You are on page 1of 6

articles nature publishing group

Behavior and Psychology

Disinhibited Eating in Obese Adolescents


Is Associated With Orbitofrontal Volume
Reductions and Executive Dysfunction
Lawrence Maayan1,2, Claire Hoogendoorn3, Victoria Sweat3 and Antonio Convit2–4

In adults, obesity has been associated with disinhibited eating, decreased cortical gray matter (GM) volume, and
lower performance on cognitive assessments. Much less is known about these relationships in adolescence and
there are no studies assessing behavioral, cognitive, and neurostructural measures in the same group of study
participants. This study examined the relationship between obesity, executive function, disinhibition, and brain
volumes in relatively healthy youth. Participants included 54 obese and 37 lean adolescents. Participants received a
cognitive battery, questionnaires of eating behaviors, and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Neuropsychological
assessments included tasks targeting frontal lobe function. Eating behaviors were determined using the Three Factor
Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ), and structural MRIs were performed on a 1.5 T Siemens Avanto MRI System (Siemens,
Erlangen, Germany) to determine brain GM volumes. Lean and obese adolescents were matched on age, years of
education, gender, and socioeconomic status. Relative to lean adolescents, obese participants had significantly
higher ratings of disinhibition on the TFEQ, lower performance on the cognitive tests, and lower orbitofrontal cortex
(OFC) volume. Disinhibition significantly correlated with BMI, Stroop color-word score, and OFC volume. This is the
first report of these associations in adolescents and point to the importance of better understanding the associations
between neurostructural deficits and obesity.

Obesity (2011) 19, 1382–1387. doi:10.1038/oby.2011.15

Introduction key area in inhibition of behavior stretches back to the case of


The prevalence of child and adolescent obesity in the United Phineas Gage, the unfortunate 19th century rail worker who
States has more than tripled since 1970. Although recent evi- survived an accident likely damaging his OFC, which resulted
dence suggests that childhood obesity may have leveled off, the in personality changes and increased impulsivity (7).
present high rates predict an impending public health problem Neurostructural findings have also been correlated with BMI.
involving cardiovascular and endocrine illness (1). In a small study of women 55 years of age and older, which
Disinhibition in eating behavior, which is characterized in employed voxel-based morphometry, BMI was negatively cor-
part as the propensity to eat opportunistically in response to related with gray matter (GM) volumes in several frontal areas
environmental cues, has long been associated with obesity both including the left orbitofrontal, right inferior frontal, and right
in youth and adults (2). The related control failure in caloric precentral gyri in addition to other regions including the right
intake that leads to eventual obesity may occur at several lev- cerebellum as well as a large right posterior region encompass-
els in the brain including the hypothalamus (3) and, according ing the parahippocampal, fusiform, and lingual gyri (8). A
to more recent work, in cerebral cortex (4). A series of func- larger study of 1,428 adults found a negative correlation among
tional neuroimaging studies of lean and obese individuals in males between BMI and overall GM as well as specific brain
both hungry and fed states have shown several cortical regions regions such as bilateral medial temporal lobes, occipital lobes,
including the anterior cingulate, medial prefrontal (5), insula, frontal lobes, precuneus, midbrain and anterior lobe of the cer-
posterior cingulate, temporal, and orbitofrontal cortices (6) to ebellum (9). Another voxel-based morphometry study showed
be differentially activated depending on the level of satiety and that obese adults have lower GM density in areas such as fron-
BMI, suggesting their involvement in the regulation of caloric tal operculum, middle frontal gyrus, postcentral gyrus, as well
intake. The understanding of orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) as a as putamen (10). Our group has described neurostructural
The first two authors contributed equally to the work.
1
Department of Child Psychiatry, New York University School of Medicine, New York, New York, USA; 2Nathan Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, Orangeburg,
New York, USA; 3Department of Psychiatry, New York University School of Medicine, New York, New York, USA; 4Department of Medicine, New York University School
of Medicine, New York, New York, USA. Correspondence: Antonio Convit (antonio.convit@med.nyu.edu)
Received 17 August 2010; accepted 8 January 2011; published online 24 February 2011. doi:10.1038/oby.2011.15

1382 VOLUME 19 NUMBER 7 | july 2011 | www.obesityjournal.org


articles
Behavior and Psychology

abnormalities among obese adolescents with type 2 diabetes Assessments


mellitus (T2DM) (11), but to our knowledge no such deficits Neuropsychological assessment. We conducted a broad evaluation
of neurocognitive functions, including intellectual achievement, recent
have been described among obese youth without T2DM.
memory, working memory, attention, and executive function. We
In addition to structural findings, cognitive assessments have hypothesized that there would be differences in frontal lobe functions
demonstrated that executive functioning and response inhibi- between lean and obese adolescents and therefore restricted our analy-
tion may be compromised in both adult and adolescent obese ses to neurocognitive tests that reflect frontal lobe integrity and intact
individuals. One study employing positron emission tomogra- executive functions, namely the Controlled Oral Word Association
Test (COWAT), Trail Making Test parts A&B, Stroop Task, Attention/
phy and cognitive tests found obese adults to have decreased
Concentration Index of the Wide Range Assessment of Learning and
baseline prefrontal glucose metabolism as well as diminished Memory (WRAML) and Working Memory Index of the WRAML.
performance on the Stroop task, a test of selective attention With the exception of the WRAML and the Stroop, which provide age-
and executive function (12). Other studies of executive func- corrected standard scores, raw scores are reported. All tests adminis-
tion and response inhibition in adults have shown a negative tered are standard neuropsychological instruments described in detail
elsewhere (17).
association of those variables with BMI (13–15). Furthermore,
extremely obese adolescents show decreased functioning on TFEQ. Characteristics of eating behavior were assessed by using the
executive tasks compared to normative data (16). TFEQ. The TFEQ is a 51-item instrument, composed of three subscales
measuring restraint (i.e., cognitive control of eating behavior; 21 items),
We hypothesized that consistent with previous findings
disinhibition (i.e., the susceptibility of eating in response to emotional
using the Three Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ), obese factors and sensory cues; 16 items), and hunger (i.e., the susceptibility
adolescents would have higher ratings of self-reported dis- of eating in response to feelings of hunger; 14 items). The TFEQ was
inhibition in eating behaviors. We further hypothesized administered ~1 h after subjects had lunch.
that obese adolescents would have lower scores on assess- MRI acquisition and image analyses. All subjects were studied
ments of executive function and reduced integrity in neu- on the same 1.5 T Siemens Avanto MRI System, which has a 65-inch
rostructural measures of frontal lobe (magnetic resonance diameter bore and a table suitable for up to a 400 pound individual. We
imaging (MRI)-based GM volumes as well as regional brain acquired T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradi-
ent echo images (MPRAGE; repetition time 1300 ms; echo time 4.38 ms;
volumes). In addition, we posited that disinhibition on the
inversion time 800 ms; field of view 250 × 250; slice thickness 1.2 mm;
TFEQ will be negatively associated with cognitive scores on NEX 1; flip angle 15°; matrix size 256 × 256; 192 coronal slices).
relevant domains as well as with MRI-based measurements
WM/GM volumetric analysis. Spatial normalization and segmenta-
of brain areas involved in response inhibition and executive
tion of MPRAGE images utilized automated procedures as described in
control. ref. (18) the statistical parametric mapping software (SPM5). MPRAGE
images were first corrected for signal non-uniformities and spatially
Methods and Procedures normalized to the standard T1 Montreal Neurological Institute tem-
Participants and procedures plate. Using the tissue classification algorithm in SPM5, we segmented
Ninety-one youth (14–21 years old), thirty-seven lean (BMI <25 kg/m2 the normalized MPRAGE images into their GM, white matter (WM),
or waist to height ratio <0.5), and fifty-four obese (BMI ≥30 kg/m2 or and cerebrospinal fluid partitions, which are maps representing the
>95 percentile for BMI for age and gender) participated in the study. probability for each voxel being classified as GM, WM, or cerebrospi-
Eighty-one of these (36 lean, 45 obese) received an MRI. Ten adoles- nal fluid. These segmented partitions were subsequently normalized to
cents did not receive an MRI for the following reasons: two did not their respective standard templates. In addition to performing whole
keep their appointments, one was pregnant and we chose to err on the brain assessment, and given that during adolescence frontal lobe myeli-
side of safety, one could not tolerate the MRI (claustrophobia), and six nation is still ongoing, we utilized two different templates to derive the
had a BMI >50 kg/m2 and exceeded the body size that could be accom- regions of interest in the frontal lobe. These were the SPM automatic
modated by the scanner. anatomic labeling (19) template and our published reliable frontal lobe
Lean participants had a mean age of 17.3 ± 1.6 years and obese parcelation method (20). The automatic anatomic labeling template
17.5 ± 1.6 years. The two groups were also matched on years of educa- was used to derive a total frontal lobe, an anterior cingulate region,
tion, gender, and socioeconomic status and were all in the cognitively and an orbitofrontal region. Our own parcelation method was used to
normal range. Evidence of neurological, medical (other than dyslipi- derive a prefrontal region (frontal lobe minus the supplementary motor
demia, insulin resistance short of T2DM, polycystic ovary disease, or region). We quantified the proportions of WM, GM, cerebrospinal fluid
hypertension), or psychiatric (including depression and alcohol or volumes in the whole brain and frontal regions at the case level by first
other substance abuse) illness excluded individuals from participa- mapping the regions to each segmented partition and then averaging
tion in the study. T2DM also excluded individuals from participation. the values across subjects for each of the two groups.
Participants and their parents gave written informed consent and were
compensated for their time and inconvenience. The study protocol was Statistical analyses
approved by the New York University School of Medicine institutional We conducted two-tailed independent samples t-tests examining group
review board. differences in demographics, endocrine data, cognitive data, and brain
All study participants had a blood sample taken after a 10-h over- volumes as well as Pearson correlations between the TFEQ disinhibi-
night fast for the assessment of glucose, insulin, lipid, and inflammatory tion score and BMI, Stroop color-word score, and OFC GM volume.
marker (high-sensitivity C-reactive protein (CRP)) levels. Glucose was Data that were further than 2 s.d. from the mean of the group for that
measured using a glucose oxidase method (VITROS 950 AT; Ortho variable were excluded. Given that there is individual variability in
Clinical Diagnostics, Amersham, England), insulin by chemilumines- regional brain volumes related to overall head size, we measured each
cence (Advia Centaur; Siemens), and CRP was measured in plasma individual’s intracranial vault size and utilized the intracranial vault val-
using an enzymatic immunoassay (Vitros CRP slide; Ortho Clinical ues to adjust the regional brain volumes. Therefore, to allow compara-
Diagnostics). Insulin sensitivity was estimated using the homeostasis bility to other studies and give the reader a sense of the size of the brain
model assessment of insulin resistance. regions studied, the table describing regional brain volumes shows the

obesity | VOLUME 19 NUMBER 7 | july 2011 1383


articles
Behavior and Psychology

raw (nonresidualized) volumes. However, statistical comparison and vs. 3.91 ± 1.96, P < 0.000, Cohen’s d (d) = 1.07), as well as the
the significance and effect size for all imaging presented utilized the hunger factor (6.60 ± 3.37 vs. 4.68 ± 2.84, P = 0.008, d = 0.81)
volumes adjusted (residualized) brain volumes.
and the cognitive restraint factor (9.19 ± 4.30 vs. 6.78 ± 4.11,
Results P = 0.012, d = 0.57). Please note that we repeated these analyses
Demographics and endocrine data for the subset of 81 participants with an MRI and the results
Subject groups were matched for age, gender, school grade, and were essentially unchanged (data not shown).
Hollingshead socioeconomic status. Obese participants were,
by definition, higher in BMI, and as expected also had higher Cognitive measures
systolic and diastolic blood pressure, fasting insulin, and glu- Relative to lean adolescents, obese adolescents had worse cog-
cose levels (but all in the normoglycemic range) as well as the nitive performance in every frontal lobe task, most pronounced
homeostatic model assessment of insulin resistance, triglycer- for the Stroop (a measure of inhibition), and the working
ides, low-density lipoprotein cholesterol, and high-sensitivity memory index of the WRAML, even when we controlled for
CRP. Obese subjects also had significantly lower levels of high- estimated IQ (Table 2).
density lipoprotein (see Table 1). Because 10 subjects did not receive an MRI evaluation (for
details please refer to the participants and procedures section
TFEQ above), we repeated our analyses for the subgroup of 81 ado-
Obese adolescents scored significantly higher than lean par- lescents who had an MRI and the direction and significance of
ticipants on the disinhibition factor of the TFEQ (6.85 ± 3.55 the cognitive results remained unchanged (data not shown).

Table 1 Demographics and endocrine characteristics of lean and obese adolescent groups
Lean Obese
n = 37 n = 54
Mean ± s.d. t P d
Age (years) 17.32 ± 1.59 17.50 ± 1.59 0.538 0.592 0.08
Education (years) 11.77 ± 1.99 11.63 ± 1.71 0.322 0.748 0.10
Gender (% F) 56.8% 63.6%
Hollingshead SES 2.5 ± 1.59 2.1 ± 1.17 1.62 0.109 0.37
BMI (kg/m2) 21.67 ± 2.49 39.86 ± 9.46 −11.41 <0.01 2.45
Systolic BP (mm Hg) 101.0 ± 8.68 114.4 ± 12.46 −6.00 <0.01 1.29
Diastolic BP (mm Hg) 62.97 ± 7.72 69.51 ± 9.33 −3.73 <0.01 0.80
HOMAIR 1.03 ± 1.80 3.06 ± 3.70 −3.50 <0.01 0.73
Fasting insulin (U/ml) 6.95 ± 2.99 19.43 ± 11.82 −6.02 <0.01 1.59
Fasting glucose (mg/dl) 74.44 ± 7.02 77.90 ± 8.00 −2.11 0.038 0.48
Triglycerides (mg/dl) 66.20 ± 24.08 85.29 ± 35.58 −2.77 <0.01 0.61
HDL (mg/dl) 52.56 ± 9.55 43.50 ± 7.44 4.88 <0.01 1.09
LDL (mg/dl) 85.24 ± 22.17 98.29 ± 21.26 −2.73 <0.01 0.60
C-reactive protein (mg/dl) 0.53 ± 0.56 3.41 ± 2.19 −7.53 <0.01 1.73
BP, blood pressure; HDL, high-density lipoprotein; HOMAIR, homeostasis model assessment of insulin resistance; LDL, low-density lipoprotein.

Table 2 Cognitive differences between lean and obese adolescent groups


Lean Obese
Frontal lobe tasks Mean ± s.d. t P d
Stoop color-word score (no. correct/min.) (response inhibition) 46.3 ± 8.3 38.9 ± 7.6 4.30 <0.001 0.94
Trail making test part A (secs.) (attention) 22.6 ± 5.2 28.2 ± 8.1 −3.90 <0.001 0.88
Trail making test part B (secs.) (cognitive flexibility) 51.3 ± 16.3 68.1 ± 32.0 −2.71 0.008 0.62
COWAT—sum of F, A, S (verbal fluency) 39.3 ± 13.1 33.8 ± 7.9 2.21 0.032 0.50
WRAML working memory index 110.7 ± 13.3 99.4 ± 13.8 3.79 <0.001 0.83
WRAML attn./concent. index 108.6 ± 15.6 100.8 ± 12.4 2.58 0.012 0.56
Estimated full-scale IQ 108.6 ± 12.5 102.6 ± 10.9 2.37 0.020 0.51
COWAT, Controlled Oral Word Association Test; WRAML, Wide Range Assessment of Learning and Memory.

1384 VOLUME 19 NUMBER 7 | july 2011 | www.obesityjournal.org


articles
Behavior and Psychology

60 for obese youth in OFC (32.3 ± 3.68 vs. 33.3 ± 3.99; residual-
ized 0.00781 ± 0.024944 vs. −0.01227 ± 0.018947, P = 0.005,
50 d = 0.66). The OFC volume group differences were unchanged
after controlling for systolic blood pressure or homeostasis
40
model assessment of insulin resistance. Other brain regions
assessed, including prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate
BMI

30
cortex were not significantly different between obese and lean
participants. Covarying for age did not change any of these
20 Lean
relationships.
Obese
10 R 2 = 0.165
Associations
We found significant associations between the TFEQ and
0
−1 1 3 5 7 9 11 13 15 cognitive, BMI, and MRI volume measures. Specifically, the
Three factor eating questionnaire—disinhibition disinhibition factor score on the TFEQ showed a significant
correlation with BMI (r(81) = 0.406, P < 0.001), Stroop color-
Figure 1 Association between BMI and Three Factor Eating word score (r(77) = −0.272, P = 0.017; Figure 1), and OFC
Questionnaire (TFEQ) disinhibition score. The association between BMI
GM volume (r(71) = −0.273, P = 0.021). In order to further
(in kg/m2) and TFEQ disinhibition factor score have been shown. Obese
individuals are noted by filled pyramids and lean individuals by filled understand the relationship between OFC volume and dis-
circles. inhibition we explored the association separately for the two
groups (Figure 2). We found that there was no association
a 0.08
Lean and obese
between disinhibition and OFC volume for obese individuals
0.06
R 2 = 0.075
(r(40) = −0.028, P = 0.864), whereas there was a strong associa-
Residualized gray matter

0.04 tion for the lean group (r(31) = −0.460, P = 0.009). The associa-
volume—OFC

0.02
tions between the disinhibition factor score and the BMI and
0
−0.02
stroop remained significant for the subset of individuals with
−0.04
MRI (data not shown).
−0.06
−1 3 7 11 15
Three factor eating
Discussion
questionnaire—disinhibition
Obese adolescents had significantly higher ratings of disinhibi-
b 0.08 Lean
c 0.08
Obese tion, hunger, and cognitive restraint on the TFEQ. Although
0.06 R 2 = 0.213 R 2 = 0.001
higher levels of cognitive restraint among obese adolescents
Residualized gray matter

0.06
Residualized gray matter

0.04 0.04 would on first inspection appear counterintuitive, it conforms


volume—OFC

volume—OFC

0.02 0.02 to the described model of “rigid restraint” in which an indi-


0 0
vidual with disinhibited eating and cognitive restraint may
−0.02 −0.02

−0.04 −0.04
tend to restrict food in some situations but grossly overeat in
−0.06 −0.06
others (21).
−1 3 7 11 15 −1 3 7 11 15 Our novel neurostructural results among obese adolescents
Three factor eating Three factor eating
questionnaire—disinhibition questionnaire—disinhibition are consistent with findings in the adult literature (8,9) dem-
Figure 2 Association between BMI and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC)
onstrating GM volume reductions. In our adolescent sample
volume. The association between BMI and intracranial vault (ICV)- these decreases were most marked for the OFC, a brain region
residualized orbitofrontal cortex volume have been shown. (a) All study important in impulse control, but also showed a weak trend
participants, (b) the significant association for lean youth, and (c) the for whole frontal lobe. We speculate that the more subtle vol-
lack of association for obese adolescents. ume reductions that exist in other brain regions among obese
adolescents may actually reach statistical significance in an
Brain imaging expanded sample.
Frontal lobe grey matter volume (in cm3) trended smaller, Importantly for this report, we found the group with excess
although not at a level of statistical significance, among weight to not only have higher disinhibition scores on the
obese adolescents (265.3 ± 29.5 vs. 269.6 ± 26.7; residual- TFEQ, but lower performance on cognitive tests reflecting
ized 0.00369 ± 0.018312 vs. −0.00609 ± 0.014076, P = 0.139, brain functions thought to be central to behavioral inhibition,
d = 0.35). Please note that although the absolute differences even when controlling for IQ. Out of the frontal lobe regions
between these volumes was small, the analyses were conducted and functions we measured, we were particularly interested in
after residualizing to the intracranial vault and significance ascertaining the relationship between the disinhibition factor
values and effect sizes reflect these analyses. Furthermore in of the TFEQ and the OFC, a brain region that is very impor-
order to control for the possible developmental effects of age tant for behavioral inhibition (impulse control). We selected
on frontal and cerebral volumes we re-ran our analyses covary- the Stroop because it is the only one of our frontal lobe tasks
ing for age. We found significantly lower grey matter volumes (including those that tap executive functions) that specifically

obesity | VOLUME 19 NUMBER 7 | july 2011 1385


articles
Behavior and Psychology

tests the ability to inhibit automated responses. This is the of adolescents with excess weight may contribute to decreased
direct cognitive parallel of the behavioral (disinhibition factor executive function and structural deficits. We have described
of the TFEQ) and brain region (OFC) also involved in inhibi- a possible model for these effects (27) in which we hypoth-
tion of automatic responses. Our interest was to ascertain the esize that insulin resistance is associated with decreased
functional (Stroop vs. other frontal tasks that do not measure brain vascular reactivity related to endothelial dysfunction.
response inhibition) and anatomic (OFC) specificity of our We know that during brain activation, such as occurs when
findings and their association to the disinhibition factor of the performing a cognitive task, there is an increase in synaptic
TFEQ. activity in the brain region involved. In the normal brain this
We also found significant associations between disinhibi- results in regional vasodilation and thus an increase in glucose
tion factor scores and both BMI and OFC volume. When availability to that region to support the increased cognitive
the relationship between disinhibition and OFC volume was demand (28). Therefore, vascular reactivity, which is integral
examined separately in lean and obese participants, we found to well-regulated cerebral blood flow, is key for maintaining
a strong negative association only for the lean group. It is pos- an optimal neuronal environment during brain activation (29).
sible that obese individuals have already experienced a critical Research showing endothelial dysfunction in obese children,
level of disinhibition (which as we demonstrated is associated even before the development of diabetes (30), further supports
with BMI), whereby additional disinhibition is not as clearly this premise. In addition, the inflammatory marker CRP was
reflected in further changes in OFC, but perhaps in different elevated in our obese adolescents. In studies examining large
brain regions or networks not assessed as part of this study. cohorts of adults, investigators have found increased levels
Another possibility for these different findings for each of the of inflammatory cytokines as putative mediators of cognitive
two weight groups is that given that the obese groups has higher decline among individuals with metabolic syndrome (31–34).
degree of item endorsement, they may be more susceptible to A possible mechanism for these cognitive effects is provided by
issues of social desirability and therefore they may be less likely animal data demonstrating that excess inflammatory cytokines
to fully report the extent of their behavioral disinhibition in can decrease long-term potentiation, a process understood as
eating, dampening the association in this group. Lastly, it is essential in consolidation of memory in the hippocampus.
also possible that range restriction, namely the phenomena Inflammatory cytokines may also cause impairment in neuro-
of correlations decreasing when the variance is decreased as genesis and neuroplasticity, processes vital to the formation of
occurs when we divide our sample in two could be affecting memories and the maintenance of structural neural integrity.
our results. A third possibility is that these effects are bidirectional
Although our study finds that disinhibition in feeding behav- whereby behavioral disinhibition predisposes to obesity, which
ior is associated with reductions in executive functioning and may negatively impact brain areas responsible for executive
frontal GM volumes, the cross-sectional nature of our design function and inhibition of caloric intake, thus causing a vicious
does not allow us to address the issue of directionality or cau- cycle of dysfunction. This third possibility could help explain
sation. With that being said, there are several plausible theories why it so difficult for individuals to lose weight once it has been
regarding the direction of these associations. gained.
One possibility is that primary structural or functional brain We are encouraged by the fact that among the few brain
deficits lead to disinhibited eating and reductions in neurocog- regions that we evaluated, the OFC, a brain region that has
nitive function. This line of reasoning is partially supported been demonstrated as important in behavioral inhibition in
by work showing disinhibtion in eating behavior to presage both animal and human studies, had the most significant vol-
increased caloric intake (22) and obesity (23). It is also con- ume reduction among obese adolescents. Our findings, includ-
sistent with functional imaging work demonstrating that indi- ing lower performance on cognitive tests thought to require
viduals, who in response to visualized intake of palatable foods intact OFC, coupled with volume reductions in this area asso-
show weaker activation of brain reward circuits, are at elevated ciated to behavioral disinhibition point to its likely importance
risk for future weight gain (24); perhaps they need a larger in weight gain.
stimulus (more food) to derive the same reward response. This study has some clear limitations. First, it is a cross-
Another possible explanation is that brain structural deficits sectional view that does not allow us to comment on clear
like those demonstrated in this study result from obesity and causality. Second, given our relatively modest sample size we
its associated insulin resistance. This possibility is supported restricted our measurements to brain regions that in previous
by a 24-year longitudinal study showing increased BMI begin- studies had either been found to have been associated to obes-
ning in middle age correlated with decreased temporal lobe ity or disinhibition, or those that we had good theoretical rea-
volume in later life (25). Also supporting this effect order is sons to believe could be involved. Therefore, it is possible that
our own work in adults where we find that hippocampal vol- there are other brain areas, which we did not evaluate, that may
umes were associated with impairments in glucose tolerance also be involved. A third limitation of our study is that we only
(26) as well as that in adolescents with T2DM, where we find have participants’ current weight and we cannot comment on
cognitive impairments and reductions in frontal lobe volumes duration of obesity; the sample that we studied is likely to have
and in WM microstructural integrity (11). We posit that the considerable variability in duration of obesity and its associ-
obesity-associated insulin resistance exhibited by our group ated insulin resistance. Nevertheless, our study has significant

1386 VOLUME 19 NUMBER 7 | july 2011 | www.obesityjournal.org


articles
Behavior and Psychology

strengths, including the careful matching between groups, the 12. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F et al. Inverse association between BMI
and prefrontal metabolic activity in healthy adults. Obesity (Silver Spring)
multidimensional evaluations conducted, and the unbiased
2009;17:60–65.
MRI methods utilized in the analyses of the MRI data. 13. Elias MF, Elias PK, Sullivan LM, Wolf PA, D’Agostino RB. Lower cognitive
To better understand the issues described here, future work function in the presence of obesity and hypertension: the Framingham heart
should evaluate subjects longitudinally, tracking the develop- study. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 2003;27:260–268.
14. Gunstad J, Paul RH, Cohen RA et al. Elevated body mass index is
ment of obesity across time while concomitantly measuring cog- associated with executive dysfunction in otherwise healthy adults. Compr
nitive, behavioral, and neurostructural changes. Alternatively, Psychiatry 2007;48:57–61.
our understanding could also be improved through a study 15. Waldstein SR, Katzel LI. Interactive relations of central versus total obesity
and blood pressure to cognitive function. Int J Obes (Lond) 2006;30:
designed to examine the consequences of a successful obesity 201–207.
treatment (e.g., bariatric surgery), and thus ascertain whether 16. Lokken KL, Boeka AG, Austin HM, Gunstad J, Harmon CM. Evidence of
some of these deficits are reversible. Furthermore, future work executive dysfunction in extremely obese adolescents: a pilot study. Surg
Obes Relat Dis 2009;5:547–552.
should evaluate other possible associated factors such as pro- 17. Lezak MD, Howleson DB, Loring DW, Hannay HJ, Fischer JS.
and anti-inflammatory cytokines as well as utilize more sensi- Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford University Press: New York, 2004.
tive MRI techniques such as diffusion tensor imaging. 18. Good CD, Scahill RI, Fox NC et al. Automatic differentiation of anatomical
patterns in the human brain: validation with studies of degenerative
Acknowledgments dementias. Neuroimage 2002;17:29–46.
The authors wish to acknowledge the children and families who participated 19. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D et al. Automated
in this research as well as Po Lai Yau and Valentin Polyakov in the collection anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical
and processing of the data and the assistance of Allison Larr in the parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 2002;15:
preparation of this manuscript. The study was supported by grants from 273–289.
the National Institutes of Health R21 DK070985 and RO1 DK083537 and, 20. Convit A, Wolf OT, de Leon MJ et al. Volumetric analysis of the pre-frontal
supported in part by grant 1UL1RR029893 from the National Center for regions: findings in aging and schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 2001;107:
Research Resources. 61–73.
21. Westenhoefer J, Broeckmann P, Münch AK, Pudel V. Cognitive control of
Disclosure eating behaviour and the disinhibition effect. Appetite 1994;23:27–41.
The authors declared no conflict of interest. 22. Yeomans MR, Leitch M, Mobini S. Impulsivity is associated with the
disinhibition but not restraint factor from the Three Factor Eating
© 2011 The Obesity Society Questionnaire. Appetite 2008;50:469–476.
23. Hays NP, Bathalon GP, McCrory MA et al. Eating behavior correlates of adult
REFERENCES weight gain and obesity in healthy women aged 55-65 y. Am J Clin Nutr
1. Ogden CL, Carroll MD, Flegal KM. High body mass index for age among US 2002;75:476–483.
children and adolescents, 2003-2006. JAMA 2008;299:2401–2405. 24. Stice E, Yokum S, Bohon C, Marti N, Smolen A. Reward circuitry
2. Stunkard AJ, Messick S. The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure responsivity to food predicts future increases in body mass: moderating
dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. J Psychosom Res 1985;29: effects of DRD2 and DRD4. Neuroimage 2010;50:1618–1625.
71–83. 25. Gustafson D, Lissner L, Bengtsson C, Björkelund C, Skoog I. A
3. Schwartz MW, Woods SC, Porte D Jr, Seeley RJ, Baskin DG. Central 24-year follow-up of body mass index and cerebral atrophy. Neurology
nervous system control of food intake. Nature 2000;404:661–671. 2004;63:1876–1881.
4. Korner J, Leibel RL. To eat or not to eat - how the gut talks to the brain. 26. Convit A, Wolf OT, Tarshish C, de Leon MJ. Reduced glucose tolerance
N Engl J Med 2003;349:926–928. is associated with poor memory performance and hippocampal atrophy
5. Martin LE, Holsen LM, Chambers RJ et al. Neural mechanisms associated among normal elderly. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2003;100:2019–2022.
with food motivation in obese and healthy weight adults. Obesity (Silver 27. Convit A. Links between cognitive impairment in insulin resistance: an
Spring) 2010;18:254–260. explanatory model. Neurobiol Aging 2005;26 Suppl 1:31–35.
6. Del Parigi A, Gautier JF, Chen K et al. Neuroimaging and obesity: mapping 28. Benton D, Parker PY, Donohoe RT. The supply of glucose to the brain and
the brain responses to hunger and satiation in humans using positron cognitive functioning. J Biosoc Sci 1996;28:463–479.
emission tomography. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2002;967:389–397. 29. Drake CT, Iadecola C. The role of neuronal signaling in controlling cerebral
7. Damasio H, Grabowski T, Frank R, Galaburda AM, Damasio AR. The return blood flow. Brain Lang 2007;102:141–152.
of Phineas Gage: clues about the brain from the skull of a famous patient. 30. Karpoff L, Vinet A, Schuster I et al. Abnormal vascular reactivity at rest and
Science 1994;264:1102–1105. exercise in obese boys. Eur J Clin Invest 2009;39:94–102.
8. Walther K, Birdsill AC, Glisky EL, Ryan L. Structural brain differences and 31. Dik MG, Jonker C, Comijs HC et al. Contribution of metabolic syndrome
cognitive functioning related to body mass index in older females. Hum Brain components to cognition in older individuals. Diabetes Care 2007;30:2655–
Mapp 2010;31:1052–1064. 2660.
9. Taki Y, Kinomura S, Sato K et al. Relationship between body mass index 32. Roberts RO, Geda YE, Knopman DS et al. Metabolic syndrome,
and gray matter volume in 1,428 healthy individuals. Obesity (Silver Spring) inflammation, and nonamnestic mild cognitive impairment in older persons:
2008;16:119–124. a population-based study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2010;24:11–18.
10. Pannacciulli N, Del Parigi A, Chen K et al. Brain abnormalities in human obesity: 33. Sweat V, Starr V, Bruehl H et al. C-reactive protein is linked to lower cognitive
a voxel-based morphometric study. Neuroimage 2006;31:1419–1425. performance in overweight and obese women. Inflammation 2008;31:
11. Yau PL, Javier DC, Ryan CM et al. Preliminary evidence for brain 198–207.
complications in obese adolescents with type 2 diabetes mellitus. 34. Yaffe K, Kanaya A, Lindquist K et al. The metabolic syndrome, inflammation,
Diabetologia 2010;53:2298–2306. and risk of cognitive decline. JAMA 2004;292:2237–2242.

obesity | VOLUME 19 NUMBER 7 | july 2011 1387

You might also like