You are on page 1of 13

Body Image 28 (2019) 53–65

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Body Image
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bodyimage

From negative to positive body image: Men’s and women’s journeys


from early adolescence to emerging adulthood夽
Kristina Holmqvist Gattario ∗ , Ann Frisén
Department of Psychology, University of Gothenburg, Sweden

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This study examined the developmental journeys of individuals who have overcome negative body image
Received 21 June 2018 in early adolescence and developed positive body image on their way to emerging adulthood. Interviews
Received in revised form 6 December 2018 were conducted with 15 women and 16 men (aged 26–27) recruited from a large longitudinal sample.
Accepted 6 December 2018
Results demonstrated different patterns of positive body image development, but most participants had
Available online 21 December 2018
overcome their negative body image by age 18. Factors contributing to their negative body image in early
adolescence included negative peer influence and discontent with life in general. Turning points included
Keywords:
finding a new social context, experiencing agency and empowerment, and using cognitive strategies to
Negative body image
Positive body image
improve body image. Characteristics of the participants’ current positive body image coincided with
Body appreciation established features of positive body image; novel findings were that the women were more likely to
Body image development think of positive body image as needing constant work to maintain and were also more likely to have
Turning points a feminist identity, whereas the men were more likely to try to improve their body shape and perceive
Qualitative research their body as resembling the ideal. In conclusion, body image interventions need to target not only
Bullying matters related to physical appearance but also adolescents’ general sense of belonging, agency, and
empowerment.
© 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction Neumark-Sztainer, & Paxton, 2006; Frisén, Lunde, & Berg, 2015)
and maintain their negative body image throughout adolescence
Negative body image, conceptualized as poor body esteem, and into emerging adulthood (Frisén et al., 2015; Rogers, Webb, &
body dissatisfaction, or body shame, dramatically affects the lives Jafari, 2018). Body image in emerging adulthood tends to remain
of many young people as it is linked to low overall well-being stable (Grogan, 2017). Altogether, these findings indicate that the
(Meland, Haugland, & Breidablik, 2007), low self-esteem (Davison & adolescent years are critical for many in shaping the body image
McCabe, 2006), depression (Ohring, Graber, & Brooks-Gunn, 2002), that they will subsequently carry with them into adulthood.
and disordered eating (Westerberg-Jacobson, Edlund, & Ghaderi, Although the most common developmental pattern implies
2010). Adolescence is a critical time for body image develop- a drastic increase in negative body image in early adolescence
ment as many of the changes during this phase in life (biological, followed by stability into emerging adulthood, investigations of
emotional, cognitive, as well as social) channel individuals’ atten- individual trajectories demonstrate that there are also other pat-
tion towards their developing physical bodies. The few large-scale terns of body image development (Frisén et al., 2015; Nelson,
longitudinal studies examining long-term body image develop- Kling, Wängqvist, Frisén, & Syed, 2018; Rogers et al., 2018). One
ment demonstrate that most adolescents experience a dramatic particularly interesting pattern, explored further here, pertains to
increase in negative body image in early adolescence (Bucchianeri, individuals who, consistent with the majority, develop negative
Arikian, Hannan, Eisenberg, & Neumark-Sztainer, 2013; Eisenberg, body image in early adolescence, but who then overcome their neg-
ative body image and develop positive body image on their journey
to emerging adulthood. We are unaware of any in-depth study of
this interesting developmental pattern, although the experiences
夽 This research was supported by grants from the Swedish Research Council for of individuals following this trajectory may be of particular value to
Health, Working Life and Welfare (grant number 2014-1729). We thank Åsa Nord- body image research, prevention, interventions, and clinical work.
ström for assisting in the data collection and the participants for sharing their stories.
∗ Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, University of Gothenburg, Indeed, these individuals have managed to overcome a negative
Box 500, 405 30 Gothenburg, Sweden. body image and their experiences of their journey toward a pos-
E-mail address: kristina.holmqvist@psy.gu.se (K.H. Gattario).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2018.12.002
1740-1445/© 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
54 K.H. Gattario, A. Frisén / Body Image 28 (2019) 53–65

itive body image can be helpful in distinguishing the factors that body appreciation (Andrew, Tiggemann, & Clark, 2016; Augustus-
may help turn a negative body image into a positive one. Horvath & Tylka, 2011; Avalos & Tylka, 2006; Tylka & Homan, 2015).
This study will accordingly explore men and women’s develop- This model emphasizes the importance of support and acceptance
ment from a negative body image in early adolescence to a positive from others in promoting positive body image, but it provides little
body image in emerging adulthood. One focus of this study is to information about whether and how these aspects may play a role
examine the range of individual patterns of body image develop- in turning a negative body image into a positive one. Also, there may
ment that these men and women display. For example, does the be additional factors serving as turning points that are not covered
change from negative to positive body image tend to happen grad- in the model. Another theoretical model, the developmental theory
ually or does it take dramatic turns and, if so, when? Answering of embodiment (2017, Piran & Teall, 2012; Piran, 2016), suggests that
these questions will advance our understanding of positive body positive embodied experiences, that is, activities that enhance the
image development and provide valuable information about the awareness of bodily experience, connectedness with the body, and
stage at which interventions should be implemented. Another focus feelings of competence, empowerment, and inclusion, can promote
is to investigate the factors underlying this pattern of development positive body image. While this model has a wider scope than does
from a negative body image in early adolescence to a positive body the acceptance model, a deficiency is that it was developed based on
image in emerging adulthood. Specifically, what contributed to assessments of girls and women only, and more research is needed
the participants’ negative body image in early adolescence? What to evaluate the application of the embodiment construct in young
characterized their positive body image in emerging adulthood? men.
Perhaps most importantly, what were the turning points in their The acceptance and developmental theories of embodiment
development? The answers to these questions can provide valu- provide a preliminary idea of factors that may turn a negative body
able knowledge of the factors characterizing negative body image image into a positive one, but turning points need to be exam-
that can be overcome, the factors that can help turn this negative ined more thoroughly, through studies including both men and
body image into a positive body image, and the characteristics of women. To obtain a deeper understanding of men’s and women’s
positive body image that have been acquired. journeys from negative to positive body image, it is essential to
Several theoretical perspectives can help us understand the hear the stories of the individuals who have followed this particu-
factors underlying individuals’ body image development. The dom- lar developmental trajectory. Their reflections on their own body
inant theoretical models (e.g., Smolak, 2012; Thompson, Coovert, image journey can be valuable in order to identify factors with the
& Stormer, 1999), however, tend to focus on the risk factors potential to change the path of body image development and to
undermining body image development and do not specifically understand individual body image trajectories.
reflect on those that may promote positive body image (Tylka
& Wood-Barcalow, 2015b). For years, it was assumed that posi- 1.1. Aim and research questions
tive body image was simply the opposite of negative body image,
and the factors assumed to promote positive body image were The aim of this study was to examine young men and women’s
therefore simply seen as the opposites of those undermining development from a negative body image in early adolescence to
body image (Striegel-Moore & Cachelin, 1999). However, recent a positive one in emerging adulthood. Four specific research ques-
research into positive body image has provided evidence that this tions guided the investigation: (a) What are the different patterns of
picture may be too simplistic. The few studies exploring the expe- body image development displayed by individuals who start with
riences, thoughts, attitudes, and behaviors of individuals with a a negative body image in early adolescence and then acquire a pos-
positive body image have been helpful in conceptualizing pos- itive one on their way to emerging adulthood?; (b) What factors
itive body image as featuring body appreciation, functionality contributed to their negative body image in early adolescence?; (c)
appreciation, body acceptance, inner positivity, interpreting infor- What factors served as turning points in their body image devel-
mation in a body-image–protective manner, and conceptualizing opment?; and (d) What characterized their positive body image in
beauty broadly (Alleva, Tylka, & Kroon Van Diest, 2017; Frisén emerging adulthood?
& Holmqvist, 2010b; Holmqvist & Frisén, 2012; Wood-Barcalow,
Tylka, & Augustus-Horvath, 2010). Yet, previous studies have not
2. Method
specifically examined individuals’ journeys toward a positive body
image and have therefore not considered the factors that may help 2.1. Participants
turn a negative into a positive body image. In addition, previous
studies have focused on adolescent girls’ and young women’s expe- The participants, 15 women and 16 men (Mage = 26.19,
riences of having a positive body image; in fact, only one sample of SD = 0.48), were recruited from the longitudinal research project
adolescent boys has previously been studied (Frisén & Holmqvist, MoS (Mobbning och Skola [Bullying and School]), which has stud-
2010b; Holmqvist & Frisén, 2012). This indicates a lack of knowl- ied individuals’ body image development from ages 10 to 24 years
edge of young men’s positive body image and how it may differ (see, e.g., Frisén & Holmqvist, 2010a; Frisén et al., 2015; Gattario
from young women’s. This study accordingly adds to the litera- et al., 2015; Lunde & Frisén, 2011). In the first wave, 960 10-year-
ture by examining patterns of positive body image development in olds participated (515 girls and 445 boys). There have been in total
young men as well as women. In addition, it is the first study to six waves in the study, at ages 10, 13, 16, 18, 21, and 24 years.
qualitatively examine what characterizes positive body image in Rates of attrition have overall been low: 91% (n = 874) of the origi-
young men. nal sample participated at age 13 years, 79% (n = 758) at age 16, 74%
When it comes to understanding the factors that can promote (n = 715) at age 18, 64% (n = 607) at age 21, and 56% (n = 544; 302
positive body image, specifically, theoretical models are still in their women and 242 men) at age 24.
infancy (Halliwell, 2015). Longitudinal, quantitative studies have In this longitudinal project, body image has been assessed
contributed insights into what can promote body appreciation, one using the Body-Esteem Scale for Adolescents and Adults (BESAA;
of several aspects of positive body image, which is defined as indi- Mendelson, Mendelson, & White, 2001 see Measures). To recruit
viduals’ acceptance of, favorable opinions of, and respect for their participants to the present study, we used the mean score of the
bodies (Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, 2015b). For example, the accep- BESAA Appearance and Weight subscale items, since the Attribu-
tance model (Avalos & Tylka, 2006) suggests that social support tion subscale has displayed lower reliability than have the other
and body acceptance by others are important factors promoting two subscales (Mendelson et al., 2001). Expectation maximization
K.H. Gattario, A. Frisén / Body Image 28 (2019) 53–65 55

in SPSS was used to handle missing data when participants had


fewer than four missing values on the BESAA on one occasion. Those
recruited to this study met the inclusion criteria of (a) having par-
ticipated in at least four of the six waves of the study, (b) having
had low body esteem in early adolescence (ages 10 and 13 years),
and (c) having high body esteem in emerging adulthood (age 24
years). Negative body image (at ages 10 and 13 years) and positive
body image (at age 24 years) were defined relative to the BESAA
scores of the larger longitudinal sample at those same ages. At each
age, all participants were divided into four quartiles ranging from
those with low body esteem (first quartile) to those with high body
esteem (fourth quartile). We defined negative body image as hav-
ing a BESAA score in the first or second quartiles, that is, lower than
the median of the total sample (at age 10, < 3.46 for boys and <
3.30 for girls, and, at age 13, < 3.14 for boys and < 2.60 for girls).
We defined positive body image as having a BESAA score in the
fourth and highest quartile (at age 24, > 3.13 for men and > 2.96
for women). From a developmental perspective, we considered the
participants’ relative position in body esteem in relation to their
Fig. 1. Female participants’ body image development (as measured combining
same-age peers a better indicator of their body image than using BESAA Weight and Appearance; Mendelson et al., 2001) between ages 10 and 24
identical cut-off points for positive and negative body image at all years. The median (dotted line) is from the larger longitudinal sample of partici-
ages. pants (N = 960) from which the current sample was recruited. BESAA ranges from 0
For consistency, throughout this paper we refer to individu- to 4, with higher values indicating a more positive body image.
als’ negative body image ‘turning into’ a positive one; however,
negative body image and positive body image should not be consid-
ered opposites of one another, and aspects of negative body image
may co-exist with positive body image (Tylka & Wood-Barcalow,
2015b). Yet, when examining body image development over time
as in this study, it is necessary to be consistent in our conceptualiza-
tions of body image from wave to wave in order to study changes in
development. Therefore, for the recruitment of participants to this
study, we defined both negative and positive body image according
to their levels of body esteem.
In total, 25 men and 30 women met the inclusion criteria of
having had negative body image in early adolescence and having
positive body image in emerging adulthood. We set a predefined
goal of including 30 participants (15 men and 15 women) in
the study. Hence, we regularly contacted small sets of randomly
selected participants from the 25 men and 30 women until we had
achieved this goal. The final sample consisted of 16 men and 15
women. Their BESAA scores at age 10 years ranged from 1.95 to
3.89 (M = 3.18, SD = 0.52) for boys and 2.37–3.53 (M = 2.90, SD = 0.37)
for girls; at age 13 years 1.43–3.95 (M = 2.98, SD = 0.75) for boys
Fig. 2. Male participants’ body image development (as measured combining BESAA
and 1.00–3.95 (M = 2.75, SD = 0.75) for girls; and at age 24 years Weight and Appearance; Mendelson et al., 2001) between ages 10 and 24 years. The
3.23–3.70 (M = 3.44, SD = 0.14) for men and 2.94–4.00 (M = 3.34, median (dotted line) is from the larger sample of participants (N = 960) from which
SD = 0.34) for women. For further illustration of their body esteem the current sample was recruited. BESAA ranges from 0 to 4, with higher values
at different ages, see Figs. 1 and 2. Demographic data from age 24 indicating a more positive body image.
years demonstrated that the participants’ highest educational level
ranged from lower secondary school (n = 1), upper secondary school tioned from a negative to a positive body image) until right before
(n = 11), university degree initiated (n = 13), to university degree fin- the fifth and final part (e) of the interview, to prevent this informa-
ished (n = 6). The majority of the participants were born in Sweden tion from affecting how they described their body image journey.
(n = 29) and the remaining were born in other European countries Within a week of the participants’ receiving the information let-
(n = 1) or in the Middle East (n = 1). Six of the participants had par- ter, we contacted them by telephone to ask whether they agreed
ents that were born in other countries than Sweden, mainly other to take part in the study. We used the contact information they
European countries (n = 3) and the Middle East (n = 3). Twenty- had supplied online during their previous participation in the lon-
five of the participants identified as heterosexual, one as gay, four gitudinal study. As an incentive, participants were offered two
as bisexual, and one participant preferred not to report his or her movie tickets as well as a book published by the authors, which
sexual orientation. describes many of the previous findings of the longitudinal research
project (Frisén, Holmqvist Gattario, & Lunde, 2014). Written con-
2.2. Procedure sent was collected from each participant before the interview. Most
participants were interviewed at the Department of Psychology,
Eligible participants first received a written letter with informa- University of Gothenburg, in a setting made as comfortable as pos-
tion about the study. The study aim described in the information sible for them. If the participants lived in another city, we arranged
letter was “to gain a richer understanding of people’s thoughts to meet with them at a suitable place in their area (in total, eight
and feelings about their bodies.” Participants were not informed men and five women). Interviews were audio recorded and lasted
of their BESAA developmental pattern (i.e., that they had transi- approximately 1.5 h. To ensure that the participants still had a
56 K.H. Gattario, A. Frisén / Body Image 28 (2019) 53–65

positive body image at the time of the interview, which occurred Attribution subscale has demonstrated lower reliability than the
approximately two years after the age 24-year time point in the lon- other two subscales (Mendelson et al., 2001). The two subscales
gitudinal study, participants were asked to fill in measures of body have, in previous studies, displayed high internal consistency and
esteem and body appreciation in-between part (b) and (c) of the test-retest reliability among Canadian adolescents and emerging
interview. After the interview, there was a short debriefing where adults (Mendelson et al., 2001). In the large longitudinal sample,
participants were asked about how they experienced the interview reliability scores for the Weight subscale ranged from ␣ = .90 to .94
and were able to ask questions about the study. The data collection and for the Appearance subscale from ␣ = .89 to .92 at ages 10 to 24
was approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board in Gothenburg, years.
Sweden, project name “Bullying and Body Image – A Longitudinal
Study From Childhood to Adulthood,” protocol number T446-15.
2.4.2. Body appreciation
The Body Appreciation Scale-2 (BAS-2; Tylka & Wood-Barcalow,
2.3. Semi-structured interview
2015a) was used to quantitatively assess body appreciation. The
BAS-2 has a unidimensional factor structure and consists of 10
An interview schedule was constructed in accordance with the
items measuring individuals’ acceptance of, favorable opinions of,
guidelines provided by Smith and Osborn (2003). The interview
and respect for their bodies (e.g., “I appreciate the different and
topics were based on the existing body image literature, but also
unique characteristics of my body”). Respondents indicate their
included parts that were more explorative. The different parts of
degree of agreement with each statement on a Likert scale ranging
the interview assessed: (a) a general idea of the participant’s cur-
from 1 = never to 5 = always. Item scores are averaged, with higher
rent body image, (b) participants’ reflections on the influence of
scores reflecting higher levels of body appreciation. The BAS-2
family and peers on their body image when growing up, (c) par-
has, in previous studies, displayed high internal consistency, test-
ticipants’ reflections on a picture of themselves that they liked and
retest reliability, and construct validity, among U.S. community and
that they were asked to email us before the interview (this was also
undergraduate women and men (Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, 2015a).
included to promote discussion of the participants’ current body
To translate the BAS-2 into Swedish (see Appendix B), the scale was
image), (d) participants’ reflections on appearance ideals conveyed
translated by the first author and then back-translated by a research
in the media, and (e) participants’ reflections on their body image
assistant (Brislin, 1970). The translations and backtranslations were
journey. The questions for each part are presented in Appendix A.
reviewed and discussed until we agreed on the translation that we
Introductory questions (e.g., regarding family and peer group con-
considered most adequate and relevant. The scale was pilot-tested
stellations when growing up) were asked to help the participants
before data collection. Lemoine et al.’s (2018) recent evaluation of
remember their life as adolescents. Several unstructured questions
an almost identical version of the Swedish BAS-2 that they based
were asked to follow up the different aspects raised by the par-
on our translation, demonstrated excellent psychometric proper-
ticipants. The following definition of body image was provided at
ties in a sample of Swedish-speaking adolescents and young adults.
the beginning of the interview: “Body image includes experiences,
The reliability score in our sample was ␣ = .83.
feelings, attitudes, and behaviors in relation to the body. It can
relate to the body’s appearance, function, as well as health. Peo-
ple may differ in the importance they ascribe to these different 2.5. Analyses
aspects of their body image.” Right before the fifth and final part
(e) of the interview where participants were asked to reflect on To answer research question (a) regarding the participants’ dif-
their own body image journey, participants were informed that ferent patterns of body image development toward a positive body
they belonged to a group of participants who, in relation to the image, their body esteem scores from ages 10 to 24 years were
large longitudinal sample, had a negative body image in early ado- extracted from the longitudinal database. Based on these scores,
lescence and then had developed a positive one on their way to we created graphs illustrating the 31 participants’ individual body
emerging adulthood. esteem patterns; see Figs. 1 (women) and 2 (men). We also cal-
Two pilot interviews, conducted to discern any potential con- culated their body esteem and body appreciation scores collected
cerns with the interview, allowed the two interviewers (the first at the time of the interview. To answer research questions (b) to
author and a trained research assistant with a Degree of Master (d), thematic analysis was used. Thematic analysis is used for the
of Science in Psychology) to discuss their biases. Both interview- systematic identification and analysis of patterns, or themes, in
ers were 35-year-old females with experience in semi-structured data (Braun & Clarke, 2006). In the present study, research ques-
interviewing and that were unknown to the participants. tions (b) to (d) guided the analysis so that the themes identified
should reflect (b) what contributed to the participants’ negative
2.4. Measures body image in early adolescence, (c) what the turning points were
in their development, and (d) what characterized their current
2.4.1. Body esteem positive body image. Three thematic analyses were accordingly
The BESAA (Mendelson et al., 2001), translated into Swedish conducted, one for each research question. The two authors started
(Erling & Hwang, 2004), was used to quantitatively assess partici- by separately reading the interview transcripts to become famil-
pants’ body image throughout the broader longitudinal project as iar with the data, assigning initial codes to meaningful features.
well as at the time of the interview. The BESAA consists of 23 items During this process, the authors met regularly to discuss poten-
that can be divided into three subscales: Weight (weight-based tial patterns and themes in the data. After having read the first 12
body esteem; eight items; e.g., “I really like what I weigh”), Appear- transcripts, the authors met and agreed on a preliminary coding
ance (appearance-based body esteem; 10 items; e.g., “I like what scheme that the first author then used to code the remaining inter-
I look like in pictures”), and Attribution (beliefs about how others views. The two authors had regular contact during this process to
view one’s body and appearance; five items; e.g., “People my own discuss any necessary adjustments to the coding scheme. Only a
age like my looks”). Respondents indicate their degree of agreement few small adjustments were made to the preliminary scheme.
with each statement on a Likert scale ranging from 0 = never to 4 = Two individual case stories (one woman and one man) were
always, with higher scores indicating higher body esteem. For the chosen to illustrate the participants’ different body image journeys
present study, we used only the items on the Weight and Appear- and portray the range of themes distinguished in their stories (see
ance subscales to create a mean score of the BESAA, because the Appendix C).
K.H. Gattario, A. Frisén / Body Image 28 (2019) 53–65 57

3. Results women (M for men = 3.46, SD = 0.86, M for women = 3.22, SD = 0.96;
Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, 2015a), the present participants had sig-
The first part of this section presents the participants’ body nificantly higher body appreciation, both men t(203) = –3.92, p <
image development in quantitative terms. This includes descrip- .01, and women t(205) = –4.51, p < .01. These results further demon-
tions of their body esteem as measured using BESAA from ages 10 to strate that the present sample experienced positive body image
24 years. It also includes their body esteem and body appreciation in line with recent conceptualizations (Tylka & Wood-Barcalow,
scores at the time of the interview. The second part of this section 2015b), and not only high levels of body esteem.
illustrates the participants’ body image in qualitative terms. We
present the results of the thematic analysis and use fictive names 3.3. Thematic analyses: the participants’ own descriptions of
to illustrate the themes. their body image development

3.1. Patterns of development from a negative body image in early 3.3.1. What factors explained the participants’ negative body
adolescence to a positive body image in emerging adulthood image in early adolescence?
The factors forming the participants’ negative body image
The participants’ body esteem development from ages 10 to 24 in early adolescence were: the perception of having a deficient
years are illustrated in Figs. 1 (women) and 2 (men). The median appearance relative to peers, experiences of negative peer influ-
at each age (indicated by a dotted line) is from the larger sam- ence, and experiences of overall discontent and lack of self-esteem
ple of participants (N = 960) from which the current sample was extending to the participants’ body image.
recruited. In line with the inclusion criteria, all 31 participants’
body esteem values were below the median at age 10 and/or 13 3.3.1.1. Perception of having a deficient appearance (13 men, 11
years and in the positive quartile at age 24 years. Although the par- women). The majority of the participants experienced having a
ticipants were recruited based on the same inclusion criteria, the deficient appearance relative to peers in early adolescence (e.g.,
graphs show a range of individual patterns in the sample, provid- having been shorter, taller, chubbier, larger, or thinner than peers,
ing brief preliminary insight into their many different trajectories or having protruding ears, bad acne, braces, or too-small or too-
from a negative to positive body image. Some participants expe- large breasts). Some participants expressed that their perceived
rienced dramatic turns, both positive and negative, in their body deficient appearance was related to maturing early or late in terms
image development before they finally stabilized around a more of puberty. Emma, for instance, said, “I was a bit chubby right before
positive body image. Others experienced a steadier, gradual pos- puberty and during high school. I felt like I wasn’t good enough.”
itive development toward a positive body image. Interestingly, a
few participants did not experience any increase in body esteem, 3.3.1.2. Experiences of negative peer influence (11 men, 11 women).
but rather maintained a similar level of body esteem while the The majority of the participants also experienced peer appearance
larger longitudinal sample to which they were compared gener- teasing or even physical (e.g., being hit, pushed, or kicked) or ver-
ally became more negative; this, in turn, resulted in the current bal (e.g., being called nasty names) bullying at school. Appearance
sample’s more positive relative position. Note that most partic- teasing often targeted the features of the participants’ appearance
ipants (11 women and 12 men) already reported above-median perceived as deficient by the participant. Jakob said, “I was always
body esteem at age 18 years, overcoming negative body image teased about my height. Everyone else was like one head taller than
according to the definition used here. However, it takes until the me so that was something they always commented on.” Kristoffer
last measurement at age 24 for all participants to be in the most said, “People have hit me and spit on me. I’ve always been bul-
positive quartile. Clearly, however, age 18 seems to be the time by lied, from the first day I went to school.” The theme also includes
which most participants have acquired a more stable body image a few descriptions by participants who experienced high appear-
(no one is in the lowest quartile at this age) and have started their ance pressure in their peer group at school. Jenny had a close friend
process of developing toward, or even maintaining, a positive body who was very appearance-oriented and influenced her to engage
image. in disordered eating: “She said that if she would be as tall as me,
she would be thinner. Because then she would want to be a model.
3.2. Participants’ body esteem and body appreciation at the time [. . .] She used toothbrushes to try to throw up food. So I tried it too.
of the interview [. . .] Her eating disorder was somehow transmitted on to me.”

Participants generally displayed high levels of body esteem at 3.3.1.3. Overall discontent and lack of self-esteem extended to body
the time of the interview, indicating that they had maintained their image (8 men, 7 women). Several participants felt discontent with
positive body image from the 24-year measuring point. Women’s life in general and experienced low self-esteem in early adolescence
body esteem scores ranged from 2.50 to 3.78 (M = 3.16, SD = 0.42). and this, in turn, extended to their body image. The theme also
Men’s body esteem scores ranged from 2.94 to 3.50 (M = 3.28, includes experiences of having difficulties finding oneself or one’s
SD = 0.17). If we compare the participants’ body esteem at the time place in life during early adolescence, or general feelings of fail-
of the interview with the cutoff value used for recruiting them (i.e., ure and psychological ill-being. A shared feature of the experiences
BESAA scores > 2.96 for women and > 3.13 for men), two women within this theme is that they are not directly related to the partic-
and three men reported body esteem scores slightly below these ipant’s physical body, but still influence body image through their
cutoffs at the time of the interview. They were retained in the anal- impact on the participant’s life and well-being in general. Karl, for
ysis, however, because all of them displayed relatively high levels instance, experienced difficulties at school because he had dyslexia,
of body appreciation (> 3.60). and when asked about his negative body image at this time he said,
The participants generally displayed high levels of body appre- “I think it had to do with all of it. I wasn’t too happy about myself
ciation at the time of the interview, further indicating their current at all at that time.”
positive body image (Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, 2015b). Women’s
body appreciation scores ranged from 3.60 to 5.00 (M = 4.35, 3.3.2. What factors served as turning points in the participants’
SD = 0.45). Men’s body appreciation scores ranged from 3.30 to 4.90 body image development?
(M = 4.34, SD = 0.44). Compared with mean values of body appreci- We identified three factors that served as turning points in the
ation previously obtained in U.S. community samples of men and participants’ development from a negative body image to a pos-
58 K.H. Gattario, A. Frisén / Body Image 28 (2019) 53–65

itive one: the participants found a social context in which they agency and control in life not directly related to their body. Several
felt belonging and acceptance, the participants developed a sense participants succeeded at work or in their studies and, by doing
of agency and empowerment in their lives, and the participants so, felt competent and gained self-esteem, which extended to their
started to actively use strategies to improve their body image. We body image. Karl, for instance, who had difficulties succeeding in
coded for turning points when the participants described events school because of his dyslexia, told us about how eventually man-
or new ways of thinking that changed the direction of the partici- aging to get employed had a positive impact on his whole life: “It
pants’ life story. Although turning points were mostly identified on [school] was a real struggle for me. And then, when I managed to
the basis of the participants’ reflections of their body image jour- get a job, and then another job, and things started going well. . .
ney, they were also identified on the basis of events or new ways of Everything just fell into place.” A few participants also described
thinking that they described occurred between their negative and the empowerment they felt when they became adults and were
positive body image. We identified more than one turning point in finally able to make decisions for themselves.
most participants’ interviews, so the themes may overlap.
3.3.2.3. Participants started to actively use strategies to improve their
3.3.2.1. Participants found a social context in which they felt belong- body image (12 men, 6 women). Many participants also described
ing and acceptance (13 men, 11 women). Many participants said that using various strategies to improve their negative body image:
having found a new social context in which they felt belonging practicing body acceptance, avoiding or ignoring negative body-
and acceptance had been essential for their positive body image related information, focusing on other things in life, and realizing
development. The new social context was provided by support- that pursuing the ideal means having to sacrifice other things in
ive and like-minded friends as well as affirmative and encouraging life.
romantic partners. 3.3.2.3.1. Practicing body acceptance (7 men, 4 women). Sev-
3.3.2.1.1. Friends who were supportive and like-minded (10 men, eral participants tried to think of their body imperfections as part
6 women). Many participants described how finding friends who of themselves and began to accept their bodies. Simon, who had
were similar to them in their interests and/or personality repre- received negative comments about the size of his ears, said, “If you
sented a turning point for them. By feeling supported and accepted would tell me now that I have small ears I would say ‘Ok, what am I
by peers and experiencing belonging, the participants became more supposed to do about that? I have small ears. It’s not like I can make
confident in their bodies and their body image. Kristoffer, who had them bigger.’”
been bullied at school, described how finally finding new friends 3.3.2.3.2. Avoiding or ignoring negative body-related information
was a turning point for him: “After having gone through a long (1 man, 2 women). A few participants started to actively avoid
time of no positive [input] at all, I found people who respected me, or ignore negative body-related information, including appearance
who said that ‘you’re OK and we like you for who you are.’ [. . .] comments from peers and media images promoting the appearance
To me, it was like finding another family outside of my [original] ideal. Amanda said, “I’ve done some pretty drastic choices in my life
family. I found a place where I was accepted and I felt like ‘wow, because of this thing with my body. . . I don’t read the blogs I used
this is where I belong.’” to read. I don’t watch the TV-programs I used to watch. Because I
3.3.2.1.2. Affirmative and encouraging romantic partners (5 men, noticed that they make me feel uncomfortable with my body.”
5 women). Some participants described meeting a boyfriend or a 3.3.2.3.3. Focusing on other things in life (8 men, 7 women). Sev-
girlfriend who, by giving encouraging appearance-reassuring com- eral participants started to pay attention to other things than their
ments, helped the participants to be more comfortable in their physical appearance. Instead, they focused on having a nice per-
body. Sara described how her first boyfriend in ninth grade was sonality, being healthy, developing their intellect, learning new
a real “boost” to her body image: “I had a lot of acne at the time things, doing well at work, or more generally “other more impor-
and couldn’t for the world understand why he still wanted to be tant things.” Linnea said, “You need to let go of that constant focus
with me, even though my face was full of acne. But he wanted to, on the body. It’s not useful to sit at home and read a magazine. It
and we were so in love.” Other participants described how having won’t help increase your self-esteem.”
a partner with whom they could discuss difficult matters and that 3.3.2.3.4. Realizing that pursuing the ideal means having to sac-
listened to them was a relief when experiencing negative feelings. rifice other things in life (7 men, 1 woman). A strategy that was
brought forward almost exclusively by the young men was real-
3.3.2.2. Participants developed a sense of agency and empowerment in izing that pursuing the appearance ideal was too strenuous and
their lives (13 men, 10 women). For the majority of the participants, time-consuming and entailed giving up certain pleasures in life
positive body image development was also related to experiences (e.g., food and drink). This ultimately led the participants to stop
of increased agency and empowerment. pursuing the ideal and become more satisfied with their bodies.
3.3.2.2.1. Agency through the body (11 men, 10 women). Several Lukas told us that he regularly worked out, but that the goal of
participants started to use their body as a means to feel agency his training had changed over time: “I now exercise in order to
and control in life. Most frequently, this was related to excelling feel good. That’s what’s important, not because I want big arms or
in physical activities or sports or, less frequently, in other body- abdominals or anything. That is such a waste of time. [. . .] I would
related activities (e.g., singing or exploring one’s sexuality). Elin have to quit my job in that case. It was different when I was a stu-
said, “When I started training and dancing, that affected me a lot in dent and [I thought] ‘I want a six-pack, and I want all of that’. . . and
the sense that I got a more positive. . . more control over my body.” now it’s more important for me to feel good.”
A few women described using small acts of body activism (e.g.,
refusing to shave one’s legs) to gain power and control. This theme 3.3.3. What characterizes the participants’ positive body image in
also includes descriptions of exploring and eventually finding one’s emerging adulthood?
own personal style in clothing and grooming, and how this helped Several features were identified as characteristics of the par-
the participants to feel more confident in their bodies. Oskar, for ticipants’ current positive body image. These included body
instance, said, “I feel like I found a clothing style that fits with my appreciation, perception of the body as integral to the self, body
body. And that has made me feel good about my clothes as well as acceptance, body attunement and self-care, mentoring others
the way I look.” to like their bodies, filtering media images in a body-image-
3.3.2.2.2. A general sense of agency and empowerment in one’s life protective manner, constantly practicing positive body image,
(7 men, 8 women). Some participants developed a general sense of feminist approach to body image, appearance-enhancing invest-
K.H. Gattario, A. Frisén / Body Image 28 (2019) 53–65 59

ment, and the perception of having a body that is close to the were constantly exposed to societal messages asserting that they
ideal. should not be satisfied with their bodies. Sara said, “It requires con-
stant practice [not to care about negative comments]. If someone
3.3.3.1. Body appreciation (12 men, 12 women). The majority of the says that I’m short I usually say that I’m pretty happy with being
participants expressed appreciation of the features, functionality, short. I wouldn’t want to be taller. Because I’m a dancer and I would
and health of their bodies. Some participants, like Elin, had friends feel clumsy.”
with health problems or experienced health problems themselves
(e.g., back pain), and this new awareness of the body’s vulnerabil- 3.3.3.8. Feminist approach to body image (5 women). A third of the
ity made them appreciate their body’s health even more. Elin said, female participants expressed having a strong feminist identity,
“One of my friends has gastric problems and another one has back using feminist arguments to fight appearance concerns, and possi-
problems, so I feel fortunate not to have any of that. I think more bly also engaging in various types of activism. Sara, who described
about that now than when I was younger.” herself as a body activist, told us about an incident she had on an
internet dating site where a potential date asked her to send a full
3.3.3.2. Perception of the body as integral to the self (1 man, 6 women). figure photo. Sara, who felt objectified by this request, confronted
Some participants, the majority women, described viewing their him: “The more I encounter these appearance prejudices, the more
bodies as integral to their self-identity. Emma said, “Every single I speak out.”
part of my body is me.” Other participants emphasized that eth-
nic features that were earlier a source of discontent were now a 3.3.3.9. Appearance-enhancing investment (11 men, 4 women). Sev-
source of pride as they were perceived to say something about the eral participants, mostly men, paid attention to the physical
participants’ origins. appearance of their bodies and strove to change their bodily
appearance through strategies such as exercise. In contrast to the
3.3.3.3. Body acceptance (12 men, 12 women). The majority of the participants aligned with the theme “Filtering media images in
participants accepted their bodies the way they were and did not a body-image-protective manner,” participants aligned with this
strive to change their appearance in any way. Jenny, for instance, theme may describe feeling motivated by the appearance ideals
said, “I’m happy with my body. [. . .] I try to make the most of it. and viewing them as positive inspiration. Some participants, how-
I don’t put any effort into trying to change it. I have accepted that ever, described that they were both critical of media images and
this is the way I look.” engaged in appearance-enhancing investment. Alexander said that
he exercised to be healthy, but “primarily in order to look good.
3.3.3.4. Body attunement and self-care (11 men, 8 women). Many I’ve got my own little goals and an ideal body that I strive for.”
participants described that they were attuned to their bodies, lis- Karl described viewing appearance ideals as inspiring: “They are
tened to how their bodies signaled pain, stress, and anxiety, and an incentive for me to make a change.”
tried to adjust their activities according to these signals. Partic-
ipants took good care of their bodies via health-promoting and 3.3.3.10. Perception of having a body that is close to the ideal (9 men,
stress-reducing activities (e.g., physical activity and relaxation). 3 women). Several participants, again mostly men, perceived they
Sebastian said, “You only live once so you need to take good care of have a body type not too far from current appearance ideals. When
your body so it will last your whole life.” Axel was asked about how he felt about appearance ideals in the
media, he said, “They are not a problem to me because I look more
3.3.3.5. Mentoring others to like their bodies (1 man, 6 women). Some like them than most people. It’s an appearance ideal that fits my
participants, in particular women, tried to mentor other people body.” Some of the men believed that they would probably be able
(friends and family; e.g., the participants’ own children) to have to attain an ideal body if they wished to put the time and effort into
a positive body image, by encouraging them to love their bodies doing so.
and not to care too much about what other people think. Julia, for
instance, told us that her female friends often asked her about what 4. Discussion
their male dates would think about their appearance and she would
answer, “It doesn’t matter what he thinks, what matters is what This study is the first to examine the experiences of individuals
you think. If you think you look good you look good, you don’t need who have overcome having a negative body image in early adoles-
anyone else to tell you that.” cence and have developed a positive body image on their way to
emerging adulthood. The study advances our understanding of dif-
3.3.3.6. Filtering media images in a body-image-protective man- ferent journeys to a positive body image and of the factors involved
ner (11 men, 10 women). Many participants expressed how they in this notable body image trajectory, including the risk factors for
interpreted media images of current appearance ideals in a body- negative body image early in life, the factors that may serve as pos-
image-protective manner. This included being critical of the itive turning points, and the characteristics of positive body image
appearance ideals, describing them as objectifying, disempower- in emerging adulthood.
ing, retouched, unreal, non-functional, homogenous, unattainable, The graphs illustrating the participants’ different patterns of
and even tragic. These participants did not identify with the models body image development reveal that the journey from a negative
in the images and were not motivated to try to attain bodies like body image in early adolescence to a positive one in emerg-
theirs. Peter said, “Just because a body looks healthy on the outside ing adulthood can differ greatly. Some participants experienced
doesn’t mean it’s healthy on the inside. [. . .] and what good does dramatic turns, both positive and negative, in their body image
it do to me that they look fit?” Filtering media images in a body- development before finally stabilizing around a more positive body
image-protective manner not only included filtering out negative image. Others experienced a steadier, gradual positive develop-
messages but also filtering in beneficial media. For example, a few ment toward a positive body image. Regardless of the participants’
of the women were followers of body activists in social media. different developmental patterns, we found that most had devel-
oped a body image above the median of the large longitudinal
3.3.3.7. Constantly practicing positive body image (1 man, 8 women). sample by age 18 years. This finding, that it took until the end
Some participants, almost exclusively women, described how of adolescence for many individuals to make this developmental
maintaining a positive body image implies constant work, as they step, is noteworthy. It adds a novel finding to existing research into
60 K.H. Gattario, A. Frisén / Body Image 28 (2019) 53–65

body image development in the critical phase of youth, suggest- or bullying). This finding is consistent with both the acceptance
ing that body image development can take positive turns late in model, which proposes that social support and body acceptance
youth development. However, the finding that none of the partic- by others can promote positive body image (Andrew et al., 2016;
ipants had a very negative body image at age 18 years and that no Augustus-Horvath & Tylka, 2011; Avalos & Tylka, 2006), and the
one was under the median at age 21 also indicates that this shift developmental theory of embodiment (Piran & Teall, 2012; Piran,
towards a positive body image takes place around age 18 to 21, not 2017), which emphasizes the importance of feeling included in
later. This could indicate that around the age of 18 years would be desired social contexts for the development of positive body image.
an ideal time for effective body image interventions. Although these models have been developed for girls’ and young
To understand the factors behind overcoming negative body women’s body image experiences, the present findings suggest that
image, we thematically analyzed the participants’ stories. We iden- they can also be applicable to boys’ and young men’s experiences.
tified three factors that contributed to the participants’ negative In line with these findings, there are already some body image
body image in early adolescence, which partly coincide with pre- interventions that target and try to improve adolescents’ peer rela-
vious findings on the risk factors for negative body image. The first tions. However, the activities in these interventions tend to focus on
is that most participants perceived themselves to have a ‘deficient’ body image-related discussions of the acceptance of diversity and
appearance in comparison with their peers in early adolescence. on psychoeducational lessons about how peers can influence body
They perceived themselves to be shorter, taller, chubbier, larger, image through appearance-related conversations and appearance
or thinner than their peers, or had other deficient appearance fea- comparisons (Bird, Halliwell, Diedrichs, & Harcourt, 2013; McCabe,
tures that troubled them. This finding supports previous research Connaughton, Tatangelo, Mellor, & Busija, 2017). For the present
demonstrating that individuals’ own negative perceptions of their participants, though, it was not the improvement of existing rela-
appearance in comparison with peers plays a major role in body tions that enhanced their body image but the experience of finding
dissatisfaction (Lev-Ari, Baumgarten-Katz, & Zohar, 2014). Another a new social context. Parents and teachers can play an important
factor contributing to the participants’ negative body image in role in encouraging adolescents to explore new social contexts to
early adolescence was the experience of negative peer influence. find one in which they feel belonging and acceptance.
The impact of peer appearance teasing (Menzel et al., 2010) and Another turning point in the participants’ positive body image
bullying (Duarte, Pinto-Gouveia, & Stubbs, 2017; Lunde & Frisén, development was related to increased agency and empowerment.
2011) as well as experiences of peer appearance pressure (Helfert The participants’ stories include a range of body-related expe-
& Warschburger, 2011) on adolescents’ body image is well estab- riences of agency and empowerment—for example, excelling in
lished and in line with theoretical frameworks, such as the tripartite physical activities or sports, participating in acts of body activism,
influence model (Thompson et al., 1999) emphasizing the influence or exploring and eventually finding a personal style of clothing and
of peers on body image development. However, the fact that many grooming—and these experiences had helped the participants feel
of the participants developed a positive body image despite being more confident in their body. Indeed, previous research has demon-
victimized in early adolescence is noteworthy and deserves further strated that physical exercise generally improves body image,
attention. It may be that the turning points identified here could although the effects have been found to be smaller for adolescents
also inform bullying interventions in order to reduce the negative than for older participants (Hausenblas & Fallon, 2006). Engaging
impact of victimization on body image. For example, bullying inter- in physical exercise has been suggested to encourage people to
ventions could guide adolescents into finding new social contexts appreciate the functionality of their bodies, which is characteris-
in which they feel accepted the way they are, or encourage adoles- tic of positive embodiment and body image (Holmqvist & Frisén,
cents to develop effective coping strategies to improve body image, 2012; Mahlo & Tiggemann, 2016; Wood-Barcalow et al., 2010).
as identified in this study. Exercise interventions targeting young adolescents have produced
The third and final factor contributing to the participants’ neg- positive results in terms of increased body esteem, but have failed
ative body image in early adolescence was perhaps our most novel to maintain long-term effects (Burgess, Grogan, & Burwitz, 2006;
finding in this part, and concerned a feeling of discontent with life Duncan, Al-Nakeeb, & Nevill, 2009). For the present participants, it
in general and a lack of self-esteem extending to their body image. was not only engaging in physical exercise that improved their body
Indeed, many researchers have connected negative body image to image but also the sense of excelling in that exercise. This may be
low self-esteem (e.g., Davison & McCabe, 2006), but few theoretical one explanation of why exercise interventions do not successfully
models point to ‘discontent with life in general’ as a risk factor for promote positive body image in all adolescents. In addition, ado-
negative body image. Depressive symptoms are often considered a lescents may need to be given the choice of which physical activity
consequence of negative body image, but recent research into the to engage in, to promote that they find one that they enjoy, since
temporal association between the two factors has suggested that enjoyable physical activity is key to positive embodiment (Piran &
this may not always be the case (Sharpe et al., 2017). For some early Teall, 2012; Piran, 2017).
adolescent boys (but not girls), depressive symptoms were the pre- As indicated by the participants’ stories, however, various other
cursors of body dissatisfaction, indicating that how young people body-related experiences and acts other than physical activity can
experience life is mirrored in how they experience their bodies. promote young people’s agency and empowerment (e.g., singing,
Previous research has also found experiences of life satisfaction to sexual exploration, body activism, or exploration of personal style
be correlated with less body dissatisfaction and more body appre- of clothing and grooming), thereby also improving their body
ciation in adolescents and young men and women (Góngora, 2014; image. In addition, some participants’ stories included the devel-
Swami & Ng, 2015). opment of a more general, non-body-related sense of agency and
We identified three factors that helped turn the participants’ empowerment that could involve succeeding at work or in stud-
negative body image into a positive one. A first turning point was ies. This led to greater feelings of competence and self-esteem,
that the participants found a new social context, for example, new which extended to their body image. Although the body-related
friends or an encouraging romantic partner, providing them with a and the non-body-related experiences of agency and empower-
sense of belonging and acceptance that helped them improve their ment may differ greatly from each other, they both give a sense of
body image. The benefits of a new positive social context for body control over one’s life. Theoretically, these experiences are linked
image development are plausible, considering that the participants’ to the concepts of physical and mental freedom, as identified in the
negative body image in early adolescence was often related to neg- developmental theory of embodiment (Piran & Teall, 2012; Piran,
ative peer relations (e.g., appearance pressure, appearance teasing, 2017), as they contributed to the participants’ increased feelings
K.H. Gattario, A. Frisén / Body Image 28 (2019) 53–65 61

of competence and empowerment, both physically and psycho- described the necessity of constantly practicing body acceptance
logically. Intriguingly, this turning point of increased agency and in order to maintain positive body image, as they were unceasingly
empowerment, as well as the turning point of finding a new social exposed to various body-image threats, such as societal messages
context, both generally reflect the suggested core basic psycholog- asserting that they should not be satisfied with their bodies as they
ical needs that all humans share: autonomy (perceiving oneself and are. Researchers have emphasized the idea of examining positive
one’s behavior as an expression of the self), competence (a sense body image not only as a stable trait but also as a state that can
of confidence and self-efficacy) and relatedness (connecting to and fluctuate over time (Webb, Wood-Barcalow, & Tylka, 2015). Pre-
caring of others, being cared for, and belongingness; Deci & Ryan, vious research has recognized this process of fluidity, for example
2000). These connections can help provide a link between positive in that women with a positive body image sometimes have ‘bad
body image and the field of psychology more broadly. body image days’ but tend to recover to an overall positive body
A third and final factor that served as a turning point in the image (Wood-Barcalow et al., 2010). Based on these findings, more
participants’ body image development was their own active use of research is needed into when and why positive body image fluc-
strategies to improve their body image. These strategies included tuates and that further explores young women’s constant practice
practicing body acceptance, avoiding or ignoring negative body- of maintaining a positive body image. In the case story of Sara, for
related information, focusing on other things in life than physical example, we learned that every time she felt bloated due to her
appearance, and realizing that pursuing appearance ideals means menstrual cycle, she reminded herself to appreciate it as a sign
having to sacrifice other things in life. It is clear that many of the par- that her body was functioning properly. This can be considered a
ticipants had successfully coped with and overcome their negative strategy of maintaining positive body image.
body image by actively regulating their thoughts regarding their A few of the young women also described having a strong
bodies. We are not aware of any participant in this study who had feminist identity and used feminist arguments to address body
received clinical treatment to address their negative body image, image concerns and media ideals. Previous experimental research
but it should be noted that some strategies used by the partici- has demonstrated that exposure to a feminist perspective can
pants (e.g., practicing body acceptance) are strategies encouraged improve young women’s body image (Peterson, Tantleff-Dunn, &
in Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) for treating body Bedwell, 2006). A recent study of the role of social media messages
dissatisfaction as well as disordered eating (Fogelkvist, Parling, in young women’s self-objectification found that strong feminist
Kjellin, & Gustafsson, 2016). A cognitive strategy mentioned almost beliefs played an important protective role (Feltman & Szymanski,
exclusively by the male participants was to realize that pursuing 2017). Yet, in focus group interviews, women themselves have
the appearance ideal meant having to sacrifice other things in life. pointed out the that their feminist awareness and cognitions can-
As in the case story of Jakob, several of the young men had con- not totally prevent them from experiencing body image concerns
cluded that having an ideal muscular body would entail giving up (Kling et al., 2017; Rubin, Nemeroff, & Russo, 2004). In fact, some
some of life’s pleasures, such as certain foods and drinks, and being women felt that their feminist cognitions sometimes led to more
forced to spend most of their time in the gym. For these reasons, distress, as now they did not only experience body image concerns,
they deprioritized having an ideal body, which in turn helped them but also felt guilty for continuing to engage in gender stereotypic
improve their body image. One may wonder why this cognitive behavior although they ‘should know better’ (Kling et al., 2017;
strategy was not brought up by the female participants. One possi- Rubin et al., 2004). Feminist ideas, therefore, need to be cautiously
ble explanation is that engaging in one’s appearance and trying to implemented into body image interventions, and preferably in
pursue the thin ideal are some of the most central feminine norms combination with ideas promoting a broader conceptualization of
that women need to adhere to in order to gain social acceptance beauty in order to de-emphasize the conflict that feminist women
(Kling, Holmqvist Gattario, & Frisén, 2017). Women may therefore may experience (Rubin et al., 2004) and compassion with body
find it more challenging than men to deprioritize this pursuit. Still, image threats such as body image flexibility via ACT (Rogers et al.,
realizing that pursuing the ideal means having to sacrifice other 2018).
things in life is likely to be a useful strategy for girls and women Positive body image characteristics that were more common
too, although interventions implementing this approach on girls among the young men were making appearance-enhancing invest-
will also need to challenge the associated feminine norms. Future ments and perceiving one’s body as close to the ideal. These
research could further examine how this ACT-related component appearance-oriented characteristics were somewhat unexpected
can be integrated into body image programs targeting both adoles- considering that positive body image has mostly been related to
cent boys and girls. non-appearance-related, embodying activities (Tiggemann, Coutts,
We identified several characteristics of the participants’ positive & Clark, 2014) and a functional, non-appearance-related view of
body image in emerging adulthood. Some are consistent with previ- the body (Alleva, Martijn, Van Breukelen, Jansen, & Karos, 2015;
ous findings from interviews with young women (Wood-Barcalow Frisén & Holmqvist, 2010b). The concept of “adaptive appearance
et al., 2010) and adolescent girls and boys (Frisén & Holmqvist, investment” has, however, been discussed as one important com-
2010b; Holmqvist & Frisén, 2012) with a positive body image. These ponent of positive body image, defined as “regularly engaging in
include, for example, the features of body appreciation, body accep- appearance-related self-care, such as grooming behaviors that pro-
tance, body attunement and self-care, mentoring others to like their tect an individual’s sense of style and personality—it is enhancing
bodies, and filtering media images in a body image-protective man- one’s natural features via benign methods” (Tylka & Wood-
ner. Interestingly, we did not specifically ask about these themes Barcalow, 2015b, p. 123). For the young men in the present study,
in the interview and yet they map onto the definition of positive appearance-enhancing investments often concerned paying atten-
body image proposed by Tylka and Wood-Barcalow (2015b), which tion to the physical aspects of their bodies or engaging in physical
provides further evidence for the validity of their definition. This is exercise to improve body shape. Although these behaviors could
particularly noteworthy considering that this is the first study to possibly be included among adaptive appearance investments, it
qualitatively explore positive body image in young men, suggest- can be difficult to distinguish between adaptive and maladap-
ing that several of the features of positive body image found among tive investments (Gattario & Lunde, 2018). However, according to
women apply to men. the present findings, appearance-enhancing investments can be
Other identified characteristics contribute novel findings to part of having a positive body image, at least among young men.
the literature on what constitutes positive body image, adding to More research is needed to understand how appearance-enhancing
the complexity of the construct. The young women in particular behaviors relate to positive body image.
62 K.H. Gattario, A. Frisén / Body Image 28 (2019) 53–65

The young men’s perception of having a body that resembles the participants now inhabit a positive body image and look back
the male body ideal is likely to be of importance to their positive at their journey with “new eyes.” It is also possible that the par-
body image. With this perception, socially comparing themselves ticipants did not remember all the aspects that were important for
to male body ideals does not necessarily generate negative feel- their positive body image development and therefore left out valu-
ings, and instead, ideals can be viewed as positive inspiration, as able information in the interviews. Future studies may benefit from
described by some of the men. It should be noted, however, that methods that aim at helping the participants remember their ado-
although the men perceived themselves as being close to the ideal lescence, for example by asking them to bring in photographs of
does not necessarily mean that they were objectively close to the themselves as adolescents or conducting several interviews over
ideal. Instead, they had ‘the feeling’ that the ideal was within reach. multiple days, to allow participants time to reflect in between.
This was also expressed by some of the participants who said that Finally, the present study is limited in that it has examined only
they would probably be able to attain the ideal if they wished to one pattern of body image development. It would be valuable for
put the time and effort into doing so. Why did the women with future research to focus on other patterns of body image develop-
a positive body image not express this view? It may be that the ment, for example, by assessing individuals who did not overcome
male body ideal is perceived as more attainable by the men than their negative body image in early adolescence, or individuals who
the female body ideal is by the women. It may also be that it is more have had a stable positive body image from early adolescence and
accepted for men in general to verbally express that they resemble onwards.
the ideal as doing so can be seen as more coherent with the mascu-
line gender role featuring characteristics such as self-reliance and
dominance (Mahalik et al., 2003). 4.2. Conclusions

4.1. Limitations The present study examined the stories of individuals who over-
came a negative body image in early adolescence and developed a
It should be noted that the participants with a positive body positive body image on their way to emerging adulthood. Graphs
image in emerging adulthood were recruited for this study based illustrating the participants’ body image development reveal a
on their high body esteem at age 24 years as measured using BESAA. range of patterns all leading to positive body image in emerg-
One may argue that high scores on BESAA, which assesses feelings ing adulthood. However, most participants had already obtained a
about appearance and weight, do not adequately capture positive positive body image by age 18 years. Thematic analyses of the par-
body image according to recent conceptualizations, but rather the ticipants’ stories demonstrated that the factors contributing to their
lack of body dissatisfaction. However, to control for this limitation, negative body image in early adolescence were that they perceived
we assessed the participants’ body appreciation (a component of themselves as having a deficient appearance, had experiences of
positive body image) at the time of the interview. Our findings negative peer influence, or experienced overall discontent and lack
indicated that, compared with mean values of body appreciation of self-esteem that extended to their body image. Three factors
previously obtained in community samples, the present partici- served as positive turning points in the participants’ body image
pants had higher body appreciation, supporting the assumption development: they found a new social context in which they felt
that the members of the sample had a positive body image at the belonging and acceptance; they developed an increased sense of
time of the interview. Several characteristics of the participants’ agency and empowerment; or they used various cognitive strate-
current positive body image were also in line with characteristics gies to improve their body image. Several of the characteristics
identified in other studies of positive body image (Tylka & Wood- that summarized the participants’ positive body image in emerg-
Barcalow, 2015b). In addition, it should be noted that using BESAA ing adulthood were in line with the findings of the few previous
was helpful in identifying participants with a negative body image qualitative studies of positive body image. However, the present
in early adolescence, and it was also the general measure of body findings add evidence of new characteristics of positive body image,
image used in the longitudinal project from which the current sam- which may differ between men and women. Namely, the women
ple was recruited. In fact, measures assessing specific components in this study were more likely to think of positive body image as
of positive body image (e.g., body appreciation) had not yet been needing constant work to maintain, and were also more likely to
developed when the longitudinal project started in the year 2000. have a feminist identity and address body image concerns by mar-
Future studies could further explore how development of body shalling feminist arguments. In contrast, the men were more likely
esteem relate to development of body appreciation over time. to engage in appearance-enhancing behaviors in order to improve
Another limitation associated with the recruitment of partic- their body shape, and were more likely to perceive their body as
ipants concerns the fact that we were stricter in our criteria for resembling the ideal. These gender differences are noteworthy con-
positive body image in emerging adulthood than in our criteria sidering that this is the first study to qualitatively examine young
for negative body image in early adolescence, considering that men’s positive body image.
we defined positive body image as having a BESAA score in the Overall, the present findings suggest that positive body image
fourth and highest quartile and negative body image as having a development can be related to adolescents’ specific body expe-
BESAA score below the median. This was chosen as the best solution riences, as well as to their life in general, general feelings of
because we wanted to ensure that the participants had a positive acceptance, belonging, agency, and empowerment. Based on these
body image in emerging adulthood while still obtaining a suffi- findings, we propose that body image interventions would bene-
ciently large sample. fit from taking a more ecological approach (Piran & Mafrici, 2012).
Another limitation worth mentioning concerns the thematic This implies developing programs and actions that not only con-
analyses and the fact that the themes identified in the analyses sider matters directly related to the physical body but also work
depended on our interpretations of the participants’ stories. Espe- with adolescents’ general sense of acceptance, belonging, agency,
cially regarding the turning points, these were not always described and empowerment. Interventions could also target the relation
as turning points by the participants, but were rather just parts of between adolescents’ bodies and their lives more broadly, for
the participants’ descriptions of their body-image journeys. Also, it example by encouraging them to reflect on how their body can
should be noted that many of the findings of this study are from help them live a valued life. In addition, the present study gives
participants’ retrospective reflections on their body image journey. hope that body image development can take positive turns late in
It is possible that these reflections were affected by the fact that adolescence.
K.H. Gattario, A. Frisén / Body Image 28 (2019) 53–65 63

Appendix A 5Jag är uppmärksam på min kropps behov


6Jag känner kärlek till min kropp
Interview topics and main questions: 7Jag uppskattar min kropps olika och unika egenskaper
8Mitt beteende visar min positiva inställning till min kropp, till
(a) General idea of current body image: exempel sträcker jag på mig och ler
• Could you please name the first three things that come to 9 Jag är bekväm i min kropp
mind when you think about your body? Why do you think 10 Jag tycker att jag är vacker även om jag inte ser ut som attraktiva
these specific things come to mind? människor (t ex modeller, skådespelare) på bilder i media
• Is there anything that you are particularly satis-
fied/dissatisfied with when it comes to your body? Appendix C
Why?
(b) Influence of family and peers on body image when growing up: To further illustrate the participants’ different body image jour-
• Could you tell me a little about the configuration of your fam- neys, the cases of Sara and Jakob are presented (for illustrated
ily/peer group when you were growing up? trajectories of their BESAA development, see Figs. 1 and 2).
• How did your family/peer group discuss aspects related to the
body?
• If your parents/peers commented on your body in any way, Sara
positively or negatively, what did they say?
• If your parents did something to make you feel better about Sara’s BESAA scores in the longitudinal research project were
2.98 (10 years), 2.80 (13 years), 2.74 (16 years), 2.71 (18 years),
your body, what did they do?
• What do you think your peers thought about how you looked 2.71 (21 years) and 3.40 (24 years). Sara’s BAS-2 score at the time
of the interview (age 26 years) was 4.00. In the interview, Sara
when you were younger?
• Do you think your parents/peers have influenced how you described being unhappy with several aspects of her body as an
early adolescent. She perceived herself to be shorter and skinnier
think and feel about your body? How?
• If you had a romantic relationship with someone when grow- than her peers, and as she entered puberty she suffered from facial
acne, which she was very preoccupied with and strove to cover
ing up, do you think that person influenced how you think
with makeup. She was also annoyed that her skin was much fairer
and feel about your body?
than her peers’ skin and that she was unable to get a tan. From a
(c) Reflections on their own picture:
• Could you please tell me what the picture features? young age, Sara was a dancer and spent much of her time in the
• Why did you choose this specific picture? dancing community. Appearance pressure was high in this context
• What is it that you like about this picture? and many of her peers suffered from eating disorders. Although Sara
• What kind of message does this picture send? did not develop an eating disorder, she clearly paid a lot of negative
• What does this picture say about you as a person? attention to her physical appearance. Several turning points were
identified in Sara’s story. For example, she described how her first
(d) Appearance ideals in the media:
boyfriend in ninth grade was a real “boost” to her body image: “I
(Participants are shown a set of typical images of appearance
had a lot of acne at the time and couldn’t for the world understand
ideals from the media).
• What do you think when you see these images? why he still wanted to be with me, even though my face was full of
• Are these examples of appearance ideals that you strive for? acne. But he wanted to, and we were so in love.” Sara also described
several experiences in relation to feelings of increased agency and
Why/why not?
• Do you usually compare yourself to such images? Why/why empowerment, and how these helped improve her body image. For
example, in terms of sexuality, she said that she was very inhibited
not?
• Do you feel down when you think about appearance ideals? when she was younger, but that as she became older she learned to
be more attuned to her body, her needs and desires. Sara also expe-
Why/why not?
• Do you have any specific strategies for coping with these rienced empowerment in relation to the physical appearance of her
body. She said that by having explored many alternative styles of
images?
clothing and makeup, she realized that she did not need certain
(e) Reflections on their own body image journey:
clothing or makeup to be comfortable in her body, and that while
(Participants are informed about their positive body image
she still occasionally enjoyed dressing up and wearing makeup, she
development at this stage of the interview).
• Why do you think you have been able to overcome a negative preferred looking natural. Sara also said that she had worked hard
to finally dare to “take up space” in the world and to have a voice
body image and develop a positive one?
• What role do you think your physical appearance has played in social contexts, by relying not on her physical appearance but on
her personality. She also described actively using various strategies
in your positive body image development?
• What advice would you give to other people who are dissat- to improve her body image, such as trying to accept her body and
not engaging with appearance ideals. At the time of the interview,
isfied with their bodies—what should they do to feel better
Sara felt integrated with her body and had come to accept the acne
about themselves?
scars on her cheeks, her short height, and her fair skin, although
she admitted that she constantly strove to maintain acceptance of
Appendix B. The Swedish BAS-2 these aspects of her body. She said that her body image varied over
the month with her menstrual cycle and that she needed to remind
Directions for participants: Var god ange om följande påståenden herself every time she felt swollen, to appreciate it as a sign that
stämmer in på dig aldrig, sällan, ibland, ofta eller alltid: her body was functioning properly. Sara had a strong feminist iden-
tity, following feminist fat activist blogs. Her stance towards beauty
1 Jag respekterar min kropp ideals in the media was critical, although she admitted a part of
2 Jag trivs med min kropp her envied such beauty. In the future, she believed it would be an
3 Jag tycker att min kropp har åtminstone några bra egenskaper important task to teach her children to be accepting of their own
4 Jag har en positiv inställning till min kropp appearance.
64 K.H. Gattario, A. Frisén / Body Image 28 (2019) 53–65

Jakob Burgess, G., Grogan, S., & Burwitz, L. (2006). Effects of a 6-week aerobic dance
intervention on body image and physical self-perceptions in adolescent girls.
Body Image, 3, 57–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.10.005
Jakob’s BESAA scores in the longitudinal research project were Davison, T. E., & McCabe, M. P. (2006). Adolescent body image and psychosocial
3.39 (10 years), 2.06 (13 years), 3.68 (16 years), 3.74 (18 years), 3.60 functioning. Journal of Social Psychology, 146, 15–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.3200/
(21 years) and 3.49 (24 years). Jakob’s BAS-2r score at the time of the SOCP.146.1.15-30
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human
interview (age 26 years) was 4.20. In the interview, Jakob said that needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11,
he was always much shorter than his peers and that, throughout 227–268. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327965PLI1104 01
early adolescence, felt as though he was always treated differently Duarte, C., Pinto-Gouveia, J., & Stubbs, R. J. (2017). The prospective associations
between bullying experiences, body image shame and disordered eating in a
because of his height. He was pushed, beaten, and excluded from
sample of adolescent girls. Personality and Individual Differences, 116, 319–325.
the peer group. Although he was comforted by his mother, who http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.05.003
said that he was fine the way he was, it was hard for him to believe Duncan, M. J., Al-Nakeeb, Y., & Nevill, A. M. (2009). Effects of a 6-week circuit
training intervention on body esteem and body mass index in British primary
her. To endure this period in his life, he tried to focus on other
school children. Body Image, 6, 216–220. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.
things, such as doing well at school. The real turning point for Jakob, 2009.04.003
however, was when he started to do weight training at the gym, Eisenberg, M. E., Neumark-Sztainer, D., & Paxton, S. J. (2006). Five-year change in
encouraged by a bet he had made with a peer. Jakob realized that body satisfaction among adolescents. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 61,
521–527. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2006.05.007
weight training was different from all the other sports he had tried Erling, A., & Hwang, C. P. (2004). Body-esteem in Swedish 10-year-old children.
and disliked, and he enjoyed seeing the fast and increasing physical Perceptual and Motor Skills, 99, 437–444. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/PMS.99.5.
results of his dedication in the gym. His new hobby gave him a new, 437-444
Feltman, C. E., & Szymanski, D. M. (2017). Instagram use and self-objectification:
central role in the peer group: “When I started doing weight train- The roles of internalization, comparison, appearance commentary, and
ing, we had something in common. Something they wanted from feminism. Sex Roles, 78, 311–324. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-017-0796-
me. ‘You’re more muscular than me, could you give some advice?’ 1
Fogelkvist, M., Parling, T., Kjellin, L., & Gustafsson, S. A. (2016). A qualitative
. . . All of a sudden, people started to listen to me . . . I was no longer analysis of participants’ reflections on body image during participation in a
picked last in physical education. Sometimes they even picked me randomized controlled trial of acceptance and commitment therapy. Journal of
first. When people start acknowledging you, that really affects you Eating Disorders, 4, 1–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s40337-016-0120-4
Frisén, A., & Holmqvist, K. (2010a). Physical, sociocultural, and behavioral factors
from within.” Jakob’s increased confidence was also a springboard
associated with body-esteem in 16-year-old Swedish boys and girls. Sex Roles,
for him to dare to get to know new people and find friends with 63, 373–385. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-010-9806
similar interests, not only related to weight training. At the time of Frisén, A., & Holmqvist, K. (2010b). What characterizes early adolescents with a
positive body image? A qualitative investigation of Swedish girls and boys.
the interview, Jakob still enjoyed weight training but said he had a
Body Image, 7, 205–212. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2010.04.001
soberer attitude to it and exercised because it was a natural part of Frisén, A., Holmqvist Gattario, K., & Lunde, C. (2014). Projekt perfekt. Om
his lifestyle. He did not put too much time into it any longer because utseendekultur och kroppsuppfattning [Project perfect. About appearance culture
“you need to have time to live a little as well.” Jakob came to accept and body image]. Stockholm, Sweden: Natur och Kultur.
Frisén, A., Lunde, C., & Berg, A. I. (2015). Developmental patterns in body esteem
his height and appreciated that his body was flexible and adapt- from late childhood to young adulthood: A growth curve analysis. The
able and could do many different things. He said that his weight European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 99–115. http://dx.doi.org/10.
training had helped him listen to his body and be more attuned to 1080/17405629.2014.951033
Gattario, K. H., & Lunde, C. (2018). Appearance-related practices: Can they be part
what it could and could not do. When exposed to muscular media of a positive body image? In E. A. Daniels, M. M. Gillen & C. H. Markey (Eds.),
images, his strategy was to think that the subject spent too much The body positive: Understanding and improving body image in science and
time training and had probably abandoned more important things practice (pp. 111–134). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Gattario, K. H., Frisén, A., Fuller-Tyszkiewicz, M., Ricciardelli, L. A., Diedrichs, P. C., &
along the way. Yager, Z., et al. (2015). How is men’s conformity to masculine norms related to
their body image? Masculinity and muscularity across Western countries.
References Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 16, 337–347. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0038494
Góngora, V. C. (2014). Satisfaction with life, well-being, and meaning in life as
Alleva, J. M., Martijn, C., Van Breukelen, G. J., Jansen, A., & Karos, K. (2015). Expand protective factors of eating disorder symptoms and body dissatisfaction in
Your Horizon: A programme that improves body image and reduces adolescents. Eating Disorders, 22, 435–449. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
self-objectification by training women to focus on body functionality. Body 10640266.2014.931765
Image, 15, 81–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.07.001 Grogan, S. (2017). Body image: Understanding body dissatisfaction in men, women
Alleva, J. M., Tylka, T. L., & Kroon Van Diest, A. M. (2017). The Functionality and children (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge.
Appreciation Scale (FAS): Development and psychometric evaluation in U.S. Halliwell, E. (2015). Future directions for positive body image research. Body
community women and men. Body Image, 23, 28–44. http://dx.doi.org/10. Image, 14, 177–189. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.03.003
1016/j.bodyim.2017.07.008 Hausenblas, H. A., & Fallon, E. A. (2006). Exercise and body image: A meta-analysis.
Andrew, R., Tiggemann, M., & Clark, L. (2016). Predictors and health-related Psychology & Health, 21, 33–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
outcomes of positive body image in adolescent girls: A prospective study. 14768320500105270
Developmental Psychology, 52, 463–474. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000095 Helfert, S., & Warschburger, P. (2011). A prospective study on the impact of peer
Augustus-Horvath, C., & Tylka, T. L. (2011). The acceptance model of intuitive and parental pressure on body dissatisfaction in adolescent girls and boys.
eating: A comparison of women in emerging adulthood, early adulthood, and Body Image, 8, 101–109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.01.004
middle adulthood. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 58, 110–125. http://dx.doi. Holmqvist, K., & Frisén, A. (2012). Ï bet they aren’t that perfect in reality:”
org/10.1037/a0022129 Appearance ideals viewed from the perspective of adolescents with a positive
Avalos, L. C., & Tylka, T. L. (2006). Exploring a model of intuitive eating with college body image. Body Image, 9, 388–395. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2012.
women. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 486–497. http://dx.doi.org/10. 03.007
1037/0022-0167.53.4.486 Kling, J., Holmqvist Gattario, K., & Frisén, A. (2017). Swedish women’s perceptions
Bird, E. L., Halliwell, E., Diedrichs, P. C., & Harcourt, D. (2013). Happy being me in of and conformity to feminine norms. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 58,
the UK: A controlled evaluation of a school-based body image intervention 238–248. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12361
with pre-adolescent children. Body Image, 10, 326–334. http://dx.doi.org/10. Lemoine, J. E., Konradsen, H., Lunde, J. A., Roland-Lévy, C., Ny, P., & Khalaf, A., et al.
1016/j.bodyim.2013.02.008 (2018). Factor structure and psychometric properties of the Body Appreciation
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Scale-2 among adolescents and young adults in Danish, Portuguese, and
Research in Psychology, 3, 77–101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1191/ Swedish. Body Image, 26, 1–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2018.04.004
1478088706qp063oa Lev-Ari, L., Baumgarten-Katz, I., & Zohar, A. H. (2014). Show me your friends, and I
Brislin, R. W. (1970). Back-translation for cross-cultural research. Journal of shall show you who you are: The way attachment and social comparisons
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 1, 185–216. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ influence body dissatisfaction. European Eating Disorders Review, 22, 463–469.
135910457000100301 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/erv.2325
Bucchianeri, M. M., Arikian, A. J., Hannan, P. J., Eisenberg, M. E., & Lunde, C., & Frisén, A. (2011). On being victimized by peers in the advent of
Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2013). Body dissatisfaction from adolescence to young adolescence: Prospective relationships to objectified body consciousness. Body
adulthood: Findings from a 10-year longitudinal study. Body Image, 10, 1–7. Image, 8, 309–314. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.04.010
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2012.09.001
K.H. Gattario, A. Frisén / Body Image 28 (2019) 53–65 65

Mahalik, J. R., Locke, B. D., Ludlow, L. H., Diemer, M. A., Scott, R. P. J., & Gottfried, M., Rubin, L. R., Nemeroff, C. J., & Russo, N. F. (2004). Exploring Feminist Women’s Body
et al. (2003). Development of the conformity to masculine norms inventory. Consciousness. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 28, 27–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.
Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 4, 3–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1524-9220. 1111/j.1471-6402.2004.00120
4.1.3 Sharpe, H., Patalay, P., Choo, T.-H., Wall, M., Mason, S. M., & Goldschmidt, A. B.,
Mahlo, L., & Tiggemann, M. (2016). Yoga and positive body image: A test of the et al. (2017). Bidirectional associations between body dissatisfaction and
embodiment model. Body Image, 18, 135–142. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. depressive symptoms from adolescence through early adulthood.
bodyim.2016.06.008 Developmental Psychopathology, 30, 1447–1458. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/
McCabe, M. P., Connaughton, C., Tatangelo, G., Mellor, D., & Busija, L. (2017). S0954579417001663
Healthy me: A gender-specific program to address body image concerns and Smith, J. A., & Osborn, M. (2003). Interpretative phenomenological analysis. In J. A.
risk factors among preadolescents. Body Image, 20, 20–30. http://dx.doi.org/10. Smith (Ed.), Qualitative psychology: A practical guide to research methods (pp.
1016/j.bodyim.2016.10.007 51–80). London, UK: Sage Publications.
Meland, E., Haugland, S., & Breidablik, H. (2007). Body image and perceived health Smolak, L. (2012). Body image development – Girl children. In T. F. Cash (Ed.),
in adolescence. Health Education Research, 22, 342–350. http://dx.doi.org/10. Encyclopedia of body image and human appearance (pp. 212–218). London, UK:
1093/her/cyl085 Academic Press.
Mendelson, B. K., Mendelson, M. J., & White, D. R. (2001). Body-Esteem Scale for Striegel-Moore, R. H., & Cachelin, F. M. (1999). Body image concerns and
adolescents and adults. Journal of Personality Assessment, 76, 90–106. http://dx. disordered eating in adolescent girls: Risk and protective factors. In N. G.
doi.org/10.1207/S15327752JPA7601 6 Johnson & M. C. Roberts (Eds.), Beyond appearance: A new look at adolescent
Menzel, J. E., Schaefer, L. M., Burke, N. L., Mayhew, L. L., Brannick, M. T., & girls (pp. 85–108). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Thompson, J. K. (2010). Appearance-related teasing, body dissatisfaction, and Swami, V., & Ng, S. (2015). Factor structure and psychometric properties of the
disordered eating: A meta-analysis. Body Image, 7, 261–270. http://dx.doi.org/ Body Appreciation Scale-2 in university students in Hong Kong. Body Image, 15,
10.1016/j.bodyim.2010.05.004 68–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.06.004
Nelson, S., Kling, J., Wängqvist, M., Frisén, A., & Syed, M. (2018). Identity and the Thompson, J. K., Coovert, M. D., & Stormer, S. M. (1999). Body image, social
body: Trajectories of body-esteem from adolescence to emerging adulthood. comparison, and eating disturbance: A covariance structure modeling
Developmental Psychology, 54, 1159–1171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ investigation. The International Journal of Eating Disorders, 26, 43–51. http://dx.
dev0000435 doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199907)26:1<43::AID-EAT6>3.0.CO;2-R
Ohring, R., Graber, J. A., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2002). Girls’ recurrent and concurrent Tiggemann, M., Coutts, E., & Clark, L. (2014). Belly dance as an embodying activity?
body dissatisfaction: Correlates and consequences over 8 years. The A test of the embodiment model of positive body image. Sex Roles, 71, 197–207.
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 31, 404–415. http://dx.doi.org/10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-014-0408-2
1002/eat.10049 Tylka, T. L., & Homan, K. J. (2015). Exercise motives and positive body image in
Peterson, R. D., Tantleff-Dunn, S., & Bedwell, J. S. (2006). The effects of exposure to physically active college women and men: Exploring an expanded acceptance
feminist ideology on women’s body image. Body Image, 3, 237–246. http://dx. model of intuitive eating. Body Image, 15, 90–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2006.05.004 bodyim.2015.07.003
Piran, N. (2016). Embodied possibilities and disruptions: The emergence of the Tylka, T. L., & Wood-Barcalow, N. (2015a). The Body Appreciation Scale-2: Item
Experience of Embodiment construct from qualitative studies with girls and refinement and psychometric evaluation. Body Image, 12, 53–67. http://dx.doi.
women. Body Image, 18, 43–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.04. org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.09.006
007 Tylka, T. L., & Wood-Barcalow, N. (2015b). What is and what is not positive body
Piran, N. (2017). Journeys of embodiment at the intersection of body and culture: The image? Conceptual foundations and construct definition. Body Image, 14,
developmental theory of embodiment. San Diego, CA: Elsevier, Academic Press. 118–129. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.04.001
Piran, N., & Mafrici, N. (2012). Preventing body image problems: Ecological and Webb, J. B., Wood-Barcalow, N., & Tylka, T. L. (2015). Assessing positive body
activism approaches. Encyclopedia of Body Image and Human Appearance, image: Contemporary approaches and future directions. Body Image, 14,
674–679. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-384925-0.00107-3 130–145. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.03.010
Piran, N., & Teall, T. (2012). The developmental theory of embodiment. In G. Westerberg-Jacobson, J., Edlund, B., & Ghaderi, A. (2010). A 5-year longitudinal
McVey, M. P. Levine, N. Piran & H. B. Ferguson (Eds.), Preventing eating-related study of the relationship between the wish to be thinner, lifestyle behaviours
and weight-related disorders: Collaborative research, advocacy, and policy change and disturbed eating in 9-20-year old girls. European Eating Disorders Review,
(pp. 171–199). Waterloo, ON: Wilfred Laurier Press. 18, 207–219. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/erv.983
Rogers, C. B., Webb, J. B., & Jafari, N. (2018). A systematic review of the roles of Wood-Barcalow, N., Tylka, T. L., & Augustus-Horvath, C. (2010). “But I like my
body image flexibility as correlate, moderator, mediator, and in intervention body”: Positive body image characteristics and a holistic model for
science (2011-2018). Body Image, 27, 43–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. young-adult women. Body Image, 7, 106–116. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
bodyim.2018.08.003 bodyim.2010.01.001

You might also like