You are on page 1of 8

Psychological Science http://pss.sagepub.

com/

Is High Sex Drive Associated With Increased Sexual Attraction to Both Sexes?: It Depends on Whether
You Are Male or Female
Richard A. Lippa
Psychological Science 2006 17: 46
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01663.x

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://pss.sagepub.com/content/17/1/46

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

Association for Psychological Science

Additional services and information for Psychological Science can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://pss.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://pss.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

>> Version of Record - Jan 1, 2006

What is This?

Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on December 8, 2014


P SY CH OL OG I C AL S CIE N CE

Research Article

Is High Sex Drive Associated


With Increased Sexual Attraction
to Both Sexes?
It Depends on Whether You Are Male or Female
Richard A. Lippa

California State University, Fullerton

ABSTRACT—If sex drive is a generalized energizer of sexual higher levels of sex drive associated with increased levels of
behaviors, then high sex drive should increase an indi- sexual attraction to both men and women (i.e., is sex drive a
vidual’s sexual attraction to both men and women. If sex generalized energizer of sexual attractions?), or are higher levels
drive energizes only dominant sexual responses, however, of sex drive associated with increased sexual attraction to one
then high sex drive should selectively increase attraction to sex or the other, but not to both (i.e., does higher sex drive in-
men or to women, but not to both, depending on the indi- crease the probability of dominant sexual responses, but not the
vidual’s sexual orientation. Data from three studies as- probability of nondominant sexual responses?)? The hypothesis
sessing a total of 3,645 participants show that for most that follows most directly from classic drive theories is that
women, high sex drive is associated with increased sexual higher levels of sex drive are associated with increases in in-
attraction to both men and women. For men, however, high dividuals’ dominant patterns of sexual attraction and response.
sex drive is associated with increased sexual attraction to Thus, high-drive heterosexual men and women should show
only one sex or the other, depending on the individual’s increased attraction to other-sex but not to same-sex individu-
sexual orientation. These results suggest that the corre- als, whereas high-drive homosexual men and women should
lates of sex drive and the organization of sexual orientation show increased attraction to same-sex but not to other-sex in-
are different for women and men. dividuals.
However, this hypothesized pattern might differ for men and
women if other-sex and same-sex attractions are more strongly
Classic learning theories of the mid-20th century proposed that dominant and nondominant in men than they are in women.
higher levels of drive increase the probability of dominant or Previous research has in fact shown that men report more mu-
well-learned behaviors, but decrease the probability of non- tually exclusive patterns of same-sex and other-sex attraction
dominant or poorly learned behaviors (Hull, 1943; Spence, than women do, and conversely, women report more bisexual
1956; Zajonc, 1965). Although this hypothesis has received patterns of attraction than men do (Pattatucci, 1998). Related
empirical support for drives such as hunger and thirst, and for research shows that same-sex attraction is often more negatively
the arousal that results from the presence of other people, it has correlated with other-sex attraction in men than in women
not been tested in relation to another powerful and socially (Lippa, 2000; Lippa & Arad, 1997). A number of studies show
significant human drive—the sex drive. that, over the course of individuals’ lives, women tend to be more
The current research applied classic drive theory to human flexible and variable in their same-sex and other-sex attractions
sexual behavior, posing a basic question about the behavioral than men are, which suggests that same-sex and other-sex at-
correlates of individual differences in human sex drive. Are tractions are less mutually exclusive for women than they are for
men (Baumeister, 2000; Diamond, 2000). Corroborating these
self-report data, recent physiological research shows that het-
Address correspondence to Richard Lippa, Psychology Department,
California State University, Fullerton, Fullerton, CA 92834, e-mail: erosexual men show genital arousal primarily to female sexual
rlippa@fullerton.edu. stimuli, and homosexual men show genital arousal primarily to

46 Downloaded fromCopyright r 2006


pss.sagepub.com Association
at TEXAS for Psychological
SOUTHERN Science 8, 2014
UNIVERSITY on December Volume 17—Number 1
Richard A. Lippa

male sexual stimuli, whereas heterosexual and homosexual differ for men and women. Specifically, high sex drive in men
women show genital arousal to both male and female sexual may be associated with increased sexual attraction to one sex or
stimuli (Chivers, Rieger, Latty, & Bailey, 2004). the other, depending on the individual’s sexual orientation. For
Differences in the organization of men’s and women’s same- women, in contrast, high sex drive may be associated with in-
sex and other-sex attractions may result, in part, from sex dif- creased sexual attraction to both sexes. Three studies were
ferences in the physiological processes of masculinization and conducted to test this hypothesis.
feminization that underlie sexual orientation. Such sex differ-
ences could result if the neural and hormonal processes that
underlie the development of sexual orientation differ for men STUDY 1
and women. For example, perhaps there is more of a bisexual
‘‘default’’ neural structure in females, whereas masculinization Method
of neural structures in males, under the influence of androgens, Participants in Study 1 were 1,735 college students (633 men
also entails defeminization of structures (see Hines, 2004, for a and 1,102 women). Seven hundred twenty-one were students in
recent review). four large human-sexuality classes at California State Univer-
Social-environmental theories may also help explain why sity, Fullerton (CSUF; see Lippa, 2000, Study 1), and 1,014 were
men’s and women’s other-sex and same-sex attractions are pat- participants in a study on finger-length ratios and sexual ori-
terned differently. Many cultures construe men’s other-sex and entation (Lippa, 2003). Forty-two percent of participants were
same-sex sexual attractions to be more binary and mutually White, 22% were Hispanic, 21% were Asian, and the remainder
exclusive than they construe women’s corresponding attractions fell into other categories. Participants’ median age was 22.
to be (see Lippa & Tan, 2001). The ‘‘either-or’’ nature of male Participants completed an anonymous questionnaire that in-
sexual orientation may result, in part, from gender socialization cluded a number of personality scales, a ‘‘sexual attitudes and
that is more rigid and rigorous for boys than for girls and from behavior’’ scale, and a cover sheet that asked for demographic
socialization agents who disapprove more strongly of feminine information. They also reported their sexual orientation. The
behaviors in boys and men than they do of masculine behaviors scale measuring sexual attitudes and behavior included three
in girls and women (Fagot & Hagan, 1991; Jacklin, DiPietro, & items used for the present study: ‘‘I am sexually attracted to
Maccoby, 1984; Lippa, 2005). Such socialization pressures men,’’ ‘‘I am sexually attracted to women,’’ and ‘‘I have a strong
could lead heterosexual men to suppress same-sex attractions sex drive.’’ Participants used a 7-point scale (ranging from
more than heterosexual women do. strongly disagree to strongly agree) to respond to these items.
Socialization processes may also influence the degree to
which sex and sex-linked physical traits are central to sexual
attraction. Peplau (2001) has argued that women tend more than Results and Discussion
men to develop a person-centered and relationship-centered The analyses that follow are restricted to the 95% of men and the
orientation to sex, whereas men tend more than women to de- 97% of women who described themselves as heterosexual. Be-
velop a body-centered and recreational orientation to sex. Thus, cause the distributions of men’s and women’s responses to the
relative to men, women may be more attracted to partners’ attraction-to-men and attraction-to-women items were quite
personalities and their ability to provide satisfying emotional skewed, nonparametric correlations (Spearman’s r), as well as
and personal relationships, whereas relative to women, men may Pearson product-moment correlations, are reported.
be more attracted to partners’ physical traits and their potential Table 1 presents, separately for men and women, means and
to provide satisfying sexual relationships (Diamond, 2003). By standard deviations for self-reported sex drive, attraction to
implication, the traits that women find attractive in a sexual men, and attraction to women. Table 2 presents intercorrelations
partner may be less dependent on the potential partner’s sex of self-reported sex drive, attraction to men, and attraction to
than is the case for men, whereas the traits that men find at- women, for men (above the diagonal) and for women (below the
tractive in a sexual partner may be more dependent on the po- diagonal).
tential partner’s sex than is the case for women. Men’s sex drive was significantly associated with their at-
In the language of classic learning theories, the greater bi- traction to women, but not with their attraction to men, and the
polarity of men’s sexual orientation implies that men’s attraction difference between these correlations was significant, t(580) 5
to one sex tends to be strongly dominant, whereas their attraction 4.05, p < .0001, using a t test for differences between corre-
to the other sex tends to be strongly nondominant. In contrast, lations when two variables are correlated with the same third
women’s other-sex and same-sex attractions may not be as variable (see McNemar, 1962, p. 140; all tests are two-tailed
strongly segregated into dominant and nondominant responses. and, unless stated otherwise, computed for Spearman’s r). In
If other-sex and same-sex attractions are more polarized and contrast, women’s sex drive was positively correlated with both
mutually exclusive in men than in women, then the association their attraction to men and their attraction to women, and these
between sex drive and same-sex and other-sex attractions may two correlations did not differ significantly, t(1035) 5 1.52, n.s.

Volume 17—Number 1 Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on December 8, 2014 47
Sex Drive and Sexual Attraction

TABLE 1 Study 2 extended these results by assessing samples of gay men


Participants’ Mean Sex Drive, Attraction to Men, and Attraction and lesbian women.
to Women

Attraction Attraction STUDY 2


Group Sex drive to men to women
Study 1 Method
Heterosexual men 5.65 (1.43) 1.31 (1.01) 6.73 (0.93) Most of the gay and lesbian participants in Study 2 were vol-
Heterosexual women 4.74 (1.63) 6.61 (1.00) 1.47 (1.13) unteers from gay and lesbian clubs, political organizations, and
Study 2
student organizations in southern California (see Lippa, 2000,
Gay men 5.77 (1.24) 6.80 (0.72) 1.86 (1.53)
Lesbian women 4.53 (1.85) 2.66 (1.78) 6.53 (1.27) Study 2). Fifteen gay men and 11 lesbian women from Study 1
Study 3 served as additional participants. Of the 111 gay men in Study 2
Heterosexual men 0.40 (0.76) 1.22 (0.47) 6.53 (0.55) (median age 5 31), 57% were White, 27% were Asian, 11%
Heterosexual women 0.30 (0.91) 5.49 (0.79) 2.25 (1.07) were Hispanic, and the remainder fell into other categories. Of
Gay men 0.39 (0.68) 6.46 (0.66) 1.60 (0.75) the 58 lesbian women in Study 3 (median age 5 36), 70% were
Lesbian women 0.17 (1.00) 1.87 (0.91) 6.31 (0.70) White, 12% were Hispanic, 7% were Asian, and the remainder
Note. Standard deviations are in parentheses. For Study 3, sex-drive scores fell into other categories. Participants completed an anonymous
are expressed as Z scores. questionnaire that included the items described in Study 1.

The negative correlation between attraction to men and at-


traction to women was stronger for men than for women (Z 5 Results and Discussion
5.00, p < .0001). This sex difference was even stronger when The bottom panel of Table 2 presents findings for gay men (above
homosexual participants (15 men and 11 women) and bisexual the diagonal) and for lesbian women (below the diagonal). Gay
participants (14 men and 24 women) were included in analyses men’s sex drive was significantly associated with their attraction
(men’s r 5 .51, p < .001; women’s r 5 .17, p < .001; Z to men, but not with their attraction to women, and the difference
difference 5 7.90, p < .0001). between these correlations was significant, t(107) 5 2.58, p <
On average, heterosexual men reported having higher levels .05. In contrast, lesbian women’s sex drive tended to be posi-
of sex drive than heterosexual women (see means in Table 1), tively correlated with their attraction to both men and women,
t(1621) 5 11.28, p < .0001, d 5 0.58. Also, women’s self-re- and the magnitude of these correlations did not differ signifi-
ported levels of sex drive were significantly more variable than cantly, t(54) 5 0.43, n.s. The correlation between sex drive
men’s, F(1, 1621) 5 12.58, p < .001 (Levene’s test for equality of and other-sex attraction was positive for lesbian women and
variances). negative for gay men; however, these correlations did not differ
Overall, the results of Study 1 showed that, for heterosexual (Z 5 1.50, n.s.). Correlations between sex drive and same-sex
men, sex drive correlated with attraction to women, but not with attraction and correlations between same-sex and other-sex at-
attraction to men. For heterosexual women, in contrast, sex drive traction also did not differ significantly between lesbian women
correlated with both attraction to men and attraction to women. and gay men.

TABLE 2
Intercorrelations of Sex Drive, Attraction to Men, and Attraction to Women in Study 1 and
Study 2

Sex drive Attraction to men Attraction to women


Measure r r r r r r
Study 1: heterosexual men and women
Sex drive — — .08 .07 .23nnn .21nnn
Attraction to men .27nnn .26nnn — — .49nnn .38nnn
Attraction to women .21nnn .19nnn .08n .13nnn — —

Study 2: gay men and lesbian women


Sex drive — — .29nn .28nn .07 .11
Attraction to men .21 .14 — — .34nnn .37nnn
Attraction to women .28n .23w .26w .29n — —

Note. In Study 1, n 5 583–584 for men and n 5 1,038–1,040 for women. In Study 2, n 5 110–111 for gay men and n 5
57–58 for lesbian women. Correlations above the diagonal are for men, and those below the diagonal are for women.
w
p < .1. np < .05. nnp < .01. nnnp < .001.

48 Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on December 8, 2014 Volume 17—Number 1
Richard A. Lippa

On average, gay men reported having higher levels of sex TABLE 3


drive than lesbian women did, t(167) 5 5.16, p < .0001, d 5 Items Included in the Sex-Drive, Attraction-to-Men, and
0.84, and lesbian women’s self-reported sex drive was signifi- Attraction-to-Women Scales Used in Study 3
cantly more variable than gay men’s, F(1, 167) 5 19.93, p < Sex drive
.0001 (Levene’s test of equality for variances). (See Table 1.) I have a strong sex drive.
Overall, the results of Study 2 showed that gay men’s sex drive I frequently think about sex.
correlated with their attraction to men, but not with their at- It doesn’t take much to get me sexually excited.
traction to women. However, the correlations between sex drive I think about sex almost every day.
Sexual pleasure is the most intense pleasure a person can have.
and same-sex attraction and between sex drive and other-sex
Attraction to men
attraction did not differ for lesbian women. One limitation of I am sexually attracted to men more than women.
Studies 1 and 2 is that they employed single-item measures of sex I am sexually attracted to men.
drive, attraction to men, and attraction to women. To address this I fantasize a lot about having sex with men.
problem, Study 3 employed more reliable, multi-item measures. I think the male body is sexy to look at.
I enjoy looking at magazines like Playgirl (magazines that have
STUDY 3 pictures of nude men).
I am romantically attracted to men.
I fantasize a lot about romantic relations with men.
Method
If I am in a setting where there are lots of attractive people, I spend
Participants in Study 3 were 1,767 individuals (687 men and more time looking at men than at women.
1,080 women) who participated in an Internet survey on ‘‘sexual If I were looking through a catalog with pictures of sexy swimsuit
attitudes and interests’’ between June 16, 2003, and November models, I would spend more time looking at the men than at the
16, 2004. Fifty-eight percent of participants were White, 17% women.
were Hispanic, 13% were Asian, and the remainder fell into other Attraction to women
categories. The participants’ median age was 20. Most partici- I am sexually attracted to women more than men.
I am sexually attracted to women.
pants were CSUF psychology students who completed the survey
I fantasize a lot about having sex with women.
for subject-participation credit. However, 28% of survey re- I think the female body is sexy to look at.
spondents were not from California, indicating that there were also I enjoy looking at magazines like Playboy (magazines that have
many non-CSUF participants. Forty-one participants (2.3% of the pictures of nude women).
total sample) were excluded from analyses because they indicated I am romantically attracted to women.
on the survey that they had not answered questions honestly. I fantasize a lot about romantic relations with women.
The Internet survey included a ‘‘sexual attitudes and feelings’’ If I am in a setting where there are lots of attractive people, I spend
more time looking at women than at men.
scale, the Sexual Desire Inventory (Spector, Carey, & Steinberg,
If I were looking through a catalog with pictures of sexy swimsuit
1996), and questions about demographic information and par- models, I would spend more time looking at the women than at the
ticipants’ sexual orientation. The Sexual Desire Inventory in- men.
cludes a subscale assessing ‘‘dyadic sexual desire’’ (i.e., sex
Note. These items come from the ‘‘sexual attitudes and feelings’’ scale. Par-
drive directed toward partners); scores on this subscale were ticipants rated their degree of disagreement or agreement with the items on a
used for the analyses presented here. The 34-item ‘‘sexual at- 7-point scale that ranged from strongly disagree to strongly agree.
titudes and feelings’’ scale included additional items assessing
sex drive, as well as items that assessed attraction to men and Table 1 presents means and standard deviations for the
attraction to women. A principal components analysis with measures of sex drive, attraction to men, and attraction to women,
varimax rotation identified subsets of these 34 items that tapped separately for heterosexual men and women and for gay men and
attraction to men, attraction to women, and sex drive (scale items lesbians. Table 4 presents intercorrelations of self-reported sex
are listed in Table 3). drive, attraction to men, and attraction to women, both for men
(above the diagonal) and for women (below the diagonal).
Results and Discussion Once again, heterosexual men’s sex drive was significantly
Reliabilities (Cronbach’s alpha) were computed for all scales— associated with their attraction to women, but not with their
for all participants, men, and women. All alphas exceeded .80, attraction to men, and the difference between these correlations
except for the 5-item drive scale (see Table 3), computed for men was significant, t(579) 5 8.34, p < .0001. In contrast, hetero-
(a 5 .73). Because the two self-report sex-drive scales (the sexual women’s sex drive was positively correlated with both
dyadic sexual-desire subscale and the 5-item scale presented in their attraction to men and their attraction to women. However,
Table 3) were substantially correlated (r 5 .70, p < .001), the first correlation was slightly larger than the second, t(934) 5
participants’ scores on these two sex-drive scales were converted 1.85, p 5 .06.
to Z scores and averaged to form one composite sex-drive score The correlation between heterosexual men’s sex drive and
(a 5 .82). their other-sex attraction was stronger than the corresponding

Volume 17—Number 1 Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on December 8, 2014 49
Sex Drive and Sexual Attraction

TABLE 4
Intercorrelations of Multi-Item Measures of Sex Drive, Attraction to Men, and Attraction to
Women in Study 3

Sex drive Attraction to men Attraction to women


Measure r r r r r r
Heterosexual men and women
Sex drive — — .01 .01 .52nnn .47nnn
Attraction to men .32nnn .38nnn — — .31nnn .13nn
Attraction to women .30nnn .30nnn .29nnn .20nnn — —

Gay men and lesbian women


Sex drive — — .44nn .38nn .31n .18
Attraction to men .09 .13 — — .56nnn .32n
Attraction to women .62nnn .60nnn .20 .21 — —

Note. n 5 582 for heterosexual men, n 5 937–938 for heterosexual women, n 5 48 for gay men, and n 5 45 for
lesbian women. Correlations above the diagonals are for men, and those below the diagonals are for women.
n
p < .05. nnp < .01. nnnp < .001.

correlation for women (Z 5 2.17, p < .05). In contrast, the cor- men and 2.53 for women), t(1708) 5 32.44, p < .0001, d 5 1.61,
relation between heterosexual women’s sex drive and their same- as did heterosexual men and women specifically (M 5 3.79 for
sex attraction was much stronger than the corresponding cor- heterosexual males and 2.53 for heterosexual females), t(1517)
relation for men (Z 5 5.56, p < .0001). Finally, the negative 5 34.44, p < .0001, d 5 1.82. However, homosexual men and
correlation between attraction to men and attraction to women women differed only slightly in their distance from the midpoint
did not differ between heterosexual men and women (Z 5 (M 5 3.49 for gay males and 3.26 for lesbians), t(93) 5 1.45,
0.37, n.s., for Pearson rs and Z 5 1.38, n.s., for Spearman rs). one-tailed p 5 .08, d 5 0.30.
The bottom panel of Table 4 shows that gay men’s sex drive The absolute value of the difference between an individual’s
was significantly associated with their attraction to men, but was attraction to men and his or her attraction to women served as
negatively associated with their attraction to women, and these another measure of the bipolarity of same-sex and other-sex
two correlations differed significantly, t(45) 5 2.51, p < .05. attraction. All men and all women differed substantially on this
Lesbian women’s sex drive was positively correlated with their measure, t(1708) 5 30.32, p < .0001, d 5 1.51, as did hetero-
attraction to women, but not with their attraction to men, and sexual men and women specifically, t(1517) 5 32.89, p < .0001,
these two correlations also differed significantly, t(42) 5 3.78, d 5 1.74. However, homosexual men and women differed only
p < .001. Correlations between same-sex and other-sex attrac- slightly, t(93) 5 1.55, one-tailed p 5 .06, d 5 0.32.
tion did not differ significantly for lesbian women and gay men. Men’s sex drive correlated significantly with the absolute
On average, heterosexual men reported having substantially difference between their attraction to men and their attraction to
higher levels of sex drive than heterosexual women did, t(1518) women (all men: r 5 .28, p < .001; heterosexual men: r 5 .35,
5 15.60, p < .0001, d 5 0.82. Heterosexual women’s levels of p < .001; gay men: r 5 .42, p < .01). Thus, for men, higher
sex drive were again significantly more variable than hetero- sex drive was associated with greater polarization of same-sex
sexual men’s, F(1, 1518) 5 29.51, p < .0001 (Levene’s test of and other-sex attractions. In general, women’s sex drive did
equality for variances). Gay men and lesbian women did not not correlate with their absolute difference scores (all women:
differ on sex drive, t(93) 5 1.24, n.s.; however, lesbians’ levels of r 5 .01, n.s.; heterosexual women: r 5 .01, n.s.). However,
sex drive were more variable than gay men’s, F(1, 93) 5 4.48, lesbian women’s sex drive did correlate with their absolute
p < .05 (Levene’s test of equality for variances). difference scores (r 5 .43, p < .01). Thus, for most women,
Figure 1 presents scatter plots, for all men (top panel) and for higher sex drive was not associated with greater polarization of
all women (bottom panel), that place individuals in the two-di- same-sex and other-sex attraction, but lesbian women provided
mensional space defined by their degree of attraction to men and an exception to this pattern.
their degree of attraction to women. Visual inspection of these In summary, the results for heterosexual participants in Study
plots suggests that women were more evenly spread between the 3 replicated the findings of Study 1, and the results for gay men
two most populated poles (high attraction to women and low in Study 3 replicated the findings of Study 2. However, the data
attraction to men at the upper left corner, and low attraction to for lesbians in Study 3 suggested more strongly than results from
women and high attraction to men at the lower right corner). This Study 2 that lesbian women had the male-typical pattern: Higher
sex difference was quantified by computing each participant’s sex drive was associated with their dominant (same-sex) sexual
euclidean distance from the midpoint of the space. All men and attraction, but not with their nondominant (other-sex) sexual
all women differed substantially on this measure (M 5 3.72 for attraction. Finally, Study 3 provided strong evidence that men’s

50 Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on December 8, 2014 Volume 17—Number 1
Richard A. Lippa

men. For heterosexual women, in contrast, sex drive correlated


with both attraction to women and attraction to men. Thus, for
heterosexual women, sex drive seems to act as a generalized
energizer of sexual attractions to both women and men. For
heterosexual men, in contrast, sex drive seems to energize only
dominant attractions. In addition, all three studies corroborated
previous findings that there are significant sex differences in sex
drive (Baumeister, Catanese, & Vohs, 2001).
In Studies 2 and 3, the pattern for gay men was the converse of
that for heterosexual men—gay men’s sex drive was correlated
with attraction to men, but not with attraction to women. Thus,
gay men showed the male-typical pattern—that sex drive en-
ergizes dominant, but not nondominant attractions. However,
what constituted dominant and nondominant attraction was re-
versed for gay and heterosexual men.
Lesbian women were the one group that showed somewhat
inconsistent results. In both Studies 2 and 3, there was evidence
that higher sex drive was associated with greater attraction to
women among lesbians. In Study 2, the correlations between
lesbians’ self-reported sex drive and attraction to men were
nonsignificant but positive, whereas in Study 3, the corre-
sponding correlations were nonsignificant but negative. In Study
3, lesbian women strongly showed the male-typical pattern of
heterosexual and gay men—higher sex drive was associated
with their dominant attraction, but not with their nondominant
attraction. One possible explanation for these differences across
studies is that some subgroups of lesbians (e.g., ‘‘butch’’ lesbi-
ans) show more male-typical traits than others (see Singh, Vi-
daurri, Zambarano, & Dabbs, 1999), and Studies 2 and 3 may
have sampled somewhat different lesbian subpopulations.
In general, the current results show that for most women (with
the possible exception of lesbian women), high sex drive is as-
sociated with increased sexual attraction to both men and
women. For men, however, high sex drive is associated with
increased sexual attraction to only one sex or the other, de-
Fig. 1. Results from Study 3: scatter plot of men (top panel) and women
pending on the individual’s sexual orientation. These findings
(bottom panel) in the two-dimensional space defined by their attraction to are consistent with the hypothesis that sexual orientation is or-
men (x-axis) and their attraction to women (y-axis). ganized differently in women and men. Indeed, the current data
provide new evidence that men’s same-sex and other-sex at-
tractions are much more polarized than women’s. Furthermore,
same-sex and other-sex attractions were organized in a more among men, higher sex drive is associated with greater polari-
bipolar fashion than women’s same-sex and other-sex attractions zation of same-sex and other sex attractions. However, most
were. Furthermore, high sex drive was associated with greater women (again, with the possible exception of lesbian women) do
polarization of same-sex and other-sex attraction for all men, not show this pattern.
heterosexual men, gay men, and lesbian women, but there was The current research suggests that classic drive theories may
no association between sex drive and polarization of same-sex be usefully applied to the study of sex drive and sexual orien-
and other-sex attraction for all women or for heterosexual tation. Future research can investigate the degree to which the
women. current findings apply to other populations (e.g., to other age
groups, to participants from other countries and cultures) and to
GENERAL DISCUSSION physiological as well as to self-report measures of sexual at-
traction. In addition, experimental studies can investigate
Studies 1 and 3 showed that, for heterosexual men, sex drive whether independent variables that influence sex drive (e.g.,
correlated with attraction to women, but not with attraction to manipulated testosterone levels, exposure to erotic stimuli)

Volume 17—Number 1 Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on December 8, 2014 51
Sex Drive and Sexual Attraction

affect same-sex and other-sex attractions differently in men and Lippa, R.A. (2000). Gender-related traits in gay men, lesbian women,
women. and heterosexual men and women: The virtual identity of homo-
sexual-heterosexual diagnosticity and gender diagnosticity. Jour-
REFERENCES nal of Personality, 68, 899–926.
Lippa, R.A. (2003). Are 2D:4D finger-length ratios related to sexual
Baumeister, R.F. (2000). Gender differences in erotic plasticity: The orientation? Yes for men, no for women. Journal of Personality and
female sex drive as socially flexible and responsive. Psychological Social Psychology, 85, 179–188.
Bulletin, 126, 347–374. Lippa, R.A. (2005). Gender, nature, and nurture (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ:
Baumeister, R.F., Catanese, K.R., & Vohs, K.D. (2001). Is there a Erlbaum.
gender difference in strength of sex drive? Theoretical views, Lippa, R.A., & Tan, F.D. (2001). Does culture moderate the relationship
conceptual distinctions, and a review of relevant evidence. Per- between sexual orientation and gender-related personality traits?
sonality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 242–273. Cross-Cultural Research, 35, 65–87.
Chivers, M.L., Rieger, G., Latty, E., & Bailey, M.J. (2004). A sex dif- McNemar, Q. (1962). Psychological statistics (3rd ed.). New York: Wiley.
ference in the specificity of sexual arousal. Psychological Science, Pattatucci, A.M.L. (1998). Biopsychosocial interactions and the develop-
15, 736–744. ment of sexual orientation. In C.J. Patterson & A.R. D’Augelli
Diamond, L.M. (2000). Sexual identity, attractions, and behavior among (Eds.), Lesbian, gay, and bisexual identities in families: Psycho-
young sexual-minority women over a two-year period. Develop- logical perspectives (pp. 19–39). London: Oxford University Press.
mental Psychology, 36, 241–250. Peplau, L.A. (2001). Rethinking women’s sexual orientation: An
Diamond, L.M. (2003). What does sexual orientation orient? A bio- interdisciplinary, relationship-focused approach. Personal Rela-
behavioral model distinguishing romantic love and sexual desire. tionships, 8, 1–19.
Psychological Review, 110, 173–192. Singh, D., Vidaurri, M., Zambarano, R.J., & Dabbs, J.M., Jr. (1999).
Fagot, B.I., & Hagan, R. (1991). Observations of parents’ reactions to Lesbian erotic role identification: Behavioral, morphological, and
sex-stereotyped behaviors: Age and sex effects. Child Develop- hormonal correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psycholo-
ment, 62, 617–628. gy, 76, 1035–1049.
Hines, M. (2004). Brain gender. Oxford, England: Oxford University Spector, I.P., Carey, M.P., & Steinberg, L. (1996). The Sexual Desire
Press. Inventory: Development, factor structure, and evidence of relia-
Hull, C.L. (1943). Principles of behavior theory. New York: Appleton- bility. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 22, 175–190.
Century. Spence, K.W. (1956). Behavior theory and conditioning. New Haven,
Jacklin, C.N., DiPietro, J.A., & Maccoby, E.E. (1984). Sex-typing CT: Yale University Press.
behavior and sex-typing pressure in child/parent interaction. Zajonc, R.B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science, 149, 269–274.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 13, 413–425.
Lippa, R., & Arad, S. (1997). The structure of sexual orientation and its
relation to masculinity, femininity, and gender diagnosticity: (RECEIVED 6/1/04; REVISION ACCEPTED 3/3/05;
Different for men and women. Sex Roles, 37, 187–208. FINAL MATERIALS RECEIVED 3/18/05)

52 Downloaded from pss.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on December 8, 2014 Volume 17—Number 1

You might also like