You are on page 1of 6

Body Image 10 (2013) 509–514

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Body Image
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bodyimage

The impact of thin idealized media images on body satisfaction: Does


body appreciation protect women from negative effects?
Emma Halliwell ∗
Centre for Appearance Research, Department of Psychology, University of the West of England, Frenchay, Coldharbour Lane, Bristol, BS16 9QY, UK

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This article examines whether positive body image can protect women from negative media exposure
Received 13 December 2012 effects. University women (N = 112) were randomly allocated to view advertisements featuring ultra-thin
Received in revised form 16 July 2013 models or control images. Women who reported high levels of body appreciation did not report negative
Accepted 17 July 2013
media exposure effects. Furthermore, the protective role of body appreciation was also evident among
women known to be vulnerable to media exposure. Women high on thin-ideal internalization and low
Keywords:
on body appreciation reported appearance-discrepancies that were more salient and larger when they
Positive body image
viewed models compared to the control group. However, women high on thin-ideal internalization and
Body appreciation
Media
also high on body appreciation rated appearance-discrepancies as less important and no difference in
Body dissatisfaction size than the control group. The results support the notion that positive body image protects women
Women from negative environmental appearance messages and suggests that promoting positive body image
Experimental may be an effective intervention strategy.
© 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Introduction positive body image avoid influences that have a potentially detri-
mental impact. Consistent with this assertion, there is correlational
Recently, body image researchers have paid increasing atten- evidence that body appreciation is associated with low levels of
tion to the experience of positive body image. This attention has consumption of appearance focused media (Swami, Hadji-Michael,
been prompted by a desire to expand our understanding of body & Furnham, 2008) and is negatively correlated with internalization
image beyond the prevention of distress and to foster the promo- of sociocultural ideals (Tylka & Kroon Van Diest, 2013). Moreover,
tion of body appreciation (Tylka, 2011). As a consequence, there qualitative evidence suggests that adolescent girls with positive
has been a flurry of research activity around positive body image. body image are highly critical of unrealistic appearance ideals in
Positive body image is conceptualized as an overarching way of the media (Holmqvist & Frisén, 2012). However, to date, no experi-
experiencing the body that encompasses love and respect (Wood- mental research has examined the claim that women with positive
Barcalow, Tylka & Augustus-Horvath, 2010), and this construct body image are protected from negative environmental messages
has been operationalized in research as body appreciation (Avalos, around appearance.
Tylka, & Wood-Barcalow, 2005). One of the most potent sources of sociocultural messages about
The Body Appreciation Scale (Avalos et al., 2005) taps into four appearance is the media. Meta-analyses confirm that exposure to
essential qualities of positive body image: holding positive eval- thin bodies idealized in the media has a small to moderate neg-
uations of the body, body acceptance, respecting and attending ative effect on body- and weight-dissatisfaction, negative affect,
to bodily needs, and protecting the body by rejecting unrealistic internalization of the thin-ideal, and eating behavior among ado-
appearance ideals. Body appreciation is not simply the experience lescent girls and young adult women (Grabe, Ward, & Hyde, 2008;
of body satisfaction, rather it is a way of valuing one’s body and Groesz, Levine, & Murnen, 2002; Want, 2009). In addition, indi-
orientating cognitive processing to protect and promote a positive vidual studies suggest that this negative exposure effect is also
view of the body. The conceptual distinction between body appre- experienced by women in later adulthood, and by girls as young
ciation and body dissatisfaction has been supported empirically as five (Dittmar, Halliwell, & Ive, 2006; Dittmar & Howard, 2004;
(Avalos et al., 2005). Positive body image incorporates a protec- Halliwell & Dittmar, 2004). A number of individual difference fac-
tive processing style; therefore, it is expected that women with tors have been shown to moderate these media exposure effects.
Two meta-analyses have found that women who report body sat-
isfaction and who have not internalized the thin beauty ideal are
∗ Tel.: +44 (0)117 3282154. protected from media exposure effects (Groesz et al., 2002; Want,
E-mail address: emma.halliwell@uwe.ac.uk 2009). Although body appreciation is related to body satisfaction

1740-1445/$ – see front matter © 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2013.07.004
510 E. Halliwell / Body Image 10 (2013) 509–514

and thin-ideal internalization, it is a conceptually and empirically participants (87%) were White, their mean age was 20.04 years
distinct construct (Tylka, 2011). Therefore, it represents an addi- (SD = 2.61, range = 18–39), and their mean BMI was 22.33 (SD = 2.99,
tional potential protective factor against negative media exposure range = 14.90–34.80).
effects.
As well as moderating media exposure effects, thin-ideal inter- Advertising Images
nalization is a key risk factor for the development of negative body
image and disordered eating (Stice, Ng, & Shaw 2010; Thompson & There were five print advertisements in each condition. The
Stice, 2001). Although all women will be aware that cultural ideals adverts in the model condition were selected from popular
of attractiveness value thinness for women, not all women buy into women’s magazines and showed full body shots of thin mod-
this dominant ideal. Thin-ideal internalization refers to the extent els advertising perfume, clothes, and a soft drink. In the control
that someone incorporates sociocultural ideals for women’s beauty condition, product-only advertisements for makeup, perfume, and
into their own personal ideals and values (Thompson, Heinberg, handbags were selected from popular women’s magazines.
Altabe, & Tantleff-Dunn, 1999). In prospective studies, thin-ideal
internalization predicts negative body image and disordered eat-
Measures
ing, yet not all women who internalize the thin-ideal develop eating
disorders (Stice et al., 2010). Women who buy into sociocultural
Positive body image. The Body Appreciation Scale (Avalos
ideals of attractiveness but who also accept their bodies despite
et al., 2005) was used to assess positive body image. Participants
not matching this ideal and view their bodies with respect and
responded to 13 items on a 5-point scale ranging from never (1) to
love maybe less likely to experience the negative consequences of
always (5). The scale has a unidimensional structure and demon-
thin-ideal internalization. Research has not yet examined the inter-
strated evidence of reliability (internal consistency and 3-week
action between body appreciation and known risk factors for body
test–retest) and construct validity for women (Avalos et al., 2005).
dissatisfaction and eating disorders.
Cronbach’s alpha for the current sample was .92.
The current study examines whether body appreciation protects
women from negative media exposure effects. In addition, it exa-
mines the interaction between positive body image and thin-ideal Thin-ideal internalization. The internalization subscale of
internalization. Women who are high on thin-ideal internalization the Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire
are known to be vulnerable to negative exposure effects. One key (SATAQ-3: Thompson, van den Berg, Roehrig, Guarda, & Heinberg,
feature of body appreciation is processing sociocultural messages 2004) was administered. Respondents indicated their agreement
about appearance in a self-protective way. Therefore, high levels of with nine statements on a 5-point Likert-type scale, ranging from
body appreciation may change the way women process images of definitely disagree (1) to definitely agree (5). Construct validity and
idealized female models and may protect women who internalize internal consistency reliability of this measure have been demon-
the thin-ideal from experiencing negative effects of viewing media strated for women (Thompson et al., 2004). Cronbach’s alpha for
images. the current sample was .90.
Consistent with previous research findings, it is hypothesized
that thin-ideal internalization will increase women’s vulnerability State body image. State body image was measured unobtru-
to media exposure. Specifically, women who are high on thin-ideal sively through ratings of appearance self-discrepancies. The state
internalization will report more negative state body image after version of the Self-Discrepancy Index (state-SDI; Dittmar, Beattie,
viewing models than control images. For women low on thin-ideal & Friese, 1996; Halliwell & Dittmar, 2006) was used. The measure
internalization, there will be no difference between conditions on asks participants to complete five sentences of the format, “I. . ..
outcome variables. but I would like. . .,” describing aspects of themselves that they
It is hypothesized that body appreciation will protect women would ideally like to change right now. They then rate each self-
from media exposure effects. For women with high levels of body discrepancy statement in terms of magnitude, ranging from a little
appreciation, there will be no difference in measures of state body different (1) to extremely different (6) and salience, ranging from a
image after viewing images of idealized models or control images. little important (1) to extremely important (6).
In contrast, women low on body appreciation will report more Self-discrepancies are then coded into a category of interest (e.g.,
negative state body image after viewing model images than after appearance-related self-discrepancies). The extent and salience of
control images. self-discrepancies were measured separately because they can be
Furthermore, it is hypothesized that the protective nature of independent of each other. For instance, there may be a large
body appreciation will also be evident among women who inter- discrepancy between a person wanting to be “thin” rather than
nalize the thin-ideal. For women high on thin-ideal internalization “overweight”, but it may not be important to them. Therefore, for
and also high on body appreciation, there will be no difference each appearance-discrepancy two scores were calculated. The size
between state body image after viewing models or control images. of the state appearance-discrepancies is calculated from the sum of
Yet, women high on thin-ideal internalization but low on body all the size ratings assigned to discrepancies coded as appearance-
appreciation will report more negative state body image after view- related. The salience of appearance-discrepancies is the sum of the
ing models than control images. importance rating assigned to discrepancies coded as appearance-
related. The measure is additive because respondents had the
opportunity to write about anything concerning themselves, list-
Method ing more appearance-related aspects is an indication of larger and
more salient discrepancies. The possible range of both the size and
Participants salience rating are 0–30. Zero means the person did not list any self-
discrepancies relating to appearance. Thirty means that all five of
Female psychology students (N = 130) enrolled in a large uni- their listed self-discrepancies were related to appearance and had
versity in the United Kingdom were recruited to take part in a magnitude or importance ratings of 6.
study on “Attitudes to Advertising” in return for course credits. The self-statements were coded as appearance-related discrep-
Complete data were obtained for 112 women; 49 women in the ancies if they explicitly referred to appearance (e.g., my weight, my
control condition and 63 in the model condition. The majority of boobs), as opposed to other aspects of the self (e.g., my finances,
E. Halliwell / Body Image 10 (2013) 509–514 511

Table 1
Pre-exposure BMI, age, thin-ideal internalization, and body appreciation by condition.

Measure Model, M (SD) Control, M (SD) t-value df p-value

BMI 22.03 (2.92) 22.40 (3.48) 0.60 107 .55


Age 19.94 (1.62) 20.18 (3.46) 0.78 106 .44
Thin-ideal internalization 3.36 (0.73) 3.13 (0.72) −1.76 113 .08
Body appreciation 3.17 (0.67) 3.24 (0.70) 0.64 106 .64

Note: N = 112; n model condition = 63, n control condition = 49.

my love life). Two coders independently coded the data, and there part of a separate study to eliminate demand characteristics. How-
was 100% agreement between the coders. ever, the naivety check in the present research confirmed that the
The SDI has good convergent validity with other measures of use of an unobtrusive measure of appearance concerns combined
women’s and men’s body image and criterion-related validity with with the post-only design did not set up demand characteristics.
eating behaviors (Halliwell & Dittmar, 2006). The state-SDI is also
appropriate for assessing media exposure effects among under-
Results
graduate women and men (e.g., Dittmar & Halliwell, 2007; Dittmar,
Halliwell, & Stirling, 2009).
t-tests, reported in Table 1, revealed that there was no significant
difference in BMI, age, thin-ideal internalization, or body apprecia-
Advertising evaluation. To support the cover story, women tion between women assigned to each condition, assuring that the
were asked to rate their impression of the advert, their impression randomization was effective in creating equal groups. Body appre-
of the brand, and how much they liked the advert. ciation and thin-ideal internalization were moderately correlated,
r = −.36, p < .001, supporting the notion that they are independent
Procedure factors. High and low groups on thin-ideal internalization and body
appreciation were created using a median split on each variable,
The study was approved by the University’s Ethics Committee. It the median for thin-ideal internalization was 3.5 and the median
was advertised as a study investigating women’s attitudes towards for body appreciation was 3.27.
advertising and personality factors influencing adverting effective-
ness. On agreeing to take part, women were randomly allocated to Appearance-discrepancy Size
a condition via a computer generated randomization table. Partic-
ipants were then sent a link to an online questionnaire in which A 2 (condition) × 2 (level of thin-ideal internalization) × 2 (level
they were asked to complete at their convenience. They were told of body appreciation) ANOVA was used to examine whether
that personality, mood, and thoughts about oneself influence pre- thin-ideal internalization and body appreciation, independently
ferences for advertising and that the online questionnaire included or in combination, moderated the impact of media exposure on
questions relating to these factors. They completed measures of appearance-discrepancy size. There was no statistically significant
body appreciation and thin-ideal internalization as well as filler main effect of media exposure on appearance-discrepancy size,
items about personality and consumer attitudes to disguise the F(1, 104) = 0.98, partial 2 = .01, p = .33. Surprisingly, neither level
true focus of the study. Women also reported their age, height, of thin-ideal internalization, F(1, 104) = 0.81, partial 2 = .01, p = .37,
weight and ethnicity. One week later participants attended a lab nor level of body appreciation, F(1, 104) = 1.43, partial 2 = .01,
session. Initially, 65 women were randomly assigned to each condi- p = .23, had a significant main effect on appearance-discrepancy
tion. However, 18 of these women did not attend the experimental size.
session due to illness, transport problems, and an unplanned uni- Consistent with previous research, there was a significant
versity closure which reduced the sample size to 112. Women were interaction between exposure and thin-ideal internalization, F(1,
tested individually. They rated the advertisements at their own 104) = 3.93, partial 2 = .04, p = .05. Among women low on thin-
pace and then completed the appearance-discrepancy measure. At ideal internalization, there was no significant difference in the
the end of the experiment, women were asked to state the pur- size of appearance-discrepancies reported in the control condition
pose of the study in their own words in order to check that they (M = 5.97, SD = 4.30) and the model condition (M = 5.48, SD = 3.82),
had believed the cover story. All of the women were naïve to the F(1, 52) = .49, partial 2 = .01, p = .49, d = 0.12. However, women
actual aims of the research. The participants were thanked and, high on thin-ideal internalization reported larger appearance-
when data collection was complete, they were emailed a written discrepancies if they had viewed models (M = 8.06, SD = 4.42)
debrief describing the true aims of the study. They were also given compared to those who had viewed control images (M = 5.50,
the opportunity to retract their data from the study. None of the SD = 3.33), F(1, 52) = 4.40, partial 2 = .08, p = .04, d = −0.65.
participants asked for their responses to be removed. Body appreciation did not moderate the effect of media expo-
A post-test only design was chosen to avoid setting up demand sure, as there was no significant interaction between media
characteristics. Some researchers have successfully used Visual exposure and level of body appreciation, F(1, 104) = 1.37, partial
Analogue Scales (VAS) in pre- and post-test designs (e.g., Birkeland 2 = .01, p = .24. The interaction effect between thin-ideal inter-
et al., 2005), and VAS scales are well suited to repeated measure- nalization and body appreciation approached significance, F(1,
ment designs. However, when using more detailed measures of 104) = 3.72, partial 2 = .04, p = .06. Central to the current research
body image, repeated measurement is likely to make the aims of question, the 3-way interaction between condition, level of thin-
the study apparent e.g., participants may be able to remember, and ideal internalization and level of body appreciation was significant,
reproduce, their pre-test responses or shape their answers accord- F(1, 104) = 11.66, partial 2 = .10, p = .001. This finding suggests that
ing to what they think the experimenter wants to find (Thompson, the moderating role of body appreciation on media exposure differs
2004). Thus, when participants suspect the true purpose of media for women depending on their level of thin-ideal internalization.
exposure studies, the identification of adverse effects is more likely To interrogate the 3-way interaction, analysis was run
(Mills, Polivy, Herman, & Tiggemann, 2002). Mills et al. (2002) sug- separately for women according to their level of thin-ideal inter-
gested that, ideally, body image measures should be presented as nalization. The means and SDs for appearance-discrepancy size are
512 E. Halliwell / Body Image 10 (2013) 509–514

Table 2 12
Mean appearance-discrepancy size and salience scores in each condition by level of
thin-ideal internalization and level of body appreciation.
10
Model, M (SD) Control, M (SD)

Appearance-discrepancy size 8
High TII
High BA 6.29 (3.79) 7.50 (3.34) 6 Low body appreciaon
Low BA 9.18 (4.51) 3.50 (1.84)
High body appreciaon
Low TII
High BA 5.24 (4.12) 4.29 (3.47) 4
Low BA 5.90 (3.41) 8.33 (4.35)
Appearance-discrepancy salience 2
High TII
High BA 6.29 (3.14) 9.10 (3.18)
0
Low BA 10.05 (4.78) 4.20 (1.87)
Low TII Model Control
High BA 4.65 (4.05) 4.88 (4.33)
Low BA 6.60 (4.43) 7.08 (4.03) Fig. 2. Appearance-discrepancy salience scores for women high in thin-ideal inter-
nalization by condition and level of body appreciation.
Note: TII, thin-ideal internalization; BA, body appreciation.

reported in Table 2. In the low thin-ideal internalization group, internalization, F(1, 104) = 4.21, partial 2 = .04, p = .04, but there
there was no interaction between media exposure and level of was no main effect of body appreciation, F(1, 104) = 0.93, partial
body appreciation, F(1, 52) = 2.53, partial 2 = .05, p = .12. In the 2 = .01, p = .34. In this case, there was no significant interaction
high thin-ideal internalization group, the interaction between between exposure and thin-ideal internalization, F(1, 104) = 1.44,
media exposure and level of body appreciation was significant, partial 2 = .01, p = .23. Instead, there was a significant interac-
F(1, 52) = 10.47, partial 2 = .17, p = .01. This interaction is displayed tion between media exposure and level of body appreciation,
in Fig. 1. Women who were high on thin-ideal internalization F(1, 104) = 7.23, partial 2 = .07, p = .01. For women high on
and low on body appreciation who viewed models reported body appreciation, there was no significant difference in the
larger appearance-discrepancies compared to women who viewed salience of appearance-discrepancies reported by women who
control images, F(1, 22) = 14.55, p = .001, partial 2 = .33, d = 1.65. had viewed models (M = 5.39, SD = 3.70) and women who had
However, there was no significant media exposure effect among viewed control images (M = 6.44, SD = 4.40), F(1, 56) = 0.99, par-
women high on thin-ideal internalization and high on body appre- tial 2 = .02, p = .32, d = 0.26. In contrast, women who were low
ciation, F(1, 22) = 0.66, p = .42, partial 2 = .03, d = −0.34. on body appreciation and viewed models reported more salient
In sum, these findings indicate that the size of appearance- appearance-discrepancies (M = 8.97, SD = 4.88) than women low on
discrepancies was only impacted by media exposure for some body appreciation who viewed control images (M = 5.77, SD = 3.49),
women. Specifically, the size of appearance-discrepancies was not F(1, 52) = 6.98, partial 2 = .12, p = .01, d = −0.75.
impacted by media exposure for women who are low on thin-ideal The interaction effect between level of thin-ideal internalization
internalization or for women who are high on thin-ideal internal- and level of body appreciation was not significant, F(1, 104) = 2.86,
ization but are also high on body appreciation. However, viewing partial 2 = .03, p = .09. Similar to the findings with appearance-
models was associated with larger appearance-discrepancies when discrepancy size, the 3-way interaction between condition, level
women were high on thin-ideal internalization but low on body of thin-ideal internalization, and level of body appreciation was
appreciation. significant, F(1, 104) = 8.11, partial 2 = .07, p = .001.
Analysis was run separately for women according to their level
Appearance-discrepancy Salience of thin-ideal internalization. The means and SDs for appearance-
discrepancy salience are reported in Table 2. The interaction
The same analysis was run on appearance-discrepancy salience. between media exposure and level of body appreciation was not
Again, there was no statistically significant main effect of media significant among women low on thin-ideal internalization, F(1,
exposure on appearance-discrepancy salience, F(1, 104) = 0.55, 52) = 0.01, partial 2 = .00, p = .92. However, the interaction was
partial 2 = .01, p = .46. There was a main effect of thin-ideal significant among women high on thin-ideal internalization, F(1,
52) = 16.85, partial 2 = .25, p = .001. This interaction is shown in
Fig. 2. Women high on thin-ideal internalization but low on body
12
appreciation reported more salient appearance-discrepancies in
the model condition than in the control condition, F(1, 30) = 13.80,
10
partial 2 = .32, p = .001, d = 1.61. Interestingly, women high on thin-
ideal internalization and also high on body appreciation reported
8
less salient appearance-discrepancies when they viewed models
than when they viewed control images, F(1, 22) = 4.63, partial
6 Low body appreciaon
2 = .17, p = .04, d = −0.89.
High body appreciaon
These findings indicate that viewing media models, compared
4
to control images, did not significantly impact on the salience of
appearance-discrepancies among women low on thin-ideal inter-
2
nalization. The combination of high thin-ideal internalization and
low body appreciation was associated with negative media expo-
0
sure effects because viewing models was associated with more
Model Control
salient appearance-discrepancies than viewing control images. In
Fig. 1. Appearance-discrepancy size scores for women high in thin-ideal internal- contrast, when high thin-ideal internalization was coupled with
ization by condition and level of body appreciation. high body appreciation viewing models was associated with less
E. Halliwell / Body Image 10 (2013) 509–514 513

salient appearance-discrepancies compared to viewing control actively downplay the importance of appearance relative to func-
images. tional aspects of their bodies or other aspects of their self-identity.
It would be informative for future research to examine salience rat-
ing for other self-domains. In addition, the lack of exposure effects
Discussion on appearance-discrepancy size for women high in both thin-ideal
internalization and body appreciation suggests that these women
This study provides preliminary evidence to suggest that are avoiding, or undoing, evaluative appearance comparisons with
body appreciation protects women from negative media exposure media models. It would also be informative to develop methods to
effects. Moreover, this protection is evident among women who directly assess women’s processing of ultra-thin media images.
internalize the thin-ideal and are known to be vulnerable to body
image disturbance. Limitations
The findings replicate previous research demonstrating that
women who strongly internalize the thin-ideal report larger The sample size of the current study was relatively small and
appearance-discrepancies after viewing thin models than control consisted of mainly White, young, undergraduate women. Clearly,
images. Interestingly, exposure to media models did not impact the these results also need replication with a larger more diverse sam-
salience of appearance-discrepancies among this group of women. ple. In the current study, different products were used in the control
This suggests that women who internalize the thin-ideal feel more condition and the model condition. Only women in the control con-
dissatisfied with their appearance after viewing models, but that dition saw advertisements for make-up and a handbag and only
they do not place increased importance on this appearance dissat- women in the model condition viewed advertisements for clothes
isfaction. and a soft drink. It is possible that the products may have had an
The findings relating to the moderating role of body apprecia- impact on self-discrepancies. This is less concerning because pre-
tion are novel and incremental to the body image literature. For vious research has demonstrated that while women’s body image
women low on body appreciation, viewing models, compared to is strongly affected by the presence of models in advertising, it
viewing control images, did not impact on the size of appearance- is not affected by viewing appearance-related products compared
discrepancies but it did make appearance-discrepancies more to household-related products (Birkeland et al., 2005). However,
salient. Interestingly, body appreciation also moderated the effects future research should match the products shown in each condi-
of media exposure among thin-ideal internalizing women. Pre- tion.
vious research has demonstrated, and this study confirms, that
women who internalize the thin-ideal are vulnerable to negative Implications for Clinical Practice
exposure effects. When women were high on thin-ideal internaliza-
tion and low on body appreciation, viewing models was associated Research has established a number of individual differences
with larger and more salient appearance-discrepancies compared variables that make women vulnerable to negative societal mes-
to the control group. However, when women high on thin-ideal sages around appearance. The current findings add support to
internalization were also high on body appreciation, the negative the theory that positive body image protects women from exter-
exposure effects on appearance-discrepancy size disappeared and nal appearance pressures. There is evidence that media literacy
on appearance-discrepancy salience were reversed. This group of interventions that give women and girls information about the
women reported less salient appearance-discrepancies after view- artificial nature of media images can prevent negative media expo-
ing models compared to the control group. This suggests that sure effects (Halliwell, Easun, & Harcourt, 2011; Posavac, Posavac, &
women who buy into the thin-ideal but who also appreciate their Weigel, 2001; Yamamiya, Cash, Melnyk, Posavac, & Posavac, 2005).
bodies downplay the importance of appearance-discrepancies after The results of this study suggest that promoting body acceptance,
viewing images of media models. a central aspect of body appreciation, should also protect women
The inclusion of measures assessing both the size and the from negative societal messages about appearance.
salience of appearance dissatisfaction has proved useful in this In support of this assertion, body acceptance activities have
research. Findings relating to the extent of appearance dissatis- been identified as an effective component of interventions for body
faction, as assessed through the size of appearance-discrepancies, dissatisfaction and disordered eating (Stice, Shaw, & Marti, 2007).
replicate findings of previous research on media exposure. The Furthermore, a body image intervention incorporating body accep-
moderating impact of internalization was only seen on this mea- tance has been shown to increase girls’ resilience to thin-idealized
sure of state body image. In contrast, body appreciation moderated media (Halliwell & Diedrichs, 2013). To date, body image inter-
the impact of media exposure on the importance women place on ventions have been designed to target negative body image, and
appearance-discrepancies. The results suggest that when women their effectiveness is measured through their impact on nega-
low on body appreciation view ultra-thin models they place greater tive body image. However, it may be that existing programs are
importance on their appearance dissatisfaction. The role of body also promoting positive body image and that this is strengthen-
appreciation is particularly interesting when it is considered in ing their impact. It would be useful to include measures of body
combination with thin-ideal internalization, and it is striking that appreciation in intervention evaluations to assess this question. In
even when women buy into dominant appearance ideals, body addition, the growing evidence that positive body image is central
appreciation seems to protect them from exposure to these ideals. to psychological health suggests that interventions should specif-
The findings are consistent with the conceptualization of posi- ically target body appreciation. Therapeutic techniques such as
tive body image (i.e., body appreciation) as guiding self-protective mindfulness that focus on self-acceptance and promoting psycho-
cognitive processing of media images to minimize the impact of logical well-being more broadly have been tailored to promote
environmental messages about appearance. Indeed, women who body appreciation (Stewart, 2004). Mindfulness interventions have
internalize the thin-ideal but who also appreciate their bodies place not yet been evaluated in relation to positive body image. There
less importance on appearance-discrepancies after viewing models is emerging evidence that mindfulness can reduce negative body
than control images. This may provide insight into one of the pro- image and problematic eating behaviors (e.g., Alberts, Thewissen,
tective psychological mechanisms used by women with positive & Raes, 2012). Future research should address whether mindful-
body image. When confronted with idealized and artificial media ness techniques present a useful addition to existing body image
images of female beauty, women with positive body image may interventions.
514 E. Halliwell / Body Image 10 (2013) 509–514

References Holmqvist, K., & Frisén, A. (2012). I bet they aren’t that perfect in reality: Appearance
ideals viewed from the perspective of adolescents with a positive body image.
Alberts, H. J. E. M., Thewissen, R., & Raes, L. (2012). Dealing with problematic eating Body Image, 9, 388–395.
behavior. The effects of a mindfulness-based intervention on eating behavior, Mills, J. S., Polivy, J., Herman, P., & Tiggemann, M. (2002). Effects of exposure to thin
food, cravings, dichotomous thinking and body image concerns. Appetite, 58, media images: Evidence of self-enhancement among restrained eaters. Person-
847–851. ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1687–1699.
Avalos, L., Tylka, T. L., & Wood-Barcalow, N. L. (2005). The Body Appreciation Scale: Posavac, H. D., Posavac, S. S., & Weigel, R. G. (2001). Reducing the impact of media
Development and psychometric evaluation. Body Image, 2, 285–297. images on women at risk for body image disturbance: Three targeted interven-
Birkeland, R., Thompson, J. K., Herbozo, S., Roehrig, M., Cafri, G., & van den Berg, P. tions. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 20, 324–340.
(2005). Media exposure, mood, and body image dissatisfaction: An experimental Stice, E., Ng, J., & Shaw, H. (2010). Risk factors and prodromal eating pathology.
test of person versus product priming. Body Image, 2, 53–61. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 418–525.
Dittmar, H., Beattie, J., & Friese, S. (1996). Objects, decision considerations and self- Stice, E., Shaw, H., & Marti, C. N. (2007). A meta-analytic review of eating disorder pre-
image in men’s and women’s impulse purchases. Acta Psychologica, 93, 187–206. vention programs: Encouraging findings. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 3,
Dittmar, H., & Halliwell, E. (2007). Think ideal and feel bad? Using self-discrepancies 207–231.
to understand negative media effects. In H. Dittmar (Ed.), Consumer culture, Stewart, T. (2004). Light on body image treatment: Acceptance through mindfulness.
identity, and well-being (pp. 147–172). London: Psychology Press. Behavior Modificiation, 28, 783–811.
Dittmar, H., Halliwell, E., & Ive, S. (2006). Does Barbie make girls want to be thin? Swami, V., Hadji-Michael, M., & Furnham, A. (2008). Personality and individual dif-
Developmental Psychology, 42, 283–292. ference correlates of positive body image. Body Image, 5, 322–325.
Dittmar, H., Halliwell, E., & Stirling, E. (2009). Understanding the impact of thin media Thompson, J. K. (2004). The (mis)measurement of body image. Body Image, 1, 7–14.
models on women’s body-focused affect: The roles of thin-ideal internaliza- Thompson, J. K., Heinberg, L. J., Altabe, M. N., & Tantleff-Dunn, S. (1999). Exacting
tion and weight-related self-discrepancy activation in experimental exposure beauty: Theory, assessment and treatment of body image disturbance. Washington,
effects. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28, 43–72. DC: American Psychological Association.
Dittmar, H., & Howard, S. (2004). Professional hazards? The impact of models’ body Thompson, J. K., & Stice, E. (2001). Thin-ideal internalization: Mounting evidence
size on advertising effectiveness and women’s body-focused anxiety in profes- for a new risk factor for body-image disturbance and eating pathology. Current
sions that do and do not emphasises the cultural ideal of thinness. British Journal Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 181–183.
of Social Psychology, 43, 477–497. Thompson, J. K., van den Berg, P., Roehrig, M., Guarda, A. S., & Heinberg, L.
Grabe, S., Ward, L. M., & Hyde, J. S. (2008). The role of the media in body image con- J. (2004). The Sociocultural Attitudes Toward Appearance Scale-3 (SATAQ-
cerns among women: A meta-analysis of experimental and correlational studies. 3): Development and validation. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 35,
Psychological Bulletin, 134, 460–476. 293–304.
Groesz, L. M., Levine, M. P., & Murnen, S. K. (2002). The effect of experimental pre- Tylka, T. L. (2011). Positive psychology perspectives on body image. In T. F. Cash &
sentation of thin media images on body satisfaction: A meta-analytic review. L. Smolak (Eds.), Body image: A handbook of science, practice, and prevention (2nd
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 31, 1–16. ed., pp. 56–64). New York: Guilford Press.
Halliwell, E., & Diedrichs, P. C. (2013, May 13). Brief Report: Testing a dissonance Tylka, T. L., & Kroon Van Diest, A. M. (2013). The Intuitive Eating Scale-2: Item refine-
body image intervention among young girls. Health Psychology. Advance online ment and psychometric evaluation with college women and men. Journal of
publication. doi: 10.1037/a0032585. Counseling Psychology, 60, 137–153.
Halliwell, E., & Dittmar, H. (2004). Does size matter? The impact of model’s body Want, S. C. (2009). Meta-analytic moderators of experimental exposure to media
size on women’s body-focused anxiety and advertising effectiveness. Journal of portrayals of women on female appearance satisfaction: Social comparisons as
Social and Clinical Psychology, 23, 104–122. automatic processes. Body Image, 6, 257–269.
Halliwell, E., & Dittmar, H. (2006). Associations between appearance-related self- Wood-Barcalow, N. L., Tylka, T. L., & Augustus-Horvath, C. L. (2010). But I like my
discrepancies and young women’s and men’s affect, body satisfaction, and body: Positive body image characteristics and a holistic model for young-adult
emotional eating: A comparison of fixed-item and participant-generated self- women. Body Image, 7, 106–116.
discrepancies. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 447–458. Yamamiya, Y., Cash, T. F., Melnyk, S. E., Posavac, H. D., & Posavac, S. S. (2005). Women’s
Halliwell, E., Easun, A., & Harcourt, D. (2011). Body dissatisfaction: Can a short media exposure to thin-and-beautiful media images: Body image effects of media ideal
literacy message reduce negative media exposure effects amongst adolescent internalization and impact-reduction interventions. Body Image, 2, 74–78.
girls? British Journal of Health Psychology, 16, 396–403.

You might also like