You are on page 1of 16

Sex Roles, Vol. 52, Nos.

7/8, April 2005 (


C 2005)

DOI: 10.1007/s11199-005-3711-0

The Language of Love: Sex, Sexual Orientation, and


Language Use in Online Personal Advertisements

Carla J. Groom1 and James W. Pennebaker1,2

Stereotypes and biological theories suggest that psychological gender differences found in
predominantly heterosexual samples are smaller or reversed among gay men and lesbians.
Computerized text analysis that compares people’s language style on a wide range of dimen-
sions from pronoun use to body references offers a multivariate personality marker to test
such assumptions. Analysis of over 1,500 internet personal advertisements placed by hetero-
sexual men, heterosexual women, gay men, and lesbians found little evidence that orientation
alters the impact of gender on linguistic behaviors. Previously reported gender differences
were replicated in the gay as well as the heterosexual advertisements studied. Main effects of
sexual orientation indicated that gay people of both sexes apparently felt less need to differ-
entiate themselves from potential mates than did heterosexual people. Virtually no crossover
sexual orientation by sex interactions emerged indicating that several popular models of sex-
ual orientation are not supported on a linguistic level.

KEY WORDS: homosexuality; sex differences; language; word use; text analysis.

Compare random samples of men and women than like her heterosexual sisters. Being a gay man
on a psychological or behavioral dimension, and and being “effeminate” are similarly assumed to be
there is a good chance of finding a group difference. highly correlated.
Eagly (1995) noted that despite early attempts to On the largest psychological gender differ-
claim that gender differences are basically a figment ence—the sex of preferred mates—the lay model is
of the popular imagination, meta-analyses have re- true by definition. Gay men are like heterosexual
ported large effect sizes (>0.8 standard deviations) women in that they seek male partners, and lesbians
for dimensions as diverse as mental rotation abil- are like heterosexual men in that they seek female
ity and facial expressiveness. Numerous studies have partners. Whether the model can be appropriately
demonstrated that gender stereotypes correspond extrapolated to other behavioral and psychological
surprisingly well to actual group differences (e.g., domains is an empirical question. Or rather, it is a
Swim, 1994). However, popular stereotypes would series of empirical questions with a large number
also have people believe that gender-typical charac- of possible outcomes. In principle, on any given
teristics and sexual orientation are two sides of the characteristic, gay men could resemble heterosexual
same coin. According to the stereotypes, if a woman women, or heterosexual men, or lesbians, or could
walks and talks more like a man than like the fem- be quite different from all three. Lesbians might
inine norm, she is probably a lesbian. Conversely, if be similar to heterosexual men, or to heterosexual
you know someone is a lesbian, you can reasonably women, or to gay men, or quite different from each.
expect that she will walk and talk more like a man The four groups might cluster in generally consistent
ways across a variety of characteristics, or they might
1 University
not. Stereotypes and many psychological researchers
of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas.
2 To whom correspondence should be addressed at Department
have proposed theories that directly or indirectly
of Psychology, A8000, The University of Texas, Austin, Texas suggest that some configurations are more likely
78712; e-mail: Pennebaker@mail.utexas.edu. than others.

447 0360-0025/05/0400-0447/0 
C 2005 Springer Science+Business Media, Inc.
448 Groom and Pennebaker

A word here about terminology. The existence their gender norms, on average, and generally fall
of English homonyms, the ethical need to minimize somewhere in-between the two heterosexual groups.
linguistic bias, and the conceptual ambiguity that sur- A meta-analysis by Bailey and Zucker (1995) showed
rounds questions of gender and sexual orientation that gay men and lesbians both recalled gender-
pose challenges to researchers working in this gen- atypical childhood behaviors at rates higher than did
eral area. Because we explore the interface between heterosexuals. Such atypicality included feeling more
chromosomal sex, sexual orientation, and gender- like members of the other sex, preferring to play with
typical behavior, and seek to test common assump- members of the other sex, and choosing activities as-
tions that conflate the three into two concepts or even sociated with the other sex. Furthermore, mothers’
one, linguistic clarity is essential. For the purposes of recollections tend to corroborate these reports (e.g.,
the present paper, sex refers to chromosomal sex, and Bailey, Nothnagel, & Wolfe, 1995). Adults’ observa-
gender-atypical refers to behaviors or characteristics tions of their own gender typicality also vary with
not normally associated with that person’s own sex orientation. Drawing on self-report data concerning
(cross-gender-typed will be used occasionally where thoughts and emotions, Pillard (1991) concluded that
atypicality refers specifically to having the charac- gay men and lesbians tend to fall between the modes
teristics of the other sex). Orientation refers to sex- for heterosexual men and heterosexual women.
ual orientation. People who prefer sexual partners Other researchers have explored whether the as-
of their own sex, including women, are referred to sociation extends beyond behavioral characteristics
by the adjective gay. The term homosexuality will that are visible “to the naked eye” to include low-
be used only where a counterpart is needed to the level psychological and physical characteristics. Find-
noun heterosexuality. The four sex-orientation com- ings include male-typical patterns of spontaneous
binations (heterosexual men, heterosexual women, sounds emitted by the inner ear in lesbians (sounds
gay men, and gay women) will be referred to as generated by the outer hair cells in the cochlea in re-
mate-preference groups. sponse to noise stimulation; McFadden & Pasanen,
1999), and different patterns of handedness in gay
and heterosexual men (e.g., Lindesay, 1987, although
Unidimensional Models not replicated by Mustanski, Bailey, & Kaspar, 2002).
Evidence has been drawn not only from gay
The lay model can be thought of as a unidi- samples that show reduced rates of gender-typical
mensional model in that for any sex-dimorphic characteristics, but also from samples selected for
characteristic, individuals can be categorized accord- biological abnormality that show an elevated rate of
ing to both sex typicality and orientation on a single homosexuality. Collear and Hines (1995) evaluated
bipolar continuum. This could run from “masculine” the evidence for a biological theory of behavioral
to “feminine,” with heterosexual men and heterosex- gender differences in general. A key part of their
ual women at the extremes. Gay men and lesbians human evidence is comprised of observations that
would fall somewhere in between, significantly dif- when the developing fetus is exposed to abnormal
ferent from the heterosexual members of their own amounts of certain hormones, gender-typical be-
sex, and maybe even crossing over to the point where havior is affected alongside physical characteristics.
they are indistinguishable from the heterosexual For example, girls exposed prenatally to excess
members of the other sex. Money (1970) envisioned androgens or progestins either through a genetic
a unipolar version of the continuum, beginning deficiency as in the case of congenital adrenal hyper-
at one end with mild deviance from normatively plasia (CAH), or as a result of medication prescribed
gender-typical social roles, to homosexuality at the to the mother appear relatively more likely to show
midpoint, and transsexualism at the extreme. Both physical and psychological characteristics associated
contain echoes of Freud’s (1973/1905) “inversion with boys, compared to controls. Girls with CAH
theory,” which explained homosexuality as part of a show not only virilized genitalia at birth, but also
package of gender-atypical behavior that arose from tend to show cross-sex play preferences, less secure
failure to identify with the same-sex parent. core sex identities, masculine profiles on personality
There is good evidence for an association be- inventories, and an elevated rate of homosexuality
tween sexual orientation and behavioral gender- and bisexuality. Maternal progestin medication pro-
typicality. For highly gender-typed characteristics, duces daughters with a similar pattern of masculin-
gay men and lesbians both differ significantly from ization. Androgen-based maternal medication has
Language Use in Personals 449

the mildest effects, producing only an elevated rate dren’s dispositions and skills so that they fit neatly
of homosexuality. The implication is that sexual ori- into either the domestic, childrearing role or the
entation is at least sometimes part of a biologically- breadwinner role. This division of labor originated
based package of sex-atypical characteristics. in women’s physical capacity to bear and nurse chil-
Other researchers in this school such as Bailey, dren, but social norms ensure that women and men
Kim, Hills, and Linsenmeier (1997) are careful to also develop different psychological dispositions and
note that they are focusing on group means; they ac- skills. Just as contemporary girls are socialized to be
knowledge substantial within-group variability and nurturant even if they aim to be trial lawyers without
imply that inferences about individuals (i.e., stereo- children, so lesbians and gay men should not be im-
typing) are not necessarily warranted. However, such mune to socialization just because they will not be as-
evidence is frequently used to support theories that suming the traditional sexual division of labor found
maintain that having the same mate preferences as in the nuclear family. However, the theory would be
the majority of members of the other sex is very compatible with a weak form of the unidimensional
likely to be accompanied by other cross-gender- model, because the forces driving the sexes apart psy-
typed characteristics. This underlying assumption is chologically (e.g., role-differentiation following mar-
shared even by researchers who do not espouse a riage and childbirth) may be weaker among gay men
“gay gene” hypothesis. For example, Bem (1996) of- and women.
fered what he called the “exotic becomes erotic” Status-based accounts of gender differences
theory (EBE). This proposes that sexual orientation (e.g., Ridgeway & Smith-Lovin, 1999) presume that
depends on which peer groups a person felt most dif- behavioral gender differences derive from people’s
ferent from as a child. For example, a gender-typical tendency to behave in accordance with their expec-
girl who forms early friendships with other girls tations about status (their own social status and that
will feel most different from gender-typical boys. of others) in a world where sex is a powerful status
This difference is arousing, and this arousal becomes cue. Indeed, Ridgeway and Smith-Lovin argued that
transformed around puberty into sexual attraction. many behavioral gender differences weaken or van-
For most people, therefore, heterosexual orientation ish in same-sex situations. Because gay people are
develops. However, for a boy with a gentle tempera- deemed inferior by broader societal norms, such the-
ment that makes him particularly suited to feminine- ories imply that (openly) gay people will suffer from
typical activities and peer groups, gender-typical low status in the same way as women in general do,
boys will become the object of arousal and attraction. with lesbians most likely at the bottom of the hierar-
Conversely, girls with a particularly active temper- chy because both their sex and their orientation are
ament will seek out masculine-typical activities and low-status cues.
friendships, and most likely come to develop lesbian Performance-based social constructionist the-
tendencies. The more atypical one is, at least as a ories (e.g., West & Zimmerman, 1991) argue that
child, the more likely one is to be gay, and vice versa. gender differences arise not from enduring trait dif-
ferences or even roles, but from gender-appropriate
ways of conducting social interaction, or “doing
Socialization-Based Alternatives gender.” Society obliges people to make their sex
to the Unidimensional Model clear during interpersonal interactions, and a deviant
orientation does not exempt one from being heav-
In socialization-based accounts of psychological ily pressured to display gender-typical behaviors.
and behavioral gender differences, the relationship Furthermore, these displays double as signals of
between sex and orientation is much less direct than heterosexuality. By employing emphatic behavioral
in the case of biological theories. Even where gen- demonstrations of their sex, heterosexuals are able
der differences in the gay population are not ad- to ensure that interaction partners are in no doubt
dressed directly, predictions can be readily derived. as to whether this is a sexually-charged, cross-sex
Generally speaking, socialization forces that man- interaction, or a platonically (perhaps competitively)
date gender-typical characteristics ought to apply to flavored same-sex interaction. West and Zimmerman
gay as well as to heterosexual people. argued that lesbians and gay men use gender displays
Social role theory (e.g., Eagly, Wood, & as camouflage, and they have the option of not per-
Diekman, 2000) traces behavioral and psychological forming these displays and thus being recognized as
gender differences to societal norms that mold chil- gay (less clear is what form the alternative behavior
450 Groom and Pennebaker

might take, because cross-gender-typed behavior us), or third person pronouns in the singular form
is not the only option). Such rejection of hetero- (e.g., he, she), or even the collective form (e.g., they,
sexuality is, of course, not without its social costs. their). All these options achieve the semantic goal of
However, this theory does imply that “un-closeted” referring to the speaker’s kin, but they imply quite
interactions between gay people of the same sex fall different qualities of relationship.
outside of society’s schema. Whereas gender-based Technological innovations have made it much
differentiation is central to heterosexual interactions, easier to detect linguistic correlates of individual dif-
gay men and lesbians may choose to structure their ferences. Unlike human coders, a computer program
interactions around asymmetrical displays of gender- can analyze a virtually unlimited number of texts
typical characteristics (e.g., butch/femme role distinc- on a very wide range of linguistic dimensions, and
tions in some lesbian relationships), or they may not. code them consistently and rapidly. One such pro-
In summary, social role theory emphasizes the gram is Linguistic Analysis and Word Count (LIWC;
ability of society to perpetuate gender differences Pennebaker, Francis, & Booth, 2001). LIWC calcu-
regardless of underlying individual differences. The lates the proportion of words in a given text sample—
difference between men and women should be either written language or transcribed speech—that
paramount. Other theories tie sex-dimorphic behav- fit into one of its 70+ word categories. The cate-
ior to gender differences in status. The low status of gories cover linguistic dimensions such as pronouns
homosexuality, at least when it is out of the closet, and articles, psychological processes (e.g., emotions,
should promote overall differences between gays and thoughts), social processes (e.g., friendship refer-
heterosexuals, in addition to the continuing impact ences), words used to communicate temporal or spa-
of sex-based cues on men and women. A third class tial context (e.g., past tense verbs), and words that
of theories argue for the active role of the indi- describe the explicit topic under discussion (e.g.,
vidual in creating gender differences, and the role money, sex). Raters were used to help determine
of heterosexuality in motivating emphatic displays which words belonged in which categories, allowing
of gender difference. Gender differences are likely an element of human qualitative judgment to be in-
to persist even among gay people because gender- corporated into an otherwise automated procedure.
typical characteristics serve a protective function, The breadth of the categories makes LIWC a
but to the extent that gay people choose to identify potent inductive tool. Researchers who have used
themselves as gay, the gender differences might be the program have discovered linguistic markers for
weakened. distinguishing between poets who did and did not
commit suicide (Stirman & Pennebaker, 2001), be-
Language as a Multivariate Personality Marker tween people of different ages (Pennebaker & Stone,
2003), between people who are lying and people who
The question of how the four sexual-orientation are telling the truth (Newman, Pennebaker, Berry,
groups cluster in general on psychological and behav- & Richards, 2003), and other individual differences
ioral dimensions is a multivariate issue. The best an- (Pennebaker & King, 1999).
swer is likely to come from a simultaneous compari- Most relevant to the present research is a re-
son of members of the four mate-preference groups cent study of the language of more than 11,000 men
on a whole range of dispositional variables, from and women speaking or writing across a broad va-
explicit interests and concerns to implicit tenden- riety of contexts from student essays to transcribed
cies to think or do things in a particular way. The talk shows to classic novels (Newman, Groom, Stone,
study of language use as a personality marker (e.g., & Pennebaker, 2005). A surprisingly high number
Groom & Pennebaker, 2002; Pennebaker, Mehl, & of gender differences were found in the ways peo-
Niederhoffer, 2003) may offer a fresh strategy for ad- ple wrote and talked. In univariate analyses, women
dressing this challenge. were found to make more references than men
Producing natural language in the form of writ- to psychological processes, social processes (espe-
ing or speech involves many choices: what to talk cially pronouns), motion, and the home. Women
about, and what words, phrase forms, and sentence also used more negations (e.g., no, never) and past
structures to use in talking about it. For example, and present tense verbs. Men used longer sentences,
people can discuss their families using references to longer words, and swore more often. Men also pre-
social roles (e.g., brother, father), or proper nouns dominated in the use of words related to object
(e.g., Tom, Sue), or first person pronouns (e.g., we, properties (articles, prepositions, numbers), and in
Language Use in Personals 451

discussion of current concerns (e.g., money, occupa- psychological work, hailed as a window into both so-
tion, metaphysical topics, sports). ciety and individuals (e.g., Parekh & Bersesin, 2001).
A similar LIWC study by Brownlow, The advertisements provide a unique way of learning
Rosamond, and Parker (2003) using a sample of tran- about the lives of individuals who would never come
scribed television interviews found that women used into a lab. The norms for personals add a structure
more social words and pronouns, whereas men used that makes them easier to compare than would be
more articles, unique words (i.e., a greater variety of the case for less structured media, such as weblogs.
words as a proportion of total words) and long words The most popular research use has been qualitative
(at least six letters). These findings using the LIWC coding of advertisement content in order to better
program fit well with findings reported by Koppel, understand mate-selection strategies (e.g., Cicerello
Argamon, and Shimoni (2002). These authors ap- & Sheeham, 1995; Hatala & Prehodka, 1996). The
proached computerized text analysis from a different general findings have been that women emphasize
angle, using a program that established inductively psychological characteristics (e.g. Deaux & Randel,
the linguistic features that best distinguished texts 1984), and lesbians in particular devote very little
written by men from those written by women. The attention to body shape or other physical features
program was able to categorize samples by gender (e.g., Epel, Spanakos, Kasl-Godley, & Brownell,
with about 80% accuracy. Features that predicted 1996). In a study where fictitious ads were placed,
male authorship included a, the, and one, consistent men responded more to looks, and women more
with the findings for articles and numbers in the to success (Goode, 1996). Gay men were generally
LIWC study. Consistent with the LIWC results for more interested in sex and in the partner’s physical
pronouns and negations, features that predicted fe- attractiveness, as indicated, for example, by a request
male authorship included she and not. However, cer- for a photograph (e.g., Hatala & Prehodka, 1996).
tain prepositions (for, with, and) actually predicted Although there may still be stigma attached to
female authorship, in contrast to the LIWC data. placing and responding to personals, work dating
The LIWC results are also compatible with more back almost 10 years suggests that it is becoming an
qualitative observations of the differences between increasingly popular way to meet people (Merskin
men’s and women’s language (e.g., Tannen, 1990). & Huberlie, 1996). Those who choose this route are
It is reasonable to assume that the vast major- relatively well-educated and successful, but simply
ity of the men and women studied were heterosex- feel disconnected from traditional ways of meeting
ual. Thus, a comparison of text samples from all people, and generally report having had good ex-
four mate-preference groups would be a way to ex- periences with personals (Jason, Moritsugu, & De-
amine the predictions of the unidimensional mod- Palma, 1992). The internet has increased the appeal
els from several angles to address several questions. of personals by minimizing or eliminating the cost
First, would a multivariate effect be found for sex, of placing an advertisement and ensuring anonymity
as in the earlier study, and would it be accompa- beyond an exchange of email addresses.
nied by effects for orientation and the sex by orienta- For the purposes of the present research, in-
tion interaction? Second, how would these predictor ternet personals have several advantages. First, per-
variables impact individual LIWC categories? Third, sonals provide an unambiguous operational defini-
when the full array of LIWC categories is used to tion of sexual orientation, because the ads state both
predict mate-preference group membership, which the sex of the ad-poster and the sex of the desired
groups would prove to be most easily distinguished mate. Second, any strategies that gay people use to
from each other, and which the most easily confused? conceal orientation should be minimized, and the
salience of their orientation in the process of writ-
ing the advertisement should maximize the chance
THE PRESENT STUDY that any gender-atypical linguistic patterns should be
displayed. Third, personals provide access to a very
Using Personal Ads as Language Samples large community-based sample.

In the present research we examined the lin- Overview and Predictions


guistic style of the four mate-preference groups by
studying the language used in internet personal ads. The relationship between psychological gender
Personals have been used repeatedly in previous differences and sexual orientation is critical both
452 Groom and Pennebaker

for our understanding of sex differences and of ori- that places heterosexual men and women far apart,
entation. Biological approaches and most laypeo- with gay men and lesbians somewhere in between.
ple generally hold that gender-atypicality in terms Thus, gay men should more often be misassigned to
of the sex of preferred mates correlates with gen- the heterosexual woman category than should het-
der typicality in other psychological and behavioral erosexual men. Lesbians should more often be misas-
domains. Sex differences established in heterosexual signed to the heterosexual man category than should
samples would be expected to be smaller or reversed heterosexual women. In addition, gay men and les-
in gay populations. Socialization-based approaches bians should be more easily confused with each other
construe psychological gender differences as the than should heterosexual men and women.
product of sociocultural prescription, and therefore
likely to be at least partly independent of individu-
Predictions Based on Socialization Approaches
als’ mate preferences. In the present study we sought
to establish how sex and orientation impacted behav-
Socialization-based theories of gender differ-
ior in a particular domain: language. Language was
ences, particularly role theory, would predict a main
chosen as the marker of gender-typicality because it
effect of sex to be the most prominent determinant of
is a domain in which gender differences have been
linguistic style. However, many of these approaches
widely reported, because it provides a snapshot of
would be compatible with a small interaction effect in
naturally occurring behavior, and because multiple
the same direction as the biological theories predict,
features of language can be assessed simultaneously
because the forces driving the sexes apart psycho-
using computerized text analysis. Language samples
logically may be just a little weaker among gay men
were obtained by collecting personal ads from a pop-
and lesbians. In the case of status-based accounts,
ular internet personals site. The resulting linguistic
the fact that gay personals are a type of same-sex
profile of heterosexual men, heterosexual women,
interaction should weaken sex differences, although
gay men, and lesbians could potentially offer support
there may also be a main effect for orientation such
for unidimensional theories, socialization-based the-
that being gay and being female push behavior in the
ories, or point toward alternative conceptions of the
same direction (e.g., more self-references).
sex-orientation intersection.

METHOD
Predictions Based on Alternatives
to the Unidimensional Approach Search Protocol

The unidimensional approach to sex and orien- The internet site chosen for the study was
tation would predict that the linguistic gender differ- AOL’s Match.com. This particular site boasts over
ences observed for heterosexuals would be smaller three million members and has a detailed question-
or reversed in the language of gay men and women. naire, which provided us with extensive standardized
In other words, there should be a large multivari- information about each member. People who post
ate effect of the orientation by sex interaction, pos- personal ads supply demographic and physical infor-
sibly accompanied by a sizeable main effect of sex, mation, an optional personality questionnaire, and
with little or no main effect of orientation. These two narratives, one that primarily describes them-
multivariate results should translate into discovery selves, and one that describes their ideal match. Po-
of many univariate crossover interactions between tential responders can access all of this information
orientation and sex when each linguistic dimension by selecting the sex and age of the partner they seek,
is considered separately, possibly accompanied by their own sex, and a variety of other optional crite-
main effects of sex, with few main effects of orien- ria. Table I provides an example of the information
tation. For example, gay men should use fewer arti- contained in a typical ad.
cles and more present tense verbs than heterosexual For the purposes of this study, the Match.com
men, whereas lesbians should use more articles and database was searched to obtain 400 ads from
fewer present tense verbs than heterosexual women. each of four mate-preference groups, approximately
Finally, if scores on all the linguistic variables were matched on age. Because we were using the same
used to predict mate-preference group membership, search mechanism as a person seeking a mate, the
errors should reflect a configuration of the groups procedure was counterintuitive. For example, in
Language Use in Personals 453

Table I. Basic Information Contained in a Typical Personal sexual men, 394 heterosexual women, 395 gay men,
Advertisement Retrieved From Match.com and 402 lesbians).
Responses to For the purposes of analyzing writing style, the
multiple-choice questions Narratives two narratives (i.e., describe self and describe ideal
20 years old (Self) I am fun loving, friendly, match) were combined into a single text file. This
Austin, Texas crazy, wild, enjoyable, and maximized the reliability of estimates of writing style,
Blonde hair, blue eyes, 5 9 love men. Very curious with an average of 170.77 total words per participant
Average body type about others, I’m not nosy, I
English speaker just like to hear what you
(SD = 103.05; range = 32–731). Finally, each partic-
Religion: Other tell me, as long as it’s the ipant’s text sample was run through a spell-checker
High school education truth. I recently moved back and an additional macro program designed to re-
Works in sales & marketing to Austin and am looking move certain common abbreviations that would be
Earns less than $24,000/year for new ways to have fun. misinterpreted by LIWC. For example, w/ was con-
Smokes socially (Seeking) Talk, dark, and
Drinks socially/occasionally looking for me. Likes to
verted to with.
Divorced have fun but knows the time
Has children living at home and the place for
everything. Responsible for Customizing the Text Analysis
doing the right thing but
also knows how to laugh.
The default LIWC dictionary codes each text file
on 74 language dimensions. In the interests of min-
order to obtain personals posted by heterosexual imizing the number of analyses performed, we ex-
men, we selected “I am a woman seeking a man” cluded categories that were unique to conversation
from the four mate search options, along with a (e.g., nonfluencies such as hmm) or highly redundant
minimum and maximum age, with the further stip- with another category. The category friends was ex-
ulation that searches were restricted to the United cluded because it was essentially uninterpretable in
States. We defined nine age ranges from which equal the context of personal ads, mixing terms such as
numbers of advertisements from each of the four boyfriend and girlfriend with platonic terms such as
groups would be selected, ensuring approximate age- roommate and buddy. We also added a new pro-
matching of the mate-preference groups. The first noun subcategory: third person singular (e.g., him,
eight age groups each spanned 4 years (18–21, 22– her, his). This allowed us to explore whether gay men
25, 26–29, 30–33, 34–37, 38–41, 42–45, 46–49), but and lesbians would be less likely than their hetero-
because the number of ads dropped so dramatically sexual counterparts to use gender-specific singular
after age 50, especially in the lesbian category, we pronouns. Each cleaned text sample was analyzed us-
defined the last category as 50–59. The four gender ing the modified LIWC program, which counted the
combinations and nine age ranges yielded 36 cells. number of times that words from each category ap-
Advertisements were selected within each cat- peared in the advertisement, and calculated this as a
egory, copied, and pasted into a spreadsheet in the proportion of all the words in the advertisement. The
order in which they appeared, which itself depended grand means for each measure revealed that 22 word
on how recently the ad-posters had adjusted their categories had extremely low base rates (<0.5%).
profiles. All ads were read, and they were excluded These included most categories with negative conno-
if it was obvious that they had been placed in the tations (e.g., death, anger, swear words), along with
wrong orientation category (e.g., an ad appeared in several other narrow categories such as eating and
a female-to-female search that referred to seeking “a music. Such low base rates indicated that these cat-
man who knows how to treat a lady”). This exclusion egories were not relevant to an analysis of personal
criterion was used sparingly, and only if there was ads, and these categories were dropped from further
explicit reference to the wrong sex or a photo that inferential analyses. Analyses were performed on the
clearly depicted someone of the wrong sex. A hand- remaining 44 categories.
ful of advertisements were excluded on the grounds
that they were obviously jokes. Each excluded ad
was replaced with approximately one additional ad in RESULTS
that search category. In the final sample, there were
41–47 ads per cell, 1,586 in total, distributed evenly Overall, LIWC recognized an average of 80.5%
across the four mate preference groups (395 hetero- of the words in each text sample (SD = 5.79). An
454 Groom and Pennebaker

alpha level of .01 was used for all analyses to take tained higher proportions of words falling into the
into account the large sample size and large number psychological categories of physical words and sex-
of analyses conducted. uality words (a subset of physical words) than did
men’s advertisements. Even when the analysis was
restricted to the 797 gay personals, the pattern of ef-
Multivariate Analyses fects was virtually unchanged, even maintaining the
rigorous .01 alpha level. In these highly conservative
A MANOVA was performed on the remain- analyses, the only differences were that effects fell
ing 44 LIWC categories, with sex (male vs. fe- below significance for articles, third person singular,
male) and sexual orientation (heterosexual vs. gay) and long words, and two new effects reached signifi-
as the two dichotomous predictor variables. All cance (actually consistent with previous research but
three effects were significant: for the main effect of not significant in the personals sample as a whole):
sex, F (45, 1538) = 3.70, p < .001; for the main ef- the tendency of women to use more spatial words and
fect of orientation, F (45, 1538) = 5.57, p < .001. The fewer long words.
interaction effect was smaller, but still significant,
F (45, 1538) = 1.94, p < .001.
Main Effects of Orientation

Univariate Analyses With respect to orientation, a striking effect


was that heterosexuals were more prolific users of
Table II summarizes the main effects from the pronouns all round, including first-person singular
univariate analyses of each individual LIWC dimen- as well as third-person singular. Heterosexuals also
sion. The organization of the word categories is based wrote longer ads (on average, 36 words longer) than
on the way that LIWC was originally constructed. did gays. Heterosexuals were more likely than gays
Word categories are divided into five broad dimen- to mention occupation-related topics, whereas gay
sions: (1) linguistic properties, (2) social processes, people predominated in the use of sexual and body
(3) psychological processes, (4) temporal and spa- words. These effects persisted even when the sex-
tial context, and (5) current concerns that are almost ual category was modified to exclude explicitly gay-
always used as literal content words. A total of 10 related words (e.g., gay, bi), and the body category
effects were found for sex, and 10 were found for was modified to remove the terms AIDS, HIV, and
orientation. disease. A potentially related finding was that gay
people used more words from the “seeing” cate-
gory, which incorporates phrases such as “straight
Main Effects of Sex looking” or “feminine looking.”

Table III presents the main effects of sex and


sexual orientation in more detail. Generally speak- Sex by Orientation Interactions
ing, the results replicate many of the gender differ-
ences reported in previous work (Newman et al., The 10 effects of sex and 10 effects of orienta-
2004). Even with a sample that contained 50% gay tion were qualified by just five significant interactions
participants, it was still the case that women were between sex and orientation. Furthermore, the in-
more likely to use pronouns, especially first per- teractions that did occur were not of the crossover
son singular, discuss emotions, and use present tense form that unidimensional models would predict, and
verbs. Also as before, men used more articles and the effects were not consistent with any of the a pri-
made more references to their jobs. Women were ori models. Instead, the effects reflected situations
also more likely to use the newly-created category where one group, generally the lesbians, was the
of third person singular. Some findings for categories “odd one out.” The effect of orientation on word
that yielded null results in previous research indi- count appeared to be driven by a tendency of les-
cate the influence of the personal ad context. Specif- bians to write particularly short ads, and heterosex-
ically, women’s personal ads contained more refer- ual women to write particularly long ads; men of both
ences to physical and sexual issues than did men’s. In orientations fell in between. Lesbians also used the
other words, women’s personal advertisements con- shortest words, but this time the contrast was greatest
Language Use in Personals 455

Table II. Univariate Effects of Sex and Orientation on Word Category Usage
LIWC Dimension Examples Effect of sex (d) Effect of orientation (d) Interaction effect (eta2 )
Linguistic dimensions
Word count −.06 .35∗∗ .012∗∗
Words per sentence −.01 −.06 .000
Question marks −.07 .02 .000
Words of > six letters .11 −.02 .005∗∗
Numbers .12 −.11 .000
Negations no, never, not .03 −.09 .000
Articles a, an, the .14∗∗ .13 .000
Prepositions on, to, from .07 .03 .001
Inclusive words with, and, include −.01 −.02 .000
Exclusive words but, except, without .09 .07 .000
Social processes
Social words −.11 −.10 .001
Communication talk, share, converse .01 −.03 .000
Humans boy, woman, group .13 −.07 .000
Pronouns −.23∗∗ .19∗∗ .000
1st person singular I, we, me −.18∗∗ .17∗∗ .000
1st person plural we, us, our .02 −.10 .000
2nd person you, you’re −.04 .09 .000
3rd person singular he, she, his −.13∗∗ .18∗∗ .000
Psychological processes
Emotions
Positive emotions happy, pretty, good −.23∗∗ .03 .000
Optimism certainty, pride, win .01 .11 .002
Positive feelings happy, joy −.29∗∗ .02 .003
Negative emotions afraid, hate, cry .00 .01 .002
Sensations .01 −.10 .002
Hearing heard, listen, sound −.03 .04 .003
Seeing view, saw, look .05 −.17∗∗ .000
Physical words −.16∗∗ −.32∗∗ .000
Sexuality lust, pregnant, gay −.32∗∗ −.26∗∗ .001
Body states ache, heart, cough .10 −.27∗∗∗ .006∗∗
Cognitive processes −.08 .05 .000
Insight think, know, −.02 .06 .001
Discrepancy should, would, could −.11 .01 .000
Tentative perhaps, guess .10 −.12 .002
Certainty always, never −.04 −.04 .000
Time and space
Time till, started, hour .01 .06 .000
Past tense verb walked, were, had .05 .11 .000
Present tense verb walk, is, be −.29∗∗ −.07 .007∗∗
Future tense verb will, might, shall −.06 −.06 .000
Space here, up, around .11 −.04 .003
Motion verbs walk, move, go −.11 .05 .000
Current concerns
Occupation .06 .28∗∗ .002
Job employ, boss, career .14∗∗ .09 .005∗∗
Achieve try, goal, win .07 .20∗∗ .001
Leisure −.02 .06 .002
Home house, kitchen, lawn .01 .06 .001
Sports football, play, game .04 .10 .004

Note. Effect sizes for main effects are based on Cohen’s d to show directionality and for interpretability. For effects of sex,
positive numbers indicate higher values for men than women. For effects of orientation, positive numbers indicate higher values
heterosexuals than gays. The appropriate index for interactions, eta2 , yields smaller numbers and is therefore reported to three
decimal places.
∗∗ p < .01.
456 Groom and Pennebaker

Table III. Means for Significant Effects of Sex and Orientation on Word Category Usage
Means for sex main effects
LIWC dimension Category Men Women
Linguistic Articles 6.03 5.74
Social Pronouns 11.22 12.06
1st person singular 7.15 7.68
3rd person singular .59 .73
Psychological Positive emotions 8.77 9.64
Positive feelings 2.91 3.48
Physical 2.12 2.40
Sexuality .98 1.38
Present tense 13.41 14.46
Current concerns Job .74 .62
Means for orientation main effects
Category Heterosexuals Gay men & lesbians
Linguistic Word count 188.79 152.93
Social Pronouns 11.99 11.29
1st person singular 7.67 7.17
3rd person singular .76 .56
Psychological Seeing 1.32 1.52
Physical 1.99 2.54
Sexuality 1.01 1.35
Body .50 .74
Current concerns Occupation 2.01 1.63
Achieve 1.00 .83
Means for sex × orientation interaction
Heterosexual Gay/Lesbian
Category Men Women Men Women
Linguistic Word count 174.24 203.39 160.95 145.05
Words of 6+ letters 15.72 15.88 16.57 15.22
Psychological Body 0.47 0.53 160.95 145.05
Time and space Present tense 13.60 14.02 16.57 15.22
Current concerns Job 0.72 0.72 0.85 0.62

with gay men, who had the largest proportion of long group ensured that the analyses of the raw data were
words in their ads; both heterosexual groups fell in not substantially skewed by the effects of age, which
the middle. Lesbians also used the most present tense has been shown to affect individuals’ word usage
verbs; the tendency of women to use more present (Pennebaker & Stone, 2003). A second demographic
tense verbs than men, as found overall and in pre- variable was a second potential source of bias: socioe-
vious research, was much larger when lesbians were conomic status. Because we were measuring use of
compared to gay men than it was when heterosex- language, and because our sample was drawn from
ual women and men were compared. Finally, lesbians the community rather than from a more education-
were much less likely than any of the other three ally homogenous university population, it was impor-
groups to use words from the job category. In the tant to check for variations in education level among
case of one interaction, it was the gay men who stood the four gender combination groups. The finding of
out as the most prolific users of body references. an interaction effect on use of long words signaled a
possible socioeconomic bias such that gay men, the
most prolific users of long words, were also the best
Controlling for Socioeconomic Status educated, and lesbians, who used the fewest, were the
least educated.
The strategy of selecting equal numbers of ad- Of the 1,586 participants, only 7.8% failed or
vertisements by age as well as by mate-preference refused to report their educational level (1 = High
Language Use in Personals 457

Table IV. Percentage Classification Success of Discriminant Analysis Predicting Mate-Preference Group Based
on Linguistic Indicators
Predicted group membership
Actual group membership Heterosexual men Heterosexual women Gay men Lesbians Total
Heterosexual men 39.0 25.6 18.7 16.7 100
Heterosexual women 21.6 47.2 14.5 16.8 100
Gay men 19.5 15.7 44.6 20.3 100
Lesbians 16.7 20.9 19.2 43.3 100

school, 2 = Some college, 3 = Associates degree, groups based on individuals’ scores for the 44 linguis-
4 = bachelor’s degree, 5 = graduate degree, 6 = tic categories. The overall success rate was 42.3%,
PhD/Post Doctoral, 7 = Prefer not to say). An which is substantially higher than the 25% hit rate
ANOVA on the remaining responses revealed that that would be expected by chance. As shown in Table
the men (M = 3.21, SD = 1.37) were better educated IV, the resulting functions successfully classified 39%
than the women (M = 3.00, SD = 1.34), F (1, 1456) = of heterosexual men, 47.2% of heterosexual women,
9.8, p < .01. The effect of orientation was not sig- 44.6% of gay men, and 43.3% of lesbians. Contrary to
nificant. However, there was a very large sex by stereotypes, gay men were least likely to be miscate-
orientation interaction, F (1, 1456) = 20.56, p < .01. gorized as heterosexual women, and vice versa. Sim-
Post-hoc tests confirmed that lesbians (M = 2.79, ilarly, lesbians were least likely to be miscategorized
SD = 1.35) had indeed completed less education, as heterosexual men, and vice versa. In fact, the most
on average, than any of the other groups (Het- common classification errors involved classifying het-
erosexual men: M = 3.10, SD = 1.36; Heterosexual erosexual men as heterosexual women, and gay men
women: M = 3.20, SD = 1.31; Gay men: M = 3.33, as heterosexual men or as lesbians.
SD = 1.37). Gay men were also educated to a signifi-
cantly higher level than were heterosexual men.
We repeated our analysis of the effects of sex DISCUSSION
and orientation on language after controlling for ed-
ucation level (we replaced “Prefer not to say” re- By comparing the language used by heterosex-
spondents with the group mean) and also for exact ual men, heterosexual women, gay men, and les-
age. The impact on the pattern of results was mini- bians in over 1,500 personal ads, we were able to test
mal. All three multivariate effects were again signif- whether the four groups clustered in ways predicted
icant (two main effects and an interaction). In terms deductively by current lay and scientific thinking
of univariate analyses, the interaction effect with re- about sex and orientation. In particular, we examined
spect to long words fell below significance, as did the whether psychological gender differences are weak-
interaction and main effects for job-related words, ened or even reversed when the men and women
which are also likely to be tied to education level. compared are gay rather than heterosexual. Lay
There were only two other changes. First, men’s ten- stereotypes and many biological theories would ex-
dency to use more numbers than women (men = pect this to be the case, whereas socialization-based
1.08, women = .92), as found in previous research, gender theories would not necessarily make this
was now significant, F (1, 1583) = 8.06, p < .01. Sec- assumption.
ond, men referred to the humans (a subcategory of
social words including boy, woman, and group) more
often than did women (men = 2.12, women = 1.91), The Effects of Sex and Sexual Orientation
contrary to previous null results, F (1, 1583) = 7.57, on Language Use
p < .01.
Men’s vs. Women’s Language

Classification Analysis Those who argue for main effects of sex can ar-
gue on the basis of these findings that psycholog-
A stepwise discriminant analysis was computed ical/behavioral gender differences transcend differ-
to predict membership of the four mate-preference ences in orientation. In addition to a multivariate
458 Groom and Pennebaker

main effect of sex, many of the previous differences significant orientation effects emerged on as many
identified between men’s language and women’s lan- occasions as gender differences had done (10 in each
guage [especially in Newman et al.’s (2004) study] case), and they emerged on many of the same dimen-
were replicated, despite the fact that one-half of sions. Socialization-based theories offer a possible in-
the sample were gender-atypical at least to the ex- terpretation for the particular orientation effects that
tent of seeking romantic involvement with same-sex were found. Essentially, a fundamental difference in
partners. The general linguistic patterns were consis- the concerns that gay and heterosexual people have
tent with Tannen’s (1990) distinction between men’s when seeking a mate may well arise from the fact that
“report” communication styles and women’s “rap- societal norms that regulate heterosexual courtship
port” communication styles, the former focused on do not apply, at least in the same way, in gay rela-
information exchange and the latter on fostering con- tionships.
nections with other people. As in previous research, First, heterosexual ads contained more refer-
women displayed a robust tendency to use more ref- ences to occupation, which is traditionally a key
erences to people, more emotion words, more first- issue in mate selection (e.g., Buss, 1989; Eagly &
person singular pronouns, and more present tense Wood, 1999). In gay relationships, there is no a priori
verbs. Men displayed a robust tendency to use more assumption that one partner should contribute more
articles, and more numbers (after SES was con- resources than the other, which renders occupation
trolled), both of which signal an interest in object a more optional topic for mention in a gay personal
properties. Remarkably, the sex effects were almost ad. The role structure for heterosexual relationships
identical when the analyses included only gay people. that Eagly focuses on (e.g., Eagly et al., 2000) may
Gender differences also emerged on dimensions be recreated in some form by gay couples and
related to sex and physical characteristics that had subcultures, but it does not supply the same degree
yielded null results in previous research, which sug- of relationship predictability that heterosexual roles
gests a special role for those words in the specific con- do (e.g., the more masculine partner in a lesbian
text of personal ads. This is consistent with earlier relationship may have children). Perhaps even more
observations that heterosexual men tend to be less important, gay singles do not face the same a priori
explicit about sexual and physical topics in personal status—and power—differentials that heterosexual
ads, either because of taboo or because their desires singles have to deal with. Consistent with a different
are taken for granted (Laner & Kamel, 1977). set of relationship priorities and taboos, gay people
The one inconsistency with previous gender dif- in our study used more references to sex and body,
ference work was that when SES was controlled for which apparently demonstrates a more up-front atti-
in this study, men used abstract, indefinite references tude to relationships. Such results are also consistent
to humans (e.g., man, woman) more than did women, with earlier work that showed that heterosexual
which is the opposite of the result reported by ads focused more on long-term relationships and
Newman et al. (2004). In the context of personal ads, financial security than did ads placed by gay people
it is not really possible for men to avoid social ref- (e.g., Gonzales & Meyers, 1993).
erences in the way that appears habitual for men in Second, heterosexual ads used pronouns that
other contexts. Indeed, it is interesting that men pre- emphasize differences between speaker and audi-
fer indefinite social references in contrast to women’s ence (for a discussion of pronoun choice and the
preference for definite social references (e.g., he vs. speaker–audience relationship, see Solomon, 1978).
a man), which suggests perhaps that women have a In particular, heterosexuals used more first-person
more elaborated vision of their ideal mate. This sug- singular and more third-person singular pronouns.
gestion would be consistent with earlier findings that Use of such pronouns gives the impression that the
women tend to put more effort into the personals ad-poster was drawing a mental contrast between
process, whereas men tend to respond briefly and rel- “me” and “him” or “her.” This finding may point
atively indiscriminately to many ads (Goode, 1996). toward a tendency for gay relationships to be more
fluid and focused more on similarity than on comple-
mentarity as desirable qualities, at least in the early
Heterosexuals’ vs. Gays’ Language stages of courtship. West and Zimmerman’s (1991)
analysis is useful here in linking displays of gender-
A multivariate main effect of sexual orientation typical characteristics with displays of heterosexual-
was also found, and univariate analyses revealed that ity. Given that gay people who post personal ads are
Language Use in Personals 459

not trying to signal heterosexuality, emphatic demon- It is especially remarkable that as many as one-half
strations of sex dimorphic behaviors are not rele- of the gender differences and orientation differences
vant to courtship in the way that they are for het- fell along the same dimensions. Pronouns, body ref-
erosexuals. In same-sex relationships, fundamental erences, and sexuality words were all subject to joint
differentiation in this sense is optional, and degree of influences of both factors. It was not simply the case
gender typicality is a matter of personal preference. that sex effects swamped orientation in some cases,
Indeed, as previous studies have shown (e.g., Bailey and orientation trumped sex in others. Instead, they
et al., 1997; Phua, 2002), the current results suggest had independent effects on the exact same linguistic
that posters of gay personals tend to spell out their markers. Nor was it possible to try to draw sex and
own degree of typicality as well as that of their de- orientation under a single banner such as status. If we
sired partner. In particular, the “seeing” LIWC cat- look specifically at the five dimensions on which par-
egory on which gays were higher than heterosexuals allel effects of sex and orientation were found, they
includes “straight looking,” “feminine looking,” and are split roughly evenly between effects where be-
similar phrases that directly communicate gender- ing female and being gay pull language in the same
typicality. direction (i.e., physical, sexuality) and effects where
they pull in opposite directions (i.e., pronouns, first-
person singular, third person singular).
Effect of Sex by Orientation Interactions

The most startling finding was the complete ab- Predicting Mate-Preference From Linguistic Style
sence of crossover interactions between sex and ori-
entation, as would be predicted by unidimensional Classification analyses dealt another blow to
theories and stereotypes that group lesbians with unidimensional assumptions. Instead of finding gay
heterosexual men and gay men with heterosexual men to be more easily confused with heterosex-
women. In the raw data, a small multivariate inter- ual women than were heterosexual men, the exact
action effect was found, but there were only one-half reverse was the case. Gay men and heterosexual
as many significant interactions on a univariate level women were the furthest apart of all the groups.
as had been the case for sex and orientation. Even Lesbians and heterosexual men were closer together,
the interactions that were observed did not conform but still more likely to be confused with members of
to a crossover pattern. Instead, it seemed that the their own sex.
lesbian group was in some way qualitatively differ-
ent from the other three groups in terms of present
tense, word count, word length, and job concerns. Limitations and Future Directions
For example, heterosexual women used the most
words, followed by heterosexual men, gay men, and The results of the present study provide strong
lesbians. Further analyses established that there was evidence that, in terms of linguistic style, gay men
indeed something special about the lesbian group: and lesbians who place personal ads on mainstream
they were less educated than ad-posters from any of internet sites show similar patterns of gender differ-
the other three mate-preference groups. Controlling ences in language as do heterosexuals, which under-
for socioeconomic status eroded the collection of in- mines assumptions based on theories and stereotypes
teractions even further, eliminating effects for word that predict general gender-atypical characteristics in
length and job words but leaving the vast majority of gay people. In addition, gay people’s ads suggested
the main effects unchanged. some different overall motives and concerns than
Even theorists of gender or orientation who do are found in ads of their heterosexual counterparts.
not adhere to such a unidimensional approach would These differences themselves may arise in part from
be surprised at the parallel quality of the effects of the irrelevance of courtship norms based on gen-
sex and orientation. It undermines our very assump- der differences. The present research underlies the
tion that orientation and gender-typicality are at least need for gender researchers to consider orientation,
related, to observe that sex has effects that apply to rather than taking heterosexuality for granted. Simi-
the exact same extent in gays as they do in hetero- larly, sexual orientation researchers need to consider
sexuals, and that orientation has effects that apply to the implications of gender differentiation for mat-
the exact same extent in men as they do in women. ing strategies among heterosexual and gay people, as
460 Groom and Pennebaker

well as the relevance of traditional gender difference computerized techniques that allow a very large sam-
research for this population. ple of texts to be analyzed virtually instantaneously,
There remains the possibility that the people linguistic approaches offer the possibility of a fresh
who placed the ads we studied were not represen- perspective on long-standing questions of individ-
tative. In particular, gay advertisers on Match.com ual difference. Text analysis programs operate with-
may be qualitatively different from people who post out regard to traditional theories about a particu-
on gay-oriented websites, or who seek mates in more lar domain. In the case of the current study, this
face-to-face gay community contexts. There is evi- naturalistic, behavioral, multivariate index paints a
dence that ads in specifically gay newspapers differ startling, yet coherent picture of gender and sexual
from those in more mainstream personals outlets. orientation.
For example, advertisers in gay newspapers are less
likely to talk about being/seeking a “straight acting”
guy (Phua, 2002). Even if the advertisements we stud- ACKNOWLEDGMENT
ied were technically representative, it may well be
the case that previous researchers who recruited par- Preparation of this paper was aided by a grant
ticipants at gay pride events or asked people to come from the National Institutes of Health (MH52391).
into a psychology lab were studying a different, less
closeted, more experienced, and highly-identified
REFERENCES
members of the gay community, which may explain
the conflicting findings. It would also be helpful
Bailey, J. M., Kim, P. Y., Hills, A., & Linsenmeier, J. A. W. (1997).
to compare the writing styles of people who rate Butch, femme, or straight acting? Partner preferences of gay
themselves as relatively gender-typical or atypical. men and lesbians. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
It might also be argued that people’s writing ogy, 73, 960–973.
Bailey, J. M., Nothnagel, J., & Wolfe, M. (1995). Retrospectively
style in personal ads is not a perfect indicator of measured individual differences in childhood sex-typed be-
personality because of the high self-presentational havior among gay men: Correspondence between self- and
concerns involved and the possibility that advertisers maternal reports. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 24, 613–622.
Bailey, J. M., & Zucker, K. J. (1995). Childhood sex-typed behav-
adjust their language to complement or match the ior and sexual orientation: A conceptual analysis and quanti-
perceived style of readers. Previous studies have tative review. Developmental Psychology, 31, 43–55.
indeed shown that, for example, women tend to offer Bem, D. J. (1996). Exotic becomes erotic: A developmental theory
of sexual orientation. Psychological Review, 103, 320–335.
instrumental traits and men tend to offer expressive Brownlow, S., Rosamond, J. A., & Parker, J. A. (2003). Gender
traits, in direct contrast to the more general gender differences in linguistic behavior during televised interviews.
differences found in instrumental and expressive Sex Roles, 49, 121–131.
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences:
characteristics (e.g., Cicerello & Sheehan, 1995). Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and
However, the close correspondence between the Brain Sciences, 12, 1–49.
gender differences observed in the present study and Cicerello, A., & Sheehan, E. P. (1995). Personal advertisements: A
content analysis. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality,
those reported across a wide range of contexts in 10, 751–756.
previous work testify to the representative quality Collear, M. L., & Hines, M. (1995). Human behavioral sex dif-
of personal ads. Previous studies have often been ferences: A role for gonadal hormones during early develop-
ment? Psychological Bulletin, 118, 55–107.
skewed toward dimensions with strong gender or Deaux, K., & Randel, H. (1984). Courtship in the personals col-
orientation stereotypic differences, such as interest umn: The influence of gender and sexual orientation. Sex
in fashion or childhood play with dolls. In contrast, Roles, 11, 363–375.
Eagly, A. H. (1995). The science and politics of comparing women
in the present study we took full advantage of new and men. American Psychologist, 50, 145–158.
text analysis technology to obtain a broad snapshot Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences
of linguistic behavior from self-reference habits to in human behavior: Evolved dispositions versus social roles.
American Psychologist, 54, 408–423.
interest in psychological characteristics. From this Eagly, A. H., Wood, W., & Diekman, A. (2000). Social role the-
broad snapshot, sizeable and systematic differences ory of sex differences and similarities: A current appraisal. In
did emerge, but they point strongly toward an in- T. Eckes & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social
psychology of gender (pp. 123–174). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
fluence of gender and of orientation and away from Epel, E. S., Spanakos, A., Kasl-Godley, J., & Brownell, K. D.
notions that gay men “are like” straight women, and (1996). Body shape ideals across gender, sexual orientation,
lesbians men “are like” straight men. socioeconomic status, race and age in personal advertise-
ments. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 19, 265–273.
The study of language as a marker of psycholog- Freud, S. (1973/1905). Three essays on the theory of sexuality.
ical state is a relatively new research strategy. With London: Pelican.
Language Use in Personals 461

Gonzales, M. H., & Meyers, S. A. (1993). “Your mother would Newman, M. L., Pennebaker, J. W., Berry, D. S., & Richards,
like me”: Self-presentation in the personals ads of heterosex- J. M. (2003). Lying words: Predicting deception from linguis-
ual and homosexual men and women. Personality and Social tic styles. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 665–
Psychology Bulletin, 19, 131–142. 675.
Goode, E. (1996) Gender and courtship entitlement: Responses to Parekh, R., & Bersesin, E. V. (2001). Looking for Love? Take
personal ads. Sex Roles, 34, 141–169. a cross-cultural walk through the personals. Academic Psy-
Groom, C. J., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2002). Words. Journal of chiatry, 25, 223–233.
Research in Personality, 36, 615–621. Pennebaker, J. W., Francis, M. E., & Booth, R. J. (2001). Linguistic
Hatala, M. N., & Prehodka, J. (1996). Content analysis of gay male inquiry and word count: LIWC 2001. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
and lesbian personal advertisements. Psychological Reports, Pennebaker, J. W., & King, L. A. (1999). Linguistic styles:
78, 371–374. Language use as an individual difference. Journal of Person-
Jason, L. A., Moritsugu, J. N., & DePalma, D. M. (1992). Adver- ality and Social Psychology, 77, 1296–1312.
tising as a strategy for meeting people. Psychological Reports, Pennebaker, J. W., Mehl, M. R., & Niederhoffer, K. (2003). Psy-
71, 1311–1314. chological aspects of natural language use: Our words, our
Koppel, M., Argamon, S., & Shimon, A. R. (2002). Automati- selves. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 547–577.
cally categorizing written texts by author gender. Literary and Pennebaker, J. W., & Stone, L. D. (2003). Words of wisdom:
Linguistic Computing, 17, 401–412. Language use over the lifespan. Journal of Personality and
Laner, M. R., & Kamel, G. L. (1977). Media mating I: Newspaper Social Psychology, 85, 291–301.
“personals” ads of homosexual men. Journal of Homosexual- Phua, V. C. (2002). Sex and sexuality in men’s personal advertise-
ity, 3, 149–162. ments. Men and Masculinities, 5, 178–191.
Lindesay, J. (1987). Laterality shift in homosexual men. Neuropsy- Pillard, R. C. (1991). Masculinity and femininity in homosexual-
chologia, 25, 965–969. ity: “Inversion” revisited. In J. C. Gonsiorek & J. D. Weinrich
McFadden, D., & Pasanen, E. G. (1999). Spontaneous otoacous- (Eds.), Homosexuality: Research implications for public pol-
tic emissions in heterosexuals, homosexuals, and bisexuals. icy (pp. 32–43). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 105, 2403– Ridgeway, C. L., & Smith-Lovin, L. (1999). The gender system and
2413. interaction. Annual Review of Sociology, 25, 191–216.
Merskin, D., & Huberlie, M. (1996). Companionship in the classi- Solomon, R. C. (1978). A pronoun is a small world. Chicago
fieds: The adoption of personal advertisements by the daily Review, 29, 9–22.
press. Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, 73, Stirman, S. W., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2001). Word use in the
219–229. poetry of suicidal and non-suicidal poets. Psychosomatic
Money, J. (1970). Sexual dimorphism and homosexual gender Medicine, 63, 517–522.
identity. Psychological Bulletin, 74, 425–440. Swim, J. K. (1994). Perceived versus meta-analytic effect sizes: An
Mustanski, B. S., Bailey, J. M., & Kaspar, S. (2002). Dermato- assessment of the accuracy of gender stereotypes. Journal of
glyphics, handedness, sex, and sexual orientation. Archives of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 21–36.
Sexual Behavior, 31, 113–122. Tannen, D. (1990). You just don’t understand: Women and men in
Newman, M. L., Groom, C. J., Stone, L. D., & Pennebaker, conversation. New York: Ballantine Books.
J. W. (2005). Sex differences in language use: An analysis of West, C., & Zimmerman, D. H. (1991). Doing gender. In J. Lorber
text samples from 70 studies [University of Texas at Austin, & S. A. Farrell (Eds.), The social construction of gender
Austin, TX]. Manuscript under review. (pp. 13–36). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

You might also like