minati, L., Piacentini, S., Ferrè, F., Nanetti, L., Romito, L., Voineskos A. N., Farzan, F., Barr, M. S., Lobaugh, N. J.,
Mariotti, C., et al. (2011). Choice-option evaluation is Mulsant, B. H., Chen, R., et al. (2010). The role of the
preserved in early Huntington and Parkinson’s disease. corpus callosum in transcranial magnetic stimulation
Neuroreport, 22(15), 753–757. induced interhemispheric signal propagation. Biological
Nitsche, M., & Paulus, W. (2000). Excitability changes Psychiatry, 68, 825–831.
induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial von Neumann, J., & Morgenstern, O. (1953). Theory of
direct current stimulation. Journal of Physiology, 527, games and economic behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
633–639. University Press.
Nitsche, M. A., Cohen, L. G., Wassermann, E. M., Priori, A., Wilkinson, N. (2008). An introduction to behavioural eco-
Lang, N., Antal, A., et al. (2008). Transcranial direct nomics. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave-Macmillan.

You might also like