You are on page 1of 12

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/288128002

Cognitive performance and memory complaints in chronic patients: with


fibromyalgis versus without fibromyalgia

Article in Revista de la Sociedad Española del Dolor · August 2008

CITATIONS READS

5 27

7 authors, including:

Antoni Castel Jose Sala


Institut Català de la Salut Universidad Peruana de Ciencias Aplicadas (UPC)
37 PUBLICATIONS 272 CITATIONS 13 PUBLICATIONS 246 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

M. Rull
Hospital Universitari Joan XXIII de Tarragona
85 PUBLICATIONS 721 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Doctors' professional status View project

Multidisciplinary Fibromyalgia Treatment Adapted for Women With Low Educational Levels. Marató TV3. View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Antoni Castel on 26 April 2022.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 2021, 62, 328–338 DOI: 10.1111/sjop.12706

Cognition and Neurosciences


Memory complaints and cognitive performance in fibromyalgia and
chronic pain: The key role of depression
ANTONI CASTEL1,2 
ROSALIA CASCON-PEREIRA2,3
and SERGI BOADA1
1
Pain Clinic, Hospital Universitari de Tarragona Joan XXIII, Tarragona, Spain
2
Multidimentional Pain Research Group, Institut d’Investigacio Sanit
aria Pere Virgili, Reus, Spain
3
Business Management Unit, Universitat Rovira i Virgili, Reus, Spain

Castel, A., Cascon-Pereira, R. & Boada, S. (2021). Memory complaints and cognitive performance in fibromyalgia and chronic pain: The key role of
depression. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 62, 328–338.

To explore the relationship between perceived cognitive problems and cognitive performance in three different samples, taking into account the possible
influence of depression, catastrophizing, pain intensity, or medication. Seventy individuals with fibromyalgia, 74 with non-malignant chronic pain and 40
pain-free controls, completed measures of verbal episodic memory, sustained attention, response inhibition, depression, catastrophizing, and pain intensity.
Fibromyalgia and chronic pain patients performed worse than controls in verbal memory and sustained attention, but these differences disappeared when
depressed participants were excluded from the analyses. Memory complaints were related with depression in all pain patients. However, in the case of
fibromyalgia, memory complaints were also related by pain intensity and inversely related by short-term episodic memory. This case-control study shows
the importance of jointly assessing cognitive performance and memory complaints and of controlling for variables such as depression, catastrophizing, pain
intensity and medication in the studied samples. Accordingly, this study highlights the differences in memory complaints, between the patients with
fibromyalgia and the patients with other chronic pain conditions. Finally, it has highlighted the important role played by depression in cognitive
performance and memory complaints considering the Neurocognitive Model of Attention to pain.
Key words: Fibromyalgia, chronic pain, memory complaints, verbal memory, attention, executive functions, medication.
Antoni Castel, Pain Clinic. Hospital Universitari de Tarragona Joan XXIII. Spain. Email: antonicastel.hj23.ics@gencat.cat

INTRODUCTION Sanchez et al., 2011), while others did (Bell et al., 2018; Correa, Mir
o,
Martınez, Sanchez & Lupia~nez, 2011). The evidence from studies on
In clinical settings, patients suffering from chronic pain often episodic memory remains controversial (Glass, 2009; Grace, Nielson,
reported difficulty in carrying out daily tasks involving cognitive Hopkins & Berg, 1999; Kim et al., 2012; Landrø, Stiles & Sletvold,
functions such as memory and attention. This has prompted a 1997; Leavitt & Katz, 2006; McDermott & Ebmeier, 2009; Munguia-
variety of studies to examine the cognitive performance of patients Izquierdo & Legaz-Arrese, 2007; Oosterman et al., 2011; Park, Glass,
with chronic pain (Dick & Rashiq, 2007; Esteve, Ramırez & Minear & Crofford, 2001; Suhr, 2003; Walitt, Roebuck-Spencer,
Lopez-Martınez, 2001; Landrø, Fors, V apenstad, Holthe, Stiles & Bleiberg, Foster & Weinstein, 2008).
Borchgrevink 2013; Oosterman, Derksen, Van Wijck, Veldhuijzen, Episodic memory can be understood as the ability to
& Kessels, 2011), and more specifically, with back pain (Jorge, remember specific events or episodes. In a laboratory setting,
Gerard & Level, 2009; Ling, Campbell, Heffermnan & Greenough, episodic memory is considered as the ability to remember a
2007), musculoskeletal pain (S€oderfjell, Molander, Johansson, list of words. To the present, differences in episodic memory
Barnekow-Bergkvist & Nilsson, 2006), neuropathic pain have been reported between chronic whiplash patients and
(Povedano, Gascon, Galvez, Ruiz & Rejas, 2007), or fibromyalgia control subjects (Antepohl, Kiviloog, Anderson & Gerdle,
(Bell et al., 2018; Dick, Verrier, Harker & Rashiq, 2008; Gelonch, 2003; Schmand, Lindeboom, Schagen, Heijt, Koene &
Garolera, Valls, Li & Pifarre, 2017; Gelonch, Garolera, Valls, Hamburger, 1998), or among patients with localized pain,
Rossello & Pifarre, 2016; Glass, 2006, 2008, 2009; Glass & Park, generalized pain, and neuropathic pain (Landrø et al., 2013),
2001; Kim et al., 2012; Pidal-Miranda, Gonzalez-Villar, Carrillo- but not between patients suffering from chronic pain of a
de-la-Pe~na, Andrade & Rodrıguez-Salgado, 2018; Tesio, Torta, different aetiology and patients with fibromyalgia (Suhr, 2003;
Colonna et al., 2015; Wu, Huang, Fang, Ko & Tsai, 2018). Walitt et al., 2008).
However, not all cognitive functions would be affected in the With regard to attention, current data strongly suggest the
same proportion. There is currently agreement on the existence of presence of attentional impairment in chronic pain and fibromyalgia
deficits in working memory among patients with chronic pain and patients when the tasks require high attentional demand (Harker,
fibromyalgia (Berryman, Stanton, Bowering, Tabor, McFarlane & Klein, Dick, Verrier & Rashiq, 2011). Nevertheless, contradictory
Moseley, 2013; Glass, 2009; Pidal-Miranda et al., 2018). In results emerge when sustained attention is studied. Differences are
response inhibition, a component of the executive functions found between pain-free control subjects and rheumatoid arthritis
(Berryman, Stanton, Bowering, Tabor, McFarlane & Moseley, patients (Dick, Eccleston & Crombez, 2002), or between chronic
2014), some authors found no differences between fibromyalgia pain patients under different medication conditions and control
patients and healthy controls (Cuevas-Toro, Lopez-Torrecillas, Dıaz- subjects (Sjøgren, Christrup, Petersen & Højsted, 2005), but not
Batanero & Perez-Marfıl, 2014; Glass, Williams, Fernandez- among pain-free controls, and fibromyalgia and musculoskeletal

© 2021 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Scand J Psychol 62 (2021) Memory complaints and cognitive performance 329

pain patients (Dick et al., 2002), or between chronic pain patients MATERIALS AND METHODS
and control subjects (Oosterman et al., 2011).
It is common for patients in a pain clinic to take various types Subjects
of medication, including opioids, non-steroidal analgesics, The sample used in this study consists of 70 patients with fibromyalgia
antidepressants or anticonvulsants that can interfere with cognitive (FM), diagnosed according to the American College of Rheumatology
performance. However, previous research shows not all drugs diagnostic criteria (Wolfe, Smythe, Yunus et al., 1990), 74 patients with
chronic pain without a diagnosis of fibromyalgia (CP) and 40 healthy
influence equally on cognitive functions. There is agreement on
controls (HC). As inclusive criteria, all participants were required to
the side effects of anticonvulsants in cognitive function (Loring, know reading and writing, be fluent in Spanish and be between 18 and
Marino & Meador, 2007; Mula, 2012), but as far as we know, 60 years old. The reason for the upper age limit set at 60 years old was
there are not conclusive results on the influence of antidepressants to minimize the possible interference of ageing on cognitive performance.
(Herrera-Guzman, Herrera-Abarca, Gudayol-Ferre et al., 2010; Exclusion criteria were severe psychopathology (e.g., psychosis), or
moderate to severe cognitive impairment secondary to a pathology with
Mohs, Mease, Arnold et al., 2012), non-steroidal analgesics
neuropsychological disorders. The free-pain controls were required not to
(Moriarti, McGuire & Finn, 2011) or long-term consumption of present any form of chronic disease and not to be taking regular
opioids (Chapman, Byass-Smith & Reed, 2002; Højsted et al., pharmacological treatment 188 out of the 289 subjects who were initially
2012; Jamison, Schein, Vallow, Ascher, Vorsanger, & Katz, contacted for the study (194 pain patients and 95 control subjects) were
2003; Tassian, Attal, Fletcher et al., 2003) on cognitive finally included. Regarding the pain patients, 12 out of them were
excluded for not meeting the inclusion criteria and 38 refused to
impairment. But cognitive performance in CP patients is not only
participate. With regard to the healthy controls, 37 were excluded for not
influenced by medication. Several studies evidence that pain meeting the inclusion criteria and 18 refused to participate. Therefore, the
intensity (Moriarty, McGuire, B.E. & Finn, 2011; S€oderfjell et al., initial sample consisted of 144 patients under treatment in our Pain Unit
2006) and catastrophizing (Crombez, Eccleston, Baeyens & and 44 control subjects recruited from civic centers and among hospital
Eelen, 1998; Crombez, Eccleston, Van den Broeck, Van volunteers. The pain patients were classified according to the primary
pain diagnosis recorded in their medical chart. Since the control group
Houdenhove & Goubert, 2002) can also interfere in cognitive
was younger and had a higher level of education than the pain group,
performance. four outliers from the HC group were excluded: two after considering
Memory complaints in chronic pain patients have also both age and educational level, and the other two only after considering
attracted considerable research attention (Castel, Cascon, Salvat the age in order to make the three groups homogeneous in age and
et al., 2008; Dufton, 1989; Gelonch et al., 2017; Katz, Heard, educational level. Nonetheless, differences were maintained in the
distribution by sex, since the FM group contained a higher percentage of
Mills & Leavitt, 2004; Mu~noz & Esteve, 2005; Park et al.,
2001; Schnurr & McDonald, 1995; Suhr, 2003, Walitt et al.,
2016). In this regard, although some studies indicate depressive
Table 1. Demographic and clinical data
symptoms as the best single contributor to memory complaints
in chronic pain patients (McCracken & Iverson, 2001; Pidal- Groups
Miranda et al., 2018; Roth, Geisser, Theisen-Goodvich &
Dixon, 2005), more recent findings have also demonstrated the Chronic painChronic Healthy
Fibromyalgia pain control
relevance of some cognitive functions in the perception of
Variables (N = 70) (N = 74) (N = 40)
cognitive impairment (Gelonch et al., 2016, 2017; Landrø
et al., 2013; Tesio et al., 2015). Age 46.8 (S.D. 49.0 (S.D. 7.7) 45.1 (S.D.
Despite the growing interest in these issues, there are still 8.8) 9.3)
relatively few studies which have jointly focused on both Sex
Male 6 (8.6%) 31 (41.9%) 10 (25.0%)
cognitive performance and the perception of cognitive problems
Female 64 (91.4%) 43 (58.1%) 30 (75.0%)
in patients with fibromyalgia and chronic pain from different Marital Status
aetiologies (Castel et al., 2008; Gelonch et al., 2016, 2017; Grace Single 6 (8.6%) 4 (5.5%) 4 (10.0%)
et al., 1999; Landrø et al., 2013; Park et al., 2001; Pidal-Miranda Married 58 (82.9%) 58 (79.5%) 31 (77.5%)
et al., 2018; Suhr, 2003; Tesio et al., 2015; Walitt et al., 2016). Widow 5 (7.1%) 8 (10.8%) 5 (12.5%)
Separated 1 (1.4%) 3 (4.1%) 0 (0%)
Therefore, this study attempts to address the lack of
Formal Education
comprehensive studies in this regard, by jointly focusing on Low 52 (74.3%) 51 (68.9%) 23 (57.5%)
cognitive performance and memory complaints in different Mid 12 (17.1%) 17 (23.0%) 14 (35.0%)
samples of patients and control subjects, taking into account the High 6 (8.6%) 6 (8.1%) 3 (7.5%)
possible influence of variables such as depression, catastrophizing, Taking medication
NSAIDs, % 63% 60% 0%
pain intensity or medication. In particular, it aims to accomplish
Opioids, % 70% 47% 0%
the following objectives: (1) to compare the cognitive Anticonvulsants, 23% 47% 0%
performance in verbal episodic memory, sustained attention and %
response inhibition among patients with fibromyalgia, patients Antidepressants, 80% 36% 0%
with chronic pain but without fibromyalgia and healthy controls; %
(2) to compare the perception of memory problems among these
Note: Age (mean and standard deviation); qualitative variables (number of
three samples of subjects: and (3) to determine the relationship of subjects and percentage); low education = completed their primary
memory complaints with cognitive performance, and with clinical education; mid education = completed their secondary education; high
characteristics such as depression, catastrophizing or pain education = completed higher education; NSAIDs = non-steroidal anti-
intensity. inflammatory drugs

© 2021 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
330 A. Castel et al. Scand J Psychol 62 (2021)

women. The final sample consisted of 184 participants. The mean age the same as the written color name. From these three different scores, a
was 47.3 years old (S.D. 8.6). The youngest participant was 21 years old final measure of interference is calculated.
and the oldest was 60. A total of 25.5% were men and 74.5% were
women. Subscale of Catastrophizing from the Coping Strategies Questionnaire
The FM group (70 patients) consisted of 33 patients with primary (CSQ). (Rodrıguez-Franco, Cano-Garcıa & Blanco-Picabia, 2004;
fibromyalgia and 37 patients with fibromyalgia concomitant with other Rosenstiel & Keefe, 1983). This subscale measures catastrophizing related
pathologies of chronic pain. The CP group (74 patients) consisted of 18 to pain, and is widely used in clinical settings and research contexts. The
patients with musculoskeletal pain, 43 patients with mixed neuropathic questionnaire generates one score, and has demonstrated adequate validity
pain, and 14 patients with neuropathic pain. Finally, the HC group and reliability among different pain conditions (Rodrıguez-Franco et al.,
consisted of 40 participants. The demographic and clinical characteristics 2004). The higher the score, then the higher the catastrophizing is.
of the various groups are shown in Table 1.
Numerical Pain Intensity Rating Scale. The patient indicates the
Measuring instruments maximum, minimum and usual intensities of pain suffered in the last week
using a numerical scale with values ranging from 0 to 10. A value of 0
Memory Failures in Everyday Memory (MFE) (Garcia-Martınez & indicates “no pain,” while a value of 10 indicates “the maximum pain
S
anchez-Canovas, 1994; Sunderland, Harris & Cleave, 1984). It possible.” The average of these three scores was obtained and used as a
evaluates the extent to which subjects perceive that their memory is measure of retrospective pain intensity. This procedure has demonstrated a
affected. It consists of 28 items which ask about the frequency with which high degree of reliability as a measure of the pain suffered during a
a particular memory failure has occurred in the three months prior to the particular period of time (Dworkin & Siefried, 1994; Jensen, Turner,
administration of the test. The test generates one total score, being the Romano & Fisher, 1999).
higher the score, the higher the perceived memory impairment. The
questionnaire demonstrated good psychometric properties (Cornish, 2000;
Sunderland, Harris & Gleave, 1984). Procedure
Before being included in the study, the medical chart of the patients was
Subscale of Depression from the Hospital Anxiety and Depression assessed and participants were individually interviewed by a clinical
Scale (HADS). (Tejero, Guimera, Farre & Peri, 1986; Zigmond & psychologist in order to determine the presence of possible
Snaith, 1983). The complete scale was designed for detection and psychopathological exclusion criteria. After, they had signed the informed
assessment of anxiety and depression in the setting of Hospital and consent form. Participants were tested individually. Tests were given in
Surgical Clinics. The subscale of depression consists of seven items with the following order: (1) the Memory Failures in Everyday Memory
four possible responses which are scored with values between 0 and 3. (MFE); (2) the subscale of Catastrophizing from the Coping Strategies
Due to its optimal sensitivity and specificity, a cutoff of ≥ 9 inHADS Questionnaire (CSQ); (3) the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale
depression score was considered as indicator of depression (Bejland, Dahl, (HADS); (4) the first part of the TAVEC (five learning trials for list A,
Haug & Neckelmann, 2002). immediately followed by learning the interference list – list B – and then
free recall of list A); (5) the Toulouse – Pieron Perceptual and Attention
Test de Aprendizaje Verbal Espa~na-Complutense (TAVEC): Test (TP); (6) the Stroop Color-Word Test (SCWT); and (7) exactly
Verbal Learning Test. (Benedet & Alejandre, 1998). This test is 20 minutes after the first part of the TAVEC test had ended, the second
based on the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) and on the part was applied, which consisted of long-delay free recall (list A) and the
California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT), and it measures verbal subtest of long-delay recognition. The estimated time to complete all the
memory and learning capacity. TAVEC consists of three lists of words exploration was about 90 minutes.
that are presented as “shopping lists”: a learning list that is repeated
five times (list A), an interference list (list B) and a recognition list.
Statistical analysis
Lists A and B have 16 words each classified in four semantic To explore the homogeneity of the various groups of subjects in terms of
categories. The recognition list consists of 44 words, among which the sex and educational level, chi-square analyses were carried out. The
words from list A are included. homogeneity of the three groups in terms of age was also determined
From all the available indices that TAVEC provides, we selected: (1) using ANOVA. To achieve our first and second goals, that is, to compare
Level of Immediate Recall: Immediate Recall Total, Immediate Recall the cognitive performance in verbal episodic memory, response inhibition
Interference List; (2) Level of Delayed Recall: Short-Delay Free Recall, and sustained attention, and to compare the memory complaints among the
Long-Delay Free Recall; (3) Recall Errors: Total Repetitions, Free Recall three groups of participants, ANOVA analysis was carried out. Given the
Total Intrusions; and (4) Delayed Recognition Trials: Discriminability unequal samples size, homogeneity of variances was tested and post hoc
Index, Total False Positives, Total Recognition. The selection of indices analyses were adjusted with Tukey’s method whenever necessary. Also,
was made considering their relevance in determining the cognitive Cohen’s d effect size was determined by calculating the mean differences
performance. between two groups and then dividing the difference by the pooled
standard deviation (Cohen, 1988). Later on, in order to control the
Toulouse–Pieron Perceptual and Attention Test (TP). (Toulouse & possible influence of depression on memory complaints and on cognitive
Pieron, 1998). It measures sustained attention. The test has 1,600 performance, given the different mean and distribution of the groups, we
graphic elements distributed in 40 rows and the task consists in followed the procedure of considering only participants without depression
indicating the squares that are identical to the two models presented, (Miller & Chapman, 2001). In particular, we compared the performance of
with a limited time of 10 minutes. Although the test provides a single the different groups in this new condition. Also, to control for the possible
Total Score, a Number of Errors and Omissions score can also be influence of medication on cognitive performance, patients taking
obtained. anticonvulsants were excluded, and new analyses were carried out without
considering them.
Stroop Color-Word Test (SCWT). (Golden, 1978, 2001). This Later on, to examine the association between memory complaints, and
instrument is a commonly used measure of executive functions (Berryman pain intensity, catastrophizing, depression and cognitive performance,
et al., 2013). The test consists in performing three different tasks. The first bivariate correlations were calculated by separate in each group of
task consists in reading different color names (red, green, and blue) participants. Finally, multiple linear regression analyses were carried out
printed in black ink. The second task requires naming the color (red, separately for each group of participants. Selected variables were depression,
green, and blue) in which different spots are printed. Finally, there is the catastrophizing, pain intensity, and the cognitive performance variables that
interference task. In this, the participant is required to name the color of correlated with a significance ≤ 0.01 with memory complaints. Variables
the ink in which the color words are printed. The color of the ink is never were introduced according with the stepwise regression method.

© 2021 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Table 2. Subjective variables and neuropsychological performance measures: Fibromyalgia, Chronic Pain and Healthy Controls (mean, standard deviation, signification). All participants, and participants with
HADS-D < 9

groups

Fibromyalgia Chronic painChronic pain Healthy controls


Scand J Psychol 62 (2021)

All HADS-D < 9 All HADS-D < 9 All HADS-D < 9


Measures (N = 70) (N = 25) (N = 74) (N = 47) (N = 40) (N = 39)

Pain Intensity 6.1  1.8 5.2  1.8 5.8  1.9 (d2 = 0.162) 5.6  2.1 (d2 = 0.204) – –
MFE Test 99.1  43.2 ***,‡ (d1 = 1.514) 75.8  30.6*** (d1 = 1.058) 78.3  44.9††† (d2 = 0.472) 64.0  37.2 (d2 = 0.346) 49.3  17.2 (d3 = 0.852) 49.5  17.3 (d3 = 0.499)
HADS-D 9.4  4.8*** ‡‡ (d1 = 1.897) 4.0  2.5** (d1 = 0.964) 6.9  4.8††† (d2 = 0.520) 3.7  2.5††† (d2 = 0.120) 2.2  2.4 (d3 = 1.238) 1.9  1.8 (d3 = 0.826)
Catastrophizing (CSQ) 18.9  8.7 14.6  8.7 18.5  9.5 (d2 = 0.043) 14.6  9.1 (d2 = 0.0) – –
TAVEC Immediate Recall:
Total 48.7  10.4* (d1 = 0.500) 52.5  9.4 (d1 = 0.077) 47.6  10.4†† (d2 = 0.105) 48.4  10.9 (d2 = 0.488) 53.5  8.7 (d3 = 0.615) 53.2  8.6 (d3 = 0.488)
Interference list 5.7  2.1 (d1 = 0.0) 5.4  2.2 (d1 = 0.020) 5.3  2.0 (d2 = 0.195) 5.6  2.2 (d2 = 0.090) 5.7  2.1 (d3 = 0.195) 5.7  2.1 (d3 = 0.006)
TAVEC Delayed Recall:
Short-delay free- 10.3  2.9 (d1 = 0.315) 11.6  1.9 (d1 = 0.257) 10.2  3.1 (d2 = 0.033) 10.5  3.1 (d2 = 0.427) 11.2  2.8 (d3 = 0.338) 11.0  2.7 (d3 = 0.172)
recall
Long-delay free-recall 10.8  3.0 (d1 = 0.398) 12.0  2.1 (d1 = 0. = 0.086) 10.6  3.5 (d2 = 0.061) 10.8  3.2 (d2 = 0.443) 11.9  2.5 (d3 = 0.427) 11.8  2.5 (d3 = 0.348)
TAVEC Recall Errors:

© 2021 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Total repetitions 6.4  4.8 (d1 = 0.140) 7.5  5.5 (d1 = 0.032) 6.0  5.6 (d2 = 0.197) 5.3  5.0 (d2 = 0.418) 7.2  6.5 (d3 = 0.076) 7.3  6.6 (d3 = 0.341)
Free recall total- 3.3  3.5* (d1 = 0.561) 2.2  3.1 (d1 = 0.153) 3.5  3.8†† (d2 = 0.054) 3.2  3.5 (d2 = 0.302) 1.7  2.0 (d3 = 0.592) 1.8  2.0 (d3 = 0.491)
intrusions
TAVEC Delayed Recognition Trials:
Discriminability 92.8  7.7 (d1 = 0.201) 96.7  3.1‡‡ (d1 = 0.467) 92.5  7.1 (d2 = 0.040) 93.4  5.7 (d2 = 0.719) 95.1  4.9 (d3 = 0.163) 95.0  4.1 (d3 = 0.322)
index
Total false positives 1.5  1.9 (d1 = 0.193) 0.96  1.1 (d1 = 0.218) 1.6  2.4 (d2 = 0.046) 1.4  1.3 (d2 = 0.365) 1.2  1.1 (d3 = 0.214) 1.2  1.1 (d3 = 0.166)
Total recognition 14.4  2.2 (d1 = 0.276) 15.5  0.8 (d1 = 0.555) 14.4  2.1 (d2 = 0.0) 14.5  1.8‡ (d2 = 0.717) 14.9  1.3 (d3 = 0.286) 14.9  1.3 (d3 = 0.254)
TP Test:
Total score 135.4  66.2** (d1 = 0.661) 151.7  68.3 (d1 = 0.406) 135.9  73.4†† (d2 = 0.007) 153.0  61.7 (d2 = 0.019) 184.6  81.8 (d3 = 0.620) 182.3  81.6 (d3 = 0.405)
Numbr of errors and 42.7  29.8 (d1 = 0.013) 41.3  30.1 (d1 = 0.015) 46.2  45.1 (d2 = 0.091) 38.8  30.8 (d2 = 0.082) 42.2  44.0 (d3 = 0.089) 40.8  35. (d3 = 0.059)
omissions
Stroop Color-Word Test:
Interference 0.37  9.8 (d1 = 0.082) 1.0  10.7 (d1 = 0.009) 0.1  9.6 (d2 = 0.027) 0.5  10.8 (d2 = 0.046) 1.2  10.4 (d3 = 0.109) 11  10.5 (d3 = 0.055)

Note: Mean differences were analyzed by ANOVA and post hoc analyses were corrected with Tukey’s method.
MFE: Memory Failures in Everyday Memory Test; CSQ: Coping Strategies Questionnaire; HADS-D: Subscale of Depression from the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; TAVEC: Verbal Learning Test; TP:
Toulouse - Pieron Perceptual and Attention Test.
Significance between fibromyalgia and healthy controls: *p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Significance between chronic pain and healthy controls: † p <.05; †† p < 0.01; ††† p < 0.001.
Significance between fibromyalgia and chronic pain patients: ‡ p < 0.05; ‡‡ p < 0.01.
d = Cohen’s Standardized Difference: d = 0.2 – 0.5 small; d = 0.5 – 0.8 medium; d > 0.8 large.
d1 = Cohen’s d between FM and HC; d2 = Cohen’s d between CP and FM; d3 = Cohen’s d between HC and CP.
Memory complaints and cognitive performance 331
332 A. Castel et al. Scand J Psychol 62 (2021)

RESULTS Medication and neuropsychological performance


Given the huge variety of medication that the patients took, we
Subjective symptoms: memory complaints, pain intensity, decided to group the medicines into four categories (see Table 1),
catastrophizing and depression without considering neither the dose nor the specific compounds. In
ANOVA showed differences among the groups in terms of memory order to assess the differences among the groups taking into account
complaints [F (2, 181) = 19.952; p < 0.001] and depression [F (2, the interference of anticonvulsants in cognitive performance (Loring
181) = 33.404; p < 0.001]. No differences were found between et al., 2007; Mula, 2012), we performed a new comparative analysis
FM and CP in pain intensity or catastrophizing. Post hoc analyses excluding the participants who took anticonvulsants.
showed that FM and CP patients evidenced more memory After participants taking anticonvulsants were excluded,
complaints and more depression) than HC participants. When both ANOVA showed differences among the groups in terms of
groups of patients (FM and CP) were compared, FM participants memory complaints [F (2, 130) = 20,690; p <0.001], depression
presented more memory complaints and more depression than CP [F (2, 130) = 31.276; p < 0.001] and TP Total Score [F (2,
(see Table 2). When looking in depth at the FM group, no 130) = 5.513; p < 0.01]. Post hoc analyses showed that FM
significant differences were found in any of these subjective patients evidenced more memory complaints, more depression,
symptoms between patients with primary fibromyalgia and patients and worse performance in TP Total Score than HC participants.
with fibromyalgia concomitant with other chronic pain pathologies. Also, FM patients exhibited more memory complaints and more
To correct the possible effect of depression on memory depression than CP patients. Overall, CP patients had more
complaints, a new analysis was carried out excluding the memory complaints, more depression and worse performance in
participants with depression. In this case, only the 111 TP Total Score than HC (for more details see Table 3).
participants with a cutoff of < 9 in the HADS depression subscale Finally, when depressed participants were also excluded,
were considered. ANOVA showed differences in memory ANOVA analysis showed differences among the groups in terms of
complaints [F (2, 108) = 6.0699; p < 0.01] and depression [F (2, memory complaints [F (2, 83) = 5,643; p < 0.01] and depression [F
108) = 8.731; p < 0.001] among the groups. Despite the (2, 83) = 7.281; p < 0.001]. Post hoc analyses showed that only FM
restriction limiting participants with depression, post hoc analyses patients showed more memory complaints and more depression than
evidenced that FM and CP patients scored higher than HC in the HC participants (for more details see Table 3).
subscale of depression. Moreover, FM participants expressed
more memory complaints than HC participants (see Table 2). Memory complaints and its relation with cognitive performance
and clinical characteristics
Performance in neuropsychological tests
The Pearson correlation indicated that memory complaints
Post hoc analyses showed that the participants from the FM group correlated significantly with depression and with catastrophizing
performed worse than the HC group participants in Immediate in both groups of pain patients, but not in the HC group (see
Recall Total, Free Recall Total Intrusions, and TP Total Score. Table 4). Nonetheless, differences emerged in the variables
Also, participants of the CP group performed worse than the HC correlating with memory complaints when comparing both groups
group participants in Immediate Recall Total, Free Recall Total of pain patients. In particular, while in FM patients the correlation
Intrusions, and TP Total Score (see Table 2). No differences were of memory complains was negatively related with recall and
found between FM and CP, or between patients with primary recognition, in CP patients it was negatively related with
fibromyalgia and patients with fibromyalgia concomitant with sustained attention (see Table 4).
other chronic pain pathologies. Furthermore, in the CP group, no When regression analyses were carried out, in the FM group
differences were found between patients with musculoskeletal depression entered first (b = 0.522, D R2 = 0.273, F change [1,
pain and patients with neuropathic pain. 62] = 23.282, p < 0.0001), followed respectively by Short-Delay
In addition, when participants with depression (HADS Free Recall (b = 0.338, D R2 = 0.104, F change [1, 61] = 10.149,
depression subscale ≥ 9) were removed from the analysis, the p < 0.01), and pain intensity (b = 0.362, D R2 = 0.106, F change
differences described above disappeared. Nevertheless, new [1, 60] = 12.283, p < 0.01). In the CP group only depression
differences appeared, in this case between FM and CP entered in the model (b = 0.483, D R2 = 0.236, F change [1,
participants. FM patients performed worse than CP patients in 69] = 21.341, p < 0.0001). Finally, in the HC group none of the
Discriminability Index and better than CP patients in Total included predictor variables was significant.
Recognition (see Table 2). When only the participants without depression were
The Pearson correlation indicated the lack of correlation of both considered, regression analysis showed that pain intensity was a
pain intensity and catastrophizing with cognitive performance, both significant predictor of memory complaints in the FM group
in FM and CP groups regardless of the level of depression. (b = 0.562, D R2 = 0.316, F change [1, 23] = 10.640, p < 0.01).
Finally, to avoid the probability of insufficient effort with the In the other two groups, none variable was predictive.
TAVEC, patients with a score ≤ 72 in the Discriminability Index
were excluded (Vilar-Lopez, Aparicio, Gomez-Rıo & Perez- DISCUSSION
Garcıa, 2013). With this cutoff three patients were removed, two
This case-control study has explored the relationship between
from the FM group and one from the CP group. After eliminating
perceived cognitive problems and cognitive performance (verbal
these participants from the analyses, no changes were observed in
episodic memory, sustained attention, and response inhibition)
the reported results.

© 2021 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Table 3. Subjective variables and neuropsychological performance measures: Fibromyalgia, Chronic Pain and Healthy Controls (mean, standard deviation, signification). All participants, and participants with
HADS-D < 9, excluded patients taking anticonvulsants

groups
Scand J Psychol 62 (2021)

Fibromyalgia Chronic pain


Excluded: Patients taking anticonvulsants Excluded: Patients taking anticonvulsants Healthy controls

All HADS-D < 9 All HADS-D < 9 All HADS-D < 9


Measures (N = 54) (N = 20) (N = 39) (N = 27) (N = 40) (N = 39)

Pain Intensity 6.0  1.9 5.1  2.0 5.5  2.1 (d2 = 0.249) 5.4  2.3 (d2 = 0.139) – –
MFE Test 96.8  40.7*** ‡ (d1 = 1.520) 76.2  31.0** (d1 = 2.148) 71.0  41.6† (d2 = 2.179) 62.1  39.9 (d2 = 0.498) 49.3  17.2 (d3 = 1.325) 49.5  17.3 (d3 = 0.409)
HADS-D 9.0  4.6*** ‡‡ (d1 = 1.853) 4.1  2.6** (d1 = 0.983) 5.8  4.7††† (d2 = 0.688) 3.0  2.1 (d2 = 0.465) 2.2  2.4 (d3 = 0.964) 1.9  1.8 (d3 = 0.562)
Catastrophizing (CSQ) 17.9  8.8 14.1  8.9 16.7  10.5 (d2 = 0.123) 12.0  8.3 (d2 = 0.244) – –
TAVEC Immediate Recall:
Total 50.3  9.9 (d1 = 0.343) 52.7  10.2 (d1 = 0.053) 49.5  9.9 (d2 = 0.080) 48.9  11.0 (d2 = 0.523) 53.5  8.7 (d3 = 0.429) 53.2  8.6 (d3 = 0.701)
Interference list 6.0  2.2 (d1 = 0.139) 5.5  2.3 (d1 = 0.090) 5.5  2.1 (d2 = 0.232) 5.9  2.1 (d2 = 0.181) 5.7  2.1 (d3 = 0.095) 5.7  2.1 (d3 = 0.095)
TAVEC Delayed Recall:
Short-delay free recall 10.7  2.8 (d1 = 0.178) 11.8  1.9 (d1 = 0.342) 10.9  2.8 (d2 = 0.071) 10.7  3.1 (d2 = 0.427) 11.2  2.8 (d3 = 0.107) 11.0  2.7 (d3 = 0.103)
Long-delay free recall 11.2  2.7 (d1 = 0.269) 12.2  2.2 (d1 = 0.169) 10.9  3.2 (d2 = 0.101) 11.0  3.3 (d2 = 0.427) 11.9  2.5 (d3 = 0.348) 11.8  2.5 (d3 = 0.273)
TAVEC Recall Errors:

© 2021 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Total repetitions 6.7  5 (d1 = 0.086) 8.3  5.6 (d1 = 0.163) 6.3  4.9 (d2 = 0.114) 5.2  3.6 (d2 = 0.658) 7.2  6.5 (d3 = 0.156) 7.3  6.6 (d3 = 0.395)
Free recall total intrusions 3.0  3.5 (d1 = 0.456) 1.8  3.1 (d1 = 0) 3.5  3.7† (d2 = 0.138) 2.8  3.5 (d2 = 0.302) 1.7  2.0 (d3 = 0.605) 1.8  2.0 (d3 = 0.350)
TAVEC Delayed Recognition Trials:
Discriminability index 93.8  5.7 (d1 = 0.259) 96.8  3.4 (d1 = 0.477) 94.0  5.6 (d2 = 0.035) 94.0  6 (d2 = 0.574) 95.1  4.2 (d3 = 0.222) 95.0  4.1 (d3 = 0.194)
Total false positives 1.4  1.7 (d1 = 0.139) 1.0  1.2 (d1 = 0.173) 1.3  1.1 (d2 = 0.069) 1.3  1.3 (d2 = 0.239) 1.2  1.1 (d3 = 0.090) 1.2  1.1 (d3 = 0.083)
Total recognition 14.7  1.7 (d1 = 0.132) 15.5  0.8 (d1 = 0.555) 14.7  2.1 (d2 = 0) 14.6  2.0 (d2 = 0.590) 14.9  1.3 (d3 = 0.114) 14.9  1.3 (d3 = 0.177)
TP Test:
= 0.548)
Total score 144.4  63.6* (d1 155.8  69.2 (d1 = 0.350) 130.3  85†† (d2 = 0.187) 151.0  61.9 (d2 = 0.073) 184.6  81.8 182.3  81.6 (d3 = 0.432)
(d3 = 0.650)
Number of errors and 37.7  25.2 (d1 = 0.125) 36.4  28 (d1 = 0.138) 53.2  57.3 (d2 = 0.350) 39.8  33.6 (d2 = 0.109) 42.2  44.0 (d3 = 0.215) 40.8  35.0 (d3 = 0.029)
omissions
Stroop Color-Word Test
Interference 1.2  8.9 (d1 = 0) 1.0  8.7 (d1 = 0.090) 0.8  9.3 (d2 = 0.101) 0.3  10.6 (d2 = 0.020) 1.2  10.4 (d3 = 0.139) 1.1  10.5 (d3 = 0.018)

Notes: Mean differences were analyzed by ANOVA and post hoc analyses were corrected with Tukey’s method.
MFE: Memory Failures in Everyday Memory Test; CSQ: Coping Strategies Questionnaire; HADS-D: Subscale of Depression from the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; TAVEC: Verbal Learning Test; TP:
Toulouse - Pieron Perceptual and Attention Test.
Significance between fibromyalgia and healthy controls: * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Significance between chronic pain and healthy controls: † p <.05; †† p < 0.01; ††† p < 0.001.
Significance between fibromyalgia and chronic pain patients: ‡ p < 0.05; ‡‡ p < 0.01.
d = Cohen’s Standardized Difference: d = 0.2 – 0.5 small; d = 0.5 – 0.8 medium; d > 0.8 large.
d1 = Cohen’s d between FM and HC; d2 = Cohen’s d between CP and FM; d3 = Cohen’s d between HC and CP.
Memory complaints and cognitive performance 333
334 A. Castel et al. Scand J Psychol 62 (2021)

Table 4. Person’s correlation coefficients between memory complaints and clinical characteristics, and between memory complaints and cognitive
performance, separately for each group of participants (Fibromyalgia, Chronic Pain and Healthy Controls). All participants, and participants with HADS-
D<9

groups

Fibromyalgia Chronic pain Healthy controls

All HADS-D < 9 All HADS-D < 9 All HADS-D < 9


Measures (N = 70) (N = 25) (N = 74) (N = 47) (N = 40) (N = 39)

Pain Intensity r = 0.545*** r = 0.562*** r = 0.180 r = 0.114 – –


HADS-D r = 0.517*** r = 0.312 r = 0.493*** r = 0.095 r= 0.066 r= 0.026
Catastrophizing (CSQ) r = 0.386** r = 0.398* r = 0.442*** r = 0.257 – –
TAVEC Immediate Recall
Total r= 0.436*** r = 0.064 r= 0.073 r = 0.036 r= 0.179 r = 0.180
Interference list r= 0.252* r = 0.167 r= 0.074 r = 0.082 r= 0.179 r = 0.183
TAVEC Delayed Recall
Short-delay free recall r= 0.454*** r = 0.082 r= 0.074 r = 0.089 r= 0.254 r= 0.283
Long-delay free recall r= 0.449*** r = 0.124 r= 0.046 r = 0.091 r= 0.309 r= 0.263
TAVEC Recall Errors:
Total repetitions r = 0.184 r= 0.083 r= 0.084 r = 0.004 r = 0.131 r = 0.114
Free recall total intrusions r = 0.123 r= 0.347 r= 0.084 r = 0.061 r = 0.187 r = 0.270
TAVEC Delayed Recognition Trials:
Discriminability index r = 0.468*** r = 0.210 r = 0.153 r = 0.088 r = 0.459** r = 0.296
Total false positivesTotal r = 0.196 r = 0.014 r = 0.181 r = 0.093 r = 0.277 r = 0.324*
Total recognition r = 0.483*** r = 0.306 r = 0.051 r = 0.016 r = 0.112 r = 0.130
TP Test:
Total score r = 0.130 r = 0.082 r = 0.326** r = 0.177 r = 0.236 r= 0.232
Number of errors and omissions r = 0.113 r = 0.155 r = 0.240* r = 0.049 r = 0.027 r= 0.012
Stroop Color-Word Test:
Interference r= 0.006 r= 0.143 r = 0.081 r = 0.055 r= 0.292 r= 0.290

Notes: HADS-D: Subscale of Depression from the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; CSQ: Coping Strategies Questionnaire; TAVEC: Verbal
Learning Test; TP: Toulouse - Pieron Perceptual and Attention Test.
Significance of the correlation: * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.

among patients with fibromyalgia, patients with chronic pain findings of Kim and colleagues (2012) in FM patients that used
without fibromyalgia and healthy controls, taking into account the the RAVLT as a measure instrument of verbal episodic memory.
possible influence of variables such as depression, catastrophizing, However, our findings are opposed to other studies that found
pain intensity, or medication. differences between patients with FM and healthy controls
The most important findings from this study are that: (1) FM (Munguia-Izquierdo & Legaz-Arrese 2007; Tesio et al., 2015).
and CP patients performed worse than controls in immediate Nonetheless, in these latter studies the effect of depression was
recall, free recall intrusions and sustained attention. However, not controlled.
when participants with depression were removed from the We obtained a similar result in sustained attention. In
analysis, these differences disappeared. (2) When participants particular, we found differences between patients and healthy
taking anticonvulsants were removed from the analysis, FM and controls when depression was not considered but these differences
CP patients still performed worse than HC in sustained attention. disappeared once depression was considered. These results
Nevertheless, when participants with depression were also support those of Dick, Eccleston and Crombez (2002), who found
removed from the analysis, these differences disappeared. (3) no differences in sustained attention between pain-free controls
Patients with FM expressed more memory complaints than CP and fibromyalgia patients with the Test of Everyday Attention
and HC participants. Even after eliminating the participants taking (TEA), and are also consistent with the study of Oosterman and
anticonvulsants and with depression from the analysis, the FM colleagues (2011), who assessed sustained attention with the
patients continued expressing more memory complaints than HC. Bourdon–Vos test after controlling for depression. Our work also
(4) Memory complaints were directly predicted by depression in adds to the evidence provided by other studies (C^ ote &
FM and CP. Moreover, memory complaints of FM patients were Moldofsky, 1997) on the lack of differences between patients and
also directly predicted by pain intensity and inversely by verbal healthy controls with regard to the precision with which they
episodic memory. carry out attentional tasks. Also, our study adds evidence about
We have structured our discussion regarding the above- the lack of differences between pain patients and controls in
mentioned main findings. Firstly, in relation to the performance in response inhibition measured with the Stroop Color-Word Test
verbal episodic memory our findings indicate that fibromyalgia (Cuevas-Toro et al., 2014; Oosterman, Derksen, Van Wijck,
and chronic pain patients were impaired in comparison to healthy Kessels & Veldhuijzen, 2012; Suhr, 2003).
controls, although these differences disappeared after eliminating All in all, and in line with the Neurocognitive Model of
participants with depression. This result is in line with the Attention to pain (Legrain et al., 2009), our results suggest that,

© 2021 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Scand J Psychol 62 (2021) Memory complaints and cognitive performance 335

depression has a role in explaining cognitive performance in CP with other studies (Gelonch et al. 2016, 2017; McCracken &
and FM patients. According to this model, the cognitive and Iverson, 2001; Roth et al., 2005). However, depression was not the
emotional response to pain may impair top-down attentional only subjective symptom related with memory complaints. In fact,
control mechanisms which filter out task-irrelevant stimuli catastrophizing in FM and CP (Mu~ noz & Esteve, 2005) and pain
resulting in impaired task performance. In this regard and intensity in FM (Grace et al., 1999; Suhr, 2003) proved to be
according to our results, depression might have interfered top- significant in our work as in the mentioned. With regard to the
down attentional control mechanisms and in consequence, relationship between memory complaints and cognitive
cognitive performance in CP and FM patients. Accordingly, we performance, our study showed interesting findings. According to
suggest that previous treatment of depression might improve previous research in FM, there is an inverse relationship with verbal
cognitive performance as the studies of Herrera-Guzman et al. episodic memory (Glass, Park, Minear & Crofford, 2005; Tesio
(2010) with antidepressants in patients with depression evidence. et al., 2015). In CP, the inverse relationship has been found with
The Neurocognitive Model of Attention to pain (Legrain et al., sustained attention (Landrø et al., 2013). When multiple linear
2009, p. 231) also “predicts that when we try to discard attention regression analysis was performed interesting results appeared.
from pain, a nociceptive stimulus can still capture attention in two Depression explained about 30% of the variance of memory
ways: (1) when it is salient enough and (2) when it shares one of complaints in both groups of patients, although differences appeared
the perceptual features defined by the attentional set” in a bottom- between them. In particular, in FM, verbal episodic memory and
up capture process of attention by pain. In relation to this, our pain intensity emerged as predictors of memory complaints,
results have not evidenced that pain intensity is related with lower explaining each one about 10% of the variance. On the contrary, in
performance of attentional processes as other studies have done CP, neither of the commented variables appeared as predictors.
(see Moriarty et al., 2011; S€oderfjell et al., 2006). This Nonetheless, if memory complaints in daily tasks of FM patients are
discrepancy might be explained when considering the also related with verbal episodic memory and pain intensity, an
characteristics of our study. The Neurocognitive Model of explanation of this fact can be due to the bottom-up attentional
attention to pain considers that attention is unintentionally control mechanisms suggested by Legrain et al.’s (2009) model and
captured by pain when it is intense, novel and threatening. These the interference of pain in attentional processes. If we consider that
pain features are not present in our CP and FM patients as we daily tasks are less cognitive demanding than laboratory tasks,
further explain. First, the pain experienced by our patients was attention would be more likely captured by non-task related stimuli
not novel as it uses to be in the case of experimental or acute such as pain. From this perspective, FM patients are more sensitive
pain. Secondly, the intensity of pain was assessed in a to this interference that CP patients, and this interference cannot be
retrospective way in our study (a numerical scale of the pain clearly detected by laboratory tests.
suffered in the last week). And finally, the task to perform was In conclusion, our results support the existing evidence about the
highly demanding which might diminish the possibility of influence of depression on memory complaints, but they also alert
attentional capture by task-irrelevant stimuli. All in all, we can about the possible differences depending on the chronic pain
conclude that the characteristics of pain did not act as a bottom- diagnosis. Specifically in FM, although the performance in Short-
up capture process of attention by pain in our study. By contrast, Delay Free Recall was not lower than in the other groups, their
depression interfered top-down attentional control mechanisms. memory complaints did have a significant inverse relationship with
Nevertheless, given the amount of medication that clinic pain the performance in this cognitive function. This finding suggests a
patients usually take, our study also tried to avoid the possible possible attentional bias in which difficulties or errors are
confusion effect of medication in cognitive performance, and in emphasized. In this regard, it would be important for future research
doing so, to contribute to extant research. Therefore, we decided to determine the significance of variables such as generalized
to compare cognitive performance among the groups excluding hypervigilance (Grisard, Van der Linden & Masquelier, 2002;
participants who took anticonvulsants, given their recognized McDermid, Rollman & McCain, 1996) or self-efficacy (Glass et al.,
effects on cognitive functions (Loring et al., 2007; Mula, 2012). 2005) in this finding.
After doing this, differences in memory performance disappeared, Last but not least, this study has some limitations that should
but differences in sustained attention did not. However, these be pointed out. Although the samples were homogeneous as far as
differences disappeared after excluding depressed patients. This age and level of education were concerned, there were gender
finding suggests that although medication can influence differences. In particular, the FM group had a higher percentage
differently on cognitive performance, its influence is probably of women. Nevertheless, we do not believe this to be a
lower than the influence of depression itself. determining factor since gender did not prove to be a predictive
Secondly, as far as memory complaints are concerned, interesting variable of the cognitive performance of our sample. Other factors
findings emerged about the level of memory complaints and about we have not considered in our study are: the heterogeneity of the
the variables related with them. Our results confirm the existing diagnoses of the CP group; malingering (Bar-On-Kalfon, Gal,
findings in the literature; that is to say, patients with fibromyalgia Shorer & Ablin, 2016; Etherton, Bianchini, Ciota, Heinly &
express more memory complaints than healthy controls (Grace et al., Greve, 2006; Gervais et al., 2001; Suhr, 2003) and sleep quality
1999; Park et al., 2001; Suhr, 2003), even after the participants with (Grace et al., 1999; Oosterman et al., 2013). Although the
depression were removed from the analysis, or when the effect of heterogeneity of the CP group might be considered as a limitation
medication was considered. of this study, it is not determinant since there were not significant
Our data also indicated that in both groups of patients, memory differences among the CP subgroups once factors of correction
complaints were significantly related with depression, in agreement were introduced. As for malingering, it does not seem to have

© 2021 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
336 A. Castel et al. Scand J Psychol 62 (2021)

been a relevant factor in this study because only three patients Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences
showed insufficient effort according to Vilar-Lopez and (2nd Ed.) Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.
Cornish, I. M. (2000). Factor structure of the of the everyday memory
colleagues (2013) criteria and after having removed them, our
questionnaire. British Journal of Psychology, 91, 427–438.
results did not change. As for the latter, although sleep quality is a Correa, A., Mir o, E., Martınez, M. P., Sanchez, A. I. & Lupia~ nez, J.
variable that can affect episodic memory, contradictory findings (2011). Temporal preparation and inhibitory deficit in fibromyalgia
have been found (Grace et al., 1999; Oosterman et al., 2013). One syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 75, 211–216.
final limitation is that no assessment was made about the occasional C^
ote, K. A. & Moldofsky, H. (1997). Sleep, daytime symptoms, and
cognitive performance in patients with fibromyalgia. The Journal of
consumption of medication, or of the influence of such variables as
Rheumatology, 24, 2014–2023.
fatigue (Suhr, 2003), or the hour of the day in which the assessments Crombez, G., Eccleston, C., Baeyens, F. & Eelen, P. (1998). When
were done considering the circadian typology of the subjects somatic information threatens catastrophizing thinking enhances
(Bennett, Petros, Jonhson & Ferraro, 2008; Cavallera, Boari, Giudici attentional interference. Pain, 75, 187–198.
& Ortolano, 2012), which were subsequently proved to be Crombez, G., Eccleston, C., Van den Broeck, A., Van Houdenhove, B. &
Goubert, L. (2002). The effects of catastrophic thinking about pain on
significant for the cognitive performance of individuals.
attentional interference by pain: No mediation of negative affectivity in
Notwithstanding the above limitations, the results of this case- healthy volunteers and in patients with low back pain. Pain Research
control study are interesting for a variety of reasons. In the first and Management, 7, 31–39.
place, the study control for important confounds, as depression Cuevas-Toro, A. M., L opez-Torrecillas, F., Dıaz-Batanero, M. C. & Perez-
and medication. Also, the study shows the importance of jointly Marfıl, M. N. (2014). Neuropsychological function, anxiety,
depression and pain impact in fibromyalgia patients. Spanish Journal
assessing cognitive performance and memory complaints in
of Psychology, 17, 1–9.
various clinical samples. Further, this study highlights the subtle Dick, B., Eccleston, C. & Crombez, G. (2002). Attentional functioning in
differences, specifically those related with perceived memory fibromyalgia, rheumatoid arthritis, and musculoskeletal pain patients.
complaints, between the patients with fibromyalgia and the Arthritis and Rheumatology, 46, 639–644.
patients with other chronic pain conditions. Finally, the results of Dick, B. D. & Rashiq, S. (2007). Disruption on attention and working
memory traces in individuals with chronic pain. Anesthesia and
this study have given rise to new unknowns that should be
Analgesia, 104, 1223–1229.
cleared up by future research. In particular, further studies should Dick, B. D., Verrier, M. J., Harker, K. T. & Rashiq, S. (2008). Disruption
examine how the diagnosis of chronic pain can affect differently of cognitive function in fibromyalgia syndrome. Pain, 139, 610–616.
various cognitive functions, and the relation between cognitive Dufton, B. D. (1989). Cognitive failure and chronic pain. The
performance and the perception of memory problems. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 19, 291–297.
Dworkin, R. H. & Siefried, R. N. (1994). Are all those pain ratings
necessary? [letter to the editor]. Pain, 58, 279.
Esteve, M. R., Ramırez, C. & L opez-Martınez, A. E. (2001). Alteraciones
REFERENCES
de la memoria en pacientes con dolor cr onico. Revista de la Sociedad
Antepohl, W., Kiviloog, L., Anderson, J. & Gerdle, B. (2003). Cognitive Espa~ nola del Dolor, 8, 119–127.
impairment in patients with chronic whiplash-associated disorder: A Etherton, J. L., Bianchini, K. J., Ciota, M. A., Heinly, M. T. & Greve, K.
matched control study. NeuroRehabilitation, 18, 307–315. W. (2006). Pain, malingering and the WAIS-III Working Memory
Bar-On-Kalfon, T., Gal, G., Shorer, R. & Ablin, J. N. (2016). Cognitive Index. Spine, 6, 61–71.
functioning in fibromyalgia: The central role of effort. Journal of Garcia Martınez, J. & Sanchez-Canovas, J. (1994). Adaptaci on del
Psychosomatic Research, 87, 30–36. cuestionario de fallos de memoria en la vida cotidiana (MFE). Boletın
Bell, T. R., Trost, Z., Buelow, M. T., Clay, O., Younger, J., Moore, D. De Psicologıa, 43, 89–107.
et al. (2018). Meta-analysis of cognitive performance in fibromyalgia. Gelonch, O., Garolera, M., Valls, J., Li, R. & Pifarre, J. (2017). Cognitive
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 40, 678–714. complaints in women with fibromyalgia: are they due to depression or
Benedet, M. J. & Alejandre, M. A. (1998). TAVEC: Test de Aprendizaje to objective cognitive dysfunction? Journal of Clinical and
Verbal Espa~na-Complutense. Madrid: Ediciones TEA. Experimental Neuropsychology, 39, 1013–1025.
Bennett, C. L., Petros, C. V., Jonhson, M. & Ferraro, F. R. (2008). Gelonch, O., Garolera, M., Valls, J., Rossell o, L. l. & Pifarre, J. (2016).
Individual differences in the influence of time of day on executive Executive function in fibromyalgia: Comparing subjective and
functions. The American Journal of Psychology, 121, 349–361. objective measures. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 66, 113–122.
Berryman, C., Stanton, T. R., Bowering, K. J., Tabor, A., McFarlane, A. & Gervais, R. O., Russell, A. S., Green, P., Allen, L. M., Ferrari, R. &
Moseley, G. L. (2013). Evidence for working memory deficits in chronic Pieschl, S. D. (2001). Effort testing in patients with fibromyalgia and
pain: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Pain, 154, 1181–1196. disability incentives. The Journal of Rheumatology, 28, 1892–1899.
Berryman, C., Stanton, T. R., Bowering, K. J., Tabor, A., McFarlane, A. Glass, J. M. (2006). Cognitive dysfunction in fibromyalgia and chronic
& Moseley, G. L. (2014). Do people with chronic pain have impaired fatigue syndrome: new trends and future directions. Current
executive functions? Clinical Psychology Review, 34, 563–579. Rheumatology Reports, 8, 425–429.
Bjelland, I., Dahl, A. A., Haug, T. T. & Neckelmann, D. (2002). The Glass, J. M. (2008). Fibromyalgia and Cognition. The Journal of Clinical
validity of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale an updated Psychiatry, 69, 20–24.
literature review. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 52, 69–77. Glass, J. M. (2009). Review of cognitive dysfunction in fibromyalgia: A
Castel, A., Cascon, R., Salvat, M., Sala, J., Padrol, A., Perez, M. & Rull, convergence on working memory and attentional control impairments.
M. (2008). Rendimiento cognitivo y percepcion de problemas de Rheumatic Diseases Clinics of North America, 35, 299–311.
memoria en pacientes con dolor cronico: con fibromialgia versus sin Glass, J. M. & Park, D. C. (2001). Cognitive dysfunction in fibromyalgia.
fibromialgia. Revista de la Sociedad Espa~nola del Dolor, 6, 358–370. Current Rheumatology Reports, 3, 123–127.
Cavallera, G. M., Boari, G., Giudici, S. & Ortolano, A. (2011). Cognitive Glass, J. M., Park, D. C., Minear, M. & Crofford, L. J. (2005). Memory
parameters and morning and evening types: Two decades of research beliefs and function in fibromyalgia patients. Journal of Psychosomatic
(1990–2009). Perceptual and Motor Skills, 112, 649–665. Research, 58, 263–269.
Chapman, S. L., Byass-Smith, M. G. & Reed, B. A. (2002). Effects of Glass, J. M., Williams, D. A., Fernandez-Sanchez, M.-L., Kairys, A., Barjola,
intermediate-and long term use of opiods on cognition in patients with P., Heitzeg, M. M. et al. (2011). Executive function in chronic pain
chronic pain. The Clinical Journal of Pain, 18, S83–S90.

© 2021 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Scand J Psychol 62 (2021) Memory complaints and cognitive performance 337

patients and healthy controls: Different cortical activation during response McDermott, L. M. & Ebmeier, K. P. (2009). A meta-analysis of
inhibition in fibromyalgia. The Journal of Pain, 12, 1219–1229. depression severity and cognitive function. Journal of Affective
Golden, C. J. (1978). Stroop Color and Word Test: A manual for clinical Disorders, 119, 1–8.
and experimental uses. Illinois: Stoelting CO. Miller, G. A. & Chapman, J. P. (2001). Misunderstanding analysis of
Golden, C. J. (2001). Stroop: Test de Colores y Palabras. Madrid: covariance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 40–48.
Ediciones TEA. Mohs, R., Mease, P. H., Arnold, L. M., Wang, F., Ahl, J., Gaynor, P. &
Grace, G. M., Nielson, W. R., Hopkins, M. & Berg, M. A. (1999). Wohlreich, M. M. (2012). The effect of duloxetine treatment on
Concentration and memory deficits in patients with fibromyalgia cognition in patients with fibromyalgia. Psychosomatic Medicine, 74,
syndrome. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21, 628–634.
477–487. Moriarty, O., McGuire, B. E. & Finn, F. P. (2011). The effect of pain on
Grisard, J. M., Van der Linden, M. & Masquelier, E. (2002). Controlled cognitive function: A review of clinical and preclinical research.
processes and automaticity in memory functioning in fibromyalgia Progress in Neurobiology, 93, 385–404.
patients: relation with emotional distress and hypervigilance. Journal Mula, M. (2012). Recent and future antiepileptic drugs and their impact on
of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 994–1009. cognition: What can we expect? Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics,
Harker, K. T., Klein, R. M., Dick, B., Verrier, M. J. & Rashiq, S. (2011). 12, 667–671.
Exploring attentional disruption in fibromyalgia using the attentional Munguia-Izquierdo, D. & Legaz-Arrese, A. (2007). Exercise in warm
blink. Psychology and Health, 26, 915–929. water decreases pain and improves cognitive function in middle-aged
Herrera-Guzman, I., Herrera-Abarca, J. E., Gudayol-Ferre, E., Herrera- women with fibromyalgia. Clinical and Experimental Rheumatology,
Guzman, D., Gomez-Carbajal, L., Pe~na-Olvira, M. et al. (2010). Effects 25, 823–830.
of selective serotonin reuptake and dual serotonergic–noradrenergic Mu~noz, M. & Esteve, R. (2005). Reports of memory functioning by
reuptake treatments on attention and executive functions in patients with patients with chronic pain. The Clinical Journal of Pain, 21, 287–291.
major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 177, 323–329. Oosterman, J., Derksen, L. C., Van Wijck, A. J., Kessels, R. P. &
Højsted, J., Kurita, G. P., Kendall, S., Lundorff, L., de Mattos, A., Pimenta, Veldhuijzen, D. S. (2012). Executive and attentional functions in
C. & Sjøgren, P. (2012). Non-analgesic effects of opioids: the cognitive chronic pain: does performance decrease with increasing task load?
effects of opioids in chronic pain of malignant and non-malignant origin. Pain Research and Management, 17, 159–165.
An update. Current Pharmaceutical Design, 18, 6116–6122. Oosterman, J. M., Derksen, L. C., Van Wijck, A. J. M., Veldhuijzen, D.
Jamison, R. N., Schein, J. R., Vallow, S., Ascher, S., Vorsanger, G. J. & S. & Kessels, R. P. C. (2011). Memory functions in chronic pain.
Katz, N. P. (2003). Neuropsychological effects of long-term opioid use Examining contributions of attention and age to test performance. The
in chronic pain patients. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, Clinical Journal of Pain, 27, 70–75.
26, 913–921. Park, D. C., Glass, J. M., Minear, M. & Crofford, L. J. (2001). Cognitive
Jensen, M. P., Turner, J. A., Romano, J. M. & Fisher, L. D. (1999). function in fibromyalgia patients. Arthritis and Rheumatology, 44,
Comparative reliability and validity of chronic pain intensity measures. 2125–2133.
Pain, 83, 157–162. Pidal-Miranda, M., Gonzalez-Villar, A. J., Carrillo-de-la-Pe~ na, M. T.,
Jorge, L. L., Gerard, C. & Level, M. (2009). Evidences of memory Andrade, E. & Rodrıguez-Salgado, D. (2018). Broad cognitive
dysfunction and maladaptive coping in chronic low back pain and complaints but subtle objective working memory impairment in
rheumatoid arthritis patients: challenges for rehabilitation. European fibromyalgia patients. PeerJ, 21, e5907.
Journal of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine, 45, 469–477. Povedano, M., Gasc on, J., Galvez, R., Ruiz, M. & Rejas, J. (2007).
Katz, R. S., Heard, A. R., Mills, M. & Leavitt, F. (2004). The prevalence Cognitive function impairment in patients with neuropahic pain under
and clinical impact of reported cognitive difficulties (fibrofog) in standard conditions of care. Journal of Pain and Symptom
patients with rheumatic disease with and without fibromyalgia. JCR: Management, 33, 78–89.
Journal of Clinical Rheumatology, 10, 53–58. Rodrıguez-Franco, L., Cano-Garcıa, E. J. & Blanco-Picabia, A. (2004).
Kim, S. H., Kim, S. H., Kim, S. K., Nam, E. J., Han, S. W. & Lee, S. J. Evaluaci on de las estrategias de afrontamiento del dolor cr
onico. Actas
(2012). Spatial versus verbal memory impairments in patients with Espa~ nolas de Psiquiatria, 32, 82–91.
fibromyalgia. Rheumatology International, 32, 1135–1142. Rosenstiel, A. K. & Keefe, F. J. (1983). The use of coping strategies in
Landrø, N. I., Fors, E. A., V apenstad, L. L., Holthe, Ø., Stiles, T. C. & chronic low back pain patients: relationships to patient characteristics
Borchgrevink, P. C. (2013). The extend of neurocognitive dysfunction in and current adjustment. Pain, 17, 33–44.
a multidisciplinary pain center population. Is there a relation between Roth, R. S., Geisser, M. E., Theisen-Goodvich, M. & Dixon, P. J. (2005).
reported and tested neuropsychological functioning? Pain, 154, 972–977. Cognitive complaints are associated with depression, fatigue, female
Landrø, N. I., Stiles, T. C. & Sletvold, H. (1997). Memory functioning in sex, and pain catastrophizing in patients with chronic pain. Archives of
patients with primary fibromyalgia and major depression and healthy Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 86, 1147–1154.
controls. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 42, 297–306. Schmand, B., Lindeboom, J., Schagen, S., Heijt, R., Koene, T. &
Leavitt, F. & Katz, R. S. (2006). Distraction as a key determinant of Hamburger, H. L. (1998). Cognitive complaints in patients after
impaired memory in patients with fibromyalgia. The Journal of whiplash injury: The impact of malingering. Journal of Neurology,
Rheumatology, 33, 127–132. Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 64, 339–343.
Legrain, V., Van Damme, S., Eccleston, C., Davis, K. D., Seminowicz, D. A. Schnurr, R. F. & MacDonald, M. R. (1995). Memory complaints in
& Crombez, G. (2009). A neurocognitive model of attention to pain: chronic pain. Clinical Journal of Pain, 11, 103–111.
behavioral and neuroimaging evidence. Pain, 144, 230–232. Sjøgren, P., Christrup, L. L., Petersen, M. A. & Højsted, J. (2005).
Ling, J., Campbell, C., Heffernan, T. M. & Greenough, C. C. (2007). Neurospychological assessment of non-malignant pain patients treated in
Short-term prospective memory deficits in chronic back pain patients. a multidisciplinary pain centre. European Journal of Pain, 9, 453–462.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 69, 144–148. S€
oderfjell, S., Molander, B., Johansson, H., Barnekow-Bergkvist, M. &
Loring, D. W., Marino, S. & Meador, K. J. (2007). Neuropsychological Nilsson, L. G. (2006). Musculoskeletal pain complaints and
and behavioral effects of antiepilepsy drugs. Neuropsychology Review, performance on cognitive tasks over the adult life span. Scandinavian
17, 413–425. Journal of Psychology, 47, 349–359.
McCracken, L. M. & Iverson, G. L. (2001). Predicting complaints of Suhr, J. A. (2003). Neuropsychological impairment in fibromyalgia.
impaired cognitive functioning in patients with chronic pain. Journal Relation to depression, fatigue, and pain. Journal of Psychosomatic
of Pain and Symptom Management, 21, 392–396. Research, 55, 321–329.
McDermid, A. J., Rollman, G. B. & McCain, G. A. (1996). Generalized Sunderland, A., Harris, J. E. & Gleave, S. (1984). Memory failures in
hypervigilance in fibromyalgia: Evidence of perceptual amplification. everyday memory following severe head injury. Journal of Clinical
Pain, 66, 133–144. and Experimental Neuropsychology, 6, 127–142.

© 2021 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
338 A. Castel et al. Scand J Psychol 62 (2021)

Tassian, V., Attal, N., Fletcher, D., Brasseur, L., Degieux, P., Chauvin, M. appraisal, objective performance, and task-related brain activity during
& Bouhassira, D. (2003). Long term effects of oral sustained release a working memory task. NeuroImage: Clinical, 11, 173–180.
morphine on neuropsychological performance in patients with chronic Walitt, B., Roebuck-Spencer, T., Bleiberg, J., Foster, G. & Weinstein, A.
non-cancer pain. Pain, 104, 389–400. (2008). Automated neuropsychiatric measurements of information
Tejero, A., Guimera, E. M., Farre, J. M. & Peri, J. M. (1986). Uso clınico processing in fibromyalgia. Rheumatology International, 28, 561–566.
del HAD en poblacion psiquiatrica: Un estudio de su sensibilidad, Wolfe, F., Smythe, H. A., Yunus, M. B., Bennett, R. M., Bombardier, C.,
fiabilidad y validez. Revista De Psiquiatrıa De La Facultad De Goldenberg, D. L. et al. (1990). The American College of
Medicina De Barcelona, 13, 233–238. Rheumatology 1990 criteria for the classification of fibromyalgia:
Tesio, V., Torta, D. M., Colonna, F., Leombruni, P., Ghiggia, A., Fusaro, report of the multicenter criteria committee. Arthritis and
E. et al. (2015). Are fibromyalgia patients impaired? Objective and Rheumatology, 33, 160–172.
subjective neuropsychological evidence. Arthritis Care and Research, Wu, Y.-L., Huang, C.-J., Fang, S.-C., Ko, L.-H. & Tsai, P.-S. (2018).
67, 143–150. Cognitive impairment in fibromyalgia: A meta-analysis of case-control
Toulouse, E. & Pieron, H. (1998). T-P. Prueba perceptiva y de atenci on. studies. Psychosomatic Medicine, 80, 432–438.
Madrid: Ediciones TEA. Zigmond, A. S. & Snaith, R. P. (1983). The Hospital Anxiety and
Vilar-Lopez, R., Aparicio, M., Gomez-Rıo, M. & Perez-Garcıa, M. Depression Scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 67, 361–370.
(2013). Utilidad de los ındices de memoria verbal para detectar
simulacion en poblacion espa~nola. Clınica y Salud, 24, 169–176. Received 8 April 2020, accepted 27 November 2020

Walitt, B., Ceko, M., Khatiwadac, M., Gracely, J. L., Rayhand, R.Van
Meterc, J. W. et al. (2016). Characterizing “fibrofog”: Subjective

© 2021 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd

View publication stats

You might also like