You are on page 1of 649

APHASIA

AND OTHER ACQUIRED NEUROGENIC


LANGUAGE DISORDERS
A Guide for Clinical Excellence
Second Edition
APHASIA
AND OTHER ACQUIRED NEUROGENIC
LANGUAGE DISORDERS
A Guide for Clinical Excellence
Second Edition

Brooke Hallowell, PhD, CCC-SLP


5521 Ruffin Road
San Diego, CA 92123

e-mail: information@pluralpublishing.com
Web site: https://www.pluralpublishing.com

Copyright © 2023 by Plural Publishing, Inc.

Typeset in 10.5/13 Palatino by Flanagan’s Publishing Services, Inc.


Printed in the United States of America by McNaughton & Gunn, Inc.

All rights, including that of translation, reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored
in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, recording,
or otherwise, including photocopying, recording, taping, Web distribution, or information storage and
retrieval systems without the prior written consent of the publisher.

For permission to use material from this text, contact us by


Telephone: (866) 758-7251
Fax: (888) 758-7255
e-mail: permissions@pluralpublishing.com

Every attempt has been made to contact the copyright holders for material originally printed in another source.
If any have been inadvertently overlooked, the publisher will gladly make the necessary arrangements at the first
opportunity.

Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data

Names: Hallowell, Brooke, author.


Title: Aphasia and other acquired neurogenic language disorders : a guide
for clinical excellence / Brooke Hallowell.
Description: Second edition. | San Diego, CA : Plural Publishing, Inc.,
[2023] | Includes bibliographical references and index.
Identifiers: LCCN 2021053552 | ISBN 9781635501599 (paperback) | ISBN
1635501598 (paperback) | ISBN 9781597569552 (ebook)
Subjects: MESH: Language Disorders--physiopathology | Language
Disorders--therapy | Language Disorders--diagnosis | Clinical Competence
| Evidence-Based Practice
Classification: LCC RC425 | NLM WL 340.5 | DDC 616.85/52--dc23/eng/20211116
LC record available at https://lccn.loc.gov/2021053552
Contents

What Is Special About This Book? xxiii


Acknowledgments xxix
About the Author xxxi

Section I. Welcome and Introduction

Chapter 1. Welcome to the Fantastic World of Research and Clinical Practice in Acquired 3
Neurogenic Communication Disorders
What Are Acquired Cognitive-Linguistic Disorders? 3
Which Neurogenic Communication Disorders Are Not Acquired Language Disorders? 4
What Is Clinical Aphasiology? 5
What Is So Fantastic About the World of Neurogenic Communication Disorders? 5
We Work With Wonderful People and Become Part of Their Rich Life Stories 5
We Are Catalysts for Positive Change 6
We Enjoy Empowerment of Others Through Advocacy and Leadership 6
We Enjoy a Great Deal of Humor and Fascination 6
We Enjoy Fantastic Local and Worldwide Professional Networks 6
Our Work Is Multicultural and Multilingual 6
We Are Lifelong Learners 6
We Tap Into Our Most Scientific and Our Most Creative Selves at the Same Time 7
We Have Rich Career Opportunities 7
What Disciplines Are Relevant to Aphasia and Related Disorders? 7
What Is Known About the Incidence and Prevalence of Acquired Neurogenic 7
Language Disorders?
Where Do Aphasiologists Work? 8
What Is the Career Outlook for Clinical Aphasiologists? 8
Learning and Reflection Activities 10

Chapter 2. Becoming the Ultimate Excellent Clinician 11


What Makes a Clinician Truly Excellent? 12
What Can One Do to Become an Excellent Clinical Aphasiologist? 12
How Do the People We Serve Characterize What They Most Want? 18
What Are Some Traits of People Who Are Perceived as Unhelpful Clinicians? 18
What Content Is Important to Master? 18
What Credentials Are Required for a Career as an Aphasiologist? 20
What Credentials May Aphasiologists Earn Beyond Their Basic Academic and 21
Clinical Credentials?
Is It Best to Specialize or Generalize? 21
What Strategies Help Boost Career Development in Acquired Cognitive-Linguistic 22
Disorders?

v
vi  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

What Organizations Support Professional Information Sharing and Networking 23


Among Clinical Aphasiologists?
Learning and Reflection Activities 23

Chapter 3. Writing and Talking About the People With Whom We Work 27
What Is Important to Consider in Writing and Talking About People With 27
Neurogenic Cognitive-Linguistic Disorders?
Person-First Language 28
Alternatives to the Word Patient 28
People With Disabilities 29
Research Participants 29
Older People 29
Healthy Adults 29
What Are Important Nuances in Terms We Use to Refer to People Who Care for
People With Neurogenic Cognitive-Linguistic Disorders? 30
What Is the Difference Between the Terms Therapy and Treatment ? 30
Neurotypical People 30
What Are Pros and Cons of Terms Used to Refer to SLPs? 31
What Are the Preferred Terms When Referring to the Experts Who Work With 31
People Who Have Neurogenic Communication Challenges?
What Is Important to Keep in Mind Regarding Inclusive and Welcoming Language? 31
What Other Terms Might Unintentionally Convey Negative Connotations? 32
Why Are There Inconsistencies in the Prefixes Used in Terms for Characterizing 32
Neurogenic Symptoms, and What Is the Rationale for Varied Prefix Choices?
Learning and Reflection Activities 33

Section II. Foundations for Considering Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Chapter 4. Defining and Conceptualizing Aphasia 37


What Is a Good Way to Define Aphasia? 37
Aphasia Is Acquired 37
Aphasia Has a Neurological Cause 38
Aphasia Affects Reception and Production of Language Across Modalities 38
Aphasia Is Not a Speech, Intellectual, Sensory, or Psychiatric Disorder 38
How Have Established Aphasiologists Defined Aphasia? 40
What Are the Primary Frameworks for Conceptualizing Aphasia? 40
Unidimensional Frameworks 40
Multidimensional Frameworks 42
Medical Frameworks 42
Cognitive Neuropsychological, Psycholinguistic, and Neurolinguistic Frameworks 43
Biopsychosocial Frameworks 45
Social Frameworks 45
Social Determinants of Health Frameworks 46
Other Historically Relevant Frameworks 46
How Does One Choose a Preferred Framework for Conceptualizing Aphasia? 47
How Are the Frameworks for Conceptualizing Aphasia Relevant to Other 48
Neurogenic Language Disorders?
Learning and Reflection Activities 48
vii
Contents  

Chapter 5. The WHO ICF, Human Rights Perspectives, and Life Participation Approaches 51
What Is the WHO ICF? 51
How Is the WHO ICF Relevant to Ethics and Human Rights? 53
How Is the WHO ICF Specifically Relevant to Intervention and Research in 53
Rehabilitation?
How Is the WHO ICF Specifically Relevant to People With Neurogenic Language 54
Disorders?
Learning and Reflection Activities 55

Chapter 6. Etiologies of Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders 57


What Is a Stroke? 57
What Are Stroke Risk Factors, and What Causes Stroke? 59
What Are the Physiological Effects of Stroke? 60
How Crucial Is Timing for Medical Treatment After a Stroke? 60
How Is the Sudden Onset of Stroke Relevant to Supporting Patients and Families? 61
What Is a Transient Ischemic Attack? 61
What Is Hypoperfusion? 61
What Can Be Done to Prevent Stroke? 61
Attending to Stroke Triggers 63
What Is TBI? 64
What Are Blast Injuries? 66
What Are Concussion and Mild TBI? 67
What Can Be Done to Prevent TBI? 68
What Are Bacteria and Viruses? 68
What Other Types of Infections Affect Cortical Function? 69
What Is Neoplasm? 69
What Is Toxemia? 70
What Are Diabetes Mellitus and Diabetic Encephalopathy? 70
What Is Metabolic Syndrome? 71
What Other Metabolic Disorders Cause Encephalopathy? 72
What Is Neurodegenerative Disease? 72
What Is Dementia? 72
What Is Mild Cognitive Impairment? 72
What Is Primary Progressive Aphasia? 72
What Are Some Special Challenges in Identifying Etiologies of Cognitive-Linguistic 73
Disorders?
Learning and Reflection Activities 73

Chapter 7. Neurophysiology and Neuropathology of Acquired Neurogenic 75


Language Disorders
What Should SLPs Know About Neuroanatomy and Neurophysiology Associated 76
With Neurogenic Cognitive-Linguistic Disorders?
What Are Key Neurophysiological Principles Pertinent to Acquired 78
Cognitive-Linguistic Disorders?
Specialization of Structure and Function 78
Interconnectivity Throughout the Brain 79
The Brain’s Plasticity 79
What Is the Most Clinically Pertinent Knowledge an Aphasiologist Should Have 80
About the Blood Supply to the Brain?
viii  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

What Factors Affect a Person’s Prognosis for Recovery From a Stroke or Brain Injury? 83
Why Is It Important for Clinical Aphasiologists to Know About the Visual System? 84
What Aspects of the Visual System Are Most Relevant to People With Neurogenic 85
Language Disorders?
Anatomy and Physiology Associated With Visual Deficits 85
How Are Visual Field Deficits Characterized? 89
What Are Ocular Motor Deficits? 93
What Are Visual Attention Deficits? 93
What Are Higher-Level Visual Deficits? 93
What Aspects of the Neurophysiology of Hearing Are Most Relevant to People 94
With Neurogenic Language Disorders?
Learning and Reflection Activities 95
Supplemental Review of Neuroanatomy Related to Aphasiology 96
Supplemental Review of Blood Supply to the Brain 96
Supplemental Review of the Visual System 98
Supplemental Review of the Auditory System 98

Chapter 8. Neuroimaging and Other Neurodiagnostic Instrumentation 101


What Are the Most Relevant Neuroimaging Techniques for Aphasiologists to 101
Know About?
Computed Axial Tomography (CAT or CT) 101
Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) 103
Single Photon Emission Computed Tomography (SPECT) 109
Cerebral Angiography 109
What Other Neurodiagnostic Methods Are Important for Aphasiologists to 110
Know About?
Electroencephalography (EEG) 110
Electrocorticography 111
Additional Methods 112
Learning and Reflection Activities 113

Chapter 9. Aging, Which Is Not a Disorder, and Its Relevance to Aphasiology 115
What Is Aging? 115
What Are Key Theories About Aging That Are Especially Relevant to Cognition 116
and Communication?
What Is Aging Well? 116
How Are Demographic Shifts in Aging Populations Relevant to Clinical 117
Aphasiologists?
What Are Normal Changes in the Brain as People Age? 117
What Are Positive Aspects of the Aging Brain? 118
Memory 118
Word Finding 119
Syntactic Processing 119
Reading and Writing 120
Discourse 120
Pragmatics 121
What Are General Guidelines for Differentiating Normal From Impaired 121
Language in Older Adults?
ix
Contents  

What Theories Have Been Proposed to Account for Cognitive-Linguistic Changes 121
With Aging?
Resource Capacity Theories 123
Working Memory Theories 123
Context-Processing Deficiency Theories 123
Signal Degradation Theories 123
Transmission Deficit Theories 123
Speed-of-Processing Theories 124
Inhibition Theories 124
What Can Be Done to Ensure the Best Preservation of Language Abilities as 124
People Age?
What Is Elderspeak, and How May We Raise Awareness About It? 124
What Sensitivities Related to Ageism Are Important for Aphasiologists to 126
Demonstrate?
Learning and Reflection Activities 127

Section III. Features, Symptoms, and Syndromes in the Major Categories of


Cognitive-Linguistic Disorders

Chapter 10. Syndromes and Hallmark Characteristics of Aphasia 131


How Are the Types of Aphasia Classified? 131
What Are the Classic Syndromes of Aphasia, and What Are the Hallmark 132
Characteristics of Each?
Expressive/Receptive, Nonfluent/Fluent, and Anterior/Posterior Dichotomies 132
Classic Aphasia Classification 134
Wernicke’s Aphasia 134
Broca’s Aphasia 137
Global Aphasia 139
Conduction Aphasia 140
Transcortical Sensory Aphasia 140
Transcortical Motor Aphasia 140
Mixed Transcortical Aphasia 140
What Is Primary Progressive Aphasia? 140
What Other Syndromes of Aphasia Are There, and What Are Their Characteristics? 141
Crossed Aphasia 141
Subcortical Aphasia 141
Anomic Aphasia 141
How Might Dyslexia and Dysgraphia Be Conceptualized as Symptoms Versus 142
Syndromes?
What Are Limitations of Classification Systems Based on Relating Function to 142
Neuroanatomical Structure?
Learning and Reflection Activities 143

Chapter 11. Cognitive-Communicative Challenges Associated With Traumatic Brain Injury 145
Why Is It Hard to Generalize About TBI Survivors? 145
What Communication Symptoms Are Likely to Be Experienced by TBI Survivors 146
What Are Special Challenges for War- and Terrorism-Related TBI Survivors? 148
x  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

What Are Special Considerations for Clinicians Working With TBI Survivors? 148
Scope of Practice 148
Interdisciplinary Collaboration 149
Assessment Challenges 149
What Are Special Challenges Faced by TBI Survivors in Health Care Contexts? 149
What Special Economic Considerations Affect Clinical Work With TBI Survivors? 150
Learning and Reflection Activities 151

Chapter 12. Cognitive-Communicative Disorders Associated With Right Hemisphere 153


Syndrome
What Is Right Hemisphere Syndrome? 153
How May RHS Affect Communication and Life Participation? 154
Conversation, Discourse, Pragmatics 154
Combined Receptive and Expressive Challenges 154
Receptive Challenges 156
Expressive Challenges 157
Attention Deficits 158
Memory Challenges 158
Executive Function Challenges 158
Visual-Perceptual Impairments 158
Auditory-Perceptual Impairments 159
Reading and Writing Impairments 159
What Are Special Challenges That SLPs Face in Serving People With RHS? 159
Underdiagnosis and Lack of Awareness of RHS 159
Symptom Classification 159
Identifying Neurological Structure-Function Relationships 160
Characterizing What Is Normal 160
What Are Special Challenges Faced by People With RHS in Health Care Contexts? 160
Learning and Reflection Activities 161

Chapter 13. Cognitive-Communicative Disorders in Primary Progressive Aphasia 163


and Dementia
What Neurodegenerative Conditions Most Commonly Affect Cognitive-Linguistic 163
Abilities?
What Are General Types of Cognitive-Communicative Impairments in People 164
With MCI and Dementia?
What Communication Challenges Are Typically Associated With MCI and Dementia? 164
What Symptoms Are Associated With Common Forms of Dementia? 166
Alzheimer’s Disease 166
Vascular Dementia 167
Dementia With Lewy Bodies (DLB) 167
Parkinson’s-Associated Dementia 167
Frontotemporal Dementia (FTD) 167
Huntington’s Disease 168
Korsakoff ’s Syndrome 168
Creutzfeldt-Jakob Disease 168
AIDS Dementia Complex 168
What Are Symptoms of the Primary Forms of PPA? 168
Is There Such a Thing as “Reversible” Dementia? 169
xi
Contents  

What Are Implications of an Incorrect Diagnosis of Dementia? 171


What Is the Role of the SLP in Working With People Who Have PPA and Dementia? 171
Learning and Reflection Activities 172

Section IV. Delivering Excellent Services

Chapter 14. Contexts for Providing Excellent Services 175


What Do SLPs Who Specialize in Neurogenic Communication Disorders Do? 175
Clinical Intervention (Screening, Assessment, Treatment, Counseling, Educating) 175
Interprofessional Collaboration and Interdisciplinary Learning 175
Advocacy 176
Marketing, Negotiating Contracts, Billing, Recordkeeping, Documentation, 176
Scheduling and Coordinating Care, Quality Assurance, and Fundraising
Leadership and Management 176
Research 176
Teaching and Mentoring 176
In What Types of Settings Do We Provide Clinical Services? 176
Hospitals 176
Rehabilitation Centers 177
Health Maintenance Organizations 177
Skilled Nursing and Long-Term Care Facilities 177
Continuing Care Retirement Communities (CCRCs) 177
Home Health Agencies 178
Private Practice and Not-for-Profit Clinics 178
University-Based Clinics 178
Adult Day Care Centers 179
Aphasia Centers 179
Hospice 179
In What Ways May Services Be Provided at a Distance? 180
With What Types of Teams Do Clinical Aphasiologists Engage? 182
How Do SLPs Get Paid? 182
Where Does the Money Come From to Pay for SLP Services? 183
Government-Sponsored Programs 183
Health Insurance 184
Private Pay 184
Mixed Funding Options 185
Philanthropic Donations 185
How Do Service-Providing Agencies Get Paid? 185
What Makes Services Provided by SLPs Reimbursable? 185
Effective Documentation Meeting All Requirements for Reimbursement 185
A Physician’s Order 186
Preauthorization for Services by the Third-Party Payer 186
Evidence That the Services Are Actually Covered by the Plan 186
Evidence of the Need for Skilled Services 186
Confirmation That the Methods Used Are Evidence Based 187
Documentation of the Life-Affecting Nature of Services 187
Evidence of Treatment Progress 187
Good Relationships With Decision Makers at Third-Party Payer Agencies 188
xii  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

What Are the Primary Reasons for Which Reimbursements for SLP Services 188
Are Denied?
What Do We Do if We Are Denied Reimbursement for Our Services? 188
How Do Health Care Finance and Cost-Control Systems Affect Clinical Services? 189
What Are the Impacts of Health Care Cost Cutting and Cost Control on Services 189
for People With Neurogenic Communication Challenges?
Learning and Reflection Activities 192

Chapter 15. Engaging Proactively in Advocacy and Legal and Ethical Concerns 193
How May Clinicians and the People We Serve Promote Access to SLP Services and 193
Communication Support?
Enhance Awareness of Communication as a Human Right 194
Raise Awareness About Neurogenic Communication Challenges and Ways to 195
Support People and Loved Ones Coping With Them
Help Educate Professionals in Health Care Contexts 200
Encourage Referrals 201
Advocate for Reduced Medicalization of Communication Disabilities 202
Promote Community-Based Approaches 202
Expand Knowledge Translation 203
How Are Human Rights, Morality, Ethics, and Law Relevant to Advocacy for 203
People With Acquired Neurogenic Disorders of Language and Cognition?
What Is the Role of the SLP in Supporting the Rights of Individuals With Aphasia 204
and Related Disorders?
How Do SLPs Engage in Decisions Regarding Competence and Decision-Making? 205
How Might Financial Conflicts of Interest Affect the Practice of Clinical 206
Aphasiologists?
Learning and Reflection Activities 207

Chapter 16. Clinical Aphasiology Around the World 209


What Global Trends Are Affecting the Incidence and Prevalence of Neurogenic 209
Communication Disorders?
A Rapidly Expanding Aging Population 209
Ongoing Demographic Shifts 210
Increasing and Disproportionate Incidence and Prevalence of Conditions That 210
Cause Neurogenic Communication Disorders
Health Care and Prevention Infrastructure Challenges 210
Global Health Priorities Undermining Essential Values 210
What Are Important Priorities for Global Capacity Building to Serve People With 210
Acquired Neurogenic Communication Disorders?
Build Culturally Contextualized Academic and Clinical Programs 210
Expand Life Participation Approaches 211
Attend to Cultural Aspects of Health, Aging, and Disability That May Affect 211
Receptivity to Services
What Are Key Challenges to Enhancing Global Engagement in Acquired 212
Neurogenic Communication Disorders?
What Are Important Ethical Considerations for Aphasiologists Engaging in 213
Transnational Work?
Learning and Reflection Activities 215
xiii
Contents  

Section V. Strategic and Meaningful Assessment

Chapter 17. Best Practices in Assessment 219


Where and When Does Assessment Happen? 219
Assessment Happens Throughout Intervention 219
Treatment Begins the Moment Assessment Starts 219
What Are the Purposes of Assessment? 219
What Aspects of Assessment Are Truly Relevant to Actual Clinical Practice? 220
What Are the Best Practices in Assessment of Acquired Neurogenic Language 221
Disorders?
Do Not Underestimate How Impactful Your Role Is 221
Focus on the Person 221
Keep the Person at the Center of the Process 221
Focus on Life Participation Goals From the Start 222
Focus on Strengths 222
Have a Clear Purpose 224
Ensure the Best Possible Assessment Conditions 224
Be Strategic in Setting the Location 224
Be Strategic About Timing 224
Include Others in the Process 224
Be Mindful of Multiple Perspectives on Real-Life Impacts of Communication 224
Disability
Speak Directly to the Person 225
Collaborate 226
Appreciate That Experts, Not Tests, Are What Determine Diagnoses 227
Attend to Cultural and Linguistic Differences 227
Learning and Reflection Activities 233

Chapter 18. Psychometrics of Assessment and Components of Assessment Processes 235


What Psychometric Properties Should Be Addressed in Assessment Processes? 235
What Are Potentially Confounding Factors? 237
Factors Related to Concomitant Challenges to Health and Well-Being 237
Test Design Factors 237
Assessment Context Factors 237
Interpersonal Factors 237
What Is Entailed in Screening for Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders? 242
What Are the Typical Components of a Comprehensive Assessment Process? 245
What Information Is Pertinent to Collect During the Case History? 246
Learning and Reflection Activities 247

Chapter 19. Problem-Solving Approaches to Differential Diagnosis and 251


Confounding Factors
How Are Potentially Confounding Factors Relevant to Differential Diagnosis? 251
What Are Important Potentially Confounding Factors in Language Assessment, 252
and How Do We Address Them?
Age 252
Intelligence, Literacy, and Education 252
xiv  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Visual Problems 253


Hearing Problems 259
Motor Challenges 260
Reading Problems 263
Dysgraphia and Other Writing Deficits 263
Problems of Awareness and Arousal 263
Attention Problems 265
Lack of Awareness of Deficits 266
Executive Function Deficits 266
Pragmatic Deficits 267
Memory Problems 267
Other Concomitant Cognitive and Linguistic Deficits 267
Depression and Other Mood Disorders 268
Anxiety 269
Emotional Lability 270
Other Challenges to Health and Well-Being 270
How Does a Process Analysis Approach to Assessment Help Address Potentially 270
Confounding Factors?
Learning and Reflection Activities 273

Chapter 20. Tests, Scales, and Screening Instruments 275


What Are the Most Important Factors in Selecting an Assessment Instrument? 275
What Is the Reason for Your Assessment? 275
Who, Specifically, Is Being Assessed? 277
Does It Provide an Appropriate Index of the Constructs You Wish to Assess? 278
Does the Tool Allow for Alternative Response Modes in Cases Where Clients May Have
Trouble With Traditional Response Modes? 278
Might Instructions and Tasks Involved Confound Results? 279
What Is the Quality of a Given Tool? 279
Is It Up to Date and Appropriate in Terms of Content? 280
Does the Tool Complement Your Own Preferences and Preferred Theoretical
Frameworks? 280
How Practical Is the Tool Under Consideration? 281
Do Others on Your Rehabilitation Team Understand the Results You Report and 282
Your Interpretation of Them?
What Are the Most Important Factors in Evaluating Assessment Instruments? 282
What Assessment Tools Are Available? 282
Learning and Reflection Activities 312

Chapter 21. Discourse and Conversation as Vital Aspects of Assessment 313


What Is Discourse? 313
What Are General Categories, Types, or Genres of Discourse? 314
What Is Conversational or Discourse Analysis? 315
Why Is Discourse Sampling and Analysis Important? 315
Discourse, Especially the Social Use of Language, Is Highly Relevant to Every 315
Type of Acquired Neurogenic Disorder
Discourse Analysis Helps Determine Strengths and Weaknesses Not Evident 315
Through Other Forms of Assessment
Discourse Analysis May Yield Critical Information for Differential Diagnosis 316
xv
Contents  

Discourse Analysis Is Vital to Treatment Planning 316


Discourse Analysis Is an Essential Aspect of Research 316
What Are Key Strategies for Sampling Discourse? 316
What Are Key Measures for Indexing Discourse Competence? 318
What Are Best Practices in Interpreting Discourse Analysis Results? 323
What Challenges Do Aphasiologists Face in Applying Discourse Analysis in 324
Clinical Practice and Research?
Time 324
Training and Mentorship 324
Equipment and Software 324
Clear Communication and Perceived Relevance 324
Replicability and Variability in the Evidence Base 324
How May Aphasiologists Confront Challenges in Applying Discourse Analysis in 325
Clinical Practice and Research?
Learning and Reflection Activities 325

Chapter 22. Documenting Assessment Results and Considering Prognosis 327


What Are Best Practices in Sharing Assessment Results With Adults Who 327
Have Acquired Cognitive-Linguistic Disorders and the People Who Care
About Them?
How Do We Best Make Judgments About Prognosis? 328
What Are Best Practices for Reporting Assessment Results in Writing? 329
What Information Is Typically Included in Assessment Reports? 330
What Abbreviations Are Commonly Used in Clinical Reporting? 330
Learning and Reflection Activities 340

Section VI. Theories and Best Practices in Intervention

Chapter 23. Best Practices in Intervention 343


What Are the Best Practices in the Treatment of Neurogenic Language Disorders? 343
Embrace Communication as a Human Right 343
Recognize Assessment as an Ongoing Intervention Process 343
Be Person Centered 344
Include Family Members, Caregivers, and Others Whose Roles Are Relevant 344
Have a Clear Sense of Purpose and Goals 344
Engage Communication Partners Outside of the Client’s Immediate Circle of 345
Friends and Family
Embrace Cultural and Linguistic Differences 345
Encourage Self-Coaching 345
Consider Optimal Timing 345
Consider Optimal Locations and Conditions 345
Focus on Functional Communication 346
Engage the Person Actively and Meaningfully in Goal Setting 347
Focus on Relevant Material 348
Focus on Strengths 348
Be an Interprofessional Team Player 348
Integrate Evidence-Based Practice With Practice-Based Evidence 349
Blend Art With Science 349
xvi  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Encourage Aphasia-Friendly Communication 349


Attend to Behavioral Challenges That Impede Successful Interactions 350
What Does the Excellent Clinical Aphasiologist Know About Evidence-Based 350
Practice?
Where Can We Find Pertinent Information to Support Evidence-Based Practice? 353
How Does the Excellent Clinician Integrate Evidence-Based Practice With 353
Practice-Based Evidence?
How May Excellent Clinicians Support Knowledge Translation Through 355
Implementation and Systems Science?
Learning and Reflection Activities 355

Chapter 24. Treatment Theories and Types of Treatment to Enhance Language and 357
Cognition Across All People With Neurogenic Communication Challenges
What Are the Purposes of Treatment Methods? 357
What Are the Mechanisms of Recovery After Stroke and Brain Injury? 358
How May Behavioral Treatment Facilitate Brain Recovery? 359
How May Pharmacological Agents Facilitate Brain Changes? 359
How May Brain Stimulation Facilitate Brain Changes? 360
What Other Types of Intervention May Facilitate Brain Changes? 361
Can We Differentiate Spontaneous Recovery From Progress Made Through 362
Treatment?
What Are the Optimal Times During Recovery to Initiate Treatment? 362
What Is the Optimal Focus of Initial Treatment Soon After a Stroke or Brain Injury? 362
Focus on Communication Needs 362
Counsel and Share Information 362
Promote Rest 363
Consider the Balance of Compensatory With Restitutive Approaches 363
Consider Pros and Cons of Focusing on Attention 363
What Is the Optimal Intensity and Duration of Treatment? 363
What Is the Best Level of Complexity for Treatment Foci? 364
What Other Treatment Parameters Are Important to Consider? 365
How Might Intervention in Neurodegenerative Conditions Slow 365
Cognitive-Linguistic Decline?
What Is the Best Time to Initiate Treatment With People Who Have 366
Neurodegenerative Conditions?
Learning and Reflection Activities 366

Section VII. General Approaches to Treatment

Chapter 25. General Approaches for Enhancing Cognitive-Linguistic Abilities in 369


Traumatic Brain Injury, Stroke Survivors, and People With Primary
Progressive Aphasia and Dementia
What Is Treatment Fidelity, and How Is It Relevant to Clinical Aphasiology? 369
What General Social and Life Participation Approaches Are Applicable to Treatment? 370
Life Participation Approach to Aphasia 370
Supported Communication 371
What General Treatment Methods Fit Within Social and Life Participation Models? 372
Total Communication Approaches 372
xvii
Contents  

Partner and Caregiver Training 373


Reciprocal Scaffolding 374
Workplace Immersion Programs 374
Aphasia Mentoring Programs 375
Toastmaster Programs 375
Humor as Therapy 376
Online Games 376
Other Socially Focused Programs 378
What General Cognitive Neuropsychological Approaches Are Applicable to 378
Treatment?
What Is Cognitive Rehabilitation? 379
What Is the Stimulation-Facilitation Approach? 381
How May Group Treatment Be Implemented, and How Can It Help People With 381
Aphasia and Related Disorders?
How May AAC, Apps, and Software Be Used to Support Communication and Aid 382
in Treatment?
Alternative and Augmentative Communications 382
What Are Intensive and Residential Aphasia Programs, and How Can They Help 385
People With Aphasia and Related Disorders?
Learning and Reflection Activities 387

Chapter 26. Facilitating Communication in People With Primary Progressive Aphasia 389
and Dementia
What Are Special Service Delivery Challenges for Serving People With PPA and 389
Dementia?
How Is Working With People Who Have PPA and Dementia Recognized as a 390
Component of the SLP’s Scope of Practice?
What SLP Services for People With Dementia Are Reimbursable? 390
What Types of Direct Treatment May Help People With PPA and Dementia? 391
What Are Important Approaches for Caregiver Coaching, Training, and Support? 392
What Are Memory Books and Memory Wallets, and How Are They Implemented? 393
What Is Spaced Retrieval Training, and How Is It Implemented? 395
What Is the FOCUSED Program, and How Is It Implemented? 397
What Are Montessori Approaches to Dementia Management? 397
What Are Additional Forms of Programming to Support People With PPA and 399
Dementia?
In What Other Ways May Clinical Aphasiologists Professionally Support the 400
Communication Needs of People With PPA and Dementia and the People
Who Care About Them?
Learning and Reflection Activities 400

Chapter 27. Counseling and Life Coaching 403


How Might an SLP Become an Effective Counselor and Coach? 403
Is the SLP Working With Adults to Be a Counselor, Life Coach, or Both? 404
What Are Important Considerations Related to Counseling and Scope of Practice? 405
How Might a Speech-Language Clinician Adopt a Counseling Mindset? 406
How Does a Clinician Listen and Respond Empathetically and Compassionately? 406
How Do We Promote a Positive Outlook Without Conveying a Pollyanna Attitude? 407
How Might Multicultural Differences Affect Counseling and Coaching? 407
xviii  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

How Might Counseling Moments Be Influenced by the Time Course of Recovery 407
and Intervention?
Counseling Following a Traumatic Change 408
Counseling at the Start of Intervention 409
Counseling Related to Assessment Results and Sharing Prognosis 409
Counseling During Treatment 409
Counseling at Discharge 409
How May Coaching Enhance Self-Advocacy? 409
What Are Best Practices in Responding to Seemingly Misguided Statements? 410
What Are Effective Ways to Address Emotional Lability During Clinical Interactions? 411
What Is the Role of the SLP in Addressing Depression in People With Neurogenic 411
Communication Disorders?
How Can Communication Counseling Enhance End-of-Life Care? 412
What Are Ways in Which Opportunities for Counseling Can Be Missed? 412
How Might Some Aspects of Life Improve After Onset of an Acquired Neurogenic 413
Communication Disorder?
How May People With Acquired Communication Challenges Support One Another? 413
What Are Some Helpful Information-Sharing Strategies and Resources? 413
Learning and Reflection Activities 417

Chapter 28. Complementary and Integrative Approaches 419


What Are Complementary and Integrative Approaches to Wellness? 419
How Are Complementary and Integrative Approaches Relevant to Neurogenic 420
Disorders of Language and Cognition?
Why Is It Important for Clinical Aphasiologists to Learn About Complementary 420
and Integrative Approaches?
How Are Mind-Body Practices Relevant to People With Cognitive-Linguistic 421
Challenges?
How Might Hypnosis and Visualization Be Relevant to People With Neurogenic 423
Communication Disorders?
What Are the Potential Roles of Religion and Spirituality in Acquired Neurogenic
Communication Disorders? 424
How Might Natural Product Use Be Relevant to People With Cognitive-Linguistic
Challenges? 424
Why Are Complementary and Integrative Approaches Increasing in Popularity? 425
Frustration With Current Options 425
Increasing Awareness 426
Expanded Funding 426
Increasing Evidence 426
Aggressive Marketing 426
What Is the Status of the Evidence Base Supporting Alternative Approaches to 426
Improving Cognitive-Communicative Abilities?
How Might SLPs Support People Considering Complementary and Alternative 427
Approaches to Cognitive-Communicative Wellness?
Stay Within Your Scope of Practice 427
Engage Only in Methods You Are Trained in and Competent to Carry Out 427
Emphasize Complementary Over Alternative Approaches to Direct Intervention for
Communication and Cognition 427
Keep an Open, Nonjudgmental Attitude and Appreciate Multicultural Differences 427
xix
Contents  

Encourage Caution When Counseling People Considering Alternative and 428


Complementary Approaches
Learning and Reflection Activities 429

Section VIII. Specific Treatment Approaches

Chapter 29. Specific Approaches for Promoting Compensatory Communication Strategies 433
What Is Promoting Aphasics’ Communicative Effectiveness (PACE)? 433
On What Principles Is PACE Treatment Based? 433
How Is PACE Treatment Implemented? 434
What Is the Status of PACE in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice? 436
What Is the Communicative Drawing Program? 436
On What Principles Is CDP Based? 436
How Is CDP Implemented? 436
What Is the Status of the CDP in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice? 437
What Is Back to the Drawing Board? 438
On What Principles Is BDB Treatment Based? 438
How Is BDB Implemented? 438
What Is the Status of BDB in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice? 439
What Is Visual Action Therapy? 439
On What Principles Is VAT Treatment Based? 440
How Is VAT Implemented? 440
What Is the Status of VAT in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice? 440
Learning and Reflection Activities 441

Chapter 30. Specific Approaches for Enhancing Expressive Language 443


What Is Constraint-Induced Language Therapy? 443
On What Principles Is CILT Based? 443
How Is CILT Implemented? 444
What Is the Status of CILT in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice? 444
What Is Script Training? 445
On What Principles Is Script Training Based? 445
How Is Script Training Implemented? 445
What Is the Status of Script Training in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice? 445
What Is Melodic Intonation Therapy? 446
On What Principles Is MIT Based? 446
How Is MIT Implemented? 447
Level I 447
Level II 447
Level III 448
Level IV 448
What Is the Status of MIT in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice? 449
What Is Voluntary Control of Involuntary Utterances? 451
On What Principles Is VCIU Treatment Based? 451
How Is VCIU Implemented? 451
What Is the Status of VCIU in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice? 451
What Is Response Elaboration Training? 451
On What Principles Is RET Based? 452
xx  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

How Is RET Implemented? 452


What Is the Status of RET in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice? 452
What Is Treatment for Aphasic Perseveration? 452
On What Principles Is TAP Based? 453
How Is TAP Implemented? 453
What Is the Status of TAP in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice? 454
Learning and Reflection Activities 454

Chapter 31. Specific Approaches for Improving Word Finding and Lexical Processing 455
What Are Cueing Hierarchies for the Treatment of Anomia? 455
On What Principles Are Cueing Hierarchies for the Treatment of Anomia Based? 455
How Is Cueing Hierarchy Treatment Implemented? 456
What Is the Status of Cueing Hierarchies for the Treatment of Anomia in Terms of 456
Evidence-Based Practice?
What Is Semantic Feature Analysis? 457
On What Principles Is SFA Treatment Based? 457
How Is SFA Treatment Implemented? 457
Baseline Phase and Target Selection 457
Semantic Feature Analysis Chart Method 458
Graphic Organizer Method 459
What Is the Status of SFA in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice? 460
What Is Phonological Components Analysis? 461
On What Principles Is PCA Treatment Based? 461
How Is PCA Treatment Implemented? 461
What Is the Status of PCA in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice? 463
What Is Verb Network Strengthening Treatment? 463
On What Principles Is VNeST Based? 463
How Is VNeST Implemented? 464
Baseline 464
Stimulus Selection and Creation 464
Generation of Agent-Patient Pairs 464
Wh- Questions About Agent-Patient Pairs 465
Semantic Judgments 465
Generation of Agent-Patient Pairs Again 465
What Is the Status of VNeST in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice? 465
What Is Verb as Core? 466
On What Principles Is VAC Treatment Based? 466
How Is VAC Treatment Implemented? 466
What Is the Status of VAC in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice? 467
Learning and Reflection Activities 467

Chapter 32. Specific Approaches for Improving Syntax 469


What Is Treatment of Underlying Forms? 469
On What Principles Is TUF Based? 470
How Is TUF Implemented? 470
Ensuring Metalinguistic Awareness 470
Creating Noncanonical Sentences 471
Thematic Role Training 471
Practice 472
xxi
Contents  

What Is the Status of TUF in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice? 472


What Is Mapping Therapy? 472
On What Principles Is Mapping Therapy Based? 473
How Is Mapping Therapy Implemented? 473
What Is the Status of Mapping Therapy in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice? 473
What Is the Sentence Production Program for Aphasia? 474
On What Principles Is SPPA Treatment Based? 474
How Is SPPA Implemented? 474
What Is the Status of SPPA and HELPSS in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice? 475
Learning and Reflection Activities 476

Chapter 33. Specific Approaches for Improving Reading and Writing 477
What Are Basic Principles That Underlie Most Writing- and Reading-Focused 477
Programs for People With Aphasia?
What Is Copy and Recall Treatment? 478
On What Principles Is CART Based? 478
How Is CART Implemented? 478
What Is the Status of CART in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice? 479
What Is Anagram and Copy Treatment? 480
On What Principle Is ACT Based? 480
How Is ACT Implemented? 480
What Is the Status of ACT in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice? 480
What Is the Problem-Solving Approach? 481
On What Principles Is the Problem-Solving Approach Based? 481
How Is the Problem-Solving Approach Implemented? 481
What Is the Status of the Problem-Solving Approach in Terms of Evidence-Based 481
Practice?
What Is Multiple Oral Rereading? 481
On What Principles Is MOR Treatment Based? 481
How Is MOR Implemented? 482
What Is the Status of MOR in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice? 482
What Is Oral Reading for Language in Aphasia? 483
On What Principles Is ORLA Treatment Based? 483
How Is ORLA Treatment Implemented? 483
What Is the Status of ORLA in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice? 483
Learning and Reflection Activities 485

Epilogue 487
Glossary 489
References 509
Index 575
What Is Special About This Book?

Many books about acquired neurogenic disorders of


language and cognition are replete with important
A Focus on What It Takes to Become
information about neurological aspects and theoret- a Truly Exceptional Clinician
ical accounts of normal and disordered cognitive-
​linguistic processing. Many offer content on assess- What does it take to become an excellent clinician?
ment and treatment. More and more have focused What can we do to become that person? What makes
on biopsychosocial models of body structure and one clinician great and another not so great? It is not
function, quality-of-life goals, multicultural matters, just knowledge and skill, although these are cer-
and evidence-based practice. The aim of this book is tainly crucial components of clinical excellence; it is
to do all of this, with additional special and import- also a host of other qualities. What are those quali-
ant emphases. ties? How does one develop them? The intent is to
motivate you, foster your learning, encourage you,
lead you, and support you to gain not just all-im-
An Emphasis on Person-Centered, portant knowledge but to practice skills and chal-
Empowering Approaches to Life Participation lenge attitudes and values on your path to becoming
the ultimate excellent clinical aphasiologist. Some-
times for efficiency, I will be directive. Do this. Do
In many of the clinical contexts in which we work, not do that. I offer an insider’s view of what many
there tends to be a greater focus on deficits than experienced experts in this area think you need to
strengths, and a greater focus on specific areas of know, what you should be able to do, and what you
communicative challenge than on how people most ought to appreciate and consider. In the end, each
want to participate in what is important to them. reader is the one best suited to define and become
Throughout this book, we emphasize the Life Par- the ultimate best clinician.
ticipation Approach to Aphasia (Chapey et al., 2008),
while recognizing the importance of helping peo-
ple regain specific cognitive and linguistic abilities.
Treatment of a Full Gamut of Acquired
Many adults with neurogenic disorders struggle to
Neurogenic Cognitive-Linguistic Challenges
be recognized as fully human and competent. Read-
ers are encouraged to commit to strength-based
approaches that heighten the self-efficacy of the This book is meant for practicing clinicians as well
people we serve. Throughout our work in assess- as for students and others wishing to learn about
ment, treatment, advocacy, counseling, education, clinical aphasiology. Students, faculty, and practic-
and research, we have ample opportunities — and a ing clinicians we polled wholeheartedly requested
moral imperative — to foster empowering, affirma- a book that cuts across specific etiologies and diag-
tive means of considering and coping with chronic nostic categories. The majority of us who teach in
aspects of communication challenges. this area must combine multiple topics within single

xxiii
xxiv  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

courses or course sequences. Some texts in this arena


address specific clinical syndromes, focusing exclu-
Appreciation for and Integration of
sively, for example, on communication and aging, Diverse Frameworks and Theoretical
aphasia, right hemisphere syndrome, or traumatic Perspectives Related to Neurogenic
brain injury (TBI). Those who have the luxury of Disorders of Language and Cognition
more courses to cover content in this arena contrib-
uted ideas about how they divvy up content across Excellent clinicians do not adhere to a single the-
courses. So in this book, with intention, we consider oretical framework alone and stick to that at all
theories, causes, and neurological underpinnings costs. Rather, they learn a great deal about multiple
associated with diverse neurogenic cognitive-​linguistic approaches based on multiple frameworks and inte-
disorders in Section II. Then, in Section III, we delve grate multiple theories in making clinical decisions.
further into each of the distinct categories of apha- They are open to revising their favored theoretical
sia syndromes, cognitive-communicative challenges perspectives based on new information. They con-
associated with TBI, cognitive-communicative chal- stantly reflect on the results of published studies
lenges associated with right brain injury, primary with results observed clinically in an ongoing way
progressive aphasia, and cognitive-communicative with every individual with whom they work. To this
challenges associated with dementia. We continue end, readers are challenged to grasp and integrate
to build on that content within and across each of multiple perspectives at once and to think critically
those categories as we engage more deeply in ser- about their own preferences, biases, and needs for
vice delivery (Section IV), assessment (Section V), further learning.
and general and specific approaches to treatment
(Sections VI and VII). This complements the cycling
approach, which we consider further in a moment.
This approach also helps us appreciate how Thoughtful Attention to Culturally Responsive
what has been done to advance work within each Practice and Diversity, Equity, and Inclusion
specialty area can be shared across other areas. Exam-
ples abound. Great work on supported communica-
tion for people with aphasia can be embodied in our In recognition that there is systemic bias and dis-
work on communication challenges associated with crimination that affects all of our social systems,
right hemisphere injury, TBI, dementia, and so on. we take seriously the role of clinical aphasiologists
Much work in interprofessional practice, individual- as allies and accomplices in supporting the people
ized approaches, coaching models, and environmen- we serve as well as our colleagues. Throughout the
tal systems models for supporting TBI survivors can book, we promote means of addressing the many
be further extended to people with aphasia. Wonder- “-isms” (e.g., racism, ageism, ableism, sexism, het-
ful progress in focusing on reminiscence strategies, erosexism, cisgenderism, linguicism) that are inher-
functional memory enhancement, identity support, ent in the systems within which we work to promote
and recognition of strengths in people with demen- access, equity, and human rights for the people we
tia can be applied to all people with all other types of serve. This includes careful attention to terminology
neurogenic disorders. Principles of critical thinking we use.
applied to assessment and diagnostic problem solv-
ing in people with right hemisphere syndrome can
be transferred to clinical challenges when working
Global Perspectives for a Global Readership
with people with any type of acquired neurogenic
communication disorder. Many assessment tools and
treatment methods can be applied across diagnostic The content in this book is intended to be relevant
categories. There can be great advantage in treating globally. Worldwide resources are provided, for
each diagnostic group specifically and also strength- example, in terms of related professional associ-
ening synergies in our understanding of and work ations and resources to support people with neu-
with people from diverse diagnostic groups. rogenic communication disorders and the people
What Is Special About This Book?   xxv

who care about them. Where content is specific to made lists of desired and undesired features from
particular regions, such as in sections addressing students’ perspectives.
health care trends and cultural factors that affect Here are additional requests from students and
clinical practice, this is noted, along with observa- instructors that were considered in the development
tions regarding general trends and national varia- of this text:
tions. Although content and resources are geared
toward an English-speaking readership, numerous • a useful clinical resource for years to come,
references point to further opportunities for clinical not just for a course
and research work and advocacy all over the world. • strong theoretical foundations
Global and multicultural perspectives are infused • an academically rigorous orientation,
throughout. conveyed with a friendly and personal voice
• coverage of the broad spectrum of the science
and art of clinical practice
• recognition of the importance of
An Evidence-Based How-To Clinical Guide
interdisciplinary and interprofessional
education, research, and clinical practice
Many of us who teach and/or supervise students • thorough coverage of diagnostic processes,
and beginning clinicians are especially familiar with including extensive resources on assessment
the disconnect between what clinical students learn • a process analysis approach for analyzing
in their academic programs and what they feel pre- communicative performance and strategically
pared to do when working as clinical professionals. interpreting results of ongoing assessment
Clear guidelines, along with references to theoret- processes infused throughout intervention
ical principles and research-based suggestions, are • an evidence-informed how-to guide to
provided for how to carry out over 50 different gen- treatment with clear guidelines on how to
eral and specific treatment approaches. The book’s carry out treatment approaches as prescribed,
direct style and practical orientation will be useful all with an appreciation for practice-based
to clinical students and professionals alike and will evidence
continue to be helpful to students long after they • functional and practical approaches
graduate from clinical programs. • key terms bolded within each chapter and
listed in a cumulative glossary
• diagrams, charts, illustrations, summary
Addressing What Instructors tables, and a detailed index
• substantial up-to-date references
and Students Requested
• inclusion of multicultural and multinational
content as well as content on counseling,
A great deal of research on textbook needs was done ethics, and legal aspects of working with
before launching the writing of the first and second people with neurogenic communication
editions of this book. Student interns in business disorders
administration joined forces with students in com- • use of gender-attuned and person-first
munication sciences and disorders to help engage language, embracing and inclusive of readers,
in multifaceted needs assessments. We polled hun- colleagues, and the people we serve clinically
dreds of students in clinical speech-language pathol- regardless of race, ethnicity, gender identity,
ogy (SLP) programs, instructors at over 75 different gender expression, age, sexual orientation,
programs who teach in related areas, plus leaders of and other attributes
clinical programs serving adults with TBI, demen- • clear and concise clinical examples to ensure
tia, and aphasia. We studied curricular requirements relevance of information based on realistic
of over 200 academic programs to see what topic scenarios
groupings are most commonly taught and in what • ancillary online materials with links to videos
combination. We reviewed existing textbooks and and other teaching/learning resources
xxvi  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

• size and weight such that the book is not and between cognitive-linguistic challenges and
cumbersome to carry or impossible to fit in a quality of life, are aware that the more we learn, the
backpack more questions we generate for ourselves and oth-
ers. There are few definitive or concrete answers to
In addition, many said that having multiple clinical questions in the world of aphasiology. Still,
authors contribute to a single text, if it is to be used it is vitally important that we continue to ask ques-
as a primary book for a course, can lead to inconsis- tions and do our best to probe for answers. In this
tencies in approach, rigor, and voice, such that a text light, this book is organized around queries — prob-
by a single author who integrates the work of thou- ing questions that have varied levels of superficiality
sands of others has important advantages. and profundity, of simplicity or complexity, and of
That’s a tall order! We invite you to provide definitiveness or open-endedness— with a vast array
feedback on how we may do better in terms of any of possible answers. I hope that you will find it use-
of these goals in future editions of this text. ful to pose these queries to yourself as an upcoming
or established clinician. I hope you will find queries
that tempt you further into an even deeper dive into
this fascinating world.
Incorporation of Adult Learning Theory Queries tend to make us contemplate and make
and Evidence-Based Pedagogy associations related to our possible responses before
we actually start to answer them; they foster reflec-
tion. “It is in the interstices between the questions
Pedagogic approaches embraced in the design of this
and the answer that minds turn,” observes Weimer
book consist of two broad categories: those directly
(2014, p. 1). Any query ideally leads to new reader-​
implemented in the structure and content of the book
generated queries, encouraging self-directed study
and those recommended through learning activities,
so vital to adult learning and critical thinking (Brook-
online resources, and suggestions to instructors. The
field, 2012; Knowles, 1984). A secondary benefit of
book content incorporates means of guiding readers
the query structure is that it clarifies the learning
with intent, through levels of learning akin to the
objectives related to each content area. Readers may
components of Bloom’s Taxonomy (Bloom, 1956)
use the queries as opportunities for self-assessment
and its more recent variations (e.g., Anderson, 1999;
as they study, reflect, and answer the queries in their
Laddha et al., 2021): conceptual development, syn-
own words.
thesis, analysis, application to content already mas-
And for instructors who tailor assessments to
tered, and fostering of broader understanding and
the contents, this structure may help address the
perspectives on new applications. Here, the levels
age-old question from students, “What will be on
of learning are treated as interdependent, not linear
the test?”
and hierarchical, as if one must pass from one level
to the next. A focus on the reader’s own develop-
ment as a clinician (“personal characteristics” within
the adult learning framework; see Cross, 1981) is Engaged Learning
intertwined with potential “situational characteris-
tics” for learning (e.g., independent study, online or
Many of the exercises in the Activities for Learn-
in-person coursework, studies to complement clini-
ing and Reflection sections are offered in a learn-
cal practicum).
by-doing rather than just a learn-by-reading mode.
Although students certainly can learn through
lectures and readings, means of ensuring active
Query-Based Approach and Enlivening engagement with what they are learning helps to
of Learning Objectives ensure better retention and likelihood of application
(Fink, 2003; Kember et al., 2008). As readers attach
personal relevance to what they are learning, they
Any of us who study the complex relationships are more likely to take ownership of the correspond-
between cognitive-linguistic abilities and the brain, ing content.
What Is Special About This Book?   xxvii

Strijbos & Fischer, 2007), case-based learning (Cha-


Cycling Approach bon & Cohn, 2011), and problem-based learning (Jin
& Bridges, 2014; Lawlor et al., 2015; Prosser & Sze,
Engaging in a cycling approach to adult learning, we 2014) are all directly amenable to teaching and learn-
recognize the importance of presenting content with ing related to the contents of this book. Service-learn-
redundancy, not such that it is presented the same ing approaches (Corless et al., 2009; Doherty & Lay,
way over and over again, but such that one aspect of 2019; Kosky & Schlisselberg, 2013; Sabo et al., 2015;
coverage of a topic complements another. One exam- Stevens, 2009) are ideal for much of the practical
ple of the cycling approach is the way that we con- content in this book. Examples of related projects
tinuously build upon knowledge related to specific include providing in-services at a health care agency,
diagnostic categories, extending that knowledge assisting with a caregiver support group, develop-
across varied acquired neurogenic conditions (as ing reminiscence projects for residents of a long-term
discussed earlier). Another example is that we start care facility, or developing a respite volunteer pro-
out exploring the life participation approach as an gram for adults with neurological disorders in your
especially valuable framework for considering our local community. Such activities are also amenable
work, not only with people who have aphasia but to study-abroad global health projects, if carefully
with anyone coping with a communication disorder; designed with clear ethical principles in mind (Hal-
then, we revisit it from varied perspectives through- lowell, 2012b; Hallowell, Combiths, et al., 2021).
out the book. We consider its relevance to advocacy Additionally, students engaged in interpro-
and to education of people with language disorders fessional learning opportunities (Interprofessional
and the people who care about them. We also think Education Collaborative Expert Panel, 2011; World
about it as a model to use in contextualizing spe- Health Organization, 2010; Zraick et al., 2014) may
cific theoretical and evidence-based approaches to make use of several aspects of this book. For exam-
assessment and treatment. ple, basic content will ideally lead to an appreciation
for the types of interdisciplinary and interprofes-
sional teams and collaborations through which
Adaptability for Multiple Pedagogic much work in aphasiology is accomplished. Addi-
tionally, suggestions for outreach, advocacy, coun-
Methods for Classroom-Based Courses,
seling, and global health experiences may be carried
Independent Study, and Online Coursework
forth in planning interprofessional activities among
students, academic and clinical faculty members,
The book is organized to be adaptable for varied and community groups or agencies.
teaching and learning methods. A flipped classroom
approach (Hoepner & Hemmerich, 2018; Keengwe
et al., 2014) may be ideal for content that students
Online Materials
need to study primarily on their own, such as ter-
minology, basics of neurophysiology, and the blood
supply to the brain, prior to integrating the related Supplemental materials include PowerPoints to guide
knowledge into in-class activities and discussions. discussions pertaining to content in each chapter,
It can also be optimal when students study about additional discussion points and learning activities,
assessment and treatment methods before related links to video examples and helpful online resources,
hands-on activities and discussion. Using the learn- and a test bank that includes multiple-choice, fill-in-
ing activities sections in each chapter to prepare the blank, matching, true/false, short-answer, and
ahead of class sessions can also be effective in this essay items, all cross-referenced to the content areas
regard and can be combined with collaborative addressed. Visit the companion website and explore:
www
learning methods. http://www.pluralpublishing.com/publication/
Team-based and collaborative learning (Abdel­ aoanld2e
khalek et al., 2010; Barkley et al., 2014; Hoepner If you have ideas you would like to share for
et al., 2021; Johnson & Johnson, 2009; John-Steiner, the website for the next edition of this book, please
2006; Michaelsen et al., 2008; Millis & Cottell, 1998; be in touch.
xxviii  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

• updated attention to culturally responsive


Updates in the Second Edition terminology, pronouns, and content
throughout to promote diversity, equity,
Updates in this second edition include the following: and inclusion, and advocacy for clinicians to
support these values in tangible ways
• expansion of content to reflect important recent
developments, with findings of over five
hundred new studies integrated throughout About the Book Cover
• enhanced foci on primary progressive aphasia
and cognitive-communicative challenges
associated with dementia, TBI, and right brain The phoenix rising from a changed brain represents
injury the human spirit moving onward and upward from
• amplification of chapter content in chapter neurological challenges. It is a symbol of honor and
titles and introductions, while maintaining the affirmation for people with neurogenic commu-
order of chapters nication disorders and the people who care about
• added content by and about people them — all of whom ideally continuously heal and
with the lived experience of neurogenic re-create themselves with the strengths they still
communication challenges have, even discovering new strengths along the way.
• many more images to support learning
• greater devotion to relevance for readers Cover design by Taylor Reeves.
regardless of nationality
Acknowledgments

Profound motivation for this book has come from rial, and keep our focus on empowering people to
numerous people who have shared with me deeply communicate and engage meaningfully in life.
about their own experiences as phoenixes rising Throughout my work on this book, I contin-
from challenges of changed brains; in this light ued to draw inspiration from colleagues dedicated
I especially thank Seth Teicher, Jane Hamlin, and to making life better for people with acquired neu-
Deb Dakin. I acknowledge inspiration for this book rogenic cognitive-linguistic disorders through my
from my precious friend Sadanand Singh, who engagement with Aphasia Access, the National
convinced me that it had an essential purpose and Aphasia Association, the Academy of Neurologic
that I would be the right vehicle for it. I thank, too, Communication Disorders and Sciences (ANCDS),
my longtime buddy and partner in global shenan- the Clinical Aphasiology Conference, Special Inter-
igans, Angie Singh, for love and encouragement est Group on Neurophysiology and Neurogenic
not to let that inspiration diminish. Since the first Speech and Language Disorders of the American
edition, my partner, James Latimer, has enlightened Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASHA),
me greatly through our collaboration in work with Virtual Connections for Aphasia, Aphasia Recov-
couples coping with aphasia, and we have learned ery Connection, and numerous additional networks
much together through wondrous life participation across the globe.
escapades and musical fun with people with neu- I have been honored by thoughtful suggestions,
rogenic communication challenges and people who feedback, and support from numerous colleagues
love them. This has added to new approaches and (who are also dear friends), especially Barbara
wisdom reflected throughout this edition. James has Shadden, Audrey Holland, Terry Wertz, Michelle
also been a vibrant champion for this book, espe- Bourgeois, Natalie Douglas, Melinda Corwin, Jerry
cially during distractions and existential challenges. Hoepner, Darlene Williamson, Carol Dow-Richards,
I extend warm thanks to the amazing students Ellen Bernstein-Ellis, Edye Strand, Pam Smith, Hyang
who have helped with a great deal of background Hee Kim, Hughlet Morris, Lynn Maher, Craig Line-
work, most especially Laura Chapman, Carley Moy- baugh, Travis Threats, N. Shivashankar, Sonal Chi-
her, and Lindsay LaClaire. I thank Taylor Reeves, the tris, Roberta Elman, Kathryn Shelley, Janet Hawley,
gifted artist who designed its cover and patiently and my adept anonymous reviewers. I appreciate,
worked with me through numerous iterations of too, the staff at Plural Publishing, especially my dili-
illustrations in the book. Special appreciation goes gent editor, Christina Gunning, Lori Asbury, produc-
to Nicole Linn and Seth Breitenstein for their exten- tion manager, Jessica Bristow, production editor, Lori
sive needs analysis that helped determine what Eby, copy editor, and other essential experts, includ-
it is that students and instructors most wanted in ing Valerie Johns and Elisa Andersen. In addition,
such a book. I also thank additional current and for- my administrative sidekick, Donna Wisniowski, has
mer students who helped with editing, literature been a fabulous ally since the day we met.
updates, feedback, and development of companion I thank my treasured friends who gave me for-
website materials, including Fatimah Hani Hassan, titude and helped me see how I could use challeng-
Javad Anjum, Mohammad Haghighi, Sabine Heuer, ing experiences in my own life to enrich my voice
and Maria Ivanova. I am indebted, too, to all of the as a writer and teacher. Many thanks especially to
students over the many years who have been fun- Michele Kaufman, Molly Morris, Mary Nossek, Kar-
damental in considering how to prioritize content, tini Ahmad, John Burns, Elle Morgan, Bob Berardi,
enhance active engagement with challenging mate- Manon Floquet, Patty Mitchell, Dianne Bouvier,

xxix
xxx  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Tim LaVelle, the Athens Friends Meeting commu- Nicholas, Nikki, and Oliver Linn; Elizabeth and Max
nity, the Llewellyn Beach community, the Valley Rego; Kirk and Katie Hallowell; Vickie and Steve
Improv, Phantom Sheep Players, and the Lost in Kracke; Todd and Lisa Hallowell; Harold Smith; Jean
Lodi Arm-wrestling clan, all of whom help me stay Latimer; Becky and Alan Kreczko; Barbara and Scott
grounded and refresh my inner joy. Butterworth; Jennifer and Doug Squires; Thomas
I am extremely grateful to members of my fam- Merton Latimer; Victor Hallowell Latimer; and all
ily who have supported me all along, especially of the rest of our sweet eclectic network of kin.
About the Author

tored and supported others in winning over US$150


million in gifts, grants, and contracts. She is a pio-
neer in the use of eye tracking and pupillometry to
study complex diagnostic issues related to cognition
and language in adults with neurological disorders,
and holds U.S. and international patents on related
technology. Additionally, she has long been active
in interdisciplinary research and advocacy related to
aging and end-of-life care, support for caregivers of
adults with disabilities, and technology to aid peo-
ple with disabilities.
A former president of the Council of Academic
Programs in Communication Sciences and Disorders
(CAPCSD), Hallowell chaired the first-ever Global
Summit on Higher Education in Communication Sci-
ences and Disorders and is deeply engaged in help-
ing to foster new academic and clinical programs,
Brooke Hallowell, PhD, CCC-SLP, brings to this emphasizing cultural responsiveness and invest-
book over 30 years of clinical, research, teaching, ment in local expertise, especially in underserved
and advocacy experience to support adults with regions of the world. She has held appointments at
acquired neurogenic communication challenges. She universities in Korea, Malaysia, and Honduras and
serves on boards and committees of several national is involved in academic and clinical collaboration in
and international organizations, including Aphasia Brazil, Cambodia, China, Honduras, India, Russia,
Access, the National Aphasia Association, the Acad- and Vietnam. A Fellow of ASHA, Hallowell is also
emy of Neurologic Communication Disorders and the recipient of the CAPCSD Honors of the Council,
Sciences, and the American Speech-Language-Hear- the Asia Pacific Society in Speech-Language-Hearing
ing Association (ASHA). She is a founding represen- Association Outstanding Contribution Award, and
tative of the Global Rehabilitation Alliance (GRA), the ASHA Certificate of Recognition for Outstanding
an affiliate of the World Health Organization (WHO) Contributions in International Achievement. She is
in Geneva, Switzerland, and serves on the GRA’s a former Fulbright Fellow, and U.S. national finalist
advocacy committee. She is an active consultant for the Thomas Ehrlich Award for Service Learning.
to the WHO on guidance regarding rehabilitation Hallowell serves as dean of the School of Health
related to COVID-19, and she chairs ASHA’s com- Sciences and professor in Communication Sciences
mittee on ethics in global engagement. She is also and Disorders at Springfield College. She previously
an editorial board member and reviewer for many served as executive director of the Collaborative on
scholarly journals and granting agencies. Aging, associate dean for Research and Sponsored
Hallowell has garnered over $15 million in Programs in the College of Health and Human Ser-
funded grants, with extramural support from vices; director of the School of Hearing, Speech and
such agencies as the National Institutes of Health, Language Sciences; director of the Neurolinguistics
the National Science Foundation, and the Health Laboratory; founding co-director of the Global Health
Resources Service Administration. She has men- Initiative; founding coordinator of the Diabetes

xxxi
xxxii  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Research Initiative; and co-director of the Appa- adulthood, she entered the realm of clinical apha-
lachian Rural Health Institute at Ohio University. siology with a personal connection to those who
Passionate about interdisciplinary collaboration, have important things to say but are not able to
she has held adjunct and affiliated appointments in express them fully. Being the mother of a child
family medicine, neurology, biomedical engineering, with a severe traumatic brain injury who is now a
gerontology, and Asian studies. Her career history thriving and extremely competent adult, she has
also includes employment as a professional musi- firsthand knowledge about what it is that people
cian, interpreter/translator, and French instructor. need and want throughout the course of rehabili-
She holds an A.B. (magna cum laude) from Brown tation. According to Hallowell, these experiences
University, an M.S. from Lamar University, a Cer- are a large part of what drives her passion to help
tificat d’Etudes Supérieures from the Conservatoire clinicians and clinicians-in-training focus not only
National de France, and a PhD from the University on gaining clinical knowledge and skills but also
of Iowa. on wisdom, compassion, humility, and other char-
Having had selective mutism as a child and acteristics that will propel them to become ultimate
having been an extremely shy person into young excellent clinicians.
To Jimmy and my marvelous family, with zealous gratitude for so much love.
SECTION I

Welcome and Introduction


2  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

In this section, we affirm the intriguing nature of this arena truly excellent, consider how we might
neurogenic communication disorders and set the best strategize to become such a person, and review
stage for key points and concepts to be affirmed in key resources that will be useful along the way. In
later sections of the book. In Chapter 1, we review Chapter 3, we review basic yet critically important
basic content about the nature of neurogenic lan- considerations related to the way we talk and write
guage disorders and consider the fascinating inter- about people with ability differences, the people
disciplinary nature of clinical aphasiology and the who care about them, and the professionals who
many associated career opportunities. In Chapter 2, work with them.
we delve into the topic of what makes a clinician in
CHAPTER
1
Welcome to the Fantastic World of
Research and Clinical Practice in Acquired
Neurogenic Communication Disorders

I could not imagine any academic or professional


pursuit more rewarding than diving into the amaz-
What Are Acquired
ing world of adult neurogenic disorders of cognition Cognitive-Linguistic Disorders?
and language. I took my first dive as an undergrad-
uate student, having no idea of the fabulous adven- When we talk about “aphasia and related disorders,”
tures and opportunities to which that would lead. we are typically referring to acquired neurogenic lan-
Whether you are a certified speech-language pathol- guage disorders and acquired cognitive-linguistic disor-
ogist (SLP), a clinician in a related field, a neuroscien- ders. These are any of a wide array of disorders of
tist with clinical interests, a student, or an otherwise language formulation, comprehension, and cogni-
engaged reader, and whether you are immersing tive processing caused by problems in the brain of a
yourself or just getting your toes wet in this clinical person who had previously acquired language. They
arena, I hope that you find your experience with this are part of a larger category of acquired neurogenic
book and with this topic informative, inspiring, and communication disorders, which also includes neuro-
challenging. genic speech disorders, most commonly referred to as
After reading and reflecting on the content in motor speech disorders.
this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in The definitions, etiologies, and descriptions of
your own words, the following queries: specific types of acquired neurogenic language dis-
orders are discussed in detail in subsequent chapters.
1. What are acquired cognitive-linguistic As a means of introduction here, let’s briefly con-
disorders? sider which types of disorders constitute acquired
2. Which neurogenic communication disorders neurogenic language and cognitive-linguistic dis-
are not acquired cognitive-linguistic disorders? orders versus other types of communication disor-
3. What is clinical aphasiology? ders. Aphasia is by definition an acquired language
4. What is so fantastic about the world of disorder. Ever since the term aphasia was first coined
neurogenic communication disorders? in 1864 by Armand Trousseau (Tesak & Code, 2008),
5. What disciplines are relevant to aphasiology? it has been defined in many different ways. Aphasia
6. What is known about the incidence and has also been examined from a multitude of perspec-
prevalence of acquired neurogenic language tives or frameworks, each of which may lead people
disorders? studying aphasia to focus on specific aspects of how
7. Where do aphasiologists work? it is defined. The wide array of perspectives from
8. What is the career outlook for clinical which we might consider, study, and theorize about
aphasiologists? aphasia need not distract us from clarity in defining

3
4  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

just what it is and is not. If you plan to work with regulation, perception, and other important aspects
people who have aphasia in any context, it is vitally of everyday functioning in our information-rich
important that you be able to clearly and succinctly and social world. Cases in which a language prob-
define what aphasia is. A simple way to do this is to lem is secondary to a cognitive problem are broadly
make sure that, however you define it, you include categorized as cognitive-linguistic disorders, not
four elements in your definition: simply language disorders. Some categories of neu-
rogenic cognitive-linguistic disorders are referred
1. It is acquired. to according to symptom constellations; they have
2. It has a neurological cause. labels that are based on one or more impairments
3. It affects reception and expression of language (e.g., dyslexia, dysgraphia). Others are referred to
across modalities. according to the associated cause. For example, one
4. It is not a sensory, psychiatric, or intellectual might refer to cognitive-linguistic disorders asso-
disorder. ciated with traumatic brain injury (TBI) to capture
any of a constellation of symptoms related to lan-
We consider each of these elements in more guage and information processing that may occur
detail in Chapter 4. We also explore how, as individ- due to TBI. Some have labels associated with an
ual scholars and clinicians, we might choose differ- underlying cause, even though the etiology is not
ent words to define aphasia based on our preferred incorporated into the label. For example, a favored
theoretical perspectives regarding aphasia. term for language problems resulting from dementia
Dyslexia is a reading disorder that may or may is language of generalized intellectual impairment.
not be an actual language disorder per se. Deep dys- A favored term for language problems associated
lexia is a language disorder. This form of dyslexia with transient confusional states is language of con-
and its varied manifestations entail problems of fusion. Still other categories of neurogenic cogni-
actual linguistic processing of written material, as tive-linguistic disorders are referred to according to
opposed to more superficial visual processing of the the location of the injury to the brain that caused the
physical characteristics of graphemes (any written loss (e.g., right hemisphere syndrome [RHS], also
representation, such as letters, words, and punctu- called right brain syndrome [RBS]).
ation marks, and characters in non-Western scripts).
Dysgraphia is a writing disorder. Like dys-
lexia, it has deep and superficial forms; the deeper Which Neurogenic Communication Disorders
forms, which entail converting semantic content to
Are Not Acquired Language Disorders?
graphemes, are those that qualify as true language
disorders. Both dyslexia and dysgraphia may be
congenital (present from birth or at the earliest Once you are clear about what acquired neurogenic
stages when associated abilities are typically man- cognitive-linguistic disorders are, you can distin-
ifested during development) or acquired. Dyslexia guish them from other disorders that do not fit into
and dysgraphia occur as symptoms of aphasia but this category. By general convention, any problem
may also occur as distinct acquired neurogenic lan- that a person is born with is not an acquired disorder.
guage disorders in people without aphasia. Neurological syndromes present from birth, includ-
We consider the notion of literal, conventional, ing developmental language disorders associated
and recommended uses of the a- and dys- prefixes with cognitive and learning disabilities or delays, are
further in Chapter 3. For now, note that although not acquired. Thus, we do not consider them within
the term aphasia is most often used instead of dys- the scope of this book. This distinction is important.
phasia, the term dyslexia tends to be used instead of The result of losing a previously acquired cognitive
alexia (the latter literally meaning the complete loss or linguistic ability is remarkably different from not
of reading ability). having ever developed such an ability in the first
Several other types of acquired cognitive-lin- place. The result is different in terms of actual brain
guistic problems result from injuries to the brain that structure and function. It is also different in terms
affect behavior, information processing, emotional of the ways that people (and their caregivers and
1. Welcome to the Fantastic World of Research and Clinical Practice in Acquired Disorders   5

others who are important to them) cope with their scientific aspects of these varied areas, even though
disabilities, the specific types of intervention that the literal sense of term is more restricted. For exam-
may be helpful, and the ways in which diagnostic ple, if you were to attend the Clinical Aphasiol-
and treatment services might be made available. Of ogy Conference or a conference of the Academy of
course, people who have congenital disorders may Aphasia (annual international meetings for research
also at some point have a stroke or TBI and may aphasiologists) or read the journal Aphasiology, you
develop dementia. would be exposed to numerous topics reaching
In light of the crucial differences between con- beyond the specific syndrome of aphasia per se. Keep
genital and acquired disorders, most experts agree this in mind as you continue to read this book, as the
that the term child aphasia, as used in the past to cap- term aphasiologist (erring on the side of being too spe-
ture the notion of a congenital language disorder, cialized) is sometimes used interchangeably with the
is a misnomer. Aphasia, by definition, is acquired. term SLP (erring on the side of being too general, as
The preferred term for a condition characterized not all SLPs are truly expert in working with people
by language deficits in the face of relatively age-​ who have neurogenic cognitive-linguistic disorders).
appropriate cognitive abilities in children is specific
language impairment. Certainly, a child may expe-
rience a stroke or TBI resulting in a true aphasia; in What Is So Fantastic About the World of
such cases, it is appropriate to classify the condition
Neurogenic Communication Disorders?
as an acquired language disorder. Still, the course of
recovery and the means of intervention for children
with aphasia are likely to be different in significant There are many enticing aspects of working and
ways compared to people with adult-onset aphasia. studying in the realm of clinical aphasiology. I will
The most common acquired neurogenic motor describe a few of my favorites here in this list of
speech disorders are apraxia of speech (a problem things that we clinical aphasiologists get to do.
of motor programming for speech articulation) and
dysarthria (a problem of innervation of the speech
mechanism for articulation). Although many peo- We Work With Wonderful People and
ple with neurogenic language disorders also have Become Part of Their Rich Life Stories
motor speech disorders, knowing how to distinguish
these general categories of disorders is vital to clin- People with acquired neurogenic cognitive-linguistic
ical excellence. Although motor speech disorders disorders and the people who care about them are
are addressed in this book in terms of clinical prob- diverse in every aspect: age, ethnicity, race, lan-
lem-solving and differential diagnosis in people who guage, education, sexual orientation, gender iden-
also have language disorders, they are not a primary tity, gender expression, life experience, personality,
focus of this book. preferences . . . you name it. As we discuss in more
detail later in this book, when people acquire apha-
sia or related disorders, all aspects of their lives may
be affected, not just their cognitive-linguistic abili-
What Is Clinical Aphasiology?
ties. Thus, all aspects of a person’s life are relevant
to our work. Clinical aphasiologists do not simply
Because of the overlapping areas of scientific and learn about a medical diagnosis, treat it in some
clinical knowledge and skill involved, and because prescriptive way, and then discharge a person from
of the contexts in which we tend to work, many pro- treatment. We get to learn about people’s assorted
fessionals who specialize in research and/or clini- interests, beliefs, relationships, and life goals, and
cal practice in aphasia (aphasiologists in the literal how language use is relevant to what is most import-
sense) are also expert in related neurogenic cogni- ant in their lives. We often become part of the fab-
tive-linguistic, speech, and swallowing disorders in ric of life change and adjustment, helping consider
adults. When we use the term aphasiology, we tend alternatives and possibilities, listening to life stories,
to incorporate topics related to the vast clinical and and nurturing fresh perspectives. We get to assist in
6  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

their career and educational considerations and help level than there can be when working with children.
family members, friends, and professionals learn to People with aphasia, for example, often have a won-
best support them. derful sense of humor about their own unintended
utterances — and about consequences of unintended
aspects of communication — in their daily lives.
We Are Catalysts for Positive Change

A problem with communication affects every aspect We Enjoy Fantastic Local and
of our lives and the lives of those around us. The fact Worldwide Professional Networks
that there is much that can be done to make a differ-
ence in people’s everyday activities and interactions In light of the vastness of life consequences associ-
makes it especially gratifying to work in this arena. ated with acquired neurogenic communication dis-
orders and the interdisciplinary nature of the work
of aphasiologists, we depend on teamwork with a
We Enjoy Empowerment of Others host of professionals in our local clinical and research
Through Advocacy and Leadership work environments. Additionally, there are wonder-
ful local, state/regional, national, and international
Beyond our direct clinical work, we also work to organizations and networks that bring together and
raise awareness of the importance of communication foster continuing education of aphasiologists. Infor-
as a basic human right and of the need to support mation about some key professional organizations
people with communication challenges in protecting and how to get involved is given later in this chapter.
that right. Many of us become leaders in our pro-
fessional contexts as well as in local, national, and
international professional organizations. Our roles Our Work Is Multicultural and Multilingual
as leaders can help us become powerful catalysts
not only for awareness but also for social reform and If you love working across languages and cultures,
policy changes. there are ample opportunities to work in the area of
neurogenic communication disorders with diverse
people throughout the world. There is a dire need
We Enjoy a Great Deal of for aphasiologists who are multicultural and speak
Humor and Fascination more than one language or dialect to assist in fur-
thering the development of assessment and inter-
The variety in the types of errors associated with lin- vention materials across languages and cultures.
guistic structure and social language use in people In many countries, the field of aphasiology is just
with aphasia is vast. Some of the linguistic errors now developing; there is a need for culturally and
and communication mishaps we observe are not linguistically sensitive consultants and volunteers
only fascinating; they can also be charming, quirky, to assist in building new academic and clinical pro-
and downright funny. grams. Opportunities for cross-cultural learning and
In some clinical situations, there is a fine line transnational collaborative adventures abound.
between enjoying humor about something a person
has said or done and respecting their dignity as a
person with a serious disability. In general, though, We Are Lifelong Learners
enjoyment of fun and laughter throughout rehabil-
itation and recovery is shared among all involved, Given the vast scope of our work and the fact that
especially people with communication challenges our expertise crosses many disciplinary boundaries
themselves. One of the delightful aspects of work- daily (as we discuss further in this chapter), there is
ing with a primarily adult population is that there is no way for us to really master all that would be ideal
much more tolerance for humor at a metalinguistic for us to master as excellent aphasiologists. If you
1. Welcome to the Fantastic World of Research and Clinical Practice in Acquired Disorders   7

enjoy studying, reading, and learning, you can find


Box
intriguing challenges to do this in your everyday life 1–1 Examples of Disciplinary Areas
as an aphasiologist. Relevant to Acquired Neurogenic
Cognitive-Linguistic Disorders

We Tap Into Our Most Scientific and Our • Actuarial sciences • Law
Most Creative Selves at the Same Time • Anthropology • Linguistics
• Art • Mathematics
In our work to help foster recovery, we must be • Art therapy • Multiculturalism
strong scientists. For example, we must be knowl- • Audiology • Music therapy
edgeable about neurophysiology and theories • Bilingualism and • Neurology
behind fostering brain changes as well as about multilingualism • Neuroscience
neuroimaging, statistics, information processing, • Biology • Occupational
and psycholinguistic and neurolinguistic modeling. • Biomechanics therapy
At the same time, we must be passionately creative • Biomedical • Otolaryngology
artists, appreciating things that sometimes surpass engineering • Pet therapy
scientific description and logical explanation. Our • Business and • Pharmacology
investment in supporting rehabilitation reflects management • Philosophy
wonder, creative listening, and incorporation of aes- • Cognitive science • Physiatry
thetics, art, and music in our work. • Communication • Physical therapy
• Computer science • Physics
• Counseling • Political science
We Have Rich Career Opportunities • Education • Public Health
• Electrophysiology • Psychology
There are ample career opportunities for clinical • Engineering • Rehabilitation
aphasiologists in terms of the availability of profes- • Foreign languages • Social work
sional positions, as well as the number and diversity • Gender studies • Social justice
of employment contexts in which they may work. • Gerontology • Sociology
We explore these in an introductory way later in this • Global health • Software
chapter and then delve further into varied aspects of • Health care development
clinical practice settings in Section IV. administration • Speech-language
• Hospice and pathology
palliative care • Statistics
What Disciplines Are Relevant to
Aphasia and Related Disorders?

What Is Known About the Incidence


There is no single field of study that “owns” apha-
and Prevalence of Acquired
siology. The expertise of clinical aphasiologists
Neurogenic Language Disorders?
depends on the integration of content across numer-
ous disciplines. Examples of relevant fields are listed
in Box 1–1. It is important that we respect the scope In the chapters addressing specific categories of
of practice of the professional disciplines in which neurogenic language disorders, introductory infor-
we hold academic degrees, certification, and/or mation is provided about the incidence (the likely
licensure. Still, recognizing the relevance of a mul- number of newly diagnosed cases per specified unit
titude of disciplines to what any one aphasiologist of time) and prevalence (the proportion of specified
has to offer is vital to our career-long development populations that had or have the disorder at a partic-
of expertise. ular time). Many such statistics are biased in terms of
8  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

representing the United States and other minority- ingly recognized, much more will be known about
world countries and regions. This is because such the distribution of neurogenic language disorders
statistics pertaining to much of the rest of the world’s throughout the world.
populations are lacking. For some regions, no data
are available, and for others, the validity of the data
is questionable. Overall, the validity of reported data
Where Do Aphasiologists Work?
about the worldwide incidence and prevalence of
disabilities in general and of communication disor-
ders specifically is suspect. There are five primary SLPs specializing in acquired neurogenic cognitive-
reasons for this: linguistic disorders are employed in a wide range
of settings. One of the great advantages of a career
• Health survey sampling methods as practiced in this area is that, once one has the required clini-
in much of the world are often inadequate, cal credentials, one has countless options to change
most commonly due to transportation career settings multiple times if desired without
challenges, a lack of thorough staff training, having to change careers per se. The most common
a mismatch in languages and culture professional contexts include hospitals, rehabilitation
between surveyors and people surveyed, and centers, skilled nursing facilities, long-term care facil-
inconsistent sampling methods across regions. ities, continuing care retirement communities, home
• What people report on health surveys is not health agencies, private practice clinics, not-for-profit
necessarily accurate, often due to the stigma communication disorders clinics, and aphasia cen-
associated with disabilities, sociocultural ters. We explore the nature of clinical aphasiology as
taboos about asking health-related questions, practiced in these diverse settings in Chapter 14.
and privacy concerns. Aphasiologists employed by colleges and uni-
• Perceptions about what constitutes disability versities typically engage in research, teaching, and
vary across individuals, families, and service as lecturers, instructors, readers, clinical
communities (Munyi, 2012; National Center supervisors, or professors. Some universities require
for Dissemination of Disability Research, 1999; academic staff members to engage in direct clinical
Shogren, 2011; Smart & Smart, 1997). The services and in clinical supervision of student clini-
same symptom constellation may constitute a cians on their path to becoming SLPs. Others hire
disability to some but not to others. separate clinical supervisory professionals and pro-
• Communication disabilities in particular mote dedication of greater proportions of academic
are commonly underestimated all over the instructors’ time to teaching and research.
world (Wylie et al., 2013) because of a lack of A small proportion of researchers specializing
education and awareness of what they are (see in aphasiology are employed by research centers or
Code et al., 2016) . institutes. Throughout the world, most of these are
• Survey instruments used to track health closely affiliated with college and university con-
data rarely include items specific to texts, and many are embedded within higher educa-
communication. Those that do typically tion campuses. Some are operated separately from
combine speech, language, and hearing institutions of higher education, even though they
disabilities such that it is not possible to tend to have strong academic collaborations.
know the specific types of communication
disabilities people have; they also do not
typically offer any breakdown according to What Is the Career Outlook for
etiology.
Clinical Aphasiologists?
As expertise in communication disorders
throughout the world expands, and as the need for Employment for clinicians, educators, and research-
treatment of disabilities and the fostering of commu- ers in clinical aphasiology has been strong world-
nication abilities as basic human rights is increas- wide for the past three decades. This is in line with
1. Welcome to the Fantastic World of Research and Clinical Practice in Acquired Disorders   9

trends for SLP employment overall. Global projec- communication disorders, the future looks even
tions based primarily on national shortage statistics brighter in light of an aging baby-boomer popula-
include SLP as one of the world’s most in-demand tion (the fastest growing segment of most national
professions (U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2021). populations), global increases in life expectancy, and
In projections for 2016 to 2026, SLP was listed as greater likelihood of survival from stroke and brain
one of the top 20 occupations in terms of new jobs injury, all resulting in greater needs for rehabilitative
that typically require a master’s, doctoral, or pro- services. The proportion of clinical SLPs employed in
fessional degree (Brook, 2019; U.S. Bureau of Labor health care contexts (in contrast to schools) has been
Statistics, 2017). According to the U.S. Bureau of rising steadily over the past several years (American
Labor Statistics (2021), demand for SLPs is likely Speech-Language-Hearing Association, 2014b). Sala-
to continue to be among the fastest growing of pro- ries for those working in health care contexts tend to
fessions. U.S. News and World Report (2021) listed be superior to those of clinicians working in schools
SLP as the fourth-ranked “Best Health Care Jobs.” (Marquardt, 2015).
SLPs working in medical and health care contexts, The expanding population of older people,
especially skilled nursing facilities, tend to earn along with public awareness campaigns, has contrib-
the highest salaries among clinical (nonmanage- uted to greater recognition of the need for research
rial) professionals in the field (ASHA, 2019) in the in aphasia and related disorders, as well as exposure
United States. In Australia, the profession is rated of young prospective scholars to study in this area.
as “very strong” in terms of future growth (Aus- The demand for PhD-level teaching staff members
tralian Government, 2021). Increasing demand in in communication sciences and disorders has been
the United Kingdom is strongly predicted through great for the past two decades in virtually every
2032, and changing demographics will require con- country where the SLP profession is established.
tinued greater demand through 2050 (Sugarman As more seasoned faculty members retire, there
Education, 2021). In Canada, projected job openings have been serious challenges in meeting teaching
for SLPs “are expected to be substantially higher to demands within academic programs in communi-
job seekers, creating a shortage of workers over the cation sciences and disorders (ASHA & CAPCSD,
2019–2028 period” (Government of Canada, 2019). 2003, 2008, 2010; Royal College of Physicians, 2014).
This is also the case in Finland, where the field is PhD-level candidates with specialization in adult
considered among the top three “hot jobs” in light neurogenic communication disorders continue to be
of labor shortages (EURES, 2021). Similar continuous highly sought after internationally (Lucks Mendel
increases in demand are predicted in Ireland (ESRI et et al., 2004).
al., 2017), China (Lin et al., 2016), Malaysia (Hassan In many countries, the field of SLP does not
& Hallowell, 2021b; Malay Mail, 2016), and South yet exist or is currently under development. In
Africa (Pillay et al., 2020). There is a dire need for most of the majority world where health-related
SLPs in much of the majority world, including much services are a focus of humanitarian efforts, the
of Africa (Wylie et al., 2016). According to a national primary focus tends to be on the needs of children
study of career opportunities in India, “There is an rather than adults. There is a dire need for cultural
acute shortage of qualified speech therapists and as insiders within those regions to become clinical and
awareness spreads. . . . Speech therapists will be in educational leaders, helping to shape the future of
high demand and have tremendous scope for pro- service for adults with acquired neurological condi-
fessional growth in the future” (India Today, 2014). tions, and services in general to meet the needs of
These claims have been further substantiated more older people, in their own countries. There is also a
recently (Kumari, 2020). Reports from South America need — and thus there are terrific opportunities — for
indicate major shortages of SLPs (Fernandes et al., passionate, multiculturally and multilingually com-
2010). In sum, employment opportunities abound petent aphasiologists around the world to help fos-
and will continue to do so. ter the career development and work of such leaders
For those seeking clinical, research, and teach- (Fernandes et al., 2010; Hallowell, 2014; Hassan &
ing career options related to acquired neurogenic Hallowell, 2021b).
10  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. Make a list of bolded terms used in this chap- a stroke survivor with aphasia who has no
ter. Practice defining them in your own words. accompanying motor speech disorder. What
2. What are the four key elements of a would you say to explain to the partner the
definition of aphasia? distinction between language and speech as
3. List and define several acquired neurogenic affected by the stroke?
language disorders. 8. Summarize the five primary reasons why
4. List and define several acquired neurogenic reported data pertaining to the incidence
speech disorders. and prevalence of acquired neurogenic
5. Why is it important to make a distinction language disorders are questionable.
between congenital and acquired language 9. What could be done to improve the validity
disorders? of data pertaining to the incidence and
6. Explain why it is important to characterize prevalence of neurogenic language disorders
a language disorder more specifically than throughout the world?
simply by using a general etiological term 10. Of all the places where aphasiologists might
(e.g., “language of generalized intellectual work, which are the ones that would suit
impairment” instead of “dementia” or you best?
“language disorder associated with TBI”
www
instead of “TBI”). See the companion website for additional learning
7. Imagine you are talking with the partner of and teaching materials.
CHAPTER
2
Becoming the Ultimate
Excellent Clinician

The intent of this book is to be a guide for clinical with them are multifaceted. The potential impact
excellence. In this chapter, we consider introductory of neurological conditions on a person’s sense of
notions about what makes clinicians truly excellent, self — their very identity — let alone the ability to
especially those who work in the realm of acquired interface with others in medical, professional, social,
communication disorders. We do this near the start familial, and other contexts is profound. So where do
of the book to encourage you to have a vision for we start if we want to become the best person we can
what you want to become as you proceed with your be to help improve the abilities and lives of people
learning. As Covey (1989) so famously conveyed in with aphasia and related neurogenic language dis-
his popular book, beginning with the end in mind is orders? If you are reading this, you probably have
one of the most important steps in becoming mas- already started. You have probably already thought
terful in any realm. We consider what we might do about being a person who could help bring about
to achieve the aspects of clinical excellence as we meaningful changes in people’s lives by enhancing
might define that term ourselves, and as seasoned their abilities to communicate and to cope with the
aphasiologists define it, as well as people with neu- loss of vital abilities. Fortunately, research and view-
rogenic communication disorders and others who points about this topic have been offered by hun-
care about them have defined it. We also review the dreds of other experts.
kind of academic and clinical content that aphasiolo- After reading and reflecting on the content in
gists (broadly defined to include experts in acquired this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in
neurogenic communication disorders) are expected your own words, the following queries:
to master, introduce varied types of required and
optional academic and clinical credentials, and 1. What makes a clinician truly excellent?
consider how individuals might strive for balance 2. How do the people we serve characterize what
between clinical generalization and specialization. they most want?
Specific means of career development and related 3. What are some traits of people who are
professional organizations are reviewed. Themes perceived as unhelpful clinicians?
introduced in this chapter are later amplified in Sec- 4. What content is important to master?
tion IV, where we delve further into details about 5. What credentials are required for a career as an
excellent service delivery. aphasiologist?
The study of aphasia and related neurogenic 6. What credentials may aphasiologists earn
language disorders is complex. There are myriad beyond their basic academic and clinical
causes that may underlie any particular individual’s credentials?
difficulties with language and communication. The 7. Is it best to specialize or generalize?
relationships between communication abilities and 8. What strategies help boost career development
the underlying neurological pathologies associated in acquired cognitive-linguistic disorders?

11
12  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

9. What organizations support professional background and earned the same SLP degrees from
information sharing and networking among the same academic programs. They tied for the top-
clinical aphasiologists? rated spots in their SLP program’s graduating class.
Both have the same amount of clinical experience
and have had the same savvy and skilled clinical
mentors at the same well-reputed clinical agencies.
What Makes a Clinician Truly Excellent?
How will you choose one over the other? What are
the qualities of the excellent clinician that cannot be
What an excellent clinician is and does is difficult so easily described in terms of what they know or
to capture. Thankfully, we have many decades of can do? Your own answers to these questions will
research that comprise a wonderful set of best prac- likely guide how you proceed on the path toward
tices for assessment, treatment, and counseling. clinical excellence.
There are myriad criteria for what constitutes best
practice. Likewise, there are numerous strategies
for achieving excellent clinical treatment results. What Can One Do to Become an
Throughout this book, we delve into best-practice Excellent Clinical Aphasiologist?
criteria and strategies to achieve excellent clinical
services. We can learn what those best practices are, Given that there are no absolute definitive answers
and we can keep honing our skills in implementing to what constitutes the ultimate best clinical aphasi-
them, continually improving how we practice. ologist, there is clearly not just one path for becom-
What an excellent clinician knows is a lot about ing such a person. The qualities that constitute
everything related to best practices, plus much excellence vary according to who defines it and in
about the associated science, art, theory, and history. what context it is defined. Characteristics of excel-
We may not all agree on what the most important lent clinical aphasiologists as derived from scholars
knowledge is, which is why there are so many var- in the field are summarized in Box 2–1.
ied foci among existing textbooks in aphasiology. Although some clinical educators comment that
We probably all agree, though, that there is a lot one some people are “born clinicians” or have native
must know in order to be a truly excellent clinician. clinical talents, many of the features on the list can be
And there is a lot more than knowledge required to learned and developed with conscientious practice,
make a clinician excellent. education, solid clinical mentoring, and good role
Imagine that the person you love most in the modeling (see Wagner et al., 2002; Zraick et al., 2003).
world has had a car accident and has just emerged Here we review some key strategies to keep in mind.
from a three-week comatose state during which
time you did not know if they would live or die, let Commit to Lifelong Learning
alone be able to think, communicate, or do any of
the amazing things most of us do in our everyday Aphasia and related disorders in and of themselves
lives. You have been through the wringer. Your life are complex and cannot be fully understood even
as you knew it has come to an abrupt and terrifying through a lifetime of study. We also cannot possibly
stop. You would do anything and everything to help master all of the ways we may help people through
this person you love to regain every ability possi- counseling and other clinical skills. Nor can we
ble, wouldn’t you? Imagine that you are in charge ever fully grasp all of the ever-changing social and
of this person’s care, and now it is time to choose a political forces that influence our work. The field
speech-language pathologist (SLP). You want to find of clinical aphasiology and all of its myriad associ-
the very best one, don’t you? How will you know ated disciplines continue to evolve. No matter how
who that is? Is it just based on what the clinician much we read, how many degrees we earn, and
knows? What else will you consider? how many conferences and workshops we attend,
Imagine that you are choosing between two there is always much more to learn. It is best that we
clinical aphasiologists, both highly recommended accept this fact and commit to readily learning more
to you. Both have the same academic and clinical throughout our careers.
2. Becoming the Ultimate Excellent Clinician   13

Box
2–1 Possible Characteristics of the Excellent Clinical Aphasiologist

They must do the following: • Effective, efficient, helpful, and useful


• Compassionate, empathetic, warm,
• Meet relevant certification and licensure
thoughtful, and considerate
requirements
• Sincerely interested in others
• Have been mentored and seasoned by
• Nonthreatening
excellent clinicians
• Reassuring
• Have excellent oral and written
• Good listeners
communication skills
• Flexible
• Skillfully interpret nonverbal communication
• Polite, patient, kind, tolerant, and sensitive
• Exhibit strong leadership qualities
• Practical
• Have a profound sense of curiosity and
• Committed to the relevance of their work to
inquiry
life participation and quality of life
• Protect confidentiality and privacy of others
• Bilingual or multilingual
• Have a rich and warm sense of humor
• Fair and equitable
• Exhibit follow-through with
• Creative and imaginative
recommendations and promises
• Confident
• Dress in appropriate professional ways
• Humble
• Advocate for people with neurogenic
• Aware of their own biases and prejudices
communication disorders and the people who
• Committed to addressing their biases and
care about them
prejudices
• Serve as role models in terms of healthy
• Well organized
living, including fitness, stress management,
• Responsible
work-life balance, and practices to prevent
• Self-directed and autonomous
stroke and brain injury
• Collaborative, cooperative
• Acknowledge personal weaknesses and
• Open to others’ views, willing to change
failures
• Timely
• Make sound judgments
• Healthy
• Love their role as a clinical aphasiologist
They are able to do the following:
They are as follows:
• Reason scientifically
• Knowledgeable, up to date, and well
• Balance and blend art and science
educated
• Integrate technical, intellectual, and
• Committed to evidence-based practice,
interpersonal competence
practice-based evidence, and lifelong learning
• Integrate content from numerous disciplines
• Ethical, honest, and trustworthy
• Complement knowledge and skills with
• Person centered
insight
• Multiculturally responsive
• Express complex ideas simply
• Reflective, self-aware, and self-evaluative
• Build trust
• Enthusiastic, passionate inspiring, motivating,
and empowering

Sources: Bendapudi et al., 2006; Carvalho et al., 2011; Comité Permanent de Liaison des Orthophonistes-​
Logopèdes de L’Union Européenne, 2007; Douglas & Smyth, 2021; Ebert & Kohnert, 2010; Fourie, 2009; Hal-
lowell & Chapey, 2008a; Khayum & Mooney, 2021; Leahy et al., 2010; NetQues Project Management Team, 2014;
O’Sullivan et al., 2008; Simmons-Mackie, 2021; Strong & Shadden, 2021; Threats, 2010b.
14  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Engage in Best Practices for cognitive-linguistic rehabilitation with that group.


Assessment and Intervention However, if the SLP were to move to work in com-
munity-based rehabilitation in urban Cape Town in
In the chapters on assessment and intervention, we the Western Cape of South Africa, the SLP may not
explore many aspects of well-documented best prac- be so culturally competent there. Cultural compe-
tices, including evidence-based practice, in clinical tence is not an achievable learning outcome that we
aphasiology. It is vital to know what those are and to can check off on our personal or professional devel-
make sure that you work in a way that is consistent opment to-do list. Humility and responsiveness better
with them. connote the ongoing process of our humble learning
about what is ideal in any particular context, and the
Commit to Being Culturally Responsive recognition of intersectionality (see Crenshaw et al.,
and Demonstrate Cultural Humility 2019); all humans represent a complex set of social
categories and personalities, such that we always
No matter where in the world we live and work, have more to learn about being responsive to every
the multiethnic diversity of the people we serve is person.
ever-expanding (Centeno, 2017). Worldwide migra- As Wesby (2009) states, it is vital for us to be
tion patterns are leading to dire needs for clinicians “able to think, feel, and act in ways that acknowl-
who are adept at connecting with people in cul- edge, respect, and build on ethnic, sociocultural, and
turally responsive ways (Grandpierre et al., 2018; linguistic diversity” (p. 280). Threats (2010a) adds to
Howells et al., Westerfeld, 2016). As Centeno, Kiran, this by highlighting the importance of self-reflection,
and Armstrong (2020) state, in “growing hyper-​ advising us to examine our own “culture, biases, and
diverse environments, individuals represent mul- views toward communication and communication
tiple linguistic profiles (i.e., monolingual speakers disability. . . . To effectively realize the influence of
of minority languages, bi-/multilingual speakers culture, persons must first realize the influence of
and users of dialectal varieties of local languages) culture on themselves” (p. 163).
and life realities (i.e., socioeconomic circumstances, We tend to recognize that cultural responsiv-
educational experiences, sociocultural histories, and ity is absolutely essential to excellence in clinical
worldviews)” (P.1315). practice, yet we have trouble defining measurable
Minoritized people experience tremendous indices of such responsivity (even though many
health disparities due to numerous factors, and a have attempted to do so). National Standards for
lack of clinicians with the cultural and linguistic Culturally and Linguistically Appropriate Services
abilities to serve them exacerbates the influence of (CLAS) in Health Care were established by the
these factors tremendously. Although some have U.S. Department of Health and Human Services
used the term cultural competence in referring to (2010) as “a blueprint for organizations to deliver
the goal of our own education in diversity, equity, effective, understandable and respectful services at
and inclusion, more appropriate terms for the ideal every point of patient contact. They are designed to
we strive for might be cultural responsiveness, advance health equity, improve quality and help elim-
or cultural humility (see Hyter & Salas-Provance, inate healthcare disparities.” There are numerous
2019). Competence suggests that there is a clear and additional resources supporting the development of
achievable set of skills and abilities that we can mas- cultural competence, including skills and knowledge
ter to do things the right way. Importantly, cultural for working with multilingual speakers and people
competence can only be judged relative to a specific who differ according to culture, ethnicity, geographic
context. A sense of mutual trust and comfort may origin, socioeconomic status, sexual orientation, sex-
develop over years of interaction between a White ual identity, gender, gender identity, disability, abil-
Australian SLP working effectively for years with ity, and the social construct of race. Some of these are
low-income Aboriginal people near Perth, Austra- available for general audiences, some for health care
lia. The SLP may learn to appreciate deeply myriad workers, some for rehabilitation professionals, some
aspects of culture and language that are essential for SLPs, and some for aphasiologists in particular
to the SLP being a strong and supportive agent for (see Alizadeh, & Chavan, 2016; Battle, 2012; Hamil-
2. Becoming the Ultimate Excellent Clinician   15

ton, 2020; Hays & Erford, 2014; Hickey & Douglas,


Box
2021; Hyter & Salas-Provence, 2019; Orozco et al., 2–2 Principles Constituting
2014; Payne, 2014). It is important for all of us to tap Interprofessional Collaborative
into these as we continue to grow professionally. Competencies

Participate Actively in Interprofessional • Person/family centered


Collaborative Practice • Relationship focused
• Community/population oriented
Interprofessional collaborative practice is when • Process oriented
health care professionals from different professions • Linked to learning activities, educational
work together with patients, families, care partners, strategies, and behavioral assessments that
and communities to provide optimal care (Inter- are developmentally appropriate for the
professional Education Collaborative [IPEC], 2016; learner
World Health Organization, 2010). • Able to be integrated across the learning
Collaboration across professions is essential to continuum
excellent clinical practice. According to an expert • Sensitive to the systems context/applicable
panel representing several health professional asso- across practice settings
ciations (IPEC, 2011): • Applicable across professions
• Stated in language common and meaningful
Interprofessional education . . . requires moving across the professions
beyond . . . profession-specific educational efforts • Outcome driven
to engage students of different professions in
interactive learning with each other. Being able Source: IPEC, 2016 (p. 10).
to work effectively as members of clinical teams
while students is a fundamental part of that learn-
ing. (p. 3) know enough or may not live up to others’ expecta-
tions in some way, we may get so distracted by our
The panel developed a consensus on the most own ego and desire to succeed that our attention is
important principles of interprofessional collabora- taken away from what matters most: actually being
tive competence, which was then updated in 2016. the ultimate excellent clinician, in action. This tends
Topical areas for collaboration, the key areas of com- to be highly problematic, especially in students
petencies, and examples of subcompetencies are and beginning clinicians. Imagine again being that
shown in Table 2–1. Ideally, these are principles to be family member at the bedside of a loved one with
embraced by all clinicians and by clinical educators a head injury. Imagine a clinician coming in and
committed to helping foster the best growth in future seeming unsure of themself. Perhaps this is made
clinicians. It is important that clinical students, too, apparent when the clinician looks at notes, acts shy,
take an active role in seeking out opportunities to speaks unassuredly, or is not clear about a plan for
pursue learning activities that embody principles of a diagnostic or treatment session. This is not what
interprofessional collaboration. you want to see as a family member. You want that
Four domains of interprofessional collaborative clinician to be the best. You want to know that the
competence are values and ethics, roles and respon- clinician knows what they are doing. It is lovely if
sibilities, interprofessional education, and teams and you are a humble person, but if your humility is such
teamwork. Examples of each as suggested by the that you are not a strong clinical presence, then be
expert panel are summarized in Table 2–1. sure to do something overtly to convey the presence
of an excellent clinician.
Be a Vehicle How can you do this? A key suggestion is this:
Be a vehicle. Do not let any contact with a person with
When we are worried or stressed about our clinical whom you are working professionally be about you. It
performance, perhaps because we think we may not is not about you. It is about the people you are serving.
Table 2–1. Examples of Interprofessional Collaborative Competencies

Topical Areas Within


Interprofessional
Collaboration Competencies Examples of Subcompetencies
Values and ethics Work with individuals of other • Place the interests and needs of the people they
professions to maintain a are serving at the center of care
climate of mutual respect and • Maintain confidentiality in team-based care
shared values.
• Respect people’s dignity and privacy
• Develop trusting relationships with individuals,
families, and other team members
• Respect the expertise, roles, culture, and values
of other team members
• Manage ethical dilemmas adeptly
• Cooperate with others involved in prevention
and health services
Roles and Use the knowledge of one’s • Recognize limitations in skills and knowledge
responsibilities own role and those of other • Communicate clearly about scope of practice
professions to appropriately and related roles and responsibilities to patients,
assess and address the health significant others, and team members
care needs of patients and
to promote and advance the • Work collaboratively to ensure “safe, timely,
health of populations. efficient, effective, and equitable” care
• Engage in continuing education to enhance
interprofessional teamwork
Interprofessional Communicate with patients, • Optimize use of communication tools and
communication families, communities, and techniques to enhance team effectiveness
professionals in health and • Communicate in an effective, understandable
other fields in a responsive way, avoiding specialized professional jargon
and responsible manner that
supports a team approach • Engage in active listening with other team
to the promotion and members
maintenance of health and the • Encourage expression of ideas and opinions
prevention and treatment of from other team members
disease. • Use respectful language in challenging
discussions and conflict resolution
Teams and teamwork Apply relationship-building • Develop a consensus among team members
values and the principles of about roles and best practices in collaboration
team dynamics to perform • Manage disagreements constructively
effectively in different team
roles to plan, deliver, and • Evaluate, reflect on, and continuously improve
evaluate patient-/population- overall team performance and the collaborative
centered care and population performance of individuals on the team
health programs and policies • Support collaborative work with evidence-based
that are safe, timely, efficient, practice
effective, and equitable. • Share accountability with people served, other
professionals, and others in the community
or environment for prevention and health care
outcomes
Note. From IPEC core competencies, by Interprofessional Education Collaborative (IPEC), 2016. https://www.ipecollaborative​
.org

16
2. Becoming the Ultimate Excellent Clinician   17

You are a conduit for empowering, effective work. the vehicle through which that competence flows.
Great knowledge and skills must be conveyed Do not be an obstruction to your expression of your
through you, regardless of how you feel, what you true competence. If you do not let your competence
wish to prove to the world about yourself, and flow, you help no one.
what you may perceive about your lack of ability. This notion of being a vehicle may seem obvi-
In Figure 2–1, showing a thriving crew of up-and-​ ous in the abstract as you consider it while reading
coming aphasiologists, you will see that you are this chapter. Imagine, though, a typical situation
in good company in espousing the goal of being of the busy clinician preparing for an assessment
a vehicle. session. They may feel pressured preparing assess-
Of course, if you truly do not have the knowl- ment materials, which often include a plentiful mix
edge and skills to do something specific in your clin- of real objects and pictures that must be carefully
ical role, you should not do it. That basic tenet is a organized. They may also require a review of scor-
key point in the codes of ethics of professional bod- ing procedures and must have the proper forms in
ies overseeing any clinical practice area, including place. They may wonder if the chosen assessment
those related to communication sciences and disor- materials are even appropriate for the situation, if
ders. However, if you have the knowledge and skills they are competent enough in the given disorder
to be in a clinical situation in the first place, then area to provide a high-quality assessment, if they
you must be considered competent to be there. Let may appear too young, if they will pronounce and
that competence be part of your clinical presence. Be explain complex terms correctly, whether there is

Figure 2–1. Students being vehicles. Photo credit: Stephanie Luczkowski. A full-color version of this figure can
be found in the Color Insert.
18  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

enough time to complete an evaluation, and so on.


It is great to be humble, especially with regard to
What Are Some Traits of People Who
multi­cultural engagement, as discussed earlier. Still, Are Perceived as Unhelpful Clinicians?
the degree of self-doubt that some clinicians feel as
they enter into a clinical encounter even with peo- In a book dedicated to positive, proactive approaches
ple of the same culture and language — especially to fostering recovery and self-empowerment, per-
early in their careers — often exceeds the degree of haps it is ironic to examine what it might be like
humility and self-effacement that characterizes the when we fail at being helpful. Still, in the interest of
excellent clinician. deepening our thinking about this, let’s take a brief
detour to consider what does not constitute clinical
excellence. Note Taylor’s (2006) description of a stu-
How Do the People We Serve
dent clinician taking a medical history the day after
Characterize What They Most Want? the author’s stroke:

What people with neurogenic communication dis- This young girl was an energy vampire. She
orders want in a clinician likely mirrors the sorts of wanted to take something from me despite my
things we discussed in the previous section and the fragile condition, and she had nothing to give me
description you would create for yourself in terms in return. . . . In her haste, she was rough in the
of how you wish to be as a clinician. See Box 2–3 for way she handled me and I felt like a detail that
suggestions to clinicians generated by adults with had fallen through someone’s crack. . . . She might
aphasia and related disorders. have gotten more from me had she come to me
Care partners and supportive friends also note gently with patience and kindness, but because
what is most important to see in a clinician; the fea- she insisted that I come to her in her time and at
tures they most report closely complement those her pace, it was not satisfying for either of us. Her
of professional clinicians as well as people with demands were annoying and I felt weary from
neurogenic language disorders. A close friend of a the encounter. (p. 81)
man with aphasia captured this in this wonderful
description he wrote to an aphasiologist he espe- Of course, the opposite of any of the abilities
cially admired after inconspicuously standing out- and qualities listed in Table 2–1 would be considered
side the man’s room during a treatment session: undesirable. Additional unhelpful traits commonly
mentioned by people with aphasia and related dis-
My gift is to recognize Genius. That’s what I saw. orders include failure to attend to the most practical
You have taken science, neurons, transmitters, aspects of supported communication, a tendency
billions of pathways, and have made it your art. to engage in treatment activities that do not lead to
The science part is the three primary colors. Your real-life gains in communication abilities, and a lack
part is as Vincent van Gogh. Just hearing you of attention to information needed, presented in a
outside his room I could feel the art. Each ques- way that the individual can understand (see Parr
tion and statement was a brushstroke on what et al., 1997).
minutes before was a blank canvas. You had the
colors mixed, the subject picked, the end results
mapped. The hard part was already done. The
What Content Is Important to Master?
only thing you couldn’t do was speed up time.
Impressive. A musician was once complemented.
He said, “Thank you, I play all the notes and play Neurogenic communication challenges are com-
them well. But Mozart . . . he didn’t play notes. plex. So are the lives and life contexts of the people
He played music.” who have them, so are clinicians and clinical schol-
ars, and so are the service delivery environments in
As an excellent clinician, you are an artist. You are a which we work. This is why there is virtually no area
musician. You are a scientist. of study that is not somehow relevant to develop-
2. Becoming the Ultimate Excellent Clinician   19

Box
2–3 Suggestions to Clinicians From Adults With
Neurogenic Communication Disorders

• Treat me as if I will recover.


• Don’t sap my hope.
• Believe in my brain’s plasticity and in my ability to help it learn and change.
• Help protect me from noise and too much stimulation.
• Help me get rest.
• Don’t talk for me as if you know what I want to say.
• Accept that I may not be the same as I was and let that be okay.
• Don’t talk about me to others as if I’m not there. Talk to me.
• Help others see that the person I am becoming now is just as lovable as the
person I was. Maybe I’m even better.
• Don’t talk down to me. Respect me.
• Don’t raise your voice if I don’t understand you.
• Value me as someone with important contributions to make.
• Encourage me to have things to look forward to.
• Focus on my strengths, not just on what I can’t do.
• Celebrate my successes.
• Be patient with me and take your time.
• Help me accept help from others.

Source: Parr et al. (1997); Simmons-Mackie et al. (2017); Taylor (2006); Worrall et al.
(2011); personal communication from numerous people with acquired neurogenic
disorders.

ing excellence in clinical aphasiology. Reviewing the rigorous academic and professional standards, typ-
table of contents of this book, you can see what areas ically going beyond the minimum requirements for
of knowledge many in our field think are import- state licensure. They have the knowledge, skills, and
ant for you to know. However, it certainly does not expertise to provide high quality clinical services,
end there. Review Box 1–1 in Chapter 1 and consider and they actively engage in ongoing professional
again the relevance of the disciplines mentioned in development to keep their certification current”
relation to the aspects you have listed in your own (ASHA, n.d.a) in specific designated areas. Speech
description of the ultimate excellent clinical apha- Pathology Australia (SPA) has a well-developed set
siologist. Appreciate that no matter how much you of competency-based professional standards (SPA,
learn as a student or clinician, you will still have 2020) and an internationally esteemed model for
much more to learn. assessing clinical competence (COMPASS; McAllis-
In countries where SLPs are certified by a pro- ter et al., 2006, 2011) that has been put to use in sev-
fessional body, specific areas of knowledge and eral other countries.
skill are typically dictated by the organization that In Europe, a set of standards for speech-​language
administers certification. The designated areas are therapists (SLTs) and logopedists was collabora-
based on the scope of practice and, ideally, on valida- tively developed through a multinational network
tion studies regarding expert clinicians’ perceptions of 65 experts representing 31 European Union (EU)
of required knowledge and skills. For example, in countries, plus Iceland and Turkey (EU candidate
the United States, the American Speech-Language-​ countries), and Norway and Lichtenstein (NetQues
Hearing Association (ASHA) requires evidence that Project Management Team, 2014; Patterson et al.,
applicants for certification “have voluntarily met 2015). It includes a set of subject-specific and generic
20  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

benchmarks representing targeted competencies, tralia (SPA), and in New Zealand through the New
now embraced by the EU’s Comité Permanent de Zealand Speech-Language Therapists’ Association
Liaison des Orthophonistes–Logopèdes de L’Union (NZSTA). There is a Mutual Recognition Agreement
Européenne. Most countries that have a national (MRA) for certified SLPs in Australia, Canada, the
certification for SLPs also have a national examina- United States, the United Kingdom, Ireland, and
tion that individuals must pass to demonstrate their New Zealand that helps to facilitate consideration
knowledge in designated content areas. Addition- of applicants from one of these countries wishing to
ally, most countries require evidence of continuing practice in another. To date, the agreement includes
education to ensure that those certified to practice only English-speaking countries, although this may
continue to expand their knowledge bases. change as interest in the international transportabil-
ity of credentials increases and as the required com-
ponents for consideration as a member of the MRA
What Credentials Are Required for are developed in additional countries.
In most countries other than the United States,
a Career as an Aphasiologist?
an undergraduate degree is currently the entry-level
degree for clinical practice in SLP and psychology; a
If you wish to become a practicing clinical SLP, logo- move to graduate degree requirements is under con-
pede, logopedist, orthophonist, clinical linguist, or sideration in many countries. Information about the
neuropsychologist, the type of degree you will likely current requirements for education and clinical cer-
pursue is governed primarily by the accreditation tification, licensure, or work permits in the country
and/or certification requirements in the country and where you wish to work is typically available on the
region where you intend to practice. In some coun- websites of the national professional association that
tries (e.g., Argentina, Brazil, Israel, India, Malay- oversees clinical practice regulations in that country.
sia, and Israel), the SLP qualifications are blended In some countries, the field of communication sci-
with qualifications in audiology, and clinicians are ences and disorders is just beginning, and there is
expected to be competent in both professional areas. not yet an agreed-upon set of standards for special-
In the United States, certification is administered ists or generalists in the field.
through ASHA. The minimum degree requirement If you choose to practice in clinical aphasi-
for U.S. SLPs is a master’s degree, typically requir- ology as a medical doctor, you would most likely
ing about 2 years of full-time study in an ASHA-​ pursue specialization in neurology, neuropsychi-
accredited SLP program, after completion of a bach- atry, or physiatry. Most medical schools across the
elor’s (undergraduate) degree (during which time globe, though, do not provide substantial education
certain prerequisite courses are taken) (ASHA, 2020). and training in clinical aphasiology per se through
Certification also requires passing of the national curricula, internships, and residencies. For this rea-
Praxis examination (Educational Testing Service, son, some choose to pursue both an SLP entry-level
2014) and completion of a 9-month supervised clin- degree and a medical degree, although this typically
ical fellowship. Most states within the United States entails a substantial extension of the time invested
have separate processes required for clinical licen- in education. Certification in psychology and neu-
sure, as do most provinces in Canada and some ropsychology in the United States requires the min-
states in Australia. imum of a clinical doctorate.
In the United Kingdom, SLPs are qualified to If you choose to become a clinical aphasiologist
practice through the Royal College of Speech and as a person with a degree in a related nonclinical
Language Therapists (RCSLT). After graduation field, you may do so by completing an additional
from a program accredited by the Health Professions degree in one of the areas mentioned earlier. Some
Council, they must complete 1 year of supervised academic programs offer opportunities for com-
clinical practice before being recognized as fully bining master’s and PhD coursework, either as
independent and certified clinicians or research- two separate programs in which you might enroll
ers. Similar requirements are in place in Canada sequentially or as blended, overlapping programs.
through Speech-Language and Audiology Canada One alternative for those without any clinical
(SAC), in Australia through Speech Pathology Aus- education or certification is to engage in a career as
2. Becoming the Ultimate Excellent Clinician   21

a research aphasiologist. This is sometimes a path requirements and a review team’s approval of two
chosen by individuals with advanced degrees in case studies, which first must be submitted and
fields such as experimental psychology, linguistics, approved in writing and then must be reviewed
or neuroscience. There are examples of wonderful and approved through an oral presentation and
scholars in aphasiology who have taken this route; discussion (ANCDS, n.d.). Eligibility to apply for
still, the route has inherent challenges. One is that the BC-ANCDS credential typically requires SLP
employment opportunities are reduced because certification from Australia, Canada, Ireland, New
many college or university programs that hire clin- Zealand, the United Kingdom, or the United States,
ical aphasiologists require their faculty members to although special consideration may be given to
hold clinical credentials. A second is that there may applicants from other countries. Almost all ANCDS
be concerns about ethics as well as relevance if apha- board-certified members are from the United States,
siologists without clinical training or experience are although U.S. residence or citizenship is not a
hired into positions in which they will be teaching requirement.
future clinicians about clinical content. For those wishing to pursue a research and
A third challenge is that without ample exposure advanced teaching career in clinical aphasiology,
to people with neurogenic language communication a PhD in SLP or a closely related area is typically
disorders and experience working with them and considered an optimal degree. For those wishing to
the people who care about them, it is more difficult teach primarily clinical courses and mentor student
to develop important counseling, critical-thinking, clinicians, a clinical doctorate may be a better fit than
and interpersonal skills that are typically fostered a PhD. Possibilities for ensuring quality and stan-
through supervised clinical mentorship and ongoing dards through expanded clinical doctoral opportu-
practice experience. A fourth is that, whether war- nities have been explored over the past several years
ranted or not, clinical aphasiologists without clinical (ASHA & CAPCSD, 2010; ASHA Academic Affairs
credentials may be accorded less credibility as clin- Board, 2012; ASHA, 2013, 2020). The importance
ical scholars among clinically qualified colleagues, and relevance of clinical doctoral programs to clini-
clinical students, and consumers of their research. cal aphasiology have been highlighted through the
ANCDS (ANCDS, 2014). The number of clinical doc-
toral programs with a special focus on neurogenic
What Credentials May Aphasiologists communication disorders is on the rise.
Earn Beyond Their Basic Academic Other types of credentials that might boost
the career development of clinical aphasiologists,
and Clinical Credentials?
depending on geographic context, expertise, and mar-
ket demands, include certificate programs and grad-
A credential that some choose to pursue is board uate degrees in related areas, such as gerontology,
certification in adult neurogenic communication global health, health policy, epidemiology, linguis-
disorders and sciences (BC-ANCDS) through the tics, neuroscience, and international development.
Academy of Neurologic Communication Disorders
and Sciences (ANCDS, n.d.). Board certification
was developed in recognition of the fact “that meet-
Is It Best to Specialize or Generalize?
ing minimum requirements for practice . . . is not
an indicator of specialized expertise or excellence”
(Duffy, 2014, p. 2). It is a means of externally vali- SLPs who work primarily in medical and rehabili-
dating one’s professional knowledge, especially by tation contexts sometimes specialize in certain areas
other colleagues who hold the credential. It does not based on type of disorder(s). For example, hospi-
typically have an effect on salary potential or job tal-based SLPs may specialize in acquired neuro-
opportunities, although some hiring professionals genic language disorders, acquired motor speech
value board certification as a credential when com- disorders, swallowing disorders, voice disorders,
paring job applicants. or communication challenges of people with tra-
Procedures for obtaining the BC-ANCDS cre- cheostomy and of people who are ventilator depen-
dential include evidence of meeting eligibility dent. Alternatively, they may specialize in diverse
22  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

disorders associated with certain neurological etiol- • applying for research and/or training grants,
ogies, such as traumatic brain injury (TBI), stroke, scholarships, and awards.
or neurodegenerative diseases. Of course, they may
also specialize in a host of areas of particular inter- Pursuing advanced graduate work toward
est to children in medical contexts (see Johnson & a clinical or research doctorate is an optimal way
Jacobson, 2017); these are not within the scope of to specialize further following undergraduate or
this book. Due to the extensive knowledge needed master’s-​level clinical education (ANCDS, 2014).
to work in each clinical area, specialization can be Among aphasiologists whose work is dedicated
highly beneficial. At the same time, many medical primarily to research and teaching, greater spe-
settings do not have sufficient caseloads to justify cialization is helpful in terms of being able to stay
hiring separate specialists in each area, thus requir- abreast of the constantly proliferating research base
ing clinicians to be well prepared to practice in mul- in any given area and in terms of developing a pro-
tiple areas. fessional reputation for expertise in a specific area.
Many SLPs choose to specialize in terms of gen- However, some academic positions require teaching
eral age groups served rather than disorder types. in content areas outside of one’s area of primary
Many hospitals and larger clinical practices have expertise, and some instructors prefer to teach in
SLPs who are especially well versed in services for diverse areas.
adults or some even more specifically geared to work In sum, if one wishes to be a true expert in clin-
with older adults. SLPs working in smaller private ical aphasiology, specialization in medical or reha-
practices or community agencies often do not have bilitation SLP, work with older populations, or in
the opportunity to specialize and must be competent specific related diagnostic or etiologic categories is
to work with people of diverse ages, disorder types, a great idea. At the same time, let’s recognize that
and etiologies. many excellent clinicians are competent in multiple
In most countries, requirements for clinical cer- areas of practice. A combination of personal prefer-
tification and/or licensure include demonstration of ence, individual strengths, influences for mentors,
knowledge and skills in areas across a wide range and educational and professional opportunities
of the SLP scope of practice. In the United States, will likely guide one’s decision about how special-
for example, a 2-year master’s degree required for ized to become on the path of excellence in clinical
clinical certification entails course work and super- aphasiology.
vised clinical practice experiences across all areas of
clinical practice, leaving little room for specialized
course work. Still, students may take advantage of What Strategies Help Boost
special opportunities for specialized career growth, Career Development in Acquired
for example, by
Cognitive-Linguistic Disorders?
• enrolling in specialized elective courses and
independent studies; Many expert clinicians and academic mentors have
• getting involved in research on neurogenic provided excellent advice to those wishing to build
language communication disorders (carrying their careers as aphasiologists. As Threats (2010b) so
out a master’s thesis or working in a research aptly states, “Simply having aspirations is not suf-
laboratory); ficient to achieve them. Concrete actions and tools
• volunteering in local student groups or are needed to reach lofty goals” (p. 88). See Box 2–4
community agencies that serve adults with for a list of doable strategies. No matter what your
neurogenic disorders; current context, consider that there are serious time
• selecting clinical practicum and externship constraints within universities and clinical contexts.
sites that avail specialized exposure and Do not wait for your next practicum assignment,
training; your new clinical fellowship, your next job, your
• observing assessment and treatment sessions next course or workshop, and so on to learn new
run by master clinicians; and content and skills.
2. Becoming the Ultimate Excellent Clinician   23

collaboration among clinical aphasiologists. See


What Organizations Support Professional Table 2–2 for examples across the globe. Information
Information Sharing and Networking about additional organizations that provide help
Among Clinical Aphasiologists? and support to people with neurogenic communi-
cation disorders and people who care about them is
Numerous professional organizations throughout provided in Chapter 27, Table 27–1.
the world help to enhance lifelong learning and

Box
2–4 Strategies for Boosting Career Development
in Clinical Aphasiology

• Delve into your studies wholeheartedly; engage in deep learning.


• Do not study just to pass tests and get good grades; consider the relevance
of everything you learn.
• Continuously reflect on how you will integrate new learning into your
professional work.
• Do not cut corners when carrying out academic projects.
• Master related medical terminology, not just to be able to understand
terms but to use them correctly yourself.
Know how to spell and pronounce terms correctly. When you mispronounce
and misspell terms, this reflects poorly not only on your own professional
competence but on the respectability of SLPs in general.
• Gain experience in learning and working with interprofessional teams.
• Pursue interdisciplinary studies.
• Take advantage of learning opportunities outside of coursework,
continuing education, and clinical practica; take initiative to pursue such
opportunities if they do not simply present themselves.
• Be a constant advocate for your area of expertise and your profession.
• Serve as an admirable example for others on your academic and clinical
teams.
• Be a role model for future clinicians and researchers.

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. Develop your own list of qualities for what If this is a goal for you, consider posting it as
represents the ultimate excellent clinical a conglomerate affirmation where you will
aphasiologist. Compare and contrast your see it regularly.
list with that of a partner. 4. What do you think might be some barriers to
2. Discuss specific strategies that will help truly interprofessional collaborative practice
you to become an excellent clinical in clinical contexts?
aphasiologist. 5. Consider the affirmation, “I am a vehicle.”
3. Make a collage representing what you think As a clinician, for what would you
constitutes an excellent clinical aphasiologist. personally like most to serve as a vehicle?
24  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

6. When determining what constitutes the will you need to take to earn those
best SLP, how might the priorities of a credentials?
person with a language disorder differ 8. Check out the websites of several professional
from those of a life partner? How might organizations related to acquired neurogenic
both of their opinions about what matters communication disorders. Which most
most about clinical competence differ from intrigue you personally and professionally?
what an employer of an aphasiologist Why?
might want?
www
7. What clinical credentials do you hope See the companion website for additional learning
to seek, if any? What specific steps and teaching materials.
2. Becoming the Ultimate Excellent Clinician   25

Table 2–2. Professional Organizations Supporting Professionals in Aphasiology

Organization Organization Website


Academy of Aphasia https://www2.academyofaphasia.org
Academy of Neurological Communication https://www.ancds.org
Disorders and Sciences
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association https://www.asha.org/SIG/02/
Special Interest Group 2 (SIG 2), Neurogenic
Communication Disorders
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association https://www.asha.org/SIG/15/
Special Interest Group 15 (SIG 15), Gerontology
Aphasia Access https://www.aphasiaaccess.org
Aphasia Alliance https://aphasiaalliance.org/
Aphasia United https://www.aphasiaunited.org/
Association Internationale Aphasie http://www.aphasia-international.com/
Australian Aphasia Association https://www.aphasia.org.au
Brain Injury Association https://www.biausa.org
British Aphasiology Society https://www.bas.org.uk/
Clinical Aphasiology Conference https://www.clinicalaphasiology.org
Cognitive Neuroscience Society https://www.cogneurosociety.org
European Brain Injury Society https://www.ebissociety.org
Collaboration of Aphasia Trialists https://www.aphasiatrials.org/
International Association of Logopedics and https://ialpasoc.info
Phoniatrics (now doing business as International
Association of Communication Sciences and
Disorders)
International Behavioral Neuroscience Society https://www.ibnsconnect.org
International Clinical Phonetics and Linguistics https://www.icpla.org
Association
International Cognitive Linguistics Association https://www.cognitivelinguistics.org
International Neuropsychological Society https://www.the-ins.org
National Aphasia Association https://www.aphasia.org
National Institutes of Health Neuroscience@NIH https://neuroscience.nih.gov
UK Stroke Forum https://www.stroke.org.uk/professionals/
uk-stroke-forum
World Stroke Association https://www.world-stroke.org
Note. A listing of additional organizations and corresponding websites supporting people with neurogenic
communication disorders is given in Chapter 27 (Table 27–1).
CHAPTER
3
Writing and Talking About the
People With Whom We Work

The words we use, especially in describing peo- 6. What is important to keep in mind regarding
ple, matter tremendously. Our choices of terms inclusive and welcoming language?
and construction of phrases as we label, categorize, 7. What other terms might unintentionally
characterize, and refer to others have tremendous convey negative connotations?
implications in terms of how the things we say will 8. Why are there inconsistencies in the prefixes
be perceived. Of course, how our communication is used in terms for characterizing neurogenic
perceived is vastly important in advancing our mis- symptoms, and what is the rationale for varied
sions as excellent clinicians. Some terms are overtly prefix choices?
pejorative. Some are deeply ingrained in our edu-
cational, clinical, and general societal cultures and
continue to be used without careful attention to their What Is Important to Consider in Writing
meanings, or in disregard for the import of word and Talking About People With Neurogenic
choice. In this chapter, we review principles related
Cognitive-Linguistic Disorders?
to various categories of terms used in acquired
neurogenic cognitive-linguistic disorders and the
importance of attending to them. After reading and Wonderful progress is afoot worldwide in the pro-
reflecting on the content in this chapter, you will motion of human rights, equity, and inclusion for
ideally be able to answer, in your own words, the all people. We have a moral imperative to continue
following queries: learning about and implementing strategies to con-
front racism, sexism, ageism, ableism, linguicism,
1. What is important to consider in writing and a host of other -isms that detract from any per-
and talking about people with neurogenic son’s rights, opportunities, and sense of belonging.
cognitive-linguistic disorders? In light of our roles of enhancing empowerment,
2. What are important nuances in terms we use self-efficacy, and opportunities for the people we
to refer to people who care for people with serve, every attempt to reduce discrimination and
neurogenic cognitive-linguistic disorders? bias matters. Part of this learning involves atten-
3. What is the difference between the terms tion to the words we use. Often dismissed as a
therapy and treatment? mere matter of political correctness or trivial issue
4. What are pros and cons of terms used to refer of semantics, the words we use in our personal and
to speech-language pathologists (SLPs)? professional communications matter greatly (Hal-
5. What are the preferred terms when referring to lowell, 2012c; Hallowell, Combiths, et al., 2021). At
the experts who work with people who have the same time, there are few definitive rules for ter-
neurogenic communication challenges? minology usage that all agree on, and differences in

27
28  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

terminology across cultures, languages, countries In medical contexts, such as hospitals and reha-
and regions abound. Also, usage preferences con- bilitation centers, it is typical to use the term patient.
tinuously evolve even among sophisticated users of The term represents the notion, tied especially to
terms characterizing groups of people. In light of the medical models, that deficits — the focus of atten-
need for aphasiologists to be culturally responsive tion — dwell within the person without taking into
and convey cultural humility, as discussed in Chap- consideration the many facets of that person’s envi-
ter 2, let’s consider important principles of terminol- ronment, and that there is an unequal relationship
ogy use for referring to people in the realm of our between the health care professional and the person
clinical and scholarly work in aphasiology. being treated. Consider this argument from Neu-
berger and Tallis (1999):

Person-First Language Patient comes from the Latin “patiens,” from


“patior,” to suffer or bear. The patient, in this
When referring to people with aphasia and related language, is truly passive — bearing whatever
disorders, it is vital that we keep in mind two basic suffering is necessary and tolerating patiently the
suggestions: interventions of the outside expert. The active
patient is a contradiction in terms, and it is the
• use person-first language assumption underlying the passivity that is the
• never use a disability label as a noun most dangerous. It is that the user of services will
remain passive in sickness, allowing the health-
This is consistent with worldwide growing con- care professional to take the active part and tell
sciousness to affirm the humanity of people with the user what to do. The passive patient will do
varied abilities, and to recognize that their disabili- what he or she is told, and will then wait patiently
ty-associated challenges do not define who a person to recover. The healthcare professional is the
is. For example, the expression a person with aphasia is healer, while the recipient of healthcare services
preferable to an aphasic person, and there is no context is the healed, and does not need to take a part
in which it is appropriate to say an aphasic. Refer to a in any decision making or in any thinking about
person with traumatic brain injury, or a traumatic brain alternatives. (p. 1756)
injury survivor, not a TBI. This follows for people
who have other disabilities. Refer to a person with Others argue that the term has continued to
quadriplegia, not a quadriplegic. Refer to a person evolve, with the understanding that the roots of the
with apraxia of speech, not an apraxic. term are no longer relevant to those who appreciate
that patients are active seekers of wellness who see
themselves as equal partners with health care pro-
Alternatives to the Word Patient viders. Another argument for retaining the terms is
that many who are called patients do not object to it.
The terms we use to refer to our customers or the In any case, many proponents of more sensi-
consumers of our services depend largely on the tive language use recommend that we refrain from
context in which we work and on our degree of referring to people with neurogenic communication
responsiveness to the perceptions of people to whom disorders as patients outside of medical contexts; one
we are referring. In many contexts, terminology is only literally qualifies as a patient while enrolled in
passed along without challenges to the origins and medically related services. Most people with apha-
implications of terms. There is often resistance to sia and related disorders have lifelong challenges,
change even when attention is called to pejorative not just conditions for which they are treated and
or demeaning aspects of words. Whoever gets to discharged from medical or rehabilitation facilities.
decide what to call someone holds power over that Thus, many of the contexts in which people with
person. This, as Maggio (1997) stated in his book on aphasia and related disorders engage in activities to
talking about people, “is why naming is one of the enhance communication abilities and life participa-
most critical issues for fairness and accuracy in lan- tion are not medical facilities. They may be group
guage” (p. 15). treatment or social centers in which participants are
2. Writing and Talking About the People With Whom We Work   29

considered members or participants. They may be study involves and what its risks and benefits are)
organizations or private practices that do not offer and who have the right to choose not to participate
medical services or accept medical insurance. Other altogether or stop participation at any time.
terms are used to avoid the term patient, yet also have
negative connotations. User, for example, connotes
the use of technology, not services focused on com- Older People
munication and relationships. Client connotes a busi-
ness relationship, yet many of us use this term for Given that the older we get, the greater are our
lack of a better term at times. Consumer connotes that chances of having an acquired neurogenic language
a person is ingesting what is prescribed or using up disorder, it is important for us to be sensitive to
a commodity. Especially outside of medical contexts, the appropriateness of terminology in the context
we might consider using the term person instead of of aging, especially when we are referring to older
patient. The former term is more humanizing and adults. Many times, professionals working with
reduces the connotation of a person whose deci- older adults inadvertently perpetuate ageism. Fol-
sions and life circumstances are being managed by lowing suit with previous suggestions, it is advisable
a medical expert as opposed to the person themself to use person-first language, refraining from calling
and those that they care about. For people who live people by labels such as the aged.
long term in skilled nursing establishments, those Although more commonly accepted in some
are their homes, so it is probably more appropriate circles, the term elderly is discouraged by many
to call them residents than patients. because of its ageist connotations. Some people read-
ily accept the terms senior citizen or senior. Others
discourage their use because these terms so readily
People With Disabilities evoke stereotypic associations, for example, about
coffee shop discounts and television advertise-
The term disability is used widely and is preferred by ments for dentures and adult diapers. “After all,”
most over the term handicap because it is less focused write Dahmen and Cozma (2009), “we don’t refer to
on pathology and more on a perspective of life people under age 50 as ‘junior citizens.’ Instead say
impacts. Still, the word disability does not meet with ‘man’ and ‘woman,’ and give their age, if relevant”
the approval of some people who have impaired (p. 36). Those authors report that people under age
abilities in any particular dimension — or of some of 55 years, on average, are not bothered by the terms
the people who care about them. Some prefer the elderly, retiree, and senior citizen; however, those over
term differently abled. At least appreciating that there age 55 years are.
is no one clear term that pleases all helps us be sen- Some older people are not bothered by euphe-
sitive in our word choice in any given context. No mistic terms such as golden ager, golden years, or age
matter what, use of a label such as the disabled is not of maturity. Others are. To avoid being perceived
acceptable from a person-first perspective. as condescending, it is best simply not to use such
terms. It is also important that professionals working
with older adults refrain from other terms that per-
Research Participants petuate ageist stereotypes, such as cute, spry, miserly,
doughty, and cranky. It is also important to avoid
In the research context, several organizations and patronizing language. Examples include referring
style convention authorities (such as the American to someone as 95 years young, a grandfatherly type, or
Psychological Association) recommend that we refer a sweet little old lady.
to research participants and not subjects; the latter
connotes an uneven power relationship in which
experimenters have authority or control over the Healthy Adults
people participating in research. In fact, fundamen-
tal to ethical research practice are the rights of par- The term healthy suggests that an individual is not
ticipants, who must give informed consent before an in an active state of disease or illness. In this sense,
experiment begins (knowing specifically what the people with neurogenic language disorders tend to
30  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

be healthy; if they are not, their health problem is friend, for whom the person being cared for also
not due to their neurogenic language disorder per cares? Many of us prefer the term care partner in
se but to a concomitant illness. In the research con- situations where a person with a disability shares
text, some authors inappropriately refer to control responsibility for a healthy reciprocal relationship;
participants (those who do not have neurological their partner may also have needs that are met by a
disorders) as healthy in contrast to groups of partic- person with a disability. Some loved ones of stroke
ipants with neurological disorders. Consistent with and brain injury survivors consider themselves
efforts to empower adults with neurological disor- co-survivors, in recognition of the fact that neuro-
ders to maximize their participation in meaningful genic communication disorders affect not just the
everyday social engagement and life activities (often person whose brain was injured.
not possible for people who are sick or unhealthy), it
is best to avoid the term healthy to differentiate con-
trol participants, or people who have not had major What Is the Difference Between the
neurological incidents, from stroke and brain injury
Terms Therapy and Treatment ?
survivors or people with dementia.
Qualifying the term control participant with the
features that contrast participants with and without Although the terms therapy and treatment are used
neurological disorders is preferable. For example, in interchangeably by many people, it is important to
a study on aphasia, one might refer to participants consider the difference and reflect on which term to
with aphasia and participants without aphasia, ensur- use when. The term therapy has a narrow connota-
ing that there are clear inclusion and exclusion crite- tion of working directly with an individual person
ria that will make clear what that distinction is; in a or group; for most, it conjures up the image of a
study on cognitive aspects of TBI, one might refer to clinician and a person engaged together in a room
participants without any history of neurological disorder within a hospital, rehabilitation center, or clinic. The
and people with language disorders subsequent to TBI. people we serve, colleagues from other professions,
and laypeople in general often hold the stereotype
that therapy is the main thing that SLPs do. The terms
Neurotypical People treatment and intervention, in the SLP’s realm, convey
the notion of any aspect of helping to improve com-
No two brains are alike. There are numerous dif- munication, compensate for communication defi-
ferences between any two living brains in terms cits, or cope with communication challenges. They
of structure, function, and neurochemical environ- include all of the elements of therapy but also such
ment. Humans differ vastly in terms of genetics, diverse activities as ongoing assessment, research
personality, experience, intelligence, education, and on options for intervention, analyses of commu-
a host of other factors, all of which are reflected in nication environments in workplaces and homes,
the complex functioning of their brains. No one is counseling, coordination of support groups, dis-
neurotypical. Saying that people with acquired brain charge planning, and training of staff, family, and
challenges are not typical conveys an ignorance of caregivers.
the many ways in which no one is typical. Unless one is referring to the narrower con-
struct of therapy, then it may be best to use the term
treatment or intervention. This helps to reinforce the
What Are Important Nuances in Terms We Use notion that SLPs carry out their work in diverse
to Refer to People Who Care for People With ways and settings to help improve lives through
improved communication, and the notion that far
Neurogenic Cognitive-Linguistic Disorders?
more is involved in clinical practice than sitting in a
therapy room and providing therapy. This is a small
The term caregiver may refer generically to anyone but important way to advocate for our profession,
providing any type of care to another. What about through heightened appreciation and respect for
when it applies to a family member, life partner, or what it is that SLPs do.
2. Writing and Talking About the People With Whom We Work   31

Again, it is important to acknowledge that, by defi-


What Are Pros and Cons of Terms nition, certain terms may not literally or precisely
Used to Refer to SLPs? capture what we might mean — and that a clear
understanding of what is meant among all taking
The narrow connotation of the word therapy dis- part in a given communicative interaction is what is
cussed earlier carries over to the word therapist. most important.
Many prefer more comprehensive terms, such as
SLP or clinician, again to capture the broader conno-
tation of the scope of what we do. In most countries, What Is Important to Keep in Mind Regarding
the terms speech therapist (ST) or speech-language ther- Inclusive and Welcoming Language?
apist (SLT) are typically used by convention rather
than SLP. Although some clinicians in those coun-
tries would prefer to change that convention, such a When writing and talking about people in general ​
change does not come easily. The notion of pathology — whether it be patients, clients, caregivers, signif-
and pathologist implicated in the term SLP, common icant others, families, spouses, colleagues, and so
in the United States, has clearly negative associations on — it is important to avoid terms that may make
with disease and bodily tissues. Pathologies connote people feel excluded, judged, or unwelcome based,
deficits, illness, and things that are wrong; certainly, for example, on their race, ethnicity, language,
as clinicians, we do more than focus on those. In much nationality, religious or political beliefs, gender
of Europe, Canada, and Central and South America, identity, or sexual orientation. Health care settings
SLPs are commonly referred to as orthophonists, pho- are especially rife with use of terms and expressions
niatrists, logopedists, and logopedes (or their trans- that blatantly disregard differences among people
lated equivalents according to one’s language), with in terms of sexual orientation, gender identity, and
distinctions in scope of practice and requirements gender expression.
associated with such titles varying by region. Even those who are welcoming and accepting
of people whose sexual orientations and family con-
texts differ from their own sometimes unknowingly
What Are the Preferred Terms When Referring perpetuate heterosexist language or convey stereo-
typical assumptions about gender identity and the
to the Experts Who Work With People Who
nature of family structures and living arrangements.
Have Neurogenic Communication Challenges? For example, case history forms that include ques-
tions about marital status or clinical interviews in
As mentioned in Chapter 1, aphasiology is a term that which there are assumptions conveyed about the
has come to be much more comprehensive in scope interviewee having a “wife” or “husband” may
than just the study of aphasia. Many clinical apha- be perceived as alienating. People who identify as
siologists specialize not only in aphasia but also in being lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgendered, queer,
other neurogenic communication and even swallow- questioning, or asexual — or who may be perceived
ing disorders. In this book, the term clinical aphasi- to be different from what others consider to be nor-
ologist is often used interchangeably, if imperfectly, mal or socially conforming — often experience dis-
with SLP, given the context of discussing research crimination and sometimes even harassment, abuse,
and clinical work with adults who have neurogenic and refusal of services (see GLADD, 2012, 2014,
cognitive-linguistic disorders. Of course, many SLPs and 2016, for tutorial materials on this topic). Peo-
are not considered aphasiologists because many do ple who differ in terms of race, ethnicity, regional
not have specialized expertise in neurogenic com- and national identity, language, religion, and other
munication disorders or training in this area beyond characteristics often experience the same. Although
what is required for clinical certification and licen- this is illegal in some countries and regions, the
sure in SLP. Also, many aphasiologists study apha- practice continues, sometimes surreptitiously, and
sia from theoretical and experimental perspectives sometimes due to unconscious biases; it remains tol-
and are not clinically certified or educated as SLPs. erated in much of the world. This makes it all the
32  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

more vital for clinicians to strive to use inclusive and Consider, too, the commonly used term brain
supportive language that embraces people’s val- damage. Many of us do not think to challenge the
ues and acknowledges that people want the same word damage as we say it in this context. Let’s reflect,
opportunities as anyone else to seek and receive though, on the potential negative connotation asso-
excellent services and participate in caring for the ciated with damaged goods or products. A preferable
people they love. Of course, our work to enhance term might be brain injury or a more specialized
diversity, equity, inclusion, and welcoming must go medical term if it applies in a given health care con-
far beyond the terms we use. text (e.g., infarct or lesion).
An aspect of usage recommended by many
globally is to standardize the use of the singular
form of the pronouns they, them, themselves and the Why Are There Inconsistencies in the
pronominal adjective their instead of the gender-spe- Prefixes Used in Terms for Characterizing
cific pronouns he/she, him/her, himself/herself, and his/
Neurogenic Symptoms, and What Is the
her. Unless we know the gender and preferred pro-
nouns of a specific person to whom we are referring,
Rationale for Varied Prefix Choices?
using the singular they keeps us from conveying false
assumptions about gender identification and risking As noted in Chapter 1, the prefix dys- before a term
offense. Today, most style guides promote the use of indicates a problem, deficit, or weakness in function.
the singular they. For example, a person with dyslexia has difficulty
reading. A person with dysgraphia has difficulty
writing. The prefix a-, meaning without, indicates
What Other Terms Might Unintentionally that a person is without that function. Despite these
literal meanings, these prefixes are often used inter-
Convey Negative Connotations?
changeably when referring to symptoms in the
realm of neurogenic communication disorders. For
Sometimes we use words we have heard or read example, according to the literal definition of the
without giving a lot of thought to how they might be terms, a person with alexia is unable to read, and a
perceived by others. Keep in mind that the fact that person with agraphia is unable to write. However,
certain terms are used commonly, even by experts we when talking or writing about a literate person who
respect, does not make their usage well conceived or has completely lost the ability to read, one clinician
appropriate. Let’s consider some common examples. may say she has dyslexia while another may say she
A term frequently used to refer to the time prior has alexia. For the most part, we simply accept that
to the onset of a neurological disorder (or any state of actual usage does not always reflect the literal mean-
disease or disability) is premorbid, literally meaning ing of these prefixes.
before disease. The word morbid has connotations for A case in point is our use of the term aphasia
many, though, not only of disease and unhealthiness (literally without language) almost exclusively over
but also death and ghoulishness. To better convey an the term dysphasia, even though almost everyone
appreciation that people with neurological disorders with aphasia has some intact language abilities. I am
are not necessarily unhealthy or macabre, we might one of many who overtly discourage the use of the
instead use such terms as preinjury, pre-onset, or pre- word dysphasia, especially because it can be so easily
stroke (depending on the context). We might also use misunderstood due to its similarity to the word dys-
descriptive terms (e.g., before she showed signs of phagia (see Worrall et al., 2016).
memory loss, before he had his head injury). The term anomia (literally without words or
The terms suffer (e.g., he suffered a brain injury) names for things) is often used even when describ-
and victim (e.g., she is a stroke victim) are words not ing the symptoms of people who may produce lots
to use in general because of their disempowering of words but have frequent difficulty coming up with
connotations. By simply using person-first language, specific words they want to say (dysnomia). In sum, the
we can avoid conveying the idea that people with choice of prefixes in actual usage is more a matter of
disabilities are in miserable or helpless states. stylistic convention than of the literal interpretation.
2. Writing and Talking About the People With Whom We Work   33

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List alternatives for the word patient. For 2. Imagine you are giving an in-service to
each, describe in what type of context each health care professionals on sensitivity in
word would or would not be appropriate. the use of terms referring to people with
• How might you revise the following disabilities and older people. Create a set of
statements to demonstrate sensitivity and examples of inappropriate use of language
inclusiveness? to have them correct and discuss.
• Ms. DeRose is a spry 90-year-old apraxic. 3. What term(s) do you think best capture(s)
• I have three TBIs on my caseload. an active participant in SLP services without
• It is unclear if the grandmotherly power differential between the clinician and
demented woman was withdrawing the person being served and without an
socially because of her general assumption that the participant is ill?
crotchetiness or because of clinical 4. What are some effective and ineffective
depression. strategies for helping other health care and
• Subjects in this research were 30 elderly research professionals to improve their use
aphasic patients from the community. of appropriate terms in clinical contexts?
• He already had brain damage from a prior
www
accident, and then he suffered a stroke. See the companion website for additional learning
• Dr. Liu is a stroke victim. and teaching materials.
SECTION II

Foundations for Considering Acquired


Neurogenic Language Disorders
36  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

In this section, we lay out a foundation for under- orders in adults. Then, in Chapter 7, we review basic
standing the basic aspects of acquired language aspects of neurophysiology and anatomy most per-
disorders. First, in Chapter 4, we consider how it is tinent to clinical aphasiology, including basic prin-
that one may define and conceptualize aphasia. This ciples and neurological landmarks and systems. In
content lays important groundwork for considering Chapter 8, we address neurodiagnostic methods.
the entire category of acquired neurogenic commu- Finally, in Chapter 9, we address key content related
nication disorders. Then, in Chapter 5, we engage in to aging as it may affect cognitive-linguistic abilities
a concise tutorial about the World Health Organi- and communication in general. We focus on aging in
zation’s (WHO) International Classification of Func- this foundational content to underscore that aging is
tioning, Disability, and Health and its relevance to not pathological or a causal factor for acquired neu-
people who have neurogenic communication disor- rogenic language disorders; rather, aging is a multi-
ders and the people who care about them. Next, in faceted construct that is important for SLPs to know
Chapter 6, we examine the basic nature of conditions about and take into account in many aspects of their
that tend to cause acquired neurogenic language dis- work, especially in support of anti-ageism.
CHAPTER
4
Defining and Conceptualizing Aphasia

A great way to delve into the study of acquired neu- 4. How does one choose a preferred framework
rogenic language disorders is to first consider apha- for conceptualizing aphasia?
sia in detail. From there, once you master certain 5. How are the frameworks for conceptualizing
factual knowledge while also considering ways of aphasia relevant to other neurogenic language
embracing multiple theoretical perspectives regard- disorders?
ing aphasia, you will have a strong foundation on
which to base more learning and reflection on other
types of acquired neurogenic language disorders.
What Is a Good Way to Define Aphasia?
This is why we begin this section by defining and
conceptualizing aphasia.
Aphasia has tremendous variability in terms of In Chapter 1, we considered that a good way to
how it affects people. Still, describing aphasia and define aphasia is to make sure we include four ele-
its various manifestations may be less complex than ments in our definition:
describing some of the other neurogenic language
disorders, especially those that tend to result from 1. It is acquired.
more diffuse areas of injury to the brain. Also, the 2. It has a neurological cause.
fact that aphasia has been studied for over 150 years, 3. It affects reception and production of language
in contrast to most other neurogenic language disor- across modalities.
ders, makes it a good starting topic for broader study 4. It is not a sensory, motor, psychiatric, or
of neurogenic cognitive-linguistic disorders. In this intellectual disorder.
chapter, we consider what aphasia is and how to
define it. We review various ways of thinking about Incorporating these four elements yields a defi-
it, studying it, and assessing it, from a variety of nition of aphasia that meets Darley’s (1982) criteria
perspectives or frameworks. We also consider how of clarifying features sufficiently to make the disor-
frameworks for conceptualizing aphasia are relevant der recognizable while differentiating it from other
to other acquired neurogenic language disorders. disorders. Let’s consider each of those elements in
After reading and reflecting on the content in this more detail.
chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in your
own words, the following queries:
Aphasia Is Acquired
1. What is a good way to define aphasia?
2. How have established aphasiologists defined Aphasia is a loss of a degree of language ability. It is
aphasia? a loss because it occurs in people who have already
3. What are the primary frameworks for learned language. As we noted in Chapter 1, although
conceptualizing aphasia? it tends to occur most commonly in adults, children

37
38  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

also can acquire aphasia, inasmuch as a child who Aphasia Affects Reception and Production
has developed competence in one or more languages of Language Across Modalities
may then lose language abilities. However, aphasia
is not a congenital language disorder. A person must Aphasia affects all modalities of language. Recep-
already have acquired language to be able to lose tion is affected in terms of auditory comprehension,
aspects of it. reading comprehension, and understanding of sign
We say aphasia is a degree of loss because peo- language (in those who have already acquired sign
ple with aphasia typically retain many linguistic language). Production is affected in terms of the
abilities and communicative strengths. Also, many ability to formulate spoken, written, or signed lan-
symptoms of aphasia relate to problems of access to guage. Some people with aphasia have more diffi-
stored linguistic representations, not necessarily to a culty expressing themselves than understanding
loss of stored representations. This fact is at the heart others. Some have more difficulty understanding
of the following: than expressing. The terms expressive aphasia and
receptive aphasia are sometimes used to capture the
• communication improvements that many notion that there are predominant problems with
people with aphasia continue to make over production or understanding, respectively. Still, it is
years post-onset vital to recognize that aphasia affects all areas of lan-
• intervention approaches that have been guage, both expressive and receptive. People with
shown to enhance abilities in people with expressive aphasia, for example, have problems that
aphasia affect their comprehension. Even people with mild
• evidence that intact linguistic abilities can expressive forms of aphasia tend to have more dif-
be brought out by varying the modality, ficulty processing complex grammatical structures
complexity, and difficulty of tasks and stimuli, than people without aphasia. Also, most people
and by providing communicative and social with receptive aphasia produce speech and writing
support that is not typical of their language abilities before
• evidence that linguistic abilities demonstrated aphasia onset.
by people with aphasia typically fluctuate
from moment to moment and day to day
• theoretical models focusing on competence
(one’s true underlying knowledge and Aphasia Is Not a Speech, Intellectual,
abilities) versus performance (one’s ability to Sensory, or Psychiatric Disorder
demonstrate knowledge and abilities in some
overt way) Aphasia is a language disorder. Given how common
it is for language problems to be confused with other
problems, it is important that we use exclusionary
Aphasia Has a Neurological Cause criteria in defining aphasia. The exclusionary ele-
ments most commonly confused or misunderstood
Aphasia is most commonly caused by stroke. It can in everyday use of the term aphasia are the speech and
also be caused by a traumatic brain injury (TBI), neo- intellectual aspects, so let’s consider those further.
plasm (tumors) affecting the brain, surgical ablation Some laypeople inappropriately refer to aphasia
of brain tissue, infections, and metabolic problems. as a speech disorder because the content of the speech
This element of the definition relates to the acquired of people with aphasia tends to be atypical. The
nature of aphasia in that there is a loss of language abnormal content in aphasia, though, is not caused
due to some type of neurological event or condition by a motor problem affecting the speech mechanism
that leads to a loss of language ability. The onset but rather a problem in the formulation of linguistic
of aphasia is most frequently abrupt because most messages. Motor speech disorders (such as apraxia
of its underlying neurological causes tend to occur of speech and dysarthria) often occur concomitantly
suddenly. in people with aphasia.
4. Defining and Conceptualizing Aphasia   39

Some may mistakenly consider aphasia to be which are parts of the aphasia syndrome. The reason
an intellectual problem because it may sometimes one might say “to the extent possible” is that it can
seem, given the interaction abilities of people with be difficult to discern nonlinguistic aspects of cog-
aphasia, that their intelligence is reduced. This is not nition, such as certain aspects of memory and atten-
so. Educating people in general about this point is tion, from language abilities. There are two reasons
an important aspect of advocacy on behalf of peo- for this:
ple with aphasia. The National Aphasia Association
(NAA) and other groups promote such advocacy • Using language expressively and receptively
through buttons, bumper stickers, magnets, and requires essential memory and attention
other products emblazoned with the slogan, “Apha- functions; as such, we cannot assess language
sia is a loss of language, not intellect.” See an exam- abilities without tapping into memory and
ple in Figure 4–1. Like motor speech disorders, attention, too.
disorders of cognition (such as nonlinguistic prob- • Most of the stimuli and tasks used to study
lem-solving abilities) may co-occur with aphasia, but memory and attention require understanding
they are not part what defines aphasia. and processing of verbal (or at least symbolic)
Given the complex combinations of symptoms material and often require verbal responses; if
a person with any type of injury to the brain may verbal abilities are impaired, poor responses
experience, it is important to identify to the extent may be inappropriately attributed to memory
possible which deficits co-occur with aphasia versus and attention problems.

Figure 4–1. Aphasia advocacy in action, at a performance of the Aphasia Tones, a choir
in the California State University East Bay Aphasia Treatment Program. Aphasia is a loss
of language, not intellect. Photo courtesy of Ellen Bernstein-Ellis. A full-color version of this
figure can be found in the Color Insert.
40  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

We talk more about this as we further consider


ways of conceptualizing aphasia. For now, since we
What Are the Primary Frameworks
are still talking about defining aphasia, our focus is for Conceptualizing Aphasia?
on keeping the definition simple yet comprehensive
and not especially imbued with theoretical principles Some of the differences in how aphasiologists define
that are important to consider yet not essential to the aphasia are the result of differences in their theoret-
definition. Note that our present query is, “What is ical perspectives on aphasia. Some do not agree on
a good way to define aphasia,” not “What is the best the wording for a single definition because of the
definition of aphasia.” The principles underlying framework through which they conceptualize apha-
the definition are more important than the specific sia. From the earliest days of aphasiology to the
wording we choose. present, trends and developments in research and
practice have led to a wide array of options for think-
ing about and discussing aphasia in clinical practice,
How Have Established research contexts, and everyday life.
Aphasiologists Defined Aphasia? The way many aphasiologists conceptualize
aphasia reveals something about their own academic
roots, that is, the way they were taught to think about
The ways that aphasiologists define aphasia may be it. Others have changed the way they consider apha-
categorized as general neurolinguistic definitions, sia because of personal and professional experiences
definitions that include nonlinguistic cognitive they have had with people who have aphasia. Still
symptoms (e.g., working memory and attention) as others are influenced by emerging research, educa-
inherent components of aphasia, and broader defi- tion, and advocacy campaigns that challenge them
nitions of aphasia as a challenge to social interac- to consider differently what the “best” framework
tion and the impact of that challenge on quality of for conceptualizing aphasia is.
life and life participation. Examples of each are No matter what our personal viewpoints, it is
shown in Box 4–1. Note that these are given for important that we know about the varied ways that
illustrative purposes; several of these definitions of the construct of aphasia might be considered. This
aphasia do not meet the definitional requirements helps us appreciate differences among diagnostic
given earlier. and treatment approaches, aphasia research pro-
Once you have a clear idea of what aphasia is grams and projects, aphasia textbook contents and
and is not, it is important to practice defining apha- emphases, and orientations of individual clinicians
sia until you are able to do it accurately and suc- and scholars. Being able to grasp and appreciate the
cinctly without any notes, in writing and speaking. validity of multiple viewpoints at the same time is
No matter what the work setting, speech-language a fundamental quality of the clinical aphasiologist.
pathologists (SLPs) are often defining and explain- Note that many of the frameworks are not mutually
ing the nature of aphasia. We must be able to do this exclusive, although some are.
clearly and adeptly at varied levels of sophistication
depending on the background of people with apha-
sia and their family members, friends and caregivers,
and health professionals and laypeople in general.
Unidimensional Frameworks
In research contexts, it is important that the
definition of aphasia used to qualify participants In a unidimensional framework, all of language is
with aphasia for a given study be clearly stated. seen as one inseparable whole. Every level of lan-
This is essential for enabling researchers to inter- guage, from phonology to morphology to syntax to
pret the findings and evaluate conclusions based on semantics to pragmatics, is included in one cohe-
the assumptions that underlie the way the study’s sive ability or set of abilities. Likewise, production
authors define aphasia (McNeil & Pratt, 2001; Rob- and comprehension are not seen as separable com-
erts et al., 2003). ponents of language but rather as interwoven. An
4. Defining and Conceptualizing Aphasia   41

Box
4–1 Examples of Definitions of Aphasia

General neurolinguistic definitions

An acquired communication disorder caused by brain damage, character-


ized by an impairment of linguistic expression and/or reception; it is not
the result of a sensory deficit, a general intellectual deficit, or a psychiatric
disorder. (Hallowell & Chapey, 2008a, p. 3)

A family of clinically diverse disorders that affect the ability to communicate


by oral or written language, or both, following brain damage. (Goodglass,
1993, p. 2)

The disturbance of any or all of the skills, associations and habits of spoken
and written language produced by injury to certain brain areas that are spe-
cialized for these functions. (Goodglass & Kaplan, 2001, p. 5)

An impairment, due to acquired and recent damage of the central nervous


system, of the ability to comprehend and formulate language. It is a multi-
modality disorder represented by a variety of impairments in auditory com-
prehension, reading, oral-expressive language, and writing. The disrupted
language may be influenced by physiological inefficiency or impaired cogni-
tion, but it cannot be explained by dementia, sensory loss or motor dysfunc-
tion. (Rosenbek et al., 1989, p. 53)

Definitions that include cognitive symptoms as inherent components of aphasia

Impairment, as a result of brain damage, of the capacity for interpretation


and formulation of language symbols; multimodality loss or reduction in
efficiency of the ability to decode and encode conventional meaningful lin-
guistic elements (morphemes and larger syntactic units); disproportionate
to impairment of other intellective functions; not attributable to dementia,
confusion, sensory loss, or motor dysfunction; and manifested in reduced
availability of vocabulary, reduced efficiency in application of syntactic rules,
reduced auditory retention span, and impaired efficiency in input and output
channel selection. (Darley, 1982, p. 42)

Broader definitions of aphasia as a challenge to social interaction and the impact of


that challenge on quality of life and life participation

Aphasia is a communication disability due to an acquired impairment of


language modalities caused by focal brain damage. Aphasia may affect par-
ticipation and quality of life of the person with aphasia as well as their fam-
ily and friends. Aphasia masks competence and affects functioning across
relationships, life roles and activities, thereby influencing social inclusion,
social connectedness, access to information and services, equal rights, and
wellbeing in family, community and culture. (Berg et al., 2020, p. 7)
42  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

injury to the brain that results in language deficits abilities. Considering patterns of performance may
in any given aspect of language ability may affect all increase the efficiency with which we develop opti-
aspects of language ability. Hildred Schuell is known mal treatment programs. Likewise, considering the
as the major historic proponent of this framework corresponding structural changes in the brain may
(Schuell & Jenkins, 1959; Schuell et al., 1964). The help us to think critically about why a person is hav-
Minnesota Test for Differential Diagnosis of Apha- ing a particular linguistic problem.
sia (MTDDA; Schuell, 1973), the aphasia language Two people who have poor auditory comprehen-
assessment tool that she developed (no longer in sion, for example, may have starkly different lesion
press), is based on this framework. locations; knowing the locations of their lesions may
Although to this day there are proponents of help us differentiate the nature of their comprehen-
this framework, it is generally considered outmoded sion deficits. Weaknesses of this approach are that
in light of evidence for a more multidimensional it is not a panacea for understanding the nature of
framework of aphasia that might better capture any individual’s manifestations of aphasia, let alone
variations among differing manifestations of apha- for knowing how we might best support a person’s
sia. Still, it has the strength of recognizing the inter- meaningful real-life communication and life partici-
dependence of all aspects of language, receptive and pation. Given the commonality of multidimensional
expressive, from phonology to pragmatics. Also, views in clinical and research practices, we explore
it fits with evidence for a great deal of functional multidimensional classification schemes in much
interconnectivity among structures thought to be greater detail in Chapter 10.
specialized for language — not just a set of discreet
specialized structures. Finally, it is a framework that
helps us to consider each individual with aphasia Medical Frameworks
as having a unique set of challenges requiring indi-
vidualized assessment that leads to the design of an Medical frameworks typically incorporate multi-
individually tailored treatment program. dimensional views and thus may be considered a
subset of that category of viewpoints. In medical con-
texts, aphasia is considered primarily at the impair-
Multidimensional Frameworks ment level, that is, at the level of specific linguistic
deficits. There is a focus on analyzing the cause in
Multidimensional frameworks are characterized by terms of a disease state (e.g., stroke) or change in
the view that there are varied forms or syndromes body structure (e.g., trauma or neoplasm). Assess-
of aphasia, each syndrome corresponding to a site ment entails identifying deficits, and treatment plans
of the lesion. Any syndrome of aphasia may be char- are designed to address those deficits. Operating
acterized by a set of hallmark features. The way the from this perspective may be consistent with the
syndromes are classified has varied over the decades. viewpoints of other rehabilitation team members,
Still, there are common aspects across many classifi- especially those focused on physical impairments,
cation schemes. Classifications of fluent versus non- and thus help an SLP feel more easily understood
fluent and anterior versus posterior forms of aphasia when communicating with others about assessment
fit this framework. So do the “classical” classification and plans of care. Being able to document the med-
systems suggesting specific aphasia syndromes (e.g., ical nature of language deficits may also be essen-
Wernicke’s, Broca’s, transcortical sensory, transcor- tial to being reimbursed financially for SLP services.
tical motor, mixed transcortical, and conduction Serious drawbacks, though, are that there tends to
aphasia, all of which are discussed in detail in Chap- be a focus on weaknesses, not strengths, and on
ter 10). A strength of this approach is that it recog- attempting to “fix” problems at the expense of help-
nizes well-established patterns of brain-behavior ing people compensate for and cope with challenges
relationships, which may help us predict particular they will likely continue to have long after they are
difficulties with language as well as concomitant discharged from the medical contexts in which we
problems that may affect a person’s communication work with them.
4. Defining and Conceptualizing Aphasia   43

Cognitive Neuropsychological, Psycholinguistic, Many aphasiologists whose work is characterized by


and Neurolinguistic Frameworks a psycholinguistic framework focus on specific areas
of processing, such as lexical perception, sentence
comprehension, or naming.
A cognitive neuropsychological framework is based Within a cognitive neuropsychological frame-
on models of mental representation and types and work, aphasia may be seen as a disruption in the
stages of information processing. Psycholinguistic processing required for any given linguistic task or
frameworks, which are focused on the processing set of tasks. A proponent of this view is likely to see
of linguistic information in particular, are a subset a primary goal of assessment as determining at what
of this framework. Components of information pro- level in the system there is a problem. Some cogni-
cessing (or modules) are often conceptualized within tive neuropsychological models proposed, espe-
boxes in flowcharts, with arrows showing the order cially early ones from the 1970s and 1980s, are serial
of processing stages and interconnections among models, suggesting that information passes from one
components. Assumptions are typically made about stage and then to the next. As theories of language
the degree of functional modularity of any given processing have evolved, there has been greater rec-
component (i.e., its independence from or interde- ognition of the interdependence and simultaneous
pendence with other components). Although some engagement of multiple processes even for a rela-
who ascribe to this type of framework attempt to tively simple task, such as understanding a spoken
associate anatomical structures or networks of struc- word. Greater degrees of complex, interactive, and
tures to specific components, the notion of modules overlapping processes are involved in more complex
rather than brain structures helps to circumvent the tasks, such as sentence and discourse comprehen-
challenges of relating language and cognitive defi- sion. Computational models have been developed
cits to specific anatomical lesion sites, and vice versa. to capture some of this complexity, focusing on what
Kay, Lesser, and Coltheart’s (1997) PALPA is an the arrows rather than just the boxes might represent
aphasia assessment battery that is well known for its in cognitive neuropsychological models (e.g., Dell
grounding in psycholinguistic theory. A schematic & O’Seaghdha, 1992). Some computational models
diagram based on their psycholinguistic model have been further developed to capture neural cor-
for comprehension and production, amplified to relates of theoretical stages of language processing
include additional components and influences, is (e.g., Dell et al., 2013; Halai et al., 2017; Mirman et al.,
shown in Figure 4–2. Auditory lexical perception 2011; Nozari & Dell, 2013; Ruml et al., 2000; Zeng
is shown as beginning with the acoustic input from a et al., 2020).
speech signal, which first goes through an auditory It is important for the clinical aphasiologist to
phonological analysis process, then passes through have a grasp of theories of language processing in
a phonological input buffer, to a phonological input people without neurological problems and how
lexicon, to the semantic system. Orally naming an processing may go awry when a person’s brain is
object begins with seeing the object, processing the injured. Neurolinguistic frameworks may be con-
visual stimulus through the visual object recognition sidered a subset of the cognitive neuropsychological
system to the semantic system, then formulating the frameworks. A distinction is that they incorporate
associated word through the phonological output connectionist models, models of neuroanatomical
lexicon and phonological output buffer, finally lead- structures and functions, or neural networks, distrib-
ing to speech. In a repetition task, since the listener uted through cortical and subcortical structures that
need not process the auditory stimulus in terms of are associated with various information-process-
its semantic properties in order to repeat it, they can ing components. A strength of cognitive neuropsy-
simply bypass the processes associated with the chological frameworks is that they help clinicians
phonological input buffer, the phonological input think critically about the underlying nature of overt
lexicon, and the sematic system and simply engage symptoms observed. Similar symptoms noted in
in acoustic-to-phonologic conversion and the phono- two different individuals with aphasia may result
logical output buffer to engage in the speech process. from different types of disruptions. For example,
44  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Figure 4–2. Psycholinguistic model of language processing. Image credit: Mohammad Haghighi.

difficulty naming an object may result from any of • difficulty assembling phonological units (at
the following: a level known as the phonological output
lexicon) to be able to articulate the object name
• difficulty seeing the object through the • a deficit in the short-term storage of
primary visual system phonological units (at a level known as the
• difficulty processing the visual stimulus phonological output buffer) to be able to
through the visual object recognition system articulate the object name
• a deficit in processing the object, once it is • impaired neuromotor innervation or motor
recognized, through the semantic system control precluding a correct spoken response
4. Defining and Conceptualizing Aphasia   45

Thus, there are several possible levels of dis- ronmental factors, social support, and a person’s
ruption that could lead to object naming problems. desire for active engagement in varied life contexts.
Determining the level or levels of an individual’s A system that fits this framework has been developed
naming problem helps guide treatment planning through the WHO to help foster holistic perspectives
by focusing on the deficient processes. Several on disabilities and the people who have them.
impairment-focused approaches to treatment have The WHO International Classification of Func-
grounding in neuropsychological theory. tioning, Disability, and Health (ICF) is a system for
A limitation of cognitive neuropsychologi- classifying disabilities that takes into consideration
cal frameworks is that the components said to be not just medical or organic aspects of health-related
responsible for any particular processing stage can- challenges but also the complex consequences of
not capture the complexity of what must really hap- having those challenges. Its primary influence on
pen in the brain to achieve whatever processing is the way we conceptualize aphasia is by having us
intended to be captured at that stage. For example, consider aphasia in terms of the following:
what processes must happen within the “semantic
system” to be able to comprehend a complex sen- • body structure and the associated functions
tence? What about to name an object? These ques- that are affected (i.e., an individual’s brain
tions require further models of their own with more and its ability to process cognitive and
finely grained details pertaining to those specific linguistic information)
functions. Some critics mention a lack of focus on • activities and participation (i.e., the ability to
environmental factors, supported communication, speak, listen, read, and write and the way a
and life participation as a limitation of neuropsy- person uses those abilities to engage in social,
chological frameworks. Some mention a focus on professional, and other daily life activities)
weaknesses and deficits at the expense of a focus
on communicative strengths. Thankfully, as we con- Any given individual’s aphasia must be char-
tinue to discuss, we have the wonderful prerogative acterized in ways that extend beyond the level of
to be able to combine approaches rather than choose damaged physical structures and the basic cogni-
any one exclusively. Thus, we are free to mix neu- tive-linguistic functions that are affected. Whether
ropsychological methods with social and supported we are engaging in assessment, treatment, research,
communication approaches, always thinking criti- education, or advocacy work, we ideally take into
cally about what is most appropriate when working account the impact of aphasia on the life context and
with a particular person. everyday experiences of the actual people who have
Details about cognitive neuropsychological the- it. We also ideally take into account how it affects
ories of language processing are explored later in other people within that person’s social spheres.
this book as they pertain to specific clinical aspects We explore this notion in greater detail in Chap-
of assessment and treatment associated with var- ter 5 and revisit it in subsequent chapters because it
ied disorders and symptoms. Readers interested is relevant to virtually every aspect of our work as
in learning more about these fascinating aspects aphasiologists.
of research and clinical practice are encouraged to
explore the numerous resources cited.
Social Frameworks

Biopsychosocial Frameworks Another set of frameworks for considering the


nature of aphasia is more focused on interpersonal
A biopsychosocial framework is one that high- contexts of communication. Aphasia is seen in this
lights attention to the complex interaction of mul- set of frameworks as a social condition. A prob-
tiple factors that constitute “disabilities” and affect lem with communication is considered a problem
health. For example, proponents of this framework because the person with aphasia and/or people in
suggest that it is important to consider the complex their social environment consider it to be a problem.
relationship among genetics and other risk factors, Some social frameworks may be considered a sub-
etiologies, impaired structures and functions, envi- set of biopsychosocial frameworks because of their
46  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

focus on the real-life social use of language, an essen- argue that a limitation is that social approaches to
tial component of life participation in the WHO ICF. assessment and treatment are not easily encap-
Some may be considered subsets of social determi- sulated or described. Actually, this is not so much
nants of health frameworks. However, some social a limitation in the approach as it is a challenge to
frameworks do not address the underlying causes the clinician to think and work expansively and
of aphasia in a direct way. creatively in light of each individual’s desires and
Many adherents to social frameworks consider needs for social connection.
the biomedical aspects of aphasia and specific sorts
of deficits that might be explained through a neuro-
psychological approach as far less relevant than the Social Determinants of Health Frameworks
impact aphasia has on a person’s ability to engage
in meaningful interactions in everyday life. Aphasia Inequities in health care and social and economic
is seen as a life-affecting condition, contextualized conditions lead to inequities in how well people
in everyday life experiences. People with aphasia with any type of health or wellness challenge may
benefit from compensatory and adaptive resources live. Social determinants of health are any societal
and services plus adaptations to their physical and factors that may impact people and others in their
social contexts to boost their full life participation lives, and the long-term consequences of those in
(Hallowell & Chapey, 2008a). The Life Participation terms of health and well-being. When we consider
Approach to Aphasia (LPAA; LPAA Project Group, social determinants that affect the long-term con-
2000) is an example of a social model. This and sev- sequences of aphasia, our attention is drawn to the
eral others are highlighted throughout this book, tremendous variability in environmental support for
most of which are compatible with LPAA. people with aphasia throughout the world. Impor-
A strength of social frameworks is the height- tantly, addressing the many forms of inequity is crit-
ened awareness they bring to the reason we do ical to local and global work to advance social justice
the work that we do: to improve people’s lives in and human rights. Like social frameworks, a social
meaningful ways through improved communication determinants of health framework (Commission on
and socialization in ways that they think are import- Social Determinants of Health, 2008) includes a focus
ant. The severity of a person’s aphasia is seen not on the chronic nature of aphasia. We consider how
in terms of cognitive-linguistic deficits that may be a person’s strengths, challenges, and needs related
quantified and qualified through isolated tasks in a to communication and life participation evolve over
clinical environment; rather, the severity of aphasia time (O’Halloran et al., 2017) and how it is important
has to do with the severity of its impact on a per- to address varied forms of contextual support in an
son’s well-being. Potential direct consequences of ongoing way as conditions change.
aphasia include loss of self-esteem, social isolation
and loneliness, depression, reduced participation
in important activities, and changed relationships. Other Historically Relevant Frameworks
These consequences are seen as fundamental to the
very nature of aphasia, not just concomitant chal- Other types of frameworks have been proposed by
lenges (Berg et al., 2020; Elman, 2016; Holland, 2021; scholars studying aphasia in the past. Their frame-
Parr, 2007; Shadden, 2005; Simmons-Mackie, 2013a). works have relevance to aphasiology in terms of
Another strength of social approaches is that appreciating the evolution of our understanding
they take into account the ongoing, usually lifelong, about aphasia, even if there are few people who
consequences of aphasia. The clinical aphasiologist would ascribe to these frameworks in particular
might only see a particular person with aphasia for today. The following four, highlighted by Hallowell
1 to 12 sessions, for example, but that person will and Chapey (2008b), are examples.
continue to cope with the condition for years to
come. Thus, adherents to this approach are mindful Concrete-Abstract Framework
of the need to prioritize attention to long-term sup-
ports for improved communication to be relied upon Goldstein (1948) suggested that aphasia reflects
long after discharge from formal services. One might a loss of “abstract attitude,” the ability to express
4. Defining and Conceptualizing Aphasia   47

and comprehend thoughts that cannot be captured Thought Process Framework


merely through sensory experience with objects
and actions that are physically present. Although Wepman (1972) suggested that people with apha-
there is evidence that more physically describable sia have impaired thought processes (due at least in
and apparent objects and actions are easier for peo- part to the unintended utterances they tend to make)
ple with aphasia to talk about and understand than that interfere with their actual thinking. There is lit-
more abstract ones, the same is true of people with- tle empirical support for this framework, other than
out neurogenic language disorders. Most clinical through findings regarding the literal meaning of
aphasiologists today would likely agree that indices unintended utterances or paraphasias — a phenom-
of concrete expression and comprehension are better enon we explore further as we delve into the varied
than abstract expression and comprehension in most types of aphasia in Chapter 10. In fact, suggesting
people with aphasia. Still, we now have a greater that people with aphasia have an inherent problem
appreciation for the various levels of influence on with thinking suggests that they have an intellectual
such findings than is captured in the concrete ver- deficit, which is an affront to people with aphasia
sus abstract distinction at the time of Goldstein’s and detracts from important advocacy efforts.
work. For example, the words commonly associated
with high levels of “abstraction” are also considered Microgenetic Framework
more difficult, according to measures of the age at Brown (1972, 1977) and Raleigh (Brown & Raleigh,
which words are acquired, the familiarity of words 1979) proposed that language abilities reflect the pro-
to speakers of a given language, and the frequency gression of evolutionary development of the brain.
with which words are used in a particular language Limbic structures, phylogenetically older compo-
(Chapman & Hallowell, 2015). Binney, Zuckerman, nents of the brain, are said to mediate basic and early
and Reilly (2016) describe efforts to frame challenges stages of language processing, while more recently
with abstract versus concrete words within a neuro- evolved structures mediate higher cortical functions
psychological perspective. of language and cognition. Various manifestations of
aphasia are said to correspond to lesions that affect
Propositional Language Framework levels of evolutionary progression, from basic abili-
ties initially mediated by structures developed ear-
Hughling Jackson (1878) characterized aphasia as lier in humans to more complex abilities mediated
an inability to make propositions. Propositions are by structures developed later. Increasing evidence
intentional, meaningful expressions (written oral, of subcortical structures being implicated in varied
or signed) meant to convey informational content. forms of aphasia, with and without identifiable cor-
Jackson reasoned that people with aphasia tend to tical lesions, makes this framework questionable in
express more easily what he called “subproposi- terms of its potential to explain variations in types of
tional” than propositional language. That is, they aphasia. Still, it has an important place in our over-
most easily express language that is more auto- all appreciation of the potential role of subcortical
matic and highly learned, such as days of the week, structures in language and cognition. It also helps
months of the year, basic social responses (e.g., us consider the simultaneous interactivity of multi-
“Fine thanks, how are you? ” when asked, “How ple cortical and subcortical structures as opposed to
are you?”), and words to a well-known song or a system of structures that operate in turn through
nursery rhyme. Although there is some empirical discrete stages of information processing.
support for this framework, much of it is without
appropriate control for the influence of concomitant
motor speech disorders, such as apraxia of speech,
on verbal production. Also, the theory underlying
How Does One Choose a Preferred
this framework lacks explicit evidence in terms of Framework for Conceptualizing Aphasia?
corresponding comprehension abilities (although
it is difficult to study the comprehension of highly Historically, many aphasiologists adopted the pre-
learned utterances, which may occur without any vailing framework that they learned about when
intent to convey meaning). they first studied aphasia. However, in more recent
48  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

years, our expanding clinical and research litera- ant, as long as we do not sacrifice what we truly
ture, plus advocacy on the part of social-model pro- value as professionals and as long as we extend the
ponents, has led to more open challenging of our very best service possible to people with aphasia and
reflections on this topic. As we noted earlier, clinical the people who care about them.
SLPs grasp and appreciate multiple viewpoints at
the same time. Doing so helps them best understand
the appropriateness of various assessment and treat- How Are the Frameworks for
ment methods based on their related assumptions
Conceptualizing Aphasia Relevant to
about what aphasia is. One need not choose one
Other Neurogenic Language Disorders?
viewpoint and stick with that one at all costs. Flexi-
bility of interpretation in context is vital.
For example, it is possible to work in a medi- Most of the frameworks for considering aphasia are
cal context and address what needs to be addressed applicable to other acquired neurogenic language dis-
in terms of medical documentation, yet not take a orders. A primary example is that clinicians working
strong “medical” approach to intervention. One with survivors of TBI and people with dementia often
may combine approaches. Another example is that are immersed in medically focused contexts, so they
one may digest and translate into clinical practice must be culturally and linguistically adaptable to med-
the conclusions of a research study without neces- ically oriented team meetings and documentation. At
sarily agreeing on the means by which the authors the same time, they may engage in cognitive neuro-
of the study conceptualized aphasia. Being able to psychological assessments and treatment approaches
adapt our conversations about aphasia to meet the and, ideally, apply their clinical work in socially ori-
demands of a given professional context is import- ented ways with a focus on life participation.

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter 5. Compare your own definition of aphasia
that are new to you or that you have not yet with those of other authors whose definitions
mastered. were reviewed in this chapter. You may wish
2. Why is it important to include exclusionary to look up others’ definitions as well.
criteria when we define aphasia? a. How is your definition similar to
3. Review the definitions of aphasia given in and different from those of other
Box 4–1. aphasiologists?
a. For each, consider whether it meets the b. Does making such comparisons make
criteria for the definition of aphasia given you want to change anything in your own
in this chapter. written definition? If so, how and why
b. Are there some definitions you prefer might you revise the definition that you
over others? If so, why? wrote?
c. Are there some definitions with which 6. With one or more partners, practice defining
you do not agree or that you think need aphasia orally as if you were speaking to
improvement? If so, how would you each of the following:
improve them? a. A social worker working in a
4. Practice defining aphasia in your own words. rehabilitation facility
Write your own definition without looking b. A caregiver with an advanced graduate
at any notes, and then check it. Practice degree
defining aphasia orally with a partner. c. A spouse with a high school education
4. Defining and Conceptualizing Aphasia   49

d. A person with mild aphasia the exercise in Item 9 above? What


e. A person with severe aphasia improvements should be made in the
7. How might the way you define aphasia diagram to address those shortcomings?
when talking to the partner or spouse of 11. With a partner, use the diagram in Figure 4–2
a newly diagnosed person with aphasia to talk through the level of breakdown when
differ from the way you would define it in a a person has the following difficulties:
scientific paper? a. Reading a word aloud but not
8. Describe in your own words the strengths understanding it
and limitations of: b. Trying to read a word but not being able
a. Unidimensional frameworks of aphasia to make sense of the letters
b. Multidimensional frameworks of c. Hearing a spoken word but not
aphasia understanding it
c. Medical frameworks of aphasia d. Hearing a spoken word but not being
d. Cognitive neuropsychological able to repeat it
frameworks of aphasia e. Seeing an object but not being able to
e. Biopsychosocial frameworks of aphasia think of a word to represent it
related to the WHO International f. Seeing an object and thinking of a word
Classification of Functioning, Disability, to represent it but not being able to say
and Health (ICF) the word
f. Social frameworks of aphasia g. Thinking of a word and not being able to
g. Other historically relevant frameworks write it
9. With a partner, use the diagram in Figure 4–2 12. Using a model or image of the brain, discuss
to talk through the stages of processing the possible structures underlying areas of
involved in breakdown that correspond to the problems
a. Reading a word and understanding it listed in Item 3.
b. Reading a word aloud and understanding it 13. What is the role of pragmatic language
c. Hearing a spoken word and and “other cognitive skills” in the model
understanding it depicted in Figure 4–2?
d. Hearing a spoken word and repeating it 14. Describe how your own theoretical
aloud perspectives on brain and language and on
e. Hearing a spoken word and writing it the nature of neurogenic language disorders
f. Hearing a spoken word and interpreting influence the way you personally:
it into sign language a. Define aphasia
g. Thinking of a concept and expressing it in b. Teach others about aphasia
writing c. Assess the communication abilities of a
h. Thinking of a concept and expressing it in person with aphasia
a spoken word d. Develop and carry out a treatment plan
i. Thinking of a concept and expressing it as for a person with aphasia
a gesture
www
10. What were shortcomings of using the Please see the companion website for supplemen-
diagram in Figure 4–2 when addressing tal material and activities.
CHAPTER
5
The WHO ICF, Human
Rights Perspectives, and Life
Participation Approaches

In Chapter 4, we considered how the World Health resents an important approach that followed previ-
Organization’s (WHO) International Classification of ous attempts by the WHO to classify functional areas
Functioning, Disability, and Health (ICF) represents of health and disability.
an important framework for considering the nature An initial system published in 1980, the Inter-
of aphasia. Given its relevance to virtually every area national Classification of Impairments, Disabilities,
of clinical and research work in the realm of acquired and Handicaps (ICIDH; WHO, 1980) was based on
neurogenic communication disorders, we review the notion that any health condition could be classi-
it in greater detail in this chapter and then highlight fied according to each of three levels: impairment,
its relevance to service delivery, assessment, and disability, and handicap. Impairments were defined
intervention in later chapters. After reading and as being “concerned with abnormalities of body
reflecting on the content in this chapter, you will structure and appearance and with organ or system
ideally be able to answer, in your own words, the function, resulting from any cause” (p. 14). Disabil-
following queries: ities were defined as “consequences of impairment
in terms of functional performance and activity by
1. What is the WHO ICF? the individual” (p. 14). Handicaps were defined as
2. How is the WHO ICF relevant to ethics and “disadvantages experienced by the individual as a
human rights? result of impairments and disabilities” (p. 14).
3. How is the WHO ICF specifically relevant to A subsequent modification was made in 1999 to
intervention and research in rehabilitation? better emphasize the importance of engagement in
4. How is the WHO ICF specifically relevant to daily life activities and participation in one’s social
people with neurogenic language disorders? context as central to the construct of health. Reframed
as the ICDH-2, the major components of health and
disability were modified to include impairments,
activity limitations, and participation restrictions.
What Is the WHO ICF?
Activity limitations were defined as “difficulties an
individual may have in the performance of activi-
The WHO ICF is an acronym for the WHO Inter- ties” (WHO, 1999, p. 16). Participation restrictions
national Classification of Functioning, Disability, were defined as “problems an individual may have
and Health. It is a conceptual framework and also in the manner or extent of involvement in life sit-
a system for classifying health and health-related uations” (WHO, 1999, p. 16). Following criticisms
domains for clinical and research applications in a that the ICDH components were all focused on neg-
consistent way throughout the world. The ICF rep- ative constructs and not neutral or positive aspects

51
52  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

of health, the ICF was developed in 2001. A sum- According to the ICF (WHO, 2001), any health
mary of the varied WHO models is encapsulated in condition a person may have may be viewed accord-
Box 5–1. ing to two domains:

The stated aims of the ICF are to: • Functioning and disability
• Contextual factors
• Provide a scientific basis for understanding
and studying health and health-related
Functioning (defined as “body functions, activ-
states, outcomes and determinants . . . ;
ities, and participation”) and disability (defined as
• Establish a common language for
“impairment, activity limitations or participation
describing health and health-related
restriction,” WHO, 2001, p. 1), combined, include
states . . . to improve communication
two components:
between different users, such as health
care workers, researchers, policy-makers
• body structures (anatomical parts) and
and the public, including people with
body functions (the physiological and
disabilities . . . ;
psychological aspects of the body)
• Permit comparison of data across
• activity (execution of tasks) and participation
countries, health care disciplines, services
(involvement in real-life activities, events,
and time; and . . .
situations, and relationships)
• Provide a systematic coding scheme for
health information systems. (WHO, 2001,
Activities and participation are each seen as
p. 5)
having aspects of both performance and capacity.
Performance represents what a person actually
does in their current context. Capacity represents
Box
5–1 Summary of WHO Models what a person can do when appropriate supports
are in place in their environment. That is, one’s
WHO, 1980 (ICIDH) environment may be modified and supports may
• Impairment be put in place to enhance performance in a partic-
• Disability ular aspect of activity or participation. Contextual
• Handicap factors, according to the ICF, include personal and
environmental factors that should be analyzed and
WHO, 1999 (ICIDH-2) addressed regarding each individual’s health con-
• Impairment ditions. Personal factors are characteristics of an
• Activity limitation individual outside of their health condition. They
• Participation restriction include, for example, age, race, ethnicity, sexual
orientation, gender identity, education, profession,
WHO, 2001 (ICF) habits, beliefs, attitudes, perspectives, and life expe-
• Part 1: Functioning and disability rience. Environmental factors are those outside of an
• Body structure and function individual person, including the physical surround-
  n Anatomical parts ings, the economy, social support, education, health
  n 
Physiological and psychological care, and the affect and attitudes of relevant people.
aspects They entail factors that are specific to an individual
• Activity and participation and people important to or involved with that per-
  n Performance son. They also entail societal factors.
  n Capacity No contextual factor is a direct component of
• Part 2: Contextual factors the specific health condition being considered at any
• Personal factors given point in time. Personal and environmental fac-
• Environmental factors tors can be enhancing or limiting in the context of an
individual’s everyday life participation. Contextual
5. The WHO ICF, Human Rights Perspectives, and Life Participation Approaches   53

factors are highly influential upon one another and of the fundamental rights of every human being
not always easy to tease apart in real life (Threats, without distinction of race, religion, political belief,
2010b). Any given individual’s degree of disability economic or social condition” (WHO, 2006, p. 1).
is influenced by environmental and personal factors. Every person’s health and active participation in
The ICF is not a model in that it provides no what is important to them on a daily basis are basic
specific predictions about the nature of the inter- human rights.
action of the complex factors that comprise health Several authors, including those who created
or disability (Threats, 2010b). Importantly, the ICF the World Report on Disability (WHO & the World
enables us to classify health conditions, not people Bank, 2011), call attention to unfortunate patterns
(Threats, 2010a). Classification of any individual’s of exclusion based on the type of disabling condi-
condition necessarily involves the individual and tion a person has. In general, disabilities related to
their caregivers and significant others, not just the cognition, communication, and behavior are more
health care professional (Threats & Worrall, 2004). marginalizing than are physical disabilities and
Applications of the ICF continue to evolve. blindness; yet in many health care contexts across
“Core sets” of categories relevant to specific areas the globe, there is a far greater focus on physical dis-
of real-life functioning according to specific health abilities and disorders than there is on cognitive and
conditions (diseases, disorders, impairments, etc.) linguistic wellness. This makes our work in counter-
have been proposed (Bickenbach et al., 2012). The ing the stigma experienced by people with commu-
intent is to help tie the specific conditions to spe- nication-related problems all the more challenging
cific categories of everyday life functions. ICF core (Deal, 2003; Wickenden, 2013). It also highlights the
set categories include such constructs as communi- importance of our work to promote communication
cation, interpersonal interactions and relationships, wellness as a basic human right (International Com-
and community, social, and domestic life. For each, munication Project, n.d.).
an individual may have any number of associated
limitations in activity and barriers to optimal partic-
ipation. In the upcoming chapters in this book, we How Is the WHO ICF Specifically Relevant to
further discuss how these constructs may be applied
Intervention and Research in Rehabilitation?
in clinical practice.

The ICF helps rehabilitation professionals and schol-


How Is the WHO ICF Relevant to ars to
Ethics and Human Rights?
• focus on health, well-being, and quality of life;
• acknowledge that every human being can
The WHO’s focus in developing the ICF over the experience a decrement in health and thereby
years has not been only to classify limitations and experience some degree of disability;
abilities. It has been to do this within a broader moral, • recognize health and disability as universal
ethical, and human rights perspective (Threats, human experiences;
2010b). The WHO is the public health component • take into account the dynamic interaction
of the United Nations, an international organiza- among such life-affecting variables as social
tion “committed to maintaining international peace support, religion, spirituality, environment,
and security, developing friendly relations among genetics, and health risks in all aspects of
nations and promoting social progress, better living assessment, treatment, and clinical research;
standards and human rights” (United Nations, n.d.). • consider disability not as an attribute of any
The constitution of the WHO includes the principles given person but as a construct that can only
that “health is a state of complete physical, mental be considered in an individual’s life context,
and social well-being and not merely the absence especially their social environment;
of disease or infirmity” and that “the enjoyment • deemphasize an individual’s health status
of the highest attainable standard of health is one according to medical diagnostic categories,
54  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

focusing instead on holistic functional concerns intimacy, inability to return to work or educational
and what might be done to address them; and pursuits, loss of independence and autonomy, and a
• address intervention through interdisciplinary host of challenges of communication access that we
approaches, combining the best areas of continue to discuss throughout this book. Thus, these
expertise to address an individual’s needs are seen as vital areas for clinical aphasiologists to
(Arthanat et al., 2004; Centers for Disease focus on in assessment and treatment.
Control and Prevention, 2020; Leach et al., 2010). In addition to the attributes of the ICF in terms
of its relevance to all rehabilitation professionals and
scholars, listed earlier, the ICF helps clinical aphasi-
How Is the WHO ICF Specifically Relevant to ologists to
People With Neurogenic Language Disorders?
• be mindful that our aim as clinicians is
not simply to assess and treat language
The WHO framework represents an important impairments but to address all aspects
departure from the traditional classification of indi- of communication that could affect an
viduals with aphasia based on neuroanatomical individual’s life;
models, which are focused largely on impairments • commit to enhancing the fullest life
of body structure and function. For example, in a participation in the people we serve;
typical neuroanatomical model, such as Darley’s • recognize that we must be humble about
(1982), one might consider an individual’s auditory what we think we know, especially given
comprehension or word-finding problems to be asso- the diverse cultures, ethnicities, languages,
ciated with identified lesions in Brodmann’s areas sexual orientations, gender identities, ages,
44, 45, and/or 22. These impairment-level areas of relationships, religious beliefs, spiritual
deficit would be identified and their severity quan- practices, socioeconomic conditions, interests,
tified during assessment. Treatment goals would be educational and employment backgrounds,
established with an emphasis on making improve- personal life stories, and personalities that
ments in those deficient areas. Treatment approaches are represented by each individual with
would be selected based on their likelihood of neurogenic communication disorders and by
improving abilities that are typically associated with each person who cares about them;
the impaired structures and their associated areas • commit to lifelong learning so that we may
of communication function. Treatment outcomes continue to improve our responsiveness as
would be monitored in terms of measures of audi- clinicians to the myriad factors that affect
tory comprehension or word finding. health and well-being;
In contrast, using the ICF framework, we focus • acknowledge that we each have biases (only
on aphasia as a contextualized life-affecting condi- some of which we are aware) and that our
tion, and we emphasize resources and compensa- own experiences and beliefs affect our work;
tory and adaptive services for full life participation. • tune into how people with language
We may work on deficits at the level of body struc- impairments prioritize their own life
ture and function, but we do so with an eye toward participation goals;
affecting activity limitations through such work. We • consider the social exclusion related to
may also work on activity limitations directly. communication disability as a fundamental
Embracing the ICF framework affects assess- area of need in intervention;
ment in that we must focus on learning about many • consider social inclusion in the contexts in
factors other than an individuals’ cognitive and com- which a specific person wants and needs to
municative deficits per se. It affects treatment in that communicate as fundamental to improving
all of our work to help improve communication abil- engagement in daily life activities;
ities is geared toward greater life participation and • appreciate that family members and friends
quality of life. Limitations in quality of life commonly may be supportive as well as obstructive with
associated with neurogenic language disorders regard to communication activities and social
include reduced quality of personal relationships and inclusion; and
5. The WHO ICF, Human Rights Perspectives, and Life Participation Approaches   55

• consider how concomitant health challenges et al., 2011). Clinical aphasiologists are challenged to
affect communication (Hallowell, 2021; continue to assess the validity and practicality of the
Hallowell & Chapey, 2008a; Hopper, 2007; framework in their work as catalysts for communi-
Howe, 2008; Kagan, 2011; Kagan & Simmons- cation enhancement in people with acquired neuro-
Mackie, 2007; Mathisen & Threats, 2018; genic language disorders.
O’Halloran & Larkins, 2008; Power et al., 2011; Given that the ICF is often used to consider
Quintas et al., 2012; Threats, 2010a, 2010b; an individual’s status at a given point in time, it
Worrall et al., 2011). is important to keep in mind the chronic nature of
most acquired neurogenic communication disor-
Challenges we face in applying the ICF may ders. Addressing evolving needs and goals related
include the fact that health care contexts vary widely to functioning/disability and life participation over
across the globe, the lack of explicit and objective time is essential. Addressing the environmental fac-
means of characterizing the subjective dimension of tors and social determinants of health that influence
functioning and disability, and the complexity and those needs and the ability to pursue those goals is
challenges with reliability in ICF coding applied to vital to our responsibility as clinicians.
people with a variety of neurogenic language dis- In the upcoming chapters, we continue to
orders (Boles, 2004; Laxe et al., 2011; Power et al., explore ways of taking into account the WHO ICF
2011; Salter et al., 2011; Ueda & Okawa, 2003; Worrall in our varied roles as aphasiologists.

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter person’s status according to each component
that are new to you or that you have not yet of the WHO ICF.
mastered. 10. Why is knowledge of the WHO ICF
2. Define the terms impairment, disability, and especially important in clinical practice
handicap as they pertain to the ICIDH. with people who have acquired neurogenic
3. Define the terms activity limitation and communication disorders?
participation restriction as they pertain to the 11. What is the relationship between the WHO
ICIDH-2. and the UN?
4. Explain why the terms activity limitation and 12. How is the consideration of global human
participation restriction were replaced in the rights relevant to consideration of acquired
ICIDH-2. neurogenic communication disorders?
5. In your opinion, was there a loss of 13. How does the abstractness of
meaningfulness or clarity by making the communication make SLPs’ work both
shift in the major categories of disability in challenging and important in promoting
the ICIDH-2? Why or why not? communication as a basic human right?
6. Define the terms now used in the ICF. 14. In your own mission to become an excellent
7. Why do you think the WHO changed its clinician, what specific pledge would you
classification system from the ICIDH-2 to the be willing to make about integrating ICF
ICF? principles into your clinical practice?
8. In this chapter, you read that the ICF enables 15. How should research scholars integrate values
us to classify health conditions, not people. associated with the ICF in their research?
Why is that distinction important? 16. What do you see as limitations of the ICF?
9. Imagine a hypothetical case of a person
www
with aphasia due to stroke. Give illustrative Please see the companion website for supplemen-
examples of the various aspects of that tal material and activities.
CHAPTER
6
Etiologies of Acquired
Neurogenic Language Disorders

The most common etiologies, or causes, of neuro- 20. What is neurodegenerative disease?
genic communication disorders are stroke, traumatic 21. What is dementia?
brain injury (TBI), bacterial and viral infections, neo- 22. What is mild cognitive impairment?
plasm (or tumors), toxemia, diabetes and other met- 23. What is primary progressive aphasia (PPA)?
abolic disorders, and neurodegenerative diseases 24. What are some special challenges in identifying
(including dementia). After reading and reflecting on etiologies of cognitive-linguistic disorders?
the content in this chapter, you will ideally be able to
answer, in your own words, the following queries:
What Is a Stroke?
1. What is a stroke?
2. What are stroke risk factors, and what causes
stroke? Stroke is defined as a temporary or permanent dis-
3. What are the physiological effects of stroke? ruption in blood supply to the brain. Stroke is the
4. How crucial is timing for medical treatment most common cause of primary neurogenic speech
after a stroke? and language disorders. It is also the leading cause
5. How is the sudden onset of stroke relevant to of death, after heart attack, globally (WHO, 2020).
supporting patients and families? The term cerebrovascular accident (CVA), synony-
6. What is a transient ischemic attack (TIA)? mous with stroke, has fallen out of favor in recent
7. What is hypoperfusion? decades. Many discourage its use because the word
8. What can be done to prevent stroke? accident is suggestive that strokes are caused by hap-
9. What is TBI? penstance rather than being associated with known
10. What are blast injuries? risk factors, many of which are modifiable (Finger et
11. What are concussion and mild TBI? al., 2010). The term brain attack is commonly used
12. What can be done to prevent TBI? in public education campaigns to draw parallels
13. What are bacteria and viruses? between lifestyle risks associated with stroke and
14. What other types of infections affect cortical heart attack.
function? There are two types of stroke: occlusive and
15. What is neoplasm? hemorrhagic. Occlusive strokes are far more com-
16. What is toxemia? mon; only about 15% to 20% of strokes are hemor-
17. What are diabetes mellitus and diabetic rhagic. Occlusive strokes entail a blockage of all or a
encephalopathy? portion of an artery, while hemorrhagic strokes entail
18. What is metabolic syndrome? leakage of blood from the arteries. Occlusive strokes
19. What other metabolic disorders cause are also sometimes called ischemic strokes, the term
encephalopathy? ischemic referring to a restriction in blood supply.

57
58  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

There are two types of occlusive stroke: thrombotic ing part and using cauterization or plastic sealants
and embolic. A thrombotic stroke occurs when an to restore the integrity of the arterial wall. Unfortu-
arterial blockage has accumulated in the same area nately, aneurysms tend to go unnoticed until they
of an artery where the blockage eventually occurs. burst, causing a hemorrhagic stroke. This is most
The clot that blocks the artery in a thrombotic stroke likely in cases of hypertension, or high blood pres-
is called a thrombus. The plural form of the word sure, due to greater force against the arterial walls.
is thromboses. Most occlusive strokes are thrombotic. See Figure 6–1 for a schematic illustration of an
An embolic stroke entails a blockage caused by embolism, thrombus, and ruptured aneurysm.
matter (typically a blood clot or a piece of athero- An arteriovenous malformation (AVM) is an
sclerotic plaque) that travels from elsewhere in the atypically developed artery or vein, usually arising
bloodstream to the point where it eventually blocks during embryonic or fetal development. If an artery
an artery. The arterial blockage in an embolic stroke is particularly twisted or tangled, blood flow may be
is called an embolism. The plural form of the word restricted, making it more susceptible to changes in
is emboli. blood pressure and increasing the chance of rupture.
A hemorrhagic stroke occurs when a blood ves- The condition is worsened by the fact that AVMs
sel ruptures. This most commonly occurs when there lack sufficient capillary networks, systems of tiny
is an aneurysm or an arteriovenous malformation interconnecting vessels, that otherwise help regulate
that bursts. An aneurysm is a bulging out at a weak- blood flow. Such malformations may not ever cause
ened spot along an arterial wall. When the arterial noticeable symptoms. Others may remain unprob-
wall bulges further, it is increasingly stretched and lematic for years until a hemorrhage occurs. Dr. Jill
weakened. If detected and surgically accessible, an Bolte Taylor, who wrote the book My Stroke of Insight
aneurysm may be operated on by clipping the bulg- (2006), had her stroke because of an AVM and has

A B C
Figure 6–1. Embolism, thrombus, and ruptured aneurysm. Each image set shows the progression from top to
bottom. A. The development of a thrombus; atherosclerotic plaque builds up over time until there is a complete
blockage. B. An embolus; a particle of atherosclerotic plaque or a blood clot travels from elsewhere in the blood-
stream and lodges in an artery, blocking the flow of blood from that point onward. C. An aneurysm leading to
a hemorrhage; the external wall of the artery progressively balloons outward until it bursts. Image credit: Taylor
Reeves. A full-color version of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.
6. Etiologies of Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders   59

since illuminated much of the world through her modifiable. Sickle cell anemia, a hereditary abnor-
talks and writing about her own experience of hav- mality in the cellular structure of hemoglobin that
ing a stroke. AVMs are relatively rare, accounting for occurs in some people of African, Mediterranean,
only about 2% of hemorrhagic strokes, but, as Taylor Asian Indian, Saudi Arabian, Caribbean, and South
highlights, “it is the most common form of stroke and Central American ancestry, is an example of a
that strikes people during their prime years of life genetic risk factor. So are hematological pathologies,
(ages 25–45)” (p. 25). such as hemophilia, which prevents the blood from
When a hemorrhagic stroke occurs within the coagulating. Having Type I diabetes (described later
brain, it not only disrupts blood supply to the areas in this chapter) significantly increases one’s risk of
of the brain targeted by the ruptured artery but also stroke; this may be associated with concomitant high
results in leakage of blood into the brain. As blood levels of triglycerides and low levels of high-density
accumulates, it creates compression against sur- lipoprotein (the lay term for which is “good” cho-
rounding brain tissue, further affecting blood flow as lesterol). In terms of age, one’s chance of having a
well as structural alignment and dynamic function- stroke doubles with each decade after age 55 years.
ing of associated functional areas. The accumulation In recent years, adjustments to modifiable fac-
of blood due to hemorrhage is called a hematoma. tors have been shown to influence the role of factors
When leakage occurs within the brain tissue, it considered to be nonmodifiable. As such, genetic
is called an intracerebral or intraparenchymal hem- factors that interact with environmental factors may
orrhage, and an intracerebral hematoma is likely to actually be more modifiable than has traditionally
form. When it occurs on the surface of the brain, been appreciated. Also, structural abnormalities in
between the pia and arachnoid mater, it is called the blood supply system (as in AVM and aneurysms)
a subarachnoid hemorrhage, and a subarachnoid may sometimes be treated surgically to reduce risks.
hematoma is likely to form. A hematoma may also The primary cause of ischemic strokes is ath-
form between the arachnoid layer and the dura erosclerosis (also called arteriosclerosis), which is
mater (the outermost layer of tissue surrounding the a buildup of lipids (fatty acids and cholesterol) and
brain), forming a subdural hematoma; this is more cellular debris within the arteries. Atherosclerotic
likely to happen in cases of TBI than in stroke. plaque accumulates in the arterial walls over time,
Intracerebral hemorrhages may have dramatic leading to narrower channels through which blood
impacts on cortical and subcortical areas at once, often may flow. As the plaque builds up, it also causes a
resulting in loss of consciousness and basic nervous loss of arterial elasticity, which diminishes the abil-
system functions. Emergency surgical intervention to ity of the arterial walls to adapt to changes in blood
stop the flow of blood is often implemented, although pressure (hence the layperson’s term “hardening
with a clear eye toward avoiding further damage to of the arteries”). Narrower channels and reduced
vital brain tissue. Decisions about pharmacological elasticity are a horrid combination, especially with
treatments to reduce edema are balanced with assess- surges in blood pressure. Given that atherosclerotic
ing possible heightened risks for further leakage. plaque builds up over long periods of time, without
consistent attention to ensuring a healthy lifestyle,
the buildup continues to get worse as people age.
What Are Stroke Risk Factors, There are four primary lifestyle-related conditions
that lead to atherosclerosis: poor diet, lack of exer-
and What Causes Stroke?
cise, high stress, and smoking. These are addressed
further as we consider ways to prevent stroke.
Causes of and risk factors for stroke are heteroge- In many cases, it is difficult to discern which
nous. Risk factors may be considered modifiable or factors causing stroke in any one individual are
nonmodifiable. For example, smoking, poor diet, related to genetic predisposition versus lifestyle.
physical inactivity, and exposure to pollution are This is because it is not always possible to sort out
modifiable, while genetic factors (including some sociocultural factors associated with diet, exercise,
hereditary disorders) and age are considered non- smoking, socioeconomic status, and access to health
60  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

care from one’s biological predisposition to stroke.


Consider, for example, that women have a greater
How Crucial Is Timing for Medical
predisposition to stroke than men. It is difficult to Treatment After a Stroke?
know whether this is associated more with lifestyle
and pharmaceutical patterns (e.g., use of oral contra- Interruption of the blood supply to the brain for even
ceptives and hormone therapies) versus genetic ones. a few seconds (8–16) may result in loss of conscious-
ness. After only 20 to 25 seconds, electrical activity
in neurons typically supplied by the vessel that has
been restricted may cease. After only 3 to 5 min-
What Are the Physiological Effects of Stroke?
utes of restricted blood supply, irreversible damage
may occur. If a person who has an occlusive stroke
Immediately following a stroke and for a few days can get to a hospital quickly, there is a great chance
to a few months afterward, there tends to be edema that the amount of permanent damage to the brain
(swelling) in the surrounding area of the brain. This can be minimized. This is because a thrombolytic
may increase intracranial pressure and lead to fur- (“clot-busting”) drug can be administered to loosen
ther complications because of its effects on other the blockage and increase the likelihood of restoring
brain structures. Stroke also causes electrochemi- blood flow to the associated cortical areas. The most
cal changes in the brain, including neurochemical common thrombolytic treatment is tissue plasmin-
surges and changes in the generation and uptake of ogen activator, or tPA.
neurotransmitters, metabolic function affecting glu- The likelihood of tPA’s effectiveness at restoring
cose and oxygen sufficiency, and removal of toxins blood flow lessens over time, as the area of blood
through the venous sinus system. Additionally, due supply loss expands, so it is vital that it be admin-
to disruptions of neuronal pathways, functions asso- istered within a few hours after an occlusive stroke.
ciated with brain structures that are remote from the Although early studies suggested that tPA be admin-
area of damage often become impaired. This phe- istered within 3 hours, more recent studies have
nomenon is called diaschisis. All of these temporary suggested that it may still be effective within 3 to
and sometimes dramatic changes make it difficult 4.5 hours. For a person who has had a hemorrhagic
to tell what the long-term impacts of a stroke will stroke, administering any treatment that thins the
be. Changes in communication ability, cognitive sta- blood or reduces coagulation may actually do much
tus, behavior, and virtually every aspect of function- more harm. Thus, it is essential to obtain a brain scan
ing typically do not remain as dramatic as they are of the stroke survivor as quickly as possible to rule
immediately following a stroke. out hemorrhage prior to administering such agents.
The long-term impact of reduced oxygen and In some cases, clots may be removed surgically.
glucose supply to the brain is neural tissue necrosis Knowing the profound impact of timing in
(death). An area of dead tissue is called an infarct or responding to the occurrence of a stroke, many pro-
infarction. The larger the area of blood supply dis- fessionals working in medical contexts are saddened
rupted, the larger is the infarct. The margin neural to hear story after story of people who experience
tissue surrounding an infarct is called the ischemic stroke symptoms yet ignore them, take a great deal
penumbra. The penumbra consists of living tis- of time considering how to respond, or wait to see
sue but it is hypoperfused, meaning that its blood if symptoms subside before seeking help. Imagine
supply is reduced. It is logical that its blood supply what happens when a person experiences blurry or
would be reduced given the blood supply disruption tunnel vision, or weakness on one side of the body.
and associated necrotic tissue adjacent to it. Thus, it She may decide to lie down and rest for a few hours
is at risk of ischemic necrosis itself. This is one of the to see if it subsides. Perhaps she will tell her hus-
reasons that the earliest possible treatment through band that she does not feel well and he will get her
clot-busting drugs for nonhemorrhagic strokes is some over-the-counter pain relievers. Perhaps she
essential. Even if there is an area of infarct, the size will call her adult son living in another state, and
of the infarct may be limited by pharmacologically he will reassure her and tell her not to worry, that it
restoring blood flow to the penumbra (reperfusion). will likely pass. Perhaps she will wait another day
6. Etiologies of Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders   61

to see how things go. And all the while her brain but then be dissolved, or a change in blood pressure
is incurring greater and greater oxygen and glucose may release a point of arterial blockage. TIAs often
deprivation, causing greater likelihood of permanent occur before a full-blown stroke and thus are import-
damage and a greater probability that the damaged ant warning signs that should be taken seriously so
area will be enlarged. Keep in mind that anyone who that appropriate preventive measures can be put into
has had a prior stroke or transient ischemic attack place, such as lifestyle changes and pharmacological
is at greater risk (compared to those who have not intervention. A common fallacy associated with TIA
had a stroke) of having another. Professionals who is that it will not cause permanent damage. This is
work with stroke survivors thus have a higher like- not necessarily the case. Many people have clear
lihood than others of being with someone who has noticeable challenges with motor, sensory, cognitive,
a repeat stroke; it is important for clinicians to be or linguistic functions following TIA. The warning
aware of warning signs so that they may act quickly signs for TIA are the same as those for stroke.
to seek appropriate help. Also, it is important that
all members of a rehabilitation team educate stroke
survivors, caregivers, and significant others about
What Is Hypoperfusion?
stroke warning signs.

Hypoperfusion is an insufficiency in blood sup-


How Is the Sudden Onset of Stroke Relevant ply to the brain. It is typically caused by heart
problems or hemorrhaging elsewhere in the body,
to Supporting Patients and Families?
such that blood flow to the brain is limited. Unlike
strokes, which tend to occur in main arteries or their
Consider how the suddenness of changes in the direct branches, hypoperfusion tends to affect the
brain can lead to shocking and emotionally devas- watershed areas of the brain that are supplied by
tating changes in abilities immediately after stroke. smaller arterial extensions. Although not a stroke
Consider, too, how the very word stroke has high or TIA per se, hypoperfusion can cause brain tissue
visceral impact on most people. Hearing the word death if blood flow is not resumed within a critical
may evoke fear and sometimes images of worst- time period.
case scenarios among people who have just had a
stroke and the people who love them. When there
is a recent onset of stroke, it is important that clini-
What Can Be Done to Prevent Stroke?
cians be sensitive to the degree of shock, fear, and
disbelief the stroke survivor may have and help
ease that shock by providing much-needed infor- A focus on prevention should be primarily focused
mation and counseling. This is one of many reasons on modifiable factors, with pharmacological and
why it is essential for the clinical aphasiologist to surgical intervention as needed. Most strategies
have strong counseling skills, a topic we address in for reducing stroke risk are important strategies for
Chapter 27. healthy living in general. See Box 6–1 for means of
preventing or reducing the risk of stroke. Note that
almost all of the actions we may take to prevent
stroke are the same actions we should take to reduce
What Is a Transient Ischemic Attack?
our risk of heart disease, cancer, and diabetes. Note,
too, that modifying one risk factor may lessen other
A transient ischemic attack (TIA) is a temporary risk factors. For example, engaging in physical activ-
blockage of the blood supply to any area of the brain. ity may help reduce blood pressure, glucose levels,
The lay term for a TIA is a mini-stroke. Most TIAs last and body weight and elevate mental well-being.
less than 30 minutes, although they are still consid- Diet modifications may reduce depression, diabetes,
ered TIAs if they last up to 24 hours. A thrombus or and overall glucose regulation. Reducing salt intake
embolus may cause sudden blood flow disruption is associated with decreased hypertension.
62  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Box
6–1 Preventing or Reducing the Risk of Stroke by Modifying Risk Factors

• If you smoke, stop smoking. • The previous steps tend to help with all of
• If you drink alcohol, drink in moderation. these. In some cases, surgical procedures
• Exercise regularly. may be recommended; a common one
• Keep weight to an ideal range for your height is endarterectomy, the removal of
and body type. atherosclerotic plaque from arterial walls,
• Avoid dietary fat (although some forms of fat most often the carotid artery. Another is
may actually lower risks). angioplasty, which involves inserting a
• Avoid sugar, especially refined sugar. catheter into the arteries and using
• Attend to your mental well-being. a balloon-like tip to expand the arterial
• Seek social support. Spend time with walls.
people who support you and whom you • In addition to immediate treatment
support. to restore blood flow, longer-term
• If your mood is low or if you experience a pharmacological treatments are often
life situation that feels overwhelming, seek recommended. Examples are hypertension
professional help. medications to reduce high blood pressure,
• Reduce stress. anticoagulants to reduce blood clotting,
• Numerous books, podcasts, webinars, antiplatelets to reduce the degree of
smartphone apps, courses, and therapies adhesion of blood by-products to the
are available to foster techniques such as arterial walls, and vasodilators to reduce
meditation, relaxation, gratitude, conflict arterial constriction.
resolution, anger management, and • Reduce risk of infections that may lead
mindfulness practices. to cerebrovascular disease, especially
• Engage in activities that are important and by immunization, social distancing,
meaningful to you. handwashing, and wearing a mask.
• A healthy diet and exercise also help with • Know the stroke warning signs and seek
stress reduction. immediate medical attention when one
• Control cholesterol, high blood pressure, high occurs.
blood coagulation levels, and diabetes.

Several stroke risk scoring systems have been ing the risks, such as living in environments with
proposed. The Framingham Stroke Risk Profile (Wolf high exposure to pollutants, conflict, and violence,
et al., 1991) is one that has been repeatedly updated. and those where healthy foods and health care are
It combines predictors based on sex, age, smoking, inaccessible or unaffordable. In such cases, we
cholesterol, blood pressure, glucose regulation, and might call such risk factors potentially modifiable
disease factors. Others include additional metrics, and recognize the tremendous need to address pop-
such as depression, disability, and marital status. ulation-based health, focusing on the social deter-
A quick Internet search will lead you to online stroke minants of health that relate to stroke (and other
risk assessments you may take yourself. Given that disabling conditions) within local communities as
people at risk for stroke are also at risk of other car- well as in geographic regions and countries (O’Don-
diovascular events, risk scores that take into account nell et al., 2010). Although the top 10 potentially
cardiac events and conditions may be more useful. modifiable risk factors have been found to be com-
Note that many of the modifiable risk factors mon across 90% of the world, regardless of age, sex,
for stroke do not seem so modifiable to those bear- and ethnic group, there are important regional vari-
6. Etiologies of Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders   63

ations in the frequency, intensity, and combinations urinary tract infection, COVID-19, flu, and colds).
of these factors (O’Donnell et al., 2016). This further There is a need for more research in this area, and
highlights the need for region-specific and global for research that addresses design shortcomings in
prevention programs. existing studies (Guirard et al., 2010; Law, 2021). If
the import of certain triggers can be validated, then
avoiding those triggers may be an important aspect
Attending to Stroke Triggers of stroke prevention (a topic that will also require
further study).
Recent research efforts have shown that, in addition The warning signs for stroke are summarized
to risk factors, exposure to certain stroke triggers in Box 6–3. The American Stroke Association (n.d.),
may precipitate stroke. These are momentary con- in a public health campaign to help raise awareness
ditions that might occur months, weeks, days, or of warning signs for stroke, has summarized these
hours prior to a stroke. Boehme et al. (2017) suggest with the acronym FAST, to which many others have
that we consider risk factors as helping answer the added the additional BE, as follows (see Figure 6–2):
question “Why me?” and triggers as helping answer
the question “Why now?” Some of the main triggers • B: balance problems
purported are listed in Box 6–2. • E: eye/vision problems
Much of the research on potential triggers has • F: face drooping
been focused on alcohol use and infections (such as • A: arm weakness
• S: speech difficulty
• T: time to call 9-1-1
Box
6–2 Possible Stroke Triggers
Box
Excessive alcohol intake within the past day 6–3 Stroke Warning Signs
or week
Warning signs for stroke include sudden
Infection (e.g., COVID-19, cold, flu, urinary
onset of
tract infection) within the last week or month
Excessive eating • Numbness of face, arm, or leg, especially
on one side of the body
Positive emotions • Weakness on one side or both sides of the
Negative emotions, psychological distress, body
anger • Asymmetric muscle control
• Loss of balance, falling
Sudden posture change, especially when • Vertigo
startled within the previous 2 hours • Confusion
Birthday • Trouble speaking
• Sudden word-finding difficulty
Drug use • Comprehension difficulty
Anesthesia • Slurred speech
• Dizziness
Surgery • Blurred vision
Physical exertion • Double vision
• Trouble swallowing
Sources: Guirard et al., 2010; Hallowell, Enderby, • Headache
et al., 2021; Koton et al., 2004; Nagre, 2018; Nobleza • Nausea or vomiting
et al., 2021; Sebastian et al., 2019; Vogrig et al., 2021. • Blacking out, loss of consciousness
64  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Figure 6–2. Stroke warning signs. Source: Shutterstock. A full-color version of this figure can be
found in the Color Insert.

(e.g., when a passenger’s head hits the windshield


What Is TBI? during a car accident) or the object is in motion and
is suddenly stopped by the head (e.g., when a brick
A TBI occurs when a sudden trauma causes dam- falls from a building and lands on a person’s head).
age to the brain. In all age groups and worldwide, An illustration of an acceleration-deceleration
males tend to have about 50% greater likelihood of injury is shown in Figure 6–3. In this case, a force
sustaining a TBI than do females, a fact that is related hitting the head from behind causes the head to
to males’ general predisposition toward dangerous accelerate toward a brick wall, and the wall causes
and thrill-seeking activities. The highest incidence the sudden deceleration. CHIs may be translational
of TBIs occurs under the age of 4 years, between the or rotational. In a translational injury, also called
ages of 14 and 25 years, and over the age of 65 years. a direct injury, the object-head contact is at a rela-
TBIs are generally classified as open- or closed-head tively perpendicular angle to one of the main axes
injuries, depending on whether the skull is fractured of the head, causing the brain to rebound against the
as a result of trauma to the head. In a closed-head side of the skull opposite the site of contact. Injuries
injury (CHI), the head suddenly hits an object, or an to the brain at the point of contact are called coup
object hits the head, without breaking through the injuries. Injuries caused by the brain’s contact with
skull. CHIs are sometimes referred to as accelera- the skull at the opposite side of the skull are called
tion-deceleration injuries. This is because the head contrecoup injuries. The locations of coup and con-
is in motion and is suddenly stopped on contact trecoup injuries are illustrated in Figure 6–3.
6. Etiologies of Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders   65

Figure 6–3. Top and side views of acceleration-deceleration injury. The arrow shows the direction of a blow
to the head. In the left-most image (A) of the top and side views, the arrow indicates the direction of the accelerat-
ing force. In the middle views (B), the head hits the wall and the brain is thrust in the direction of the accelerating
force. Where the brain is injured at the point of contact is the coup injury. In the right-most views (C), the brain
rebounds from the front of the skull to the back of the skull. Where the brain hits the back of the skull is where
the contrecoup injury occurs. Image credit: Taylor Reeves. A full-color version of this figure can be found in the
Color Insert.

In rotational injuries, also called angular inju- or more specific areas of the brain) but also diffuse
ries, the contact of an object with the head creates (involving multiple areas of the brain at once). One
more of a spinning motion of the head, causing the reason for diffuse damage is widespread severing or
brain to rotate in relation to the skull and often to shearing of axons through a process called diffuse
hit against multiple skull areas. In Figure 6–4, the (or traumatic) axonal injury, common in all forms of
smaller arrows represent the direction of an object acceleration-deceleration trauma to the head. Dif-
hitting the person’s head. The curved arrow shows fuse damage may also be due to space-occupying
the twisting motion of the head in response to a rota- and pressure-raising effects of edema and subdural
tional blow. Note the coup injury highlighted in the and intracerebral hematomas that result from the
rotational blow and the coup and contrecoup inju- trauma. These, in turn, may lead to secondary dam-
ries highlighted in the direct blow image. age comprising a combination of interrupted blood
An open-head injury (OHI) involves breakage supply, neurochemical changes (such as pathological
or penetration of the skull. Examples are falls that surges of certain neurotransmitters), and nerve cell
lead to skull fracture, gunshot wounds to the head, death. Of course, further complications may arise
and lacerations by sharp objects, such as a knife, from hemorrhages. Much of the diffuse damage may
spear, or ax. Whether or not the skull is fractured, not be localizable through neuroimaging, making
the consequences of TBI can be extremely complex diagnosis of the neuropathology underlying some
because injuries are not only focal (confined to one symptoms especially challenging.
66  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

A B
Figure 6–4. Direct and translational blows to the head. The smaller arrows represent the direc-
tion of the force of an object hitting the head. A. A direct blow to the head. The highlighted portion
on the left side of the brain indicates the site of a coup injury, while the highlighted portion on
the right shows the site of a contrecoup injury. B. A rotational blow to the head. The larger arrow
represents the twisting motion of the head and neck in response to the angular force against the
head. Image credit: Taylor Reeves. A full-color version of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.

Factors affecting the extent of damage due to the


What Are Blast Injuries? blast include peak pressure, duration, distance from
the explosion, and whether it was in open air or in
Blast injuries result from rapid phases of over- and a confined space (Howe, 2009; Wallace, 2006). There
under-pressurization of air compared to normal are four broad categories of blast injuries: primary,
atmospheric pressure. They are most frequently secondary, tertiary, and quaternary (for overviews,
associated with exposure to war-related explosives. see BrainLine, 2015; DePalma et al., 2005; DePalma &
Due to an increasing frequency of blast attacks asso- Hoffman, 2018; Yamamoto et al., 2018). Primary blast
ciated with newly developed war technology, there injuries result from “wave-induced changes in atmo-
are more TBIs in current wars than in previous wars spheric pressure” (Wallace, 2006, p. 399). The brain,
throughout history (Arnold et al., 2004; Gondusky because it has air-fluid interfaces and tissues of dif-
& Reiter, 2005; Hoge et al., 2008; Jaffee et al., 2009; ferent densities, is especially vulnerable when con-
Langlois et al., 2006; Murray et al., 2005; Warden, nected components are stretched and sheared due
2006; Xydakis et al., 2005). Also, there are increas- to acceleration at different rates (Taber et al., 2006).
ing numbers of people surviving injuries that in the Shear and stress waves may cause direct injury (e.g.,
past would have killed them (Cernak & Noble-Hae- concussion, cortical edema, diffuse axonal injury),
usslein, 2010; Cifu et al., 2010; Mernoff & Correia, as well as gas emboli, leading to infarction (Ros-
2010; Rosenfeld & Ford, 2010; Vasterling et al., 2009). enfeld & Ford, 2010; Taber et al., 2006). Secondary
6. Etiologies of Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders   67

blast injuries are caused by objects set into motion the impact of a TBI until attempting to return to pro-
(e.g., flying debris, structural collapse). Tertiary blast fessional, educational, or military service. The onset
injuries are caused by an individual being set into of symptoms may occur months or even years post-
motion as a result of an explosion and then hitting injury, symptom severity may fluctuate, and some
a solid surface. Quaternary blast injuries are caused symptoms may be triggered by life events occurring
by blood loss from bodily injury or exposure to toxic long after the actual physical trauma (Hicks et al.,
gas associated with an explosion. 2010; Quinlan et al., 2010). Third, stigma-​associated
Most modern warfare casualties involve a com- TBI symptoms confound self-report data and thus
bination of all four blast injury categories (Elder & estimates of incidence.
Cristian, 2009; Gondusky & Reiter, 2005; Murray
et al., 2005; Taber et al., 2006). In humans, it is gener-
ally considered impossible to tease apart what part of
What Are Concussion and Mild TBI?
the injury is due to these different blast components,
although some clear delineations in effects have
been obtained through animal studies (Belanger A concussion is a “complex pathophysiological pro-
et al., 2009; Courtney & Courtney, 2009; Howe, 2009; cess affecting the brain, induced by biomechanical
Rosenfeld & Ford, 2010; Taber et al., 2006). The most forces” (McCrory et al., 2013, pp. 250–251). The term
common types of blast-induced (BI) TBI are diffuse concussion is often considered synonymous with mild
contusions, subdural hematomas and hemorrhages, TBI (or mTBI) (e.g., Department of Veterans Affairs,
and axonal injuries (Gondusky & Reiter, 2005; Hicks Department of Defense, 2009), although some pre-
et al., 2010; Murray et al., 2005). fer to consider it as a syndrome on the mildest end
Penetrating TBIs and fatal injuries to the body of the mTBI spectrum of severity (King et al., 2014,
are increasingly prevented by advanced body armor p. 452). There is no widely accepted threshold for
technology; however, it may be impossible to pre- when a blow to the head (or a blow to another part
vent the effects of blast injuries on the brain (Galar- of the body, which then transmits a force to the head)
neau et al., 2008; Okie, 2005). A majority of soldiers is to be labeled a concussion as opposed to an mTBI
who survive blast injuries are diagnosed with BI TBI (Harmon et al., 2013). Concussion and mTBI may be
(Keltner & Cooke, 2007), and increasing numbers are caused by an acceleration-deceleration injury, by the
surviving their injuries due to improved wound care head or body being violently shaken, or by exposure
in war zones. Continued exposure to bombs, rock- to blast. A concussion or mTBI may or may not be
et-propelled grenades, mortar rounds, and other accompanied by loss of consciousness.
types of heavy artillery leads to greater risk of life-​ mTBI is most commonly caused by sports inju-
affecting head injuries. ries, falls, and motor vehicle accidents. Symptoms
Estimates of the incidence of both BI and non- may include confusion, disorientation, headache,
blast-induced (NBI) TBI among injured military blurred vision, tinnitus, balance problems, leth-
service personnel are highly variable. For example, argy, sleeplessness, nausea, seizure activity, mood
research on BI TBI incidence associated with U.S. sol- changes, weakness and numbness of the extremities,
diers returning from Iraq and Afghanistan war zones agitation, attention problems, memory problems,
in the early 2000s ranges from 8% to 97%. There are speech and language deficits, and executive func-
three primary reasons for this variability. First, in tioning problems such as poor judgment, impulsiv-
war zones and following terror attacks, those with ity, and lack of inhibition of inappropriate words or
the most apparent life-threatening injuries are given behavior. There is tremendous variability in the inci-
medical assistance first. Most people with head dence of these symptoms within any individual, and
injuries are treated first for their most obvious con- recovery trajectories vary greatly across individuals
comitant physical injuries. Many with CHIs may be (Kontos & Collins, 2018).
unaware of a brain injury and may not even be exam- Although the terms concussion and mTBI are
ined for TBI (Belanger et al., 2009; Cherney et al., often considered to indicate that the consequences
2010; Langlois et al., 2006; Schneiderman et al., 2008; are only temporary, this is not always the case;
Warden, 2006). Second, many people do not realize a history of even one may cause lifelong problems
68  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

with cognitive, linguistic, sensory, and motor func- addition to achieving a host of other benefits to the
tions (McCrory et al., 2013). By far the most com- human race, reducing war and violence is vital to
mon lingering complaints are memory problems. reducing brain injuries.
A history of concussion or mTBI is also a known
risk factor for future onset of mild cognitive impair-
ment and dementia (Guskiewicz et al., 2005; Plass-
What Are Bacteria and Viruses?
man et al., 2000), conditions discussed later in
this chapter.
Bacterial infections and viruses are both microscopic
organisms that may cause inflammation in the brain.
Bacteria are single celled and thrive in many envi-
What Can Be Done to Prevent TBI?
ronments. Most bacteria are harmless; many, such
as those involved in digestion, are essential to good
Falls are the most common cause of TBI. In the United health. Viruses, which are much smaller than bacte-
States, these tend to be greatest in children under ria, are typically harmful and require a host to sur-
4 years of age and in adults over age 75 years. A large vive. They are invasive, taking over the host’s cells to
proportion of falls could be prevented by ensuring genetically replicate themselves. Bacterial and viral
appropriate safety monitoring and accommodations infections may cause neurogenic communication
for those two age groups and by reducing high-risk disorders when they affect the brain. Infections that
behaviors across age groups. Motor vehicle-related affect the cortex are called encephalopathies. Both
TBIs are the second most common cause across age types of infection may cause meningitis, an inflam-
groups. Wearing of seat belts, obeying traffic rules, mation of the meninges surrounding the brain (also
driving at moderate speeds, and ensuring cogni- called meningoencephalitis when it is caused by an
tive, visual, and motor abilities for safe driving are infection). The space-occupying nature of the inflam-
all effective means of reducing TBI risk (Baldwin mation may obstruct blood flow as well as healthy
et al., 2016). connections in the brain’s complex circuitry. Addi-
Sports-related injuries are also common world- tionally, infectious processes in meningitis tend to
wide. The wearing of helmets during contact sports alter the blood-brain barrier, thus exposing sensitive
(e.g., rugby, football, soccer, boxing) and in activities brain tissue to toxicity. When functioning properly,
with high fall risk has been shown to significantly the blood-brain barrier prevents toxic substances
reduce the incidence, severity, and within-individ- from entering brain tissue and provides selective
ual frequency of mild to severe head injuries. Helmet permeability for certain substances (such as sugar
use has been shown to dramatically affect incidence and alcohol) to pass.
and severity of TBI in motorcycle riders, bicycle rid- Antibiotics can be administered to treat some
ers, skiers, snowboarders, all-terrain vehicle users, harmful bacteria but not to treat viruses. Some
and skateboarders, among others (Bowman et al., viruses (e.g., polio, chickenpox, COVID-19, some
2009; Ganti et al., 2013; Giza et al., 2013; Sulheim forms of hepatitis) can be largely prevented with
et al., 2006; Weiss et al., 2010). vaccines. Antiviral medications are sometimes used
Awareness of or attention to basic aspects of to treat herpes simplex and HIV/AIDS, two viruses
head injury prevention is lacking worldwide, espe- that tend to affect brain function.
cially in the arena of sports and motorcycling, in HIV/AIDS stands for human immunodefi-
which the trade-off of risk versus a sense of thrill, ciency virus/acquired immunodeficiency syn-
avoidance of inconvenience, and social stigma asso- drome. HIV is a basic virus that targets the human
ciated with using proper head protection are often immune system. By substantially invading immune
swayed in a dangerous direction. There is a great cells, it causes AIDS. According to the Centers
need worldwide for enhanced public education, for Disease Control and Prevention (2014), AIDS
advocacy, and policy changes in the arena of head involves 1 of 12 opportunistic infections if there is
injury prevention (see Kontos & Collins, 2018). Of no other detectable cause of cellular dysfunction in
course, there is a great need for world peace, too. In the immune system. Although infections in most
6. Etiologies of Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders   69

minority-world counties have been declining since a rare, rapidly progressing condition that causes
the mid-1980s, progress has not continued at a con- rapid onset of dementia in addition to neuromotor
sistent rate. HIV prevention and treatment are not and visual problems; people who get it typically die
reaching many who would most benefit from them, within a year of onset.
especially men who have sex with men, transgen-
der persons, and racial and ethnic minority groups
already experiencing disproportionately poor access
What Is Neoplasm?
to health care (CDC, 2021).
The primary reason that HIV/AIDS is relevant
here is that neurogenic language disorders may Neoplasm (literally “new growth”), or tumors, when
arise from both primary and secondary infections. they occur within the brain, may cause neurogenic
An example of a primary infection is AIDS demen- speech and language disorders. They may do this by
tia complex. Examples of secondary infections are directly impinging upon functional communication
meningeo-encephalitis or toxoplasmosis. Secondary areas in the brain or on vital pathways between such
infections are also called opportunistic infections areas. Symptoms tend to develop slowly as the brain
because viruses (and/or bacteria) selectively take adapts to the gradual increase in pressure on sur-
advantage of compromised immune systems. Infec- rounding areas. Sometimes symptoms go unnoticed
tions that may not put healthy people at risk under until significant tumor growth has occurred. Symp-
normal circumstances may be particularly hazard- toms tend to be highly variable, depending on the
ous for people with immunodeficiencies. location, size, and type of tumor. Common symp-
Neurogenic language disorders caused by toms are progressive; they may include loss of vision
HIV/AIDS may be associated with three general eti- or visual field disturbances, memory and attention
ologies: neoplasm (e.g., due to some form of lym- problems, confusion, nausea, and seizure activity.
phoma), systemic disorders (e.g., metabolic and Although tumors have been reported to cause
nutritional problems such as anemia, hypoglycemia, aphasia, there is little research consistently docu-
and vitamin B12 deficiency), and stroke. Of course, menting their effects on language abilities. Davie et
as any etiology affecting multiple interconnected al. (2009) report that 30% to 50% of people with pri-
neural functions, people with HIV/AIDS are likely to mary brain tumors have aphasia and that the most
have far more complications than just those involv- common associated symptom is anomia. Paratz
ing cognitive and communication abilities. Given (2011) reports that language deficits are highly vari-
that the array of disorders that may accompany able and tend to be less severe than for poststroke
HIV/AIDS is wide and highly variable, it is impossi- aphasia. Duffau (2005) suggests that the slow growth
ble to predict the cognitive, linguistic, social, mental, of many brain tumors enables greater gradual reor-
and quality-of-life status of any individual if all one ganization of brain functions associated with affected
knows is that they have a diagnosis of HIV/AIDS. structures even in adults. This may be a key reason
that long-term impacts of tumors on language abil-
ities are less severe than those of stroke, even when
What Other Types of Infections similar areas of the brain are affected.
Tumors may be malignant (cancerous) or
Affect Cortical Function?
benign (noncancerous). The degree of malignancy
is indexed on a scale of I (benign) to IV (high grade
There are numerous additional infections that may and malignant). Primary tumors in the brain result
affect cognitive-linguistic abilities. These include the from uncontrolled growth of glial or meningeal cells.
family of prion disorders, also called spongiform Glial cell tumors are called gliomas and are the most
encephalopathies, because they cause brain tissue common form of brain tumor. One common form of
to look spongy. Prion disorders are neurodegenera- glial tumors is astrocytoma, a benign, slow-growing
tive conditions characterized by the aggregation of tumor. Another is glioblastoma multiforme, a
deformed protein fibers (amyloid fibrils) outside of malignant and fast-growing tumor. Meningioma
brain cells. An example is Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease, is a benign tumor that arises from the meninges. If
70  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

removed early enough such that it does not impinge cellular level. Exposure to substances that may be
on other structures, it may have no impact on corti- toxic to the nervous system is common, and myr-
cal functioning. Secondary or metastatic tumors are iad factors influence one’s ability to tolerate and
spreading tumors that typically arise from elsewhere get rid of such toxins. Examples of toxins that are
in the body and travel to the brain via the blood harmful to brain tissue are alcohol (especially when
supply or lymphatic system, most commonly subse- used excessively), recreational drugs such as meth-
quent to breast, lung, and skin cancers. amphetamines (“ecstasy”), and heavy metal (e.g.,
Diagnosis of brain tumor depends on biopsy, lead and mercury) poisoning. Many argue that cer-
the clinical examination of tissue removed from the tain dietary substances, such as refined sugars, are
body. Sometimes an entire tumor is removed and neurotoxic. Symptoms of toxemia are variable, from
later biopsied. Given that surgical tumor removal reduced cognitive functioning to somnolescence to
may cause additional damage, needle or stereotactic personality changes. Treatment is typically focused
biopsy, in which tiny amounts of tissue are removed on removing the source and purging the toxins.
via a narrow cannula, is often preferred. Treatment
of intracranial tumors may involve surgical excision
(either by open-skull surgery or gamma knife sur- What Are Diabetes Mellitus and
gery, which does not require physical cutting of the
Diabetic Encephalopathy?
skull), chemotherapy, and radiation.
Clinical aphasiologists may be involved in pre-
operative assessments of people with brain tumors Diabetes mellitus (DM) is a chronic disorder of car-
to establish baselines with which to compare post- bohydrate metabolism caused by abnormal insulin
operative assessments. Some speech-language function or insulin deficiency, resulting especially in
pathologists (SLPs) participate as team members in elevated or poorly controlled blood sugar (glucose)
intraoperative monitoring, helping to ensure that levels. The influence of diabetes on human health
awake patients’ cognitive and linguistic abilities is enormous and is increasing steadily worldwide,
are carefully observed so that functional anatomi- in terms of overall health, mortality, and economic
cal structures associated with communication are impacts. This is the case across every age, sex, race,
spared during craniotomy. and education category. DM is a leading cause of
Treatment of language symptoms, depending death worldwide, ranking fifth among causes of
on their nature, may be informed by recommended death in many countries; its incidence has dramat-
treatments for stroke-induced aphasia as well as lan- ically increased in recent decades across the globe.
guage deficits associated with right brain injury and DM can refer to a spectrum of related patholo-
TBI. People with progressive neurological symptoms gies, yet, specifically, the term almost always refers
due to cancer are increasingly likely to develop com- to one of the three main types of DM: Type 1, Type 2,
munication challenges toward the end of life. In such and gestational. Type 1 diabetes entails decreased or
cases, the role of the clinical aphasiologist is vital in absent insulin production from the beta cells of the
terms of recommendations and training related to pancreas. It originates as an autoimmune attack on
augmentative communication and assistive technol- the pancreas; insufficient pancreatic insulin produc-
ogy (Pollens, 2004). tion in turn leads to the chronically high blood glu-
cose (hyperglycemic) levels in the blood.
Type 2 DM is characterized by gradual insulin
resistance of cellular tissue to the normal production
What Is Toxemia?
of insulin. With time, a person with Type 2 diabe-
tes experiences a diminishing of insulin production
Toxemia is the poisoning, irritation, or inflamma- in the pancreas. Insulin resistance takes place over
tion of nervous system tissue through exposure to several years. A person with Type 2 diabetes may be
harmful substances. Toxic encephalopathy refers able to control blood glucose levels to a large extent
to brain dysfunction related to metabolism at the by controlling weight, exercising, eating healthfully,
6. Etiologies of Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders   71

and, if necessary, taking prescribed medication to


Box
decrease cellular insulin resistance and/or increase 6–4 Areas of Cognitive-Linguistic Deficits
pancreatic insulin production. Associated With Diabetes Mellitus
Type 1 DM was once commonly referred to as
childhood or juvenile diabetes because it tends to • Memory
affect people at a young age. Those terms have been • Attention
replaced by the less misleading term, Type 1. This • Psychomotor skills
has become especially important as more and more • Visuospatial and visuoconstructional
children are affected by Type 2 diabetes. Gestational ability
diabetes is similar to Type 2 DM. It occurs due to • Orientation
hormonal changes during pregnancy. The condition • Word finding, verbal fluency, and
often goes away postnatally, although mothers who generative naming
have had it have a greater disposition toward Type 2 • Problem-solving
DM later in life. • Executive functions
Diabetic encephalopathy is any type of brain
disorder caused by diabetes. People with diabetes Sources: Arvanitakis et al., 2004; Coker & Shumaker,
have significantly higher incidence than others of 2003; Greenwood, 2003; Hallowell, Enderby, et al.,
numerous conditions likely to affect cognition and 2021; Hallowell et al., 2015; Hassing et al., 2004;
language, such as stroke, brain atrophy, atheroscle- Kanaya et al., 2004; Meneilly et al., 1993; Perlmuter
et al., 1987; Ravona-Springer et al., 2010; Ryan, 1988;
rosis, peripheral and autonomic neuropathies, and
Ryan et al., 2003; Skenazy & Bigler, 1984; Toro et al.,
dementia. Even in the absence of such conditions
2009; Weinger & Jacobson, 1998.
common in people with diabetes, hyperglycemia
(high blood glucose levels) and hypoglycemia (low
blood glucose levels) are related to cognitive changes
resulting from vascular defects in the blood-brain emia (a high level of blood lipids) and elevated blood
barrier and from hypertension (Hallowell et al., sugar. Given that each of those four elements is a
2015). Numerous studies demonstrate associations risk factor for stroke, it is no wonder that the com-
between diabetes and problems of cognition and bination of them is related to increased stroke risk.
language. A summary of key cognitive-linguistic Metabolic syndrome is also a risk factor for diabetes,
deficits that have been shown to occur dispropor- heart disease, and a host of other health complica-
tionately in people with DM is given in Box 6–4. tions. Metabolic syndrome is especially prevalent in
Additionally, hearing loss is more common in people people with sedentary lifestyles and poor diet, with
with DM, further complicating associated commu- hyperlipidemia (high blood lipids and low high-den-
nication impairments. Much remains to be known sity lipoprotein [HDL, “good cholesterol”]). Once
about the degree to which performance of adults considered a disease of affluence most prominent
with diabetes differs from those without diabetes in minority-world countries, its incidence is steadily
when general variations in glucose values and real- rising throughout the world and now constitutes a
time moment-by-moment glucose values are taken global pandemic. Global prevalence is estimated to
into consideration (Hallowell et al., 2015). be about one-fourth of the world’s population. In the
United States, about 37% of the population is said
to have it. Two causes for its rise are decreases in
physical activity and increases in consumption of
What Is Metabolic Syndrome?
high-calorie, low-fiber foods. It has been increasing
significantly among women, adults from 20 to 39
Metabolic syndrome represents the clustering of years old, and Asian and Hispanic adults (Hirode
obesity, hypertension, dyslipidemia (an abnormal & Wong, 2020). Similar trends of increasing rates for
amount of lipids such as fat phospholipids, tri- certain ethnic groups and for children and young
glycerides, and cholesterol in the blood), hyerlipid- adults are being noted throughout the world.
72  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

ric Association, 2000; Román et al., 1993). The most


What Other Metabolic Disorders common symptoms that first lead to concerns about
Cause Encephalopathy? possible dementia are problems with memory and
behavior. People with dementia develop problems
Thyroid disorders may also affect brain functioning, with attention, executive functions, critical thinking,
often resulting in cognitive deficits, low energy lev- and language.
els, and reduced affect. Deficiencies in vitamin B12
also may cause cognitive problems. Thiamine defi-
ciency, often associated with chronic alcohol abuse, What Is Mild Cognitive Impairment?
tends to lead to loss of cognitive abilities in addition
to motor signs, gait abnormalities, and ocular motor
problems. This condition is sometimes referred to as Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is a condition of
Wernicke’s encephalopathy. Chronic alcohol abuse cognitive decline that is not typical of normal aging.
is another example of a toxic cause of cognitive and It often results from neurodegenerative disease
behavioral problems. Interestingly, recent research and head injury, but it may also be associated with
on the relationships between gut bacteria and met- neoplasm, infectious processes, or metabolic dis-
abolic, immune, and nervous system functions sug- orders. Terms previously used to characterize MCI
gests promise for improved understanding of brain include age-associated cognitive decline or memory
health through gut health (Morais et al., 2020). It will loss and benign senescent memory loss or forget-
be important to stay tuned to how this might influ- fulness. MCI is the current term of choice in light
ence risk, prevention, and treatment of neurogenic of international efforts to standardize terminology
communication disorders in the future. in related research and clinical practice (Winblad
et al., 2004). Memory problems are the most com-
mon complaints of people with MCI, although there
may be associated deficits in attention, visuospatial
What Is Neurodegenerative Disease? perception, language, and speed of processing.
For many people with MCI, the condition does not
Neurodegenerative disease is any neurogenic condi- affect everyday activities except for complex tasks
tion that progressively gets worse over time. Neuro- (Mansbach et al., 2012; Alcauskas & Galetta, 2018),
degenerative diseases that affect cognitive-linguistic although the real-life effects are highly variable
abilities include any of the many types of dementia across individuals.
and some forms of mild cognitive impairment. We
consider these briefly here and then more in depth
in Chapter 13 as well as in the intervention chap- What Is Primary Progressive Aphasia?
ters (Sections IV through VIII). Primary progres-
sive aphasia is the category of aphasia types that is
degenerative, eventually devolving to dementia. Primary progressive aphasia (PPA) is a neurogene-
rative dementia syndrome, unlike the other types of
aphasia. The onset is insidious rather than sudden;
symptoms get worse over time. The first symptoms
What Is Dementia?
of PPA to be noticed tend to be linguistic, and then
they evolve into symptoms more characteristic of
The criteria for the diagnosis of dementia include other forms of dementia. We review PPA as a syn-
memory impairment along with one or more cog- drome of aphasia in Chapter 10, highlight its varied
nitive or linguistic impairments having a noticeable subtypes and consider its clinical manifestations in
impact on social and occupational interactions and Chapter 13, and address it through varied interven-
representing an observable change from previous tion approaches in Sections IV through VIII (Chap-
levels of everyday functioning (American Psychiat- ters 14 through 33).
6. Etiologies of Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders   73

symptoms have been acquired or exacerbated,


What Are Some Special Challenges let alone what the causes are.
in Identifying Etiologies of • Symptoms are variable within and between
Cognitive-Linguistic Disorders? individuals having the same underlying
causal condition.
As we have just reviewed, a wide array of condi- • Any given individual may experience more
tions may cause a person to acquire a neurogenic than one causal condition, complicating
language disorder. The potential complexity and or even rendering impossible the task
variability of each of these underlying conditions of discerning what symptomatology is
alone is great. Further complicating our understand- associated with which condition.
ing of what may underlie any given individual’s • Countless additional factors, such as age,
acquired communication challenges are the follow- socioeconomic status, cultural and linguistic
ing facts: background, health status, emotional health,
and social support, may affect the influence
• Each individual has their own unique set of of varied etiologies as well as an individual’s
cognitive-linguistic and social strengths and prognosis for improvement.
weaknesses before acquiring a communication
problem, making it hard to discern which The excellent SLP embraces these challenges.

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. Make a list of bolded terms used in this 9. Check the list of stroke prevention tips in
chapter. Practice defining them in your own Box 6–1. How are you doing in terms of
words. your everyday lifestyle as it pertains those
2. What are the most common causes of suggestions?
acquired neurogenic language disorders in 10. Describe examples of causes of open- and
adults? closed-head injuries.
3. Describe, compare, and contrast the primary 11. Describe why the consequences of TBI tend
types of stroke. to be extremely complex.
4. What predisposing factors increase a given 12. Imagine that you are helping the partner of a
individual’s risk of stroke? TBI survivor understand the nature of a TBI
5. What are examples of modifiable versus due to an acceleration-deceleration injury.
nonmodifiable risk factors for stroke? How might you explain the types of injuries
6. How is the sudden onset of stroke and TBI that are entailed?
related to the need for clinical aphasiologists 13. Describe the four broad categories of blast
to have strong counseling and life coaching injuries.
skills? 14. In what way do sports-related TBI differ
7. List warning signs for stroke and TIA. from blast-injury TBI?
8. It is important that we consider how we, 15. How would you describe your current
as clinicians, might be role models for status in terms of reducing your own risk
others in terms of preventing neurogenic of TBI?
communication disorders. How would you 16. What examples of social stigma associated
describe your current status in terms of with TBI prevention have you observed or
reducing your own risk of stroke? experienced personally?
74  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

17. What are some examples of good strategies 22. What is intraoperative monitoring, and why
for addressing personal and societal might an aphasiologist be involved in it?
resistance to improved protection from head 23. What are examples of varied types of
injury in sports, automobile operation, and neoplasm that might affect language abilities?
leisure activities? 24. What are the primary types of diabetes
18. Describe specific examples of bacterial and mellitus (DM)? How do they differ from one
viral infections that may affect the brain another?
and cause neurogenic communication 25. In what ways is diabetes relevant to
disorders. language processing in the brain?
19. What might be the impact of the space- 26. What are the most common types of
occupying nature of the inflammation in the symptoms that first lead to concerns about a
brain? possible diagnosis of dementia?
20. What are blood-brain barriers, and how 27. In what ways is PPA like other forms of
might they be affected as a result of aphasia? In what ways is it different?
infectious processes?
www
21. How is HIV/AIDS relevant to the scope of Check out additional learning and teaching mate-
practice of SLPs? rials on the companion website.
CHAPTER
7
Neurophysiology and
Neuropathology of Acquired
Neurogenic Language Disorders

In this chapter, we review aspects of neuroanatomy courses that aspiring speech-language pathologists
and neurophysiology and related principles that (SLPs) take. In Chapter 10, we review the key ana-
are vital to clinical practice with people who have tomical landmarks of the brain associated with each
acquired neurogenic cognitive-linguistic disorders. of the classic types of aphasia.
It is not within the scope of this book to educate the After reading and reflecting on the content in
reader in depth about the neuroscience of cognition this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in
and communication. It is highly recommended that your own words, the following queries:
readers of this book take at least one comprehensive
neuroscience course, enroll in intensive workshops, 1. What should SLPs know about neuroanatomy
engage in hands-on neuroanatomy lab experiences, and neurophysiology associated with
and study neuroscience texts if you have not already neurogenic cognitive-linguistic disorders?
done so. Also, there are a number of online tutorial 2. What are key neurophysiological principles
programs to help you review key aspects of neuro- pertinent to acquired cognitive-linguistic
anatomy and neurophysiology pertinent to neuro- disorders?
genic communication disorders. See the Learning 3. What is the most clinically pertinent
and Reflection Activities section at the end of this knowledge an aphasiologist should have about
chapter for suggestions. Be sure to follow up by fill- the blood supply to the brain?
ing in any gaps in your basic background knowl- 4. What factors affect a person’s prognosis for
edge in this area. Do not restrict your studies to just recovery from a stroke or brain injury?
communication functions. Supplemental studies 5. Why is it important for clinical aphasiologists
with clinical relevance for those without solid back- to know about the visual system?
ground in neuroscience, or needing a review, are 6. What aspects of the visual system are most
recommended. relevant to people with neurogenic cognitive-
At varied points throughout this book, we refer linguistic disorders?
to neuroanatomical components and principles as 7. How are visual field deficits characterized?
well as the functions of the nervous system as they 8. What are ocular motor deficits?
affect the people with whom we work. For example, 9. What are visual attention deficits?
in this chapter, we review key neurophysiological 10. What are higher-level visual deficits?
principles, the blood supply system, and the visual 11. What aspects of the neurophysiology of
system because these are vital to clinical practice and hearing are most relevant to people with
tend not to be covered in tremendous detail in other neurogenic language disorders?

75
76  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

developed a solid background in human neurophys-


What Should SLPs Know About iology, especially as it relates to language and cogni-
Neuroanatomy and Neurophysiology tion, it is important that you do so.
Associated With Neurogenic The structural components of the brain that cor-
Cognitive-Linguistic Disorders? respond to language functions are clustered around
the perisylvian region of the language-dominant
Competent SLPs must be able to identify the basic hemisphere. Many of the most critical areas import-
landmarks of the brain associated with neurogenic ant to acquired neurogenic language disorders are
communication disorders. They must also be famil- visible on a lateral view of the language-dominant
iar with landmarks associated with other types of hemisphere, as shown in Figure 7–1. You may find it
problems that people with neurological disorders helpful to use the lists of structures, landmarks, and
due to strokes, brain injury, neoplasm, dementia, concepts in Box 7–1 as a checklist in evaluating your
and metabolic disturbance might have. If you are basic knowledge of neuroanatomy and neurophysi-
studying to become an SLP, and you have not yet ology related to clinical aphasiology.

Figure 7–1. Examples of functional areas of the brain and Brodmann’s areas
vital to clinical aphasiology, according to classical models. Highlighted regions
represent examples of major functional areas and corresponding Brodmann’s
areas visible on a left lateral view of the brain. A. Broca’s area. B. Primary
auditory area. C. Wernicke’s area and surrounding auditory association area.
D. Primary motor area. E. Primary sensory area. F. Primary visual area. Image
credit: Taylor Reeves. A full-color version of this figure can be found in the
Color Insert.
7. Neurophysiology and Neuropathology of Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders   77

Box
7–1 Structures, Landmarks, and Concepts Relevant to Clinical Aphasiology

Major components of the central nervous system • Superior temporal gyrus


• Cerebrum, cerebral hemispheres • Superior temporal sulcus
• Cerebellum • Inferior temporal sulcus
• Brainstem • Transverse temporal gyri
• Spinal cord • Primary auditory area
• Anterior temporal gyrus, Heschl’s gyrus,
Views of the brain auditory association area (or cortex)
• Lateral • Orbital gyri and sulcus
• Superior • Planum temporale
• Ventral • Arcuate fasciculus
• Medial • Primary visual area (or cortex)
• Sagittal/midsagittal • Visual association area
• Coronal • Glial cells
• Basal ganglia
Hemispheric lobes
• Frontal White matter
• Temporal • Myelin
• Parietal • Commissures
• Occipital • Corpus callosum
• Insula (central) • Association fibers
• Limbic • Projection fibers

Major cortical landmarks, structures, and Visual system structures


functional areas • Retina
• Central fissure (central sulcus or fissure of • Optic nerve
Rolando) • Optic tract
• Lateral fissure or sylvian fissure • Optic chiasm
• Perisylvian area
• Precentral gyrus, primary motor area, Brainstem structures
motor strip • Diencephalon
• Premotor area (or strip or cortex) • Thalamus
• Prefrontal cortex • Hypothalamus
• Postcentral gyrus, primary sensory area, • Midbrain
sensory strip • Pons
• Primary sensory area (or cortex) • Medulla oblongata
• Sensory association area (or cortex)
• Occipital pole Blood supply system
• Hippocampus • Right and left internal carotid arteries
• Parahippocampal gyrus • Right and left vertebral arteries
• Broca’s area, pars triangularis and pars • Basilar artery
opercularis (frontal operculum) of the third • Circle of Willis
frontal convolution • Right and left posterior cerebral arteries
• Wernicke’s area • Right and left posterior communicating
• Supramarginal gyrus arteries
• Angular gyrus • Right and left anterior cerebral arteries
78  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

• Right and left middle cerebral arteries • Pia mater


• Anterior communicating artery • Subdural space
• Deep cerebral veins • Subarachnoid space
• Superficial cerebral veins
• Internal jugular vein Concepts in neurophysiology
• Synaptic transmission
Ventricular system • Contralateral motor control
• Cerebrospinal fluid • Contralateral sensory perception
• Lateral ventricles • Hemispheric dominance
• Third ventricle • Motor and sensory homunculi
• Fourth ventricle • Efferent and afferent systems
• Cerebral aqueduct • Somatic nervous system
• Foramina of Munro • Autonomic nervous system
• Choroid plexuses • Sympathetic nervous system
• Parasympathetic nervous system
The meninges • Anastomosis
• Dura mater • Collateral circulation
• Arachnoid mater • Blood-brain barrier

People with any neurological problem that case in general in terms of differences between the
leads to a communication disorder are also likely to right and left hemispheres, and specifically in terms
have numerous additional problems that affect their of differences among precise structures within each
everyday abilities. The more we know about how hemisphere. The notion that each side of the brain
the brain functions and how injury to the brain may houses specialized abilities in most people is called
affect diverse areas, such as autonomic functions, hemispheric specialization. Despite looking basi-
wakefulness, vision, olfaction, sensation, attention, cally the same structurally, the two hemispheres
memory, movement, speech, hearing, personality, house contrasting functions in the adult brain. In
mood, eating, and swallowing, the more adept we most people, for example, the left hemisphere is
are at understanding the people we treat and the dominant for speech, language, and analytical func-
more effective we are as rehabilitation team members tions, whereas the right hemisphere in most people
facilitating the best outcomes. Of course, if you wish is dominant for musical skills, emotional interpre-
to delve even further, you could spend a lifetime tations, and paralinguistic functions, such as stress,
focused on the study of the neurophysiology of com- intonation, pitch, humor, and metaphor. In a clas-
munication. The pertinent literature is ever-growing, sic study of hemispheric specialization for speech
especially in light of advancements in neuroimag- and language, Wada and Rasmussen (1960) applied
ing methods. Also, opportunities to get involved in the Wada test (described later) in 140 right-handers
related research and academic study are abundant. and 122 left-handers to study patterns of left versus
right cerebral dominance. They interpreted their
results to indicate that 96% of right-handers and
What Are Key Neurophysiological 70% of left-handers were left hemisphere dominant
Principles Pertinent to Acquired for language. Only 15% of left-handers had right
hemisphere language dominance, and only 15%
Cognitive-Linguistic Disorders?
had bilateral dominance; 4% of right handers were
right hemisphere dominant, and none had bilateral
Specialization of Structure and Function dominance.
Geschwind and Levitsky (1968) followed up
Specific regions of the brain have long been asso- soon after that study with an examination compar-
ciated with specific functional abilities. This is the ing the postmortem size of the planum temporale
7. Neurophysiology and Neuropathology of Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders   79

(the frontotemporoparietal region encompassing the Interconnectivity Throughout the Brain


speech and language areas) of 100 left- and right-
handers. They reported that the planum temporale
Of course, there are important exceptions to patterns
was larger in the left hemisphere of 65%, in the
of structure-function correlates. An individual may
right hemisphere of 24%, and the size of the struc-
exhibit symptoms that suggest a specific area of
ture was equal between hemispheres in 11%. Using
damage but may not actually have any damage to
functional transcranial Doppler, Njemanze (2003)
that area. Likewise, a person may have a lesion in a
concluded that only 61.5% of right-handers with-
specific, known functional area but not demonstrate
out neurological disorders showed left hemisphere
the symptoms that would typically be predicted
lateralization for language, with 38.5% showing
given the lesion’s location. Another reason to be cau-
right hemisphere lateralization for language. Using
tious about overgeneralizing structure-function rela-
functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI),
tionships is that the brain does not act as a system
however, Hund-Georgiadis et al. (2001) reported
of separate parts, each functioning independently.
results similar to those of Wada and Rasmussen
Rather, interconnections among distributed net-
(1960) for right-handers: 94% had clear left hemi-
works of brain structures, all operating in a dynamic
sphere dominance for language. Hund-Georgiadis
electrochemical environment, are vital to the brain’s
et al. (2001) further reported that only 2% of left-
functioning. Fiber tracts throughout the brain con-
handers had right hemisphere dominance for lan-
nect the two hemispheres and also connect cortical
guage, whereas 76% had left brain dominance and
with subcortical structures. Subcortical relay sta-
the remainder mixed laterality. Numerous addi-
tions enable complex integration and interpretation
tional studies have been completed to study struc-
of sensory and motor signals. In sum, although the
tural and functional asymmetries in greater detail.
principle of specialization of structure and function
Overall, there continues to be a lack of agreement
is important, it is equally important that we chal-
about the relationship between handedness and lan-
lenge our assumptions about this as we consider
guage dominance and also on the actual proportion
patterns of interconnectivity as well as any given
of people with left-, right-, and mixed-hemisphere
individual’s unique condition.
language dominance. Still, two conclusions are
clear: most people, regardless of handedness, are
left brain dominant for language, and the methods
used to test for language dominance influence the The Brain’s Plasticity
results obtained.
Intrahemispheric specialization, the notion Neuroplasticity is the ability of the nervous system
that specific structures within each hemisphere are to change and adapt to internal or external influences.
associated with specific abilities, is another key con- The brain’s plasticity is at the heart of spontaneous
struct in this arena. Since the 18th century, it has been recovery, or the natural pattern of improvement in
clear that injury to certain areas of the brain tends to functioning after an injury to the brain. The clinical lit-
result in predictable types of communication prob- erature is replete with cases of individuals who have
lems (see Tesak & Code, 2008, for a detailed history). lost major portions of brain tissue yet who have expe-
Just how those areas are defined has been a topic rienced significant brain reorganization; that is, struc-
of debate for over 150 years (see Fridriksson et al., tures other than the damaged ones have taken over
2015; Krestel et al., 2013; Paciaroni & Bogousslavsky, functions initially associated with the damaged area.
2011). A basic example is Wernicke’s area in the Plasticity is also at the heart of learning, as
left superior temporal lobe; a person with a lesion experience leads to improved connections among
in that area is likely to display signs of Wernicke’s neural networks. Taking advantage of plasticity is
aphasia. Likewise, when we observe a person with key to helping foster brain changes through behav-
Wernicke’s aphasia, we might reason that there is a ioral intervention, which is the primary type of SLP
high probability that she or he has a lesion in that intervention. Although the brain continues to lose
area. This is a topic into which we delve further as plasticity in many ways from birth to old age, and
we explore varied types or syndromes of aphasia in although there are clearly critical periods in brain
Chapter 10. development that determine whether and how well
80  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

certain abilities will be acquired, even the oldest For now, let’s briefly review the most critical
brains retain plasticity and continue to be influenced content that every SLP should know about the cor-
by learning and exposure (Aghaz et al., 2018). We dis- tical blood supply. Four main arteries, arising from
cuss neuroplasticity further in Sections VI through the heart, supply the blood that eventually reaches
VIII as we explore principles of treatment and means the cortex: the right and left carotid arteries, as well
of enhancing brain change through intervention. as the right and left vertebral arteries. The common
carotid artery stems from the heart. As it ascends
toward the brain, the right and left internal carotid
What Is the Most Clinically Pertinent arteries and the right and left external carotid arter-
Knowledge an Aphasiologist Should Have ies arise from it. Of these two pairs of arteries, focus
your attention now on the internal carotid arteries.
About the Blood Supply to the Brain?
The blood supply from the internal carotid arteries
flows into the right and left anterior cerebral arteries
Blood supplies two nutrients that are essential to and the right and left middle cerebral arteries. These
brain function: glucose and oxygen. Neurons can- are the main arteries that supply blood to most of
not store these nutrients; constant replenishing is the functional areas of the brain involved in cogni-
needed. Areas of the brain engaged in greater acti- tion and communication. Thus, disruptions in these
vation at any given moment require more nutrients arteries are directly related to most brain-based cog-
and thus a greater flow of blood. An additional func- nitive-communicative disorders.
tion of blood in the brain is that, as it circulates, it The right and left vertebral arteries arising
removes elements that are toxic to the nervous sys- from the heart join together as the basilar artery at
tem, especially carbon dioxide. Toxins and deoxy- the brainstem, travel up the ventral pons, and then
genated blood are transported back to the heart and directly join the circle of Willis. The basilar artery
lungs for reoxygenation through the venous sinus then bifurcates at the circle of Willis into the right and
system. Given that the problems of blood supply left posterior cerebral arteries. Given that right and
to the brain are fundamental to all types of brain left vertebral arteries join as the basilar artery, many
pathology — whether the pathology arises from of us find it helpful to refer to this arterial division
stroke, brain injury, neoplasm, metabolic distur- as the vertebral-basilar system. Be sure that you can
bance, or neurodegenerative disease— it is extremely identify the juncture of the internal carotid arteries
important that SLPs be familiar with basic aspects of and the vertebral-basilar arteries at the circle of Willis
the cortical blood supply system. (Figure 7–2). The circle of Willis is an anastomosis,
Typical medical records for people with stroke a protective feature allowing collateral circulation
and brain injury include details about components of of blood in case one channel of blood flow becomes
the blood supply that have been disrupted. A solid blocked. See Figure 7–2 for a schematic illustration.
understanding of the major channels through which An occlusion in any channel within the circle results
blood is supplied from the heart to the brain, and the in blood pressure changes that then cause the blood
basic aspects of blood flow dynamics, is important to flow in a different direction, away from the occlu-
for understanding the nature of neurogenic deficits sion. The closer a blockage is to an emerging portion
and for meaningful discussions with rehabilitation of any given cerebral artery forming the circle, the
team members, clients, and clients’ significant oth- more likely it is that effective collateral circulation
ers. Given that content about the basics of the blood will occur. Also, when the occlusion builds up over
supply to the brain — with an emphasis on its clini- time, the system is more likely to adapt and allow
cal relevance — is often lacking in otherwise excellent alternative channeling of blood than it is if there is a
neuroscience texts and courses, specific suggestions sudden blockage.
for studying and reviewing the blood supply system This system of collateral circulation in the circle
are given in the Learning and Reflection Activities of Willis is a wonderful safety mechanism that saves
section of this chapter. If this is not a content area lives and reduces long-term damage by allowing
that you know well, be sure to invest time and effort blood to reach critical areas in the brain even when
in learning about it. their typical source is cut off. Still, it is not a per-
7. Neurophysiology and Neuropathology of Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders   81

anterior cerebral artery


middle cerebral artery
anterior communicating
artery

ophthalmic artery

internal carotid artery anterior choroidal artery

posterior communicating
artery
posterior cerebral artery

superior cerebellar artery

pontine arteries

anterior inferior cerebellar


artery

basilar artery

vertebral artery

Figure 7–2. The circle of Willis. Image credit: Taylor Reeves. A full-color version of this figure can be found
in the Color Insert.

fect system, and collateral blood flow tends not to faces of frontal and parietal lobes, and the medial
be nearly as effective as flow through the standard surfaces between the two hemispheres. Given the
channels. Also, there is enormous variability in the structural-functional associations in those areas, one
actual configuration of the circle of Willis across may predict that reduced blood flow in the ante-
individual people, and some have much better sys- rior cerebral arteries is likely to affect functioning
tems for enabling collateral circulation than others associated with the prefrontal cortex. Some related
(see Brohi et al., 2018). functional problems are within the direct purview
The right and left anterior cerebral arteries of SLPs: deficits in executive functions, including
emerge from the internal carotid arteries and extend problems of decision-making, planning, self-moni-
to the anterior portion of the cortex, the lateral sur- toring, and social appropriateness. Other impacts of
82  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

reduced blood flow from the anterior cerebral arter- posterior, inferior parts of the brain, including the
ies include contralateral motor control and strength posterior inferior temporal lobe and the occipital
of the lower body. lobe. Given the structural-functional associations
The right and left middle cerebral arteries are in those areas, one may predict that reduced blood
the largest channels arising from the internal carotid flow in the posterior cerebral artery will lead to
arteries. From the circle of Willis, they extend into visual acuity and visual attention problems, read-
the posterior frontal lobe, major portions of the tem- ing problems, and deficits in sensory integration,
poral lobes, and anterior parietal lobes, plus the including recognition and interpretation of visual
basal ganglia and diencephalon on their respective information.
sides. Given the structural-functional associations To recap, given that most people are left brain
in those areas, one may predict that reduced blood dominant for language, and given that the middle
flow in the middle cerebral artery on the language-​ cerebral artery supplies blood to the areas most
dominant side is likely to affect any of a wide array involved in speech and language, disruptions in the
of speech and language functions, including reading left middle cerebral artery and its extensions into the
and writing. Other impacts may include contralat- cortex are the most likely to be associated with neu-
eral sensory deficits, contralateral deficits in motor rogenic speech and language disorders. A schematic
control and strength of the upper body, difficulty illustration of areas of the brain supplied by each of
with spatial relations, and visual problems. the left cerebral arteries as seen on a lateral view of
The right and left posterior cerebral arteries the brain is shown in Figure 7–3. It is essential that
emerge from the circle of Willis and extend to the you be able to draw a simple sketch of patterns of

Figure 7–3. The cerebral arterial blood supply to the left lateral cortex. Image
credit: Taylor Reeves. A full-color version of this figure can be found in the
Color Insert.
7. Neurophysiology and Neuropathology of Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders   83

each of the areas of blood supply, at least to the lat-


Box
eral portion of the brain. This is important, not only 7–2 When Structure Bleeds
to test and ensure your own knowledge about this,
but also because it is often useful to do this when When structure bleeds, chaos rules.
helping stroke survivors and their family members Words, numbers, thoughts get shuffled.
understand the nature of a stroke. See Box 7–2 for a In my mind with thieves and fools.
personal take by a person with aphasia on the con- My perception get muffled.
sequences of bleeding in the brain. Everything has its own place.
Drawers and shelves are concrete.
Folders, files of database.
What Factors Affect a Person’s Prognosis for All things are in a spreadsheet.
Recovery From a Stroke or Brain Injury? When structure clots, havoc reigns.
Sensations, feelings get shocked.
The factors that may affect the severity of an acquired Anxiety, fears, and chains —
language disorder due to stroke or brain injury Like I’m was laughed at and mocked.
are the same as those that influence prognosis for Emotions, memory, write,
recovery. These are summarized in Box 7–3. Some Understand language, words, read,
are related to etiology (e.g., site and size of lesion), Touch, pain, temperature, sight,
others to pre-onset characteristics (e.g., intelligence Speak, concentrate — things you need.
educational history), and still others to present sta- But structure stores my psyche.
tus (e.g., access to excellent services, social support, Housing spirit, mind, and soul.
and the nature and severity of concomitant deficits). Back with Phoenix and Nike.
Note that many of these factors are highly interde-
Seize the light from a black hole.
pendent. Within a given individual, it is often impos-
sible to tease apart the influence of one factor versus By Mark Harder, a professional plasterer who became
another. An important example is that the influence a poet after having a stroke. He now leads Poems in
of age, considered by many to be an important prog- Speech, online poetry sharing sessions for people
nostic factor, may be confounded by other factors, with aphasia and related challenges.
such as health status. In and of itself, especially when
applied to an individual and not a group, age is not
necessarily a strong predictor of prognosis. Note,
too, that some factors are balanced by others. The For people with aphasia, aphasia severity is a
size of the lesion is definitely an important factor, but strong predictor of recovery potential. Type of apha-
it also matters greatly how much tissue surround- sia may also play a role in people with similar levels
ing an infarct is still perfused. Although educational of aphasia severity. For example, people with global
history matters, one’s actual intelligence may have a aphasia tend to have poorer prognosis than those
stronger influence. with other types of aphasia. People with Wernicke’s
There is no clear agreement among experts per- aphasia generally have poorer prognosis than peo-
taining to some of the factors that have been asso- ple with Broca’s aphasia; however, there are many
ciated with prognosis. For example, although some reasons this pattern may not hold true for a given
have suggested that gender may play a role in prog- individual.
nosis, research findings on that topic are equivocal. As we discuss further in Chapter 22, clinical
Another example is that SLP intervention at the right aphasiologists should be well versed in the influ-
time with the appropriate duration and frequency is ence of such factors when making any statements
generally accepted as a positive influence on recov- about the likelihood of recovery for any given stroke
ery; there is controversy, though, regarding just what or brain injury survivor. Let’s always recognize that
the right timing, frequency, and duration are (a topic any individual may defy statistical odds in terms of
we consider in Section VI). predictions about recovery.
84  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Box
7–3 Factors That May Influence Severity of an Acquired Language Disorder
and Prognosis for Recovery in Stroke and Brain Injury Survivors

General positive influences (e.g., more tends to • Memory


be better) • Attention
• Time post-onset • Appropriateness of judgments
• Amount of intact perilesional tissue • Coping skills
• Good health, glucose regulation, rest, • Self-esteem
exercise/fitness, nutrition, hydration • Motivation (intrinsic and extrinsic)
• Motivation • Vocational and avocational goals
• Pre-onset intelligence • Tolerance of frustration
• Pre-onset academic skills • Anger management strategies
• Educational history
• Professional history General negative influences (e.g., less tends to
• Access to intervention through a clinical be better)
aphasiologist • Age
• Appropriate quality, type, frequency, and • Size of lesion
duration of SLP intervention at the right • Presence of bilateral lesions
time • Inclusion of subcortical white matter in
• Access to holistic rehabilitation addition to cortical tissue
programming • Length of coma (if any)
• Psychosocial support • Concomitant memory and attention deficits
• Awareness of deficits • Depression
• Independent use of compensatory • Psychiatric disorder
strategies and self-cuing • Alcohol abuse (past and current)
• Independence/history of independent • Drug and other substance abuse (past and
living current)
• Stimulability for engaging in specific
cognitive-linguistic tasks Other influences
• Willingness to engage in and practice • Site of lesion
compensatory strategies • Pharmacological effects and side effects
• Willingness to participate in multimodal • Locus of control (belief in what or who can
communication influence one’s condition and outcome; see
• Organizational abilities Chapter 27)

Note. These influences are termed general because they are often associated with better or worse prog-
nosis. Any given individual may defy the influence of any factor on the list. Note also that many of
these factors are not independent from one another.

deficits. These may be generally categorized as visual


Why Is It Important for Clinical sensory deficits, visual attention deficits, visual inter-
Aphasiologists to Know About pretation deficits, and ocular motor deficits. Visual
the Visual System? sensory deficits include problems of visual acu-
ity, problems of color perception (achromatopsia/
People with brain-based communication disorders dyschromatopsia), and visual field deficits (or visual
often have one or more types of concomitant visual field cuts). A visual sensory deficit entails a problem
7. Neurophysiology and Neuropathology of Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders   85

with actually seeing, that is, getting visual informa- we review clinically relevant basics about the visual
tion registered in the brain. Visual sensory deficits system with a focus on content that is most essential
can be due to a problem or combination of problems for clinical aphasiologists.
anywhere from the eye to the primary visual cortex.
Visual attention deficits are problems with
being aware of information that is actually reg- What Aspects of the Visual System
istered in the brain; they are not sensory deficits, Are Most Relevant to People With
because visual stimuli are physically “seen” but Neurogenic Language Disorders?
are not noticed or attended to. Visual interpretation
or visual integration deficits are problems with
making sense of visual information that is physi- Anatomy and Physiology Associated
cally seen and also attended to. Ocular motor defi- With Visual Deficits
cits include problems of adjusting the shape of the
lens, problems with pupillary dilation, problems in Visual sensory deficits include problems of visual
achieving visual reflexes, and problems of moving acuity, problems of color perception, and visual field
the eye within its socket. deficits (or visual field cuts). Visual acuity entails
A key reason that it is so important to know refraction of light rays onto the lens and cornea, the
about the visual system is that much of our everyday conversion of light to neural impulses within the
use of language can be affected by visual problems retina, the transmission of those impulses to the pri-
experienced by people with aphasia and related dis- mary visual cortex within the occipital lobe, and the
orders. Consider, for example, how important it is to actual perception of those impulses throughout the
see who is in our shared space as we are conversing, primary and visual association areas. As you read
to see others’ nonverbal responses as we communi- about the visual system, refer to Figures 7–4 and 7–5
cate, to be able to see the things to which people are to locate each of the structures mentioned.
referring within the conversational context, and to The lens is the transparent connective tissue that
see print well enough so that we can read it. Without underlies the pupil. It refracts light rays and focuses
understanding the visual abilities of a person we are them on the retina. Refraction is simply the act of
serving, we may overlook highly relevant influences changing the angle of the light rays as they hit an
on their communication abilities. Furthermore, with area of contrasting density. The retina is the inside
few exceptions, the diagnostic and treatment materi- layer of the eyeball. There are two types of photore-
als we use as clinicians entail visual material. We ask ceptors in the retina: rods (important for low-light
our clients to point to things they see in the room. We and peripheral vision) and cones (functional in
ask them to label and describe pictures and objects. bright light and responsible for central discrimina-
We test their reading abilities. Likewise, the majority tive vision and color detection). Action potentials
of experimental tasks used in research studies with from the photoreceptors are transmitted to ganglion
people who have aphasia involve materials that are cells within the retina, cells that are specialized for
presented visually (Hallowell, 2008). transmitting visual neural impulses. The axons of
We also must know about the visual system to these cells travel to the optic disk and pass in bun-
be able to explain visual challenges to patients and dles through the sclera (the outer coating of the eye-
their significant others, who might not have oth- ball) to become the optic nerve (cranial nerve II).
erwise learned about them, and to be able to help Optic nerve fibers travel through the optic chiasm
our rehabilitation colleagues be aware of them and (the x-shaped structure housing the optic nerve
take them into account in their diagnostic and inter- fibers at the base of the brain). Some of those fibers
vention work. In sum, although it is not within the remain ipsilateral; that is, they continue to travel on
scope of practice of clinical aphasiologists to diag- the same side of the brain as the eye from which they
nose or treat visual deficits per se, it is essential that carry visual information. Other fibers cross over to
we be aware of them, understand their implications the contralateral side (i.e., the opposite side of the
for communication, and know when to refer for brain relative to the eye from which they carry visual
follow-up by a neuro-ophthalmologist. To this end, information) inside the optic chiasm.
Figure 7–4. Components of the eye. Image credit: Taylor Reeves. A full-color ver-
sion of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.

Figure 7–5. Overview of the visual system. Image credit: Taylor Reeves. A full-color
version of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.

86
7. Neurophysiology and Neuropathology of Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders   87

All of the optic nerve fibers continue to travel to four quadrants of the visual field. Each quadrant is
the lateral geniculate body of the thalamus. Then, defined as temporal or nasal and as upper or lower.
as the optic tract, they travel through the internal Because of the crossing over of fibers from each
capsule. Next, they curve around the lateral ven- eye within the optic chiasm, components of the
tricles and travel, through the optic radiations, to visual system from the optic chiasm all the way to the
the primary visual cortex. The primary visual cortex primary visual cortex contain information from both
(Brodmann’s area 17; see Figure 7–1) is located on eyes. Thus, even if a person were completely blind in
the posterior portion of each occipital lobe. one eye due to a lesion or severing of the optic nerve
The visual field refers to the entire space from of that eye, they would still have representation from
which we take in visual information as we look the other eye in the primary visual cortex of both
forward. If you have good eyesight and have both hemispheres. Information from the right side of the
eyes open, you could trace with your finger the visual field in each eye is represented in the left side
shape of your entire binocular field of view (what of the cortex. Information from the left side of the
is seen with both eyes jointly). It would appear as visual field in each eye is represented in the right
an ellipsis, wide horizontally and narrower in the side of the cortex.
vertical dimension, idealized in Figure 7–6. The pri- The optic nerve fibers from the temporal half
mary visual cortex on each side of the brain receives (outside, toward the temples) of each retina are the
visual information from one side of the visual field ones that travel ipsilaterally through the optic chi-
in each eye. asm and on the lateral geniculate body of the thal-
We also have a visual field for each eye, as amus. They carry information about the nasal half
shown in Figure 7–7. If you were to close your left (inside, toward the nose) of the visual field (coming
eye while looking at the scene depicted, your right from temporal portion of each retina). Fibers from the
eye’s monocular visual field would appear in the nasal half of each retina are the ones that decussate
stylized shape shown on the right side of Figure 7–7. in the optic chiasm along their path to the thalamus.
Although visual fields are elliptical and not They carry information about the temporal half of the
completely circular, they are typically diagrammed visual field (coming from the nasal portion of each
as circles for the purpose of depicting the visual retina). The calcarine fissure is a prominent sulcus
fields, as shown in Figure 7–8. Imaginary horizon- seen on the medial surface of each hemisphere of the
tal and vertical lines bisecting the retina define brain. Optic radiations arriving above each calcarine

Figure 7–6. A binocular field of view. Image credit: Barbara Butterworth.


A full-color version of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.
Figure 7–7. The field of view for each independent eye. Image credit: Barbara
Butterworth. A full-color version of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.

Figure 7–8. Visual fields. Image credit: Taylor Reeves.

88
7. Neurophysiology and Neuropathology of Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders   89

fissure convey information about the lower half of Another is the occurrence of cataracts, the accumula-
the visual field from both eyes. Optic radiations arriv- tion of fibrous proteins on the lens, which degrades
ing below each calcarine fissure convey information the quality of images seen.
about the upper visual field from both eyes. As it is Keeping in mind the retinotopic organization
represented in the primary visual cortex, the image of throughout the visual system, we can predict the
the object in the visual field is inverted and reversed effects that a lesion will have on the visual system,
from left to right. As noted earlier, the left visual field given information about where the lesion occurs.
is represented in the visual cortex of the right hemi- Consider these examples.
sphere, and the right visual field is represented in the A lesion of the optic nerve (before the fibers
visual cortex of the left hemisphere. Furthermore, reach the optic chiasm) may result in complete or
the upper half of the visual field is represented below partial blindness of one eye. This is represented sche-
the calcarine sulcus; the lower half of the visual field matically in the visual fields shown in Figure 7–9.
is represented above the calcarine sulcus. A lesion within a specific set of fibers within the
The visual association areas (Brodmann’s areas optic nerve on one side may result in a scotoma, or
18 and 19), which surround area 17, have myriad blind area within the visual field only for that eye. An
complex connections to other areas of the brain. For example of a scotoma is represented stylistically in
example, they have fiber tracts projecting to areas the visual field for the left eye shown in Figure 7–10.
of the parietal and temporal cortex and to the thal- A lesion of the decussating fibers in the optic
amus. These projections enable the integration of chiasm (sparing the ipsilateral fibers) may result
visual information with cognitive operations vital in bitemporal (heteronymous) hemianopsia. See
to motion perception, spatial representation of ele- the visual field representation in Figure 7–11. It is
ments within the field of view, object recognition, called bitemporal because the temporal portions of
reading, and interpretation of pictographic stimuli. both visual fields are affected. It is called heterony-
In addition to the primary visual pathways, mous because it involves the right side of one visual
there are other visual pathways that we will not field and the left side of the other visual field. It is
explore in detail. Some of them project to subcorti- called hemianopsia because half of the visual field is
cal nuclei and some to various cortical regions via affected. The term hemianopia is synonymous with
the thalamus. They are important for control of the hemianopsia.
visual reflexes and for the integration of visual sensa- A lesion of the optic tract (after the fibers have
tion with sensorimotor perception and activity. One passed through the optic chiasm) on the left side of
of these other pathways, the tectal pathway, provides the brain may result in right homonymous hemi-
input to the tectospinal pathway. Tectal fibers travel anopsia (Figure 7–12). Reference to the term right is
from the eyes to the superior colliculi and contribute used because the right visual field is affected (due to
to our ability to orient to visual stimuli. damage on the contralateral side of the brain). It is
called homonymous because the same side is affected
in both visual fields.
A lesion of the optic tract fibers projecting to
How Are Visual Field Deficits Characterized?
visual cortex below the calcarine fissure on the left
side of the brain may result in bilateral upper right
Visual deficits are described in terms of visual field quadrantopsia (Figure 7–13). Lesions that affect the
rather than in terms of the retinal location disturbed. occipital lobe are most often caused by disruptions
Most of the visual deficits associated with brain inju- in blood supply from the posterior cerebral artery.
ries are beyond the level of the retina. However, some In some cases, there is enough collateral circulation
visual deficits associated with the surface of the eye of blood supplied by the middle cerebral artery for
are increasingly likely to occur with advancing age, some of the affected visual area to be spared.
so they may occur concomitantly with neurogenic A lesion of the optic tract fibers projecting to
visual deficits. One example is a change in the shape visual cortex above the calcarine fissure on the left
of the lens, resulting in reduced near visual acuity side of the brain may result in bilateral lower right
(hypermetropia), far visual acuity (myopia), or both. quadrantopsia (Figure 7–14).
Figure 7–9. Monocular blindness of the left eye. Image credit: Taylor Reeves.

Figure 7–10. A scotoma. Image credit: Taylor Reeves.

90
Figure 7–11. Bitemporal (heteronymous) hemianopsia. Image credit:
Taylor Reeves.

Figure 7–12. A right (homonymous) hemianopsia. Image credit: Taylor


Reeves.

91
Figure 7–13. A bilateral upper right quadrantopsia. Image credit: Taylor
Reeves.

Figure 7–14. A bilateral lower right quadrantopsia. Image credit: Taylor


Reeves.

92
7. Neurophysiology and Neuropathology of Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders   93

processing, and responding to visual items during


What Are Ocular Motor Deficits? assessment, it is important that clinicians take care
to screen for it. It is also vital that they address visual
The neuromuscular system controlling movements neglect appropriately during intervention. Specific
of the eyes functions to control reflexes (e.g., changes means of doing this are addressed in Chapter 18.
in pupil dilation and curvature of the lens), rotation
of the eyes within their sockets to allow fixation from
point to point so that a person may look at things What Are Higher-Level Visual Deficits?
(saccadic eye movements), and following of moving
targets with the eyes (pursuit movements). Periph-
eral nerve damage may affect ocular movements. Lesions in the visual association areas and their pro-
Eye-movement problems sometimes co-occur with jections may lead to problems of higher-level visual
cognitive-linguistic disorders such that it is import- processing and integration of visual information.
ant for the clinician to be aware of them and to refer Examples are the following:
people suspected of having them for a full evalua-
tion by a neuroophthalmologist. • Dyslexia, an impairment in understanding
written materials. Dyslexia has varied forms,
including surface dyslexia, an impairment in
visual decoding of graphemes (printed units of
What Are Visual Attention Deficits?
meaning, such as letters), and deep dyslexia,
an impairment in higher-level interpretation
In addition to visual field defects and deficits in visual and understanding of written words.
acuity, many people with neurogenic cognitive-​ • Apperceptive agnosia, the inability to
linguistic disorders also have neglect of the visual recognize an object. This may be tactile,
fields. People with visual neglect are able to “see” the olfactory, visual, auditory, or gustatory (often
visual world in front of them in the sense that neu- characterized according to the modality
rological impulses carrying visual information from affected, e.g., visual agnosia, the inability to
the retina are received in the occipital lobe. However, recognize an object, image, or written word
they do not, or are not able to, attend to a portion of the even though one can see it).
visual space such that they do not know that they see it. • Associative agnosia, a failure to associate
Visual neglect is most typical in the visual field contra- meaning to what is seen (e.g., an object’s
lateral to the site of lesion. Given that a person with a relevance and function).
neurogenic language disorder is most likely to have a • Prosopagnosia, an impairment in the ability
left hemisphere lesion, if they have visual neglect, it is to recognize faces.
most likely of the right hemispace. However, reported • Optic aphasia, an impairment in naming an
incidence of visual neglect is actually higher in people object presented visually, despite being able to
with right compared to left hemisphere lesions. describe the object.
A challenge in working with people who neglect • Visuoconstructive deficits, problems
part of the visual space is that they are typically with being able to process two- or three-
unaware of the problem and thus do not adjust for dimensional relationships in space.
it. One way visual neglect might be noticed is when
a person eats only half of a meal placed before her Visuoconstructive deficits may be seen in a per-
on a tray, not because she is not interested in items son’s inability to re-create a pattern, such as copying
on the other side but simply because she does not a drawing, or assembling blocks in a pattern that
know they are there. Another clue might be when matches a preassembled set of blocks. They may also
a person ignores visitors on one side of a room, ori- be seen as topographic disorientation, the inability
entating to and speaking with only the ones on the to interpret visuospatial relationships such as those
nonneglected side. Because visual neglect affects needed for using a map, or following a set of driving
interpersonal communication as well as reading, or walking directions.
94  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Exercises to help foster learning and reflection auditory processing problems. The latter category
about the visual system with special relevance to is fundamental to the understanding of most neu-
aphasiology are given in the Learning and Reflection rogenic language disorders; higher-level auditory
Activities section. Take care to study any related con- processing problems are often part of the nature of
tent you have not already mastered and to consider aphasia and related disorders, not just concomitant
how you may be likely to use and talk about that conditions. Auditory agnosia, a challenge with
information in your clinical work. recognizing or interpreting sounds, may manifest as
auditory sound agnosia or auditory verbal agno-
sia. Although hearing problems are a more central
What Aspects of the Neurophysiology component of the scope of practice of audiologists
of Hearing Are Most Relevant to People than aphasiologists, cortical-level addressing prob-
lems related to the linguistic processing of auditory
With Neurogenic Language Disorders?
information is well within the scope of SLPs.
As acoustic information is processed at higher
People with brain-based cognitive-linguistic disor- levels from the brainstem and beyond, there is
ders commonly have one or more types of hearing greater interaction among components and greater
and auditory processing deficits. Just as vision and contralateral representation of auditory input. The
visual processing abilities are key to much of the fact that there is greater contralateral than ipsilat-
work that is typically done in research and clinical eral representation of sound at cortical levels is the
practice in aphasiology, so are hearing and auditory reason for what is known as the right ear advan-
processing abilities. For most people, the auditory tage for speech processing. That is, listeners who
modality is key to socialization and everyday com- are left-brain dominant for language tend to process
munication and thus to quality of life. Fortunately, linguistic stimuli with greater efficiency when the
most SLPs have formal background in at least basic information is presented through the right as com-
aspects of audiology such that they tend to know pared to the left ear. This fact is not always function-
more about the nature of hearing and auditory ally relevant, in that the advantage tends to be small,
processing than they do about vision. Many have and most everyday communication is binaural.
experience with at least rudimentary aspects of the The auditory association area, surrounding
screening for hearing disorders as well as experience Heschl’s gyri, functions to identify and recognize
collaborating with audiologists to address clinical sounds. From there, basic sound information is inte-
and research questions relevant to people with neu- grated with input from other cortical and subcortical
rogenic communication disorders. Thus, we engage structures to associate sounds with semantic asso-
here in only a brief review of important aspects of ciations and interpretations. The multimodal inte-
neurophysiology of hearing and auditory processing gration of sounds with other types of stimuli, along
as they tend to be affected in people with acquired with influences of linguistic, paralinguistic, and
neurogenic communication disorders. social contexts, is not easily localizable within spe-
The basic anatomical components involved in cific structures. For this reason, many psycholinguis-
auditory linguistic processing and their primary tic and neurolinguistic models of auditory language
functions, along with deficits that might result from processing in the brain (introduced in Chapter 4)
problems with each component, are summarized in depend on the association of constructs with stages
Table 7–1. From the cochlea to the primary cortex, of processing that might not be associated to spe-
tonotopic organization of the frequency of sounds cific structures in a definitive way. We consider rel-
is preserved. That is, neurons contiguous with one evant aspects of hearing as they relate to aging in
another represent frequencies closest to one another; Chapter 9. We address the varied types of auditory
the further apart auditory fibers are, the more distant processing deficits associated with categories of lan-
the frequencies of the acoustic information they convey. guage disorders in Section III. Basic means of tak-
Auditory problems may be generally catego- ing hearing and auditory processing problems into
rized as auditory acuity deficits, problems of pitch account when thinking critically about language-
or tone perception, auditory attention deficits, defi- specific problems in an assessment context are
cits in sound localization, and higher-level “central” reviewed in Chapter 18.
7. Neurophysiology and Neuropathology of Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders   95

Table 7–1. Basic Components and Associated Functions and Potential Deficits in Auditory
Linguistic Processing

Anatomical Component Function Associated Potential Deficits


Outer ear, tympanic Air and bone conduction, mechanical Ipsilateral conductive hearing loss
membrane, middle ear transmission of sound waves
(ossicles to round window)
Middle ear (cochlea, hair Hydraulic converted to electrical Ipsilateral sensorineural hearing
cells, spiral ganglia) transmission of sound loss, loudness recruitment, reduced
speech discrimination
Vestibulocochlear nerve Transmission of electroacoustic signal Ipsilateral sensorineural hearing
to brainstem loss, loudness recruitment, reduced
speech discrimination
Brainstem cochlear nuclei, Relay of sound information to other Ipsilateral or bilateral sensorineural
superior olivary nuclei, brainstem structures and thalamus hearing loss, loudness recruitment,
lateral lemniscus, inferior reduced speech discrimination,
colliculus impaired reflexes that require
auditory-visual integration, reduced
auditory attention
Thalamus (medial Relay of sound information to Reduced auditory attention, reduced
geniculate body) multiple cortical and subcortical areas speed of processing, difficulty
processing pitch and rhythm
Primary auditory cortex Initial interpretation of speech sounds Auditory agnosia, pure word
(Heschl’s gyri, superior deafness, auditory comprehension
temporal convolution) deficits
Auditory association areas Phonemic analysis and interpretation Auditory-visual dissociation, auditory
comprehension deficits
Multiple cortical and Parallel and distributed processes Difficulty with multimodal integration
subcortical structures integrating multimodal sensory of linguistic with nonlinguistic
information with top-down linguistic, information, auditory comprehension
cognitive, social, and environmental deficits, challenges with abstraction
influences; elaboration, synthesis, and synthesis
and abstraction influenced by auditory
information processing

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. Make a list of bolded terms used in this 3. How does the neurophysiological principle
chapter that are new to you or that you have of “interconnectivity” throughout the brain
not yet mastered. Practice defining them in challenge the structure-function correlates?
your own words. 4. Use the structures, landmarks, and concepts
2. What are inter- and intrahemispheric listed in Box 7–1 as a basic checklist for
specializations of the brain, and how are evaluating your basic knowledge of
they relevant to clinical aphasiology? neuroanatomy and neurophysiology
96  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

related to clinical practice with people who than just about structures and functions
have acquired neurogenic communication associated with language and speech?
disorders. Rate yourself using a rating scale 6. Describe examples of specialization of
of your choosing (e.g., 1 to 10, 1 to 3, plus or structure and function in the brain.
minus). 7. Define and give examples of inter- and
a. How do you fare? intrahemispheric specialization.
b. What specific content do you need to 8. In what ways might a clinical aphasiologist
review or study further? discuss the principle of interconnectivity in
c. What steps will you take to fill in areas the brain when counseling people who have
of knowledge about neuroanatomy and recently had a stroke or brain injury?
neurophysiology pertinent to clinical 9. How is neuroplasticity related to
aphasiology? Review the supplemental spontaneous recovery?
review contents at the end of these 10. How is neuroplasticity related to professional
learning and reflection activities. treatment dedicated to language recovery?
Complete any items that would help 11. How is knowing about prognostic factors
you in terms of filling in your basic related to stroke and brain injury vital
knowledge in this area. Additional in counseling people with neurogenic
materials and helpful web links to communication disorders?
support your studies are available on the 12. When considering a given stroke or brain
companion website. injury survivor’s prognosis for language
5. Why is it important for clinical recovery, what are the limitations of
aphasiologists to know more about interpreting findings from a given research
neuroanatomy and neurophysiology study about prognostic factors?

notes or diagrams. Then do the same again


Supplemental Review of Neuroanatomy by covering all but the Brodmann’s area
Related to Aphasiology column. Next, try it once more looking only
at the associated abilities or functions.
1. What gyri and sulci demarcate each of the 3. In general, what is the relationship between the
lobes of the brain? Be sure that you can identify site of lesion and whether an individual will
these, where possible, on superior, lateral, and have ipsilateral versus contralateral deficits in
medial images of the left hemisphere. motor control or sensation?
2. Print or view online various images of the 4. Describe in basic terms the nervous system’s
brain showing medial, superior, and ventral logical organization of information pertaining
views. to sight (retinotopic organization), sound
a. Identify each of the structures in the table (tonotopic organization), and sensation and
in the next section on as many of the views motor control (somatotopic organization).
as appropriate for that structure.
b. Complete the empty fields in the table.
Note that some structural areas listed Supplemental Review of Blood
have multiple associated Brodmann’s area
Supply to the Brain
numbers.
c. Once you have completed the table,
practice covering the content in all columns 1. With a partner, describe the course of the
but the “structure” column, and see if you major arteries from the heart to the brainstem,
can fill in the rest without looking at any cerebellum, and cortex.
7. Neurophysiology and Neuropathology of Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders   97

Classically
Associated Lobe(s) Where
Structure or Brodmann’s the Structure Is Classically Associated Abilities
Structural Area Area(s) Located or Functions
Primary motor strip
(or cortex)
Premotor strip (or
cortex)
Primary sensory area
(or cortex)
Sensory association
area (or cortex)
Broca’s area

Wernicke’s area

Perisylvian area

Auditory association
area (or cortex) or
Heschl’s gyrus
Primary visual area
(or cortex)
Prefrontal cortex

Be sure to include how blood reaches each 4. What is the circle of Willis?
of the functional areas of speech, language, a. What are its key components that are most
hearing, vision, and motor control of the relevant to the blood supply to the parts
body. As you mention these functional of the brain that are most essential for
areas, practice your use of neuroanatomical communication?
information in context by referring to the b. How is the functioning of the circle of
lobes, major gyri and sulci, and other relevant Willis relevant to clinical practice in
neuroanatomical structures, and include aphasiology?
references to corresponding Brodmann’s areas. 5. Draw the circle of Willis several times, and
Use illustrations, pointing out key structures to make sure that you are able to label its key
enrich your explanation. components. Do this until you can do it
2. Now, do this again, this time drawing your correctly without looking at any notes or
illustrations yourself freehand to support your diagrams.
explanation. 6. Make a three-dimensional model of the circle
3. What is an anastomosis? What is a watershed of Willis. Get creative. Wonderful replicas may
region? How do anastomoses and watershed be made of clay, aluminum foil, wire, cardboard
regions help protect the brain during a stroke? tubing, straws, and red licorice. Use your
98  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

imagination. Label the key components. Work 3. With a partner, read aloud the subsection of
with a partner and describe to one another the this chapter under the heading “How are
key components, where blood enters (from the visual deficits categorized?” Before you move
heart) and exits (toward the brain), and how on to each subsequent sentence, be sure you
anastomosis functions in the circle of Willis. understand what you have just read. Try
Share strategies for remembering the layout of paraphrasing any content that is new to you to
its arteries and the areas of the central nervous be sure that you grasp it.
system that they supply. 4. Draw a diagram to illustrate the visual
7. What are common functional deficits that result pathway identifying the optic nerve, optic
from disruptions in the blood supply to the chiasm, optic tract, lateral geniculate body, and
cortex from visual cortex. Then use your diagram to discuss
a. The right and left anterior cerebral arteries the visual field representation at the level of
b. The right and left middle cerebral arteries each of these major structures.
c. The right and left posterior cerebral arteries 5. Imagine a person with bitemporal
8. Imagine you are counseling a person who has hemianopsia. What visual fields are affected?
had a stroke affecting the left anterior cerebral Where do you think her lesion is likely to be
artery. Explain to him how the stroke might be located?
associated with certain functional deficits that 6. A person with Wernicke’s aphasia may
he may be having. To illustrate your key points, exhibit no vision in the right half of both
draw freehand sketches of lateral and medial visual fields.
views of the brain and illustrate possible a. Name this deficit.
areas of blood supply disruption that might b. What site of lesion is commonly implicated?
have occurred. Note that referring to images 7. Create your own diagram of the visual system
with motor homunculi overlaid on key brain and the visual fields and illustrate
structures might be helpful in this exercise. a. Monocular blindness
a. Repeat this, now imagining that you are b. Bitemporal (heteronymous) hemianopsia
counseling a person who has had a stroke c. Nasal hemianopsia
affecting the left middle cerebral artery. d. Homonymous hemianopsia
Note that referring to images with sensory e. Upper left quadrantopsia
homunculi overlaid on key brain structures f. Lower left quadrantopsia
might be helpful in this exercise. 8. List and describe examples of “higher-level”
b. Repeat this, now imagining that you are visual processing problems.
counseling a person who has had a stroke 9. What are some special challenges in language
affecting the left posterior cerebral artery. intervention for people with visual neglect?
In this case, referring to sensory or motor
homunculi may be irrelevant.
Supplemental Review of the Auditory System

Supplemental Review of the Visual System


1. List and describe five general categories of
auditory deficits common in people with
1. List and describe four general categories of brain-based communication disorders.
visual deficits common in people with brain- 2. Draw a diagram to illustrate the main
based communication disorders. auditory pathways and structures. On it,
2. Why is it important to explore patterns of identify the outer ear, middle ear, inner ear,
visual perception deficits and visual attention vestibulocochlear nerve, brainstem structures,
problems of people with neurogenic language thalamus, primary auditory cortex, and
disorders? auditory association areas. Then use your
7. Neurophysiology and Neuropathology of Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders   99

diagram to discuss the type of auditory adults with acquired cortical-level auditory
problems a person might have if they had a linguistic processing deficits?
lesion at each level.
www
3. In what ways might SLPs and audiologists Please see the companion website for additional
collaborate in addressing challenges faced by resources.
CHAPTER
8
Neuroimaging and Other
Neurodiagnostic Instrumentation

In this chapter, we address basic information about (MRA), event-related potentials (ERPs), and elec-
common neurodiagnostic techniques used in work trocorticography. Here we briefly review how each
with people who have neurogenic communication of these functions and how each might be used to
disorders. We broadly categorize these as neuro- gain insights about acquired neurogenic language
imaging techniques and “other” forms of instru- disorders. There is ample literature available to sup-
mentation-based methods. Although much of what port further learning of those with special interests
speech-language pathologists (SLPs) do in everyday in applying any of these and other techniques.
practice is not necessarily based on the use of such
instrumentation, it is important for the excellent cli-
nician and clinical researcher to have an appropriate Computed Axial Tomography (CAT or CT)
level of sophistication for reading research articles
and medical charts to support work as a savvy inter- Also called x-ray computed tomography, computed
professional rehabilitation team member. axial tomography (CAT or CT) measures energy
After reading and reflecting on the content in transmission through tissue. Although it entails
this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in exposure to radiation, it is one of the most common
your own words, the following queries: imaging technologies available because it may be
used quickly, is relatively inexpensive, and allows
1. What are the most relevant neuroimaging a view of gross brain structures. CT entails quickly
techniques for aphasiologists to know about? rotating narrow x-ray beams detected by CT. An
2. What other neurodiagnostic methods are x-ray tube, as shown in Figure 8–1, actually circles
important for aphasiologists to know about? around the head. As the x-ray photons are trans-
mitted through tissue, the tissue through which
they course attenuates them to some degree. The
What Are the Most Relevant Neuroimaging degree of that attenuation is measured by contrast-
ing the initial x-ray intensity with its postabsorp-
Techniques for Aphasiologists to Know About?
tion strength and is expressed as a fraction or linear
attenuation coefficient. By taking a series of x-rays
Neuroimaging techniques pertinent to aphasiology at many angles at progressive locations throughout
include computed axial tomography (CAT or CT), the brain, images of slices of brain tissue are recon-
magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), functional structed, reflecting the relative density of tissue
magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), positron emis- throughout. High density is seen as lighter, or white,
sion computed tomography (PET), single photon and low density as darker. The thickness of slices
emission computed tomography (SPECT), cerebral may be adjusted by varying the thickness of the
angiography, magnetic resonance angiography x-ray beam.

101
102  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Figure 8–1. CT scanner. Source: Shutterstock. A full-color version of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.

CT has been used for almost four decades to stroke for diagnosing whether a stroke is hemor-
study brain-behavior relationships in acquired com- rhagic or occlusive. This is critical because a person
munication disorders and has been used to confirm experiencing a hemorrhage should not be adminis-
original multidimensional models of aphasia clas- tered thrombolysis following stroke.
sification that were based on postmortem studies CT perfusion studies entail the use of enhanced
of people who had exhibited various symptoms of contrast intravascular tracers to enable indexing of
aphasia and related disorders. CT is especially use- cerebral blood volume, cerebral blood flow, and the
ful for detecting subarachnoid hemorrhage, necrotic speed of perfusion. CT has great utility for study-
infarction, trauma, edema, cysts, and excessive pro- ing changes in perfusion following stroke and brain
duction or blocking of cerebrospinal fluid that com- injury and differences in perfusion associated with
presses brain tissue. It is not ideal for detecting small certain pharmacological or behavioral interventions
infarcts and/or acute infarcts that are not hemor- meant to reduce the degree of permanent impact
rhagic. In Figure 8–2, see an example of a CT image of stroke. Although CT entails radiation and is not
of an intracranial hemorrhage.. ideal for detecting acute lesions, it is less costly than
Enhanced CT, incorporating radiopaque dye other imaging techniques, such as MRI. Also, it is
injected into the arteries to enhance the degree of not as sensitive as MRI to motion artifacts, which is
x-ray attenuation, shows where there is bleeding in especially important for use with people who cannot
the brain. CT is important immediately following or will not remain still.
8. Neuroimaging and Other Neurodiagnostic Instrumentation   103

within the magnetic field. When they do this, they


release RF energy, which is recorded via a receiving
coil. Two- or three-dimensional images are created
based on relative degrees of RF signal at each point
in space. Given that bones contain little water, they
produce fewer MRI signals. An example of an MRI
scanner is shown in Figure 8–3.
MRI provides better resolution than CT, does
not entail radiation, and is better than CT for imag-
ing the effects of ischemic strokes. MRI can be used
to detect axonal injury and by-products of intracra-
nial hemorrhage within hours after a brain injury,
so it may be helpful in assessing prognosis and
changes over time in survivors of traumatic brain
injury (Massaad et al., 2021). Challenges with MRI
are primarily that it is highly sensitive to motion,
which can be a problem with fidgety or uncooper-
ative people, and that it cannot be used around any
type of metal, precluding use with people who have
metallic implants. Sometimes placing a person in an
MRI scanner, especially if they are claustrophobic,
poses stress that is not warranted by an increase in
Figure 8–2. CT image. This CT sample shows the detection over CT (Myburgh, 2009).
space-occupying nature and resultant midline shift of Varied levels of proton relaxation are used
brain tissue due to an intracranial hemorrhage. Left and in MRI. T1-weighted images, more sensitive to
right are reversed, as they are with most neuroimaging
lipids, provide better gray versus white matter
scans. Source: “Intracranial bleed with significant mid-
line shift” by James Heilman, licensed under CC BY-SA
contrast, enabling greater anatomical resolution,
3.0. but do not reflect edema and infarcts very well.
T2-weighted images, sensitive to water molecule
contrasts, are more sensitive to pathology, such as
edema and ischemia. When used with contrastive
dyes, vascular changes may be highlighted. Exam-
Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) ples of T1- and T2-weighted images are given in
Figure 8–4.
MRI makes use of an applied magnetic field around Diffusion MRI, also called diffusion tensor
the head and brief and repeated bursts of radiof- imaging (DTI), is the detecting and mapping of the
requency (RF) wave exposure. Spinning hydrogen diffusion of water molecules within myelinated fiber
protons within water-rich living tissue, each having tracks. Thus, it is sensitive to pathology in associa-
positive and negative aspects, constantly convey tion fibers in the brain. Fiber tracking, or tractogra-
magnetic signals. For an MRI of the brain, the per- phy, is a DTI technique that can be applied to study
son’s head is placed within a magnetic field. This the course and nature of specific nerve fiber bundles.
causes the hydrogen protons to align rather than Tracks are color coded to reflect whether they run
continue to spin randomly. Then an RF signal is in anterior-posterior, left-right, or superior-infe-
emitted in repeated short pulses, exciting the hydro- rior directions (Figure 8–5). DTI is likely to become
gen protons. Protons associated with different tissue increasingly vital in neurolinguistic studies aimed at
types and immersed in differing chemical environ- distinguishing models of specialized brain centers
ments align differently. When the RF signal stops, versus distributed processes controlled by connec-
the protons return to their previous orientation tions among varied brain regions.
104  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Figure 8–3. MRI scanner. Source: “Vantage Atlas MRI scanner from Toshiba” by Sergey Kiselev, licensed under
CC BY 4.0. A full-color version of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.

MRI diffusion-weighted imaging (DWI) is a ences among protons is used to enhance contrasts that
means of indexing the rate of water diffusion within enable evaluation of active or acute stroke processes.
voxels (specific units of magnetic resonance images). It can detect the effects of ischemia within minutes. It
DWI is better than T1- and T2-weighted MRI at may also be used to study blood flow in and around
detecting areas of acute infarction soon after stroke brain tumors and to help discern between neoplasm
onset, so is especially helpful in diagnosing ischemic and necrotic tissue due to radiation. Given that PWI is
strokes sooner. DWI may be used to detect an infarct sensitive to areas of hypoperfusion contiguous with
within 15 to 20 minutes; MRIs tend not to detect an areas of infarction, it helps to determine areas with
infarct until 4 to 6 hours poststroke, and CTs require potential for improved blood flow in the penumbra
36 to 48 hours. (the area tissue surrounding an infarct). This is vital
Perfusion weighted imaging (PWI) is a method in that the volume of hypoperfused tissue has been
of indexing microscopic levels of blood flow. A special shown to be a better predictor of language recovery
contrast medium that is sensitive to specific differ- than the volume of actual lesions (Hillis et al., 2000).
8. Neuroimaging and Other Neurodiagnostic Instrumentation   105

B
Figure 8–4. Examples of T1- and T2-weighted magnetic resonance images. Left and
right are reversed, per standard practice. Axial T1 (left) and T2 (right) images of two
separate individuals. The top images (A) show slices of the temporal and occipital lobes
of a man with chronic Wernicke’s aphasia due to stroke. The bottom images (B) show
slices of the frontal and parietal lobes in a woman with chronic anomic aphasia. Images
courtesy of Dr. Julius Fridriksson, University of South Carolina.
106  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Figure 8–5. Diffusion tensor image showing a left lateral view of the brain.
Tractography shows anterior-posterior, left-right, and superior-inferior nerve
fiber bundles. Source: Shutterstock. A full-color version of this figure can be
found in the Color Insert.

Functional MRI (fMRI) is a means of indexing ciated with specific functions. fMRI enables moni-
dynamic changes in blood flow as indicated by vary- toring of hemodynamic changes during stroke and
ing levels of oxygen in the brain. As such, it is not brain injury recovery and of changes in brain tumor
a direct measure of neuronal activity; it is a means growth or reduction. Research aphasiologists use
of quantifying hemodynamic changes associated fMRI to study not only neuropathology and sponta-
with active metabolism during ongoing neuronal neous aspects of recovery associated with commu-
activity. The activity of hemoglobin is differentiated nication disorders but also basic aspects of normal
from deoxyhemoglobin within the magnetic field. cognitive and linguistic processing (Barberini et al.,
As one or more specific areas of the brain are acti- 2017; Crosson et al., 2007; Fridriksson et al., 2010;
vated to perform particular cognitive, linguistic, Saur, 2006; Saur et al., 2006; Scheibel, 2017; Thomp-
or motor tasks, more oxygen is consumed in those son & den Ouden, 2008).
areas, resulting in the release of greater amounts Positron emission computed tomography (PET).
of deoxyhemoglobin. Deoxyhemoglobin generates PET enables studies of regional cerebral blood flow
greater inflow of oxygenated blood to the activated (rCBF). It works by detecting radioisotopes (often
area within about 3 to 6 seconds, which is indexed radioactive oxygen) injected into the bloodstream as
as the blood oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) they travel through the brain. Given that blood flow
effect. Repeated sets of images are taken as partic- increases to more active areas of the brain, greater
ipants rest (at baseline) and as they engage in spe- absorption of oxygen in those areas is contrasted
cific tasks. Indices from baseline are contrasted with with areas consuming lower levels of oxygen. Using
task-associated indices to suggest cortical areas asso- different types of radioisotopes, glucose metabolism
8. Neuroimaging and Other Neurodiagnostic Instrumentation   107

may also be studied. PET images are coded in colors. metabolic changes associated with the progression
Shades from dark to light red indicate the highest of neurodegenerative disease.
degree of metabolism, then shades of orange, yel- PET can also be helpful for showing differences
low, green, blue, and black. A strength of PET is in regions of the brain activated during specific
that, even when there is no evidence of structural types of tasks. For example, PET has been useful
damage, changes in metabolism may be detected. for demonstrating that various areas of the brain
See Figure 8–6 for an fMRI image showing reduced other than those transitionally associated with spe-
metabolism in a person with primary progressive cific speech and language functions are involved in
aphasia (PPA) whose MRI image was reported to be language processing. See, for example, Figure 8–8.
normal, including no evidence of atrophy. See Fig- However, PET has been shown to have poor resolu-
ure 8–7 for examples of how PET images can depict tion and measurement accuracy in people abusing

Figure 8–6. PET imaging example of functional changes. Left and right are reversed in the
images. This series of fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG) PET images shows reduced metabolism (shown
by arrows pointing toward areas of reduced activation in the left superior temporal, inferior pari-
etal, and lateral thalamic regions) in a person with primary progressive aphasia whose MRI was
reported to be normal. Image courtesy of Dr. Sultan Tarlaci, Sifa University, Turkey. A full-color
version of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.
108  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Figure 8–7. PET images associated with neurodegenerative changes. Left: Normal FDG-PET
activity. Center: Metabolic changes in mild cognitive impairment. Right: The brain of a person
with Alzheimer’s disease. Image courtesy of Drs. Susan M. Landau and William J. Jagust, Uni-
versity of California Berkeley. A full-color version of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.

Figure 8–8. PET images associated with speech and language tasks. Each image of the left
lateral view of the brain shows a typical activation pattern during a linguistic task. The image
on the upper left is associated with reading, the upper right with listening to words, the lower
left with thinking about words, and the lower right with saying words. Image courtesy of Marcus
E. Raichle, Washington University School of Medicine. A full-color version of this figure can be
found in the Color Insert.
8. Neuroimaging and Other Neurodiagnostic Instrumentation   109

drugs and taking neurotropic medications. Also, Cerebral Angiography


interpretation in traumatic brain injury (TBI) survi-
vors is not well standardized (Granachar, 2003; Shin Cerebral angiography (or arteriography) helps deter-
et al., 2017). mine the extent of vascular problems within cerebral
blood vessels (see Georgeadis et al., 2015). It allows
visualization of the arterial blood supply to the cor-
Single Photon Emission Computed tex and the degree of collateral circulation occurring
Tomography (SPECT) in cases of occlusion. While the patient is under local
anesthesia, a catheter is placed in the femoral artery
Like PET, single photon emission computed tomog- and extended up into the carotid or vertebral artery.
raphy (SPECT) makes use of intravenously injected A contrast medium is injected through the catheter,
radioisotopes, but the effects last longer. It detects and then x-rays are taken to show the contrast as
diffuse and focal damage and is useful for differ- it courses through arteries, then capillaries, then
entiating stroke from other types of brain pathol- veins. Carotid artery injection allows visualization
ogy, such as neurodegenerative disease. It has been of the anterior and middle cerebral arteries and
shown to be more sensitive than CT or MRI for their extensions, whereas injection into the vertebral
detecting lesions subsequent to TBI, including small artery enables visualization of the posterior cerebral
lesions not seen via static neuroimaging and lesions arteries (Figure 8–9). Cerebral angiography is espe-
at earlier acute stages (Meller et al., 2008). cially important for identifying cerebral aneurysms,

Figure 8–9. Cerebral angiography image


featuring the posterior portion of the circle
of Willis. Left and right are reversed. Before
this image was taken, a contrast medium
had been injected into the left vertebral
artery (the large artery on the right). The
fact that the contrast is seen not only in
the left but also the contralateral (right)
vertebral artery demonstrates that there
has been retrograde blood flow through
the posterior communicating artery at the
base of the circle of Willis. Source: “Cere-
bral angiography, arteria vertebralis sinister
injection” by Lypothymia, licensed under
CC BY 3.0.
110  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

arteriovenous malformation, and tumors within the each of the four primary cortical lobes. An example
vascular system. It has been shown not to be so effec- is shown in Figure 8–10. The number of electrodes
tive in detecting shallow ulcerating lesions. varies widely, typically from 19 to 256.
One form of angiography is sodium amytal Where dendrites are more highly concentrated,
infusion, also known as the Wada test. It entails the there are greater voltages generated. Differences
injection of amobarbital (an anesthetic), diluted with in electrical potential between any two electrodes
saline solution, into the carotid artery to enable deter- correspond to cortical brain waves. EEG indexes
mination of hemispheric dominance for language activity summed across thousands (sometimes even
(discussed in Chapter 7). When it reaches brain tissue, millions) of neurons. Comparisons may be made
sodium amytal renders whatever tissue it reaches between specific areas within either hemisphere and
nonfunctional for a brief period of up to 10 minutes. between corresponding functional areas in each of
The person under study is asked to extend their arms the two hemispheres. Brain wave symmetry, ampli-
and fingers and to count backward from 10. For most tude, and timing are all important aspects of brain
people, injection on the left side leads to an almost wave monitoring.
immediate ceasing of contralateral motor control of EEG is most frequently used in studies of brain
the arms and fingers and cessation of speech. Sodium activity of people with epilepsy and is helpful in
amytal infusion is especially critical for planning the differential diagnosis of people with suspected
surgical management of epilepsy so that language- metabolic (or toxic) encephalopathy. EEG is key in
dominant areas can be strategically spared. evaluation of basic cortical functioning in people
CT angiography (CTA) is increasingly used to who are nonresponsive, especially those who may
measure stenosis or occlusion of the carotid arter- be vegetative or comatose. Absence of brain activity
ies and aneurysms. A contrast medium that quickly is seen as flat lines on an EEG brain waves, suggest-
targets the arteries is imaged through fast dynamic ing brain death.
CT scanning. Software then enables two- and three-​ Although EEG has been used to study prog-
dimensional reconstructions of images. nosis of language recovery in aphasia due to stroke
Magnetic resonance angiography (MRA) is the (Nicolo et al., 2015; Szelies et al., 2002), it is not
use of MRI methods to image vascular functions commonly used for that purpose, given the relative
in the arterial system. It may be used, for example, merits of the more modern neuroimaging methods
to detect arterial stenosis (narrowing), aneurysms, such as MRI and CT (e.g., greater special resolution,
and occlusions. Magnetic resonance venography more precision, reduced setup time). Still, the por-
(MRV) is the use of MRI to image blood flow in the tability and relatively low cost of EEG make it more
venous system. accessible. Additionally, the fact that people under
study do not have to hold still, that EEG use does
not induce claustrophobia, and that EEG technology
What Other Neurodiagnostic Methods Are does not generate noise are all important additional
advantages compared to other neuroimaging meth-
Important for Aphasiologists to Know About?
ods. EEG may be used in conjunction with fMRI to
study the relationship between electrical activity (as
Electroencephalography (EEG) indexed by EEG) and metabolic activity (as indexed
by the BOLD effect).
EEG is a means of studying electrical potential dif- Event-related potentials (ERPs, also called
ferences between two or more points on the scalp. evoked potentials) entail the use of EEG during
It takes advantage of the fact that electrical activity specific cognitive, linguistic, or behavioral tasks and
is generated through the dynamic fluctuation of during any type of somatosensory, olfactory, visual,
voltage differences between the two ends of cortical or auditory stimulation. Depending on the focus
dendrites. EEG can be applied in research or diag- of the study, a sensory stimulus (often auditory in
nostic laboratories, as well as at bedside. Electrodes the realm of aphasiology) is presented repeatedly.
are placed in a prescribed pattern on the scalp over The electrical response to each stimulus is added
8. Neuroimaging and Other Neurodiagnostic Instrumentation   111

Figure 8–10. Example of scalp electrode setup for EEG. Source: “EEG
recording” by Petter Kallioinen, licensed under CC BY 3.0. A full-color ver-
sion of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.

across the repeated presentations, and the measures Electrocorticography


are then amplified, summated, and averaged. This
reduces artifacts of electrical activity that typically Electrocorticography, or cortical stimulation brain
co-occur with the activity in response to the inten- mapping, is the use of EEG intracranially. Electrodes
tional stimulation. Auditory evoked potentials can are placed directly on the surface of the brain, not
be used to identify problems in the auditory system on the scalp, as illustrated in Figure 8–11. Thus, it
and may also be used to determine approximate is a highly invasive procedure. Cortical mapping is
hearing thresholds in people who are nonresponsive most commonly used to avoid key areas of the brain
or otherwise difficult to test. vital to sensory, motor, cognitive, and linguistic func-
ERPs are used in experimental studies geared tions during surgical removal of brain tissue. It is
toward improved understanding of the timing of especially important for surgeries for treatment of
neural activity relative to specific tasks, such as lexi- epilepsy, tumors, and arteriovenous malformations.
cal processing or real-time sentence comprehension. Electrically stimulating specific areas of the brain
See Silkes and Anjum (2021) for a scoping review while the person under study remains awake (only
of research that includes the use of ERPs to study under local anesthesia) helps determine which spe-
aphasia; this has relevance to all acquired neuro- cific areas are associated with important functions
genic cognitive-linguistic challenges. Magneto- and thus should be avoided during surgery. It also
encephalography (MEG) is a means of recording helps determine the specific diseased tissue that is
ERPs in the brain in response to specific tasks and relatively safe to remove. Although external cortical
then mapping those ERPs onto magnetic resonance stimulation was used as early as the late 19th century
images to reflect cortical mapping of task-induced (Bhatnagar, 2013; Fritsch & Hitzig, 1870), extensive
brain functioning. brain mapping of sensory, speech, language, and
112  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Figure 8–11. Electrocorticography. M = motor; S = sensory. Numbers


correspond to standardized sites for electrode placement. Source: “Intra-
cranial electrode grid for electrocorticography” by Blausen.com staff,
licensed under CC BY 3.0. A full-color version of this figure can be found
in the Color Insert.

motor functions determined through intraoperative including comprehension, varied forms of attention,
cortical stimulation was first reported by Penfield and sematic priming, and working memory (Ablinger
Roberts in 1959 and since has been reported further in et al., 2014; Anjum & Hallowell, 2019; Binney et al.,
relation to people with aphasia and related disorders 2018; Cho & Thompson, 2010; Dickey et al., 2007;
(Bhatnagar & Andy, 1983; Bhatnagar et al., 2000). Hallowell, 1999, 2012; Hallowell et al., 2002; Heuer
& Hallowell, 2009, 2015; Heuer et al., 2017; Ivanova
& Hallowell, 2012; Mirman et al., 2011; Odekar et al.,
Additional Methods 2009; Yee et al., 2015). Eyetracking can also be com-
bined with EEG and fMRI in studies of challenging
Other instrumentation-based methods are used to constructs in people with acquired neurogenic com-
study language processing in the brain in less direct munication disorders.
ways than through neuroimaging. Eyetracking Pupillometry entails the measurement of pupil-
entails monitoring of the location and duration of lary diameter. When the myriad factors that influ-
eye fixations as people look at real-world scenes, ence pupil size are carefully controlled, pupillometry
objects, or computer-projected still images and vid- may be used to index cognitive effort (rather than
eos. It is used in conjunction with auditory and writ- just accuracy and reaction time) associated with
ten linguistic stimuli to index abilities that are often engaging in a certain task (Chapman & Hallowell,
hard to assess in people with neurological disorders, 2015, 2020, 2021).
8. Neuroimaging and Other Neurodiagnostic Instrumentation   113

Sensorimotor tracking entails having a person task. In addition, computerized applications for
engage in a sensorimotor task (such as following a indexing accuracy, reaction time, and speed of pro-
moving target with one’s finger as it moves about a cessing on numerous types of cognitive-linguistic
computer screen) while engaging in a cognitive or tasks are increasingly available and may be used in
linguistic task; changes in sensorimotor performance conjunction with neuroimaging data.
may indicate changes in the ease or difficulty of the

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. Review and define the bolded terms in this at various positions and angles via a
chapter that are new to you or that you have rotating x-ray tube
not yet mastered. 3. Describe why EEG might be used instead of
2. Match each of the methods listed with the MRI or CT to examine neuropathology, even
descriptions below. though the latter methods are more precise.
a. Computed axial tomography (CAT or CT) 4. What are the relative benefits of CT over
b. Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) MRI?
c. Functional MRI (fMRI) 5. What are the relative benefits of MRI over
d. Positron emission computed tomography CT?
(PET) 6. What are the relative strengths and
e. Single photon emission computed weaknesses of PET compared with CT and
tomography (SPECT) MRI?
f. Cerebral angiography 7. What is functional magnetic resonance
g. Electroencephalography (EEG) imaging (fMRI)?
• Brain imaging technique that makes use 8. How is indexing of hemodynamic changes
of an applied magnetic field around the relevant to cognitive and linguistic
head and brief and repeated bursts of functions?
radiofrequency (RF) wave exposure 9. Describe examples of the potential
• Means of indexing dynamic changes in use of tractography to study language
blood flow as indicated by varying levels comprehension and/or production.
of oxygen in the brain 10. How might an aphasiologist be involved
• Means of studying electrical potential in clinical or research studies involving
differences between two or more points intraoperative monitoring using
on the scalp electrocorticography?
• Following injection of radioisotopes 11. What are some potential combinations of
into the bloodstream, enables studies of imaging and other instrumentation-based
regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) methods that help elucidate language-
• Means of visualizing the arterial blood processing abilities in the brain?
supply to the cortex and the degree of 12. In what ways might information gained
collateral circulation occurring in cases of through neuroimaging be relevant to the
occlusion ICF?
• Technique for measuring energy
www
transmission through tissue, enabling See the companion website for additional learning
images of “slices” of brain tissue recorded and teaching materials.
CHAPTER
9
Aging, Which Is Not a Disorder, and
Its Relevance to Aphasiology

This final chapter in our section on Foundations 3. What is aging well?


for Considering Acquired Neurogenic Disorders is 4. How are demographic shifts in aging
offered in light of the fact that many people with populations relevant to SLPs?
aphasia and related disorders are “older.” There is a 5. What are normal changes in the brain as people
tremendous amount of system ageism in everyday age?
social cultures, health care systems, clinical practices, 6. What are positive aspects of the aging brain?
and even the literature by expert aphasiologists. 7. What are general guidelines for differentiating
Numerous sociocultural factors influence one’s per- normal from impaired language in older
ception of “age” and how one considers the cognitive adults?
and linguistic abilities of older people. Deep-seeded 8. What theories have been proposed to account
stereotypes, plus a great deal of misinformation and for cognitive-linguistic changes with aging?
poorly designed research regarding aging and older 9. What can be done to ensure the best
people, hamper approaches that promote aging as preservation of language abilities as people
an important component of development through age?
the life span. In this chapter, we consider what we 10. What is elderspeak, and how may we raise
mean by the construct of “aging,” demographic pat- awareness about it?
terns that make it especially compelling that speech-​ 11. What sensitivities related to ageism are
language pathologists (SLPs) learn about aging, important for aphasiologists to demonstrate?
means of differentiating normal from pathological
changes in the brain and in cognitive-linguistic abili-
ties, theories addressing cognitive-linguistic changes
What Is Aging?
with aging, and issues related to ageism and elder-
speak. All of these are vital topics for the excellent
clinician to know about and consider in pursing pos- When most of us think about the term aging, we
itive and empowering ways to serve and advocate think of older people (as in “the elderly,” a term
for older adults. I hope that you will consider not using to describe
After reading and reflecting on the content in people). In fact, we begin aging at the moment we
this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in are conceived. Aging is an ongoing process expe-
your own words, the following queries: rienced by all people at all times, no matter how
old they are. These points may seem obvious; still,
1. What is aging? they are important in the context of considering
2. What are key theories about aging that the influence of age on people’s abilities. A com-
are especially relevant to cognition and mon way of defining age is though chronological
communication? age: an index of how long a person has lived since

115
116  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

birth. Other means of defining aging are functional and logical reasoning), others have suggested a post-
in nature: formal operational stage in which reasoning becomes
more flexible and more meaningfully connected to
• biological age, an index of the functioning of life experience (Labouvie-Vief, 1984). During that
one’s bodily organs over time stage, older adults are said to improve in coping
• cognitive age, an index of how one’s with daily life challenges and changes, especially
intelligence, memory, and learning abilities those with enriched life experience and engagement
change over time in higher education (Dunn, 2011).
• psychological age, an index of how one’s The life-span model of postformal cognitive
personality changes over time development (Schaie, 2005; Schaie & Willis, 2002)
• social age, an index of aging according to entails seven stages, with only the first occurring
one’s social roles and according to changes in before adulthood. Stages are said to occur at vari-
one’s environment over time able rates and times, with observed traits emerging
differently according to individual differences. The
adult stages emphasize taking on of responsibilities,
What Are Key Theories About Aging shifting from foci on the self and family to commu-
That Are Especially Relevant to nity and society, and from professional to nonpro-
fessional activities. The last two stages emphasize
Cognition and Communication?
greater selectivity in activities due to increasingly
limited energy and the creation of legacies through
Biopsychosocial models of aging have gained favor stories and passing on of possessions. This model
in parallel to the increasing acceptance and inte- may help clinicians consider changes in communi-
gration of the World Health Organization (WHO) cation abilities and needs with age as not only due
models of disability and health among the health to changes in body structure and function but also
care professions. Such models emphasize the com- evolving life priorities.
plex interactions among biological, psychological, Heckhausen et al. (2010) motivational theory
and sociological factors that influence how people of life-span development focuses on adults’ highly
age. Our sense of identity according to these three individualized abilities to choose, adapt to, and
aspects of aging is seen as central to how we age, and pursue life changes and opportunities. Ideally,
it is considered flexible in that the way we view and aging involves gaining “self-regulatory skills” that
define ourselves evolves over time. “involve anticipating emergent opportunities for
Many of us prefer to emphasize the positive goal pursuit, activating behavioral and motivational
aspects of later life by referring to “adult develop- strategies of goal engagement, disengaging from
ment” and not just “aging” (Overton, 2010). For many goals that have become futile and/or too costly, and
decades, formal models of cognitive development replacing them” (p. 54). Considering this theory may
tended to focus on the period from birth through be helpful in terms of developing an appreciation
adolescence or early adulthood, not on the entire life for how older adults’ motivation to address concerns
span. Piaget’s (1936) popular stages of development, about health and communication may evolve in a
for example, are often still studied in current courses way that is consonant with their specific life circum-
on cognitive development. The stages are said to stances. It may also be helpful in considering ways
occur from birth to about 6 years, from about 7 to to support a given person in compensating for chal-
about 11 years, and from about 12 years “to adult- lenges, rather than simply dismissing them as prob-
hood.” Consider that the latest stage encompasses lems that are inherent in aging.
most of a typical individual’s life. It is a stage that
includes tremendous cognitive development.
More recently, theorists have suggested frame-
What Is Aging Well?
works and theory to support continued stages of
development in the adult years. Where Piaget’s stages
of development stop at the “formal operational stage” “Healthy aging” and “aging well” are terms that
(when a person learns to engage in abstract thought help us to embrace the importance of “culture,
9. Aging, Which Is Not a Disorder, and Its Relevance to Aphasiology   117

function, engagement, resilience, meaning, dignity, opportunities discussed in Chapter 1 are associated
and autonomy, in addition to minimizing disease” with expanding opportunities in contexts in which
(Freidman et al., 2018, p. 18). The construct of aging older people are served.
well is complex and multidimensional, including
body structure and function plus life participation
and its inherent environmental influences. Further- What Are Normal Changes in
more, the notion of aging well is influenced by cul-
the Brain as People Age?
tural factors across the globe. In a survey of studies
on “successful aging, “ Depp and Jeste (2006) noted
29 different definitions of that construct across 28 Changes that may be associated with aging are not
studies, plus tremendous variability in the propor- necessarily caused by aging. Teasing apart the role of
tion of populations said to be aging successfully. age versus illness and disability on human function-
Adding to the complexity of defining “aging well” ing is challenging. Many aspects of dysfunction ste-
is that having physical, cognitive, or communicative reotypically thought to decline with age (cognitive,
challenges does not preclude a fulfilling life in terms linguistic, and motor abilities) are actually the result
of meaning, engagement with others, learning, and of factors such as genetic predisposition, poor nutri-
resilience through hardships. tion, glucose fluctuation, lack of exercise, low levels
of social engagement, environmental contamination,
illness, and stress. Let’s review some of the key lit-
How Are Demographic Shifts in erature that highlights cognitive-communicative
Aging Populations Relevant to changes that have been purported to be attributable
to aging.
Clinical Aphasiologists?
There is a great deal of variability in structural
changes in the brain over time. The following gen-
The aging population is rising steadily in most of eral patterns — all having effects on cognition — have
the world. There are three primary reasons for this: been noted:
decreasing infant mortality, decreasing fertility, and
improved longevity. As of 2017, about 13% of the • neuron shrinkage and reduced dendritic
world’s population was estimated to be over age 60 branching leading to decreased brain
years; by 2050, about 25% will be (with the exception volume, beginning at about age 30 years and
of Africa, the region with the youngest age distri- continuing throughout life (Kramer et al.,
bution, still expected to continue to grow rapidly) 2006), accelerating after about age 70 years
(United Nations, 2017). The number of people aged (Christensen et al., 2008)
80 years or older globally is expected to triple by • atrophy, primarily in the frontal lobes and
2050 and to be seven times its 2017 level by 2100. hippocampus (Kramer et al., 2006)
One contributor to the expanding aging pop- • reduction in neurotransmitters (e.g.,
ulation is increased life expectancy. Globally, life acetylcholine and dopamine) (Christensen
expectancy from birth rose from age 65 years for et al., 2008)
men and 69 years for women in 2002 to 2005, to age • decreased white matter, especially in the
69 years for men and 73 years for women in 2010 to frontal lobes (Dennis & Cabeza, 2008)
2015. There are large disparities in life expectancy. • reduced cerebral blood flow (Christensen
For example, life expectancy averages 82 years for et al., 2008)
both sexes in Australia, Hong Kong, Iceland, Italy, • accumulation of amyloid beta or
Japan, Macao, Singapore, Spain, and Switzerland. amyloid plaques (without accompanying
In contrast, life expectancy is below 55 years in the neurofibrillary tangles associated with
Central African Republic, Chad, Cote d’Ivoire, Leso- Alzheimer’s disease)
tho, Nigeria, Sierra Leone, Somalia, and Swaziland
(United Nations, 2017). Given these demographic Many adults exhibiting such physiological
trends, it is essential that SLPs be well prepared to changes do not exhibit problems with cognitive or
work with older people. Many of the rich career linguistic abilities, whereas others do.
118  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

At a neurological level, vast differences across


individuals in terms of their intact abilities in the
What Are Positive Aspects of the Aging Brain?
face of measurable brain changes are likely due to
differences in their abilities to recruit other areas Typically, when we think of the aging brain, we think
of the brain to compensate for declining functional of an ongoing dissolution of structure and function
areas. For example, some older people demonstrate leading to more and more problems over time. It is
greater activation of bilateral brain regions while certainly true that as we live longer, we are prone to
completing complex cognitive tasks that tend to more challenges, some associated with normal aging
involve primarily one hemisphere in younger peo- processes and some merely because of our greater
ple, a phenomenon called hemispheric asymmetry likelihood of experiencing conditions that negatively
reduction in older adults (Dennis & Cabeza, 2008). affect our brains. However, it is also true that some
Likewise, some show more activation of the inferior brain changes over time lead to positive aspects of
frontal gyrus, the insula and frontal operculum, the aging. Consider the following, for example:
anterior cingulate cortex, the inferior parietal sulcus,
and medial and temporal hippocampal areas during • The ongoing storage of semantic, procedural,
a variety of word production, working-memory, and episodic memories, as well as the ability
mathematical, and logical problem-solving tasks to integrate and reflect on thematic elements of
(Cabeza, 2002; Fedorenko et al., 2012; Hoyau et al., stored long-term memories, leads to richness
2017; Woolgar et al., 2011). of life experience; such experience, in turn,
Individual differences may also pertain to dif- leads to one’s ability to better teach, mentor,
ferences in abilities that are valued and the degree guide, inspire, and even entertain others.
of effort one wishes to invest in performance to • For some people, age-related changes in
demonstrate one’s abilities. The results of several prefrontal and limbic interactions, along with
published studies support the notion that older hormonal changes in the brain, may also lead
adults maintain a reserve capacity, which supports to clearer balance of basic drives associated
the ability to perform in ways that are typically not with sexual pursuits, career ambition, greed,
tested or demonstrated. When motivated to perform and self-centeredness, thus allowing older
well and to improve performance through learning adults to shift priorities to deeper, more
and practice, one may achieve test results that sur- meaningful, and benevolent pursuits.
pass those that might be captured in typical clini- • Gradual gray and white matter necrosis
cal and research contexts (Friedman & Ryff, 2012; and loss of synaptic connections through
Hoyau et al., 2017; Kemper et al., 2011; Tucker-Drob decreased dendritic branching and reduced
& Salthouse, 2013). The fact that older adults per- production of some neurotransmitters
form as well as younger adults in semantic pro- may even be part of the brain’s way of
cessing tasks, despite evidence of clear neuronal increasingly specializing in areas of cognitive
changes associated with aging (Lacombe et al., 2015), and linguistic strength and gaining what is
provides further evidence of the role of reserve perceived as wisdom.
capacity.
In sum, as we age, our likelihood of acquiring
cognitive and language disorders increases. That Memory
is, the longer we live, the greater our likelihood of
stroke, brain injury, tumors, and diseases that might Abilities that tend to be especially robust in the
affect brain functioning. However, the greater sta- face of normal aging are semantic memory, proce-
tistical likelihood of acquired neurological problems dural memory (recall for how to accomplish specific
in older versus younger people should not be inter- tasks), and autobiographical memory (memory
preted to mean that cognitive and linguistic impair- about important aspects of one’s past). These aspects
ments associated with such acquired etiologies are are also often relatively well preserved in the face of
a part of the normal aging process. Aging is not a other types of memory impairment, such as those
disease or pathology. of people with various forms of dementia. Memory
9. Aging, Which Is Not a Disorder, and Its Relevance to Aphasiology   119

challenges associated with normal aging include • reduced verbal fluency, which is indexed by
minor, if any, impairments in working memory, the number of words fitting in a particular
episodic memory (recall of personal experiences), category (e.g., types of animals or furniture)
source memory (memory of where and how one or words beginning with a specified letter,
acquired knowledge or where and when a previous within a given time period
event took place), and short-term memory (recall of
recent events). Word-finding abilities tend to decline in the 30s
Many of the memory challenges that have been and continue to worsen across decades. The decline
reported to be associated with normal aging in the may accelerate in the 70s, especially in terms of nam-
published literature must be scrutinized carefully; a ing response times (Spieler & Balota, 2000).
lack of careful control in terms of participant inclu- Word-finding problems in older adults have
sion and exclusion criteria, stimulus design, and the to do with accessing words when formulating lan-
nature of tasks and measures used to detect mem- guage, not with recognizing words or loss of vocabu-
ory problems threatens the validity of many claims lary. An older person who has a difficult time coming
made about memory and aging. These same prob- up with the name of an object or person is unlikely
lems are important to consider during assessment of to have trouble identifying the correct name when
older adults, as we discuss in further detail in Sec- given two or more choices. In fact, vocabulary — or
tion VI. Further complicating efforts to study mem- knowledge of words and their meanings — actually
ory in older people is age-related identity threat, the tends to improve with age, at least until the early 70s
implicit or explicit belief that one will fail because one (Ben-David et al, 2015; Verhaeghen, 2003), and this is
is “old.” In social contexts where negative views of the case for individual words as well as words in sen-
memory and aging are readily expressed (i.e., where tence contexts (Thornton & Light, 2006). When older
jokes, insults, and derogatory terms about memory adults have trouble understanding complex spoken
and aging are common in everyday life), such beliefs sentences, such difficulty is associated more with
may be the reason that some older adults tend to challenges in perceptual processing of speech input
perform worse on memory tests (Armstrong et al., and working memory than it is with understanding
2017; Cuddy et al., 2009; Fiske, 2008; Nelson, 2008). of individual words (Federmeier et al., 2003). Chal-
Even when older adults do not perform worse than lenges at the phonological level seem to be at the heart
younger adults, they may be perceived by younger of word retrieval difficulties (Connor et al., 2004).
adults as less competent in those very tasks (Voss
et al., 2018).
Syntactic Processing

Word Finding Difficulties with syntactic processing are most read-


ily studied during auditory comprehension tasks.
Word-finding problems are one of the few consis- People of all ages tend to have more difficulty
tently reported linguistic challenges that correspond understanding long compared to short sentences
to increased age even when all other influences on and grammatically complex compared to simple
language and cognition are taken into account. sentences. Challenges with understanding long and
Overall, when compared with younger people, older complex sentences tend to increase with age; this is
people demonstrate the following: primarily attributable to declines in working mem-
ory (Byczewska-Konieczny & Kielar-Turska, 2017;
• more tip-of-the-tongue experiences (i.e., Caplan et al., 2011; Kemper & Sumner, 2001). Hav-
knowing that they know the word they want ing to hold both semantic meaning and syntactic
to say but not being able to come up with it) structure from the early part of a sentence in mem-
• slower response times during confrontational ory while continuing to process later parts of a sen-
naming (i.e., when shown a picture or object tence, and then integrate meaning and form from the
and asked to name it) entire sentence, is challenging. Although reducing
• less accuracy in confrontational naming background noise and other distractions facilitates
120  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

syntactic processing in people of all ages, it leads topsychological and social changes related to life pri-
even greater improvements in older people. Also, orities and interests than in light of actual linguistic
sometimes slowing speech rate, especially as long abilities per se. Based on a robust review of research
as the speech is not perceived as unnaturally slow, literature, Shadden (2011) summarized factors that
may facilitate comprehension. influence discourse performance as having to do
In terms of syntactic production, some research- with three aspects: emotional regulation, personal
ers report that older speakers tend to use fewer discourse goals, and the nature of specific discourse
complex syntactic structures than younger people. tasks. Emotional regulation influences discourse in
At the same time, sentence length is not necessarily that emotional themes tend to be increasingly elab-
reduced with age; this appears to be due to the fact orated, and emotional topics may be inserted with
that an individual’s vocabulary tends to have more greater frequency in conversations. The goals of dis-
of an effect on sentence length than does their use of course are said to evolve with an increasing desire to
complex syntax (Kemper & Sumner, 2001). engage in autobiographical storytelling and discus-
Reported research findings indicating age-​ sion of values as one ages.
related declines in sentence comprehension should The nature of specific discourse tasks also influ-
be interpreted carefully. Emerging research results ences discourse comprehension. Older people tend
suggest that experience-based knowledge, rooted in to demonstrate better understanding during narra-
content with which any given individual has par- tives (storytelling) than during expository speech
ticular familiarity or expertise, has an important (informative discourse for explaining ideas or pro-
influence on sentence comprehension performance cesses, recounting events, or defining constructs).
(DeDe, 2013). Of course, so do hearing loss and tin- In terms of discourse production, aging may have
nitus, both of which are increasingly common with positive influences. For example, listeners tend to
age but not central to language functioning per se. judge older adults’ discourse as clearer and more
interesting than that of younger speakers (Glisky,
2007; Kemper & Kemtes, 2000).
Reading and Writing Overall, older people tend to have more dis-
fluencies (i.e., pauses, interjections, revisions, and
In terms of language competence, reading and writ- repetitions) in their speech production in conver-
ing abilities tend to mirror those of listening and sation compared to younger people (Schiller et al.,
speaking as people age. For example, the word-​ 2007). One must be careful to interpret the cause of
finding issues described earlier may also be seen such disfluencies, which may be due to such diverse
during writing, and syntactic processing challenges factors as word-finding problems, attention prob-
may be apparent in reading as well as writing. Of lems, working memory limitations, or higher-level
course, reading is heavily influenced by visual acuity organizational demands. Discourse coherence, the
problems and visual processing deficits that increase ability to tie together elements of a story and main-
with age. Likewise, writing by hand or through typ- tain the thematic content, has been shown to decline
ing is influenced not only by visual changes but by with age; however, researchers who have demon-
motor control challenges that may increase with age. strated this may not have appropriately controlled
Changes in reading and writing ability with age are for underlying cognitive abilities in the participants
more likely to be due to sensory and motor deficits assessed.
than to linguistic factors per se, unless there are In general, the more difficult and complex the
mediating pathological conditions, which are typi- form and content of discourse are, whether spo-
cally not primarily due to aging. ken or written, the more marked differences there
are between older and younger people. However,
making judgments about any given individual’s
Discourse discourse competence requires a great deal more
consideration than simple conversational sampling,
The use of language in interaction with others (dis- especially within a single session. It is essential to
course), spoken and written, and expressive and consider their previous discourse abilities, education
receptive, tends to change with age more in light of level, vocabulary, interest in the subject matter, and
9. Aging, Which Is Not a Disorder, and Its Relevance to Aphasiology   121

degree of motivation to engage in a discourse task at about losing her car keys. “I know I had them this
any particular time. afternoon when I came home. Usually, I keep them
on this key rack. But today I must have forgotten,
Pragmatics and I have no idea where they are now.” One might
judge her to be distracted or disorganized but not
Pragmatic abilities, knowledge and skill in the social pathologically impaired.
use of language, are intricately intertwined with Now imagine an 85-year-old making the same
discourse abilities because both are carried out and statement. “I know I had them this afternoon when
observed in the context of written and oral interac- I came home. Usually, I keep them on this key rack.
tion. Examples are topic maintenance, turn taking, But today I must have forgotten, and I have no idea
use of prosody for emphasis, disambiguation, use of where they are now.” In this case, one might easily
and response to humor, and use and interpretation of conclude that the individual speaking has an age-
facial expressions and gestures. Aging in and of itself related memory problem. Our predisposition to think
does not appear to influence pragmatic abilities in that aging necessarily entails significant memory
significant ways, beyond the fact that priorities and decline may reflect our collective underlying ageism.
interests evolve across life stages, and these, in turn, We may easily rationalize that a young person simply
have important influences on pragmatics. Of course, has memory lapses, but we often interpret the same
pathological declines in cognition, language, mobil- lapses in older people to be indicative of impairments
ity, and sensory functioning (especially hearing acu- due to age. As Shadden (2011) aptly states, “If the
ity and higher-level auditory processing challenges) expectation is that the elderly are somewhat incom-
have important influences on social interaction that petent, any changes in language and communication
may be observed as changes in pragmatics. appear to confirm that incompetence” (p. 207).
Further complicating the matter, what might
be “normal” for one person based on their history
might be considered problematic in a person expe-
What Are General Guidelines for
riencing a definite change in ability. Some charac-
Differentiating Normal From Impaired teristics observed in a given older individual, such
Language in Older Adults? as attention deficits or lapses in pragmatic appro-
priateness, might be typical of their tendencies in
Characteristics of older adults, often described by younger years. Some people in the early stages of
individuals and by their care partners, are given in dementia may have excellent abilities in some areas,
Table 9–1, along with general guidelines for whether such as short-term memory and speed of process-
each characteristic is typical of normal aging or ing. In Section V, we delve into specific methods
not according to the broad literature on aging and and published tests that may be useful in discerning
cognition. When referring to such guidelines, it is linguistic changes due to normal aging versus those
important to be cautious about relying too much that reflect impairment. It is vital that SLPs strive to
on subjective impressions and self or care partner distinguish normal aging from disease and injury. As
report. It is also important to keep in mind that there team members, it is important that we not dismiss a
is great variability in what constitutes “normal” abil- person’s clinical symptoms as being due to age.
ities versus deficits that may be due to an underlying
impairment that is not directly age associated. Some
authors differentiate primary (normal) aging from What Theories Have Been Proposed
impairment-based or secondary aging. to Account for Cognitive-Linguistic
Even though certain deficits of cognitive and
Changes With Aging?
linguistic performance may seem obvious in some
older adults, it is important to consider whether those
“deficits” might be due to something other than age. Theories about why cognitive and linguistic changes
Often our stereotypes about aging are so pervasive occur in normal aging may be divided into three cat-
that we are not aware of our own judgments in this egories: resource capacity, speed of processing, and
regard. For example, imagine an 18-year-old talking inhibition theories (Shadden, 2011).
Table 9–1. General Guidelines for Considering Whether Cognitive-Linguistic Characteristics of
Older Adults Are Normal

May Be Associated With Normal Aging a Not Characteristic of Normal Aging


Distractibility, sometimes considered Forgetting essential information related to
absent-mindedness activities of daily living
Word-finding problems, especially increasing Semantic confusion at the discourse level
tip-of-the-tongue experiences (knowing that one
knows the word and perhaps knows something
about the word but is not able to actually say the
word)
Occasional trouble finding proper names Dysnomia for important names
Occasional math errors Acalculia or problems with basic
mathematical functions
Normal or near-normal syntactic production Major changes in syntactic structures that
(speaking and writing), with greater difficulty in were used in younger years
distracting conditions; possible reduced use of
complex grammatical forms and reduced length
of sentences
Normal or near-normal syntactic comprehension Moderately or severely impaired
(auditory and reading); greater difficulty with comprehension
longer and more complex stimuli and in dual-task
and distracting conditions
Normal or mildly impaired working memory; Moderately or severely impaired working
greater difficulty with longer and more complex memory
stimuli and in dual-task and distracting conditions
Normal or mildly decreased speed of processing Moderately or severely slowed speed of
during cognitive and motor tasks; greater difficulty processing
with longer and more complex stimuli and in
dual-task and distracting conditions
Normal or mildly impaired episodic memory Moderately or severely impaired episodic
(recall of personal experiences) memory
Normal procedural memory (recall for how to Impaired procedural memory
accomplish specific tasks)
Normal or mildly impaired procedural learning Impaired procedural learning
Normal or mildly impaired source memory Impaired source memory
(memory of where and how one acquired
knowledge or where and when a previous event
took place)
Normal short-term memory (recall of recent events) Impaired short-term memory
Normal autobiographical memory (memory about Impaired autobiographical memory
important aspects of one’s past)
Near or near-normal pragmatic and executive Impaired pragmatic abilities and executive
function abilities function deficits
Note. aNote that several of the characteristics in this column may be lifelong characteristics, not necessarily
characteristics that have developed in older age. Recall, too, that there is tremendous inter- and intraindividual
variability in these characteristics.

122
9. Aging, Which Is Not a Disorder, and Its Relevance to Aphasiology   123

Resource Capacity Theories keeping in mind a task to be performed (Braver &


Barch, 2006; Paxton et al., 2006; Rush et al., 2006). An
Resource capacity theories (Burke & Shafto, 2008) example might be difficulty keeping the task in mind
attribute cognitive and linguistic deficits to a reduc- during a dual-task activity that requires remember-
tion in overall cognitive capacity, not the ability to ing a set of images to be recalled later while at the
accomplish individual simple tasks. The fact that same time listening to auditory verbal stimuli that
older adults tend to have greater difficulty with more one must comprehend.
complex tasks and with engaging in two tasks at the
same time supports this theory. Resource capacity
Signal Degradation Theories
theories may be further broken down into working
memory, context-processing deficiency, signal deg-
radation, and transmission deficit theories. According to signal degradation theories, a key rea-
son for declines in language comprehension and
production in normal aging is a decline in processing
of auditory and visual information (Wingfield et al.,
Working Memory Theories
2005). When visual and auditory acuity decline — as
is typical during normal aging — this, of course,
Working memory theories that implicate aging are affects one’s ability to read and to understand spo-
based on evidence that working memory capacity ken language. Additionally, problems with the inte-
declines with age, especially in older age (past age gration and interpretation of visual and auditory
70 years) (Alatorre-Cruz et al., 2018; Caplan et al., information, also common in aging, affect reading
2011; Mella et al., 2015; Miyake et al., 1994; Stanley and listening comprehension. The greater the sen-
et al., 2015). The effects of working memory decline sory deficits are for a given person, the greater the
are most readily observable in tasks that involve lon- degree of effort they will have to exert during lan-
ger and more grammatically complex verbal stimuli. guage tasks. The limited pool of cognitive resources
The increasing bilateral involvement of the frontal is thus more strained, resulting in poorer compre-
lobes with age in many people, described earlier, hension, recognition, and recall of information that
may be what accounts for such declines; older peo- is read or heard. Auditory and visual distractions
ple who maintain more left hemisphere dominance further tax an individual’s capacity to process infor-
during cognitive and linguistic tasks tend not to mation meaningfully (Schneider et al., 2005).
demonstrate declines in working memory abilities.
The hippocampus, too, vital for transforming and
consolidating short-term memory into long-term Transmission Deficit Theories
memory, appears to play a role in working memory
changes associated with aging (Ystad et al., 2009). Transmission deficit theories suggest that declines
are due to reduced efficiency of neuronal transmis-
sion (Burke et al., 2000). Weaker networks among
Context-Processing Deficiency Theories neurons and among specialized functional areas or
systems in the brain are associated with aging. Such
Context-processing deficiency theories are based on weakening requires greater allocation of limited
the hypothesis that as we get older, we have increas- cognitive resources to compensate for reduced neu-
ing difficulty judging and taking into account the ral transmission, thus affecting speed and accuracy
context of a cognitive or linguistic task and thus of task performance. Transmission deficits are less
adjusting to the context. This may, in turn, lead to severe in people who regularly and actively engage
less efficient allocation of resources to accomplish a in the types of tasks that typically decline with aging.
task. Evidence to support this hypothesis includes That is, active cognitive and linguistic engagement
challenges with speed and accuracy during contin- strengthens connections among neurons and func-
uous performance tasks in which one is required to tional systems, leaving a larger pool of resources to
maintain sustained attention on a set of stimuli while allocate to cognitive and linguistic tasks.
124  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Speed-of-Processing Theories task. Likewise, failure to inhibit irrelevant thoughts


during conversation may lead to the intrusion of
Speed-of-processing theories are based on the gen- more tangential comments in discourse production
eral slowing hypothesis (Salthouse, 1996), the (Bell et al., 2008).
notion that our cognitive processing at all levels A source of distraction that is often unrecog-
slows as we age. The influence of neurologically nized clinically and in research is a person’s own
based slowing on performance varies according to worry and doubt about being able to perform well
the type and complexity of the task at hand. In terms cognitively and linguistically. Taking on a mentality
of its influence on communication, reduced speed of of not being able to perform well because of one’s
processing is especially relevant to the processing of age is a type of identity accommodation associated
auditory linguistic input, which is intricately time with one’s own awareness of, or even belief in, ste-
bound (Caplan et al., 2003; Hartley, 2006; Salthouse, reotypes about aging.
2000). Thus, older adults perform well on linguis-
tic tasks that involve simple grammatical structures
and easy and familiar words; overall slowing may What Can Be Done to Ensure the
limit their speed when processing more difficult syn- Best Preservation of Language
tax and longer linguistic stimuli, although this accu- Abilities as People Age?
racy may or may not be affected, depending on the
task (Byczewska-Konieczny & Kielar-Turska, 2017;
Zhu et al., 2018). Adults have been shown to be as For the most part, regardless of the specific theories
accurate as young adults in the naming of pictures, of language changes associated with aging, there are
despite slower response times (Britt et al., 2016). clearly things we can all do to promote brain health
Memory impairments during information process- and language abilities as we grow older. Mainte-
ing may be attributed to slower speed of processing nance of overall good health is key. Factors promot-
because there is an increasing backlog of multiple ing brain health are summarized in Box 9–1. Two
cognitive operations to be accomplished in a finite factors that have been shown to be related to delayed
amount of time. onset of cognitive deficits and to slow the progres-
sion of the dementia may be difficult to manipulate
in older adults: higher education levels and average
Inhibition Theories (or better) socioeconomic status.
Increasing numbers of apps, computer games,
Inhibition theories (also called inhibitory deficit books, and online resources are being developed in
theories) are based on the rationale that older peo- response to growing public awareness of the impor-
ple have greater challenges than younger people tance of preventing cognitive decline with age. It is a
with inhibiting irrelevant information and focusing good idea for clinical aphasiologists to stay abreast of
attention to a particular task in the face of multiple such developments and the quality of the evidence
competing stimuli or task requirements (Butler & base supporting specific applications marketed to
Zacks, 2006; Hasher & Zacks, 1988; Zacks & Hasher, older people.
1993). Examples of irrelevant information may be
distracting auditory and visual stimuli in the imme-
diate environment. Older people also appear to be What Is Elderspeak, and How May
more sensitive to internally generated distractions.
We Raise Awareness About It?
For example, when trying to come up with a partic-
ular word, an older person may generate multiple
possible words that are related to the target word but Elderspeak is the adaptation of language to a person
are not the word itself; attending to those nontarget because of their age. Elderspeak may be likened to
words may consume cognitive resources that then speaking to an older adult as if they were a child
become unavailable to the primary word-finding and has many of the features common in motherese,
9. Aging, Which Is Not a Disorder, and Its Relevance to Aphasiology   125

enculturated to use elderspeak within their employ-


Box
9–1 Factors That Promote Brain Health ment settings because it is modeled so commonly
Through the Life Span by others.
Elderspeak includes prosody, lexical choice,
• Active intellectual engagement on a daily and pragmatic aspects of conversation such as role
basis assumption. Elderspeak may be characterized by
• Regular active and pleasurable social any of the features listed in Box 9–2. Many laypeo-
engagement and support ple as well as clinical professionals use elderspeak
• Limitation of time spent in passive unintentionally and may even think it is helpful or
activities, such as watching television nurturing. This fact makes it all the more important
• Constructive and proactive management of
low mood, stress, and anger
• Cardiovascular fitness, including well-
Box
regulated blood pressure 9–2 Features That May
• Maintenance of low body fat Constitute Elderspeak
• Sufficient rest
• A well-balanced diet with well-controlled • Speaking loudly even when the older
glucose regulation person has normal or near-normal hearing
• Alcohol consumption only in moderation if • Speaking unnaturally slowly
at all • Exaggerating intonation
• When possible, avoidance of drug effects • Speaking with raised pitch (actually
and pharmacological interactions that inhibitory to many older adults’ hearing,
negatively affect brain and language which tends to be worse at higher
functions frequencies)
• Simplifying vocabulary (e.g., “thing”
instead of “remote control”)
• Using childish terms
or maternal language to young children. However, • Simplifying grammar
although many aspects of motherese have been • Using multiple short sentences to convey
shown to be beneficial in interaction with children, content that would be more naturally
elderspeak generally has greater negative influences conveyed using longer sentences
on the quality of communication and often no facil- • Use of first person rather than second (e.g.,
itating effects on success in conveying communica- “let’s pull up our pants now,” or “have we
tive intent. finished our dinner yet?”)
Elderspeak conveys stereotypes of older people • Addressing older people with diminutive
as childish, incomplete, cognitively impaired, and or inappropriately intimate terms such as
dependent. Additionally, there is substantial evi- “dear,” “honey,” or “sweetie”
dence that it is perceived by older adults as demean- • Assuming that an older person knows
ing and that it detracts from the quality of social and nothing about certain contemporary
clinical interactions. Several authors have suggested subject areas such as social networking,
that it may lead to lower self-esteem, social with- smartphone use, and popular culture
drawal, depression, and increased behaviors asso- • Speaking in a bossy, threatening, or overly
ciated with dependency (Kemper & Harden, 1999; authoritarian manner (e.g., “What do you
Ryan et al., 1991; Williams et al., 2005, 2017). Elder- think you’re doing in here where you don’t
speak has long been found to be common in nursing belong,” “Stop your jabbering,” or “Take
home settings (Caporael, 1981; Williams et al., 2017) your pills or I’m not going to help you
and in health care contexts in general (Schroyen make that phone call”)
et al., 2018). In fact, many staff members seem to be
126  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

that clinicians working with older adults pay close reotypes perpetuated through everyday television
attention to their own speech directed at older peo- programming include older sexually inactive, frail,
ple and model appropriate interactions to others. dependent people (especially women) with poor
Aphasiologists working in residential settings may memories, often acting irrationally.
find it especially helpful to include training on In fact, the nature of aging is highly variable and
avoiding elderspeak in ongoing staff in-service pro- complex; few generalizations can be made about an
grams (Corwin, 2018; Williams et al., 2003). individual based on age alone. A majority of older
There may be particular instances when commu- people are physically and cognitively competent
nication with certain older individuals is enhanced and, in much of the world, financially self-sufficient.
by modifying specific aspects of speech and lan- National and local economies and child-rearing
guage that happen to be characteristic of elderspeak practices depend greatly on older people. Due to
(Kemper & Harden, 1999). In such cases, it is best to improvements in health care, disease prevention,
consider those facilitative modifications rather than exercise, nutrition, sanitation, and assistive technol-
elderspeak per se; the very term elderspeak connotes ogies, life expectancy continues to rise throughout
inappropriate and demeaning modification of com- the world, and so does the proportion of people over
munication. Examples of appropriate modifications age 65 years without disabilities.
include reducing grammatical complexity and utter- With the barrage of ageism in our daily lives,
ance length to a person with aphasia for whom this it is no wonder that we all need to be reminded to
strategy has been demonstrated to facilitate compre- do our part to counter such influences. SLPs work-
hension, or speaking louder to a person with a hear- ing with older adults have ample opportunities to
ing impairment that precludes hearing at a normal consider the rights of older people as basic human
conversational level. rights and to advocate actively for fairness. Advo-
cacy in this arena may be as simple as suggesting
edits to wording in written medical reports about
What Sensitivities Related to Ageism Are older adults, modeling appropriate word choice
during conversations with clinical colleagues, and
Important for Aphasiologists to Demonstrate?
providing professional in-services to counter age-
ism. It might entail political activity to change leg-
In Chapter 3, we discussed the importance of terms islation in favor of older people, work to change
we use when we talk about older adults. It is also ageist policies (or enforce anti-ageist policies) in
important that we consider the general concept of our local agencies and communities, and efforts to
ageism and how experts working with older adults help build awareness of age-related discrimination
may play a role in challenging ageist stereotypes. and abuse.
Consider the sheer amount of exposure that people In sum, aging is not a disease. Aging is not an
have to ageist stereotypes. According to the Inter- etiology that causes neurogenic language disorders.
national Longevity Center–USA and Leading Age Age is a vital construct in a life span perspective on
California (Dahmen & Cozma, 2009), people over human development. Given that people have greater
age 65 years comprise 12.7% of the U.S. population likelihood of acquiring neurogenic language disor-
but less than 2% of people seen in prime-time televi- ders as they age, opportunities for clinical aphasi-
sion. Twice as many older people portrayed on tele- ologists to work with and empower older people
vision are men, whereas in reality the proportion of abound and are ever expanding due to the world-
women over age 60 years is greater than men. Ste- wide growth of the aging population.
9. Aging, Which Is Not a Disorder, and Its Relevance to Aphasiology   127

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter 9. What factors influence discourse
that are new to you or that you have not yet performance as people age?
mastered. 10. Describe subtypes of resource capacity
2. Describe the life span model of postformal theories of aging. How are they relevant to
cognitive development. How is it relevant cognitive-linguistic abilities and challenges?
to consideration of age-related changes in 11. Consider the positive aspects of the aging
communication abilities and needs? brain summarized in this chapter. Do you
3. How might models of aging that promote have experience with an older person that
conceptualization of aging from a life span reinforces the positive aspects of age-related
perspective influence your role in: changes in cognitive-linguistic development?
a. Helping a family member consider If so, share stories of such experiences, tying
prognosis for recovery from an acquired them to the content in this chapter.
language disorder due to stroke or brain 12. Why is it important to scrutinize carefully
injury? and not necessarily accept as fact the results
b. Responding constructively to colleagues of research studies that demonstrate declines
who make misguided statements about in cognitive and linguistic abilities as people
the influence of age on an older person’s age?
language abilities? 13. How will you continue to challenge your
c. Working with a social worker on own stereotypes about aging that may
discharge planning at a subacute negatively affect your role as a clinical
rehabilitation facility? aphasiologist?
4. How are demographic shifts related to age 14. Give examples of stereotypes about aging
relevant to clinical aphasiologists? that could affect a person’s own potential for
5. Describe how individual differences in age- progress in language rehabilitation.
related cognitive-linguistic abilities might be 15. Is elderspeak helpful or is it discriminatory?
attributable to changes in the brain. Support your answer.
6. Why is it that physical changes that might be 16. Give examples of how elderspeak might
detected in the brains of older people do not include various aspects of conversation,
necessarily correspond to problems in their including prosody, lexical choice, and
functional cognitive and linguistic abilities? pragmatics.
7. What patterns of word-finding difficulties 17. What are some positive modifications that
are typically associated with normal aging? can be used instead of elderspeak?
8. What are some challenges in attributing
receptive and expressive syntactic processing Additional learning and reflection materials and
www
difficulties to age? activities may be found on the companion website.
SECTION III

Features, Symptoms, and Syndromes


in the Major Categories of
Cognitive-Linguistic Disorders
130  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

The background in Section II sets the stage for important content related to major categories of
returning to consideration of aphasia in Chapter acquired language disorders: cognitive-linguistic
10, this time with a focus on specific hallmark fea- challenges associated with traumatic brain injury
tures and syndromes of aphasia as they have been (Chapter 11), right hemisphere syndrome (Chap-
characterized by numerous authors over the years. ter 12), and dementia and other neurodegenerative
Subsequent chapters in this section, then, address conditions (Chapter 13).
CHAPTER
10
Syndromes and Hallmark
Characteristics of Aphasia

In this chapter, we consider varied means of classi- communication deficits are often associated with
fying types or syndromes of aphasia. In doing so, certain types, or syndromes, of aphasia. There
we review many of the symptoms and associated have been many variations on that scheme pro-
terminology that are important to know about as posed over the years, and aphasiologists to this
we study aphasia in general and as we learn about day vary widely in terms of their means of classi-
the challenges of particular individuals with aphasia fying and describing the various types of aphasia.
with whom we work. We also consider critically the One reason it is important to know about the ways
strengths and weaknesses of classification schemes aphasia types are classified is that classification
and the need for excellent clinicians to remain flexi- schemes are used widely in clinical practice and
ble in the ways they consider and interpret aphasia in research. Many commonly used aphasia assess-
classification systems as they work with individual ment batteries are designed to aid in classifying a
people who have aphasia. given test taker’s constellation of language symp-
After reading and reflecting on the content in toms according to one of several aphasia types, also
this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in called syndromes of aphasia. Note, though, that the
your own words, the following queries: classification of any individual’s aphasia may differ
according to which test they take, a topic to which
1. How are the types of aphasia classified? we return after reviewing the classic syndromes
2. What are the classic syndromes of aphasia, and of aphasia.
what are the hallmark characteristics of each? It is important that we understand what others
3. What is primary progressive aphasia (PPA)? mean when they refer to specific types of aphasia,
4. What other syndromes of aphasia are there, even if we do not agree with the particular classi-
and what are their characteristics? fication scheme they may be using. Also, knowing
5. How might dyslexia and dysgraphia be con- the basic schemes used for classifying aphasia syn-
ceptualized as symptoms versus syndromes? dromes helps us to think critically about possible
6. What are limitations of classification systems alternatives and exceptions to those schemes. Thus,
based on relating function to neuroanatomical in this chapter, we review the most prevalent ways
structure? in which aphasia is classified and also consider
exceptions and challenges to existing classification
schemes. Excellent clinical aphasiologists are flexible
thinkers, not getting so caught up in one theoreti-
How Are the Types of Aphasia Classified?
cal argument over another that they do not see each
individual person with aphasia as a special, complex
Aphasia is manifested in many ways, with varying individual regardless of how anyone might catego-
symptoms and severity levels. General patterns of rize their linguistic symptoms.

131
132  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

a concomitant motor speech disorder but, due to a


What Are the Classic Syndromes of moderate-to-severe aphasia, may have many pauses
Aphasia, and What Are the Hallmark as he struggles to formulate words into sentences to
Characteristics of Each? express his ideas. Both could be considered nonflu-
ent, and both may even produce the same number
of words, content units, or utterances per unit of
Expressive/Receptive, Nonfluent/Fluent, time. However, their actual speech output would be
and Anterior/Posterior Dichotomies noticeably dissimilar, and the underlying cause of
their disfluency would be different.
One general way of considering aphasia types is in Here is another example. Consider two people
a simple dichotomy of fluent or nonfluent, anterior with conduction aphasia due to a similar type of
or posterior, and expressive or receptive aphasias. lesion in the arcuate fasciculus. One may not be able
Fluent aphasias (or fluent types of aphasia) are to repeat what is said to her, so her linguistic output
those in which people can speak or write readily during a repetition task would certainly be disfluent.
and have few hesitations or struggles when gen- Yet in spontaneous conversation, it may seem that
erating language, even though some of the words her fluency is near normal. The other may also have
that are spoken or written may not be real words or milder repetition problems but more severe anomia,
may not accurately convey the intended meaning. including more naming errors and self-corrections
People with fluent aphasia tend to have more diffi- of nontarget words. Her speech would thus be noted
culty understanding language as opposed to formu- as disfluent, but for a different reason than the first,
lating language; for this reason, fluent aphasias are despite having a similar lesion and the same clas-
also sometimes called receptive aphasias. Consider sic aphasia type. In sum, knowing that a person is
how the term fluent is based on the intact abilities disfluent does not help us to understand very much
manifested, while — in contrast — the term receptive is about the nature of their communication problems.
based on the primary area of deficit. Fluent aphasias An additional challenge with the fluent versus
tend to be caused by temporal lobe lesions and thus nonfluent distinction is that just how fluent or disflu-
are also sometimes called posterior types of aphasia. ent a person is considered to be has a great deal to do
Nonfluent aphasias (or nonfluent types of with how fluency is indexed. A person who speaks a
aphasia) are those in which people generate few great deal but with no meaning may be considered
words, content units (elements of meaning), or fluent in terms of words per minute but disfluent in
utterances per unit of time. Given that people with terms of content units. A person who speaks slowly
nonfluent aphasia tend to understand language bet- and with great effort may express more actual con-
ter than they produce language, nonfluent forms of tent per unit of time.
aphasia are also sometimes called expressive aphasia. People who are categorized as having nonflu-
Nonfluent aphasias tend to be caused by frontal lobe ent aphasia are generally thought to correspond
lesions and thus are sometimes called anterior types to people with anterior lesions, whereas people
of aphasia. with fluent aphasia are generally thought to cor-
Despite their common usage, the terms fluent respond to those with posterior lesions. However,
and nonfluent to describe aphasia syndromes are not when a fluent versus nonfluent distinction is based
generally helpful or informative. They are catchall on language testing, and then lesion sites are com-
terms that gloss over important differences among pared, the lesion sites often have overlapping
people who fit into each of the two categories. For components. An example is shown in Figure 10–1,
example, consider two individuals, each with a fron- depicting lesion mapping for people with “fluent”
tal lobe lesion that led to Broca’s aphasia. One may versus “nonfluent” aphasia based on standardized
have only mild aphasia (mild language formulation language testing. Note that when brain lesions are
deficits consisting primarily of difficulty generating superimposed on a common lateral brain template,
complex syntactic forms); still, he may speak halt- some people classified as being nonfluent have areas
ingly, struggling to initiate words because of a con- of involvement that correspond to “anterior” lesions
comitant apraxia of speech. The other may not have (e.g., see BY in Figure 10–1). Likewise, some people
133
Figure 10–1. Normalized magnetic resonance images for people categorized as having fluent versus nonfluent aphasia based on test scores. Lesion
maps shown are superimposed on a common lateral brain template across people with aphasia. The top panel corresponds to brains of people described
as having “nonfluent” aphasia, whereas the bottom panel represents people with “fluent” aphasia. The distinction between fluent and nonfluent in this
case was based on standardized testing (Boston Diagnostic Aphasia examination; Goodglass et al., 2001).The initials correspond to names of research
participants. See Speer and Wilshire (2014) for details about a study pertaining to the same participants. Image courtesy of Dr. Carolyn Wilshire, Victoria
University of Wellington. A full-color version of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.
134  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

classified as being fluent have areas of involvement to consider associated concomitant deficits that are
that correspond to “posterior” lesions (e.g., see SW likely to arise in a person with a given syndrome of
in Figure 10–1). Still, many have overlapping areas aphasia. Such concomitant deficits are often predict-
of involvement. able given the location of the lesion(s) causing any
particular type of aphasia.
The fact that some aspects of language are
Classic Aphasia Classification affected differently than others for any given syn-
drome has led some authors to refer to aphasia as
For the purpose of establishing a basic understand- dissociation syndrome. The term dissociation in this
ing of commonly referenced forms of aphasia, we context refers to the fact that some abilities remain
review those summarized in Table 10–1. These are relatively intact while others are relatively impaired.
based largely on the Wernicke-Lichtheim model
(Graves, 2009), which the Boston Group Classifica-
tion (Benson, 1979) approximates. Note that such Wernicke’s Aphasia
tables are readily available in textbooks, aphasia bat-
teries, and online formats. Note also that the specific Wernicke’s aphasia is classically associated with a
details indicated across such tables are often incon- lesion in Wernicke’s area, in the superior temporal
sistent. Such inconsistencies are due to varied the- lobe, which corresponds to Brodmann’s area 22.
oretical views, not to mention inconsistent clinical Recent research, though, has shown that actual asso-
observations such as those about the fluent versus ciated areas of the brain affected in people with Wer-
nonfluent distinction described earlier. nicke’s aphasia include not only Wernicke’s area but
Of course, there is also a great deal of infor- other parts of the left temporal lobe (e.g., anterior
mation about aphasia, especially in online formats portions of the temporal lobe, the posterior middle
without peer review, which is simply incorrect. temporal gyrus, the posterior superior temporal sul-
Additionally, when condensing complex informa- cus), plus parts of the inferior frontal gyrus, includ-
tion into tables or brief summaries, vital information ing Broca’s area, and the middle frontal gyrus and
about possible exceptions and important nuances is the dorsal premotor cortex (Dronkers et al., 2004;
lost. As always, it is incumbent upon the reader to Mesulam et al., 2015; Turken & Dronkers, 2011).
weigh carefully and critically the veracity of infor- People with this aphasia syndrome typically
mation found in print. Also, no matter how clear the have language output that sounds fluent even
ideal description of each aphasia type may be, indi- though a great deal of content may not be conveyed
vidual people with aphasia often do not fit neatly by what they say or write. Wernicke’s aphasia is
into the set of features that characterize any partic- sometimes called jargon aphasia because of the ten-
ular type. Even when they do, their symptoms may dency to produce nonwords, or neologisms (liter-
change over time. ally, “new words”). An example is saying, “Bring me
Again, it is important to know the details behind a trunket,” instead of, “Bring me a drink.”
classification schemes, and once we are sophisti- Another hallmark feature is the production of
cated about those details, it is important to not get paraphasias, or words substituted for target words.
so hung up in them that it keeps us from tuning into Paraphasias may be semantic or literal. Semantic
the most essential strengths and needs of each indi- paraphasias (also called verbal or global parapha-
vidual person with aphasia so that we may offer the sias) entail the substitution of a real word for the tar-
best intervention. get word. Examples are saying the word ear instead
Hallmark characteristics of the various syn- of nose (in this instance sharing the same semantic
dromes of aphasia are, by definition, characteristics category) and saying the word car instead of ladder
of linguistic abilities. Given that changes in brain (where the paraphasia has less of a clear semantic
tissue affect many other aspects of a person’s abil- relationship with the target word). Phonemic para-
ities, and given that most people with aphasia have phasias (also called literal paraphasias) entail the
concomitant nonlinguistic deficits, it is also helpful substitution of one or more sounds in the target word.
Table 10–1. Types of Aphasia, Associated Lesions, and Hallmark Features

Auditory
Classically and Reading Oral and Written
Aphasia General Associated Comprehension Expression
Syndrome Type* Lesion Site Impairment Impairment Other Features

Classical Aphasia Syndromes

Wernicke’s R/F/P Posterior portion Moderate to Moderate to Poor self-


aphasia of the superior severe severe; anomia; monitoring, lack
temporal gyrus, semantic of awareness of
Brodmann 22 and literal deficits
paraphasias,
jargon,
neologisms,
circumlocutions,
press of speech,
logorrhea

Broca’s E/N/A Inferior frontal Mild to Mild to severe; Typically aware


aphasia lobe, Brodmann moderate, agrammatism/ of deficits,
44 and 45 (third difficulty with telegraphic sometimes with
frontal convolution; passives speech, anomia, catastrophic
frontal operculum) and complex literal paraphasias reaction and
grammar more common emotional lability;
than semantic; often concomitant
circumlocutions apraxia of speech,
dysarthria,
contralateral
hemiparesis

Conduction Either Arcuate fasciculus, Mild to moderate Mild to moderate,


aphasia (depends supramarginal especially
on gyrus (Brodmann with repetition
severity) area 40) impairment;
phonemic
paraphasias,
conduit
d’approche

Global E/N/A Large perisylvian Severe Severe; may be


aphasia lesion, including nonverbal; may
frontal and have jargon and
temporal lobe and stereotypy
often parietal lobes

Transcortical R/F/P Temporal lobe Moderate to Moderate to


sensory watershed regions; severe severe, especially
aphasia angular gyrus with paraphasias,
(Brodmann’s area neologisms
39), posterior logorrhea, poor
middle temporal self-monitoring;
gyrus (Brodmann’s intact repetition
area 37)

continues

135
Table 10–1. continued

Auditory
Classically and Reading Oral and Written
Aphasia General Associated Comprehension Expression
Syndrome Type* Lesion Site Impairment Impairment Other Features

Transcortical E/N/A Frontal lobe Mild to Mild to moderate,


motor watershed regions; moderate, telegraphic; intact
aphasia Brodmann’s areas difficulty with repetition; literal
6, 8, 9, 10, and 46 passives and semantic
and complex paraphasias
grammar

Mixed E/N/A Inferior frontal lobe Mild to Mild to moderate,


transcortical moderate, telegraphic
aphasia difficulty with
passives
and complex
grammar

Other Types of Aphasia

Anomic Either Variable, often Mild Mild to severe,


aphasia (depends angular gurus word-finding
on difficulty;
severity) paraphasias,
circumlocutions,
fillers; use of
generic terms

Primary Either Depends on Mild to severe, Mild to severe, Unlike other


progressive subtype progressive progressive forms of aphasia,
aphasia worsens over
time; individuals
eventually
develop dementia

Crossed Either Right hemisphere See See Concomitant


aphasia (depends in a right-handed corresponding corresponding impairments
on type person type of aphasia type of aphasia more typically
and above above associated with
severity) right hemisphere
syndrome are
likely

Subcortical Either Thalamus, Depends Depends upon


aphasia basal ganglia, upon subtype; subtype; variable
cerebellum, variable across across individuals
connecting white individuals
matter

*E/N/A = expressive/nonfluent/anterior; R/F/P = receptive/fluent/posterior.


Note. The “classically associated lesion sites” are important to consider in the context of the complexity of structure-function
relationships in aphasia and related disorders, as reviewed in this chapter. From Damasio, 2008; Goodglass et al., 1964, 2001;
Goodglass & Wingfield, 1961; Hallowell & Chapey, 2008a.

136
10. Syndromes and Hallmark Characteristics of Aphasia   137

Examples are saying “tegetable” instead of “vege-


Box
table” and “bady” for “baby.” When a neologism is 10–1 Examples of Three Forms of
substituted for a real word, it is sometimes called a Recurrent Perseveration
neologistic paraphasia.
People with Wernicke’s aphasia tend to have • Semantic perseveration: When asked to
relatively intact syntactic production compared to name body parts, a person correctly names
those with more anterior types of aphasia. They may arm, nose, and knee but then repeats the
have logorrhea, or spoken language that is overly word “nose” when the clinician points to
fluent. They tend to exhibit press of speech, continu- an ear. The actual response is semantically
ing to speak even when what they are saying makes related to the intended response.
no sense to the listener, and often without attend- • Lexical perseveration: The individual
ing to social conventions about turn taking in con- names the following items, colors, and
versation. They also tend to lack awareness of their letters correctly: feather, glove, yellow,
deficits and may seem unconcerned about the fact brown, P, T, but then says “brown” when
that what they are saying does not make sense to the shown the letter H. The actual response is
person with whom they are speaking. a word that was spoken previously and is
Recurrent perseveration is one of three forms not semantically related to the intended
of perseveration that are common in people with a response.
variety of acquired neurogenic disorders. It is “the • Phonemic perseveration: When asked to
recurrence of a previous response to a subsequent name body parts, a person correctly names
stimulus within the context of an established set” arm and nose, but then, instead of naming
(Albert, 1989, p. 427). The other forms are continuous ear when the clinician points to an ear, he
perseveration (continuation of a behavior when it is says “near”; when the clinician points to an
no longer appropriate) and stuck-in-set persevera- ankle, he may say “nearkle” or “nackle.”
tion (the inappropriate persistence in continuing a The actual response has phonemic features
task or activity) (Sandson & Albert, 1984). Recurrent in common with a previous word spoken
perseveration is the most common in people with and is not semantically related to the
aphasia and tends to occur in all forms of aphasia; it intended response.
is most common in Wernicke’s aphasia. Examples of
each type of recurrent perseveration (semantic, lexi-
cal, and phonemic) are shown in Box 10–1. aphasia immediately after a stroke, but whose lan-
Verbal perseveration is sometimes used syn- guage abilities continue to improve, eventually have
onymously with the term lexical perseveration. It is what would be classified as anomic rather than Wer-
the tendency to say a word or sounds within a word nicke’s aphasia.
spoken previously but not the word intended at the
moment; it is common in most types of aphasia. It
is especially prevalent in people with Wernicke’s Broca’s Aphasia
aphasia. This is typically exacerbated when asking
a person to name objects or pictures. For example, Broca’s aphasia is classically associated with a lesion
after having said the word fork when shown a fork, in Broca’s area, in the inferior, posterior portion of
a person may continue to say the word fork when a the frontal lobe. Broca’s area is often said to corre-
pencil, book, or chair is shown. spond to Brodmann’s areas 44 and 45, also called the
Although hallmark features of language pro- frontal operculum, although there is ample variation
duction described earlier are the key factors that lead in how various researchers describe the nature of
clinicians to suspect that a person has Wernicke’s lesions that lead to Broca’s aphasia. Even the brains
aphasia, people in this clinical group also tend to of the very first two patients identified as having this
have impaired receptive (auditory and reading) form of aphasia by Pierre Paul Broca in 1861, when
abilities as well. Many people who have Wernicke’s reinspected over 140 years later via high-resolution
138  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), were found to Another set of theoretical perspectives on
have more extensive lesions that also affected the agrammatism is characterized as primarily linguistic
superior longitudinal fasciculus, a fiber tract con- in nature. The “tree pruning hypothesis” is based on
necting language regions of the frontal and tempo- the notion of generative grammar. Imagining a sen-
ral lobes (Dronkers et al., 2007). Associated areas tence mapped as a syntactic tree, one may conceive
affected also may involve the white matter underly- of people with aphasia having difficulty reaching up
ing the frontal operculum, plus the basal ganglia and to the highest nodes of the tree (Friedmann, 1994,
precentral gyrus, as well as portions of the tempo- 2001; Friedmann & Grodzinsky, 1997). The mapping
ral and parietal lobes (Damasio, 2008; Flinker et al., hypothesis suggests that agrammatism results from
2015; Fridriksson et al., 2015). difficulty mapping thematic roles (meaning) onto
A primary hallmark feature of Broca’s aphasia grammatical constituents (structure) of sentences
is agrammatism, a deficit in formulating and pro- (Schwartz et al., 1980). Other perspectives empha-
cessing syntax. Auditory comprehension is impaired size phonological (e.g., Kean, 1977) or lexical (e.g.,
especially for more complex types of grammatical Bradley et al., 1980) bases.
constructs. One example of a grammatical construct A third set of perspectives includes hypotheses
that people with Broca’s aphasia tend to have trouble related to the effort entailed in grammatical pro-
with is reversible passives. For example, it may be duction. The stress-saliency hypothesis (Goodglass,
hard to tell who did the kissing and who got kissed 1962, 1968) emphasizes that formulating speech
when trying to process the sentence, “Yangfan was requires effort and that different parts of speech vary
kissed by Jacques”; this is because it would make in terms of the degree of effort required to express
logical sense for either the subject (Jacques in this them. Similarly, the economy-of-effort hypothesis
case) or the object (Yangfan in this case) to do the (Lenneberg, 1967, 1973; Pick, 1931) suggests that
kissing. In contrast, a passive sentence such as, “The people with agrammatism adapt by using the most
ball was kicked by Wanda,” is not reversible (in that meaningful words to reduce their overall production
a ball cannot kick a person) and would be relatively effort, leaving some words out. Additional varia-
easy for a person with Broca’s aphasia to under- tions of these perspectives have been proposed and
stand. Studies in which people with agrammatism tested and are an interesting area for further explo-
have been asked to make grammaticality judgments ration. The results of ongoing research suggest that
(decisions about whether sentence constructions not all manifestations of agrammatism are similar
are correct or incorrect) have generated evidence across people with aphasia and that perhaps there
that their knowledge of grammatical rules tends are varying causes underlying syntactic challenges
not to be lost; rather, it seems that their access to across individuals.
and implementation of grammatical rules are what People with Broca’s aphasia tend to produce
is impaired. short and simple phrases and tend not to produce
Several theories about the underlying nature of complex grammatical constructions. Language pro-
agrammatism have been proposed. Earlier notions duction is typically telegraphic, a term that harkens
that agrammatism is a central deficit in the linguis- back to the day when people communicating by tele-
tic ability to process syntactic information have been graph had to pay by the word and thus were careful
discounted at least partially by the fact that there to use only the most necessary, meaningful words to
tend to be dissociations between expressive and pro- convey what they wanted to say. The words that are
ductive modalities. A more recent account is based missing tend to be function words, such as preposi-
on the speaker’s adaptation to limited working tions, pronouns, determiners, conjunctions, and aux-
memory and speed of processing capacity. Reduced iliary verbs. These are also referred to as closed-class
capacity leads to challenges in handling incoming words because there is a relatively small set of these
information at the rate that it must be processed to in a language compared to open-class words, and
be understood and a simplification of grammatical there are rarely new words in this category added
production (Kean, 1985; Kolk, 1995; Kolk & Hee- to a language.
schen, 1990; Kolk et al., 1985; Shiani et al., 2018) in The words that are spoken or written tend to be
relation to the speaker’s capacity overload. content words, primarily nouns, verbs, adjectives,
10. Syndromes and Hallmark Characteristics of Aphasia   139

and adverbs. These are also referred to as open-class Keep in mind that Broca’s aphasia is a language
words because words in this class continue to be disorder. The reason this is important to emphasize
added to languages and the way such words are used is that many people tend to get distracted by con-
and combined with others continues to evolve in a comitant motor speech deficits that so many people
language. Verbs in English are often used without with Broca’s aphasia have. The proximity of areas
inflection by people with Broca’s aphasia; that is, just or structures essential to speech production may be
the verb stems may be used (e.g., “I go” instead of the reason that there is such a high incidence of a
“I went” or “Melanie study . . . ” instead of “Melanie Broca’s type of aphasia in patients who have apraxia
studies . . . ”). Just how and why different classes of speech (and vice versa).
of words may be relatively spared while others are Based on a seminal study by Dronkers (1996),
retained, and how these patterns differ across different the insula, located just beneath Broca’s area in the
types of languages, is an intriguing area of research. frontal operculum, has been implicated in motor
People with Broca’s aphasia typically have speech disorders and has been thought by many
dysnomia (problems with word finding). Often, to be a center for motor programming for speech.
they use circumlocutions, or words other than However, more recently, this has been contested,
the intended words, to get around the words they and other areas have been implicated (see Basilakos
are striving to say and yet still communicate their et al., 2015; Fedorenko et al., 2015; and Richardson
intended meaning. An example might be saying, et al., 2012, for intriguing arguments about this).
“Hand me the cutters,” rather than “Hand me the Still, whatever regions are specifically responsible
scissors.” Literal paraphasias are more frequent than for motor speech versus language, discerning which
semantic paraphasias in people with Broca’s apha- aspects of a person’s disfluency and struggle to com-
sia. Their speech tends to be disfluent in that fewer municate are due to language deficits versus apraxia
words and less meaningful content are conveyed of speech, when that person has both of these, is an
per unit of time compared to people without apha- important challenge for clinicians. We consider this
sia and also to most people with Wernicke’s aphasia. carefully in Section V.
Their speech tends to be effortful, and they tend to Many people with Broca’s aphasia also have
be aware of their errors, especially in contrast to peo- additional concomitant challenges. For example, in
ple with aphasia who have more posterior (temporal light of the proximity of Broca’s area to the motor
lobe) lesions. Some have extreme frustration when strip in the frontal lobe, they commonly have paresis
struggling to communicate, referred to by some in the face and body on the contralateral (usually
authors as catastrophic reaction. right) side. They are also likely to have dysarthria.
Additionally, although most stroke and brain
injury survivors tend to experience depression
(due to direct neurophysiologic changes as well as Global Aphasia
life impacts), people with Broca’s aphasia tend to
have higher rates and severity of depression than Global aphasia is classically associated with mul-
those with more posterior forms of aphasia. Some tiple areas of the frontal, parietal, and temporal
also experience emotional lability, the tendency to areas of the brain that receive their blood supply
cry, swear, and otherwise openly emote, in a way from branches of the middle cerebral artery in the
that is uncharacteristic of how the person typically language-dominant hemisphere. That is, large
responded prior to a stroke or brain injury. lesions throughout the planum temporale tend to
Consider this speech sample from a person with be affected. Global aphasia entails a combination
Broca’s aphasia: of expressive and receptive language deficits in all
modalities. Some people with global aphasia are
Many years back, uh, stroke. Speech, uh. No. No not able to speak at all, especially soon after stroke.
speech. But uh work. Hospital. Work. Rehab. Some speak only via stereotypy, the production of
Work. Home. Work. This one and that one. And the same few words or nonwords regardless of the
now . . . speech pretty good. Not speak like you. meaning intended. For example, a person may say
No like old me. But speak way better. only “hello,” “wonderful,” or “kippish” in any verbal
140  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

context and no other words. Communication is car- tal lobe, extending to the prefrontal areas. Damasio
ried out largely through gesture, tone of voice, and (2008) reports that lesions may be in Brodmann’s
facial expression. areas 6, 8, 9, 10, and 46. The hallmark symptoms are
similar to Broca’s aphasia, with the exception that
repetition is intact in transcortical motor aphasia.
Conduction Aphasia Note that the terms sensory and motor in the
labels for transcortical aphasia are misnomers in
The classic lesion site associated with conduction that, by definition, no form of aphasia is a motor or
aphasia is the arcuate fasciculus, within the supra­ sensory disorder. These misnomers are persistent
marginal gyrus (Brodmann’s area 40), although artifacts remaining from years of use in the clinical
other fiber tracts in pathways between Wernicke’s and research literature, dating back at least as far as
and Broca’s areas have been implicated (Anderson et Wernicke’s classic text, Der aphasische symptomen-
al., 1991; Buchsbaum et al., 2011; Sidiropoulos et al., complex, in 1874. Their persistence may have been
2014). The hallmark symptom is impaired repetition exacerbated by the fact that not all languages include
relative to good comprehension and spontaneous terms that clearly distinguish speech from language.
production of spoken and written language. The
longer and more complex the words, phrases, and
sentences people with conduction aphasia are asked Mixed Transcortical Aphasia
to repeat, the more difficulty they have with the task.
Awareness of errors is typically good, and they may There is no clear agreement about a classic associ-
engage in conduit d’approche, or repeated attempts ated site of lesion for mixed transcortical aphasia,
to articulate a verbal stimulus that they are trying although it may be associated with combined mul-
to repeat. In spontaneous speech, the most common tifocal lesions in the frontal and temporal water-
error types are phonemic paraphasias. shed regions. It is similar to global aphasia, with the
exception of intact repetition ability.
Transcortical Sensory Aphasia

The classic lesion site associated with transcortical What Is Primary Progressive Aphasia?
sensory aphasia is the area surrounding Wernicke’s
area, excluding Wernicke’s area itself, namely, the
angular gyrus (Brodmann’s area 39) and the pos- As introduced in Chapter 6, primary progressive
terior portion of the middle temporal gyrus (Brod- aphasia (PPA) is distinguished from other types of
mann’s area 37). These are known as the temporal aphasia as follows:
lobe watershed regions, or areas of overlapping
peripheral blood flow near Wernicke’s area. Accord- • PPA is a dementia syndrome, so it is
ing to Damasio (2008), associated lesions may pro­ neurodegenerative; other forms of aphasia
ject into the lateral portions of the occipital lobe are not.
or anterior portions of the middle temporal gyrus • PPA has an insidious onset; other forms of
(Brodmann’s areas 18, 19, and 21). The hallmark aphasia tend to develop suddenly.
symptoms are similar to Wernicke’s aphasia, with • PPA symptoms get progressively worse;
the exception that repetition is intact in transcortical symptoms of other forms of aphasia continue
sensory aphasia. People with this type of aphasia to improve or at least stabilize.
also sometimes echo others’ words as they are lis-
tening to others speak. It meets the definition of aphasia in that it entails
a loss of previously acquired language abilities
due to a neurological cause and is not attributable
Transcortical Motor Aphasia to psychiatric, sensory, motor, or intellectual defi-
cits. Its early impairment-level linguistic mani-
The classic site of lesion for transcortical motor apha- festations may be similar to those in varied forms
sia is in the anterior watershed area of the left fron- of aphasia.
10. Syndromes and Hallmark Characteristics of Aphasia   141

Although PPA is a degenerative condition, its Just as there are cases of crossed aphasia, there
initial manifestations in communication are different are cases of crossed nonaphasia or crossed right
from degenerative conditions in which initial man- hemisphere syndrome, as well. That is, a right-
ifestations consist primarily of memory loss, confu- handed individual may have lesions in areas asso-
sion, and executive function challenges. Noticeable ciated with specific syndromes of aphasia, yet have
word-finding problems that are uncharacteristic of a no symptoms of aphasia (Cohen et al., 1991; Fischer
person’s prior status are especially prevalent in the et al., 1991; Hund-Georgiadis et al., 2001; Judd, 1989;
early stages of PPA. PPA tends to occur, on average, Junqué et al., 1986; Taylor & Solomon, 1979).
at a younger age than does dementia; it is often first
diagnosed in people in their 50s or 60s. PPA was not
substantially reported in the literature until much Subcortical Aphasia
more recently than most other neurodegenerative
conditions. Given a lack of awareness of the syn- Subcortical aphasia is any form of aphasia that is
drome, many people with PPA are likely to be misdi- associated with a lesion below the cortex. The term
agnosed, and many are not referred for appropriate defies the traditional viewpoint that aphasia can only
support and clinical services. be associated with cortical lesions. Researchers have
PPA occurs in varied forms and, as such, may provided neuroimaging evidence associated with
be characterized as a group of syndromes. The syn- psycholinguistic profiles of people who have sub-
dromes have in common the fact that brain regions cortical lesions but no evidence of cortical damage
and neural networks involved primarily in linguistic (Kuljic-Obradovic, 2003; Marien et al., 2000; Nadeau
processing are affected first and that further degen- & Crosson, 1997; Schmahmann & Sherman, 1998). If
eration then affects other regions and networks. The the specialized structures and neural networks for
most widely accepted classification to date includes language processing are all in the cortex, then how
three major types: semantic, logopenic, and agram- could aphasia be caused by a subcortical lesion? Pri-
matic (nonfluent). We explore these in detail in Chap- mary structures that have been implicated in subcor-
ter 13. Approaches to facilitating communication in tical aphasia are the thalamus, the basal ganglia, and
people with PPA are explored in Chapter 26, and we the cerebellum. The subcortical white matter path-
consider relevant service delivery, assessment, and ways that connect subcortical structures with one
treatment content in each of the chapters in Sections another (including the internal capsule) and with the
V, VI, and VII. cortex have also been implicated (Alexander et al.,
1987; Craver & Small, 1997; de Boissezon et al., 2005;
Hillis et al., 2004; Kang et al., 2017; Krishnan et al.,
What Other Syndromes of Aphasia Are 2012; Kuljic-Obradovic, 2003). Reported associated
symptoms vary.
There, and What Are Their Characteristics?

Crossed Aphasia Anomic Aphasia

Crossed aphasia is any form of aphasia that is due A lesion site commonly associated with anomic
to damage to the right hemisphere instead of the left aphasia is the angular gyrus; although other asso-
in a person who is right-handed. It is extremely rare ciated lesion sites have been reported, associated
(probably less than 3% of people with aphasia). The lesions still tend to be in the same region, around
lesion sites most often mirror the sites associated the intersection of the superior temporal and infe-
with classic aphasia syndromes in the left hemi- rior parietal lobes. As its name implies, the hallmark
sphere, although some are said to be associated feature of anomic aphasia is word-finding difficulty.
with unique and unpredictable sites of lesion. Many What differentiates this from other syndromes of
people with crossed aphasia also have concomitant aphasia in which anomia is common is that compre-
symptoms that are typically associated with right hension and syntactic production in anomic aphasia
hemisphere lesions, such as left visual neglect and are relatively spared. Typical symptoms are circum-
visuospatial deficits. locutions, the use of generic terms (e.g., “thing”
142  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

instead of an intended noun or “girl” instead of a • The actual classification assigned based
specific girl’s name), and production of fillers such on aphasia assessment results may vary
as “uh,” “you know,” and “like.” depending on which assessment battery is
used (Crary et al., 1992; Henseler et al., 2014).
• People with right or bilateral hemispheric
How Might Dyslexia and dominance for language still acquire aphasia.
Dysgraphia Be Conceptualized as • People with aphasia secondary to subcortical
lesions, degenerative conditions, traumatic
Symptoms Versus Syndromes?
brain injury, and multiple or unknown sites
of lesion further challenge us in classifying
In all types of aphasia, reading abilities tend to mir- of aphasia types based on neuroanatomical
ror auditory comprehension abilities and writing correlates.
abilities tend to mirror speaking abilities, unless • Current classification systems do not take
there are concomitant deficits that would differen- into account important findings relevant to
tially affect any of those abilities, or unless a per- the distinction of highly specialized brain
son had low or no literacy prior to onset of aphasia. regions (e.g., Broca’s area) versus domain-
Thus, dyslexia and dysgraphia may be considered general brain regions that contribute to
symptoms of aphasia. There are individuals, though, cognitive aspects of language processing (e.g.,
who have significant reading deficits but relatively working memory, executive functioning,
good auditory receptive language abilities and good and processing of actions; Binder, 2015, 2017;
language production. In such cases, the primary Fedorenko et al., 2012; Flinker & Knight, 2018;
diagnosis may be dyslexia rather than aphasia. Dys- Fridriksson et al., 2018).
lexia may occur with or without dysgraphia.
Whitaker (1984) suggests that use of the term
syndrome is problematic when considering aphasia.
What Are Limitations of Classification By definition, he argues, a syndrome is a set of symp-
Systems Based on Relating Function toms that co-occur to constitute a certain condition.
For an individual to be considered to have a certain
to Neuroanatomical Structure?
type of aphasia, we tend not to be very strict about
whether all of their symptoms fall within the symp-
All aphasia classification systems have limitations. tom constellation that is characteristic of that type of
Those based on associating linguistic deficits to aphasia. If not all of the set of symptoms associated
structural deficits are listed here. with a type of aphasia have to be present for a per-
son to have that type of aphasia, then in what way is
• Given that language and related aspects it helpful to characterize a person as having a certain
of cognition depend on multicomponent syndrome of aphasia?
networks throughout the brain, functional Brookshire (1983) suggested that categorization
correlates corresponding to specific structures of aphasia is more a matter of the beliefs and biases
are not truly separable (Fridriksson et al., 2018). of aphasiologists than it is of the actual characteris-
• Linguistic functioning relies not only on tics of people who have aphasia. Furthermore, even
structural components but also on electro- if we could be completely confident of an objective
chemical properties and dynamic blood flow. means of characterizing a person’s symptoms and
• A minority of people with aphasia can be corresponding neuropathology according to a clas-
reliably classified based on their site of lesion sic aphasia type, we still may not know a great deal
alone (Marshall, 1984). about the underlying nature of their deficits.
• There are people whose aphasia symptom There is no classification system that is agreed
constellations fit into one particular category upon by the worldwide community of clinical apha-
at one time but evolve to another type of siologists (Byng et al., 1990; Holland et al., 1986;
aphasia at another time. Luria & Hutton, 1977; Marshall, 1983; McNeil &
10. Syndromes and Hallmark Characteristics of Aphasia   143

Kimelman, 2001). Even if there were a universally predictive of actual cognitive-linguistic


accepted classification scheme, consider the limita- symptoms), there would still be overlap of
tions that would persist. corresponding sites of lesion across people
with varied types of aphasia.
• There would be many people with • Clinical aphasiologists would still have
aphasia whose symptoms would defy the to engage in excellent clinical judgment
classification boundaries. and problem-solving to best promote an
• There would still be tremendous variability individual’s best recovery of language and
in the symptom constellations and levels social participation.
of severity according to each area of deficit
within each aphasia type. Given this state of affairs, some aphasiologists
• There would still be overlap in symptoms suggest that we may benefit by being flexible in using
noted (e.g., word-finding problems and different classification models toward different ends
challenges in life participation). (Bartlett & Pashek, 1994; Schwartz, 1984). As Bay so
• Clinicians would likely remain inconsistent cleverly stated in 1964, “To ponder too much over
in terms of judgments about which specific one or another system of classification appears to be
type of aphasia any given person would be an idle and sterile occupation” (p. 122). If you wish
considered to have. to be challenged further in considering alternative
• Unless the classification scheme were based classification schemes for aphasia, be sure to check
on site of lesion alone (thus not necessarily out the relevant references cited in this chapter.

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter 7. Consider each of the following types of
that are new to you or that you have not yet aphasia:
mastered. • Broca’s aphasia
2. Practice explaining to a partner what is • Wernicke’s aphasia
meant by the dichotomous classifications • Conduction aphasia
of fluent/nonfluent aphasia, expressive/ • Anomic aphasia
receptive aphasia, and posterior/anterior • Global aphasia
aphasia. • Transcortical sensory aphasia
3. Why is it problematic to classify aphasia • Transcortical motor aphasia
dichotomously as “fluent” versus • Primary progressive aphasia
“nonfluent”? • Crossed aphasia
4. What does it mean when one refers to • Subcortical aphasia
aphasia as a “dissociation syndrome”? For each:
5. Give examples of phonemic (literal) and a. Identify typical associated site(s) of lesion
semantic (global) paraphasias. according to classic models of structure
6. Describe varied ways in which the cause of and function. Be sure to refer to names of
agrammatism in people with Broca’s aphasia structures, names of any associated gyri
might be explained. What are the strengths and sulci, and numbers for associated
and weaknesses of arguments supporting Brodmann’s areas.
any given explanation for why people with b. Identify the origin of the blood supply for
Broca’s aphasia tend to have trouble with associated structures/brain regions.
grammatical processing? c. List the hallmark features and symptoms.
144  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

8. Compare and contrast each aphasia type r. Have word-finding problems?


listed above with other types. s. Show catastrophic reactions during
9. Describe the similarities and differences interaction?
between primary progressive aphasia (PPA) t. Use jargon in conversation?
and dementia. u. Perseverate on a word?
10. Imagine two people engaging in v. Perseverate on a task?
conversation with you, one with Broca’s As you consider each of these items, be sure
aphasia and the other with Wernicke’s to consider how any of these tendencies
aphasia. Which one is most likely to: might be different within specific individuals
a. Speak “fluently”? with aphasia.
b. Have concomitant hemiplegia? 11. Observe video samples of people with
c. Have relatively good comprehension in varied types of aphasia. For each individual
conversation? you observe, discuss the following:
d. Perseverate on a word or a task? a. Physical characteristics (specific aspects
e. Be depressed? of appearance, muscle tone, coordination,
f. Have phonemic paraphasias? speech, posture, etc.) that you think may
g. Have press of speech or logorrhea? have been affected by their stroke or brain
h. Use fewer function (closed-class) words injury
than content (open-class) words? b. Aspects of language you think may have
i. Use more nouns than verbs? been affected by their stroke or brain
j. Demonstrate emotional lability? injury
k. Use agrammatic speech? c. Aspects of speech you think may have
l. Use telegraphic speech? been affected by their stroke or brain
m. Have poor self-monitoring of their injury
linguistic errors? d. The likely impact of aphasia on that
n. Be especially upset or depressed about individual’s participation in activities
their loss of language abilities? that might have been important to them
o. Struggle overtly with articulating before the stroke
words?
p. Have concomitant apraxia of speech? Additional learning and teaching materials are
www
q. Have concomitant dysarthria? available on the companion website.
CHAPTER
11
Cognitive-Communicative Challenges
Associated With Traumatic Brain Injury

In Chapter 6, we reviewed basic aspects of the


nature, types, and causes of traumatic brain injury
Why Is It Hard to Generalize
(TBI). In the present chapter, we follow up with a About TBI Survivors?
discussion of the nature of the varied populations of
TBI survivors and the many cognitive-communica- We introduced this chapter by describing the popula-
tive challenges that they (and those who care about tion of TBI survivors as “extremely heterogeneous.”
them) face. We consider special clinical practice chal- There is no clear diagnostic profile to describe this
lenges for clinicians working with this extremely group. They represent a wide range of ages, nation-
heterogeneous population. We also review situations alities, cultures, ethnicities, socioeconomic back-
related to economic influences and health care con- grounds, and education levels. The majority are
texts in which speech-language pathologists (SLPs) people who have fallen or who have been in motor
may play a role, not only by providing direct clinical vehicle accidents. Many have been injured in sport-
services but also by serving as consultants, expert ing activities. Some have been wounded in military
witnesses, educators, and advocates. This content is combat. They may be victims or perpetrators of vio-
integrated further in most of the remaining chapters lence, or innocent bystanders caught in unexpected
of this book, as we consider service delivery (Section trauma (e.g., acts of terror, structural collapses of
IV), assessment (Section V), theories of intervention buildings and bridges, falling objects).
(Section VII), and treatment (Section VIII) across the Overall, TBI survivors constitute a much
array of acquired neurogenic communication disor- younger population than the stroke or dementia
ders, including those associated with TBI. population. The most commonly affected group is
After reading and reflecting on the content in males between 14 and 25 years old, although adults
this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in over age 75 years and children under age 4 years
your own words, the following queries: have a relatively high incidence of TBI, too. Males
are about two times more likely than females to
1. Why is it hard to generalize about TBI survivors? experience a TBI. This is largely attributable to neu-
2. What communication symptoms are likely to rophysiological differences in gender-related hor-
be experienced by TBI survivors? mones and frontal lobe development in males in the
3. What are special challenges for war- and 14- to 25-year-old range and the corresponding rel-
terrorism-related TBI survivors? ative lack of inhibition of dangerous impulses, risk-​
4. What are special considerations for clinicians taking behaviors, and aggression during that time of
working with TBI survivors? development. Given the younger age range of TBI
5. What are special challenges faced by TBI survivors, those who are seen clinically by SLPs are
survivors in health care contexts? more likely than people in other diagnostic groups
6. What special economic considerations affect with acquired neurogenic communication disorders
clinical work with TBI survivors? to have concerns about return to employment and

145
146  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

education. Thus, intervention goals and activities are lectively as cognitive-communicative impairments.
more likely to focus on communicative competence Common sequelae related to cognition, communica-
required for work and schooling. tion, and behavior in TBI survivors are summarized
The severity of challenges faced by TBI survivors in Box 11–1. Of course, any given individual with a
ranges from barely noticeable symptoms to complete TBI experiences their own unique set of challenges
loss of functional abilities in all activities of daily that may be only grossly summarized as a subset
living or even coma or vegetative state. Their con- of symptoms on such a list. Given the focus of this
comitant medical concerns and disorders are highly book, our emphasis here is on cognitive-linguistic
variable. For some, recovery is quick; for others, it is symptoms, not speech disorders. Still, knowing a
protracted over decades. Given the sheer diversity great deal about motor speech disorders is crucial
of causes that lead to TBI, clinicians working with for excellent clinical practice to help TBI survivors
this population ideally have a great deal of flexibility improve communication.
in adjusting to unpredictable needs and behaviors, Considering the ICF framework (see Chapter 5)
embracing complexity, and potentially accepting dif- helps us to reflect on both the body structure and
ferences in values that may not align with their own. function of TBI survivors as they pertain to commu-
nication as well as the absolutely paramount aspects
of activities and life participation. The items listed
What Communication Symptoms Are Likely in Box 11–1 pertain primarily to specific aspects of
functioning related to brain injury. Recall the details
to Be Experienced by TBI Survivors
about the diverse types and causes of head injury that
we explored in Chapter 6. Most of the body structure
TBI may cause aphasia, and about one-third of TBI challenges associated with communication problems
survivors have aphasia. Their language problems in people with TBI result from damage to the frontal
are more commonly associated with cognitive and lobes, limbic structures, and critical axonal connec-
behavioral challenges and are often grouped col- tions among prefrontal and limbic structures.

Box
11–1 Common Sequelae Related to Cognition,
Communication, and Behavior in TBI Survivors

Cognitive Characteristics n Retrograde (loss of recall for events


• Impaired verbal and nonverbal memory preceding injury)
• Areas of deficit n Paramnesia (disturbance in

n Working memory identification of location and


n Short-term memory surroundings)
n Long-term memory • Impaired sensory integration
n Procedural memory/implicit memory • Reduced attention
n Declarative memory/explicit memory • Decreased arousal
n Episodic memory • Decreased and variable alertness
n Semantic memory • Problems with selective/focused attention
n Prospective memory • Problems with sustained attention
n Source memory • Deficits in attention switching/shifting/
• Memory-associated learning problems divided/alternating attention
n Encoding • Variations in cognitive effort
n Storage • Challenges with resource allocation
n Retrieval • Slowed speed of processing/reduced
• Amnesia cognitive efficiency
n Anterograde (loss of recall for events • Problems with executive functions
following injury) • Reasoning
11. Cognitive-Communicative Challenges Associated With Traumatic Brain Injury   147

• Judgment Hearing Characteristics


• Decision-making • Conductive and sensorineural hearing loss
• Goal setting, planning, strategizing • Speech discrimination problems
• Awareness of strengths and weaknesses • Central auditory processing disorders
• Organizing
• Sequencing Behavioral Characteristics
• Impaired reasoning • Personality changes due to increased
• Convergent thinking irritability
• Divergent thinking • Sudden mood changes
• Anxiety and frustration
Language Characteristics
• Depression
• Word-finding difficulties
• Restlessness
• Difficulty with comprehension and
• Reduced self-esteem and self-confidence
production of abstract language
• Hyperactivity
• Impaired verbal reasoning
• Impaired concentration
• Impaired verbal learning
• Impulsivity
• Dyslexia
• Egocentricity
• Dysgraphia
• Emotional lability and excessive laughing
• Paraphasias
or crying
• Impaired discourse and pragmatic abilities
• Inappropriate social judgment
• Impaired organization and cohesion of
• Fluctuating moods
written and spoken language
• Hypersexuality
• Inappropriate topic switching
• Inability to self-monitor
• Impaired turn taking
• Lack of motivation
• Inappropriate use and interpretation of
• Self-centeredness
facial expressions
• Inability to control emotions
• Difficulty interpreting and using prosody
• Lack of insight
• Challenges interpreting metaphor and
• Denial of physical and mental limitations
humor
• Literal interpretations of environmental
• Social disinhibition/impaired
situations
monitoring of appropriateness of
• Confusion
language use in social contexts
n Inappropriate humor
• Confabulation
n Inappropriate self-disclosure in light
• Perseverations
• Stimulus boundedness
of social contexts
• Decreased initiation
• Difficulties using language to aid
• Impaired visual processing skills
memory and logical organization
• Fatigue
Speech Characteristics • General mental slowing
• Slowed speech • Low tolerance for visual and auditory
• Slurred speech extraneous stimuli
• Inappropriate intonation • Motor control difficulties

Note. Characteristics are highly variable within and across individual TBI survivors, in terms of occurrence
as well as severity. Social and environmental contexts are absolutely vital in determining the influence of any
characteristic on an individual’s communicative competence at any given point in time. Many of the terms listed
here may be found in the Glossary.
Sources: Chan, 2000; Elbourn et al., 2018; Keltner & Cooke, 2007; Musiek et al., 2004; Myers et al., 2009; Niemann
et al., 1996; Sohlberg & Mateer, 2010.
148  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Given the bony prominence of the frontal com- growing. Although blast injury (BI) TBI incidence has
ponents of the skull, and thus the likelihood that the continued to rise dramatically, non-blast-induced
brain will contact those during a traumatic injury, (NBI) TBI remains a serious concern as well in the
most TBI survivors have some involvement of the context of terrorism and war. Most reported military
frontal lobes, regardless of the locus of coup or con- injuries since the early 2000s have been blast related;
trecoup injuries. Left and right orbital frontal lobe the next most common causes are vehicular accidents,
injury tends to be associated with a constellation of falls, penetrating fragments, and bullets (Agimi et al.,
symptoms often called frontal lobe syndrome (FLS). 2019; Terrio et al., 2009). The true incidence of war-re-
FLS symptoms and the severity are variable within lated TBI is unknown, especially because many do
and across individuals. The most common aspects not seek immediate care; many mild injuries are not
of FLS are executive function deficits (challenges detected immediately and only reported months or
with self-regulation, reasoning, making judgments years later (Cornis-Pop et al., 2012).
and decisions, goal setting, planning, strategizing, Recall the complex nature of BI TBI etiologies
being aware of strengths and weaknesses, organiz- discussed in Chapter 6. Clearly, impacts of BI TBI
ing, sequencing, allocating attention, and inhibiting on individuals’ cognitive-communicative abilities
in appropriate behaviors) and pragmatic deficits are also complex, and much remains to be studied
(problems with the social use of language). Some about them. Hallmark symptoms of war- and ter-
people with FLS have difficulty with use and inter- rorism-related BI TBI include deficits in language
pretation of gestures, facial expressions, and speech comprehension, language formulation, speech,
prosody, as we explore further in Chapter 12. Many attention, memory, judgment, and decision-making,
have depression. FLS manifestations are often per- as well as hyperactivity, personality changes, irrita-
ceived as aspects of personality change. Involvement bility, anxiety, headaches, fatigue, increased sensitiv-
of the hippocampus and surrounding limbic tissue ity to noise and light, and insomnia (Cherney et al.,
is associated with impaired declarative and explicit 2010; Trudeau et al., 1998). Even mild forms of BI
memory. Injury to other limbic structures and fron- TBI may lead to severe, life-affecting consequences
tolimbic connections contributes to difficulty in emo- in all of these areas. Indexing these consequences is
tional and behavioral self-regulation. complicated by the fact that both BI and non-BI TBI
Challenges with engaging in and maintaining are commonly accompanied by post-traumatic stress
meaningful interactions and relationships tend to disorder (PTSD) and depression (Agoston, 2017;
occur across TBI survivors, regardless of the specific Cherney et al., 2010; Elder & Cristian, 2009; Hicks
nature of their symptoms. Recall that the activities and et al., 2010; Hoffman et al., 2010; Hoge et al., 2008;
participation components of the ICF include a distinc- Huckans et al., 2010; Martin et al., 2008; Rosenblatt
tion of capacity versus performance. Many people et al., 2019; Rosenfeld & Ford, 2010; Schneiderman et
with TBI have highly variable performance character- al., 2008; Vasterling et al., 2009), conditions that tend
istics based on the actual environments in which they to affect many of the same important aspects of daily
engage. For this reason, any aspect of assessing and life participation. Additionally, associated motor,
treating the body structure and function challenges hearing, and visual impairments commonly affect
of a TBI survivor should be in the context of a focus the validity of patients’ responses during behavioral
on real-life social engagement and other aspects of assessment of cognition and language.
life participation. All aspects of intervention should
include friends, family members, and any others who
play important roles in the TBI survivor’s life. What Are Special Considerations for
Clinicians Working With TBI Survivors?

What Are Special Challenges for War- and


Terrorism-Related TBI Survivors? Scope of Practice

Clinical practice with TBI survivors has led to a deep-


The body of research on the neuropsychological ening and widening of SLPs’ roles in the assessment
aspects of war- and terrorism-related TBIs is steadily and treatment of cognitive challenges that affect
11. Cognitive-Communicative Challenges Associated With Traumatic Brain Injury   149

communication. Knowing a great deal about neuro- Psychiatric Association) used to delineate TBI
genic communication disorders, and having clinical sequelae
expertise with people with aphasia, is a great founda- • inconsistency in design and lack of
tion for work with TBI survivors. The best practices thoroughness among many of the available
and principles for effective assessment, treatment, assessment instruments
counseling, and advocacy are similar across all clin- • discrepancies between standardized test
ical groups within the realm of acquired neurogenic results and how individuals actually perform
communication disorders. Still, expert clinicians in in real-life contexts
this arena are ideally tooled with additional special- • a lack of sophisticated and detailed research on
ized knowledge and skills pertinent to TBI. cognition, language, and psychosocial aspects
of the life affecting consequences of TBI
• a lack of understanding about how cognitive
Interdisciplinary Collaboration and linguistic challenges in TBI survivors
are related and how deficits in these areas
Given the numerous and variable physical, medical, may be associated with other concerns, such
cognitive, communicative, emotional, and mental as depression, PTSD, and motor (including
health challenges of TBI survivors, it is vital that speech), vision, and hearing deficits
SLPs work as strong interprofessional team mem- • variability in performance within and across
bers in assessing, treating, educating, counseling, individuals, especially relative to the social
and advocating for TBI survivors and the people use of language
who care for them.
All forms of TBI, whether involving blast, acceler-
ation/deceleration, rotational, or penetrating injuries,
Assessment Challenges and whether war related or not, pose critical diagnos-
tic challenges due to the complexity of overlapping
Although it is relatively simple to list cognitive, symptoms. It is not always clear just which aspects of
linguistic, and behavioral characteristics (as in cognition and language are affected, in what ways, and
Box 11–1), and to observe them in individual TBI sur- to what degree for a given individual. As we explore
vivors, it is far more complex to address fundamen- in Section V, there are many tests, survey instruments,
tal questions related to assessment and rehabilitation. questionnaires, and rating scales that have been tested
For example, consider the following questions: for administration with TBI survivors. Still, given the
sheer variability in etiology and complexity of symp-
• Given that many of these characteristics affect tomatology across individuals, using and interpret-
one another, is it possible to individually ing the results of such assessments requires in-depth
describe or index each characteristic distinctly problem-solving on the clinician’s part. Furthermore,
using standardized assessment methods? unless one assesses an individual in real-life commu-
• How does one sort out which of these nicative situations, it cannot be clear how any aspect
characteristics may have been present in an of their deficits influences their meaningful life par-
individual before the brain injury? ticipation. Ongoing creative and strategic assess-
• What impact does each of these characteristics ment throughout treatment in multiple contexts,
have on a particular individual’s actual com- using standardized and nonstandardized methods,
municative effectiveness in real-life situations? is essential (Mashima et al., 2019; Turkstra, Coelho, &
Ylvisaker, 2005; Turkstra, Ylvisaker, et al., 2005).
Special difficulties in the differential diagnosis
of the many symptoms of TBI are rooted in six crit-
ical challenges:
What Are Special Challenges Faced by
• inconsistencies across varied classification TBI Survivors in Health Care Contexts?
systems (e.g., the American Congress of
Rehabilitation Medicine, the American In much of the world, specialized interdisciplinary
Academy of Neurology, and the American rehabilitation services for TBI survivors are lacking.
150  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Even when such services are available, these indi- memory, judgment, moods, or concentration,
viduals face particular difficulties in accessing them. for example, and thus do not know that it is
That is, in addition to the myriad communicative possible to pursue services to address them.
and life participation challenges of people with TBI, • Insufficient screenings. Many health care
they face vital challenges in terms of getting the ser- facilities do not incorporate effective head
vices they need. Let’s consider why this is. injury screening protocols. Survivors of
accidents and violent incidents causing bodily
• Invisibility of deficits. Their communicative harm are often not even screened using
deficits are often not physically observable. basic mental status and cognitive-linguistic
They may look fine. They may speak well. screening tools or surveys. In fact, many are
They may seem, especially during a brief visit not even asked whether they are experiencing
with a health care provider, to be just fine. related challenges. If problems go undetected,
• Concomitant conditions. They often have they are discharged from health care facilities
medical conditions that distract health care without referrals to professionals who are
providers during acute and subacute phases qualified to help them.
of care. A person admitted to the emergency
room with facial lacerations, a broken leg, and Often more than one of these six factors come
dislocated shoulder following a car accident, into play and interact in any given individual’s post-
for example, will likely immediately be trauma health care experience. Further complicating
treated for those relatively easily detectable the situation is the fact that many members of a given
physical injuries and then be referred for health care team may not know that assessment and
further care associated with those particular intervention of communicative challenges associated
injuries. Even though they may have with cognitive-linguistic deficits are within SLPs’
sustained a serious brain injury during the scope of practice. Even when cognitive-linguistic
same accident, it may go undetected because challenges might be apparent, appropriate referrals
of the focus on other problems. may not be made. The ultimate excellent aphasiolo-
• Lack of self-awareness. TBI survivors are not gist is alert to ongoing opportunities to raise aware-
always the best judges of what they need in ness of head injury sequelae and available services to
terms of rehabilitation. Many are unaware help address them, promote interprofessional post-
of their own deficits. Without education trauma screening protocols, and provide educa-
and counseling, typically including family tional and referral materials to TBI survivors and the
members or caregivers, many are unlikely to people who care about them. A person-centered and
identify their own needs for further help. This interdisciplinary approach is paramount to address-
is often the case even in the face of significant ing needs at every stage of recovery (Mashima et al.,
impacts of a head injury on their ability to 2019). Life participation approaches are best suited
engage meaningfully with others. to long-term support and empowerment of TBI sur-
• Lack of service-seeking initiative. TBI vivors and their co-survivors.
survivors often do not independently
seek out services to address cognitive and
communicative challenges. This may be What Special Economic Considerations
because they feel embarrassed about certain
Affect Clinical Work With TBI Survivors?
symptoms they are experiencing or are afraid
of consequences of having their deficits
noticed (e.g., due to social stigma or fear of Unfortunately, the validity of statistics regarding
not being able to return to work or other pre- employability and potential for return to work for
accident activities). TBI survivors is often suspect. This is in part because
• Insufficient knowledge. Many TBI survivors some TBI survivors receive greater financial benefits
and their family members do not know that by not being employed and thus choose unemploy-
it is normal to experience challenges with ment even when they actually could return to work.
11. Cognitive-Communicative Challenges Associated With Traumatic Brain Injury   151

In the United States, financial benefits may include with the goals of finding that a person in question
Social Security Income (SSI), which often includes is malingering or trumping up the consequences of
support not only for TBI survivors but for their an injury for financial gain. TBI survivors often need
dependents as well. In many counties, people who help substantiating the real-life impacts of cognitive
are injured while working may receive workers’ and linguistic deficits because these deficits are less
compensation from their employers or from their visible and concretely apparent to laypeople. SLPs
employers’ insurance agencies. Additionally, some may serve as advocates to TBI survivors by justify-
people receive funding by winning or settling law- ing and documenting concerns about lifelong chal-
suits related to their injuries. lenges due to head injuries.
Potential financial benefits related to not return- An additional area in which financial consider-
ing to work and to legal cases associated with TBI ations might affect clinical practice is in the pursuit
may lead to inherent conflicts of interest. Sometimes of funding to support durable medical equipment
survivors stand to gain more financially if the long- (DME) for injured patients. TBI survivors who
term consequences of their injuries are documented require augmentative and alternative communica-
as being worse than they may actually be. Thus, tion devices or treatment software often rely on the
SLPs may be called upon to provide expert opinions help of SLPs to pursue such funding through insur-
and testimony about the validity of deficits claimed ance arrangements, state or federal funding pro-
and the actual consequences of an individual’s grams and agencies, or private foundations.
brain injury. We continue to consider important aspects of
Of course, financial benefits to TBI survivors serving TBI survivors in the next chapter focusing on
are sometimes highly warranted. SLPs may be asked right brain injury, as well as in the upcoming sections
to provide professional judgments, not necessarily on service delivery, assessment, and intervention.

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter • A drug dealer who has been struck in
that are new to you or that you have not yet the head during a violent brawl in a
mastered. transaction that went bad
2. In this chapter, we noted the tremendous • A drunk driver who has a head injury due
diversity of causes that lead to TBI, such to a car accident that also killed a man, his
that clinicians working with this population wife, and two of their four small children
ideally have special strengths. With a partner because the driver ran a red light
or small group, discuss what strengths you 5. Consider the general relative age of TBI
would hope to have as an SLP specializing survivors compared to stroke survivors.
in TBI. How will this influence general diagnostic
3. As an SLP working with TBI survivors, processes and formulation of treatment goals
what are some ways that you might you for people within each etiological category?
need to draw clear boundaries between 6. Refer to Box 11–1.
your professional responsibilities and your a. How might the symptoms listed
personal values and preferences? influence the reliability of your diagnostic
4. With a partner or small group, discuss evaluation of a TBI survivor?
how you might best serve each of these b. How might the various symptoms
hypothetical TBI survivors: influence a person’s prognosis?
• An injured terrorist who killed and c. Which symptoms would most influence
maimed innocent people at a mass your prioritization of treatment goals and
bombing how?
152  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

d. Give specific examples of how you, as an to SLP services for TBI survivors, even in
SLP, will collaborate with team members locations where such services are available.
to address these factors. 11. Describe two hypothetical situations in
7. Describe key difficulties in the differential which you might be called to act as an expert
diagnosis of the many symptoms of TBI. witness, documenting and perhaps testifying
8. How is the application of the International about the severity of a TBI survivor’s
Classification of Functioning, Disability, cognitive-linguistic symptoms and their
and Health (ICF) framework relevant to likely lasting impact on their livelihood.
intervention in the context of TBI? a. Give an example of a situation in which
9. Give an explicit rationale for how treatment you might be asked to detect malingering
of a person with cognitive sequelae or exaggeration of a person’s deficits.
associated with TBI is within the SLP’s scope b. Give an example of a situation in which
of practice. you might be advocating for a person
10. Describe how you would plan and legally by emphasizing the long-term and
implement a facility-wide screening protocol far-reaching negative consequences of
for trauma survivors seen at an acute care a TBI.
hospital or rehabilitation center. Be sure to
consider how you would address the six You may be interested in additional activities for
www
reasons given for a common lack of access learning and reflection on the companion website.
CHAPTER
12
Cognitive-Communicative
Disorders Associated With
Right Hemisphere Syndrome

Speech-language pathologists (SLPs) play an import- 3. What are special challenges that SLPs face in
ant role in assessing and researching the cognitive-​ serving people with RHS?
communicative challenges in people with right 4. What are special challenges faced by people
hemisphere syndrome (RHS) and in advocating for with RHS in health care contexts?
provision and reimbursement of related diagnostic
and treatment services. In this chapter, we build on
related content from the first two sections of this
What Is Right Hemisphere Syndrome?
book and Chapters 10 and 11. We summarize what
is meant by the term RHS, explore the diverse and
myriad cognitive-communicative difficulties associ- RHS is a constellation of symptoms associated with
ated with RHS, and review key challenges faced in right hemisphere damage (RHD), also called right
health care contexts by people with RHS and their hemisphere disorder or right brain injury (RBI). It
friends, caregivers, and clinicians. We conclude by may be associated with any neurological etiology,
emphasizing how essential it is that we continuously such as stroke, traumatic brain injury (TBI), tumors,
educate medical professionals, family members, or infectious processes, and the resultant damage
stroke and brain injury survivors, and laypeople may be located in any part of the right hemisphere.
about the legitimacy and significance of the life-af- People with RHS are an extremely heterogeneous
fecting challenges of people with RHS. This content population in terms of the types and severity of chal-
is reflected further in the remaining chapters of this lenges they face with communication and social par-
book, as we consider service delivery (Section IV), ticipation. Further complicating attempts to study
assessment (Section V), theories of intervention this population is the fact that people with RBI may
(Section VII), and treatment (Section VIII) across the have bilateral lesions and thus additional concomi-
array of acquired neurogenic communication disor- tant deficits associated with left hemisphere injury.
ders. After reading and reflecting on the content in Some people with RHS may not have any trouble
this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in communicating. After all, the term RHS is based on
your own words, the following queries: the location of some (any kind of) physical damage
in the right hemisphere, not on specific symptoms.
1. What is right hemisphere syndrome? RHS (or RBI) is not a communication disorder. Still,
2. How may RHS affect communication and life a majority of people with RHS have difficulty with at
participation? least some aspect of communication (Tompkins, 2012).

153
154  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

leisure pursuits. In the following paragraphs, we


How May RHS Affect Communication consider major categories of potential challenges and
and Life Participation? examples of each. As we do so, let’s keep in mind
that people with RHS also have important strengths
Challenges of people with RHS that are within in terms of linguistic, intellectual, and social abilities.
the purview of SLPs are primarily in the area of As always, it is important that we balance any focus
cognitive-communicative or cognitive-linguistic on challenges with an appreciation that all people
impairments, and their consequences in term of life have vital strengths.
participation. There are myriad ways one might
categorize communication-related impairments
in people with RHS. This is evident across varied Conversation, Discourse, Pragmatics
descriptions of RHS in the literature. For example,
Myers (1999) designates the following categories of The complex interrelationships among cognitive
deficits that affect cognition and communication in and linguistic deficits in this population make it dif-
some way, whether the effects are direct or indirect: ficult or impossible to study and assess them sepa-
attentional deficits, neglect, visuospatial deficits, rately (Blake, 2017, 2021; Côté et al., 2007; Monetta
cognitive-communicative deficits, and affective and et al., 2003). Overall, the language deficits of people
emotional deficits. Blake (2006) suggests three cate- with RHS are unlike those of people with aphasia,
gories of challenge in people with RHS: communi- although there may be common symptoms between
cation, attention/perception, and cognition. Myers members of either group. Here, let’s review the com-
and Blake (2008) suggest general categories of lin- bined expressive-receptive and then expressive and
guistic, extralinguistic, and nonlinguistic deficits. receptive types of challenges that may be faced by
Blake, Frymark, and Venedictov (2013) categorize people with RHS.
symptoms as verbal, nonverbal, and both verbal and
nonverbal combined (pragmatics). Symptoms asso-
ciated with RHS as reported by many authors com- Combined Receptive and Expressive Challenges
bined are summarized in Box 12–1. Note that any of
those symptoms could be reorganized into any of Deficits in pragmatics, the social use of language in
the categorical schemes mentioned earlier. context, attributed to RHS include difficulty with
There are two especially important points to topic maintenance and discourse cohesion, impul-
keep in mind regarding RHS-associated impair- sivity, disinhibition of inappropriate utterances,
ments. One is that their incidence and severity are challenges with judging the appropriateness of con-
highly variable within and across individuals. The versational content, poor eye contact, poor conver-
second is that, even if mild, the associated challenges sational turn taking, failure to interpret nonverbal
may affect the quality of social interactions and com- cues from a conversational partner, problems notic-
municative effectiveness. Thus, they may have sig- ing communicative breakdown, and failure to back
nificant impacts on educational, career, social, and up and repair communicative breakdown.

Box
12–1 Cognitive-Communicative Challenges Associated With Right Brain Syndrome (RHS)

Challenges in conversation/discourse/pragmatics • Poor turn taking, frequent interruptions


during conversation
Expressive and receptive challenges
• Poor eye contact
• Lack of perspective regarding another
• Problems making use of contextual cues
person’s feelings or point of view (theory
of mind) Receptive challenges
• Codeswitching deficits • Problems interpreting themes, morals,
• Inattentiveness main ideas
12. Cognitive-Communicative Disorders Associated With Right Hemisphere Syndrome   155

• Problems with making inferences Executive function deficits


• Tendency toward literal interpretation of
Problems with
figurative language (difficulty with idioms,
• Reasoning
indirect requests, sarcasm)
• Judgment
• Difficulty shifting topics
• Decision-making
• Difficulty interpreting facial expressions
• Goal setting, planning, strategizing
• Difficulty interpreting humor
• Self-monitoring, awareness of strengths
• Receptive aprosodia
and weaknesses
Expressive challenges • Problem-solving
• Poor topic maintenance, relevance, discourse • Organizing
cohesion, organization of content, use of • Sequencing
macrostructure, main ideas, and themes • Considering new hypotheses in light of
• Inefficient expression, inappropriate level new information
of detail
• Frequent tangential comments Reading problems
• Flat affect or inappropriate emotional • Visuospatial difficulties in interpreting
expression letters and words
• Dysprosody • Problems interpreting content, as noted for
• Limited initiation of conversations auditory comprehension
• Reduced information units in discourse
• Reduced use of facial expressions to convey Writing problems
emotion and meaning • Visuospatial difficulties with writing or
• Disinhibition of inappropriate language copying letters, words, ideographs, and
and humor symbols
• Confabulation • Problems with expression, as noted for
• Hypoaffectivity discourse
• Hyperaffectivity
• Expressive aprosodia Visual-perceptual impairments
• Visual memory problems
Attention problems • Prosopagnosia
• Anosognosia • Visuoconstructive deficits
• Hemispatial (left) neglect • Visuospatial disorientation
• Problems with vigilance, orientation, • Topographical disorientation
sustained attention, focused attention,
selective attention, attention allocation, and
Auditory-perceptual impairments
alternating attention/attention switching
• Amusia
Memory challenges (see also specific aspects of • Auditory agnosia
memory listed in Box 11–1) • Sound localization deficits
• Verbal • Tone perception deficits
• Nonverbal

Note. Characteristics are highly variable within and across individuals with RBI, in terms of occurrence as well as
severity. Many of these characteristics are interrelated. Social and environmental contexts are vital in determining
the influence of any characteristic on an individual’s communicative competence at any given point in time.
Sources: Balaban et al., 2016; Blake, 2005, 2021, 2021; Côté et al., 2007; Crottaz-Herbette et al., 2017; Cummings,
2019; Foerch et al., 2005; Myers, 1999; Myers & Blake, 2008; Stöttinger et al., 2018; Tompkins, 2008; Tompkins et al.,
1994, 2002; Tompkins & Lehman, 1998.
156  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Expression and appreciation of humor may Tompkins (2012) reports that once strict stimulus
change due to RBI. A person with RHS may be drawn development and metalinguistic demand controls
to off-color, childlike, or slapstick humor, sometimes are implemented, impaired performance based on
in stark contrast to their pre-onset traits. In terms of theory of mind in people with RHS has not been
production, this may relate to disinhibition of inap- supported.
propriate content and a preference for more concrete Another theory for overall cognitive-linguistic
over abstract content. In terms of comprehension, deficits in people with RBI is the cognitive resources
challenges seem to relate more to cognitive demands hypothesis (Tompkins et al., 2002). According to this
of tying usually incongruous information between view, the communication deficits seen in people with
the body of the joke and its punchline (Brownell & RHS are highly dependent on the degree of attention
Gardner, 1988). and working memory demands of a given commu-
Changes in affect associated with RHS include nicative task. Also, deficits in linguistic performance
both hypo- and hyperaffectivity. Hypoaffectivity are seen as being attributable to limited cognitive
may be demonstrated as flat expression of emotion resources mediated by the right hemisphere, not to
conveyed by reduced prosody and a lack of conver- linguistic impairments per se.
sational or social initiative. Hyperaffectivity may
be evidenced as a degree of exuberance and inces-
sant talking. Receptive Challenges
Several theories have been proposed to account
for conversational, pragmatic, and social challenges RBI survivors do not typically have much difficulty
of people with RHS. One is the social cognition with lexical and grammatical processing. Written
deficit hypothesis (Brownell & Martino, 1998), the and auditory comprehension tends to be good or
notion that right hemisphere networks are import- only mildly impaired. When they do have trouble
ant for critical aspects of relating to others, such as with comprehension, it often may be attributable
empathy, and understanding and responding to to cognitive deficits, especially attention and work-
others’ perspectives. Some authors have proposed ing memory, and to challenges with inferencing
that people with RHS may have deficits in theory of and interpretation. Impairment of comprehension
mind, the ability to interpret, infer, and predict the of written or spoken discourse may be exhibited
thoughts, beliefs, feelings, and intentions of others through problems with identifying main ideas
and to differentiate the thoughts and perceptions (Brookshire & Nicholas, 1984; Hough, 1990).
of others from one’s own. This has been suggested Several authors have reported that people
especially in light of a tendency not to take the per- with RHS tend to literally interpret figurative lan-
spectives of another person into account when com- guage — that is, expressions that require abstraction
municating (Balaban et al., 2016). For example, it to infer meaning that cannot be gained through lit-
might explain in part why an individual may not eral interpretation. Examples of figurative language
orient to a listener’s lack of knowledge about a topic are often found in idioms (e.g., “it’s raining cats and
and leave out important details the listener would dogs” when it is pouring), indirect requests (e.g.,
need to know in order to understand the main idea “look at all those dishes piling up,” to suggest that
or intent of what is being expressed. Likewise, it may one’s housemate pitch in with housework), and sar-
help explain why they may include too much infor- casm (e.g., “nice job,” when someone has made a
mation, not taking into account the listener’s exist- mistake). Kempler et al. (1999) suggested that part of
ing knowledge. the trouble with a tendency toward literal interpre-
Critics of the theory-of-mind explanation have tation may have to do with difficulty with abstract
suggested that it does not help us understand thinking in general.
the nature of pragmatic problems any more than Myers and Linebaugh (1981) suggested that
describing the problems themselves does. Find- tasks used to study understanding of figurative
ings regarding theory of mind as a construct that language may lead to inflated findings of deficits
can be indexed using explicit dependent measures because of their heavy reliance on metalinguistic
have not been consistently reported in the literature. skills to explain explicitly what is meant by expres-
12. Cognitive-Communicative Disorders Associated With Right Hemisphere Syndrome   157

sions in isolation. Actual interpretation may be in a logical way to express ideas effectively and
more accurate than is evidenced by overt explana- efficiently. They may leave out important elements
tions of interpreted meanings. In any case, the actual when telling a story, include irrelevant information,
occurrence of incorrect interpretation of figurative and get sidetracked into completely different topics
language appears to be less when studied during without fully expressing something they seemed to
conversational contexts, where there are more ample have set out to express. When given elements of a
cues about what a speaker’s intended meanings are. story, conveyed through pictures or text, they may
Inferencing in the context of communication is have trouble assembling them in a logical order
the act of making a logical conclusion about intended (Schneiderman et al., 1992).
meaning based on what has been communicated. We It has also been suggested that people with RHS
make inferences whenever we make connections have greater difficulty than others with codeswitch-
between different components of discourse, even ing, or taking into account the person with whom
when we simply understand the relevance of one one is speaking in considering appropriate adap-
sentence or phrase based on a prior sentence. People tations of what is being expressed and how it is
with RHS tend to have more difficulty with inferenc- being expressed. The ability to tailor our conversa-
ing when making judgments about the intentions or tional style and content in light of what is socially,
emotions of characters in spoken language or writ- culturally, and linguistically acceptable according
ten text. For example, explaining the motives of a to a given communicative context is vital to our
character in a story is more difficult than compre- codeswitching competence. For example, a bilin-
hending what the character did. gual speaker may not change readily back and forth
Inferencing difficulty has been attributed at between speaking Spanish to his mother and English
least in part to three factors: challenges with abstract to his daughter. A person who swears readily with
thinking in general, metalinguistic abilities that are friends and family who are comfortable with swear-
taxed when people are asked to explain their inter- ing at home may fail to cease swearing when engag-
pretations, and difficulty in taking the emotional ing in conversations with professional colleagues or
perspectives of characters about whom inferences acquaintances in a religious organization who may
are to be made when the emotions of the respondent find swearing offensive.
differ from the characters’ emotions. Challenges in Sometimes right hemisphere injuries lead to
interpreting emotions of speakers and of characters deficits in the use of prosody, the intonation, stress,
in stories have been attributed by some authors as and rhythm of speech. The degree to which the right
related to changes in processing of emotions. The hemisphere is implicated directly in expressive and
nature of such changes is far from clear and may not receptive language is influenced by whether an indi-
be similar across groups of people with RHS (Blake, vidual speaks a tonal language. Tonal languages
2005, 2017, 2021). The suppression deficit hypoth- are languages in which changes in tones (or pitch
esis (Tompkins & Lehman, 1998) suggests that peo- and pitch contours) change the literal meaning of a
ple with RHS are typically able to generate multiple word. Examples are Mandarin Chinese, Cantonese,
interpretations of words, sentences, and stories but Taiwanese, Vietnamese, the class of Bantu languages
have a harder time selecting a most plausible inter- in Africa, and some Mayan languages in Central
pretation when given suggested interpretations from America. In nontonal languages, pitch changes may
which to choose. As with literal interpretation, the be used for emphasis or to convey certain nuances
way that inferencing is studied in people with RHS of meaning, but they do not affect the literal mean-
has a strong influence on the results obtained (Blake, ing of words. Thus, speakers of tonal languages are
2017; Lehman-Blake & Tompkins, 2001). at risk for greater language deficits associated with
dysprosodia or dysprosody (prosodic deficits).
Regardless of whether a person speaks a tonal
Expressive Challenges language or not, comprehension may be impaired
due to a lack of appropriate interpretation of prosody
Some people with RHS tend to have challenges with and of nonverbal cues of another speaker. Addition-
discourse coherence, the tying together of content ally, challenges in interpretation of stress and into-
158  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

nation may be at the root of some of the figurative sognosia, the lack of awareness of an illness or defi-
language deficits explored earlier. In conversation, cit, often resulting in denial of problems. One of the
some people with RHS have trouble using prosody conditions neglected is often the condition of neglect
for emphasis and to enliven engagement with a lis- itself. Anosognosia can be a serious impediment to
tener. Speech output may seem monotonous. The the communication rehabilitation process (Jehkonen
rate of speech may be unusually fast or slow, and it et al., 2006); acknowledging that there is a problem
may be difficult to interpret the speaker’s emotional and wanting to address it are key motivational ele-
state from the way they are talking. ments supporting an individual’s active engagement
in treatment and associated activities to support the
integration of treatment gains into everyday com-
Attention Deficits municative contexts.

Attention deficits that have been attributed to RHS


include reduced alertness and orientation to the Memory Challenges
external environment, a decrease in the ability to
sustain attention and vigilance, and a decrease in Memory impairments in RHS have been generally
selective and alternating attention. Deficits in atten- categorized just as those considered in Chapter 11
tion tend to co-occur with learning and memory for people with TBI. Everyday functional challenges
problems; this is logical in that attention is vital to related to memory may include difficulty with
learning and memory. Also, the construct of atten- working memory for processing of long or complex
tion is implicated in any means of indexing the con- sentences, remembering of instructions, and remem-
structs of learning and memory. bering to carry out specific actions such as taking med-
Left visual neglect, also called hemi-inattention ications, replacing batteries, or turning off a strove.
or hemispatial neglect, may also occur due to RBI. As
reviewed in Chapter 7, left visual neglect is a reduced
ability to attend to or generate an internal represen- Executive Function Challenges
tation of information on a person’s left side. It is not
a sensory problem in that the information neglected Other deficits attributed to RHS include deficits in
is actually registered in the primary sensory areas of executive functions, such as difficulty with reason-
the brain. It may be perceived as a lack of sensitivity ing, judgment, decision-making, goal setting, plan-
to, awareness of, and responsiveness to visual, audi- ning, self-monitoring, sequencing, problem-solving,
tory, somatosensory, and olfactory stimuli (Myers, and organization. Note that many of the deficits
1999). The area neglected may correspond to an described earlier actually include elements of these
individual’s bodily midline (egocentric neglect). constructs, further highlighting the complexity of
Alternatively, the neglected area may be relative to attempts to study and describe the nature of RHS.
the individual’s subjective frame of reference at any Some problems with executive functioning lead to
given moment (allocentric neglect); for example, it difficult challenges in rehabilitation for people with
may be relative to an object on which they are focus- RHS. For example, memory supports such as to-do
ing or to the walls of a room. The degree of informa- lists, calendars, medication reminders, and memory
tion neglected can often be modified by changing books may be in place, yet an individual may fail
the position of stimuli, adding auditory stimuli to to actually engage in using such supports in real-
a visual task, or changing the person’s intentional life situations.
focus of attention. Hemispatial neglect often resolves
within weeks or months following stroke or brain
injury but can persist indefinitely, especially in peo- Visual-Perceptual Impairments
ple with large cortical lesions (Maguire & Ogden,
2002). Means of screening for visual neglect are RBI survivors may have any of an array of visual
given in Chapter 18. processing deficits. Aside from visual acuity or
As with TBI survivors in general, another form color perception deficits, their challenges primarily
of neglect that some people with RHS have is ano- involve integrating and interpreting visual informa-
12. Cognitive-Communicative Disorders Associated With Right Hemisphere Syndrome   159

tion. For example, they may have difficulty judging right hemisphere, and characterizing what is “nor-
spatial relationships, drawing, copying figures, dis- mal” in the context of real-life communication.
tinguishing important components of images from
background details (figure-ground problems), and
interpreting visual cues in the environment or in pic- Underdiagnosis and Lack of Awareness of RHS
tures. Some experience visual agnosia or prosopag-
nosia, or difficulty recognizing familiar faces. One need only consider the long list of RHS symp-
toms described in this chapter to know that they cer-
tainly have an impact on one’s ability to participate
Auditory-Perceptual Impairments fully in daily life activities, especially in those involv-
ing interactions with others. Still, many among the
As mentioned earlier, people with RHS may have complex array of RHS symptoms are less obvious
impairments in processing and interpreting tonal and less easily describable than those of left brain
and melodic aspects of speech (dyprosody). They injury survivors who have overt, obvious speech
may also have the following: and language deficits.
Although it is logical that people would have an
• amusia, an impairment of processing, equal likelihood of having a left or right hemisphere
remembering, and recognizing music stroke or brain injury, in fact many fewer cases of
• auditory agnosia, an inability to recognize right brain compared to left brain strokes and injuries
sounds are diagnosed in medical charts and reported in the
• sound localization deficits research literature. According to Foerch et al. (2005),
who studied data collected for over 20,000 stroke
survivors, the reason for underdiagnosis and under-
Reading and Writing Impairments reporting is not that the incidence in right versus left
strokes is different but that medical professionals, fam-
Reading challenges associated with RHS tend to be ily members, and patients are less likely to notice or
associated with visuospatial problems as well as with complain about the symptoms associated with RHS.
the general cognitive-linguistic problems described
earlier. The degree to which the right hemisphere is
involved in reading and writing at visuospatial and Symptom Classification
cognitive-linguistic levels may be influenced by the
languages one uses. The use of graphemes that cor- The very heterogeneity of the population of people
respond to sounds or words (e.g., the letter-based with RHS is daunting for researchers attempting to
Roman alphabet system), with general letter-to- capture patterns of deficit that may characterize this
sound relationships, may be affected differently in group or even subgroups within the population. For
RHS than the use of ideographic scripts, such as in example, some people with RHS are hyperrespon-
Chinese, Korean, and Japanese, in which meaning is sive, speaking incessantly, or hyporesponsive, with-
conveyed through ideograms, graphemes that rep- drawing from communication (Blake, Duffy, Myers,
resent concepts or ideas. & Tompkins, 2002). Some have flat affect while oth-
ers are ebullient, cheerful, and dramatic in nature
when engaged in conversation.
What Are Special Challenges That SLPs Classifying the symptoms is also challenging
because there are so many relationships among the
Face in Serving People With RHS?
abilities affected. For example, difficulty interpreting
facial expressions might be a pragmatic problem, a
In clinical and research contexts, SLPs navigate chal- problem of attention, or both. Failure to understand
lenges associated with underdiagnosis and lack of another person’s joke may be due to a language com-
awareness of RHS as a clinical syndrome, classifying prehension problem but also may be associated with
diverse RHS symptoms, understanding underlying problems of focused attention, flat affect, and literal
neurological structure-function relationships in the interpretation.
160  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Given the multitude of diverse symptoms that course measures. People with RHS tended to have
may occur with RHS, it is important that the diagnos- more tangential comments, demonstrate more ego-
tic process be multifaceted and that it include stan- centrism, and have differences in quantity of produc-
dardized testing and qualitative observation during tion (relatively too much or too little). Although such
tasks that tap the wide range of abilities that may results are helpful in considering ways to determine
be affected, at varied levels of difficulty. Integrating communicative strengths and weaknesses, we are
input from significant others who may have import- still challenged with the issue of figuring out what
ant insights about challenges of which the individual is normal for a given person. We might attempt to
with RHS might be unaware is essential. These topics assess this by in-depth interviewing of people who
are addressed in greater detail in Section V. knew a given person with RHS prior to the neuro-
logical change that caused RHS.
Another level of challenge in differentiating
Identifying Neurological normal from disordered abilities in people with
Structure-Function Relationships RHS is that they frequently appear to be normal,
competent communicators and thinkers unless they
Much less research has been published about right are engaged in conversations, problem-solving, and
brain as compared to left brain communication func- other cognitive-linguistic activities that rely on the
tions. Also, much less has been defined in terms of sorts of abilities that are affected in their own par-
specialization of cognitive, linguistic, and behav- ticular cases. A person with RHS is most likely to
ioral functions associated with specific right com- overcome or mask deficits during easy tasks, experi-
pared to left hemisphere structures. This may be encing more breaking down of abilities during diffi-
because there really are fewer specialized structures cult tasks. What makes a task more difficult depends
and, rather, more distributed processing through on its nature. It might be, for example, having to per-
interconnected neural networks in the right hemi- form under time pressure or in front of an audience.
sphere and associated subcortical structures (Blake, It might be trying to comprehend or express abstract
2005, 2017). as opposed to concrete ideas. It might be having to
make judgments about social appropriateness of
utterances, describe emotions that are typically asso-
Characterizing What Is Normal ciated with certain facial expressions, or switch back
and forth between the use of two languages.
When determining what is normal, it is important
to ask ourselves: Normal in comparison to whom?
Is it relative to others? Is it relative to how a per- What Are Special Challenges Faced by People
son used to be? Note how many of the long list of
With RHS in Health Care Contexts?
RHS symptoms are traits that we notice in people in
our everyday lives who have never experienced any
neurological impairment. Consider poor eye contact, The challenges that RBI survivors face in getting
turn taking, and disinhibition of inappropriate lan- services by qualified clinicians are akin to those dis-
guage, for example. Although you might not know cussed in Chapter 11 regarding TBI survivors.
one person who demonstrates all of those traits at
once, I imagine you know at least one person who • Specialized interdisciplinary rehabilitation
has trouble looking others in the eye, another who services are lacking.
interrupts others regularly, and another who swears • Communicative deficits of people with
a lot. There are personality and sociocultural influ- RHS are often invisible; they may look fine
ences on such factors that have nothing to do with and seem normal, especially during brief
brain injury. interactions in clinical contexts.
Blake (2006) analyzed the discourse of adults • They may have concomitant medical
with RHS and without any neurological disorder conditions that distract medical staff from
and found three characteristics that were more use- attending to their cognitive-communicative
ful in distinguishing these two groups through dis- challenges.
12. Cognitive-Communicative Disorders Associated With Right Hemisphere Syndrome   161

• They may not be adept at making judgments people are aware of related expertise and
about what their deficits are or what they services, problems are often not identified and
need in terms of rehabilitation. appropriate referrals are not made.
• They may not take initiative in seeking
services to address cognitive and Excellent SLPs ensure active interprofessional
communicative challenges due to fear of RHS screening protocols wherever we work, seek
stigma or other negative consequences. opportunities to educate others about RHS and
• People with RHS, their families and available associated services, raise awareness that
caregivers, and even health care professionals assessment and intervention of cognitive-communi-
may not know that RHS symptoms are cative challenges are within SLPs’ scope of practice,
treatable and that there are SLPs and provide educational and referral materials to peo-
neuropsychologists (among others) with ple with RHS and the people who care about them,
expertise to address various RHS challenges. and recognize that people with RHS have numerous
• Most clinical agencies lack effective RHS intellectual and interpersonal strengths in addition
screening protocols; as a result, even when to their challenges.

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define all bolded terms in this cognitive-communicative impairments in


chapter and any terms used in Box 12–1 that RHS from the linguistic impairments in
were unfamiliar to you or that you have not aphasia?
yet mastered. 12. Describe the pros and cons of using the
2. Explain why it is hard to summarize construct theory of mind to account for some
succinctly just what RHS is. of the social and pragmatic deficits of people
3. Consider Myers and Blake’s (2008) with RHS.
taxonomy of RBI symptoms entailing three 13. How might the cognitive resource
broad categories of linguistic, extralinguistic, hypothesis account for some of the
and nonlinguistic deficits. How would each variability in the communicative
of the symptoms listed in Box 12–1 fit into performance of an individual with RHS?
each of these categories? 14. List and describe key challenges to carrying
4. Why is RHS better characterized as a out research on that underlying nature and
cognitive-communicative syndrome than a cause of specific cognitive-communicative
linguistic syndrome? symptoms in people with RHS.
5. What linguistic deficits may be attributed to 15. What impacts might anosagnosia have on
RHS-associated impairments? the rehabilitation of a person with RHS?
6. What executive function problems may be 16. Why is RHS likely underdiagnosed
associated with RHS-associated impairments? worldwide?
7. What attention deficits may be associated 17. In what ways might the social
with RHS-associated impairments? communication skills of a person with RHS
8. What memory challenges may be attributed be difficult to differentiate from those of a
to RHS-associated impairments? person who might be considered “normal”?
9. What visual challenges may be associated 18. How might you, as an SLP, implement a
with RHS-associated impairments? screening protocol for RHS in an acute care
10. What auditory-perceptual impairments may be hospital?
associated with RHS-associated impairments?
www
11. How would you explain to a family member See the companion website for additional learning
of a person with RHS the difference between and teaching materials.
CHAPTER
13
Cognitive-Communicative Disorders in
Primary Progressive Aphasia and Dementia

In this chapter, we explore communication and life 2. What are general types of cognitive-
participation challenges of people with neurodegen- communicative impairments in people with
erative conditions. These are the most prevalent eti- MCI and dementia?
ologies for neurogenic communication disorders. In 3. What communication challenges are typically
Chapter 6, we explored the defining characteristics of associated with MCI and dementia?
neurodegenerative disease, dementia, mild cognitive 4. What symptoms are associated with common
impairment (MCI), and primary progressive aphasia forms of dementia?
(PPA). In Chapter 10, we considered an overview of 5. What are symptoms of the primary forms of PPA?
the varied forms of PPA and clarified how PPA meets 6. Is there such a thing as “reversible” dementia?
the definition of aphasia, yet is remarkably different 7. What are implications of an incorrect diagnosis
from other types of aphasia. In this chapter, we review of dementia?
common forms of dementia and PPA. We also discuss 8. What is the role of the SLP in working with
the importance of differential diagnosis, especially as people who have PPA and dementia?
it pertains to types of dementia, PPA, and symptoms
that may be misinterpreted as being due to dementia.
Given the vastness of this topic, we focus selectively What Neurodegenerative Conditions
on conditions for which speech-language patholo-
Most Commonly Affect
gists (SLPs) are most often called upon for education,
advocacy, counseling, assessment, and treatment. As
Cognitive-Linguistic Abilities?
always, person-first, empowering approaches on
the part of SLPs are recommended. This content is As introduced in Chapter 6, neurodegenerative con-
reflected further in the remaining chapters of this ditions or diseases are a broad category of disorders
book, as we consider service delivery (Section IV), entailing progressive changes in the brain that result
assessment (Section V), theories of intervention (Sec- in progressive loss of neurological functioning. Of
tion VI), and treatment (Section VIII) across the array course, in the realm of the SLP, the primary focus is
of acquired neurogenic communication disorders. on those that entail progressive loss of communica-
Means of enhancing communication in this special tion abilities and associated challenges for life par-
population are the focus of Chapter 26. ticipation. In this chapter, we focus on three primary
After reading and reflecting on the content in categories of neurodegenerative conditions with
this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in great relevance to SLPs:
your own words, the following queries:
• mild cognitive impairment (MCI), a condition
1. What neurodegenerative conditions most of cognitive decline that is not consistent with
commonly affect cognitive-linguistic abilities? normal aging

163
164  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

• dementia, a constellation of memory key feature that differentiates these conditions from
impairment plus one or more cognitive aphasia. Another important distinguishing char-
and/or linguistic impairments that have an acteristic between language disorders associated
impact on everyday functioning and social with dementia versus aphasia is that symptoms in
interactions, and represent a remarkable dementia continue to worsen over time, whereas the
change from previous levels of functioning language of people with aphasia (with the exception
• primary progressive aphasia (PPA), the of PPA) tends to get better, or at least to stabilize,
category of dementias characterized by the rather than regress.
insidious loss of linguistic abilities, eventually Although memory impairment is considered
leading to general cognitive impairments, and by many to be a hallmark or defining feature of
associated with frontotemporal degeneration dementia, some experts suggest that executive func-
and Alzheimer’s disease (AD). tion deficits are more prominent traits for dementia
than memory loss. Results from a study of 547 adults
In contrast to mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI), over age 70 years indicate that when controlling for
MCI tends to be associated with neurodegenerative age and ability to engage in activities of daily living,
changes, not injury, although some use the terms memory loss is a key component of dementia only
interchangeably. when there are concomitant impairments in execu-
Significant changes were made in how neurode- tive functioning (Royall et al., 2005).
generative conditions are classified in the fifth edi-
tion of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association,
What Communication Challenges Are
2013). Dementia and amnestic disorders, which were
previously considered separately, are now subsumed
Typically Associated With MCI and Dementia?
under a common term: neurocognitive disorder (NCD).
NCDs are considered according to whether they are Like people with TBI, the language problems of
major or mild. The term dementia is still frequently people with MCI and dementia are sometimes aptly
used and has largely not fallen out of favor because characterized as cognitive-communicative impair-
of those changes. Recognition of mild NCDs helps ments. Although MCI and dementia may co-occur
to legitimize the fact that people may have minor with aphasia, most people with MCI or dementia do
degenerative changes that nevertheless have import- not have aphasia. An increasingly widely accepted
ant impacts on their everyday activities. term for the loss of language abilities in people with
MCI and dementia is language of generalized intel-
lectual impairment. This term highlights the fact that
What Are General Types of the language loss is secondary to a loss of cognitive
Cognitive-Communicative Impairments abilities; this is the key feature that differentiates
these conditions from aphasia. Another important
in People With MCI and Dementia?
distinguishing characteristic between language dis-
orders associated with dementia versus aphasia is
Although MCI and dementia may co-occur with that symptoms in dementia continue to worsen over
aphasia, most people with MCI or dementia do time, whereas the language of people with aphasia
not have aphasia. Like people with traumatic brain (with the exception of PPA) tends to get better, or at
injury (TBI), the language problems of people with least to stabilize, rather than regress.
MCI and dementia are sometimes aptly character- Although memory impairment is considered
ized as cognitive-communicative impairments. An by many to be a hallmark or defining feature of
increasingly widely accepted term for the loss of dementia, some experts suggest that executive func-
language abilities in people with MCI and dementia tion deficits are actually more prominent traits for
is language of generalized intellectual impairment. dementia than memory loss. Results from a study
This term highlights the fact that the language loss of 547 adults over age 70 years indicate that when
is secondary to a loss of cognitive abilities; this is the controlling for age and ability to engage in activities
13. Cognitive-Communicative Disorders in Primary Progressive Aphasia and Dementia   165

of daily living, memory loss is a key component of The use of humor to cover up or draw attention
dementia only when there are concomitant impair- away from such symptoms is common. Some inten-
ments in executive functioning (Royall et al., 2005). tionally isolate themselves to avoid embarrassment.
Cognitive symptoms associated with many Many people with early dementia are adept at using
forms of MCI and dementia are listed in Box 13–1. It circumlocution to compensate for their word-finding
is not difficult to imagine how any type of memory, problems. Most have better preservation of compre-
executive function, or other cognitive impairment hension and production of concrete compared to
may, in some way, affect the quality and effective- abstract semantic content.
ness of communication. Often, early signs of demen- In mid-stages of dementia, pragmatic and exec-
tia include symptoms of communication impairment utive functioning abilities decline and word-finding
related to memory loss, such as the following: continues to worsen, whereas speech and writing
abilities remain relatively intact. Semantic deficits
• word-finding problems (including reduced through early and mid-stages are more commonly
verbal fluency, indexed in terms of the associated with impaired access rather than with
number of object names within semantic impaired storage, a topic we explored in Chapter 9
categories a person can generate within timed as we considered theories about language changes
intervals) in normal aging.
• semantic confusions in word usage Problems with syntax and phonology tend not
• errors or gaps in spoken recall of recent events, to occur until later in the disease progression; many
especially in contrast to talking about topics people with slowly progressing dementia are able to
that draw more from procedural memory carry on meaningful conversations for several years,
especially if topics from the distant past are the focus
of conversational content. Discourse-level deficits
Box
13–1 Cognitive Changes Commonly are primarily in the area of pragmatics, including
Associated With MCI and Dementia problems with the following:

• Memory problems • maintaining a topic for a cohesive


• Working versus short-term versus conversation
long-term memory • organizing a logical progression of ideas
• Episodic versus semantic versus • adjusting to an appropriate level of formality
procedural memory and politeness
• Anterograde and retrograde amnesia • orienting to the identity, needs, and desires of
• Attention problems the person to whom they are speaking
• Divided attention • self-monitoring of inappropriate language use
• Attention switching (such as swear words, sexual references, and
• Visual neglect racial slurs or use of other offensive terms)
• Focused/sustained attention
• Executive function problems Toward the end of life, many people with dementia
• Disinhibition cease to speak at all, and some produce only echo-
• Verbal and task perseveration lalic or repeated stereotypic utterances.
• Inflexibility Many of the problematic behaviors reported by
• Impaired judgment caregivers of people with dementia entail commu-
• Problems of codeswitching and social nication problems, such as repeated commenting
appropriateness or questioning, and expressions of frustration and
• Perceptual problems anger over misunderstandings (Murray et al., 1999;
• Visuospatial discrimination Orange et al., 1995; Powell et al., 1995). As the dis-
• Prosopagnosia ease progresses, the sense of grief that family mem-
• Achromatopsia bers report due to lost companionship and their
loved one’s changing identity relates largely to lost
166  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

communication abilities (Murray et al., 1999). Addi- mon form of dementia, estimated to comprise about
tional problems causing the greatest sense of burden 60% to 80% of the population of people with demen-
to caregivers include depression, anxiety, agitation, tia (Alzheimer’s Association, n.d.). It is increasingly
and wandering. Some people with AD become phys- common with age. AD is estimated to affect about
ically combative at times. Symptoms tend to worsen 45% to 60% of people over age 85 years (National
in early evening hours; this is referred to as sun- Institute on Aging, 2004). People at greater risk
downing or sundowner’s syndrome. for AD include those with a family history of AD,
In considering the communication challenges prior experience of brain injury or stroke, and risk
of people with dementia, it is important to appre- factors for cardiovascular problems, such as high
ciate that there is tremendous variability within blood pressure, high cholesterol, and diabetes (Alz-
and between people who have it, not to mention heimer’s Association, 2013). Although there have
the social and structural contexts in which they are been suggestions that environmental and nutritional
living. There is also great variability in symptoms exposure to toxins may cause AD, there are no defin-
relative to type of dementia, as well as to severity, itive studies to date supporting such claims.
time post-onset, and rate of deterioration. Not all AD entails progressive diffuse brain atrophy
people with dementia experience problems in all and accumulation of neuritic plaque (also called
areas of communication and cognition that might be amyloid plaque) and neurofibrillary tangles (Braak
expected, especially in early stages of progression. & Braak, 1991). The neuropathological changes tend
to begin in the medial and anterior temporal lobes,
then progress to the hippocampi, and then eventu-
What Symptoms Are Associated With ally to the neocortex and limbic systems. A definitive
diagnosis of AD requires an autopsy; clinical diagno-
Common Forms of Dementia?
ses of AD to living people are characterized as “prob-
able” or “possible.” AD is often suspected if there is
Varied forms of dementia may be categorized dif- no clear evidence of another type of dementia.
ferently depending on the motivation for classifica- The onset of AD is gradual. It affects many
tion. For example, one may categorize dementia in aspects of cognition, including memory, attention,
terms of the localization of associated brain pathol- and executive functions. Short-term and working
ogy, such as cortical, subcortical, or mixed demen- memory deficits are apparent in the early stages,
tias; cortical dementias might be further classified making remembering recent events and learning new
as frontal, temporal, or temporoparietal dementias. material especially challenging. Long-term memory
Dementia types may also be classified in terms abilities, in contrast to short-term, are relatively well
of etiology. For example, they may be classified as preserved in the early to mid stages.
being primarily vascular in nature, as being caused MCI due to Alzheimer’s disease (MCI due to
by changes in brain cell structure (e.g., AD), or as AD) is a condition of cognitive decline that is not
being due to some other type of medical condi- typical of normal aging and occurs prior to the onset
tion (e.g., HIV, TBI, amyotrophic lateral sclerosis of AD. A working group of the National Institute on
[ALS], Parkinson’s disease, Korsakoff’s syndrome, Aging and the Alzheimer’s Association (Albert et al.,
or Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease, each of which is dis- 2011) established two sets of criteria for diagnosing
cussed briefly later). Clinicians and laypeople alike MCI due to AD: a clinical set that does not involve
also tend to refer to early, mid, or late stages of the advanced laboratory-based testing and a research set
disease or to use scales that characterize dementia (still requiring validation for clinical use).
progression over time (see Chapter 20). The first step in the clinical diagnostic process
is ruling out stroke, vascular disease, frontotempo-
ral lobar degeneration (likely to be detected through
Alzheimer’s Disease communication or behavioral disorders as symp-
toms first emerge), neurodegenerative causes other
AD, also referred to in the literature as dementia of than dementia, or rapid decline within weeks or
the Alzheimer’s type (DAT), is by far the most com- months. Of course, the process of ruling out other
13. Cognitive-Communicative Disorders in Primary Progressive Aphasia and Dementia   167

causes is difficult, because there is always a possi- is disproportionate risk of both AD and ICVD in
bility that any given individual will have more than older people, especially over age 79 years.
one of these conditions, such as a history of stroke
in addition to evidence of AD. The second step is
documenting a change in cognitive status over time Dementia With Lewy Bodies (DLB)
reported by the patient, family member, or a clini-
cian who has observed the patient’s decline. Third, Dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) is the third most
informal or formal testing is done to provide objec- common form of dementia, accounting for about
tive evidence of and to describe specific cognitive 10% to 25% of reported dementia cases (Alzheimer’s
deficits, and also to ensure that the individual retains Association, 2013). It affects more men than women.
general functional abilities for most activities of daily It is characterized by abnormal protein (alpha-
living and does not qualify as having dementia. synuclein) deposits that are also commonly found in
Research steps for diagnosis recommended by people with AD and in people with dementia associ-
Albert et al. (2011) include use of imaging to rule out ated with Parkinson’s disease. In addition to symp-
traumatic and vascular causes of cognitive decline toms of confusion, variable states of awareness and
and, if possible, to show patterns of brain dissolu- alertness, memory loss, autonomic nervous system
tion over time. Also, genetic factors that may be rele- problems, and visual hallucinations, many people
vant are considered. Biomarkers, such as evidence of with DLB also have neuromuscular problems that are
amyloid pathology detected through cerebrospinal common to Parkinson’s disease. The latter include
fluid or positron emission tomography (PET), may muscle rigidity, tremors, and balance problems.
also be detected.

Parkinson’s-Associated Dementia
Vascular Dementia
Parkinson’s-associated dementia is a form of
Vascular dementia (also called ischemic dementia) is dementia that entails Lewy bodies and co-occurs
the second most common form of dementia, account- with Parkinson’s disease. Some people with demen-
ing for about 20% to 30% of dementia cases (Alzhei- tia associated with Parkinson’s disease also have
mer’s Association, 2013). As the name suggests, it neuritic plaques and neurofibrillary tangles typi-
is caused by problems of blood supply to the brain. cally associated with AD. It is important to note that
It may result from a stroke, or a series of strokes or dementia occurs in only some people with Parkin-
transient ischemic attacks (TIAs). As strokes and son’s disease; among those, some develop demen-
TIAs occur, symptoms tend to worsen. Thus, the tia only in the late stages of disease progression.
progression of cognitive-communicative symptoms Estimates of the proportion of people with Parkin-
tends to be stepwise, occurring in sudden, if minor, son’s disease who have an associated dementia vary
spurts rather than in a slowly continuous fashion as widely (from 18% to 80%). Regardless of the actual
is typical in other forms of dementia. When there is incidence, one should not expect that any given
evidence of multiple focal lesions, the condition may person with Parkinson’s disease will necessarily
be called multi-infarct dementia. develop dementia.
Many people who are diagnosed with AD may,
in actuality, have concomitant vascular dementia or
ischemic cerebrovascular disease (ICVD), compli- Frontotemporal Dementia (FTD)
cating the nature and manifestation of their demen-
tia (Kalaria, 2016). Royall (2005) suggests that 40% to Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) (also called fronto-
60% of people diagnosed with AD also have ICVD temporal lobal degeneration [FTLD], and originally
and that the diagnosis of the latter is missed because called Pick’s disease) is a form of dementia caused
associated frontal lesions are often not apparent in by atrophy of the anterior frontal and temporal
neuroimaging. The probability that the underlying lobes. It accounts for about 10% to 15% of demen-
etiologies will be mixed increases with age, as there tia cases overall. It is more likely than most forms
168  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

of dementia to occur in people younger than age 65 may be AIDS, cancer, poor nutrition, and chronic
years. Its cause is unknown. In about 40% of cases, infections. Cognitive-communicative challenges
there is a hereditary link (Association of Frontotem- include short- and long-term memory deficits and
poral Degeneration, n.d.; Baez et al., 2019). The age confabulation.
of onset is typically in the 40s, 50s, and 60s, although
it can occur in the 20s through the 80s. Symptoms
are determined largely by the associated functions Creutzfeldt-Jakob Disease
of specific areas of the brain that are affected. Behav-
ioral variant FTD (bvFTD) is the most common Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease is a rare, rapidly progres-
form. It manifests through personality and behav- sive, degenerative viral disease. It entails a common
ioral changes, often entailing apathy (observed as a bodily protein, prion, which forms into misshapen
lack of initiation or response in conversation), a lack configurations that destroy brain cells. Symptoms
of insight about communication challenges, diffi- are a rapid loss of cognitive and linguistic abilities
culty expressing emotions and considering others’ and cortical and cerebellar muscular coordination,
emotions, and disinhibition of inappropriate behav- plus mood changes. It typically (about 85% of the
iors and expressions during social interaction. The time) occurs for no obvious reason in people between
semantic and nonfluent forms of PPA (described age 60 and 65 years. There is also a hereditary form,
later) are also considered types of FTD. Communica- comprising about 10% to 15% of cases, and an infec-
tion problems include expressive and receptive lan- tious form associated with contamination through
guage deficits, and discourse challenges, especially infected meats (sometimes referred to popularly as
arising from executive function deficits, manifesting mad cow disease, which, in actuality, does not occur
in a lack of code switching, empathy, and foresight. in humans) or medical instruments (National Insti-
Certain types of movement disorders entailing tute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, 2015b).
involuntary or automatic motor functions (e.g., cor-
ticobulbar degeneration, progressive supranuclear
palsy, and ALS) are also associated with some forms AIDS Dementia Complex
of FTD (National Institute of Neurological Disorders
and Stroke, 2015a). AIDS dementia complex is one of the many poten-
tial complications that may arise from HIV infection.
An immune disorder, AIDS may affect any aspect of
Huntington’s Disease human functioning through primary and secondary
infectious processes. Sometimes referred to as HIV/
Huntington’s disease is a hereditary condition char- AIDS-associated dementia or HIV/AIDS-associated
acterized by chorea and psychiatric and cognitive-​ encephalopathy, it occurs when the brain is affected
linguistic problems. Symptoms tend to emerge by the HIV virus or by any type of associated oppor-
between age 30 and 50 years, although it can occur tunistic infection. Symptoms may involve challenges
in children and in adults in their 80s. Communica- with executive functions, pragmatic abilities, atten-
tion challenges include poor language organization, tion, and memory. Symptom progression may often
confabulation, short- and long-term memory prob- be slowed through retroviral treatments. The term
lems, dysnomia, irritability, and emotional lability HIV-associated mild neurocognitive disorder is sometimes
(Sturrock & Leavitt, 2010). used to characterize mild versions of this condition.

Korsakoff ’s Syndrome
What Are Symptoms of the
Primary Forms of PPA?
Korsakoff’s syndrome is a condition of gradual
cognitive decline associated with cortical atrophy.
It is caused by a thiamine (vitamin B1) deficiency, In Chapter 10, we considered how PPA qualifies as
most commonly (but not exclusively) due to chronic a type of aphasia, yet is also considered a form of
alcohol abuse (Kopelman et al., 2009). Other causes dementia, the first symptoms of which are linguis-
13. Cognitive-Communicative Disorders in Primary Progressive Aphasia and Dementia   169

tic in nature. It is remarkably different from other (e.g., forgetfulness, praxis for use of household tools
syndromes of aphasia, especially in that PPA has and gadgets) and visuospatial deficits (e.g., finding
an insidious onset, is degenerative, and eventually close-by objects, judging distances, locating exits)
manifests as full-blown dementia. People with non- (Marshall et al., 2018). Thus, the initial diagnosis of
fluent agrammatic PPA (nfvPPA) tend to have dif- lvPPA is often confused with AD or other forms of
ficulty with syntax, especially with understanding dementia. Diagnosis is supported by imaging reveal-
and producing longer and more complex sentences. ing temporoparietal atrophy and hypometabolism.
Their speech is characterized by speech sound errors Upon autopsy, pathology is often characteristic of
and omission of articles and function words. They AD (Rising & Beeson, 2020; Stierwalt, 2020).
also often have concomitant apraxia of speech, so A fourth subtype has been suggested, although
speech is effortful, sometimes with articulatory grop- some consider it to be another form of FTD, while
ing. Many develop parkinsonism, postural instabil- others consider it to be a type of svPPA. This is a
ity, and limb apraxia. Single-word comprehension is form in which the right rather than left hemisphere is
typically good. Diagnosis is supported by imaging first affected, such that pragmatic and extralinguistic
that shows atrophy of the left posterior regions of aspects of communication are affected initially; later,
the frontal lobe, especially around the insula, and this form tends to develop into semantic PPA.
the superior temporal gyrus. See Schaffer and Henry As with people who have aphasia in general,
(2020) for a discussion of varied viewpoints on pos- many people with PPA have symptoms that do not
sible subtypes of nfvPPA. fit neatly into a single syndrome description (Mar-
People with semantic variant PPA (svPPA) tend shall et al., 2018; Mesulam & Weintraub, 2014) and
to have challenges with word finding, especially in may be categorized as having mixed PPA or simply
confrontation naming, and comprehension; even PPA of an unidentified type. Brain imaging is often
understanding at the single-word level tends to be not conducive to clear identification of underlying
impaired in reading as well as auditory comprehen- pathologies. Guidelines for improved diagnosis and
sion. In contrast to people with logopenic variant subtyping continue to be refined to help improve the
PPA (lvPPA), anomia in svPPA stems from a loss of meaningfulness of diagnostic labeling and the sensi-
semantic knowledge, not just access to that knowl- tivity and specificity of PPA assessment methods (see
edge. Loss of knowledge of more specific semantic Mesulam et al., 2021). Most people diagnosed with
information that is not used regularly tends to pre- PPA live for many years following the diagnosis.
cede loss of knowledge about familiar and typical Diagnosis of these primary types of PPA
constructs (Botha & Josephs, 2019; Marshall et al., depends on clinical observation, cognitive-linguis-
2018). Other than being characterized by dysnomia, tic assessment, brain imaging, and follow-up lab-
verbal output is relatively good, including intact oratory testing as needed. A clinical road map for
syntax. People with svPPA tend not to have concom- diagnosis for the primary forms of PPA is shown in
itant motor speech impairments. Diagnosis is sup- Figure 13–1.
ported by imaging revealing atrophy in the ventral As mentioned at the start of this chapter, we
and lateral anterior temporal lobes, especially in the delve into approaches to facilitating communication
left hemisphere. Cases with greater atrophy in the in people with PPA in Chapter 26. We consider rel-
right hemisphere, sometimes with accompanying evant service delivery, assessment, and treatment
prosopagnosia, may be characterized as right later- content in each of the chapters in Sections V, VI,
alized semantic dementia (Chiou & Allison, 2020). and VII.
People with logopenic variant PPA (lvPPA)
tend to have problems with word finding, espe-
cially in spontaneous conversation. This appears to Is There Such a Thing as
be due to a challenge of access, not a loss of seman-
“Reversible” Dementia?
tic knowledge. They may have frequent speech
sound and spelling errors. Repetition of phrases or
sentences as compared to single words tends to be Although the term reversible dementia is sometimes
more challenging than it is for other PPA variants. used in the research literature and in clinical prac-
Many have concomitant extralinguistic challenges tice, it is inaccurate and misleading. This is because,
Figure 13–1. “Clinical road map” for diagnosis of the primary forms of PPA: nfvPPA, nonfluent–agrammatic
variant primary progressive aphasia; lvPPA, logopenic variant primary progressive aphasia; svPPA,
semantic variant primary progressive aphasia. Adapted from Marshall et al., 2018, licensed under CC
BY 4.0. Image credit: Taylor Reeves. A full-color version of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.

170
13. Cognitive-Communicative Disorders in Primary Progressive Aphasia and Dementia   171

by definition, dementia is progressive; it gets worse unable to remember what they learned from one
over time. Although its progression may be slowed treatment session to the next, then it makes no sense
in some cases, and although some symptoms may to provide one-on-one treatment to improve their
reach plateaus for weeks or even months at a time, communication strategies.
it cannot be reversed. For this reason, a condition Some third-party payors consider dementia to
that seems like dementia but in which significant be a “red flag” diagnosis. This means that as soon
improvements in cognitive abilities are noticed for as a diagnostic code for dementia is recognized on
a sustained period of time is most likely not truly an electronic medical record via computerized track-
dementia. For example, a person newly admitted ing, approval of reimbursement for many types of
to a long-term care facility after years of living at rehabilitation services is automatically withdrawn,
home, or a person who has recently lost a spouse, at least without substantial further documentation
may show signs of confusion and disorientation and review. Making matters worse, once a demen-
that are characteristic of dementia. However, cogni- tia code is actually recorded in a medical record, it
tive decline and behavioral symptoms caused by a may be very difficult to have it deleted later once it
sudden life change that has led to major personal becomes clear that an original diagnosis was wrong.
adjustment do not constitute symptoms of dementia. Although medical doctors in most countries, not
These typically dissipate over time, especially with SLPs, have the ultimate authority to record specific
the support of family, friends, and professionals. diagnoses in a person’s medical records, it is incum-
Other situations in which dementia-like symp- bent on SLPs and other rehabilitation professionals,
toms may be noted despite the absence of true as well as family members, to advocate for great care
dementia are cases of depression, dietary imbalances over a period of time that is long enough and that
or vitamin deficiencies, drug effects, drug interac- entails sufficient probing to rule out possibilities
tions, and postsurgical states. Some clinicians and other than dementia that could account for memory
scholars refer to such false cases of dementia as pseu- difficulties and confusion.
dodementia. However, a more accurate and descrip-
tive term that does not include the word dementia
is preferable. Many prefer the terms transient confu- What Is the Role of the SLP in Working With
sional state, acute confusional state, or delirium.
People Who Have PPA and Dementia?

What Are Implications of an Incorrect Overall, the role of the clinical aphasiologist is vital
in assessment, treatment, counseling, caregiver
Diagnosis of Dementia?
coaching and training, and advocacy for people
with neurodegenerative conditions. Given that
It is important to avoid having a diagnosis of demen- these conditions continue to change, dynamic clin-
tia be documented in a person’s medical records ical decision-making with an interprofessional team
unless there is a high degree of certainty about that approach, centered on the individual, is paramount.
diagnosis. An incorrect diagnosis of dementia may The SLP’s role is vital in anticipating future needs.
lead to unnecessary stress and grief for clients and Treatment tends to be more adaptive than rehabili-
caregivers, hardships related to untoward stigma tative. This is a very large and diverse category of
on a social level, and incorrect prescription of med- highly underserved people whose needs — and the
ication and other interventions. It may also lead to needs of those who care about them — often fall
a person being deemed ineligible for coverage of between the cracks in health care systems better
crucial rehabilitation-related services. For example, suited to treating acute rather than chronic needs
third-party payors (e.g., case managers for insurance and better geared toward continued improvement as
companies or federal or state health care plans) may opposed to support and work to slow deterioration
refuse to reimburse service-providing agencies or and loss (Hovland, 2018). Person-first, empowering
clinicians for direct SLP intervention for a person approaches aimed at making the most of residual
with AD; their rationale may be that if a person is abilities, slowing progression, providing personal
172  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

and environmental supports, and promoting qual- eral intervention approaches explored in Chapter 25
ity of life of people with neurodegenerative condi- are pertinent to this population. In Chapter 26 we
tions can make a tremendous difference in the lives consider more specific means of facilitating commu-
of individuals, families, and communities. The gen- nication in people with PPA and dementia.

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter 9. Why is it important for SLPs to act as
that are new to you or that you have not yet advocates to hold off on any formal
mastered. diagnosis of dementia until there is a great
2. What are the primary categories of deal of certainty about the diagnosis?
neurodegenerative conditions with great 10. List and describe key features of the types of
relevance to clinical aphasiology? dementia mentioned in this chapter.
3. How does mild cognitive impairment 11. Compare and contrast PPA with dementia.
(MCI) differ from age-related cognitive- 12. Compare and contrast PPA with classical
communicative decline? syndromes of aphasia.
4. How does language of generalized 13. Describe the primary types of PPA.
intellectual impairment differ from aphasia? 14. In what ways are person-first and empower­
5. What are different schemes by which varied ment approaches to assessment, intervention,
forms of dementia might be classified? counseling, and advocacy for people with
6. What are the primary causes of dementia? neurodegenerative conditions essential to
7. Why is the term reversible dementia individuals, families, and communities?
misleading? What term is preferable?
www
8. How is dementia different from transient Visit the companion website to see more sugges-
confusional state? tions for learning and reflection.
SECTION IV

Delivering Excellent Services


174  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

This section is dedicated to helping readers learn ing legal and ethical concerns, we take a proactive
about the service delivery contexts in which we approach in considering how to promote access to
work and how we may serve as dedicated, vigor- our services and support the rights of people with
ous, passionate, constructive advocates for people aphasia and related disorders. We also review crit-
with acquired neurogenic communication disorders. ical ethical dilemmas related to our own financial
Many practicing clinicians working in health care conflicts of interest as clinicians and consider what
contexts say they wish they had been equipped with we can do about them. In Chapter 16, we consider
such information before they hit the clinical trenches global aspects of aphasiology, including what consti-
as full-time aphasiologists. In Chapter 14, we review tutes cultural responsivity across cultural and natu-
several aspects of the contexts in which we work, ral borders, global trends that are affecting our field
the way our services are paid for, and the impact and the people we serve, and helpful resources for
of health care finance and cost-control systems on supporting transnational work in aphasiology.
clinical services in our field. In Chapter 15, address-
CHAPTER
14
Contexts for Providing Excellent Services

In Chapters 1 and 2, we considered the rich career 10. What do we do if we are denied reimbursement
possibilities for speech-language pathologists (SLPs) for our services?
who specialize in neurogenic communication disor- 11. How do health care finance and cost-control
ders in clinical and research settings. In this chapter, systems affect clinical services?
we delve into the myriad roles that clinical aphasi- 12. What are the impacts of health care cost cutting
ologists play and consider the varied contexts for and cost control on services for people with
clinical practice, telepractice options, and aspects neurogenic communication challenges?
of interprofessional teamwork. Note that our focus
continues to be on delivering excellent services,
even when service delivery contexts may be far What Do SLPs Who Specialize in Neurogenic
from excellent. We also explore a series of queries
Communication Disorders Do?
about how clinicians get paid and related issues
about reimbursement for our services. Finally, we
address highly relevant aspects of health care finance Just as the work environments and cultural contexts
and cost-control systems as they affect our clinical of aphasiologists vary widely, so do their profes-
services. sional responsibilities. Here, let’s consider some of
After reading and reflecting on the content in our primary roles.
this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in
your own words, the following queries:
Clinical Intervention (Screening, Assessment,
1. What do SLPs who specialize in neurogenic Treatment, Counseling, Educating)
communication disorders do?
2. In what types of settings do we provide clinical Our roles in these diverse aspects of intervention are
services? not separable, although it is helpful to examine each
3. In what ways may services be provided at a in detail as one gains knowledge, skills, and values
distance? related to each.
4. With what types of teams do clinical
aphasiologists engage?
5. How do SLPs get paid? Interprofessional Collaboration and
6. Where does the money come from to pay for Interdisciplinary Learning
SLP services?
7. How do service-providing agencies get paid? As discussed in Chapter 1, no aphasiologist rep-
8. What makes services provided by SLPs resents a single discipline. The knowledge, skills,
reimbursable? and values required for clinical excellence span
9. What are the primary reasons for which numerous content areas. Continuous lifelong learn-
reimbursements for SLP services are denied? ing across disciplines is vital. Also, ideally every SLP

175
176  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

works with one or more teams of professionals from in research in some way. Clinicians need not pursue
a wide range of areas of expertise, a topic we discuss research careers to be strong consumers of research
further in this chapter. and to contribute to research through collaboration
with others. Opportunities for those wishing to
engage in research careers in acquired neurogenic
Advocacy disorders in particular are summarized in Chapter 1.

Supporting the rights of people with communication


challenges is fundamental to all of the professional Teaching and Mentoring
roles and responsibilities of SLPs, regardless of the
context in which we work. We already discussed this University-based aphasiologists and clinical super-
vital role in earlier chapters regarding work with visors have teaching and mentoring roles as part of
older people and people who have varied types of their ongoing professional responsibilities. Those
neurogenic communication disorders, and we con- working in primarily service enterprises are often
sider it further in Chapter 15 and in the context of called upon, or often volunteer, to offer on-the-job
intervention in Sections VI and VII. mentoring and supervision of student clinicians,
clinical fellows, and new hires who do not have a
great deal of experience working in the area of neu-
Marketing, Negotiating Contracts, rogenic cognitive-linguistic disorders.
Billing, Recordkeeping, Documentation,
Scheduling and Coordinating Care,
Quality Assurance, and Fundraising
In What Types of Settings Do We
Provide Clinical Services?
The business aspects of clinical practice are fun-
damental to sustaining clinical services. Some cli-
nicians have a great deal of help with many of the Decisions about where an individual will receive
business-related aspects of clinical practice, but services depends on the type and severity of prob-
many do not. Such efforts require substantial time, lems they are facing and the related type and extent
expertise, and leadership (Hallowell, 2021; Hallow- of services needed, geographic location, insurance
ell & Chapey, 2008b). coverage, financial status, and personal and family
preferences.

Leadership and Management


Hospitals
Many aphasiologists have the communication and
leadership skills, making them sought after for lead- Aphasiologists who work in hospitals typically
ership positions. They may become heads of clinical provide inpatient or outpatient services, or both.
SLP departments or rehabilitation units, managers of Inpatient services may be categorized as acute care
rehabilitation companies, and owners of businesses. or subacute care. In recent years, there has been a
In academic departments, they frequently rise in trend to reduce the amount of time that a patient
the ranks of leadership from department chairs and may stay in the hospital. This is primarily due to the
clinical directors to deans, provosts, presidents, and high costs of acute care. Given that stroke and brain
chancellors. injury survivors are typically only in acute care for
a short time, SLPs may engage in screenings, assess-
ment, treatment planning, counseling, and family
Research education with them but are less likely to actually
get to implement ongoing treatment programs.
Given that evidence-based practice relies on practice-​ A great deal of subacute service takes place bedside
based evidence and vice versa, all SLPs are engaged in patients’ rooms. Ongoing treatment is more likely
14. Contexts for Providing Excellent Services   177

provided in subacute care, rehabilitation settings, dential setting. Most SNFs identify as rehabilitation
or patients’ homes. Hospitals ideally have compre- centers, too. Residents may come for short-term
hensive stroke management and trauma response rehabilitation stays following hospitalization (e.g.,
programs, both of which require concerted interpro- following a stroke or brain injury) so that they may
fessional teamwork among professionals with var- continue to recover prior to going back home. Some
ied areas of clinical expertise. SNFs offer rehabilitation services that are provided
by their own staff members; others contract with
agencies that provide rehabilitation staffing and
Rehabilitation Centers oversight. Many SNFs have special areas dedicated
to ongoing care of people with dementia.
Whether focused on inpatient or outpatient services,
rehabilitation centers are key contexts for diagnosis
and ongoing poststroke and post-traumatic brain Continuing Care Retirement
injury services. Many are equipped with special Communities (CCRCs)
facilities, such as dining rooms with restaurant-like
components, mock shopping areas, and kitchens, all CCRCs are increasingly popular alternatives to liv-
set up for practicing needed life skills to maximize ing as long as possible at home and only moving to
independence. A typical rehabilitation gym includes a long-term care facility as a last resort, when no rel-
ample equipment to support occupational and phys- atives or paid caregivers are available or able to care
ical therapy services. SLPs often engage in cotreat- for a person with a disability. At increasing rates in
ments with clinicians from other disciplines in such much of the world and especially in Western coun-
gyms and sometimes may even provide their own tries, older adults are actively choosing to move into
direct services there because the gym may be the CCRCs while they are healthy. Their goal is typically
rehabilitation team’s primary dedicated space. Ide- to live there independently in apartments or condo-
ally, private spaces for direct communication inter- miniums, all the while accessing shared recreational
vention and counseling are also available. and social facilities and programming, as well as
health care and dining facilities. Later, if they lose
basic functional abilities required to maintain that
Health Maintenance Organizations level of independence, they may move to assisted
living areas within the same community — or even
Health maintenance organizations (HMOs) are have staff members who come to the places they
agencies that provide health care services through already reside — to receive help as needed on an
contracts with clinical professionals rather than individualized basis. Help might include tracking
having patients see separate independent provid- and administering medications, dressing wounds,
ers. People who have the choice between an HMO or assisting with housekeeping. Still later, if they
and another agency for SLP services may choose the need more intensive nursing care, they may move
HMO primarily because copays and other out-of- to a skilled nursing component within the same
pocket costs tend to be lower and because HMOs community.
offer a one-stop location for health care services and The continuity of care within the same location
thus tend to be convenient and familiar. HMOs some- has many benefits. Life transitions due to health con-
times provide on-site SLPs to serve their members. cerns need not be terribly disruptive; if one partner
of a couple needs more care, the two need not be
split up to live in different locations. Also, residents
Skilled Nursing and Long-Term Care Facilities maintain access to friends, health professionals,
and other staff members within the other com-
In several countries, what once were called such munity, regardless of their level of care. Although
ageist names as “old age homes” or “rest homes” they may engage in hobbies such as gardening and
are now typically called skilled nursing facilities woodworking, they are free of such responsibili-
(SNFs). SNFs offer health care services in a resi- ties as groundskeeping and home repair. Most such
178  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

communities are brimming with activities. Resi- run by not-for-profit groups with a mission to serve
dents may take academic courses offered on-site by people regardless of their ability to pay for services.
local colleges, make use of workout facilities and In the United States, the National Association of
swimming pools, attend concerts, go on day trips, Speech and Hearing Centers (NASHC, http://www​
compete on sports teams, participate in discussion .nashc.net) helps to foster networking and informa-
groups, serve on committees charged with oversight tion sharing among leaders of not-for-profit clinics.
of various aspects of the community, and take part in The American Academy of Private Practice in Speech
groups with shared interests, such as political action, Pathology and Audiology (AAPPSPA, http://www​
religious or cultural traditions, sewing, cooking, .aappspa.org) is an organization supporting private
or computer use, just to name a few. Many people practitioners working in a wide array of clinical con-
choose the continuing care community option even texts through continuing education opportunities,
if they do have adult children who would be glad networking, and legal consultation. There are paral-
to look after them, and even if they could afford lel national and regional networks across the globe.
in-home caregiving that would enable them to keep Unless a private practice has a large enough
living in their family homes. CCRCs typically offer a caseload to be able to afford specialized clinicians
full range of rehabilitation services, including those in specific areas of practice, SLPs working in such
described earlier for SNFs. contexts are apt to practice across a vast array of
areas within the field’s scope of practice. Still, this is
a viable context for providing intervention for adults
Home Health Agencies with acquired neurogenic language disorders. Also,
some private practice clinics are set up especially to
Given the cost of care for hospitalization and follow-​ address people with specific types of communica-
up inpatient rehabilitation services, and time limits tion needs; as such, some are dedicated to practice
on access to these, many stroke and brain injury sur- in clinical aphasiology.
vivors still benefit from services delivered in their
homes. These services may be offered at less cost
and in many cases have been shown to lead to the University-Based Clinics
best long-term results, especially in terms of carry-
over of progress made in rehabilitation to natural Many universities with programs in SLP and audi-
living environments. In-home care is ideally coor- ology have on-site clinics that provide services
dinated among several rehabilitation and medical to the local community. The case mix tends to be
disciplines. Housekeeping, shopping, and home diverse rather than specialized, as in many free-
maintenance services are also sometimes contracted standing communication disorders clinics. Dramatic
through home health providers or through commu- changes in funding models for universities as well
nity-based groups dedicated to helping people live as in health care funding models have had serious
at home for as long as possible. financial implications affecting the way such clinics
operate. For example, clinical supervisors whose
salaries were once supported through a university’s
Private Practice and Not-for-Profit Clinics ongoing annual operating budget are now increas-
ingly based on clinical revenues generated. Thus,
Private clinical practices serving people with com- many clinical supervisors have fiscal productivity
munication disorders typically provide a wide range requirements in addition to their clinical teaching
of services to people from birth through old age with and mentoring duties. While most university clinics
any and all types of hearing, speech, language, bal- in the past served community members at low or no
ance, or swallowing disorders. In addition to having cost as deemed helpful to the clients and beneficial
their own clinical facilities, some contract services to to students’ clinical learning, this is rarely possible
local schools, hospitals, rehabilitation providers, and in today’s cost-strapped clinics. Most now operate as
other agencies. Although some are for-profit busi- real businesses, billing private and federal insurance
nesses, some communication disorders clinics are programs for services, and requiring copayments
14. Contexts for Providing Excellent Services   179

or out-of-pocket fees from clients. The changes are or months. Most aphasia centers are not-for-profit
not all negative. Students offering clinical services and allow for sliding payment scales for people
in a real business-oriented operation have wonder- who lack financial resources to be seen elsewhere.
ful opportunities to learn about key areas of clinical Affordability is certainly not the only benefit of such
practice management, such as billing and documen- group-based practice models. As we discuss further
tation to meet payer requirements. Also, they do not in Section VII, there are ample reasons that group
develop false expectations about how long a typical intervention may be optimal for many people.
client may be kept on caseload or the amount of time Most aphasia centers are dedicated to the Life
available to engage in nonbillable activities. Participation Approach to Aphasia (LPAA). Most
do not operate based on a medical model of service
delivery, in contrast to many of the types of contexts
Adult Day Care Centers described earlier, because few rely on reimburse-
ment through health insurance. Instead, aphasia
Families wishing to care for adults with disabilities centers tend to derive income through private out-
at home but who are unable to do so during the day of-pocket payment from the people served, plus
may rely on adult day care centers. These may be donations and fundraising efforts. Without having
freestanding practices, or they may be offered in to meet strict medically focused documentation and
conjunction with hospitals, rehabilitation centers, service provision criteria, SLPs in these settings are
and the like. Many adult day services are dedicated free to focus on life participation and social models of
to people with dementia who cannot be left unsu- service delivery (Elman, 2016). For example, instead
pervised and to people with severe motor disabil- of being called clients or patients, people who seek
ities that preclude their being left alone at home. services at aphasia centers tend to be called “mem-
Although there are not typically salaried positions bers.” At many such centers, members are welcome
for SLPs at such centers, they may offer intervention to visit with one another in shared kitchen or living
and caregiver training services on a contractual or room spaces when they are not necessarily engaged
volunteer basis. in programmed sessions, further fostering a sense
of mutual support and camaraderie. Groups are
formed based on patterns of needs and interests of
Aphasia Centers members. Some groups engage in creative commu-
nity-based activities outside of center facilities.
Given the severe restrictions that federal, state, and
private insurance programs impose on rehabilitation
services (discussed later in this chapter), and given Hospice
that most people with neurogenic language disor-
ders have chronic challenges that require long-term Hospice services, designed to support people who
intervention, there has been a trend over the past are dying and the people who care about them
three decades to provide long-term language inter- through the end of life, are provided in myriad types
vention services to people with aphasia and related of clinical contexts. They are most commonly pro-
disorders through independent aphasia centers. vided in private homes, skilled nursing and rehabili-
Most aphasia centers offer primarily group inter- tation facilities, hospitals, and specialized residential
vention, although some offer private diagnostic and centers for hospice and palliative care, all of which
treatment sessions, too. By attending a group session, are contexts in which SLPs work. The primary roles
a participant need only pay a fraction of the cost of of the SLP in hospice that are central to addressing
a typical private session lasting the same duration; neurogenic disorders of cognition and communica-
the cost of professional intervention is shared among tion include
members of a group, making the experience more
affordable for all. This makes it possible for some • providing information and consultation to
to continue to participate in communication reha- patients, families, and other hospice team
bilitation for years as opposed to just days, weeks, members; and
180  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

• developing supportive communication 1998; Martínez et al., 2004; Richmond et al., 2017;
strategies to facilitate the patient’s Whitten, 2006).
participation in decision-making, participation Much of the evidence for the effectiveness of
in social relationships, and expression and telerehabilitation is based on research from disci-
fulfillment of end-of-life wishes (Chahda plines other than SLP. Several studies support the
et al., 2017; Pollens, 2004). feasibility of using distance technologies, including
simple telephone contact, videoconferencing, chat
We discuss those roles further in the context of messaging, patient portals and platforms, mobile
counseling and life coaching in Chapter 27. Clini- health apps, and gaming platforms (Cohn & Cason,
cal aphasiologists are also often at the forefront of 2016; Coufal et al., 2018; Parker Oliver et al., 2017;
helping to determine decision-making capacity and Richmond et al., 2017).
competency of people in hospice care (Brody, 2005; The evidence base for the effectiveness of
Chahda et al., 2017; Dietz et al., 2013; Griffith & telepractice in neurogenic language disorders is
Tengnah, 2007; Horton-Deutsch et al., 2007; Moberg growing. There is mounting evidence that in-person
& Rick, 2008; Pollens, 2004; Sabat, 2005). and telerehabilitation diagnosis and treatment may
be similarly effective when used in specific ways
for particular purposes with stroke and brain injury
In What Ways May Services Be survivors and people with dementia (Brennan et
al., 2004; Duffy et al., 1997; Freckmann et al., 2017;
Provided at a Distance?
Georgeadis et al., 2004; Hall et al., 2013; Hill et al.,
2006; Lasker et al., 2010; Theodoros et al., 2008;
There are many reasons for which a person with a Wertz et al., 1992). Although a majority of the pub-
neurogenic cognitive-linguistic disorder may need lished work in this area to date has been focused on
or desire diagnostic or treatment services from a cli- impairment-level assessment and treatment, there
nician who is not in the same location. For exam- is increasing evidence that telepractice delivery can
ple, those who do not speak the language of local enhance everyday communication, life participation,
clinicians, those who are too weak or ill to travel to and quality of life for people with aphasia (e.g., Pitt
clinical centers, those without transportation, those et al., 2018).
who live in areas remote from qualified clinicians, Not only may it be effective, but telepractice
and those who live in war zones or unsafe neigh- may be the only way of reaching people who would
borhoods may wish to access clinicians at a distance not otherwise have access to our services (Hallow-
(Doolittle et al., 1998; Khazei et al., 2005). ell, 2021). In addition, it enables networks of peo-
Telepractice is the application of technology to ple with aphasia and related challenges — and their
deliver health, counseling, consulting, assessment, care partners — opportunities to network with one
or rehabilitative services at a distance. It offers prom- another despite geographic distances. During the
ise for providing access to services where access is COVID-19 pandemic, the recognition of the extreme
otherwise reduced or nonexistent due to distance, isolation of people with aphasia in lockdown, and
lack of availability of appropriate expertise, lack of the curtailing of not only in-person SLP services but
mobility, or lack of transportation. also family and social activities in general, inspired
The terms telerehabilitation, telehealth, and tele- many of us to initiate online offerings if we had
medicine are sometimes used interchangeably with not done so prior to the pandemic, and expand our
this term, or to convey varied connotations in refer- individual and group treatment and support service
ring to service delivery from afar. Telepractice is the offerings if we had.
preferred term in the context of this book, in recog- One remarkable example of successful engage-
nition that it may occur in or outside of medical and ment of thousands of people with aphasia and care
health care contexts. Telepractice may be synchro- partners that was borne from the pandemic is Virtual
nous (live and in real time, usually via audio and Connections for Aphasia. Thanks to a partnership
video connection) or asynchronous (recorded and between the not-for-profit Aphasia Recovery Con-
later shared for review or interpretation) (Berman nections with for-profit Lingraphica — and the com-
& Fenaughty, 2005; Brown, 2005; Hickman & Dyer, mitment of time and talent of over 60 professionals
14. Contexts for Providing Excellent Services   181

(SLPs, music therapists, professors, and specialists Most aphasia centers and a large proportion of
in assistive technologies) from five continents — Vir- private practice clinics initiated and increased online
tual Connections offers three to four free online clinical offerings during the COVID-19 pandemic.
sessions 6 days a week, and one or two sessions on Ageist and ableist assumptions that older adults and
Sundays. To date, registered members participate people with communication challenges would have
from more than 50 countries, and new members join trouble or be resistant to engaging in online services
every week. In addition, 30 to 50 students, mostly in have been readily countered.
speech-language pathology, participate each semes- Clinicians practicing through distance technol-
ter. Through the Zoom cloud-based video commu- ogy are accountable in terms of ethical and scope-
nications app, participants with aphasia engage of-practice issues just as they are for in-person
in diverse activities, including supported conver- treatment and must adhere to related local, regional,
sations about numerous intriguing topics, games, and federal regulations and policies (Cason & Bran-
book groups, singing, improvisational theater, exer- non, 2011; Cohn, 2012; Cohn et al., 2011; Cohn &
cise, and mindfulness. Couples coping with aphasia Watzlaf, 2011; McGill & Fiddler, 2021). Also, if the
engage with professional coaches and share about intent is to bill for services, it is important to ver-
their experiences with other couples. Care partners ify whether distance services are reimbursable. Fac-
join in special partner-only sessions for informa- tors that have the greatest impact on options for
tion sharing, self-empowerment, and networking expanding telerehabilitation for people with neuro-
with others. Although the impetus for Virtual Con- genic communication disorders are summarized in
nections was the desire to counter forced isola- Box 14–1. Since access to appropriate technology and
tion of people with aphasia during the COVID-19 an Internet connection are essential for online partic-
pandemic, it is unlikely to stop once the virus is no ipation, addressing disparities in such access among
longer a significant threat. people who would benefit from online services is an

Box
14–1 Factors Influencing Options of Expanding Telerehabilitation
for People With Neurogenic Communication Disorders

• Means of ensuring relationship-centered services


• Training in use of telepractice technology
• Client abilities (technology use and control, hearing and visual abilities,
local support)
• Attitudes of providers and people being served
• Clinicians’ cultural responsiveness and linguistic competence to ensure
appropriate therapeutic alliance with people being served
• Establishment and enforcement of clinical standards
• Reimbursement rules and related considerations of government and
insurance programs
• Licensure and certification issues, especially for services provided
between states, provinces, and regions, or across national borders
• Physician referrals
• Means of ensuring patient confidentiality
• Means of adapting and designing assessment and treatment materials
• Methods for tracking and reporting clinical outcomes
• Internet connectivity and other telecommunications infrastructure and cost
• Potential cost savings

Sources: Akamoglu et al., 2018; American Telemedicine Association, 2010; Edwards-


Gaither, 2018; Hallowell, 2021; Hallowell & Chapey, 2008b.
182  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

essential aspect of promoting communication and lowing during mealtimes, and use a series of basic
life participation for all people with communication commands rather than complex instructions when
disorders. helping her engage in self-care tasks. Interdisciplin-
ary teams represent much of the current educational
and clinical focus of interprofessional collaboration.
With What Types of Teams Do In a transdisciplinary team, there is further
cross-training of team members; the lines clearly
Clinical Aphasiologists Engage?
demarcating the expertise of one discipline’s scope
of practice may be blurred. This type of team is
Teamwork is central to coordination and providing increasingly common in home health care environ-
the best clinical services, and ensuring effective doc- ments, where the sheer expense and the lack of suf-
umentation and reimbursement for services. Team ficient numbers of specialists in each discipline make
members often include the person with a neurogenic it difficult or impossible to send a separate clinician
communication disorder and any friends, relatives, representing each area of rehabilitation need to a
or caregivers they choose to include; physicians patient’s home. Each member of the rehabilitation
(physiatrists, geriatricians, neurologists, radiologists, team, for example, may engage in monitoring vital
family physicians, hospitalists, etc.); occupational signs; checking up on physical therapy, occupational
therapists; physical therapists; neuropsychologists; therapy, and SLP homework; and counseling. In the
rehabilitation counselors; social workers; nurses; transdisciplinary context, it is important to ensure
nursing assistants; and dietitians. Ideally, through that the proper training required to carry out any
true interprofessional collaboration, all team mem- type of service is provided, and that each discipline’s
bers orient their expertise toward holistically foster- recognized scope of practice is respected. Many tools
ing a person’s reach toward their fullest potential for evaluating interprofessional practice teams and
(Hallowell & Chapey, 2008b). In Chapter 2, we teamwork have been developed and tested. Refer-
considered the interprofessional collaborative com- ring to these may help administrators as well as
petencies that lead to clinical excellence. Here let’s team members tune into their collective strengths
briefly discuss three basic types of teams in which and weaknesses and establish plans for ongoing goal
we might serve as collaborators: multidisciplinary, setting and improvements. The National Center for
interdisciplinary, and transdisciplinary teams. Interprofessional Practice and Education offers a rich
In a multidisciplinary team, each team member set of such tools on its website (https://nexus​ipe.org).
represents their own expertise and also ideally con-
fers with other team members regularly about dis-
cipline-specific and general rehabilitation goals. In
How Do SLPs Get Paid?
an interdisciplinary team, there is greater synergy
across team members and a high degree of collabo-
rative decision-making about strategies for working SLPs are paid directly or indirectly for their services
together to achieve the best outcomes for the patient’s in a variety of ways. These include being paid
overall health and well-being. Cotreatments by clini-
cians from two or more disciplines at once are more • a salary;
likely. Also, clinicians tend to implement strategies • an hourly rate for all services rendered;
recommended and modeled by those from other dis- • an hourly rate only for billable services rendered;
ciplines to complement each team member’s efforts. • a per diem (daily) rate for covering the
For example, a physical therapist may train the SLP, caseload within one or more facilities serviced
occupational therapist, and nursing staff to remind by the same agency;
a stroke survivor with aphasia and hemiparesis to • a specified dollar amount per unit of time
engage in her prescribed range-of-motion exercises (day, week, month) based on a proportion of
at varied times during the day. The SLP may train billable revenue generated; or
others on the team to check on the patient’s hearing • privately, by an individual client (also called
aid use, provide specific reminders about safe swal- out of pocket).
14. Contexts for Providing Excellent Services   183

In the United States, federal health care plans


Where Does the Money Come include Medicare, Medicaid, and funding for mil-
From to Pay for SLP Services? itary veterans, all initiated in the 1960s. Medicare
is the U.S. federal and state health insurance pro-
If SLPs are to be paid, the money must be gener- gram for people who are age 65 years or older and
ated to supply the coffers that keep the payments for people with disabilities and end-stage renal dis-
coming. Most are paid by an employing agency (a ease. Medicare Part A addresses inpatient care in
private practice, hospital, rehabilitation center, etc.). skilled nursing, hospital acute care, rehabilitation
Self-employed SLPs typically pay themselves a fixed hospital, and home health settings. To qualify for
salary or a proportion of profits from their business Part A coverage, the patient must meet criteria for
revenues. Worldwide, funds paid to SLPs working needing a certain amount of “skilled” nursing and/
with adults tend to be generated through govern- or rehabilitation care. Medicare reimbursement rates
ment-sponsored programs, health insurance plans, are higher for addressing needs for intensive inter-
private pay, philanthropic donations, or a combina- vention compared to lesser levels of care. Further
tion of these. aspects of skilled care are detailed later. Medicare
Part B addresses outpatient rehabilitation and long-
term care. The scheme for Medicare’s reimburse-
Government-Sponsored Programs ment for services within skilled nursing facilities
was overhauled in 2019 to address concerns that bill-
In much of the world, in fact in most countries out- ing for service provided leads to conflicts of interest
side of the United States, national health care plans related to financial incentives. Under a prospective
cover most of the costs of health-related services. payment system, providing more services leads to
That is, the government pays for a large proportion greater reimbursement; providers face ethical dilem-
of health care costs for citizens who need health-​ mas in being rewarded for billing for more services
related services or medicine. Throughout the world, than may be needed. The new patient-driven pay-
nationalized health care plans were established to ment model (PDPM) is based on clinical diagnoses
provide free or low-cost needed health care to all cit- and health conditions, with the intent of focusing
izens. Examples are the United Kingdom’s national on patients’ holistic needs rather than on the vol-
health care system, the National Health Service ume of services provided. However, the new model
(NHS), initiated in the mid-1940s, and Australia’s has raised serious concerns about reimbursements
similar health care system launched in the late 1970s. and job security for SLPs, and about SLPs’ ability
These and the national systems of Canada, France, to make important clinical judgments about patient
Germany, Singapore, and Switzerland are consid- care. Ongoing updates regarding payment models
ered among the best, in terms of costs relative to the may be found through the Centers for Medicare and
national gross domestic product figures, per-capita Medicaid Services (https://www.cms.gov), and asso-
costs, wait time for appointments, and infant mortal- ciated updates and resources for SLPs may be found
ity rates (Amadeo, 2020) through the American Speech-Language Hearing
A majority of SLPs who work primarily with Association (ASHA; https://www.asha.org/).
adults in non-U.S. countries in which SLP is a recog- Medicare Part D is a program to help defray
nized profession are paid a salary by an employer, the costs of prescription medication. Medicaid is the
such as a hospital or rehabilitation agency. Their U.S. federal and state health insurance program for
employer typically receives all or a majority of the people with limited income and financial resources,
funds to pay them from a government agency, such including older people and people with disabilities.
as a national ministry of health. In such cases, the Medicaid can help cover some of the costs not cov-
SLP is typically expected to work for a specified ered through Medicare, such as preventive care and
number of hours per day for a certain number of glasses. The distribution of Medicare and Medicaid
days per week, providing diagnostic and treatment funds, and the management of how those funds
services as needed to anyone who needs them in a are approved based on the justification for services
given facility or set of facilities. provided, is often coordinated by an insurance
184  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

intermediary, a professional insurance company ance arrangements, the employer and the employee
that ensures that Medicare and Medicaid policies are share the cost of enrollment in a medical plan. Often,
obeyed and that funds are distributed as government employees have choices about the level of insurance
regulations dictate. The U.S. Department of Veter- coverage they wish to have. More expensive insur-
ans Affairs (VA) funding helps pay for health care ance plans tend to cover more types of services (such
services incurred by people who have served in the as vision care and dentistry) and may entail lower
U.S. armed forces. Such services are usually provided deductible and copay amounts.
through specialized VA hospitals or clinics but are Some people who do not have employer-​sponsored
sometimes offered through facilities that serve the insurance opt to buy insurance for themselves and,
general public when VA facilities are not available. if appropriate, their families. They typically do this
Worldwide, as the proportion of the world’s as protection in case there is a traumatic accident or
population over age 65 years continues to swell, there devastating illness that leads to unwieldy medical
are more and more people who are outliving their costs. Some people who have employer-sponsored
accrued savings, and there is more and more deple- insurance may opt for additional insurance cover-
tion of and competition for government-sponsored age so that they are especially well protected in case
funds. This is a key reason why advocacy for health there are needs for costly medical care in the future.
care reform and assurance of government sources to Although recent regulatory initiatives at the fed-
cover the costs of SLP services is an important aspect eral level have improved the affordability of some
of the role of the excellent clinician, a topic we revisit forms of health care (see Tolbert & Orgera, 2020, for
in Chapter 15. a summary), many U.S. citizens who do not have
employer-sponsored insurance live without sufficient
insurance coverage and are vulnerable to major finan-
Health Insurance cial hardships if costly medical conditions arise.

Health insurance plans are contracted arrangements


that enable individuals to receive health care at a set Private Pay
or reduced rate. The care may include diagnostic and
treatment services, medicine, and durable medical Private pay, or out-of-pocket pay, is payment from
goods (supplies needed for medical care or rehabil- an individual. It does not involve any insurance
itation, which might include hearing aids, alterna- company or government-sponsored plan. It is paid
tive and augmentative communications devices, and through an arrangement between an individual (the
adaptive technology). Typically, the people who are client or a significant other) and the SLP or the SLP’s
insured still have to pay something for their care. employer. Private-pay arrangements allow for SLPs
This is often in the form of to provide services to people with chronic commu-
nication problems for much longer periods of time
• an annual deductible, a certain amount of than tend to be allowed by other means. Many free-
expense they must pay for themselves before standing aphasia centers offer services to people
the insurance funds start to cover their health who will benefit from them no matter how long such
care bills; or services might be needed. Such centers offer private
• a copay, or a portion of the costs they pay diagnosis and treatment sessions, along with ther-
themselves, for various services, prescription apy, conversation, and support for groups of people
medications, and medical equipment. with aphasia and related disorders and their care-
givers. Clients or members of such centers pay pri-
Given the lack of a national health care system vately for the services they need for as long as they
for all, the majority of people in the United States wish to engage in those services. Group formats, in
who have health insurance get their insurance addition to being effective for enhancing communi-
through their employer or through the employer of cation skills and quality of interactions, also allow
a parent or spouse. In employer-sponsored insur- for hourly rates for services to be less costly than
14. Contexts for Providing Excellent Services   185

they would be for individual sessions, especially in professional in each discipline, regardless of
a more medically focused context. their duration
• case rate, a fee for treating a patient based on
their diagnosis, regardless of which specific
Mixed Funding Options services they are provided
• per diem funding scheme, a set fee paid on
In and outside the United States, even where gov- a daily basis, regardless of which specific
ernment programs pay for health care coverage, services are provided
individuals often opt to purchase private insurance. • capitation, a fixed sum based on the number
This is to enable them to afford more expensive and of people enrolled in a contracted health care
at times better quality services should their health plan, regardless of how many of those people
care needs exceed what is typically covered by a actually receive services and regardless of
government plan. Individuals can also pay privately which services are provided
to supplement what might be covered through a
government or private insurance plan. Complex derivatives and combinations of these
schemes are applied through varied health care
systems.
Philanthropic Donations

Many service-providing agencies, especially not- What Makes Services Provided


for-profit and nongovernmental agencies, rely on
by SLPs Reimbursable?
financial contributions from donors to support the
financial base from which they pay clinical employ-
ees. Even for-profit agencies have been developing For insurance-sponsored services to be covered (i.e.,
their own affiliated not-for-profit foundations or part- reimbursable), they must meet the requirements of
nering with other philanthropic groups to help raise the program or company that offers such services.
funds to support clinical services. Fundraising cam- Those requirements are often enforced by a third-
paigns may include special events (e.g., 5k or 10k runs party payer (the agency that is actually handling the
or marathons, dance-a-thons, galas, concerts), annual payment for services). The third-party payer is often
fund campaigns, and planned giving programs (e.g., the insurance company through which an individ-
gift annuities and charitable remainder trusts, charita- ual has an insurance plan. In cases of government-​
ble life insurance, and gifts of goods (e.g., real estate, sponsored plans, the third-party payer might be an
antiques, artwork) to be bequeathed to the agency insurance company that has a contract with a gov-
upon the donor’s death (Hallowell, 2021). ernment agency to handle administration of the
plan, and the administration is carried out much
as would be done through regular insurance plans.
The requirements vary according to the specific plan
How Do Service-Providing Agencies Get Paid?
that an individual has. Factors that promote the like-
lihood of successful billing are described here and
There are varied schemes that determine just what summarized in Box 14–2.
a government-based or insurance program will pay
a service-providing agency for clinical services. At a
very basic level, these include the following: Effective Documentation Meeting All
Requirements for Reimbursement
• fee-for-service, a rate paid for a specific
diagnostic or intervention service, which No matter how justifiable and high quality our
may be based on units of time or on numbers services, if we do not provide the type and quality
of visits or sessions provided by each of documentation required for reimbursement, a
186  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

preauthorization is required; if it is, they must know


Box
14–2 Factors That Promote the how to obtain it. Sometimes it requires just a sim-
Likelihood of Successful Billing ple telephone call. It may also be Internet based or
accomplished through a paper document. Of course,
• Effective documentation meeting all the clinician must also be savvy about requirements
requirements for reimbursement for covered services and documentation and about
• A physician’s order for SLP services effective ways to justify services. Services tend to be
• Preauthorization for services by the third- preauthorized if they meet the criteria for reimburs-
party payer able services and the appropriate documentation
• Evidence that the services are actually is provided.
covered by the plan
• Evidence of the need for services
• Confirmation that the methods used are Evidence That the Services Are
evidence based Actually Covered by the Plan
• Documentation of the life-affecting nature
of services and carryover to real-world No matter how much good a specific type of treat-
contexts ment might be for a given individual, if their insur-
• Evidence of treatment progress ance or national health plan does not cover it, the
• Good relationships with decision makers at treatment will not be reimbursable. Although it is
the agencies that reimburse us increasingly uncommon, some private and employ-
er-sponsored health plans, for example, do not
allow for coverage of any SLP services. Most limit
third-party payer is unlikely to pay. The items listed the number of diagnostic and treatment sessions (or
in Box 14–2 serve as a good checklist to consider in units of time spent in such sessions) allowed. Part
submitting reimbursement requests. of the process of ensuring coverage is requesting
authorization to continue providing services once a
previously approved amount of service has already
A Physician’s Order been provided.

Sometimes the first order for a given patient new to


a caseload is for a diagnostic session and a second Evidence of the Need for Skilled Services
order is made later for treatment based on what is
learned in the diagnostic process. More commonly, Important qualifications for reimbursable services
an order is given for an assessment and a specified in much of the world, and (as mentioned earlier)
number of treatment sessions per week for a spec- required by Medicare and Medicaid in the United
ified number of weeks. There may be other for- States, include that the service provided requires
mulas, especially for skilled nursing residents, as the skills and abilities of a highly trained clinician.
mentioned earlier. The physicians’ order may be on There are two types of skill entailed in skilled
paper, through electronic media, or conveyed by services. One pertains to the skills of the clinician
telephone, depending on the specific requirements providing the services. The other pertains to the
of the insurance intermediary. level of skill required to carry out the actual services
being rendered.
In the United States, a skilled clinician is consid-
Preauthorization for Services ered as one with a minimum of a master’s degree in
by the Third-Party Payer a program with national accreditation by the Coun-
cil on Academic Accreditation in Speech-Language
Preauthorization is commonly yet not always re- Pathology (or an approved equivalent degree). There
quired. If it is, it must be obtained before any SLP may be an exception to this in some cases where stu-
services are offered. The SLP must know whether dents or clinical fellows (e.g., U.S. clinicians who
14. Contexts for Providing Excellent Services   187

have a master’s degree but who are still working evidence for each method they use unless the third-
toward national certification and/or state licensure) party payer denies preauthorization or reimburse-
engage in clinical activities that meet stringent crite- ment. In cases where an appeal must be made to
ria for skilled services supervised by a licensed and contest a denial (discussed further in this chapter),
certified SLP. In most countries outside the United the SLP almost always must substantiate the evi-
States where SLP is a recognized profession, a skilled dence base that justifies the service provided.
clinician is one with a minimum of a 3- or 4-year
undergraduate degree in which the majority of the
degree program has been focused on clinical educa- Documentation of the
tion (coursework and supervised clinical practice) in Life-Affecting Nature of Services
the field.
In the United States, “skilled” services are those It is not sufficient to rely on our own confidence that
for which it can be documented that people need our services have meaningful impacts on the lives of
certain types and intensity of care that would not the people we serve. We must document how this is
typically be available for extended periods of time. the case. Just what is to be documented in this regard
These may include, for example, intensive wound varies according to clinical contexts. We typically
care by nursing, dysphagia management or commu- must provide support, for example, for our effective-
nication intervention by speech-language pathology, ness in enabling communication in real-life contexts,
intervention to improve safety and independence by medical management, independence, and quality of
occupational therapy, or treatment for strengthening life of the people we serve.
and mobility by physical therapy. Often, a combina- A common requirement is documentation of
tion of such services is provided in SNFs. how our services are “medically necessary.” Medical
Services that are considered to require the skills necessity is a loosely defined term, applied incon-
of a qualified SLP include ongoing monitoring, sistently among third-party payers. Suggestions for
assessment, feedback, and treatment modification defending the medical necessity of intervention to
based on patient needs, desires, and performance. address cognitive-linguistic challenges include docu-
Services that do not require such skills (unskilled menting how improved communication abilities may
services) include oversight of rote exercises, repeti-
tive drill and practice, and no requirement for eval- • enhance medical management through better
uation or feedback to the patient (Sampson et al., interaction with providers,
2013). Given the expectation that the skills of a fully • prevent further decline,
qualified clinician are required for the services to be • promote intact abilities,
paid for, the onus is on the SLP to document clearly • improve safety, and
and convincingly that all services provided require • enhance independence.
such skills.
Including statements from the people we serve,
significant others, and other members of our inter-
Confirmation That the Methods vention team helps to support such documentation.
Used Are Evidence Based

Evidence-based practice is at the heart of justifiable Evidence of Treatment Progress


health care services. If there is not documented evi-
dence that a certain procedure, process, or method If we cannot document that a person we are treat-
has been shown to lead to clear improvements in ing is making gains in treatment, then payers are
people with conditions similar to the person we unlikely to continue reimbursing us for our services
are treating, then the service we are providing is for that person. Daily, weekly, and monthly progress
not considered to constitute evidence-based prac- notes help us to track gains relative to specific func-
tice. Requirements for such evidence vary. In some tional communication goals. Specific documentation
cases, the SLP may not be required to document the requirements vary among payers, so it is important
188  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

to be familiar with what is required by the third-


party payers associated with each of the people we
What Are the Primary Reasons for Which
serve. If there are changes in an individual’s health Reimbursements for SLP Services Are Denied?
or social status that might negatively affect commu-
nication improvement, it is important to document The reasons for denying reimbursement are typi-
these. The onus is on the clinician to justify how a cally related to the following:
lack of continued improvement is not a failure of
the client to engage, the client’s lack of potential for • clerical errors on the part of the clinician or
improvement, or a failure of the clinician or treat- other professionals in the service-providing
ment program. When working with people who agency
have degenerative conditions, documenting treat- • misunderstanding or lack of knowledge
ment progress can be a major challenge. We must on the part of the third-party payer’s case
address that challenge directly by manager
• failure to document each of the items listed
• strategically selecting metrics used to track earlier regarding what makes services
improvement, and reimbursable
• advocating vigorously for people who need
and benefit from direct intervention to help By proactively addressing to these key reasons
slow the progression of symptoms and for denial, we can promote effective reimbursement
enhance quality of life, medical management, (Hallowell, 2021).
and social interaction.

Good Relationships With Decision What Do We Do if We Are Denied


Makers at Third-Party Payer Agencies Reimbursement for Our Services?

When we have positive professional relationships If we have adhered to all of the requirements of
with decision makers who understand and respect reimbursable services described earlier, we ideally
what we do, they are more likely to make judgments will receive payment for our services from a third-
in our favor. Ongoing educational and advocacy party payer. Of course, maintaining ongoing posi-
efforts with case managers and utilization review- tive and cooperative relationships with insurers is
ers at insurance companies are vital to healthy busi- the best means of avoiding denials in the first place
ness relationships. If possible, do not wait to have a and appealing them successfully if we must. How-
conflict with them to educate them about the great ever, no matter how superb our efforts, sometimes
service that you provide. Do so in a constructive and our requests for reimbursement are denied. If this
friendly way. If you are new to an agency or you is the case, it is important that we appeal the denial.
learn of new case managers or utilization reviewers The success of our appeal depends on the excellence
working for a payer with which you are likely to of our documentation and on our persistence in
have frequent billings, send a welcoming introduc- pursuing the appeal (Hallowell, 2021; Hallowell &
tory email with web links or a letter and brochures Chapey, 2008b). The first step in appealing a denial is
about what you do. Take advantage of the fact that typically the submission of an appeal form or letter
many insurance companies hold regular continuing along with associated clinical documentation to the
education events; offer to provide a workshop on third-party payer.
evidence-based practice in SLP. Most case managers Additional steps to contest a denial may involve
and utilization reviewers are not SLPs. Their back- having a patient or significant other make a com-
grounds are often in business or nursing, not com- plaint to the insurance company. Most payer orga-
munication disorders. Thus, helping them to learn nizations have a customer liaison who serves as a
about the life-changing work that we do helps to point of contact for inquiries or complaints from
promote our services as well as payment of our bills. patients covered by the plans the agency oversees.
14. Contexts for Providing Excellent Services   189

Patients who have employer-sponsored insurance sibility for allocating health care funds has been
plans may file complaints with the human resources transferred from federal to provincial or state gov-
department of their employers. ernments, a movement that has coincided with
In the United States, the clinician, a representa- ongoing reforms in policies and reimbursement
tive of the service-providing agency, the patient, or schemes aimed at cutting costs.
a caregiver may also file a complaint with the state Given drastic cost-saving measures, national
insurance commissioner. For continued, egregious insurance schemes worldwide now tend to be aimed
denials of reimbursement, other possibilities include largely at the needs of people who have insufficient
attracting media coverage to shed public light on resources to pay for their own health care needs.
ethical questions related to coverage for services. Of Many with the means to pay for private care through
course, it is always best to foster and maintain the private hospitals and clinics opt to do so. Those with
most positive of professional relationships and use higher income also tend to obtain their own pri-
such tactics only as a last resort. Still, pursuing what vate insurance coverage. As a result, in much of the
is right is important and may build public aware- world, a divergence in public versus private health
ness of the needs of people with communication care facilities has evolved. In most of Australia, Asia,
disorders. Europe, Africa, Central America, and South America,
In the everyday hectic work life of a clinician, wherever there are significant disparities in income
the time and effort required to appeal denials for among local citizens, the nature of public versus pri-
reimbursement — and the fact that this type of work vate hospitals is readily apparent. Private hospitals
is truly not enjoyable for most of us — make it easy tend to have shorter wait times for appointments,
to avoid the task altogether. It does not help that newer facilities, more modern technology, and more
such efforts are not reimbursable. However, it is “boutique” or elective sorts of services (e.g., cos-
of paramount importance that we all appeal every metic plastic surgery).
denial. Why? First, if we do not appeal it, we will Worldwide, as costs for private medical care
not get reimbursed for worthy services we already and insurance have risen over the past two decades,
provided. Second, we would be setting a precedent even citizens with relatively higher income have
(or providing further incentive) for that payer to sought publicly funded care. This has led to greater
leave our bills unpaid. Third, we would be tacitly financial strain on public hospitals and clinics, which
agreeing that our services are not valuable. Fourth, has led to even more dire cost-cutting schemes.
when we do not promote the value of our own pro- The impact on the private sector has been great as
fessional services, we detract from the perceived well, as there is greater competition for dwindling
value of SLP services (McCarty & Warren, 2017). private-payer funds. Across varied national sys-
In sum, making a commitment to appealing every tems globally, cost-cutting measures have reduced
denial and following through with that commit- the amount and types of services that may be pro-
ment are important acts of advocacy, not only for vided to people who want it and need it. Health
ourselves and the people we serve but for the whole care cost-saving tactics used by government agen-
of our profession. cies and insurance companies are summarized in
Box 14–3.

How Do Health Care Finance and


Cost-Control Systems Affect Clinical Services? What Are the Impacts of Health Care
Cost Cutting and Cost Control on
Services for People With Neurogenic
In the late 1980s and early 1990s, spiraling govern-
Communication Challenges?
ment health care expenditures led to ongoing and
sometimes dramatic changes in the way health-​
related services are funded. Related cost-cutting Reimbursement rates for SLP services, and the
schemes have affected virtually every area of means by which they have been determined, have
health-related practice. More and more, the respon- fluctuated over the past three decades. Managed
190  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

emergency rooms without even being admitted


Box
14–3 Health Care Cost-Savings Tactics or are admitted for only brief hospital stays and
Used by Government Agencies then are discharged to home or to rehabilitation
and Insurance Companies facilities. This has led to decreased continuity of
care and to shorter time windows in which people
• Required preauthorization for diagnostic with neurogenic communication disorders may be
and treatment services seen by SLPs. Those factors have made it such that
• Incentives to physicians for not authorizing the timing of treatment and prioritization of what
specialty or rehabilitation services aphasiologists actually do in varied service deliv-
• Stringent billing review and increased ery contexts are vital considerations in interven-
denials for billed services tion planning, a topic we discuss in further detail in
• Reduced reimbursement rates Chapters 23 and 24, on theories and best practices in
• Reduced frequency, intensity, and duration intervention.
of services paid for In a system focused on cost control, the per-
ceived “customers” in health care were once the
patients/clients/consumers. Now the customer is
care is a term used to capture the combined more typically seen as the third-party payer. The
goals of professionals making decisions about allowable ser-
vices (case managers and utilization reviewers) are
• controlling health care costs through a often not educated about the nature of neurogenic
system of care authorization that is controlled communication disorders. Having people other than
largely by physicians plus case managers and qualified providers determine which people may
utilization reviewers employed by third-party receive which services and at what intensity and
payers; for what duration generally does not constitute opti-
• coordinating care to reduce waste, abuse, and mal care.
redundancy of services; and Overall, the frequency, intensity, and duration
• ensuring access to care, quality of services, of services we are allowed to provide have been
and positive outcomes. reduced and held under greater scrutiny. Treatment
for neurogenic disorders is limited often to brief
Now, across health care contexts globally, aspects acute and subacute care stages, and rehabilitation
of managed care are so pervasive that the term is care within the first 6 months post-onset (Katz et al.,
used less and less; it is now commonly assumed that 2000; Verna et al., 2009). Requirements for evidence
such strategies for managing care are in place. The of progress in treatment have led to reduced cov-
goals of managed care are echoed in the frameworks erage of people with degenerative conditions, who
of several current regulatory initiatives. Consider, often do not meet the criteria for consistent improve-
for example, the similar goals espoused by the Insti- ment from the initiation of treatment to discharge.
tute for Healthcare Improvement, in its “Triple Aim Also, people with low income and people from
Initiative”: underrepresented ethnic and racial groups have dis-
proportionately greater challenges in accessing cov-
• Improving the patient experience of care erage for the services they need (Hallowell, 2021).
(including quality and satisfaction) Given that SLPs typically provide interven-
• Improving the health of populations tion for both swallowing and cognitive-communi-
• Reducing the per capita cost of health care cative disorders, another serious challenge is that
(Institute for Healthcare Improvement, n.d.) far more time, effort, and funding are allocated to
dysphagia over intervention for cognitive-linguis-
Many people who would have been hospital- tic disorders in medical settings — almost three
ized for serious neurological events (e.g., stroke times as much in countries where such data have
and brain injuries) in the past are discharged from been reported (ASHA, 2013; Enderby & Petheram,
14. Contexts for Providing Excellent Services   191

2002; Foster et al., 2014; Rose et al., 2014; Verna et al., ernment policies can also impose barriers to access
2009). A trend to allow reimbursement services that to care. Thus, it is important for aphasiologists to
are considered “medically necessary” as opposed to stay abreast of local, regional, and federal regula-
those promoting quality of life has supported this tions that affect access to SLP services for people
prioritization. Relatedly, professional colleagues who need them and to take seriously their role as
from other disciplines more easily understand the politically engaged citizens, speaking up for and
physical nature of swallowing and eating than they with people who may not be able to communicate so
do the abstract nature of language and communi- well on their own behalf. In Chapter 15, we consider
cation. It is thus easier for SLPs to rationalize and specific actions that clinical aphasiologists may take
receive authorizations and reimbursement for dys- to promote access to services.
phagia services (Enderby & Petheram, 2002). Lest we end this chapter with only the bleak-
Adding to this challenge is that many SLPs est of news about health care cost controls, let’s also
working in medical contexts actually prefer the recognize that some good things have arisen from
concreteness of working with swallowing disor- managed health care. Most important, the height-
ders over the abstractness and complexity of work ened focus on evidence-based practice and account-
in cognition and language. Furthermore, practicing ability has stimulated new research initiatives and
SLPs have also reported that the clinical education priorities of clinicians, researchers, academic insti-
programs from which they graduated underempha- tutions, and professional organizations. We are now
sized content about cognitive-linguistic disorders. required to consider how our services lead to life-​
Foster et al. (2014) report that practicing SLPs rate affecting changes and to document that the methods
themselves as more confident in swallowing than we use are evidence based. Such advancements help
language and rate their mastery of knowledge and not only to justify our services but also to improve
skills in swallowing as greater than their mastery in the way we diagnose and treat people with neuro-
language disorders. genic cognitive-communicative disorders.
Another repercussion of a cost-focused health Changes in the positive direction also include
care system is that the integrity of separate health improved coordination of care through enhanced
professions has been challenged through the use of recordkeeping, and increasingly automated billing
aides and assistants. Use of support personnel saves and documentation programs that free up more
on costs because they are paid far less than fully time for clinicians to engage in direct intervention.
qualified professionals. Such individuals are often Also, to reduce costs of health care services, many
trained in transdisciplinary modes to provide basic insurance companies have stepped up support for
drill-and-practice routines as they attempt to replace preventive care and healthy lifestyle education.
skilled services from qualified professionals. Such efforts may help to reduce the risk of stroke
When agencies that employ rehabilitation pro- and brain injury and slow the progression of neuro-
fessionals face financial pressures, these pressures degenerative conditions.
are often felt directly by those professionals in the Clinicians and clinical administrators who have
form of cost-savings and revenue generation pres- been in practice over the past two or three decades
sure. Such pressures often lead to serious ethical commonly observe that the business savvy of clini-
challenges for clinical professionals, a topic that we cians has been enhanced through an increased per-
consider in further detail in Chapter 15. sonal sense of investment in their agencies’ revenue
Globally, government policies have been devel- generation and professional operations. Clinicians
oped to alleviate some of the challenges of health now tend to play a more critical role in marketing,
care cost controls and access to care. Of course, gov- billing, and quality assurance, for example.
192  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter 10. Summarize the sources of funding that
that are new to you or that you have not yet support services in clinical aphasiology in
mastered. your local region.
2. With a partner, review the list of roles of 11. Describe the opportunities for accessing SLP
SLPs who specialize in acquired neurogenic services in your region for adults who have
communication disorders. Discuss which are little or no income.
your personal most and least favorite roles 12. Consider the list of factors that promote the
and why. likelihood of successful billing, summarized
3. With a partner, review the list of clinical in Box 14–2. Describe how a potential
service settings described. Discuss the pros employer might be interested in your
and cons you would consider if you were knowledge about these if you were seeking a
contemplating a professional position in clinical position in their agency.
each type of setting. 13. Describe the ways in which promoting
4. If you are a student, consider organizing successful billing practice is an act of
a panel of professional SLPs who work advocacy for adults with neurogenic
in varied contexts to come speak to your cognitive-communicative disorders.
class or to an extracurricular group about 14. Describe what is meant by the following
their experiences working in varied statement: “Making a commitment to
settings. appealing every single denial and following
5. If you had a neurogenic language disorder, through with that commitment are
in what type of clinical setting would you important acts of advocacy, not only for
choose to seek treatment for it? Why? ourselves and the people we serve but for
6. Research the agencies that provide services the whole of our profession.”
for adults with neurogenic communication 15. List specific steps that you might take to
disorders in your local your community. enhance the proportion of time you spend as
Learn about the types of services provided a clinician treating cognitive-communicative
there. If possible, contact an SLP at each (or, disorders compared to swallowing disorders
if appropriate, make such contacts through in a rehabilitation setting.
a university-based aphasiologist). Consider 16. Describe how trends in health care over
visiting and perhaps even observing related the past three decades have influenced
clinical or social activities. the following for people with aphasia and
7. Describe what types of SLP services would related disorders:
be appropriate for a dying person under a. Affordability of care
hospice care. b. Access to care
8. Describe specific ways that you might ensure c. Quality of care
relationship-centered interactions when d. Ethics in care delivery
providing clinical sessions via telepractice.
www
9. Compare and contrast the three primary See the companion website for additional learning
types of rehabilitation teams. and teaching materials.
CHAPTER
15
Engaging Proactively in Advocacy
and Legal and Ethical Concerns

As we note throughout this book, speech-language


pathologists (SLPs) play critical roles as advocates
How May Clinicians and the People We
in virtually every aspect of clinical and scholarly Serve Promote Access to SLP Services
work in the field. In this chapter, we consider the and Communication Support?
importance of advocacy related to ethical and legal
concerns. We also consider the interplay among Worldwide changes in health care policies, along
morality, ethics, and law and how these constructs with sweeping changes in the ways that health
are so vital to our work related to access to care, care is delivered and paid for, have dramatically
human rights, and judgments about competence and affected access to rehabilitation services for peo-
decision-making. Finally, we explore how the finan- ple who would benefit from them. Although there
cial pressures felt in many of our work environments have been recent significant gains in access to care in
lead to ethical dilemmas for clinicians and what can much of the world, there have also been increasing
be done to proactively address such dilemmas. limitations — and threats of further limitations — for
After reading and reflecting on the content in people with aphasia and related disorders. This is
this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in especially the case in countries where private insur-
your own words, the following queries: ance and federally sponsored health care plans have
undergone continuing policy and cost-cutting mod-
1. How may clinicians and the people we ifications over the past three decades. In addition,
serve promote access to SLP services and heightened awareness of disparities in access to
communication support? health care, and in support for lifestyles that lead to
2. How are human rights, morality, ethics, and law reduced risk of stroke, traumatic brain injury (TBI),
relevant to advocacy for people with acquired and dementia (among other causes of neurogenic
neurogenic disorders of language and cognition? communication disorders) make it critical that we
3. What is the role of the SLP in supporting the step up our work to promote communication as a
rights of individuals with aphasia and related human right for all.
disorders? Actions that aphasiologists may take to pro-
4. How do SLPs engage in decisions regarding mote access to their professional services are listed
competence and decision-making? in Box 15–1. Key strategies are described here.
5. How might financial conflicts of interest affect
the practice of clinical aphasiologists?

193
194  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Box
15–1 Actions That Aphasiologists May Take to Promote
Access to Their Professional Services

• Educate others about neurogenic communication disorders, their


life-affecting consequences, and the need for intervention and
support
• Provide truly excellent clinical services
• Enhance awareness of communication as a human right
• Stay abreast of political forces and policy developments that influence
health care and wellness
• Educate and mobilize consumers to advocate for themselves
• Educate and market to current and potential referral sources
• Appeal denials of all treatment authorization and reimbursement
• Contribute financially to political action committees that engage in
professional advocacy with governmental agencies
• In writing and through in-person visits, educate and express concerns and
needs to elected officials and those running for office
• Join professional advocacy networks through professional associations
• Engage in and promote evidence-based practice research
• Disseminate information about positive clinical outcomes
• Mentor future clinicians about their roles as advocates, and provide
opportunities for hands-on advocacy work for clinical students

Enhance Awareness of HelpAge International and Handicap Interna-


Communication as a Human Right tional (2012) provide evidence that less than 1% of
humanitarian aid is allocated for older people and
people with disabilities. From this, we can deduce
As we discussed in previous chapters, human com- that far less aid is allocated for people with commu-
munication is a right. People with disabilities have nication challenges. The World Report on Disability
equal rights; older people have equal rights, too. (World Health Organization [WHO] & World Bank,
Yet humanitarian programs and policies globally 2011) emphasizes a hierarchy of societal exclusion,
are sorely lacking in terms of a focus on both aging in recognition that disabilities related to cognition,
and disability (Jubeh & Abdalla, 2020; Small, 2018). communication, and behavior are more marginal-
Despite great efforts since the 1980s represented izing than are physical disabilities and blindness.
through the First and Second World Assemblies on Yet physical disabilities tend to attract the greatest
Ageing, the United Nations Principles for Older attention from the medical community and capture
Persons, the Humanitarian Standards Partnership, the majority of already limited rehabilitation fund-
the World Bank Office of Disability and Develop- ing globally. This is a travesty, given how vital one’s
ment, HelpAge International, the World Human- ability to communicate is to one’s sense of humanity,
itarian Summit, and the Global Rehabilitation independence, sense of fulfillment in a social world,
Alliance — and efforts of thousands of nongovern- and quality of life.
mental organizations addressing health, aging, and In 2017, the WHO launched an information and
disability — few such initiatives address communi- advocacy program entitled “Rehabilitation 2030: A
cation disorders or even mention communication Call for Action,” which brought together stakehold-
disorders in policy statements or working papers. ers from many disciplines and countries to engage
15. Engaging Proactively in Advocacy and Legal and Ethical Concerns   195

in action planning toward improving rehabilitation materials for promoting patients’ rights, assessment
access worldwide. Many initiatives have been gain- tools, and information about laws and other regu-
ing momentum globally since then, with foci on the lations. The National Aphasia Association (http://
following: www.apha sia.org) offers print and downloadable
documents pertaining to the rights of people with
• promoting rehabilitation for anyone with aphasia and also to the rights of care partners.
a health condition or disability (acute or In addition to supporting awareness of rights
chronic) that limits life participation of people with communication challenges, clinical
• recognizing rehabilitation as an essential aphasiologists have roles to play in ethical and legal
component of health care issues related to the rights of the people they serve.
• revamping health systems to strengthen These are discussed later in this chapter.
rehabilitation
• expanding implementation and systems
science approaches (see Chapter 23) to
Raise Awareness About Neurogenic
research costs and benefits of rehabilitation, Communication Challenges and
access to care, and rehabilitation outcomes Ways to Support People and Loved
Ones Coping With Them
Having been personally engaged in leadership
and consultation with initiatives of the WHO and Educating others about aphasia and related disor-
affiliated organizations for several years, I have great ders is fundamental to our role as advocates. For
hope that the momentum will continue. It will be someone who has never learned about aphasia and
important that national and international profes- related disorders, communicating with a person
sional organizations related to aphasiology around who has one may lead to incorrect conclusions about
the world step up their advocacy for the field of the person’s intelligence or mental stability. As Tom
communication sciences and disorders to be well Broussard, a stroke educator with aphasia, aptly
represented in leadership and advocacy in these states, “No one wants to talk, meet, understand or
important areas. build a relationship with a person with aphasia until
The International Communication Project one learns what aphasia means” (Broussard, 2020, p.
(https://internationalcommunicationproject.com), a 2). Despite the fact that aphasia has high incidence
movement to advance communication as a human and prevalence rates in comparison to many other
right, was launched in 2014 (see Mulcair et al. [2018] conditions, several studies have shown that, across
for an overview). Anyone interested in supporting the globe, laypeople generally lack awareness of
that cause is invited to sign a pledge on the organiza- what it is (Code et al., 2001, 2016; Flynn et al., 2009;
tion’s website and to share information about related Hill et al., 2019; Mavi, 2007; McCann et al., 2013; Pat-
awareness raising and advocacy campaigns. terson et al., 2012; Simmons-Mackie et al., 2002).
Many hospitals, clinics, and other health and People with aphasia report that a lack of aware-
wellness agencies across the globe have policies ness of aphasia and resultant negative assumptions
regarding the rights of the people they serve. With and imposition of stereotypes contribute to the neg-
increasing frequency, these are seen posted on the ative impacts of their communication challenges
walls of such agencies and distributed in print and (Worrall et al., 2011). Lack of knowledge about neu-
made available online. In some countries, the accred- rogenic communication disorders may also be a bar-
iting bodies overseeing health care agencies provide rier to community reintegration, including return
guidance and sometimes accreditation requirements to work (Hinckley, 2002; Patterson et al., 2012; Sim-
pertaining to human rights of health care consumers. mons-Mackie, 2018). Other negative consequences
An example is the Joint Commission (2010) “road include that agencies and donors that could help
map” for hospitals, which addresses such important fund services for people with aphasia and related
topics as communication standards, patient-centered challenges are unlikely to do so if they are unfamiliar
care, and cultural competence and provides tutorial with such challenges (Elman et al., 2000; Ganzfried,
196  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

2018; Morrow-Odom & Barnes, 2019; Prins et al., for enhanced legibility and readability of
2019). It is vital that we help people with neurogenic signage, menus, insurance and health care
communication disorders educate others about their documents, activity schedules, calendars, and
conditions. Also, having family and friends clarify web pages (Figure 15–1)
with others just what aphasia and related disorders • first-responder training programs, offering
are and are not can be a great way for them to advo- awareness and sensitivity training to police,
cate for a loved one. firefighters, and other emergency personnel
Means of raising awareness about neurogenic (Figure 15–2)
cognitive-linguistic disorders include sharing sto- • awareness-raising programs (Figure 15–3),
ries, videos, and photos via social media and pub- events, and celebrations, perhaps including
lishing articles for laypeople in newspapers, blogs, performances of actors and musicians
and the popular press. When doing so, it is import- with language disorders or films that
ant to use relevant terminology rather than simpli- promote empowering views of people with
fying explanations so much that an opportunity to communication disabilities
enhance others’ educated reference to such condi- • assessments of businesses for communication
tions is lost. For example, when writing an article for accessibility and other aspects of aphasia
a local newspaper, do not avoid use of the term apha- friendliness, and presentation of awards for
sia. Rather, use the term and explain what it means. aphasia-friendly businesses
When celebrities and public officials acquire • promotion of local and national aphasia
neurogenic communication disorders, draw on pub- awareness activities and advocacy with
lic interest by turning news stories into public edu- city, state, provincial, and national political
cation opportunities. Celebrities sharing on social representatives including proclamations on
media, speaking publicly, and writing books about the importance of empowering people with
their experiences with acquired communication cognitive-linguistic challenges
challenges can raise awareness while also empow-
ering others coping with such challenges, including Aphasia awareness activities may be woven
families and friends. Examples of books by famous into group treatment activities, often with leadership
people with aphasia and their care partners include from group members (see Chapter 25). Substantial
Gabrielle Giffords’s and Mark Kelly’s Gabby: A Story information and materials to support such activities
of Courage and Hope (2011); Joe Biden’s Promise Me, are provided through many of the organizations for
Dad: A Year of Hope, Hardship, and Purpose (2017); which websites are listed in Chapter 2 (Table 2–2)
Randy Travis’s Forever and Ever, Amen (2019); Mark and Chapter 27 (Table 27–1).
McEwen’s Change in the Weather: Life After Stroke Numerous people with neurogenic communi-
(2008); Diane Ackerman’s One Hundred Names for cation challenges and their partners have done (and
Love (2011); and Lee and Bob Woodruff’s In an Instant are doing) amazing work to raise awareness about
(2007). See the National Aphasia Association website acquired neurogenic communication challenges.
for examples of celebrity stories that help advance They do this while educating others about resources
aphasia awareness. for information, services, funding, and by providing
Initiate or take part in expanding aphasia and support and inspiration for others’ coping and resil-
dementia-friendly communities. Several organi- ience. Consider these wonderful examples:
zations and aphasia centers have launched effec-
tive outreach programs to develop and implement • Stroke activist Debra Meyerson (Figure 15–4),
aphasia-friendly businesses and communities and who has aphasia and (along with her son and
promote awareness (Cruice et al., 2005; Howe et al., with input from her entire family) wrote the
2004; Pound et al., 2007; Worrall et al., 2007). Let’s book Identity Theft: Rediscovering Ourselves
consider the following examples: after Stroke (Meyerson & Zuckerman, 2019).
With her husband, she launched Stroke
• communication access training for community Onward, the mission of which is “to provide
leaders and businesses, including suggestions stroke survivors, families and caregivers
15. Engaging Proactively in Advocacy and Legal and Ethical Concerns   197

Figure 15–1. Members of the California State University East Bay Aphasia Treatment Program engage in a col-
laborative project with park rangers at Yosemite National Park. Using supported communication and educating
park rangers about aphasia, the group re-designed a Yosemite visitor’s guide. By making maps and other printed
material aphasia friendly, they enhanced communication access to park information for all people. Photo courtesy
of Ellen Bernstein-Ellis. A full-color version of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.

with more resources to help them navigate co-authored two books (Dow et al., 2017;
the emotional journey to rebuild their Dow & Dow-Richards, 2013), presents
identities and rewarding lives” (https://stroke​ frequent talks on aphasia, and leads online
onward.org). interactive sessions for people with aphasia
• David Dow (Figure 15–5), who had a stroke and related disorders.
at age 10 years, now (as an adult) serves as • Tom Broussard (Figure 15–6), a three-
vice president and co-founder of Aphasia time stroke survivor with aphasia, is the
Recovery Connection (ARC), the mission president of Stroke Educator, Inc. (https://
of which is “to improve the quality of life www​.stroke​educator.com). He is a tireless
for people recovering from aphasia and aphasia advocate and educator, gives
their families and friends” (https://www​ frequent presentations, offers a regular
.aphasiarecoveryconnection.org). In addition newsletter, and has written four books on
to providing leadership for ARC, he has aphasia.
Figure 15–2. Members of the Aphasia Education and Advocacy Team of the Lansing (Mich-
igan) Area Aphasia Support Group along with emergency personnel. Prior to presenting
six two-hour emergency personnel trainings, the team, composed of people with aphasia,
care partners, an SLP, and a graduate intern, spent 4 months reviewing research, creating
training materials, and sharing opinions and experiences with emergency personnel. Photo
courtesy of Katie Strong. A full-color version of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.

Figure 15–3. The dynamic podcast crew of stroke and brain injury survivors at the Stroke Comeback Center
(https://strokecomebackcenter.org) produce The Slow Road to Better, an ongoing fun, informative, and often poi-
gnant podcast series (http://theslowroadtobetter.blogspot.com) about “the good, the bad and the ugly living life with
aphasia.” Photo courtesy of Melissa Richman. A full-color version of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.

198
Figure 15–4. Stroke activist Debra Meyerson, with her Figure 15–5. Carol Dow-Richards, president and exec-
husband, Steve Zuckerman, and co-author son, Danny utive director of ARC, along with her son, David Dow,
Zuckerman, displaying their insightful, inspiring, and aphasia survivor, educator, author, and advocate. Photo
information-rich book on aphasia. Photo courtesy of courtesy of David and Carol Dow. A full-color version of
Debra Meyerson. A full-color version of this figure can this figure can be found in the Color Insert.
be found in the Color Insert.

Figure 15–6. Tom Broussard, president of


Stroke Educator, Inc. and a person with
aphasia, sharing information about aphasia
with passersby near his home. Photo courtesy
of Tom Broussard. A full-color version of this
figure can be found in the Color Insert.

199
200  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

• Lauren Murphy, a TBI survivor with aphasia, ety as well as missed communication opportunities
and her mother, Colleen Murphy, pair up for people with acquired language disorders. One
to share inspiring stories of survival and cannot assume that because someone has a medical
recovery throughout the Unites States or health professional degree that they know much
(Figure 15–7). Colleen also authored a book to about cognitive-communicative disorders, let alone
share about the perspectives of parents, care know optimal ways of supporting people who have
partners, and families affected by TBI and them (Simmons-Mackie, Kagan, et al., 2007).
aphasia (Murphy, 2021). Consider Jill Bolte Taylor’s personal observa-
• Avi Golden (Figure 15–8), a former paramedic, tion following her stroke and onset of aphasia:
travels locally and internationally to raise
awareness of aphasia and to empower people To someone looking on, I may have been judged
with physical, cognitive, and communicative as less than what I had been before because
disabilities to engage in adventure sports. I could not process information like a normal
person. I was saddened by the inability of the
medical community to know how to communi-
Help Educate Professionals in cate with someone in my condition . . . I wanted
Health Care Contexts my doctors to focus on how my brain was work-
ing rather than on whether it worked according
Lack of knowledge about aphasia and other disor- to their criteria or timetable. I still knew volumes
ders, and about how to best facilitate communica- of information and I was simply going to have to
tion, is common among professionals in health care figure out how to access it again. (Taylor, 2006,
settings and is a serious source of stress and anxi- p. 78)

Figure 15–7. Lauren Murphy, a traumatic brain injury survivor with aphasia, presenting the
commencement speech at Fontbonne University, her alma mater. Photo courtesy of Colleen
Murphy. A full-color version of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.
15. Engaging Proactively in Advocacy and Legal and Ethical Concerns   201

Figure 15–8. Avi Golden, stroke and aphasia awareness advocate and enthusiast for adventure
sports for all people, regardless of physical, cognitive, or communicative challenges. Photo courtesy
of Avi Golden. A full-color version of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.

Providing regularly scheduled practical, dynamic, when an insurance plan is involved. Thus, for better
and interactive in-services to staff members, be they or worse, physicians may often control your access
physicians, nurses, dietitians, housekeepers, and so to patients and patients’ access to you. Some phy-
on, is a valuable form of advocacy. So is informal sicians are phenomenal, rehabilitation-promoting,
coaching of clinical colleagues and other staff mem- relationship-centered colleagues. Some, however,
bers as they interact with people with communica- have closed minds related to the work that SLPs
tion disorders on a daily basis. do. Some ignore or reject the literature on treatment
efficacy for cognitive-linguistic disorders. Some
treat nonphysicians as inferior professionals. Let’s
Encourage Referrals be articulate, assertive, and proactive in having
our physician colleagues learn about our scope of
One important way to ensure ongoing referrals is to practice, expertise, and competence. Working zeal-
develop a positive working relationship with refer- ously, affirmatively, and professionally, rather than
ring physicians. Recall that a physician’s order is typ- conveying a resigning attitude, often pays off, as in
ically required for diagnostic or treatment services many other areas of professional practice and life.
202  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Advocate for Reduced Medicalization Promote Community-Based Approaches


of Communication Disabilities
Extending communicative support throughout local
In much of the Western world, definitions of dis- communities helps increase the likelihood that peo-
ability, and disability-related laws and policies, tend ple who need such support will be able to access it. In
to be medicalized. That is, they are based on “the addition to raising community awareness about and
highly esteemed science of medicine” (Harry, 1992, support for people with communication disorders,
p. 113), with a strong focus on physical impairments. we may extend communication support through
Medicalizing may actually do more harm than good volunteer communication partner programs (Kagan
in the sense of fostering normal versus abnormal or et al., 2001; Lyon et al., 1997), in-home respite pro-
disordered dichotomies, especially in the context of grams (Hallowell, 1999, 2000), and caregiver training
developing regions of the world and underrepre- programs and support groups (Fox et al., 2004; Lyon,
sented groups. In what Ndi (2012) refers to as the 1996; Purdy & Hindenlang, 2005; Ripich et al., 2000).
“professionalized legacy of the post-industrial rev- We may also provide opportunities for enhanced
olutionary epoch” (also referred to as neocolonial- access through aphasia centers and telepractice (see
ism), “it was presumed that there was an objective Chapter 14).
condition in which the concept of normality of the Such locally oriented activities are key to our
body was to be referred, and the role of the rehabili- role in promoting community-based rehabilitation
tation professional was to make changes on the body (CBR). CBR is defined by the WHO (n.d.-b) as follows:
of the disabled person in order to bring it as closely
as possible to the condition of normality” (p. 1). Of [A means of] enhancing the quality of life for peo-
course, more important than how we define dis- ple with disabilities and their families; meeting
abilities across cultures and groups is how we treat basic needs; and ensuring inclusion and partici-
people who have them (Barron & Amerena, 2007; pation. It is a multi-sectoral strategy that empow-
Ingstad & Reynolds Whyte, 1995; Wickenden, 2013). ers persons with disabilities to access and benefit
Let’s be mindful that some people whom we from education, employment, health and social
might consider to have disabilities do not necessar- services. CBR is implemented through the com-
ily agree that they have disabilities (Edmonds, 2005). bined efforts of people with disabilities, their fam-
An interesting case in point is Schensul, Torres, and ilies and communities, and relevant government
Wetle’s (1992) finding that older Puerto Ricans living and nongovernment health, education, voca-
in the United States were aware of their cognitive tional, social and other services.
and behavioral changes associated with Alzhei-
mer’s disease but considered them to be normal. Some of our community-based work is not reim-
It is important to note that those authors’ findings bursable. Many of us volunteer substantial amounts
are not necessarily valid, current, or representative of time to such activities, above and beyond the
of people in broader Puerto Rican or Hispanic com- requirements of our paid positions, because of our
munities. Still, the implications bring up potentially commitment to the people who benefit from our
far-reaching questions. Is it important for clinicians support and expertise. As professionals, when we
to prove wrong people who do not believe they have promote networks of consumers, volunteers, and pro-
a problem? If so, why? Are there ways that cogni- fessionals who are accessible in local communities, we
tive-communicative strengths can be emphasized
in people with differing communication abilities • facilitate meaningful engagement with and
without insisting on diagnostic categorization? Of for people with neurogenic communication
course, even when there is clarity or agreement that disorders and other disabilities;
a person has a disability per se, what an individ- • advance public education and understanding
ual and their family or others might consider to be about neurogenic communication disorders
important outcomes for rehabilitation also is highly and their impacts on people’s lives; and
variable (Dilworth-Anderson, Pierre, & Hilliard, • provide extended opportunities for inclusive
2012; Shogren, 2011). socialization.
15. Engaging Proactively in Advocacy and Legal and Ethical Concerns   203

CBR approaches are the emphasis of many pro- • Should people who are near death be
grams being developed through nongovernmental provided rehabilitative services?
agencies and volunteers in areas of the world where • Who has the right to decide whether a person
SLP services are minimal or nonexistent. Those of us is to have access to rehabilitative services?
in more privileged regions have much to learn from • At what point is it time to stop promoting
the effective models of extended care and opportu- curative solutions and instead provide
nity provided through CBR in regions where no or palliative care?
few rehabilitation resources exist. • Who has the right to decide how long a
person should be enabled to live?
• By what means should a given individual be
Expand Knowledge Translation kept alive or allowed to die?
• How much of our limited financial resources
Representatives of international governmental and should be used to prolong a given person’s
nongovernmental agencies and health care providers life?
are engaged in ongoing global efforts to promote the
use of research evidence to shape health care policy, Excellent clinical SLPs simultaneously uphold
working to bridge the gaps between research, policy, standards according to ethics, morality, and law
and practice endorsing translation of research into (Horner, 2003). Morality consists of subjective judg-
best practices (Global Ministerial Forum on Research ment of what conduct and consequences are good
for Health, 2008; WHO, 2004). Health-focused fund- and bad. Moral principles include the following:
ing agencies are implementing policies to help
ensure the relevance of health-related scholarship to • respect for people, including respect for
the lives of actual people with the conditions. Still, it choices that others make or would make for
is not clear how much of the work being published themselves, and respect for their autonomy
in aphasiology can be or is being translated to clin- • beneficence, acting for others’ good
ical use (Douglas & Burshnic, 2018; Onslow, 2008; • nonmaleficence, avoiding doing harm to
Raymer et al., 2008). SLPs’ roles in clinical trans- others
lation of research are essential to every aspect of • justice, making decisions and sharing
clinical practice. It is important that research aphasi- resources fairly
ologists not only consider and emphasize the clinical
relevance of their scholarly work but also collaborate The intersection of values and moral princi-
with members of clinical populations under study ples is represented in the codes of ethics of many
and clinicians to ensure that their findings are trans- national professional associations in communication
lated to actual practice. We delve into this topic in disorders and sciences. See Figure 15–9, based on an
greater detail as we discuss evidence-based practice, example from Speech Pathology Australia. Ethics
practice-based evidence, and implementation sci- involves subjective decision-making about what is
ence in Chapter 23. right or wrong, what our obligations to others are,
and what is appropriate. Law consists of locally,
regionally, or nationally adopted rules and princi-
ples about rights, equality, and fairness and involves
How Are Human Rights, Morality, the balancing of varied interests.
Ethics, and Law Relevant to Advocacy The principle of equal protection of the laws
for People With Acquired Neurogenic maintains that people with disabilities have the
same opportunities as everyone to participate in
Disorders of Language and Cognition?
society. All people have rights to do certain things
and not to have their rights restricted, according to
Consider some of the tough questions being asked applicable laws. Among the many international cov-
by the people we serve and the professionals with enants that have addressed (directly or indirectly)
whom we work: the rights of people with disabilities, the most recent
204  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

ety with the same rights as others governed by the


same laws. Rather than beings seen as “patients”
as defined by medical needs, environmental sup-
ports and social contexts are considered vital com-
ponents of equal rights. As mentioned earlier, there
are numerous additional national and international
policies and laws that support the rights of people
with disabilities.

What Is the Role of the SLP in


Supporting the Rights of Individuals
With Aphasia and Related Disorders?

Despite significant documented commitments to


the rights of people with disabilities, and despite
institutions’ commitments to those rights, people
with communication disorders often fall through
the cracks when it comes to actually realizing such
rights. Challenges with communication and others’
Figure 15–9. Depiction of the intersection of values and lack of awareness of their needs and desires often
principles as they relate to standards of ethical practice. impede full participation in social relationships.
Speech Pathology Australia (https://www.speechpathol- These challenges may also restrict appropriate and
ogyaustralia.org.au) highlights ethical decision-making
ideal participation in decision-making about con-
as being contextualized in cultural and linguistic respon-
siveness and person centeredness. Source: Adapted
cerns that profoundly affect lives of people with
from Johnson et al., 2021. communication disorders, such as decisions about
driving, independent living, financial management,
legal affairs, medical services and medications,
eating and nutrition, rehabilitation goals, and sex-
far-reaching one is the United Nations Convention ual consent (Bingham, 2012; Boswell, 2011; Horner,
on the Rights of Persons with Disabilities (United 2013). Although it is not within the SLP’s scope of
Nations, 2006). According to the convention, its practice to provide legal advice, supporting commu-
purpose is to “promote, protect and ensure the full nication to help people with communication chal-
and equal enjoyment of all human rights and fun- lenges engage in self-advocacy related to their rights
damental freedoms by all persons with disabilities, is important. Many SLPs are well suited to play a
and to promote respect for their inherent dignity” key role in
(Article 1).
The convention includes provisions for accessi- • helping physicians, legal professionals, and
bility, independent living, and inclusion in the com- courts to determine decision-making capacity
munity, health, habilitation, and rehabilitation. One and competence of people with aphasia and
hundred eighty-one countries plus the European related disorders;
Union have signed and/or ratified the convention • raising awareness about potential rights
at the time of this writing. The convention comple- violations;
ments the WHO’s efforts to consider disability from • advocating for communication supports
a biopsychosocial perspective (see Chapters 4 and and intervention required to enable each
5). Rather than being seen as charity cases, people individual to participate actively in their own
with disabilities are seen as equal members of soci- decision-making; and
15. Engaging Proactively in Advocacy and Legal and Ethical Concerns   205

• providing communication support and Interprofessional collaboration in such efforts is


training others to do so during competency important (Barton et al., 1996; Brady Wagner, 2003;
evaluations, decision-making discussions, and Finestone & Blackmer, 2007; Togher et al., 2006).
legal proceedings. When there are concerns or disagreements
about a person’s competence to make decisions, all
SLPs should review accreditation or licen- health and wellness professionals have the duty to
sure standards with which the facility or agency is advocate for intervention, be it through consulta-
required to comply. In the United States, the Joint tion with an ethicist, engagement of a mediator, or
Commission, which accredits hospitals, has explicit launching of or involvement in a legal case. Legal
standards for promoting provider-patient commu- processes that help to ensure the self-determination
nication to ensure that patients’ communicative of people whose decision-making competence or
participation is facilitated so that they may actively capacity is in question (or may eventually come into
participate in their own care (Joint Commission, question) are summarized in Box 15–2.
2010; Simmons-Mackie, 2013b). Patients’ rights For those wishing to learn more about this topic,
statements are often required to be posted publicly Horner (2013) and Brady Wagner (2003) provide
in hospitals and clinics in many countries. SLPs may tutorials on communication rights and policies rel-
support communication about these with people evant to people with aphasia and related disorders,
who need such support. and the role of SLPs in promoting ethical practice in
this regard. Kagan and Kimelman (1995), Palmer and
Patterson (2011), Penn, Frankel, Watermeyer, and
How Do SLPs Engage in Decisions Regarding
Competence and Decision-Making?
Box
15–2 Legal Procedures to Help to
Despite the fact that engagement in issues of com- Ensure Self-Determination
petence and decision-making is central to the role
of the SLP, many do not have training or experience • Advance directives: Documentation of a
in this area (Ferguson et al., 2010). Others are highly person’s wishes for medical care in case
sophisticated about this topic and some are engaged they become unable to convey them
as expert witnesses in related legal cases. • Living will: A document detailing a
Important elements of decision-making capacity person’s wishes in case they have a
and competence include determining the following: terminal condition and are near death and
cannot make their own decisions about
• comprehension, the ability to understand potential life-prolonging treatments
written or spoken communication in the • Durable power of attorney for health
language being used care: A document used to appoint a trusted
• choice, the ability to deliberate about and person to make health care decisions for a
logically consider possible alternatives person if they become incapacitated
• appreciation of consequences and risks and • Guardianship of a person: Full or limited,
benefits of decisions temporary or permanent oversight of an
• whether a person is being coerced individual
• Conservatorship of a person’s property:
There is no simple way to index capacity, and Full or limited, temporary or permanent
there is no single method or tool that can be applied oversight of the things a person owns
to all people with cognitive-linguistic challenges to • Statutory surrogacy: Legal designation
determine capacity or competence (Ferguson et al., of a person to make decisions for an adult
2003; Karlawish, 2008; Pachet et al., 2010; Rowland who is deemed incompetent
& McDonald, 2009; Stein & Brady Wagner, 2006).
206  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Müller (2009), Salima and Tammy (2016), and Zus- • providing intervention to people with limited
cak, Peisah, and Ferguson (2016) provide guidance potential for rehabilitation, who are unlikely
on how the rights of people with aphasia may be to benefit from skilled treatment
taken into account for informed consent in research • overstating a person’s potential for
contexts. Pape, Jaffe, Savage, Collins, and Warden improvement to justify billable services
(2004) suggest special challenges in this regard to be • misrepresenting actual progress made in
addressed with TBI survivors. Chang and Bourgeois treatment
(2015) provide tutorial materials about ways to pro- • misrepresenting the time spent in billable
mote end-of-life decision-making capacity through sessions
visual supports for people with dementia. • avoiding or limiting nonbillable activities
that are important to quality of service (e.g.,
in-services, informal discussions with team
How Might Financial Conflicts of Interest members, staff meetings) (Cutter & Polovoy,
2014; Hallowell, 2021)
Affect the Practice of Clinical Aphasiologists?
High productivity requirements challenge the
Clinicians often have conflicts of interest related to ethical standards not only of clinicians but also of
their own financial gains. For example, they may service-providing agencies. In some contexts, cer-
be paid bonuses in addition to their regular salary tain important activities are considered non-reve-
for achieving target rates of clinical income. Some- nue-generating. Examples include report writing,
times such bonuses are paid to teams for meeting billing, attending team meetings, screening, read-
or exceeding collective revenue targets. Certain rates ing of medical charts, marketing, providing and
of productivity are required for some SLPs to keep attending in-services, conferring with professional
their jobs, which poses even more severe inherent colleagues, providing information and counseling
financial conflicts of interest. for people who are not on the SLP’s caseload, and
Ways in which the quality and ethics of our training and mentoring student clinicians or clinical
services may be influenced by financial incentives fellows. SLPs may have to engage in such activities
include the following unethical and sometimes without being paid, often in addition to a full-time
fraudulent activity: job (Cutter & Polovoy, 2014).
Regardless of the pressures felt regarding poten-
• seeing patients too long or beyond the point tially unethical activities, clinicians are responsible
of expecting significant continued progress for maintaining their own ethical and legal stan-
• imposing clinical services even when patients dards. When investigating employment opportuni-
refuse to participate ties with a particular agency, it is a good idea to ask
• when serving low-income or low-resourced about how productivity requirements are addressed
patients who qualify for low-cost service and to query clinicians currently employed there
rates, providing intervention too briefly or at a about perceived ethical pressures.
frequency and intensity that is not optimal
15. Engaging Proactively in Advocacy and Legal and Ethical Concerns   207

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter and SLP services is lacking. Describe how
that are new to you or that you have not yet the principles of CBR might be applied to
mastered. expanding access to services even in high-
2. Describe what is meant by the statement that income regions where SLP programs and
communication is a human right. expertise are well developed.
3. If you have not yet done so, peruse the 9. Describe specific examples of how research
International Communication Project findings may be translated to clinical use.
website. If you have not yet done so, 10. How are morality, ethics, and law
consider signing the associated pledge. interrelated? How are the three constructs
4. As mentioned in this chapter, resources to distinguishable from one another?
help people learn about the rights of people 11. What knowledge and skills are required for
with aphasia and related disorders and their an SLP to be able to assist in determining a
care partners are readily available, yet many person’s cognitive-linguistic competence to
people are still not aware of them. What are make important life decisions? Outline an
some specific steps you might take to ensure action plan for an SLP wishing to become
that the people with neurogenic disorders expert in this area.
with whom you work, their care partners, 12. Look up examples of legal documents
and other professionals are aware of and related to the procedures listed in Box
understand their rights? 15–2. Provide specific examples of how
5. Make a list of celebrities with acquired the language in such documents may pose
neurogenic communication disorders. In special challenges for people with language
what way might it be helpful to refer to such disorders. Describe how you might support
a list in your role as advocate for awareness comprehension of such language for people
about aphasia and related disorders? with dementia or MCI.
6. Outline a plan to make your own 13. High productivity standards for billable
community a dementia-friendly and/or SLP services pose several types of ethical
aphasia-friendly community. challenges. Describe these in your own
7. Describe what is meant by medicalization words. Then list specific actions you would
of disabilities. In what ways can it be take, as a practicing SLP, to maintain your
detrimental to people with neurogenic own ethical standards and integrity in the
communication disorders? face of those challenges.
8. CBR is seen as an effective way of expanding
www
access to care in underserved regions, where See the companion website for additional learning
access to health care, rehabilitation care, and teaching materials.
CHAPTER
16
Clinical Aphasiology Around the World

In recognition of its absolute importance and rel- 2. What are important priorities for global
evance to every area of clinical practice in neuro- capacity building to serve people with acquired
genic disorders of communication and cognition, neurogenic communication disorders?
information relevant to multicultural, multilingual, 3. What are key challenges to enhancing
and international content is infused throughout this global engagement in acquired neurogenic
book. We considered global trends, transnational communication disorders?
opportunities, and varying educational and certi- 4. What are important ethical considerations for
fication requirements for aphasiologists across the aphasiologists engaging in transnational work?
globe in the introduction (Section I), issues of mul-
ticulturalism and multilingualism relative to basic
aspects of neurogenic cognitive-linguistic disorders What Global Trends Are Affecting the
(Section II), and categories of cognitive-linguistic Incidence and Prevalence of Neurogenic
disorders in aphasiology (Section III). In Chapter 15,
Communication Disorders?
we considered global and multicultural topics rele-
vant to human rights, ethics, and our roles as advo-
cates, plus variations in how health care systems Five important global trends are influencing the inci-
affect service delivery and the ways that speech-lan- dence and prevalence of neurogenic communication
guage pathologists (SLPs) are reimbursed for clinical disorders: a rapidly expanding aging population,
services. In Section V, we consider issues of multicul- ongoing demographic shifts, increasing and dispro-
turalism and multilingualism as they affect assess- portionate incidence and prevalence of conditions
ment, and in Sections VI through VIII, we consider that cause neurogenic communication disorders,
them in light of treatment principles, best practices, health care and prevention infrastructure challenges,
and methods. This chapter is dedicated to specific and global health priorities undermining essential
queries on global aspects of aphasiology that may values. Each is summarized briefly here.
help foster further important reflection and encour-
age you to learn more about these topics.
After reading and reflecting on the content in A Rapidly Expanding Aging Population
this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in
your own words, the following queries: As discussed in Chapter 9, the proportion of the
world’s population composed of older adults rel-
1. What global trends are affecting the incidence ative to younger adults and children is ever-in-
and prevalence of neurogenic communication creasing. This is especially the case in most of the
disorders? relatively lower-income and majority regions of the

209
210  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

world. Despite this, few global health programs and ties (see Chapter 15), children over adults, young
initiatives focused on health include foci on older adults over older adults, and physical challenges
adults (Van Eetvelt et al., 2020). over disabilities in cognition, communication, and
mental health.
Ongoing Demographic Shifts
What Are Important Priorities for
As highlighted in Chapter 2, the multiethnic diver-
sity of the people we serve is ever-expanding, and Global Capacity Building to Serve
global migration patterns are leading to dire needs People With Acquired Neurogenic
for clinicians who are adept at connecting with peo- Communication Disorders?
ple in culturally responsive ways.
Despite the fact that SLP intervention can lead to
Increasing and Disproportionate Incidence vital life participation outcomes, people with neu-
and Prevalence of Conditions That Cause rogenic language disorders in much of the world do
Neurogenic Communication Disorders not have SLP services available in their communities
or cannot access them. Of course, worldwide advo-
cacy efforts are needed, as we discussed in detail in
A predisposition to language disorders is culturally
Chapter 15. Here, let’s consider three categories of
and geographically contextual. Three of the most
high-priority actions that may be taken to expand
common causes of language disorders in adults are
the global capacity to meet needs for clinical ser-
most prevalent in low- and middle-income regions
vices and communication support: building cultur-
of the world: stroke, brain injury, and dementia. The
ally contextualized academic and clinical programs;
incidence of stroke and brain injury in developing
expanding life participation approaches; and attend-
regions is twice that of developed regions. Demen-
ing to cultural aspects of health, aging, and disability
tia is the greatest cause of years lost to disability in
that may affect receptivity to services.
high-income regions and second greatest worldwide.
By 2050, 71% of the expected 152 million people with
Alzheimer’s disease and other forms of dementia Build Culturally Contextualized
worldwide will be living in low- and middle-income Academic and Clinical Programs
countries (Alzheimer’s Disease International, 2021).
Worldwide recognition of the need for services to
Health Care and Prevention address communication disorders in general, and
Infrastructure Challenges acquired neurogenic communication disorders in
adults in particular, is ever-growing. Those of us
Throughout the world, where there is a lack of pre- with expertise in transnational academic and clinical
ventive and medical care, limited transportation, program development are being called on more and
poverty, poor nutrition, and low levels of health lit- more to help build capacity in our field in regions
eracy, the incidence and prevalence of acquired neu- where the field is not yet officially recognized or is
rogenic communication disorders are greater. just beginning. As such, representatives from highly
resourced regions (often White and English-speaking)
tend to serve in “expert” roles outside of their own
Global Health Priorities countries, advising cultural insiders elsewhere on
Undermining Essential Values program development. It is critical that those of
us engaged in such work take seriously the need
Global humanitarian programs and policies tend to to help foster within-region sustainable leadership
prioritize disease prevention and primary medical and expertise and not enable dependence on our
care over chronic health problems and disabili- ongoing roles there (Hallowell, 2012b; Hallowell &
16. Clinical Aphasiology Around the World   211

Hickey, 2014, 2015; Hallowell et al., 2021). It is also in press-a, in press-b; Hilari et al., 2015; Northcott
important that we take great care to appreciate the et al., 2017).
cultural factors that might affect how students learn
to become clinical experts in those contexts and how
people in their region who might benefit from their Attend to Cultural Aspects of Health,
future services would access such services (Howells Aging, and Disability That May
et al., 2016). Affect Receptivity to Services
As we consider planning for future academic
and clinical programs focused on communication At the same time that we focus on contextual infra-
disorders in adults, we must consider important structure, we must consider cultural aspects of
infrastructure factors that will affect access to SLP health care, aging, disability, and society in gen-
services, as summarized in Box 16–1. eral that may restrict access to services for people
with acquired communication disabilities once they
become more available. Availability of qualified cli-
Expand Life Participation Approaches nicians and clinical facilities does not necessarily
lead to service accessibility.
Increasing awareness and practice of life participa- Pioneers in aphasiology in underserved regions
tion approaches are vital to worldwide efforts to of the world must proactively address cultural
enhance the quality of life of people with commu- aspects of aging and disability that have a direct
nication challenges and those who care about them. impact on access to communication intervention.
Although there is clear evidence that SLPs across One important consideration is that throughout
many countries and continents care about qual- much of the world, there is a long-standing tradition
ity of life through clinical and research endeavors, of older people living as long and as independently
many acknowledge not being well informed about as possible at home. The idea of moving to a long-
or confident in practicing specific life participation term care center would be the last and least accept-
approaches (Cruice et al., 2015; Hassan & Hallowell, able alternative for many, only to be considered if

Box
16–1 Infrastructure Factors That Affect Access to SLP Services

• Appropriately educated and experienced local SLPs who are culturally


and linguistically attuned to the people they are serving (see Chapter 2)
• Equipment and culturally and linguistically appropriate materials to
support SLP services
• Inclusion of older people and people with communication challenges and
their families in our efforts to understand critical needs
• Facilities or agencies that hire SLPs and offer their services
• Government-recognized, salaried positions for SLPs
• National and local policies requiring provider accountability and quality
assurance
• Opportunities for continuing education
• Public education about communication disorders and the benefits of SLP
services
• Affordability of SLP services
• Transportation for potential clients to access SLPs
• Distance to facilities that offer SLP services
212  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

one could not live with their adult children or hire multiple needs, often with priorities geared
paid caregivers, or if one had such extensive care- toward younger family members
giving needs that they simply could not be met at
home. In many regions of the world, including much Despite these trends, cultural acceptance of
of Asia, Africa, Eastern Europe, Central America, minority-world models of care (e.g., hospitalization,
and South America, and even in regions and groups rehabilitation stays, nursing home care, and hospice
in North America, there continue to be strong cul- care) is not what many majority-world families con-
tural taboos regarding having one’s older relatives sider to be acceptable alternatives. Considerations
live in institutional environments; it is incumbent on of alternative living arrangements, health care, and
families to make sure they meet caregiving needs at rehabilitation services and means of family support
home. The notion of importing Western models of for care of adults with communication challenges
skilled nursing facilities and retirement communi- all require the utmost of cultural attunement and
ties is often rejected by those who were not raised adaptability.
in environments where such options were the norm,
regardless of where they are located.
Relatedly, while many SLPs and students pre- What Are Key Challenges to Enhancing
paring to be SLPs indicate a lack of knowledge about Global Engagement in Acquired
and comfort with addressing matters of religion and
Neurogenic Communication Disorders?
spirituality in practice (Mathisen et al., 2015), these
are fundamental components of clinical practice
about which we must constantly learn and enhance Despite wonderful efforts to expand transnational
our cultural responsiveness as we work across research, in many countries and regions, there is a
diverse cultures (Chahda et al., 2021; Mathiesen & lack of training and designated personnel with the
Threats, 2018). time and encouragement to engage in research.
At the same time, despite cultural resistance Given that the specialty of aphasiology, let alone the
to reaching outside the home and family structure field of communication sciences and disorders, does
in caring for older people, sociodemographic shifts not exist in many countries, there is frequently a lack
require that new strategies for care be considered. of guidance and mentorship for cultural insiders
Consider, for example, the following global trends: willing to engage in research. Also, many research-
ers and clinicians are simply unfamiliar with global
• rural to urban migration such that parents are efforts in research on aphasia and related disorders
left in rural areas with children working and (Hassan & Hallowell, 2021b; Lavis et al., 2010).
living in cities An additional challenge to transnational
• migration of adult children outside of their approaches to research in aphasiology is the extreme
home countries language bias of our scholarly journals (Karanth,
• full-time employment of adult children who 2000), which mirrors the English language bias in
may live in the same home as a parent but many science- and health-related areas (Amano
who are not home during the day et al., 2016; Huttner-Koros, 2015; Jackson & Kuri-
• decline of three-generation households such yama, 2019; Neimann Rasmussen & Montgomery,
that fewer people are present to assist 2018). A large majority of aphasia research studies are
• increased numbers of women working outside based on observations of English-speaking people
the home (Beveridge & Bak, 2011). The literature is not repre-
• hiring of in-home caregivers who often are sentative of the world’s languages or of its speakers.
unskilled and not proficient in the household This bias toward English poses a serious limitation
language and who do not have training to the applicability of reported results. There is a
in supporting adults with communication need for advocacy on the part of journal editors and
challenges reviewers as well as research mentors to promote
• competing priorities of time, effort, and research pertaining to non-English-speaking par-
finances within the family for meeting ticipants and research done by those who are cul-
16. Clinical Aphasiology Around the World   213

tural and linguistic insiders in non-English-speaking and the proclamation of communication as a human
regions. There is also a need for seasoned research right in the International Communication Project
aphasiologists who are proficient in English to col- (2014). Many of our national professional bodies in
laborate with and help build the research capacity of the field of communication sciences and disorders
cultural insiders. have codes of ethics, but these (or components of
them) may not be relevant or enforceable beyond
national boundaries.
What Are Important Ethical Let’s recall the distinction we drew between
Considerations for Aphasiologists cultural competence and cultural responsiveness in
the context of our delineating what constitutes an
Engaging in Transnational Work?
excellent clinician (see Chapter 2). When we interact
with people who differ from us according to one or
As discussed in Chapter 1, one of the joys of being more traits, which will certainly be the case as we
a clinical aphasiologist is the connections across cul- engage transnationally, it is vital that we continue
tures, nations, languages, and ethnicities that are to enhance our cultural awareness, humility, and
inherent in our professional organizations and in the knowledge while focusing on responsiveness, rec-
populations with which we work. Several interna- iprocity, and sustainability (Hallowell, Combiths,
tional research organizations for professionals and et al., 2021; Hyter & Salas-Provence, 2019). A helpful
researchers are dedicated to acquired neurogenic model to keep in mind is shown in Figure 16–1.
cognitive-linguistic disorders, as summarized in Entire tomes might be dedicated to ethics in
Table 2–2. Likewise, there are many country-specific global engagement for clinical aphasiologists. For
organizations for professionals and researchers spe- the sake of simplicity, let’s consider the three key
cializing in this area. Information about many of the areas of guidance among those identified by Hallow-
organizations providing support to people coping ell, Combiths, et al. (2021) and some brief examples
with aphasia and related disorders worldwide is of queries we might present to ourselves as we con-
given in Chapter 27 and summarized in Table 27–1. sider our own roles:
With an increase in globalization (interconnec-
tions and interdependencies across multiple domains • Engaging in clinical practice outside of
transcending national borders [Steger, 2013]), SLPs our home county, supervising students, or
have been engaging professionally more and more carrying out service or humanitarian projects
across national boundaries. Ideally, global engage- from our home counties in other countries
ment is “a collaborative or bilateral and sustainable • Have we ascertained that any “service”
interaction with communities in parts of the world we are providing is addressing something
different from the part of the world with which one wanted and needed by the people we are
may be most familiar” (Hyter, 2014, p. 115). Ethics supposedly “serving”?
is a construct that has the connotation of universal • Have we acknowledged overtly and
values, although different groups of people vary in addressed proactively the ways in which
terms of their consideration of what is ethical. What our presence may take up limited time and
we consider ethical depends on our world views resources of our hosts?
and the social environments in which we have lived, • Have we engaged in ample pretravel/
and it may change as we take on new viewpoints service reflective training to ensure a focus
and consider new experiences. Still, many attempts on cultural responsiveness and humility,
have been and are being made to promote ethical and anticolonial approaches to “service”?
principles globally. Consider, for example, the com- Have we learned at least key phrases in the
mon standards for all people described in the United local language of our hosts?
Nations Universal Declaration of Human Rights • Are our services sustainable? What will
(United Nations, 1948), the ethical principles for be done to follow up on and sustain any
research involving humans outlined in the Belmont positive aspects of what we have done?
Report (1974) and the Declaration of Helsinki (2013), • Are we providing assessments that identify
214  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Figure 16–1. Hyter’s model of the intersecting and interactive


aspects of cultural responsiveness. Source: Adapted from John-
son et al., 2021.

needs yet not providing a means of necessarily be effective or appropriate in


addressing those needs? If so, how might the local context of our hosts?
we reinvent our offering to be of more • Are we empowering leaders from within
meaningful and sustainable service? the cultural contexts to sustain any
• Are we avoiding any semblance of neocolo- progress where we are working rather than
nialist approaches, including potential per- encouraging dependence on our expertise
ceptions that people from minority-world and presence?
and nonminoritized groups are the saving • Engaging in research with colleagues in other
grace of those being “served”? countries
• Are we enabling students (or others) to • Are we adapting our research plans to
engage in clinically related services without fit the local culture and the nature of
credentials that would permit them to do people who will participate, through close
the work they are doing within their home collaboration with cultural insiders?
country environments? • Have we addressed cultural differences in
• Consulting, aiding in the development of ethics/institutional review board review
clinical and academic programs, teaching in of our work before we engage in data
academic programs in other countries collection with humans?
• Are our recommendations grounded in deep • Have we addressed in depth any cultural
learning about the ethnic, linguistic, and aspects of informed consent, assessment
other cultural aspects of the people who are of risks and benefits, and remuneration for
to benefit from our consulting or assistance? participation in our research?
• Are we acknowledging that how things • Are we aware of our own biases regarding
are done in our home contexts will not the relevance of the research in which we
16. Clinical Aphasiology Around the World   215

are engaging? Have we ensured means • In our collaborative work, have we ensured
of translating our research to real-world ways to be inclusive of those from diverse
applications that are likely to benefit the backgrounds who speak languages other
populations represented by participants in than our own, including any publications
our research? and professional presentations?

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter own curiosity and enjoy the corresponding
that are new to you or that you have not yet intellectual and cultural stimulation, this
mastered. constitutes “voluntourism.” What are some
2. Imagine that you are an aphasiologist from of the potential ethical hazards of clinical
an English-speaking country with well- aphasiologists engaging in voluntourism?
developed academic and clinical programs 4. Describe the features of an ideal program
in aphasiology and strongly established for engaging volunteer clinical
licensure and clinical certification standards aphasiologists to engage in meaningful,
in SLP. Imagine that you are invited to help ethical, and sustainable services in
build a clinical program to serve people with countries and cultures other than their
aphasia and related disorders in a country own.
where the field of SLP is not established 5. Even if programs to serve adults with
and in which there are no experts in clinical acquired communication challenges are
aphasiology. developed in regions that previously
a. List the primary challenges you would had no such programs, older adults with
face if you accepted such an invitation. such disabilities may still not access those
b. Describe specific steps you might programs.
recommend for building a sustainable a. List aspects of infrastructure that might
clinical program that would not affect their accessing such programs.
indefinitely rely on leadership from b. List cultural factors that might affect their
cultural outsiders like you. accessing such programs.
c. Describe features of a clinical program c. Describe how the items you listed for (a)
that you might recommend that would and (b) could be proactively addressed
differ from features of a clinical program within the cultural contexts of such
in your own local community. programs.
3. Some people love to travel to other countries 6. List steps that could be taken to reduce the
and experience new cultures. Some English-language bias of scholarly journals
health care professionals take advantage reporting research relevant to aphasia and
of opportunities to travel to new places related disorders.
to serve as volunteers, building clinical
programs or providing clinical services. Additional teaching and learning materials may
www
When people do this primarily to serve their be found on the companion website.
SECTION V

Strategic and Meaningful Assessment


218  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

In this section, we address assessment as an ongoing choose an assessment instrument based on assess-
problem-solving process that occurs in all of the con- ment goals, and we review published scales, tests,
texts and stages of rehabilitation. In Chapter 17, we and screening tools used for people with any of a
draw from several decades of work by clinical and wide variety of acquired neurogenic cognitive-lin-
research aphasiologists as we explore recommended guistic disorders. In Chapter 21, we consider dis-
best practices in assessment. We then review psy- course sampling, a means of describing important
chometric aspects of assessment and components of aspects of communication that are typically not well
assessment processes in Chapter 18. In Chapter 19, captured through published tests and instruments.
we focus on a problem-solving approach to assess- To conclude this section on strategic and meaning-
ment, emphasizing the need for critical thinking ful assessment, we review means of documenting
and process analysis as we strive to describe, clas- assessment results in Chapter 22. We also consider
sify, and interpret the nature of any individual’s how assessment results might be used to inform our
neurogenic language disorder and its life-affecting thinking about an individual’s prognosis for recov-
consequences. In Chapter 20, we consider how to ery and to guide treatment planning.
CHAPTER
17
Best Practices in Assessment

In this chapter, a strengths-based, empowering, tic than testing people and diagnosing impairments.
person-​centered approach to assessment is advo- This is why we refer to the “assessment” process as
cated. Wisdom gained from leading aphasiologists opposed to the “diagnostic” process, the latter term
over the past several decades is shared. The aim is to having the narrow connotation of labeling impair-
set the stage for a holistic approach to clinical excel- ment-level problems — a very small part of what
lence in assessment. After reading and reflecting on assessment ideally entails. In fact, assessment hap-
the content in this chapter, you will ideally be able pens constantly throughout intervention and even
to answer, in your own words, the following queries: afterward (e.g., in follow-up counseling, screening,
or group activities).
1. Where and when does assessment happen?
2. What are the purposes of assessment?
3. What aspects of assessment are truly relevant Treatment Begins the Moment Assessment Starts
to actual clinical practice?
4. What are the best practices in assessment of We need not wait to complete an assessment (as if it
acquired neurogenic language disorders? were a final product) to be able to make a difference
in helping improve communication and socialization.
From the moment we meet a person with a language
disorder, we ideally bring our healing presence and
Where and When Does Assessment Happen?
affirming attitude into their space, as Taylor (2006)
suggests. Even during initial case history interviews
Assessment is any means of evaluation. Two import- and testing, we motivate, counsel, and share infor-
ant points about assessment and treatment as inter- mation. We also model the types of communicative
woven components are that support likely to be used throughout intervention, not
just during a designated “assessment” session.
• assessment happens throughout intervention,
and
• treatment begins the moment assessment starts.
What Are the Purposes of Assessment?
Let’s briefly consider each of these points.
We most often engage in assessment so that we may

Assessment Happens Throughout Intervention • support initial and ongoing intervention


through rapport building, counseling,
We do not first assess and then treat. As noted in the motivation, information sharing, and
introduction to this section of the book, the nature of demonstrating communication enhancement
assessment is much deeper, broader, and more holis- strategies most likely to be effective;

219
220  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

• contribute to the diagnostic process, particular domain or set of domains is influenced by


determining whether a person has one or the context in which we work, and so is the amount
more communication disorders and, if so, of time that we are allotted for engaging in specific
what is the nature of each; types of assessment tasks, such as administration of
• index and describe the severity of language formal tests. Whether we are making a quick judg-
and related cognitive impairments and the ment call on the fly based on observable behavior
nature of cognitive and linguistic strengths; during a spontaneous conversation, or engaging in
• index and describe declining abilities hours of testing and methodical discourse analysis,
throughout a neurodegenerative process, the more savvy we are about best practices in assess-
or after a recent health incident, such as a ment, the more effective we will be. The ultimate
repeated stroke; excellent clinician is highly skilled and knowledge-
• index and describe the impacts of language able about many facets of assessment and constantly
and related cognitive impairments on life engages in problem-solving to achieve the best
participation, including quality of life, medical assessment outcomes.
management, and independence; Consider this comment from a clinical supervi-
• help inform prognosis; sor working in a busy rehabilitation center to a new
• inform decisions to recommend further student clinician:
assessment, treatment, discharge from
treatment, referrals to other professionals, and I don’t care if you learned anything about tests in
patient and family education and counseling; your clinical education program. I don’t care what
• plan intervention with substantial patient and theories you know. Theories don’t fix anyone.
family input, including determining long-and I just want you to get in there and figure out what’s
short-term goals, prioritizing goals, deciding going on with a person and move as quickly as
on which strengths and weaknesses to focus, possible into helping them communicate. Besides,
and selecting appropriate treatment methods we’re only allowed to bill for 15 minutes of assess-
and materials; ment in most cases anyway.
• measure, describe, and document baselines
and progress during treatment; Unfortunately, the disturbing attitude conveyed
• justify treatment to payors; by that supervisor is not uncommon in many of
• determine when a person has met goals such today’s clinical practice contexts. The ability to help
that new goals should be set, or treatment people with neurogenic communication disorders
should be discontinued; and is directly connected to a profound understanding
• collect data to be aggregated with data from of the complex nature of each person’s challenges
others to document clinical outcomes related in light of their real-life goals. Even if you rarely
to our services, or to the services within our administer published standardized tests in your
discipline, facility, rehabilitation team, and actual practice, knowing about tests and their design
so on. is essential to your sophistication as a skilled clinical
problem solver. Even if you do not typically engage
Note that many of these purposes apply to assess- in thorough discourse analysis, knowing about
ment in clinical research as well as in clinical practice. methods of assessing conversational and written
competence in meaningful communicative contexts
is paramount to your astute observations and judg-
What Aspects of Assessment Are Truly ments. Even if you adhere to a social framework for
intervention geared toward supportive discourse, if
Relevant to Actual Clinical Practice?
you do not know what underlies the problems caus-
ing barriers to social participation, the type, inten-
The degree of organization and preparation in which sity, and timing of support that you encourage may
we may engage to assess a person’s abilities in any be misguided.
17. Best Practices in Assessment   221

profession? Where do they come from? What hob-


What Are the Best Practices in Assessment of bies and interests does she have? What languages
Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders? does he speak? What are they experiencing right
now? What are their wants and needs that are within
Best practices in assessment entail a set of strong your realm of expertise to address? You are the vehi-
principles, a solid theoretical base supporting assess- cle to getting the best information you can and syn-
ment and the constructs to be assessed, knowledge thesizing it so that the best-informed decisions can
of numerous assessment methods and tools, and be made about further intervention.
adherence to pearls of wisdom that have been shared Could they be nervous or worried about what
and honed by seasoned clinical aphasiologists. These deficits you might find? Could they be unaccus-
elements of best practice are interwoven in the list tomed to formal testing situations? Consider alter-
and descriptions of guidelines provided here. native words you might say, the tone of voice you
might use, and the reassuring nonverbal signs you
might give to express the following to a person with
Do Not Underestimate How aphasia or a related disorder:
Impactful Your Role Is
• Mistakes and wrong answers are okay and
As discussed in Chapter 2, what makes one clinician even help us learn more about how we can
more excellent than another is complex, depends on help you.
the context at hand, and is difficult to define. Still, • Assessment materials are designed so that
there are many ways to continuously improve one’s most people cannot do every task perfectly.
degree of excellence. This is especially important in • A lot of other people have trouble with
the arena of assessment. particular items or tasks, too.
The person doing the assessing has a tremen- • Incorrect answers do not reflect a lack of
dous influence on assessment results. Of course, the personal worth or the quality of who you are
influence is partly shaped by the clinician’s knowl- as a person.
edge and skills. It is also partly due to the fact that • Performance on a test does not capture the
the clinician typically determines what assessment whole picture of what is relevant to your
questions are asked, what tools are used, and what communication abilities.
logistical arrangements are made for assessment
activities. There is even more to it. The nebulous Could an older person feel devalued due to
but very real influence of assessor traits related to age? Especially if you are younger than a person
affect, values, culture, and interpersonal behav- with whom you are working, consider how you
iors has been documented over several decades of might demonstrate respect and appreciation for
interdisciplinary research in the behavioral sciences their experience and wisdom, and guard against
(Björklund et al., 2011; Decker & Martino, 2013; Kel- conveying ageist stereotypes (see Chapter 9). Also
ley et al., 2014; Keren & Willemsen, 2009; Michaelson be sure to have age-appropriate materials available
et al., 1967; Rosenthal & Rosnow, 2009). as assessment stimuli.

Focus on the Person Keep the Person at the Center of the Process

Recall our discussion in Chapter 2 about being a Excellent clinicians are person-centered starting the
vehicle. The assessment process is not about you, the first time they meet a client. This means we do not
clinician. No matter how worried you are about your come with our own agenda about what assessment
preparation, competence, or knowledge, let your tools and processes will be used, how results will
focus on your own ego slide into the background be interpreted, what goals will be set, and what
as you focus on this person. Who is he? What is her treatment methods will be used. Rather, we include
222  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

the person from the start in learning about chal- replete with limiting statements. Be sure to bal-
lenges and priorities, and collaborate with the per- ance this with findings of what is possible despite
son in decision-making about goals and treatment impairments.
directions. See Figure 17–1 for an illustration of the When assessing at the impairment level, focus
contrast between clinician-centered and person-cen- on much more than deficits. Be sure to note intact
tered assessment. This approach is consistent with skills and abilities as well as challenges. For exam-
the Life Participation Approaches, and complements ple, a summary of assessment results for a woman
the Living with Aphasia: Framework for Outcome with global aphasia may include a long list of things
Measurement (A-FROM) approach for people with she cannot do. Consider these statements:
aphasia (Kagan & Simmons-Mackie, 2007). The
A-FROM is a model for treatment planning based on • She is unable to read at the single-word level.
the person’s life participation goals rather than based • She cannot identify more than 2 of 20 simple,
on tests. Although originally developed with apha- concrete everyday objects in an auditory
sia in mind, it can be applied to any disorder. For comprehension task.
example, Bourgeois and colleagues (Hickey et al., • Her verbal output is primarily in the form of
2018; Hickey & Douglas, 2021) promote a parallel one stereotypic utterance.
approach called the Flip the Rehab Model in work- • Her verbal imitation abilities are poor.
ing with people who have dementia. Meulenbroek
and Keegan (2021) provide a helpful tutorial on how What can she do? Does she have a reliable
a similar approach can support social integration for yes/no response? Is she affectionate with people
traumatic brain injury (TBI) survivors. she loves? Is she responsive to others’ touch? Does
she use meaningful intonation patterns even if her
utterances are not literally meaningful? Does she
Focus on Life Participation sometimes use appropriate gestures? Does she show
Goals From the Start responsiveness to any particular variety of sup-
ported communication?
The clinical aphasiologist is ideally a catalyst for Be sure to note strengths pertaining to life par-
improved quality of life, socialization, activity ticipation for every individual. Focus not only on
engagement, independence, medical management, barriers, isolation, and the risk of loneliness. Does
safety, and wellness. This applies to every step of a person take initiative to engage others in inter-
the intervention process, including during our first actions? Does the person join in-group activities
introduction to the client and significant others and enjoyed pre-onset? Perhaps there are even some
through all aspects of assessment. This notion is things that have improved since the onset of an
central to clinical excellence. We explore it in fur- acquired disorder, such as willingness to try new
ther detail in Chapter 23. In terms of assessment, things, and a deepened relationship and sense of
no matter how specific our evaluation is in terms closeness with a partner or adult child.
of identifying deficits at the impairment level, it is People with dementia merit special consider-
only important if it is relevant to the person we are ation of strengths. As reviewed in Chapter 13, many
serving and it can be tied to a person’s actual use of people with Alzheimer’s disease and related disor-
communication in ways that are important to them ders retain abilities to participate in conversations
in everyday life. and meaningful interaction even in the late stages
of disease progression. Tuning into performance
during reminiscence activities and tasks that tap pro-
Focus on Strengths cedural memory, for example, is vital to demonstrat-
ing cognitive-communicative strengths. So is the use
Strengths-based assessment is all the more essen- of supported communication strategies that make
tial in the deficit-focused contexts that characterize the most of intact abilities (Bayles et al., 2020; Braun
many medical environments. Assessment reports et al., 2017; Davidson et al., 2003; Hickey & Douglas,
in the arena of acquired neurological disorders are 2021; Hopper et al., 2001; Mahendra et al., 2005).
A

B
Figure 17–1. Clinician-centered (A) versus person-centered (B) assessment. The person’s priorities are
at the heart of assessment, and the clinician collaborates with the person at every stage of goal setting,
assessment, and treatment planning. Image credit: Taylor Reeves. A full-color version of this figure can be
found in the Color Insert.

223
224  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Have a Clear Purpose Be Strategic About Timing

Excellent clinicians know what they are doing for a Be thoughtful about scheduling an assessment ses-
person with communication challenges, and why, at sion. The time of day can make an important dif-
any given time. When engaging in assessment, we ference in people whose moods and energy levels
are often addressing more than one of the purposes may shift cyclically. Consider scheduling around
listed earlier. Still, we clinicians should have a clear moments when such potential confounds as fatigue,
idea of our goals and purpose in the process, and pain, hunger, or low mood may be especially prob-
these should be clearly in line with the individual’s lematic. Important considerations about timing may
own personal goals. come up within a given assessment session as well.
If too much frustration develops over difficult lan-
guage tasks during assessment, think about when it
Ensure the Best Possible is time to stop. Although some insurance companies
Assessment Conditions and health care plans require the use of some sort of
standardized test in a diagnostic session, most do
Clinicians often have little control over the types not require that the entire examination be given or
of physical space available within facilities where even that the actual scores be reported.
they work. We may be expected to carry out an
assessment session in a hospital room, a busy reha-
bilitation gym in which occupational and physical Include Others in the Process
therapists are working with others at the same time,
or in shared office space. No matter what the cir- Keep in mind how personal and environmental factors
cumstances, it is important to ensure that the room are critical contextual factors for life participation
is well lit and that distracting noise, activities, and (WHO, 2001). Language disorders affect relation-
visual clutter be minimized. Anyone in the room ships. Assessment of personal and environmen-
who may be distracting or unhelpful at any given tal factors includes assessment of these complex
moment should probably simply not be in the room. impacts, which differ according to whose relation-
It may be necessary to have a caregiver in the room ships one is considering. Consider, for example, the
for assistance with documenting case history infor- diagrams in Figure 17–2, depicting the most import-
mation, but it may be a good idea to have that per- ant relationships to a specific person with aphasia
son leave during other aspects of the assessment and to those of that person’s partner. This could
if their presence leads to distraction, interruption, apply to an adult with any type of communication
embarrassment, or stress on the part of the person disorder. We might consider either person’s most
being assessed. important relationships from the perspective of the
other. How we do this has great relevance to the way
we might assess the impact of the language disorder
Be Strategic in Setting the Location (Brumfitt, 2009; Hickey & Douglas, 2021; Holland,
2021; Khayum & Mooney, 2021; Meulenbroek & Kee-
If it is feasible, get out of a clinical space or hospi- gan, 2021; Palmer et al., 2016; Sather & Howe, 2021;
tal room. Get outside. Go to a store, the person’s Shadden, 2005; Strong & Shadden, 2021).
home, workplace, or school. Assess the individual’s
functioning in true daily communication environ-
ments. Go to whatever context might help tie in the Be Mindful of Multiple Perspectives on
carryover of strategies on which you are working. Real-Life Impacts of Communication Disability
Changing contexts is important for generalizing
treatment results and may also help boost motiva- A spouse, partner, or adult child of a person with a
tion by enhancing the relevance and applicability of language disorder often has a different perception
treatment strategies. of the severity of the language disorder, and of the
17. Best Practices in Assessment   225

A
Figure 17–2. Varied perspectives on social circles. A. The person with aphasia (or
any acquired communication disorder) is the one whose primary everyday relation-
ships are being considered. The other circles represent important people or groups
in that person’s life. Closer circles represent people seen more often. The size of the
circle represents the degree of personal importance of that person as indicated by the
person assessed. continues

life-affecting consequences of the disorder, com- Speak Directly to the Person


pared to the individuals’ own perception. In some
cases, the perception of a significant other is that People with communication disabilities often men-
the severity and impact are worse than the individ- tion that their greatest pet peeve in health care con-
ual with the language challenge thinks they are. In texts is being talked about as if they are not in the
other cases, the individual with the language disor- room. When we are collecting case history informa-
der suggests greater severity. Acknowledging these tion, it can be easy to shift attention away from a
differences in viewpoints is important. So is taking person who has trouble answering those questions,
them into account throughout intervention. focusing instead on a spouse, partner, or adult child,
226  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

B
Figure 17–2. continued  B. The importance and frequency of contact in terms of the
care partner’s perspective. Although there are several individuals and groups that are
important to both, there are differences, and the degree of importance and frequency
of contact is rarely the same for both. Source: Adapted from Shadden, 2001. Image
credit: Taylor Reeves. A full-color version of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.

for example, to seek information most efficiently. der. Foster a sense that their own role is essential to
Efficiency is not usually as important as the qual- the process.
ity of our connection to the people we are serving.
Making eye contact and directly addressing a person
with a language disorder, using whatever adaptive Collaborate
communication techniques will help exchange infor-
mation, and allowing plenty of time for process- Assessment is a collaborative process with the indi-
ing, are vital to empowering that person as the key vidual assessed and anyone else involved in their
player in their intervention. In cases where it is nec- care. Many aspects of assessment require and benefit
essary to ask questions of or address someone else from input from other professionals with whom we
in the room, it is important to keep drawing in the work. As is apparent when we consider assessment
participation of the person with a language disor- problem-solving challenges in Chapter 19, drawing
17. Best Practices in Assessment   227

on input from neuropsychologists, social workers, ple without any neurological impairment; still, there
nurses, physicians, occupational therapists, physical is likely to be overlap between the distributions of
therapists, audiologists, and dieticians, among oth- scores for the two groups (Richardson et al., 2021;
ers, is key. Ross & Wertz, 2003, 2004).
Being knowledgeable about the scope of prac-
tice of other disciplines and showing respect for
real and perceived boundaries may help ensure Attend to Cultural and Linguistic Differences
sustainable and effective working relationships. For
example, in some contexts, neuropsychologists are Attending to cultural and linguistic differences
considered the designated experts in assessment of when assessing people with neurogenic communi-
cognitive constructs (e.g., memory, attention, visu- cation disorders is a topic worthy of an entire tome.
ospatial perception), and speech-language patholo- In fact, there are several texts focused on multicul-
gists are not encouraged — and sometimes are even tural approaches to intervention in adults with neu-
disallowed — to administer corresponding tests. rogenic communication disorders. Readers of this
Occupational therapists and physical therapists, book are encouraged to continue study in this area
too, have important areas of overlapping expertise throughout their professional lives. Here we review
with clinical aphasiologists. For example, all three of important strategies to address multicultural aspects
these disciplines have scope of practice components of every type of assessment.
related to executive functions, expressive modalities,
and information processing. Recognize That Our Multicultural
Strengths Are Always Limited
Appreciate That Experts, Not Tests, Of course, it is important that we engage in lifelong
Are What Determine Diagnoses learning about other cultures and languages so that
we may be sensitive, thoughtful, and strategic in
Tests do not diagnose language disorders; clinicians working with people who differ from us culturally
do. Tests are tools for helping to better understand and linguistically. We probably all know that we
the nature of a person’s strengths and weaknesses. should, for example, pay close attention to prag-
They are not clinical decision makers; expert clini- matic differences across cultures in terms of our use
cians are. If you administer an aphasia battery to of body language, expression of affection, and use
people with no congenital cognitive disabilities and of formal titles versus first names. Still, no matter
no acquired neurogenic conditions, your results will how sophisticated we may be about multicultural
likely indicate that they have aphasia. You could aspects of health, well-being, language, and human
even classify them in terms of type and severity of interaction, it is important that we recognize our lim-
aphasia. The same could happen if you administered itations in terms of appreciating cultural and linguis-
an aphasia battery in English to a person with lim- tic differences that may influence our assessments
ited English proficiency. Your results would be mere (Björklund et al., 2011; Hallowell, Combiths, et al.,
bunk. Certainly, you would not do this. 2021; Hyter, 2021; Hickey & Douglas, 2021; Ivanova
Using test scores to diagnose neurogenic lan- & Hallowell, 2013; Pascoe et al., 2018; Teoh et al.,
guage disorders is misguided for another important 2018; Threats, 2005, 2010a).
reason. People who do not have the condition for
which you are assessing sometimes do worse than Index Language Proficiency Across
the people who do have it. Consider, for example, All Languages Spoken
the fact that people without aphasia do not necessar-
ily perform without errors on aphasia tests. People It is ideal if a clinician who is proficient in each lan-
with aphasia may score within “normal” ranges on guage the individual spoke prior to the onset of a
an aphasia battery yet clearly have what, by defini- disorder directly assesses the individual’s commu-
tion, is aphasia. The mean aphasia battery score of a nicative abilities. However, this is rarely the case. It
group of people with aphasia is, of course, signifi- is often helpful to incorporate an interpreter; how-
cantly lower than the mean score of a group of peo- ever, this is not always feasible. Also, the role and
228  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

actions of interpreters are important considerations Hallowell (2013) provide a table of aphasia tests in
in their own right; for example, it is especially chal- 20 of the world’s most commonly spoken languages,
lenging to work with an interpreter who is not will- along with information about reported normative
ing or able to compartmentalize their own opinions sampling and references for each. Clinicians assess-
and ideas during the process. Computer and phone ing people who speak languages in which tests are
apps for translation and interpretation are available unavailable are challenged with having to depend
to provide spoken and written output in many lan- on qualitative descriptions and observations, or
guages, and their capabilities are readily improv- translated versions of existing tests.
ing. Of course, they are unlikely ever to replace the Given differences in phonology, morphology,
strengths of actual multilingual speakers in terms semantics, syntax, and pragmatics across any two
of the flexibility and creativity required for effective languages, literal translations of any given test are
interpersonal communication. never appropriate (Bates et al., 1991; Pauranik et al.,
2019). Also, psycholinguistic controls implemented
Use Dynamic and Standardized Methods in testing stimuli developed in one language are typ-
ically not applicable in a different language. Exam-
A mixture of standardized testing and flexibly ples of important aspects of control that would not
adapted assessment is recommended regardless of necessarily apply once translated into a different lan-
who is being assessed. When working with a person guage include the following:
whose cultural background is different from yours
and different from the background of those included • phonetic complexity and articulatory
in test standardization, this is especially critical. difficulty of words to be spoken
Because this is an issue for all people, regardless of • graphemic complexity of words to be written
culture, we address it further later. • morphological length and complexity
• word frequency, familiarity, and associated
Consider Potential Cultural Impacts age of acquisition
• syntactic complexity
on Assessment Results
• specific grammatical structures (e.g., articles
The very act of testing may be unfamiliar to some and inflections, verb tense and mood, and
people we assess. Medical or clinical environments noun case and gender)
may be unfamiliar and even alienating to some. • verbal stimulus length
Further, the situation of having a clinician who is in • cultural relevance of visual and linguistic
charge probe a person through questions and tasks stimuli and tasks
with which that person has difficulty results in an
imbalance of power and, for some, perceived respect. Even when a test is translated well and appro-
When clinicians are younger than the people they are priately adapted to as many of these factors as pos-
assessing, this may exacerbate such imbalances. A sible, it is still important that it be standardized
sort of teacher-student or boss-underling role estab- anew in the target language. Norms and patterns
lishment between clinician and patient is inherently of performance for people with similar types and
demeaning to many. We clinicians cannot know with severity levels of language impairments are not
certainty just how we are being perceived in terms equivalent across languages into which tests have
of the cultural acceptance and respect we intend to been translated (Bates et al., 1991; Ivanova & Hal-
convey. In any case, it is important to consider how lowell, 2013).
such factors might influence assessment results.
Promote Acceptance Regardless of
Attend to Special Needs of Multilingual Speakers Sexual Orientation, Gender Identity,
and Gender Expression
The number of tests for aphasia and related disor-
ders is limited in most of the world’s languages, even Throughout this book, we noted the importance of
among those spoken most commonly. Ivanova and cultural responsiveness and humility in our work as
17. Best Practices in Assessment   229

it pertains to working with people who differ accord- Recognize That There Is Always a
ing to language, age, socioeconomic status, ethnicity, Chance of Measurement Error
and the social construct of race. Ensuring a context
that welcomes diversity in terms of sexual orienta- Measurement error is a reality. No matter how clever
tion, gender identity, and gender expression is also we are as diagnosticians, how phenomenal our
paramount. There are ample challenges that people assessment tools, and how hard we try to achieve
who are lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, or queer the most valid results possible, there is always a
(see the Human Rights Campaign [n.d.] Glossary of chance that we will be mistaken when we draw
Terms, for a discussion on nuances related to such conclusions from results. We may misunderstand
terms) face when navigating health care systems; the nature of a problem. We may misdiagnose a
these go beyond the myriad challenges already faced condition. We may underestimate or overestimate
when coping with life-altering aspects of acquired severity. Recognizing the possibility of question-
neurological disorders (Adler et al., 2018; Hancock able results and wrong conclusions is part of clinical
& Haskin, 2015). Avoid use of heterosexist language. excellence.
Do not ask questions that convey an assumption A strong body of research in the area of judg-
about the gender of a person’s partner. Do not ask ment and decision-making underscores that the
whether a patient has a “spouse.” Although legal degree of confidence that experts in a variety of dis-
marriages of same-sex couples are on the rise in most ciplines have about the professional decisions they
Western countries, they are not the norm in much of make has little relationship to the accuracy of their
the world; questions about marital status are usu- actual decisions. Also, the confidence that clinicians
ally not appropriate. Heterosexist language is often have about their knowledge and skills is often unre-
used unintentionally, merely out of ignorance, yet lated to their actual knowledge and skills. Recogniz-
conveys judgment and discrimination. It is import- ing these facts, and accepting what we do not know,
ant that we clinical aphasiologists play an active makes us more honest clinicians, helps us remain
advocacy role in this area, perhaps nudging other open to lifelong learning, and makes it more likely
colleagues to revise the wording they use, and per- that we will make valid conclusions.
haps editing clinical forms and records that include
heterosexist and cisgender-centric language.
Embrace Assessment as a Research Process
Adopt a Process Analysis Approach
There is much to be gained by thinking of the assess-
A process analysis approach to assessment entails ment of any individual’s communication abilities as
the following: a research project. Research consists of methodically
formulating questions and hypotheses, designing a
• considering all possible skills, abilities, method to test the hypotheses and answer the ques-
predispositions, and aptitudes that a person tions, collecting data, analyzing the data, interpret-
must have to carry out a particular assessment ing the data in light of the questions and hypotheses,
task and reflecting on and planning for the possible next
• considering which of those directly pertain steps. The next steps might include contributing to
to the construct under study and which do any of the assessment goals listed earlier as purposes
not, and determining the potential impact of of assessment or perhaps generating new questions
confounding factors on our conclusions (see and hypotheses to be addressed in a continuing
Chapter 18) assessment process. See Figure 17–3 for examples of
• acknowledging any uncertainty we may how assessment activities may be conceived within a
have about the validity of results in light of research process. Given that we typically have many
inherent and potentially confounding factors hypotheses we aim to test and questions we aim to
answer in any given assessment process, we might
Examples and illustrations of this approach are actually view the process as several research projects
explored further in Chapter 19. or experiments combined.
Research process steps Examples of research steps during assessment

Quesons: Does he have aphasia? Would she be likely to benefit from a treatment program?
Formulate Hypotheses: Her aphasia is impacng her ability to manage her finances independently. He is having
quesons and severe difficulty with reading for leisure. She has a moderate word-finding deficit in spontaneous
hypotheses conversaon.

Design a Select tests, quesonnaires, screening instruments, etc. Plan for ming and scheduling of significant
method others, organize materials and supplies. Address funding issues.

Collect Carry out the method as planned, adapng as necessary to the individual’s needs and responses
data
throughout the process.
230  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Analyze Analyze quantave and qualitave results from standardized tesng, screenings, interviews, and
data observaons.

Interpret Use results to address hypotheses and answer quesons. Consider shortcomings of the approach taken
results and what might be done to remedy those. Recognize addional informaon needed.

Reflect on
and plan
for next Make recommendaons for treatment, discharge, paent and family educaon and support, referrals to
steps other professionals, etc. Document addional assessment needs.

Figure 17–3. Assessment activities corresponding to steps in the research process. A full-color version of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.
17. Best Practices in Assessment   231

Balance Dynamic Nonstandardized familiar to less familiar content to gauge


With Standardized Assessment differences in the quality of interaction
• continuing to converse in depth in some
Dynamic assessments are those that allow tailoring content areas to observe discourse cohesion
of assessment materials to the interests, ability level, • trying out varied types of cueing to elicit
and cultural and linguistic background of the person responses
being assessed. Standardized assessments are those • inviting varied levels of partner support
that
Shortcomings of dynamic assessment are that it
• have normative data, ideally collected
from people without any neurological • does not enable comparison of the individual
disorders and from people with the disorder tested to a normative sample,
corresponding to the assessment tool’s target • does not ensure consistency or comparable
clinical population, enabling comparisons of types or quality of information gained from
individual results to group results; and one session to the next,
• entail explicit instructions for test • is less likely than standardized assessment
administration and scoring. to entail methodical assessment of simple to
complex levels in a prespecified array of tasks
Ideally, standardized tests include directions for and language domains.
how many and what types of cues can be given, how
many times any item may be repeated, and ceiling/ Shortcomings of standardized testing are that cur-
floor rules. A ceiling rule indicates how many times rently available published assessment tools tend to
a person may get consecutive items or items within
a subtest wrong before the test administrator stops • fail to meet a wide range of criteria for test
or moves on to the next section or subtest. A floor design, psychometric properties, and means
rule (also called a basal rule) indicates when certain of controlling for potentially confounding
items or groups of items may be skipped because the factors;
test taker gets so many correct that those items are • fail to provide an account of the underlying
apparently too easy. neuropsychological nature of and reasons for
Dynamic assessments allow great flexibility in the deficits indexed;
terms of how much any particular domain is sam- • have an impairment-level focus, limiting our
pled, what types of cues are given, and how relevant ability to index treatment outcomes related to
the type of ability being assessed is to an individual’s social participation and quality of life across a
life participation and quality of life. Discourse is an wide range of individuals;
essential component of dynamic assessment, as dis- • fail to lead to clear suggestions for appropriate
cussed in greater detail in Chapter 21. It is important treatment approaches; and
to observe varied communication strategies and their • be deficient in indexing meaningful changes
relative effectiveness across a variety of discourse in language recovery over time.
tasks. Examples of discourse manipulation that may
yield important information include the following: Given that clinical aphasiology has a much
longer history of focusing on what the Interna-
• varying discourse genres (conversational, tional Classification of Functioning, Disability, and
persuasive, descriptive, expository, Health (ICF) refers to as body structure and function
procedural, narrative) than on activity and participation, there are far more
• varying grammatical complexity of statements established published assessment protocols that
and questions to note differential impacts on entail testing at the level of body functions. Most
comprehension published tests for assessing people with aphasia
• drawing on content from the distant past and language impairments associated with stroke,
versus the immediate past and from highly TBI, and dementia are focused on specific aspects
232  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

of impairment, such as reading, writing, word find- for that balance is an important task of the excellent
ing, auditory comprehension, working memory, clinical aphasiologist.
and attention. These are certainly important areas
to focus on in terms of identifying potential body- Integrate Criterion-Referenced Measures
function-level areas to target in treatment. They are
not, however, necessarily meaningful without con- Criterion-referenced measures (also called domain-​
sidering an individual’s vast array of personal and referenced measures) are indices used to gauge a
environmental factors. person’s own ability without direct comparison to
Knowing just the severity of a given person’s others. This is in contrast to norm-referenced mea-
post-onset reading deficits, for example, is not nec- sures (indices compared to a sample of a popula-
essarily relevant to a rehabilitation program. To tion with similar traits). In clinical practice, we use
interpret reading assessment scores and apply them criterion-referenced measures in several practical
in intervention, we must also know how important ways. We use them when we establish a person’s
reading is to that person, what about their reading initial performance abilities on a certain task, such
activities has been lost, what they need and want to as object naming or comprehension of sentences
read on a regular basis, and what reading topics are having a certain grammatical construction, and
most interesting and relevant to them. We explore then measure changes from that baseline during the
the factors that guide the selection of an assessment course of treatment. We also use them to determine
tool despite its possible weaknesses in Chapter 20. whether a person might be appropriate for a given
The distinction between standardized versus treatment. For example, if a given person is able to
nonstandardized testing by definition is clear in that demonstrate accurate reading comprehension at a
one either administers a test in a way that meets the certain prespecified level, we may consider them
psychometric criteria of a standardized test or one eligible for a book discussion group for stroke and
does not. Nonstandardized testing is considered brain injury survivors. Researchers often similarly
“dynamic” because it is altered based on the needs use criterion-referenced measures to determine
and desires of the clinician and the person being whether a person qualifies to participate in a study.
assessed. In actuality, the distinction between stan- For example, depending on the nature of a study,
dardized and dynamic assessment is often blurred, we may say that a person must be able to accurately
as some clinicians use published standardized tests read aloud a certain set of sentences or to correctly
in a dynamic way. Examples include the following: name a certain percentage of the verbs depicted in a
series of action scenes.
• altering the type of cues and feedback given Further, we use criterion-referenced measures
and the kinds of responses interpreted as to determine when a certain treatment goal has been
“correct”; selecting only certain subtests or met. Consider, for example, a TBI survivor who does
items within subtests to administer not self-monitor or control their tendency to say off-
• altering the content of verbal or visual stimuli topic comments during spontaneous conversation.
to adjust for cultural and linguistic differences We may have a goal to help them work toward
• using materials from a standardized test that topic maintenance or discourse cohesion and oper-
has not been normed on people with the ationalize their target for meeting that goal as inde-
same type of communication impairment or pendently demonstrating on-topic comments 80%
etiology as the person being assessed of the time during spontaneous conversation with
their partner.
A key point in comparing and contrasting stan- Sometimes tasks and stimuli from standardized
dardized and nonstandardized assessment methods tests are used in a nonstandardized way when deter-
is not to determine which approach is better but to mining criterion-referenced performance. We might
recognize that the effectiveness, validity, and qual- index information units produced when a person
ity of assessment for a given person are often based describes a picture from the Western Aphasia Bat-
on an appropriate balance between the two. Striving tery–Revised (WAB-R; Kertesz, 2007), the number of
17. Best Practices in Assessment   233

commands followed accurately on one of the sub- The list of best practices for assessment is sum-
tests of the Revised Token Test (McNeil & Prescott, marized in Box 17–1. Of course, reading about and
1978), or the number of phone numbers identified studying best practices is not sufficient. We must
in a minute during a phone book search task on the engage holistically in ongoing efforts to become the
Test of Everyday Attention (Ridgeway et al., 1994). most excellent aphasiologists we can be.

Box
17–1 Summary of Best Practices for Assessment

• Do not underestimate how impactful your • Index language proficiency across all
role is. languages spoken.
• Focus on the person. • Use dynamic and standardized methods.
• Focus on life participation goals from the start. • Consider potential cultural impacts on
• Focus on strengths. assessment results.
• Have a clear purpose. • Promote acceptance regardless of
• Ensure the best possible assessment conditions. sexual orientation, race, religion, and
• Be strategic in setting the location. socioeconomic status.
• Be strategic about timing. • Attend to special needs of multilingual
• Include others in the process. speakers.
• Be mindful of multiple perspectives on real- • Adopt a process analysis approach.
life impacts of communication disability. • Recognize that there is always a chance of
• Speak directly to the person. measurement error.
• Collaborate. • Embrace assessment as a research process.
• Appreciate that experts, not tests, are what • Consider the appropriate balance of
determine diagnoses. dynamic nonstandardized and standardized
• Attend to cultural and linguistic differences. assessment.
• Recognize that our multicultural strengths • Integrate criterion-referenced measures.
are always limited.

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter 5. How might knowing about standardized
that are new to you or that you have not yet tests be helpful in clinical practice even
mastered. when there are no published tests available
2. What are some ways in which treatment or when there is insufficient time allotted for
might be integrated into the assessment test administration?
process? 6. In this chapter, it was noted that the person
3. What are some ways in which assessment carrying out assessment has a tremendous
might be integrated into the treatment influence on assessment results. Describe at
process? least three examples of how this could be
4. How is the ICF framework relevant to the case.
life participation goals in the context of 7. Recall a situation (personal, academic, or
assessment? professional) in which you found it difficult
234  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

to let go of your own ego and self-awareness work to ensure an accepting and welcoming
to simply be present in terms of doing your atmosphere for people with diverse sexual
best work. orientations?
a. How might you have handled yourself 16. Describe the limitations of each of the
differently in that situation if you had strategies below, used to help reduce bias
not been so worried about your own when evaluating people from culturally and
performance and instead focused more ethnically diverse populations.
on the mission of what you were doing? a. Translate existing tests for speakers of
b. How might you prepare yourself to be a other languages.
vehicle for empowerment and strength b. Modify existing standardized tests to
finding in assessment contexts? make them appropriate for clients from
8. Recall a situation in which you were other cultures.
assessed and felt that your weaknesses were c. Use tests that include a small percentage
emphasized far more than your strengths. of minorities in the standardization
a. Was the assessment helpful? Why or why sample when developing tests.
not? d. Standardize existing tests on minority
b. What could the assessor have done to populations.
help you feel more empowered to do e. Use a language sample and naturalistic
something to address your weaknesses? observations.
c. What might you do as a clinician to f. Use criterion-referenced measures.
ensure a strengths-based focus in your 17. What strategies will you use to demonstrate
assessment practices? your recognition of the fact that there is
9. How might location and timing affect the always a chance of measurement error in
reliability and validity of assessment results? your clinical assessments?
10. How would tuning into varied social circles 18. Describe how an excellent clinical
of a person with a communication disorder aphasiologist can adopt a process analysis
affect your treatment and discharge planning? approach during the course of assessment.
11. Describe a situation in which 19. Describe how the assessment process
interprofessional collaboration would be with each individual you assess could be
essential for assessing the nature of person’s conceived as a research process.
language disorder. 20. Summarize the relative advantages and
12. Why can test scores alone not be used to weaknesses of standardized and dynamic
determine the diagnosis of a language (nonstandardized) assessment.
problem? 21. Describe how the distinction between
13. List ways in which cultural and linguistic standardized and dynamic assessment can
differences may be addressed during be blurry.
assessment. For each, describe strengths and 22. What are potential advantages and
weaknesses. disadvantages for using norm-referenced
14. What are the pros and cons of having an versus criterion-referenced scores in
interpreter assist in testing a person with a the assessment of neurogenic language
language disorder whose language you do disorders?
not speak?
www
15. What are some specific strategies you would See the companion website for additional learning
recommend in a clinical agency where you and teaching materials.
CHAPTER
18
Psychometrics of Assessment and
Components of Assessment Processes

In this chapter, we review psychometric properties of Reliability refers to the consistency with which
assessment methods and tools. We also summarize something is measured or evaluated. Four types of
the basic elements of diagnostic tests and processes. reliability are particularly relevant to clinical apha-
Practical examples of screening and case history siology: test-retest reliability, inter-examiner reliabil-
items are provided. After reading and reflecting on ity, intra-examiner reliability, and internal reliability.
the content in this chapter, you will ideally be able Test-retest reliability is the consistency with which
to answer, in your own words, the following queries: the same result is achieved when a test is admin-
istered at two different times. Several factors may
1. What psychometric properties should be influence this in people with neurological disor-
addressed in assessment processes? ders. If we give the same test twice within a brief
2. What are potentially confounding factors? period of time, they may do better the second time
3. What is entailed in screening for acquired because they have recently had a chance to practice
neurogenic language disorders? and remember stimuli and answers when taking
4. What are the typical components of a the test the first time. If we allow a longer period of
comprehensive assessment process? time until we assess them again, they may actually
5. What information is pertinent to collect during improve due to spontaneous recovery, practice, or
the case history? intervention; in that case, it is the person’s ability
that is inconsistent, not the test’s ability to capture
similar results from one time to the next.
What Psychometric Properties Should Be Inter-examiner reliability is the consistency
of results obtained by two different assessors. The
Addressed in Assessment Processes?
two examiners may score the same administration
of a test (both being in the room at the same time or
For readers who already have ample sophistica- using a video recording of the same administration).
tion in assessment, the definition and relevance of Alternatively, they may each administer the test on
psychometric constructs may seem obvious. Still, separate occasions to the same individual. In the lat-
clinicians who may have mastered these constructs ter case, the reliability indexes the degree to which
during their academic studies do not always use the individual test administrator and the nature of
them correctly in clinical practice. For example, the their interaction with the test taker influences the
constructs of reliability and validity are often errone- results. Intra-examiner reliability refers to the con-
ously used interchangeably. sistency with which an individual assessor gets the
It is a good idea even for seasoned clinical apha- same assessment results. Using high-quality video-​
siologists to brush up periodically on psychometric recorded assessments helps to rule out the influ-
terminology. ence of test-retest inconsistencies on intra-examiner

235
236  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

reliability. Internal reliability (also called internal ative naming or verbal fluency in a person without
consistency) refers to the consistency with which a motor speech disorder; however, having apraxia
results are obtained across items or components of of speech could certainly negatively affect the index
items within a test. One type of internal validity without reflecting anything about their language for-
sometimes reported is split-half reliability; this is mulation ability. To achieve high-content validity, we
the degree to which half of the items on a test yield must operationalize exactly what we mean to index
consistent results compared to the other half. Of and then ensure that our assessment addresses all of
course, for a test that is designed to index perfor- the components of what we have operationalized.
mance across varied difficulty levels in any given Construct validity is the ability of a means of
domain, one would not expect results for easy items assessment to capture what it is intended to assess.
on the test to correlate highly with results for diffi- It can be quantified by measuring its degree of agree-
cult items. At times, when a smaller number of items ment with other measures of the same construct (i.e.,
on a test yield similar results to the test as a whole, through convergent validity indices). It can also be
shortened forms of the test may be recommended. quantified by measuring its lack of relationship with
However, it matters a great deal which items have measures of constructs that differ from the target
been shown to lead to similar results as the test in construct (i.e., though discriminant validity).
its entirety. Ivanova and Hallowell (2013) provide Criterion validity reflects how predictive per-
detailed guidance for the calculation of varied met- formance on a measure is of a certain outcome. It
rics of internal reliability on adult language tests. may be measured by calculating the correlation
Validity refers to the degree to which what between scores from one test with scores on another
one intends or purports to measure really is what that is intended to assess a similar construct (con-
is being measured. Validity is not inherent in a test, current validity). It may also be measured by calcu-
test item, or task. Rather, it is the inferences we make lating the correlation between test results and future
based on performance we observe, or our interpre- performance in a relevant area (predictive valid-
tations of test results, that can be judged in terms of ity). For example, a great deal of attention is paid
their validity. Some argue that validity is a singular to word-finding abilities through lexical-semantic
construct without distinct subtypes (Strauss et al., approaches to treatment in people with anomia. It is
2006; Wolfe & Smith, 2007). Even if that is the case, logical that an index of the number of well-controlled
one may analyze the construct in varied ways. Any objects or pictures a person names correctly is a valid
test, test item, or task may be judged in terms of its index of her naming ability. However, it may not be
content, construct, criterion, and face validity. a valid predictor of how accurately or how often she
Content validity is the degree to which the uses those same words in spontaneous conversation.
items on a test or scale tap into the construct to be Very few tests in the realm of clinical aphasiology
assessed. Nicholas et al. (1986) provided an illus- have been studied in terms of their predictive valid-
tration of challenges to content validity in testing ity. In their review of 31 instruments used to index
people with and without aphasia. They presented varied aspects of cognitive-communicative abilities
questions taken from aphasia tests meant to assess in traumatic brain injury (TBI) survivors, Turkstra et
reading ability but without first showing test takers al. (2005) report that only four had been evaluated
the corresponding reading passages. Both groups of according to whether they predicted abilities outside
participants were able to answer many of the ques- of clinical settings.
tions accurately. Thus, many of the reading test items Criterion validity may also be evaluated in
were tapping into general knowledge and reason- terms of how well a test distinguishes between two
ing, not necessarily reading comprehension. An area groups of individuals. It is said to have good sensi-
of frequent challenge to content validity is the use tivity if scores generated are likely to help identify
of speaking tasks to index language formulation in the impairment you are testing if there really is one
people who have concomitant language and motor (avoiding false negatives) and good specificity if the
speech problems. An index of the number of words a scores are likely to help rule out impairment if there
person can say in a minute when given a certain cat- is not one (avoiding false positives).
egory (e.g., types of transportation, words that begin Face validity is the degree to which a test or
with the letter t) may be a good indicator of gener- measure is judged by others to be valid. For exam-
18. Psychometrics of Assessment and Components of Assessment Processes   237

ple, an aphasiologist applying for a competitive disorders). Because they are so important for clinical
research grant may wish to select a test that is well aphasiologists to know about and address proactively,
respected by senior scientists studying in a simi- these are discussed separately in our exploration of
lar area. A speech-language pathologist (SLP) may how to screen for and take into account concomitant
value the face validity of a test in terms of the cred- challenges to health and well-being during language
ibility it has among their clinical supervisors or the assessment, in Chapter 19.
respect it was accorded by professors during their
formal clinical education. Ecological validity is the
degree to which a test, or any specific stimulus or set Test Design Factors
of stimuli within a test, represents actual real-world
types of stimuli that would be encountered in the There are ample ways in which aspects of the test
everyday life of the person being tested. For exam- could influence results in ways that are not directly
ple, for a picture-naming task, consider the lack of related to the language abilities of the person being
ecological validity for an image of a sled for a person assessed. Factors that are important to control for
in a tropical region, a Christmas tree for a person in in test design are listed in Table 18–1 in relation to
a country where Christian holidays are not widely constructs typically assessed. Any one of these fac-
celebrated, or a popular singer from the 1950s for a tors not well controlled for could be considered a
person born in the 2000s. potentially confounding factor. For example, if lexi-
cal stimuli are not developed with careful attention
to psycholinguistic factors that are known to influ-
ence word difficulty, such as word frequency, word
What Are Potentially Confounding Factors?
familiarity, age of acquisition, concept imageability,
level of abstractness/concreteness, and grammatical
A confounding factor in assessment is any char- class, then any one of these factors or a combination
acteristic of a person’s abilities; any aspect of the of them could influence the validity of conclusions
assessment tools, procedures, and processes that drawn about the test taker’s lexical abilities.
we use; and any aspect of the testing context or sit-
uation in the assessment context that could lead to
invalid results. Let’s consider four broad categories Assessment Context Factors
of potential confounds in the assessment of people
with neurogenic language disorders: factors related Additional potentially confounding factors are related
to concomitant challenges to health and well-being, to the assessment context. Poorly lit or noisy areas
test design factors, testing context factors, and inter- may influence a person’s ability to respond well, and
personal factors. so may visual clutter and the presence of other people.

Factors Related to Concomitant Interpersonal Factors


Challenges to Health and Well-Being
Another category of potentially confounding fac-
A large and important set of potential confounding tors relates to the relationship between the examiner
factors relates to the myriad impairments and condi- and the person being tested. If the examiner does
tions that people with neurogenic language disorders not speak the test taker’s language at a native-like
may have that can affect their linguistic performance. level or has a strong nonnative accent, this could
Key categories of such sources of potential confounds certainly affect the validity of assessment. Distract-
are visual problems, hearing problems, apraxia of ing violations of politeness as perceived by the test
speech, dysarthria, limb apraxia, paralysis or paresis, taker could also affect validity; this might result, for
constructional apraxia, intellectual and learning dis- example, when a male patient with strict beliefs that
abilities, attention deficits, working memory deficits, a woman should not touch him is touched repeat-
concomitant language deficits, and other challenges edly by a female clinician who believes that being
to health and well-being (e.g., metabolic and mood affectionate with her clients helps build rapport.
Table 18–1. Cognitive-Linguistic Functions, Corresponding Test Item Types, and Potentially
Confounding Factors

Additional
Cognitive-Linguistic Potentially Confounding Potentially
Functions and Associated Cognitive-Linguistic Confounding
Abilities and Deficits Assessment Item Types Factors Factors
Receptive Language
Auditory processing and • Lexical decision (word • Word frequency These apply to
comprehension: versus non-word • Word familiarity all aspects of
• Phonological processing discrimination) assessment:
• Noun case and gender
• Word recognition • Selection of a • Clinician
• Age of acquisition
multiple-choice image expertise
• Word comprehension • Imageability
corresponding to a
• Grammatical processing • Clinician-client
verbal stimulus • Concreteness/
and comprehension; rapport
• Commands abstractness
receptive agrammatism • Arousal,
• Yes/no questions • Word, phrase, sentence
• Discourse comprehension alertness,
length
• True/false questions/ attention
• Phonemic complexity
• Statements • Concomitant
• Grammatical complexity cognitive,
• Questions following
(e.g., semantically linguistic,
a story (complex
constrained versus sensory, and
ideational material)
not; canonicity; clausal motor problems
• Story retell tasks types, verb tense,
• Pre-onset
• Spontaneous mood)
intelligence,
conversation • Density of propositions education, and
• Metaphor • Plausibility of content abilities in all
interpretation areas assessed
• Audibility, clarity, and
• Semantic and rate of spoken language • Pre-onset
phonological priming proficiency in
• Discourse genre
language of
Reading comprehension: • Matching cases/script/ • Script, font assessment
• Reading ability, dyslexia numbers • Word frequency • Literacy
• Copying letters, words, • Word familiarity • Time
phrases, sentences
• Noun case and gender • Pain
• Orthographic lexical
• Age of acquisition • Overall health
decision
• Imageability • Depression,
• Reading aloud
• Concreteness/ mood
• Word/sentence/
abstractness • Self-esteem
paragraph reading
with picture matching • Word, phrase, sentence • Shyness
length • Test anxiety
• Paragraph/
text reading with • Phonemic composition • Fear of stigma
comprehension and articulatory difficulty
• Sociocultural
questions • Grammatical complexity factors
• Metaphor • Density of propositions • Motivation
interpretation • Plausibility of content • Locus of control

238
Table 18–1. continued

Additional
Cognitive-Linguistic Potentially Confounding Potentially
Functions and Associated Cognitive-Linguistic Confounding
Abilities and Deficits Assessment Item Types Factors Factors
Expressive Language
Repetition: • Repetition of • Phonetic/phonemic • Desire to deceive,
• Repetition phonemes, words composition and malinger
(nonsense words, articulatory difficulty of • Location,
• Perseveration
single words, series stimulus to be repeated context,
of words), phrases, • Word, phrase, sentence ambiance
sentences length • Presence of
• Grammatical complexity others in the
• Grammatical and room
semantic • Ecological
• Plausibility validity, personal
relevance of
• Audibility, clarity, and
constructs
rate
assessed and of
Automatic speech: • Recitation of • Articulatory difficulty stimulus /topic
• Rote, highly learned speech automatic sequences • Familiarity of rote content
(numbers, days of the sequences
• Perseveration
week, months)
• Recitation of nursery
rhymes, poems,
songs
• Spontaneous
automatic utterances
during conversation
Naming: • Confrontation naming • Word frequency
• Word retrieval, dysnomia • Word descriptions/ • Word familiarity
• Paraphasias (literal/ definitions requiring • Age of acquisition
phonemic, semantic/global/ naming response
• Imageability
verbal) • Cloze sentences or
• Concreteness/
• Perseveration phrases
abstractness
• Circumlocution • Word (verbal) fluency
• Phonemic composition
tasks
• Stereotypy and articulatory difficulty
• Word length
• Semantic category
• Visual/tactile stimulation
• Real objects versus
images
• Degree of control
of physical stimulus
properties of images

continues

239
Table 18–1. continued

Additional
Cognitive-Linguistic Potentially Confounding Potentially
Functions and Associated Cognitive-Linguistic Confounding
Abilities and Deficits Assessment Item Types Factors Factors
Spontaneous spoken (or sign) • Picture description • Degree of
language: • Conversation/ conversational structure
• Expressive language, discussion and support
agrammatism, telegraphic • Topic complexity,
speech concreteness/
• Word finding, dysnomia abstractness
• Paraphasias • Topic familiarity
• Perseveration • Personal relevance
• Circumlocution • Relationship to
conversational partner
• Stereotypy
• Means of scoring/rating
Writing: • Letter matching • Word frequency
• Writing ability, dysgraphia • Writing of words, • Word familiarity
phrases, sentences to • Age of acquisition
dictation
• Imageability
• Copying
• Concreteness/
• Written picture naming abstractness
• Narrative writing • Length of word, phrase,
sentence, discourse
stimuli
• Phonemic complexity
• Regular versus irregular
words
• Topic complexity
• Topic familiarity
• Personal relevance
• Relationship to intended
reader
• Type of writing
instrument/keyboard
Discourse/Pragmatics • Spoken and written • All aspects noted above
• Topic maintenance, discourse sampling
cohesion, politeness, and analysis:
informativeness, conversational,
codeswitching, narrative, procedural,
appropriateness of expository,
word choice, impulsivity, persuasive,
confabulation, hyper-/ descriptive
hypoaffectivity, use and • Use of gestures/
interpretation of prosodic pantomime
cues; use and interpretation
of facial expressions

240
Table 18–1. continued

Additional
Cognitive-Linguistic Potentially Confounding Potentially
Functions and Associated Cognitive-Linguistic Confounding
Abilities and Deficits Assessment Item Types Factors Factors
Cognition
Verbal and nonverbal memory • Interview, • All aspects noted above
abilities and challenges: conversation
• Working memory • Verbal and visual
• Short-term memory span tasks
• Long-term memory • Immediate and
delayed story retelling
• Procedural/implicit memory
• Immediate and
• Declarative/explicit memory
delayed recall and
• Episodic memory recognition of words,
• Semantic memory objects, object
• Prospective memory locations (spatial
memory), symbols,
• Source memory
patterns
• Encoding, storage, retrieval
• Copy and delayed
• Memory for events preceding copy of simple to
and following injury complex figures
Attention • Auditory, visual, and • All aspects noted above
• Arousal tactile vigilance tasks
• Alertness • Visual scanning and
tracking
• Selective/focused attention
• Sorting tasks
• Sustained attention
• Category switching
• Attention switching/shifting/
tasks
divided/alternating attention
• Cognitive effort
• Resource allocation
• Speed of processing/
cognitive efficiency
Executive function and • Verbal and nonverbal • All aspects noted above
reasoning: problem-solving
• Reasoning • Spontaneous
• Judgment language and
discourse tasks (see
• Decision-making
above)
• Goal-setting, planning,
• Verbal and nonverbal
strategizing
analogy tasks
• Awareness of strengths and
weaknesses
• Organizing
• Sequencing
• Convergent thinking
• Divergent thinking
continues

241
242  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Table 18–1. continued

Additional
Cognitive-Linguistic Potentially Confounding Potentially
Functions and Associated Cognitive-Linguistic Confounding
Abilities and Deficits Assessment Item Types Factors Factors
Calculation, acaculia • Verbal and nonverbal • Task difficulty
mathematical • Sensory deficits
problem-solving
• Response deficits
• Counting and number
concept tasks
Note. Data from Edwards & Bastiaanse, 2007; Hallowell & Ivanova, 2009; Hallowell et al., 2002; Heuer & Hallowell, 2007, 2009;
Ivanova & Hallowell, 2012, 2013, 2014; Lorenzen & Murray, 2008; Murray & Clark, 2006; Odekar et al., 2009; Roberts, 2008;
Roberts & Doucet, 2011. Many of the terms here are listed in the Glossary.

In Chapter 19, we explore ways to address and cally performed at no charge to the person screened.
control for potentially confounding factors during Despite the fact that screenings do not directly
assessment. generate revenue in such contexts, they are vital
to the recruitment of patients into one’s caseload,
and bigger caseloads ideally lead to more revenue
What Is Entailed in Screening for Acquired through billable services provided. They are also
vital to promoting access to services for people who
Neurogenic Language Disorders?
may not know about the full scope of practice of
SLPs and the related services that might help them.
A screening is typically a brief evaluation of whether Many hospitals, clinics, rehabilitation centers, and
a person has a problem that may benefit from fur- long-term care facilities have policies and proce-
ther professional attention and, if so, what the prob- dures in place for the screening of newly admitted
lem may be and what type of services might help. patients or residents, and periodic screening of indi-
Initially, where appropriate, the SLP may consult a viduals staying at the facility for extended periods
patient’s medical chart to consider any notes about of time.
basic challenges to body structure and function and Basic interview questions are essential to
about the patient’s recent living situation and social screening as well as comprehensive assessment. The
and caregiving support noted by a physician, nurse, nature of the questions we ask depends on the com-
or others on the rehabilitation team. If other mem- munication abilities of the individual and whether
bers of the rehabilitation team have already screened there is a care partner or someone who knows the
the patient, it is often helpful to hear their impres- patient well who can help to fill in information if
sions directly. If possible, observing the patient com- and when the patient has trouble answering. Of
municating with a family or staff member is helpful course, it is vital to include the person with the lan-
for noting spontaneous use of language, and facili- guage disorder as wholly as possible regardless of
tative or maladaptive strategies that the patient and the severity of their communication impairments.
others use to support communicative effectiveness. Not only is this essential to empowering the person
For in-person screening, the SLP may visit a person with the language disorder but also to enabling the
at bedside or in their room if they are an inpatient or clinician to directly observe their communication
resident at a particular facility. strengths and weaknesses.
In insurance-based, fee-for-service, and govern- Although open-ended requests (e.g., starting
ment-sponsored contexts, SLP screenings are typi- with “Tell me about . . . ”) are best for getting rich
18. Psychometrics of Assessment and Components of Assessment Processes   243

descriptions of the problems in the patient’s own Asking people directly what bothers them
words, some people with language challenges are most about communication and thinking and what
best able to respond to more direct or yes/no ques- they most wish to improve is a vital component
tions. Prior to asking interview-type questions, it is of screening. Answers to interview questions and
important to engage in rapport-building conversa- open-ended requests for information (e.g., Tell me
tion and to learn enough about the patient’s back- about . . . , Please describe . . . ) provide rich infor-
ground to ensure the relevance of questions to be mation, not only in terms of the content conveyed
asked. The interview questions shown in Box 18–1 but also in terms of what we may glean about their
serve as an example and may be adapted according insights, concerns, fears, level of awareness of poten-
to the person’s abilities and the appropriateness of tial deficits, life context and pre-onset aspects of life
the content. As shown in parentheses in Box 18–1, participation, family and social support, memory,
basic screening tasks may also be administered and attention, phonology, morphology, semantics, syn-
direct observations made as you proceed with an tax, pragmatics, discourse abilities, and concomitant
interview. impairments.

Box
18–1 Possible Screening Interview Questions and Tasks

A. Tell me about what is troubling you about • Getting “stuck” on the same words or
your communication abilities. phrases even if they do not convey what
you mean
1. Tell me about trouble you are having with
• Use of swear words
communication.
• Understanding others speaking
• Speaking • When listening to single words (Sample
• Coming up with names of people (Point to object naming with body parts and objects
people or pictures of family members and in the room and descriptions of objects.)
ask names.) • In a one-on-one conversation (Engage in
• Finding words for things you want to say social conversation; observe following
(Show common objects and point to objects of one-, two-, and three-step commands,
in the room and basic body parts and ask responses to yes/no and open-ended
the person to name them.) questions, object naming; compare
• Repetition (Provide words and sentences of automatic with propositional speech.)
increasing complexity to be repeated.) • In a group discussion
• Automatic/rote speech (Ask to recite days • Responding to requests
of the week, months of the year, nursery • Following spoken directions
rhymes, song lyrics, numbers from 1 to 20, • Listening to speech on the radio
etc.) • Watching and understanding television
• Propositional language (Ask questions shows and movies
requiring spoken responses; tell a story • Reading and understanding
and have the patient retell it; ask for • Single words
descriptions of objects and pictures; ask for • Numbers
procedural descriptions. Ask, for example, • Signs
how to make toast or change a diaper; ask • A newspaper or magazine
for a description of distant past and recent • A restaurant menu
past events.) • At your computer
244  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

• On your mobile phone • Doing simple arithmetic


• Other things you have typically read • Telling time
• Writing (by typing and handwriting) • Keeping track of time
• Individual letters of the alphabet • Planning activities
• Your name • Working
• Single words • Keeping track of your medications
• Sentences • Drawing
• Paragraphs
2. Tell me about what you most wish you could
• Written correspondence, e-mails
improve about your thinking abilities.
• Other things you have typically written
• Copying letters, words, sentences
C. Tell me how you are coping with your
• Managing e-mails and using the Internet
challenges.
• Engaging in social activities
• Using the telephone 1. How do you feel about the changes in your
• Using social media communication and thinking abilities?
• Connecting in person with your family 2. How is your mood?
and friends 3. Describe any changes in your mood since
• Going out to restaurants before you had this condition.
• Participating in activities you typically 4. Describe any changes in your personality
enjoyed before since before you had this condition.
5. What strategies do you use to communicate
2. Tell me about what you most wish you could
when you face barriers with talking and
improve about your communication.
listening?
6. What types of support do you think you
B. Tell me about your thinking abilities overall.
most need?
1. Describe the trouble you are having: 7. What questions do you have about your
condition?
• Thinking clearly
8. What questions do you have about your
• Remembering things (Request a recounting
assessments?
of an event [episodic]; request instructions
9. What questions do you have about
for carrying out a common task [procedural];
rehabilitation or treatment plans?
request a description of the purpose of
10. What additional information would you like
common and uncommon objects [semantic].
to have?
Ask questions about childhood, about events
just prior to and following onset, and about
D. Tell me about your everyday participation in
recent events within the past few hours.)
activities now and before.
• Paying attention (Observe ability to notice
changes [vigilance], stay focused with 1. What activities that involve communication
distraction [sustained and selective attention], are most important to you?
multitask [attention switching].) 2. What challenges are you having in doing
• Counting change things that are most important to you?
• Managing your finances

Note. Attend to the individual’s perception of how important each item is, now and prior to onset of a language
disorder. These items are framed as questions and requests to the person; all may be reframed to also be asked of
caregivers or significant others; all may be reframed in a less linguistically loaded format through supported com-
munication. In addition to noting content offered by the individual, observe evidence of each ability in context,
as feasible. See Box 18–2 for case history content that may be integrated into a screening form and/or process.
18. Psychometrics of Assessment and Components of Assessment Processes   245

It is often helpful, too, to give a client and a care company, or another source). Thorough assessment
partner separate questionnaires or rating scales (see for any of the adult neurogenic language disorders
Chapter 20) for answering basic questions about should include assessment of the following:
communication challenges; when given to both, it
may be informative to compare responses between • what a person wants to do in life and the
the two. Examples of language and mental sta- environment needed to support that
tus screening activities are shown in items within • perceived strengths and challenges associated
parentheses in Box 18–1. Further screening tasks to with communication, socialization, and
address concomitant areas of functioning, such as quality of life (ways in which challenges are
for visual neglect, attention, and memory, reviewed affecting the individual’s life participation
in Chapter 19, may be presented as appropriate. according to their own perceptions and those
Published screening protocols may be administered of others who are important to them; the
to assess practically any aspect of language and cog- type and degree of communication supports
nition. Information about published screening tools and barriers in their social and physical
is given in Chapter 20. Many clinicians prefer to use environment)
their own screening materials and processes. • language formulation and production at all
A screening occurs in a small window of time language levels (phonology, morphology,
as we begin to learn about a person’s communica- semantics, syntax, and pragmatics) and in
tive, cognitive, and life participation status. It is not written and spoken modalities (plus sign
in-depth enough to provide a full picture of a per- language in cases of sign language users)
son’s wide array of communicative strengths and • cognition, including multiple aspects of
weaknesses and their relevance to quality of life. mental status or orientation, memory,
Even when standardized screening tests and proto- attention, and executive functions
cols are used, screening results do not entail enough
detailed and repeated sampling to provide highly Of course, many people with neurogenic lan-
reliable and valid results on which to base firm prog- guage disorders also have motor speech and swal-
nostic, diagnostic, or treatment decisions. Imagine lowing disorders, among other concomitant chal-
having a time limit of only 5 or 10 minutes to glean lenges, each of which must be carefully assessed.
information about every item listed in Box 18–1. Is The ultimate excellent clinical aphasiologist will
it possible? No. You would simply do your best to pursue knowledge and skills in those areas and
glean the most information possible across a wide ensure effective assessment and intervention to meet
array of areas of function and harness the best in holistic needs.
your reservoir of clinical judgment capacities to Perceived strengths and challenges associated
draw conclusions pertinent to next steps. Whether with communication, socialization, and quality of
your time is so severely restricted or not, be sure to life may be ascertained through the case history pro-
keep your mind open to new information and ongo- cess (see later) and also through the use of published
ing changes in status. assessment tools and rating forms. A thoughtful dis-
cussion of the ultimate goals of each person assessed,
from their own perspective, is essential. Use of sup-
What Are the Typical Components of a ported communication to explore such goals from
the start is important. Haley et al. (2010) developed
Comprehensive Assessment Process?
a set of Life Interests and Values Cards to be used
for this purpose. The process of using such supports
A comprehensive assessment of a person with a neu- ideally promotes autonomy and self-efficacy in
rogenic cognitive-linguistic disorder includes a case goal setting (Helm-Estabrooks & Whiteside, 2012).
history, discourse sampling, and speech, language, Talking Mats (Murphy et al., 2016) is a visual frame-
and cognitive testing. Unlike screening, in many work based on which picture symbols can be used
contexts, there is typically a financial charge for a full to spark conversation with people who have com-
assessment (whether paid by the client, an insurance munication challenges and support conversations
246  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

about hobbies and life priorities. It is available in a amount importance. Examples of items on a case his-
printed format, with images and printed word cards, tory form that are especially pertinent to acquired
and in a computerized form. adult neurogenic language disorders in adults are
Examples of specific assessment tasks used listed in Box 18–2. Many examples of case history
to index cognitive-linguistic abilities are listed in forms are available online and in related textbooks
Table 18–1. Examples of tasks used to index cogni- and can be adapted to fit assessment goals and con-
tive abilities are more challenging to encapsulate textual constraints. Each agency or facility in which
into such a cohesive listing; cognitive constructs to an SLP might work is likely to have its own case his-
be assessed are referred to with tremendous incon- tory forms, and these are increasingly generated in
sistency, and the means of indexing cognitive abili- electronic rather than paper formats.
ties is extremely variable. An effective way of getting Note that, depending on the context and the
a good grasp on means of indexing such constructs nature of the patient’s problems, a complete case
as memory (in its multiple forms), attention (in its history form may include information about many
multiple forms), reasoning, problem-solving, infer- other topics as well, including items related to motor
encing, sequencing, discourse organization, and speech and swallowing. Many case history forms
humor is to read about and peruse published instru- also include the types of items listed as screening
ments designed to assess these. content in Box 18–1. In the examples given, case his-
tory information might be collected through sources
other than direct screening or interviewing; in actu-
What Information Is Pertinent to ality, the processes for obtaining both types of infor-
mation are often intermingled.
Collect During the Case History?
Keep in mind that many case history forms in cur-
rent use include heterosexist language (e.g., regard-
Some case history information may be gleaned from ing marital status) and biased content about gender
medical charts; some may be learned through other identity (e.g., no choices to check anything other than
health professionals. Still, input from the person “male” or “female” as gender). These require revision
being assessed and their significant others is of par- to convey greater equity and inclusion.

Box
18–2 Sample Cognitive-Communicative Items on a Case History

General information • Handedness


• Date of case history • Native language
• Name • Additional languages spoken and levels of
• Identification number proficiency in each
• Contact information • Where they grew up
• Gender identity/preferred pronouns • Highest level of education
• Family physician • Professional background
• Referral source • Current educational and/or professional
• Previous services received through this status
clinic/agency • Hobbies, volunteer work, and personal
• Dates of previous services interests
• Everyday types of communication
Personal history activities in which the person wants and
• Birth date needs to participate
• Age • Living arrangement
18. Psychometrics of Assessment and Components of Assessment Processes   247

• Physical location and type of housing • Dysphagia


• Other people living in the home • Vision problems
• Current care partners • Hearing problems

Background Previous services or consultation: nature of


• Date of injury or onset, or when symptoms services and corresponding dates
were first noticed • Speech-language pathology assessment
• Etiology and/or intervention
• Site and extent of lesion and other • Psychological assessment
neurological findings • Psychological counseling
• Loss of consciousness and duration • Vocational counseling
• Description of any of the following through • Physical therapy
records or referrals • Occupational therapy
• Language problems • Speech and language information
• Cognitive problems
• Speech problems Patient/client and care partner/significant other
• Motor control problems in the body descriptions of communication problemsa
(weakness, paralysis)

a
See Box 18–1 for interview questions and tasks that may be integrated into a case history form and/or process.
Note. Be sure any actual case history form does not include heterosexist language or convey assumptions about
gender identity and the nature of family structures and living arrangements. The items listed are items to con-
sider in context; actual wording should fit the intended context and purpose. Content should be integrated with
that of other areas of clinical practice as appropriate.

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter 5. Provide two examples of how specific
that are new to you or that you have not yet concomitant deficits associated with
mastered. cognitive-communicative deficits subsequent
2. Compare and contrast different types of to TBI might affect the reliability of language
reliability. assessment results with a TBI survivor.
3. Provide two examples of how two 6. List three factors that might influence inter-
nonlinguistic challenges that often co-occur examiner reliability for a specific published
with aphasia might affect the reliability of language test.
language assessment results for a person 7. List three factors that might influence intra-
with aphasia. examiner reliability for a specific published
4. Provide two examples of how specific language test.
cognitive-communicative deficits associated 8. Compare and contrast different types of
with right brain syndrome (RBS) might affect validity.
the reliability of language assessment results 9. Provide two examples of how two
with a survivor of a right brain injury (RBI). nonlinguistic challenges that often co-occur
248  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

with aphasia might affect the validity of 16. Consider the “assessment item types” listed
language assessment results for a person in the second column of Table 18–1. As a
with aphasia. clinician or researcher, you are not likely
10. Provide two examples of how specific to have sufficient time to assess abilities
cognitive-communicative deficits associated using all of the possible types of items for
with RBS might affect the validity of any specific ability you wish to assess.
language assessment results with an RBI What are the most important criteria for
survivor. deciding which types of assessment items to
11. Provide two examples of how specific administer for a given individual?
concomitant deficits associated with 17. Consider the list of potentially confounding
cognitive-communicative deficits factors listed in the second and third
subsequent to TBI might affect the validity columns in Table 18–1.
of language assessment results with a TBI a. Note how the items in the third column,
survivor. labeled “potentially confounding factors,”
12. Describe specific means by which a test, are largely controlled by the authors of
subtest, or test item may have weak content assessment instruments and/or clinicians
validity. who are carrying out assessment tasks.
13. Describe how one would establish each of Describe specific ways in which the
the following (in general terms, not in terms potential for such factors to confound
of statistical procedures): assessment results could be avoided by
a. Construct validity (convergent validity test authors and clinicians.
and discriminant validity) b. Note how the items in the fourth
b. Criterion validity (concurrent validity, column, labeled “additionally potentially
predictive validity, sensitivity, and confounding factors,” have little to do with
specificity) design of assessment tools and more to do
c. Face validity with how, when, by whom, and to whom
d. Ecological validity assessments are administered. Describe
14. Describe why it is important for a test, specific ways in which the potential for
subtest, or test item to have each of the such factors to confound assessment
following: results could be avoided by clinicians.
a. Construct validity (convergent validity 18. Compare and contrast a screening from a
and discriminant validity) comprehensive evaluation of a person’s
b. Criterion validity (concurrent validity, cognitive-linguistic abilities.
predictive validity, sensitivity, and 19. Describe why it is important to establish
specificity) rapport with an individual prior to engaging
c. Face validity in any formal screening or assessment.
d. Ecological validity 20. Consider the screening interview items
15. Consider the “cognitive-linguistic functions and tasks listed in Box 18–1. As a clinician
and associated abilities and deficits” listed in or researcher, you are not likely to have
the first column of Table 18–1. As a clinician sufficient time to implement all of the items
or researcher, you are not likely to have and tasks listed for a screening. What are the
sufficient time to directly assess all of these most important criteria for deciding which
abilities and challenges for every individual. items and tasks to administer in a screening
What are the most important criteria for for a given individual?
deciding which constructs to assess for a 21. Many clinical facilities have preestablished
given individual? screening forms for cognitive-linguistic
18. Psychometrics of Assessment and Components of Assessment Processes   249

assessment in adults. What would be the language disorder during a case history
strengths and weaknesses of having one session, what would they be?
standard form to be used for all screenings? 24. During a screening or case history session, if
22. Many clinical facilities have preestablished you could ask only five questions of a person
case history forms for adult clients. What most socially and emotionally connected
would be the strengths and weaknesses of with a person with an acquired language
having one standard form to be used for all disorder, what would they be?
case histories?
23. If you could ask only five questions of You may find additional learning and teaching
www
a person with an acquired neurogenic materials on the companion website.
CHAPTER
19
Problem-Solving Approaches
to Differential Diagnosis and
Confounding Factors

Some of the greatest challenges for speech-language 3. How does a process analysis approach
pathologists (SLPs) relate to the complexity of to assessment help address potentially
problem-​solving required to understand any indi- confounding factors?
vidual’s neurological and psychosocial condition
and to draw conclusions about the myriad influences
on that person’s ability to communicate, participate How Are Potentially Confounding Factors
maximally in desired daily life activities, and have a
Relevant to Differential Diagnosis?
strong sense of identity and well-being. In terms of
assessment, there are two critical considerations as
we work to address those great challenges: The term differential diagnosis typically refers to iden-
tifying disorder labels that either apply or do not
• How to differentially diagnose one condition apply to an individual according to an evaluation of
from another his or her body structure and function.
• How to be sure that our assessments truly In the differential diagnostic process, we tend
reflect a given condition, and its severity to label the disorders that a person has and clarify
and impact, in the face of many potentially which disorders they do not have. Differential diag-
confounding factors nosis in a person with a neurogenic communication
disorder can be particularly perplexing for four pri-
This chapter is intended to help address those mary reasons. First, given the interconnectivity of
challenges. structures throughout the brain — as well as with
After reading and reflecting on the content in subcortical structures and systems — plus their func-
this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in tional connections and the interrelationships with one
your own words, the following queries: another, it is not always easy to tell just what a per-
son with a neurogenic language disorder can or can-
1. How are potentially confounding factors not do. Second, there are many underlying reasons
relevant to differential diagnosis? why a person may have trouble with any particular
2. What are important potentially confounding aspect of cognition or communication; identifying the
factors in language assessment, and how do we symptoms does not necessarily lead to clarity about
address them? the nature of those symptoms or their causes. Third,

251
252  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

any person we are assessing is likely to have multi-


ple concomitant conditions, such that we are not just
What Are Important Potentially Confounding
making conclusions about one diagnosis at a time. Factors in Language Assessment, and
Fourth, many of the influences on a person’s ability How Do We Address Them?
to communicate cannot be distilled neatly into health
or medical conditions that can be readily labeled. Age
The term confounding factor, as we noted in
Chapters 17 and 18, refers to any aspect of a per- “Old age” is not a diagnosis; nor is it an explanation
son’s abilities; any aspect of the assessment tools, for impairment. Chronological age, as defined by
procedures, and processes that we use; and any time since birth, has been shown to correlate with
aspect of the testing context or situation that could some patterns of brain change that correspond with
lead to invalid results. We add the term potentially some symptoms in a majority of people, particularly
confounding factor in many cases because we cannot when tested in research contexts. Such patterns are
always know when a particular factor is affecting explored and summarized in Chapter 9. In the assess-
our assessments. Several categories of problems that ment process, knowing about such patterns may be
tend to co-occur with language problems are poten- helpful if we are considering hypotheses about the
tially confounding factors. In Chapter 18, we noted potential influence of a person’s age on their com-
many test design factors, testing context factors, and munication abilities. At the same time, knowing that
interpersonal factors that may influence the actual any given individual may defy such patterns is of
assessment process as well as our interpretation of paramount importance. Also, attributing a person’s
assessment results. Many of those are factors that communicative deficits to age alone is almost always
we may control for or at least take into account by inappropriate; when we find ourselves or others
implementing best practices in assessment. doing so, it is good to reflect on whether overt or
Any condition that we might consider as a pos- covert ageism may be at play.
sibility in the differential diagnostic process is also
likely to be associated with factors that confound
assessment of cognition and communication. In this Intelligence, Literacy, and Education
chapter, we focus on potentially confounding fac-
tors that relate especially to concomitant challenges A person’s levels of intelligence, literacy, and education
to health and well-being. All of these may influence prior to the onset of an acquired neurogenic disorder
the validity and reliability of differential diagnosis as are all important to gauge. These are crucial potential
well as of the overall conclusions that may be drawn confounds to consider because they can directly and
about any aspect of a person’s communication and indirectly influence language assessment. Pre-onset
socialization status. Potentially confounding factors intelligence is a potential confounding factor in the
represent a mixed collection of constructs. Some are differential diagnosis of any and all cognitive deficits.
personal traits or demographic factors (e.g., age, Below-average intelligence may influence the valid-
intelligence), some relate to prior learning (e.g., edu- ity of assessment results when using tools normed
cation, literacy), and some are diagnostic categories on people of average or higher intelligence. Know-
(e.g., hearing problems, visual problems, reading ing about any prior intellectual challenges helps us
disorders, depression). Still others relate to the social interpret current performance and has implications
and communication contexts of any given assessment for guiding treatment plans and implementing reha-
process and the interpersonal dynamics between the bilitation strategies (Gao et al., 2000; Leritz et al., 2008;
person assessing and the person being assessed. For Makin et al., 2018). At the same time, we must always
some potentially confounding factors, specific assess- be alert to socioeconomic, ethic, cultural, and social
ment procedures and tools may be recommended to constructs of race as potential confounds in indices of
help in differential diagnosis. For others, means of intelligence, literacy, and education.
taking them into account during the assessment pro- Measures that control for intelligence versus
cess and weighing their potential influences are con- years of education do not necessarily lead to par-
sidered in light of theory and research to date. allel results; it is often important to consider both
19. Problem-Solving Approaches to Differential Diagnosis and Confounding Factors   253

intelligence and education levels (Steinberg et al., disorder. Visual problems also may be exacerbated
2005). The Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR; by aging and by peripheral neuropathies associated
Wechsler, 2001) is one of the few tests that have been with metabolic disorders such as diabetes mellitus.
shown to be correlated with intelligence test scores Given the great deal of reliance on visual material
obtained prior to the onset of brain injury (Mathias (e.g., text, pictures, objects) and to visual aspects of
et al., 2007). However, illiteracy would certainly be communicative situations (e.g., facial expressions,
a significant confound in WTAR results, as would gestures, postures, location of people involved),
socioeconomic, cultural, and ethnic factors that may screening for visual deficits is fundamental to the
influence intelligence test scores. cognitive-linguistic assessment process.
It is impossible to tease apart the influence of Hallowell et al. (2004) tracked assessment and
education in general and literacy in particular on experimental task materials used in every research
language abilities of an individual living in a region article on neurogenic language disorders in each of
where people are typically engaged in formal edu- 17 journals over a 10-year period. We found that all
cation throughout childhood. Further complicating but one of 668 studies (99.85%) included the use of
matters, literacy has been shown to be a confound visual stimuli. However, less than 3% of those that
in assessment of skills beyond reading. For exam- involved visual materials included mention of any
ple, low literacy has been linked to deficits in phono- aspect of vision (e.g., whether it was included in
logical awareness, naming, word finding, drawing, inclusion/exclusion criteria, participant descrip-
abstract thinking, sentence-picture matching, and tions, or methods of experimental control). Clearly,
lexical decision tasks in people with and without there is a need for greater attention to vision in our
neurogenic language disorders (Colaço et al., 2010; work as clinical aphasiologists.
Coppens et al., 1998; Reitan & Wolfson, 1997; Tse- Failing to take visual deficits into account
gaye et al., 2011) and thus may affect language test in assessment and in the interpretation of results
results in assessment of constructs beyond reading leads to serious validity problems. Ideally, stroke
and writing. Again, to what degree literacy per se and brain injury survivors should be referred to a
versus education in general may influence such neuro-ophthalmologist for a full visual evaluation
links — and to what extent economic, social, ethnic, early in the rehabilitation process and periodically
and other contextual factors play a role — is probably as needed. Whether this is feasible or not, it is still
indeterminable for a given person. important for SLPs to consider the potential influ-
Given that socioeconomic status is often tied ences of visual deficits throughout assessment and
to one’s level of education and literacy, it, too, may treatment. Hallowell (2008) offers details on design-
be considered an assessment confound. Its specific ing screening protocols and case history procedures
impact on language impairment severity is unclear to capture information about visual abilities in peo-
(González-Fernández et al., 2011). A complicating ple with acquired neurogenic disorders. Here, let’s
factor in understanding the nature of the influence review specific means of indexing key problems
of socioeconomic status on any cognitive-linguistic in terms of acuity and color vision deficits, ocular
ability is that low socioeconomic status is linked to motor problems, visual neglect, and varied types of
poor overall health, limited access to health care, and higher-level visual integration problems.
increased risk for cardiovascular and neurological
disorders (Connor et al., 2001; Kuller et al., 1995). Visual Acuity Deficits
Age-related macular degeneration is the most com-
Visual Problems mon cause for loss of visual acuity. Other common
causes are diabetic retinopathy, glaucoma, and cat-
Many people with neurogenic communication disor- aracts. Be sure to ask if the person you are assessing
ders have some type of visual dysfunction, and many normally wears glasses or contact lenses. Especially
have multiple concomitant visual problems. Many if they need glasses for near vision and/or correction
already had visual acuity, color vision, and ocular of nystagmus or double vision, it is vital that they
motor problems prior to onset of a communication use them during assessment. Far vision (for viewing
254  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

at a distance of 3 feet or more) is important for every- what was intended, especially in terms of color, res-
day functioning (e.g., viewing traffic signals, read- olution, and projected size of images and characters.
ing signs, watching movies or slides projected on a Visual acuity tests typically entail identifying
screen) but is less likely to influence performance on the smallest characters that can be read at a speci-
the types of tasks typically used in cognitive-linguistic fied distance. Snellen charts, composed of rows of
assessment. letters in progressively smaller font, are typically
Charts and other tools for visual screening are used. However, for people with language disorders,
readily available through ophthalmologic, optomet- it may be preferable to use nonlinguistic symbols,
ric, medical, and educational supply companies. The such as shapes and simple images. When using the
Rosenbaum Pocket Vision Screener is commonly dis- Tumbling E chart, the person may simply hold up
tributed at no or low cost by medical supply com- three fingers to indicate the direction of an E pattern
panies. Some computerized assessment tools are shown in varied orientations. With the Lea chart,
available; it is important to scrutinize these carefully, symbols for a ball, apple, house, and square are
as actual computerized displays may differ from shown (Figure 19–1). The Allen chart includes every-

Figure 19–1. Example of a visual screening chart requiring no verbal responses. The Lea chart (shown on the
left) depicts line drawings of four objects. As a clinician points to images on the chart, a person who has difficulty
speaking may simply point to a corresponding image on a multiple-choice card (top right). They may also give a
yes/no response when shown one image at a time on a separate card (bottom right). Image courtesy of Lea test
International, LLC.
19. Problem-Solving Approaches to Differential Diagnosis and Confounding Factors   255

day objects that are typically easy to recognize (bird, formats and picture matching, is Color Vision Test-
horse, cake, telephone, hand). With any of these ing Made Easy (Waggoner, 1994).
adapted charts, if a person is unable to speak, the
respondent may point to a multiple-choice display Ocular Motor Problems
to indicate what is seen. For people with low vision
(acuity worse than the top row of a far-vision acu- Problems of alignment and movement of the eyes
ity chart 20 feet away), screening tasks may involve may be caused by impairments of the extraocular
having them count the number of fingers you hold muscles that control eye movement, ocular motor
up or detect nearby motion or light. nerves, or cortical systems involved in motor con-
trol of those nerves. Ocular motor control in itself
Visual Field Deficits is not likely to negatively affect assessment of lan-
guage and cognition for most people. In cases where
Visual field deficits, or visual field cuts, are a loss of an individual’s communication ability may depend
visual information regarding specific components of on eyetracking input, or when ocular motor control
the visual space. They are very common, especially deficits impair processing of orthographic or picture
immediately post-onset, in stroke and brain injury stimuli, it is a good idea to screen for ocular motor
survivors. Types of visual field deficits are reviewed deficits and consult with a neuro-ophthalmologist.
in Chapter 7. They include homonymous hemianop- See Hallowell (2008) for suggestions.
sia or quantrantopsia, heteronymous hemianopsia,
and scotoma. Visual fields are tested when the indi- Visual Neglect
vidual’s head is held stable. While SLPs may carry
out visual field screening, it is important to confer As reviewed in Chapter 7, visual attention deficits
with an ophthalmologist for precise visual field test- have a direct impact on communication abilities, so
ing because of the instrumentation and expertise they may pose an important barrier to valid test-
required. Visual field deficits tend not to be as con- ing. Note that not all researchers agree that visual
founding in cognitive-linguistic assessment as visual neglect is a problem of attention per se. As indicated
acuity problems and neglect; this is because people in Chapter 12, a minority of scholars in this area pre-
with visual field deficits tend to compensate for fer to consider it a cognitive problem of being able
them via head movement, even if they are unaware to generate an abstract internal representation of the
of doing so. Basic screening procedures for visual external physical world.
field and peripheral vision screening are provided People with hemineglect of the visual space also
by Hallowell (2008). often ignore nonvisual aspects of the same hemis-
pace. For example, they may not attend to or use the
Color Vision Deficits limbs on the neglected side and may not attend to
olfactory, auditory, or somatosensory input on the
To the degree that color is implicated in assessment neglected side. As reviewed in Chapter 12, some-
stimuli, color vision deficits (of which there are many times the space neglected is relative to the midline
varied forms) are potentially confounding factors in of an individual’s own body (egocentric neglect).
indexing communication abilities. Ishihara plates Sometimes it is relative to whatever object, display, or
are a common screening tool. These are images or real-world scene they are focused on at the moment
shapes composed of small dots in primary colors (allocentric neglect) (Ting et al., 2011). Differences
superimposed on a background of dots in a second- among subtypes of neglect have been attributed to
ary color. They are shown on a computer monitor or differing types and locations of underlying lesions
on printed cards. If color perception is deficient for (Karnath & Rorden, 2012). Hemispatial neglect often
a given color contrast, the respondent is unable to resolves itself within the first few weeks or months
identify the image or shape. An example of a screen- after a stroke or brain injury. It also can persist indef-
ing tool that is amenable to adaptation through sup- initely, which is more likely in people with large cor-
ported communication, especially multiple-choice tical lesions (Maguire & Ogden, 2002).
256  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Estimates of the incidence of hemispatial neglect Deficits in visual attention may be a serious con-
in people with right and left hemisphere lesions vary founding factor in many of the most common types
widely. Reasons for this include the following: of assessment tasks used by SLPs to index perfor-
mance according to any of a host of constructs. For
• different means of indexing neglect lead example, imagine the impact that neglect of half of a
to differing levels of sensitivity and picture could have on language formulation scores
specificity based on a picture description task. Imagine how not
• some tasks depend on the person’s attending to half of the images in multiple-choice
communication abilities and thus have displays could affect auditory and reading compre-
inherent confounds for people with speech hension test results. Imagine how pragmatic appro-
and language disorders priateness in conversation might be misjudged if a
• numbers of people sampled and sampling person completely ignores visitors on one side of
procedures differ across reported incidence the room. Keep in mind that people with neglect are
studies unlikely to be aware of it, even if they are told that
• people with lesions due to varied etiologies they have it, and so they tend not to report it or com-
are included within and across studies pensate for it. It is thus important that we go out of
• symptoms evolve quickly in many people our way to look for it. Input from family members is
soon after onset so the timing of assessment important; still, they may be unaware of it as well.
may influence the proportions reported One means of screening for visual neglect
is through observation. People with hemispatial
There is general agreement in the literature that visual neglect may ignore people or activities on
the incidence of left neglect is high in people with the neglected side of the room. Some fail to eat the
right hemisphere lesions (up to about 66%; Schenken- food placed on the neglected side of a plate or cafe-
berg et al., 1980). Incidence of right-sided neglect in teria tray. Some who wear makeup may fail to apply
people with left hemisphere lesions is often reported makeup to one side of the face. You may learn that
to be lower, although visual neglect is much more an individual has walked across a street without
prevalent than once thought in people with aphasia attending to oncoming traffic from one direction; this
and related disorders (occurring in up to 65% of sur- is a potentially serious consequence of not attending
vivors of left brain injuries; Beume et al., 2017; Kar- to one side.
nath et al., 2002; Pedersen et al., 1997). It may be that A series of simple screening tasks may be car-
left hemineglect is more readily detected in people ried out. The line bisection task entails asking a
with right brain injury (RBI) because clinicians and person to mark the midpoint of a straight line. Note
researchers are more predisposed to screen for it in the result for a person with hemispatial neglect in
that population, and because language disorders in Figure 19–2. It is as if the neglected portion of the line
people with left hemisphere lesions may confound does not exist in the view of the individual, and the
assessment of visual attention. bisection mark is made toward the nonneglected side.

Figure 19–2. Line bisection task. A. The bisection mark made by a person without
visual hemineglect. B. The mark made by a person with left visual field neglect.
19. Problem-Solving Approaches to Differential Diagnosis and Confounding Factors   257

A line cancellation task (also called the Albert Reading tasks also may be helpful. If a person
test; Albert, 1973) entails presenting a series of lines is able to read and speak, give them a page of text to
in varied orientations on a page and asking the indi- read aloud. People with visual neglect often fail to
vidual to mark each line to create a cross or plus read words on the neglected side of a page or col-
sign. This is illustrated in Figure 19–3. Note that the umn, as illustrated in Figure 19–5. Considering the
lines in the neglected portion of the visual field (in lost content during a reading task, it is apparent how
this case, the left side) are not marked. Variations of visual neglect may be falsely interpreted as a linguis-
line cancellation tasks entail pages of varied shapes tic or reading comprehension problem, per se.
or letters; respondents are asked to circle or other- Another means of using reading to screen for
wise mark all of any shape (e.g., all the squares) or neglect is to present individual cards with printed
letter (all the As). compound words, such as mailman, girlfriend, or
The clock drawing task is commonly used in matchbox. If a person reading these aloud consistently
screening for visual neglect. Simply ask the individ- pronounces only one component morpheme on the
ual to draw a clock. A tendency to draw the numbers right or left, this may indicate neglect of one side of
and hands of the clock on one side of the clock’s face the visual space. For people who have sufficient lin-
suggests that the opposite side is neglected, as illus- guistic abilities to engage in a picture description or
trated in Figure 19–4. Interestingly, the circular struc- reading-aloud task, we may gain additional insight
ture of the clock face is often appropriately drawn, as into the condition by adding irrelevant visual stimuli
highly predictable, common symmetrical shapes are. on the neglected side of the stimulus of interest. Peo-
Drawing or copying other items, such as a flower, ple with allocentric neglect may neglect the added
person, or tree, may also yield insights about poten- stimulus on the contralateral side instead of the con-
tial visual neglect. tralateral portion of the stimulus and attend to all or

Figure 19–3. Line cancellation task result for a person with left visual hemineglect.
Figure 19–4. Clock drawing by a TBI survivor with left visual field
neglect.

Figure 19–5. Right-sided visual neglect during a reading task. Highlighted words indicate portions of text
neglected for an individual with a left hemisphere lesion reading the letter aloud.

258
19. Problem-Solving Approaches to Differential Diagnosis and Confounding Factors   259

at least more of the stimulus of interest; thus, their ities. Auditory acuity and discrimination challenges
responses may improve. Another strategy to try is to tend to occur with advancing age, as do most etiolo-
present multiple-choice stimuli or rating scales in a gies of neurogenic language disorders. Still, there is
vertical rather than horizontal orientation. wide variability in how age and neurological inju-
Many language batteries include screening ries affect hearing, such that it is essential to consider
tools for visual neglect. The Behavioral Inattention each older person’s hearing status individually.
Test (BIT; Wilson et al., 1987a) includes means of Traumatic brain injury (TBI) survivors have a
indexing varied forms of hemispatial neglect. The high rate of hearing loss due to otologic injuries,
severity of hemispatial neglect may be modified by and many are said to have central auditory pro-
manipulating the degree of alertness and attention cessing difficulties (Lubinski et al., 1997; Musiek
required, such as through imposing time limits on et al., 2004) or difficulty responding to rapid rates
cognitive-linguistic tasks (George et al., 2008). of speech (Poeck & Pietron, 1981). Injuries of the ear
are among the most common of all injuries reported
Higher-Level Visual Integration Problems among soldiers engaging in modern warfare; ears
are especially susceptible to blast injury (Myers et
Visual agnosia is a deficit in recognizing visual stim- al., 2009). The actual incidence of hearing loss within
uli. Types of visual agnosia include visual object any of the same diagnostic groups with neurogenic
agnosia (deficit in recognizing objects or images loss of language abilities is difficult to estimate due
of objects), prosopagnosia (deficit in recognizing to multiple potential assessment confounds and
faces), or autotopagnosia (deficit in recognizing due to the fact that many are unaware of their hear-
body parts). Although they are not common, any ing deficits and thus do not report them or seek
of these may affect assessment as well. There are hearing assessment.
several assessment tools for indexing such prob- It is a great asset to have an audiologist on your
lems, although challenges with language may rehabilitation team, one experienced in adapting
potentially confound corresponding results. Assess- instructions and procedures as needed and engag-
ment for these problems is often considered more ing in supported communication with people with
within the scope of practice of neuropsychology neurogenic disorders; where there is not one, it is
than of SLPs. often appropriate to refer to an outside audiolo-
Visuoconstructive deficits involve challenges in gist for a hearing evaluation to index, at least, the
drawing original images, copying images, or manip- person’s pure-tone thresholds (air and bone) and
ulating three-dimensional objects into specified auditory discrimination and processing abilities.
designs or patterns. Some use the term constructional Auditory evoked response testing is recommended
apraxia, although it is less accurate in a literal sense, by some; auditory evoked response results have
because the behavioral manifestation may occur due even been validated, at least to some degree, as a
to a perceptual disorder not involving a motor com- prognostic indicator for language recovery in people
ponent (Benton & Tranel, 1993; Rothi et al., 1997). with aphasia due to stroke (Sosa et al., 2009). Most
Visuoconstructive disability may be a confounding central auditory testing procedures entail long and
factor in any cognitive-linguistic assessment involv- complex verbal instructions and response tasks that
ing drawing tasks. Some language batteries include are linguistically loaded and have low ecological
screening tools to index this construct. validity. Thus, incidence statistics for central audi-
tory processing disorders in people with neurogenic
language deficits typically have questionable valid-
Hearing Problems ity (Feeney & Hallowell, 2000).
It is within the SLP’s scope of practice to screen
Hearing problems are more likely in people with for hearing difficulties. When performing a hearing
neurogenic language disorders than in the general screening, attend to any appropriate adaptations for
population. Some of the disproportion is due to the people with language deficits, such as allowing for
effects of age on hearing abilities, and some is due to alternative instructions (auditory, written, graphic,
the impact of neurological changes on hearing abil- gestural) and response modes (hand raising, button
260  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

pressing, eye blink, written responses). For intelligi- exists, many clinicians as well as people with neuro-
bility testing, picture-pointing tasks may be a more genic language disorders describe it as problematic
valid indicator than word repetition. in terms of decreased language processing abilities
Does the person have a hearing aid (or two)? as an assessment session continues. A challenge in
If so, make sure they are wearing it and that it is indexing the phenomenon as a clinical symptom
clean and functioning correctly, with functional bat- is that many of our assessment tools are designed
teries. If the hearing aid is not present or functional to begin with easy tasks and then progress to more
or if the user wishes not to use it, it may be helpful difficult tasks. Given that people are more likely to
to use a simple battery-powered personal listening become fatigued, frustrated, and challenged with the
device, such as a sound amplification system that later tasks, then, it may not be clear whether decreased
may be worn around the neck and a microphone abilities noted over time are due to this rather nebu-
and headphone set. They are available at low to lous but still potentially aggravating symptom.
moderate cost, depending on quality and features.
Some people even without hearing impairment
seem more alert and interactive when wearing these, Motor Challenges
perhaps because they are aided by the reduction in
background noise, or the physical sensation of the Any of an array of motor challenges may occur
headphones causes them to attend more closely to in people with neurological disorders. They may
auditory stimuli and conversation. Individual peo- involve innervation, motor programming, control,
ple or the people who interact with them may wish and coordination of musculature required for speech
to have one of their own if it seems to improve com- or bodily movement.
munication. It is well worth the investment to have
at least one of these on hand to try out as appropriate Apraxia of Speech (AoS)
during assessment.
Whether or not the individual is using amplifi- Apraxia of speech (AoS) is impairment in motor
cation, be sure to determine at what loudness level programming and sequencing of movements of the
you should be speaking to accommodate hearing articulators for intentional or volitional speech. It is
problems. Do not assume that a person has a signifi- characterized by articulatory groping, inconsistent
cant hearing impairment just because they are older. articulatory errors (sound substitutions, omission,
Ask directly for feedback about whether you are substitutions, repetitions, and distortions), slow
speaking too loudly or too quietly, too quickly or too speech rate, and abnormal stress patterns. It is not a
slowly. It is also important, throughout intervention, problem of muscular innervation (Croot, 2002; Duffy,
to tune into possible auditory agnosia (difficulty rec- 2013). The primary way in which AoS threatens the
ognizing sounds) — especially auditory sound agno- validity of language assessment is when problems
sia (inability to recognize nonlinguistic sounds) or speaking are interpreted as language problems. For
pure word deafness (inability to recognize spoken a person who has both AoS and aphasia, it can be
language in the face of good recognition of nonlin- especially challenging to sort out which aspects of
guistic sounds) (Burns, 2004). that person’s spoken production problems are due
Finally, consider whether the individual expe- to which underlying impairment (Knollman-Porter,
riences noise buildup, that is, increased difficulty 2008; Mumby et al., 2007).
with cognitive-linguistic tasks over time. It is said Following are a few strategies to help discern
to occur within sentences (such that the end of a AoS from a language problem:
long sentence may be especially difficult to under-
stand compared to the beginning) or within a spe- • If the individual is literate, look for a marked
cific activity, conversation, or clinical session (Porch, distinction in content expressed through
1967; Schuell, 1953, 1954; Schuell et al., 1964). speaking versus writing. A difference between
Although there is no consensus among aphasi- the two is a good indicator of AoS; a similar
ologists about the underlying nature of the phenom- level of expression is more likely across
enon of noise buildup, or even about whether it truly modalities in people with aphasia.
19. Problem-Solving Approaches to Differential Diagnosis and Confounding Factors   261

• Compare the production of automatic or example, items within a category named per minute
highly learned utterances with propositional and spoken language indices of average information
speech. Both a person with AoS and a person units expressed per unit of time may reflect, at least
with aphasia may speak better in an automatic partially, a motor speech as opposed to a language
speech task; however, the distinction may be deficit. Still, the distinction between the two is much
more marked in a person with AoS. simpler than with AoS and aphasia. Here are some
• Note articulatory groping behaviors. These strategies to distinguish dysarthria from aphasia:
are common in AoS but not in aphasia
without AoS. • Observe the individual’s muscular control
• Note whether there are inconsistencies and carry out an oral mechanism examination;
in articulatory errors. A person with AoS a primary distinguishing feature of people
typically has more of these relative to a person with dysarthria (with or without concomitant
with only aphasia. aphasia) is oral and/or facial asymmetry.
• Try testing articulation with longer and • Note semantic, syntactic, and pragmatic
increasingly complex utterances; people from errors; these are not likely in a person with
both groups tend to have more difficulty on dysarthria, but they are in a person with
longer and more phonologically complex aphasia.
sound sequences than on shorter, simpler • Note difficulty following directions; these,
ones, but the contrast may be more marked in too, are not likely in a person with dysarthria,
a person with AoS. but they are in a person with aphasia.
• Use reading and writing tasks to index
If an individual has little or no speech, be sure language reception and production abilities;
to have writing paper and pens or pencils handy. people with dysarthria and not aphasia
Also, yes/no cards, picture cards, image books, and perform relatively well through written and
like/dislike rating scales may be helpful for elicit- spoken modalities.
ing responses. Textbooks on motor speech disorders
typically provide protocols and suggested tasks for As with AoS, several language assessment bat-
diagnosis of AoS — and the differential diagnosis of teries include tasks designed to screen for dysarthria,
AoS in people with aphasia. Several aphasia batter- and most textbooks on motor speech disorders (e.g.,
ies include screening tools or tasks to help identify Duffy, 2013; Lowit & Kent, 2011) address specific
AoS. Separate assessment tools may also be used, for means of differential diagnosis of various types of
example, the Test of Oral and Limb Apraxia (TOLA; dysarthria. Tools developed to assess presence, type,
Helm-Estabrooks, 1992b) and the Apraxia Battery and severity of dysarthria include the Assessment
for Adults, Second Edition (ABA-2; Dabul, 2000). of Intelligibility of Dysarthric Speech (AIDS; York-
ston & Beukelman, 1984), the Frenchay Dysarthria
Dysarthria Assessment (Enderby & Palmer, 2008), the Dysarthia
Examination Battery (Drummond, 1993), and the
Dysarthria is an impairment of neuromuscular Quick Assessment for Dysarthria (Tanner & Culb-
innervation of the muscles involved in speech. It ertson, 1999).
results in slow, weak, and poorly coordinated speech
production. Most people with dysarthria due to Limb Apraxia
unilateral cortical lesions are still able to speak with
at least moderate intelligibility due to the bilateral Limb apraxia is a deficit in motor programming of
innervation of the articulatory musculature. Still, the arm, elbow, wrist, hand, or fingers for volitional
intelligibility may be affected. Dysarthria may affect movement. Limb apraxia has been associated with
the validity of language assessment in cases when two underlying processes: ideational apraxia (a
the clinician does not understand an individual’s problem generating a plan to carry out a purpose-
intended utterances. Also, given that speech may ful movement) and ideomotor apraxia (a problem
be slower, scores may be lower for timed tasks. For executing the plan); some argue that these two
262  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

constructs are not actually separable (Duffy, 1974). (such as making a hitchhiking gesture) (Pazzaglia
Heilman and Gonzalez Rothi’s (2003) dual-component et al., 2008). Theories regarding differences among
model of limb apraxia suggests a mix of conceptual manifestations of limb apraxia and underlying asso-
and preparatory aspects involving perception and ciated neuropathologies continue to be developed
movement. and tested through functional neuroimaging and
People with limb apraxia may perform reflexive behavioral studies (Foundas, 2013; Gonzalez Rothi
or highly rote movements using the same body parts & Heilman, 2014).
but have difficulty when they try to do so intention-
ally. For example, if you approach a North American Paralysis or Paresis
person with limb apraxia with your arm extended
as if to shake their hand, they may easily extend a Many people with neurogenic language disorders
hand to shake yours. This is because handshaking is have right-sided paralysis or hemiparesis. Given
so common in their culture that it happens without that most are right-handed, use of the dominant
planning or a great deal of attention. However, when hand may be slowed or poorly coordinated. Also,
you ask them to show you the arm and hand move- for most people, using the dominant left hand in lieu
ment required for a handshake, they may be unable of the right results in slower and less coordinated
to do so. Limb apraxia is likely to be underreported use of the hand and arm for writing, typing, but-
because it is overlooked in comparison to other ton pressing, drawing, and gesturing. Paralysis or
motor symptoms, especially paresis or paralysis of paresis of the body in general may affect the ability
the limbs (Buchmann & Randerath, 2017; Pazzaglia to demonstrate comprehension of commands. Thus,
et al., 2008). It may also be mistaken for poor coordi- paralysis and paresis may affect the speed and accu-
nation of the nondominant but more functional arm racy of responses in a way that affects the validity of
or hand in a person whose has paralysis or paresis language test results that depend on such control.
on the side contralateral to the stroke. Often, problems of muscular innervation of the face,
Limb apraxia may lead to incorrect or unreliable hands, or body are visibly noticeable. Ideally, the
pointing, gesture, responses to commands, and use SLP will have access to records of a full neurological
of some response modes, such as button pressing or examination performed by a neurologist. If not, it is
typing, during testing. Sequencing of multiple limb a good idea to engage in a basic screening for weak-
movements typically results in more errors than ness, including asking the person and significant
single movements (Neiman et al., 2000). To screen others directly about related symptoms.
for limb apraxia informally, engage the person you
are testing in a range of simple to complex panto- Other Motor Deficits
mime tasks and motor commands using the arms
and hands (Bartolo et al., 2008; Duffy, 1974; Nei- Additional motor-related symptoms may interfere
man et al., 1994; Rothi et al., 1997; Rothi & Heilman, with cognitive-linguistic assessment. Bradykinesia,
2014) or use one of the apraxia screening tests men- a condition of excess muscle tone, results in slowed
tioned earlier (i.e., ABA-2; Dabul, 2000, and TOLA; movements with reduced range of motion; it typi-
Helm-Estabrooks, 1992b). cally leads to problems with writing and manip-
Limb apraxia is distinct from the spontaneous ulating objects, and it may cause reduced facial
use of gestures to convey meaning during conver- expression. Bradykinesia is common in Parkinson’s
sation. In fact, a caution with more explicit testing is disease, and some TBI survivors have it. Ataxia, a
that gestures or pantomime movements elicited via problem of muscle coordination that may affect
commands and requests of imitation are not likely speaking and voluntary movements of the eyes and
to be as accurate as gestures occurring in natural limbs, is often associated with cerebellar lesions. It
conversation in people with language disorders and is common in multiple sclerosis and cerebral palsy
concomitant limb apraxia (Rose & Douglas, 2003). and may also be caused by stroke, brain injury, or
There may be differences in how an individual uses tumors. It may affect accuracy of pointing responses,
pantomime gestures for transitive object use (such command following, and object manipulation. Invol-
as hammering or toothbrushing) versus intransitive untary hyperkinetic movement disorders such as
19. Problem-Solving Approaches to Differential Diagnosis and Confounding Factors   263

chorea (rapid, repetitive, jerky movements), atheto- is the Reading Comprehension Battery for Aphasia
sis (slow, writhing movements), or ballismus (jerk- (RCBA; LaPointe & Horner, 2017); the Psycholinguis-
ing, flinging movements, especially of the arms and tic Assessments of Language Processing in Aphasia
legs) may also affect assessment performance. (PALPA; Kay et al., 1997) also has reading-specific
subtests. These tools and many subtests from lan-
guage batteries intended for people with neurolog-
Reading Problems ical disorders entail relatively easy material and do
not necessarily require the reader to have understood
Indexing of reading abilities at least to some degree what they have read to answer correctly some of
is typical of most screenings or assessment sessions. the corresponding comprehension questions (Nich-
Before evaluating what reading deficits a person olas et al., 1986). Several reading assessment tools
might have due to some sort of change in the brain, normed on children and young adults have better
be sure to stop and learn about their degree of liter- controls for reading level, means of error analysis,
acy prior to the onset of any acquired neurological and psychometric properties. Many of these norma-
condition. This is a critical step that far too many cli- tive data do not pertain to people with neurological
nicians ignore. Given their highly educated, highly disorders but still may be helpful if administered
literate backgrounds, clinical aphasiologists are at and interpreted appropriately.
risk of ethnocentric assumptions about another per-
son’s ability and desire to read. Keep in mind that
a person who is unable to read may not have ever Dysgraphia and Other Writing Deficits
been able to read or to read well.
Reading is a potential assessment confound To the degree that written responses are relied upon
whenever we rely on written material for instructions for any aspect of assessment not directly intended to
or for evoking any type of communicative response index writing, they may confound assessment. Basic
when reading is not what is being assessed. In some writing abilities may be assessed using most general
cases, we may seek to learn generally whether a per- language assessment batteries listed and described
son can read, if there are acquired reading problems, in Chapter 20. For more in-depth assessment, there
and what are the life-affecting aspects of any reading are few tools explicitly developed for older adults, let
deficits. In other cases, when reading deficits are an alone adults with neurological impairments. Some
important concern to an individual and we are con- use spelling and expository writing subtests from
sidering treatment options specifically focused on writing batteries developed for use with younger
reading, we may delve more deeply into the nature people with and/or without disabilities.
of a person’s reading deficits. By analyzing perfor-
mance according to a cognitive processing model of
reading (Hillis & Caramazza, 1992), we may con- Problems of Awareness and Arousal
sider where in the reading process there could be
a breakdown. Arousal (also called alertness or vigilance) and
Almost all general language assessment bat- awareness are components of the construct of con-
teries include reading components, and additional sciousness. It can be extremely challenging to dis-
reading tests enable more detailed assessment to cern the influence of potentially altered states of
help determine severity of functional reading defi- consciousness on cognitive and linguistic abilities.
cits, whether a person may have pure alexia or alexia Consciousness is indexed on a continuum rather
with agraphia, and whether reading challenges may than in terms of a present/not present dichotomy
be due to grapheme-to-phoneme (letter to sound) (Bayne et al., 2016). Consciousness states include
conversion, lexical-sematic access, or other aspects coma (no arousal or awareness), vegetative state
involved in reading aloud or copying what one has (arousal without awareness, also called unrespon-
read. Some commonly recommended tools are listed sive wakefulness syndrome), deep sleep, para-
and described in Chapter 20. One in particular that doxical sleep (rapid eye movement [REM] sleep),
was designed for use with people who have aphasia anesthesia, minimally conscious state (arousal with
264  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

fluctuating awareness), and wakefulness (Gosseries body’s voluntary muscles (with the exception of
et al., 2011). The boundaries between these states certain types of eye movement). She describes her
are often unclear, and transitions from one to the experience being hospitalized without recognition
next are often unnoticeable through mere obser- of her intact abilities for 6 years until, finally, an SLP
vation. Potential stages that follow a coma due to discovered that she was alert, highly intelligent, and
stroke or brain injury are shown schematically in had no language disability whatsoever.
Figure 19–6. The SLP is not likely to take the lead in differ-
Signs of consciousness may easily be missed. ential diagnosis of consciousness states; still, it is
Further complicating this situation is that people important that they be involved as a critically think-
who are motorically unresponsive may be per- ing member of a rehabilitation team. The fact that
ceived as cognitively and linguistically incompetent coma and persistent vegetative state are frequently
when in fact they merely lack a means of expressing misdiagnosed (estimated at 37% to 42% of the time;
their intact cognitive and linguistic abilities. There Andrews et al., 1996; Schnakers et al., 2009; Wade,
are heart-wrenching cases of people without cogni- 2018) suggests that there is a dire need for all rehabili-
tive-linguistic disorders who remained institution- tation team members to engage in astute observation,
alized for years as if they were vegetative, without inquisitiveness, and advocacy for thorough assess-
appropriate stimulation, socialization, rehabilitation, ment even for people who are not on their caseload.
information, and inclusion in daily activities and There is no universally accepted technique or
decision-making, and without assistive technology tool for indexing levels of consciousness, and diag-
that would have enabled them to communicate. An noses are often made based on behavioral obser-
example is described in an autobiographical account vation. Standardized assessments help to improve
by Tavalaro (1997), who had locked-in syndrome, validity and reliability. Differential diagnosis includes
a condition caused by a brainstem-level stroke or neuroimaging (e.g., computed tomography, mag-
injury and resulting in complete paralysis of the netic resonance imaging, regional cerebral blood

Figure 19–6. Potential consciousness conditions after stroke or brain injury. Sources: Gosseries et al.,
2011; Laureys et al., 2004.
19. Problem-Solving Approaches to Differential Diagnosis and Confounding Factors   265

flow, positron emission tomography), electroen- clinical contexts, there is one particular scale of choice
cephalography, event-related potential, behavioral established for agency or facility-wide use.
observation, and behavioral testing for reflexes
and responses to commands (hand squeeze, eye
blink, eye movement for those who are not other- Attention Problems
wise overtly responsive). Repeated screenings for
responsiveness at each translational stage of recov- As we reviewed in detail in Chapter 19, attention
ery should be implemented because of the high and the ability to allocate it efficiently during the
potential for marked inconsistencies. Once any con- processing of linguistic and nonlinguistic informa-
sistent pattern of responding or means of eliciting tion is often a fundamental consideration in charac-
behavioral responses has been identified, the role of terizing language abilities. Attention is essential to
the SLP is critical, not only for direct clinical inter- all communication and learning tasks, so it affects
vention but also for consultation regarding ethical virtually all aspects of assessment and treatment.
issues and decision-making capacity. It is important that the SLP be aware of the myriad
The Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS; Teasdale & Jen- types or manifestations of attention and attention
nett, 1974), summarized in Box 19–1, is, worldwide, deficits, and consider whether there are particular
the most widely used index applied to people in and challenges with any particular aspects of attention.
emerging from coma. Individuals are scored on a scale The Test of Everyday Attention (TEA; Ridgeway
of 3 to 15 points for eye, verbal, and motor behaviors. et al., 1994) is an example of a published battery that
Several other coma scales are in use as well. In most includes tasks to index various forms of attention:

Box
19–1 Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS)

The GCS is scored between 3 and 15, with 3 being the worst and 15 the best. It
is composed of three parameters: best eye response, best verbal response, and
best motor response, as follows:

Best eye response (4) Best motor response (6)


1. No eye opening 1. No motor response
2. Eye opening to pain 2. Extension to pain
3. Eye opening to verbal command 3. Flexion to pain
4. Eyes open spontaneously 4. Withdrawal from pain
5. Localizing pain
Best verbal response (5) 6. Obeys commands
1. No verbal response
2. Incomprehensible sounds
3. Inappropriate words
4. Confused
5. Oriented

Note that the phrase “GCS of 11” is essentially meaningless, and it is import-
ant to break the score down into its components, such as E3V3M5 = GCS 11.
A Coma Score of 13 or higher correlates with a mild brain injury, 9 to 12 a
moderate injury, and 8 or less a severe brain injury.

Source: Teasdale & Jennett, 1974.


266  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

focused or selective attention, sustained attention, • comparing reports of deficits from the
attention allocation or divided attention, and atten- individual being assessed with those of
tion switching. significant others
Unfortunately, as described in Chapter 19, there • comparing the individual’s prediction of how
are serious methodological challenges associated they will perform on a given task with actual
with indexing attention allocation in people with performance
acquired neurogenic language disorders. Even when • analyzing error detection and correction
using published batteries, the clinician must reflect • directly observing interactions with the
critically on what deficits other than attention might individual
influence results. Also, it is important to observe
how well the person being assessed manages atten- Factors that potentially confound self-report
tion resources during everyday communication and of deficits in general are the stigma an individual
problem-solving tasks in naturalistic contexts. may feel about the deficit and the fear of losing inde-
pendence, such as driving privileges or control of
finances. Potential confounds of reports from signif-
Lack of Awareness of Deficits icant others include personal biases and resistance to
noticing changes in self-awareness over time (Bach
A lack of awareness and unwillingness to consider & David, 2006). Potential confounds in comparing
the importance of a deficit are executive function predicted with actual task performance include the
deficits common in the clinical syndromes addressed possible lack of relevance of such tasks to real-world
by SLPs. Lack of awareness may be caused by cog- functioning (Schlund, 1999).
nitive impairments that limit self-reflection, psycho- Sohlberg (2000) outlines important assessment
logical reactions to changes in function, and organic items relevant to self-awareness, including the fol-
changes in brain regions involved in awareness lowing:
(Sohlberg, 2000). Deficits in general self-awareness
and the ability to reflect on and have insights about • whether the individual knows about their
one’s own condition are associated with prefrontal strengths and weaknesses
lesions, especially in TBI survivors and people with • whether any denial of deficits appears to be
some forms of dementia. More specific forms of anos- organically based, psychologically based, or
agnosia are associated with damage in other areas of both
the brain. For example, people with left hemisphere • whether the individual engages in
lesions in the superior temporal lobe often appear compensatory or self-corrective behaviors
unaware of or not bothered by their language defi- • what the consequences of the lack of
cits. Symptoms of failing to notice or use one or more awareness might be
contralateral limbs, denial of hemiplegia or of the
severity of its impact, and denial that one’s limbs are Drawing attention to limitations through dis-
truly one’s own tend to be associated with parietal cussion or demonstration typically has little influ-
lesions (Myers, 1999; Sohlberg, 2000). Anosognosia is ence; brief attempts to do so may be helpful in terms
much more prevalent in right than left brain injuries. of getting a notion of how strong any associated
A person with anosognosia, who denies or is resistance to treatment may be.
unaware of any particular impairment, may not be
particularly motivated to participate in the assess-
ment process or in treatment. Thus, it is an important Executive Function Deficits
characteristic to tune into, as it is highly relevant to
the communication rehabilitation process. Sohlberg In addition to lack of awareness of deficits, other
(2000) recommends the following means of indexing executive function deficits may negatively affect the
awareness of deficits: validity of cognitive-linguistic assessment by mask-
ing intact cognitive-linguistic abilities, interfering
• having the person describe their own abilities with compliance during assessment tasks, restrict-
and disabilities ing assessment time and access due to problematic
19. Problem-Solving Approaches to Differential Diagnosis and Confounding Factors   267

behavior, and distracting clinicians such that key person’s receptive and expressive abilities are not as
communication strengths may go unappreciated. strong as they actually are. Excellent pragmatic skills
Indices of executive functioning have been shown may also be deceiving. For example, people with
to independently influence independence in activi- word-finding deficits often find clever ways to cover
ties of daily living in older people with and without them up through astute circumlocution and by redi-
neurological disorders (Royall et al., 2005). recting conversations. People with mild cognitive
The complexity and diversity of constructs impairment are often adept at redirecting conversa-
subsumed under the broad term executive function tional topics and engaging in lively social banter in a
(planning, initiative, problem-solving, judgment, way that makes their cognitive-linguistic deficits less
organization, sequencing, inhibition, cognitive flex- apparent. Means of conversational analysis focused
ibility, self-monitoring, self-reflection, etc.) make it on pragmatic behavior are discussed in Chapter 21.
difficult to pinpoint just what is meant when we refer Repeated observation in conversational contexts and
to executive function impairments. It is important, gaining insight through caregivers and family mem-
then, to be clear about areas of performance about bers help clarify the influence of pragmatic abilities
which we have particular assessment hypotheses or on specific cognitive-linguistic abilities.
questions, to choose assessment stimuli and methods
that capture constructs related to those areas, and to
present results in light of those particular constructs. Memory Problems
The Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System
(D-KEFS; Delis et al., 2001) enables indexing of a Memory deficits are key potential sequelae of RBI,
variety of executive function constructs. The Exec- TBI, and stroke and are also inherent characteris-
utive Interview (Royall et al., 1992) is a screening tics of varied forms of dementia. Numerous assess-
instrument designed to assess executive functions ment batteries include subtests created to index
in adults in varied types of living arrangements memory relative to the timing of onset (retrograde
(nursing homes, retirement communities, dementia and anterograde), the duration for which content is
units, and private family homes). It has been shown remembered (working memory, short-term memory,
to be sensitive to disruptive and other problematic long-term memory), the nature of what is remem-
behaviors. Additional assessment tools include the bered (e.g., episodic or semantic), and the means
Behavioral Assessment of Dysexecutive Syndrome of indexing memory (e.g., recall or recognition).
(BADS; Ufer & Wilson, 2000) and subtests of most Memory may be assessed in any relevant modal-
batteries developed for survivors of TBI and RBI. ities; it is most commonly examined in visual and
Given that actual executive performance is verbal modalities but may also be examined in tac-
highly dependent on the nature of the tasks, stim- tile and olfactory modalities. As with intelligence
uli, assessment environments, and ecological valid- tests, even when tests are said to be nonverbal, this
ity of assessment tools and methods, it is important does not mean that people who take them do not
to scrutinize test results carefully and to observe use verbal strategies to accomplish supposedly non-
executive functioning in a variety of structured and verbal tasks.
unstructured everyday tasks over time. Just as exec-
utive function deficits may confound assessment
of other cognitive and linguistic constructs, so may Other Concomitant Cognitive
other cognitive and linguistic deficits confound the and Linguistic Deficits
assessment of executive functions.
Even aspects of language impairment may interfere
with assessment of other cognitive and linguistic
Pragmatic Deficits abilities. For example, if a task requires comprehen-
sion of complex instructions, then not being able
Pragmatic abilities are closely tied to executive func- to understand those instructions could confound
tion abilities. They may affect the validity of assess- responses to the task when it is administered to
ment of other constructs in a variety of ways. Poor people with comprehension impairments. In fact,
social use of language may make it appear that a people without cognitive-linguistic disorders often
268  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

experience challenges with comprehending task


Box
instructions (Keren & Willemsen, 2009). Likewise, if 19–2 American Psychiatric Association
a particular task is cognitively demanding, for exam- Criteria for Depression
ple, requiring a long bout of sustained attention or
demanding working memory allocation that exceeds The American Psychiatric Association (APA,
a person’s capacity, then the individual’s true com- 2013) defines depression as a low mood for a
petence according to any other construct the clini- period of at least 2 weeks, entailing any of the
cian is attempting to evaluate may be masked. following symptoms:

• Low mood or irritability for most of the


Depression and Other Mood Disorders day, almost every day, as reported by the
individual or others
Almost everyone with a neurogenic loss of commu- • Decreased interest or pleasure in
nication ability, regardless of etiology and regardless everyday activities most of the day,
of site of lesion, experiences depression. The Ameri- almost every day
can Psychiatric Association (APA) criteria for clinical • 5% change in weight or change in
diagnosis of depression are given in Box 19–2. Symp- appetite
toms of depression also include any of the following: • Sleep disturbance (insomnia or
persistent sadness, frequent crying; a sense of help- hypersomnia)
lessness, hopelessness, anxiousness, or empty feel- • Slowed or agitated psychomotor activity
ings; social withdrawal; and associated aches, pains, • Fatigue or reduced energy
headaches, and digestive problems that do not ease • Feelings of worthlessness or
with treatment (National Stroke Association, 2006). inappropriate guilt
A person with a neurogenic language disorder • Difficulty concentrating or indecisiveness
may have depression for any or all of the following • Suicidal thoughts or planning
reasons:
Having at least five of the nine symptoms listed
• continuation of preexisting depression the is considered major depression, while having
individual had before the stroke or brain fewer than five is considered minor depression.
injury (which may be linked to multiple
causes, such as changes in life circumstance,
illness, loss of a loved one, and genetics) ders may be confounding factors in the assessment
• depression caused by neurochemical and of depression and other mood disorders. Sambunaris
structural changes in neuronal functioning and Hyde (1994) illustrate this point through two
• situational depression (Lyon, 1998) or grief case studies of people with aphasia whose commu-
response (Währborg, 1991) over the loss of nication-based symptoms led to erroneous diagnoses
abilities and independence, and changes in of psychotic disorders. A case report on a similar sit-
relationships and identity (Patterson, 2002) uation is described by Owolabi and Yakasai (2012).
• depression exacerbated by coping challenges It is often difficult to identify depression, and
related to changes in life participation it may be impossible to understand its nature for a
given person. This is because (a) there are so many
Depression may be a confounding factor in possible causes underlying it, any of which may
assessment in that a lack of responsiveness may be occur in combination; (b) communication and cog-
inappropriately perceived as an inability to respond, nitive impairments can limit expression of feelings
and associated difficulties concentrating may affect and/or limit capacity for judgment and personal
the ability to respond during formal and informal reflection; and (c) many people experiencing depres-
assessment. Additionally, a lack of motivation to sion do not feel comfortable talking about it even if
participate and do one’s best may negatively affect they are capable of doing so, due to guilt, shame,
assessment results. Likewise, communication disor- embarrassment, fear of judgment, and associated
19. Problem-Solving Approaches to Differential Diagnosis and Confounding Factors   269

stigma. In addition, sometimes diagnosis is based mood face with a word at the bottom. Respondents
on caregiver report and observation, the validity of mark the spot on the line that corresponds to how
which may be in question. they feel. The VAMS-Revised (VMS-R; Kontou et al.,
Rating scales are commonly used to learn about 2012) is a newer version that positions the more pos-
the mood of people with language disorders. One itive aspects of moods (sometimes “neutral” and
specifically developed for people with aphasia is the sometimes positive descriptors such as “happy” or
Stroke Aphasic Depression Questionnaire (SADQ-H; “energetic”) consistently at the top of the vertical
Lincoln et al., 2000). It is designed to be completed line and more negative aspects (sometimes “neu-
through caregiver (including nursing staff) input tral” and sometimes negative descriptors such as
and may be supplemented with input from the per- “sad” or “angry”) consistently at the bottom. The
son assessed. It is a revised version of the original authors of both versions report high internal consis-
21-item SADQ (Sutcliffe & Lincoln, 1998), which had tency and high concurrent validity with other mood
fewer rating-scale response options. Bennett et al. indexes. A related analog scale created expressly for
(2006) tested a briefer 10-item version and demon- people with aphasia is the Visual Analogue Self-​
strated its concurrent validity with the Hospital Esteem Scale (VASES; Brumfitt & Sheeran, 1999). An
Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS; Zigmond & example of a basic rating scale that could be applied
Snaith, 1983). Cobley et al. (2012) report internal con- in varied contexts is shown in Figure 19–7. Brum-
sistency and concurrent validity of the SADQH-10 fitt (2009) provides an overview of a wide range of
for people with aphasia due to stroke. Hacker et al. assessment instruments for indexing depression,
(2009) demonstrated its concurrent validity with the mood, self-esteem, and well-being. Laures-Gore
Brief Assessment Schedule Depression Cards (BAS- et al. (2017) provide evidence that perceived stress
DEC; Adshead et al., 1992). may be a good predictor of depressive symptoms in
A disadvantage of indexing the self-ratings of stroke survivors. In any case, whether or not there
people with language disorders themselves, rather is an official diagnosis of depression or even a tran-
than through caregivers, is that they are highly lin- sient low mood, it is critical that the SLP consider
guistically loaded (typically composed of words how strong the influence of low mood may be on the
and no images; Brumfitt & Sheeran, 1999; Patterson, validity and reliability of assessment results.
2002). Of course, a disadvantage in having others
provide ratings is that the degree to which their rat-
ings reflect the actual feelings of the person being Anxiety
assessed is usually unknown. Conferring with a
psychiatrist, psychologist, and/or physiatrist who Many people with acquired neurogenic disorders
understands the nature of acquired language dis- experience anxiety, which differs from depression.
orders can be helpful; still, it does not necessarily Poststroke anxiety has been attributed to medical
ensure the validity of formal or informal assessments factors (e.g., higher incidence in people with epi-
of mood in people with communication disorders. lepsy, migraine, and frontal lobe lesions), insomnia,
The assistance of others, be they relatives, friends, and depression (Leppävuori et al., 2003). Anxiety
or professionals, increases the feasibility of assessing symptoms include ongoing worry, fear, restlessness,
mood but does not ensure validity (Laska et al., 2007). and irritability; low energy levels; poor concentra-
Visual analog scales have been created to cir- tion; muscle tension; panicky feelings; and physical
cumvent the linguistic load of depression scales symptoms of headache, shaking, or feeling sick in
based on self-ratings. In the Visual Analog Mood the stomach. Any one of these symptoms may affect
Scales (VAMS; Stern, 1997), instead of verbal labels, the reliability and validity of assessment and may be
iconic or cartoon-like faces are used to indicate exacerbated by the assessment process; thus, these
self-ratings according to each of eight moods, along symptoms should be considered when scheduling
with verbal labels. The moods indexed are afraid, and managing assessment activities. Also, each
confused, sad, angry, energetic, tired, happy, and should be considered as a potentially confounding
tense. Each scale has a neutral face and accompany- factor when interpreting assessment results (Cahana-​
ing label “neutral” at the top of a vertical line and a Amitay et al., 2011).
270  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

inconsistent with the appropriate degree of emotion


or even the appropriate emotion for a given situa-
tion. For example, a person may cry when welcome
visitors arrive or laugh when someone is express-
ing sadness over a death in the family. Emotional
lability is a potential confound in assessment in that
the individual, the clinician, and anyone else present
may be distracted from the assessment process. As
you work with a person, if you notice sudden cry-
ing or an emotional outburst that does not appear to
have been evoked by a corresponding event or situ-
ation, then it may be best to continue, perhaps men-
tioning or showing that you notice the feelings being
expressed but not focusing on them at the expense of
continuing with the tasks or activities at hand. If the
individual and any significant others present seem
not to know about emotional lability, this may be
a good occasion to help educate and counsel them
about the condition and things they might do to
lessen its impacts (see Chapter 27).

Other Challenges to Health and Well-Being

Any influence on mood, perception, and behavior


may confound cognitive-linguistic behavior. Think
about how painkillers, sleep aids, and antidepres-
sants, as well as fatigue and hunger, might affect a
person’s ability to attend and think clearly during
assessment (Chaumet et al., 2008). Imagine taking
a language test while you are in severe pain from
a decubitus ulcer, migraine headache, or surgical
Figure 19–7. A nonverbal visual analog scale that can
be adapted for personalized ratings of person-centered
incision — examples of conditions common among
factors. A vertical scale is recommended for people people served by clinical aphasiologists.
with neurogenic challenges due to potential confounds
with visual fields and visual attention (especially hemi­
neglect). Redundancy through color, images, and num- How Does a Process Analysis
bers, plus live cues given by the clinician, support Approach to Assessment Help Address
person-centered responses. Image credit: Taylor Reeves.
A full-color version of this figure can be found in the
Potentially Confounding Factors?
Color Insert.
A process analysis approach is one in which we con-
sider the ability, skill, or construct we wish to assess
in light of the tasks used to assess it and analyze
Emotional Lability the other aspects of performance that may also be
reflected in what is assessed. A simple scheme for
Emotional lability, also called pathologic lability these process analysis components is shown in Fig-
and pseudobulbar affect (PBA), causes people with ure 19–8. The process (middle) component depicted
brain injury to exhibit emotional reactions that are is the focus as we analyze the skills and abilities we
19. Problem-Solving Approaches to Differential Diagnosis and Confounding Factors   271

Figure 19–8. Components of the process analysis approach to assessment.

actually tap into when we attempt to index an ability Adding time pressure such that the task must
according to any particular construct. be completed within a brief time frame may increase
For example, we may wish to index auditory the difficulty at each level of processing. Although
comprehension (the ability of interest) through visual processing is not a part of the construct ide-
a pointing response (what is assessed) during a ally measured in the multiple-choice task, the task
multiple-choice task. The task entails our asking the clearly entails several aspects of visual processing;
person to point to an image that best corresponds to each of those aspects could be a confounding fac-
a word, phrase, or sentence. We present the verbal tor when assessing auditory comprehension. Note,
stimulus and note whether they point to the correct too, that once a target image is identified and local-
image. If they do well on this task across repeated ized, the individual must plan and execute a point-
exposures to various types of verbal stimuli, we ing response. Thus, any deficits that affect motor
might deduce with confidence that they understand planning, neuromuscular control, or coordination
those types of verbal stimuli. If they do not do well, could affect the response and thus be confound-
however, what do we know? To answer that ques- ing factors. The process analysis approach helps us
tion, we could first analyze the components of the to conceptualize and be mindful of such potential
task and all of the skills and abilities that are required confounds. During comprehension assessment, an
to perform it. One way of analyzing the task is illus- incorrect response is typically attributed to a com-
trated in Figure 19–9. prehension deficit.
To point to an image corresponding to the ver- Let’s return to the earlier question: What do we
bal stimulus, the individual must have sufficient know about a person’s comprehension if they get
auditory and visual sensory processing abilities to an item wrong? Not much. In fact, there are many
perceive accurately the visual and auditory stimuli reasons for an incorrect response that have nothing
at each level of processing. Deficits in visual acu- to do with comprehension. Intact comprehension
ity, visual attention, color vision, visual integration, abilities are easily underestimated and unappreci-
visual memory, visual search, and eye-hand coor- ated (Hallowell, 2012a; Hallowell et al., 2002). This
dination could all influence the response. Locat- is especially problematic, considering the great fre-
ing the target among foils requires processing of quency with which multiple-choice testing is used to
spatial relationships, exerting greater demands on assess linguistic comprehension.
processing capacity than simple target detection. Throughout this chapter, we have explored
Furthermore, the efficiency and accuracy of visual numerous assessment challenges and accompany-
perceptual processing and also of visual search may ing solutions to address them. Although myriad
be influenced by stimulus complexity, the degree of published tests are available as tools for assessment,
similarity between the target and the foils, the num- all fall short without keen clinical problem-solving
ber of colors in a display, and the number of images strategies and expert judgment. Let’s keep this in
presented (Heuer & Hallowell, 2007, 2009; Heuer mind as we proceed to examining published assess-
et al., 2017). ment tools in the upcoming chapter.
272  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Figure 19–9. Process analysis for a multiple-choice auditory comprehension task. A full-color version of this figure
can be found in the Color Insert.
19. Problem-Solving Approaches to Differential Diagnosis and Confounding Factors   273

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter f. Dysarthria


that are new to you or that you have not yet g. Limb apraxia
mastered. h. Paralysis or paresis
2. For each of the following constructs, i. Visuoconstructive disability
describe (1) how it may confound cognitive- j. Reading problems
linguistic assessment results in a person with k. Dysgraphia and other deficits
a neurogenic language disorder, and (2) how l. Problems of awareness and arousal
you might control for it as you strive to m. Attention problems
achieve a valid and reliable assessment: n. Lack of awareness of deficits
a. Age o. Executive function deficits
b. Intelligence, literacy, and education p. Pragmatic deficits
c. Visual problems q. Memory problems
i. Visual acuity deficits r. Depression and other mood disorders
ii. Visual field deficits s. Anxiety
iii. Color vision deficits t. Emotional liability
iv. Ocular motor problems
v. Visual neglect
d. Hearing problems There are additional teaching and learning activi-
www
e. Apraxia of speech ties on the companion website.
CHAPTER
20
Tests, Scales, and Screening Instruments

In the previous chapters in this section, we empha- • the quality of any given tool under
sized that clinicians, not tests, determine diagnoses consideration
and that assessment processes depend on much • our own preferences and preferred theoretical
more than the use of published tests, scales, and frameworks
screening instruments. Still, it is important for the • the practicality of using a particular tool
excellent clinical aphasiologist to be knowledgeable
about available assessment tools and how to select Each of these five categories pertains to a relevant
which ones to use in a given context and with given query highlighted here.
individuals. In this chapter, we review important fac-
tors in selecting and evaluating assessment instru-
ments. Substantial information about available tools What Is the Reason for Your Assessment?
for use with people who have acquired neurogenic
language disorders is provided. Do You Need to Determine a Baseline
After reading and reflecting on the content in
and Detect Changes Over Time?
this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in
your own words, the following queries: If so, it is best that you select an instrument that has
a sampling of many similar items within a domain.
1. What are the most important factors in
selecting an assessment instrument? Do You Wish to Determine Receptive
2. What are the most important factors in
and Expressive Strengths and Weaknesses
evaluating assessment instruments?
3. What assessment tools are available?
Across Spoken and Written Modalities
in a Variety of Language Domains?
If so, you want a tool that taps items of varied diffi-
What Are the Most Important Factors in culty levels with several types of stimuli and tasks
(Goodglass et al., 2001). Typical aphasia batteries,
Selecting an Assessment Instrument?
for example, include items that help assess lan-
guage abilities and impairments in auditory com-
The most critical factors that influence our selection prehension, reading, naming, spontaneous speech,
of assessment tools vary according to five categories: automatic speech, repetition, and writing. In Chap-
ter 18, we reviewed tasks typically used to index per-
• the reason we are carrying out a given formance in each of these areas (Table 18–1). Some
assessment tools are designed to index a specific domain, such
• the nature of the individual being assessed as reading or auditory comprehension.

275
276  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Do You Wish to Index Nonverbal Do You Wish to Index Aspects Such as Life
Aspects of Cognition? Participation, Social Engagement, Self-Esteem,
Coping Strategies, and Quality of Life?
There is ample evidence that nonlinguistic defi-
cits exacerbate neurogenic language impairments. I hope you do. Be sure to consider strategic ways
Some aphasia batteries and most traumatic brain of doing so. Most tools and batteries addressing
injury (TBI) and right hemisphere syndrome bat- specific impairment-level cognitive and linguistic
teries include subtests or screening tools to index deficits do not include substantial means of index-
attention, memory, and executive skills, as you see ing such constructs that are vital to life participation.
by perusing the tables of assessment instruments Examples of tools that may be used for this purpose
(Tables 20–1 through 20–7, at the end of this chapter). are the Assessment for Living with Aphasia (ALA)
There are also many means of testing such abilities (Kagan et al., 2011), Life Interests and Values Cards
using separate instruments. (Haley et al., 2010), and the Stroke and Aphasia
Quality of Life Scale (SAQOL-39; Hilari et al., 2003).
Several additional examples are given in Tables 20–3
Do You Wish to Index Motor Speech and 20–7. If your assessments do not address criti-
in Addition to Language Abilities? cal factors that influence life participation, then you
will not be able to substantiate clinical effectiveness.
Some batteries include means of assessing concomi-
In a systematic review of intervention for people
tant motor speech problems. Separate motor speech
with dementia, Heuer and Willer (2020) summarize
batteries are also available.
challenges in person-centered assessment that are
especially critical for that population. These include
If You Are Testing a Person With relying on:
Aphasia, Do You Wish to Determine
• proxy-reported measures, given that care
the Type of Aphasia They Have?
partners tend to rate well-being as worse than
If so, you will want an aphasia battery that provides their partners with dementia;
such a diagnostic profile. For example, the Boston • inventories of adverse behaviors, which are
Diagnostic Aphasia Examination–3 (BDAE-3; Good- based on the false assumption that behaviors
glass et al., 2001); the Western Aphasia Battery, that are bothersome to caregivers and health
Revised (WAB-R; Kertesz, 2007); and the Aphasia care staff necessarily indicate the well being of
Diagnostic Profiles (ADP; Helm-Estabrooks, 1992a) the person being assessed; and
provide means of interpreting test results to assign • assessment of the person without assessing
diagnostic categories according to classical aphasia the physical, social, and attitudinal factors
subtypes as well as severity levels. The ADP sug- in the person’s environment that are
gests interpretation according to behavioral, error, fundamental to quality of life.
and alternative communication profiles. The WAB
provides separate indices, including an “aphasia Is Your Goal to Collect Data That Will Be
quotient” (based on oral language subtest scores), Used to Document Outcomes Across a Group of
a “language quotient” (based on speaking, reading, People (e.g., People Seen in a Certain Facility
and writing scores), and a “cortical quotient” (over- or Throughout a Multisite Organization)?
all cognitive-linguistic abilities in terms of the con-
structs represented through diverse subtests). Keep If so, it will be important to consider whether the tool
in mind that there are inconsistencies in how an indi- is appropriate for that purpose. Outcomes assess-
vidual’s aphasia may be classified through tests, and ment tools must be general enough to be applied to
that many people have symptoms of aphasia that do a wide range of people with varied types of impair-
not fit any specific typification, as discussed in detail ments and associated etiologies. Some rehabilitation
in Chapter 10. outcomes measures are designed to be applied across
20. Tests, Scales, and Screening Instruments   277

disciplines, such as the Therapy Outcome Measure sia impacts on everyday living. More recently, and
(TOM; Enderby & John, 2015), in common usage in more specific to aphasia, Kagan et al. (2011) and
the United Kingdom, or the Australian Therapy Out- Simmons-Mackie et al. (2014) developed the Assess-
come Measure (AusTOM; Perry et al., 2004), adapted ment for Living with Aphasia (ALA), and Hula et al.
for use in Australia. Both of those tools enable mon- (2015) developed the Aphasia Communication Out-
itoring of changes based on International Classifi- come Measure (ACOM), both based on self-report
cation of Functioning, Disability, and Health (ICF) of people with aphasia. Tools that are more specific
constructs of impairment, activity, participation, and to particular disorders hold promise for increas-
well-being. ing consistency across research studies reporting
ASHA FACS and the related Functional Com- outcomes associated with specific treatment meth-
munication Measure, a 7-point scaling system that ods (Wallace et al., 2014). An international group
speech-language pathologists (SLPs) may use for a of researchers (Wallace et al., 2019) developed a
similar purpose in medical and rehabilitation con- consensus statement on a set of tools to be used in
texts but with greater relevance to language, cogni- research on aphasia with the aim of developing more
tion, and swallowing (Frattali et al., 2017). The ASHA consistency across studies. Additional details about
FACS includes 43 items for rating cognitive-linguis- these and several other measures that may be used
tic abilities across four domains (see Table 20–4). to index treatment outcomes are given in Tables 20–1
Scoring according to qualitative dimensions such as through 20–7.
appropriateness, promptness, and adequacy is also
encouraged. The tool is not a panacea in terms of
needed indices for outcomes for people with neu- Who, Specifically, Is Being Assessed?
rogenic communication disorders. However, it is
more appropriate than other scores applied across Is the Tool in the Appropriate Language, and
disciplines other than SLP. A limitation for some Does It Account for Dialectic Differences?
SLPs is that training is required for valid and reli-
able scoring. Be sure to check the demographic data related to any
A tool specifically developed for outcomes norms you may be using for comparative purposes
assessment in speech-language pathology and audi- and consider whether they are appropriate for a
ology is ASHA’s National Outcomes Measurement given person. Consider the ecological validity of the
System (NOMS; American Speech-Language-Hear- stimuli in light of potential linguistic and cultural
ing Association, n.d.). The NOMS includes 15 Func- differences (see Chapter 18).
tional Communication Measures (FCMs) for tracking
changes in communication and swallowing abili- Is the Tool Normed and Standardized on a
ties. Cognitive-linguistic abilities may be indexed Sample of People With a Similar Condition?
by scales that address attention, memory, spoken
language comprehension, spoken language expres- For example, if you are assessing a person with TBI,
sion, reading, and writing. In addition to enabling does the tool include appropriate normative sam-
outcomes assessment for a given program, agency, ples of TBI survivors? Although particular items and
or organization, NOMS data are collected nationally tasks on an aphasia test may be of interest for the
in the United States. This enables participating agen- sake of performance description or criterion-based
cies to compare their data with national statistics. indices for a person with TBI or dementia, it is not
Other tools commonly used for programmatic appropriate to compare their scores to norms for
outcomes assessment are the Communicative Effec- the test unless that test has norms for TBI survivors.
tiveness Index (CETI; Lomas et al., 1989), which Some tools are developed with norms for certain
indexes communication strengths and weaknesses clinical populations by the tests’ authors and then
as perceived by family members, and the Com- are normed for additional populations by the same
munication Disability Profile (CDP; Swinburn & or different authors. Even when a particular test’s
Byng, 2006), which is based on self-report of apha- title suggests a certain clinical group, the test still
278  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

may have associated norms corresponding to other sion; the Philadelphia Naming Test (Roach et al.,
groups. If you wish to investigate what clinically 1996), the Action Naming Test (ANT; Obler & Albert,
relevant norms are available for any given tool, it is 1979), and Boston Naming Test (BNT; Goodglass
important to search the research literature, not just et al., 2001) for naming; the SOAP Test of Syntactic
the manual accompanying the published test. Complexity (Love & Oster, 2002) and Northwestern
Assessment of Verbs and Sentences (NAVS; Cho-
Is the Instrument Normed and Standardized on Reyes & Thompson, 2012) for syntax; and the Johns
a Sample of People With Similar Attributes? Hopkins University Dysgraphia Battery (Goodman
& Caramazza, 1985) for writing. The Psycholinguis-
For example, does the person you are assessing have tic Assessments of Language Processing in Aphasia
the same general age, educational level, socioeco- (PALPA; Kay et al., 1992) enables in-depth testing of
nomic status, cultural background, and ethnicity as many different aspects of receptive and expressive
others represented in the tool’s norms? All of these language processing, and any of these may be sepa-
factors have been shown to influence language test rately administered to index performance according
results (Lezak et al., 2004; Mitrushina et al., 2005). to a particular construct of interest.
It is also important that the standardization sample Keep in mind that just because a test is intended
include a clinical population with similar attributes, to index a particular construct does not mean it does
for example, people with a similar type of aphasia, so well or holistically. For example, the most pop-
head injury, or dementia, and people with a similar ular test of word finding across English-speaking
level of language impairment. countries (see Katz et al., 2000), the BNT, addresses
only nouns, not other important parts of speech, and
Does the Instrument Have Items at lacks recent detailed normative information, espe-
an Appropriate Difficulty Level? cially pertaining to older adults. Another example
pertains to the picture description tasks, common in
Some tools are too easy for people with mild lan- popular aphasia batteries; these often fall far short
guage impairments such that the tool is not helpful of capturing real-life expressive language abilities.
in identifying the challenges they are having. Some Also, the means of administering, scoring, and
tests, and some subtests in particular, are just too interpreting corresponding results are fraught with
difficult for some people with severe impairments. potential confounds.
Some tests include items with greater demands on
working memory, general knowledge, and general Are There Sufficient Means of Controlling
intelligence than others; if not accounted for, such for Potentially Confounding Factors?
demands may confound language assessment results.
Recall the myriad potential assessment confounds
we explored in Chapters 18 and 19. As many of those
Does It Provide an Appropriate Index of as possible should be controlled for in a language
the Constructs You Wish to Assess? assessment tool’s design and in test administration
to heighten assessment validity.
Language and cognitive batteries tend to enable
sampling of a wide array of communicative behav-
iors at varied levels of difficulty. Often, we wish to Does the Tool Allow for Alternative Response
learn about an individual’s abilities in a particular Modes in Cases Where Clients May Have
area in greater detail. Several tests developed to Trouble With Traditional Response Modes?
index specific language functions are listed among
the batteries in tables in this chapter. Examples are As discussed in the previous chapter, motor and
as follows: the Reading Comprehension Battery visual disabilities may impose serious threats to
for Aphasia-2 (RCBA-2; LaPointe & Horner, 1998) the assessment validity. Some tests allow varied
for indexing reading; the Revised Token Test (RTT; types of response modes, such as yes/no respond-
McNeil & Prescott, 1978) for auditory comprehen- ing through eye blink, button press, hand squeeze,
20. Tests, Scales, and Screening Instruments   279

or use of an alternative and augmentative commu- predictive), and face validity? Does it have good
nications device. For more complex tasks, such as sensitivity in that scores generated are likely to help
comprehension assessment using multiple-choice identify the impairment you are testing for, if it is
images, there may be no suitable response alterna- there? Does the tool have good specificity in that
tives given the way the tasks were designed. Some people who do not have a disorder or impairment
tests are designed to reduce reliance on visual and for which you are testing are not likely to score at a
motor abilities. An example is the Putney Auditory level that would suggest an impairment?
Comprehension Screening Test (Beaumont et al.,
2002). Eyetracking-based assessment methods are Is the Normative Group Substantial?
promising alternatives as well (Anjum & Hallowell,
2019; Hallowell, 2012a; Hallowell & Lansing, 2004; If there are not sufficient data to support valid and
Hallowell et al., 2002; Heuer et al., 2017). reliable test interpretation, then the use of norms is
not recommended. A good guideline for a minimally
sufficient normative sample is 100 people (Franzen,
Might Instructions and Tasks 2003).
Involved Confound Results?
Is the Normative Group Well Defined?
Many tools used for assessing abilities across a wide
array of domains require comprehension of instruc- If the test developers have not taken care to care-
tions as well as of verbal stimuli. When the intent is fully describe the clinical and control groups from
not to index comprehension but rather some other which normative data have been derived, it may
construct, the reliance on comprehension abilities not be possible to know if the norms are applicable
may invalidate responses. Likewise, reliance on to a person you are assessing. Consider the degree
speech and limb-motor abilities to demonstrate that factors such as age, gender, education level, pre-​
cognitive-linguistic abilities is often problematic. onset intelligence, socioeconomic status, literacy,
An example of a common tool used in many clin- and any concomitant disabilities are relevant to the
ical environments that is problematic in terms of constructs you wish to index.
linguistic load as well as motor requirements is the
Mini-Mental Are the Psychometric Strengths of a Subtest
State Examination, Second Edition (MMSE-2; Well Substantiated for Stand-Alone Use?
Folstein et al., 2010). For people with cognitive and
linguistic impairments, visual analog rating scales If you intend to use a subtest of a more compre-
may be used to minimize the influence of cogni- hensive battery to index a particular construct, it
tive-linguistic impairments on response validity. For is important to consider whether the psychometric
example, the Stroke Aphasia Depression Question- properties of that subtest have been verified.
naire (SADQ; Sutcliffe & Lincoln, 1998) has rating
scales with response selections consisting of faces Are There Two or More Forms of the Tool
that represent varied moods. See similar scale exam- With High Consistency Between Them?
ples depicted in Figure 19–7.
Having more than one form of a test, with all forms
having been shown to result in similar scores for a
What Is the Quality of a Given Tool? given person, reduces the likelihood that learning
during the first administration will improve scores
Be sure to evaluate the instrument according to the on a subsequent administration. Many of the most
psychometric properties summarized in Chapter 18. commonly used language tests do not have multiple
For example, does the tool have robust indices of forms. An example of one with two forms having
test-retest, inter-examiner, intra-examiner, and inter- strong test-retest validity is the Amsterdam Nijme-
nal reliability? Does it have strong evidence of con- gen Everyday Language Test (ANELT; Blomert et al.,
tent, construct, criterion (including concurrent and 1994). A greater proportion of tests for indexing
280  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

cognitive constructs has parallel forms. Examples Let’s keep in mind that the fact that a tool is
are the Test of Everyday Attention (TEA; Ridgeway published does not mean that it has been developed
et al., 1994), the Wechsler Memory Scale–III (WMS- well, with proper attention to test construction and
III; Wechsler, 2009), and the Rivermead Behavioral psychometric principles (Ivanova & Hallowell, 2013;
Memory Test–3 (RBMT-3; Wilson et al., 2008). Roberts, 2001). Psychometric indices that should
ideally be addressed were reviewed in Chapter 19.
Most, if not all, tests are lacking in some aspects of
Is It Up to Date and Appropriate test design and psychometric principles (see McCau-
in Terms of Content? ley & Swisher, 1984; Skenes & McCauley, 1985;
Spreen & Risser, 2003). Furthermore, many do not
Cognitive-linguistic assessment tools are ideally address potential confounds and inappropriateness
periodically updated to reflect current research in terms of cultural and linguistic diversity. When
findings, enhance validity and reliability, and (when selecting a test, it is the clinician’s responsibility to
applicable) improve standardization and related scrutinize such features, as they are fundamental
norms. If you do not have access to the most recent to clinical excellence.
version of a test, be sure to learn about what was
changed in a more recent version and consider that
information as you decide whether to use the one Does the Tool Complement Your
you have. Such information may also help you in Own Preferences and Preferred
advocating for the purchase of the latest version. In Theoretical Frameworks?
some cases, using an old version of a test leads to
violations of professional codes of ethics (e.g., stan-
dards regarding integrity, evidence-based practice, Does It Have a Framework for Conceptualizing
competence, and high-quality service; Jakubowitz & Language and Communication
Schill, 2008). That You Respect and Value?
Appropriateness of content is vitally important.
Although some have defended a swastika symbol For example, what you consider to be “functional”
in the Boston Assessment of Severe Aphasia (CITE) and most relevant to life participation will likely
as tolerable in light of the aim to stimulate a person influence your choice of a tool. The Porch Index of
with severe aphasia to respond, most of us would Communicative Ability–Revised (PICA-R; Porch,
not show such symbols as compassionate clinicians. 2001) was for a long time touted as a robust test
As Bernstein-Ellis et al. (2021) state, with a long-standing history in our field. At the same
time, it lacks a focus on spontaneous speech or dis-
It is our ethical charge to ensure that our diagnos-
course and does not, in and of itself, include refer-
tic tools are appropriate and valid for the popula-
ences to life participation and contextual supports.
tions we are working with. . . . This responsibility
In addition, many of its stimuli are outdated.
includes removing test items that may be cultur-
ally and racially insensitive — and even egregious ​ Is It Based on a Theoretical
— ​to our clients, so that we do not alienate and Model That You Favor?
cause further emotional distress to the people we
are trying to help. Some tests have very loose theoretical bases. Others
are based on certain models of language process-
Those authors refer to an image of a noose in the ing, functional communication, and life participa-
Boston Naming Test-2 (Kaplan et al., 2001). Pro-Ed, tion. Some of these are directly related to the first
the publisher, now recognizing how injurious that set of factors, pertaining to what you most want to
symbol can be, offers a replacement sticker of a boo- know. For example, when assessing a person with
merang, which can be requested for a fee by email- aphasia, if you believe that testing should include
ing a request to the publisher or by ordering one assessment of a range of specific linguistic abilities,
from their website. you might select a language-focused battery such as
20. Tests, Scales, and Screening Instruments   281

the BDAE-3. If you believe that associated cognitive-​ Do You Have Enough Time to
communicative deficits (e.g., use of gesture, arithme- Administer the Tool?
tic skills, visual neglect, and semantic and recognition
memory) are fundamental to the person’s assess- In busy clinical environments with high productivity
ment, you might choose the Comprehensive Apha- standards, clinicians often do not have time to admin-
sia Test (CAT; Swinburn et al., 2004); alternatively, ister an entire language test battery, let alone a series
you may choose to administer additional screenings of screenings and additional tools. What we desire
or tests, such as the Cognitive Linguistic Quick Test to do in terms of best practice is often not consonant
(CLQT; Helm-Estabrooks, 2001), in addition to an with the demands of our professional contexts.
aphasia battery. If you believe that assessing the
impact of language loss on psychosocial well-being Are the Instructions Clear and Easy to Follow?
(or perhaps of changes in mood or life participation
over time) is important, you may choose to prioritize Are there clear floor (basal) and ceiling rules and
your assessment time to include tools for indexing guidelines on the types and numbers of cues that
such constructs as mood (e.g., Kontou et al., 1997), are allowable? If needed, are normative data easily
self-esteem (e.g., Brumfitt & Sheeran, 1999), confi- found along with the instrument, or must those be
dence (Babbitt & Cherney, 2010), participation (see procured separately?
Eadie et al., 2006), or quality of life (e.g., Paul et al.,
2004; von Steinbüchel et al., 2010). Do You (or the Person to Be Administering
the Assessment) Have Sufficient Training to
Administer and Score a Particular Test?
How Practical Is the Tool
Under Consideration? Most assessment tools have scoring procedures that
are not difficult for a skilled clinician to implement,
Just because you may wish to use a tool does not provided the instructions are clear. Most batteries
mean it will be practical for you to do so. administered by clinicians use a variety of scoring
procedures. For example, the ADP includes 4-point
scale ratings, plus/minus scoring, frequency of
Are You Seeking a Test That Meets Certain correct information units, and phrase-length indi-
Standards for Billable Diagnostic Services? ces. The BDAE-3 incorporates plus/minus scoring,
5-point and 7-point rating scales, and frequency
In a health care business context, it is important that counts. Many tests provide guidance for converting
you comply with regulations. Be sure that the tool raw scores to percentile and z scores.
you document in a report is not one that will lead to Some tests, though, require substantial training
a denial of reimbursement. to ensure appropriate validity and reliability of scor-
ing. Examples are the PICA-R, for which a 40-hour
Is the Tool Available to You Physically training course has been recommended (Martin,
in the Context Where You Work or 1977; McNeil, Prescott, & Chang, 1975), and the RTT,
in Any Other Way Accessible? which uses a multidimensional scoring system mod-
eled after that of the PICA. PICA-R and RTT per-
Be sure to consider whether the instrument is phys- formance is scored according to a multidimensional
ically available to you. If not, has it been published scoring system (16-point system for PICA, 15-point
and is it in print? Can you afford to purchase it? The for RTT), both based on five dimensions of perfor-
authors of the Brisbane Evidence-Based Language mance: completeness, accuracy, promptness, respon-
Test have addressed these concerns with a language siveness, and efficiency. To the degree that training
test (with easily accessible reliability and validity requirements alone keep some clinicians from using
data and norms) available in five versions for free otherwise potentially valuable instruments, sim-
download (https://brisbanetest.org) (Rohde et al., pler scoring alternatives have been proposed and
2020). tested. An example is a method of correct/incorrect
282  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

scoring on every element of every command in the the use of such tools is that determining a stage or
RTT, accompanied by astute qualitative observation level of recovery does not help predict the extent or
(Odekar & Hallowell, 2005). Some tests have com- rate of expected recovery.
puterized scoring options. This may ease the burden
of scoring and scoring training but does not obviate
the chance of error in terms of data entry or response
What Are the Most Important Factors in
tracking, and certainly does not replace a clinician’s
expert administration, judgment, and interpretation.
Evaluating Assessment Instruments?
With more assessment tools available online for elec-
tronic administration, some vendors have imposed The factors that are important for selecting an assess-
training as a requirement prior to enabling access to ment instrument can be applied to evaluating them.
those tools. In some cases, this appears to be more As with tool selection, the way we evaluate any
about generating revenue than about enhancing given tool depends a great deal on our reasons for
clinical effectiveness. Consider the example of the assessment, the specific characteristics of the indi-
initiation of required training at a cost (and required viduals and groups we are serving, our theoretical
re-certification after 2 years) for most people wishing principles and values, and the sheer practicality for
to administer the Montreal Cognitive Assessment use in the context where we work. A review form
(MoCA; Nasreddine, 2021), a basic screening tool. for assessing assessment tools is given in Box 20–1.
For many of us who have administered this easy-to-
use tool for years, this seems blatantly opportunistic
(see Borson et al., 2019).
What Assessment Tools Are Available?

Do Others on Your Rehabilitation Team Aphasiologists working with speakers of English


Understand the Results You Report are highly privileged in terms of the number of stan-
and Your Interpretation of Them? dardized tests available in English. For the majority
of the world’s languages other than English, there
Some measures used by SLPs and neuropsycholo- are few standardized tests of aphasia and related
gists index constructs that are not easily understood disorders. Of those that do exist, most lack valid-
by those without related education and experience. ity and reliability statistics and do not meet other
Being able to convey results clearly so that others, important psychometric criteria. Many are transla-
including clients, family members, and other profes- tions of assessment batteries in English. Others are
sionals, may interpret them in ways that are relevant developed originally in the target language. Many
to everyday concerns is essential. are unpublished. Refer to Ivanova and Hallowell
An additional tool often used to share infor- (2013) for substantial details about aphasia tests in
mation across rehabilitation team members is the non-English languages and suggestions for devel-
Rancho Los Amigos Scale of Cognitive Functioning oping new tests in any language. See Rohde et al.
(Hagen et al., 1979; Malkmus & Stenderup, 1974). (2018) for their systematic review of language tests.
Intended for use with TBI survivors, it includes eight In the tables that follow, crucial information about
items that apply to varied types of daily functioning, English-language tests, scales, and screening instru-
with scoring based on observation of the person’s ments is presented. Of course, resources are already
responses to stimulation. An important caution in published and in progress in many other languages.
20. Tests, Scales, and Screening Instruments   283

Box
20–1 Review Form for Assessment Tools

Title of the test, author(s), and publisher:

1. Target population • Face validity?


• Is the tool intended for use with a person 7. Normative information
or group of people with whom you wish • What norms are available?
to use it? • What are the characteristics of the
• Are any norms available that are pertinent normative sample(s)? Do they match
to your clinical population(s) of interest? those of the client(s) whose abilities you
2. Purpose are assessing?
• Is there a clear operational definition of • Are aspects of gender, socioeconomic
what is being assessed? status, age, concomitant disorder, and/or
• Are specific aspects of language, culture controlled for so as to reduce the
cognition, life participation, or other influence of such potentially confounding
constructs of interest being assessed? If so, factors?
are they pertinent to the way you would • Is it normed by age in a way that would
use this instrument? be useful for the population of interest?
3. Theoretical framework • How large is the standardization sample?
• Is the theoretical framework on which this • Is the standardization sample sufficient
tool is based consistent with your own? for the population of interest?
4. Specific abilities tested and types of items 8. Administration modality
used to elicit responses • Does it allow for adaptive instructions
• Does it take into account a person’s ability and stimuli in cases where a person
to communicate meaning as well as form may have difficulty with the standard
and content? modalities?
• Does it take into account accuracy? Speed • How might hearing problems, speech
of processing? Levels of effort involved in perception problems, language problems,
carrying out the tasks? visual deficits, and attention problems
5. Reliability information affect the ability to understand task
• What is the quality of reported data (if instructions?
there are any) regarding 9. Response modes
• Test-retest reliability? • Does it allow for alternative response
• Intra-rater reliability? modes in cases where a person may have
• Inter-rater reliability? trouble with traditional response modes?
• Split-half reliability? • How might concomitant motor speech
• Is there adequate sampling of the specific disorders, paralysis, paresis, limb
constructs you most wish to assess? apraxia, dysgraphia, dyslexia, or visual
• Can the test be readministered? If so, in deficits confound results?
what form and how often? • Are there response modifications
6. Validity information suggested for special populations that
• What is the quality of data regarding meet the needs of the population of
• Construct validity (convergent validity interest?
and discriminant validity)? 10. Estimated time to administer and score
• Criterion validity (concurrent validity, • Is the administration and scoring time
predictive validity, sensitivity, and realistic in light of the context in which
specificity)? you would use it?
284  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

11. Ease of administration, scoring, and • Have the stimulus materials been
interpretation of results developed with attention to appropriate
• Are administration instructions clear? psycholinguistic controls (e.g., word
• Are there ceiling and floor rules, if frequency and familiarity, imageability,
appropriate? concreteness/abstractness, grammatical
• How long does it take to learn to score the complexity, plausibility, script, font, verbal
test validly and reliably? stimulus length, pronounceability)?
• Does the clinician require training to • Are the materials appealing?
administer it? If so, is it logistically • If there are objects, are they appropriately
feasible and affordable? sized, manipulable, and durable?
• Are raw scores easily converted to • Are the stimuli age appropriate?
percentiles, standard deviations, language • Is the tool portable?
age equivalents, and so on? • Are memory and attention demands
• Is guidance provided on how to interpret controlled?
scores in a way that fits your purpose? 13. Administrative manual, instructions,
12. Quality and appropriateness of test scoring, and reporting documentation for
materials/stimuli the clinician
• Is there an appropriate rationale for the • Are scoring forms provided?
design of stimulus items? • Are there clear means of summarizing
• How ecologically valid are the test items? results in a meaningful way?
• Are the stimuli relevant to the 14. Relevance of results to determination of
everyday lives of the people with prognosis and to treatment planning
whom you would use the tool? • Is it clear how results would be pertinent
• Do the stimuli convey cultural, age, or to treatment planning?
other biases? 15. Cost
• Are there items that could potentially • Can you afford it?
be considered offensive? 16. Any additional limitations
• Would familiarity with test items be 17. Any additional strengths
typical in the intended population?

A summary of available screening instruments samples of people without neurological disorders.


and tests is given in Tables 20–1 through 20–7, along Some authors report means and standard deviations
with information about targeted clinical groups, con- of ages, others report age ranges, still others report
structs assessed (as expressed by the authors), and all three of these indices, and some do not specify
the approximate time it takes to administer each. At ages. In any case, all of the instruments are applica-
times, such information is reported inconsistently. ble to adults.
This is the case, for example, when targeted clini- The tools listed in Tables 20–1 through 20–7 are
cal groups differ from information subsequently highly representative but not exhaustive. Also, they
published separately. Also, the age range of norma- do not include comprehensive sets of published
tive samples is not included in these tables simply informal assessment tools and screening instruments
because age is so inconsistently reported, making in related areas, such as auditory processing, motor
comparisons of the ages of intended target groups speech ability, vision, and mathematical ability,
across tests ineffectual. Age ranges associated with unless these constructs are addressed in components
published tools sometimes apply to clinical groups of batteries included in the list. New instruments
tested as normative samples and sometimes to are continuously appearing in the literature, while
20. Tests, Scales, and Screening Instruments   285

some older tests come out in new editions or go out this is especially crucial. Every assessment tool has
of print. Thus, it is important to keep abreast of new strengths and weaknesses. No tool meets all of the
developments in assessment tools. criteria for what one might consider to be most
For the most part, the tables exclude tools that important in terms of inherent properties and design
are no longer readily available, even though they characteristics and in terms of its relevance and prac-
may have had a strong influence on past research ticality in a given assessment situation. When select-
and clinical practice (e.g., Schuell, 1965). Not all tools ing an assessment instrument, it is always important
fit neatly into the categories listed. For example, to consider the five sets of factors summarized ear-
some tests and screening instruments listed as cogni- lier and to choose wisely based on a balance of the
tive tools also include means of indexing language; relative importance of all factors. Excellent online
several language-focused tools also include means resources for information about psychometric prop-
of assessing varied aspects of cognition; and many erties of many tools used by aphasiologists and other
tests are applicable to multiple clinical groups. rehabilitation professionals are the Academy of
Similarly, many assessment batteries include screen- Neurologic Communication Disorders and Sciences
ing tools that may be used separately from the pri- (https://www.ancds.org), the Canadian Partnership
mary tests. for Stroke Recovery (https://strokengine.ca), the
Of course, the excellent clinical aphasiologist is Shirley Ryan Ability Lab (https://www.sralab​.org),
highly discriminating and knows to consider pub- and the University of Sydney Brain and Mind Centre
lished work critically. In the arena of assessment, (https://www.sydney.edu.au/brain-mind).
Table 20–1. Language Screening Tools

Acute Aphasia Screening Protocol (AASP; Crary et al., 1989)


Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Attention/orientation to communication, auditory comprehension,
expressive ability, conversational style
Typical time to administer: 10 minutes
Aphasia Language Performance Scales (ALPS; Keenan & Brassell, 1975)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Listening, talking, reading, writing
Typical time to administer: 30 minutes
Aphasia Screening Test, Second Edition (AST; Whurr, 1996)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Auditory and reading comprehension, oral and written language
production, calculation
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Bedside Evaluation Screening Test, Second Edition (BEST-2; Fitch-West et al., 1998)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Conversational expression, object naming, object description, sentence
repetition, single-word comprehension, reading
Typical time to administer: 15–20 minutes
Frenchay Aphasia Screening Test, Second Edition (FAST-2; Enderby et al., 2006)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Comprehension, expression, reading, writing; to be assessed by
professionals who are not speech-language pathologists
Typical time to administer: 3–10 minutes
Mississippi Aphasia Screening Test (MAST; Nakase-Thompson, 2004)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Naming; automatic speech; repetition, yes/no accuracy; object recognition;
verbal instructions; reading instructions; verbal fluency; writing/spelling to
dictation
Typical time to administer: 5–15 minutes
Multilingual Aphasia Examination, Third Edition (MAE; Benton et al., 1994)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Naming, repetition, fluency, articulation, spelling, aural comprehension,
reading, writing
Typical time to administer: Unspecified

286
Table 20–1. continued

Multimodal Communication Screening Task for Persons with Aphasia (MCSTPA; Lasker &
Garrett, 2005)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia, including aphasia with concomitant apraxia of
speech
Constructs assessed: Likelihood of benefitting from augmentative and alternative communication
(AAC) use, and partner dependence versus independence of AAC use
Typical time to administer: Unspecified; subtests may be administered across multiple days
Naming and Oral Reading for Language in Aphasia 6-point scale (NORLA-6; Pitts et al., 2018)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Naming and oral reading, especially to index changes in naming and oral
reading associated with oral reading intervention
Typical time to administer: 20–30 minutes
Putney Auditory Comprehension Screening Test (PACST; Beaumont et al., 2002)
Target clinical population: People with severe motor and visual disabilities
Constructs assessed: Auditory comprehension
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Quick Assessment for Aphasia (QAA; Tanner & Culbertson, 1999)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Naming, answering questions, providing basic information, conversational
ability
Typical time to administer: 10–15 minutes
Reitan-Indiana Aphasia Screening Test (AST; Reitan, 1981)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Language and other neurocognitive abilities via naming and copying of
line drawings, reading, verbal repetition, simple arithmetic problems, and
following simple commands
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Sheffield Screening Test for Acquired Language Disorders (SSTALD; Syder et al., 1993)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Spoken language and understanding (no reading or writing items)
Typical time to administer: Approximately 10 minutes
Sklar Aphasia Scale (SAS; Sklar, 1983)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Auditory and visual decoding, oral and graphic decoding
Typical time to administer: 20–30 minutes
Note. Several additional screening tools listed in Table 20–3 include relevant indices for aphasia screening.

287
Table 20–2. Cognitive Screening Tools

Brief Cognitive Assessment Tool (Mini-ACE; Hsieh et al., 2015)


Target clinical population: People suspected of having dementia or mild cognitive impairment (MCI)
Constructs assessed: Orientation, memory, verbal fluency, and visuospatial function
Typical time to administer: Less than 5 minutes
A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed (AQT; Wiig et al., 2003)
Target clinical population: Adults with (or suspected of having) Alzheimer’s disease
Constructs assessed: Naming response time and accuracy
Typical time to administer: 3–10 minutes
Birmingham Cognitive Screen (BCS; Humphreys et al., 2012)
Target clinical population: Stroke survivors
Constructs assessed: Attention and executive function, language, memory, number processing,
action planning and control
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Brief Cognitive Assessment Tool (BCAT; Mansbach et al., 2012)
Target clinical population: People with (or suspected of having) dementia and MCI
Constructs assessed: Contextual memory, executive control, attentional capacity
Typical time to administer: 10–15 minutes
Cognistat (Kiernan et al., 1987; Mueller et al., 2014)
Target clinical population: Adults
Constructs assessed: Neurocognitive functioning (consciousness, orientation, and attention
span), language, constructional ability, memory, calculation skills,
reasoning/judgment
Typical time to administer: 15–30 minutes (shortened version, the Cognistat Five, also available
which takes approximately 5 minutes)
Cognitive Linguistic Quick Test (CLQT; Helm-Estabrooks, 2001)
Target clinical population: People with stroke, dementia, and traumatic brain injury
Constructs assessed: Orientation, attention, memory, language (naming, auditory
comprehension), visuospatial skills, executive functions
Typical time to administer: 15–30 minutes
General Practitioner Assessment of Cognition (GPCOG; Brodaty et al., 2002)
Target clinical population: Older adults, people with (or suspected of having) dementia
Constructs assessed: Cognitive abilities and caregiver report of cognitive abilities
Typical time to administer: 6 minutes
Mini-Mental State Examination, Second Edition (MMSE-2; Folstein et al., 2010)
Target clinical population: Adults
Constructs assessed: Orientation to time and place, attention, mental calculation, immediate
memory, delayed memory, visuospatial construction, object relations
Typical time to administer: 10 minutes; MMSE-2 Brief version is even shorter

288
Table 20–2. continued

Modified Mini-Mental State Examination (3MS; Teng & Chui, 1987)


Target clinical population: Adults
Constructs assessed: Orientation, attention, calculation, immediate memory, delayed memory,
visuospatial construction (copied)
Typical time to administer: 10 minutes
Nonverbal BriefScreen (Economou et al., 2021)
Target clinical population: People with limited verbal and/or motor abilities; people with possible MCI
or dementia
Constructs assessed: Memory, nonverbal reasoning, conceptualization, fluid intelligence
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA; Nasreddine, 2021)
Target clinical population: People with (or suspected of having) cognitive impairment
Constructs assessed: Attention/concentration, executive functions, memory, language,
visuoconstructional abilities, conceptual thinking, calculation, orientation
Typical time to administer: 10 minutes
Saint Louis University Mental Status Examination (SLUMS; Tariq et al., 2006)
Target clinical population: Adults suspected to have MCI or dementia
Constructs assessed: Orientation, short-term memory, attention, calculations, naming, clock
drawing, and recognition of geometric figures
Typical time to administer: Approximately 7 minutes
Scales of Cognitive and Communicative Ability for Neurorehabilitation (SCCAN; Milman &
Holland, 2012)
Target clinical population: People with cognitive-communicative deficits and those for whom a
diagnosis is not established
Constructs assessed: Oral expression, orientation, memory, speech comprehension, reading
comprehension, writing, attention, problem-solving
Typical time to administer: 35–40 minutes

289
Table 20–3. Aphasia Assessment Tools

Action Naming Test (ANT; Obler & Albert, 1979)


Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Verb naming
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Activity Sort Cards (ACS; Baum & Edwards, 2008)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia; applicable to anyone with an acquired neurogenic
communication challenge
Constructs assessed: Life participation before and after onset of aphasia; categories include
activities of daily living, low- and high-demand leisure activities, and social
activities
Typical time to administer: Variable; the approach is individualized
Amsterdam Nijmegen Everyday Language Test (ANELT; Blomert et al., 1994)
Target clinical population: Stroke survivors, people with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Change in communication over time; understandability and intelligibility of
responses to scripted interview questions about everyday life situations
Typical time to administer: 10 minutes
Aphasia Communication Outcome Measure (ACOM; Hula et al., 2015)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Patient-reported communicative functioning
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Aphasia Diagnostic Profiles (ADP; Helm-Estabrooks, 1992a)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Speaking, listening, reading, writing, gesture; type and severity of aphasia;
includes aphasia severity profile, alternative communication profile,
classification profile, behavioral profile, and error profile
Typical time to administer: 40–50 minutes
ASHA Functional Assessment of Communication Skills for Adults (ASHA FACS; Frattali et al., 2017)
Target clinical population: Adults with speech, language, or cognitive impairment
Constructs assessed: Social communication, communication of basic needs, reading, writing,
number concepts, daily planning
Typical time to administer: 20 minutes
Assessment of Communicative Effectiveness in Severe Aphasia (ACESA; Cunningham et al.,
1995)
Target clinical population: People with severe aphasia due to stroke
Constructs assessed: Has modified tasks, stimuli, and scoring procedures
Typical time to administer: Unspecified

290
Table 20–3. continued

Assessment for Living with Aphasia (ALA; Kagan et al., 2011; Simmons-Mackie et al., 2014)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Pictographically supported self-report of the impacts of aphasia on daily
life; includes participation in life situations, communication and language
environment, language and related impairments, personal identity,
feelings, and attitudes, moving on with life, and descriptive questions
Typical time to administer: 10–95 minutes
Assessment of Language-Related Functional Activities (ALFA; Baines et al., 1999)
Target clinical population: People with a history of “neurological episodes”
Constructs assessed: Telling time, counting money, addressing an envelope, solving math
problems, writing a check and balancing a checkbook, understanding
medicine labels, using a calendar, reading instructions, using the
telephone, and writing a phone message
Typical time to administer: 30–90 minutes
Boston Assessment of Severe Aphasia (BASA; Helm-Estabrooks, Ramsberger, et al., 1989)
Target clinical population: People with severe aphasia
Constructs assessed: Auditory comprehension, buccofacial and limb praxis, gesture recognition,
oral and gestural expression, reading comprehension, writing, and
visuospatial abilities; includes scoring of verbal and nonverbal responses,
refusals, affect, and perseveration
Typical time to administer: 20–30 minutes
Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination, Third Edition (BDAE; Goodglass, Kaplan, & Baresi,
2001); includes Boston Naming Test and Visuospatial Quantitative Battery
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Conversational and expository speech, auditory comprehension, oral
expression, repetition, reading, writing; helps identify type of aphasia
Typical time to administer: Short form: 30–45 minutes; Long form: dependent on how many tests
the examiner chooses to administer; Extended testing options: more
thoroughly probe particular language functions within each area of testing
Boston Naming Test (BNT; Kaplan et al., 2000; also included in the BDAE-3)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia, dementia, and/or concerns about naming
Constructs assessed: Naming/lexical retrieval (based on naming of black-and-white line
drawings), including responsiveness to semantic and phonemic cues
Typical time to administer: 35–45 minutes; may also be given in a short form (Fastenau et al., 1998)
Brisbane Evidence-Based Language Test (Rohde et al., 2020)
Target clinical population: Aphasia and high-level language disorders
Constructs assessed: Auditory comprehension, verbal expression, reading, writing, perception
Typical time to administer: 45 minutes for complete test; 15–25 minutes for shorter versions
(profound/severe, moderate, mild)
continues

291
Table 20–3. continued

Butt Nonverbal Reasoning Test (BNVR; Butt & Bucks, 2004)


Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Everyday problem-solving; identifies cognitive and/or linguistic deficits
Typical time to administer: 10–20 minutes
Code-Müller Protocols (CMP; Code & Müller, 1992; Code et al., 1999)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia and other communication disorders including
dysarthria, laryngectomy, and acquired deafness
Constructs assessed: Psychosocial state, associated changes over time, optimism, and
predicted future adjustment to aphasia and related disorders
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Communication Confidence Rating Scale for Aphasia (CCRSA; Babbitt & Cherney, 2010)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Communication confidence
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Communication Disability Profile (CDP; Swinburn & Byng, 2006)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Self-report of the impact of aphasia on everyday life; activities,
participation, and emotions
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Communicative Activities of Daily Living, Third Edition (CADL-3; Holland, Fromm, & Wozniak,
2018)
Target clinical population: People with neurogenic communication disorders, including aphasia,
Alzheimer’s disease, and traumatic brain injury (TBI)
Constructs assessed: Communication and interaction abilities for functional interaction (reading,
writing, using numbers; social, divergent, and contextual communication;
nonverbal communication; sequential relationships; humor/metaphor/
absurdity)
Typical time to administer: Approximately 30 minutes
Communicative Effectiveness Index (CETI; Lomas et al., 1989)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Functional verbal and nonverbal communication, as assessed by
significant others; especially designed to allow detection of change in
function
Typical time to administer: 15 minutes
Comprehensive Aphasia Test (CAT; Swinburn et al., 2004)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Recognition, comprehension, production of spoken and written language;
includes cognitive screening and disability questionnaire in addition to
language battery
Typical time to administer: 90–120 minutes; can be completed over one or two assessment sessions

292
Table 20–3. continued

Discourse Comprehension Test, Second Edition (DCT-2; Brookshire & Nicholas, 1997)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia, right hemisphere damage, and TBI
Constructs assessed: Listening and reading comprehension at a discourse level
Typical time to administer: 20 minutes
Everyday Communication Needs Assessment (ECNA; Worrall, 1992); and the more recent
Functional Communication Therapy Planner (FCTP; Worrall, 1999)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Pre-onset communicative style and everyday activities; elicited via
nonstandardized questionnaire to help develop, administer, and evaluate
aphasia intervention
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Examining for Aphasia, Fourth Edition (EFA-4; LaPointe & Eisenson, 2008)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Visual, tactile, and auditory recognition; auditory comprehension; speech;
writing
Typical time to administer: 30–60 minutes
Galveston Orientation and Amnesia Test for Aphasia (A-GOAT; Jain et al., 2000)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia due to head injury
Constructs assessed: Orientation to person, place, and time, and memory for events preceding
and following the injury
Typical time to administer: 3–15 minutes
Inpatient Functional Communication Interview (IFCI; McCooey-O’Halloran et al., 2004)
Target clinical population: Hospital inpatients with communication difficulties
Constructs assessed: Everyday communication needs and abilities of patients while they are in
hospital.
Typical time to administer: 30–45 minutes (includes medical history/chart review, patient interview,
and interview of relevant members of the health care team)
Kissing and Dancing Test (KDT; Bak & Hodges, 2003)
Target clinical population: People suspected having frontal variant frontotemporal dementia (fvFTD)
or semantic variant primary progressive aphasia (svPPA)
Constructs assessed: Verb processing; semantic access from words and pictures
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Life Interests and Values Cards (LIV; Haley et al., 2010)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia; applicable to anyone
Constructs assessed: Interests and values to be considered and prioritized in intervention
planning focused on life participation. Categories 12: physical; home and
community; relaxing and creative; and social
Typical time to administer: Variable; the approach is individualized
continues

293
Table 20–3. continued

Neurosensory Center Comprehensive Examination for Aphasia (NCCEA; Spreen & Benton, 1977)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Visual and tactile naming, repetition, verbal fluency, object description,
immediate verbal memory, auditory comprehension of single words
and commands of varied length and complexity, reading of words and
sentences, writing (dictation, copying, and naming), articulation
Typical time to administer: 24 subtests, most of which can be administered in less than 5 minutes
Naming and Oral Reading for Language in Aphasia 6-Point Scale (NORLA-6 Scale; Gingrich et al.,
2013)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: 6-point scale used for quantifying naming and oral reading
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Northwestern Syntax Screening Test (NSST; Lee, 1971)
Target clinical population: Sometimes applied to people with aphasia (originally developed for
children)
Constructs assessed: Expressive and receptive portions: prepositions, personal pronouns, noun-
verb agreement, tense, possessives, present progressives, active and
passive voice, and wh- questions
Typical time to administer: 20 minutes
Philadelphia Naming Test (PNT; Roach et al., 1996) (Also can be administered in two matched short
forms; Walker & Schwartz, 2012)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Object naming
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Porch Index of Communicative Ability, Revised (PICA-R; Porch, 2001)
Target clinical population: People with brain injury and aphasia due to stroke
Constructs assessed: Gestural, verbal, and graphic abilities; entails multidimensional scoring
according to accuracy, responsiveness, completeness, promptness, and
efficiency of response
Typical time to administer: 60 minutes; requires extensive training to administer
Progressive Aphasia Severity Scale (PASS; Sapolsky et al., 2014)
Target clinical population: People with primary progressive aphasia
Constructs assessed: Articulation, fluency, syntax/grammar, word retrieval/expression,
repetition, auditory comprehension, single-word comprehension, reading,
writing, functional communication; includes supplemental domains for
communication initiation, turn-taking, and language generation
Typical time to administer: 10 minutes (in addition to accompanying evaluation and interviews on
which ratings are partially based)

294
Table 20–3. continued

Psycholinguistic Assessment of Language Processing in Aphasia (PALPA; Kay et al., 1992)


Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Auditory processing; reading and spelling; picture and word semantics;
sentence comprehension
Typical time to administer: Length beyond feasibility in its entirety in clinical environments; individual
subtests can be administered
Pyramids and Palm Trees Test (PPT; Patterson & Howard, 1992)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia, visual agnosia, general semantic impairment
Constructs assessed: Semantic access from words and pictures
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Quality of Communication Life Scale (QCLS; Paul et al., 2004)
Target clinical population: Adults with neurogenic communication disorders
Constructs assessed: Impact of a communication disorder on relationships, communication,
interactions, and participation in social, leisure, work, and education
activities; overall quality of life
Typical time to administer: 15 minutes
Reading Comprehension Battery for Aphasia, Second Edition (RCBA-2; LaPointe & Horner, 1998)
Target clinical population: Adults with acquired language disorders
Constructs assessed: Reading of single words; includes manipulations of visual, auditory, and
semantic confusions; synonyms; sentences; paragraphs; addresses silent
reading, not just reading aloud
Typical time to administer: 30 minutes
Revised Token Test (RTT; McNeil & Prescott, 1978)
Target clinical population: People with auditory processing impairment associated with brain
damage, aphasia, and language and learning disabilities
Constructs assessed: Auditory comprehension of commands of varying length and complexity
Typical time to administer: 30 minutes
Sentence Production Test for Aphasia (SPTA; Wilshire et al., 2014)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Production of words in sentences, including contrast of production in
sentences with single-word production
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
SOAP Test of Syntactic Complexity (SOAP-TSC; Love & Oster, 2002)
Target clinical population: People with TBI and aphasia
Constructs assessed: Comprehension of sentences (matched for length) of four syntactic
construction types: active, passive, subject-relative, and object-relative
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
continues

295
Table 20–3. continued

Stroke and Aphasia Quality of Life Scale (SAQOL-39; Hilari et al., 2003)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Self-report of health-related quality of life; includes self-care, mobility,
upper-extremity function, work, vision, language, thinking, personality,
mood, energy, and family and social roles
Typical time to administer: 10–15 minutes
Stroke Aphasia Depression Questionnaire (SADQ; Sutcliffe & Lincoln, 1998)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: 21-item questionnaire completed by the client’s caregiver, developed
based on observable behaviors thought to be associated with depressed
mood
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Verb and Sentence Test (VAST; Bastiaanse et al., 2002)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia
Constructs assessed: Understanding of verb forms (transitive and intransitive), derivational
morphemes, inflectional morphemes; understanding of canonical and
noncanonical sentences; morphosyntactic production of words and
sentences
Typical time to administer: 2–3 hours; individual subtests may be given
Western Aphasia Battery, Revised (WAB-R; Kertesz, 2007; and WAB-Extended (WAB-E) an
“extension” of the WAB-Revised, Kertesz, 2006)
Target clinical population: People with aphasia due to stroke, TBI survivors, people with dementia
Constructs assessed: Fluency, auditory comprehension, repetition, naming, word finding,
reading, writing, drawing; supplemental tools to index block design,
calculation, praxis, and differentiation of deep, surface, and visual
dyslexia; suggests classification according to classical aphasia types;
includes bedside evaluation
Typical time to administer: Full battery 30–45 minutes; additional 45–60 minutes for reading, writing,
praxis, and construction sections; 15 minutes for bedside form
Note. Many of the tools listed in this table are applicable also to people with TBI, RBI, and dementia; to avoid duplication,
they are not listed in the additional tables for those target clinical groups.

296
Table 20–4. Traumatic Brain Injury Assessment Tools

Attention Process Training Test (APT-Test; Sohlberg & Mateer, 1987)


Target clinical population: Adolescents, adults, and veterans with mild, moderate, and severe
traumatic brain injury (TBI), postconcussion syndrome, and other
neurological disorders
Constructs assessed: Sustained, selective, divided, and alternating attention on paced tasks
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Behavioral Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS; Wilson et al., 1996)
Target clinical population: TBI survivors
Constructs assessed: Executive functioning (mental flexibility, problem-solving, abstract thinking,
temporal judgment)
Typical time to administer: 40 minutes
Brief Test of Head Injury (BTHI; Helm-Estabrooks & Hotz, 1991)
Target clinical population: TBI survivors
Constructs assessed: Orientation/attention, command following, linguistic organization, reading
comprehension, naming, and visual-spatial skills
Typical time to administer: 20–30 minutes
Comprehensive Assessment of Prospective Memory (CAPM; Roche et al., 2002)
Target clinical population: TBI survivors
Constructs assessed: Prospective memory
Typical time to administer: 10–15 minutes
Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS; Baldo et al., 2001; Delis et al., 2001)
Target clinical population: Adults with mild brain injury, especially with frontal-lobe challenges
Constructs assessed: Executive functions within verbal and spatial modalities, including
initiation, flexibility of thinking, inhibition, problem-solving, planning,
impulse control, concept formation, abstract thinking, and creativity;
sustained, focused, and divided attention
Typical time to administer: 90 minutes; specific subtests may be given
Functional Assessment of Verbal Reasoning and Executive Strategies (FAVRES; MacDonald, 2005)
Target clinical population: TBI survivors
Constructs assessed: Everyday life reasoning and executive functioning skills (reasoning
accuracy, rationale, and efficiency), and reasoning skills (getting facts,
eliminating irrelevant information, weighing facts, flexibility, generating
alternatives, and predicting consequences)
Typical time to administer: Approximately 60 minutes
Galveston Orientation and Amnesia Test (GOAT; Levin et al., 1979; A-GOAT form available for
individuals with aphasia)
Target clinical population: People at a subacute stage of recovery from closed head injury
Constructs assessed: Duration of post-traumatic amnesia; orientation to person, place, and time,
and memory for events preceding and following the injury
Typical time to administer: 3–15 minutes

continues

297
Table 20–4. continued

The Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS; Teasdale & Jennett, 1974)


Target clinical population: TBI survivors
Constructs assessed: Responsiveness following coma; 3–15 points: best eye, verbal, and motor
behaviors
Typical time to administer: Approximately 1 minute
LaTrobe Communication Questionnaire (LCQ; Douglas et al., 2000)
Target clinical population: TBI survivors
Constructs assessed: Perceived communication ability in adults with TBI based on information
gathered from patient and significant other
Typical time to administer: 20–40 minutes
Measure of Cognitive-Linguistic Abilities (MCLA; Ellmo et al., 1995)
Target clinical population: TBI survivors
Constructs assessed: Reading comprehension, functional reading, pragmatics in discourse,
narrative discourse, written narrative abilities, story recall, verbal abstract
reasoning, confrontation naming, oral mechanism function
Typical time to administer: 45–60 minutes
Mount Wilga High Level Language Test (MWHLLT; Clark et al., 1986) and Mount Wilga High Level
Language Test, Revised (MWHLLT-R; Simpson, 2006)
Target clinical population: People with mild language problems due to head injury
Constructs assessed: Naming skills, verbal explanation, planning, auditory memory, auditory
comprehension, reading comprehension, written expression, numeracy
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT; Gronwall, 1977)
Target clinical population: People with mild head injury
Constructs assessed: Measure of cognitive function that assesses auditory information
processing speed and flexibility, and calculation ability
Typical time to administer: 10–15 minutes
Quality of Life After Brain Injury (QOLIBRI; von Steinbüchel et al., 2010)
Target clinical population: TBI survivors
Constructs assessed: Satisfaction in the areas of cognition, self, daily life, autonomy, and social
relationships; perception of “feeling bothered” by emotions and physical
problems
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Rancho Los Amigos Scale of Cognitive Functioning, Revised (RLASCF-R; Reimer et al., 1995)
Target clinical population: TBI survivors
Constructs assessed: Stages of recovery after brain injury; responsiveness to stimuli, ability
to follow commands, presence of nonpurposeful behavior, cooperation,
confusion, attention to environment, focus, coherence of verbalization,
appropriateness of verbalizations and actions, memory recall, orientation,
and judgment and reasoning
Typical time to administer: Unspecified

298
Table 20–4. continued

Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test, Third Edition (RBMT-3; Wilson et al., 2008)
Target clinical population: People with acquired, nonprogressive brain injury
Constructs assessed: Verbal and nonverbal episodic memory, spatial memory, prospective
memory, and procedural memory during everyday functional tasks
Typical time to administer: 25–30 minutes
Ross Information Processing Assessment, Second Edition (RIPA-2; Ross-Swain & Fogle, 1996)
Target clinical population: TBI survivors
Constructs assessed: Recent memory, temporal orientation (recent memory), temporal
orientation (remote memory), spatial orientation, orientation to
environment, recall of general information, problem-solving and abstract
reasoning, organization auditory processing and retention
Typical time to administer: 45–60 minutes
Ruff Figural Fluency Test (RFFT; Ruff, 1996)
Target clinical population: Adults
Constructs assessed: Nonverbal capacity for initiation, planning, and divergent reasoning
Typical time to administer: 5 minutes
Scales of Cognitive Ability for Traumatic Brain Injury (SCATBI; Adamovich & Henderson, 1992)
Target clinical population: TBI survivors
Constructs assessed: Perception/discrimination, orientation, organization, recall, reasoning
Typical time to administer: 2 hours; subtests can be given separately
Wiig-Semel Test of Linguistic Concepts (W-STLC; Wiig & Semel, 1974)
Target clinical population: TBI survivors
Constructs assessed: Comprehension of a range of complex grammatical structures, including
passive, comparative, temporal, spatial, and familial structures
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Note. Many of the tools in Table 20–5 are also applicable to TBI survivors.

299
Table 20–5. Right Brain Injury Assessment Tools

Burns Brief Inventory of Communication and Cognition: Right Hemisphere Inventory (BBICC-
RHI; Burns, 1997)
Target clinical population: Adults with right hemisphere injury
Constructs assessed: Scanning and tracking, visuospatial skills, prosody and abstract language
Typical time to administer: 30 minutes
Mini Inventory of Right Brain Injury, Second Edition (MIRBI-2; Pimental & Knight, 2000)
Target clinical population: Adults with right hemisphere injury
Constructs assessed: Attention, ability to explain incongruities, absurdities, figurative language
and similarities, affective language, emotions and affect processing,
understanding humor, praxis, and expressive ability
Typical time to administer: 30 minutes
The RIC Evaluation in Right Hemisphere Dysfunction-3 (RICE-3; Halper et al., 2010)
Target clinical population: Adults with right hemisphere injury
Constructs assessed: Characteristics of pragmatics, visual scanning and tracking, analysis of
writing, metaphorical language
Typical time to administer: 60 minutes
Right Hemisphere Language Battery, Second Edition (RHLB-2; Bryan, 1994)
Target clinical population: Adults with right hemisphere injury
Constructs assessed: Metaphor, comprehension of inferred meaning, humor, lexical semantic
comprehension, emotional and linguistic prosody, discourse
Typical time to administer: Approximately 60 minutes
Note. Some of the tools in Table 20–3 and many in Table 20–4 are also applicable to right brain injury survivors.

300
Table 20–6. Tools for Assessing People With Mild Cognitive Impairment, Dementia, and Primary
Progressive Aphasia

Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination-III (ACE-III; Hsieh, 2013)


Target clinical population: People suspected of having dementia or mild cognitive impairment (MCI)
Constructs assessed: Attention, memory, language, visuospatial function, and verbal fluency
Typical time to administer: 15 minutes
Arizona Battery for Communication Disorders of Dementia (ABCD; Bayles & Tomoeda, 1993)
Target clinical population: People with dementia
Constructs assessed: Mental status, story retelling (immediate and delayed), command
following, comparative questions, word learning (free recall, total recall,
recognition), repetition, object description, word and sentence reading
comprehension, generative naming, confrontation naming, concept
definition, generative drawing, and figure copying; includes screening for
visual perception, literacy, and speech discrimination
Typical time to administer: 45–90 minutes
Dementia Rating Scale–2 (DRS-2; Jurica et al., 2001)
Target clinical population: People with dementia
Constructs assessed: Attention, initiation-perseveration, construction, conceptualization,
memory
Typical time to administer: 15–30 minutes
The Executive Interview (EXIT25; Royall et al., 1992)
Target clinical population: People with mild dementia
Constructs assessed: Executive functions, including verbal fluency, design fluency, frontal
release signs, motor/impulse control, imitation behavior, and other
symptoms associated with frontal lobe changes
Typical time to administer: 15 minutes
Functional Linguistic Communication Inventory (FLCI; Bayles & Tomoeda, 1994)
Target clinical population: People with dementia
Constructs assessed: Greeting, naming, answering questions, writing, comprehension of signs,
following commands, conversation, reminiscing, gesture/pantomime, and
word reading
Typical time to administer: 30 minutes
Global Deterioration Scale (GDS; Reisberg et al., 1982)
Target clinical population: People with dementia
Constructs assessed: Seven stages of cognitive decline
Typical time to administer: 5–10 minutes
Location Learning Test, Revised (LLT-R; Kessels et al., 2011)
Target clinical population: Older adults with and without dementia
Constructs assessed: Visuospatial learning
Typical time to administer: 30 minutes
continues

301
Table 20–6. continued

Quality of Life in Alzheimer’s Disease (Qol-AD; Logsdon et al., 1999)


Target clinical population: People with dementia
Constructs assessed: Patient and caregiver report of quality of life
Typical time to administer: 10 minutes for each (self-report and caregiver)
Quality of Life in Dementia (DEMQOL; Smith et al., 2005)
Target clinical population: People with dementia
Constructs assessed: Patient and caregiver report of quality of life
Typical time to administer: 10 minutes for each (self-report/interview and informant)
Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS; Randolph et al., 1998)
Target clinical population: Adults, especially people with dementia
Constructs assessed: Immediate recall, visuospatial construction, language, attention, delayed
recall
Typical time to administer: 30–45 minutes
Ross Information Processing Assessment–Geriatric, Second Edition (RIPA-G:2; Ross-Swain &
Fogle, 2012)
Target clinical population: People older than 55 years; people with mild cognitive impairment,
Alzheimer’s disease, right cerebrovascular accident, traumatic brain injury
Constructs assessed: Immediate memory, temporal orientation, spatial orientation, general
information, situational knowledge, categorical vocabulary, listening
comprehension
Typical time to administer: 25–35 minutes
Rowland Universal Dementia Assessment Scale (RUDAS; Storey et al., 2004)
Target clinical population: Older adults
Constructs assessed: Memory, gnosis (body orientation), praxis (fist-palm alternation), visuospatial
ability (cube copying), judgment, language (generative naming).
Typical time to administer: 5 minutes
Scales of Adult Independence, Language, and Recall (SAILR; Sonies, 1997)
Target clinical population: Older adults
Constructs assessed: Functional independence (assessed via checklists and interviews with
clients and caregivers), language and recall (confrontation naming,
sentence comprehension, paragraph recall)
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
The Severe Impairment Battery (SIB; Saxton, 2004)
Target clinical population: Adults with severe dementia who are unable to complete standard types
of neuropsychological testing
Constructs assessed: Measures cognitive dysfunction in advanced stage dementia based on
behavioral observations and direct performance on a wide variety of
low-level tasks (one-step questions and commands; social interaction,
memory, orientation, language, attention, praxis, visuospatial ability,
construction, orienting to name)
Typical time to administer: 20 minutes

302
Table 20–7. Other Tools for People With Acquired Neurogenic Cognitive-Linguistic Disorders

Apathy Evaluation Scale (AES; Marin et al., 1991)


Target clinical population: Stroke survivors, people with dementia or depression
Constructs assessed: Apathy
Typical time to administer: 10 minutes for each (caregiver and patient)
Assessment of the Intelligibility of Dysarthric Speech (AIDS; Yorkston & Beukelman, 1984)
Target clinical population: People with dysarthria
Constructs assessed: Single-word intelligibility, sentence intelligibility, and speaking rate
Typical time to administer: 30 minutes
Balloons Test (BT; Edgeworth et al., 1998)
Target clinical population: Adults
Constructs assessed: Visual inattention
Typical time to administer: 5–10 minutes
Barkley Deficits in Executive Functioning Scale (BDEFS; Barkley, 2011)
Target clinical population: Adults
Constructs assessed: Executive functioning in daily life activities
Typical time to administer: Long form: 15–20 minutes; short form: 4–5 minutes
Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult Version (BRIEF-A; Roth et al., 2005)
Target clinical population: Children and adolescents with executive function/self-regulation
impairment (may have relevance to adults)
Constructs assessed: Self and informant report of inhibition, self-monitoring, planning/
organization, shifting attention, initiating, monitoring tasks, emotional
control, working memory, organization of materials
Typical time to administer: 10–15 minutes
Behavioral Inattention Test (BIT; Wilson et al., 1987a)
Target clinical population: Adults
Constructs assessed: Visual neglect in everyday activities
Typical time to administer: 30–40 minutes
Benton Visual Retention Test, Fifth Edition (BVRT-5; Benton & Benton Sivan, 1992)
Target clinical population: Adults, especially those with reading disabilities, nonverbal learning
disabilities, traumatic brain injury (TBI), attention deficit hyperactivity
disorder, and dementia
Constructs assessed: Visual perception, memory, visuoconstructive abilities
Typical time to administer: 15–20 minutes

continues

303
Table 20–7. continued

Bilingual Verbal Ability Tests (BVAT; Munoz-Sandoval et al., 1998; normative update edition available
through Munoz-Sandoval et al., 2005)
Target clinical population: People who are bilingual
Constructs assessed: Overall verbal ability: Picture vocabulary, oral vocabulary, verbal analogies
measures first in English, with supplementation in another language;
provides an index of English language proficiency and of overall bilingual
ability for speakers of English and 17 other languages
Typical time to administer: 30 minutes
Brief Test of Attention (BTA; Schretlen, 1997)
Target clinical population: Adults
Constructs assessed: Attention
Typical time to administer: 10 minutes or less
Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R; Benedict, 1997)
Target clinical population: Adults
Constructs assessed: Visuospatial memory
Typical time to administer: 45 minutes (including 25-minute delay)
The Burden of Stroke Scale (BOSS; Doyle et al., 2003)
Target clinical population: Stroke survivors with and without communication disorders
Constructs assessed: Health status and related clinical outcomes; patient-reported difficulty in
well-being and multiple domains of functioning following stroke
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Burns Brief Inventory of Cognition and Communication (Burns Inventory; Burns, 1997)
Target clinical population: People with left hemisphere lesions, right hemisphere lesions, and
complex neuropathologies
Constructs assessed: Language, speech prosody, visuospatial abilities
Typical time to administer: 30 minutes
California Verbal Learning Test, Second Edition (CVLT-II; Delis et al., 2000)
Target clinical population: Adults, including those with left or right hemisphere stoke or TBI
Constructs assessed: Verbal learning and memory (recall and recognition of word lists over
immediate and delayed memory trials)
Typical time to administer: 30 minutes testing plus 30 minutes of delay; short form: 15 minutes testing
plus 15 minutes of delay
Cambridge Prospective Memory Test (CAMPROMPT; Wilson et al., 2005)
Target clinical population: Adults
Constructs assessed: Prospective memory
Typical time to administer: 25 minutes

304
Table 20–7. continued

Color Trails Test (CTT; D’Elia et al., 1996)


Target clinical population: Adults
Constructs assessed: Sustained attention, sequencing
Typical time to administer: 3–8 minutes
Comb and Razor Test (CRT; McIntosh et al., 2000)
Target clinical population: Adult stroke survivors
Constructs assessed: Unilateral spatial neglect
Typical time to administer: 5 minutes or less
Common Objects Memory Test (COMT; Kempler et al., 2010)
Target clinical population: Adult stroke survivors
Constructs assessed: Memory for common objects based on pictures
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Communication Profile for the Hearing Impaired (CPHI; Demorest & Erdman, 1986)
Target clinical population: Adults with hearing impairment
Constructs assessed: Self-ratings of hearing and auditory processing in four areas:
communication performance, communication environment,
communication strategies, and personal adjustment
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Comprehensive Test of Nonverbal Intelligence, Second Edition (CTONI-2; Hammill et al., 2009)
Target clinical population: People with language disabilities, hearing impairment, motor control
problem, history of stroke or brain injury
Constructs assessed: General intelligence
Typical time to administer: 60 minutes
Comprehensive Trail-Making Test (CTMT; Reynolds, 2002)
Target clinical population: People with TBI and other neurogenic disorders, especially frontal lobe
deficits
Constructs assessed: Visual search, scanning, speed of processing, mental flexibility, and
executive functions, attention, concentration
Typical time to administer: 5–12 minutes
d2 Test of Attention (d2TA; Brickenkamp & Zillmer, 1998)
Target clinical population: Adults
Constructs assessed: Attention, concentration, processing speed, rule compliance (originally
developed to measure driving aptitude and efficiency)
Typical time to administer: 8 minutes
Doors and People (DP; Baddeley, Emslie, & Nimmo-Smith, 1994)
Target clinical population: Adults
Constructs assessed: Long-term memory; visual and verbal recall and recognition
Typical time to administer: 35–40 minutes

continues

305
Table 20–7. continued

Detroit Test of Learning Aptitude–Adult (DTLA-A; Hammill & Bryant, 1991)


Target clinical population: Adults with learning disabilities
Constructs assessed: General intelligence, word opposites, form assembly, sentence imitation,
reversed letters, mathematical problems, design sequences, basic
information, quantitative relations, word sequences, design reproduction,
symbolic relations, story sequences
Typical time to administer: 40 minutes–2 hours
Dysarthria Examination Battery (DEB; Drummond, 1993)
Target clinical population: People with dysarthria
Constructs assessed: Respiration, phonation, resonation, articulation, intelligibility, prosody, oral
sensitivity to tactile stimulation
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Executive Control Battery (ECB; Goldberg et al., 2000)
Target clinical population: Adults
Constructs assessed: Executive functioning/control
Typical time to administer: 60 minutes (15 minutes per subtest)
Expressive Vocabulary Test (EVT; Williams, 1997)
Target clinical population: Children and adults
Constructs assessed: Expressive vocabulary and word retrieval
Typical time to administer: 10–20 minutes
Florida Affect Battery, Revised (FAB-R; Bowers et al., 1999)
Target clinical population: People with neurological or psychiatric disorders
Constructs assessed: Perception and understanding of nonverbal (i.e., facial and prosodic)
communicative signals of emotion under a variety of task demands
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Frenchay Dysarthria Assessment (FDA; Enderby, 1983); Frenchay Dysarthria Assessment,
Second Edition (FDA-2; Enderby & Palmer, 2008)
Target clinical population: People with motor speech disorders
Constructs assessed: Reflexes, respiration, lips ratings, palate ratings, laryngeal ratings, tongue
ratings, intelligibility, influencing factors (hearing, sight, teeth, language,
mood, posture, rate, and sensation)
Typical time to administer: 20 minutes
Hearing Handicap Inventory for the Elderly (HHIE; Ventry & Weinstein, 1982) and the Hearing
Handicap Inventory for the Elderly–Spouse (HHIE-SP; Newman & Weinstein, 1986)
Target clinical population: Older adults with hearing impairment and their spouses, respectively
Constructs assessed: Self-ratings of hearing and auditory processing
Typical time to administer: Unspecified

306
Table 20–7. continued

Johns Hopkins University Dysgraphia Battery (JHUDB; Goodman & Caramazza, 1985)
Target clinical population: Adults
Constructs assessed: Spelling of dictated words and nonwords, transcoding, written picture
naming
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Location Learning Test, Revised (LLT-R; Kessels et al., 2011)
Target clinical population: People with dementia, amnesia, and milder memory deficits
Constructs assessed: Visuospatial recall and learning
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Memory for Intentions Test (MIST; Raskin et al., 2010)
Target clinical population: Adults
Constructs assessed: Prospective memory
Typical time to administer: 30 minutes
Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (M-WCST; Schretlen, 2010)
Target clinical population: Adults
Constructs assessed: Abstract reasoning, perseveration
Typical time to administer: 7–10 minutes
Neuropsychological Assessment Battery (NAB; White & Stern, 2003)
Target clinical population: People with known or suspected disorders of the central nervous system
Constructs assessed: Attention, oral and written language production, memory, spatial abilities,
executive functions
Typical time to administer: 4 hours; also includes screening modules; individual subtests may be given
Northwestern Assessment of Verbs and Sentences (NAVS; Cho-Reyes & Thompson, 2012)
Target clinical population: People with neurological disorders
Constructs assessed: Comprehension and production of action verbs, production of verb
argument structure in sentence contexts, and comprehension and
production of canonical and noncanonical sentences
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Neuropsychological Assessment Battery (NAB; White & Stern, 2003)
Target clinical population: Adults with neurological disorders
Constructs assessed: Attention, oral and written language production, memory, spatial abilities,
executive functions
Typical time to administer: 4 hours; also includes screening modules; individual subtests may be given
Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test, Adapted Version (PASAT; Rao et al., 1989)
Target clinical population: People with multiple sclerosis
Constructs assessed: Measure of cognitive function that assesses auditory information
processing speed and flexibility, and calculation ability
Typical time to administer: 10–15 minutes
continues

307
Table 20–7. continued

Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Fourth Edition (PPVT-4; Dunn & Dunn, 2007)
Target clinical population: Children and adults
Constructs assessed: Single-word receptive vocabulary
Typical time to administer: 10–15 minutes
Quick Assessment for Dysarthria (QAD; Tanner & Culbertson, 1999)
Target clinical population: People with dysarthria
Constructs assessed: Respiration, phonation, articulation, resonance, and prosody
Typical time to administer: 10–15 minutes
Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices (RAPM; Raven, 2007)
Target clinical population: Adults with high intellectual ability
Constructs assessed: High-level observation skills, clear thinking ability, intellectual capacity
Typical time to administer: 40–60 minutes
Raven’s Progressive Matrices (RMP; Raven et al., 2003)
Target clinical population: Anyone
Constructs assessed: Reasoning ability, nonverbal abilities, general intelligence
Typical time to administer: 40 minutes
Rey Complex Figure Test (RCFT; Meyers & Meyers, 1995)
Target clinical population: Adults with and without neurological and psychiatric impairments
Constructs assessed: Visuospatial recall memory, visuospatial recognition memory, response
bias, processing speed, visuospatial constructional ability, and ability to
use cues to retrieve information.
Typical time to administer: 45 minutes, including a 30-minute delay interval (timed), and 15 minutes
scoring time
Ross Test of Higher Cognitive Processes (RTHCP; Ross & Ross, 1976)
Target clinical population: Students (used to identify students for gifted programs in schools;
sometimes applied to adults)
Constructs assessed: High-level thinking skills (analysis, synthesis, and evaluation, organization
and reasoning); includes verbal analogies, deduction, assumption,
identification, word relationships, sentence sequencing, interpreting
answers to questions, information sufficiency and relevance in
mathematical problems, and analysis of attributes of complex stick figures
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
SCAN-3:A Test for Auditory Processing Disorders in Children and Adults (SCAN3-A; Keith, 2009)
Target clinical population: People age 13 years and older
Constructs assessed: Auditory processing
Typical time to administer: Screening: 10–15 minutes; diagnostic assessment: 30–45 minutes

308
Table 20–7. continued

Speed and Capacity of Language Processing Test (SCOLP; Baddeley et al., 1992)
Target clinical population: People with brain injury, dementia, schizophrenia, older people, and
people exposed to drugs, stressors, and alcohol
Constructs assessed: Speed of cognitive processing, cognitive capacity
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
The Speech, Spatial, and Qualities of Hearing Scale (SSQ; Gatehouse & Noble, 2004)
Target clinical population: Adults, with or without hearing aids or cochlear implants
Constructs assessed: Self-ratings of hearing and auditory processing in a variety of competing
contexts, and related to directional, distance, and movement components
of spatial hearing
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Stroop Color and Word Test-Adult (SCWT-A; Golden & Freshwater, 2002)
Target clinical population: Adults
Constructs assessed: Attention, executive functioning
Typical time to administer: 5 minutes
Symbol Digit Modality Test (SDMT; Smith, 1973)
Target clinical population: Adults
Constructs assessed: Psychomotor speed and attention/integration without requiring linguistic
responses
Typical time to administer: 5 minutes
Test of Adolescent/Adult Word Finding-2 (TAWF-2; German, 2016)
Target clinical population: People age 12 years and above
Constructs assessed: Word finding
Typical time to administer: 20–30 minutes
Tasks of Executive Control (TEC; Isquith et al., 2010)
Target clinical population: Children and adolescents with executive function impairment (may be
applied with adults if relevant)
Constructs assessed: Word finding
Typical time to administer: 20–30 minutes
Test of Everyday Attention (TEA; Ridgeway et al., 1994)
Target clinical population: People suspected of having attention deficits
Constructs assessed: Selective attention, sustained attention, divided attention, attentional
switching
Typical time to administer: 45–60 minutes
continues

309
Table 20–7. continued

Test of Language Competence, Expanded Edition (TLC-E; Wiig & Secord, 1989)
Target clinical population: People with delayed language (may be applied with adults if relevant)
Constructs assessed: Processing of lexical and syntactic ambiguities, logical inferencing, syntax
and semantics in sentence generation, interpretation of metaphor, and
recall of word pairs
Typical time to administer: Less than 60 minutes
Test of Nonverbal Intelligence-4 (TONI-4; Brown et al., 2010)
Target clinical population: Anyone
Constructs assessed: Intelligence, aptitude, abstract reasoning, problem-solving
Typical time to administer: 5–20 minutes
Visual Analog Mood Scales (VAMS; Stern, 1997) and Visual Analog Mood Scales, Revised
(VAMS-R; Kontou et al., 2012)
Target clinical population: Adults with neurological impairments, especially in adults in medical and
psychiatric settings
Constructs assessed: Internal mood states (Afraid, Confused, Sad, Angry, Energetic, Tired,
Happy, and Tense)
Typical time to administer: 5–15 minutes
Visual Analogue Self-Esteem Scale (VASES; Brumfitt & Sheeran, 1999)
Target clinical population: People with communication impairment
Constructs assessed: Self-esteem
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Wechsler Memory Scale-III (WMS; Wechsler, 2009)
Target clinical population: Adults with suspected memory problems
Constructs assessed: Auditory and visual learning, auditory and visual short- and long-term
memory; includes brief cognitive status screening tool
Typical time to administer: 60–90 minutes
Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR; Wechsler, 2001)
Target clinical population: Adults
Constructs assessed: Pre-onset intellectual and memory abilities
Typical time to administer: 5–10 minutes
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST; Heaton, Thompson, Psychological Assessment Resources, &
Business Video Productions, 1995)
Target clinical population: People with TBI, neurodegenerative disease, or mental illness such as
schizophrenia
Constructs assessed: Abstract reasoning and executive function: strategic planning; organized
searching; and ability to utilize environmental feedback to shift cognitive
sets, direct behavior toward achieving a goal, and modulate impulsive
responding
Typical time to administer: 20–30 minutes

310
Table 20–7. continued

Woodcock Johnson III Normative Update (NU) Tests of Cognitive Abilities (WJIII NU; Woodcock
et al., 2007)
Target clinical population: Children and adults
Constructs assessed: General intellectual ability, information-processing abilities, (working
memory, planning, naming speed, attention, and executive functioning),
oral language, and academic achievement
Typical time to administer: About 5 minutes per test; Cognitive Standard 7 tests (35–45 minutes);
Achievement Standard 11 tests (55–65 minutes)
Word Test-Revised (TWT-R; Huisingh et al., 1990)
Target clinical population: School-age children (may be applied with adults if relevant)
Constructs assessed: Semantics at the word level (categorizing, finding relationships among
words, generating synonyms and antonyms, detecting semantic
incongruities, and defining words)
Typical time to administer: Unspecified
Note. Additional tools that may be used to index potentially confounding factors in assessment are described in Chapter 19.

311
312  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter 4. Name and briefly describe at least two
that are new to you or that you have not yet screening instruments used to get a quick
mastered. idea of an adult’s linguistic strengths and
2. How might each of the following help you weaknesses.
determine which assessment tools to use in a 5. Name and briefly describe at least two
given situation: screening instruments used to get a quick
a. The reason you are carrying out a given idea of an adult’s cognitive strengths and
assessment? weaknesses.
b. The nature of the individual being 6. List and briefly describe at least three
assessed? standardized aphasia batteries.
c. The quality of any given tool under 7. List and briefly describe at least one
consideration? published assessment tool for indexing the
d. Your own preferences and preferred cognitive-communicative abilities of a TBI
theoretical frameworks? survivor.
e. The practicality of using a particular 8. List and briefly describe at least one
tool? published assessment tool for indexing
3. How might standardized tools other the cognitive-communicative abilities of a
than those designed to explicitly examine person with right brain syndrome.
aphasia be used in the diagnostic process 9. List and briefly describe at least one
for a person with aphasia? Discuss what published assessment tool for indexing
information such tools may yield to help in the cognitive-communicative abilities of a
the differential diagnosis of aphasia. Why is person with dementia.
it important to distinguish this information
from that generally obtained through For additional learning and reflection activities,
www
standardized aphasia batteries? see the companion website.
CHAPTER
21
Discourse and Conversation as
Vital Aspects of Assessment

As discussed in Chapter 17, learning about individ- 7. What are best practices in interpreting
uals’ abilities in a variety of discourse contexts is discourse analysis results?
essential to dynamic assessment. Challenges in the 8. What challenges do aphasiologists face in
interactive use of language are the most detrimen- applying discourse analysis in clinical practice
tal impacts of a language disorder in terms of social and research?
relationships, quality of life, independence, self-es- 9. How may aphasiologists confront the
teem, and professional and educational opportu- challenges in applying discourse analysis in
nities. If we do not assess how a person functions clinical practice and research?
in authentic communicative contexts, then we are
failing to assess an absolutely fundamental aspect
of that person’s true communicative strengths and
What Is Discourse?
weaknesses. In this chapter, we consider what we
mean by discourse, discourse genres, and discourse
analysis. We then consider specific ways in which Discourse is the interactive use of language, encom-
discourse analysis is clinically important, means of passing comprehension and production, regardless
sampling and analyzing discourse, and best prac- of modality (written, spoken, or signed language,
tices in analyzing results. Finally, challenges in dis- and verbal as well as nonverbal communication). In
course sampling and conversational analysis in the the 1970s, Bloom and Lahey (1978) conceptualized
context of real-world clinical and research practice language as having three subsystems: form, content,
are considered. and use. Form (phonology, morphology, and syntax)
After reading and reflecting on the content in and content (semantics) are subjects that had long
this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in been studied in linguistics, speech-language pathol-
your own words, the following queries: ogy, and related areas. However, the emphasis on
the actual use of language was novel at the time
1. What is discourse? and led to a host of new analytic methods as well
2. What are general categories, types, or genres of as treatment strategies focused on the social use of
discourse? language (Bates, 1976). Pragmatic abilities include
3. What is conversational or discourse analysis? an integrated combination of cognitive and linguis-
4. Why is discourse sampling and analysis tic abilities (Coelho et al., 1995; Duff et al., 2012; Lê
important? et al., 2012; Mozeiko et al., 2011).
5. What are key strategies for sampling discourse? Just as the systems underlying the form and
6. What are key measures for indexing discourse content of language are rule based, so is pragmat-
competence? ics. People sharing interactive communication must

313
314  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

share and apply knowledge about how discourse is production and comprehension in narrative tasks are
to be carried out. They must know, for example, how easiest because narrative structures are so familiar
and when to take turns; how to take into account and so well practiced from an early age. Procedural
the listener’s (or reader’s) point of view and prior and expository speech has less predictable structure,
knowledge; how to adjust to the appropriate level of and the proportion of new information exchanged
formality; which language and dialect to use; what is much greater than in narrative discourse; thus,
words and expressions are appropriate or inappro- producing and comprehending it is more difficult
priate in the given context; which gestures, facial (Shadden, 2011).
expressions, and intonation to use and how to inter- One way of categorizing aspects of the social
pret those of others; how close to stand or sit to oth- use of language, regardless of genre, is by designat-
ers; and in what patterns of eye contact to engage. ing three levels:
Many use the word conversation interchange-
ably with the word discourse. Others argue that con- • linguistic (the verbal form and content)
versation connotes primarily oral communication; • extralinguistic (the environmental context of
written communication is also important when con- language use and the roles and backgrounds
sidering discourse. Given the degree of conversation of the individuals involved in an exchange)
that occurs in written form through letters, texting, • paralinguistic (prosodic aspects of intonation,
e-mail, and social media, the separation of written pitch, and stress; use of gestures and facial
versus nonwritten aspects of conversation is blurred. expressions) (Davis, 1986)
For that reason, some prefer the word discourse when
the purpose is to convey a more all-encompassing This categorical scheme was discussed in Chapter 12
notion of expressive, receptive, written, oral, and as a way of categorizing areas of discourse or prag-
nonverbal communication. matics challenge for people with right brain injury
(RBI).
Discourse may also be considered in terms of
What Are General Categories, speech acts, the intended purposes underlying the
specific communicative intent of what is expressed
Types, or Genres of Discourse?
at any given moment or in a given context (Grice,
1975; Searle, 1969). The following are examples of
Different genres of discourse entail different under- speech acts:
lying rules and require different types of knowledge
and skill. Commonly studied genres include • greetings
• requests for information
• conversational (social, interactive); • assertions
• narrative (telling and retelling stories); • persuasion
• procedural (giving instructions); • protests
• expository (providing information, explaining • agreements
ideas or processes, defining constructs); • conversational repair
• persuasive (convincing, providing evidence to
support an opinion or request for action); and Speech acts may be direct or indirect, verbal or
• descriptive (conveying attributes). nonverbal. Different types of speech acts require dif-
ferent types of knowledge and skill.
Each genre requires engaging in certain aspects Additional means of classifying types of dis-
of discourse. For example, storytelling typically course include the following:
includes establishing who the characters are, estab-
lishing the time and place, and conveying main • register (the level of formality/informality, or
ideas, actions, and outcomes; most of these aspects the degree of highly specialized jargon used
are less relevant to other discourse types. Typically, within specific professional or social groups)
21. Discourse and Conversation as Vital Aspects of Assessment   315

• context (e.g., clinical facility, home, workplace, memory impairments tend to have difficulty with
etc.) discourse cohesion and coherence, as discussed fur-
• means of elicitation (e.g., solicited versus ther later (Kurczek & Duff, 2011; Peach, 2013). Still,
unsolicited, naturalistic versus constrained) given the diversity of pre-onset pragmatic abilities
and the complexity of communication in social con-
texts, it is impossible to predict any individual’s
What Is Conversational or Discourse Analysis? pragmatic strengths and weaknesses without care-
fully assessing them.

Discourse analysis entails the methodical record-


ing of discourse samples as people communicate Discourse Analysis Helps Determine
in well-described contexts, selection of specific seg- Strengths and Weaknesses Not Evident
ments of discourse for analysis, and application of Through Other Forms of Assessment
specific analytic methods using specific units or
measures of discourse performance. Discourse anal- The cognitive-linguistic profile assembled based on
ysis enables us to tune in systematically to linguistic, speech and language test results is often in stark con-
extralinguistic, and paralinguistic aspects of perfor- trast to statements that caregivers, friends, and fam-
mance across varied discourse genres. ily members make about an individual’s functional
communication abilities. Discourse abilities are often
at the heart of such contrasts and are the primary
Why Is Discourse Sampling reasons for which psychosocial aspects of acquired
and Analysis Important? language disorders can be more disabling than the
neuropsychological ones.
A person with a severe language disorder may
Let’s consider how discourse sampling and analysis have intact pragmatic abilities that serve as com-
are vital to clinical practice and research. municative strengths. Many people with aphasia
demonstrate exceptional pragmatic abilities that
enable them to communicate effectively in situa-
Discourse, Especially the Social Use of tions in which one would predict poor performance
Language, Is Highly Relevant to Every based on verbal abilities alone. Varying the dis-
Type of Acquired Neurogenic Disorder course genre sampled often helps uncover strengths
in people with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and
Regardless of etiology, severity, type, or lesion loca- dementia that might otherwise go unnoticed. Con-
tion, a language disorder is likely to affect discourse versations about topics of varied levels of relevance
abilities. At the same time, impaired or unusual pat- to a person’s past (e.g., entailing career expertise,
terns of pragmatic performance tend to be associated hobbies, and upbringing versus world events or
with people who have certain types of neurogenic general knowledge of less personal relevance) help
language disorders. For example, people with Wer- elucidate strengths that are often left unassessed
nicke’s aphasia often violate turn-taking rules and in older adults and brain injury survivors (Coelho,
fail to repair conversational breakdown. People with Ylvisaker, & Turkstra, 2005; Dijkstra et al., 2004).
Broca’s aphasia tend to initiate new topics less than Knowing about such strengths is especially import-
people without neurological disorders. People with ant for those in institutional settings where there is
frontal lobe lesions due to traumatic brain injury a paucity of information available to staff members
(TBI) often demonstrate challenges with inhibition regarding the sorts of conversational topics that will
of inappropriate content and with discourse cohe- best bring out an individual’s best communicative
sion. People with RBI tend to have more difficulty competence (Hickey & Douglas, 2021).
than others with interpreting facial expressions and Conversely, a person with even a mild language
speech intonation patterns. People with neurogenic disorder may have impaired pragmatic abilities that
316  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

dramatically affect everyday communication. Many Discourse Analysis Is Vital to


TBI survivors, for example, are discharged from Treatment Planning
inpatient acute care or rehabilitation stays and are
never offered SLP assessment or treatment because The actual use of language is fundamental to the
of their high-level verbal abilities, only to later con- most important treatment outcomes. For many
front profound interpersonal communication chal- people with neurogenic communication disorders,
lenges associated with pragmatic deficits. Such focusing on pragmatic abilities is vital to educational
deficits are often ignored in assessments that rely and professional opportunities and success and to
primarily on standardized aphasia batteries (Coelho, social relationships. It is thus important to incorpo-
Grela, et al., 2005). rate discourse-related goals in treatment planning
(Cherney et al., 1998; Ehlhardt et al., 2008; Hengst,
Discourse Analysis May Yield Critical 2020; Kennedy et al., 2008; Kilov et al., 2009; Ylvi-
Information for Differential Diagnosis saker et al., 2005). Doing so requires that discourse-​
related metrics be incorporated as baselines before
treatment, as ongoing measures of progress during
Just as certain patterns of discourse strengths and treatment, and as indices of treatment outcomes. As
weaknesses tend to characterize people with varied Lyon (1999) aptly states, “No matter how good, valid,
types of language disorders, examining these pat- or accurate our clinical constructs and solutions, they
terns may help in substantiating diagnostic results. will not endure unless the living of life is measurably
Consider, for example, the challenge of differenti- and decisively better for those we treat” (p. 689).
ating characteristics of normal aging from MCI, as
discussed in Chapter 9. If a person leaves out a few
details in a story retelling task (which could occur in Discourse Analysis Is an
normal aging), this may be contrasted with leaving
Essential Aspect of Research
out main ideas or getting distracted from the task
of retelling the story (which is more suggestive of
MCI or dementia). People with dementia tend to For all of the reasons stated earlier, discourse analy-
have trouble with productive narrative organization, sis is an important focus of research on neurogenic
whereas age-matched older people tend not to. Also, language disorders, not just of clinical practice. Con-
people with dementia are less competent at identify- sideration of discourse is fundamental to research
ing the main points of a story (Welland et al., 2002) underlying evidence-based practice.
and deducing the moral of a story (Shadden, 2011).
Adults with closed head injuries have been shown
to have poorer organization of critical content when What Are Key Strategies for
retelling stories than those without brain injuries
Sampling Discourse?
(Lindsey et al., 2019).
There are limitations, of course, to basing con-
clusions about differential diagnosis solely on dis- All the principles for best practice for assessment
course analysis. Consider, for example, the concerns mentioned in Chapter 17 apply to discourse sampling
noted in Chapter 10 regarding the terms fluent ver- and analysis. A foremost consideration in deciding
sus nonfluent as diagnostic categories for people with on discourse sampling strategies is what the goals of
aphasia. People classified as having “nonfluent” sampling are. Are there certain clinical hypotheses to
aphasia have been shown to have reduced gram- be explored? Are there questions pertaining to differ-
matical structural complexity and shorter utterance ential diagnosis? Is there a discourse-related concern
length in conversation for different underlying rea- that the person you are serving or that a significant
sons (Saffran et al., 1989). It is important to explore other wishes to explore further? Is there a critical
those underlying reasons through methodical testing aspect of interaction that you think may become the
in addition to studying discourse production. focus of an intervention goal?
21. Discourse and Conversation as Vital Aspects of Assessment   317

Examining discourse does not necessarily Gurland, 1989), and the Discourse Abilities Profile
restrict us to using nonstandardized methods. Sev- (Terrell & Ripich, 1989). Each includes a distinct set
eral examples of standardized tests that address dis- of discourse or pragmatic behaviors to be monitored
course and pragmatic performance are included in and metrics to be tracked. Still, given that there is
the listings of assessment tools in Chapter 20. The no consensus as to which measures are the best to
Discourse Comprehension Test (Brookshire & Nich- recommend as core measures in aphasia assessments
olas, 1997), for example, is based on paragraph-level (see Kurland & Stokes, 2018), there is no consensus
spoken stories. Communicative Abilities of Daily on the types of published instruments that would be
Living–2 (CADL-3; Holland et al., 2018) contains most useful for clinical practice.
items that address several aspects of pragmatics, Regardless of the specific approach selected, it
including social interaction, use of humor and meta- is important that the approach be methodical so as
phor, “functional” reading and writing abilities, and not to miss important strengths and weaknesses. It
contextual communication. The Amsterdam Nijme- is also critical to sample both production and under-
gen Everyday Language Test (ANELT; Blomert et al., standing during a variety of discourse tasks and
1994) includes scripted interview items pertaining to genres. Quantitative and qualitative results tend to
common situations. Other tools for indexing com- differ across analyses of varied types of discourse
municative effectiveness, such as the CETI (Lomas elicited from a single person (Armstrong, 2000; Cher-
et al., 1989), include ratings of the use of language ney, 1998). Thus, it is essential to assess performance
in daily situations. Still, there is no single compre- across a variety of the genres listed earlier.
hensive tool for assessing discourse abilities that is In addition to varying genre and discourse
agreed upon by all or that fits all discourse analysis tasks, there are many varied means of eliciting
needs. Furthermore, there is no consensus to date on responses. Probe questions during a discourse com-
tools or measures that should be used to ensure the prehension task, for example, may entail asking yes/
validity and reliability of either for aphasia assess- no questions, asking what the main idea is, request-
ment (de Reisthal & Diehl, 2017; Dietz & Boyle, 2018; ing that the individual tell what happens next in a
Kim & Harris Wright, 2020; Stark, 2019). story, or an instruction to analyze facts versus opin-
Most general standardized language assess- ions expressed in a conversation or written passage.
ment batteries also include some form of discourse Results of discourse analysis tend to differ in
elicitation; however, they tend to comprise primar- evoked conversation compared to naturally occur-
ily picture description tasks. Although these may ring spontaneous conversation, especially if the scor-
be informative and lend themselves to standard- ing rules are not sufficiently detailed and explicit to
ized means of scoring language production, picture be interpreted and applied consistently by multi-
descriptions alone are not sufficient for develop- ple evaluators (Oelschlaeger & Thorne, 1999). This
ing a holistic appreciation for a person’s discourse makes it challenging to judge the carryover from
competence (Capilouto et al., 2005, 2006; Wright more to less structured conversation and from con-
et al., 2011). When describing pictures, many peo- versations in a clinical environment to those in real-
ple — even with mild linguistic challenges — limit life contexts.
themselves to labeling objects, people, and actions The context in which discourse is elicited for
and to using simple syntactic structures. The content each genre may also affect, sometimes dramatically,
of pictures shown is often not personally relevant to the indices of communicative competence. Context
them. Also, scoring systems tend not to allow credit may be manipulated, for example, in terms of the
for expounding on related ideas or personal inter- following:
pretations or associated ideas.
Several aphasiologists have developed means • degree of short- or long-term recall implicated
of methodically analyzing language samples once • degree of communicative support provided
they are collected. These include the Profile of • relationship and roles between the individual
Communication Appropriateness (Penn, 1988), the being assessed and the conversational partner
Assessment Protocol of Pragmatic Skills (Gerber & and/or clinician
318  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

TBI survivors have been shown to have greater the discourse input in terms of aspects and metrics
dependence on examiners than those without brain of interest. A variety of computerized transcription
injuries for maintaining conversational topics, shar- and analysis programs may be used to aid in this
ing information, and keeping conversations going process. Examples are given in Table 21–1. Some,
(Coelho et al., 2002). Interactive support provided such as Systematic Analysis of Language Tran-
by an examiner or other conversational partner may scripts (SALT), were designed especially for clinical
have a strong influence on discourse production use, whereas others have been designed for varied
measures. The role of the clinician with regard to the types of qualitative research and data mining. Appli-
person being assessed, including perceived status, cations for such tools range far beyond the types of
comfort level, and social connectedness, are addi- discourse analysis discussed in this chapter. Many of
tional factors that might influence performance. these tools may be applied to ethnographic research
The degree of communicative support depends and to analyses of such diverse sources of input as
on input from the communication partner and also interviews, surveys, focus group discussions, Internet
on the tasks at hand. For example, results may vary content, and written and pictographic material in gen-
for video description and narration, telephone con- eral (e.g., book, articles, newspapers, etc.). Although
versation, role-playing, picture-based descriptions, computerized transcription, coding, and analysis
assembly of pictures into a story sequence, and real- certainly reduce the labor and increase the reliability
life use of language (say, asking for help in a store, of discourse analysis compared to what can be done
or informing people about the nature of one’s stroke without such software, coding can be time-consum-
or TBI). ing, and many software programs require training or
Task instructions matter, too. Simply asking a at least significant learning and practice.
person to describe an event, retell a story, or describe A network of hundreds of clinicians and
a picture may yield different results compared to researchers who contribute clinical data, including
requesting specific information. For example, ask- discourse videos and analyses, is organized through
ing a person to tell a story with a beginning, middle, TalkBank (talkbank.org). TalkBank consists of data,
and end rather than simply to tell a story has been resources, stimuli, and additional information that
shown to yield qualitatively and quantitatively dif- support communication analyses. It includes CHAT,
ferent narratives (Wright & Capilouto, 2009). a transcription and coding tool; CLAN and MOR,
analysis tools; and a database tool. Clinical areas
include several areas of practice related to children
and adults, and include AphasiaBank, TBIBank,
What Are Key Measures for RHDBank, and DementiaBank. Each of these banks
Indexing Discourse Competence? include rich resources, such as assessment protocols
(several across diverse languages), stimulus items,
scripts, and bibliographies of works presented and
Once decisions have been made about discourse published based on each bank’s data.
tasks, associated stimuli, and means of evoking The specific measures to be used in discourse
responses and instructing participants, a speech sam- analysis depend on the information needed to
ple is collected, ideally using audio-video recording. address assessment questions or track gains in dis-
Nicholas and Brookshire (1993) recommended at course abilities over time. Unfortunately, there is a
least 300 to 400 words per sample for sufficient reli- lack of information about the psychometric proper-
ability over time; similarly, Doyle et al. (2000) recom- ties of most measures, and insufficient evidence to
mend a sample of about 400 words. However, given support the choice of measures for most applications
what we now know about variability in discourse and contexts (Pritchard, Hilari, Cocks, & Dipper,
tasks, elicitation procedures, assessment contexts, 2018; Spell et al., 2020; Stark et al., 2020, 2021). Bryant
and measures, these are not necessarily appropriate et al. (2016) report 536 distinct discourse measures.
guidelines for all discourse assessment. A summary of the types of metrics commonly used
If spoken, the discourse sample is then tran- in published work on discourse analysis is given in
scribed and coded. Coding is any means of marking Box 21–1.
21. Discourse and Conversation as Vital Aspects of Assessment   319

Table 21–1. Programs for Transcribing and Analyzing Discourse

Name Source Description


Coding Analysis CAT: Open-source free software for
Toolkit (CAT) https://cat-help.texifter.com coding, managing, and analyzing
text; allows for multiple collaborators
and indexes inter-rater reliability
Dedoose Dedoose: Web-based app for qualitative and
https://www.dedoose.com/ mixed methods analysis; includes
text, video, and spreadsheet
features
General GATE: Open-source free software for
Architecture for https://gate.ac.uk/ tagging and quantifying several
Text Engineering types of linguistic and extralinguistic
(GATE) elements; available in several
languages
Kwalitan Kwalitan: Program that permits coding,
https://www.kwalitan.nl retrieval, and categorization of text,
image, audio, and video input
Natural Language NLTK: Program that enables coding
Toolkit (NLTK) https://www.nltk.org and tagging for syntactic and
sematic analysis with a basis in
computational linguistics
MAXQDA MAXQDA: Software for organization,
https://www.maxqda.com/ evaluation, coding, annotation, and
interpretation of data plus report
generation and data visualization
QDA Miner Provalis Research: Qualitative software for coding,
https://provalisresearch. annotating, retrieving, and analyzing
com/products/qualitative-​ transcripts and other documents,
data-analysis-software/ plus images

Systematic Analysis SALT Software: Software for elicitation and


of Language https://www.saltsoftware. transcription of language samples;
Transcripts (SALT) com/ enables comparisons with “typical
speakers”; offers free online training.
Tropes Semantic Knowledge: Free natural language software
https://www.semantic- enabling semantic analysis, keyword
knowledge.com/ extraction, and identification of
“fundamental propositions”
UAM Corpus Tool UAM Corpus tool: Tool that enables text annotation for
http://www.corpustool.com/ linguistic analysis, from single words
to whole texts
WordStat Provalis Research: Software for content analysis and
https://provalisresearch. text mining, automatic tagging
com
320  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Box
21–1 Examples of Categories of Discourse Analysis Measures

Surface-level linguistic measures • Relevance, numbers and proportions of


• Number of words, mean number of words on- and off-topic comments
per sample • Correctness of pronoun use
• Number of words after fillers, revisions, • Number and type of literal and semantic
and false starts are removed paraphasias
• Number of utterances • Number of ambiguous terms
• Type-token ratio for semantic performance, • Number and type of circumlocutions
syntactic performance, vocabulary use • Use of alternative strategies such as
• Core lexicon (index of the most critical gesture, AAC, drawing, and writing
lexical items required for a meaningful and
coherent narrative) Indices of discourse comprehension
• Syntactic, phrase, or utterance length (in • Responses to yes/no questions about stated
words or morphemes) or implicit content and about main ideas
• Completeness (of utterances, ideas, or • Written and spoken questions regarding
syntactic structures) inferences
• Number of open- and closed-class words
• Number of nouns and verbs Category constructs related to information
exchange and pragmatics
Indices of informativeness • Eye contact
• Content units • Gaze direction
• Propositions • Joint visual attention
• CIU and mean CIUs • Turn-taking behaviors
• Number and accuracy of main ideas • Codeswitching behaviors
• Number and accuracy of main events • Topic initiation
or other important elements mentioned • Topic maintenance patterns, frequency of
during storytelling/retelling on- and off-topic utterances
• Main events indexed during narrative • Conversational repair behaviors
tasks • Response appropriateness
• Steps described when explaining a • Elaboration
procedural task • Requests for repetition, clarification,
• Story goodness (story grammar and story additional information
completeness) • Use of compensatory strategies (e.g.,
drawing, gestures, pantomime, vocalizing,
Efficiency and intoning without words)
• Syllables, words, clauses, phrases, or • Verbal and nonverbal cues
content information units per minute, • Feedback that the listener had understood
per given communicative task, T-unit, or content conveyed
written or spoken discourse sample
Cohesion analysis indices
Symptom- and compensatory strategy-related • Number of cohesive ties, e.g.,
metrics • Personal pronouns (e.g., we, you, they)
• Pauses, interjections, revisions, and • Demonstrative pronouns (e.g., that,
repetitions those, here, there)
• Paraphasias, neologisms • Reiteration
21. Discourse and Conversation as Vital Aspects of Assessment   321

• Causal conjunctives (e.g., because, in Coherence analysis


order to) • Ratings of local and global relatedness of
• Temporal conjunctives (e.g., yet, while, utterance or T-units
during)
• Additive conjunctives (e.g., and, Speech act analysis
furthermore, likewise) • Type and frequency of speech acts (e.g.,
• Ellipsis (leaving out information greetings, requests for information,
previously stated because it is assumed assertions, statements of protests)
the listener knows it) • Adherence to social rules
• Proportion of cohesive ties that are • Strengths and weaknesses in sending and
complete/incomplete or appropriate/ receiving information
erroneous

Note: These are mere examples. There are numerous derivatives of these categories and types of measures.

Surface-level metrics (as listed in Box 21–1) are of discourse informativeness, is proposed as a more
those that are primarily based on linguistic aspects sensitive measure of discourse for people with apha-
of communication, especially phonology, morphol- sia (Dalton & Richardson, 2019).
ogy, syntax, and semantics. A basic form of a lexically Another way to index informativeness is by
based type-token ratio (measure of lexical diversity) indexing specific ideas conveyed within a sentence
is calculated as the total number of unique words in (Coelho et al., 2005). A main event index was devel-
a sample divided by the total number of words in oped by Wright et al. (2005) to focus on an individ-
the sample. A core lexicon measure is based on the ual’s ability to convey the relationships and causal
number of important lexical items that are essential connections between ideas in narrative discourse.
for producing a meaningful, coherent narrative (Kim The number and accuracy of main events identi-
& Wright, 2020). A basic form of syntactically based fied are especially relevant in storytelling/retelling
type-token ratio is calculated as the total number of and picture description (Capilouto et al., 2005, 2006;
distinct syntactic structures produced divided by the Nicholas & Brookshire, 1995).
number of sentences or informational units. Given A story goodness index has been proposed to
that the length of the discourse sample analyzed has capture indices of the organizational structure of
an influence on these ratios, additional metrics have storytelling (story grammar) and story completeness
been proposed to adjust for that influence (e.g., Dal- (Lê et al., 2012; Mozeiko et al., 2011). Story grammar
ton et al., 2020; Fergadiodis & Wright, 2011). is based on the proportion of units within a story
Indices of informativeness are based on the narrative that contribute to conveying the story con-
accuracy and relevance of information conveyed. tent. Story completeness is based on comparisons
Nicholas and Brookshire’s (1993) analysis is a stan- with the most common events and characters men-
dardized rule-based scoring system for indexing tioned by people without any neurological disorder
informativeness that has been studied for people who have retold stories based on a standard set of
with aphasia and adults without neurological dis- pictures. Measures of efficiency take into account
orders. The correct information unit (CIU) metric is that not only the amount and accuracy of discourse
derived by counting the number of words and con- matter; the amount of information conveyed per unit
tent information units that are accurate, relevant, of time, task, or specified discourse sample length
and informative in terms of the stimulus used for is also important (Fergadiotis et al., 2015; Yorkston
elicitation, then dividing the number of words by the & Beukelman, 1980). Measures of speaking rate are
number of CIUs. Main concept analysis, a measure included in this category.
322  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Metrics corresponding to language disorder Examples are pronoun use, use of causal conjunc-
symptoms or compensatory strategies are also tives (specifying a cause, reason, or result, e.g., oth-
frequently tracked as a means of indexing com- erwise, because), temporal conjunctives (referring
municative strengths and weaknesses as well as to time, e.g., afterward, beforehand, then, simulta-
improvements or regression over time. For exam- neously), additive conjunctives (introducing added
ple, pauses, interjections, revisions, and repetitions information, e.g., furthermore, in addition, in con-
may indicate word-finding problems or challenges trast), ellipsis (leaving out information previously
related to organizational demands (Schiller et al., stated because it is assumed the listener knows it),
2007). Tracking such symptoms and strategies at and instances of reiterating content previously stated
the discourse level is vital when they are relevant to to help tie components of discourse together. These
treatment goals. Tracking the relevance and appro- may be tracked in terms of frequency (i.e., the num-
priateness of comments is important for an individ- ber used per unit of time or number of words) as
ual working toward improved executive strategies well as in terms of correctness and appropriateness.
during conversation. Indexing independent and Although challenges with cohesion in discourse
supported use of compensatory strategies to com- may occur in language-normal adults and in people
municate content regardless of modality or linguistic with any type of language disorder, they have been
correctness is important for people with a wide array especially noted in TBI survivors and people with
of communication problems. dementia (Coelho et al., 1995, 2002; Kurczek & Duff,
Means of indexing discourse comprehension in 2011; Mentis & Prutting, 1987).
people with neurogenic cognitive-linguistic disor- Discourse coherence refers to the continuity
ders are addressed less frequently in the literature of meaning throughout conversation or text. It may
than those focused on discourse production. Written be local (referring to continuity across consecutive
and spoken discourse comprehension assessment utterances) or global (referring to topic maintenance
is often focused on one of two areas: the ability to or interrelatedness across larger samples of dis-
make inferences and the ability to understand main course). It may be indexed via subjective ratings of
ideas. Inferencing in the context of discourse entails how hard or easy it is to relate an utterance or min-
the ability to draw conclusions that are not explicitly imal terminal unit (T-unit) to a topic (Kurczek &
stated by synthesizing information across compo- Duff, 2011; Shadden, 1998).
nents of discourse content. Understanding of main Speech acts, too, discussed earlier as important
ideas entails detecting what information is most aspects of discourse may be analyzed according to
salient and discerning critical aspects of what is said the apparent intention of the person communicating,
or written from details. what is expressed, what is understood by others, and
Indices of information exchange are those that the social rules that pertain to a given communica-
pertain more to dyads or groups during interaction, tive exchange. People with aphasia have been shown
not just to the individual with a communication dis- to have strengths in roles as senders and receivers
order. Examples are use of eye contact and joint visual during speech acts despite specific linguistics defi-
attention, turn-taking behaviors, conversational cits (Armstrong, 2001; Wilcox et al., 1978).
repair strategies, compensatory strategies, reminders Speech acts may be largely influenced by the
and cues to use supported communication strategies, specific discourse genre or task being targeted for
and feedback about communicative effectiveness. analysis. For example, it would be unlikely that a
Discourse cohesion entails individual and person engaged in a picture description task would
collective means of making connections across engage in greeting or protest as part of the picture
utterances, individuals, and topics in spoken or description process. For this reason, speech act anal-
written communication. Metrics for cohesion anal- ysis is most pertinent to spontaneous conversation.
ysis include cohesive ties, that is, discourse markers If specific types of speech acts are particularly rel-
indicative of a speaker’s or writer’s ability to main- evant to an individual and an important focus of
tain a common theme or topic, weaving together treatment, then it is logical that those particular
content at varied levels (e.g., phrases, sentences, speech acts would be monitored in conversational
paragraphs, conversations, stories) (Cherney, 1998). analysis.
21. Discourse and Conversation as Vital Aspects of Assessment   323

vocabulary, education level, and general interest in


What Are Best Practices in Interpreting and knowledge of topics discussed are all important
Discourse Analysis Results? factors influencing discourse performance. Cultural
background also has a strong influence on discourse
Valid and reliable coding takes a great deal of training patterns, so it must be taken into account in any
and practice. Even then, changing contexts, content, evaluation of effectiveness. One may not confidently
and partners may make evaluations inconsistent. If draw discourse assessment conclusions without
discourse indices are to be used to document treat- ongoing thorough evaluation, observation, and con-
ment outcomes, it is critical to obtain stable baselines sultation with others who have known the individ-
prior to treatment (Cameron et al., 2010; Wright et al., ual for a long time and can attest to which aspects
2005). Primary sources of intraindividual variability of discourse performance appear to have changed
to be taken into account in discourse analysis are compared to specific points in the past.
summarized in Box 21–2. An essential element of best practice in dis-
When interpreting discourse analysis results, course analysis is to apply assessment findings to
it is essential to keep in mind the great variability effective intervention planning. Discourse-based
in discourse production and comprehension across intervention may entail a combination of attention
individuals, too, whether or not they have a lan- process training and memory treatment from sen-
guage disorder. A person’s general cognitive ability, tence to discourse (oral or written) contexts, practice

Box
21–2 Primary Sources of Intraindividual Variability to Be
Taken Into Account in Discourse Analysis

• Discourse genre
• Discourse modality (spoken, written, signed)
• Task and elicitation procedure
• Sampling method
• Sample length
• Specific discourse indices tracked and analyzed
• Task instructions
• Cues
• Topic difficulty
• Topic familiarity
• Transcription procedures
• Abstractness/concreteness of content
• Degree of short- and long-term recall implicated
• Availability and use of supports (e.g., gesture, AAC devices, drawing and
writing during conversation)
• Physical environment
• Mood, motivation, interests, and other aspects of psychological states of
participants
• Concomitant medical challenges of participants that may influence
reliability (e.g., pain, glucose level, somnolescence)
• Skills, abilities, interpersonal familiarity, roles, and affective qualities of
participants (all involved in a given discourse context)
• Expertise, prior training and practice, and biases of the person analyzing
the sample
• Metrics used for analysis
324  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

with specific conversational strategies, and coach- and video recording equipment, computers, and
ing with conversational partners (see Sections VI software for transcription analysis and reporting are
through VIII). important tools to have in place.

Clear Communication and Perceived Relevance


What Challenges Do Aphasiologists
Face in Applying Discourse Analysis
Few medical and rehabilitation professionals other
in Clinical Practice and Research?
than SLPs, let alone people with communication
disorders and their families, have training about
Challenges in carrying out discourse analysis in the detailed aspects of discourse. It can be difficult to
everyday work life of the SLP include issues of time, describe aspects of discourse in ways that are mean-
training, and mentorship; equipment and software; ingful and relevant to the people for whom our
communication and perceived relevance; replicabil- results are ideally most pertinent.
ity; and variability in the evidence base. Let’s con-
sider these briefly.
Replicability and Variability
in the Evidence Base
Time
Given that naturalistic settings entail myriad uncon-
Discourse analysis can be time and labor intensive. trolled contextual aspects, and given the numer-
In addition to transcribing and coding conversa- ous intraindividual variables, as summarized in
tional samples, the aphasiologist must also spend Box 21–2, the same findings may not be easily rep-
time analyzing findings and then, ideally, verifying licated across sessions or contexts (Kim & Wright,
2020; Stark et al., 2020).
the validity and reliability of findings through trian-
gulation (repeated evaluation across varied contexts Discourse analysis includes a mixture of quan-
and discourse genres) and verification (separate titative and qualitative methods and measures.
evaluation by trained independent evaluators). Analysis tends to be based less on specific a pri-
Given productivity demands and busy schedules in ori hypotheses about the nature of communicative
most clinical and research environments, engaging strengths and weaknesses than on observation of
in extensive discourse analysis on a regular basis and reflection on patterns of communicative behav-
is often not an option. Still, attention to conversa- iors observed. It is difficult to draw firm conclusions
based across diverse studies employing vastly differ-
tions and personal stories is vital to life participation
approaches. ent approaches, methods, and measures. To this end,
an international group of clinicians and researchers
has formed a network to “create recommendations
Training and Mentorship for field-wide standards in methods, analysis, and
reporting of spoken discourse outcomes” (Stark
There are relatively few clinicians and researchers et al., 2020, p. 41). Identified as FOQUS Aphasia
who are sophisticated in discourse analysis. This (Focusing on the Quality of Spoken Discourse in
makes it such that even relatively fewer student cli- Aphasia, https://foqusaphasia.com/), the group
nicians and researchers are mentored in discourse-​ has two primary foci, with corresponding sub-
analytic approaches. groups: “Best Practices” for improving standards
for use, analysis, and reporting of discourse data;
and “Methodology and Data Quality,” for address-
Equipment and Software ing a test-retest database to enable formation about
psychometric properties of the stability of measures
Discourse analysis is relatively low-tech compared (Stark et al., 2020). The group welcomes clinical and
to many clinical and research methods. Still, audio research collaborators.
21. Discourse and Conversation as Vital Aspects of Assessment   325

such experience, and practicing professionals may


How May Aphasiologists Confront take advantage of continuing education opportuni-
Challenges in Applying Discourse Analysis ties to do so.
in Clinical Practice and Research? Consider, too, how you might address concerns
about the relevance of discourse analysis processes
Although not all of the challenges noted earlier are and results. Conveying results to people with cog-
easily solvable, clinicians may be proactive in con- nitive-linguistic disorders and the people who care
structively addressing them. Consider, for exam- about them requires thoughtful attention to avoid-
ple, the serious issue of time constraints discussed ing jargon and explaining findings in the most rel-
in Chapter 14. Despite the fact that extensive use of evant ways possible. Showing clips of recorded
discourse analysis is not feasible in most of today’s conversational samples to the people being assessed
clinical environments, having at least some experi- is often helpful in terms of making clear what dis-
ence and mentorship in this vital area is important. course-level challenges seem to be most problematic
We may not always have time to transcribe, code, for a person or dyad and what strategies seem to
and analyze in detail communicative interactions be most effective in terms of enhancing discourse.
with the people we serve. Still, learning to carry out Contrasting earlier videos with more recent sam-
such analyses may help to sharpen our observational ples may also be helpful for conveying the degree
skills and make us more aware of the vast array of of improvement an individual, dyad, or group has
parameters of language use that affect the contextual made in the course of communication intervention.
language use of a person with a language disorder In any case, knowledge of discourse collection
and those who communicate with that person. This, and analysis methods is important for research apha-
in turn, may help to stimulate inclusion or related siologists whose work is grounded in real-world lan-
metrics of ongoing naturalistic assessments and guage use. Those who are interested in engaging in
observations. Aphasiologists in training may seek extensive discourse analysis are encouraged to con-
special courses or independent study options to gain sult texts dedicated to the topic.

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter discourse analysis in everyday clinical
that are new to you or that you have not yet environments.
mastered. c. Describe specific means of addressing the
2. List and describe varied discourse genres. challenges you mentioned in Item 5b.
What are some specific examples of each? 6. Review some of the discourse assessment
3. In what specific ways is discourse analysis tools mentioned in this chapter. Describe
important in clinical practice? their strengths and weaknesses in terms of
4. In what specific ways is discourse analysis clinical relevance and practicality of use.
important in clinical research? 7. Describe key ways in which discourse
5. Imagine that you are an SLP supervising production may vary based on means of
SLP students in a clinical practicum in a elicitation, context, degree of conversational
rehabilitation setting. support, memory load, and roles of
a. Outline the points you would make conversational partners.
to them regarding the importance of 8. Describe key ways in which discourse
learning to engage in discourse analyses. comprehension may vary based on
b. List the points that you would make in tasks, modality of presentation, means
recognizing challenges of engaging in of assessment, degree of conversational
326  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

support, memory load, and roles of clinical scenarios in which indexing specific
conversational partners. aspects of discourse would provide you
9. Investigate some of the online programs with important information for treatment
for transcribing and analyzing discourse planning and assessment of treatment
samples that are listed in Table 21–1. progress and outcomes.
a. Which ones seem user-friendly for 13. In what ways might each of the sources of
clinicians? variability in Box 21–2 influence discourse
b. What would be your personal criteria for analysis results?
selecting such a program for clinical or 14. How might you defend or rationalize the
clinical research use? need to engage in discourse analysis despite
10. Peruse the AphasiaBank, TBIBank, the time constraints in most clinical practice
RHDBank, and DementiaBank resources settings?
(talkbank.org). How might you, as a 15. Even if you are not able to carry out
clinician, contribute to and take advantage of extensive discourse analysis in your
these resources? everyday clinical practice, how might your
11. Consider the diverse types of discourse experience with and knowledge about
analysis measures listed in Box 21–1. What discourse analysis enrich your clinical
factors would determine your selection expertise?
of specific measures to track for a given
www
individual or dyad? See the companion website for additional learning
12. Describe three different hypothetical and teaching materials.
CHAPTER
22
Documenting Assessment Results
and Considering Prognosis

Sharing assessment results in writing and through 4. What information is typically included in
interpersonal interactions is a fundamental com- assessment reports?
ponent of clinical practice with people who have 5. What abbreviations are commonly used in
neurogenic communication disorders. The way we clinical reporting?
convey assessment findings is important for build-
ing rapport with and empowering the people we
serve, helping them understand the nature of their What Are Best Practices in Sharing
challenges, and helping them consider the relevance Assessment Results With Adults Who Have
of our findings to their daily lives and future plans.
Acquired Cognitive-Linguistic Disorders
Documenting assessment results is also central to
and the People Who Care About Them?
information sharing with other professionals and
for ensuring that services meet criteria for reim-
bursement by third-party payers. In this chapter, Whether in writing or in person, there are important
we review strategies for effectively sharing and strategies for sharing assessment results with people
documenting assessment results. Given that con- who have acquired neurogenic language disorders.
sideration of prognosis is essentially linked with Consider how you will do each of the following in
assessment, we also consider means of making and information-sharing meetings:
conveying prognostic judgments. Given that we
are expected to use and understand abbreviations • Adjust the expression of content to the
used in clinical documentation, a listing of common communication needs of the individual and
abbreviations is provided. family member, friends, and caregivers.
After reading and reflecting on the content in • Include the person with a communication
this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in disorder as a central and active member of the
your own words, the following queries: process.
• Support communication through images,
1. What are best practices in sharing assessment written cues, use of simple terms and phrases,
results with adults who have acquired and gestures.
cognitive-linguistic disorders and the people • Have brain images and models on hand
who care about them? to help support understanding of affected
2. How do we best make judgments about structures and associated functions.
prognosis? • Acknowledge limitations of findings, keeping
3. What are best practices for reporting in mind that there are always potentially
assessment results in writing? confounding factors and reliability challenges.

327
328  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

• If you have used assessment tools and


Box
methods that largely focus on impairments, 22–1 A Stroke Survivor’s Account of
be especially careful about making Receiving Assessment Results
interpretations regarding life participation.
• Focus on the relevance of results to life- That morning before I left the Neuro ICU room
affecting aspects of communication. for my new room, a speech pathologist came
• If your assessments have been carried out by. She talked to me about the possibility of
in clinical contexts, be careful about the having a problem with my language. That is
limitations for generalizing what you have to say, she explained things to me that I still
observed to naturalistic everyday contexts. didn’t understand. . . . I certainly didn’t under-
• Focus on strengths while remaining realistic stand my deficits at the time. It was not until
about challenges. I received the medical records . . . (two and a
• Anticipate questions that the people you are half years later) that I discovered the conversa-
serving may have. tion between us. Of course, it was hardly a con-
• Avoid discussing initial assessment results versation. She talked and I imagine I blabbered.
with individuals or family members by The speech-language pathologist related
telephone; in-person meetings allow for better that I appeared to be aware of all those things.
inclusiveness of the primary person being If that was the case, I was trying to focus;
discussed and allow better communication I was trying to express myself; I was trying to
supports for all. understand. But if I knew of my deficits in the
• Be careful about information overload. moment, I certainly couldn’t remember them
Imagine that people with neurogenic later. (Broussard, 2015, p. 3)
disorders and caregivers may be exhausted,
stressed, and fearful, and may feel bombarded
with too much information at once. rate and extent of recovery or decline that is likely.
Before making any statements about a person’s prog-
Even when we attend to all of these practices, nosis, we must be extremely thoughtful. We must be
it is usually not sufficient to explain assessment honest and at the same time sensitive and careful. Be
results and their implications just once. Regardless especially careful not to
of whether people with neurogenic communica-
tion disorders and their family members appear to • overstate gravely negative or cheerfully
understand your explanations, do not assume that positive predictions
they do. And even if they do understand, do not • oversimplify the complex interaction of
assume they will retain what you have discussed. factors that influence recovery
This point is nicely captured by Thomas G. Brous- • be overly confident about what you think
sard (2015), a person with aphasia, in his book Stroke you know
Diary: A Primer for Aphasia Therapy (Box 22–1).
In considering prognosis, we must also consider
the following: Prognosis for what? What aspect of
How Do We Best Make recovery? For improved language in terms of use of
grammar? Word-finding? Social engagement? Life-
Judgments About Prognosis?
long coping? Use of compensatory strategies? Return
to work? Return to school? Someone with a bleak
In Chapter 7, we noted that there are many factors prognosis with respect to one such domain may
that determine the pattern of recovery expected for have an excellent prognosis with respect to another.
a given individual who has had a stroke or brain Keep in mind, too, that many prognostic factors may
injury and the progression of decline expected for be manipulated through behavioral intervention,
a person with a neurodegenerative condition. It is counseling, medication, nutrition, family support,
important to review each of those in considering the enhanced awareness of deficits, relief of depression,
22. Documenting Assessment Results and Considering Prognosis   329

and positive coping skills. Like assessment in gen- early signs of dementia may not be ready or willing
eral, prognostic considerations are ongoing through- to see that actual label. A person in denial of cer-
out intervention. tain deficits may be offended by the suggestion of
such deficits.
An example of what might be considered con-
What Are Best Practices for Reporting fusing is the use of abbreviations and jargon that
the reader does not understand or the mention of
Assessment Results in Writing?
diagnostic labels that no one has previously men-
tioned in person. An example of what might be dis-
The format for reporting assessment results is deter- couraging is a focus on deficits without balanced
mined largely by the clinical context in which we information about strengths or a clear statement
work. In some agencies, assessment results are encap- supporting hope for improvement. A tough chal-
sulated in brief boxes within paper or online forms, lenge related to documentation in the clinical context
whereas others allow for more open-ended summa- is to provide empowering information and support
ries. Often the official assessment (or “diagnostic”) to clients and their families while highlighting defi-
report formatting is made to align with documenta- cits that make clear the need for skilled services to
tion requirements for reimbursement. Key strategies third-party payers.
for making speech-language pathologist (SLP) ser- As an excellent clinician, be strategic about these
vices reimbursable are detailed in Chapter 14. issues, for example,
Many clinicians keep separate files with greater
amounts of detail that can be included on official • anticipate how written reports of any type
forms. Sometimes more detailed notes are kept in will be interpreted by an individual with
an individual’s file in a centralized location within language challenges and the people who care
a given facility. SLPs involved in documentation for about them;
medicolegal cases must address additional docu- • write and edit reports with sensitivity about
mentation requirements in line with their contracted perceptions by varied readers;
responsibilities. • be sensitive to the perspectives of people
Written content should be free of ageist stereo- with language disorders and the people
types and heterosexist biases (see Chapters 16, 18, who care about them in terms of what gets
and 19) and adhere to important principles for writ- documented;
ing about people with disabilities and health prob- • proactively meet with the person assessed — ​
lems (see Chapter 3). In many cases, assessment and anyone that person chooses to have
reports are given to a client or caregivers, either included — to review report content; and
by the clinician or through the service-providing • use supportive communication strategies
agency. Imagine that any written report you send to ensure comprehension and address
by mail may be opened by the person you have potential confusion or concerns, and provide
assessed or a caregiver with no one else around to compassionate education and counseling
help interpret or explain it. Is there content that may about the diagnostic labels and deficits noted.
be disturbing, confusing, or discouraging?
All of us can relate to the fact that it is disturbing No matter how excellent your documentation,
to be characterized in a way we would rather not be. unless it is read by others, it will not be helpful.
Subjective terms such as “cooperative” and “pleas- Make sure it is concise, clear, and relevant and that
ant” are often used in reports, with the intention of it gets shared with others who will make use of the
portraying a person in a positive light; however, information you provide. One of the intended audi-
such terms are often interpreted negatively: “Well ences often left out of the loop includes clinicians
of course!” a client might say, “Why would I not who will be working with the same individual in
be cooperative?” Another example of what might be the future (King, 2013a). Every time a person is dis-
disturbing is diagnostic labeling about deficits with charged, be sure that copies of assessment and treat-
which an individual might not agree. A person with ment progress reports are sent along to be shared
330  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

with future clinicians. With appropriate permissions types of services (e.g., assessment or treatment ses-
in place and in adherence with related policies, you sions). The International Classification of Diseases–
may also send copies of reports directly to clinicians Clinical Modification, 10th Revision (ICD-10-CM)
that you know will be working with an individual is a system of classification and coding for diseases,
you are discharging. conditions, and symptoms. In the United States,
ICD-10 codes are required on most documents used
in justifying, documenting, and billing for health-​
What Information Is Typically related services that are supported through govern-
ment or private health insurance.
Included in Assessment Reports?
With productivity requirements as they are in so
many health care contexts, most SLPs have very little
The amount of detail to be included in an assess- time in their workday for documentation. In many
ment report depends on the documentation policies cases, time spent on documentation is not billable
and forms required in a given context. Typically, an time, or if it is, only a small amount of billable time
assessment report includes the following items: can be allocated to documentation. At the same time,
excellent documentation is essential for reimburse-
• A brief description of the individual’s ment. No matter how justifiable and high quality our
background, including the reason they were services, if we do not provide the type and quality of
referred to you, current communication- documentation required for reimbursement, a third-
related diagnoses and other related diagnoses, party payer is unlikely to pay.
living and social context, vocation, education,
and age
• A brief summary of communicative strengths What Abbreviations Are Commonly
and weaknesses based on a synthesis of
Used in Clinical Reporting?
observation, case history, interviews, and
formal and informal screening and testing
results. In some contexts, reference to a Just as documentation and coding requirements vary
published, standardized test is required. by agency, region, and country, so do abbreviations
Contextualizing assessment results in light used for clinical reporting. Common abbreviations
of the real-life communication impacts of the used in work with people who have acquired neu-
impairments is vital. rogenic communication disorders are summarized
• A brief summary of recommendations. If in Table 22–1. They are used throughout interven-
treatment is recommended, suggest long- and tion, including in progress and discharge notes. Note
short-term goals (see Chapter 23) that are that some employers request or require that certain
consistent with the individual’s own wants abbreviations be used and that others be avoided.
and needs. Also include a statement about Some abbreviations are considered to be frequently
how evidence-based intervention will likely misinterpreted and thus have increased likelihood of
lead to the person’s improved independence, leading to potentially serious communication errors
medical management, socialization, and (Tariq & Sharma, 2021; see Institute of Safe Medica-
quality of life. tion Practices, 2021, for a list). In cases where con-
sequences of misinterpretation could be serious, or
Many agencies require specific coding schemes when the intended readers of our notes and reports
for documenting clinical information, including would not be familiar with abbreviations, it is best to
diagnostic codes and codes that represent various use actual words instead of abbreviations.
Table 22–1. Common Abbreviations Used in Rehabilitation Documentation

@ At; each
♀ Female
♂ Male
− Negative; no; none; deficiency; subtract; not ordered
+ Positive; added; ordered
< Smaller than; less than; caused by
= Equal; equal to
≠ Not equal; not equal to
> Larger than; greater than; causes
↑ Above; elevated; enlarged; improved
∅ Null, zero
↓ Below; decreased; falling; depressed
A Assessment (in problem-oriented medical record)
a Before


A Assist, assistance
AC Auditory comprehension
ADD Attention deficit disorder (ADHD now typically preferred)
ADHD Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
ADL Activities of daily living
AER Auditory evoked response
A fib Atrial fibrillation
AIDS Acquired immune deficiency syndrome
ALS Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis
AMA Against medical advice
amb Ambulatory, ambulate, ambulation
angio Angiogram
ant Anterior
ax Assessment
A&O Alert and oriented
AOD Arterial occlusive disease
A/P Assessment/plan
AP Anterior-posterior
A-P Anterior-posterior

continues

331
Table 22–1. continued

apt Appointment
ASAP As soon as possible
ASCVD Arteriosclerotic cardiovascular disease
ASHD Atherosclerotic heart disease


B Bilateral
BG Blood glucose
bid Twice daily (bis in die)
bil Bilateral
biw Twice weekly
bp Blood pressure
buc Buccal
Bx Biopsy
c With (cum)
CA Carcinoma
Ca Calcium
CABG Coronary artery bypass graft
CAD Coronary artery disease
CAT Computed axial tomography
CC Chief complaint
cc Cubic centimeter
CCU Coronary care unit
cea Carotid endarterectomy
CGA Contact guard assist
CHD Congenital heart disease; coronary heart disease
CHF Congestive heart failure
CHI Closed head injury
chol Cholesterol
CI Cardiac index
cm Centimeters
CNS Central nervous system; clinical nurse specialist
CO Carbon monoxide
c/o Complains of
CO2 Carbon dioxide
cont. Continue

332
Table 22–1. continued

COPD Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease


COTA Certified occupational therapy assistant
CSF Cerebrospinal fluid
CT Computed tomography
CV Color vision
CVA Cerebrovascular accident
CVD Cardiovascular disease
CVI Cerebrovascular incident
CX Cancellation, cancel
CXR Chest x-ray
D Dependent


D Dependent
d Day
D/C Discharge, discontinue
Dep Dependent
diff Differential
DM Diabetes mellitus
DME Durable medical equipment
DNI Do not intubate
DNR Do not resuscitate
DNT Did not test
DOB Date of birth
DOC Doctor on call
DOI Date of injury
DRG Diagnosis/diagnostic-related group
DVT Deep vein thrombosis/deep venous thrombosis
DZ, Ds Disease
Dx Diagnosis
EBRT External beam radiation therapy
ECA External carotid artery
ECG, EKG Electrocardiogram
ECHO Echocardiogram
EEG Electroencephalogram

continues

333
Table 22–1. continued

EMG Electromyogram
EMS Emergency medical services
EMT Emergency medical technician
ENT Ear, nose, throat
E/O Expected outcome
Equip Equipment
ER Emergency room
eval Evaluation
Ex Exercise
Exam Examination
exp Expired
F Female
FBS Fasting blood sugar
FHx Family history
freq Frequency
FROM Full range of motion
F/U Follow-up
GCS Glasgow Coma Scale
Geri Geriatrics
gluc Glucose
gm Gram
GSW Gunshot wound
G-tube Gastrostomy tube
GTT Glucose tolerance test
HA Headache
h/a Headache
HASHD Hypertensive arteriosclerotic heart disease
HBO Hyperbaric oxygen
HEENT Head, eyes, ears, nose, and throat
HGB/Hgb Hemoglobin
HHD Hypertensive heart disease
HIV Human immunodeficiency virus
HMO Health maintenance organization
h/o History of

334
Table 22–1. continued

H&P History and physical


HPI History of present illness
hr Hours, hour
HSV Herpes simplex virus
HTN Hypertension
HVD Hypertension vascular disease
Hx History
I&O In and out/input and output/intake and output
ICH Intracranial hemorrhage
ICP Intracranial pressure
ICU Intensive care unit
ID Infectious disease
IDD Insulin-dependent diabetes
IDDM Insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus
inf Inferior
inj Injection
int Internal
ip Inpatient
IV Intravenous
IVUS Intravascular ultrasound
JVD Jugular venous distention
JVP Jugular venous pressure
kg Kilogram


L Left
L Left
L&R Left and right
lab Laboratory
lat Lateral
LOC Loss of consciousness
LP Lumbar puncture
L-Spine Lumbar spine
LTG Long-term goal
lytes Electrolytes

continues

335
Table 22–1. continued

M Male
MAC Monitored anesthesia care
MAR Medication administration record
max Maximum, maximal
mcg Microgram
MD Doctor of medicine
ME Medical examiner
meds Medications
mes Mesial
mg Milligram
MI Myocardial infarction
Min A Minimum assist
misc Miscellaneous
ml Milliliter
mm Millimeters
mod Moderate
Mod A Moderate assist
MRI Magnetic resonance imaging
MR Scan Magnetic resonance scan
MRSA Methicillin-resistant Staphylococcus aureus
MSE Mental status examination
NA Not available; not applicable
NAD No apparent (acute) distress
Narc Narcotic
NC Noncontributory
NCV Nerve conduction velocity
Neg Negative
Neuro Neurology
Ng Nanogram
NG Nasogastric
NG tube Nasogastric tube
NIDDM Non-insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus
NKDA No known drug allergies
noc Nocte (at night)

336
Table 22–1. continued

NOS Not otherwise specified


n.p.o. Nothing by mouth (nil per os)
NPO Nothing by mouth (nil per os)
NT Not tested
O Oral
occ Occupational
OCD Obsessive-compulsive disorder
o.p. Outpatient department, osteoporosis
OR Operating room
OSA Obstructive sleep apnea
OT Occupational therapy
OTC Over the counter
OU Both eyes
p After, following
PACU Postanesthesia care unit
PCA Patient-controlled analgesia
PCP Primary care physician
PE Physical examination
PEG Percutaneous endoscopic gastrostomy
PMH Past medical history
PM & R Physical medicine and rehabilitation
p.o. By mouth
POD Post-op day (#)
pos Positive
p.r.n As needed (pro re nata)
pt Patient
PT Physical therapy/physical therapist
PTSD Post-traumatic stress disorder
q Each, every (quaque)
Q Each, every (quaque)
qad Every other day (quoquealternis die)
qam Every morning
qh Every hour (quaque hora)

continues

337
Table 22–1. continued

q2h Every 2 hours


q.d. Every day (quaque die)
R Right


R Right
reg Regular
rehab Rehabilitation
RN Registered nurse
R/O Rule out
ROM Range of motion/rupture of membranes
ROS Review of systems
rpt Repeat; report
RT Radiation therapy
RTW Return to work
Rx Prescription; treatment
S/P Status post
s Without (sine)
sec Seconds
SNF Skilled nursing facility
SOAP Subjective, objective, assessment, plans
SSEPs Somatosensory evoked potential
SSI Sliding scale insulin
SSRI Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor
STAT Immediately
STG Short-term goal
subcu/subq Subcutaneous; subcutaneously
sup Superior
TBI Traumatic brain injury
TIA Transient ischemic attack
tid Three times a day (ter in die)
T-O Temperature, oral
Tx Treatment, therapy
UTI Urinary tract infection
vs Versus
VTE Venous thromboembolism

338
Table 22–1. continued

w/ With
W/C Wheelchair
WC Wheelchair
WFL Within functional limits
WNL Within normal limits
wt Weight
w/u Workup
x Times
YO Years old
y.o. Years old
yr Year

339
340  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter linguistic deficits due to mild cognitive
that are new to you or that you have not yet impairment.
mastered. d. The clinician summarizes findings
2. With two colleagues, practice sharing suggesting that the person has primary
assessment information through role- progressive aphasia.
play in each of the scenarios below. Take Be sure to practice suggestions for
turns in playing three roles: person with information sharing and making prognostic
a communication disorder, caregiver statements given in your readings.
or partner of that person, and clinician. 3. Describe important strategies to use in an
Clinicians, for each scenario, convey assessment information-sharing session to
information about the condition and its empower a person with a language disorder
severity, the limitations of your assessment and focus on strengths while remaining
findings, your best guess at prognosis, and realistic about challenges.
limitations to guessing about prognosis. 4. Describe how the contents of a written assess-
Others, ask questions and respond to the ment report to be sent to an insurance company
clinician in ways you think would be likely as part of a billing procedure may differ from
in such scenarios. a written report shared with the family.
a. The clinician conveys that the person 5. Review the abbreviations in Table 22–1. How
has global aphasia due to a stroke that might each be relevant to your own reading
occurred 5 days ago. and writing of documents in medical and
b. The clinician conveys that the person has rehabilitation settings?
a mild posterior form of aphasia due to a
stroke that occurred 1 year ago. More materials to foster teaching and learning on
www
c. The clinician confirms that findings are this chapter’s content may be found on the com-
consistent with a diagnosis of cognitive- panion website.
SECTION VI

Theories and Best Practices in Intervention


342  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

This section addresses best practices and theories professional work. In Chapter 24, we consider the
of intervention for adults with acquired cognitive-​ theories that support the wide range of treatment
linguistic disorders. In Chapter 23, we focus on what methods (and components of those methods) used
we mean by “best practices,” what we need to know in clinical practice serving people with cognitive-lin-
about them, and how to find information about the guistic challenges. This content serves as an import-
evidence base in our field to support our work as ant foundation for the final two sections of the book
clinicians. You are encouraged to reflect on how you regarding treatment methods.
will implement evidence-based practice in your own
CHAPTER
23
Best Practices in Intervention

The excellent clinician is a dedicated vehicle for fos-


tering brain changes and helping people compensate
What Are the Best Practices in the Treatment
for and cope with chronic challenges. As we dis- of Neurogenic Language Disorders?
cussed in the context of best practices in assessment
(Chapter 17), homing in on the people with whom Let’s start this chapter with a list of best practices
we are in an empowering, affirming clinical role is for engaging in clinical excellence in treatment of
paramount in serving as that vehicle. In this chapter, neurogenic language disorders. All of the best prac-
we consider wisdom about intervention best prac- tices recommended for assessment in Chapter 17
tices offered by clinical aphasiologists over many also apply to treatment, although some noted here
decades. We also review means of determining lev- have a special nature that is important to consider
els of evidence for methods of intervention, ways in the treatment context. Of course, there are addi-
excellent clinicians integrate and balance evidence tional best practices that are particular to treatment
from the research literature with evidence from the itself.
people we serve, and the importance of knowledge
translation through implementation science.
After reading and reflecting on the content in Embrace Communication as a Human Right
this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in
your own words, the following queries: The ability to communicate is a fundamental human
right. Advocate vigorously for the people you serve.
1. What are the best practices in the treatment of Be sure to review the strategies for advocacy to boost
neurogenic language disorders? access to services and supports for people with neu-
2. What does the excellent clinical aphasiologist rogenic communication disorders in Chapter 14.
know about evidence-based practice?
3. Where can we find pertinent information to
support evidence-based practice? Recognize Assessment as an
4. How does the excellent clinician integrate Ongoing Intervention Process
evidence-based practice with practice-based
evidence? We noted in Chapter 17 that, although we might
5. How may excellent clinicians support be expected to document that we are engaged in a
knowledge translation through implementation formal assessment session prior to initiating treat-
and systems science? ment, we must continue to ask and answer questions

343
344  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

about a particular person’s communication abilities are in gear as clinicians, we ideally focus primarily
throughout the entire time that they are seen for pro- on those we serve and not on ourselves or our own
fessional services. Every treatment session entails performance.
assessment of some type. For example, how much
has been retained from one clinical interaction to the
next? Is there carryover of progress from one area Include Family Members, Caregivers, and
to another? How is this person progressing toward Others Whose Roles Are Relevant
their goals? We constantly learn new information
about a person’s life context that might influence our Communication is not typically an activity that we
goals, the content and location of our sessions, and carry out alone. It relies on interaction with and
who might be included in any of our interactions. engagement of others. Achieving communicative
Monitoring of progress throughout intervention success with a person who has a communication
fits into the single-case hypothesis-testing scheme disorder depends not only on the abilities and strat-
akin to the assessment-focused research process egies of the person with the disorder but also on the
illustrated in Chapter 17. With an individual person, abilities and strategies of the people with whom that
we may apply a specific approach for which there person communicates.
is empirical support detailed in the literature based Include care partners and significant others
on individual cases or groups. Then, as we assess its to enhance and expand the network of supportive
effects with a particular individual, we may continu- people in a person’s environment, and to ensure
ously tweak the process to best suit that individual’s maximal generalization of progress to everyday
needs (Turkstra, 2010). Ylvisaker (e.g., in Ylvisaker use in naturalistic environments. Authors of several
et al., 2002) referred to this as patient-specific hypothe- studies have demonstrated that training of partners
sis testing and case-based decision-making. in the use of supportive communication strategies
improves short- and long-term treatment outcomes
(see Simmons-Mackie et al., 2010). We explore this
Be Person Centered notion further in Chapter 24.

Person-centered care is reflected in our demonstrat-


ing that the individuals we serve are the core of our Have a Clear Sense of Purpose and Goals
purpose as clinical professionals (DiLollo & Favreau,
2010; Hickey & Bourgeois, 2018; Hickey & Doug- A key tenet of Covey’s (2013) strategies for “highly
las, 2021; Kitwood, 1997; Kitwood & Bredin, 1994; effective people” is to begin with the end in mind. In
Leach et al., 2010; Lewin et al., 2001; Peri et al., 2004; Chapter 2, we noted that this is important to keep in
Worrall, 2006). At the heart of person-centered care mind in terms of our own development as excellent
is the direct inclusion of the individual with a com- clinicians. Kagan and Simmons-Mackie (2007) elo-
munication disorder in decisions about all aspects quently declare that the concept of beginning with the
of assessment, treatment planning, and intervention end in mind is central to outcomes-focused treatment
alternatives and follow-up after direct interven- planning. It is unlikely that we will reach a goal if
tion (Hinckley et al., 2014; Lund et al., 2001). This we do not know what the goal is, let alone how to
may seem obvious when considered in the abstract. get there. As noted earlier and throughout this book,
However, in our everyday busy clinical and research our ultimate goal is to help people with neurogenic
environments, we have constant time and fiscal pres- language disorders foster the most successful and
sures. We have many demands that do not involve fulfilling lives possible. The way toward that goal is
direct contact with the people we serve. Thus, the shaped by the people we serve — their strengths and
goal of person-centered care can easily be under- weaknesses, their support systems, and their own
mined (Rohde et al., 2012). No matter how stressed sense of what is “successful” and what is “fulfill-
we may be, who is observing or not observing us, ing.” As mentioned in our discussion of assessment
how much confidence we may lack . . . when we best practices, excellent clinicians know what they
23. Best Practices in Intervention   345

are doing with a person and why at any given time. Encourage Self-Coaching
They begin with the end in mind. If we do not know
our clients’ own personal goals — what defines the Ylvisaker, well known for person-centered approaches
“end” for them — then we cannot possibly have the that promote emotional self-regulation in TBI sur-
end in mind. At least not the right end. vivors, preferred the metaphor of self-coaching to
Unfortunately, stories of unprepared SLPs self-regulation to enhance the relevance of the notion
demonstrating their ignorance of the literature on to clients. Most of us would probably rather be
treatment methods are not uncommon. For some, coached than regulated (Ylvisaker, 2006). Let’s keep
it seems that completing photocopied workbook this in mind as we work with people who have
sheets from a book of language exercises or having a executive function problems, lack of awareness of
person name objects depicted on a stock set of cards deficits, and needs for improved self-monitoring or
constitutes a treatment approach. Practices without self-cuing.
sound theoretical foundations and evidence of effec-
tiveness are unethical and unworthy of the skills and
abilities of a qualified SLP.
Consider Optimal Timing
Directly tied to having a clear purpose and
goals is having a strong basis supporting the meth-
ods we use and the activities in which we engage in We must make every effort to time treatment ses-
throughout intervention. As we discuss throughout sions to minimize negative influences of pain, ill-
this book, excellent clinicians typically do not cham- ness, fatigue, distraction, and so on. Additionally,
pion one theoretical framework without recognizing there is important research emerging indicating
the validity or utility of others. They know about that the timing of intervention in terms of stages
multiple approaches and frameworks, integrate of recovery from stroke or brain injury is of vital
multiple theories, evaluate critically the results of importance. We discuss this further in Chapter 24,
published research results in light of what is most under queries about optimal times in recovery to
relevant to specific people with language disorders, initiate treatment, and about intensity and duration
and remain open to modifying theoretical perspec- of treatment.
tives based on new learning.

Consider Optimal Locations and Conditions


Engage Communication Partners Outside of the
Client’s Immediate Circle of Friends and Family Often the ideal environment for treatment meets the
same criteria as those for optimal assessment con-
In cases where there are not sufficient numbers of truly ditions: well lit, quiet, without clutter and excess
supportive people, find ways to enlist volunteers who visual stimulation, and including or excluding spe-
may be helpful. Members of community service orga- cific people as appropriate. It is also important that
nizations, students in clinical or preclinical academic we contextualize treatment in real-world environ-
programs, and retired people can become wonder- ments to enhance the likelihood of maximal carry-
ful communication partners to assist in supporting over of treatment gains to everyday life contexts. Be
social communication in general and carryover of sure to get outside of typical clinical contexts. When
specific communication goals in particular. possible and where appropriate, engage in home
visits and go to other places where communication
is paramount for the person with whom you are
Embrace Cultural and Linguistic Differences working (e.g., workplaces, civic organization meet-
ings, shopping centers, restaurants, and school, col-
Treatment content and goals must take into account lege, or university settings). We discuss this further
each individual’s unique background. This is essential as we consider “functional communication” later in
to our focus on the person and on life participation. this chapter.
346  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Focus on Functional Communication to embracing aspects of life participation that may


be improved through improved communication
The term functional in clinical practice has multiple abilities. Examples (far from an exhaustive list) of
interpretations. In the 1980s and 1990s, our field saw pioneers in this area are as follows:
a surge of “functional” approaches to treatment of
communication disorders. Audrey Holland has been • Roberta Chapey, Judith Duchan, Roberta
a magnificent catalyst of such approaches, through Elman, Linda Garcia, Aura Kagan, Jon Lyon,
her early work on functional communication per and Nina Simmons Mackie, The LPAA Project
se (Holland, 1982) and her more recent work since Group, who launched the Life Participation
then on quality of life in aphasia. Many others joined Approach to Aphasia (e.g., LPAA Project
forces to enrich foci on enhancing quality of life Group, 2000)
through communication intervention. • Barbara Shadden, who highlighted the
What was typically intended by the authors of impact of neurogenic language disorders
such approaches was to get away from focusing on on caregivers and the effects of aphasia on
specific linguistic constructs in decontextualized identity (e.g., Shadden, 2005)
clinical exchanges and orient intervention more • Mark Ylvisaker, Jon Lyon, Susie Parr, Sally
toward the use of language in real communicative Byng, Sue Gilpion, Judith Duchan, and Carole
situations in ways that are important to the person Pound, who emphasized the importance of
being treated. For example, some early treatment helping stroke and brain injury survivors to
approaches to help improve naming abilities (such cope with long-term life-affecting consequences
as the cueing hierarchy approach [Linebaugh & Leh- of their communication challenges (e.g., Byng
ner, 1977]), described in Section VIII, had us focus on et al., 2000; Lyon, 1998; Ylvisaker, 1992)
working through a series of cues to help people get • the multiple authors of a wonderful edited
better at naming objects. The authors of functional book on supporting communication for
approaches toward the same method encouraged us people with aphasia (see Simmons-Mackie &
to make sure that the stimuli (words, objects, and King, 2013)
pictures) were highly relevant to each individual in • the multiple authors of a fantastic edited book
terms of their own daily word-finding needs. They on life participation approaches for people
also encouraged us to make sure we probed to see with neurogenic communication disorders
that word-finding improvements at the single-word (see Holland & Elman, 2021)
level were being carried over into sentences and • Margaret Rogers, Nancy Alarcon, and
into conversations. Extending this further, several colleagues, who emphasized practical means
authors of functional approaches heightened our of training caregivers of people with aphasia
collective awareness of the need for clinicians to get to use supported communication (e.g.,
out of the clinic room and into real-world situations Alarcon & Rogers, 2006)
with the people we serve to be sure that communi- • Travis Threats, who promoted and
cation gains we saw in the clinic were transferring to spearheaded a great deal of international
real-world interactions. work aimed at incorporating communication
The functional approaches of the 1980s and needs and related services into ICF and other
1990s evolved into social models for contextualizing WHO-related programs and projects (e.g.,
work with people with neurogenic communication Threats, 2010a)
disorders near the start of the 21st century. As the • Kerry Byrne and J. B. Orange (2005), who
World Health Organization (WHO) International promoted principles for using ICF constructs
Classification of Functioning, Disability, and Health and tenets in the assessment, treatment, and
(ICF) has risen to prominence, we have been made support of people with dementia
more aware of its relevance to working with people • Aura Kagan, Nina Simmons-Mackie, Jack
with neurogenic communication disorders. In that Damico, and colleagues, who developed
light, the term functional is now frequently applied means of indexing real-life outcomes of
23. Best Practices in Intervention   347

aphasia intervention and emphasized the mean that working on reading comprehension is
importance of conversational analysis for more important to that person. If a person is having
capturing many of these (e.g., Kagan et al., trouble with comprehending and producing com-
2008; Simmons-Mackie et al., 2007) plex sentence structures yet feels that working on
• David Beukelman and colleagues, who grammatical processing is irrelevant to their com-
developed the Participation Model of AAC munication needs, it is important to pay attention
(e.g., Beukelman et al., 2007; Beukelman & to that. If you are keen on working with your client
Mirenda, 2013) in their work environment with hopes of training
• Michelle Bourgeois, Natalie Douglas, and coworkers to support professional communication
Ellen Hickey, who have promoted person- and maintain the person’s status and productivity
centered life participation approaches for at work — yet note that they are much more con-
people with dementia (Bourgeois & Hickey, cerned about the trouble they are having connect-
2009; Hickey & Douglas, 2021) ing with their teenage children — then change your
• The founding members of Aphasia Access, plans and go with their preferences. In sum, be sure
a vibrant organization created in 2014 to to tune into the person and their own perceptions,
promote LPAA approaches (http://www​ and guard against assuming that the help you might
.aphasiaaccess.org) value most if you had a communication disorder is
similar to what the client values.
All the while, some aphasiologists have taken In line with this person-centered approach is
exception to the notion that all things functional Goal Attainment Scaling (GAS). First introduced
must be beyond the level of linguistic analysis and in the context of assessing mental health treatment
couched in terms of life participation. For example, outcomes (Kiresuk & Sherman, 1968), GAS entails
what if someone has limited grammatical processing having people in any kind of rehabilitation inter-
deficits, making it difficult to process long and com- vention select their own goals as part of intervention
plex sentences? Grammatically composed phrases planning, communicating about those goals to inter-
and sentences are primary vehicles through which disciplinary team members, and collaborating across
meaning is expressed. Isn’t grammar, then, in and of types of intervention to help people meet their goals.
itself, functional, in the sense that it is useful and rel- GAS has been applied in many areas of clinical prac-
evant to real-world communication? Understanding tice and diverse practice settings, including aphasi-
reversible passive sentences, coping with syntactic ology (e.g., Escher et al., 2018; Kucheria et al., 2020;
ambiguities, parsing relative clauses, and interpret- van Alphen, 2019).
ing pronoun references are all essential to compe- Basically, here is how it works. Engaging with
tent language use in daily life. Thus, just because the client through motivational interviewing, con-
the focus of a particular treatment approach may be sider potential goals and what the outcomes of each
aimed at the impairment level, that approach should would be. Have the client choose one or more goals
not necessarily be considered nonfunctional. As long and clarify the corresponding outcome for each.
as a treatment method focused on impairments is What outcome will be used to indicate that the goal
couched in a more holistic framework of ensuring has been met? On a scale of –2 to +2, consider that
real-life relevance and carryover such that the indi- meeting that goal will be at 0, right in the middle
vidual person’s life participation is enhanced, then of the scale. If the score achieved is greater than or
certainly it, too, may be considered functional. equal to 0, this is to indicate clinically meaningful
improvement (Kiresuk et al., 1994).
Advantages of GAS include the following:
Engage the Person Actively and
Meaningfully in Goal Setting • The goals are inherently meaningful to the
person involved; playing a central role in the
If a person scores poorly on reading comprehension goal-setting process helps people engage more
but well on auditory comprehension, this does not actively in reaching for goals.
348  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

• Problems of ceiling and floor effects foci are, the greater the activation of neural networks
associated with many standardized measures involved in processing that material (Kleim & Jones,
are avoided. 2008; Raymer et al., 2006), and the greater the likeli-
• Sensitivity of measurement is greater than hood of generalization to spontaneous use.
for most standardized measures, especially
because there are no irrelevant items.
• Scores are adaptable to any domain in or level Focus on Strengths
in the ICF (Schlosser, 2004; Turner-Stokes,
2009; van Alphen, 2019). Of course, it is often appropriate to focus on a cli-
ent’s cognitive-linguistic deficits. After all, those are
Challenges in applying GAS include the following: the reasons they have likely been referred to us. We
must clearly document deficits to carry out effective
• Clinicians must have robust training in goal assessments and justify working with clients. We
setting and in building therapeutic alliance for must analyze the nature of a person’s impairments
effective motivational interviewing. and their negative effects on life participation as we
• Goal setting processes vary widely across facilitate treatment planning. Acknowledging defi-
clinicians and clinical contexts. cits directly with a stroke or brain injury survivor is
• Setting goals early in the rehabilitation process usually essential during assessment and throughout
can be difficult when it is unclear what goals intervention. For example, during treatment, we may
will be realistic for a person. be reviewing test results that highlight cognitive lin-
• Some clients may lack appropriate guistic impairments. We may be tracking the number
understanding of the process and insight of errors during a given treatment task to monitor
about impairments and associated realistic progress. We may be pointing out weaknesses as
expectations. a means of enhancing a person’s self-awareness of
communication breakdown. All of this is typically
To index achievements for a group of people, appropriate. And as we are doing such things, that
GAS scores can be converted to t-scores. The valid- person may be seeing other professionals (occu-
ity and reliability of analyses across individuals pational and physical therapists, physicians, etc.)
depend on the precision and consistency with which who are also focusing on their impairments in other
outcomes for each person are measured. Kuche- areas. Let’s keep in mind that such a focus on deficits
ria et al. (2020) developed and tested an electronic requires a hearty balance of positivity. The excellent
GAS process to assist and train SLPs in using GAS clinician is a motivator, cheerleader, and encourager.
during motivational interviews. Krasny-Pacini et al. Celebrating successes, no matter how small, is essen-
(2016) advocate for the application of appraisal tial to helping a person persist in the face of chal-
criteria to strengthen the feasibility of using GAS lenges and stay engaged in recovery.
in clinical research hospital-based SLP practices.
Such work bodes well for future research and direc-
tion to enhance the use of GAS in treatment out- Be an Interprofessional Team Player
comes reporting.
Tapping into the expertise of our colleagues across
medical, rehabilitation, and other health disciplines
Focus on Relevant Material is essential, and so is sharing complementary inter-
vention strategies, educating others in constructive
A focus on the use of relevant tasks and stimuli is not ways about our scope of practice, and advocating
only a sound principle in terms of social frameworks for attention to the life-affecting cognitive-linguistic
for intervention. It also makes the best sense in terms challenges faced by the people we serve. If regular
of neuropsychological approaches to intervention: action-focused, synergistic, interprofessional team
The more relevant and personally salient treatment meetings focused on the people we are treating
23. Best Practices in Intervention   349

are not taking place within an agency in which we tory precision of everyday informal speech has been
work, it is important that we advocate for such meet- shown to facilitate comprehension, especially in peo-
ings — or initiate them ourselves. ple with hearing impairment. Although clear speech
is sometimes confused with elderspeak (see Chap-
ter 9), the two are distinct; clear speech facilitates
Integrate Evidence-Based Practice comprehension, and elderspeak conveys ageism.
With Practice-Based Evidence There are two constructs related to the ease of
reading that are relevant to potential means of opti-
We explore the importance of this integration, and mizing print: legibility and readability. Legibility
how we might each engage in it, later in this chapter. refers to the ease or difficulty of identifying individ-
ual printed letters, numbers, or characters. Read-
ability refers to the degree of ease or difficulty of
Blend Art With Science comprehending written text. Both influence reading
time and reading efficiency.
As discussed in Chapter 2, the ability to balance art Published recommendations for aphasia-​friendly
with science in clinical work is essential to clinical print materials include use of ample white space and
excellence. Mastering important scientific knowledge emphasis of key words by underlining, capitaliz-
across key disciplinary areas that are pertinent to ing, or bolding. Some recommendations for apha-
aphasiology, and being able to call on that knowledge sia-friendly print include “easy-to-read” font (e.g.,
in applicable ways, is vital. At the same time, the Rose et al., 2009, 2010; Simmons-Mackie, 2013a).
excellent clinician uses keen and sensitive judgment Recommendations for the use of sans-serif font
and engages with people in empathetic, thoughtful, have been made; however, such recommendations
and creative ways in the art of clinical practice. have yet to be substantiated with solid published
evidence. Even in people without neurological dis-
orders, findings about legibility and readability of
Encourage Aphasia-Friendly Communication print in serif versus sans serif print is equivocal
(Poole, 2012). Some argue that use of serif markings
General recommendations for aphasia-friendly lan- aids legibility because they add redundancy to gra-
guage, whether spoken or written, include use of phemic cues, whereas others suggest that block print
short and simple sentence structures, supported is most legible. A caveat in this regard is that there
by clear photos or pictographs that lack irrelevant is a paucity of research on just which font is easi-
content. Considering that some people with apha- est to read for any particular clinical subgroup and
sia and related disorders have good reading and/ whether legibility and readability are similar across
or oral language comprehension, it is important not individuals with varied neurogenic language disor-
to simplify language too much for a given individ- ders and types and severity of aphasia.
ual. Still, written, audio, and video materials meant Varied minimum font sizes have also been rec-
for general use by people with aphasia should be ommended. For example, Simmons-Mackie (2013a)
designed to facilitate comprehension by people with suggests a minimum of 20-point font, whereas others
moderate to severe comprehension deficits so as to suggest a range of 14 to 22 (Australian Aphasia Asso-
increase content accessibility. ciation Inc., 2010; Brennan et al., 2005). Certainly, an
Recommendations for oral language include important factor in this regard is the visual acuity
use of multiple modalities (speech, gesture, facial and contrast sensitivity of the reader (Connolly,
expressions, body language, writing with key 1998; Owsley & Sloane, 1990). Further evidence for
words pointed to or underlined, and photo and pic- the improved legibility of any particular font style
tographic support) and repetitions, expansions, and and size for people with aphasia and related disor-
paraphrasing as appropriate. Some authors have rec- ders — and its potential influence on readability — is
ommended avoiding “exaggerated speech.” In fact, needed. Of course, regardless of what group studies
clearly articulated speech that exceeds the articula- may reveal, it would be ideal to try out varied font
350  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

size and style with a particular individual where dementia as the cause of unwanted behavior, rather
possible to optimize personalized written material. than reacting to such behavior through behavioral
and pharmacological approaches, is more effective
and more humane. Power highlights the importance
Attend to Behavioral Challenges That of considering behavioral problems as symptoms of
Impede Successful Interactions fundamental challenges to a person’s well-being. He
recommends that we focus on strengths from a well-
Antecedent-based behavior management is a pro- ness perspective rather than problem behavior from
active approach to reducing problem behaviors by a biomedical perspective. Power provides evidence
reducing the likelihood of their occurring in the first that approaches steeped in a wellness perspective
place. Promoted by Ylvisaker (Ylvisaker & Feeney, best sustain quality in relationships with people
2009) for use with traumatic brain injury survivors, who might otherwise be considered to be acting out,
it is applicable to work with anyone who engages unruly, or out of control. This philosophy is echoed
in disruptive or inappropriate behavior. The clini- beautifully by Hickey and Douglas (2021) in their
cian proactively notes patterns in occurrences prior book on person-centered memory and communica-
to problem behaviors (the antecedents) and then tion interventions for people with dementia.
works to prevent those from happening. Likewise, A list of best practices for treatment is summa-
the clinician tunes into the antecedents of positive rized in Box 23–1.
behavior and works to facilitate those. The goal is
to take advantage of constructive ways of helping
to shape a person’s environment and others’ roles What Does the Excellent Clinical
in it to reduce the need for negative consequences to Aphasiologist Know About
address problem behaviors (Turkstra, 2010; Ylvisa-
Evidence-Based Practice?
ker & Feeney, 2009).
A similar, more holistic approach is espoused
by Thomas Kitwood (1997) in his seminal text, Evidence-based practice entails implementing meth-
Dementia Reconsidered, and by Power in his popular ods for which there is known research support,
books, Dementia Beyond Drugs: Changing the Culture while drawing on our own clinical expertise and
of Care (2010) and Dementia Beyond Disease: Enhancing taking into account the personal traits of the peo-
Well-Being (2014). As we considered in Chapter 13, ple we serve and the traits of the local environment.
focusing on addressing unmet needs of people with Excellent clinicians know about the evidence base

Box
23–1 Summary of Best Practices in Intervention

• Embrace communication as a human right. • Consider optimal locations.


• Recognize assessment as an ongoing • Focus on functional communication.
intervention process. • Engage the person actively and meaningfully
• Be person centered. in goal setting.
• Include family members, caregivers, and • Focus on relevant material.
others whose roles are relevant. • Focus on strengths.
• Have a clear sense of purpose and goals. • Be an interdisciplinary team player.
• Engage communication partners outside of • Integrate evidence-based approaches with
the client’s immediate circle of friends and practice-based evidence approaches.
family. • Blend art with science.
• Embrace cultural and linguistic differences. • Encourage aphasia-friendly communication.
• Encourage self-coaching. • Attend to behavioral challenges that impede
• Consider optimal timing and conditions. successful interactions.
23. Best Practices in Intervention   351

for whatever they do professionally, as documented • Very low: Any estimate of effectiveness is
in peer-reviewed research literature and also as very uncertain.
grounded in their own data collection and observa-
tion with the people they serve. Here, let’s review The Clinical Guidelines for Stroke Manage-
information about evidence-based practice most ment offered by the Stroke Foundation of Australia
pertinent to clinical excellence. (https://strokefoundation.org.au) are living guide-
Four constructs are especially important as we lines, updated as evidence evolves. The strength of
consider types of evidence for treatment outcomes: any recommendation fits into one of two categories,
based on available evidence:
• efficacy, the likelihood of benefit from a given
treatment for a defined population under • strong, “where guideline authors are certain
ideal conditions (applicable to a population, that the evidence supports a clear balance
not to an individual) towards either desirable or undesirable
• effectiveness, the likelihood of benefit of effects;” or
treatment to an individual under average • weak, “where the guideline panel is uncertain
conditions (based on studies of efficacious about the balance between desirable and
treatment) undesirable effects.”
• efficiency, an index of productivity, that is,
how much can be gained with a minimum of The guidelines also include recommendations for
expense, time, and effort (especially important attention to quantity and quality of evidence, poten-
in comparing two or more treatments that tial clinical impact, generalizability, and applicability.
have been found to be efficacious) The American Academy of Neurology (AAN;
• outcome, an index of change that occurs as https://www.aan.com/) identifies four levels of
a result of time, intervention, or both (a term recommendation:
that encompasses efficacy, effectiveness, and
efficiency) (Golper et al., 2001; Wertz & Irwin, • A: established as effective, ineffective, or
2001) harmful (or established as useful/predictive
or not useful/predictive) for the given
The strength of recommendations for clinical condition in the specified population
practice is ideally based on levels of evidence. Levels • B: probably effective, ineffective, or harmful
of evidence pertaining to treatment outcomes and (or probably useful/predictive or not useful/
the strength of recommendations have been defined predictive) for the given condition in the
differently by different authors and groups. specified population
The Grading of Recommendations Assessment, • C: possibly effective, ineffective, or harmful
Development and Evaluation (GRADE) Working (or possibly useful/predictive or not useful/
Group (http://www.gradeworkinggroup.org) is an predictive) for the given condition in the
international group dedicated to helping evaluate specified population
the quality of evidence for health care practices. Evi- • U: data inadequate or conflicting; given
dence is graded as follows: current knowledge, treatment (test, predictor)
is unproven
• High: It is highly unlikely that further
research will change confidence in the The determination of classes of evidence is
estimation of treatment effectiveness. based on the combined strengths of recommenda-
• Moderate: It is likely that further research tions. The classes of evidence are as follows:
will affect confidence in the estimation of
treatment effectiveness. • I: evidence from one or more well-designed
• Low: Further research is very likely to have randomized, controlled clinical trials that
an impact on confidence in the estimation of include objective measures and baseline
treatment effectiveness. controls and that meet five additional criteria
352  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

• II: evidence from one or more well-designed populations. These include single-participant
randomized, controlled clinical trials or studies, replications of Phase III studies, and
prospective matched cohort study that large-group studies.
includes objective measures and baseline • Phase V: An effectiveness and efficiency test
controls and that meets all but one of the five phase, in which time allocation and cost are
additional criteria for Level I studied along with satisfaction and quality-
• III: all other controlled trials (including case of-life indices of large samples of individuals
history controls or participants who serve as treated as well as significant others and their
their own controls and objective measures) caregivers.
• IV: studies not meeting the criteria for Level
I, II, or III, including expert opinion and The Cochrane Collaboration (http://www.coch​
consensus statements (AAN, 2017; French & rane.org) is an international network of scholars,
Gronseth, 2008; Gronseth & French, 2008). professionals, and consumers established to help
consider the evidence base that supports any area of
An alternative approach is Robey and Schultz’s health-related intervention. The Cochrane Libraries
(1998) and Robey’s (2004) five-phase outcome research (http://www.cochranelibrary.com) include a data-
model. Rather than defining levels of evidence, five base of systematic reviews of evidence for a wide
phases of outcomes research are described: range of health care practices. The Cochrane criteria
for levels of evidence include attention to strengths
• Phase I: A discovery phase, when investigators in terms of research design (randomized, controlled
develop hypotheses about treatment, estimate clinical trials being the highest level), statistical pre-
the optimal treatment intensity, and specify cision, relevance or usefulness, and effect size (the
the population to benefit from treatment. statistical measure of the degree of likelihood that a
Phase I studies include single participants or treatment will be beneficial or harmful). Currently,
case studies, studies with small sample sizes, there are few systematic reviews specific to acquired
and studies with no control group. neurogenic language disorders in the Cochrane Col-
• Phase II: An optimizing phase, when lection. The collection in this area is likely to grow,
hypotheses are refined, a rationale for the and clinicians as well as scholars may find it helpful
treatment method is specified, the selection to stay abreast of new studies as they are added to
criteria for participants are explicitly detailed, the collection.
and the treatment protocol is standardized. A major challenge in building a solid evidence
Like Phase I studies, Phase II studies include base for the treatment of neurogenic language dis-
single-participant or case studies and studies orders is that the gold standard for evidence-based
with small sample sizes; they may include practice, the randomized, double-blind, controlled
studies with no control group. clinical trial, is extremely difficult to conduct. Rea-
• Phase III: An efficacy test phase, which sons for this include the following:
involves testing of a treatment method
developed through Phases I and II, with large • time required to complete a randomized
samples of people who represent the target clinical trial (most often several years), with
populations in a randomized control trial (a even more time needed to assess long-term
trial in which participants who meet explicit maintenance and generalization
selection criteria are assigned randomly to • funding requirements
treatment and control groups, often conducted • challenges in recruiting and retaining
across multiple sites). participants who meet strict inclusion and
• Phase IV: An effectiveness test phase, when exclusion criteria
the effects of treatment already studied in
Phase III are studied under average clinical As a consequence, we have very few such stud-
conditions. Phase IV studies allow for ies in the realm of aphasiology. In addition, excellent
variation in the frequency and intensity of clinicians recognize the shortcomings of randomized
treatment and even in the definition of target controlled trials. These include the following:
23. Best Practices in Intervention   353

• research questions generated by researchers atic reviews on its website (http://www.asha.org)


rather than through collaboration with through its National Center for Evidence-Based
representatives from the populations under Practice in Communication Disorders (N-CEP). The
study Academy of Neurologic Communication Disorders
• lack of attention to individual participant and Sciences (ANCDS; http://www.ancds.org) pro-
differences, preferences, and goals vides a rich set of evidence-based practice guidelines
• ethical problem of assigning people randomly and practice resources on its website.
to no-treatment groups, thus withholding
treatment that treatment may be helpful
• sheer heterogeneity of people in each How Does the Excellent Clinician
diagnostic category and subcategory, making Integrate Evidence-Based Practice
it such that there are always potentially
With Practice-Based Evidence?
confounding factors, and generalization of
results is always questionable
• dismissal of some results that may be important It is important to be able to judge levels of evidence for
within control conditions as “placebo” effects any treatment method we consider. This requires stay-
• known biases (see Higgins et al., 2011 for a ing abreast of the peer-reviewed research literature
description of varied forms of bias in this and scrutinizing the quality and quantity of evidence
context) (Chabon et al., 2011; Code, 2012; underlying any method. Still, there are vital aspects of
Code & Petheram, 2011; Lemoncello & Ness, any treatment situation that shape the likelihood that
2013; Ylvisaker et al., 2002). In addition, many our work with a person will lead to improvement.
studies entail a frequency and intensity of Evidence-based practice is not to be applied as cook-
treatment that is not feasible to attain in most book practice. We may create lists of “how-to” strat-
clinical environments. egies for carrying out specific approaches; this does
not mean that each step should be carried out in a
For this reason, studies that supposedly repre- prescribed way with any particular individual, unless
sent a “lower” level of evidence (e.g., multiple base- that individual is enrolled in a research study and has
line single-case and a clinical case studies) may be consented overtly to participating in such a study.
more feasible, relevant, and applicable to our every- It is essential that we consider person traits (the
day clinical work. Such studies are important to sup- person’s real-world environmental factors, motiva-
port practice-based evidence in the literature and in tion, perceived social support, personality, culture,
our everyday clinical work. severity of communication challenges, concomitant
disabilities, health status, etc.) plus our own traits
(our competence and confidence in carrying out a
Where Can We Find Pertinent Information particular approach, our personal preferences and
the individual ways in which we adapt treatment
to Support Evidence-Based Practice?
to individuals, our ability to foster and maintain an
effective therapeutic alliance with the person, etc.).
An excellent resource for SLPs seeking evidence-​ Within actual clinical practice, external scientific
based practice guidance in virtually any area of evidence is irrelevant if not considered in light of
speech-language pathology is SpeechBite (speech​ person and clinician traits, all the while taking into
bite.com). SpeechBite is a free, online, searchable account the service delivery context and its con-
database of intervention studies, along with rat- straints. There is ideally a constant integration of
ings of research quality for each study. The quality practice-based evidence with evidence-based prac-
ratings are based on the PEDro scale (Sherrington tice in our work (Figure 23–1).
et al., 2000), an 11-item rating system for assessing Using practice-based evidence in a daily way
external and internal validity and interpretability of involves
research. The American Speech-Language-Hearing
Association also provides a free, searchable online • using evidence from real-world intervention
evidence-based practice guidelines and system- to shape what we actually do as clinicians; and
354  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Figure 23–1. The essential integration and interaction of evidence-​


based practice with practice-based evidence within an intervention
environment. The integration of the two is embedded in the model
of the three primary components of evidence-based practice. Image
Credit: Taylor Reeves. A full-color version of this figure can be found
in the Color Insert.

• evaluating critically how research findings given method. Still, being armed with a wide array of
may best be interpreted and translated in a approaches and knowing when to use which — and
practical sense to the individual people with why — is essential to clinical practice no matter what
whom we work, in light of the environment, the situation. At any given time, the clinician should
person traits, and our own traits. be able to answer the following:

In the context of best practices in assessment • What is the method in use?


(Chapter 17), we examined ways in which the assess- • What is the purpose of that method?
ment process can be viewed as a research process. It • What are the theories that support the method?
is also important to view the treatment process for • What evidence is there in the research
every individual as an ongoing scientific process literature that the method works?
requiring repeated hypothesis generation, data col- • Does the method align with person-centered
lection, analysis, interpretation, and action planning goals?
to guide the next steps. • What evidence is there that a given person
Certainly, there are times that we must think is benefiting from it in a way that will affect
on our toes, not necessarily having advance notice everyday communication and participation?
about an imminent treatment session that allows
for researching an individual’s detailed case his- We consider this further in the context of the
tory or preparation time that enables us to assemble evidence base supporting each of the treatment
the materials that would be appropriate for using a approaches discussed in subsequent chapters.
23. Best Practices in Intervention   355

are conceived. (See Hinckley et al. [2014] for


How May Excellent Clinicians Support an overview.)
Knowledge Translation Through • Stakeholders (e.g., people with neurogenic
Implementation and Systems Science? disorders, families, caregivers, professionals
from diverse disciplines, policy makers,
In Chapter 15, we introduced the importance of funding agency representative) who
knowledge translation as a component of the advo- eventually influence how research may be
cacy role of aphasiologists. There is growing sup- used are included in shaping meaningful
port in aphasiology to expand our implementation research questions, evaluating the usefulness
science, which entails the study of how research of evidence.
findings may be put to use to enhance the quality of • Knowledge is translated to meaningful
clinical services and intervention outcomes. Exciting changes, such as improved intervention
progress is afoot with ever-growing opportunities for methods, policies, accessibility of services,
clinicians to collaborate with researchers to help shape equity, clinical education, and funding.
the usefulness of new evidence as it emerges (Douglas
& Burshnic, 2018; Eccles & Mittman, 2006; Goldstein To learn more about this approach, check out the
& Olswang, 2018). At the same time, research-practice information and resources on the Learning Health
partnerships are expanding with a systems science Systems Rehabilitation Research Network website
approach, in which the following occur: (https://sites.brown.edu/learrn).
With steady grounding in best practices for
• Members of clinical populations under study intervention, we proceed to the next chapter, consid-
engage not only as study participants but ering specific purposes of intervention and theories
as experts who help shape what the most that underlie the rationale for general and specific
important research questions are as studies intervention methods.

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter 4. How might the phases of Robey’s (2004)
that are new to you or that you have not yet five-phase outcome research overlap with
mastered. the American Academy of Neurology classes
2. Consider the recommended best practices of evidence and with the grading scheme for
for intervention listed in the chart at the end levels of evidence suggested by the Stroke
of this section. For each (by checking the Foundation of Australia?
corresponding box), rate the degree to which 5. Which of the means of considering levels of
you value its importance in your own role as evidence presented in this chapter would
a clinician. Use your ratings to order them be most helpful to practicing clinicians
from most to least important. Then compare in evaluating the evidence base for any
your ratings and overall order with those particular treatment method?
of other colleagues. Discuss the similarities 6. Peruse each of the online databases
and differences in your views on the relative mentioned as evidence-based practice
importance of specific aspects of best practice. resources in this chapter. How might you
3. Compare and contrast the meaning of the put such resources to use as you consider
terms efficacy, effectiveness, efficiency, and various approaches to intervention for a
outcome. given person?
356  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

7. How do you plan to implement evidence- you might implement to address those
based practice and practice-based evidence challenges?
in your own clinical work? 9. How might you go about designing a
8. Describe challenges to evidence-based study that would benefit people with brain
practice that you envision in everyday injury using an implementation science
clinical work. What are specific strategies framework?

1 = Extremely important
3 = Moderately important
5 = Of little importance 1 2 3 4 5
Embrace communication as a human right
Recognize assessment as an ongoing intervention
process
Be person centered
Include family members, caregivers, and others
whose roles are relevant
Have a clear sense of purpose and goals
Ensure the best possible treatment conditions
Engage communication partners outside of the
client’s immediate circle of friends and family
Embrace cultural and linguistic differences
Encourage self-coaching
Consider optimal timing
Consider optimal locations
Focus on functional communication
Focus on abilities the individual really needs and
wants to improve
Focus on relevant material
Focus on strengths
Be an interdisciplinary team player
Use evidence-based approaches
Blend art with science
Encourage aphasia-friendly communication
Attend to behavioral challenges that impede
successful interactions

www
Additional teaching and learning materials are available on the companion website.
CHAPTER
24
Treatment Theories and Types of
Treatment to Enhance Language and
Cognition Across All People With
Neurogenic Communication Challenges

In this chapter, we delve further into important 12. What other treatment parameters are important
aspects of best practices in intervention, focusing on to consider?
the purposes of intervention and theories that sup- 13. How might intervention in neurodegenerative
port the ever-growing gamut of intervention meth- conditions slow cognitive-linguistic decline?
ods. After reading and reflecting on the content in 14. What is the best time to initiate treatment
this chapter, you should be able to answer, in your with people who have neurodegenerative
own words, the following queries: conditions?

1. What are the purposes of treatment methods?


2. What are the mechanisms of recovery after
What Are the Purposes of Treatment Methods?
stroke and brain injury?
3. How may behavioral treatment facilitate brain
recovery? The ultimate goal of clinical aphasiologists is to help
4. How may pharmacological agents facilitate people with neurogenic language disorders foster the
brain changes? most fulfilling lives possible. To that end, there are four
5. How may brain stimulation facilitate brain primary purposes for any aspect of treatment related
changes? to acquired neurogenic communication disorders:
6. What other types of intervention may facilitate
brain changes? • facilitate brain-based recovery of abilities that
7. Can we differentiate spontaneous recovery have been lost or reduced
from progress made through treatment? • help compensate for language impairments
8. What are the optimal times during recovery to and empower use of intact abilities to
initiate treatment? maximize effective communication
9. What is the optimal focus of initial treatment • support people psychologically and socially in
soon after a stroke or brain injury? coping with lost or reduced abilities
10. What is the optimal intensity and duration of • encourage the fullest life participation
treatment? possible for people with neurogenic
11. What is the best level of complexity for communication challenges and the people
treatment foci? who are important to them

357
358  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

A majority of published treatment approaches plasticity is fundamental to spontaneous recovery as


for aphasia and related disorders address primarily well as to changes that are due to learning, behavioral
either the first or second of these goals and some- intervention, pharmacotherapy, and other treatments.
times both. These are highlighted in the descriptions Research in this area is based on animals and on peo-
of theories supporting each specific approach in the ple with and without a wide range of neurological
upcoming chapters on treatment methods. Methods disorders, including people undergoing surgery.
aimed at fostering brain-based recovery are some- Mechanisms of brain changes underlying recov-
times called restorative or restitutive approaches, ery include the following:
in contrast to compensatory approaches.
For example, let’s consider two different ap- • Reduction of edema: As swelling goes
proaches to help people with aphasia improve their down, the compression of surrounding brain
communicative ability in the face of word-finding tissue is reduced; areas that were temporarily
deficits. Semantic Feature Analysis (Boyle & Coelho, malfunctioning due to increased intracranial
1995) is a treatment method aimed at enhancing pressure begin to function more normally
activation of neural networks involved in semantic again.
representation of words. It is generally considered • Reperfusion: Blood flow is restored to areas
a restitutive approach because the goal is to foster of hypoperfusion (e.g., ischemic penumbrae);
actual changes in brain mechanisms that under- with refreshed blood supply to areas
lie semantic representation. In contrast, Promoting surrounding necrotic tissue, surrounding
Aphasics’ Communicative Effectiveness (PACE; brain tissue becomes more functional.
Davis & Wilcox, 1985) is a treatment approach that • Resolution of diaschisis: Functions
entails use of any and all modalities (speech, writing, associated with brain structures remote from
drawing, gesturing) to communicate; the measure the area of damage that had been initially
of communicative success is based on whether the impaired (a phenomenon described in
listener has understood, not on linguistic accuracy. Chapter 6) improve over time.
It is typically considered a compensatory approach • Neuronal regeneration: Although necrotic
because the goal is to help the person with apha- neurons (those whose cell bodies have died)
sia compensate for deficits by using alternative in the brain are typically not considered
and mixed modalities, not on restoring impaired revivable, components of injured neurons can
brain functions. be restored. This can happen in two ways,
Means of supporting people in coping with the through
long-lasting effects of language disability are more • dendritic branching, an increase in dendritic
rooted in counseling, coaching, and education-ori- connections and thus the number of
ented practices (see Chapter 27) than they are spe- synapses that can be made per neuron; and
cific cognitive-linguistic treatment methods. The • collateral sprouting, an increase in axonal
excellent clinician pursues a multifaceted approach receptivity per neuron to other neurons
to treatment, blending strategies that address each of through the growth of new axonal branches
the four primary purposes for intervention as appro- in uninjured axons near injured cells.
priate in a given situation. • Long-term potentiation (LTP): The efficiency
of transmission at the synaptic level is increased
in surviving neurons, thus compensating for
What Are the Mechanisms of Recovery reduced transmission from others.
• Unmasking of preexisting pathways: Neural
After Stroke and Brain Injury?
connections that already existed before
injury but that were not active (or that were
In Chapter 7, we considered the importance of neu- previously inhibited) may be activated and
roplasticity (the ability of the nervous system to thus help compensate for connections lost
change and adapt to internal or external influences) through injury.
as a basic principle enabling improvement in abil- • Cortical reorganization: Basic brain-behavior
ities following stroke or brain injury. The brain’s relationships are modified as areas of brain
24. Treatment Theories and Types of Treatment to Enhance Language and Cognition   359

tissue that were not centrally involved in The more we learn about how neurological
certain functions prior to injury take over functioning is influenced by behavioral intervention,
those functions. Examples of this are seen cognitive-linguistic practice, and general active use
within and between hemispheres. of intact cognitive and linguistic abilities in every-
day activities, the more we might be able to channel
These mechanisms of postinjury recovery are that knowledge into developing new approaches to
highly interactive and, in some cases, not separa- intervention. The rapid expansion of new knowl-
ble. For example, LTP may be a means of facilitating edge made possible through neuroimaging meth-
dendritic branching and collateral sprouting and ods bodes well for enhancing understanding in this
thus the fostering of new synapses. Cortical reor- area, as does the ever-increasing synergy of expertise
ganization is facilitated largely through neuronal across disciplines (e.g., chemistry, biology, engineer-
regeneration, LTP, and unmasking of preexisting ing, linguistics, psychology, and education), includ-
pathways. Compared to those three latter aspects ing the testing of theories of learning and memory,
of recovery, cortical reorganization as a construct is through neuroscientific methods. It is important to
more easily studied in living people with neurogenic keep in mind that, in cases where neurological func-
disorders because it can be more readily investigated tioning may not be fully restored, compensatory
through the use of neuroimaging (see Kurland et al., strategies remain a vital aspect of rehabilitation.
2017; Pataraia et al., 2004; Thompson, 2000a, 2000b; Sensory stimulation is a category of methods,
Thompson & den Ouden, 2008). All of the mecha- some loosely defined and others highly specific, that
nisms of recovery listed earlier can be facilitated, in have been purported to enhance recovery in stroke
most cases, by some type of intervention. There is and brain injury and to slow decline in neurodegen-
a tremendous need for improved research design, erative conditions. Basically, sensory stimulation
analysis, and reporting in research on aphasia and involves exposure to touch, vibration, light, scent,
related disorders in order for us to better under- sound, or taste; it may be passive or include interac-
stand the nature of recovery mechanisms (Wilson & tive experience. Sensory stimulation has been recom-
Schneck, 2021). mended by many to enhance awareness, alertness,
attention, and responsiveness in people in coma
or minimally conscious states (e.g., Urbenjaphol
How May Behavioral Treatment et al., 2009). It has been recommended as a means
of promoting intellectual and social engagement
Facilitate Brain Recovery?
for people with dementia, especially those living
in sensory-deprived contexts (Bourgeois & Hickey,
Enriched environmental input (including multi- 2009; Chung & Lai, 2009). As an approach overall,
modal stimulation) and active engagement in cog- sensory stimulation is not well described. Much of
nitive-linguistic activity may help support cortical the related research is based on small samples, does
growth and synaptic transmission at every age, not include controlled stimuli, and does not include
including very old age. Studies with animals and details pertaining to dosage, methods, and partic-
humans have demonstrated evidence of the effects ipant characteristics (Meyer et al., 2010). Further
of behavioral intervention in terms of outcomes such research is needed to validate claims about how,
as increased dendritic areas, collateral sprouting, the when, and for whom specific types of stimulation
number of synapses per neuron, the degree of reacti- may facilitate brain recovery or slow deterioration.
vation of impaired functional areas, and the degree
of enhanced activation of intact structures that
appear to help compensate for impaired structures. How May Pharmacological Agents
At a basic level, the very acts of learning and remem-
Facilitate Brain Changes?
bering increase synaptic efficiency in the brain in all
people, not just those with neurological disorders.
The role of the speech-language pathologist (SLP) is Pharmacological intervention (also called pharma-
central in fostering recovery through strategic com- cotherapy and neuropharmacology) is a potentially
munication intervention methods. important means of facilitating recovery from stroke,
360  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

brain injury, and degenerative conditions, accord- bers of people with aphasia (e.g., Tanaka et al., 1997;
ing to individual conditions and responsiveness Walker-Batson et al., 2001). Cholinergic drugs that
(Berthier et al., 2011; Dyer et al., 2018). Drugs gener- affect the thalamic nuclei and their connections to
ally work in one of three ways: by blocking the recep- the cortex have been found to improve linguistic
tion of neurotransmitters, modulating (augmenting) abilities in some stroke survivors. One such drug,
the uptake of neurotransmitters by receptors, or donepezil (Aricept), has been found to have positive
mimicking (imitating) natural neurotransmitters. Of effects on some people with aphasia (Berthier, 2005;
course, it is not within the SLP’s scope of practice Berthier et al., 2011; Berthier et al., 2006), and it might
to administer pharmacotherapy. Still, we are often have an impact beyond the acute phase of recovery.
in a consulting and information-sharing role with Unfortunately, no particular drug has been
medical professionals who do, and we play import- consistently or unequivocally demonstrated to fos-
ant roles in discussing whether certain medications ter recovery of neurological functioning associated
might be considered and whether current drugs with language (see de Boissezon et al., 2007). Also,
prescribed — or interactions among them — might be no drug in isolation appears consistently to improve
negatively affecting communication and socializa- the cognitive or linguistic abilities of people with
tion. Another important role aphasiologists play is acquired neurogenic cognitive-linguistic disorders
in helping people with cognitive and linguistic dis- without being paired with behavioral intervention
orders adhere to prescribed drug regimens through (Galling et al., 2014; Small, 2004). There is a need for
memory enhancement methods and the use of mem- drug effects reported to date to be tested at higher
ory aids. levels of evidence than has typically been done, such
Some drugs play a key role in directly enhanc- as through studies with randomized, placebo-con-
ing neural recovery mechanisms. In the acute phase, trolled designs and careful controls for placebo and
for example, edema may be reduced through the use practice effects (see Cahana-Amitay et al., 2014;
of drugs that regulate the permeability of the blood- Klein & Albert, 2004; Tanaka et al., 2013).
brain barrier to water, thus restricting the accumu- Additionally, consumers of research on phar-
lation of fluid that leads to swelling in the brain macological treatments must carefully scrutinize
after an injury. Reperfusion of ischemic penumbrae whether statistically significant effects are applica-
can be facilitated through thrombolytic agents such ble to relevant clinical outcomes for people within
as tissue plasminogen activator (tPA, or Altepase) a diagnostic category as well as for a given per-
(Chen et al., 2017; Hillis et al., 2006; Saver et al., son (Royall, 2005). Finally, for each individual, it
2013; Wardlaw et al., 2012). During post-acute care, is important to consider the many potentially neg-
drugs may also be used to enhance LTP and, in turn, ative side effects associated with any of the drugs
enhance cognitive abilities (Cooke & Bliss, 2005). mentioned here, as well as with other medications
Some researchers have studied how drugs that stim- that may be prescribed for other conditions, such
ulate the neurotransmitter dopamine (dopaminergic as pain, insomnia, and depression. Side effects and
drugs, such as bromocriptine) might help modulate consequences of drug interactions may include
brain activity in a way that enhances language com- slowed recovery, lessened affective response, re-
prehension and formulation by selectively inhibiting duced speed of processing, and diminished working
transmission of sensory information that may inter- memory and attention abilities.
fere with information processing (Albert et al., 1988;
Ashtary et al., 2006; Galling et al., 2014; Gill & Leff,
2014; Gold et al., 2000). How May Brain Stimulation
Amphetamines (such as Adderall) have also
Facilitate Brain Changes?
been considered as a possible means of neuromod-
ulation, perhaps by enhancing collateral sprouting
(Walker-Batson, 2000; Walker-Batson et al., 2016). During surgery, direct electrical stimulation to spe-
Serotonergic drugs (which block presynaptic trans- cific brain structures may activate or inhibit them;
fer of the transmitter serotonin) have been found to however, since direct stimulation is so highly inva-
improve language abilities in studies of small num- sive, it is only administered if a person’s brain is
24. Treatment Theories and Types of Treatment to Enhance Language and Cognition   361

already being operated on. Surgical interventions about implementing tDCS. Concerns include safety,
are not recommended for most people with apha- costs, and practicality of implementation within
sia and related disorders whose conditions are due their work contexts (e.g., reimbursement, adminis-
to necrosis or atrophy of brain tissue. Two noninva- trator buy-in, and training).
sive brain stimulation techniques are under study More research is needed on TMS and tDCS,
as means of enhancing language recovery in people especially to document the long-term effects and
with aphasia: transcranial magnetic stimulation specific symptoms. It will be important to further
(also called repetitive TMS, or rTMS) and transcra- validate the methods as potential adjuncts to behav-
nial direct current stimulation (tDCS). ioral intervention methods and to demonstrate
rTMS is a potentially important, noninvasive treatment outcomes in terms of everyday functional
means of enhancing recovery of language in peo- communication.
ple with aphasia (Kapoor, 2017; Li, Qu, Yuan, &
Du, 2015; Naeser et al., 2005, 2010; Otal, et al., 2015;
Weiduschat et al., 2011). Magnetic coils are placed What Other Types of Intervention
on the scalp to stimulate or inhibit activation of tar-
May Facilitate Brain Changes?
geted brain regions beneath the scalp with repeated
low-frequency magnetic pulses. One of the reasons
rTMS may be helpful is that it reduces diaschisis There are several other types of intervention that
through activation of areas that are intact but have may be administered in addition to behavioral and
ceased to function normally (Carrera & Tononi, pharmacological interventions to address cogni-
2014). Another is that rTMS helps to inhibit overac- tive-communicative impairments. It is important to
tivation of right hemisphere homologues of dam- weigh potential risks and benefits of each. In some
aged left hemisphere brain regions specialized for cases, surgical interventions may facilitate recov-
language. It may be that the natural inclination of ery. Removal of neoplasm and shunting of excess
the right hemisphere to be activated as a means of cerebrospinal fluid are examples; they work pri-
cortical reorganization to compensate for left hemi- marily by reducing intracranial pressure and thus
sphere damage is actually less helpful (at least for displacement of and pressure on functional areas of
some people with aphasia) than restoring as much the brain.
activity as possible in the left hemisphere (Post- Nutritional supplements are another category
man-Caucheteux et al., 2010; Winhuisen et al., 2005). of potential intervention to support neurological
tDCS is a technique that entails pulses of low- recovery. Antioxidants, a category of natural and
level electrical current, delivered through electrodes human-made substances that counteract the damag-
on the scalp (de Aguiar et al., 2015; Fridriksson ing effects of oxidation on bodily tissue, are under-
et al., 2012, 2018). Cathodal tDCS decreases excit- studied in animals and humans (Gasparova et al.,
ability of the underlying brain area; anodal tDCS 2014; Sarkaki et al., 2013). Several authors have rec-
increases brain activity. They may be used separately ommended them with respect to promoting brain
or at the same time over different brain regions. health in general. Antioxidants are found naturally
Several studies have shown positive results when in many fruits and vegetables; they are also avail-
tDCS has been applied along with speech and lan- able as dietary supplements. Examples include beta-​
guage intervention. A systematic review of such carotene, lycopene, and vitamins A, C, and E. Herbal
studies did not show significant gains (Elsner et supplements have been commonly prescribed in
al., 2015), although outcome measures were limited Eastern approaches to health and medicine for years,
primarily to word-finding, and many studies were a topic we explore further in Chapter 28.
excluded due to strict inclusion criteria. In a more Other means of supporting spontaneous recov-
recent study, Fridriksson et al. (2018) report results ery and brain health in general include good nutri-
that support the potential for enhancing naming tion (especially low-glycemic diets), exercise, solid
in people with aphasia. In an analysis of survey daily rest, strong coping skills and countering of
responses from 155 SLPs, Keator et al. (2020) report depression, and strong social support. Avoidance
that most (94.2%) clinicians express apprehension of negative factors on the brain is also important
362  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

(Marshall & Mohapatra, 2017). For example, it is following brain injury. Cognitive-linguistic symp-
vital to reduce the risk of further stroke or injury toms tend to evolve during the hyperacute stage
and exposure to harmful substances (e.g., cocaine, due to changes in blood flow around ischemic pen-
steroids, lead, and excessive alcohol). umbrae, resolution of diaschisis, and reduction of
edema. An approach that does not work at one time
may work later.

Can We Differentiate Spontaneous Recovery


From Progress Made Through Treatment?
What Is the Optimal Focus of Initial Treatment
Soon After a Stroke or Brain Injury?
A large body of literature provides evidence that
treatment of aphasia and related disorders leads to
improvements that cannot be accounted for by spon- A great deal of literature addresses how we should
taneous recovery alone (e.g., Allen et al., 2012; Brady prioritize precious time spent with people in the
et al., 2012, 2016; Kelly et al., 2010; Robey, 1998; Wertz days and weeks following newly acquired neu-
et al., 1986). A great deal remains to be studied to rogenic communication disorders. Let’s consider
address questions about specific outcomes associ- important guidance about how to focus our work
ated with specific areas of communication-related early post-onset.
impairment and life participation and with spe-
cific intervention methods. There is also a need for
research on how to best complement pharmacolog- Focus on Communication Needs
ical and other approaches to fostering physical neu-
rological recovery with behavioral intervention by At all levels of care (during hospitalization, in acute
clinical aphasiologists (see Raymer et al., 2008). and subacute care, during rehabilitation, and in
home, long-term care, and assisted living environ-
ments), people with language impairments need to
be able to send and receive information. They need
What Are the Optimal Times During to take an active part in their medical management
Recovery to Initiate Treatment? and in decision-making about their care, learn about
their conditions, and express themselves about such
Immediately following stroke or brain injury, the topics as pain level, dietary wants and needs, con-
focus of health care tends to be on the individual’s cerns about medications, and wishes to involve or
survival. Unfortunately, many SLPs and other reha- not involve friends and significant others in commu-
bilitation professionals working with stroke and nications and decision-making about their care.
brain injury survivors in the acute stage often have
little time to spend in assessment and treatment
because of restrictions on the amount of time an Counsel and Share Information
individual is allowed to stay in an acute care setting.
Fortunately, there is ample evidence that direct SLP Without a doubt, the earliest SLP intervention
intervention can have positive effects in acute and should entail counseling and information sharing
post-acute phases for decades following onset. with people who have communication challenges
Even if a person is not yet ready for or respon- and their friends and family. If the individual is not
sive to direct treatment, it is never too early to begin able to communicate sufficiently to express needs
the process of helping a person cope with the life-​ and desires, then setting up basic means of commu-
affecting consequences of an acquired cognitive-​ nication through communication boards, devices,
linguistic disorder. Keep in mind that a great deal and apps is most critical. Also, caregiver training
of changes occur in the brain in the days and weeks focused on supportive communication strategies is
24. Treatment Theories and Types of Treatment to Enhance Language and Cognition   363

essential. Building a strong sense of alliance with the sis of the limbs; restricting use of a functional arm or
individual and the people most important to them is leg was shown to increase motor functioning in the
an important priority through early intervention of impaired limb.
any type (Schönberger et al., 2006; Simmons-Mackie Constraint-induced language therapy (CILT, see
et al., 2016; Sohlberg et al., 2001; Turkstra, 2013). Chapter 30) shares with CIT the underlying rationale
that it is important to encourage patients to use the
language modalities that are most impaired. There is
Promote Rest no credible evidence, however, that using supported
communication across all modalities impedes recov-
Most people who have just experienced a stroke or ery of impaired modalities. Furthermore, failing to
brain injury are in dire need of rest. Sleep is essential attend to the communication needs of people with
to cortical health and healing. Unfortunately, med- acquired language disorders as soon as possible is
ical contexts are notoriously noisy and not condu- simply unethical.
cive to rest. Patients are often awakened for all sorts
of things that might be better delayed until they
waken naturally after solid sleep. SLPs are often per- Consider Pros and Cons of
petrators of this, prioritizing their own assessment Focusing on Attention
and treatment scheduling demands over the basic
needs of the individual to be served. Try not to be
Some aphasiologists have debated whether inter-
one of them.
vention during the acute phase should include a
focus on enhancing attention. The rationale for treat-
Consider the Balance of Compensatory ing attention early was that attention is essential to
With Restitutive Approaches further learning and memory. However, others have
suggested that there is no benefit from treatment spe-
cifically focused on attention (see Novack et al., 1996,
Historically, many aphasiologists (e.g., Darley, 1982) and Ponsford & Kinsella, 1988). Given that attention
have recommended the use of impairment-focused is a fundamental component of active participation
restitutive approaches during early acute and post- in cognitive and linguistic intervention, most if not
acute treatment, with the aim of enhancing the all of our treatments are tapping into practice with
brain’s physiological recovery. More recently, some attention. We consider this further in Chapter 25 as
have recommended against impairment-focused we explore general approaches to treatment.
treatment during an acute care hospital stay imme-
diately after a stroke or brain injury (Duffy et al.,
2011; Holland & Fridriksson, 2001; Simmons-Mackie,
2013b; Sohlberg & Turkstra, 2011). Some refer to ani- What Is the Optimal Intensity
mal studies demonstrating that too much focus on and Duration of Treatment?
intensive learning tasks soon after brain injury can
exacerbate the injury (see Kleim & Jones, 2008).
Some aphasiologists have argued that using The concept of treatment intensity includes con-
compensatory approaches early will reduce the sideration of the number, frequency, and duration
likelihood of greater physiological recovery. Pro- of treatment sessions; intensity of treatment over
ponents of this view often cite the work of Pul- a specified period of time is sometimes referred to
vermüller and colleagues on constraint-induced as treatment dosage. Optimal intensity depends on
therapy (CIT; Pulvermüller et al., 2001). CIT entails several factors, including the following:
restricting an individual’s use of intact abilities to
maximize reliance on, and thus use and stimulation • health and well-being of the person being
of, impaired abilities. The origin of CIT was in treat- treated, including the degree of personal and
ment of people who had hemiparalysis or hemipare- environmental support for recovery
364  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

• nature of the person’s cognitive-linguistic This makes it all the more paramount that we moni-
impairments and their life-affecting tor each individual’s progress carefully and continu-
consequences ously, making ongoing treatment decisions based on
• treatment goals practice-based evidence.
• treatment methods implemented (how and at
what pace they are administered, what type
of feedback is given, the amount of practice What Is the Best Level of Complexity
entailed, etc.)
for Treatment Foci?
• therapeutic relationship established between
the clinician and the person treated
It was once considered “best practice” to begin treat-
In general, more intense intervention leads to ment at a level of complexity that is relatively easy
the greatest brain changes as well as the greatest for an individual and then to progressively build up
functional abilities (Brady et al., 2016; Breitenstein the level of difficulty as treatment progresses (e.g.,
et al., 2017; Cherney, 2012; Enderby, 2012). However, Porch, 2008). More recently, however, evidence has
it is extremely important to recognize that this is not been mounting that using more complex stimuli and
always the case. In people with neurological disor- tasks actually seems to optimize recovery by recruit-
ders, if intensity exceeds one’s ability to attend and ing more intact neural networks and enhancing their
participate actively, or if it leads a person to tire of or interconnections through behavioral intervention
resist treatment, then it is no longer beneficial. If the (Kiran & Thompson, 2003; Thompson et al., 2003).
intensity of a particular intervention is too great for Such evidence has been incorporated as the com-
a particular individual at a particular point in time, plexity account of treatment efficacy (CATE).
it can be more harmful to recovery than providing Support for this account has been provided
no treatment at all. through evidence of the following:
The factors that determine optimal intensity
interact in complex ways that are not well under- • Training of naming for atypical exemplars of
stood. Reasons they are not well understood include words within categories leads to generalization
the following: of naming for words corresponding to
typical exemplars but not vice versa (Kiran &
• a lack of research studies in which each factor Johnson, 2008; Kiran et al., 2011).
is carefully controlled for and described and • Training of abstract words within a category
in which precise and well-validated indices of leads to generalization of naming for words
treatment outcomes are implemented corresponding to more concepts but not vice
• a higher drop-out rate for people in high- versa (Kiran & Johnson, 2008).
intensity compared to low-intensity therapy • Training of more complex verbs (based on
• the sheer complexity of those factors, argument structure, which is reflected in the
individually and in combination number of sematic roles attached to the verb
• inconsistencies among authors of various in a clause or sentence, such as subject direct
studies on the topic regarding what dosage is object) leads to generalization to untrained
considered “intense” verbs with simpler argument structure
• the fact that some individuals may reach a (Thompson et al., 2013).
maximum level of benefit after a specific dose • Training of more complex syntactic structures
of treatment and may not make gains beyond generalizes to less complex structures (Dickey
that point, regardless of intensity (Baker, 2012; & Thompson, 2007; Thompson et al., 2010).
Brady et al., 2016; Enderby, 2012)
The expert clinician balances judgments about
Overall, keep in mind that the degree of bene- evidence-based practice relative to task and stimu-
fit from and tolerance of specific levels of intensity lus complexity with judgments about the degree of
depend on the person we are treating at a given time. frustration or lost confidence that a given individual
24. Treatment Theories and Types of Treatment to Enhance Language and Cognition   365

may experience through repeated complex and chal- cognitive and learning theories suggesting
lenging tasks. that stimulation through multiple modalities
leads to greater storage of, and access to,
information. This is likely due to the fact
What Other Treatment Parameters that attempts to engage a greater number of
connections or routes leading to the activation
Are Important to Consider?
of a neural network enhance LTP and increase
the likelihood of activating that network.
Some advocate the application of principles of motor
learning to cognitive-linguistic rehabilitation (see
Maas et al., 2008, for a review). This includes the fol- How Might Intervention in Neurodegenerative
lowing notions:
Conditions Slow Cognitive-Linguistic Decline?
• Varied practice (across tasks and activities)
is better than constant practice on the same The principles of neurobiology of recovery that
thing. apply to stroke and brain injury are largely applica-
• Random practice on varied types of cognitive- ble to neurodegenerative conditions, with one criti-
linguistic stimuli is better than blocks of cal exception: People with degenerative conditions
practice on the same stimuli. may have periods of improvement or plateau; still,
• A greater number of repetitions is better than overall, their affected functional abilities will con-
fewer repetitions. tinue to decline over time. Many of the mechanisms
• Low frequency of feedback encourages of recovery mentioned earlier, and the strategies to
self-evaluation of performance and thus facilitate them, apply to people with dementia and
independence in implementing what is related conditions as well.
learned in treatment. Given that impaired LTP may be a primary cause
• Knowledge of performance (knowing how of cognitive-linguistic decline in people with primary
accurately one has accomplished a task) is progressive aphasia, Alzheimer’s disease, and other
more important than knowledge of response forms of dementia, facilitating synaptic transmission
(receiving detailed feedback about just what within intact neurons through new learning, experi-
one did correctly or incorrectly during a ence, pharmacological agents, diet, nutritional sup-
given task). plements, exercise, and social support likely boosts
the health of intact neural connections. In combina-
Other parameters that may influence learning and tion, and implemented strategically with individual
memory include the following: needs and real-life contexts in mind, intervention
may delay onset and slow progression of symp-
• Interstimulus intervals (ISIs). ISI, the amount toms. Promising results have been shown for certain
of time between presentations of cognitive- drugs, including cholinesterase inhibitors — donepe-
linguistic stimuli, influences learning and zil (the most commonly prescribed drug to address
memory related to those stimuli. Specific dementia), galantamine, and rivastigmine — as well
influences of ISI duration depend on an as memantine (Berthier et al., 2006, 2009; Falchook
individual’s unique neurological status. et al., 2014; Klein et al., 2006; Tocco et al., 2014);
• The degree of ecological validity of stimuli the effects are not consistent across varied types
used. The more the types of stimuli and of degenerative and stroke-induced and traumatic
behavior addressed in a treatment setting brain injury–induced cognitive-​communicative dis-
complement real-world use, the greater is the orders, making diagnostic processes paramount to
likelihood of carryover of treatment effects to decisions regarding possible drug prescriptions.
everyday life participation. Many drugs commonly prescribed to people
• Modalities of treatment. Recent studies with neurogenic cognitive impairments have dele-
in neuroscience support long-standing terious effects on their intact cognitive abilities. As
366  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

mentioned earlier in the context of pharmacotherapy intellectual impairment arise. It is vital to develop
for stroke and brain injury survivors, medication memory aids and communication supports while
prescribed for any number of concomitant health a person is still able to participate in active deci-
conditions (e.g., cancer, diabetes, allergies, seizure sion-making about what content and activities are
disorders, infections) may lead to exacerbation of most important. Also, to the degree that memory
cognitive-communicative symptoms. Antipsychotic and communication-enhancing technology may be
medications in particular (often administered to con- helpful, training a person to use it early is likely to
trol undesirable behavior; see Chapters 13 and 23) help ensure longer successful use as their cognitive
can increase the risk of infection, heart failure, hallu- abilities decline. As with all of our clinical work,
cination, delirium, confabulation, and death. building positive, affirming alliances with the peo-
ple served, including the people who care for them,
is essential to enhancing quality of life through qual-
What Is the Best Time to Initiate ity of communication.
Treatment With People Who Have Scientifically based theory plays a crucial role
in evidence-based clinical practice. At the same
Neurodegenerative Conditions?
time, on any given day, we treat specific people, not
groups of people categorized by clinical symptom-
For people with neurodegenerative conditions, the atology. Theories of intervention must be translated
best time to initiate treatment is as soon as possible, to applicable, clinical practice, even if the translation
once symptoms related to language of generalized is not always clearly direct or immediate.

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter behavioral, pharmacological, and other
that are new to you or that you have not yet aspects of intervention may lead to
mastered. enhanced neurobiological recovery.
2. Four basic purposes of treatment methods 5. Describe the optimal timing and foci
for people with acquired neurogenic of treatment for people with language
disorders of cognition and language are disorders due to stroke and brain injury.
listed early in this chapter. In what ways will 6. What factors influence the optimal intensity
focusing on your specific purpose at a given of treatment focused on cognitive-linguistic
moment in time with a given client help you abilities?
to be most effective? 7. What are the key challenges in determining
3. Compare and contrast restorative and the optimal intensity of treatment?
compensatory approaches to treatment. 8. What are some specific means of slowing
4. Consider the mechanisms of brain changes cognitive-linguistic decline in people with
listed and described in this chapter. neurodegenerative conditions?
a. In what ways might each be facilitated
through some sort of intervention? For additional learning and reflection activities,
www
b. Describe specific ways in which see the companion website.
SECTION VII

General Approaches to Treatment


368  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

This section addresses general approaches to inter- another or from the more “specific” approaches
vention for a wide range of cognitive-communicative discussed in Section VIII. In Chapter 26, we explore
disorders, including those associated with traumatic approaches to promoting communication, cognition,
brain injury, stroke and degenerative conditions. In and life participation of people with various forms of
Chapter 25, we address the construct of treatment dementia. In Chapter 27, we consider how we might
fidelity and discuss many general approaches, from best engage as counselors and coaches in our work
social and life participation models to cognitive to support, empower, and inform people with neu-
neuropsychological and cognitive rehabilitation rogenic disorders of cognition and language and the
programs, to stimulation-facilitation methods, and people who care about them. Finally, in Chapter 28,
on to augmentative and alternative communica- we delve into the topic of complementary and inte-
tion, smartphone apps, software, and intensive and grative approaches to wellness and their relevance
residential programs. We note that many of these to clinical practice.
approaches are not mutually exclusive from one
CHAPTER
25
General Approaches for Enhancing
Cognitive-Linguistic Abilities in
Traumatic Brain Injury, Stroke
Survivors, and People With Primary
Progressive Aphasia and Dementia

In this chapter, we review a rich set of general 4. What general cognitive neuropsychological
approaches to treatment in neurogenic language approaches are applicable to treatment?
disorders. By general approaches, we mean that they 5. What is cognitive rehabilitation?
are sets of principles and recommendations for how 6. What is the stimulation-facilitation approach?
intervention is to be delivered. They reflect experts’ 7. How may group treatment be implemented,
distillations of best practice in clinical aphasiology. and how can it help people with aphasia and
These are loosely differentiated from the approaches related disorders?
described in the chapters in Section VIII, which 8. How May AAC, Apps, and Software Be
include approaches and methods that can be encap- Used to Support Communication and Aid in
sulated through more specific guidelines about how Treatment?
a method is to be carried out. The distinction is a 9. What are intensive and residential aphasia
blurry one. Researchers have completed carefully programs, and how can they help people with
controlled intervention outcome studies on some aphasia and related disorders?
aspects of general approaches, whereas some spe-
cific methods have not been investigated at levels
of evidence higher than anecdotal, descriptive, or What Is Treatment Fidelity, and How Is
single-case studies.
It Relevant to Clinical Aphasiology?
After reading and reflecting on the content in
this chapter, you should be able to answer, in your
own words, the following queries: One consideration in determining whether a treat-
ment approach fits within a general or specific
1. What is treatment fidelity, and how is it treatment category is whether it could be described
relevant to clinical aphasiology? in sufficient detail such that it is clear when one is
2. What general social and life participation using the approach specifically as it was designed
approaches are applicable to treatment? or whether clinicians take liberties in adapting the
3. What general treatment methods fit within approach. Therapist drift, the tendency for clini-
social and life participation models? cians to vary a treatment protocol according to their

369
370  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

own predilections and in response to behaviors of • having others misunderstand the nature of
the individual being treated (Waller, 2009), is part their communication disorder
of what makes intervention research extremely • feeling disrespected and being treated as
challenging. children or as less competent or less intelligent
Treatment fidelity is the degree to which an • being left out of conversations
intervention method is administered in a reliable • being unable to follow conversations because
way or in accordance with a specific protocol. If of fast talking and multiple speakers talking at
treatment studies are carried out without consis- once
tency in research design and means of indexing treat- • being given insufficient time to respond
ment effects, then it is difficult to make conclusions during conversations
about just what aspects of a method are effective, • being treated as if they are a burden
for whom, and under what circumstances (Gearing • being treated as if they are ill or unhealthy
et al., 2011; Waller, 2009). Despite the importance of • feeling incapable of contributing meaningfully
treatment fidelity, it is rarely indexed (or even men- to others (Baar, 2021; Brown, Worrall, et al.,
tioned) explicitly in treatment studies in aphasiology 2010; Dalemans et al., 2010; Meyerson &
(Hinckley & Douglas, 2013). This is an area in need Zuckerman, 2019; Shadden, 2005; Strong &
of improvement as we continue to build the evidence Shadden, 2021; Worrall et al., 2011)
base supporting our intervention methods (Kader-
avek & Justice, 2010; Schlosser, 2002). At the same Social and life participation approaches are
time, especially outside the research context, clini- not specific treatment approaches that are typically
cians often have good reason to modify a method encapsulated into explicit lists of treatment steps.
in a given context to suit individual circumstances. Instead, they are addressed throughout this book as
vital frameworks to embrace in all aspects of clini-
cal practice. Here we review some of the key social
What General Social and Life Participation approaches as they pertain to intervention for people
with acquired neurogenic language disorders.
Approaches Are Applicable to Treatment?

Social and life participation models share common Life Participation Approach to Aphasia
tenets that quality of life is directly connected to
quality of relationships and the ability to participate The Life Participation Approach to Aphasia (LPAA)
actively in meaningful activities. Since being able to (introduced in Chapter 4, revisited throughout this
communicate is vital to relationships and activities, book, and expounded upon in Chapter 23) puts the
enhancing communication in people with communi- life concerns of people with aphasia at the center
cation disorders is fundamental to promoting their of decision-making. Although developed with a
quality of life. These approaches view aphasia as a focus on aphasia, it is applicable to all people with
chronic condition with long-term life-affecting con- acquired neurogenic communication disorders.
sequences and encourage clinicians to use creative, Excellent clinical aphasiologists may ascribe to many
socially contextualized methods to enhance func- other approaches as well; still, clinical practice with
tional outcomes. LPAA principles as a foundation is vital to clinical
The notion that enhancing life participation is excellence among all aphasiologists. For this reason,
our ultimate goal as speech-language pathologists I am supposing that you are already a devotee of this
(SLPs) may seem obvious to most. However, when approach.
one examines actual practice, the common degree Proponents of LPAA recognize that communi-
of focus on impairment-level deficits without cor- cation problems affect interpersonal bonds and thus
responding focus on real-life communication needs virtually all aspects of one’s life. We also recognize
is disheartening. Perception of social communica- the complexity of communication in real-life con-
tion challenges as expressed by people with aphasia texts and that impairment-focused treatment strat-
include the following: egies are only meaningful and effective when they
25. General Approaches for Enhancing Cognitive-Linguistic Abilities   371

are grounded in what is relevant, meaningful, and much in common with ICF foci. Adherents of these
important in the lives of the people we serve. We approaches recognized the dynamic aspects of indi-
consider holistically the many factors that influ- viduals, their communication needs, and the social
ence lifelong coping with aphasia. We see the role systems in which they play roles as part of their
of clinical aphasiologists as vital to helping people everyday activities. Proponents recommended that
live successfully with aphasia, not just in acute, sub- rehabilitation address not only the individual but
acute, and rehabilitation contexts, but over years and their family, work environment, and social circles,
decades, through various life transitions. and that it should also emphasize sociocultural
The World Health Organization (WHO) Inter- relevance.
national Classification of Functioning, Disability,
and Health (ICF; described among the conceptual
frameworks in Chapter 4 and expounded upon in Supported Communication
Chapter 5) has been the focus of several life par-
ticipation approaches to all of the acquired neuro- Supported communication is not a specific method
genic language disorders addressed in this book. but rather a philosophy and set of tenets and strat-
The Living with Aphasia: Framework for Outcome egies that should be implemented as part of all
Measurement (A-FROM; Kagan, 2011; Kagan et al., aspects of intervention. Although major proponents
2008), as discussed in Chapter 17, is a means of con- have framed it as an approach to support people
ceptualizing the outcomes of intervention for people with aphasia, it is highly relevant and directly appli-
with aphasia based largely on the ICF. Proponents cable to people with all forms of communication
recommend that clinicians and scholars attend to challenge. King et al. (2013) define communication
four interrelated life-affecting impacts of aphasia: support as “anything that improves access to or par-
ticipation in communication, events or activities,”
• language and related processing including the following:
• participation
• personal factors, identity, and feelings • “strategies, material, or resources” used by
• the environment the person with the language disorder or
anyone communicating with that person
These recommendations are certainly applicable • modifications to the person’s environment or
to all other forms of acquired neurogenic language activities
disorders, not just aphasia. • “supportive attitudes that foster
In ICF-focused approaches, aphasiologists are communicative participation” (p. 9)
recognized as playing a critical role in modifying
environmental barriers to life participation and Communication support may involve any
enhancing communication support. Adherents to modality (e.g., gestures, drawing, writing, speaking,
this approach recognize how individual differences intonation patterns, facial expressions, postures, use
are paramount in considering life impacts of com- of pictures, and use of alternative and augmentative
munication disorders. We also recognize that just as communications (AAC) devices or other forms of
family members and caregivers are important poten- technology).
tial facilitators and supporters, they also may be Simmons-Mackie (2013a) clarifies that support:
responsible for exacerbating communication barriers.
An important point to note is that we can con- . . . might also involve internal properties, such as
sider existing treatment methods through the per- respect for the inherent competence of the speaker
spectives offered by an ICF framework, even if those or knowledge about the communication disor-
methods were not originally designed to address der. It includes methods that provide the skills,
language disability through the ICF constructs. It is opportunity, resources, and assistance needed to
also important to recognize that decades before the participate in communicative exchanges, social
emergence of the ICF, many clinicians advocated for interactions, and individually relevant roles or life
environmental systems approaches, which have situations . . . [It] requires not only an understanding
372  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

of the . . . impairment and its consequences but contextual support during conversation is recom-
also insight into what each individual requires mended; it has been found to increase the number
to live successfully despite residual language and degree of success of conversational exchanges
impairments. (p. 11) and to enhance the number of conversational ini-
tiatives in people with aphasia (Garrett & Huth,
Examples of ways to make the communicative envi- 2002; Ho et al., 2005). Assistive technologies that
ronment more accessible include the following: are recommended to help support communication
are described under the query about AAC, apps,
• supporting the individual and significant and software.
others in learning more about the nature
and etiology of the communication
disorder, strengths, and weaknesses and
What General Treatment Methods Fit Within
providing information through supported
Social and Life Participation Models?
communication
• providing education and counseling to
significant others to improve attitudes and Several additional approaches represent important
reduce stigma associated with communication aspects of LPAA, ICF-focused, and supported com-
problems munication approaches. These include total commu-
• providing local community-based training nication approaches, AAC, partner and care partner
programs to raise awareness and acceptance training, reciprocal scaffolding, aphasia mentoring
of aphasia and related disorders programs, Toastmasters programs, humor as ther-
• advocating for improved policies and apy, online games, and a variety of additional and
insurance coverage to support people with highly creative types of programming — none of
communication disabilities which is mutually exclusive. Let’s consider each of
• continuously adding to and updating these briefly here.
communication supports (communication
booklets or notebooks, memory wallets and
notebooks, remnant books, reminiscence Total Communication Approaches
materials, calendars, sticky notes, scripts,
new apps, assistive technology, etc.) through Total communication approaches are those that
dynamic interactive processes with multiple encourage any means of communication to convey
communication partners and ongoing and receive information. No particular modality is
assessment of what works best for whom required, and all attempts to communicate are con-
(Garrett & Kimelman, 2000; Garrett & Lasker, sidered acceptable (Collins, 1986; Lawson & Fawcus,
2013; Hinckley et al., 2013; Ho et al., 2005; 1999). The content is appreciated as more import-
Kagan et al., 2001; King, 2013a, 2013b; Rogers, ant than how the content is delivered. Strategies
King, & Alcorn, 2000; Simmons-Mackie, include the use of gesture, mime, drawing, reading,
2013b; Simmons-Mackie & King, 2013; and writing. Some people with aphasia and related
Simmons-Mackie et al., 2016) disorders naturally gravitate toward total commu-
nication approaches, initiating their own means of
In supported communication, people with- compensating for challenges with language for-
out language disorders are encouraged to take an mulation and comprehension; others require more
active role in ensuring the best exchange of informa- assistance (Rautakoski, 2011). Combining means of
tion possible, providing help in the form of cuing, expression rather than relying on verbal expression
requesting clarification, paraphrasing, asking for alone appears to facilitate spoken language abilities.
verification of what one has understood, and sup- Use of gesture with speech, for example, has been
porting content through multiple modalities. Apha- shown to enhance word retrieval (Lanyon & Rose,
sia-friendly communication (described in Chapter 2009) and oral discourse (Kong et al., 2017). Overall,
23) is encouraged. The use of pictures and graphic using total communication approaches in conver-
25. General Approaches for Enhancing Cognitive-Linguistic Abilities   373

sation with people with neurogenic language dis-


Box
orders, rather than spoken language alone, enables 25–1 Categories of Outcome Measures for
significantly more exchange of meaningful informa- Communication Partner Training
tion (Luck & Rose, 2007; Rose, 2013).
Some people have difficulty carrying over Partner and person with aphasia together
total communication strategies learned in a clinical Conversational analyses
context to actual spontaneous use in conversation
(Purdy, 2002; Wallace, Purdy, & Skidmore, 2014). Partner outcomes
This may be especially true for people with execu- Quality of life
tive functioning impairments (Purdy, 2002). Some Knowledge of aphasia
researchers have suggested that this challenge be
Questionnaires, self-reports, self-ratings,
addressed through training focused on multiple
interview responses
means of communication in an integrated way for
a single concept at a time, enhancing the associa- Outcomes for people with aphasia
tion of semantic representations through combined Quality of life
modalities (Purdy & Van Dyke, 2011; Wallace, Purdy,
Language performance according to
et al., 2014).
impairment-based assessments
Communication use and effectiveness
Partner and Caregiver Training scales
Questionnaires, self-reports, self-ratings,
Given how vital communication partners are to interview responses
communication rehabilitation and coping with long-
term communication challenges, and how little time Note: For a summary of details pertaining to outcome
people with neurogenic communication disorders measures and associated references, see Simmons-​
have access to professional services for communi- Mackie et al. (2016).
cation rehabilitation, enlisting others to participate
in the continued process of communicative support
is essential. Lyon and colleagues (Lyon, 1996; Lyon sation partners learn to use strategies to improve
et al., 1997) described programs for recruiting and communicative interactions. Many partner and
training volunteers in local communities to support caregiver training programs have been developed
social and conversational participation of people around the world (Figure 25–1). Some are central to
with aphasia. Numerous researchers, clinicians, and interventional approaches for people with demen-
higher education personnel offer such programs. tia and their caregivers (see Bourgeois et al., 1997;
Two systematic reviews of research on commu- Ripich et al., 2000). Some are integrated into specific
nication partner training for people with aphasia and treatment approaches for people with aphasia and
their partners (Cherney et al., 2010; Simmons-Mackie for traumatic brain injury (TBI) survivors, described
et al., 2016) have included a combined total of 56 in Section VIII. Some are focused on couples, such
studies, all of which have demonstrated positive as Boles’s (2009) Aphasia Couples Therapy (ACT).
outcomes. This is a striking finding based on varied ACT entails educational components to facilitate
methods, numbers of participants, training contexts, understanding about aphasia, training and practice
ages of participants, and types of aphasia. Positive in supported communication, and mutual sharing
outcomes have also been substantiated for training of evaluations of quality of communication and
of TBI survivors’ partners (Behn et al., 2021). Out- strategy implementation. Caregiver coaching and
comes measured in partner training are diverse. Cat- training programs often extend beyond foci on com-
egories of outcomes measures for partner training munication and conversation and into the realm of
are summarized in Box 25–1. counseling and coaching to help people cope with
Conversational coaching, originally described the lifelong consequences of acquired language chal-
by Holland (1991), is a process of helping conver- lenges, a topic discussed further in Chapter 27.
374  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Figure 25–1. Communication partner training. Clement Amponsah, speech-language pathol-


ogist and faculty member at the University of Ghana, engages with a person with aphasia and
his care partner in supported communication training at Korle Bu Teaching Hospital in Accra,
Ghana. A full-color version of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.

Meulenbroek and Cherney (2021) developed not know as much about the content to be learned.
and tested a computer-based workplace communi- The novice provides language modeling and feed-
cation training program for TBI survivors, focusing back during naturalistic interactions while the per-
on increasing politeness in work-related contexts. son with aphasia teaches. The intent is to provide
They found promising results in a small sample of naturalistic means of supporting meaningful com-
adults in terms of conversation partner feedback munication and natural feedback regarding commu-
and politeness makers in eight adults with chronic nicative effectiveness, while empowering the person
brain injury. with a language disorder through a teaching role
(Avent & Austermann, 2003; Avent et al., 2009).

Reciprocal Scaffolding
Workplace Immersion Programs
Reciprocal scaffolding is a method in which a per-
son with a neurogenic language disorder serves as Given that many TBI survivors, and a significant
an expert or teacher in an interaction with a person proportion of stroke survivors, have goals to return
(called a novice, learner, or apprentice). The nov- to work, workplace-immersion programs are a vital
ice may have greater language abilities but does means of preparing people for workplace reinte-
25. General Approaches for Enhancing Cognitive-Linguistic Abilities   375

gration, or at least the highest level of employment Speech-language pathology students pair with brain
that suits them. Supported work environments injury survivors, focusing on interpersonal skills and
with holistic, person-centered goals in mind can executive function goals in a thoughtfully supported
help people close the gap between the desperation baking and retail environment (Close, 2018; Jarman
over unemployment — which adds to their sense of & Lemoncello, 2021; Lemoncello & Hoepner, 2020).
decreased worth and lost identity — and meaningful Passion Works Studio (https://passion-works-​
engagement in work. There are numerous examples studio.myshopify.com), the mission of which is to
worldwide of workplace training and career devel- “inspire and liberate the human spirit through the
opment empowerment programs for people with arts” has developed a model of intergenerational
communication disabilities. Let’s consider two illus- community-based art programming that supports
trative examples here. people in local communities to empower people of
Sarah Bellum’s Bakery and Workshop in Port- any age and any ability level to engage in art-making
land, Oregon, is a “social enterprise bakery that and related entrepreneurial activities. Their Creative
supports adults with brain injury to return to . . . Abundance model (Dlouhy & Mitchell, 2015) can be
work, play, and life” (https://www.sarahbellums​ credited with helping to transform the sequestered
bakery.org). Founded by Rik Lemoncello, Professor and isolating “sheltered workshop” approach to
at Pacific University, the bakery is an incubator of meaningful, strengths-based employment for peo-
holistic social and communicative rehabilitation and ple with disabilities. The founders lead workshops
advocacy for brain injury survivors (Figure 25–2). worldwide to help build community-based sustain-
able arts-based programming and employment for
people in the arts, regardless of ability.

Aphasia Mentoring Programs

An aphasia mentoring program (Purves et al., 2013)


is a program in which people with aphasia serve as
mentors to students in clinical education programs
in the health sciences. The people with aphasia share
their knowledge about what it is like to live with
aphasia and what they have learned as consumers
of clinical services related to neurological disorders.
Students benefit from the mentors’ personal sharing
and the humanization of clinical conditions. Men-
tors benefit through enhanced social engagement
and empowerment through making important
contributions.

Toastmaster Programs

Toastmasters International is an organization with


clubs worldwide that provide means of develop-
ing communication and leadership skills for adults.
Some clinics, aphasia centers, and universities offer
Figure 25–2. The traumatic brain injury survivor bak- special Toastmasters programs (or affiliated “Gavel
ery team in action at Sarah Bellum’s Bakery, having Clubs”) for people with aphasia and related disor-
fun mixing batters and lining cupcake pans. A full-color ders. People with language disorders prepare, prac-
version of this figure can be found in the Color Insert. tice, and deliver speeches to one another. The intent
376  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

is to provide real-world communication practice in reaction to embarrassment; it is ideal for people to


a supportive environment, while enhancing social feel sufficiently unguarded such that they may prac-
support and networking. tice and make mistakes without fear of judgment or
being demeaned.
Assessing any given individual’s sense of
Humor as Therapy humor and the sorts of content that they find funny
is important. Simmons-Mackie suggests overtly
Simmons-Mackie and colleagues have studied how explaining the importance and relevance of humor
humor can be incorporated during language treatment to the individual being served. Materials might
and can even be used as a method of treatment (Potter involve cartoons, websites, and movie and TV show
& Goodman, 1983; Sherratt & Simmons-​Mackie, 2016; clips. Activities might include joke sharing, journal-
Simmons-Mackie, 2004; Simmons-Mackie & Schultz, ing about funny stories, and making funny gestures
2003). Benefits of humor include relief of embarrass- or facial expressions. Benefits of humor use might be
ment and tension, alleviation of sadness or depres- indexed through conversational analyses, including
sion, enhanced motivation, confirmation of a sense measures pertaining to such constructs as informa-
of togetherness and shared experience, enjoyment, tional content, conversational initiation, turn taking,
and a deepened sense of connection among partici- eye contact, and facial expressions.
pants in a conversation. There may be neuropsycho- Another means of incorporating humor (and fake
logical benefits as well: laughing that might evolve into real laughter) into
therapy, or as an adjunct to therapy, is through laugh-
• Cognitive-linguistic associations evoked ter yoga. We discuss this in Chapter 28 in the context
or enhanced through humor may improve of complementary and integrative approaches.
learning and memory.
• Humor may lead to increased right
hemisphere activation, enhancing bilateral Online Games
cortical processing during language use.
• Funny material may focus attention on The expansion of online programming for people
conversational or treatment content (see with acquired neurogenic challenges, especially
Simmons-Mackie, 2004). heighted during the COVID-19 pandemic, has
yielded wonderful new opportunities for online play
Importantly, humor may be effective regard- that may foster improvements at the impairment
less of the severity of a person’s cognitive-linguistic level while promoting life participation. Having led
impairments. numerous online game sessions for mixed groups
Funny things often happen when we work with of people with varied levels of aphasia and primary
adults who have acquired neurogenic language dis- progressive aphasia through Virtual Connections for
orders. As noted in Chapter 1, sharing humor is one Aphasia (https://www.aphasia.com/virtual-con-
of the wonderful aspects of getting to work with this nections-info), I am an enthusiast for harnessing
diverse population. The positive feelings that might the power of such opportunities to engage people
be shared through occurrences of spontaneous socially, build friendships, and encourage people
humor can be harnessed by the excellent clinician as with communication challenges to empower one
a means of building rapport. Spontaneous moments another in taking risks and initiative. Consider an
of laughter often occur when a person with a lan- example in the context of a simple game of “Would
guage disorder makes an error or struggles with a You Rather” played using Zoom (Figure 25–3). In
task. Making good, immediate judgments about how this game, players make forced choices given two
to respond in such situations is an important part of alternatives. Here are some sample questions:
the art of clinical practice. Using humor to cover up
a clinician’s lack of preparation is not appropriate. • Would you rather have a pet ferret or a pet goat?
It is not helpful if a person with a communication • Would you rather marry a rich person you’re
disorder regularly resorts to superficial humor as a not in love with, or a poor person you adore?
25. General Approaches for Enhancing Cognitive-Linguistic Abilities   377

Figure 25–3. Members of Virtual Connections for Aphasia enjoying a round of responses to “Would you rather
. . . ” questions. A full-color version of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.

• Would you rather work on communicating • use the chat function or send private e-mail
more, even if it’s not all correct, or work to suggest their own “Would you rather . . . ”
on speaking more correctly, even you questions.
communicate less?
Aphasia Games for Health is “a collaborative
Players use the polling function in Zoom to effort by the Aphasia Recovery Connection, the Uni-
indicate their own preferences and then we discuss versity of Pittsburgh, Carnegie Mellon University,
who chose what and why, and why a majority might and Thorny Games to fight social isolation and lan-
have chosen one response over another. Opportuni- guage loss [with the mission of] building a gaming
ties for meaningful engagement abound. For exam- community for people with aphasia by supporting
ple, they the development of games for recovery, providing
resources for designers, and making aphasia friendly
• express themselves by talking, gesturing, games accessible” (https://www.aphasiagames​
using AAC devices, and writing in the chat for​health.com). People with aphasia participate as
box, on paper, and whiteboards; “designer partners” in generating game ideas, test-
• show emotional responses through body ing and giving feedback about games in the design
language, often exaggerating their disgust or phases. They also describe and demonstrate existing
delight at the varied responses, and through a games that they recommend. Visit their website to
great deal of laughter; learn about design principles and guidelines, and to
• encourage those who have not spoken much view game prototypes the group has developed.
to say more, patiently cheer on those having EVA Park is virtual communication environ-
trouble saying what they wish to say, and ment for people with aphasia that is played on a
applaud those who work especially hard to computer. The participants are represented as ava-
convey their ideas; tars, which they design. Communication is through
• share their own personal stories that relate to real-time speech or typing. Simulated locations on a
the choices we’re discussing; and virtual island allow multiple players to interact and
378  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

to explore a variety of locations (e.g., a restaurant, titution of brain function, and the secondary goal of
hair salon, disco, bar) and experiences (e.g., explor- helping compensate for lasting deficits. An advan-
ing a planetarium, taking a ride on a turtle). Marshall tage of these approaches is that they lend themselves
et al. (2016) describe varied results for 20 people with well to the process analysis approach to assessment
aphasia with diverse treatment goals using the plat- (see Chapter 19). Process analysis helps to delineate
form. Marshall and colleagues also describe use of areas of deficit, which, in turn, helps us plan treat-
the environment for treating naming impairments ment programs to address those deficits.
(see Chapter 31). At the same time, as discussed in previous
chapters, one limitation of cognitive neuropsycho-
Other Socially Focused Programs logical models is that they tend to oversimplify the
overlapping and parallel nature of processing by
Group treatment and intensive treatment programs suggesting that much of information processing is
(discussed later in this chapter) often provide a accomplished in a serial fashion. Another limitation
mixture of cognitive-neuropsychological and social is that the component constructs suggested (aware-
approaches. Additional models of supporting peo- ness, storage, retrieval, recall, input and output buf-
ple with acquired neurogenic language disorders fers, etc.) do not generally capture the complexity
include community-based and online stroke clubs of actual neural structures and processes required to
and support groups, although these are typically achieve what is represented by those constructs. In
not means of direct intervention by the SLP. Addi- the context of treatment, this is a problem in that by
tional creative means of enhancing communication focusing on specific neuropsychological functions
through socialization include art and music pro- (e.g., attention, memory, and executive functioning)
grams (Beard, 2012; Brotons & Koger, 2000; Cowl & and tasks (e.g., comprehension of reversible passive
Gaugler, 2014; Horowitz, 2013; Kahn-Denis, 1997; sentences, subject-verb-object sentence construction,
Luckowski, 2014; Macauley, 2006; Mihailidis et al., naming, writing words to dictation, completing sen-
2010; Seifert, 2001; Stallings, 2010; Truscott, 2004), tences), we may oversimplify what is entailed in the
aphasia theater troupes (Côté & Lafance, 2018), types of cognitive-linguistic deficits we mean to treat
aphasia choirs (Polovoy, 2014), and companion ani- and in the types of tasks we administer to treat them.
mal and pet therapy programs (Gilbey & Tani, 2015; An associated challenge in treatment is a ten-
Macauley, 2006; Matuszek, 2010). dency to focus on decontextualized impairment-​
level problems without incorporating relevant
stimuli and tasks and without working toward real
life-affecting gains. Ylvisaker (1998) recommended
What General Cognitive Neuropsychological that we address this challenge in treatment by
Approaches Are Applicable to Treatment?
• contextualizing goals for any cognitive
Oince all language abilities can be said to be cognitive activity into real-life needs and desires of the
and neuropsychological, then almost all of our treat- person being treated;
ment approaches in clinical aphasiology could be said • using decontextualized exercises to reduce
to fit this category. However, we tend to associate the impairment only when there is a clear
terms cognitive, neuropsychological, neurolinguistic, and evidence base for doing so; and
the like with information-processing models (which • not first treating deficits in a decontextualized
may or may not entail assumptions about underlying way and waiting to address them in a
neural structures and processes). We generally con- contextual way after some level of mastery,
sider approaches to belong in this category if they are but rather contextualizing all of our stimuli
based on models of mental representation and types tasks, activities, and goals from the start of
and stages of information processing (see Chapter 4). intervention.
In treatment contexts, cognitive neuropsycho-
logical approaches tend to focus on underlying Politis (2014) discusses how he was influenced
impairments, with the primary goal of fostering res- in his role as a clinician by taking on this context-​
25. General Approaches for Enhancing Cognitive-Linguistic Abilities   379

sensitive approach to neuropsychological rehabilita- Sohlberg and Mateer (2001b) set the stage for a great
tion. For example, he writes that when working with deal of research and clinical program development
a person with “language formulation difficulties”: in aphasiology and neuropsychology. Those authors
suggested a framework for intervention with TBI
Instead of creating this goal, survivors that include the following:
• Patient will produce a grammatically and
semantically correct sentence using one given • problem orientation, awareness of problems,
word (noun or verb) with 80% accuracy in and goal setting (determining what the
each of two consecutive sessions. problem is and what to do about it)
• compensation (learning how to function
I could target this: despite impairments)
• Max will use a practiced script to • internalization (enhancing the automaticity of
communicate three key ideas to his strategy use)
grandparent via Skype. (p. 7) • generalization (applying what is learned in
multiple real-life contexts)
The merging of cognitive-neuropsychologi-
cal approaches with social and life participation Kennedy et al. (2008) provide a systemic review
approaches is key to clinical excellence. of treatment methods intended to improve executive
Several treatment approaches that fit within this functions in TBI survivors. Specifically, they exam-
general category are described in Section VIII. The ined outcomes associated with methods focused
continued building of strong evidence-based treat- on problem-solving, planning, organization, and
ment methods based on neuropsychological models multitasking. Overall, results are supportive in
will depend on detailed specification of the theoret- terms of documenting positive effects of treatment
ical rationale underlying each and clearly detailed on dysexecutive syndrome. At the same time, the
aspects of treatment stimuli and procedures (Cice- authors highlight important weaknesses in out-
rone et al., 2005). comes research related to such methods. Weaknesses
include the following:

• failure to include sufficient sampling of older


What Is Cognitive Rehabilitation? adults and combat veterans
• underrepresentation of research participants
Cognitive rehabilitation is a general term used to from minority and low-income groups, who
encompass intervention to facilitate cognitive-com- are especially at risk for TBI
municative recovery following brain injury. It may • lack of data pertaining to TBI survivors
include any training, teaching, coaching, modeling, who are in acute and subacute stages of
behavior modification programming, and counsel- rehabilitation
ing. It may address executive functioning, memory, • great variability across studies in terms of
attention, and social communication, and include clinical methods, dosage, outcomes indices,
any of the cognitive-linguistic and behavioral chal- and details of participant descriptions
lenges summarized in Chapters 12 and 13. It may
be restitutive or compensatory. It may be focused Sohlberg and Mateer’s (2010) Attention Process
on impairment-level deficits or on life participa- Training (APT) for TBI survivors, a program designed
tion. Given how broad the term is, it is not highly to enhance focused, sustained, selective, alternating,
useful in terms of enabling us to make statements and divided attention, has continued to be of inter-
about underlying theories, specific methods, means est in the clinical and research literature. Other train-
of indexing outcomes, or evidence of efficacy, effec- ing programs geared toward enhanced attention and
tiveness, or efficiency. working memory have been described in the literature,
A classic text on an “integrative neuropsycho- but few are described well enough for replicability,
logical” approach to cognitive rehabilitation by an essential aspect of research supporting treatment
380  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

outcomes (see O’Neil-Pirozzi et al., 2016; Sohlberg the aim of operationalizing those guidelines through
et al., 2003; Wiseman-Hakes et al., 2010). step-by-step procedures. It is a helpful resource and
Given the vital role of attention and working supports interprofessional approaches.
memory in all aspects of language processing, and Some approaches to language treatment in apha-
given that deficits in various aspects of attention sia entail a focus on cognitive aspects of language
exacerbate or even cause language challenges, this is processing such that they, too, might be considered
a fertile area for continued growth in evidence-based to be subsumed into the general category of cogni-
practice in aphasiology (Heuer & Hallowell, 2009, tive rehabilitation. For example, Helm-Estabrooks
2015; Heuer et al., 2017; Ivanova & Hallowell, 2011; et al. (2014) describe a method they call the Cog-
McNeil et al., 2011; Murray, 2004, 2012; Sung et al., nitive Approach to Improving Auditory Compre-
2009; Wright & Shisler, 2005). The use of external hension (CAIAC). It involves tasks of “attention”
memory aids with TBI survivors is often described as and “conceptual knowledge” said to help improve
important in texts on cognitive rehabilitation; how- everyday cognition and communication from the
ever, as noted for other targets of treatment in the single word to discourse level. Treatment involves
general domain of cognitive rehabilitation, research the following:
in this area is also lacking in terms of design specific-
ity, making it difficult to determine what aspects of • abstract design cancellation tasks requiring a
intervention led to outcomes reported across studies person to cross out target designs from arrays
(Sohlberg et al., 2007). Specific treatment approaches of targets and foils
related to people with memory impairment, includ- • a variety of pattern-copying (“graphomotor”)
ing not only TBI survivors but also people with tasks
dementia, are described in Chapter 26. • a symbols trails task, requiring drawing
Several authors have recommended variations of lines between symbols within a given
of behavioral management and self-regulation category
approaches as part of cognitive rehabilitation for • “odd-man-out” tasks that require a person
TBI survivors. Across published studies, results are to select designs and images that do not fit
positive for treatments geared toward among a set of others
• tasks requiring sorting of images by size and
• contingency management (systematic and weight
intentional manipulation of consequences for
desirable and undesirable behaviors); Complexity across the first four tasks is increased
• positive behavior interventions (methods by adding more designs and background distraction
focused on an individual’s internal control, and by manipulating nontarget (distractor) designs
leading to lifestyle change as a priority, with in terms of their similarity to targets. The influence
specific behaviors receiving less attention); of progress on such tasks on actual communication
and abilities has not been well addressed in the research
• cognitive supports (e.g., assistance and literature.
prompts through memory aids, devices, and Another example of cognitive rehabilitation
interactional participants) (Ylvisaker et al., applied to people with aphasia is Chapey’s (2008)
2007). “cognitive stimulation” approach, in which lan-
guage treatment is considered to benefit from foci
Ylvisaker et al. (2005) offer a summary of on cognitive “operations,” including memory, con-
such approaches, along with a summary of social vergent thinking, divergent thinking, and evalua-
approaches for improving cognitive abilities. Build- tion. She recommends carrying out problem-solving
ing on a series of systematic reviews on cognitive tasks requiring each of these operations in discourse
rehabilitation, members of the Brain Injury Inter- contexts that are relevant to the individual treated,
disciplinary Special Interest Group of the American thus increasing the likelihood of carryover to sponta-
Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine developed an neous use in conversation. “To display higher-level
interdisciplinary manual (Haskins et al., 2012) with cognitive behavior, [clients] must go beyond infor-
25. General Approaches for Enhancing Cognitive-Linguistic Abilities   381

mation given in some way — for example, relating • abstractness


it to something else, reorganizing it, inferring from • word stress
it, and using it as a springboard for creatively solv- • parts of speech
ing new problems. It involves applying, analyzing, • psychological and physical factors
synthesizing, and evaluating” (p. 487). As is the • response modalities
case with many approaches, the translation from • appropriateness of feedback
such strong and important principles underlying
a method to a method specific enough to be tested Note that any of the factors listed might be
empirically is a daunting challenge. manipulated to enhance or detract from linguistic
performance in people with aphasia and thus are
consistent with the type of potentially confounding
factors we considered in Chapter 19.
What Is the Stimulation-Facilitation Approach?

The stimulation-facilitation approach to language How May Group Treatment Be Implemented,


treatment (Schuell et al., 1964), or Schuell’s stim- and How Can It Help People With
ulation approach, is a set of strategies and princi-
Aphasia and Related Disorders?
ples developed by Schuell in the 1960s and 1970s.
Recall that Schuell’s framework for conceptualizing
aphasia as a unidimensional disorder was one of Group treatment in aphasia and related disorders
the frameworks for conceptualizing aphasia that we has become more and more popular as restrictions
reviewed in Chapter 4. Her principles for treatment on access to individualized treatment in health care
arose from her recognition of the interdependence contexts have led to the offering of alternative means
of all aspects of language, receptive and expressive, of providing intervention. By combining multiple
from phonology to pragmatics. Her recommenda- people in one session, the cost per person is less, so
tions for treatment stem from appreciation of the group treatment can be a more affordable option
functional interconnectivity among brain structures for many, especially if they do not have insurance
involved in language. coverage for individual treatment. Cost is certainly
Coelho et al. (2008) aptly describe the stimula- not the only or even primary benefit of treatment
tion approach as a method of “strong, controlled and in groups. Bonds among group members can help
intensive auditory stimulation of the impaired audi- them to cope with multiple challenges, especially
tory symbol system” (p. 439) in people with aphasia. social withdrawal and isolation, which are serious
The focus on auditory stimulation is in recognition of consequences of acquired neurogenic language dis-
the fact that all people with aphasia tend to have at orders. Also, supported communicative interactions
least some difficulty with auditory comprehension. in authentic social contexts ideally promote general-
This is a general restitutive approach that includes ization of conversational strategies.
recommendations on best practices for treatment There are several types of treatment groups.
and includes conditions that we should consider They may be designed to facilitate recovery, coping
controlling for optimal auditory stimulation: strategies, information sharing, or social support;
they are often used to address any combination of
• linguistic structure these at once. Sometimes care partners are included
• articulatory clarity in groups for education and support. Sometimes
• discriminability among response choices they are excluded so that group members may focus
• multisensory stimulation on connecting with one another.
• repetition Groups may comprise people with similar
• rate and pause types of communication disorders and similar lev-
• prompts and cues els of severity, or they may include more eclectic
• attention to meaningfulness and frequency combinations of people. Several researchers have
of words documented the efficacy of varied types of group
382  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

treatment approaches for people with aphasia and • modifying recommendations about forms of
related disorders (Allen et al., 2012; Bollinger et al., AAC use as appropriate
1993; Booth & Swabey, 1999; Cherney et al., 2011;
Clausen & Beeson, 2003; Elman, 2007a, 2007b; Elman In recent years, researchers have reported that
& Bernstein-Ellis, 1999; Falconer & Antonucci, 2012; using visual scene displays, rather than separate
Kearns & Elman, 2008; Marshall et al., 2012; Sim- words or icons in printed or computerized AAC
mons-Mackie & Elman, 2011; van der Gaag et al., media, may help to enhance interaction in people
2005; Wertz et al., 1981). Group treatment may be with aphasia and related disorders. Visual scene dis-
impairment focused at times, incorporating specific plays are images of scenes that are “contextually rich
evidence-informed neuropsychologically based pictures that depict situations, places, or experiences
treatment approaches. Still, groups are naturally that clearly represent relationships and interactions
social in nature. Suggestions for activities that could with important people or objects” (King, 2013b,
be used to enhance opportunities to complement p. 87). They may be computer projected or printed
group goals are listed in Box 25–2. and combined with relevant text and have been
shown to enhance active supported communication
between people with aphasia and their conversation
How May AAC, Apps, and Software partners (Beukelman, Fager, et al., 2007; Beukelman
Be Used to Support Communication et al., 2015; Hux et al., 2010).
Several vendors (e.g., Lingraphica, PRC, and
and Aid in Treatment?
Tobii Dynavox) offer AAC devices to support peo-
ple with aphasia, many of which are especially help-
Alternative and Augmentative Communications ful for people with limited speech output. Apps for
use on smartphones and touchscreen tablets or iPad
As discussed, people who cannot communicate suf- computers as well as computer software programs
ficiently through spoken language often benefit from are ever increasing in number and scope. Some
alternative means of communicating (Beeson et al., enable extensive practice outside of treatment, tai-
2003; Ho et al., 2005; Marshall et al., 2012; Nicholas et lored to individual needs. This extends the amount
al., 2011). Many researchers have demonstrated the of practice in which a person can engage outside of
benefits of specific types and combinations of AAC treatment sessions with a clinician. Apps may also
strategies. AAC is broadly defined and includes help consolidate and generate treatment stimuli to
high-tech, low-tech, and no-tech means of communi- be used in treatment, saving valuable preparation
cating. The Participation Model of AAC (Beukelman, time and helping to ensure that stimulus images are
Garrett, & Yorkston, 2007; Beukelman & Mirenda, not outdated.
2013) provides a framework for proactively con- Constant Therapy (https://constanttherapy-
sidering and implementing means of enhancing health.com) is an FDA-approved app that enables
communication in people with complex and severe custom programming of exercises for cognitive-lin-
communication disorders. Interactive steps include guistic treatment tasks tailored to the individual.
the following: Artificial intelligence and data analytics enable adap-
tation of exercises in real time as a person engages in
• assessment of participation preferences and any of a large array of exercises.
needs Tactus Therapy Solutions (https://tactustherapy​
• assessment of barriers to communication .com) provides a wide range of apps that generate
access and barriers to communication activities related to a host of cognitive-linguistic abil-
opportunities ities (e.g., naming, asking and answering questions,
• trying out various AAC options reading comprehension, writing, visual attention,
• implementing promising forms of AAC and memory). Subscription-based apps and activi-
• continuously assessing and providing support ties within apps can be selected to target an individ-
for AAC use ual’s goals and track progress.
25. General Approaches for Enhancing Cognitive-Linguistic Abilities   383

Box
25–2 Group Treatment Activity Ideas

• Review and develop the group’s individual • Ask them to imagine they are stranded in a
and collective goals together. forest; have them name the top five things
• Give each group member a list of topics they would want to have.
discussed in the group during the year. Have • When a member has a grandchild, have
each member take one to review with the group members ask questions, discuss, and
group. Examples might be listing activities congratulate.
they can do over the summer, strategies for • Discuss what is involved in buying a house,
educating others about aphasia or related how to do it, and so on.
disorders, specific strategies to help when one • Write a letter to a group member who is gone,
cannot find a word for something one wants traveling, ill, or no longer in the group.
to say, ways other people can help improve • Discuss places where group members have
communication, and so on. This is a good family; talk about who has the most dispersed
review strategy and gives each member a family.
chance to lead a discussion. • Have a surprise retirement party for a group
• Have all members write down a goal they want member.
to accomplish, and then put it in an envelope • Play Scrabble.
and mail it to themselves in a month or so. • Play aphasia bingo, containing some
• Have a slide show of pictures from several information about particular members;
sessions set to music. members and other people present walk
• Take pictures of the group members. You around trying to find this information from
could have a discussion about who is the others at the meeting.
tallest, shortest, wearing the most colorful • Go out to dinner to various ethnic or theme
shirt/blouse, has the least hair, and so on. restaurants.
• Have a fire in a fireplace, burning cards • Have an aphasia grand rounds attended by
describing something negative each group SLPs and graduate students.
member wants to get rid of. • Create “advice to _________” sheets; have
• Play 20 questions with a given picture of members give advice to SLPs, RNs, doctors,
known topic (e.g., food). caregivers, as well as other people with
• Write a get-well card for a member who is ill. aphasia visiting the group.
• Bring in pictures from family vacations and • Practice a sales presentation for the group.
discuss them. • Practice explaining what aphasia is and is not.
• Given a letter, have each person name as • Play a word game: come up with the word
many of something (e.g., foods, animals, that begins with the last letter of the previously
clothes, cars, places, things that start with a named word (e.g., “beg” – “gate” – “eat” – etc.).
certain letter, etc.) as they can. • Discuss planning and execution of different
• Discuss how as a group they support each activities (making an omelet, planning a
other. vacation).
• Ask each other questions, varying types of • Decide what one should bring to a party.
information requested. • Make a list of the least favorite things to bring
• Discuss memories associated with holidays as to a party.
those holidays arise. • Have midterm evaluations of personal goals.
• Review major parts of speech; use Mad Libs– • Take a personality questionnaire and talk
type games to elicit parts of speech within about the results.
contrived stories about group members. • Play hangman.
384  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

• Discuss “What if” situations (e.g., What if you • Tell chain stories (stories in which one
could meet any person from history? Who person begins with a sentence or two and
would you want to meet?) then the next continues, and so on, around
• Play commercially available games that the room).
encourage social interaction through multiple • Make bookmarks from old greeting cards.
modalities, such as Zobmondo!!, Moods, • Have brief guest lectures on supportive topics
Pictionary, and Hilarium. (e.g., coping with depression, facilitating
• Make clay sculptures and discuss the communication, enhancing motivation)
creations that emerge. and leave plenty of time for discussion and
• Paint a large canvas together and discuss the questions.
painting that emerges. • Have a workshop on computer use.
• Go bowling. • Have a workshop on using social media, such
• Have a picnic. as Facebook and Instagram, to connect with
• Have a potluck meal. other people who have aphasia.
• Watch a film and discuss it. • Have a book club series using supported
• Invite a local performing artist to entertain the communication for reading and discussing
group in an interactive way. books.

For Constant Therapy and Tactus Therapy apps, predict likely choices, suggest spelling and gram-
features such as the difficulty level and the number matical correction, and provide auditory cues when
of items to be included in practice sessions can be suggestions are available. Coglink (http://www.per-
manipulated by the SLP. The user gets feedback on sonaltechnologies.com/coglink) is a simple program
each activity and session. Scores and reports can be that facilitates e-mail use that may be personalized
generated for the clinician. The clinician can monitor for an individual’s needs. Speech-to-text technology
the user’s performance and adjust practice assign- captures voice input and provides written output.
ments by logging in remotely from a separate device Programs vary in terms of accuracy, adaptation to
or while working in person with the user. varied languages, accents and dialects, and speaker
Several types of software packages are available diarization (adaptation to aspects of an individual
to support speaking, writing, reading, and listening. speaker’s unique features).
Some programs and apps allow for preprogramming Text-to-speech technology facilitates reading
of things people are likely to want to say frequently comprehension by augmenting written content with
and also allow for preparing in advance to tell sto- simultaneous auditory input. Features may include
ries or jokes, provide explanations, and advocate for highlighting of words and sentences and adjustable
themselves. Examples are Video-Assisted Speech voice rates. Many AAC apps are designed for people
Technology (VAST), available through the Lingraph- with little or no speech but good language skills so
ica SmallTalk and TalkPath apps (https://www​ are not appropriate for people with significant lan-
.aphasia.com) and SentenceShaper (https://sentence​ guage problems. AAC apps that are symbol based
shaper.com). AphasiaScripts (Cherney, Halper, et al., and allow for generating spoken messages include
2008) is a specialized software program for practice the following:
and use in supported conversation, allowing person-
alization (https://www.sralab.org/aphasiascripts). • Proloquo2Go (https://www.assistiveware
Word prediction to aid typing and texting, now .com/products/proloquo2go)
standard on many smartphone apps and word-pro- • TalkTablet Pro (https://talktablet.com/)
cessing programs, prompts users with likely words • Lingraphica SmallTalk (http://www.aphasia​
given words previously typed. Software varies in .com/)
terms of such features as the ability to predict mul- • TalkRocket Go (https://myvoiceaac.com/)
tiple words, learn from individuals’ usage to best • TouchChat (https://touchchatapp.com/)
25. General Approaches for Enhancing Cognitive-Linguistic Abilities   385

Many people with neurogenic cognitive-lin- listservs, and individualized testing of tools that are
guistic challenges find video captioning to be helpful easily found online.
for supporting understanding of movies, television, Commercial vendor websites abound. When
and videos. It is now a standard option for many considering available technology, it may be helpful
media platforms. Quality across platforms varies in to refer to third-party online resources to reduce the
terms of accuracy and the ability to convey content influence of commercial claims made by companies
about nonverbal aspects of what is being shown. selling the technology described. Aphasia Software
Many apps to support reading, writing, speak- Finder (ASF) (https://www.aphasiasoftwarefinder​
ing, and auditory comprehension are available for .org) offers a large array of references about software
free or low cost, and some are increasingly available and apps that may benefit people with cognitive-​
as components of existing operating systems, media linguistic disorders. ASF was established through a
software, and AAC devices. Of course, availability not-for-profit entity (Tavistock Trust for Aphasia). Its
in target languages is a major consideration, with a content is presented in a manner designed to facili-
heavy bias toward development in English-language tate navigation and comprehension by people with
programs. General programs and apps that are not aphasia, including video demonstrations about how
specifically designed for clinical groups may be help- to use almost each of its pages, simplified language,
ful for people with memory challenges. Examples clear print with important words highlighted, and
are calendars, talking photo albums, grocery lists, substantial white space. ASF provides searchable
alarms, and text reminders. An advantage of tech- databases by app name or by desired features.
nology over paper-and-pencil means is the active Additional helpful online resources for learning
prompting of participants to engage in whatever is about apps and other potentially helpful technology
being stimulated by the app (Jamieson et al., 2017). include AbleNet (http://www.ablenetinc.com) and
Word-processing programs (spell-checking, AbilityNet (https://abilitynet.org.uk/). In addition,
grammatical assistance, thesaurus) may also be NeuroAbilities, a program of the Global Initiative for
helpful for supporting written communication. In Inclusive Information and Communication Technol-
addition, other apps such as those to enhance mind- ogies (https://g3ict.org/) that promotes worldwide
fulness and self-empowerment and manage depres- sharing about assistive technology, has resource
sion and fatigue are frequently helpful. pages and frequent updates about new solutions on
A dynamic feature-matching process for apps the horizon. It may also be helpful to search online
entails identifying an individual’s current and poten- apps stores associated with the brand of any device
tial future communication needs, matching strengths being used. No matter what technology is used in
and needs to available technology with input from treatment, a tool and its use do not constitute treat-
the individual, and assessing appropriateness of the ment. Also, just using an app or practicing a skill is
match by actually trying it out in supported natural- not necessarily helpful. As with all treatment tools
istic environments (Gosnell et al., 2011). Since there and methods, attention to principles of evidence-​
are literally thousands of apps that may be benefi- informed practice is essential.
cial to adults with acquired cognitive-linguistic dis-
orders, and since there are more emerging all the
time, a more substantial listing here would quickly What Are Intensive and Residential Aphasia
become outdated. Consider, for example, the rapidly Programs, and How Can They Help People
expanding advances in brain-behavior interfaces
With Aphasia and Related Disorders?
(see Saha et al., 2021, for a summary, and check out
the National Center for Adaptive Neurotechnologies
[https://www.neurotechcenter.org/] for updates). A growing trend over the past three decades has been
Consider, too, the growing options for the use of the offering of intensive aphasia programs. This usu-
virtual reality in rehabilitation (see Alashram et al., ally involves registration for a set period (1 to 4 weeks,
2019). It is best that aphasiologists stay abreast of for example), often at a university or freestand-
new developments through the research literature, ing aphasia center. Participants join in group and
continuing education opportunities, professional individual treatment and provide mutual support.
386  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

In some programs, spouse/partner/caregiver sup- frame their results according to outcomes pertain-
port and education are also incorporated. Many pro- ing to cognitive-linguistic abilities, client-reported
vide training on use of smartphone and tablet apps effects, and caregiver (“surrogate”) assessments of
(Hoover & Carney, 2014). the client’s communicative abilities.
Although the nature of programs varies widely, Some residential programs provide lodging for
most have in common the goals of harnessing the participants, whereas some require that participants
power of intensive treatment and practice to advance from out of town secure lodging in nearby hotels.
neuroplasticity in recovery and social support Aphasia Recovery Connection (https://www.apha​
through information sharing and group activities sia​recoveryconnection.org/) offers aphasia cruises
(Rose et al., 2013). Most published literature about that focus on life participation for people with apha-
such programs is descriptive. Based on results of a sia and their friends or partners. Time at sea offers
survey of intensive programs in four countries, Rose intensive social and communication programming
et al. (2013) discuss commonalities and differences and care partner support. Adventures aboard cruise
among programs in terms of how they are staffed, ships and ports of call enable opportunities for fun
their philosophies and values, means of fund- and meaningful interactions for people of all of abili-
ing, admission criteria, types of activities in which ties (Figure 25–4). The costs of programs vary widely.
participants engage, degree of family/caregiver Given that quality may vary according to the type of
involvement, and the means by which outcomes program offered and the expertise of the clinicians
are assessed. Winans-Mitrik et al. (2014) provide an and staff members, it is important that consumers
excellent summary of important variables that influ- investigate their options carefully. Tactus Therapy
ence the outcomes of intensive programs. They also (https://tactustherapy.com) provides a listing of
detail evidence of positive treatment outcomes asso- intensive programs. Aphasia Access (https://www​
ciated with a residential aphasia program offered .aphasiaaccess.org/) has a listing of aphasia centers
through the VA Pittsburgh Healthcare System. They in the United States and Canada, many of which

Figure 25–4. People with aphasia and volunteers on an Aphasia Recovery Connections cruise take over the
karaoke stage on their ship, singing, dancing, and making sure that fellow cruisers know what aphasia is and is not.
25. General Approaches for Enhancing Cognitive-Linguistic Abilities   387

provide intensive programming. Several of the web- summaries of residential and intensive programs in
sites provided in Chapter 27 (see Table 27–1) include different geographic regions.

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter 13. Describe how Schuell’s stimulation-
that are new to you or that you have not yet facilitation approach might be described
mastered. within a general framework of best practices
2. Describe how a lack of treatment fidelity for treatment, as described in Chapter 23.
in studies of intervention is a challenge to 14. Describe the aspects of group treatment
evidence-based practice. that might have the greatest influence on
3. How do you think too much focus on treatment outcomes.
treatment fidelity might impede the best 15. What are the optimal treatment outcomes
approach to treatment for a given person measure for group treatment? Why?
with a neurogenic language disorder? 16. What do you think might be some of
4. Describe some social communication the logistical challenges in starting and
challenges reported by people with aphasia. maintaining group treatment programs?
How might you, as a clinician, help to 17. Which of the group activities listed in Box
address these? 25–2 most interest you? Why?
5. Many authors who write about and are 18. Add your own ideas to the list in Box 25–2.
proponents of LPAA are the same as those 19. Download some of the free apps and
who write about ICF-focused approaches. software programs mentioned in this
Why do you think this is the case? chapter, or others that you find on your
6. Describe how you might implement staff own. What are some features that you think
training on supported communication would be especially helpful?
within a skilled nursing or rehabilitation 20. What are advantages and challenges of using
facility. apps during actual treatment sessions? Of
7. Describe how you might include a partner having clients use them on their own outside
or significant other in the use of supported of treatment?
communication during direct treatment 21. How might you help people interested in an
sessions. intensive residential aphasia program choose
8. In what ways is AAC use relevant to the best program to fit their needs?
treatment of people with aphasia? 22. Many clinical aphasiologists are so busy
9. What do you think would be the benefits and fulfilling heavy caseloads and productivity
challenges of an aphasia mentoring program? demands, they do not have time to engage
10. How might a Toastmaster’s program bring in intervention that does not fit the mode
about benefits to a person with a neurogenic of one-on-one treatment sessions in clinical
language disorder that would be hard environments. If you did not have such
to achieve through individual treatment demands and could engage in any type of
sessions? creative programming to assist people with
11. Do you think humor is a viable component aphasia and related disorders, what types of
of treatment for neurogenic communication programs might you choose to initiate? What
disorders? Why or why not? would be the goals of such programs?
12. Is it possible to be a proponent of LPAA and
www
also of neuropsychological approaches? Why See the companion website for additional learning
or why not? and teaching materials.
CHAPTER
26
Facilitating Communication in People With
Primary Progressive Aphasia and Dementia

As reviewed in Chapter 13, speech-language pathol- 7. What is spaced retrieval training and how is it
ogists (SLPs) play a vital role in assessing and treat- implemented?
ing people with neurodegenerative conditions, and 8. What is the FOCUSED program and how is it
in counseling, coaching, and training caregivers, implemented?
while also advocating for the rights of people with 9. What are Montessori approaches to dementia
primary progressive aphasia (PPA) and dementia. In management?
this chapter, we consider further the multiple roles 10. What are additional forms of programming to
of the SLP in working with this population, and support people with PPA and dementia?
the importance of recognizing work in this area as 11. In what other ways may clinical aphasiologists
critical to the SLP’s scope of practice. We also dis- professionally support the communication
cuss service delivery and reimbursement challenges needs of people with PPA and dementia and
within health care systems. Then, we review some the people who care about them?
of the most well-known approaches for enhancing
communication and promoting life participation of
people with PPA and dementia, in addition to the What Are Special Service Delivery Challenges
general approaches discussed in Chapter 25.
for Serving People With PPA and Dementia?
After reading and reflecting on the content in
this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in
your own words, the following queries: In earlier chapters, we addressed how people who
have degenerative cognitive-linguistic disorders
1. What are special service delivery challenges for often encounter barriers to accessing treatment that
serving people with PPA and dementia? would likely benefit them. This is largely due to the
2. How is working with people who have PPA following:
and dementia recognized as a component of
the SLP’s scope of practice? • the acute care medical model on which most
3. What SLP services for people with dementia health care delivery systems are based (i.e.,
are reimbursable? people are treated and they get better or are
4. What types of direct treatment may help people cured and get on with their lives), which does
with PPA and dementia? not take into account the ongoing long-term
5. What are important approaches for caregiver needs of people with degenerative conditions
coaching, training, and support? • beliefs that people with incurable memory
6. What are memory books and memory wallets loss will not retain enough content from direct
and how are they implemented? speech and language intervention (which

389
390  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

typically requires learning) to warrant such • provide direct speech-language evidence-


intervention based intervention for some people with
• failure to recognize how strengths-based PPA and dementia, discussed further in
approaches to enhancing communication Sections VII (especially Chapter 26) and VIII
reduce care partner burden, and enhance • provide important training, coaching, and
health care management and quality of life counseling to the individual, care partners,
for the people with these conditions and the family members, and professional colleagues,
people who care about them so that effective communication strategies
and supports may be used to help enhance
Unfortunately, not all health care professionals memory, meaningful communication, and
fully appreciate that dementia management is within behavioral responsiveness
SLPs’ scope of practice. Many physicians do not refer • advocate for the provision of communication-​
people with neurodegenerative conditions or their related services to people with neurodegener-
caregivers to SLPs in the first place. This means that ative conditions and their families
the role of the SLP as an advocate for people with • participate in documentation and research
dementia by educating others of the impact that SLP to support the evidence base for enhanced
services can have is of paramount importance. diagnostic procedures, direct and indirect
treatment methods to address communication
challenges, and training and education of
How Is Working With People Who Have caregivers and health professionals
PPA and Dementia Recognized as a
It is important that aphasiologists engage as
Component of the SLP’s Scope of Practice?
early as possible in assessment and intervention.
Assessment processes must be continued so as to
SLPs are uniquely qualified to educate, coach, and track the progression of symptoms and anticipate
train people with PPA and dementia, their loved ones needed support and compensatory strategies. Coun-
and caregivers, and other health care professionals. seling and educating the people who are important
The focus of this work tends to be on lessening and to them is vital; most are not likely to have had much
compensating for communication challenges, and help or explanation regarding the condition until
fostering meaningful communication environments, they meet with an SLP.
especially through caregiver training, whether in
residential facilities, clinics, adult day care centers,
family homes, or community agencies. This role is What SLP Services for People With
consistent with the justification for SLPs providing
Dementia Are Reimbursable?
intervention for people with communication chal-
lenges related to cognitive disorders and in engaging
in related research. As we discuss in detail in Chapter 14, the way that
Given how central memory and other cognitive SLPs are paid for their services varies according
abilities are to language abilities, worsening commu- to government-regulated health care policies and
nication deficits are a fundamental component of all agencies in the country, region, or state in which
forms of PPA and dementia. There are five primary they work. There is wide variability in the types of
ways in which SLPs play a vital role in enhancing justification needed to ensure that SLP services are
quality of life through quality of communication for paid for. Many third-party payer representatives
people with PPA and dementia: who make decisions about patients’ eligibility for
services do not understand the importance of SLP
• assess communication strengths and intervention for people with dementia. Thus, it is
weaknesses and help the individual family important not only that SLPs educate medical pro-
members and rehabilitation and health care fessionals who may refer people with dementia for
colleagues understand them services but also the insurance companies and health
26. Facilitating Communication in People With Primary Progressive Aphasia and Dementia   391

plan representatives who will be making decisions of people even in late-stage dementia to learn and
about whether SLP services may be authorized for retain new information has great promise for boost-
reimbursement. ing the justification for direct SLP intervention. Res-
In the United States, since Medicare is the fed- titutive methods primarily include in-person and
eral program providing health benefits to people computer stimulation aimed at slowing the progres-
over 65 years and people with disabilities, Medicare sion of cognitive-linguistic deterioration. Compen-
is the most likely source of reimbursement for SLP satory methods include the development and use of
services for people with PPA and dementia. Thus, it memory aids and written and pictorial communica-
is important for U.S.-based SLPs to know about the tion supports.
ways that Medicare authorizes dementia services. Given the relative recency of the recognition
One way is through providing direct assessment and of PPA syndromes, the evidence-informed practice
treatment involving primarily the patient on their literature supporting PPA direct treatment meth-
own; the other is through a more indirect assess- ods is still in early stages. Still, an ever-increasing
ment and intervention approach called a functional number of studies suggest promising results (Brady
maintenance program, which includes assessment et al., 2016). Stimulation approaches for PPA have
and treatment. The assessment process involves not been especially geared toward improved naming in
only the person but significant others, including any people with semantic variant PPA (svPPA) through
frequent caregivers. The “treatment” is a training repetitive exposure to target words, ideally through
program aimed at getting communication partners auditory and orthographic stimulation in conjunc-
to understand and use specific communication strat- tion with naming tasks (Rising & Beeson, 2020).
egies that will enhance that individual’s communi- Jokel and colleagues report evidence that errorless
cation abilities. approaches are more effective than error-focused
Direct intervention for people with dementia feedback in people with svPPA (Jokel & Anderson,
entails an assessment process followed by treat- 2012). They clarify that incorporating episodic mem-
ment sessions, much as would be approved for peo- ory with words boosts gains in word finding in peo-
ple with other types of neurogenic communication ple with svPPA, while phonological treatment may
deficits, such as aphasia. To justify direct one-on- be more effective in those with nonfluent variant
one speech-language intervention for people with PPA (nfvPPA; Jokel et al., 2016).
dementia, we often must demonstrate that they have Henry et al. (2018) implemented Video Imple-
sufficiently intact attention and memory abilities to mented Script Training for Aphasia (VISTA)
demonstrate carryover of communication benefits (described in Chapter 30) in 10 people with nfvPPA.
from one session to the next, and from the clinical Participants achieved gains for trained scripts for up
treatment environment to real-world communica- to 1 year following treatment, and stability in per-
tion. In most clinical environments, there is a gen- formance with untrained scripts and standardized
eral belief that the communication benefits a person test scores. Grasso et al. (2019) present a case study
with dementia is most likely to maintain typically showing benefits of VISTA for a Spanish-English
relate to use of compensatory strategies, such as use bilingual speaker with aphasia.
of written and picture cues and other memory aids. Repeated transcranial magnetic stimulation and
transcranial direct current stimulation (see Chap-
ter 25) for people with PPA have not yet yielded
What Types of Direct Treatment May significant promising outcomes, and neither have
pharmacological interventions (Rising & Beeson,
Help People With PPA and Dementia?
2020). Errorless learning methods, as described for
Spaced Retrieval Training, later in this chapter, have
There is a growing evidence base supporting direct had mixed results. Multimodal cues and response
SLP treatment for people with PPA and dementia modes during personally relevant tasks may enhance
(Cadório et al., 2017; Chiou & Allison, 2020; Hopper responsiveness.
et al., 2013; Rising & Beeson, 2020; Schaffer & Henry, Of course, it is important to gauge whether treat-
2020; Swan et al., 2018). Evidence of the capacity ment leads to generalization in naturalistic contexts
392  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

and maintenance after treatment. A challenge with degenerative cognitive-linguistic conditions. Unfor-
evaluating treatment outcomes is that there will still tunately, they are often not intuitive. Without train-
be a degradation of cognitive-linguistic abilities; the ing, care partners and professionals may respond in
slowing of decline as well as momentary improve- ways that actually exacerbate communication chal-
ments in skills are important to evaluate. lenges, frustration, and social isolation rather than
The Academy of Neurologic Communication reduce them and promote person-first, empowering
Disorders and Sciences convened a writing group strategies.
(Wauters et al., 2021) to engage in a systematic In addition, there are many effective strategies
review of behavioral treatment of PPA and PPA of for preventing problematic behaviors by attending
speech (PPAOS). A majority of the 66 studies they proactively to a person’s unmet needs. Kitwood
included addressed naming treatment. Support wrote eloquently about this notion in his classic
for SLP intervention was considered “moderately book Dementia Reconsidered: The Person Comes First
strong.” The group recommends heightened rigor in (1997), and in his more recent Dementia Reconsid-
experimental design and more research, especially in ered, Revisited: The Person Still Comes First (2019). He
areas other than naming. emphasizes that person-centered approaches that
bring out the strengths of people with dementia are
not only the most humane, they are also the most
What Are Important Approaches for Caregiver effective. Unfortunately, especially in institutional-
ized settings, pharmacological intervention to sedate
Coaching, Training, and Support?
and otherwise regulate the “problematic” behavior
of people with dementia is used widely in much of
Many of the greatest challenges reported by formal the world.
and informal care partners of people with neuro- Power (2010, 2014) offers wonderful guidance
degenerative conditions entail challenges in com- on using nonpharmacological approaches to enhance
munication. Repeated questioning, perseverative well-being in people with dementia. He shows how
comments, and mutual expressions of confusion strategically addressing the domains of well-being
and anger over breakdowns in communication often (identity, connectedness, security, autonomy, mean-
lead to frustration and reduce the quality of relation- ing, growth, and joy; Thomas et al., 2005) helps us
ships. For people who care for a person with PPA to promote quality of life in proactive ways. This,
or dementia, the progressive loss of companionship in turn, reduces reliance on dehumanizing physical
and the ongoing change in the person’s identity lead restraints, institutional isolation, and drugs adminis-
to a sense of grief and lack of control. SLPs may sup- tered to control behavior. Hickey and Douglas (2017,
port caregivers and care partners through education, 2021) provide rich resources on person-​centered
training, and counseling. assessment and intervention for people with demen-
There is substantial evidence that people with tia with edifying perspectives for SLPs.
degenerative disorders who have difficulty engaging A multitude of care partner training programs
in meaningful conversations with untrained partners are available to support communication, slow the
tend to engage much more meaningfully with part- progression of cognitive-communicative decline,
ners trained to elicit and support quality interactions reduce care partner burden, and improve social inter-
(Byrne & Orange, 2005; Hopper, 2003; Orange et al., action for people with dementia and other forms of
1995; Ripich & Wykle, 1996; Santo Pietro & Otsuni, memory loss. An example of a care partner training
2003). Strategies to help care partners support com- program for which formal and informal care partners
munication are paramount to enhancing quality of may receive training and trainers may be certified to
communication, which we know enhances quality train others is the Savvy Caregiver Program (SCP).
of life. SCP is a packaged program focused on mediating
There are numerous strategies that help elicit care partner stress through improved interactions
the best of intact cognitive and linguistic skills with people who have dementia. Components in-
and reduce problematic behaviors in people with clude in-person workshops, Internet-based train-
26. Facilitating Communication in People With Primary Progressive Aphasia and Dementia   393

ing, a care partner manual, a DVD, and an online words; these can easily be edited and added to over
workbook. Training content includes background time. Photo albums may also be used. Some that
information about dementia, notions of control, goal allow voice recording and playback to accompany
setting, means of managing daily care and behavior, each page are available. Pocket-sized notebooks or
self-care, and decision-making strategies (Healthcare index cards connected through a metal ring, both of
Interactive, 2008; Hepburn et al., 2003). The methods which are easy to carry in a purse or pocket, may also
are said to be evidence based, although the research be used. Memory wallets are typically plastic wallet
on outcomes is primarily generated by the authors inserts containing emergency contact information,
and owners of the program. Benefits include positive words that are important to remember, schedul-
caregiver ratings of relevance, usefulness of strate- ing/calendar/appointment information, addresses,
gies learned, and increased confidence in handling names, phone numbers, and associated pictures.
their caregiving roles. Potential drawbacks are a Contents of memory books and wallets may also
lack of personalization according to the individ- be kept on tablet computers and smartphones if the
uals and situations at hand, a lack of suggested user is able to initiate using those media.
cultural and linguistic adaptations, a reliance on The rationale for the use of memory wallets and
computer and Internet access, and the possibility memory books is that people with dementia tend to
of overwhelming care partners with substantial retain long-term memory abilities far beyond the time
information. that they lose short-term memory abilities. Images
In addition to content on behavior and com- and words help to stimulate retrieval of memories
munication, it is important that caregiver training (McPherson et al., 2001). Thus, conversations that
include content on environmental modifications focus on recall of content from the distant past will
that can help heighten communication effectiveness. likely result in enhanced communicative interaction
Examples are limiting visual clutter and extraneous and social engagement. Also, repeated practice with
television and other background noise, ensuring personally relevant stimuli helps to enhance access
sufficient lighting, and providing visual supports to associated facts, words, and names. In addition
through pictures and artifacts (Bourgeois & Hickey, to providing supports of social interaction, memory
2009; Schaffer & Henry, 2020). books and wallets may also be used as reminders
to engage in certain activities (such as taking med-
icine, keeping appointments, and finding objects
What Are Memory Books and Memory [such as keys]).
Many studies have demonstrated positive out-
Wallets, and How Are They Implemented?
comes, according to a range of indices. In conversa-
tions in which memory notebooks have been used to
Memory books and wallets are collections of pic- support communication, people with dementia have
tures, phrases, and words associated with familiar been shown to
people, places, and events that a person may have
difficulty remembering (Bourgeois, 1992, 2019). They • increase the duration of engagement in
are generally used with people who have memory conversation,
loss, including people with language of generalized • produce more utterances within a conversation,
intellectual impairment and memory loss associ- • produce fewer perseverative utterances,
ated with TBI. They have been shown to be useful • produce fewer off-topic utterances and more
as memory aids, alternative and augmentative com- on-topic utterances,
munications (AAC) materials, direct treatment mate- • engage in a single topic for a greater number
rials, communication partner training strategies, and of conversational turns,
general supports to assist in meaningful conversa- • produce utterances of greater length and
tion (Burgio et al., 2001; Chiou & Allison, 2020). complexity,
Memory books are often in the form of three- • produce less ambiguous (more easily
ring binders containing photographs and printed interpretable) utterances,
394  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

• provide more appropriate answers to • I see you have two children, Jules and Yvette.
questions about related content, Tell me about how you used to celebrate their
• improve naming and accuracy of naming of birthdays when they were little.
people and object depicted and labeled, and • I see you had a corgi named Willie. He was so
• demonstrate independent use of the aid (Alm cute. I used to have a corgi, too. What did you
et al., 2004; Bourgeois, 1990, 1992, 1993, 2019; like best about having a dog?
Bourgeois & Mason, 1996; Burgio et al., 2001;
Gómez Taibo et al., 2014; Hoerster et al., 2001; Such conversations may also lead to further
Ingersoll-Dayton et al., 2013; Singh et al., meaningful positive interactions later. Consider, for
2014). example, how a nursing assistant might be more
likely to
Such benefits aid others who engage in communi-
cation while using them for conversational support; • play a recording of opera music in the
this, in turn, enhances the likelihood of continued resident’s room or ask that the activities
engagement in higher-quality conversations than director initiate an opera-related activity,
unsupported communication (Allen-Burge et al., • ask additional questions about Jules and
2001; Hoerster et al., 2001). Yvette during a subsequent visit, and
The communicative content of daily conver- • have a friend with a corgi stop by and let the
sations as nursing staff members, physicians, vol- resident pet it.
unteers, and other visitors engage with long-term
care and rehabilitation center residents tends to be Creating memory books or wallets entails devel-
focused on content that relies on recall for recent opment of stimulus pages that contain photographs
events. Consider the following typical questions and words, phrases, or sentences, organized accord-
from a nursing assistant: ing to general topics that pertain to that individual
(e.g., My daily schedule; My family; My career; Places
• When is the last time you used the restroom? I have visited; My hobbies). Biographical information
• Was your son here this morning? may be arranged from past to present or vice versa.
• Has anyone changed your sheets? Phrases and sentences are written from the perspec-
• What did you have for lunch? tive of the person (e.g., I was born on January 31,
1978; My favorite foods are artichokes, candied gin-
Quality of life may be enhanced by evoking ger, and spaghetti; My son’s name is Zhuoming).
more meaningful conversations, especially in an The ideal size and complexity of words and pictures
institutionalized setting where staff members may depend on the individual’s cognitive-linguistic abil-
not be familiar with an individual’s past and thus not ities as well as their visual acuity.
otherwise have sufficient content to support commu- Including the individual in the process of cre-
nication about personally relevant topics. According ating the materials is ideal. If a person in the early
to myriad measures of communicative competence, stages of dementia assists, it will help ensure per-
strategies that evoke reminiscence and telling of life sonal engagement in the development process as
stories are far more effective than are those that rely well as personal relevance. When possible, it is good
on recent declarative and semantic memory. to have family members or others who know the
Consider how the following might evoke more individual well tell about key past events, people,
meaningful and successful conversational inter- and accomplishments and provide personal pho-
actions as well as positive affect, when supported tographs and other memorabilia that would most
through pictures and text: likely trigger distant memories. When such peo-
ple are not available to participate or resources are
• So, I see you were an opera singer. That’s unavailable, photos may be obtained through other
amazing. Where did you perform? Do you sources, such as through online image searches and
still like to sing? What is your favorite opera? magazine clippings.
26. Facilitating Communication in People With Primary Progressive Aphasia and Dementia   395

Once the initial materials are developed, they material resources and personnel to support the
may be introduced by the SLP, a family member, or development, implementation, monitoring of use,
a trained volunteer. Uses depend on the goals for and revision of such aids on a regular basis, continu-
an individual. Activities may include the following: ously building a culture of enhanced communication
empowerment.
• looking at and reading one page at a time,
stopping to converse about the relevant
context What Is Spaced Retrieval Training,
• looking at a picture and using open-ended
and How Is It Implemented?
prompts, such as “tell me about . . . ,” or
“what was it like when you . . . ”
• elaborating on comments made by the person Spaced retrieval training (SRT) is “a method of learn-
• asking for additional details ing and retaining information by recalling the infor-
• reading words and phrases aloud or asking mation over increasingly longer periods of time”
the person to do so (Camp et al., 1996, p. 196). The goal is to enhance
• having the person show the book to someone accessibility to stored representations by repeatedly
else and explain content within it activating them and making a person aware of them
• suggesting looking at or discussing content in (Bayles & Tomoeda, 1997; Camp et al., 1996; Cherry
the book to distract the individual when they & Simmons-D’Gerolamo, 2005). It is said to target
are sad or engage in undesirable behavior implicit (unconscious, involuntary) memory, consid-
ered to be relatively robust in people with memory
Training staff and family members to use the disorders associated with TBI and various forms of
book in supported communication, with encourage- dementia. Hopper et al. (2005) suggest that SRT facil-
ment for positive aspects, enhances the likelihood itates cue-behavior associations (between verbal and
of meaningful conversations in future interactions. auditory cues and face or object name associations;
To increase the likelihood of their being used, it is Hopper et al., 2005). SRT is classified as an errorless
important to see that the materials are not stuck in a learning method, which means that the individual
drawer or covered by other items on a bedside table. is encouraged and praised for successes and not cor-
Using a bookstand or attaching the book or wallet to rected or given negative feedback when they do not
a wheelchair may be helpful. As the person’s cogni- perform a task correctly.
tive-linguistic status changes, reevaluate treatment There are two basic forms of SRT:
goals. As additional input is provided by the per-
son, staff members, and significant others, update • fixed-interval/uniform approach, in which
the memory aids. the time between trials remains constant, said
Some SLPs develop memory books and wallets to help transfer information into long-term
as a component of direct treatment. Some do so as storage
part of a functional maintenance program (a brief • randomized-interval/adjusted approach,
period of evaluation, development of an interven- in which the time between trials is adjusted
tion plan, and care partner training, as discussed in according to the individual’s performance,
Chapter 13). Unfortunately, many SLPs do not have said to enhance long-term retention (Morrow
sufficient time allocated to create memory books and & Fridriksson, 2006)
wallets for all of the people within a given facility
who might benefit from them, let alone to train each The latter form is featured in most of the rele-
staff member and family member in how to use them vant published literature. The delay between subse-
to address individual goals. Thus, the role of the SLP quent trials is increased when the person responds
is often one of trainer and coach of others, such as correctly, first in intervals of 10 seconds until correct
family members, activities directors, and volunteer responses are given with a 1-minute delay, and then
coordinators. The excellent clinician advocates for in increments of 30 seconds and 1 minute. When the
396  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

person does not respond or responds incorrectly, to other care partners. Benigas, Brush, and Elliott
the clinician or care partner restates the informa- (2016) provide more in-depth step-by-step instruc-
tion and asks the person to repeat it. The delay is tions for clinicians.
then decreased to the interval of the prior correct SRT treatment outcomes that have been docu-
response. mented include the following:
Consider the sample script for an SRT session
in Box 26–1. In the example, remembering the doc- • improved ability to remember simple
tor’s name is the focus. Use of memory aids, such associations following an initial presentation,
as a memory wallet or calendar, could also be the within minutes and days
focus, as could environmental cues and directions, • improved face-name associations
such as where the cafeteria is located and how to get • improved naming of objects
to the activities room. The technique can be taught • improved use of memory aids

Box
26–1 Sample Script for an SRT Session Using an Adjusted Interval Approach

Clinician: Let’s work on remembering your Clinician: Good. That’s right. It’s Dr. Gutmann.
doctor’s name. Her name is Dr. Gutmann. What You’re doing well with remembering.
is her name?
(40, then 50, then 60 seconds later)
Client: I’m not sure. Who?
Clinician: We’re working on remembering your
Clinician: We’re talking about your doctor’s doctor’s name. Your doctor is Dr. Gutmann.
name. Her name is Dr. Gutmann. What’s your doctor’s name?
Gutmann. What is her name? Client: It’s Dr. Gutmann. I’m pretty sure.
Client: Dr. Gutmann. Clinician: Good. You’re sure and you’re right!
Clinician: Good. You know her name. She’s Dr. (2 minutes of intervening activity or
Gutmann. conversation)
(interval of 10 seconds) Clinician: We’ve been talking about your doctor
and remembering her name. What’s her name?
Clinician: Let’s see if you can remember now.
What’s your doctor’s name? Client: Dr. Gutmann.
(20, then 30, and 40 seconds later . . . ) Clinician: You’ve got it! It’s Dr. Gutmann.
Client: Dr. Gutmann. (Then proceed to 4, 6, 12 minutes, etc.)
Clinician: Right. Dr. Gutmann. Let’s keep
The client and clinician may engage in other
practicing this. (10 seconds later) What’s your
treatment activities and conversation during the
doctor’s name?
intervals.
Client: Dr. Cohen.
At the start of the next session, the clinician
Clinician: Your doctor’s name is Dr. says, “Last time we worked on remembering
Gutmann. (30 seconds later) We’re working on the name of your doctor. What’s your doctor’s
remembering who your doctor is. What’s your name?”
doctor’s name?
The same pattern is repeated until the response
Client: Dr. Gutmann. is correct at the start of the next session or two.
26. Facilitating Communication in People With Primary Progressive Aphasia and Dementia   397

Joltin et al. (2003) demonstrated that treatment of FOCUSED has gradually decreased the empha-
effects could also be obtained over the telephone. In sis on FOCUSED in care partner training programs.
a systematic review of SRT as carried out in 12 qual- However, it is summarized briefly here given the
ifying studies, Oren et al. (2014) concluded that SRT import it has had within the literature in this area.
yields positive results in terms of learning of new FOCUSED is an acronym representing each of the
information. Benigas and Bourgeois (2016) demon- strategies listed and described in Box 26–2.
strate that supplementing content to be remembered Recommended training for professionals and
with visual stimuli may boost recall and perfor- volunteers using the approach involves six 2-hour
mance on important everyday tasks. modules. Research support is primarily in the form
Although SRT is typically thought to be an of case studies and small-group studies and does
approach to treat memory impairment per se, Frid- not tend to include quality-of-life indices or address
riksson et al. (2005) demonstrated that it could help long-term maintenance of effects (see Ripich et al.,
people with aphasia with word finding and found 2000, for an exception). Still, the simple approach to
that it was more efficient than a cueing hierarchy training, with hands-on practice with each strategy,
approach to naming. Of course, specific outcomes may lead to greater satisfaction of trained personnel
are variable according to the intervals implemented, in communicating meaningfully with people who
environmental factors, etiology of memory disor- have PPA and dementia (Ripich et al., 1995, 1998;
der, individual client and clinician factors, and test- Small et al., 2003). In addition to hands-on practice,
ing intervals (e.g., within weeks, months, or even an important component of training is care partner
years). Challenges with SRT include that a person support in recognition of the added burden of com-
may remember the specific piece of information munication on the part of care partners (Orange &
practiced but not the context for it (e.g., remember- Colton-Hudson, 1998).
ing a doctor’s name, as in Box 26–1, but not knowing
what type of doctor she is and where or why the
client might see her). What Are Montessori Approaches
to Dementia Management?

What Is the FOCUSED Program,


The Montessori approach, initially developed for
and How Is It Implemented?
use with children in educational contexts, is intended
to enhance activation of intact intellectual and com-
FOCUSED is a set of strategies for enhancing com- municative activities and improved compensatory
munication with people who have dementia. It is strategies through use of the following:
not based on a single set of theoretical principles;
it represents the original authors’ (Ripich & Wykle, • emphasis on intact abilities
1996; Ripich et al., 1995) interpretation and testing • AAC and other means of supported
of best practices in this general area. General strat- communication
egies are recommended for use in real-life contex- • multimodal stimulation
tualized communication and are intended to be the • environmental accommodations taking into
basis for training of care partners, family members, account participants’ cognitive, linguistic,
health care professionals, and volunteers (Ripich & motoric, and perceptual abilities
Horner, 2004). The goals of FOCUSED are to pro- • ecologically valid and personally relevant,
mote the best quality of interactions with people concrete stimuli
who have dementia and thus enhance quality of life • supported and contextualized cueing
for the person with dementia as well as all involved • positive feedback and opportunities for success
in such interactions. • repetition
A great deal of work on supported and total • minimal reliance on episodic and working
communication (see Chapter 25) since the inception memory (Camp, 2001, 2010; Hitzig &
398  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Box
26–2 FOCUSED Communication Strategies

F = Face to face. Face the individual directly; attract the individual’s


attention; maintain eye contact.
O = Orientation. Orient the individual by repeating key words several
times; repeat sentences exactly; give the individual time to comprehend what
you say.
C = Continuity. Continue the same topic of conversation for as long as
possible; prepare the individual if a new topic must be introduced.
U = Unsticking. Help the individual become “unstuck” when they use a
word incorrectly by suggesting the word they are looking for; repeat the
individual’s sentence using the correct word; ask, “Do you mean . . . ?”
S = Structure. Structure the questions to give the individual a simple choice
to respond with; provide only two options at a time; provide options that the
individual would like.
E = Exchange. Keep up the normal exchange of ideas we find in
conversation; begin conversations with pleasant, normal topics; ask easy
questions that the individual can answer; give the individual clues as to how
to answer.
D = Direct. Keep sentences short, simple, and direct; use specific, concrete
nouns, rather than pronouns; use hand signals, pictures, and facial
expressions.

Source: Adapted from Ripich et al., 1995, p. 16.

Sheppard, 2017; Mahendra et al., 2006; van The Montessori approach is said to comple-
der Ploeg et al., 2013; Vance & Johns, 2003) ment intervention goals of improved independence,
self-esteem, positive affect, and participation in
The first-in/last-out model of cognitive loss, meaningful social roles and activities. Given that the
the theory that the functional abilities learned ear- approach is highly adaptable, there are few parame-
liest in life are those most likely to be preserved in ters that would help ensure treatment fidelity within
people with dementia, is an important crux. The the context of carefully controlled research on the
highest-level tasks in which a person is still able to approach. Activities may range, for example, from
engage are the ones that should be implemented; art projects using varied media to seriation tasks
as functional abilities decrease, easier tasks that involving arrangement of colored tiles from light to
lead to a sense of success should be implemented. dark hues (Vance & Johns, 2003), to hair brushing
Multi­ modal sensory exploration is encouraged. or other self-grooming activities, to playing of card
Facilitators of the approach are encouraged to adapt games to practice memory skills.
instructions according to the comprehension abilities The approach may be implemented by SLPs,
of participants. Training of partners to be involved activities directors, nursing staff members, and
is a vital component of the approach (Schneider & other health care providers as well as trained vol-
Camp, 2003). unteers. Montessori-based activities to facilitate
26. Facilitating Communication in People With Primary Progressive Aphasia and Dementia   399

socialization and enhance communication in people this approach is that long-term care residents with
with dementia have been tested in long-term care PPA and dementia are at risk for social isolation and
contexts (Orsulic-Jeras et al., 2000, 2001), adult day- have reduced opportunities for social interaction.
care centers (Vance & Johns, 2003), intergenerational The basis for the approach is providing an ongo-
programs (Camp et al., 1997), and individual and ing breakfast club that encourages social participa-
group settings. Montessori-based programming for tion during a multisensory activity, encourages use
people with dementia is used in many countries, and of each participant’s strength in communication,
training materials have been developed in multiple and includes adaptations to individual preference,
languages (Camp, 2010). See Hitzig and Sheppard needs, strengths, and weaknesses. Possible activi-
(2017) for a review of outcomes for intergenerational, ties are encapsulated in the form of sequential steps
resident-assisted, volunteer- or family-​ directed, summarized in Box 26–3. Of course, progression
group, and individual programming. through each step need not be completed in a linear
fashion, as the approach is adaptable to individuals
and context.
What Are Additional Forms of Programming For additional information about intervention
related to memory problems and language of general-
to Support People With PPA and Dementia?
ized intellectual impairment, see the evidence-based
practice guidelines for dementia offered by the
Group treatment for people with dementia has the Academy of Neurologic Communication Disorders
advantage of providing opportunities for naturalis- and Sciences (ANCDS; http://www.ancds.org/).
tic social interactions (see Swan et al., 2018). Also, search SpeechBite (https://speechbite.com) for
An example of a social group approach used systematic reviews and research studies pertaining
in a nursing home context is The Breakfast Club, to several related topics as addressed from multiple
first described by Boczko (1994). The motivation for disciplinary perspectives.

Box
26–3 Breakfast Club Activities

1. Greetings and choice of nametags (varying experience by having members smell the
field of choices to promote success) coffee and talk about its temperature.
2. Introduction of a topic: juice. Reading 4. Discuss foods available and decisions on
labels and each person’s selection of juice what each person wants.
choice 5. Discuss how the foods are prepared
• Semantic cues: Which is made from a (ingredients and procedures for making
red fruit? This one is from a fruit we them).
squeeze . . . 6. Pass silverware, plates, and napkins, and
• Forced-choice: Would you like prune serve breakfast.
juice or apple juice? Do you prefer orange 7. Serve coffee. Facilitate discussion about
juice or apricot juice? how much cream, milk, and/or sugar
• Cloze sentence/phrase or carrier to add.
phrase: I would love a fresh-squeezed 8. Discussion while eating and cleaning up.
glass of . . . 9. Continue discussion based on themes
3. Introduction of a new topic: coffee. Use that arose during breakfast and clean-up
cues as above and also promote sensory activities.

Source: Adapted from Boczko, 1994; Santo Pietro & Boczko, 1998.
400  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

• promoting programming that integrates


In What Other Ways May Clinical interaction with animals (Aarskog et al.,
Aphasiologists Professionally Support 2019; Gilbey & Tani, 2015; Matuszek, 2010;
the Communication Needs of People Peluso et al., 2018), music (Brotons & Koger,
With PPA and Dementia and the 2000; Gerdner & Schoenfelder, 2010; Gómez-
People Who Care About Them? Romero et al., 2017; Horowitz, 2013; Lee et al.,
2019; Music and Memory, 2015; Peluso et al.,
In contexts where SLPs are salaried without being 2018; Schlaug et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2017),
tied to billable schemes for their services and in cases art (Beard, 2012; Brotons & Koger, 2000;
where SLPs are willing to provide volunteer services Cowl & Gaugler, 2014; Hannemann, 2006;
above and beyond their normal workday activities, Kinney & Rentz, 2005; Lee et al., 2019;
there are ample additional ways they may make Mihailidis et al., 2010; Rentz, 2002; Rusted
a difference in the lives of people with dementia. et al., 2006; Seifert, 2001; Wang & Li, 2016),
Examples are the following: and other creative activities, such as poetry
writing, acting, and photography (Truscott,
• visiting local dementia caregiver support 2004; Zientz et al., 2007) to stimulate
groups to provide caregiver training on expression and socialization, and to enhance
effective communication strategies for reducing inclusiveness.
caregiver burden and enhancing quality of life
• providing in-services on empowering Many of the general approaches for improving
approaches to communication to staff communication in older people and for addressing
members in residential, adult day care, and language of generalized intellectual impairment (see
hospice programs Section VII) can be adapted for effective volunteer
• training volunteers to provide reminiscence- and caregiver training. SLPs working in university
based programs to people with dementia contexts may offer such community-based volun-
in their local communities (e.g., through teer services by training student volunteers to carry
the development of life review diaries and out communication-focused dementia management
videos, memory books, memory wallets, and services, which may also be combined with respite
reminiscence-based activities) programming for caregivers.

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter 5. Why is it important that formal and informal
that are new to you or that you have not yet caregivers be trained in strategies to facilitate
mastered. communication in people with PPA and
2. What are some specific strategies you might dementia?
implement as an SLP to enhance referrals of 6. Describe the benefits that have been
people with neurodegenerative disorders to demonstrated through research on the use
you for clinical services? of memory books and wallets.
3. Describe specific examples of how you, as 7. What are the benefits and limitations
an SLP, will ensure person-centered care in of SRT?
assessment and treatment of people with 8. Engage in role-play with a colleague, with
PPA and dementia. one of you playing the role of a person with
4. Compare and contrast direct treatment with dementia and the other as their care partner.
functional maintenance programs for people Take turns illustrating each of the seven
with dementia. strategies in the FOCUSED program.
26. Facilitating Communication in People With Primary Progressive Aphasia and Dementia   401

9. Develop a menu of options of activities in a your professional expertise to help support


Montessori-based program for people with people with PPA and dementia?
dementia. 14. Imagine that you are the only SLP in a large
10. Describe the Breakfast Club approach. How skilled nursing and rehabilitation center
might you implement it in a context other with a large number of residents who have
than a long-term care facility? dementia. Imagine that you are unable to
11. What are the strengths and weaknesses of include work on memory books and wallets
packaged caregiver training programs, such as part of your caseload.
as the Savvy Caregiver Program? a. What programming might you develop to
12. Imagine you were in an early stage of ensure that every resident with dementia
dementia. has a memory book in their room to be
a. What content would you most want used during staff and visitor interactions?
in your memory book and memory b. How would you ensure that the memory
wallet? books and wallets continue to be used in
b. In what ways would you want your meaningful ways?
materials to be organized (e.g., themes, c. How would you ensure that these aids
topics, types of materials, media and get updated periodically?
materials to be used)? On what would
you base such preferences? More materials to foster teaching and learning on
www
13. Aside from billable services, what are some this chapter’s content may be found on the com-
other service-oriented means of offering panion website.
CHAPTER
27
Counseling and Life Coaching

As we discussed in Chapter 23, our means of sup- 5. How does a clinician listen and respond
porting people in coping with the long-lasting effects empathetically and compassionately?
of language disability are more rooted in counsel- 6. How do we promote a positive outlook
ing, coaching, and education-oriented practices than without conveying a Pollyanna attitude?
they are in direct language intervention. Given the 7. How might multicultural differences affect
life-affecting nature of acquired neurogenic commu- counseling and coaching?
nication disorders, counseling or life coaching can 8. How might counseling moments be influenced
benefit all people touched by such disorders. As cli- by the time course of recovery and intervention?
nicians, it is important for us to help determine the 9. How may coaching enhance self-advocacy?
best people, approaches, and timing for extending 10. What are best practices in responding to
support and empowerment for meaningful, fulfill- seemingly misguided statements?
ing life participation. Given dynamic fluctuations 11. What are effective ways to address emotional
in each individual’s well-being during recovery lability during clinical interactions?
from an acquired communication disorder, or while 12. What is the role of the SLP in addressing
coping with progressive loss of cognitive-linguistic depression in people with neurogenic
abilities, what constitutes optimal support is typ- communication disorders?
ically ever changing (Worrall et al., 2010). Despite 13. How can communication counseling enhance
the fact that counseling and life coaching are among end-of-life care?
the most important services that speech-language 14. What are ways in which opportunities for
pathologists (SLPs) can provide to people with counseling can be missed?
acquired cognitive-linguistic challenges, many cli- 15. How might some aspects of life improve
nicians report being underprepared for the role, after onset of an acquired neurogenic
and many wish they had much more training in this communication disorder?
realm (Sekhon et al., 2015, 2019). 16. How may people with acquired
After reading and reflecting on the content in communication challenges support one
this chapter, you should be able to answer, in your another?
own words, the following queries: 17. What are some helpful information-sharing
strategies and resources?
1. How might an SLP become an effective
counselor and coach?
2. Is the SLP working with adults to be a How Might an SLP Become an
counselor, life coach, or both?
Effective Counselor and Coach?
3. What are important considerations related to
counseling and scope of practice?
4. How might a speech-language clinician adopt a Throughout this book, we have emphasized the sup-
counseling mindset? portive and empowering role of the SLP in serving

403
404  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

people with acquired neurogenic communication and knowledge in this significant area of practice.
disorders and the people who care about them. Our Meredith and Yeates (2020) offer thoughtful guid-
counseling roles cannot be clearly distinguished ance on counseling and promoting resilience with
from our roles as wellness-focused experts in other families coping with aphasia.
areas of practice, such as assessment and treatment.
Many of the features of ultimate excellent clinicians
highlighted throughout this book (e.g., summarized Is the SLP Working With Adults to Be
in Boxes 2–1, 17–1, and 23–1) include important
a Counselor, Life Coach, or Both?
counseling traits. Given just how paramount this
aspect of practice is to our work, we explore it fur-
ther in this chapter. Of course, all SLPs should have It is worth taking a moment to reflect on similarities
mentored practice and formal education in coun- and distinctions between terms here. Counseling
seling and life coaching that extends far beyond is a professional, goal-based collaborative process
the content of this book. Counseling and coaching geared toward fostering mental health and wellness
courses, continuing education programs, readings by encouraging changes in ways of thinking, feeling,
and reflection, and mentorship from experienced and behaving (Kaplan et al., 2014; NBCC Interna-
counselors are fundamental to continuous pursuit tional, 2015). Life coaching (or wellness coaching)
of clinical excellence. is a professional means of helping people develop a
There are several texts designed to foster clear vision of what is most important to them and
empowerment skills in general. Brumfitt (2009) pro- empowering them toward wellness and maximizing
vides theoretical and practical information about a their personal potential; it is typically based on pro-
vast array of approaches to assessing and treating moting strengths, moving beyond challenges, and
anxiety and depression and promoting well-being keeping in mind a big-picture view of what they
in adults with acquired communication disorders. most want to achieve (International Coach Federa-
Luterman (2008) offers advice and instructions for tion, 2015).
counseling with people who have communication Counseling and life coaching outcomes may
disorders and their families. He provides clinical include improved perspectives on challenges and
examples along with specific counseling techniques. strengths, empowerment, coping skills, relation-
Holland and Nelson (2014) offer a wonderful text on ships, and reduced anxiety, depression, and help-
counseling from a wellness perspective for people lessness. Both approaches depend on professional
with communication disorders across the life span. relationships that are focused on the client and their
They include information and inspiration-rich chap- environment, not a mutual friendship. Although
ters on counseling work with adults with acquired some but not all counseling approaches involve
communication disorders and with people at the end delving into analysis of past experiences, life coach-
of life. Their framework is rooted in positive psy- ing is more exclusively focused on what can be
chology, the discipline of helping people to lead full, done in the present and in the future. Also, counsel-
meaningful lives and pursue well-being and hap- ing may involve working to help people struggling
piness. Strategies used in positive psychology are with mental illness, whereas life coaching tends to be
focused on optimism, resilience, hope, mindfulness, more holistically focused on wellness.
affirmation, and positive thinking, not on abnormal The term counseling for many has the conno-
behavior or personality impairment (Seligman, 2002; tation of a service provided by a qualified (certi-
Snyder & Lopez, 2002). fied and/or licensed) professional, such as a social
Payne (2015) provides a rich resource guide in worker, psychologist, or rehabilitation counselor.
her book, Supporting Family Caregivers of Adults With Although it may be practiced professionally, life
Communication Disorders. Emphasizing the impor- coaching is not necessarily practiced in a “clinical”
tance of family dynamics, the complex roles of care- environment; it has the connotation for some peo-
givers, and multicultural aspects of caregiving, she ple as being less formal, and it is not regulated. That
provides ample practical content to guide communi- is, although there are certifications for professional
cation disorders professionals for developing skills life coaches (and accreditation standards for training
27. Counseling and Life Coaching   405

programs), these tend not to be required to practice • Even when a psychologist, psychotherapist,
as a life coach. or rehabilitation counselor is available and
The type of psychosocial assistance most needed services are reimbursable, many do not
and desired by people with neurogenic communi- have a solid background in communication
cation disorders and that can be aptly provided by disabilities, and many do not have training in
SLPs tends to fit in the intersection of counseling and supported communication.
life coaching. That is, we are professionals who fos- • Some people, even when referred, do not
ter mental health and wellness through changes in pursue the referral, perhaps due to financial
thinking, feeling, and behaving, and it is incumbent concerns and lack of insurance coverage, lack
upon us to provide support and empowerment to of convenience, and unfortunately pervasive
help the people we serve set goals and reach their myths that counseling is for people who have
greatest potential in terms of life participation. We personality disturbances, are self-absorbed,
do this to the extent that such work fits in our scope or have deep-seeded secrets in requiring
of practice; that is, inasmuch as our coaching and extensive dwelling on the past.
counseling are focused on enhanced communication
and socialization, improved coping with persistent Further compounding such referral challenges,
challenges, and reduction of the disabling aspects of many SLPs do not feel qualified in making mental
cognitive-linguistic challenges. In this light, Holland health referrals to others, expressing doubt about
and Nelson (2014) refer to our blended role as “com- when to refer, to whom to refer, and what sorts
munication counselors.” This role fits with the role of referrals would lead to reimbursable services
of the clinician as “expert companion,” promoted by (Northcott et al., 2017).
Park et al. (2008) in their book on how medical chal- Ideally, mental health and rehabilitation coun-
lenges can lead to positive life change. seling goes hand-in-hand with SLP services to
support people coping with cognitive and commu-
nicative challenges and the people who care about
What Are Important Considerations Related them. Within the SLP scope of practice, Holland and
Nelson (2014) suggest that we view counseling not
to Counseling and Scope of Practice?
just as a specific service we provide, but that we see
it as integrated with the rest of the work that we do;
Counseling people with communication disorders is they aptly recommend infusing intervention with
within the scope of practice of SLPs as characterized “counseling moments.” The thrust of such moments
by virtually every national association or council may be coping, acceptance, insights, goal setting,
overseeing SLP services globally and is a rich area or future planning (Brumfitt, 1995; Carvalho et al.,
for interprofessional practice. At the same time, 2011; Simmons-Mackie & Damico, 2011). The context
it is important to recognize the boundaries of our may be in direct individual treatment, couples coun-
scope of practice and also the ethical ramifications of seling, group intervention, caregiver training pro-
engaging in any type of service for which we do not grams, or support groups (see Chapters 25 and 26).
have demonstrated training and competence. Some For reasons just described, referring a person for
SLPs and other rehabilitation professionals consider mental health services is often not sufficient, even
counseling in the realm of mental health profession- when it is warranted, such as in cases of ongoing
als; they feel that anyone with an acquired neuro- depression. Sometimes being an outsider to the
genic communication disorder who would benefit person’s immediate family context, especially in
from mental health treatment should be referred to a friendly but professional role, helps boost a per-
others. There are four major problems with this: son’s receptivity to suggestions. Many people need
repeated encouragement to seek additional support.
• Mental health services are not always For people who are depressed, their depression may
available. limit their willingness to pursue mental health ser-
• Mental health services are often not covered vices; many people are embarrassed or feel stigma-
by third-party payers. tized by having others know of their depression, let
406  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

alone their communication problems. As anyone knowledgeable, reassuring, and confident profes-
who has tried to be helpful to a person with depres- sional you can be.
sion knows, it can be a challenge finding an effective Ideally, we provide counseling and life coaching
way to reach out and be supportive. Those who may throughout the intervention process. Suggestions for
have memory, attention, and comprehension prob- doing so are summarized in Box 27–1.
lems may be more apt to follow up if they are given
written reminders of referrals and engaged repeat-
edly in conversations about the potential benefits of How Does a Clinician Listen and Respond
mental health services and if their care partners are
Empathetically and Compassionately?
included in formation sharing about opportunities
for mental health counseling.
Empathy is the ability to see the world from another
person’s point of view; it involves tuning into
How Might a Speech-Language Clinician another’s emotional state, desires, and sense of
need. Compassion is like empathy but has the con-
Adopt a Counseling Mindset?
notation of shared feeling, not just understanding;
it is also linked to a sincere desire to provide sup-
Counseling starts from the moment we connect with port and help. Being compassionate does not mean
a person we are serving professionally. Before you feeling pity for someone. In fact, feeling sorry for a
enter a person’s space, pause, even if briefly, to adopt person with a disability can be extremely disempow-
a counseling mindset. Take a moment to reflect on ering (Northcott & Hilari, 2011; Simmons-Mackie &
your role as a motivator, a comforter, and a catalyst Elman, 2011).
for hope, reassurance, and recovery. Consider what We best express compassion and empathy as
the person you are about to meet might be experi- active listeners, not as experts trying to fix a person’s
encing. Consider what people who love that person problems. Empathic responding involves listening
are thinking and feeling. You have your own unique and reflecting with true concern about a person’s
way of expressing yourself as an affirmative, recov- feelings and perceived needs. Although some people
ery-promoting presence. Whatever that is, before are more naturally empathetic than others, empathic
you go meet the person you are about to serve, take skills can be learned and practiced; this is one of
a deep breath and gear up to be the best helpful, the reasons it is so important that clinical aphasiol-

Box
27–1 Suggestions for Psychosocial Support

• Listen actively, paraphrase, and ask for • Encourage engagement in meaningful


feedback about what you think you have activities.
heard. • Encourage scheduling of pleasurable activities
• Repair communication breakdown rather on a regular basis.
than moving on as if you understand or • Enlist the support of family members and
assuming that you have been understood. friends in addressing emotional concerns.
• Provide encouragement about strengths and • Consult with others regarding environmental
progress. factors, such as room furnishings, lighting,
• Provide active supported communication wall art, photos, and decorative colors that
for expression of feelings and reflections on may positively influence moods.
coping strategies. • Take your time; do not rush interactions.

Source: Brumfitt, 2009; Dunkle & Hooper, 1983; Holland & Nelson, 2014; Payne, 2015; Strong & Shadden, 2021.
27. Counseling and Life Coaching   407

ogists invest in in-depth training and mentorship on behavior changes (see Kormanik & Rocco, 2009,
in counseling. for a review). The construct is considered broadly
in two categories of belief: internal and external.
External locus of control includes a sense that other
How Do We Promote a Positive Outlook forces, such as God, luck, fate, and other people
Without Conveying a Pollyanna Attitude? (family, friends, professionals, etc.), determine what
will happen. Internal locus of control includes
one’s sense of having the power and the ability to
Coaching from a positive psychology framework
do something about one’s situation.
does not mean that we act as if all is fine and well,
If a person believes that his stroke was caused
minimizing the degree of anger, mourning, frustra-
by God as punishment for past wrongdoings, this
tion, grief, and loss that the people we serve may
would be an example of an external locus of control
be experiencing. Although an empowering attitude
at play. Another person might consider that their
is central to being a positive presence, cheerfulness
brain injury is attributable to their own negligence
is not a goal that trumps empathy or compassion.
in not wearing a helmet while driving a motorcycle
Responding with “Great job” when a person has
fast and drunk; this would be an example of internal
failed miserably at a memory or word-finding task,
locus of control. Most of us have a mix of beliefs in
for example, is not typically empowering. A more
external and internal forces in our own lives.
authentic response might be to just move on, or to
Since Rotter’s initial work, a rich literature has
respond nonverbally in recognition of the attempt,
been developed on how locus of control influences
or periodically (not too frequently) saying, “I know,
health care, rehabilitation, and counseling (Gru-
that is really tough. Let’s keep working at it.”
ber-Baldini et al., 2009; Harris, 2014; Papadopoulos
Saying “Well at least she got to live 95 years” to
et al., 2014). Several authors have developed instru-
a client whose mother just died is not typically help-
ments in attempts to index locus of control for clin-
ful. A more empathic response would be, “I am so
ical use (e.g., Baken, 2003; Baken & Stephens, 2005;
sorry. What a painful loss that is. I know she was so
Wallston et al., 1978, 1999). One way this construct
important to you.” Nonverbal responses, too, such
comes into play in counseling is when we work to
as a simple stroke on the arm or hug (if appropriate
help a person foster a stronger sense of internal con-
in the context), may also convey empathy.
trol as a means of empowerment to engage actively
in rehabilitation, coping and moving on with plans
for a full and active life as a person with ongoing
How Might Multicultural Differences
cognitive-communicative challenges. Doing this
Affect Counseling and Coaching? effectively, however, requires a deep appreciation
for others’ beliefs, tolerance for beliefs that might be
Throughout this book, we discuss cultural compe- contrary to our own, and support for behavior and
tence as a key ingredient of clinical excellence. Given attitude changes that do not violate a person’s core
that empathic responding requires seeing the world cultural, spiritual, or religious tenets.
the way it is seen by another person, the more we
know about a person’s background, including their
values, cultural traditions, and religious or spiritual How Might Counseling Moments
beliefs, the more empathic we may be. Be Influenced by the Time Course
Tuning in to a person’s locus of control is one
of Recovery and Intervention?
aspect of the intersection of culture and personality
that can influence the effectiveness of our counsel-
ing efforts. Locus of control is a person’s own view Counseling or coaching moments often arise spon-
of what and/or who has shaped the events in their taneously, according to evolving needs and circum-
life, and of what and/or who has the power to shape stances. Still, there are key times during intervention
their circumstances. The construct was initially that certain aspects of counseling are especially
introduced in the 1950s by Julian Rotter in his work important.
408  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Counseling Following a Traumatic Change that there are likely other injuries to the body as well,
some perhaps life-threatening and painful, that alter
If you are working in an acute care setting, your basic functions of mobility, breathing, toileting, and
counseling role is key in helping people cope with eating, in addition to thinking and communicating.
immediate needs for information and reassurance Fears of dying and the shock of having a brush with
and for planning next steps. If a person has had a mortality may be foremost on the minds of stroke
stroke, imagine how suddenly the person has seem- and brain injury survivors and the minds of the peo-
ingly been plucked from everyday life and dropped ple who love and care about them.
into a strange new world of changed abilities. If Consider, too, what it is like for the person
there has been a traumatic injury to the brain, imag- just realizing that their communication abilities are
ine how suddenly all of life as one has known it impaired. See Box 27–2 for firsthand quotes from
seemingly comes to a halt, at least initially. Consider people describing their own initial realizations about

Box
27–2 Quotes on Initial Realizations About the Sudden Onset of Aphasia

J.B.T., neuroscientist, author My wedding. Just gone. She goes too. I don’t even
[When] I tried to speak, I was blown away to dis- know where she go.
cover that although I could hear myself speaking
clearly, within my mind, no sound came out of D.L., physician
my throat. Not even the grunts that I was able to I woke up with a terrible headache and felt kind of
produce earlier. I was flabbergasted. Oh my gosh! sick to my stomach. I turned on the radio, just as
I can’t talk, I can’t talk! And it wasn’t until this I do every morning. I always listen to the morning
moment when I tried to speak out loud that I had news. But I had no idea what the guy was saying.
any idea that I couldn’t. My vocal cords were Sounded just like he should: nice radio voice, all
inoperative and nothing, no sounds at all, would professional and authoritative. But what was he
come forth. (Taylor, 2006, pp. 58–59) saying? I just could not make out what he meant.
I thought I was going crazy. So I turned on the TV.
R.M., automechanic CNN. You know the scrolling words they always
Alone in bed. Um, uh. Wake up. Strange. Some- show with news updates? They looked like they
thing strange. But what? Head really strange. were in hieroglyphics. I couldn’t make any of it
Go to pee. Foot on flum, flick, I mean floor. Then out. And this person and that were on the screen
bam. All of me on flick, floor. Yell for help. Yell in and I had no idea what anyone was saying. Every
my head. But nothing. Nothing. Pfffft. No word. once and a while I could get a word. But that was
Maybe a little squeak. Scared scared scared. What it. So I just got into my car to go to the ER. So weird,
happen? No idea. No word. Scared scared scared. though. I felt so weird. So confused. I live really
close to the hospital. It’s the same one where I’ve
P.L. been working for 14 years. I came around the cor-
Before the wedding. At night. Everybody there. ner to see the name of the hospital and I just could
Everybody. And flowers and food and music and not read it. The emergency entrance sign? I knew
love. Even from England and one, or no, two what the arrow meant. And of course I knew
Tunisia came. All that way. And dancing. And, how to get there. But oh man I could not make
you know, the guys, microphone, say how happy out that word, “emergency.” That’s when it hit
we, best wishes. Then I just couldn’t. At night me. I have aphasia! I must’ve had a stroke! But of
changed everything. No wedding. Hospital. I still course I couldn’t think that in words. I just knew
don’t know. All those people? They left then? it. Changed me forever. I’ve treated people with
They stay then and marky . . . park . . . party any- aphasia for years. Now I was one of them. Way to
way? I still don’t know. I just know no wedding. completely shake up my world!
27. Counseling and Life Coaching   409

the sudden loss of language abilities. The first is from munication can help to alleviate stress and refocus
Taylor (2006), from her book My Stroke of Insight. The efforts on what still can be done. Throughout treat-
others are from individuals with aphasia with whom ment, relationships continue to evolve. People who
I have worked. were highly supportive at the start may become less
engaged as time goes on. Caregivers may move or
die. There is always a need for empathic, active lis-
Counseling at the Start of Intervention tening and coaching in modification of life partici-
pation goals.
Counseling by the SLP soon after a stroke or brain
injury is usually the first counseling that a patient or
family receives. The most immediate needs tend to Counseling at Discharge
be for information, reassurance, and hope. In terms
of information, stroke and brain injury survivors Tuning in to emotional and information needs
and the people who care about them most want to is essential to effective discharge planning. Rarely is
know the cause of the problem, the type and extent a person discharged from SLP services because they
of changes in their abilities, and what the prognosis would no longer benefit from SLP services. As we
is (whether and how soon they will improve; Hersh considered in Section IV, many other forces are at
et al., 2013; Parr et al., 1997; Payne, 2015). play in determining when and for how long a per-
People with primary progressive aphasia (PPA), son has access to SLP services. Recognize that people
mild cognitive impairment, and dementia and the may feel abandoned when treatment is terminated,
people who care about them want to know the especially if they feel they have not achieved their
expected rate and nature of the likely progression personal communication goals (Hersh, 2009; Shad-
of the condition, and what they can do to slow pro- den, 2005). Additionally, emotional attachments to
gressive loss of cognitive-linguistic abilities. All are clinicians may make it especially difficult to sud-
likely to want to know how to best cope with com- denly lose access to such a key source of support.
munication and related social challenges, how to get
further informational and emotional support, and
what sorts of services are available to them, a topic
How May Coaching Enhance Self-Advocacy?
we further discuss later in this chapter.

In Chapter 15 and elsewhere in this book, we dis-


Counseling Related to Assessment cussed the need to expand awareness about neu-
Results and Sharing Prognosis rogenic language disorders, improve the way
laypeople respond to people with communication
The way we document and share assessment results challenges, reduce stigma, and facilitate empower-
and the way we discuss prognosis are fundamen- ment. Although people with communication disor-
tally related to our empowerment and advocacy ders are typically at a disadvantage in terms of the
roles. This is discussed in detail in Chapter 22. skills required to advocate for themselves, enlisting
them as key self-advocates is important. Encourage
them to carry print material to share with others
Counseling During Treatment about the nature of their communication challenges.
For people with aphasia, a card that defines aphasia
During treatment, stroke and traumatic brain injury and suggests means of facilitating communication
(TBI) survivors may feel that they are not progress- with them is not only helpful in terms of increasing
ing sufficiently. Reassurance that plateaus in prog- the likelihood of information exchange with a given
ress are common and that, in fact, people tend to person; it is also a good way to increase awareness
continue to improve for years can be reassuring. and knowledge about aphasia. Such cards are avail-
People with neurodegenerative conditions may feel able through the National Aphasia Association and
discouraged and helpless. Reassurance regarding from many service-providing agencies. They may
persistent strengths and training in supported com- also be created by clinicians and significant others
410  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

and tailored to the preferences and needs of an indi- validation of the emotion behind the statement. Con-
vidual with any type of communication disorder. sider possible clinician responses:
Some people with aphasia take a picture of such
cards or have the information printed in a “notes” • “No, he definitely doesn’t understand a lot
page on their smartphones for ready access. of what you’re saying. I know it based on my
testing results.”
• “I understand why you might think that.
Would you like to try some things together
What Are Best Practices in Responding
to figure out just what he may or may not
to Seemingly Misguided Statements? understand?”

Consider this comment from the wife of a man with In choosing a response, it is important for the cli-
aphasia who does not use writing or gesture effec- nician to consider which is more important: that the
tively to express meaning and whose spoken lan- partner’s statement stand corrected or that the partner
guage comprises mostly jargon: be enlisted as an essential ally in the next steps toward
improved communication. It is also important to con-
“I know everything he means.” sider the partner’s likely emotional investment in the
matter. This may be a true asset in the great scheme
On one level, the astute clinician probably knows of things as we transition into a treatment program.
that it is simply unlikely that his wife understands Gentle guidance and hands-on demonstration
everything. On another, it is important to consider by someone who cares to take the caregiver’s per-
whether overtly contesting the veracity of such a spective is likely to be far more effective than refuting
statement is helpful. Often a more effective approach her “misguided” statements. The ultimate excel-
is to demonstrate the process of deducing meaning lent clinician considers creative and compassionate
through conversations and other tasks requiring ways to heighten the degree to which the partner’s
conveyance of new information — content that his assumptions are realistic, yet keep her motivated
wife would not know. Helping her take part in the and engaged in collaborating to seek ways of further
process of discerning what she thinks he means from enhancing communication.
what he is expressing may best help her accept the Consider this dialogue between an SLP and a
challenge that there are simply times when no one man with severe aphasia whose expressive language
can tell what he means. This, in turn, may help with is highly neologistic:
heightening his wife’s focus on communicative sup-
port, perhaps encouraging more use of computer SLP: “I know you’re having trouble saying what
and phone apps, pictures, gesture, and drawing. you mean. I’d like to help you improve your
Consider this comment from the partner of a communication. How do you feel about getting
man with global aphasia who is known, based on started in therapy so that we can work on this?”
extensive assessment and treatment probes, to have
severe comprehension deficits and no reliable yes/ Client: (Shaking head) “I really don’t think
no response: there’s much of a problem here. I’ve got all the
squirrels I need to be able to, you know, get what
“He understands everything we say.” you need from me.”

Can this be true? We can’t refute it entirely because What is the most constructive way for the SLP
the understanding of the person with aphasia may to respond? Is the goal to “teach” the person how
only be deduced through some overt index; we may severe his problems really are? Or is it to enlist him
simply lack any such index in this situation. Still, as the most important ally on his own recovery team?
there is a high probability that the statement is incor- The excellent clinician takes into account that lack
rect. So, is it best to tell the partner that what she is of awareness of deficits is a hallmark characteristic
saying is probably not true? It could be. However, of this person’s aphasia and thus part of what is to
many caregivers in this situation respond better to be addressed through intervention — hopefully not
27. Counseling and Life Coaching   411

a roadblock to enrolling him in treatment. Enlisting • find a means of distracting themselves


him as an ally may involve a shift from talking about (perhaps by trying to remember lyrics to
whether to enroll in treatment to actually demon- a song; counting the change in a pocket or
strating helpful strategies for detecting a commu- purse; walking around; or taking deep, slow
nication breakdown and repairing it. Rather than breaths and letting the air out gradually).
showing resistance to his resistance, having him par-
ticipate directly in what is a meaningful therapeutic
activity may be far more effective in convincing him What Is the Role of the SLP in Addressing
that you have something to offer. Depression in People With Neurogenic
Communication Disorders?

What Are Effective Ways to Address Emotional


In each of the chapters on etiologies underlying neu-
Lability During Clinical Interactions?
rogenic language disorders, we saw depression and
as an inherent challenge. Not only is low mood an
Emotional lability, or pseudobulbar affect (PBA), obvious detractor from quality of life, but it is also
common in people with neurogenic communication a cause of poorer rehabilitation outcomes (Aström
disorders, was discussed in Chapter 19 as a potential et al., 1992; Mitchell, 2016; Simmons-Mackie, 2013a;
confound in assessment. It is also a potentially debil- Spencer et al., 1995; Worrall et al., 2017). When
itating condition in terms of its impact on commu- depression continues for two or more weeks, it is
nication, for two primary reasons. First, it can easily termed by the American Psychiatric Association
sidetrack a conversation or activity. Most of us, when (APA, 2013) as persistent depressive disorder, a
we see a person burst into tears in the midst of a component of chronic major depressive disorder.
conversation, naturally feel led to comfort that per- What had previously been called dysthymic dis-
son and show sympathy. For people with PBA, our order (a chronic state of depression for most of the
comforting response may actually exacerbate the time over a period of at least 2 years) in earlier ver-
outburst and further interfere with whatever it is sions of the APA’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
that we were doing together. Although it may not be of Mental Disorders (DSM) is now included in this
intuitive because we are so driven to be nurturing, category. Bereavement over the death of a loved
often the best thing a listener can do is to acknowl- one, previously considered to fit within the defini-
edge the tears with a tender touch or an empathetic tion of depression, is now excluded from the defi-
look but move on with what we were doing, perhaps nition. This is in recognition of the fact that features
encouraging the person to change positions or take of bereavement and other aspects of depression are
some deep breaths, but not delving into discussion often intertwined and indistinguishable and that
of feelings. A second reason that emotional liability there is no specific time period for the duration of
is a detriment to communication is that the outburst bereavement (previously set at 2 months but recog-
and lack of control over them may, in turn, lead to nized to last more typically for 1 to 2 years).
feelings of shame or embarrassment around others, One of the most important ways of alleviat-
which then may lead to avoidance of social situa- ing depression, sadness, and grief is to talk about
tions and thus social isolation. You might suggest feelings with others; having restrictions in commu-
directly to people with PBA that they nication makes coping all the more challenging. In
Chapter 19, we reviewed the definition of depres-
• educate others that the emotions they show sion and means of screening for it during the assess-
do not necessarily reflect what they are really ment process. Here, let’s consider the role of the SLP
feeling; in helping, directly and indirectly, to promote mental
• ask others to move on in a conversation or health and wellness.
activity when an emotional outburst occurs The actual incidence of depression in people
rather than trying to comfort or give other with acquired neurogenic communication disor-
emotional feedback; and ders is unknown. Although incidence statistics for
412  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

depression have been published for stroke and TBI


survivors and people with right brain syndrome
How Can Communication Counseling
and dementia, the studies on which they are based Enhance End-of-Life Care?
have serious limitations. Many use language-based
indices, do not include input from the people being In Chapter 14, we noted that SLPs play important
assessed, require reflection and judgment abilities roles in hospice and palliative care contexts by pro-
that exceed the capabilities of some who are assessed, viding information, encouraging expression about
and/or exclude people with severe communica- end-of-life wishes, and supporting important com-
tion impairments (Patterson, 2002; Townend et al., munication about comfort, needs, and relationships.
2007a, 2007b). Those of us with decades of experi- Given the frequent intense focus on restoring health
ence working with this broad population will attest and curing disease in medical contexts, people
that the condition is severely underreported, and the who are nearing the end of life are often attended
rate is close to 100%. Some already had depression to medically rather than in a holistic, supportive,
prior to onset of a cognitive communicative disor- life-​affirming way (Gawande, 2014). SLPs can play
der. Additionally, most have a combination of neu- a pivotal role in reducing the focus on medicaliza-
robiologically induced mood changes and negative tion of care through counseling and communicative
emotional reactions to their life-changing disability. support and in encouraging conversations about
Stroke survivors with aphasia tend to have a higher things that matter most to people nearing the end
rate of depression than those without aphasia. The of life and those who care about them. SLPs with
prevalence of major depression in people with apha- special expertise in end-of-life and palliative care
sia has been shown to increase during the 12 months concerns may provide much-needed in-services and
following acute care for stroke (Kauhanen et al., workshops to other professionals to promote critical
2000). Regardless of whether they are depressed, reflection and planning to enhance communication
many people with aphasia experience anxiety, stress, and quality of life through their varied clinical roles
and worry associated with their self-perceptions of (Roberts & Gaspard, 2013). As professional team
communicative inadequacy and anticipation of com- members, we may also be helpful in mutual support
municative failure (Cahana-Amitay et al., 2011, 2013; of colleagues coping with client death and bereave-
Ryan et al., 2020). ment (Barton et al., 2003).
As always, whether or not a person actually has
a diagnosis of depression, a clinical aphasiologist can
play an important role as a provider and facilitator of
psychosocial support and wellness. As discussed in
What Are Ways in Which Opportunities
Chapter 19, identifying depression and understand- for Counseling Can Be Missed?
ing its nature in people with neurogenic disorders is
complex due to interactions among multiple poten- Simmons-Mackie and Damico (2011) provide a
tial causes, communication barriers, challenges with thoughtful ethnographic analysis of how some SLPs
judgment and reflection, guilt, embarrassment, and actually avoid counseling moments and thus miss
associated stigma. Although the diagnosis of mood important opportunities for providing emotional
disorders fits more within the scope of practice for support during treatment sessions. They interpret
psychiatry and psychology than for SLP, when a lan- missed counseling opportunities as being due to
guage disorder is overlaid on suspected depression, SLPs’ tendencies to take control of interactions by
the SLP may be extremely helpful by consulting in
the diagnosis of depression and by making referrals • focusing on facts rather than feelings or
for psychological counseling and possible phar- discussions about abstract or vague ideas;
macotherapy. Ongoing assessment of mood states, • engaging in “staged,” inauthentic
coping strategies, self-esteem, optimism, and level conversation;
of adjustment to changes in body function and struc- • using humor to deflect emotional expression;
ture and life participation is also important. Means and
of indexing changes according to such constructs are • transitioning from expressions of emotion to
given in Chapters 19 and 20. “objective therapy tasks.”
27. Counseling and Life Coaching   413

The authors suggest that the underlying causes widely and include increased time and availability
of these missed opportunities include a lack of train- to help others, wisdom, patience, appreciation for
ing and mentorship in supporting the value of coun- others, renunciation of workaholic tendencies, deep-
seling, a failure to appreciate counseling as part of ened loving relationships and friendships, and clar-
our scope of practice, discomfort or awkwardness ity in priorities. Of course, the prominence of benefits
with handling intimacy and depth of social inter- versus struggles fluctuates. As Debra Meyerson (in
action, and adoption of a clinician-centered rather Meyerson & Zuckerman, 2019) says, “Sometimes
than a person-centered approach to intervention. I mourn the loss of my old self and feel like life sucks.
They recommend that we explore our own beliefs, Other times, I genuinely feel like a happier person
values, and habits that might lead to missed oppor- than I was before my stroke” (p. 43).
tunities. We might do this through analysis of videos
of our clinical sessions, introspection, vigilance for
counseling opportunities that arise, and conscious How May People With Acquired
self-​monitoring of what evokes embarrassment or Communication Challenges
negative emotions in us. We might also observe what Support One Another?
happens when, instead of taking control, we simply
engage in active, reflective, empathic listening and
We explored group therapy in Chapter 25. Let’s
collaborate as communication partners in exploring
highlight briefly here the importance of groups in
the challenges at hand. Let’s keep practicing this.
counseling and life coaching. Having people get to
meet others coping with similar challenges makes
a tremendous difference in the lives of many group
How Might Some Aspects of Life members (Northcott et al., 2018). Ask any clinician
Improve After Onset of an Acquired who facilitates group therapy and social engage-
Neurogenic Communication Disorder? ment among people with aphasia and related dis-
orders, and they will regale you with touching
stories of sharing and mutual support among group
People with neurogenic communication disorders
members. Monte et al. (2020) artfully describe their
often express wonderfully affirmative comments
impression of the power of groups: “It is as if tak-
about how they have maintained positive feelings
ing part in the group could slowly awaken a per-
about their identity and life purpose — sometimes
son’s sleeping cognitive and social skills. This is an
even expressing better self-regard than they had
important therapeutic function for the group, as well
before the onset of their communication disability.
as a pattern of personal empowerment” (p. 90). On
Consider this eloquent quote from Taylor (2006):
any given day, professionals and students commit-
ted to empowering people with aphasia and related
I knew I was different now — but never once did
challenges may directly witness the power of mutual
my right mind indicate that I was “less than” what
support by viewing the powerful videos and written
I had been before. I was simply a being of light
messages shared among stroke and brain injury sur-
radiating life into the world. Regardless of whether
vivors and people coping with PPA on social media
or not I had a body that could connect me to the
(for examples, go to Facebook and check out Apha-
world of others, I saw myself as a cellular mas-
sia Recovery Connection, Poems in Speech, Living
terpiece. In the absence of my left hemisphere’s
with Aphasia, the Primary Progressive Aphasia Sup-
negative judgment, I perceived myself as perfect,
port Group, and Virtual Connections for Aphasia).
whole, and beautiful, just the way I was. (p. 71)

It is not uncommon for stroke and brain injury


What Are Some Helpful Information-
survivors to point out aspects of their lives that have
improved post-onset. See Box 27–3 for an example
Sharing Strategies and Resources?
as described by a TBI and a stroke survivor. Benefits
noted by people with acquired neurogenic commu- Counseling and coaching involve not only empathic
nication disorders and their friends and families vary support but also information sharing. This might be
414  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Box
27–3 Quote From a TBI Survivor (Lucy) About
Improvements Due to Brain Changes

Lucy: I was describing this to somebody on the phone who, um, a good
friend of mine, this one that I work with in North Carolina who a few years
ago he uh was also in an accident where he was almost killed with carbon
monoxide poisoning so he had a year of or he had some time of trying,
and when I was describing this over the phone he said, “Boy, it sounds like
depression.” But the difference was it was like but it didn’t bother me . . . it
didn’t bother me then, I mean maybe it is, I don’t know but it doesn’t well,
the what I was saying that I didn’t care about stuff anymore. And the um um
. . . the feeling is that oh, it would bother me that I don’t have any ambition
but the truth of the matter is that it doesn’t bother me in the least.
Clinician: And this concerns you that it doesn’t bother you?
Lucy: No, actually, I think it’s kind of a benefit (laugh). You know what
I mean, it’s like it’s kind of like a benefit being able to sit for two hours
with nothing to do and that’s kind of nice. Because before you’d need to be
reading something or you know to write you know. All right let’s get the
show on the road you know it’s like . . .
Clinician: Yeah.
Lucy: It’s like I mean I think that that’s almost skipped all those years of
meditating and now I’m enlightened (laugh) without having to do any of the
years of meditating.

Poem by a Stroke Survivor Conveying a Balance Between


What He Has Lost With What He Has Gained

To Be Here
Before Stroke: Confident and Competent
I’m supposed to be here.
After Stroke: A Presence and Mindfulness
I’m supposed to be here.
— Mark Harder

in the form of explanations about the causes and might be assumed given the name of the site-hosting
nature of cognitive-communication challenges as well entity. For example, most aphasia resource sites
as in referrals and extension of opportunities for sup- listed also provide information that is helpful to peo-
port. Be sure to have information on hand about any ple with dementia and TBI and the people who care
local stroke clubs, aphasia centers, and support groups about them.
for those with TBI, stroke survivors, people with neu- There are many online and print resources to
rodegenerative conditions, and caregivers, too. help people with aphasia and other disorders — and
Note, too, that almost all of these sites provide those who care about them — understand more about
information and support to a wider audience than their conditions, learn means of supporting com-
27. Counseling and Life Coaching   415

munication, and help foster positive and proactive returning to work, and various aspects of caregiving
attitudes about moving forward in pursuit of the and caregiver support.
fullest life participation possible. Examples of help- Social media provide opportunities for people
ful, informative websites are given in Table 27–1. with aphasia and their caregivers to connect with
Note that these resource sites originate from varied one another for information exchange and support.
countries all over the world; given Internet access, Using a keyword search in Facebook, for example,
one need not be restricted geographically in seeking for most of the many diagnostic categories or condi-
information and support. tions mentioned in this book will lead to a selection
Many of the websites listed in Table 27–1 have of groups and pages of potential interest to people
links to videos that can be helpful in terms of pro- seeking online interaction regarding their chal-
viding supportive information as well as examples lenges and triumphs. Many of the websites listed in
of successful coping strategies. For example, the Table 27–1 have corresponding Facebook, Instagram,
National Stroke Association’s website provides a and Twitter links. Among the offerings of several are
series of informative videos and prerecorded webi- online communication cafes, which are online vid-
nars on topics such as sex and sexuality after stroke, eoconferencing meetings enabling social interaction
depression and other emotional problems, fatigue, among adults with communication challenges.

Table 27–1. Websites to Support People With Neurogenic Cognitive-Communicative Disorders


and the People Who Care About Them

Name of Organization Organization Website


Alzheimer’s Association https://www.alz.org
Alzheimer’s Disease Education and Referral https://www.nia.nih.gov/health/alzheimers
(ADEAR) Center (part of the National Institute
on Aging)
Alzheimer’s Disease International https://www.alz.co.uk
American Brain Tumor Association https://www.abta.org
American Stroke Association (part of the https://www.strokeassociation.org
American Heart Association)
Aphasia Accessa https://www.aphasiaaccess.org
Aphasia Alliance https://www.aphasiaalliance.org
Aphasia Corner https://blog.aphasia.com
Aphasia Center of California https://www.aphasiacenter.net
Aphasia Recovery Connection https://www.aphasiarecoveryconnection.org
Aphasia Hope Foundation https://aphasiahope.wpengine.com
Aphasia Now https://www.aphasianow.org
Aphasia Institute https://www.aphasia.ca
Aphasia and Stroke Association of India https://aphasiastrokeindia.com/
Australian Aphasia Association https://www.aphasia.org.au

continues
Table 27–1. continued

Name of Organization Organization Website


Association for Frontotemporal Degeneration https://www.theaftd.org
Better Conversations with Aphasia: A Learning https://extendstore.ucl.ac.uk/product?catalog​
Resource =UCLXBCA
Brain Injury Association of America https://www.biausa.org
Brain Trauma Foundation https://www.braintrauma.org
British Aphasiology Society https://www.bas.org.uk
Communication Forum Scotland www.communicationforumscotland.org.uk
Connect: The Communication Disability http://www.ukconnect.org/index.aspx
Network
Dementia Advocacy and Support Network http://www.dasninternational.org
Different Strokes: Support for Younger Stroke https://www.differentstrokes.co.uk
Survivors
Family Caregiver Alliance https://www.caregiver.org/
Friendship and Aphasia https://friendshipandaphasia.weebly.com/
Huntington’s Disease Society of America https://www.hdsa.org
FTD Support Forum https://www.ftdsupportforum.com/
Lewy Body Dementia Association https://www.lbda.org
Music and Memory https://www.musicandmemory.org
National Aphasia Association https://www.aphasia.org
National Brain Tumor Society https://www.braintumor.org
National Health Services https://www.nhs.uk
National Institute for Health Research https://www.nihr.ac.uk
National Parkinson Foundation https://www.parkinson.org
Powerful Tools for Caregivers https://www.powerfultoolsforcaregivers.org/
Predicting Language Outcome and Recovery https://www.ucl.ac.uk/ploras
After Stroke (PLORAS)
Rare Dementia Support https://www.raredementiasupport.org/
Stroke Association https://www.stroke.org.uk
Stroke Survivor https://strokesurvivor.com
Tavistock Trust for Aphasia https://www.aphasiatavistocktrust.org
UCL CSLIR Aphasia Research Group Blog aphasiaresearch.wordpress.com/
United Kingdom Acquired Brain Injury Forum https://www.ukabif.org.uk
Note. Additional websites supporting clinical practice are also helpful for supporting information-sharing, coun-
seling, and coaching efforts. See Table 2–2.
a
The Aphasia Access site includes a listing of providers with a focus on life participation approaches.

416
27. Counseling and Life Coaching   417

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter 8. Describe a time when someone else
that are new to you or that you have not yet responded to you in a way that was not
mastered. particularly empathetic when you felt grief,
2. Some SLPs have a natural aptitude for anxiety, or fear.
counseling and coaching. Others have to a. What, specifically, do you wish that
work harder at the related competencies. person had said or done?
a. With a partner, compare and contrast b. How might you use your reflection about
what you think are your natural that incident in your preparation to
predispositions toward counseling and become more empathetic with others?
coaching. 9. With a partner or small group, share
b. What do you think you need to work on anecdotes about how you or others have
further in this regard? engaged in Pollyanna-type responses in
3. Describe what you think would be the best personal or professional situations. If it were
outline of topics to be covered in a course possible, how might those responses have
on counseling and coaching for people with been revised to be more empathetic?
neurogenic communication disorders and 10. Describe a way in which a clinician might
the people who care about them. integrate the construct of locus of control in
4. Look up scope-of-practice documents counseling or life coaching.
for your national and regional or state 11. Imagine what it would feel like to suddenly
certification as an SLP. lose your ability to communicate verbally.
a. What do they say about the extent and What support would you wish for most
limitations of your professional role in immediately?
counseling and coaching? 12. Plateaus in treatment progress can be
b. Do you think the documents are discouraging. In a role-play with a partner
appropriate in light of the services representing your “client,” practice what
that people with acquired neurogenic you might say to encourage them to
cognitive-linguistic disorders typically continue working toward their rehabilitation
need? Why or why not? and life participation goals.
5. Compare and contrast the roles of 13. How might you proactively address a
neuropsychologists, rehabilitation person’s lack of awareness of deficits when
counselors, social workers, and SLPs in encouraging them to enroll in SLP treatment?
supporting the counseling and coaching 14. With a partner, practice role-playing
needs of people with acquired neurogenic responses to emotional lability during a
cognitive-linguistic disorders. one-on-one language treatment session.
6. Describe what you would consider your Then discuss how effective you think your
optimal means of collaborating with responses were and why.
professionals mentioned in Item 5 to support 15. Describe why referral to a mental health
people with acquired neurogenic cognitive- professional may not be the only and
linguistic disorders. best option for addressing the counseling
7. Describe a time when you responded in a and coaching needs of a person with a
way that was not particularly empathetic neurogenic communication disorder.
to another person expressing grief, anxiety, 16. Describe how an SLP might best collaborate
or fear. Knowing what you know now, how with a mental health professional in
might you have responded differently? addressing the counseling and coaching
418  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

needs of a person with a neurogenic 20. Review at least 10 of the websites listed in
communication disorder. Table 27–1.
17. Consider that many people are not aware a. Note special attributes of websites
of how SLPs may be helpful in fostering that you may find particularly helpful
communication support for people who are in terms of support and information
dying and the people who love them. What sharing for people with neurogenic
might you do as an aphasiologist to promote communication disorders and people
SLP services in such situations? who care about them.
18. Write a to-do list of specific actions that b. If you are a Facebook user, “like” some
would help an SLP not miss opportunities pages or groups associated with clinical
for counseling during intervention to populations with which you work or are
address cognitive and linguistic challenges. most likely to work and get familiar with
19. Disabilities and illness can improve some what they have to offer.
aspects of life for many people. c. With a partner or small group, discuss
a. With a partner or small group, share how the merits and potential challenges of
you have witnessed this in your own having clients with neurogenic disorders
personal or professional experience. and their caregivers engage with specific
b. How might your awareness of the Facebook pages or groups.
positive aspects of acquired disabilities
affect your role as a communication Additional teaching and learning materials are
www
counselor? available on the companion website.
CHAPTER
28
Complementary and
Integrative Approaches

In this chapter, we consider examples of interven- improving cognitive-communicative


tion approaches that serve as alternatives or adju- abilities?
vants to more traditional types of intervention for 10. How might speech-language pathologists
people with cognitive-communicative disorders. (SLPs) support people considering
Many of these approaches are applied broadly in the complementary and alternative approaches to
majority of the world even if not promoted through cognitive-communicative wellness?
the minority-world, English-language scientific
literature.
After reading and reflecting on the content in What Are Complementary and
this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in
Integrative Approaches to Wellness?
your own words, the following queries:

1. What are complementary and integrative The terms alternative, complementary, integrative, and
approaches to wellness? nontraditional have each been used in varied ways
2. How are complementary and integrative by varied authors and in institutions and agencies
approaches relevant to neurogenic disorders of that support related practices. The term alternative
language and cognition? suggests approaches that are recommended in place
3. Why is it important for clinical aphasiologists of common Western medical approaches (i.e., allo-
to learn about complementary and integrative pathic approaches, which most often target specific
approaches? bodily systems or disease states). Generally, the
4. How are mind-body practices relevant to term complementary highlights approaches that do
people with cognitive-linguistic challenges? not replace allopathic approaches but rather ones
5. How might hypnosis and visualization that are used in conjunction with them, also called
be relevant to people with neurogenic adjunct or adjuvant approaches. The term integra-
communication disorders? tive has two related connotations in complementary
6. What are the potential roles of religion approaches to health. One connotation reflects a
and spirituality in acquired neurogenic holistic health focus (e.g., integration of body and
communication disorders? mind, which are seen as intertwined, inseparable
7. How might natural product use be relevant to entities); the other reflects the combination (inte-
people with cognitive-linguistic challenges? gration) of complementary and allopathic medical
8. Why are complementary and integrative approaches into mainstream health care and health
approaches increasing in popularity? promotion programs.
9. What is the status of the evidence base The term nontraditional is a term that is typ-
supporting alternative approaches to ically used in minority-world countries to refer to

419
420  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

nonallopathic approaches. However, this is prob- of the research on such approaches carried out with
lematic because many such approaches (e.g., Chi- people coping with stroke, traumatic brain injury
nese and Ayurvedic medicine) represent traditional (TBI), and dementia, for example, addresses overall
approaches; they have been used in some cultures health and well-being or specific aspects of health
for thousands of years, much longer than most allo- (e.g., insomnia, pain, or anxiety relief), not specific
pathic approaches. For most people living in Asia, aspects of cognition and language. Does this make
for example, use of herbs for healing is literally tra- it irrelevant to clinical aphasiologists? Certainly not,
ditional. Because of the cultural relativity and thus inasmuch as we are ideally team members helping
ambiguity of the term, many authors have suggested to promote wellness; wellness is an essential concern
we avoid using the terms traditional and nontradi- in our work.
tional altogether when discussing complementary
approaches to health.
The terms medical and medicine also warrant Why Is It Important for Clinical
revisiting in this context. Although some commonly Aphasiologists to Learn About Complementary
refer to complementary or alternative medicine, the
and Integrative Approaches?
focus of many complementary approaches is on
wellness, health, and prevention, in contrast to the
common physically curative approaches of allo- In clinical practice environments worldwide, there is
pathic medicine. Of course, one might also consider increasing likelihood that we will play a consultative
whether the distinction of “Eastern” versus “West- role in helping people with neurogenic communi-
ern” approaches is appropriate, given the degree of cation disorders consider complementary options
transnational and multicultural influence on formal to direct behavioral intervention to improve or slow
and informal approaches to wellness today. Com- declines in speech, language, and cognition. Pop-
plementary and integrative approaches have taken ularity of such options is long-standing in Eastern
on great import globally, as they reflect important regions and is increasing steadily in the West (Park
philosophical viewpoints regarding wellness and et al., 2015; Shah et al., 2008). Many of the people
holistic health. we serve clinically are likely to be engaged in some
Attention to such distinctions in terminology is form of complementary treatment or practice (Lund-
reflected in progressive name changes for what was gren, 2004). Most clinicians in training to become
founded as the U.S. National Institutes of Health SLPs have some experience with complementary
(NIH) Office of Alternative Medicine in 1991. The and alternative modalities in their own self-care
NIH created that office in recognition of the need for (Marshall & Laures-Gore, 2008). Some clinical SLPs
an evidence base to support practices that were con- advocate passionately for the integration of comple-
sidered “nontraditional” in comparison to Western mentary approaches to communication disorders
medicine. The office was renamed the National Cen- within SLP curricula (Marshall & Basilakos, 2014).
ter for Complementary and Alternative Medicine in As discussed in Chapter 6, promoting healthful
1998, and then given its current name, the National living for the prevention of neurological disorders
Center for Complementary and Integrative Health and for lessening their impacts on life participation
(NCCIH) in late 2014 (NCCIH, 2021). is a critical aspect of our role as advocates and health
care team members. Many authors have shown how
participating in socially engaging and health-pro-
How Are Complementary and Integrative moting activities can boost cognitive-linguistic
Approaches Relevant to Neurogenic intervention. Examples are exercise, volunteer work,
singing, listening to and playing music, dancing,
Disorders of Language and Cognition?
interacting with animals, playing games, cooking,
and art- and craft-based activities (for examples, see
It is difficult to characterize just which complemen- Beard, 2012; Brotons & Koger, 2000; Horowitz, 2013;
tary and integrative approaches are most relevant to Hurkmans et al., 2012; LaFrance et al., 2007; Luck-
acquired disorders of cognition and language. Much owski, 2014; Macauley, 2006; Mahendra & Arkin,
28. Complementary and Integrative Approaches   421

2003, 2004; Schneider & Camp, 2003; and Stallings, Ray (2001) summarize features of relaxation ther-
2010; also consider the general approaches to inter- apy and acupuncture as they have been applied to
vention and social approaches in Chapter 25). All of people with varied types of neurologic communica-
these types of activities can be carried out during tion disorders, and also provide a concise overview
or in addition to SLP treatments, in co-treatments of other methods and the scientific rationale behind
with other professionals, and through caregiver them, including biofeedback, and transcutaneous
and volunteer facilitation (Figure 28–1). Still, many electrical nerve stimulation. An in-depth description
of the methods associated with complementary of acupuncture and associated biological responses
approaches are more definitively outside of our that make it potentially relevant to treatment of neu-
scope of practice and not incorporated into most SLP rogenic cognitive-linguistic disorders is provided by
educational programs. Laures-Gore and Marshall (2008).
Marshall and Mohapatra (2004) provide a “call
to action” for aphasiologists to collaborate across
disciplines in complementary practices and to sub- How Are Mind-Body Practices Relevant to
stantiate the evidence base in this arena. Laures and
People With Cognitive-Linguistic Challenges?
Shisler (2004) provide a review of the principles
underlying some approaches to health and wellness
applied in the realm of clinical aphasiology, includ- Most complementary approaches to health in gen-
ing acupuncture, hypnosis, imagery, progressive eral may be categorized as mind-body practices or
muscle relaxation, and biofeedback. Murray and natural product use. Mind-body practices include

Figure 28–1. A highly social exercise class with an anti-ageist focus at a government-sponsored
community center for older adults with and without disabilities in Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia. A full-
color version of this figure can be found in the Color Insert.
422  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

mindfulness, mindfulness-based stress reduction, experiences of being so immersed in an activity that


hypnotherapy, guided imagery, biofeedback, mas- they momentarily lose a sense of themselves and
sage, acupuncture, herbal medicine, chiropractic, of their debilities. We may do that by focusing on
osteopathic manipulation, prayer, yoga, reiki, Tai strengths, encouraging engagement in pleasurable
Ji, and qi gong. Although all of these approaches activities, adjusting difficulty of intervention tasks so
are practiced with people who have neurogenic that they are not too easy or difficult, and counseling
communication challenges in much of the world, about people and aspects of their physical environ-
the research supporting them for specific clinical ment that may detract from or enhance flow.
populations, including methodological details and Yoga is practiced in diverse ways across the
measurable outcomes, is scant. By no means does globe. Examples of yogic practices include asanas
this mean that they are not viable. Rather, there is a (bodily posturing), pranayama (breathing-focused
tremendous need for research in this area. experiences), and varied forms of meditation. In
Consider the two key components of mindful- much of Asia, yoga entails spiritual practices that
ness: self-regulation of attention to enable immedi- tend to be less emphasized in other regions. Stroke
ate physical, mental, and emotional experience, and and brain injury survivors and people coping with
adoption of a new orientation (through curiosity, dementia, and their care partners, may benefit from
openness, and acceptance) (Crielesi et al., 2019). participating in any of these. Applications specific
Intuitively, it makes sense that people with commu- to these populations may be offered as a primary
nication challenges would benefit from these, espe- therapy, guided by a certified yoga therapist, or as
cially in the sense of not being bound by their own an adjunct to SLP intervention (Dietz et al., 2020).
thoughts and being open to new ways of improving Marshall et al. (2015) provide a fascinating descrip-
themselves. See Dickinson et al. (2016), Laures-Gore tion of unilateral forced nostril breathing and why
and Marshall (2016), Marshall et al. (2018), and Oren- it may be a helpful adjunct to language treatment
stein et al. (2012) for evidence of benefits according following stroke.
to diverse outcome measures from case studies and Dietz et al. propose that “a mind-body practice
small groups of people with aphasia. such as yoga, adapted to meet the language and phys-
Yeates (2020) suggests that alternatives to tra- ical needs of people with aphasia and co-survivors,
ditional “seated verbal exchange” that “is not suffi- can serve as a vehicle to increase resilience and
ciently flexible and innovative response to the myriad coping, thereby supporting language recovery and
of survivors’ altered relationships with their minds improved quality of life” (p. 855). Few studies to
post injury” (p. 137) are essential. As a practitioner of date have addressed specific cognitive-linguistic
Tai Ji, “characterized by slow, flowing movements of outcomes associated with specific yoga practices;
all parts of the body, alongside breath regulation and this is a promising area for further study. In any case,
deep psychological immersion in the movements” the evidence for the health benefits of yoga in gen-
(p. 140), he encourages “a focus on using the body eral, including resilience, coping, stress reduction,
to manage the mind” (p. 138). Proponents of flow and social engagement, certainly bode well for peo-
psychology and yoga also highlight the importance ple with neurogenic communication disorders.
of bodily practice, including breathing methods, to Laughter yoga (Figure 28–2) is in common use in
help individuals center themselves and attend to the India in day centers and group programs for people
present as a means of stress reduction and release with dementia and is increasing in popularity for all
of limiting thoughts and negative self-talk (both of people in much of the world. Popularized by Kataria
which tend to be challenges for people with neuro- (2011) in the 1960s, it is based on the mind-body the-
genic challenges). Sather et al. (2017) describe flow as ory that laughing for no reason brings about many
“positive experiences of intense concentration, dis- of the psychophysiological benefits of real laughter
torted time passage, and a loss of self-consciousness (e.g., reduced stress, lower blood pressure, a sense of
that result from matching task difficulty to a person’s well-being), especially when combined with breath-
skills level” (p. 25). They advocate for us to facilitate ing techniques, group engagement, and eye contact
flow for people with aphasia to enhance positive with others while laughing (Ellis et al., 2017; Heidari
emotions, sense of identity, skill development, and et al., 2020; Yoshikawa et al., 2019).
28. Complementary and Integrative Approaches   423

Figure 28–2. People with aphasia, care partners, and volunteer speech-language pathologists
enjoying a vibrant laughing yoga experience led by a certified yoga practitioner at Aphasia SG,
an aphasia center in Singapore. Photo courtesy of Evelyn Khoo. A full-color version of this figure
can be found in the Color Insert.

approaches, the ability to visualize to-be-named


How Might Hypnosis and Visualization objects and actions internally while engaging in
Be Relevant to People With Neurogenic semantic associative tasks, such as through semantic
Communication Disorders? feature analysis (see Chapter 31), may boost a per-
son’s success with such approaches. The possibility
Visualization is an internal process of associating is supported by the fact that word finding is more
images with objects, events, and ideas. Guided imag- difficult for less imageable objects and verbs than
ery is often used to support meditative practices and more imageable ones.
can be used in counseling and coaching contexts to While hypnosis, like meditation, can be used to
help people consider in realistic ways what might be help let go of troubling thoughts, it is based on the
possible in their lives. As such, including visualiza- notion of suggestibility, a person’s ability and will-
tion practices in other holistic approaches to neuro- ingness to achieve trance states. Stage hypnotists,
genic communication challenges, and to challenges while they may be entertaining and raise public
faced by care partners, may be beneficial. awareness of hypnosis, have been especially dam-
Another potential use for visualization tech- aging to the credibility associated with hypnosis in
niques is to boost naming abilities in people with medical, rehabilitation, and mental health contexts.
word-finding problems. Although not necessar- Despite this, scholarship in hypnotic applications in
ily specified within many documented treatment these areas outside of speech-language pathology
424  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

has continued to advance, with increased support • screening for spiritual and religious care
from psychophysiological and neuroimaging data options as expressed by individuals and
(Halsband et al., 2009; Sansone, 2014). Thompson families
et al. (1986) trialed a combination of visualization • supporting communication goals related to
with hypnosis in an object-naming training task, religion and spirituality
reasoning that hypnosis-induced relaxation would • incorporating treatment materials and content
enhance the ability of a person with aphasia to visu- that may be of a religious or spiritual nature
alize during treatment. Results were mixed and not when these are desired by our clients and
robust. Today, however, with advances in treatment relevant to treatment goals
methods, hypnotic sciences, and outcomes assess- • recognizing how beliefs influence how the
ment, future work in this area may be promising. people we serve conceive of their disabilities,
limitations, strengths, and opportunities
• supporting communication access for
What Are the Potential Roles of Religion engaging in spiritual and religious activities,
and Spirituality in Acquired Neurogenic for example, in the context of counseling,
palliative care, and end-of-life decision-
Communication Disorders?
making and conversations
• collaborating with related health care
In Chapter 5, we recognized that religious beliefs professionals, such as chaplains
and spiritual practices are essential components • making appropriate referrals
of holistic care and are included in the Interna-
tional Classification of Functioning, Disability, and
Health (ICF; WHO, 2001) as activity/participation How Might Natural Product Use
and environmental factors. In Chapter 16, we rec- Be Relevant to People With
ognized the crucial role of religious and spiritual
Cognitive-Linguistic Challenges?
beliefs and traditions on models of health care and
access to care across cultures and regions of the
world. In Chapter 27, we considered how religion Natural product use includes the use of herbs and
and spirituality are important in our consider- nutritional supplements. (Of course, nutrition in
ations of our roles as counselors and life coaches. general matters, as discussed in Chapter 6.) Addi-
We reviewed how religion and spirituality have an tional general complementary approaches include
important bearing on one’s locus of control, which is traditional Chinese and Ayurvedic medicine, home-
highly relevant to their role in intervention. We also opathy, naturopathy, and traditional healers (most
acknowledged that excellent clinicians demonstrate of these having overlap in terms of how they are
appreciation for others’ beliefs, exhibit tolerance defined, and methods used). Of course, there are
for beliefs that might be contrary to our own, and numerous variations and complexities within each
support the people with whom we work in ways of the categories of practice mentioned.
that do not violate their core cultural, spiritual, or Some natural products have been said to help
religious tenets. slow cognitive decline or prevent dementia. How-
Many health-related fields, especially nursing, ever, the evidence base for this to date is still grow-
primary care, osteopathic medicine, occupational ing. Consumption of omega-3 fatty acids has been
therapy, and social work, are well ahead of speech- reported in observational studies to slow cognitive
language pathology in recognizing the need to decline. A Cochrane review of three randomized
address spirituality in everyday clinical practice controlled trials (Sydenham et al., 2012) did not
(Chahda et al., 2021; Mathisen et al., 2015). Although demonstrate a significant benefit in terms of cogni-
it may be beyond our scope of practice to engage tive functioning in older people who did not have
in some aspects of religious and spiritual care, at dementia.
a minimum, we do have the following important Hecht (2008) offers a succinct review of herbal
responsibilities: treatments used to address cognitive disorders
28. Complementary and Integrative Approaches   425

in people with Alzheimer’s disease and vascular


dementia. Ginkgo biloba, a well-known supplement,
Why Are Complementary and Integrative
has also been recommended for brain health and Approaches Increasing in Popularity?
prevention of dementia in older adults (DeKosky
et al., 2008; Mahadevan & Park, 2008). A 6-year trial Several trends seem to be working together to
in over 3,000 adults did not show consistent benefits increase global interest in the search for complemen-
in terms of cognitive stability of preventive indica- tary and integrative approaches to neurogenic com-
tors for stroke such as blood pressure maintenance munication disorders. These include frustration with
and hypertension (DeKosky et al., 2008). A system- current options, increasing awareness, expanded
atic review of Ginkgo biloba extracts administered to funding for nonallopathic services, a growing evi-
people with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dence base, and aggressive commercial marketing.
dementia support overall improvements in cogni-
tion, neuropsychiatric symptoms, and activities
of daily living, especially with high daily dosages Frustration With Current Options
(Zhang et al., 2016). Still, caution is warranted, espe-
cially in light of potential side effects (NCCIH, 2020). A lack of steady improvement according to perceived
Additional examples of herbs that have been cognitive-communicative needs (or, in the case of neu-
examined for possible effectiveness in treating peo- rodegenerative conditions, the continued progression
ple with neurogenic communication disorders are of symptoms) motivates many people with acquired
given in Table 28–1. Note that the fact that certain neurological disorders and caregivers to seek alter-
herbs have been studied does not at all mean that native solutions. Relatedly, the many limitations of
their use is recommended. medically based health care systems (discussed in

Table 28–1. Examples of Herbs That Have Been Examined for Possible Effectiveness in Treating
People With Neurogenic Communication Disorders

Target Conditions or
Symptoms Addressed
Herbal Treatment in Published Reports Corresponding Citations
Bacopa monniera, an Ayurvedic herb Memory and mental Hecht, 2008
illness
Huperzine alpha, also called Huperzia Memory and cognition Hecht, 2008; Xing et al., 2014;
serrata in people with Xu et al., 1995; Yue et al.,
Alzheimer’s disease 2012; Zhang et al., 2002
Jie Yu Dan Aphasia Zhang et al., 2018
Moxibustion, jihwangeumja, Aphasia Jung et al., 2012
cheongshinhaeeo-tang,
seonghyangjeongkisan, and
cheongshindodam-tang (Korean herbs)
Sailuotiong, a Chinese medicine formula Cognitive abilities in Liang et al., 2014
of panax ginseng, ginkgo biloba, and vascular dementia
crocus sativus
Vinpocetine, derived from Vinca minor Memory in dementia Balestreri et al., 1987; Hecht,
(lesser periwinkle plant) 2008
Note. This listing in no way constitutes a recommendation for use. These are merely examples of herbs that have
been studied in the relevant research literature. Citations are provided for readers wishing to learn more about them.
426  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Section IV) are leading many to consider new possi- Individuals are often not swayed as much by re-
bilities for treatments that do not necessarily fit into search findings as they are by their own beliefs and
the traditional frameworks of health care or Western hopes, and their personal perceptions of benefits.
models of support for health and well-being.

Aggressive Marketing
Increasing Awareness
Vigorous marketing on the part of companies sell-
Due to increasing popular media exposure, public ing goods and services, most of them unregulated, is
education, and commercial promotion of alternative reaching increasing numbers of people with neuro-
and complementary approaches, more people are genic communication disorders and their caregivers.
made aware of potential treatment options. As we discuss further in this chapter, what consum-
ers are paying for in many cases may be hope rather
than actual demonstrable benefits.
Expanded Funding

Coverage for nonallopathic, preventive, and wellness-​ What Is the Status of the
focused approaches through many national health Evidence Base Supporting Alternative
care systems is on the rise. Approaches to Improving
Cognitive-Communicative Abilities?
Increasing Evidence
Summarizing research results from studies specifi-
Despite the overall challenges summarized in cally addressing the effects of complementary and
Box 28–1, the research base supporting the use of alternative types of intervention to address neuro-
some complementary approaches is ever-growing. genic cognitive-communicative disorders is chal-

Box
28–1 Challenges in Many Research Studies on Complementary
and Integrative Approaches to Intervention

• Insufficient sample sizes


• Heterogeneity of samples studied
• Poor control of cognitive and linguistic measures
• Lack of control groups
• Lack of placebo or sham groups and conditions
• Lack of randomization
• Use of nonstandardized measures
• Use of subjective descriptions without objective measures
• Lack of overall detail in published reports
• Failure to report and describe additional behavioral, pharmacological,
or nontraditional interventions being provided
• Failure to report dosage, intensity, and frequency of treatment
• Failure to address possible underlying neurobiological causes for
reported effectiveness

Note: Many of these challenges are also noted in research on cognitive-linguistic inter-
vention methods for people with acquired neurogenic communication disorders.
28. Complementary and Integrative Approaches   427

lenging, in light of the blurred boundaries between niques, or acupuncture, for example, be sure that
what might be considered relevant to cognition you make referrals, or simply acknowledge that such
and communication in the context of the measures topics are beyond the boundaries of your expertise.
reported in many studies. Many published studies Do not practice in any area that is not within the
addressing alternative and complementary treat- scope of practice as defined by any licensing or reg-
ments related to stroke, TBI, and dementia, for ulatory agency related to your professional role.
example, do not include specific indices of cognitive
and linguistic performance. An additional challenge
is that many of the studies are published in non-​ Engage Only in Methods You Are Trained
English languages and so are not accessible to a large in and Competent to Carry Out
proportion of the scientific and clinical readership
(Tatsuya et al., 2016). Liu et al. (2015), for example, Even if a certain approach might be defended as fit-
searched all systematic reviews on acupuncture and ting within your scope of practice, if you have not
stroke using a combination of major Chinese- and had appropriate education and training to do it as a
English-language databases and found that 90.7% professional, then it is unethical to provide it.
(42 of a total of 49 published from 2001 to 2014) were
in Chinese.
Limitations of the existing research in this gen- Emphasize Complementary Over Alternative
eral area are summarized in Box 28–1. Such limita- Approaches to Direct Intervention for
tions are not uncommon in many areas of clinical
Communication and Cognition
research, including much of research related to apha-
siology. Still, they have led some to campaign against
the use of taxpayer support for research on alterna- As acknowledged throughout this book, there is
tive and complementary approaches (Mielczarek & a vast and growing evidence base supporting the
Engler, 2012). Given that there is mounting evidence work that we do to improve communication and
that some complementary methods may augment life participation in people with neurogenic com-
the effectiveness of our work, and the possibility that munication disorders. We know that the potential
some methods now considered “alternative” may benefits from SLP services can continue for years
work their way into our everyday clinical practice, following onset. Consider whether recommending
further research with improved methods is needed in alternative approaches instead of speech-language
this important arena. Although many approaches lack intervention may run counter to the evidence base
consensus on methodological appropriateness and and thus be unethical. Keep in mind that in reported
standardization of intervention, several hold promise. studies where approaches such as acupuncture and
herbal medicine have been studied as adjuncts ver-
sus alternatives to speech and language intervention
in aphasia, the combined approaches have led to bet-
How Might SLPs Support People Considering ter results (Huang et al., 2020; Jung et al., 2012; Pang
Complementary and Alternative Approaches et al., 2010).
to Cognitive-Communicative Wellness?

General guidelines for SLPs in the area of comple- Keep an Open, Nonjudgmental Attitude
mentary and alternative approaches are summa- and Appreciate Multicultural Differences
rized here.
Given our own cultural perspectives and life experi-
ences, we may sometimes be shocked or amazed to
Stay Within Your Scope of Practice learn what other people believe. As excellent clini-
cians, it is important that we filter our biased responses
This is a vital principle in all of the work we do. If about the options people might be considering and
you are asked to provide herbal remedies, yoga tech- serve as vehicles for multicultural understanding.
428  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Encourage Caution When Counseling conditions. In a position statement, the FDA (2013)
People Considering Alternative and warns consumers against use of HBOT for off-label
Complementary Approaches purposes. In a Cochrane Library systematic review
of 11 studies of people in the acute state following
stroke, Bennett et al. (2014) found little evidence of
Although we may not be expert in the mechanisms any functional gains compared to control groups.
and methods associated with specific alternative and Most other studies not included in that review
complementary approaches, we may from time to are case studies, and most share many of the chal-
time be asked to weigh in on decisions being made lenges listed in Box 28–1. The authors note that high
about whether people with neurogenic communica- dosages of oxygen “may increase oxidative stress
tion disorders should pursue them. As long as we through the production of oxygen free radical spe-
are clear about the limitations of our expertise as it cies and is potentially toxic” (p. 5). They also caution
relates to any type of treatment under discussion, that “HBOT is associated with some risk of adverse
it is important that we share what is known about effects, including damage to the ears, sinuses and
the evidence base associated with particular types lungs from the effects of pressure, temporary wors-
of intervention. ening of shortsightedness, claustrophobia and oxy-
In my clinical practice in the United States, gen poisoning” (p. 5). To this list, the FDA (2013)
by far the most common inquiry I have had about adds the risk of paralysis.
alternative and complementary practice relates to In another Cochrane Library systematic review
whether or not hyperbaric oxygen therapy (HBOT) of studies testing the effectiveness of HBOT for peo-
can help stroke and brain injury survivors and peo- ple with vascular dementia (in which only one of
ple with dementia and improve their cognitive or many studies examined met criteria for the review),
communicative abilities. HBOT is a method that has Xiao et al. (2012) report that insufficient evidence
been touted by some, especially those marketing is available to support the use of HBOT with that
HBOT services and equipment, as holding promise population. They also expressed concern regarding
for people with cognitive-linguistic deficits due to the lack of information reported concerning safety
stroke and brain injury (as well as a host of other and possible adverse effects. What’s more, there is
conditions). no peer-reviewed empirical evidence to date that
HBOT involves immersing an individual in a HBOT enhances any aspect of cognition or com-
sealed tank while raising the atmospheric pressure munication in any clinical group. Sarno et al. (1972)
so that oxygen is forced into their bodily tissues at studied the effect of HBOT on language and cogni-
a rate up to three times greater than under normal tive abilities of people with aphasia due to stroke.
air pressure. HBOT may be effective for some condi- They conclude that “results revealed a total lack of
tions. For example, the U.S. Food and Drug Admin- treatment effect” (p. 14).
istration (FDA) has approved it for use in carbon Despite the evident lack of effectiveness, peo-
monoxide poisoning, decompression sickness, and ple with acquired neurological conditions and their
thermal burns. caregivers are commonly bombarded with com-
The rationale for HBOT use following stroke mercial literature, online sales material, and tele-
is that since stroke reduces the oxygen supply to marketing touting potential HBOT benefits related
the brain, infusing the brain with more oxygen will to cognitive-communicative abilities, among other
be helpful and perhaps reduce the extent of per- alleged benefits. They are encouraged to attend fre-
manent damage in acute stages following stroke. quent HBOT sessions at treatment centers and also
The rationale for use in vascular dementia is that to purchase systems for home use. Agencies provid-
patients’ brains are hypoperfused; HBOT might ing HBOT are often staffed by physicians and other
improve blood supply, supporting better cognitive licensed professionals, which boosts the perception
functioning. that the treatment must be appropriate relative to
According to the FDA, the safety and effective- their needs. I personally know several people who
ness of HBOT have not been established for demen- have been told directly by salespeople that HBOT
tia, stroke, or brain injury, or for a host of other will enhance their recovery of language and/or cog-
28. Complementary and Integrative Approaches   429

nitive abilities. This is the selling of hope at a very was no ginseng at all in most supplements labeled
high cost — a cost not only in the financial sense but as ginseng) and/or to contain ingredients not listed
also in terms of possible foregoing of other treat- on the labels (e.g., rice, wheat, beans, and other
ments with greater potential benefit, as well as expo- botanical and nonbotanical fillers were found in the
sure to unwarranted risks. products without any indication of these as ingredi-
The food supplement and herbal medicine mar- ents on the label). An additional concern about herbs
ket is another that merits healthy skepticism, even and nutritional supplements is the common yet false
though some products in this category may be bene- assumption that they cannot cause any harm. In fact,
ficial. Natural supplements are largely unregulated some have potent active ingredients and can lead to
globally and inconsistently regulated even in coun- side effects if taken with certain prescription medi-
tries with strict federal guidelines for food and drug cations. Also, some have been found to contain toxic
safety and effectiveness. People selling such prod- substances such as lead, mercury, and arsenic (Saper
ucts do not necessarily have consumers’ best inter- et al., 2008).
ests at heart. DNA testing reveals that many herbal In sum, it is important that SLPs support peo-
supplements sold have questionable ingredients. ple with acquired neurogenic impairments by advo-
The Office of the New York Attorney General (2015) cating for judicious considerations of treatment
delivered a disturbing report that 79% of herbal sup- adjuncts and alternatives. Let’s stay informed about
plements sold at four major national retailers in 16 such options and let the people we serve know about
regions in the state of New York were found to lack the evidence base supporting them (or, in the case of
the substance indicated on their labels (e.g., there HBOT, not supporting them).

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter a. In what might this be (or not be) the case
that are new to you or that you have not yet for you?
mastered. b. Discuss with one or more colleagues how
2. Compare and contrast the terms alternative, their own experience in complementary
complementary, integrative, and nontraditional and integrative practices in their own self-
as they might be used in the context care might influence their involvement
of intervention for cognitive-linguistic in the types of practice mentioned in this
disorders. chapter.
3. How are complementary methods that c. Do an online search for a few nutritional
promote stress reduction and relaxation supplements and mind-body treatments
related to clinical practice in aphasiology? said to help improve cognitive and
4. Is it possible to define clear boundaries linguistic abilities.
between what is and what is not in the SLP d. What sorts of misleading sales tactics do
scope of practice relative to alternative and you notice?
complementary methods for promoting e. How might you advise people with
life participation in people with neurogenic neurogenic communication disorders and
communication disorders? their families to consider advertising for
a. If so, describe those boundaries. such products with skepticism?
b. If not, why not? 6. Give specific examples of harm that may
5. Marshall and Basilakos (2014) suggest arise from using products and services that
that, as SLPs, we are more comfortable are not regulated and that have not been
recommending complementary practices with thoroughly studied in terms of safety and
which we have had personal experience. efficacy.
430  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

7. Given what is known about the lack of challenges associated with MCI, dementia,
efficacy of HBOT for treatment of cognitive TBI, or stroke?
and linguistic challenges, why do many
people continue to use HBOT with hopes For additional learning and reflection activities,
www
that it will lessen their communication see the companion website.
SECTION VIII

Specific Treatment Approaches


432  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

In Section VI, we considered important principles we consider approaches for fostering compensatory
and theories that underlie intervention for neuro- strategies in communication. Chapter 30 includes
genic cognitive-linguistic disorders. In Section VII, approaches aimed at enhancing expressive language
we addressed many types of general approaches to in particular. Chapter 31 is devoted to methods for
treatment and the research base supporting them. enhancing word finding and lexical processing.
In this section, we delve into a variety of what we Treatment methods for improving syntax are dis-
call “specific” treatment approaches. By “specific,” cussed in Chapter 32. Finally, in Chapter 33, we
we mean that they have an actual title or name, review methods to help with recovery of writing
they entail carrying out characteristic activities, and and reading abilities. Not all treatment methods fit
there is a dedicated literature (be it large or small, clearly within one chapter heading. For example,
strong or weak) that informs our consideration of some verb-focused treatments are described in the
using them. chapter on naming treatments (Chapter 31). How-
The distinction between general and specific ever, working on verbs ideally helps improve gram-
approaches is not always a clear one. Some approaches matical processing and performance, so verb-focused
that do not have a very clearly defined set of steps to treatments might also fit nicely in the list of methods
carry out, such as constraint-induced language ther- geared toward syntax in Chapter 32.
apy (CILT), are described here as specific approaches; In each of the methods reviewed here, we con-
they could also be considered general. Some of those sider systematically what defines the method, who
we classified in Section VII as general approaches, is most likely to benefit from it, the associated prin-
such as spaced retrieval training (SRT), could be con- ciples and rationale for the method, the specific steps
sidered specific. As the excellent clinician that you or procedures entailed in carrying out the method,
are (or are becoming), your flexibility and creativity and the status of the evidence base supporting (or
will help you organize your own learning, thoughts, not supporting) our use of the method. Methods
and ideas about treatment approaches in a way that used across different studies pertaining to any given
makes sense to you. approach tend to vary. Also, not all descriptions pro-
Most of the approaches in this section were vided in the research literature are detailed enough
developed primarily for people with aphasia. This to enable us to replicate exactly how a certain treat-
does not mean they are only relevant to aphasia. ment protocol was carried out for a given study. As
There is a great need for extending the evidence base always, if you are seeking to adhere to treatment
to support applying some of the methods described fidelity for research purposes, it is important that
in this section to people with other types of neu- you refer to the published work detailing the explicit
rogenic cognitive-linguistic disorders. Each chap- steps you wish to replicate. Otherwise, it is import-
ter in this section has a theme based on the types ant to learn the steps and the rationale for each, tai-
of treatment goals most commonly associated with loring your applications in a practice-based evidence
the treatment methods we explore. In Chapter 29, framework (see Chapter 23).
CHAPTER
29
Specific Approaches for Promoting
Compensatory Communication Strategies

Recall that in Chapter 25 we reviewed many general ing awareness of ways to work on communication
approaches focused on helping people compensate goals at the level of conversation.
for their communication challenges through sup- Despite the development of additional methods
ported conversation, augmentative and alternative focused on social communication, life participation,
communication (AAC), apps, software, and other functional communication, and discourse since that
forms of technology. In this chapter, we review a time, PACE remains an important treatment method
set of more specific compensatory approaches: Pro- to this day. (Note that the use of “aphasics” in the
moting Aphasics’ Communicative Effectiveness name of this method is reflective of the era in which
(PACE), Communication Drawing Program (CPD), it was developed; were it today, the authors would
Back to the Drawing Board (BDB), and Visual Action probably have taken a person-centered approach by
Therapy (VAT). After reading and reflecting on the calling it something like Promoting Effective Com-
content in this chapter, you will ideally be able to munication in People with Aphasia. Still, the PACE
answer, in your own words, the following queries acronym is widely used among aphasiologists and
about each of these approaches: thus is likely here to stay.)

1. What is it?
2. On what principles is it based?
On What Principles Is PACE Treatment Based?
3. How is it implemented?
4. What is its status in terms of evidence-based
practice? PACE is based on principles that differentiate it from
most other approaches in significant ways:

What Is Promoting Aphasics’ • Equal participation: The client and the


clinician take equal turns sending and
Communicative Effectiveness (PACE)?
receiving messages; during any given
exchange, one is considered the sender and
Promoting Aphasics’ Communicative Effective- the other the receiver.
ness (PACE) (Davis, 1980; Davis & Wilcox, 1985) is a • New information: The stimulus to be
method developed to foster pragmatic skills, accord- described is not seen by the receiver; this
ing to its authors. It was introduced at a time when ensures that there is true, not simulated,
the treatment literature was focused much more on information exchange.
decontextualized impairment-level goals and tasks, • Free choice of modalities: When in the role
so its introduction played an important role in rais- of sender, the client and clinician determine

433
434  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

the communication mode that each will use to possible, another option is to have so many
convey the message. cards prepared that you would not be likely
• Natural feedback: Feedback consists to easily guess which one the person has
simply of the clinician’s or client’s responses selected.
regarding whether each message was • Face the client.
successfully sent or received; communicative • Discuss with the client the goal of successful
success is considered a more natural form communication rather than linguistic
of feedback shared between clinician and accuracy. Adapt your means of expression
client than declarations of linguistic accuracy as needed. Using supported communication
(Davis, 2005). strategies, give examples of how the client
might convey a concept through speaking,
The goal in PACE exchanges is successful com- gesture, sign, facial expressions, pantomime,
munication, not accuracy of phonology, morphol- writing, or drawing. Explain how you will
ogy, syntax, and not necessarily success through any take turns sending and receiving messages.
particular mode of communication. • Decide who will take the first turn as sender.
Consider how many communication treat- • The sender picks a card and hides it from the
ment approaches entail the clinician playing the receiver’s view. A barrier may be used, or the
role of teacher or expert who already knows the card may be held under the table or placed
answers to questions being asked or problems being face down on the table.
addressed — that is, in many approaches, there is • The sender uses any modality they choose to
not equal participation, and new information is not convey what is represented on the card.
exchanged. In this sense, PACE differs from many • The receiver gives feedback about what was
other approaches. The free choice of modalities may understood. In some cases, the sender simply
be especially helpful for focusing on alternatives says a correct word, the receiver merely
to spoken language as a means of communication. repeats it, and then both look at the card
Thus, PACE may be considered a total communica- and affirm the communicative success. In
tion approach, as described in Chapter 25. other cases, the sender may use nonspeech
modalities, and more guessing is required
on the part of the receiver. This may be
followed by repeated attempts and ongoing
How Is PACE Treatment Implemented?
feedback.
• In the role of receiver, you may request
Here is how the PACE approach is carried out: the use of certain modalities and may also
request different types of written or spoken
• Obtain a set of stimulus cards (words and/or expression, such as physical description of
pictures). objects, object categories, or functional uses
• Place the deck of stimulus cards facedown for objects depicted. Expand on the client’s
on a table. It is important that you not know utterance to acknowledge the successful
what is represented on the cards. Given what components of communication initiated.
we know about the importance of ecological Keep in mind that your feedback is to focus
validity for maximizing carryover to real- on effective communication, not on accuracy.
world contexts, it is important that the stimuli In the role of sender, you may choose
be relevant to the client. Thus, the stimuli descriptions rather than naming, and you may
must be selected with their goals and interests choose varied modalities of expression.
in mind. This may be challenging, as stimulus • Keep taking equal turns until the sender
selection is ideally done by someone other has successfully communicated what is
than the clinician so that the clinician has represented on the card.
no idea what the stimuli are. If that is not • Reverse roles as sender and receiver.
29. Specific Approaches for Promoting Compensatory Communication Strategies   435

To score PACE communication attempts, a scale and objects. Complexity (see the description of
from 0 to 5 may be applied. See the scoring sum- CATE in Chapter 24) may be emphasized strategi-
mary in Table 29–1. Clinicians may wish to monitor cally, and its effects on an individual’s performance
or score other aspects of communicative attempts and carryover may be monitored. Springer et al.
as well, such as how well the client responds as (1991) reported that adding a semantic classification
receiver. task to traditional PACE treatment enhanced the
If it is important to maintain treatment fidelity effectiveness of the approach when applied to four
in terms of the method as described by its authors, people with aphasia.
such as for a research study, the clinician should A benefit of PACE that I have personally found
encourage the client to engage in any form of com- in clinical practice is the empowerment of family
munication when in the role of sender (i.e., speak- members and friends to aid in establishing collec-
ing, gesture, sign, facial expressions, pantomime, tions of meaningful stimuli. Significant others often
writing, or drawing). However, if treatment fidelity want to help in meaningful ways, yet feel powerless
is not essential, liberties may be taken by focusing to do so. Asking them to assemble pictures, objects,
on the use of specific modalities or combinations of and words that represent real-world interests and
modalities. For example, a person with severe ano- everyday experiences of the client gives them a
mia may have a goal of using written cues and ges- meaningful role to play. Having others assemble the
tures to facilitate expression; in this case, they may be materials also helps preserve the principle of con-
asked to only use those strategies and not to speak at veying new information by assuring the clinician’s
all when sending messages. Another variation that lack of prior knowledge of what the stimuli are.
may be implemented is to use stimuli other than PACE was developed for people with aphasia,
picture and word cards. For example, real objects, primarily those with word-finding challenges, but
video clips, and computer-generated images, words, may be used with people who have goals related
or phrases may serve as the stimuli, as long as the to turn-taking and other aspects of pragmatics, and
clinician does not know their contents. goals related to any of myriad expressive and recep-
Difficulty of PACE tasks may be manipulated tive linguistic abilities (Pulvermüller & Roth, 1991).
by using phrases or sentences of varying complex- PACE treatment may be easily adapted for use in
ity, pictures of complex scenes rather than simple communication partner training (Newhoff et al.,
objects, and low-frequency or less familiar words 1981) and group treatment (Elman, 2007a).

Table 29–1. Summary of PACE Scoring Scale for Client as Sender

Response Description Score


Message conveyed on first attempt 5
Message conveyed after general feedback (indicating the first attempt 4
was not completely understood)
Message conveyed after specific feedback from the clinician 3
Message partially conveyed by the client, only after general and specific 2
feedback have been attempted
Message not conveyed appropriately despite efforts by the client and 1
clinician
Client does not attempt to convey the message 0
Unscorable response due to violation of one of the four principles U
Sources: Adapted from Davis, 1980; Davis & Wilcox, 1985.
436  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

ent in PACE simulates real conversation, its structure


What Is the Status of PACE in Terms makes it such that it does not constitute actual con-
of Evidence-Based Practice? versation (Davis, 2005). As always, in clinical prac-
tice, it is important to index the gains in terms of life
Overall, outcomes research for PACE suggests that it participation goals for each person.
enhances communicative effectiveness in people with
aphasia. Challenges in documented treatment efficacy
to date include small numbers of participants within What Is the Communicative
studies and variations in participant inclusion and
Drawing Program?
exclusion criteria (e.g., type, severity, and etiology of
aphasia), treatment dosage, control for personal rele-
vance and complexity of stimuli, and the type of feed- The Communicative Drawing Program (CDP)
back provided by the clinician to the client. focuses on the use of drawing as a compensatory
Means of indexing outcomes associated with means of communication (Helm-Estabrooks et al.,
the method have also varied across studies. Some, 2014). It is intended for people with severe apha-
for example, have included indices of communica- sia who are limited in oral and written language
tion during role-play, expressive language scores in expression. It is based on and similar to the Back
aphasia batteries, storytelling, picture naming, and to the Drawing Board approach, the next approach
picture description abilities. Li et al. (1988) reported described in this chapter.
that although PACE treatment participants in their
study did not improve in word finding, they did
improve in effective circumlocutions and in pro-
On What Principles Is CDP Based?
viding multiple attempts at communicating con-
tent. Carlomagno et al. (1991) reported that PACE
participants improved their expression of relevant An underlying principle of CDP is that drawing is
content and decreased irrelevant content in referen- intrinsically nonlinguistic and so may be useful even
tial tasks; they also improved in storytelling but did in people with severe aphasia (Farias et al., 2006).
not improve in picture description abilities. Kurland Some have argued that drawing exploits use of the
et al. (2012) reported that two people with chronic intact right hemisphere (e.g., Farias et al., 2006) to
aphasia and apraxia of speech improved in naming facilitate word retrieval, although the assumptions
ability following PACE treatment but made greater underlying that argument have been contested (e.g.,
and more rapid gains during constraint-induced Gainotti, 2015). In any case, CDP was developed pri-
aphasia therapy (CIAT, discussed in Chapter 30). marily as a compensatory approach to communica-
However, in a more recent study involving 24 partic- tion. Overall, the authors of the original work on CDP
ipants with aphasia (Kurland et al., 2016), differences do not provide a strong theoretical basis other than
in the effectiveness of a constraint-based approach the general notion that drawing training through a
compared to a modified form of PACE therapy were methodical series of prescribed steps may enhance
not significant. Avent et al. (1995) and Kurland et al. communication in people with severe aphasia.
(2012, 2016) emphasize that individuals respond dif-
ferently to PACE treatment; indexing progress for
each person is important.
How Is CDP Implemented?
In sum, there has been little conformity in treat-
ment fidelity and outcomes assessment, and not
all people with aphasia benefit equally from this CDP is carried out in 10 steps, which are spelled out
approach. Still, benefits may be more far-reaching in detail by Helm-Estabrooks et al. (2014). Steps are
than documented to date if one takes into account summarized here.
the empowering nature of natural feedback and
equal roles of sender and receiver entailed. Keep in 1. Have the client identify and recognize
mind that although the turn-taking interaction inher- categories of objects. This step is intended
29. Specific Approaches for Promoting Compensatory Communication Strategies   437

to ensure “semantic-conceptual knowledge” appropriate and recognizable objects are


(Helm-Estabrooks et al., 2014, p. 392). Show drawn 100% of the time, as judged by a person
a set of 10 pictures, five of which belong to who does not know what the items to be
one semantic category (e.g., tools, vegetables, represented are.
furniture) and the other five (foils) representing 7. Name an object on which you have been
a diverse collection of other types of items. working. Ask the client to draw it. Do this until
Do not mention the name of the category to the drawing is appropriate and recognizable
the client. Simply ask the client to “circle the 100% of the time as judged by a person
objects that belong together.” Do this until the who does not know what the items to be
client can select the five that go together for at represented are. Do this for all 10 of the items
least five different category sets. used in the preceding step.
2. Provide the client 12 color markers and ask 8. Tell the individual a category name and ask that
the client to color black-and-white line-drawn the client to draw an item in that category. Do
objects. The objects should have clear target this for 10 categories (e.g., tools, transportation,
colors (e.g., banana, pea, carrot). This activity and furniture). Do this until 10 appropriate
may be supplemented as needed with the use and recognizable objects are drawn 100% of the
of real objects or colored pictures to enhance time, as judged by a person who does not know
“knowledge of object color properties” (p. 281). what the items to be represented are.
Do this until nine objects are colored correctly. 9. Ask the person to draw as many items
3. Have the client trace around the contour of black- within a category as the client can, without
and-white line drawings. The authors suggest any examples presented. Work as needed on
that this step helps clients recognize items improvements that would make the drawings
from their “outer configuration” (p. 281). Do interpretable in terms of what they are meant
this until the client conforms to the basic shape to represent. Do this until 6 to 10 recognizable
without intersecting the lines in the drawings. drawings are completed.
4. Ask the person to copy the following geometric 10. Have the client draw one-, two-, and three-
shapes: crescent, oval, star, octagon, cone, paneled cartoons representing a story or joke.
pyramid, cylinder, and cube. This is to help First, instruct the client to point out what is
the client work on drawing images of the funny about a one-paneled cartoon. Second,
correct relative size and shape that also convey ask the client to remember the picture; take it
three-dimensional aspects. If needed, color the away and then have the client draw it from
objects to provide additional cues. Do this until memory. If the picture drawn is not adequate,
the client can draw all eight shapes in proper try a simpler version for practice. For two- and
proportion. three-paneled cartoons, the individual must
5. Provide pictures of objects with missing parts draw the panels in the correct order to convey
(such as a car missing a wheel, a cat missing an a logical sequence of events. To complete this
eye, or a horse missing a leg). Ask the person to step, a nonbiased judge must be able to identify
fill in the missing parts using a black pen. This is all aspects of the picture that are necessary to
to help foster attention to the features of objects. understand the joke or story.
The specific number of items to be presented is
not mentioned by the authors. When all items in
the set of items you are using are complete and What Is the Status of the CDP in
accurate, move on to the next step.
Terms of Evidence-Based Practice?
6. Show the individual a picture and then
take it away. Ask the person to draw the
picture. This is to work on drawing from Several other authors have contributed to the evi-
“stored representations” (p. 282). Do this for dence base underlying the use of drawing to sup-
10 different pictures. Work on each picture port communication in people with aphasia (e.g.,
up to three times if needed. Continue until Lyon, 1995; Lyon & Helm-Estabrooks, 1987; Morgan
438  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

& Helm-Estabrooks, 1987; Rao, 1995; Sacchett, 2002; A salient distinction between the Sacchett et al.
Sacchett et al., 1999; Wallace et al., 2014). Although (1999) approach and CDP is the focus on the com-
studies employing drawing as part of a total com- municative effectiveness of drawing over accuracy.
munication approach abound, carefully controlled The authors reported that in a 12-week program,
studies of CDP in particular are lacking. Benefits of recognizable generative drawings improved, and
CDP are said to be enhanced accuracy of drawing for these improvements were maintained 6 weeks fol-
conveying of content and thus enhanced communi- lowing treatment. Caregiver interviews suggested
cation (Helm-Estabrooks et al., 2014). generalization to spontaneous conversation. Gener-
When considering drawing-focused approaches, alization is indeed an important factor to consider,
it is important to take individual preferences into as many people with aphasia tend not to use draw-
account. Appreciate that not all people who might ings spontaneously in conversation even when they
benefit from such an approach enjoy drawing; some have been trained to do so (Lyon, 1995). Lyon (1995)
may not want to participate in these sorts of activi- draws attention to an aspect of the interactive nature
ties. Also, many in the target population do not have of communicative drawing that is not captured in
functional use of their dominant hand for drawing common language treatment outcomes measures:
and may become frustrated, especially with the level “Interacting through common focus, reciprocal
of exactness (with high-accuracy criteria for moving turn taking, and the shared experience of building
from one step to another) required for this approach. a drawing together constitutes success, even if its
Other limitations that might affect individual enjoy- value is simply mutual satisfaction” (p. 87).
ment as well as performance include visual acuity,
visual attention, visuospatial abilities, the ability to
associate drawn objects with semantic representa-
tions and remember those associations, and the ability What Is Back to the Drawing Board?
to generate drawings based on verbal names alone.
The focus on accuracy of drawing as opposed to Back to the Drawing Board (BDB) (Morgan &
the communicative content conveyed has been noted Helm-Estabrooks, 1987) is an approach much like
by some as a challenge with this approach. One need CDP and was developed before the publication of
not be a good artist to convey a message. With peo- CDP as a specific treatment method. Like CDP, it is
ple who have severe aphasia, conveying a message intended for use with people with severe aphasia.
is likely to be far more important than drawing accu-
rately (Sacchett, 2002; Sacchett et al., 1999). Relatedly,
providing more detail than needed to convey a mes-
sage results in inefficiency of communication. On What Principles Is BDB Treatment Based?
Sacchett et al. (1999) tested a similar approach
entailing fewer steps with seven people with severe
chronic aphasia. The aims, as stated, were to BDB is based on the same principles as described for
the CDP. As with CDP, treatment goals may include
• improve the ability to “draw generatively, in answering questions; requesting assistance, objects,
other words, to think of an idea, call up its or information; or sharing information, by way of
visual representation and translate this into a drawing rather than speaking. Treatment outcomes
drawing” (p. 269); may be indexed in terms of increased accuracy of
• “promote ‘economic’ drawing” (p. 269) by drawings (Peach, 2008).
focusing on the most important aspects to be
conveyed and only drawing those;
• improve the ability of the person with aphasia
How Is BDB Implemented?
to respond to feedback from a conversational
partner; and
• improve the conversational partner’s drawing Morgan and Helm-Estabrooks (1987) provided in-
interpretation skills. structions for guiding people with aphasia in a pro-
29. Specific Approaches for Promoting Compensatory Communication Strategies   439

cess of using sequential drawings to communicate


humorous content. The steps are as follows:
What Is the Status of BDB in Terms
of Evidence-Based Practice?
• Create or gather five uncaptioned humorous
cartoon panels. Single panels should be used Research on the approach is limited. Morgan and
to illustrate a single event, and multiple Helm-Estabrooks (1987) applied the approach with
panels should be used to illustrate sequential two people with severe aphasia. Independent judges
events. Start with single panels. assessed the accuracy of pre- and posttreatment
• Show the client the first one-panel cartoon drawings according to
for a short time and then take it away. Ask
the client to draw the cartoon from memory. • whether the gist of the cartoon was conveyed,
The criterion for an acceptable drawing is that • number of components to the sequence,
the drawing must be recognizable and must • number of objects named when described,
convey the humorous aspect. If the result and
is satisfactory, move to the next step. If not, • accuracy of the gender of the people
provide more instruction, demonstration, and represented in the drawings.
practice by copying.
• Provide the second cartoon panel and give four Both participants were said to improve in drawing
trials of drawing from memory. The criteria for accuracy as assessed by independent judges.
success are the same as for the previous step. Given the similarities between the CDP and
• Once the client successfully draws three of BDB, the discussion of the strengths and weaknesses
five single-cartoon panels from memory, of the BDB overall is captured in the discussion of
introduce two-panel cartoons. CDP. Like CDP, although BDB was designed for
• Once the client successfully draws three people with severe expressive language deficits, the
of five two-cartoon panels from memory, treatment program could be used to facilitate com-
introduce three-panel cartoons. munication in any person with aphasia as part of a
total communication approach.
The authors recommend that family members
and friends be included in training, not only about
drawing but about total communication and com-
What Is Visual Action Therapy?
munication support in general. Communication
partners may be taught strategies for asking ques-
tions to extract information about the drawings, for Visual Action Therapy (VAT) is a gesture-based
example, asking the individual to point to the most nonvocal approach, intended for people with global
important aspects of the drawings and encouraging aphasia, to promote the use of symbolic gestures to
multimodal expression. Progress is measured using communicate when language expression is severely
the individual’s drawings of “accidents of living” impaired (Peach, 2008). It is geared toward foster-
(Morgan & Helm-Estabrooks, 1987, p. 65). To do this, ing the use of symbolic gestures for stimuli that are
enact the following types of activities: not visually present (Helm-Estabrooks et al., 2014;
Helm-Estabrooks et al., 1982). VAT may be consid-
• single events (e.g., dropping a pencil on the ered a compensatory approach because it is used to
floor) support an alternative modality for expressive com-
• two-part events (e.g., shuffling cards and then munication. It could also be considered restitutive
dropping them) because studies have shown that stimulation pro-
• three-part events (e.g., writing with a pencil, vided in VAT enhances brain activity (Drummond,
breaking the pencil, and sharpening it) 2006). VAT treatment goals might include the ability
to pair gestures with commonly used items and the
Then ask the client to draw what was just witnessed. use of meaningful gestures in spontaneous social
Compare these drawings before and after treatment. interactions and expression of wants and needs.
440  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

There are three types (also called phases) of VAT: objects are a screwdriver, teaspoon, telephone,
paintbrush, and tea bag. Suggested B/F items
• Proximal Limb VAT (PL VAT) focuses on the are a whistle, flower, lollipop, drinking straw,
proximal limbs (extremities closer to the torso, and lip balm. Contextual props may also be
such as arms and legs) and relates to gross used (e.g., an actual drink for showing the use
motor skills (e.g., hitting a desk with a gavel). of a drinking straw or a teacup to show use of
• Distal Limb VAT (DL VAT) involves the distal a tea bag).
limbs (extremities farther away from the torso, • Have the client match pictures to objects. First,
such as fingers and toes) and relates to fine have the client place the objects on the pictures.
motor skills (e.g., dialing a telephone). Then have the client place the pictures on the
• Bucco-facial VAT (B/F VAT) incorporates objects. Next, show the pictures and have
facial gestures (e.g., drinking from a straw). the client point to the corresponding object.
Finally, present the objects and have the client
According to Helm-Estabrooks et al. (2014),
point to the corresponding pictures.
people with severe deficits in expression but intact
• As you show each object, have the client use
comprehension of speech and written language are
gestures to demonstrate how each object is
the best candidates for DL VAT; people with severely
used.
restricted verbal output but relatively good auditory
• Present an object and a corresponding picture.
skills are the best candidates for B/F VAT.
Have the client demonstrate how the object is
used.
• Show a group of objects. Gesture how one of
On What Principles Is VAT Treatment Based? them is used. Model gestures associated with
each object.
VAT is based on the principle that people with severe • Model the same gestures again, this time
language impairments often retain symbolic abili- asking the client to choose an object that goes
ties that underlie language use (Gardner et al., 1976; with your gesture.
Glass et al., 1973; Ramsberger & Helm-Estabrooks, • Show one object at a time and have the client
1988). Helm-Estabrooks et al. (2014) offer a detailed gesture its use.
description of the rationale that led to the approach. • Model a gesture (pantomime) associated with
each object, without the object in sight.
• Have the client request an object using only a
gesture. Give the client the item indicated.
How Is VAT Implemented? VAT scoring is determined as follows:
• 1 point for correct performance without
The same stimuli are used for all three types of VAT: hesitation or delay
objects, line drawings of those objects, and pic- • 0.5 points for a self-corrected and/or delayed
tures of those objects. Steps progress from match- response
ing objects and pictures to representing concepts • 0 for any other performance
through gestures without a corresponding physical
object being present. The steps are described slightly
differently between the original published descrip- What Is the Status of VAT in Terms
tion (Helm-Estabrooks et al., 1982) and a more recent of Evidence-Based Practice?
publication (Helm-Estabrooks et al., 2014). We con-
sider them briefly here:
Little research has been published regarding the
• Assemble 15 objects, line drawings of effects of specific VAT protocols as described by
those objects, and pictures of those objects. Helm-Estabrooks et al. (1982) and Helm-Estabrooks
Recommended PL VAT items are a flag, paint et al. (2014). Ramsberger and Helm-Estabrooks (1988)
stick, gavel, saw, and iron. Recommended DL used the B/F aspect of the program with six peo-
29. Specific Approaches for Promoting Compensatory Communication Strategies   441

ple with aphasia and, in their terms, “bucco-facial Raymer et al. (2006) did not implement the
apraxia.” They reported improvements not only in detailed protocol specified by Helm-Estabrooks and
pantomime use but also in verbal repetition and colleagues but supported the effectiveness of gesture
auditory comprehension, as indexed via the PICA paired with verbal training for nouns and verbs in
(Porch, 1967). Conlon and McNeil (1989) reported people with aphasia. They noted no carryover to
improvements according to PICA scores and ges- untrained words. Daumüller and Goldenberg (2010)
tural response scores in two people with global described a different modification of gesture-based
aphasia using VAT; they cautioned that generaliza- treatment and measured significant improvement in
tion to spontaneous use and untrained items was not use of gestures that were practiced. Some carryover
noted and that additional research is needed. to untrained items was noted.

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter 5. How would you summarize the status
that are new to you or that you have not yet of evidence-based practice for specific
mastered. treatment methods intended to foster
2. For each of the approaches addressed in compensatory strategies in communication?
this chapter, describe why it would be 6. What do you see as the greatest research
considered to be restitutive, compensatory, needs related to methods for fostering
or both. compensatory strategies in communication?
3. For each of the approaches in this chapter, 7. What strategies would you use to maximize
make a list of materials you would want to transfer of treatment gains in compensatory
have on hand so that you would be prepared strategy treatments to real-world use of
to carry out the approach in actual treatment communication?
sessions.
4. With a partner, demonstrate a treatment For additional learning and reflection activities,
www
session using each approach in this chapter. see the companion website.
CHAPTER
30
Specific Approaches for Enhancing
Expressive Language

In Chapter 25, we reviewed several general approaches the modalities that are the most impaired. For exam-
that help people with reduced expressive language ple, a person with limited oral language expression
engage in meaningful social interaction, especially would be restricted from using gestures, drawing,
through supported communication. In this chapter, and writing to communicate. Although it might be
we extend that discussion to specific approaches applicable for people with neurogenic cognitive-lin-
for improving expressive language abilities: con- guistic disorders other than aphasia, the approach
straint-induced language therapy (CILT), script has been developed to date primarily for people
training, Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT), Vol- with aphasia.
untary Control of Involuntary Utterances (VCIU),
Response Elaboration Training (RET), and Treatment
for Aphasic Perseveration (TAP).
On What Principles Is CILT Based?
After reading and reflecting on the content in
this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in
your own words, the following queries about each CILT is a restitutive approach. As discussed in
of these approaches: Chapter 24, Pulvermüller et al. (2001) introduced
the notion that constraint-induced movement ther-
1. What is it? apy (CIMT) for people with neuromotor challenges
2. On what principles is it based? could be applied to people with acquired language
3. How is it implemented? disorders. They based their rationale on previous
4. What is its status in terms of evidence-based research showing that when people with hemiparal-
practice? ysis or hemiparesis of the limbs were restricted in the
use of their functional arm or leg, they demonstrated
increased motor functioning in their impaired limbs.
What Is Constraint-Induced Maximizing reliance on impaired systems seemed
to stimulate impaired abilities. Thus, the underlying
Language Therapy?
rationale for CILT is that it is important to encour-
age people with language disabilities to use the lan-
Constraint-induced language therapy (CILT) or guage modalities that are most impaired.
constraint-induced aphasia therapy (CIAT) is mod- The primary principles of CILT are that
eled on approaches to constraint-induced therapy
in areas of practice outside of communication (i.e., • communication should be restricted to verbal
treatments involving neuromotor control). In this expression (e.g., that nonverbal modality use
approach, people are restricted in their use of com- should be discouraged), and
pensatory modalities. They are encouraged to use • practice should be intense.

443
444  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

also reported increased activation measured through


How Is CILT Implemented? functional MRI (fMRI) in perilesional areas follow-
ing CILT. Meinzeret al. (2005) reported maintenance
One challenge with the state of CILT to date is that of treatment effects 6 months posttreatment in a
there is little consistency across studies in terms of study of 27 people with aphasia.
the actual treatment protocol implemented. Not only Most of the research on CILT to date has been
are the details about treatment intensity and dura- done with people who had severe Broca’s apha-
tion lacking in some studies but so are the specific sia and apraxia of speech. Faroqi-Shah and Virion
activities in which participants engaged. The focus (2009) administered CILT to two people with chronic
has been more on what participants with apha- agrammatic aphasia and reported minimal impacts
sia were not allowed to do. That is, they have been on syntactic performance. Research with people who
instructed primarily not to use nonlinguistic means have mild aphasia and/or more “fluent” forms of
to communicate and not to use the stronger of oral aphasia is needed (Cherney, Patterson, et al., 2008).
versus written modalities of communication. Also, few outcome measures have been used consis-
Typically, tasks during CILT have been focused tently across studies.
on spoken language production, often using a cue- In a systematic review, Cherney, Patterson, and
ing hierarchy approach (see Chapter 31). Treatment colleagues (2008) summarized evidence regarding
intensity has generally been about 3 to 4 hours per the intensity of treatment for CILT for people with
day for at least 5 days per week over 2 weeks or 10 aphasia. Across 10 studies, overall positive effects
consecutive days. were noted. None included people with acute apha-
sia. Only five provided sufficient detail pertaining
to intensity to qualify for systematic review accord-
What Is the Status of CILT in Terms ing to level of treatment intensity. They reported
“modest evidence” (p. 1282) for greater effects from
of Evidence-Based Practice?
more intensive treatment and suggested that treat-
ment decisions be made “in conjunction with clin-
Maher et al. (2006) reported carryover of language ical expertise and the client’s individual values”
gains in three of four people with aphasia treated (p. 1282). This is certainly a good mantra for all
with CILT. Interestingly, they reported that a con- evidence-​based practice.
trol group involved in PACE treatment also made Other authors, too, have pointed out that the
similar gains, suggesting that perhaps the inten- required intensity of CILT may have more to do with
sity of the treatment — not just the method — was its demonstrated effectiveness in research studies to
important. Kirmess and Maher (2010) applied CILT date than the notion of restricting nonverbal expres-
to three people with acute aphasia in an inpatient sion (Basso & Macis, 2011; Brady et al., 2012; Szaflar-
rehabilitation context. They reported that the great- ski et al., 2008). Rose (2013) reviewed the theoretical
est improvements were seen in the impaired modal- accounts for multimodal treatments for aphasia as
ities treated. well as CILT and concluded that “constraint treat-
Johnson et al. (2014) reported positive outcomes ments and multimodality treatments are equally effi-
following CILT with four people with chronic Bro- cacious, and there is limited support for constraining
ca’s aphasia. They noted a lack of statistical signifi- client responses to the spoken modality” (p. 227).
cance in language battery test scores but improved Overall, there is a need for more research on
use of language in natural contexts; they highlighted CILT, with larger numbers of people with varied
that indices used to measure treatment outcomes types of aphasia at varied levels of time post-onset.
should be those that reflect the most life-affecting It will be important to control and describe more
aspects of treatment. consistently what is done in treatment while certain
As noted in Chapter 29, Kurland et al. (2012) modality use is avoided and to consider methodi-
found that treatment effects for CILT in addition to cally the factors that might affect how well a per-
PACE treatment were greater than PACE treatment son may fare using CILT (see Pierce et al., 2019). It is
alone for two people with aphasia. Those authors also important to study more about the underlying
30. Specific Approaches for Enhancing Expressive Language   445

neural mechanisms that support functional changes • Have them generate topics that are most
associated with CILT. Of course, contextualizing these relevant to themselves. The scripts may be
studies in a life participation perspective, not only monologues or dialogues to be initiated by
attending to the impairment level, will be important. the client in actual communicative situations.
Regardless of the effectiveness of CILT, it is Scripts may include, for example, personal
important to note that, overall, there is no clear evi- stories, general conversational topics, content
dence that using supported communication across to provide information, and descriptions of
all modalities impedes recovery of impaired modal- personal interests (Holland et al., 2010). It
ities. As we noted in Section VI, failing to attend to might help for you to propose a few specific
the communication needs of people with acquired topics based on what you know about their
language disorders as soon as possible is unethical. interests and communication needs.
• Use supported communication strategies to
collaborate with in generating a written script
for specific content the person wants to be
What Is Script Training?
able to convey.
• Practice reading the script aloud with
Script training is a method in which the client prac- the person, then have them read it alone,
tices using personally relevant conversational scripts supporting them as needed.
that are written in collaboration with a speech-​ • For homework, assign repeated reading aloud
language pathologist (SLP). It is intended for people of the script several times a day.
with aphasia who have limited expressive language. • Have the client practice using the script
The goal is to produce relatively fluent speech in contexts where the content is socially
and natural language production in socially mean- appropriate.
ingful contexts. • Have the client practice the script with new
conversational partners.

Since the method makes use of mass practice, it


On What Principles Is Script Training Based?
makes sense to use technology to deliver the target
script to be practiced. Script text may be programmed
Script training is based on evidence that repetitive into a speech-generating device, smartphone, or tab-
practice of preestablished, personally relevant con- let computer, using human voice recordings or digi-
versational text will decrease the amount of effort tized speech output.
involved in speaking during conversation and
increase spontaneous language generation (Bilda,
2011). Although script training may be considered What Is the Status of Script Training in
impairment focused, it also fits within a social and life
Terms of Evidence-Based Practice?
participation model because it entails use of trained
scripts in actual real-life communicative contexts.
Youmans et al. (2005) reported increased accuracy of
production and good generalization to spontaneous
use for two people with aphasia. The authors found
How Is Script Training Implemented?
that 5 to 11 sessions per script led to mastery for
both people; two or three additional sessions were
The following steps are based on descriptions from provided for generalization practice with new con-
Cherney (2012); Lee et al. (2009); Manheim et al. versational partners. Goldberg et al. (2012) provided
(2009); and Youmans et al. (2005): script training delivered through a combination of
in-person and online videoconferencing sessions for
• Discuss the goals of script training with the two people with aphasia. Treatment entailed work
client. on two personally relevant scripts, three times per
446  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

week for 3 weeks, for each script. The authors noted and ending in longer and more grammatically com-
that both participants improved in terms of gram- plex utterances. Tempo is slowed down so that the
matical morpheme production, rate of speech, syn- utterance is lyrical in nature. Variation of spoken
tax, and overall conversational success. Manheim pitch is reduced and made more constant or mono-
et al. (2009) provided computer-based script train- tonic. Stress is exaggerated for emphasis using pitch
ing for 20 people with chronic aphasia. Outcomes, and volume changes (Sparks, 2008).
as indexed according to the Burden of Stroke Scale MIT is intended for people with severely limited
(Doyle et al., 2003), indicated significantly reduced oral expression, especially people with Broca’s apha-
communication difficulty. Lee et al. (2009) reported sia (with or without apraxia of speech). The best can-
that greater intensity of use of prerecorded script didates for treatment are said to be those with good
practicing software (AphasiaScripts, https://www​ auditory comprehension, the ability to self-​monitor
.sralab.org) led to better treatment gains in 17 people and self-correct, and willingness to participate
with aphasia, especially in people with more severe actively (Helm-Estabrooks et al., 1989, 2014; Norton
language impairment. Cherney et al. (2014) applied et al., 2009; Schlaug et al., 2008; Sparks, 2008; Sparks
computer-based script training with eight people & Holland, 1976). The goal is to draw on the prosodic
with chronic aphasia, varying the amount and type of features of language to facilitate verbal output. MIT
cueing (high-frequency multimodal cues versus min- targets speech output at the impairment level.
imal cues) and found that both were effective for peo-
ple with varied levels of aphasia severity. Fein et al.
(2020) describe how a script training based on texting
On What Principles Is MIT Based?
was helpful in a person with chronic anomic aphasia.
Dial et al. (2019) comment that video-​implemented
script training for aphasia (VISTA) is easily adaptable MIT was developed based on the hypothesis that
for teletherapy delivery and provide evidence of its “functions associated with the intact right hemi-
effectiveness in a sample of 10 people with nonfluent sphere might be tapped to improve the language
variant primary progressive aphasia (nfvPPA). Addi- functions of a damaged left hemisphere” (Helm-​
tional support for applying script therapy for people Estabrooks et al., 1989, p. 1). According to Albert
with PPA is discussed in Chapter 26. et al. (1973), MIT takes advantage of three principles:
Holland et al. (2010) analyzed the contents of
100 short scripts that had been collaboratively devel- • The right hemisphere mediates music and
oped by 33 people with aphasia and their SLPs. The speech prosody in most people.
most common category of monologue scripts was • The right hemisphere is typically preserved
personal stories (68%), and the most common cate- in individuals with aphasia such that singing
gory of dialogue scripts was conversations with fam- abilities are spared in most individuals with
ilies (21%). These findings support the relevance of left hemisphere lesions alone.
the approach to everyday life participation. • Preserved musical and prosodic capabilities
can be used to facilitate language production
in people with aphasia.
What Is Melodic Intonation Therapy?
Initially, utterances presented melodically are
a way for individuals to compensate for their lack
Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT) is an interven- of speech. As treatment steps progress, the underly-
tion method based on facilitating spoken language ing melody fades, and more typical speech patterns
through the exaggeration of three elements of spo- and prosody are ideally elicited. MIT is restitutive in
ken language prosody: pitch, the tempo and rhythm terms of the goal to foster brain changes to enhance
of utterances, and stress for emphasis (Sparks, 2008). speech output and prosody. It may also be consid-
Speaking tasks are gradually increased in length and ered compensatory in that a person may learn to use
complexity as treatment progresses in a hierarchical melody and rhythmic patterns to facilitate their own
fashion, starting with shorter, easier speaking tasks spoken language (Albert et al., 1973).
30. Specific Approaches for Enhancing Expressive Language   447

Level I
How Is MIT Implemented?
• Hum a melodic pattern twice holding
The original method, developed by Albert and col- the left hand of the client. Together, make
leagues (1973), is composed of clear steps organized hand-tapping movements in time with the
in a hierarchy of increasing difficulty. Difficulty humming, emphasizing rhythm, tempo, and
refers to an increased phrase length with each level stress.
and removal of melodic intonation and rhythmic • Motion for the client to join with you in
tapping within later levels (Schlaug et al., 2010; humming the same melodic pattern. Continue
Schlaug, Norton, et al., 2010). Sparks (2008) summa- repeating the same humming pattern.
rized additional suggestions: • Gradually fade out your humming, while
continuing the hand tapping. Use gesture to
• pausing for 6 seconds between presenting encourage the client to continue humming.
a target stimulus and having the person Continue to do this until the client hums in a
respond, and between the completion of one way that matches what you have modeled.
targeted verbal item and the next to enable
processing time
• avoiding excessive reinforcement for good Level II
responses
• avoiding incorporation of melodies similar to
Step 1
actual songs, which might stimulate memories
of song lyrics • Think of a sentence that would be meaningful
for the client to say. Examples might be: “I am
The recommended frequency of intervention is hungry,” “I need help,” “I love you,” or “How
two 30-minute sessions daily, 5 days per week. The are you?” Consider the intonation pattern
recommended criterion for progression from one with which the target sentence would be
level to the next is 90% or better accuracy for 10 con- naturally said.
secutive therapy sessions (Sparks, 2008). If a failure • Hum that intonation pattern while holding
occurs on a step, even when a trial is repeated from the client’s hand, tapping in rhythm to the
the previous step, then that utterance is to be discon- humming.
tinued (Sparks et al., 1974). • Intone the words of the sentence instead of
The specific steps in MIT are described vari- humming, with the same melody, stress, and
ably by the original authors. They are summarized rhythm.
here in an attempt to provide practical guidance for • Motion for the client to join with you; intone
carrying out the approach. If you are interested in the sentence together. If the client cannot do
delving further into specific processes and scoring this, wait for a few seconds, then move on to
procedures, I recommend reviewing the detailed another sentence and start again at Level II,
instructions and scoring procedures provided by Step 1.
Helm-Estabrooks et al. (2014) and Sparks (2008).
In the instructions, reference to intoning means Step 2
that instead of speaking, you sing the words in a melo-
dious pattern that exaggerates the natural pitches cor- • Intone the same sentence along with tapping
responding to how a target sentence might be said. hands.
The term sprechgesang, literally (in German) “spo- • Continue hand tapping but fade your
ken song,” refers to a blend of speaking and singing. intoning, and gesture to the client to continue
Sparks (2008) described it as being similar to intoning intoning the sentence. If the client cannot do
in terms of the exaggerated tempo, rhythm, and stress this, wait for a few seconds, then move on
but having a more constant pitch: “The utterance is to another sentence and begin at the start of
lyrical but spoken rather than sung,” he says (p. 842). Level II, Step 1 again.
448  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Step 3 gives is as follows: if the target sentence in


Step 2 was, “I want some pie,” then you might
• Signal for the client to listen to you. intone, “What kind of pie?” (p. 846).
• Present the same intoned sentence again, • If the client does not respond accurately, back
accompanied by hand tapping with the up to Level III, Step 2.
client.
• Signal the client to repeat the sentence
while you continue hand tapping, but stop Level IV
intoning.
• If the client has trouble initiating the sentence,
Step 1
provide a phonemic cue.
• If the client cannot do this, wait for a few • Signal for the client to listen while you intone
seconds, then move on to another sentence the sentence.
and begin at the start of Level II, Step 1 again. • Present the sentence twice in sprechesang
while hand tapping with the client.
Step 4 • Gesture for the client to join you in unison
sprechesang while hand tapping together.
• Without hand tapping, intone the question, • If the client does not join in, model it
“What did you say?” again, and again gesture for the client to
• Signal to the client to answer with the same join in.
intoned utterance.
• Provide hand tapping and a phonemic cue if
Step 2
the client is having trouble.
• Signal for the client to listen and not join in
while you present the same sentence again in
Level III sprechesang with hand tapping.
• Wait for 2 or 3 seconds and gesture for the
Step 1 client to repeat the sentence in sprechesang
with hand tapping.
• Present the intoned sentence again with hand • If the client cannot or does not do it, go back
tapping and gesture for the client to do it in to Level IV, Step 1.
unison with you.
• Fade your intoning as the client continues,
Step 3
only joining in again if needed.
• Signal for the client to listen, and then present
Step 2 the same sentence using typical speech
prosody and no hand tapping.
• Intone the sentence again with hand tapping. • Signal for the client to repeat the sentence
• Give a hand signal to the client to request that using typical speech prosody.
they delay the response for a second or two.
• Gesture for the client to intone the sentence Step 4
alone.
• Ask questions relevant to the sentence just
Step 3 spoken in Step 3. For example, if the sentence
was “I want to eat,” then you might ask,
• Intone a question to elicit a response that is “What do you want to eat?” “What’s your
relevant to the sentence on which you have favorite food?” and “Where would you go to
been working. The example that Sparks (2008) get that?”
30. Specific Approaches for Enhancing Expressive Language   449

Helm-Estabrooks et al. (2014) suggested that therapy. Two of those people had nonfluent aphasia
optimal treatment duration is no more than 8 weeks, and one had global aphasia, all poststroke. After 1 to 2
although there is a great deal of variation in duration months of MIT treatment, each person demonstrated
and intensity of treatment in the related research lit- increased expressive language abilities in proposi-
erature. Outcomes indices directly tied to treatment tional speech, including the ability to answer ques-
may include scoring of responses for each level, tions and converse with peers (Albert et al., 1973).
repetition accuracy, and length, informativeness, Some studies to date have helped to identify
and accuracy of responses to questions in Level IV. the best candidates for MIT. Sparks et al. (1974) con-
Additional metrics reported in evaluating MIT out- ducted a study involving eight participants and ana-
comes include mean length of utterance, informa- lyzed results in terms of how well they responded
tion content units, confrontational naming accuracy, to MIT. The four participants grouped into the best
effectiveness of communication with a partner, and recovery group commenced MIT with limited ste-
self-initiation of MIT strategies. reotypical jargon, paraphasias, and agrammatism.
The two grouped in the moderate recovery group
entered the program with almost no meaningful
What Is the Status of MIT in Terms speech and had no stereotypic responses. The last
two, who showed no significant recovery, had little
of Evidence-Based Practice?
verbal output prior to MIT and demonstrated overall
poor motivation. Naeser and Helm-Estabrooks (1985)
Many of the early articles on MIT are qualitative reported that those who responded best to treatment
and descriptive in nature, mainly focusing on the according to standardized language measures had
theory of right brain involvement in singing and the lesions in Broca’s area but not in the temporal lobe
corresponding preserved tonal and musical abili- or right hemisphere. Those with poor responses had
ties of many individuals with aphasia. Most studies bilateral lesions involving Wernicke’s area.
addressing treatment outcomes are case studies or Additional studies have addressed neurological
single-subject designs. evidence for the mechanisms of recovery associated
In a randomized, controlled single-blind study with MIT. Schlaug et al. (2008) reported significantly
conducted by board certified music therapists more fMRI activity in the right hemisphere follow-
(Conklyn et al., 2012) with 30 people with aphasia ing MIT treatment compared to a control treatment
(16 in an MIT treatment group and 14 in the con- based on speech repetition. Belin et al. (1996) mea-
trol group), the treatment group showed significant sured cerebral blood flow in seven people with
improvements compared to the control group. How- aphasia who had completed MIT. Blood flow was
ever, in a randomized controlled trial with 17 people measured via positron emission tomography while
with chronic aphasia, Van Der Meulen et al. (2016) the participants listened to and repeated words with
found that MIT was only effective for repetition of exaggerated melody and rhythm. Results indicated
trained materials. Repetition improvement did not activation of Broca’s area and the left prefrontal
transfer to untrained items, to word finding, or to cortex. This was in contrast to abnormal activation
spontaneous conversation, and it was not main- when words were presented without such empha-
tained 6 weeks after treatment. Results were similar ses. Importantly, their findings of left hemisphere
in the same authors’ 2014 study including 27 people reactivation called into question the notion that MIT
with subacute aphasia. Haro-Martinez et al. (2019), leads to right hemisphere compensation.
using a randomized group design, found no signif- The focus of still other studies on MIT has been
icant results according to standardized measures, on discerning the elements of the overall treatment
although questionnaire responses from caregivers that may account for its effectiveness for some peo-
suggested some improvement. ple. Dunham and Newhoff (1979) described the case
Albert et al. (1973) reported on results of MIT of a man with aphasia who had made little progress
for three people with aphasia who had experienced during 19 months of language treatment prior to
no resolution of symptoms after months of aphasia initiation of MIT. After 5 months of treatment using
450  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

hand tapping as a prosodic cue, he was able to use patterns than the original prescribed patterns and
four- to five-word utterances to respond to questions using complete phrases from the start of treatment)
or refer to picture stimuli. In contrast, Hough (2010) with 16 people who had acute rather than chronic
described the case of a man with chronic aphasia aphasia. They compared MIT results for untrained
who benefited from a focus on melodic cues without repetitions and responses to those of a control group
hand tapping and with verbal stimuli incorporating of 14 people with acute aphasia and found improve-
his own automatic and personally relevant sponta- ments after only one treatment session. In addition
neous utterances. to the many modified approaches for English speak-
Boucher et al. (2001) suggested that melodic ers, MIT has been adapted for use in several other
contour is not as important as rhythm and pacing. languages, including French (Belin et al., 1996), Jap-
They studied the comparative effectiveness of mod- anese (Seki & Sugishita, 1983), Persian (Bonakdar-
ifying tones versus rhythm and pacing on repeti- pour et al., 2003), Romanian (Popovici & Mihăilescu,
tion abilities in two people with chronic aphasia. 1992), and Spanish (Haro-Martinez et al., 2017).
They reported that an emphasis on tonal conditions Vines et al. (2011) provided transcranial direct
during treatment did not facilitate repetition gains; current stimulation (tDCS; see Chapter 24) along
the emphasis on rhythm and pacing of words did. with MIT treatment to six people with severe “non-
Stahl et al. (2013) compared singing versus rhyth- fluent” aphasia. They showed significant gains in
mic speech in seven people with Broca’s aphasia and “fluency” measures compared to when they were
eight with global aphasia. They concluded that both engaged in MIT with a sham treatment made to look
may be effective in eliciting formulaic expressions in and feel like tDCS.
people with “nonfluent” aphasia. In sum, reported treatment outcomes are mixed,
Laughlin et al. (1979) reported that syllable and the overall quality of the supportive research is
duration is an important parameter in MIT admin- not strong (Hurkmans et al., 2012). A challenge in
istration. They evaluated syllable duration during summarizing and interpreting the evidence base is
MIT to determine which of three syllable durations the lack of treatment fidelity across many studies
was most effective for increasing correct phrase pro- and the great variability in treatment outcomes mea-
ductions. Five people with aphasia were presented sures. Much remains to be learned about the neuro-
with natural nonintoned phrases spoken at less than logical mechanisms underlying MIT effects and the
1 second per syllable and modified MIT intoned effectiveness of MIT relative to such factors as time
phrases spoken at seconds per syllable and 2.0 sec- poststroke, cerebral dominance, treatment intensity
onds per syllable. All participants had the greatest and duration, and participants’ and clinicians’ musi-
number of correct phrase productions when pre- cal backgrounds. Differences in outcomes between
sented with the longest syllable duration. Regular speakers of tonal and nontonal languages (see Chap-
nonintoned speech led to the greatest number of ter 12) are also important to study.
response failures. As Sparks and Holland (1976) point out, a weak-
Several researchers have tested modified ver- ness of MIT is that the main areas targeted are accu-
sions of MIT. Goldfarb and Bader (1979) adapted rate progression through the prescribed tasks and a
MIT for a home-based training program described focus on linguistic form rather than true communi-
in a case study of a man with global aphasia. MIT cation. Measures of social validation and generaliza-
was administered twice per week in a clinical set- tion to naturalistic communication (effects beyond
ting and up to five times per week in the home via the impairment level) are needed. It is important to
a spouse trained to implement the method. Target note that any elicitation method that leads to speech
sentences corresponded to daily needs. After 23 ther- of any kind can be encouraging to individuals with
apy sessions, the participant obtained criterion in all severe aphasia and apraxia of speech. It can also be
levels of difficulty. Improvement was noted in imi- heartening to those who care about them. Thus, even
tation of trained sentences, as well as in responses in cases where the sung, intoned, or spoken output
to questions. does not convey literal meaning, there may be some
McKelvey and Weissling (2013) applied a mod- emotional and relational benefit to occasionally
ified version of MIT (using more natural intonation helping a person demonstrate at least some spoken
30. Specific Approaches for Enhancing Expressive Language   451

production through the use of alternative patterns of • Create a list of all the words that the client is
elicitation such as those recommended in MIT. known to have produced spontaneously, and
write each on a separate card.
• Ask the client to read one card at a time aloud.
What Is Voluntary Control of For each card, if the client reads it correctly,
keep the card; if not, discard it.
Involuntary Utterances?
• Present pictures of the target words and ask
the client to name each one. If the client cannot
Voluntary Control of Involuntary Utterances (VCIU) name it, show the corresponding written word
is a treatment approach designed to improve expres- and ask the client to read it aloud.
sive, propositional communication in people with • Any time the client produces a different real
severe nonfluent aphasia whose speech is limited to word, discard the former target word and
automatic production of few words (Helm & Barresi, replace it with the new word.
1980; Helm-Estabrooks et al., 2014). The purpose is • Provide the target word card to the client to
to stimulate the use of propositional language in practice at home.
individuals who mainly use involuntary utterances • Through supported communication,
but who are able to read and comprehend at least encourage progression from oral reading
one word at a time. The clinician uses the client’s and confrontation naming to use in natural
current automatic utterances as a starting point for conversation.
therapy. Goda (1962) and Vignolo (1964) suggested
that clinicians could effectively use correct automatic Family members and friends can be involved in
or involuntary utterances to facilitate production the VCIU approach by identifying new target words
of voluntary utterances. Even inappropriate utter- (based on utterances they hear the client say outside
ances used by individuals with aphasia can give the of the clinical setting) and by practicing voluntary
clinician information about the sounds and words use of target words in meaningful social contexts.
that the client is capable of producing. An inherent
benefit is that using the person’s spontaneous pro-
ductions may help to increase the likelihood that What Is the Status of VCIU in Terms
treatment materials are relevant to the individual.
of Evidence-Based Practice?

Very limited research has been done on the efficacy


On What Principles Is VCIU Treatment Based?
of VCIU. Helm and Barresi (1980) reported that three
participants with limited automatic speech but rel-
The assumption behind VCIU is that spontaneously atively intact reading and auditory comprehension
produced, automatic speech can be used to facilitate skills showed improvement in confrontation nam-
the production and intentional use of real words in ing. One demonstrated significant improvement in
conversation (Helm & Barresi, 1980; Helm-Estabrooks the number of words used in natural conversation.
et al., 2014). A supposition that complements the A great deal more research must be done to support
approach is that using words uniquely tailored to the evidence base for this approach.
each individual, with stimuli based on actual prior
productions, will help ensure personal relevance.
What Is Response Elaboration Training?

How Is VCIU Implemented?


Response Elaboration Training (RET) was devel-
oped by Kearns (1985) as a means of increasing the
The following are the basic steps for carrying out length and improving the information content of
VCIU: oral language of people with Broca’s or “nonfluent”
452  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

aphasia. In contrast to more formalized methods, • Do not directly correct the client’s responses;
during RET, the client is seen as the primary commu- instead, provide natural feedback through
nicator, and client-initiated topics are encouraged. conversational modeling.
A forward-chaining technique is implemented. That
is, the clinician responds directly to anything the cli-
ent says and models and reinforces longer utterances What Is the Status of RET in Terms
based on client-initiated utterances. Successful com-
of Evidence-Based Practice?
munication of novel ideas is encouraged rather than
accuracy of production.
Research studies to date, primarily case studies and
single-participant studies, have shown increases in
the amount of verbal information provided by peo-
On What Principles Is RET Based?
ple with Broca’s aphasia in response to picture stim-
uli (Gaddie et al., 1991; Kearns, 1985; Kearns & Scher,
RET is considered an interactive loose training pro- 1989; Kearns & Yedor, 1991). Treatment outcomes
gram geared toward lengthening of utterances and have been indexed in terms of number of words,
increasing variety in linguistic formulations. Loose length of utterances, sentence completeness, and
training programs are those that reduce clinician grammatical accuracy. RET effects have been shown
control over stimuli, responses, and feedback during to generalize to other conversational partners, pic-
treatment (Gaddie et al., 1991). ture stimuli, and social settings (Gaddie et al., 1991;
Kearns & Yedor, 1991).
Conley and Coelho (2003) reported improved
naming in a woman with chronic Broca’s apha-
How Is RET Implemented?
sia following a program of combined RET and
Semantic Feature Analysis treatment, with greater
In RET, people with aphasia are shown picture stim- maintenance of trained than untrained words and
uli. Instead of having them describe the pictures, the high- versus low-familiarity words.
clinician encourages them to elaborate on whatever Wambaugh and colleagues have demonstrated
thoughts they associate with the picture. The follow- that people with concomitant aphasia and severe
ing steps are based on the original description by apraxia of speech can also achieve positive treatment
Kearns (1985): gains when the essential elements of RET are imple-
mented, alone or in combination with other methods
• Show a stimulus picture depicting an (Bunker et al., 2019; Wambaugh et al., 2013; Wam-
everyday activity and elicit an initial verbal baugh et al., 2012, 2014). Kearns and Elman (2008)
response to the picture. Encourage the described how RET may be applied in group set-
client to elaborate on whatever the client is tings. Limitations of RET studies to date are the lack
reminded of when looking at the picture. of consistent metrics used to demonstrate outcomes,
Avoid having the client describe the picture or small sample sizes, and a lack of randomization and
name items depicted. control groups.
• Respond to the client’s initial response with
your own comments, and encourage the client
to expand on the initial response. Continue to
What Is Treatment for Aphasic Perseveration?
make additional comments in response to the
client’s comments as appropriate.
• Ask Wh- questions regarding the client’s own Treatment of Aphasic Perseveration (TAP) is an
responses. approach originally designed by Helm-Estabrooks
• Model sentences that combine the client’s et al. (1987) for people with aphasia who tend to per-
initial and subsequent responses. Ask the severate on speech sounds, words, and utterances
client to repeat your combined sentences. they have already said. As discussed in Chapter 10,
30. Specific Approaches for Enhancing Expressive Language   453

people with neurogenic communication disorders particular attention to their perseverations to


tend to perseverate in a variety of ways. General try to avoid them. Of course, do this with a
categories of perseveration are recurrent, continu- gentle and friendly, not corrective, tone.
ous, and stuck-in-set perseveration (Albert, 1989; • Engage in a confrontation naming activity,
Sandson & Albert, 1984). As summarized in Chap- with 5-second intervals between items.
ter 10, recurrent perseveration, common in aphasia, Arrange the stimuli according to the severity
may be semantic, lexical, or phonemic. If a reminder of perseveration the person has exhibited on
would be helpful, examples of each type are shown the confrontation naming task. The intent is
in Box 10–1. TAP is an impairment-level approach. to start where the client will have the greatest
The goals are to reduce perseverations and enhance success and then to move to more difficult
naming. Optimal candidates for TAP are people with items in a hierarchical fashion. Although the
aphasia who have at least moderately intact com- earlier descriptions of TAP recommended
prehension, good memory, and moderate to severe use of preestablished picture sets, more
recurrent perseveration. recent versions have acknowledged that it
is important to use pictures that are most
personally relevant to each individual’s daily
use of language.
On What Principles Is TAP Based?
• Continue to draw attention to moments of
perseveration. Write the incorrect utterance
Given how pervasive recurrent perseveration is in that was spoken and then rip it up in front
chronic aphasia, and given how it tends to persist of the client. If the client perseverates on the
regardless of time post-onset (Basso, 2004; Helm-­ same word again, point to the ripped paper as
Estabrooks et al., 1998), addressing it head-on may a reminder. As you have the client name one
be especially helpful for many people with aphasia. picture at a time, track
The underlying principle is that by helping people • the number of items (pictures) named
become aware of their perseverations, we may help correctly (providing up to three cues for
them suppress them (Helm-Estabrooks et al., 2014). each), and
• the number and type of words on which
the person perseverates (regardless of the
number of times the client perseverated on
How Is TAP Implemented?
each word).

The following are summarized according to the Helm-Estabrooks et al. (2014) provide scoring
treatment steps described by Helm-Estabrooks sheets for this purpose.
et al. (2014) and the original program detailed by
Helm-Estabrooks et al. (1987): • Cues may be gestures, drawings, spoken
descriptions, graphic cues (initial letters,
• Establish a baseline by calculating the syllables, or the whole word), phonemic cues,
percentage of words perseverated during the requests for repetition, or requests to speak or
confrontation naming portion of the Boston sing the word in unison.
Diagnostic Aphasia Examination-3 (BDAE-3; • If you choose to work on sets of words within
Goodglass et al., 2001) and interpreting the semantic categories (e.g., kitchen items,
percentage as follows. foods, transportation, letters, numbers), it
• minimal: 0% to 5% may help to mention that the category is
• mild: 5% to 19% about to change, to avoid stuck-in-set types of
• moderate: 20% to 49% perseverations based on the task.
• severe: 49% and higher
• Explain to the client what perseveration is It is important to test for treatment effects not
and give examples. Ask that the client pay only with trained but also untrained items.
454  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

according to generalization to untrained naming


What Is the Status of TAP in Terms stimuli or tasks. No other empirical reports on the
of Evidence-Based Practice? effectiveness of the approach appear to have been
published to date. Thus, the evidence base is weak.
Helm-Estabrooks et al. (1987) reported results of Given the pervasiveness of perseveration in acute
a single-case design study with alternating types and chronic aphasia, further research on treatment
of treatment for three people with aphasia. They methods to address it is important. Contextualizing
reported substantial reduction in perseverations for such research in a life participation framework will
all three participants. They did not report results be important.

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter 5. How would you summarize the status
that are new to you or that you have not yet of evidence-based practice for specific
mastered. treatment methods intended to enhance
2. For each of the approaches addressed in expressive language across modalities?
this chapter, describe why it would be 6. What do you see as the greatest research
considered to be restitutive, compensatory, needs related to methods for enhancing
or both. expressive language across modalities?
3. For each of the approaches in this chapter, 7. What strategies would you use to maximize
make a list of materials you would want to transfer of treatment gains made through
have on hand so that you would be prepared expressive language treatment to real-world
to carry out the approach in actual treatment use of communication?
sessions.
4. With a partner, demonstrate a treatment For additional learning and reflection activities,
www
session using each approach in this chapter. see the companion website.
CHAPTER
31
Specific Approaches for Improving
Word Finding and Lexical Processing

As you know, one of the most pervasive and persistent used in intervention with people who have neu-
problems of people with neurogenic communication rogenic language disorders. That is, we frequently
disorders, no matter what the etiology, is difficulty provide cues to our clients to support their communi-
with word finding. This makes it especially important cation. This has long been the case, and cueing strat-
to have a solid repertoire of methods to target word egies were in use before anyone decided to specify
finding in particular. In Chapter 25, we reviewed sev- this particular approach (Nickels & Best, 1996). Also,
eral approaches that may enhance word finding as most cognitive-linguistic treatment programs devel-
part of general social and stimulation-based methods. oped since the description of this particular approach
In this chapter, we consider specific approaches for involve cueing. Linebaugh, though, is known as the
improving word finding and lexical processing: cue- key pioneer in naming this particular approach and
ing hierarchies for the treatment of anomia, Semantic articulating the principles behind it (Linebaugh, 1983;
Feature Analysis (SFA), Phonological Components Linebaugh & Lehner, 1977; Linebaugh et al., 2005).
Analysis (PCA), Verb Network Strengthening Treat- What is unique about cueing hierarchy approaches
ment (VNeST), and Verb as Core. to anomia was the formalization, in the late 1970s
After reading and reflecting on the content in and early 1980s, of the principles of
this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in
your own words, the following queries about each • organizing cues in a hierarchical way to
of these approaches: enhance naming abilities in people with
naming deficits,
1. What is it? • basing the hierarchy of cues on the naming
2. On what principles is it based? performance of each individual person treated,
3. How is it implemented? • systematically presenting cues to optimize
4. What is its status in terms of evidence-based naming responses, and
practice? • making use of a person’s communicative
strengths (e.g., writing, auditory
comprehension) to facilitate word retrieval.
What Are Cueing Hierarchies for
the Treatment of Anomia?
On What Principles Are Cueing Hierarchies
for the Treatment of Anomia Based?
Numerous aphasiologists have written about cueing
hierarchy approaches for treating people with ano-
mia, as well as for just about any impairment-level Linebaugh’s principles for a cueing hierarchy ap-
focus in treatment. Cueing strategies are commonly proach to anomia are to

455
456  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

• elicit correct naming with the least amount is unable to name an item, provide a cue.
of cueing possible — that is, using the most Cues, for example, may be initial phonemes,
powerful cues in terms of their ability to elicit the printed first letter, the printed word,
a correct response; a rhyming word, an object description, a
• reduce cueing from the clinician as soon as the sentence completion task, a gesture showing
cues are not needed; and how an object to be named is used, or a verbal
• help the person generate self-cueing strategies description of the item. As you do this, keep
to enhance naming (Linebaugh, 1983; careful notes regarding which cues led to the
Linebaugh & Lehner, 1977). correct production of the word.
• Based on the data you collect during the
All cues are said to have stimulus power. The naming assessment, order the cues, or
stimulus power of a given cue refers to the likeli- therapeutic stimulus types, or tasks, along a
hood of that particular cue eliciting a target word. continuum according to their effectiveness
So, by definition, cueing hierarchies are organized in stimulating correct word retrieval
according to stimulus power. The stimulus power performance.
for a given task and the overall hierarchy of cues • Engage in confrontation naming tasks using
established for that task vary across individuals pictures and objects, first providing cues with
with anomia (a highly heterogeneous popula- the greatest stimulus power and progressively
tion, given that anomia is one of the most perva- using cues with less stimulus power. Coach
sive impairments among people with any type of the client about the importance of their
neurogenic cognitive-linguistic disorder). Cueing initiating the types of cues that you are
hierarchy approaches are generally considered providing.
impairment-based stimulation methods but may • Engage in generative naming tasks, having
also be considered compensatory in that the person the client come up with words that fit
with aphasia ideally learns to implement strategies in certain categories. Continue the same
to improve their own naming abilities when they approach to cueing, gradually reducing the
have difficulty retrieving a word. strength of your cues.
• Progress to generalization tasks such as
picture descriptions, prepared monologues,
How Is Cueing Hierarchy story retelling, and role-playing.
Treatment Implemented?
What Is the Status of Cueing Hierarchies
Since numerous authors have developed treatment for the Treatment of Anomia in Terms
approaches based on the basic principles described
of Evidence-Based Practice?
earlier, there is not just one means of carrying out
treatment in this category. However, there are com-
mon steps that reflect the approach as it was origi- As mentioned earlier, numerous studies have incor-
nally described. In the clinical context, we use certain porated the use of cueing hierarchies; it is beyond the
stimulus types or tasks, in a progression of increas- scope of this chapter to summarize them. Instead,
ing difficulty, to move from one level to the next as let’s consider key findings that have emerged in over
the client reaches a certain level of performance with 40 years of research on this topic. Representative
each task. studies supporting these findings are cited here; the
list of studies noted is far from comprehensive.
• Engage in naming assessment using a
standardized assessment battery, as well • When we work on naming as described
as by using a set of picture stimuli that are earlier, people get better at naming the items
personally relevant to the individual with practiced (Conroy et al., 2012; Freed et al.,
whom you are working. Whenever the client 2004; Lowell et al., 1995).
31. Specific Approaches for Improving Word Finding and Lexical Processing   457

• Sometimes people get better at naming items The goal is to enhance naming abilities by improving
we did not use in the training, but mostly access to semantic networks (Boyle & Coelho, 1995;
they get better at the words used in treatment. Coelho et al., 2000).
Transfer of gains to untrained words is
typically weak. This finding highlights the
importance of using words that are personally
On What Principles Is SFA Treatment Based?
relevant to the individual. Complementing
this finding, there is evidence that people
achieve greater carryover to real-word SFA is a treatment method to enhance word retrieval.
contexts when personally relevant words are It is based on the spreading activation theory of
used in treatment (Freed & Marshall, 1995; semantic processing (Collins & Loftus, 1975). By
Freed et al., 1995, 2004; Marshall et al., 2002, activating the semantic network surrounding a tar-
2018). get word, the target word may be activated above
• Generalization from naming practice to its threshold, thereby facilitating retrieval. When the
natural conversation using only a cueing sematic network involved in representing a certain
hierarchy approach is limited (Marshall & concept is activated, an individual is more likely to
Freed, 2006; Wambaugh et al., 2002). be able to produce the target word (Boyle & Coelho,
1995; Wambaugh & Ferguson, 2007). Even access
Treatment programs developed more recently to nontarget words may be facilitated by enhanc-
tend to incorporate more intentionally meaningful ing semantic activation of related concepts. SFA is
interaction about words, thus further strengthen- a restitutive approach in that it has been shown to
ing semantic networks. For example, SFA, VNeST, enhance recruitment of activation of left hemisphere
and Verb as Core (described later in this chapter) areas (Marcotte et al., 2012). One of the long-term
have generally led to more positive results than the aims of SFA treatment is to help people with dysno-
types of tasks used in traditional cueing hierarchy mia learn to cue themselves independently and in
approaches. natural everyday conversational contexts to produce
Also, findings that treating atypical exemplars target words. In that sense, it is also a compensatory
of words leads to better functional gains (please approach.
recall our discussion of CATE in Chapter 24) have
been used to challenge the notion that we should
progress hierarchically through naming treatments,
How Is SFA Treatment Implemented?
from easy to difficult levels (e.g., using cues with
high stimulus power before cues with less stimulus
power). It is important that we harness the strengths To carry out SFA treatment, use the basic steps listed
of complexity to increase carryover to untrained later in this chapter for the baseline phase and target
words, a point that we consider further in the con- selection, the semantic feature analysis chart method,
text of our next approach. and the graphic organizer method, all adapted from
Boyle (2004) and Coelho et al. (2000). The chart and
graphic organizer may be printed on paper. If lami-
nated versions are used, a dry-erase marker may be
What Is Semantic Feature Analysis?
used to mark responses, and they can be reused.

Semantic Feature Analysis (SFA) is a treatment


approach targeting word-finding abilities, so it Baseline Phase and Target Selection
is especially designed for people with dysnomia,
including but not limited to people with anomic • Select words that are most relevant and
aphasia. It is based on earlier approaches developed ecologically valid for the individual. Basic
for traumatic brain injury (TBI) survivors (Haarbau- general categories may be used, such as
er-Krupa et al., 1985; Massaro & Tompkins, 1992). names of furniture, modes of transportation,
458  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

or foods. However, if a person has particular words. Coelho et al. (2000) reported that using
interests in specific categories for which as few as 10 target words led to generalization
increased word finding would be helpful, it to untrained words. Add a few easy words
is good to explore those, too. Examples might (determined at baseline); Coelho et al. (2000)
be woodworking tools, spices used in Indian suggested about five per session, to promote a
cooking, stringed instruments, and breeds sense of success and thus boost motivation.
of dogs.
• Obtain a corresponding image for each target
word. Semantic Feature Analysis Chart Method
• Obtain baseline scores in a confrontation
naming task. Show each image, one at a time. • Select one image at a time; put it at the center
Retain those named correctly the first time as of an SFA chart (Figure 31–1).
“easy” stimuli. Retain those not named even • Ask the person to name the word
after three consecutive sessions as “target” corresponding to the image (the target word).

USE

What can we do
with it?
It’s used to…
GROUP What’s this for? ACTION

It’s a… What does it do?


What group is it in? We use it to…
What’s the category? What’s it used for?

TARGET
Target
(picture
Image to
to be named
be named)
ASSOCIATION PROPERTIES

What does this How would you


remind you of? describe it?
When you see this It has…
you think of… LOCATION

This is found…
Where you we see
one of these?

Figure 31–1. Semantic feature analysis chart. Source: Adapted from Coelho et al., 2000.
31. Specific Approaches for Improving Word Finding and Lexical Processing   459

• Acknowledge the accuracy (or lack of • Association: reminds me of . . . when my


accuracy) of the response. grandmother had me stay overnight and
• Regardless of naming accuracy, use the SFA made toast with her fresh-baked bread;
chart to prompt the person to produce words wonderful aromas in the kitchen; a fire I set
semantically related to the target word. Have by accident when I was little; the toaster
them list the categories into which it fits pastries my mom gave us for breakfast
(group), its use, actions that are taken with when our dad was out of town
it or that it takes, properties, location, and • Write the person’s correct responses in the
association. For example, if the target word is box corresponding to each type of feature.
toaster, the responses below may be evoked: More than one response in each box is fine.
• Group: kitchen appliances, things used to You may help them by saying aloud your
make breakfast, electrical gizmos own associations and writing them down,
• Use: to make toast, to heat and brown providing auditory and visual input. Even if
bread, to warm pastries they say the correct target word, continue to
• Action: heats and browns bread and fill out the SFA chart.
pastries
• Properties: electrical, made of metal (and
sometimes plastic, too), gets too hot to Graphic Organizer Method
touch, has a cord
• Location: found in the kitchen, on the • Use a graphic organizer (Table 31–1) to lead
kitchen counter the person in considering semantic features

Table 31–1. Graphic Organizer Examples: Furniture and Appliances

Furniture
Chair Table Desk Bed
It’s made to sit on + − − +/−
It’s soft +/− − − +
We have one in the kitchen + + − −
We have one in the bedroom + + − +
We have one in the office + − + −
Is usually made of wood +/− + + +/−
Appliances
Paper
Toaster Oven Shredder Blender
Usually found in a kitchen + + − +
Usually found in an office − − + −
Usually found in a bedroom − − − −
Has blades − − + +
Warms things + + − −
Has an electric plug + + + +
460  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

of the target word and distinguishing it from monitored. For example, it may also be advanta-
other words or concepts in the same category. geous to work on atypical exemplars of categories
In the top row, write the names of four objects (e.g., albatross rather than robin for bird; duodenum
within a certain semantic category. In the far- rather than nose for body parts, etc.) to enhance
left column, write features that may or may carryover to other words (Kiran, 2008; Wambaugh,
not apply to any of the objects in that category. Mauszycki, et al., 2013).
• Have the person indicate whether a certain The number of features listed and analyzed per
feature fits an object (marked by +) or not target word list may also be manipulated. Hashimoto
(marked with a −). Note that responses are and Frome (2011) found significant improvements
not necessarily absolutely correct or incorrect. in naming for a person with aphasia, 7 years post-​
For example, note the “+/−” designation for onset, when using only three features per target
whether a chair is soft or not in Chart A of word. Although designed to enhance object naming,
Table 31–1. If the client suggests that a certain SFA has been used successfully to enhance naming
feature may or may not apply, and this is of actions (verbs), too; generalization to untreated
true, then this can be acknowledged in the verbs has been less robust (Carragher et al., 2013;
conversation and marked accordingly. If the Peach & Reuter, 2010; Wambaugh et al., 2002; Wam-
client is able to write, have them complete baugh, Mauszycki, & Wright, 2014).
the chart, one row at a time, with a +, −, or Another modification is to adapt SFA to dis-
+/− in each cell. Otherwise, have the person course-level tasks, such as picture description and
tell you what to write. Having them write procedural descriptions. Peach and Reuter (2010)
may improve performance if the person has found that by carrying out SFA at a discourse
concomitant apraxia of speech (Hashimoto level, treatment effects found in connected speech
& Frome, 2011; Kiran, 2008). If the client is and naming generalized significantly to untrained
unsure, a question mark may be entered. action and object names. SFA has been shown to be
Support the activity through discussion, successfully adapted in group treatment formats
questions, answers, and encouraging feedback. (Antonucci, 2009; Falconer & Antonucci, 2012); use
• Once the chart is completed, support the in group treatment contexts may actually facilitate
client in decisions to change any question carryover to the discourse level. Versions of SFA
marks to a + or −. Also, discuss any item for treatment have been developed for computerized
which you do not agree with the response. If practice and mobile apps (Higgins et al., 2012; Tactus
the client provides a convincing argument for Therapy, 2015). Availability of computerized exer-
that response, there is no need to change it. cises and feedback bode well for enhanced practice
• Discuss which items within the category are intensity, which may boost treatment effects.
most alike (share the most features), which
rows have the most similar responses, which
cells have mixed responses and why, and What Is the Status of SFA in Terms
which responses (if any) require further
of Evidence-Based Practice?
verification.

I find it helpful to keep laminated versions of Most SFA studies to date entail single-subject,
semantic feature analysis charts and graphic orga- multiple-​baseline, pre- and post-treatment designs,
nizers on hand, along with erasable markers, so that and case studies. Significant improvements in nam-
the charts can be wiped clear and used repeatedly. ing of objects and actions have been reported for
Dry-erase boards can also be used. As progress is people with dysnomia associated with stroke and
made with the most relevant words, continue to add TBI (Boyle, 2001, 2004; Boyle & Coelho, 1995; Coelho
words in order of their personal relevance. Com- et al., 2000; Davis & Stanton, 2005; Efstratidou et al.,
plexity (see the description of CATE in Chapter 24) 2018; Hashimoto & Frome, 2011; Gravier et al., 2018;
may be manipulated strategically and its effects on Kendall et al., 2019; Maddy et al., 2014; Marcotte &
an individual’s performance and carryover may be Ansaldo, 2010; Massaro & Tompkins, 1992; Peach
31. Specific Approaches for Improving Word Finding and Lexical Processing   461

& Reuter, 2010; Rider et al., 2008). Scholl, McCabe, is also great variability in the duration of time passed
et al. (2021) provide data to support the efficacy of between completion of treatment and follow-up out-
the approach with people who have aphasia and comes assessment.
concomitant apraxia of speech. Improvements have
been noted from 4 months post-onset (e.g., Peach
& Reuter, 2010) to over 10 years post-onset (e.g.,
What Is Phonological Components Analysis?
Rider et al., 2008) of aphasia. Positive effects have
also been noted in a person with PPA (Marcotte &
Ansaldo, 2010). Phonological components analysis (PCA) is an
Improvements are generally noted for trained impairment-focused approach for the remediation of
items, and for items that share semantic features naming deficits in people with aphasia. It is based on
with trained items (Kendall et al., 2019). Most SFA the SFA approach; instead of focusing interactively
studies have concentrated on single-word training, on semantic aspects of target words, the clinician and
with inconsistent generalization of improved lexical client focus on the phonological aspects of words.
retrieval to discourse. Even within studies, certain
individuals tend to respond better to SFA than oth-
ers; also, individuals demonstrate differences in the
On What Principles Is PCA Treatment Based?
amount of carryover to naming of untrained items
(Wambaugh et al., 2013). In a meta-analysis across 12
studies, Quique et al. (2019) highlight the importance Phonological cues and contexts are known to facil-
of attending to individual factors and confirmed that itate naming (Martin et al., 2004, 2006). Having the
dosages matter. Gravier et al. (2018), based on their person generate their own responses regarding
study of 17 people with chronic aphasia, report that prompts about the phonological aspects of words is
the more features of to-be-named items a person thought to stimulate deeper processing than mere
generates themselves during SFA, the better the gen- practice with clinician-generated cues, ideally lead-
eralization and maintenance outcomes. ing to longer-lasting treatment effects (Hickin et al.,
Marshall et al. (2018) describe case studies on 2002; Leonard et al., 2015; Leonard et al., 2008). Use
the use of EVA Park, a virtual platform (described in of picture cues incorporates a semantic aspect to the
Chapter 25) to deliver SFA and VNeST (described in naming task, such that phonological activation may
this chapter) in individual treatment for two people interact with semantic activation to facilitate nam-
with aphasia. Impairment-related results were better ing. Ideally, PCA leads the person to independently
for SFA treatment. They conclude that the platform generate phonological cues when having difficulty
has promise for delivering individual treatment, coming up with a word.
overseen by those who are not speech-language
pathologists (SLPs), which may be especially advan-
tageous for expanding access for people with limited
How Is PCA Treatment Implemented?
access to in-person SLP intervention.
Overall, treatment fidelity throughout the SFA
literature is not strong. This is often by design; sev- The following steps are summarized based on Leon-
eral modifications of SFA have been methodically ard et al. (2008):
studied, as noted earlier. Additionally, SFA research
studies vary in terms of inclusion/exclusion crite- • Use a confrontation naming task to determine
ria of participants, such as those pertaining to type words the client has difficulty naming. Of
of aphasia, etiology of dysnomia, site of lesion, and those, have the client select a set of pictures
time post-onset. They also vary according to mea- representing words that the client finds
sures of improvement and whether these include relevant to typical conversational needs.
discourse-level indices (such as correct information • As with SFA, place a picture at the center of
units), confrontation naming, and/or generalization a chart and ask the person to name it. See
to untrained words before and after treatment. There Figure 31–2 for an example of a PCA chart.
462  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

First sound

First sound associate


Rhymes with…

TARGET
Target
(picture
Image to
to be named
be named)

Number of syllables Final sound

Figure 31–2. PCA chart.

Whether or not the person can name it, ask 5. How many syllables (or beats) are in the word?
them to identify five components related to the
• Regardless of whether the client can name the
word that correspond to whatever is depicted.
word or provide responses to the questions,
You may elicit this by showing categories on
review each component.
the chart and by asking questions.
• For each component, if the client cannot
1. What does it rhyme with? Can you think of provide a response spontaneously, ask them to
a word that rhymes with this? choose one from a list of up to three possible
2. What’s the first sound? What sound does it responses. Show the printed options on a card
start with? and read them aloud.
3. What other word starts with this sound? • If the client cannot provide a correct response
Can you think of another word that begins given a choice, say the correct response and
with the same sound? ask the client to repeat it.
4. What’s the last sound in the word? What • After reviewing the phonological components,
sound does it end with? ask the client to say the word that goes with
31. Specific Approaches for Improving Word Finding and Lexical Processing   463

the picture. If the client is unable to do so, say that intensive and nonintensive PCA treatments may
the word and ask them to repeat it. both be beneficial and that patterns of associated
neural changes differ for different intensity levels.
Leonard et al. (2008) provided treatment three Further research with many more people with apha-
times per week for about 1 hour at a time, with treat- sia is needed to determine what client characteristics
ment discontinued after 15 sessions (or earlier for and levels of treatment intensity may lead to the best
those not scoring above 40% accuracy). treatment gains using PCA. Also, evidence regard-
ing carryover of treatment effects to word finding in
natural conversation is needed.
What Is the Status of PCA in Terms Given that there is growing evidence in support
of the efficacy of both PCA and SFA, it is important
of Evidence-Based Practice?
to consider which approach (or perhaps a combina-
tion of the two) will lead to greater long-term gains
Studies to date have entailed primarily case series. according to specific symptom constellations and
Leonard et al. (2008) administered PCA to 10 people underlying neurological conditions of people with
with varied types of aphasia, all having difficulty aphasia (Neumann, 2018; Nickels, 2002).
with naming; seven demonstrated significantly im-
proved naming and maintained improvements when
tested 4 weeks after the completion of treatment, What Is Verb Network
with some but minimal generalization to untreated
Strengthening Treatment?
words. Those who were able to repeat words tended
to perform better. Leonard et al. (2015) tested a modi-
fied version of PCA that entailed clinician-generated Verb Network Strengthening Treatment (VNeST)
cues. They compared results with the approach in was developed to improve verb retrieval through
which clients generated the cues. Five people with enhanced activation of semantic and grammatical or
aphasia demonstrated gains in word-finding abil- relational aspects of verbs (the verb network). The
ities in PCA regardless of whether cues were gen- goal is to help the client generalize the ability to pro-
erated by the clinician or the client. All maintained duce verbs within sentences and ideally to carry this
gains after 4 weeks, and four of five maintained over to discourse contexts. The approach was devel-
their abilities at 8 weeks posttreatment. Kristensson oped to address challenges with the single-word
and Saldert (2018), in a study of PCA treatment for focus of SFA, which has been used to treat dysnomia
nouns and verbs provided to two people, one with for nouns and verbs. Although VNeST is described
mild and one with severe chronic aphasia, report by some as a method focused on verb retrieval, it is
improvements in confrontation naming and produc- discussed in the literature as a method for enhancing
tion of correct information units in discourse but not word retrieval in general; nouns as well as verbs are
in perceived functional communication. They high- the focus of treatment stimuli and activities.
light that aphasia severity and underlying impair-
ments likely influence responsiveness. According to
Leonard et al. (2015), ensuring more active engage-
On What Principles Is VNeST Based?
ment of a person with aphasia, by having them select
the phonological attributes of words to be targeted,
may help to enhance treatment effects. Since the meaning of verbs is tied to thematic roles,
Leonard et al. (2015) and Rochon et al. (2010) which entail grammatical relationships with other
studied fMRI data to elucidate potential aspects of words, the rationale is to use thematically related
neurological changes associated with PCA treat- words to enhance production at the sentence level.
ment; results are generally not conclusive due to The thematic roles of verbs are emphasized. The-
methodological variability and small numbers of matic roles are defined as the related agent (subject)
participants. Based on fMRI data for two people and patient (object) of a given verb in a given gram-
with aphasia, Marcotte et al. (2018) support findings matical context.
464  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

A single verb can be associated with many differ- patients. The selection of words to be used
ent agents and patients. These associations have been as verbs, patients, and agents may also be
shown to elicit priming effects of verbs on agents and made based on the client’s interests, word
patients that are typically associated with those verbs difficulty (familiarity or frequency of usage,
(McRae et al., 2005). For example, the word celebrate phonological composition, length, etc.), or the
may prime bride and winner as agents and wedding severity of dysnomia. Some verbs have more
and success as patients. This is important in that prim- thematic meanings than others; focusing on
ing is a vital result of semantic activation. verbs with multiple thematic meanings may
In VNeST, the client is led to associate multiple help lead to broader activation of and practice
possible agents and patients with each target verb with the nouns and verbs incorporated into
(Edmonds & Babb, 2011; Edmonds et al., 2009, 2014). treatment, thus enhancing the likelihood of
Repeated activation and use of neural networks asso- generalization to more words. For example,
ciated with verbs is thought to strengthen access to the word play could be thematically related to
verbs as well as to associated nouns that may serve toddler, musician, athlete, and other agents,
as agents and patients. Throughout, encouraging as well as to game, trumpet, football, and
divergent combinations of words, and reflections on other patients.
their semantic associations, evokes multiple sche- • Write each of the following words, each on a
mas that are not constrained by preselected pictures. separate wh- card: who, what, where, when, and
why.

How Is VNeST Implemented? Generation of Agent-Patient Pairs

To carry out VNeST, use the basic steps listed later, • Show the client a target verb card. Instruct
adapted from Edmonds et al. (2009, 2014). the client to make a sentence using that
verb, including an agent and patient. If the
client is unable to do so, ask them to select
Baseline an agent to go with that verb. For example,
if the verb is kick, a prompt might be, “Who
• First, establish a baseline for object and action can kick?” The client might say “soccer
naming. Edmonds et al. (2009) did this using player” or “horse.” Another prompt might be,
the Boston Naming Test (Goodglass & Kaplan, “What can a horse kick?” If the client still has
2001) and the Northwestern Verb Production difficulty producing a sentence, show them
Battery (Thompson, 2002). Edmonds et al. the associated agent and patient cards. Have
(2014) did this by presenting picture cards the client say a sentence while pointing to the
showing actions and possible agents and agent, action, and patient cards.
patients of those actions. Baseline indices of • Lay the who and what cards on a table. Ask
spontaneous speech and discourse will also who can carry out the action associated with
be helpful for indexing potential treatment that verb. Have the client put the agent cards
effects following treatment. under the who card and the patient cards
under the what card. Have the client read the
word pairs aloud.
Stimulus Selection and Creation • Have the client generate their own words to
make a sentence using the verb. Sentences
• Select or create about 10 verb cards for which need not be complete. Encourage use of
the client has low accuracy of naming; for specific words, such as beaver instead of
each, select or create three or four cards animal, professor instead of woman, and so on.
showing possible associated agents and three If the client cannot independently generate an
or four cards showing possible associated agent and patient, ask them to choose possible
31. Specific Approaches for Improving Word Finding and Lexical Processing   465

words from a set of appropriate agent and over to naturalistic conversations, it would be
patient words and foil words (words that do important to do so when using this approach. Also,
not fit semantically with the verb). Edmonds et al. (2014) suggested that integrating
means of explicitly eliciting more production of the
action words, not just the agents and patients, may
Wh- Questions About Agent-Patient Pairs help to better strengthen verb retrieval.

• Have the client choose an agent-patient pair.


Ask wh- questions about the pair, pointing to
What Is the Status of VNeST in Terms
and saying aloud, in context, the words on the
wh- cards, for example,
of Evidence-Based Practice?
• Who kicks balls?
• What does a monster eat? There are few published studies documenting treat-
• Where does a magician perform? When ment outcomes associated with VNeST. All have
does a gardener plant rutabaga? small numbers of participants; they entail single-case
• Why does a criminal rob a bank? designs and one small-group study. Results of a
study with four participants (Edmonds et al., 2009)
In this context, engage in natural conversation showed generalization to untrained stimuli imme-
related to the target words. diately following treatment and 1 month after study.
This included improvements in single-word noun
• Choose another agent-patient pair and ask
production. Three of the four participants showed
wh- questions incorporating those words.
improvement in the ability to produce sentences
with subjects, verbs, and objects relating to the rele-
Semantic Judgments vant thematic vocabulary.
Edmonds and Babb (2011), applying the method
• Remove the cards from the table. with two people with aphasia, reported significant
• Read 12 sentences that contain the target verb, gains for one participant but not another, at least
some that make sense and some that do not. not according to the targeted indices. For the par-
For each, ask the client whether the sentence ticipant with greater gains, they reported reduced
makes sense. Examples might be as follows: neologisms, fewer verb errors, and generalization
• The toddler kicked the ball. to untrained verbs. They interpreted these gains to
• The gerbil kicked the coyote. suggest that activation of verb networks has wide-
• The warrior kicked the enemy. spread effects in terms of semantic, phonological,
• The donkey kicked the farmer. and lexical aspects of lexical retrieval. Edmonds et al.
• The pencil kicked the bottle. (2014) reported improvements in lexical retrieval in
sentence contexts but mixed results at the discourse
level. Furnas and Edmonds (2014) tested a computer-
Generation of Agent-Patient Pairs Again ized version of VNesT with two people with aphasia
using typed responses. They found improvements
• Without referring to the cards, ask the client to in lexical retrieval for trained and untrained stimuli
name three verbs and the agents and patients and recommended further study of their influence on
that go with each. treatment dosage. The approach is represented in some
• Have the client produce a sentence using the treatment apps (e.g., through Tactus Therapy), which
words they generate. may be especially beneficial for use by SLPs who do
• Give feedback and encouragement as not have extensive background in theoretical linguis-
appropriate. tics and for those who do not have time to assemble
materials required for carrying out this approach.
Although the treatment protocol as described There has not been a high degree of treatment
does not include explicit training to enhance carry­ fidelity in terms of dosage, means of word selection,
466  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

or numbers of items used across studies to date. language (Fink et al., 1992; Loverso et al., 1988).
Indices used for outcomes assessment have varied Tasks include saying, copying, writing, and repeat-
(e.g., percentage of complete utterances, overall ing the subject (agent) and object (patient) for each
informativeness, posttreatment object and action verb and answering wh- questions.
naming, communication partner perceptions); out-
comes indices have also varied in terms of how
long post-retreatment maintenance of effects was
On What Principles Is VAC Treatment Based?
assessed. Also, eclectic groups of participants, with
a wide range of dysnomia severity, aphasia types,
and ages, have been included, making it difficult Verb as Core is based on the principle that verbs
to make conclusions regarding just what types of carry critical meaning for communication and lit-
individuals may benefit from this approach. Some erally serve as the “core” of all sentences. People
participants in studies to date may have had diffi- with agrammatism have particular challenges with
culty with sentence construction due to grammatical accessing verbs, yet impairment-level treatment
or cognitive deficits rather than verb retrieval; this approaches tend to focus much more on nouns than
might have obscured measures of positive overall they do on verbs.
effects. Edmonds (2016) offers an analysis of poten-
tial predictors and mechanisms for improvement
associated with VNesT.
How Is VAC Treatment Implemented?
Overall, it appears that not all people with
dysnomia benefit equally from this approach but
that it holds promise for some individuals. Still, Treatment is carried out methodically, beginning
the theoretical rationale and evidence to date are with a baseline assessment. The treatment steps are
compelling. More studies with larger numbers of summarized here, according to guidance provided
participants, assessment of long-term benefits, and by Loverso et al. (1988).
carryover to natural conversation in a real-world
context are needed. Of course, in clinical practice, it • Collect baseline data for each of two levels.
is important to index gains in terms of life participa- • For Level I, present verbs; for each verb,
tion goals for each individual. ask “who” and “what” questions to elicit a
subject-verb (agent-patient) response. First,
give a subject-verb pair and ask the person
to say it and write it. Second, give a set of
What Is Verb as Core?
four possible subjects and have the person
select which one of the subjects goes with
Verb as Core (VAC), also known as verbal cueing, is the verb.
another treatment approach focused on verbs. Like • For Level II, add questions with “what,”
VNeST, VAC was developed in recognition of the fact “when,” “how,” “where,” and “why” to
that verbs carry the greatest meaning about events elicit subject-action-object responses. First,
conveyed in a sentence (Fink et al., 1992). In fact, the give a subject-verb-object combination and
early research that led to the Verb as Core approach, have the person say it and write it. Second,
and clinical studies of Verb as Core, appear to have give a set of four possible subjects and
greatly influenced the development of VNeST. have the person select a subject that goes
The treatment is intended to improve expres- with the verb and object. Start treatment
sive verb use and verb understanding as well as at a level for which the individual’s
language performance in general in people with performance is about 60% accurate.
agrammatic aphasia. The approach involves audi- • Initiate treatment from one level/sublevel
tory and visual processing of sentences with a focus to the next, using words that are relevant
on verbs, and practice through spoken and written in terms of the individual’s everyday use
31. Specific Approaches for Improving Word Finding and Lexical Processing   467

of language. The criterion for progressing detailed. The influence of VAC on abilities to use
from one level or sublevel to the next is 90% verbs in the context of a variety of complex sentence
accuracy across three consecutive sessions. types has not been studied methodically. The pub-
lished work on this method does not address car-
ryover of improvements into natural conversation.
What Is the Status of VAC in Terms The authors of VAC do not specify the means
of selecting verbal stimuli for the approach or the
of Evidence-Based Practice?
number of items to be presented across sessions. In
any case, as always, it is important to select verbal
Treatment studies have indicated improvement in stimuli most relevant to the individual, given that
performance on language battery scores (Loverso we know there is a greater likelihood of carryover
et al., 1988; Loverso et al., 1979) and improved abil- for trained versus untrained words across lexically
ity to generate agents and patients for trained and oriented methods.
untrained verbs (Prescott et al., 1982) in small num- Although further research could be carried
bers of people with aphasia. Loverso et al. (1979) out on the approach, aphasiologists studying verb-​
reported that language gains were preserved for at focused treatment have tended to delve further into
least 1 month following treatment. Katz (2001) imple- other approaches on verb naming (e.g., cueing hier-
mented a computerized VAC treatment program archy approaches for verb naming and VNeST) and
with one person with aphasia and reported improve- on grammatical expression and reception (e.g., Treat-
ments in language battery scores. Few treatment ment of Underlying Forms and Mapping Therapy;
studies have incorporated the approach as originally see Chapter 32) in people agrammatic with aphasia.

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter of evidence-based practice for specific
that are new to you or that you have not yet treatment methods intended to enhance
mastered. word finding and lexical processing?
2. For each of the approaches addressed in this 6. What do you see as the greatest research
chapter, describe why it would be considered needs related to methods for enhancing
to be restitutive, compensatory, or both. word finding and lexical processing?
3. For each of the approaches in this chapter, 7. What strategies would you use to
make a list of materials you would want to maximize transfer of treatment gains
have on hand so that you would be prepared made through word finding and lexical
to carry out the approach in actual treatment processing treatments to real-world use of
sessions. communication?
4. With a partner, demonstrate a treatment
session using each approach in this chapter. Additional teaching and learning materials are
www
5. How would you summarize the status available on the companion website.
CHAPTER
32
Specific Approaches for Improving Syntax

In this chapter, we review three impairment-based processing problems, such that these are a potential
approaches focused on reducing syntactic deficits: confound in studies of people with agrammatism
Treatment of Underlying Forms (TUF), Mapping (Dickey & Thompson, 2007). Fortunately, aphasiol-
Therapy, and the Sentence Production Program for ogists are continuing to strengthen the theory base
Aphasia (SPPA, an updated version of the Language and the evidence base for treatment efficacy regard-
Program for Syntax Stimulation). Recall that two ing syntactic approaches. Some are tackling issues of
approaches reviewed as lexical approaches in Chap- individual differences that will help us better under-
ter 31, VNeST and Verb as Core, may also be consid- stand who will benefit most from specific aspects of
ered syntactic approaches because they entail work syntactically focused approaches.
with verbs in the context of phrases and sentences. After reading and reflecting on the content in
Syntactic approaches are intended for those with this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in
grammatical processing impairments, that is, people your own words, the following queries about TUF,
with agrammatism. This is stating the obvious per- Mapping Therapy, and SPPA:
haps; still, it is an important point in terms of the
overall purpose of the approaches reviewed here. 1. What is it?
Although people with acquired neurogenic disor- 2. On what principles is it based?
ders other than aphasia may have difficulty with syn- 3. How is it implemented?
tax, the target population for which the approaches 4. What is its status in terms of evidence-based
described in this chapter have been developed is practice?
people with aphasia.
One limitation of research on specific syntactic
treatment approaches to date is the lack of agree-
What Is Treatment of Underlying Forms?
ment on the nature of agrammatism, limiting the
extent of the theoretical basis for specific methods in
terms of who might benefit most. Recall from Chap- Treatment of Underlying Forms (TUF) is an approach
ter 10 that some aphasiologists theorize that people to help people with agrammatism (typically with
with agrammatism lack the basic syntactic represen- Broca’s aphasia) improve comprehension and
tation rules for grammar, or at least access to those expression of sentence structure. The focus is on
rules. Others suggest that people with agrammatic developing metalinguistic awareness of the under­
aphasia have difficulty mapping syntactic structures lying, abstract properties of language, such as the
to their thematic roles. Still others consider agram- role of verbs, verb arguments, and phrase move-
matism to be due largely to short-term and working ment. Practice is encouraged with increasingly
memory deficits, which are not necessarily limited to complex sentence structures. The ultimate goals are
linguistic processing. It is likely that there are varied to generalize treatment effects in terms of reduced
forms of syntax challenges depending on underlying agrammatic speech and improved comprehension
neurological deficits. Further complicating matters, for sentences not used in treatment and in sponta-
morphological problems may contribute to syntactic neous language use (Thompson & Shapiro, 2005).

469
470  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

An example of a subject relative sentence is, “It was


On What Principles Is TUF Based? my aunt who bought this dress.”
A first step in TUF is to help the client establish
TUF is based primarily on government binding the- knowledge of word and argument relations in non-
ory, a component of Chomsky’s derivational linguis- canonical sentences. These result from a change in
tic theory (Chomsky, 1986). The clinician need not be a order of elements within a sentence, for example,
master of linguistic theory to carry out this approach. movement of who or what did something or had some-
For a review of the relevant grammatical theory, see thing done to it (as in object cleft sentences) and noun
Shapiro’s (1997) tutorial article on syntax. Here, let’s phrase movement (as in passive sentences). In the
address the basic constructs that are important to example sentence, “I know who the cat scratched,”
understand in considering TUF (as well as Mapping the underlying meaning is that the cat (the agent)
Therapy, the next approach we are considering). scratched (the verb) someone (the theme), and that
As noted in the discussion of VNeST (in Chap- I know who was scratched (again, the theme).
ter 31), lexical information associated with verbs is
represented within sentences, not just in verbs them-
selves. Within the context of sentences, verbs carry a
How Is TUF Implemented?
great deal of the information, beyond just their own
meaning as individual words. The association of a
verb with its context within a sentence is called argu- The order and nature of steps are conveyed differ-
ment structure. ently across the various published descriptions of
Arguments are typically noun phrases assigned TUF. Still, some commonalities are noted, and these
a thematic role (agent, theme, and goal) that fill par- are summarized here to provide practical guidance.
ticipant, object, and/or indirect object positions. In Some accounts of the procedures entailed in TUF are
the sentence, “Pierre sang an aria,” Pierre (the agent, specific to certain types of sentence structure. Given
or the subject) and aria (the theme, also called the that more generalization tends to occur with training
patient or object) are the arguments of the verb sang. focused on more complex sentence structures, and
When we develop language, we learn the argument given that training with more diverse sentence types
structure of verbs. We learn, for example, that for may lead to greater gains than just focusing on single
singing to occur, there must be someone singing sentence types, this summary includes references to
(an agent). We learn, too, that singing may have an a variety of sentence structures. More sentence types
object (a song, a lullaby, an aria, etc.) or that it may can be added, and it is possible to work on specific
occur without any object (as in, “Rahaida sings”). sentence types if there is a clinically important rea-
Phrase movement, changes in word order that son to do so.
are reflected in syntactic changes, is another import-
ant aspect of using words in sentence contexts. Word
order is an important aspect of syntax and is vital Ensuring Metalinguistic Awareness
to the constraints of phrase movement in various
languages. Phrase movement is what makes sen- The first phase of TUF entails getting a person with
tences complex. aphasia to develop metalinguistic awareness of
In English, active sentence structures (which verbs, verb arguments, and the difference between
have a subject-verb-object word order, as in “Lati- canonical and noncanonical sentence structure.
sha planted nasturtiums”) are known as canonical Thompson (2008) refers to this as “thematic role
sentences. Noncanonical sentences entail phrase training.”
movement. Phrase movement occurs in passive
sentence structures (object-verb-subject, as in “The • If the person is not aware of basic
nasturtiums were planted by Latisha”) and in sen- metalinguistic terms, engage in brief tutoring.
tences with embedded clauses (object relative and For example, explain the following:
subject relative sentences). An example of an object • The agent is the one doing the action
relative sentence is, “I know who the cat scratched.” (sometimes called the subject).
32. Specific Approaches for Improving Syntax   471

• The theme is the one having the action and reiterate the sentence, pointing to the
done to it (sometimes called a patient or an relevant components of the picture.
object). • Lay out additional word cards: who and
• The verb is the action in the sentence. it was.
• Use word cards to talk about the agent, • Say, “It was the dog who chased the cat,”
verb, and theme in a canonical sentence. while pointing to the corresponding word
For example, to develop metalinguistic cards.
awareness of passive sentence structure, do • Show a picture of a lion biting a trainer.
the following: • Ask the client to make a sentence just like
• Lay down three word cards in the the last one (“It was the tiger who bit the
following order: dog, chased, cat. trainer”).
• Show a picture of a dog chasing a cat.
• Say, “The dog chased the cat.”
• Point out, on the word cards, which is Creating Noncanonical Sentences
the agent, theme, and verb, and reiterate
the sentence, pointing to the relevant The second phase entails having the client practice
components of the picture. Add comments complex noncanonical sentences. Thompson (2008)
such as the following: calls this phase “sentence building.”
n Let’s look at this word chased. It’s the
verb. It tells us what the action is. • Say a noncanonical sentence.
n Notice this word dog. The dog is the one • Show an image and have the client say a
doing the chasing. sentence using that same form but pertaining
n This word, cat, tells us who got chased. to the image. For example, say, “The cake was
• Change the order of the word cards to: cat dropped by the man who fell down.” Then
chased dog. show an image of a man lying on the floor,
• Say, “The cat was chased by the dog.” holding a cake. Provide the following word
• Put the cards back in canonical order and cards: cake, dropped, man, fell.
have the client say the sentence in the active • Ask the client to put them in order and use a
(canonical) form. similar sentence structure.
• Change the card order again (or have
the client do so) and ask them to say the
sentence in its passive (noncanonical) form. Thematic Role Training
• Review both forms again, discussing which
cards represent the action, theme, and verb. The third phase entails practice with the previous
• Depending on how well the client grasps this, structures, modeled in new combinations of words:
you might practice with a few sets of cards
and corresponding pictures (e.g., “The lion • For example, to work further on subject
bit the trainer” and “The trainer was bitten by relative sentences, you might show a picture
the lion”). of a boy who has fallen off a bike. Ask the
• Extend this same sort of explanation and client to make up a sentence of a similar
practice with other types of sentences. For structure as the previous one (in the practice
example, to develop metalinguistic awareness phase). A possible target sentence might be,
of subject relative sentence structure, do the “The boy who rode the bike crashed.”
following: • Work further on other types of sentences.
• Lay down three word cards in the Present a spoken model of the target sentence
following order: dog, chased, cat. structure:
• Say, “The dog chased the cat.” • Say a simple passive sentence while
• Ask the client to point out on the word showing an image illustrating the sentence
cards which is the agent, theme, and verb, (e.g., “The juice was drunk by the toddler”).
472  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

• Ask the client to produce a similar sentence tures treated or to less complex structures. Impor-
corresponding to a different image (e.g., tantly, training of structures that are more complex
“The bird was chased by the cat”). has been shown to lead to more wide-ranging effects
• Whether or not the client’s production was than training on simpler structures (Thompson &
accurate, line up word or phrase cards to Shapiro, 2005). In other words, treatment involv-
make up the canonical (active) form of the ing complex sentence forms leads to improvements
sentence, “The cat chased the bird.” more efficiently than treatment through a hierarchy
• Ask them to identify the verb (chased), the of simple to complex sentences. This is an important
subject (cat), and the object (bird) of the extension of CATE (see Chapter 23) as it pertains to
sentence. syntax (Thompson et al., 2003).
• Rearrange the cards to complete the target, Thompson and Shapiro (2005) reported func-
noncanonical, sentence (“The bird was tional magnetic resonance imaging findings of neu-
chased by the cat”). ral correlates of functional changes associated with
• Have the client read the sentence aloud. TUF; they indicated that the right hemisphere area
• Remove the cards and have the client homologous to left hemisphere Broca’s area had
provide the same sentence or one with a increased activation in people who were treated
similar structure. with TUF, compared to those of a control group.
Most TUF studies to date entail single-case designs.
Ballard and Thompson (1999) suggested that
Practice participants who did not achieve generalization in
their study may have been limited due to the sever-
• Shuffle sets of word cards that include ity of their aphasia and/or concomitant cognitive
semantically plausible combinations of deficits. Given that metalinguistic knowledge is the
subjects, verbs, and objects, plus the it was focus, those with prior interest in and familiarity
and who cards. Have the client lay them with grammar and linguistics may benefit from this
out and make sentences of varied syntactic approach the most (Thompson & Shapiro, 2005).
forms with them. Treatment fidelity has not been consistent across
studies, in terms of dosage or targeted sentence struc-
As noted earlier, several modifications of the tures. Also, indices used for outcomes assessment
nature and order of procedures for TUF have been vary (e.g., mean length of utterance, proportion of
implemented. Murray, Timberlake, and Eberle (2007) grammatical sentences, production of verbs relative
reported positive results for a person with Broca’s to nouns, correctness of judgments about sentence
aphasia using an adaptation of TUF focused on writ- anomalies). Participants with mild to moderate
ten as opposed to spoken language. Results based on agrammatism have been included in most studies;
Sentactics, a computerized version of TUF (Thomp- people with more severe forms have been studied
son et al., 2010) were promising. far less. More studies with larger numbers of par-
ticipants and assessments of long-term benefits and
carryover to natural conversation in real-world con-
What Is the Status of TUF in Terms text are needed. Also, additional outcomes research
pertaining to computerized practice based on TUF
of Evidence-Based Practice?
will be important.

Improvement has been demonstrated in terms of


trained as well as untrained wh- structures and noun
What Is Mapping Therapy?
phrase movement structures in people with agram-
matism (Ballard & Thompson, 1999; Jacobs & Thomp-
son, 2000; Mack & Thompson, 2017; Thompson et al., Mapping Therapy is a treatment method, rooted in
1998; Thompson et al., 2003; Thompson & Shapiro, linguistic theory, that is designed to treat deficits in
2005). Overall, generalization of treatment effects thematic role assignment in people with agramma-
appears to be limited primarily to the specific struc- tism. As noted earlier, challenges with role assign-
32. Specific Approaches for Improving Syntax   473

ment involve assigning roles to agents, patients/ • Instruct the client to underline critical words
themes, and goals within the semantic structure of on the printed sentence. For example, ask
a sentence. that they underline the adjective in an object
position, adjective in subject position, subject
noun phrase, object noun phrase, cleft subject,
On What Principles Is cleft object, object relative embedded in an
object noun phrase, object relative embedded
Mapping Therapy Based?
in a subject noun phrase, or a subject relative
embedded in a subject noun phrase.
People with agrammatism tend to have difficulty • Following each probe, provide immediate
mapping relations between assigned thematic roles feedback by affirming the correct underlined
for words and the surface syntax by which words are words if the response was correct. If the
combined to form sentences (Thompson et al., 2003). response was incorrect, underline the correct
Mapping Therapy is focused on taking apart and words.
reorganizing the structure of sentences. The overall • When the client makes an error, encourage
treatment goal is increased competence in compre- them to analyze the error and determine why
hending and producing more complex, noncanon- it is an error. If the person cannot determine
ical sentences. The goal for production is correct why their response was wrong, provide an
syntactic structure. Treatment involves analyzing explanation.
grammatical roles of nouns and verbs (e.g., nouns as
subjects, objects, etc.) and identifying semantic/the- Byng, Nickels, and Black (1994) suggested using
matic roles in sentences (e.g., agent, patient/theme, color and spatially coded templates to strengthen the
etc.; Jacobs & Thompson, 2000). association between thematic roles and word order.
Each stimulus sentence may be printed on a card with
the syntactic class of items (e.g., noun phrase, verb,
object, etc.) underlined with corresponding colors.
How Is Mapping Therapy Implemented?

Each step in this treatment method targets the rela- What Is the Status of Mapping Therapy
tionships between nouns and verbs and/or between
in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice?
semantics and syntax. Treatment progresses as the
client successfully masters analysis of canonical sen-
tence structures (typical subject-verb-object structure) Results of studies with single-subject designs and
and becomes increasingly competent in comprehend- studies with small samples of people with apha-
ing more complex, noncanonical sentences. Through- sia have demonstrated that the approach improves
out treatment, sentences are made longer and more production of semantically reversible, canonical
complex by adding direct objects, modifiers, and sentences (Byng et al., 1994; Nickels et al., 1991;
prepositional phrases (Fink et al., 1992; Schwartz Schwartz et al., 1994). Overall, participants showed
et al., 1994). The following steps are adapted from a fewer gains, if any, in comprehension. Fink et al.
description of the approach by Schwartz et al. (1994): (1998) implemented Mapping Therapy using only
transitive verbs to promote the comprehension of
• Present a printed sentence. Have the client all semantically reversible sentence types. They
read it. Support them in reading it (e.g., reported generalization to all sentence types, pos-
pointing to words as they are spoken), and sibly because mapping the subject noun to the role
also read it aloud with them. of the agent helps identify the semantic relationship
• Ask probe questions. For example, ask which between the two.
word is the verb, which is the agent, and Rochon et al. (2005) tested a version of Map-
which is the patient/theme. Mix up the order ping Therapy focused on the production of canon-
in which you ask these probe questions for ical and noncanonical reversible sentences with
each sentence. three people with nonfluent aphasia. Participants
474  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

generalized trained sentence structures across two called the Helm Elicited Language Program for Syn-
tasks that required producing constrained sentences. tax Stimulation (HELPSS). SPPA is a commercially
They made slight improvements in narrative tasks. available, packaged program. The revision from
Sentence comprehension abilities, however, did not HELPSS to SPPA entailed a reduced number of sen-
improve significantly. tence types (11 to 8), editing for a gender bias toward
Kiran et al. (2012) developed and tested a new males, and addition of wh- questions.
approach based on Mapping Therapy. In addition to
assigning thematic roles and syntactic categories
to written words, participants manipulated objects
to enact thematic roles conveyed in sentences and On What Principles Is SPPA Treatment Based?
also engaged in sentence-picture matching. Overall,
sentence comprehension improved. Importantly, The rationale behind the SPPA and HELPSS approaches
some generalization of skills was noted in less is that people with agrammatism are limited in
complex sentence structures when more complex grammatical performance because of impaired or
sentence structures were trained. This is consistent inconsistent access to grammatical knowledge, not a
with other studies suggesting that it may be espe- loss of grammatical knowledge (Gleason et al., 1975;
cially helpful to tune into CATE (see Chapter 24) Goodglass et al., 1972; Kolk & Heeschen, 1990; Line-
in considering this and other approaches aimed at barger et al., 2004). In contrast to TUF and Mapping
grammatical abilities. Therapy, SPPA is based not so much on linguistic
Further research on Mapping Therapy, includ- theory as on the notion that repeated and method-
ing studies with larger sample sizes and control ical stimulation of certain morphosyntactic forms
groups, studies controlling for treatment inten- will lead to improvements in the use of those forms.
sity, and studies addressing carryover of treatment The primary goal of SPPA is to increase the vari-
effects to naturalistic contexts, is needed. Individual ety and complexity of spoken utterances through
differences related to memory, attention, and interest sentence completion tasks tied to brief “stories” rep-
in the approach may affect effectiveness, so should resented in supportive pictures.
also be studied.
As with all approaches, it is important to indi-
vidualize the nature of the verbal stimuli used. It is
also important to support discussion about what How Is SPPA Implemented?
might seem like challenging intellectual material
about grammar (even to some speech-language
The method involves a hierarchy of eight sentence
pathologists [SLPs]). People who had limited knowl-
types, each with two controlled levels of grammat-
edge about grammar before the onset of aphasia
ical complexity. The sentence types are shown in
might not be appropriate candidates. Another lim-
Table 32–1.
itation for some could be the inability to generalize
Given that SPPA entails a specific set of pictures
mapping abilities to spontaneous comprehension
and verbal prompts in a published stimulus book,
and production during conversation and in writing.
you need to have access to the commercialized pro-
gram to actually administer the treatment (Helm-​
Estabrooks & Nicholas, 2000).
What Is the Sentence Production
Program for Aphasia? • For each sentence type, at the first level (A),
ask the client to repeat the target sentence that
The Sentence Production Program for Aphasia (SPPA; you present in a story format, along with an
Helm-Estabrooks & Nicholas, 2000) is a stimulation accompanying picture. The authors provide
approach (described in Chapter 25) designed to this example:
enhance sentence production in people with agram- Clinician: Nick’s school bus arrives in
matism. It is based on an earlier treatment program 15 minutes and Nick is still asleep, so his
32. Specific Approaches for Improving Syntax   475

Table 32–1. Sentence Types for Sentence Production Charts are provided for summarizing scores before,
Program for Aphasia during, and after treatment.

Type 1: Imperative Intransitive


Type 2: Imperative Transitive What Is the Status of SPPA and HELPSS
Type 3: Wh- Interrogative — What and Who in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice?
Type 4: Wh- Interrogative — Where and When
To date, published studies on this approach are
Type 5: Declarative Transitive based on HELPSS and not SPPA. All are case studies
Type 6: Declarative Intransitive or single-subject design studies with one to six par-
ticipants. Helm-Estabrooks and Ramsberger (1986)
Type 7: Comparative reported results for six people with Broca’s aphasia
Type 8: Yes-No Questions who received HELPSS treatment for approximately
80 sessions each. They noted significant improve-
ments in terms of the number of correct sentence
construction types and grammatical morphemes
mother tells him, “Wake up!” What does
produced in narratives. They also reported that
his mother say?
treatment via telephone was effective for one male
Client: Wake up! (p. 5) studied over three half-hour sessions per week for
• For the next level (B), ask the client to approximately 7 months.
complete your “story” by answering a Fink et al. (1995) reported evidence of general-
question. Helm-Estabrooks and Nicholas ization to a novel sentence elicitation task (involv-
(2000) provided this example: ing an untrained structure) for four nonfluent people
treated with probe questions and pictures used in
Clinician: Nick’s school bus arrives in
HELPSS. In another study based on HELPSS, Doyle,
15 minutes and Nick is still asleep. So
Goldstein, and Bourgeois (1987) showed improve-
what does his mother tell him?
ments in generalization of similar sentence con-
Client: Wake up! (p. 5) structions in people with Broca’s aphasia but not
• Present items for Levels A and B consecutively generalization to novel sentences or to natural
for each sentence. Once a set criterion level conversation.
has been met for Level A, present the items in Studies of the generalization of SPPA and
Level B alone. HELPSS to other tasks and spontaneous usage are
• Once a set criterion level has been met for problematic in that some limit the types of sentences
Level B, move on to the next sentence type. elicited. Also, cognitive and linguistic demands
• Continue until the client can produce all across tasks used to demonstrate generalization vary
sentence types at Level B. across studies (Fink et al., 1995). The generalization
of trained and untrained sentence types to everyday
Scoring sheets are provided in the commercialized communication using SPPA and HELPSS has not
program. Scores given are as follows: been thoroughly investigated.
Research is needed to demonstrate the efficacy
• 1 = Fully correct response of SPPA overall, with attention to treatment dosage,
• 0.5 = Partially correct response (only one word carryover of gains to untrained sentences and sen-
produced or word produced in error) tence types, carryover to natural conversation, and
• 0 = Incorrect response (two or more erroneous maintenance of any benefits. Also, given evidence
words or words omitted) (based on other methods for syntax treatment) that
• NA = Not administered (item not presented training with more complex syntactic structures may
because the criterion was not met for the lead to gains in untrained simpler structures (e.g.,
previous level) Dickey & Thompson, 2007; Thompson et al., 2003,
476  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

2010), it will be important to test the assumption that viewers with and without aphasia consistently
that it is best to progress from simple to complex understand what is depicted, which may be of par-
structures in SPPA treatment. In addition, it will ticular concern if the method were to be applied in
be important to validate the images used to ensure languages other than English (Reis et al., 2019).

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter 6. What do you see as the greatest research needs
that are new to you or that you have not yet related to methods for improving syntax?
mastered. 7. What strategies would you use to maximize
2. For each of the approaches addressed in transfer of treatment gains made through
this chapter, describe why it would be syntactic treatments to real-world use of
considered to be restitutive, compensatory, communication?
or both. 8. Imagine that you were asked to provide an
3. For each of the approaches in this chapter, in-service for practicing SLPs to help them
make a list of materials you would want to understand the theories underlying syntactic
have on hand so that you would be prepared approaches to language treatment.
to carry out the approach in actual treatment a. What are the most clinically relevant
sessions. concepts on which you would focus?
4. With a partner, demonstrate a treatment b. What terminology would be most
session using each approach in this chapter. important for your participants to know?
5. How would you summarize the status
of evidence-based practice for specific More materials to foster teaching and learning on
www
treatment methods intended to improve this chapter’s content may be found on the com-
syntax? panion website.
CHAPTER
33
Specific Approaches for
Improving Reading and Writing

Recall that reading and writing abilities tend to be


similar in severity to listening and speaking abili-
What Are Basic Principles That Underlie
ties in people with aphasia. Still, there are some Most Writing- and Reading-Focused
who have disproportionate challenges with read- Programs for People With Aphasia?
ing and/or writing. Functional use of both involves
interactive cognitive, linguistic, and perceptual (and Most approaches developed specifically to help fos-
in the case of writing, motor) processes (Beeson & ter reading and writing abilities are based on psy-
Henry, 2008). Many of the approaches we discussed cholinguistic models of language processing, such as
in the previous four chapters and in Section VI those discussed in Chapter 4 and illustrated in Fig-
include use of reading and writing as modalities in ure 4–2. If you check out the original work cited in
the method, and as overall potential modalities for the descriptions of each of the approaches described
communication. in this chapter, you find many well-conceived sche-
In this chapter, we focus on examples of treat- matic (mostly box-and-arrow) illustrations of writ-
ment approaches that target reading and/or writing ing and reading processes that help us consider the
specifically, at the impairment level: Copy and Recall possible levels of breakdown when a person has
Treatment (CART), Anagram and Copy Treatment trouble writing or reading.
(ACT), the problem-solving approach, Multiple Oral A process analysis approach to assessment,
Rereading (MOR), and Oral Reading for Language explored in detail in Chapter 19, is essential to deter-
in Aphasia (ORLA). Before delving into specific mining what, specifically, affects an individual’s abil-
writing- and reading-focused programs, it is a good ity to read and/or write. The selective disruption of
idea to review the psycholinguistic and cognitive any of the component processes involved in reading or
neuropsychological theories and principles that sup- writing can lead to a form of dyslexia or dysgraphia,
port impairment-oriented treatment of reading and respectively. Many of the assessment tools described
writing. After reading and reflecting on the content in Chapter 20 help us to discern whether an individu-
in this chapter, you will ideally be able to answer, in als’ dysgraphia and/or dyslexia lies at the deep, pho-
your own words, the following queries about CART, nological, or surface level. Without a solid grasp on
ACT, MOR, and ORLA: why a person is having trouble writing or reading,
we cannot tailor a treatment program to their specific
1. What is it? writing or reading needs at the impairment level.
2. On what principles is it based? Writing- and reading-specific approaches meant
3. How is it implemented? to enhance communication require that the client
4. What is its status in terms of evidence-based have a preserved semantic system (Wright et al.,
practice? 2008). This is important to consider because a person

477
478  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

can write a word without necessarily being aware writing practice through a progression of single-​
of what it means. Writing may be accomplished via word writing tasks (Beeson et al, 2003). Because it
phonological spelling — that is, segmenting audi- involves progressively taking away written exam-
tory input into its component sounds, translating ples, it has also been called delayed-copy treatment
phonemes into corresponding graphemes, and then (Rapp & Kane, 2002). Good candidates for CART
converting graphemes to letters, bypassing seman- have good visual recognition abilities and the ability
tic representations that are essential to conveying to write letters (graphomotor abilities).
meaningful content through writing (Beeson &
Henry, 2008; Raymer et al., 2003).
The goal of writing-specific treatment is to help
On What Principles Is CART Based?
a person independently and effectively communi-
cate information (convey requests and protests, ask
and answer questions, and relay personal informa- The rationale for CART is that repeated attempts to
tion) through writing. The goal of reading-specific accurately spell target words assist in activating the
treatment is to help a person independently and words’ graphemic representations.
effectively comprehend information in the written
modality. As for all types of communication inter-
vention, goals for both should be couched in the
How Is CART Implemented?
context in which a person will immediately or even-
tually incorporate the benefits of treatment.
As you know, many people with dysgraphia due Steps for carrying out CART described here are
to stroke or brain injury have paralysis or paresis of based primarily on Beeson et al. (2003):
the dominant hand. Be sure to provide encourage-
ment that it is okay for handwriting or printing not to • Assemble a set of cards with personally
look anywhere close to perfect. Also, if you are used relevant line drawings printed on each.
to having two functional hands, it is easy to forget Provide a paper and pencil or pen to the
how hard it can be to stabilize a paper while writing. client.
Using a clipboard and helping by holding onto a cor- • Turn over one card at a time. Say the word
ner of the paper as the client writes can be helpful. that corresponds to the item depicted. Then
Here are two additional important points to ask the client to write the word.
keep in mind when considering reading and writ- • If the client writes the word correctly, move
ing treatments: on to the next card. If not, write the word or
show a printed version of the word already
• Verbal stimuli used within all reading and prepared. Ask the client to copy the word
writing approaches should be personalized to three times. Depending on the client’s
each individual. responsiveness, you might cue the client to
• Not all people with an acquired neurogenic write it again as needed until three versions
language disorder were literate prior to onset; are complete.
working on reading and writing is not likely • Once the word is written three times, take
to be an appropriate goal for them. This may away all of the examples of the written word.
seem obvious, but many speech-language Show the drawing again, and ask the client
pathologists (SLPs) neglect to consider this to write the word three times. Give feedback
important fact in treatment planning. after each attempt, and then cover that written
word before the next attempt. If the client
cannot write the word without a model, start
the process again with a different picture. If
What Is Copy and Recall Treatment?
possible, come back to that picture with which
they had difficulty during the same session.
Copy and Recall Treatment (CART) is an impairment-​ • Provide CART homework to be done 6 days
focused stimulation method that entails repeated per week. For each day, the client is to (a) label
33. Specific Approaches for Improving Reading and Writing   479

pictures, copying the same written word 20 of four word sets. Interestingly, if the untrained
times for each picture, and then (b) write the words had a similar spelling to the trained words,
word on a separate “test” page on which only especially at the beginnings of the words, general-
the picture is shown, without looking at any ization was better.
written model. Keep in mind that the type of feedback we pro-
• As treatment progresses, include old and new vide to our clients may shape their degree of suc-
words in the homework. Present mastered cess when they engage in CART, and the degree of
words less frequently. improvement may not correspond to their sense
of enjoyment of the approach. Raymer et al. (2010)
Helm-Estabrooks et al. (2014) suggested a means implemented adapted versions of CART in which
of establishing a hierarchy for target words: Consider participants were given overt feedback about their
short before long words, and words with regular spell- errors versus when errors were not overtly cor-
ing before irregular words. Of course, since pictures rected. Although the participants said the errorless
are involved, it is important that words be imageable approach was preferred and less frustrating, three of
(i.e., concrete, or easy to picture in our minds). four actually made greater gains in the approach in
which errors were overtly addressed by the clinician.
Clausen and Beeson (2003) showed how group
What Is the Status of CART in Terms treatment may be useful in enhancing the effects of
CART following individualized CART treatment.
of Evidence-Based Practice?
They had four people with Broca’s aphasia engage
in weekly group sessions and daily homework.
Research on CART is based primarily on case stud- Once words were mastered in the group context,
ies and single-case design experiments. Beeson et al. person-to-person interactions were set up with unfa-
(2003) reported results of 8 months of CART treat- miliar communication partners. The authors com-
ment for eight participants with dysgraphia due to mented that the psychosocial benefit was apparent.
stroke. Half showed improvement in terms of the All participants used written communication effec-
outcomes measured. tively with an unfamiliar person but performed bet-
Wright et al. (2008) applied a version of CART ter in the group context.
in which a written cueing hierarchy was specified Beeson et al. (2013) described the incorpora-
with two people with aphasia. Both participants tion of smartphone text messaging into a modified
improved for target written words. One generalized version of CART for a person with Broca’s apha-
to other words, but one did not. The authors high- sia. Although his handwritten and spoken naming
lighted that individual differences are an import- improved more than his text messaging abilities,
ant consideration in predicting responsiveness to his functional abilities in texting increased and
treatment. were maintained 2 years after the treatment. Given
Repetitive spelling, a key element of this ap- enhanced features of predictive words and auto-
proach, has been found to be an essential aspect of correct features in ever-evolving smartphone tech-
what makes the approach effective. Mortley et al. nology, texting may be an increasingly important
(2001) reported improved spelling for a person with component of writing-related treatment.
severe dysgraphia but good oral spelling using a Helm-Estabrooks et al. (2014) recommended a
repetitive spelling exercise by typing via computer. version of CART combined with Anagram and Copy
Authors of modified versions of CART that included Treatment (the next approach we review). They also
spoken word repetition of target words suggested suggested a scoring system that gives credit for
that adding a speaking component may enhance partial correctness rather than the correct/incorrect
effectiveness (Ball et al., 2011). scoring system used in the original approach. Orjada
Raymer et al. (2003) reported results of a case and Beeson (2005) described the benefits of CART
series in which they implemented an adapted CART combined with an oral rereading method (summa-
approach incorporating real words and nonwords rized later, under reading treatments) for a person
for participants with dysgraphia. Spelling for trained with reading and spelling deficits due to stroke.
words improved. Generalization was limited to two Other authors (e.g., Brown et al., 2016) have shown
480  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

treatment gains in reading when CART is used along • Progressing from easy to more difficult words,
with other methods. show an anagram and then ask the client to
Overall, be mindful that just copying words arrange the letters in the right order to spell
is not sufficient for making semantic-orthographic the word.
links. It is important to activate the associated word • Once the letters are arranged successfully
meaning (Beeson et al., 2002). Some people may not to spell the word, have the client copy the
be compliant with following through on homework word. If the client cannot arrange the letters
assignments. It may be helpful to get friends or fam- correctly, do so for them and then have them
ily members involved in providing reminders and copy the word in their own writing.
motivation regarding homework. • Make the task more difficult by adding two
foil letters (letters that are not in the target
word) (a vowel and a consonant) and begin
What Is Anagram and Copy Treatment? the sequence again.
• Have the client write the word from memory.
Repeat this until they spell the word correctly
Anagram and Copy Treatment (ACT) is an approach three times.
for people with dyslexia who especially have diffi- • Assign daily homework. Have the client
culty at the level of the graphemic output lexicon repeatedly copy words using pictures that are
and graphemic buffer (Aliminosa et al., 1993; Bee- labeled with each target word for 30 minutes
son, 1999; Hillis, 1989). It may be especially useful per day. Also, have the client copy target
for those who have limited oral language and speech words as with CART.
output (e.g., people with Broca’s aphasia and apraxia • As treatment progresses, shift from your
of speech) because writing is an important compen- own selection of target words to the client’s
satory mode of communication. As with CART, selection of words.
good candidates for ACT have good visual recogni-
tion and graphomotor ability (the physical ability to
write). ACT is based on the use of anagrams, which
are scrambled sequences of letters. What Is the Status of ACT in Terms
of Evidence-Based Practice?

On What Principle Is ACT Based? Research on ACT consists of case series and single-​
case designs. Overall, writing for trained words has
The ACT approach is based on the notion that improved, but there has been minimal carryover to
repeated recall and practice strengthen the graphe- untrained words (Beeson, 1999). Positive results for
mic representation of words. Manipulating anagram a person with severe global aphasia, as character-
letters is easier than writing letters because the indi- ized by a reduced need for self-correction and time
vidual need not select letters from memory and can to respond, were reported by Greenwald (2004).
rearrange letters in various ways before deciding on Beeson et al. (2002) combined the ACT and CART
a correct arrangement. approaches for two people with Broca’s aphasia (one
moderate and one severe) and one with global apha-
sia. After 9 weeks of sessions once or twice per week
plus daily homework, all three could spell all of the
How Is ACT Implemented? target words. However, there was still minimal gen-
eralization to untrained words.
The following steps are summarized from Beeson Helm-Estabrooks et al. (2014) suggested a scor-
(1999): ing system that is more sensitive than the correct/
incorrect scoring typical of most of the studies on
• Develop a set of words that would be highly ACT and CART. By giving partial credit for the
relevant and useful to the client. degree of effectiveness at a spelling attempt, we
33. Specific Approaches for Improving Reading and Writing   481

might better capture the communicative effective- it sounds, and then decide if it is correct.
ness of writing. Anagram, Copy, and Recall Treat- If the client is still unsure, ask them to
ment (ACRT) is a combination of ACT and CART try correcting it. Have the client look it up
to support single-word writing (Beeson et al., 2002). using an electronic speller, computer, or
smartphone app.
• Have the client keep a notebook and write
What Is the Problem-Solving Approach? in it each day, continuing to practice the
writing, spell-checking, and self-correction
strategies. Encourage the client to select topics
The problem-solving approach entails teaching independently. If the client has difficulty with
the client with dysgraphia to implement strategies that, it may be helpful to assign topics at first.
that help facilitate spelling. Strategies may include • Have the client make a list of the words that
writing partially correct responses, self-correction, were difficult to spell.
making sound-to-letter correspondences, and using • Review the homework content during
an electronic speller or tablet/smartphone app to treatment sessions. Discuss and provide
obtain alternatives when the client is unsure about a feedback about strategy use.
spelling (Beeson et al., 2000).

What Is the Status of the Problem-Solving


On What Principles Is the Approach in Terms of Evidence-Based Practice?
Problem-Solving Approach Based?
Beeson et al. (2000) reported significant improvements
The problem-solving approach is based on the in spelling when the problem-solving approach was
notion that having the client with dysgraphia learn used for 10 weeks with each of two people who had
to evaluate their own writing problems (spelling spelling impairments following stroke.
in particular) and independently implement those
strategies will improve their writing abilities. A focus
on phoneme-to-grapheme conversion ideally helps
What Is Multiple Oral Rereading?
the individual improve spelling through repeated
stimulation and corrective feedback.
Multiple Oral Rereading (MOR) involves repeated
reading aloud of the same text to facilitate whole-
How Is the Problem-Solving word rather than letter-by-letter reading. It is a resti-
Approach Implemented? tutive, stimulation approach designed for people with
aphasia who have acquired reading impairments
(alexia with or without agraphia), especially those
Treatment sessions are geared toward discussion with difficulty accessing the graphemic input lexicon
and demonstration of strategies that a client may use but retaining orthographic knowledge (Beeson & Hil-
independently to improve spelling. Most of the work lis, 2001; Beeson & Insalaco, 1998; Beeson et al., 2005).
is done by the client through homework, which is
then reviewed interactively during treatment with
feedback from the clinician. The following steps are
On What Principles Is MOR Treatment Based?
based on Beeson et al. (2000):

• Ask the client to write about something, Many people with aphasia acquire a letter-by-letter
perhaps something about what has happened approach to reading (Beeson, 1999). This makes it
in their day. When the client comes upon especially difficult to read longer words. MOR is
a word for which they are unsure of the intended to improve access to the graphemic input
spelling, suggest that they write it just as lexicon either directly or through compensatory
482  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

processes (Beeson & Hillis, 2001). The clues provided tive in increasing reading rate and oral reading accu-
by sentence contexts and increasing familiarity with racy (Beeson, 1998; Beeson & Insalaco, 1998; Beeson
the text may be part of what makes the treatment effec- et al., 2005; Cherney, 2004; Tuomainen & Laine, 1991).
tive. Repetition is fundamental to the method, so com- Most studies on MOR have not included control
pletion of daily homework is an essential component. groups. MOR has also been shown to demonstrate
carryover to new, unpracticed text in some studies
(e.g., Beeson & Insalaco, 1998). However, there is less
evidence to support the use of MOR for facilitating
How Is MOR Implemented?
reading comprehension (Lacey et al., 2007).
Lacey et al. (2010) noted that specific words that
The steps described here are based on those origi- are practiced are the words most likely to be read
nally described by Beeson (1998): correctly in subsequent readings. They suggested
that including difficult words in the context of pas-
• In the initial session, determine the person’s sages rather than in isolation may help improve
reading rate and accuracy for paragraph- reading abilities. As noted with other approaches
level text. Choose reading material that is of requiring homework, the client’s motivation and
interest to the client. Choose a portion of text willingness to practice independently or with a
that is at an appropriate level of difficulty for partner at home are essential to improvement. Also,
the client and have the client read it aloud. rereading of erroneous words will likely not facili-
Score the client’s reading rate in words per tate correct word reading in the future, so some form
minute, and note reading errors. Also note of feedback from a partner during practice sessions
any self-corrected errors. may be helpful.
• Ask the client to reread the text. Support the In a review of treatment methods for acquired
client’s reading as appropriate and correct alexia, Starrfelt et al. (2013) questioned the veracity
errors as the client proceeds. Rereading the of the diagnosis of alexia in some studies on read-
passage helps increase the client’s familiarity ing treatments, including MOR, and pointed out the
with the text, which should help enhance diversity of people who have been studied in terms
accuracy. of the type and severity of alexia, and the number
• Provide the written text as homework. Thirty and type of concomitant deficits.
minutes of repeated reading of the same text The fact that MOR is often combined with
once or twice a day is recommended. other treatment approaches (for reading or for other
• Have the client keep a log of daily practice. aspects of language) makes it difficult to discern
• In subsequent treatment sessions, review the the specific effects of MOR in several studies (Bee-
log and discuss progress with homework. son et al., 2010; Lacey et al., 2010; Mayer & Murray,
Then assess reading rate and accuracy with 2002). This is not necessarily problematic in terms
the text the client has been practicing. Plot of actual associated outcomes, as a combination of
these data graphically. approaches, some emphasizing more comprehen-
• Set a target reading rate based on what seems sion-based foci, and especially those incorporating
realistic given the client’s current level. When more semantic processing and stimulation across
the client attains that rate with the same modalities, is likely advantageous.
passage, move on to a new passage. Kendall et al. (2003) pointed out that methods
that focus more on indirect-route processing, that
is, on phonological processing, may be more helpful
What Is the Status of MOR in Terms for people who have phonological or deep alexia.
They demonstrated that a phonological approach
of Evidence-Based Practice?
may help people with acquired alexia improve their
rate and accuracy of reading aloud, including gen-
Several descriptive case studies and single-case eralization to untrained passages, and also enhance
series results have demonstrated that MOR is effec- their comprehension. They also demonstrated
33. Specific Approaches for Improving Reading and Writing   483

maintenance of treatment effects following comple- • For Level 2, use 8- to 12-word sentences
tion of a phonologically based program (Kendall or combinations of two brief sentences.
et al., 2003). A third-grade level is recommended.
• For Level 3, use 15- to 30-word sentence
combinations, separated into sets of two
What Is Oral Reading for or three sentences. A sixth-grade level is
recommended.
Language in Aphasia?
• For Level 4, use simple paragraphs of 50 to
100 words at a sixth-grade reading level.
Oral Reading for Language in Aphasia (ORLA) is • Have the client read the material in unison
a treatment method for people with dyslexia associ- with you. Then have the client read it again
ated with any form and severity level of aphasia. The alone.
intent is to foster recovery or relearning of reading • Do not correct errors; rather, focus on
comprehension through practice using the phono- correct modeling.
logical and semantic routes and associated feedback
(Cherney, 1995, 2004; Cherney et al., 1986). Through
a hierarchical ordering of material (easy to difficult, What Is the Status of ORLA in Terms
and increasing in length), the clinician guides the
of Evidence-Based Practice?
client in reading in unison and then independently.

Most studies of ORLA to date involve case series


and case studies. Cherney (2010b) also carried out
On What Principles Is ORLA Treatment Based?
a comparative study using the traditional version of
ORLA and reported that 25 individuals improved in
ORLA is a stimulation approach based on neuropsy- overall aphasia language battery scores compared
chological models of reading. Repetitive stimulation to a no-treatment phase. ORLA has been shown to
is intended to strengthen phonological and seman- lead to improvements not only in reading but also
tic routes for reading. By incorporating connected in other modalities (speaking, writing, and auditory
speech rather than individual words, it is said to per- comprehension) in people with varied forms of “flu-
mit more natural prosody when clients read aloud. ent” and “nonfluent” aphasia (Cherney, 1995, 2004;
Using text-level stimuli permits practice with varied Cherney et al., 1986).
grammatical forms in meaningful contexts. Newer Cherney (2010a, 2010b) developed and tested
computerized and online versions of ORLA inte- computerized versions of ORLA based on the orig-
grate additional principles related to feedback and inal program and compared results to ORLA treat-
auxiliary forms of practice to help integrate semantic ment given by a clinician in person. Twenty-five
and phonological processing across modalities. participants improved their overall aphasia sever-
ity level following both forms of treatment; gains
were greater in the clinician-administered version.
Reasoning that a lack of visual feedback from the
How Is ORLA Treatment Implemented?
face and articulators of the clinician may have led to
poorer results in the computerized version, Cherney
The levels of stimulus types and the steps that follow and colleagues developed another version of ORLA,
are based on details provided by Cherney and col- the ORLA Virtual Therapist (VT) program.
leagues (Cherney, 1995, 2004; Cherney et al., 1986). In ORLA VT, an animated clinician with a dig-
itized female voice reads along with the client, with
• Assemble materials according to each of four words highlighted on the screen as they are read (Fig-
levels based on length and reading level. ure 33–1). She then fades her reading at the end of
• For Level 1, use simple three- to five-word the sentences, and then the client reads the material
sentences at a first-grade reading level. alone. Additional steps are implemented, including
484  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Figure 33–1. Person with aphasia engaging with ORLA VT. Image courtesy of
Leora Cherney, Shirley Ryan AbilityLab. A full-color version of this figure can be
found in the Color Insert.

pointing to specific content and function words, greater gains 6 weeks later. The authors highlight
and reading single words aloud. Performance data the importance of intensive delivery that is possible
are tracked for later review by the clinician. Recent with an asynchronous Internet application overseen
versions of ORLA VT have been implemented using by an SLP.
Internet-based protocols (Cherney & van Vuuren, In ongoing research on ORLA and its varied
2012; van Vuuren & Cherney, 2014). Cherney et al. modifications, it will be important to assess treat-
(2021) describe a pilot randomized control trial com- ment outcomes in terms of real-life use of reading
paring a web-based version (Web ORLA) and a com- according to individualized life participation goals.
mercially available computer game. Although there It will also be important to have researchers who are
were no differences in pre- and posttreatment gains not involved in the initial design of ORLA methods
for either group, those using Web ORLA showed independently test it and its variations.
33. Specific Approaches for Improving Reading and Writing   485

Learning and Reflection Activities

1. List and define any terms in this chapter needs related to methods for improving
that are new to you or that you have not yet writing and reading?
mastered. 7. Imagine that you were asked to provide
2. For each of the approaches addressed in an in-service for practicing SLPs to help
this chapter, describe why it would be them understand the theories underlying
considered to be restitutive, compensatory, impairment-focused stimulation approaches
or both. for improving reading and writing.
3. For each of the approaches in this chapter, a. What are the most clinically relevant
make a list of materials you would want to concepts on which you would focus?
have on hand so that you would be prepared b. What diagrams would be helpful to use
to carry out the approach in actual treatment in helping clinicians consider the levels
sessions. of breakdown that might occur in carried
4. With a partner, demonstrate a treatment forms of dyslexia and dysgraphia?
session using each approach in this 8. What strategies would you use to maximize
chapter. transfer of treatment gains made through
5. How would you summarize the status writing and reading treatment to real-world
of evidence-based practice for specific use of communication?
treatment methods intended to improve
www
writing and reading? See the companion website for additional learning
6. What do you see as the greatest research and teaching materials.
Epilogue

What a privilege to get to help guide others on their ful, scientific, reflective, ever-growing practitioner,
journey to clinical excellence! I am eager to have and advocate for our professions and the people we
you join me in sharing this privilege as a role model, serve. Through your dedication to life participation
coach, and counselor — no matter what your profes- and quality of life as the ultimate goals of your work,
sional position is or will be. I congratulate you on your may you be a vital vehicle, helping phoenixes rise in
own unique commitment to serving as a strengths- remarkable ways from challenged brains.
based, culturally responsive, knowledgeable, art-

487
Glossary

Please note that the terms in this glossary are defined in light of the context in which they are used in this
book and may not apply to all uses of each term in other contexts. Definitions of aphasia subtypes are given
only in terms of classical anatomical models, not according to associated symptoms, which are discussed
in Chapter 10.

Acceleration-deceleration injury. An injury in which AIDS dementia complex. A cognitive impairment


the neurological insult is due to a moving object associated with HIV itself or a related opportu-
hitting a nonmoving object; in the case of traumatic nistic infection; sometimes referred to as HIV/
brain injury, one of the objects is a person’s head. AIDS-​associated dementia or HIV/AIDS-associated
Achromatopsia. A color perception deficit; used inter- encephalopathy.
changeable with dyschromatopsia. Albert test. See line cancellation task.
Activity. Execution of tasks, in the context of the Alertness. Psychophysiological state of readiness to
WHO ICF (WHO, 2001) domain of functioning and react to sensory stimuli.
disability. Allocentric neglect. A form of neglect in which the
Activity limitations. A component of health and dis- neglected area is relative to the individual’s subjec-
ability added following a 1999 modification to the tive frame of reference at any given moment.
WHO ICIDH (WHO, 1999), which is defined as “dif- Allopathic approaches. Treatment approaches that
ficulties an individual may have in the performance most often target specific bodily systems or disease
of activities” (p. 16). states; common in Western medical practice.
Additive conjunctives. Words introducing added in- Alzheimer’s disease (AD). The most common form
formation (e.g., furthermore, in addition, in contrast). of dementia, associated with neurofibrillary tangles,
Adjunct or adjuvant approaches. Treatment approaches neuritic plaques, cortical atrophy, and ventricular
used in conjunction with allopathic medical ap- dilation; also referred to as dementia of the Alzhei-
proaches; sometimes termed complementary treatment mer’s type, or DAT.
approaches. Amusia. An impairment of processing, remembering,
Adult day care center. A facility in which people with and recognizing music.
disabilities may stay during the day so that they are Amyloid plaque. See neuritic plaque.
not left alone at home; provides respite to caregivers. Anagram. A scrambled sequence of letters that, when
Advance directives. Documentation of a person’s ordered properly, form a word.
wishes for medical care in case they become unable Anagram and Copy Treatment (ACT). An approach
to convey them; includes living will and durable for people with dyslexia based on the notion that
power of attorney. repeated recall and practice strengthen the graphe-
Age-related identity threat. The implicit or explicit mic representation of words.
belief that one will fail because one is “old.” Anastomosis. A protective feature allowing collateral
Agent. A thematic role corresponding to the subject of circulation of blood to the brain in case the primary
a sentence. channels of blood flow become blocked.
Agnosia. Inability to recognize or interpret sensory Aneurysm. A bulging out at a weakened spot along
input. an arterial wall.
Agrammatism. A deficit in formulating and process- Angioplasty. A procedure entailing insertion of a
ing syntax. catheter into the arteries and use of a balloon-like
HIV/AIDS-associated dementia. See AIDS dementia tip to expand the arterial walls.
complex. Angular injury. See rotational injury.
HIV/AIDS-associated encephalopathy. See AIDS Annual deductible. A certain amount that a person
dementia complex. with medical insurance must pay out of pocket each

489
490  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

year before insurance starts to cover their health Associative agnosia. A failure to associate mean-
care costs. ing to what is seen (e.g., an object’s relevance and
Anomia. See dysnomia. function).
Anomic aphasia. An aphasia syndrome in which Astrocytoma. A common form of glial tumor that is
word-finding difficulty is the primary deficit (asso- benign and slow growing.
ciated with varied lesion sites). Ataxia. A problem of muscle coordination that may
Anosognosia. A lack of awareness of an illness or affect speaking and voluntary movements of the
deficit. eyes and limbs, often associated with cerebellar
Antioxidants. A category of natural and human-made lesions.
substances that counteract the damaging effects of Atherosclerosis. A buildup of lipids (fatty acids and
oxidation on bodily tissue. cholesterol) and cellular debris within the arteries;
Aperceptive agnosia. Inability to recognize an the primary cause of stroke.
object; may be visual, tactile, olfactory, auditory, or Athetosis. A type of dyskinesia involving involuntary
gustatory. slow, writhing movements.
Aphasia. An acquired language disorder, caused by Attention Process Training (APT). A treatment pro-
brain injury (e.g., stroke, traumatic brain injury, neo- gram for traumatic brain injury survivors designed
plasm, surgical ablation of brain tissue, infections, to enhance focused, sustained, selective, alternating,
and metabolic problems) affecting all modalities of and divided attention.
language (speaking, listening, reading, and writing); Attention switching. Shifting of cognitive focus from
it is not the result of an intellectual, sensory, motor, one task or stimulus to another (also called alternat-
or psychiatric problem. ing attention; also see divided attention).
Aphasia Couples Therapy (ACT). A specific treat- Auditory agnosia. Impairment in recognition or
ment approach for couples, including education to interpretation of auditory input (includes auditory
facilitate understanding about aphasia, training and sound agnosia and auditory verbal agnosia).
practice in supported communication, and mutual Auditory sound agnosia. Impairment in recognition
sharing of evaluations of quality of communication or interpretation of nonverbal sounds.
and strategy implementation. Auditory verbal agnosia. Impairment in recognition
Aphasia mentoring program. A program in which or interpretation of spoken words; in the absence
people with aphasia serve as mentors to students in of auditory sound agnosia, also called pure word
clinical education programs in the health sciences. deafness.
Aphasiologist. A person who studies aphasia; an Autobiographical memory. Memory about import-
aphasia expert; in a nonliteral sense, commonly used ant aspects of one’s past.
to refer to an expert in aphasia and other acquired Autotopagnosia. A type of visual agnosia involving a
neurogenic cognitive-linguistic disorders. deficit in recognizing body parts.
Aphasiology. The study of aphasia; often used to refer Back to the Drawing Board. A treatment program
to the study of acquired neurogenic cognitive-lin- that focuses on the use of drawing as a means of
guistic disorders in general. compensatory communication for individuals with
Apraxia of speech (AoS). An impairment in motor severe aphasia.
programming and sequencing of movements of the Bacterial infection. Infection by single-celled micro-
articulators for intentional or volitional speech. scopic organisms that may cause inflammation.
Argument structure. The association of a verb with Ballismus. A type of dyskinesia involving involun-
its context within a sentence. tary jerking, flinging movements, especially of the
Arousal. Psychophysiological state of reactivity to arms and legs.
sensory stimuli. Basal rule. See floor rule.
Arteriography. See cerebral angiography. Behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. The
Arteriosclerosis. See atherosclerosis. most common form of FTD, which manifests
Arteriovenous malformation (AVM). An atypically through personality and behavioral changes, often
developed artery or vein (most commonly arising entailing apathy, a lack of insight about communi-
during embryonic or fetal development); typically cation challenges, difficulty expressing emotions
less adaptable to changes in blood pressure than and considering others’ emotions, and disinhibition
normal formations, increasing the chance of rupture. of inappropriate behaviors and expressions during
Asanas. Yogic practice focused on bodily posturing. social interaction.
491
Glossary  

Beneficence. A moral principle of acting for others’ The Breakfast Club. A social approach used in a
good. long-term care context in which social participation
Bilateral quadrantopsia. A visual field deficit affect- of people with dementia is encouraged during a
ing the same quadrant of the visual field in each multisensory activity; includes adaptations to indi-
eye, resulting from a lesion of the optic tract fibers vidual preferences, needs, strengths, and challenges.
projecting to the visual cortex above (resulting in Broca’s aphasia. A classic aphasia type associated
lower quadrantopsia) or below (resulting in upper with a lesion in the inferior, posterior portion of
quadrantopsia) the calcarine fissure on the one side the frontal lobe (corresponding to Brodmann’s areas
of the brain. 44, 45).
Binocular visual field. The field of view that is seen Calcarine fissure. A prominent sulcus on the medial
with both eyes jointly. surface of each hemisphere of the brain; demarcates
Biological age. An index of the functioning of one’s the upper and lower quadrants of visual fields rep-
bodily organs over time. resented in the primary visual cortex.
Biopsy. Clinical examination of tissue removed from Canonical sentences. Sentences that have a standard
the body. word order in any given language; in English, active
Biopsychosocial framework. A means of conceptual- sentence structures with a subject-verb-object word
izing health-related conditions and well-being that order are considered canonical.
highlights the complex interaction of multiple fac- Capacity. Within a legal framework, what a person
tors that constitute disabilities and disease. can do when appropriate supports are in place in
Biopsychosocial models of aging. Models that em- their environment; typically referred to in the con-
phasize the complex interactions among biological, text of decision-making.
psychological, and sociological factors that influence Capitation. A funding scheme in which there is a
how people age. fixed sum based on the number of people enrolled
Bitemporal (heteronymous) hemianopsia. A visual in a contracted health care plan, regardless of how
field deficit affecting the temporal halves of both many of those people actually receive services and
visual fields (right side of one visual field and left regardless of which services are provided.
side of the other), resulting from a lesion of the decus- Case rate. An insurance funding scheme in which the
sating fibers in the optic chiasm (sparing the ipsilat- provider receives a set amount for treating a patient
eral fibers). based on their diagnosis, regardless of which spe-
Blast injury. A type of traumatic brain injury resulting cific services they are provided.
from rapid phases of over- and under-pressurization Cataracts. The accumulation of fibrous proteins on
of air compared to normal atmospheric pressure; the lens of the eye, resulting in degradation of image
most frequently associated with exposure to war-​ quality.
related explosives. Catastrophic reaction. Extreme frustration that may
Blood oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) effect. An be experienced when struggling to communicate.
fMRI method used to index the relative flow of oxy- Causal conjunctives. Words specifying a cause, rea-
genated blood to a brain region, interpreted as acti- son, or result (e.g., otherwise, because).
vation of that region at a given point in time. Ceiling rule. The designation in a standardized assess-
Body functions. The physiological and psychological ment of how many times a person may get consec-
aspects of the body, in the context of the WHO ICF utive items or a number of items within a subtest
(WHO, 2001) domain of functioning and disability. wrong before the test administrator stops or moves
Body structures. Anatomical parts, in the context of on to the next section or subtest.
the WHO ICF (WHO, 2001) domain of functioning Cerebral angiography. A neuroimaging technique
and disability. that involves injecting a contrast medium into the
Bradykinesia. A condition of excess muscle tone that bloodstream and taking x-rays to show the contrast
results in slowed movements with reduced range as it courses through arteries, capillaries, and veins;
of motion; it may lead to problems manipulating helps determine the extent of vascular problems
and controlling objects and writing; it may cause within cerebral blood vessels; allows visualization
reduced facial expression. of the arterial blood supply to the cortex and the
Brain attack. Stroke (a term used in public education degree of collateral circulation in cases of occlusion.
campaigns to draw parallels between lifestyle risks Cerebrovascular accident (CVA). Synonymous with
associated with stroke and heart attack). stroke; the term CVA has fallen out of favor, largely
492  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

because the word accident suggests that strokes are cative task; may be applied to other types of syn-
caused by happenstance rather than being associ- dromes as well.
ated with known risk factors. Collateral sprouting. A type of neuronal regener-
Chorea. A type of dyskinesia involving involuntary ation; an increase in axonal receptivity per neuron
rapid, repetitive, jerky movements. to other neurons through the growth of new axonal
Chronological age. An index of how long a person branches.
has lived since birth. Communicative Drawing Program (CDP). A treat-
Circle of Willis. An anastomosis at the base of the ment for people with severe aphasia who are limited
brain; the major arterial network supplying blood in oral and written language expression that focuses
to the brain. on the use of drawing as a compensatory means of
Circumlocution. Word or words other than the communication.
intended word; used to express the meaning of an Community-based rehabilitation (CBR). Means of
intended word. enhancing quality of life for people with disabilities
Closed-class words. Function words belonging to a and those who care about them, focusing on meet-
relatively small part of the lexicon in a given lan- ing basic needs and ensuring inclusion and partici-
guage compared to open-class words; new words in pation; implemented through the combined efforts
this category are rarely added to a language. of people with disabilities, families, community
Closed-head injury (CHI). A traumatic brain injury members, and governmental and nongovernmen-
in which the skull is not fractured. tal health, education, vocational, social, and other
Cloze sentence or phrase. A portion of spoken dis- services.
course or written text in which certain words are Complementary treatment approaches. See adjunct
removed; typically used in tasks that require a or adjuvant approaches
respondent to fill in the missing word or words. Complexity account of treatment efficacy (CATE).
Cochrane Collaboration. An international network The theory that treatment gains made when using
of scholars, professionals, and consumers estab- more complex (as opposed to simpler) stimuli and
lished to help consider the evidence base that sup- tasks enhance generalization to less complex stim-
ports any area of health-related intervention. uli and tasks; associated with greater recruitment of
Codeswitching. The act of taking into account the intact neural networks and enhanced neural inter-
individual or individuals with whom one is speak- connections during complex tasks and when pro-
ing; the adaptation of what is being expressed, how cessing complex stimuli.
it is being expressed, and in what dialect or lan- Computed axial tomography (CAT or CT). A neuro-
guage it is expressed, based on the immediate social imaging technique entailing measurement of energy
context. transmission through tissue, allowing for visualiza-
Cognitive age. An index of how one’s intelligence, tion of gross brain structures; also called x-ray com-
memory, and learning abilities change over time. puted tomography.
Cognitive effort. The intensity of information pro- Concrete-abstract framework. A historic framework
cessing allocated to a given mental task (also called for conceptualizing aphasia, suggesting that aphasia
mental effort). reflects a loss of “abstract attitude,” or the ability to
Cognitive-linguistic disorders associated with trau- express and comprehend thoughts that cannot be
matic brain injury. Any of a constellation of com- captured through sensory experience with objects
munication problems resulting from traumatic brain and actions that are physically present (Goldstein &
injury. Scheerer, 1941).
Cognitive neuropsychological framework. A frame- Concurrent validity. A type of criterion validity that
work for conceptualizing aphasia that is based on is measured by calculating the correlation between
models of mental representation and types and scores from one test with scores on another that is
stages of information processing; in these models, intended to assess a similar construct.
aphasia may be seen as a disruption in the processing Concussion. Mild traumatic brain injury of which the
required for any given linguistic task or set of tasks. survivor may or may not be aware; may or may not
Cognitive resources hypothesis. A theory that the result in loss of consciousness; may have short-term
communication deficits of people with RHS are or lasting effects.
highly dependent on the degree of attention and Conduction aphasia. A classic aphasia type associ-
working memory demands of a given communi- ated with a lesion in the arcuate fasciculus within the
493
Glossary  

supramarginal gyrus (corresponding to Brodmann’s yields results similar to that of another means of
area 40). assessment that is intended to measure the same
Conduit d’approche. Language output characterized construct.
by repeated attempts to articulate a verbal stimulus. Conversational coaching.  A means of educating
Cone. A type of photoreceptor, located in the retina, people with aphasia and conversational partners
that is functional in bright light and responsible for to use strategies that improve their communicative
central discriminative vision and color detection. interactions.
Confabulation. Unintentional misrepresentation of Continuous perseveration. A form of recurrent per-
the truth. severation in which a behavior is continued when it
Confounding factor. Any characteristic of a person’s is no longer appropriate.
abilities or any aspect of an assessment tool, proce- Copay. A portion of health care costs individuals pay
dure, or situation that could lead to invalid assess- out of pocket for various services, prescription med-
ment results. ications, medical equipment, and so on.
Connectionist models. Conceptual schemes that asso- Copy and Recall Treatment (CART). An impairment-​
ciate neuroanatomical structures and functions with focused stimulation method that entails repeated
various aspects of information processing. writing practice through a progression of single-​
Conservatorship. In a legal framework, oversight of word writing tasks.
the things a person owns, in full or in part; may be Correct information unit (CIU). A standardized,
temporary or permanent. rule-based scoring system for indexing informative-
Constraint-induced aphasia therapy (CIAT) or ness in discourse analysis.
constraint-induced language therapy (CILT). Cortical reorganization. Modification of brain-be-
A treatment approach restricting the use of compen- havior relationships following brain injury; the
satory communication modalities and encouraging phenomenon of areas of brain tissue that were not
the use of communication modalities that are the centrally involved in certain functions prior to injury
most impaired. taking over those functions.
Construct validity. The degree to which a means of Cortical stimulation brain mapping. See electro­-
assessment captures what it is intended to assess. corticography.
Constructional apraxia. A term sometimes used to Counseling. A professional, goal-based, interpersonal
refer to a visuoconstructive disability, although it is process for fostering mental health and wellness
less accurate in a literal sense. by encouraging changes in thinking, feeling, and
Content validity. A type of validity referring to the behaving.
degree to which the items on a test or scale tap into Coup injury. Injury to the brain located at the site of
the construct to be assessed. impact between the head and an object.
Content words. Nouns, verbs, adjectives, and adverbs. Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease. A rare, rapidly progres-
Context-Processing Deficiency Theories: Theories sive, degenerative viral disease, entailing a common
based on the hypothesis that as we get older, we have bodily protein (prion) that forms into misshapen
increasing difficulty judging and taking into account configurations that destroy brain cells; may be
the context of a cognitive or linguistic task, which hereditary or infectious.
may lead to less efficient allocation of resources to Criterion-referenced measures. Indices used to gauge
accomplish a task. a person’s own ability without direct comparison to
Contextual factors. A domain of health conditions others; also called domain-referenced measures.
in the WHO ICF (WHO, 2001), including personal Criterion validity. The degree to which an individu-
and environmental factors that should be analyzed al’s performance on a certain measure is predictive
and addressed regarding each individual’s health of a certain outcome.
conditions. Crossed aphasia. Rare occurrence of aphasia (of any
Continuing care retirement communities (CCRCs). ​ type) due to damage to the right instead of the left
Facilities that offer several levels of health care in hemisphere in a person who is right-handed.
one location. Cueing hierarchy approaches to anomia. A treat-
Contrecoup injury. Injury to the brain located oppo- ment approach that formalized principles of cueing
site the site of impact between the head and an object. to aid in naming in anomia; cues are provided in a
Convergent validity. A type of construct valid- hierarchy based on their ability to elicit correct pro-
ity; the degree to which one means of assessment duction of a target word.
494  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Cultural competence. Ability to conduct oneself in Diffuse. In the context of brain injury, involving mul-
a socially, linguistically, and culturally appropriate tiple areas of the brain at once.
way with people from a culture other than one’s own. Diffusion MRI. A neuroimaging technique involving
Cultural humility. Recognition that one cannot know the detection and mapping of the diffusion of water
everything about a culture and, thus, that that one molecules within myelinated fiber tracks, allowing
always has more to learn and appreciate any partic- for visualization of connections among varied brain
ular cultures. regions, and pathologies in association fibers in the
Cultural responsiveness. Active commitment to brain; also called diffusion tensor imaging (DTI).
engage in ongoing learning about another culture Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI). See diffusion MRI.
and to recognize the limits of one’s cultural compe- Direct injury. See translational injury.
tence and maintain cultural humility in any given Disability. A level of the International Classifica-
context. tion of Impairments, Disabilities, and Handicaps
Declarative memory. Long-term factual or seman- (ICIDH; WHO, 1980) defined as “consequences
tic knowledge that can be consciously recalled or of impairment in terms of functional performance
recognized. and activity by the individual” (p. 14); a domain
Decubitus ulcer. Breakdown of skin integrity result- of health conditions in the WHO ICF (WHO, 2001)
ing from pressure (usually from lying down or sit- consisting of impairment, activity limitations, and
ting for long periods of time); also called bed sore. participation restrictions.
Deep dyslexia. An impairment in higher-level inter- Discourse. The use of spoken or written language in
pretation and understanding of written words. interaction with others.
Delirium. A temporary, often sudden, decline in cog- Discourse coherence. The tying together of semantic
nitive ability. content in a logical way to express ideas effectively
Dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB). The third most and efficiently.
common form of dementia, characterized by abnor- Discourse cohesion. The tying together of lexical and
mal protein (alpha-synuclein) deposits that are also grammatical relationships within and across phrases
commonly found in people with AD and in people and sentences in discourse.
with dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. Discovery phase. Phase I of Robey and Schultz’s
Dendritic branching. A type of neuronal regenera- (1998) five-phase outcome research model, in which
tion in which dendritic connections increase, thus investigators develop hypotheses about treatment,
expanding the number of synapses that can be made estimate the optimal treatment intensity, and specify
per neuron. the population to benefit from treatment.
Derivational linguistic theory. A linguistic theory Discriminant validity. A type of construct validity,
based on the notion that linguistic structures are quantified by measuring a test’s lack of relationship
generated through a series of operations on base with measures of constructs that differ from the con-
structures (e.g., a surface structure based on a corre- struct to be measured.
sponding deep structure or a complex word based Disfluent. Characteristic of spoken language with fewer
on its simpler components); developed by Chomsky units of verbal production (e.g., phonemes, words,
(1986) as part of his theory of transformational content information units) conveyed per unit of time
grammar. compared to a standard of “normal” fluent speech.
Diabetes mellitus (DM). A chronic disorder of car- Dissociation syndrome. A symptom constellation in
bohydrate metabolism caused by abnormal insulin which some abilities remain relatively intact while
function or insulin deficiency, typically resulting in others are relatively impaired.
elevated or poorly controlled blood glucose levels. Divided attention. Attention to multiple tasks at the
Diabetic encephalopathy. Any type of brain disorder same time; considered by many to be more aptly
caused by diabetes. called attention switching.
Diaschisis. A phenomenon in which functions associ- Domain-referenced measures. See criterion-referenced
ated with brain structures that are remote from the measures.
injured area become impaired due to disruptions in Durable power of attorney for health care.  An
neuronal pathways. advance directive; a document used to appoint
Differential diagnosis. Process of discerning which a trusted person to make health care decisions if a
disorder, disease, or disability labels apply or do not person becomes incapacitated.
apply to an individual according to an evaluation of Dynamic assessment. Evaluation that allows tailor-
their body structure and function. ing of assessment materials to the interests, ability
495
Glossary  

level, and cultural and linguistic background of the involves testing of a treatment method developed
person being assessed. through Phases I and II with large samples of people
Dysarthria. An impairment of neuromuscular inner- who represent the target population in a random-
vation of the muscles involved in speech, result- ized control trial.
ing in slow, weak, and poorly coordinated speech Efficiency. An index of productivity, measured by how
production. much can be gained with a minimum of expense,
Dyschromatopsia. A color perception deficit; used time, and effort.
interchangeably with achromatopsia. Egocentric neglect. A form of inattention in which
Dysgraphia. A writing disorder. the neglected area corresponds to the individual’s
Dyslexia. A reading disorder. bodily midline.
Dyslipidemia. An abnormal amount of lipids (e.g., fat Elderspeak. The demeaning adaptation of language
phospholipids, triglycerides, and cholesterol) in the to a person because of their age.
blood. Electrocorticography. A neurodiagnostic method
Dysnomia. A problem with word finding; often used involving the intracranial use of EEG; also called
interchangeably with anomia, although the prefix cortical stimulation brain mapping.
dys suggests a milder form. Electroencephalography (EEG). A neurodiagnostic
Dysphasia. Sometimes used instead of the term apha- method that involves studying brain waves reflect-
sia (the dys- prefix indicates a degree of impaired ing electrical potential differences between two or
language rather than the complete loss of language). more points on the scalp.
Dysprosodia. See dysprosody. Ellipsis. A grammatical cohesive device in which
Dysprosody. Deficit in the intonation, stress, or information previously stated in discourse is left out
rhythm of speech. because it is assumed the listener knows it.
Dysthymic disorder. A chronic state of depression for Embolic stroke. A type of occlusive stroke in which
most of the time over a period of at least 2 years; a blockage (typically a blood clot or a piece of ath-
included in the category of persistent depressive erosclerotic plaque) travels from elsewhere in the
disorder. bloodstream to the point where it blocks an artery.
Ecological validity. The degree to which a test, or any Embolism. Arterial blockage in an embolic stroke.
specific stimulus or set of stimuli within a test, rep- Emotional lability. The tendency to cry, swear, and
resents actual real-word types of stimuli that would otherwise openly emote, in a way that is uncharac-
be encountered in the everyday life of the person teristic of how a person typically responded prior to
being tested. a stroke or brain injury.
Edema. Swelling caused by excess fluid. Encephalopathies. Infections that affect the cortex.
Effect size. A statistical measure of the degree of like- Endarterectomy. The removal of atherosclerotic
lihood that a treatment will be beneficial or harmful. plaque from arterial walls, most often the carotid
Effectiveness. The likelihood of benefit of treatment artery.
for an individual under average conditions (based Environmental factors. Factors outside of an individ-
on studies of efficacious treatment). ual person that affect their health; include physical
Effectiveness and efficiency test phase. Phase V surroundings, services, social context, and the affect
of Robey and Schultz’s (1998) five-phase outcome and attitudes of relevant people.
research model, in which time allocation and cost are Environmental systems approaches. Treatment ap-
studied along with satisfaction and quality-of-life proaches whose proponents recognize the dynamic
indices in large samples of individuals treated as aspects of individuals relative to their communica-
well as significant others and caregivers. tion needs and their sociocultural systems in every-
Effectiveness test phase. Phase IV of Robey and day activities; includes attention to family, work
Schultz’s (1998) five-phase outcome research model, environment, and social circles.
in which the effects of a treatment already stud- Episodic memory. Declarative recollection of personal
ied in Phase III are studied under average clinical experiences.
conditions. Equal participation. In the PACE treatment method,
Efficacy. The likelihood of benefit from a given treat- the principle that the client and the clinician take
ment for a defined population under ideal conditions equal turns sending and receiving messages.
(applicable to a population, not to an individual). Equal protection of the laws. The principle that peo-
Efficacy test phase. Phase III of Robey and Schultz’s ple with disabilities have the same opportunities as
(1998) five-phase outcome research model, which everyone to participate in society.
496  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Errorless learning method. Treatment methods in Fluent aphasia. Any type of aphasia in which spo-
which individuals are encouraged and praised for ken language production in terms of morphemes
successes and not corrected or given negative feed- or words per unit of time (regardless of meaningful
back when they do not perform a task correctly. content expressed) is normal or excessive; often used
Event-related potentials (ERPs). A neurodiagnostic interchangeably with the term receptive aphasia.
method involving the use of EEG during specific Focal. In the context of brain injury, confined to one or
cognitive, linguistic, or behavioral tasks, and during more specific areas of the brain.
any type of somatosensory, olfactory, visual, or audi- FOCUSED. A set of strategies for enhancing commu-
tory stimulation; also called evoked potentials. nication with people who have dementia.
Evoked potentials. See event-related potentials. Focused attention. Dedicated concentration on a spe-
Executive function deficits. Challenges with self-reg- cific aspect of a task or stimulus; also called selective
ulation, reasoning, making judgments and decisions, attention.
goal setting, planning, strategizing, being aware of Forward-chaining. A language intervention method
strengths and weaknesses, organizing, sequencing, in which the clinician models and reinforces longer
allocating attention, and inhibiting in appropriate utterances based on utterances initiated by the client.
behaviors. Free choice of modalities. In the PACE treatment
Explicit memory. See declarative memory. method, the principle that when in the role of sender,
Expressive aphasia. A type of aphasia in which peo- the client and clinician each determine the commu-
ple have greater difficulty producing than under- nication mode used to convey a message.
standing language; often used interchangeably with Frontal lobe syndrome (FLS). A constellation of
the term nonfluent aphasia. symptoms associated with left and right orbital
External locus of control. A category of locus of con- frontal lobe injury, including executive function and
trol; includes a sense that other forces, such as God, pragmatic deficits.
luck, fate, and other people (family, friends, profes- Frontotemporal dementia. A type of dementia caused
sionals, etc.) determine what happens. by atrophy of the anterior frontal and temporal
Eyetracking. The use of instrumentation to monitor of lobes; also called Pick’s disease.
the location and duration of eye fixations as people Function words. Prepositions, pronouns, determin-
or animals look at real-world scenes, objects, still ers, conjunctions, and auxiliary verbs.
images, and videos. Functional Communication Measure (FCM). A 7-
Face validity. The degree to which a test or measure point scoring system that speech-language pathol-
is judged by others to be valid. ogists may use similarly to the way Functional
Fee-for-service. A health insurance funding scheme Independence Measures are used in medical and
in which there is a rate paid for a specific diagnostic rehabilitation contexts, with greater relevance to
or intervention service, which may be based on units language, cognition, and swallowing.
of time or numbers of visits/sessions regardless of Functional maintenance program. An intervention
duration. assessment and treatment approach involving the
Fiber tracking. See tractography. person served and significant others, including
Figurative language. Expressions that require abstrac- caregivers; the focus is on training communication
tion to infer meaning that cannot be gained through partners to understand and use communication
literal interpretation. strategies that enhance communication abilities.
First-in/last-out model of cognitive loss. The the- Functional MRI (fMRI). A neuroimaging technique
ory that the functional abilities learned earliest in life involving the indexing of dynamic changes in blood
are those most likely to be preserved in people with flow as indicated by varying levels of oxygen in the
dementia. brain; allows for the quantification of hemodynamic
Flip the rehab model. A personalized means of col- changes associated with active metabolism during
laboratively developing goals as directed by the cli- ongoing neuronal activity.
ent and care partner(s) to enhance participation in Functioning. A domain of health in the WHO ICF
meaningful life activities. (WHO, 2001), consisting of body functions, activi-
Floor rule. A rule in a standardized assessment that ties, and participation.
indicates when certain items or groups of items may General slowing hypothesis. The notion that cogni-
be skipped because the test taker gets so many cor- tive processing at all levels slows as we age.
rect that those items are apparently too easy; also Glioblastoma multiforme. A fast-growing and malig-
called basal rule. nant form of glial tumor.
497
Glossary  

Glioma. A tumor caused by uncontrolled growth of Human immunodeficiency virus/acquired immuno-


glial cells; the most common form of brain tumor. deficiency syndrome (HIV/AIDS). A virus (HIV)
Global aphasia. A classic aphasia type associated targeting the human immune system causing AIDS
with multiple areas of brain damage, typically by substantially invading immune cells.
in the frontal, parietal, and temporal areas of the Huntington’s disease. A hereditary condition char-
brain. acterized by chorea and psychiatric and cognitive-​
Global paraphasia. See semantic paraphasia. linguistic problems.
Goal Attainment Scaling (GAS). People taking part Hyperaffectivity. A heightened affective response;
in rehabilitation select their own goals as part of may be evidenced as exuberance and excessive
intervention planning and clinicians collaborate talking.
with them and with one another to help people meet Hyperbaric oxygen therapy (HBOT). A treatment
their goals. method that involves immersing an individual in a
Grammaticality judgment. A task in which individ- sealed tank while oxygen is forced into their bodily
uals are asked to make decisions about whether sen- tissues.
tence constructions are correct or incorrect. Hyperlipidemia. Dyslipidemia involving high blood
Graphomotor ability. The physical ability to write. lipids and low high-density lipoprotein (HDL)
Guardianship. Full or limited, temporary or perma- Hypermetropia. Reduced near-visual acuity, associ-
nent oversight of an individual. ated with a change in the shape of the lens.
Handicap. A level of the International Classification of Hypoaffectivity. A restricted affective response; may
Impairments, Disabilities, and Handicaps (ICIDH; be demonstrated as flat expression of emotion con-
WHO, 1980) defined as “disadvantages experienced veyed by reduced prosody and a lack of conversa-
by the individual as a result of impairments and dis- tional or social initiative.
abilities” (p. 14). Hypoperfusion. Decreased blood flow within an
Health insurance plans. Contracted arrangements organ (e.g., a part of the brain).
that enable individuals to receive health care at a set Ideational apraxia. A problem generating a motor
or reduced rate. plan to carry out a purposeful movement.
Health maintenance organizations (HMOs). Agen- Ideograms. Graphemes that represent concepts or
cies that provide health care services through con- ideas.
tracts with clinical professionals rather than having Ideographic scripts. Written languages, such as Chi-
patients see separate, independent providers. nese, Korean, and Japanese, in which meaning is
Hematoma. The accumulation of blood outside of a conveyed through ideograms (symbolic representa-
blood vessel, caused by hemorrhage. tions) rather than through letters that correspond to
Hemianopia. Synonymous with hemianopsia. speech sounds.
Hemianopsia. Loss of one half of a visual field. Ideomotor apraxia. A problem executing a motor
Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults plan to carry out purposeful movement.
(HAROLD). A phenomenon in which some older Imageable. Easy to picture mentally.
individuals demonstrate greater activation of Impairment. A level of the International Classification
bilateral brain regions while completing complex of Impairments, Disabilities, and Handicaps (ICIDH;
cognitive tasks that tend to involve primarily one WHO, 1980) “concerned with abnormalities of body
hemisphere in younger people. structure and appearance and with organ or system
Hemispheric specialization. The notion that each function, resulting from any cause” (p. 14).
side of the brain houses specific abilities. Implementation science. Study and methods ori-
Hemorrhagic stroke. A type of stroke that occurs ented toward the use of evidence (through research
when a blood vessel ruptures. and practice) to improve the quality and effective-
Holistic health. A focus on the integration of body ness of intervention.
and mind, which are seen as intertwined, insepara- Implicit memory. Long-term recollection that does
ble entities. not require conscious recall to be activated (e.g., how
Homonymous hemianopsia. A visual field deficit to steer a car or walk).
affecting the same visual field in each eye (i.e., the Incidence. The number of newly diagnosed cases per
temporal half of one field and the nasal half of the specified unit of time.
other), resulting from a lesion of the optic tract (after Infarct or infarction. An area of dead tissue.
the fibers have passed through the optic chiasm) on Inference. A conclusion that is not explicitly stated,
one side of the brain. based on synthesis of information.
498  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Inferencing. In the context of communication, the natural pitches corresponding to how target words,
act of making a logical conclusion about intended phrases, or sentences might be said in natural
meaning based on what has been communicated. conversation.
Information exchange. Indices used during discourse Intracerebral hemorrhage. Leakage of blood that
analysis that pertain to dyads or groups during occurs within brain tissue.
interaction, not just to the individual with a com- Intra-examiner reliability. The degree of consis-
munication disorder (e.g., use of eye contact, turn-​ tency of results obtained by the same assessor.
taking behaviors). Intrahemispheric specialization. The notion that
Inhibition theories. Theories based on the rationale specific structures within each hemisphere are asso-
that people have greater challenges inhibiting irrel- ciated with specific abilities.
evant information and focusing attention to a partic- Intraparenchymal hemorrhage. Intracerebral bleed-
ular task in the face of multiple competing stimuli or ing.
task requirements (often applied in studies of aging, Ischemic. Characteristic of restricted blood supply.
traumatic brain injury, right brain injury, and execu- Ischemic cerebrovascular disease. Brain disorders
tive function deficits). associated with reduced blood supply to the brain.
Inhibitory deficit theories. Theories of aging based Ischemic penumbra. An area of reduced blood flow
on the rationale that older people have greater in neural tissue surrounding an infarct.
challenges than younger people with inhibiting Ishihara plates. A common tool for color vision
irrelevant information and focusing attention on screening, consisting of images or shapes composed
a particular task in the face of multiple competing of small dots in primary colors superimposed on a
stimuli or task requirements. background of dots in a secondary color.
Insurance intermediary. A professional insurance Jargon aphasia. A type of aphasia characterized by
company that ensures that Medicare and Medicaid the tendency to produce nonwords; sometimes used
policies are obeyed and that funds are distributed as to describe Wernicke’s aphasia.
government regulations dictate. Justice. A moral principle; making decisions and shar-
Interdisciplinary team. A team in which there is ing resources fairly.
synergy across team members and a high degree of Korsakoff’s syndrome. A condition of gradual cog-
collaborative decision-making and consultation in nitive decline due to cortical atrophy caused by
clinical practice. chronic alcohol abuse.
Inter-examiner reliability. A type of reliability refer- Language of confusion. Conversational content asso-
ring to the consistency of assessment results obtained ciated with transient confusional states.
by two different assessors. Language of generalized intellectual impairment. ​
Internal consistency. See internal reliability. Language problems resulting from cognitive impair-
Internal locus of control. The sense of having the ment, typically applied in the context of language
power and the ability to do something about one’s disorders associated with neurodegenerative condi-
own situation. tions such as dementia.
Internal reliability. A type of reliability referring to Lateral geniculate body of the thalamus. A relay
the consistency with which assessment results are center for the visual pathway in the thalamus.
obtained across items or components of items within Laughter yoga. A mind-body practice entailing inten-
a test; also called internal consistency. tional laughter, ideally to evoke the psychophys-
International Classification of Diseases — Clinical iological benefits of real laughter, especially when
Modification (ICD-10-CM). A system of clas- combined with breathing techniques, group engage-
sification and coding for diseases, conditions, and ment, and eye contact with others while laughing.
symptoms. Legibility. The ease or difficulty of identifying indi-
International Classification of Functioning, Disabil- vidual printed letters, numbers, or characters.
ity, and Health (ICF). A system for classifying dis- Lexical decision task. An experimental condition in
abilities that takes into consideration not just medical which a person is asked to make a judgment about
or organic aspects of health-related challenges but also words (e.g., word versus nonword discrimination or
the complex consequences of having those challenges. whether a word has been shown before).
Interstimulus intervals (ISIs). The amount of time Lexical perseveration. A type of recurrent persever-
between presentations of cognitive-linguistic stimuli. ation involving persistence in using the same word
Intoning. In Melodic Intonation Therapy, singing used in a previous response instead of an appropri-
words in a melodious pattern that exaggerates the ate word.
499
Glossary  

Life coaching (or wellness coaching). A professional increased in surviving neurons, thus compensating
means of empowering people to develop a clear for reduced transmission from damaged neurons.
vision of what is most important to them, strive for Loose training program. A training program in
wellness, and maximize their personal potential. which the clinician exerts minimal control over stim-
Life Participation Approach to Aphasia (LPAA). A uli, responses, and feedback during treatment.
social treatment approach that puts the holistic life Knowledge of performance. A person’s perception of
concerns of people with aphasia and those who are how accurately they have accomplished a task.
important to them at the center of decision-making Knowledge of response. A person’s perception of
and intervention. what they did correctly or incorrectly during a given
Life-Span Model of Postformal Cognitive Develop- task.
ment. A model of aging entailing seven stages, in Magnetic resonance angiography (MRA). A neuro­
which only the first occurs before adulthood. imaging technique involving the use of MRI methods
Limb apraxia. A deficit in motor programming of to image vascular functions in the arterial system.
the arm, elbow, wrist, hand, or fingers for volitional Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). A neuroimag-
movement. ing technique that makes use of an applied magnetic
Line bisection task. A screening task for visual neglect; field around the head and brief and repeated bursts
entails asking a person to mark the midpoint of a of radiofrequency (RF) wave exposure, allowing
straight line. visualization of brain structures.
Line cancellation task. A screening task for visual Magnetoencephalography (MEG). A neurodiagnos-
neglect; entails presenting a series of lines in varied tic method involving recording of ERPs in the brain
orientations on a page and asking the individual to in response to specific tasks, then mapping those
mark each line to create a cross or plus sign; also ERPs onto magnetic resonance images to reflect cor-
called the Albert test (Albert, 1973). tical mapping of task-induced brain functioning.
Literal paraphasia. See phonemic paraphasia. Main event index. A discourse analysis metric indi-
Living will. An advance directive detailing people’s cating a person’s ability to identify relationships
wishes in case they have a terminal condition, are and causal connections between ideas in narrative
near death, and cannot make their own decisions discourse.
about potential life-prolonging treatments. Majority world. Counties and regions where a major-
Living with Aphasia. Framework for Outcome ity of the world’s populations live (e.g., in the region
Measurement (A-FROM). A means of conceptu- known as sub-Saharan Africa, parts of Asia, Central
alizing the outcomes of intervention for people with and South America, and the Caribbean); previously
aphasia based largely on the International Classifica- labeled as underdeveloped or third world.
tion of Functioning, Disability, and Health. Malingering. Feigning or exaggerating medical or
Locked-in syndrome. A condition caused by a brain- psychological symptoms, typically for personal gain.
stem-level stroke or injury, resulting in complete Managed care. A term used to capture the combined
paralysis of the body’s voluntary muscles (with the goals of controlling health care costs, coordinating
exception of certain types of eye movement). care, and overseeing access to care, quality of ser-
Locus of control. A person’s own view of what and/ vices, and outcomes assessment.
or who has shaped the events in their life and of Mapping Therapy. A treatment method, rooted in lin-
what and/or who has the power to shape their guistic theory, designed to treat deficits in thematic
circumstances. role assignment in people with agrammatism.
Logopenic variant PPA (lvPPA). Form of PPA char- MCI due to Alzheimer’s disease (MCI due to AD). A
acterized by difficulty with word finding, especially condition of cognitive decline that is not typical of
in spontaneous conversation. normal aging and occurs prior to the onset of Alzhei-
Logorrhea. Spoken language that is overly abundant mer’s disease.
in light of a given communicative context; also called Medicaid. The U.S. federal and state health insurance
press of speech. program for people with limited income and finan-
Long-term memory. System of information storage cial resources, including older people and people
that typically may be maintained over time whether with disabilities.
or not it is actively processed. Medicare. The U.S. federal and state health insurance
Long-term potentiation (LTP). A mechanism of program for people who are 65 years old or older
brain change following brain injury in which the and for people with disabilities and end-stage renal
efficiency of transmission at the synaptic level is disease.
500  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Medicare Part A. Component of Medicare that ad- and temporal watershed regions; similar to global
dresses inpatient care in skilled nursing, hospital aphasia, with the exception of intact repetition
acute care, rehabilitation hospital, and home health ability.
settings. Monocular visual field. The field of view that is seen
Medicare Part B. Portion of Medicare that addresses with one eye independently of the other.
outpatient rehabilitation and long-term care. Montessori approaches. Intervention methods, ini-
Medicare Part D. Portion of Medicare that addresses tially developed for use with children in educational
the costs of prescription medication. environments, adapted for use with adults who
Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT). A treatment have dementia; goals include enhanced activation
method based on facilitating spoken language of intact intellectual and communicative activities
through the exaggeration of three elements of spo- and improved compensatory strategies through var-
ken language prosody: pitch, tempo/rhythm, and ious activities.
emphatic stress. Motivational theory of life-span development. A
Memory books and wallets. Collections of pictures, model of aging that focuses on adults’ highly indi-
phrases, and words associated with familiar people, vidualized abilities to choose, adapt to, and pursue
places, and events that a person may have difficulty life changes and opportunities.
remembering; designed to enhance communicative MRI diffusion weighted imaging (DWI). A neuro-
interaction and social engagement. imaging technique that involves indexing the rate
Meningioma. A benign tumor that arises from the of water diffusion within voxels (specific units of
meninges. magnetic resonance images), allowing for the visu-
Meningitis. An inflammation of the meninges sur- alization of acute infarctions.
rounding the brain; called meningoencephalitis Multidimensional frameworks.  Frameworks for
when it is caused by an infection. conceptualizing aphasia characterized by the view
Mental effort. See cognitive effort. that there are varied forms, subtypes, or syndromes
Metabolic syndrome. A constellation of symptoms of aphasia, each corresponding to a typical site of
including elevated blood glucose, high blood pres- lesion.
sure, abnormal cholesterol or high triglycerides, and Multidisciplinary team. A team in which each team
excess body fat around the waist. member represents their own expertise and also
Microgenetic framework. A framework for concep- ideally confers with other team members regularly
tualizing aphasia proposed by Brown (1972, 1977; about discipline-specific as well as general rehabil-
Brown & Raleigh, 1979), in which impaired lan- itation goals.
guage abilities reflect the reverse order of progres- Multi-infarct dementia. A case of vascular dementia
sion of evolutionary development of the brain; the where there is evidence of multiple focal lesions.
theory that limbic structures, phylogenetically older Multiple Oral Rereading (MOR). A restitutive, stim-
components of the brain, mediate basic and early ulation approach designed for people with aphasia
stages of language processing while more recently who have acquired reading impairments; involves
evolved structures mediate higher cortical functions repeated reading aloud of the same text.
of language and cognition. Myopia. Reduced far visual acuity, associated with a
Mild cognitive impairment (MCI). A condition of change in the shape of the lens.
cognitive decline that is not typical of normal aging. Nasal. Medial, toward the nose.
Minimum terminal units, or T-units. Units used for Natural feedback. In the PACE treatment method,
discourse analysis, defined as “one main clause plus the principle that feedback about communicative
any subordinate clauses or nonclausal structures effectiveness consists simply of the clinician’s or cli-
attached to or embedded in the main clause” (Shad- ent’s responses regarding whether a message was
den, 1998, p. 22). successfully sent or received.
Minority-world. Pertaining to countries and regions Necrosis. Tissue death.
where the minority of the world’s populations live, Neglect. Inattention to or lack of conscious awareness
e.g., Europe, North America, Israel, Australia; previ- of sensory information that is not due to a sensory
ously labeled as developed or first world. deficit; may be visual, tactile, olfactory, auditory, or
Mixed transcortical aphasia. An aphasia type in gustatory.
which there is no clear agreement about a classic Neologisms. Nonwords; literally, “new words.”
associated site of lesion, although it may be associ- Neologistic paraphasia. Substitution of a neologism
ated with combined multifocal lesions in the frontal for a real word.
501
Glossary  

Neoplasm. Tumors; literally “new growth.” Open head injury (OHI). A type of traumatic brain
Neuritic plaque. Buildup of beta amyloid protein in injury involving breakage or penetration of the skull.
nerve cells. Opportunistic infections. Infections in which viruses
Neurodegenerative disease. Any neurogenic condi- and/or bacteria selectively take advantage of com-
tion that progressively gets worse over time. promised immune systems after an initial infection;
Neurofibrillary tangles. Abnormal fibrous structures also called secondary infections.
within neurons, composed of twisted tau (a protein). Optic aphasia. An impairment in naming an object
Neurolinguistic frameworks. A subset of cognitive presented visually, despite one’s ability to recognize
neuropsychological frameworks for conceptualizing or describe that object.
aphasia that incorporate connectionist models. Optic chiasm. The x-shaped structure housing the
Neuronal regeneration. A mechanism of brain optic nerve fibers at the base of the brain, where
change following brain injury in which the ability of some of the optic nerves from each eye decussate.
some components of injured neurons is restored; see Optic nerve. Cranial nerve II, which transmits visual
dendritic branching and collateral sprouting. information to the brain.
Neuroplasticity. The ability of the nervous system to Optic radiations. Optic nerve fibers arising from the
change and adapt to internal or external influences. thalamus and extending to the primary visual cortex.
New information. In the PACE treatment method, the Optic tract. A continuation of the optic nerve fibers
principle that the stimulus to be described should that travel through the internal capsule.
not be seen in advance by the receiver, thus ensuring Optimizing phase. Phase II of Robey and Schultz’s
true, not simulated, information exchange. (1998) five-phase outcome research model, in which
Noise buildup. A phenomenon in which an individ- hypotheses are refined, a rationale for the treatment
ual experiences increased difficulty with cognitive-​ method is specified, the selection criteria for partici-
linguistic tasks over time. pants are explicitly detailed, and the treatment pro-
Noncanonical sentences. Sentences that have a non- tocol is standardized.
standard word order in any given language; in Oral Reading for Language in Aphasia (ORLA). A
English, noncanonical sentences, such as passives treatment method for people with dyslexia, with
and sentences with embedded clauses, entail phrase the intent to foster recovery or relearning of reading
movement. comprehension through practice using phonological
Nonfluent agrammatic PPA. Form of PPA charac- and semantic routes and associated feedback.
terized by difficulty with syntax, especially with Out of pocket. Payment provided by an individual
understanding and producing longer and more client.
complex sentences. Outcome. An index of change that occurs as a result
Nonfluent aphasias. Types of aphasia in which spo- of time, intervention, or both; encompasses efficacy,
ken language is restricted, characterized by fewer effectiveness, and efficiency.
units of verbal production (e.g., phonemes, words, Paraphasia. Substitution of an unintended word or
content information units) conveyed per unit of time nonword for an intended word.
compared to a standard of “normal” fluent speech; Parkinson’s-associated dementia. A form of demen-
often used interchangeably with the term expressive tia that entails Lewy bodies and co-occurs with
aphasia. Parkinson’s disease; some cases may also involve
Nonmaleficence. A moral principle of avoiding doing neuritic plaques and neurofibrillary tangles typi-
harm to others. cally associated with AD.
Norm-referenced measures. Indices in which results Participation. Involvement in real-life activities,
are compared to a sample of a population with sim- events, situations, and relationships, in the context
ilar traits. of the WHO ICF (WHO, 2001) domain of function-
Occlusive stroke. A type of stroke entailing blockage ing and disability.
of all or a portion of an artery. Participation restrictions. A component of health
Ocular motor deficits. Problems with the neuro- and disability added following a 1999 modification
muscular system responsible for controlling eye to the WHO ICIDH (WHO, 1999), defined as “prob-
movements. lems an individual may have in the manner or extent
Open-class words. Category of words that continue of involvement in life situations” (p. 16).
to be added to languages and evolve in terms of the Pathologic lability. See emotional lability.
ways they are used and combined with other words; Patient. A thematic role corresponding to the object
content words (e.g., nouns and verbs). of a sentence.
502  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Penumbra. The area of tissue surrounding an infarct. Press of speech. See logorrhea.
Per diem. Daily. Prevalence. The proportion of specified populations
Per diem funding scheme. Health care funding ar- that had or have a disorder at a particular time.
rangement in which the third-party payer pro- Primary aging. Changes associated with “normal”
vides a set rate on a daily basis for a given patient’s aging.
care, regardless of which specific services they are Primary progressive aphasia (PPA). The ongoing
provided. loss of language abilities in the face of relatively pre-
Performance. What a person actually does in their served cognitive abilities, caused by neurodegener-
current context. ative disease.
Perfusion weighted imaging (PWI). A neuroimag- Primary tumors. A tumor at the site where tumor
ing technique that involves indexing microscopic progression began.
levels of blood flow, allowing for the detection of Problem-solving approach. A treatment approach
acute ischemia and the study of blood flow in and that entails teaching a client with dysgraphia to
around brain tumors. implement strategies that help facilitate spelling.
Persistent depressive disorder.  A component of Procedural memory. Implicit (nondeclarative) rec-
chronic major depressive disorder; depression con- ollection of how to carry out specific activities or
tinuing for 2 or more weeks. actions.
Personal factors. Characteristics of an individual out- Promoting Aphasics’ Communicative Effectiveness
side of their health condition, including age, race, (PACE). An intervention method developed to fos-
education, profession, habits, beliefs, attitudes, per- ter pragmatic skills during conversation in which
spectives, and life experience. new information is conveyed and in which the
Phonemic paraphasia. Substitution of one or more client and clinician exchange roles as sender and
sounds in an intended word; also called literal receiver.
paraphasia. Propositional complexity index (PCI). A metric
Phonemic perseveration. A type of recurrent per- used in discourse analysis to index semantic com-
severation involving persistence in incorporating plexity; the number of propositions in a sample
phonemic features of previous verbal responses into divided by the number of T-units.
attempts to say target words. Propositional language framework. A means of con-
Phonological components analysis (PCA).  An ceptualizing aphasia as an inability to make proposi-
impairment-focused approach for the remediation tions (Jackson, 1878).
of naming deficits in people with aphasia, with a Propositions. Intentional, meaningful expressions
focus on the phonological aspects of target words. (written, oral, or signed) meant to convey informa-
Phrase movement. Changes in word order that are tional content.
reflected in syntactic changes. Prosody. The intonation, stress, and rhythm of speech.
Pick’s disease. Term originally used to refer to fronto- Prosopagnosia. Impairment in the ability to recog-
temporal lobal degeneration. nize faces.
Positive psychology. The discipline of helping people Prospective memory. Recollection of information
to lead full, meaningful lives and pursue well-being pertinent to future events (e.g., having to return a
and happiness. library book or turn off an oven after use).
Positron emission computed tomography (PET). A Pseudobulbar affect (PBA). See emotional lability.
neuroimaging technique involving the detection of Pseudodementia. See transient confusional state.
radioisotopes (often radioactive oxygen) that have Psycholinguistic framework. A framework for con-
been injected into the bloodstream as they travel ceptualizing aphasia focused on information-pro-
through the brain, allowing for the visualization of cessing components; stages of processing are
regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF). typically conceptualized within boxes in flowcharts
Pragmatics. The social use of language. with arrows showing the order of processing stages
Pragmatic deficits. Problems with the social use of and interconnections among components.
language Psychological age. An index of how one’s personality
Pranayama. A yogic practice focused on breathing changes over time.
experiences. Randomized control trial. A trial in which partic­
Predictive validity. A type of criterion validity mea- ipants who meet explicit selection criteria are
sured by calculating the correlation between test assigned randomly to treatment and control groups,
results and later performance in a relevant area. often conducted across multiple sites.
503
Glossary  

Readability. The degree of ease or difficulty of com- Right brain syndrome (RBS). See right hemisphere
prehending written text. syndrome.
Receptive aphasia. Type of aphasia in which people Right ear advantage. The phenomenon in which
have greater difficulty understanding than produc- listeners who are left brain dominant for language
ing language; often used interchangeably with the process linguistic stimuli with greater efficiency
term fluent aphasia. when the information is presented to the right as
Reciprocal scaffolding. A treatment method in which compared to the left ear.
a person with a neurogenic language disorder serves Right hemisphere syndrome (RHS). Any combina-
as an expert or teacher in an interaction with a per- tion of a constellation of symptoms associated with
son (called a novice, learner, or apprentice). right brain injury (RBI; also called right hemisphere
Recurrent preservation. Recurrence of a response, in damage, RHD).
the context of an established set of responses. Rod. A type of photoreceptor, located in the retina,
Reduction of edema. Lessening of swelling; one of important for low-light and peripheral vision.
the key aspects of spontaneous recovery in cases of Rotational injury. A type of closed-head injury
stroke and brain injury. resulting from a spinning motion of the head, which
Register. The level of formality/informality, or the causes the brain to rotate in relation to the skull; also
degree of highly specialized jargon used within spe- called angular injury.
cific professional or social groups. Savvy Caregiver Program (SCP). A packaged pro-
Reliability. A psychometric property referring to the gram focused on mediating caregiver stress through
consistency with which something is measured or improved interactions with people who have dementia.
evaluated. Sclera. The outer coating of the eyeball.
Reperfusion. Pharmacological restoration of blood Scotoma. A blind area within the visual field for a spe-
flow to an organ or tissue. cific eye, resulting from a lesion within a specific set
Reserve capacity. The difference between a person’s of fibers within the optic nerve on one side.
maximal performance ability and their actual per- Screening. A brief evaluation of whether a person has
formance. a problem that may benefit from further professional
Resolution of diaschisis. Restoration of functioning attention and, if so, what the problem might be and
in structures remote from the area of damage; one of what type of services might help.
the key aspects of spontaneous recovery in cases of Script training. A treatment approach in which the
stroke and brain injury. client practices using personally relevant conver-
Resource allocation. The distribution of cognitive sational scripts written in collaboration with a
effort to various aspects of a task, often aligned with speech-language pathologist, with the goal of pro-
perceived task demands. ducing relatively fluent speech and natural language
Resource capacity theories. A set of theories that in socially meaningful contexts.
attribute cognitive and linguistic deficits to a reduc- Secondary aging. Impairment-based changes associ-
tion in overall cognitive capacity, not the ability to ated with aging.
accomplish individual simple tasks. Secondary or metastatic tumors. Tumors arising
Respect for people. A moral principle; respect for choices from an initial neoplasm that spread to additional
that others make or would make for themselves. parts of the body via the blood supply or lymphatic
Response Elaboration Training (RET). A treatment system, most commonly subsequent to breast, lung,
approach that focuses on increasing the length and and skin cancers.
improving the information content of oral language Selective attention. See focused attention.
of people with Broca’s or “nonfluent” aphasia. Semantic Feature Analysis (SFA). A treatment
Restitutive approach. See restorative approach. approach targeting word-finding abilities, involving
Restorative approach. A treatment approach aimed focused associations with the meanings of words.
at fostering brain-based recovery; sometimes called Semantic memory. Recollection of factual information.
a restitutive or stimulation approach. Semantic paraphasia. Substitution of a real word for
Retina. The inside layer of the eyeball containing an intended word; also called verbal or global
photoreceptors. paraphasia.
Reversible passive. A type of passive clause or sen- Semantic perseveration. A type of recurrent perse-
tence in which the subject (agent) or object (theme) veration; persistence in using words of a similar cat-
could be used interchangeably and still be semanti- egory as a previous response instead of a word from
cally plausible. the current appropriate semantic category.
504  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Semantic variant PPA (svPPA). A form of PPA en- Spaced retrieval training (SRT).  “A method of learn-
tailing challenges with confrontation naming, and ing and retaining information by recalling the infor-
comprehension. mation over increasingly longer periods of time”
Sensitivity. A statistical measure of test performance, (Camp et al., 1996, p. 196), with a goal of enhancing
reflecting the proportion of people who actually the accessibility to stored representations by repeat-
have an impairment that a test identifies as having edly activating them and making a person aware
that impairment. of them.
Sensory stimulation. A category of passive and/or Specific language impairment.  A developmental
interactive methods purported to enhance recovery condition characterized by language deficits in the
in stroke and brain injury and to slow decline in face of relatively age-appropriate cognitive abilities
neurodegenerative conditions through exposure to in children.
touch, vibration, light, scent, sound, or taste. Specificity. A statistical measure of test performance,
Sentence Production Program for Aphasia (SPPA). ​ reflecting the proportion of individuals a test identi­
A stimulation approach to enhance sentence produc- fies as unimpaired who actually are unimpaired.
tion in people with agrammatism. Speech acts. Intended purposes underlying a specific
Short-term memory. A system for holding memory communicative intent in discourse.
during active maintenance and/or rehearsal. SpeechBite.  A free, online, searchable database of
Signal degradation theories. Theories purporting intervention studies related to speech-language
that language comprehension and production defi- pathology, along with ratings of research quality for
cits are attributable to deficits in auditory and/or each study.
visual processing. Speed of processing. Mental efficiency; the rapidity
Single photo emission computerized tomography with which a cognitive task may be accomplished.
(SPECT). A neuroimaging technique involving the Speed of processing theories.  Theories based on
use of intravenously injected radioisotopes (with the notion that cognitive processing at all levels
effects lasting longer than in PET), allowing for the slows as we age due to reduced efficiency of neural
detection of diffuse and focal brain injury and the transmission.
differentiation of stroke from other types of brain Spontaneous recovery. The natural pattern of im-
pathology, such as neurodegenerative disease. provement in functioning after an injury to the brain.
Skilled nursing facilities (SNFs). Facilities that offer Sprechgesang. In Melodic Intonation Therapy, a blend
health services in a residential setting; often include of speaking and singing; literally “spoken song” in
rehabilitation and long-term care services. German.
Skilled services. Intensive medical or rehabilitation Standardized assessments. Assessments that have
services typically not available for extended periods normative data and entail explicit instructions for
of time; care that requires a certain level of clinical test administration and scoring, enabling compari-
provider credentials. sons of individual results to group results.
Social age. An index of aging according to one’s social Statutory surrogacy. Legal designation of a per-
roles and according to changes in one’s environment. son to make decisions for an adult who is deemed
Social cognition deficit hypothesis. A theory that dif- incompetent.
ficulties with empathy, understanding, and respond- Stereotypy. Language output characterized by the
ing to others’ perspectives in people with RBI are production of the same word or set of words or non-
attributable to right hemisphere networks important words regardless of the meaning intended.
for critical aspects of relating to others. Stimulation-facilitation approach to language treat-
Social frameworks. Means of considering the nature ment or Schuell’s stimulation approach. A set of
of language disorders focused on everyday inter- strategies and principles for “strong, controlled and
personal contexts of real-life communication and intensive auditory stimulation of the impaired audi-
participation. tory symbol system” (Coelho et al., 2008, p. 439) in
Sodium amytal infusion. A form of angiography people with aphasia.
entailing injection of amobarbital (an anesthetic), Stimulus power. The likelihood of a particular cue
diluted with saline solution, into the carotid artery eliciting a target word.
to enable determination of hemispheric dominance Story completeness. A component of the story good-
for language; also called the Wada test. ness index involving indexing of critical components
Source memory. Recollection of how, when, and/or in the story.
where a memory was first made. Story goodness index. A measure of organization and
completeness of discourse production.
505
Glossary  

Story grammar. A component of the story goodness Temporal conjunctives. Words referring to time (e.g.,
index that involves indexing the organizational afterward, beforehand, then, simultaneously).
structure of the story. Test-retest reliability. A type of reliability referring
Stroke. A temporary or permanent disruption in blood to the consistency with which the same result is
supply to the brain. achieved when a test is administered to the same
Stroke trigger. Momentary conditions associated person at two different times.
with increased risk or cause of a stroke. Thematic roles. The role that a noun phrase plays in
Stuck-in-set perseveration. Persistence in carrying relation to the action or state described by the verb
out a task recently performed or saying a word pre- in the sentence; defined as the related agent (subject)
viously spoken when the task or word is no longer and patient (object) of a given verb in a given gram-
appropriate. matical context.
Subarachnoid hemorrhage. Leakage of blood that Theory of mind. The ability to interpret, infer, and
occurs on the surface of the brain, between the pia predict the thoughts, beliefs, feelings, and intentions
and arachnoid mater; typically results in subarach- of others and to differentiate the thoughts and per-
noid hematoma. ceptions of others from one’s own.
Subcortical aphasia. Any form of aphasia associated Therapist drift. The tendency for clinicians to vary a
with a lesion below the cortex. treatment protocol according to their own predilec-
Subdural hematoma. A hematoma formed between tions and in response to behaviors of the individual
the arachnoid mater and the dura mater. being treated.
Sundowning or sundowner’s syndrome. A phenom- Third-party payer. The agency that manages reim-
enon in which problematic behaviors seen in people bursement for health care services.
with dementia—including depression, anxiety, agita- Thought process framework. A historic means of
tion, and wandering — worsen in the evening hours. conceptualizing aphasia suggesting that unintended
Supported communication. A philosophy and set of words thought and spoken by people with aphasia
tenets and strategies implemented throughout social interfere with their thinking abilities.
intervention with people who have communication Thrombolytic drug. A pharmacologic agent that dis-
disorders, involving anything that improves access solves blood clots.
to or participation in communication, events, or Thrombotic stroke. A type of occlusive stroke in which
activities. an arterial blockage accumulates in the same area of
Suppression deficit hypothesis. A theory suggesting an artery where the blockage eventually occurs.
that people with RHS are typically able to generate Thrombus. A clot that blocks an artery in a thrombotic
multiple interpretations of words, sentences, and stroke.
stories but are challenged in selecting the most plau- Tissue plasminogen activator, or tPA. The most
sible interpretation. common thrombolytic drug administered following
Surface dyslexia. A form of dyslexia involving an an occlusive stroke.
impairment in visual decoding of graphemes. Tonal languages. Languages in which changes in
Systems science. Research processes in which mem- tones (or pitch and pitch contours) change the literal
bers of clinical populations under study engage meaning of a word.
not only as participants but as experts shaping Total communication approaches. Treatment ap-
the research questions as studies are conceived, proaches that encourage any means of communica-
stakeholders are included in shaping meaningful tion to convey and receive information, involving
research questions and evaluating the usefulness of any and all language modalities.
evidence, and knowledge is translated to meaning- Toxemia. The poisoning, irritation, or inflammation of
ful changes, such as improved intervention meth- nervous system tissue through exposure to harmful
ods, policies, accessibility of services, equity, clinical substances.
education, and funding. Tractography. A DTI technique involving visualiza-
Telegraphic speech.  Spoken language production tion of the course and nature of nerve fiber bundles
characterized by the use of primarily open-class in the brain; also called fiber tracking.
words and omission of function words. Transcortical motor aphasia.  A classical aphasia
Telepractice. The application of technology to deliver subtype associated with a lesion in the anterior
health, counseling, consulting, assessment, or reha- watershed area of the left frontal lobe, extending to
bilitative services at a distance. the prefrontal areas.
Temporal. In the context of visual field deficits, lat- Transcortical sensory aphasia. A classic aphasia
eral, toward the temples. type associated with a lesion in the area surrounding
506  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Wernicke’s area, excluding Wernicke’s area itself, Type-token ratio. A metric examining variation in
namely, the angular gyrus (Brodmann’s area 39) and semantic (lexical) or syntactic performance within
the posterior portion of the middle temporal gyrus spoken or written discourse.
(Brodmann’s area 37). T1-weighted image. A magnetic resonance image that
Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). A is sensitive to lipids, thus enabling gray versus white
technique that involves delivering pulses of low- matter contrast and good anatomical resolution but
level electrical current through the scalp to stimulate reducing visualization of edema and infarcts relative
the brain. to T2-weighted images.
Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS; also called T2-weighted image. A magnetic resonance image
repetitive TMS, or rTMS). A technique involving that is sensitive to water molecule contrasts, thus
magnetic coils placed on the scalp to stimulate or providing enhanced visualization of pathologies
inhibit activation of targeted brain regions beneath such as edema and ischemia.
the scalp via low-frequency magnetic pulses. Unidimensional frameworks. Means of conceptu-
Transdisciplinary team. A team in which members alizing aphasia in which every level of language
are trained to work across disciplinary areas and in (phonology, morphology, syntax, semantics, and
which the lines typically demarcating each profes- pragmatics) and aspect of language use (production,
sional’s scope of practice are blurred. comprehension) is included in one cohesive set of
Transient confusional state.  Dementia-like symp- linguistic abilities.
toms noted in the absence of true dementia (e.g., in Unmasking of preexisting pathways. Restoration of
cases of depression, dietary imbalance, drug effects, neural connections that were previously inactive
and postsurgical states); sometimes called pseudode- or inhibited; one of the key aspects of spontaneous
mentia; a temporary decline in cognitive ability. recovery in cases of stroke and brain injury.
Transient ischemic attack (TIA). A temporary block- Unskilled services. Health care services that do not
age of the blood supply to any area of the brain; a require the skills of a trained clinician to carry out;
common lay term is “mini-stroke.” may include oversight of rote exercises or repetitive
Translational injury. A type of closed-head injury in drills and practice.
which the object-head contact is at a relatively per- Validity. The degree to which a means of measure-
pendicular angle to one of the main axes of the head, ment actually measures what it intended to measure.
causing the brain to hit the side of the skull opposite Vascular dementia. Dementia caused by problems of
the site of contact. blood supply to the brain (e.g., one or more strokes
Transmission deficit theories. A theory that attri- or transient ischemic attacks); also called ischemic
butes declining cognitive and linguistic functioning dementia.
to reduced efficiency of neuronal transmission. Verbal paraphasia. See semantic paraphasia.
Traumatic brain injury (TBI). Brain damage caused Verbal perseveration. Persistence in saying a word
by sudden trauma. spoken previously, often not the word intended at
Treatment dosage. Intensity of treatment over a spec- the moment; a type of stuck-in-set perseveration.
ified period of time. Verb as Core (VAC). A treatment approach intended
Treatment fidelity. The degree to which an interven- to improve expressive verb use and verb under-
tion method is administered in a reliable way or in standing as well as language performance in general
accordance with a specific protocol. in people with agrammatic aphasia.
Treatment intensity. Consideration of the number, Verb Network Strengthening Treatment (VNeST). A
frequency, and duration of treatment sessions. treatment method developed to improve verb
Treatment of Aphasic Perseveration (TAP). A treat- retrieval through enhanced activation of semantic
ment approach for people with aphasia who tend and grammatical or relational aspects of verbs, with
to perseverate on speech sounds, words, and utter- a goal of helping the client generalize the ability to
ances they have already said. produce verbs within sentences and ideally to carry
Treatment of Underlying Forms (TUF). A treat- this over to discourse contexts.
ment approach to help people with agrammatism Virus. Invasive microscopic organisms that take over
improve comprehension and expression of sentence a host’s cells to genetically replicate themselves; typ-
structure. ically harmful organisms that may cause inflamma-
Tumor. See neoplasm. tion in the brain.
507
Glossary  

Visual Action Therapy (VAT). A gesture-based non- or objects” (King, Simmons-Mackie, & Beukelman,
vocal method to promote the use of symbolic ges- 2013, p. 87) used in alternative and augmentative
tures in people with global aphasia. communication.
Visual agnosia. Impairment in recognition or inter- Visual sensory deficits. Problems with registering
pretation of visual stimuli, not attributable to sen- visual information in the brain; may be due to any
sory deficits (includes visual object agnosia and problem or combination of problems from the eye to
prosopagnosia). the primary visual cortex.
Visual attention deficits. Lack of awareness of infor- Visuoconstructive deficits. Problems with being able
mation registered in the visual cortex. to process two- or three-dimensional relationships
Visual field. The entire space from which one takes in in space.
visual information at any given moment. Voluntary Control of Involuntary Utterances (VCIU). ​
Visual integration deficits. Problems with making A treatment approach designed to improve expres-
sense of visual information that is physically seen sive, propositional communication in people with
and also attended to; sometimes referred to as visual severe nonfluent aphasia whose speech is limited to
interpretation deficits. automatic production of few words.
Visual neglect. A visual attention deficit in which indi- Wada test. See sodium amytal infusion.
viduals are able to see the visual world, but they do Wellness coaching. See life coaching.
not or are not able to attend to a portion of the visual Wernicke’s aphasia. A classic aphasia syndrome asso-
space, such that they do not know that they see it. ciated with a lesion in Wernicke’s area in the supe-
Visual object agnosia. A type of visual agnosia involv- rior temporal lobe (corresponding to Brodmann’s
ing a deficit in recognizing real, photographed, or area 22).
drawn objects. Working memory. System for temporary storage of
Visual scene displays.  Images of scenes that are information while it is being processed.
“contextually rich pictures that depict situations, Working memory theories. A set of theories that
places, or experiences that clearly represent rela- link changes in cognitive and linguistic abilities to a
tionships and interactions with important people reduction in working memory capacity.
References

Aarskog, N. K., Hunskår, I., & Bruvik, F. (2019). Animal-​ Agoston, D. V. (2017). Modeling the long-term conse-
assisted interventions with dogs and robotic animals quences of repeated blast-induced mild traumatic
for residents with dementia in nursing homes: A sys- brain injuries. Journal of Neurotrauma, 34(S1), S-44–S-52.
tematic review. Physical & Occupational Therapy In Geri- https://doi.org/10.1089/neu.2017.5317
atrics, 37(2), 77–93. https://doi.org/10.1080/02703181​ Akamoglu, Y., Meadan, H., Pearson, J. N., & Cummings, K.
.2019.1613466 (2018). Getting connected: Speech and language pathol-
Abdelkhalek, N., Hussein, A., Gibbs, T., & Hamdy, H. ogists’ perceptions of building rapport via telepractice.
(2010). Using team-based learning to prepare medi- Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 30(4),
cal students for future problem-based learning. Med- 569–585. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-018-9603-3
ical Teacher, 32(2), 123–129. https://doi.org/​10.3109/​ Alarcon, N. B., & Rogers, M. A. (2006). Supported commu-
01421590903548539 nication for intervention for aphasia. American Speech-​
Ablinger, I., Huber, W., & Radach, R. (2014). Eye move- Language-Hearing Association.
ment analyses indicate the underlying reading strategy Alashram, A. R., Annino, G., Padua, E., Romagnoli, C.,
in the recovery of lexical readers. Aphasiology, 28(6), & Mercuri, N. B. (2019). Cognitive rehabilitation post
640–657. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2014.894​ traumatic brain injury: A systematic review for emerg-
960 ing use of virtual reality technology. Journal of Clinical
Academy of Neurologic Communication Disorders and Neuroscience, 66, 209–219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jo​
Sciences (ANCDS). (2014). Position statement of the Acad- cn​.2019.04.026
emy of Neurologic Communication Disorders and Sciences Alatorre-Cruz, G. C., Silva-Pereyra, J., Fernández, T.,
on clinical doctorate programs in speech-language pathology. Rodríguez-Camacho, M. A., Castro-Chavira, S. A., &
https://www.ancds.org/assets/docs/ancds_clin_doc_ Sanchez-Lopez, J. (2018). Effects of age and working
position_statement.pdf memory load on syntactic processing: An event-related
Academy of Neurologic Communication Disorders and potential study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12,
Sciences (ANCDS). (2015). ANCDS board certification. 185. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00185
https://www.ancds.org/board-certification-process Albert, M. L. (1973). A simple test of visual neglect.
Adamovich, B. B., & Henderson, J. A. (1992). Scales of Neurology, 23(6), 658–664. https://doi.org/10.1212/
Cognitive Ability for Traumatic Brain Injury (SCATBI). WNL.23.6.658
Pro-Ed. Albert, M. L. (1989). Experimental approaches to apha-
Adler, R. K., Hirsch, S., & Pickering, J. (2018). Voice and sia therapy. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 4(3–4), 427–434.
communication therapy for the transgender/gender diverse https://doi.org/10.1016/0911-6044(89)90031-6
client: A comprehensive clinical guide (3rd ed.). Plural Albert, M. L., Bachman, D. L., Morgan, A., & Helm-​
Publishing. Estabrooks, N. (1988). Pharmacotherapy for aphasia.
Adshead, F., Cody, D. D., & Pitt, B. (1992). BASDEC: Neurology, 38(6), 877–879. https://doi.org/10.1212/
A novel screening instrument for depression in elderly WNL.38.6.877
medical inpatients. British Medical Journal, 397. https:// Albert, M., DeKosky, S., Dickson, D., Dubois, B., Feldman,
doi.org/10.1136/bmj.305.6850.397 H. H., Fox, N., . . . Phelps, C. (2011). The diagnosis of
Aghaz, A., Hemmati, E., & Ghasisin, L. (2018). Types of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s dis-
neuroplasticity and factors affecting language recovery ease: Recommendations from the National Institute on
in patients with aphasia: A systematic review. Archives Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diag-
of Neuroscience, 5(3). https://doi.org/10.5812/ans.62265 nostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s
Agimi, Y., Regasa, L. E., & Stout, K. C. (2019). Incidence of & Dementia, 7(3), 270–279. https://doi.org/10.1016/j​
traumatic brain injury in the U.S. military, 2010–2014. .jalz.2011.03.008
Military Medicine, 184(5–6), e233–e241. https://doi.org/​ Albert, M. L., Sparks, R. W., & Helm, N. A. (1973).
10.1093/milmed/usy313 Melodic intonation therapy for aphasia. Archives of

509
510  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Neurology, 29(2), 130–131. https://doi.org/10.1001/ Amano, T., González-Varo, J. P., & Sutherland, W. J.
archneur.1973.00490260074018 (2016). Languages are still a major barrier to global sci-
Alcauskas, M., & Galetta, S. (2018). Editors’ note: Practice ence. PLOS Biology, 14(12), e2000933. https://doi.org/​
guideline update summary: Mild cognitive impairment: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2000933
Report of the guideline development, dissemination, American Academy of Neurology (2017). Clinical Practice
and implementation subcommittee of the American Guideline Process Manual. https://www.aan.com/
Academy of Neurology. Neurology, 91(8). https://doi​ American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and
.org/10.1212/wnl.0000000000006040 statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.).
Alexander, M. P., Naeser, M. A., & Palumbo, C. L. (1987). American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and
Correlations of subcortical CT lesion sites and aphasia statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
profiles. Brain, 110(4), 961–988. https://doi.org/10.1093/ American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (n.d.-a).
brain/110.4.961 What does it mean to be certified? https://www2.asha.org/
Aliminosa, D., McCloskey, M., Goodman Schulman, R., About/governance/committees/CommitteeSmart​
& Sokol, S. M. (1993). Remediation of acquired dys- Forms/Council-for-Clinical-Certification-in-Audiology-​
graphia as a technique for testing interpretations of and-Speech-Language-Pathology/
deficits. Aphasiology, 7(1), 55–69. https://doi.org/​10.10​ American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (n.d.-b).
80/​02687039308249499 National outcomes measurement system (NOMS). https://
Alizadeh, S., & Chavan, M. (2016). Cultural competence www.asha.org/NOMS/
dimensions and outcomes: A systematic review of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2013a).
literature. Health & Social Care in the Community, 24(6), At a glance: Aphasia, TBI and older Americans. The
e117–e130. https://doi.org/10.1111/hsc.12293 ASHA Leader, 18, 26. https://doi.org/10.1044/leader​
Allen, L., Mehta, S., McClure, J. A., & Teasell, R. (2012). .AAG.18092013.26
Therapeutic interventions for aphasia initiated more American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2013b).
than six months post stroke: A review of the evidence. Report of the ad hoc committee on the feasibility of stan-
Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 19(6), 523–535. https://doi​ dards for the clinical doctorate in speech-language pathol-
.org/10.1310/tsr1906-523 ogy. https://Ad-Hoc-Committee-on-Feasibility-of-Stan​
Allen-Burge, R., Burgio, L. D., Bourgeois, M. S., Sims, dards-for-the-Clinical-Doctorate-in-SLP.pdf
R., & Nunnikhoven, J. (2001). Increasing communica- American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2018).
tion among nursing home residents. Journal of Clinical ASHA 2020 Standards and implementation procedures for
Geropsychology, 7(3), 213–230. https://doi.org/10.1023/​ the Certificate of Clinical Competence in speech-language
A:1011343212424 pathology. https://www.asha.org/certification/2020-​
Alm, N., Astell, A., Ellis, M., Dye, R., Gowans, G., & slp-certification-standards/
Campbell, J. (2004). A cognitive prosthesis and com- American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2019).
munication support for people with dementia. Neuro- ASHA 2019 SLP health care survey. https://www.asha​
psychological Rehabilitation, 14(1–2), 117–134. https://doi​ .org/research/memberdata/healthcare-survey/
.org/​10.1080/09602010343000147 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2020a).
Alzheimer’s Association. (n.d.). Frontotemporal dementia. Standards and implementation procedures for the Certifica-
https://www.alz.org/alzheimers-dementia/what-​ tion of Clinical Competence in speech-language pathology.
is-dementia/types-of-dementia/frontotemporal-​de​ https://www.asha.org/certification/2020-slp-certifica​
mentia tion-standards/
Alzheimer’s Association. (2013). 2013 Alzheimer’s disease American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2020b).
facts and figures. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 9(2), 208–245. Ad Hoc Committee on Graduate Education for Speech-
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2013.02.003 Language Pathologists. https://www.asha.org/site​assets/​
Alzheimer’s Disease International. (2010). World Alzhei- reports/ahc-graduate-education-for-slps-​final-report​
mer’s report 2010: The global impact of dementia. https:// .pdf https://www.asha.org/siteassets/reports
www.alz.co.uk/research/files/WorldAlzheimer Report​ American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2021).
2010.pdf Supply and demand resourcelist for speech-language pathol-
Alzheimer’s Disease International. (2021). Dementia statis- ogists. https://www.asha.org/siteassets/surveys/sup​
tics. https://www.alzint.org/about/dementia-facts-fig​ ply-demand-slp.pdf
ures/dementia-statistics/ American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Aca-
Amadeo, K. (2020). Universal health care in different coun- demic Affairs Board. (2012). Academic affairs board report
tries, pros and cons of each. The Balance. https://www.the​ to the ASHA board of directors on the clinical doctorate
balance.com/universal-health-care-4156211 in speech-language pathology. https://www.asha.org/
511
References  

siteassets/reports/2012-report-slp-clinical-doctorate​ episodic and working memory: A meta-analysis. The


.pdf Gerontologist, 57(Suppl. 2), S193–S205. https://doi.org/​
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Ad Hoc 10.1093/geront/gnx056
Committee on Guidelines for the Clinical Doctorate in Armstrong, E. (2000). Aphasia discourse analysis: The
Speech-Language Pathology. (2015). Guidelines for the story so far. Aphasiology, 14(9), 875–892. https://doi​
clinical doctorate in speech-language pathology. https:// .org/​10.1080/02687030050127685
www.asha.org/uploadedFiles/ASHA/About/gover​ Armstrong, E. (2001). Connecting lexical patterns of verb
nance/Resolutions_and_Motions/2015/BOD-22-2015- usage with discourse meanings in aphasia. Aphasiol-
Ad-Hoc-Committee-Reporton-the-Guidelines-for-the- ogy, 15(10/11), 1029–1045. https://doi.org/10.1080/​
Clinical-Doctorate-in-SLP.pdf#search=%22​Ad%22 02687040143000375
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association and Arnold, J. L., Halpern, P., Tsai, M.-C., & Smithline, H.
Council of Academic Programs in Communication (2004). Mass casualty terrorist bombings: A compari-
Sciences and Disorders. (2002). Crisis in the discipline: son of outcomes by bombing type. Annals of Emergency
A plan for reshaping our future. https://www.asha.org/ Medicine, 43(2), 263–273. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0​
siteassets/reports/crisisinthediscipline.pdf 196-0644(03)00723-6
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association and Arthanat, S., Nochajski, S. M., & Stone, J. (2004). The inter-
Council of Academic Programs in Communication national classification of functioning, disability and
Sciences and Disorders. (2008). Report of the 2008 Joint health and its application to cognitive disorders. Dis-
Ad Hoc Committee on PhD shortages in communication sci- ability and Rehabilitation, 26(4), 235–245. https://doi.org
ences and disorders. https://www.asha.org/siteassets/ /10.1080/09638280310001644889
reports/2008phdadhoccomfullreport.pdf Arvanitakis, Z., Wilson, R. S., Bienias, J. L., Evans, D. A.,
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association and & Bennett, D. A. (2004). Diabetes mellitus and risk of
Council of Academic Programs in Communication Alzheimer disease and decline in cognitive function.
Sciences and Disorders. (2010). Joint ASHA–CAPCSD Archives of Neurology, 61(5), 661–666. https://doi.org/​
research doctoral survey report, 2007–2008 academic year. 10.1001/archneur.61.5.661
https://www.asha.org and https://www.capcsd.org Ashtary, F., Janghorbani, M., Chitaz, A., Reisi, M., & Bah-
American Stroke Association. (n.d.). Stroke symptoms. https:// rami, A. (2006). A randomized, double-blind trial of bro-
www.stroke.org/en/about-stroke/stroke-​symptoms mocriptine efficacy in nonfluent aphasia after stroke.
American Telemedicine Association. (2010). A blueprint for Neurology, 66. 914–916. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl​
telerehabilitation guidelines. https://telerehab.pitt.edu/ .000​0203119.91762.0c
ojs/Telerehab/article/view/6063/6293 Association of Frontotemporal Degeneration. (n.d.).
Anderson, J. M., Gilmore, R., Roper, S., Crosson, B., Bauer, Genetics of FTD. https://www.theaftd.org/what-is-ftd/
R. M., Nadeau, S., . . . Heilman, K. M. (1991). Conduction genetics-​of-ftd/
aphasia and the arcuate fasciculus: A reexamination of Aström, M., Asplund, K., & Aström, T. (1992). Psychosocial
the Wernicke-Geschwind model. Brain and Language, function and life satisfaction after stroke. Stroke, 23(4),
70(1), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1999.2135 527–531. https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.23.4.527
Anderson, L. W. (1999). Rethinking Bloom’s taxonomy: Impli- Australian Aphasia Association Inc. (2010). Aphasia facts
cations for testing and assessment. https://eric.ed.gov/​ and figures. https://www.aphasia.org.au
?id=ED435630 Australian Government. (2021). Austalian government job
Andrews, K., Murphy, L., Munday, R., & Littlewood, C. outlook: Audiologists and speech-language pathologists.
(1996). Misdiagnosis of the vegetative state: Retro- https://joboutlook.gov.au/occupations/audiologists-​
spective study in a rehabilitation unit. BMJ, 313(7048), and-speech-pathologists?occupationCode=2527
13–16. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.313.7048.13 Avent, J., & Austermann, S. (2003). Reciprocal scaffolding:
Anjum, J., & Hallowell, B. (2019). Validity of an eyetrack- A context for communication treatment in aphasia.
ing method for capturing auditory-visual cross-format Aphasiology, 17(4), 397–404. https://doi.org/​10.10​80/​
semantic priming. Journal of Clinical and Experimental 02687030244000743
Neuropsychology, 41(4), 411–431. https://doi.org/10.10​ Avent, J., Patterson, J., Lu, A., & Small, K. (2009). Recip-
80/13803395.2019.1567692 rocal scaffolding treatment: A person with aphasia as
Antonucci, S. M. (2009). Use of semantic feature analysis clinical teacher. Aphasiology, 23(1), 110–119. https://doi​
in group aphasia treatment. Aphasiology, 23(7–8), 854– .org/10.1080/02687030802240211
866. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030802634405 Avent, J. R., Edwards, D. J., Franco, C. R., Lucero, C. J.,
Armstrong, B., Gallant, S. N., Li, L., Patel, K., & Wong, & Pekowsky, J. I. (1995). A verbal and non-verbal
B. I. (2017). Stereotype threat effects on older adults’ treatment comparison study in aphasia. Aphasiology,
512  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

9(3), 295–303. https://doi.org/10.1080/026870395082 596. https://socrates.berkeley.edu/~shimlab/2001_


48206 Baldo​_Fluency-JINS.pdf
Baar, S. (2021). Discovering functional needs in speech-lan- Baldwin, G., Breiding, M., & Sleet, D. (2016). Using the
guage therapy. In A. L. Holland & R. J. Elman (Eds.), public health model to address unintentional injuries
Neurogenic communication disorders and the life participa- and TBI: A perspective from the Centers for Disease
tion approach (pp. 53–79). Plural Publishing. Control and Prevention (CDC). NeuroRehabilitation, 39(3),
Babbitt, E. M., & Cherney, L. R. (2010). Communication 345–349. https://doi.org/10.3233/NRE-161366
confidence in persons with aphasia. Topics in Stroke Balestreri, R., Fontana, L., & Astengo, F. (1987). A dou-
Rehabilitation, 17(3), 214–223. https://doi.org/10.1310/ ble-blind placebo controlled evaluation of the safety
tsr1703-214 and efficacy of vinpocetine in the treatment of patients
Bach, L. J., & David, A. S. (2006). Self-awareness after with chronic vascular senile cerebral dysfunction. Jour-
acquired and traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychological nal of the American Geriatrics Society, 35(5), 425–430.
Rehabilitation, 16(4), 397–414. https://doi.org/10.1080/​ https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.1987.tb04664.x
09602010500412830 Ball, A. L., de Riesthal, M., Breeding, V. E., & Mendoza, D.
Baddeley, A., Emslie, H., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1992). The E. (2011). Modified ACT and CART in severe aphasia.
Speed and Capacity of Language Processing (SCOLP) test. Aphasiology, 25(6–7), 836–848. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Thames Valley Test Co. 02687038.2010.544320
Baddeley, A. D., Emslie, H., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1994). Ballard, K. J., & Thompson, C. K. (1999). Treatment and
Doors and people: A test of visual and verbal recall and rec- generalization of complex sentence production in
ognition. Thames Valley Test Co. agrammatism. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing
Baez, S., Pinasco, C., Roca, M., Ferrari, J., Couto, B., García- Research, 42, 670–707. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr​
Cordero, I., . . .Torralva, T. (2019). Brain structural cor- .4203.690
relates of executive and social cognition profiles in Barberini, L., Marrosu, F., Barbarossa, I. T., Melis, M., Suri,
behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia and elderly H. S., Mandas, A., . . . Saba, L. (2017). Networks in the
bipolar disorder. Neuropsychologia, 126, 159–169. https:// brain: From neurovascular coupling of the BOLD effect
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.02.012 to brain functional architecture. Neurovascular Imaging,
Baken, D. (2003). The development of a multidimensional sense 3(1), 3. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40809-017-0027-8
of control index and its use in analyzing the role of control in Barkley, E. F., Major, C. H., & Cross, K. P. (2014). Collabo-
the relationship between SES and health [Doctoral disser- rative learning techniques: A handbook for college faculty.
tation], Massey University. Jossey Bass.
Baken, D., & Stephens, C. (2005). More dimensions for the Barkley, R. (2011). Barkley Deficits in Executive Functioning
multidimensional health locus of control: Confirmatory Scale (BDEFS for adults). Guilford Press.
factor analysis of competing models of the structure of Barron, T., & Amerena, P. (2007). Disability and inclusive
control beliefs. Journal of Health Psychology, 10(5), 643– development. Leonard Cheshire International.
656. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105305055310 Bartlett, C. L., & Pashek, G. V. (1994). Taxonomic theory
Baker, C., Worrall, L., Rose, M., Hudson, K., Ryan, B., & and practical implications in aphasia classification.
O’Byrne, L. (2018). A systematic review of rehabilita- Aphasiology, 8(2), 103–126. https://doi.org/10.1080/​
tion interventions to prevent and treat depression in 02687039408248645
post-stroke aphasia. Disability and Rehabilitation, 40(16), Bartolo, A., Cubelli, R., & Sala, S. D. (2008). Cognitive
1870–1892. https://doi.org/10.1080/09638288.2017.131 approach to the assessment of limb apraxia. The Clinical
5181 Neuropsychologist, 22(1), 27–45. https://doi.org/10.10​
Baker, E. (2012). Optimal intervention intensity. Interna- 80/​13854040601139310
tional Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 14(5), 401– Barton, C. D., Mallik, H., Orr, W. B., & Janofsky, J. S. (1996).
409. https://doi.org/10.3109/17549507.2012.700323 Clinicians’ judgement of capacity of nursing home
Balaban, N., Friedmann, N., & Ariel, M. (2016). The effect patients to give informed consent. Psychiatric Services,
of theory of mind impairment on language: Referring 47(9), 956–960. https://doi.org/10.1176/ps.47.9.956
after right-hemisphere damage. Aphasiology, 30(12), Barton, J., Grudzen, M., & Zielske, R. (2003). Vital connec-
1424–1460. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2015.11 tions in long-term care: Spiritual resources for staff and res-
37274 idents. Health Professions Press.
Baldo, J., Shimamura, A. P., & Delis, D. C. (2001). Verbal Basilakos, A., Rorden, C., Bonilha, L., Moser, D., & Frid-
and design fluency in patients with frontal lobe lesions. riksson, J. (2015). Patterns of poststroke brain damage
Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, 7, 586– that predict speech production errors in apraxia of
513
References  

speech and aphasia dissociate. Stroke, 46(6), 1561–1566. Beeson, P. M., & Henry, M. L. (2008). Comprehension and
https://doi.org/10.1161/STROKEAHA.115.009211 production of written words. In R. Chapey (Ed.), Lan-
Basso, A. (2004). Perseveration or the tower of Babel. Sem- guage intervention strategies in aphasia and related neu-
inars in Speech and Language, 25(4), 375–389. https://doi​ rogenic communication disorders (5th ed., pp. 654–688).
.org/10.1055/s-2004-837249 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
Basso, A., & Macis, M. (2011). Therapy efficacy in chronic Beeson, P. M., Higginson, K., & Rising, K. (2013). Writing
aphasia. Behavioural Neurology, 24(4), 317–325. https:// treatment for aphasia: A texting approach. Journal of
doi.org/10.3233/BEN-2011-0342 Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(3), 945–955.
Bastiaanse, R., Edwards, S., & Rispens, J. (2002). Verb and https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0360)
Sentence Test (VAST). Thames Valley Test Co. Beeson, P. M., & Hillis, A. E. (2001). Comprehension and
Bates, E. (1976). Language and context: The acquisition of production of written words. In R. Chapey (Ed.), Lan-
pragmatics. Academic Press. guage intervention strategies in aphasia and related neu-
Bates, E., Wulfeck, B., & MacWhinney, B. (1991). Cross-​ rogenic communication disorders (4th ed., pp. 572–604).
linguistic studies in aphasia: An overview. Brain and Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
Language, 41(2), 123–148. https://doi.org/10.1016/​00​ Beeson, P. M., Hirsch, F. M., & Rewega, M. A. (2002). Suc-
93-934X(91)90149-U cessful single-word writing treatment: Experimental
Battle, D. E. (Ed.). (2012). Communication disorders in multi​ analyses of four cases. Aphasiology, 16(4–6), 473–491.
cultural and international populations (4th ed.). Elsevier/ https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030244000167
Mosby. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/ Beeson, P. M., & Insalaco, D. (1998). Acquired alexia: Les-
book/​9780323066990 sons from successful treatment. Journal of the Interna-
Bay, E. (1964). Principles of classification and their influ- tional Neuropsychological Society, 4(6), 621–635. https://
ence on our concepts of aphasia. In A. V. S. de Reuck & doi.org/10.1017/S1355617798466116
M. O’Connor (Eds.), Disorders of language (pp. 122–142). Beeson, P. M., Magloire, J. G., & Robey, R. R. (2005). Let-
John Wiley & Sons. ter-by-letter reading: Natural recovery and response
Bayles, K. A., & Tomoeda, C. K. (1993). Arizona Battery for to treatment. Behavioural Neurology, 16(4), 191–202.
Communication Disorders of Dementia (ABCD). Pro-Ed. https://doi.org/10.1155/2005/413962
Bayles, K. A., & Tomoeda, C. K. (1994). The Functional Linguis- Beeson, P. M., Rewega, M. A., Vail, S., & Rapcsak, S. Z.
tic Communication Inventory: Test manual. Canyonlands. (2000). Problem-solving approach to agraphia treat-
Bayles, K. A., & Tomoeda, C. K. (1997). Improving func- ment: Interactive use of lexical and sublexical spell-
tion in dementia and other cognitive linguistic disorders. ing routes. Aphasiology, 14(5–6), 551–565. https://doi​
Canyonlands. .org/10.1080/026870300401315
Bayles, K. A., & Tomoeda, C. (2007). Assessment of cogni- Beeson, P. M., Rising, K., Kim, E. S., & Rapcsak, S. Z. (2010).
tive-communication disorders of dementia. In Bayles, A treatment sequence for phonological alexia/agraphia.
McCullough, & Tomoeda (Eds). Cognitive-communica- Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(2),
tion disorders of dementia (pp. 139–166). Plural Publishing. 450–468. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/​
Bayne, T., Hohwy, J., & Owen, A. M. (2016). Are there lev- 08-​0229)
els of consciousness? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(6), Beeson, P. M., Rising, K., & Volk, J. (2003). Writing treat-
405–413. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.03.009 ment for severe aphasia: Who benefits? Journal of Speech,
Beard, R. L. (2012). Art therapies and dementia care: A Language & Hearing Research, 46(5), 1038–1060.
systematic review. Dementia, 11(5), 633–656. https:// Behn, N., Francis, J., Togher, L., Hatch, E., Moss, B., &
doi.org/10.1177/1471301211421090 Hilari, K. (2021). Description and effectiveness of
Beaumont, J. G., Marjoribanks, J., Flury, S., & Lintern, T. communication partner training in TBI: A systematic
(2002). PACST: Putney Auditory Comprehension Screening review, Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 36(1),
Test. Thames Valley Test Co. 56–71. https://doi.org/10.1097/htr.0000000000000580
Beeson, P. M. (1998). Treatment for letter-by-letter reading: Belanger, H. G., Kretzmer, T., Yoash-Gantz, R., Pickett,
A case study. In N. Helm Estabrooks & A. L. Holland T., & Tupler, L. A. (2009). Cognitive sequelae of blast-​
(Eds.), Approaches to the treatment of aphasia (pp. 153– related versus other mechanisms of brain trauma. Jour-
177). Singular Publishing. nal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(1),
Beeson, P. M. (1999). Treating acquired writing impair- 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617708090036
ment: Strengthening graphemic representations. Apha- Belin, P., Van Eeckhout, P., Zilbovicius, M., Remy, P.,
siology, 13(9–11), 767–785. https://doi.org/10.1080/​02​ François, C., Guillaume, S., & Samson, Y. (1996). Recov-
6870399401867 ery from nonfluent aphasia after melodic intonation
514  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

therapy: A PET study. Neurology, 47(6), 1504–1511. Bernstein-Ellis, E., Higby, E., & Gravier, M. (2021).
https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.47.6.1504 Responding to culturally insensitive test items. Leader
Bell, R., Buchner, A., & Mund, I. (2008). Age-related differ- Live. https://leader.pubs.asha.org/do/10.1044/leader​
ences in irrelevant-speech effects. Psychology and Aging, .AE​.26052021.26/full/
23(2), 377–391. https://doi.org/10.1037/​0882-​7974.23​ Berthier, M. L. (2005). Poststroke aphasia: Epidemiology,
.2.377 pathophysiology and treatment. Drugs & Aging, 22(2),
Bendapudi, N. M., Berry, L. L., Frey, K. A., Parish, J. T., & 163–182. https://doi.org/10.2165/00002512-200522020-
Rayburn, W. L. (2006). Patients’ perspectives on ideal 00006
physician behaviors. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 81(3), Berthier, M. L., Green, C., Higueras, C., Fernández, I.,
338–344. https://doi.org/10.4065/81.3.338 Hinojosa, J., & Martín, M. C. (2006). A randomized,
Ben-David, B. M., Erel, H., Goy, H., & Schneider, B. A. placebo-controlled study of donepezil in poststroke
(2015). “Older is always better”: Age-related differ- aphasia. Neurology, 67. 1687–1689.
ences in vocabulary scores across 16 years. Psychology Berthier, M. L., Green, C., Lara, J. P., Higueras, C., Bar-
and Aging, 30(4), 856–862. https://doi.org/10.1037/ bancho, M. A., Dávila, G., & Pulvermüller, F. (2009).
pag0000051 Memantine and constraint-induced aphasia therapy in
Benedict, R. H. B. (1997). Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-​ chronic poststroke aphasia. Annals of Neurology, 65(5),
Revised: Professional manual. Psychological Assessment 577–585. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.21597
Resources. Berthier, M. L., Pulvermüller, F., Dávila, G., Casares, N.
Benigas, J. E., & Bourgeois, M. (2016). Using spaced G., & Gutiérrez, A. (2011). Drug therapy of post-stroke
retrieval with external aids to improve use of compen- aphasia: A review of current evidence. Neuropsychol-
satory strategies during eating for persons with demen- ogy Review, 21(3), 302–317. https://doi.org/10.1007/
tia. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 25(3), s11065-011-9177-7
321–334. https://doi.org/10.1044/2015_AJSLP-14-0176 Beukelman, D. R., Fager, S., Ball, L., & Dietz, A. (2007).
Benigas, J. E., Brush, J. A., & Elliot, G. M. (2016). Spaced AAC for adults with acquired neurological conditions:
retrieval step by step: An evidence-based memory interven- A review. AAC: Augmentative and Alternative Com-
tion. Health Professions Press. munication, 23(3), 230–242. https://doi.org/10.1080/​
Bennett, H. E., Thomas, S. A., Austen, R., Morris, A. M., 07434610701553668
& Lincoln, N. B. (2006). Validation of screening mea- Beukelman, D. R., Garrett, K. L., & Yorkston, K. M. (2007).
sures for assessing mood in stroke patients. British Augmentative communication strategies for adults with
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 45(3), 367–376. https:// acute or chronic medical conditions. Paul H. Brookes.
doi.org/10.1348/014466505X58277 Beukelman, D. R., Hux, K., Dietz, A., McKelvey, M.,
Bennett, M. H., Weibel, S., Wasiak, J., Schnabel, A., & Weissling, K. (2015). Using visual scene displays
French, C., & Kranke, P. (2014). Hyperbaric oxygen as communication support options for people with
therapy for acute ischaemic stroke. Cochrane Database chronic, severe aphasia: A summary of AAC research
of Systematic Reviews, (11), CD004954. https://doi.org/​ and future research directions. Augmentative and Alter-
10.1002/14651858.CD004954.pub3 native Communication, 31(3), 234–245. https://doi.org/​
Benson, D. F. (1979). Aphasia, alexia, and agraphia. Churchill 10.3109/07434618.2015.1052152
Livingstone. Beukelman, D. R., & Mirenda, P. (2013). Augmentative and
Benton, A. L., & Benton Sivan, A. (1992). Benton Visual alternative communication: Supporting children and adults
Retention Test. The Psychological Corporation. with complex communication needs (4th ed.). Paul H.
Benton, A., & Tranel, D. (1993). Visuoperceptual, visuo- Brookes.
spatial, and visuoconstructive disorders. In K. M. Heil- Beume, L.-A., Martin, M., Kaller, C. P., Klöppel, S., Schmidt,
man & E. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology (pp. C. S. M., Urbach, H., . . . Umarova, R. M. (2017). Visual
165–213). Oxford University Press. neglect after left-hemispheric lesions: A voxel-based
Berg, K., Isaksen, J., Wallace, S. J., Cruice, M., Sim- lesion–symptom mapping study in 121 acute stroke
mons-Mackie, N., & Worrall, L. (2020). Establishing patients. Experimental Brain Research, 235(1), 83–95.
consensus on a definition of aphasia: An e-delphi https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4771-9
study of international aphasia researchers. Aphasiology. Beveridge, M. E. L., & Bak, T. H. (2011). The languages
https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2020.1852003 of aphasia research: Bias and diversity. Aphasiology,
Berman, M., & Fenaughty, A. (2005). Technology and man- 25(12), 1451–1468. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038​
aged care: Patient benefits of telemedicine in a rural .2011.624165
health care network. Health Economics, 14(6), 559–573. Bhatnagar, S. C. (2013). Neuroscience for the study of communi-
https://doi.org/10.1002/hec.952 cative disorders (4th ed.). Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
515
References  

Bhatnagar, S. C., & Andy, O. (1983). Language in the Blake, M. L., Duffy, J. R., Myers, P. S., & Tompkins, C. A.
nondominant right hemisphere. Archives of Neurology, (2002). Prevalence and patterns of right hemisphere
40(12), 728–731. https://doi.org/10.1001/arch​ neur​ cognitive/communicative deficits: Retrospective data
.1983.04050110046006 from an inpatient rehabilitation unit. Aphasiology, 16,
Bhatnagar, S. C., Mandybur, G. T., Buckingham, H. W., 537–547. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030244000194
& Andy, O. J. (2000). Language representation in the Blake, M. L., Frymark, T., & Venedictov, R. (2013). An
human brain: Evidence from cortical mapping. Brain evidence-based systematic review on communica-
and Language, 74(2), 238–259. https://doi.org/10.1006/ tion treatments for individuals with right hemisphere
brln.2000.2339 brain damage. American Journal of Speech-Language
Bickenbach, J., Cieza, A., Rauch, A., & Stucki, G. (2012). Pathology, 22(1), 146–160. https://doi.org/10.1044/​
ICF Core Sets: Manual for Clinical Practice. Hogrefe 1058-​0360(2012/12-0021)
Publishing. Blomert, L., Kean, M.-L., Koster, C., & Schokker, J. (1994).
Bilda, K. (2011). Video-based conversational script train- Amsterdam-Nijmegen Everyday Language Test: Con-
ing for aphasia: A therapy study. Aphasiology, 25(2), struction, reliability and validity. Aphasiology, 8(4),
191–201. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687031003798254 381–407. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687039408248666
Binder, J. R. (2015). The Wernicke area: Modern evidence Bloom, B. S. (1956). Taxonomy of educational objectives: The
and a reinterpretation. Neurology, 85(24), 2170–2175. classification of educational goals. Longmans, Green.
https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000002219 Bloom, L., & Lahey, M. (1978). Language development and
Binder, J. R. (2017). Current controversies on Wernicke’s language disorders. Wiley.
area and its role in language. Current Neurology and Boczko, F. (1994). The breakfast club: A multimodal lan-
Neuroscience Reports, 17(8), 58. https://doi.org/10.1007/ guage stimulation program for nursing home residents
s11910-017-0764-8 with Alzheimer’s disease. American Journal of Alzhei-
Bingham, S. L. (2012). Refusal of treatment and deci- mer’s Disease and Other Dementias, 9(4), 35–38. https://
sion-making capacity. Nursing Ethics, 19(1), 167–172. doi.org/10.1177/153331759400900407
https://doi.org/10.1177/0969733011431925 Boehme, A. K., Esenwa, C., & Elkind, M. S. V. (2017).
Binney, R. J., Ashaie, S. A., Zuckerman, B. M., Hung, J., & Stroke risk factors, genetics, and prevention. Circula-
Reilly, J. (2018). Frontotemporal stimulation modulates tion Research, 120(3), 472–495. https://doi.org/10.1161/
semantically guided visual search during confrontation CIRCRESAHA.116.308398
naming: A combined tDCS and eye tracking investiga- Boles, L. (2004). The ICF language of numeric adjectives.
tion. Brain and Language (180–182), 14–23. https://doi​ Advances in Speech-Language Pathology, 6(1), 71–73.
.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2018.04.004 https://doi.org/10.3109/09638288.2010.529235
Binney, R. J., Zuckerman, B., & Reilly, J. (2016). A neuro- Boles, L. (2009). Aphasia couples therapy (ACT) workbook.
psychological perspective on abstract word represen- Plural Publishing.
tation: From theory to treatment of acquired language Bollinger, R. L., Musson, N. D., & Holland, A. L. (1993).
disorders. Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports, A study of group communication intervention with
16(9), 79. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11910-016-0683-0 chronically aphasic persons. Aphasiology, 7(3), 301–313.
Björklund, F., Bäckström, M., & Jørgensen, Ø. (2011). https://doi.org/10.1080/02687039308249512
In-group ratings are affected by who asks and how: Bonakdarpour, B., Eftekharzadeh, A., & Ashayeri, H.
Interactive effects of experimenter group-membership (2003). Melodic intonation therapy in Persian aphasic
and response format. Journal of Social Psychology, 151(5), patients. Aphasiology, 17(1), 75–95. https://doi.org/​
625–634. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2010.522​ 10.1080/729254891
623 Booth, S., & Swabey, D. (1999). Group training in com-
Blake, M. L. (2005). Right hemisphere syndrome. In L. L. munication skills for carers of adults with aphasia.
LaPointe (Ed.), Aphasia and related neurogenic language International Journal of Language & Communication Dis-
disorders (pp. 213–224). Thieme. orders, 34(3), 291–309. https://doi.org/10.1080/​1368​28​
Blake, M. L. (2006). Clinical relevance of discourse char- 299247423
acteristics after right hemisphere brain damage. Ameri- Borson, S., Sehgal, M., & Chodosh, J. (2019). Monetizing
can Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 15(3), 255–267. the MoCA: What now? Journal of the American Geriatrics
https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2006/024) Society, 67(11), 2229–2231. https://doi.org/10.1111/jgs​
Blake, M. L. (2021). Communication deficits associated .16158
with right hemisphere brain damage, In J. S. Damico, N. Boswell, S. (2011). Court access for people with aphasia.
Müller, & M. J. Ball (Eds.), The handbook for language and The ASHA Leader, 16, 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1044/
speech disorders (2nd ed., pp. 571–589). Wiley-Blackwell. leader.FTR6.16022011.1
516  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Boucher, V., Garcia, L. J., Fleurant, J., & Paradis, J. Braak, H., & Braak, E. (1991). Neuropathological stageing
(2001). Variable efficacy of rhythm and tone in melo- of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathologica,
dy-based interventions: Implications for the assump- 82(4), 239–259. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00308809
tion of a right-hemisphere facilitation in non-fluent Bradley, D. C., Garret, M. E., & Zurif, E. B. (1980). Syntactic
aphasia. Aphasiology, 15(2), 131–149. https://doi.org/​ deficits in Broca’s aphasia. In D. Caplan (Ed.), Biological
10.1080/02687040042000098 studies of mental processes. MIT Press.
Bourgeois, M. S. (1990). Enhancing conversation skills in Brady, M. C., Kelly, H., Godwin, J., & Enderby, P. (2012).
patients with Alzheimer’s disease using a prosthetic Speech and language therapy for aphasia following
memory aid. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23(1), stroke. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. https://
29–42. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1990.23-29 doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD​000425.pub3
Bourgeois, M. S. (1992). Evaluating memory wallets in Brady, M. C., Kelly, H., Godwin, J., Enderby, P., & Camp-
conversations with persons with dementia. Journal of bell, P. (2016). Speech and language therapy for aphasia
Speech and Hearing Research, 35(6), 1344–1357. https:// following stroke. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews.
doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3506.1344 https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD000425.pub4
Bourgeois, M. S. (1993). Effects of memory aids on the Brady Wagner, L. C. (2003). Clinical ethics in the context of
dyadic conversations of individuals with dementia. language and cognitive impairment: Rights and protec-
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26(1), 77–87. https:// tions. Seminars in Speech and Language, 24(4), 275–284.
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1993.26-77 https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2004-815581
Bourgeois, M. S., Burgio, L. D., Schulz, R., Beach, S., & BrainLine. (2015). Blast injuries and the brain. https://www​
Palmer, B. (1997). Modifying repetitive verbaliza- .brainlinemilitary.org/content/2010/12/blast-injuries​
tions of community-dwelling patients with AD. The and-the-brain.html
Gerontologist, 37(1), 30–39. https://doi.org/10.1093/ Braun, M. J., Dunn, W., & Tomchek, S. D. (2017). A pilot
geront/37.1.30 study on professional documentation: Do we write
Bourgeois, M. S., & Hickey, E. M. (2009). Dementia: From from a strengths perspective? American Journal of
diagnosis to management — A functional approach. Psycho- Speech-Language Pathology, 26(3), 972–981. https://doi​
logical Press. .org/​10.1044/2017_AJSLP-16-0117
Bourgeois, M. S., & Mason, L. A. (1996). Memory wallet Braver, T. S., & Barch, D. M. (2006). Extracting core compo-
intervention in an adult daycare setting. Behavioral nents of cognitive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
Interventions: Theory and Practice in Residential and Com- 10(12), 529–532. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006​.10​
munity-Based Clinical Programs, 11(1), 3–18. https:// .006
doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-078X(199601)11:1<3::AID- Breitenstein, C., Grewe, T., Flöel, A., Ziegler, W., Springer,
BIN150>3.0.CO;2-0 L., Martus, P., . . . FCET2EC study group. (2017). Inten-
Bowers, D., Blonder, L. X., & Heilman, K. M. (1999). Florida sive speech and language therapy in patients with
affect battery. University of Florida, Cognitive Neurosci- chronic aphasia after stroke: A randomised, open-label,
ence Laboratory. blinded-endpoint, controlled trial in a health-care set-
Bowman, S. M., Aitken, M. E., Helmkamp, J. C., Maham, ting. Lancet (London, England), 389(10078), 1528–1538.
S. A., & Graham, C. J. (2009). Impact of helmets on inju- https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(17)30067-3
ries to riders of all-terrain vehicles. Injury Prevention, Brennan, A. D., Worrall, L. E., & McKenna, K. T. (2005).
15(1), 3–7. https://doi.org/10.1136/ip.2008.019372 The relationship between specific features of aphasia-​
Boyle, M. (2001). Semantic feature analysis: The evi- friendly written material and comprehension of writ-
dence for treating lexical impairments in aphasia. ten material for people with aphasia: An exploratory
SIG, 2 Perspectives on Neurophysiology and Neurogenic study. Aphasiology, 19(8), 693–711. https://doi​.org/​
Speech and Language Disorders, 11(2), 23–28. https://doi​ 10.1080/02687030444000958
.org/10.1044/nnsld11.2.23 Brennan, D. M., Georgeadis, A. C., Baron, C. R., & Barker,
Boyle, M. (2004). Semantic feature analysis treatment for L. M. (2004). The effect of videoconference-based tel-
anomia in two fluent aphasia syndromes. American erehabilitation on story retelling performance by
Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 13(3), 236–249. brain-injured subjects and its implications for remote
https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2004/025) speech-language therapy. Telemedicine Journal and
Boyle, M., & Coelho, C. A. (1995). Application of semantic E-Health, 10(2), 147–154. https://doi.org/10.1089/tmj​
feature analysis as a treatment for aphasic dysnomia. .2004​.10.147
American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 4, 94–138. Brickenkamp, R., & Zillmer, E. (1998). The d2 Test of Atten-
https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360.0404.94 tion. Hogrefe & Huber.
517
References  

Britt, A. E., Ferrara, C., & Mirman, D. (2016). Distinct successfully with aphasia. Aphasiology, 24(10), 1267–
effects of lexical and semantic competition during pic- 1295. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687031003755429
ture naming in younger adults, older adults, and peo- Brown, L., Sherbenou, R. J., & Johnson, S. K. (2010). Test of
ple with aphasia. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. https://doi​ Nonverbal Intelligence TONI-4. Pro-Ed.
.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00813 Brown, N. A. (2005). Information on telemedicine. Journal
Brody, H. (2005). Shared decision making and determining of Telemedicine and Telecare, 11(3), 117–126. https://doi​
decision-making capacity. Primary Care, 32(3), 645–658. .org/10.1258/1357633053688714
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pop.2005.06.004 Brownell, H., & Gardner, H. (1988). Neuropsychological
Brohi, A. H., Borges, K. J. J., Yar, G. H., Hassan, N., & Shah, insights into humour. In J. Durant & J. Miller (Eds.),
S. N. N. (2018). Configuration of circle of Willis and its Laughing matters: A serious look at humour (pp. 17–34).
clinical significance. Journal of Bahria University Medical Wiley.
and Dental College, 8(4), 270–273. Brownell, H., & Martino, G. (1998). Deficits in inference
Brook, G. (2019). SLP health care survey report: Workforce and social cognition: The effects of right hemisphere
trends 2005–2019. https://www.asha.org/siteassets/sur​ brain damage on discourse. In M. Beeman & C. Chi-
veys/2019-slp-health-care-survey-workforce-trends- arello (Eds.), Right hemisphere language comprehension:
2005-2019.pdf Perspectives from cognitive neuroscience (pp. 309–328).
Brookfield, S. D. (2012). Teaching for critical thinking: Tools Lawrence Erlbaum.
and techniques to help students question their assumptions. Brumfitt, S. (1995). Psychotherapy in aphasia. In C. Code
Jossey-Bass. & D. Müller (Eds.), Treatment of Aphasia: From theory to
Brookshire, R. H. (1983). Subject description and gen- practice. Whurr.
erality of results in experiments with aphasic adults. Brumfitt, S. (2009). Psychological wellbeing and acquired com-
Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 48(4), 342–346. munication impairment. Wiley-Blackwell.
https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.4804.342 Brumfitt, S., & Sheeran, P. (1999). The Visual Assessment of
Brookshire, R. H., & Nicholas, L. E. (1984). Comprehen- Self-Esteem Scale. Winslow Press.
sion of directly and indirectly stated main ideas and Bryan, K. (1994). The Right Hemisphere Language Battery
details in discourse by brain-damaged and non-brain- (2nd ed.). Whurr.
damaged listeners. Brain and Language, 21(1), 21–36. Buchmann, I., & Randerath, J. (2017). Selection and
https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(84)90033-6 application of familiar and novel tools in patients
Brookshire, R. H., & Nicholas, L. E. (1997). Discourse Com- with left and right hemispheric stroke: Psychomet-
prehension Test: Test manual. BRK. rics and normative data. Cortex, 94, 49–62. https://doi​
Brotons, M., & Koger, S. M. (2000). The impact of music .org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.06.001
therapy on language functioning in dementia. Journal of Buchsbaum, B. R., Baldo, J., Okada, K., Berman, K. F.,
Music Therapy, 37(3), 183–195. https://doi.org/10.1093/ Dronkers, N., D’Esposito, M., & Hickok, G. (2011).
jmt/37.3.183 Conduction aphasia, sensory-motor integration, and
Broussard, T. G. (2015). Stroke diary: A primer for aphasia phonological short-term memory: An aggregate analy-
therapy. CreateSpace. sis of lesion and fMRI data. Brain and Language, 119(3),
Broussard, T. (2020). Spreading aphasia awareness, one social 119–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2010.12.001
distancing person at a times — in the COVID age. Stroke Bunker, L. D., Nessler, C., & Wambaugh, J. L. (2019). Effect
Educator, Inc. https://www.strokeeducator.com/ size benchmarks for response elaboration training:
Brown, J., Hux, K., & Fairbanks, S. (2016). Reading recov- A meta-analysis. American Journal of Speech-Language
ery: A case study using a multicomponent treatment Pathology, 28(1S), 247–258. https://doi.org/10.1044/​
for acquired alexia. Aphasiology, 30(1), 23–44. https:// 2018_AJSLP-17-0152
doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2015.1052728 Burgio, L. D., Allen-Burge, R., Roth, D. L., Bourgeois, M.
Brown, J. W. (1972). Aphasia, apraxia, and agnosia: Clinical S., Dijkstra, K., Gerstle, J., & Bankester, L. (2001). Come
and theoretical aspects. Charles C. Thomas. talk with me: Improving communication between
Brown, J. W. (1977). Mind, brain and consciousness. Aca- nursing assistants and nursing home residents during
demic Press. care routines. The Gerontologist, 41(4), 449–460. https://
Brown, J. W., & Raleigh, M. (1979). Language represen- doi.org/10.1093/geront/41.4.449
tation in the brain. In H. Steklis (Ed.), Neurobiology of Burke, D. M., MacKay, D. G., & James, L. E. (2000). Theo-
social communication in primates. Academic Press. retical approaches to language and aging. In T. J. Per-
Brown, K., Worrall, L., Davidson, B., & Howe, T. (2010). fect & E. A. Maylor (Eds.), Models of cognitive aging (pp.
Snapshots of success: An insider perspective on living 204–237). Oxford University Press.
518  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Burke, D. M., & Shafto, M. A. (2008). Language and aging. Cahana-Amitay, D., Oveis, A., & Sayers, J. (2013). Feeling
In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The handbook anxious can affect language performance in chronic
of aging and cognition (3rd ed., pp. 373–444). Psychology aphasia: A case report. Procedia — Social and Behavioral
Press. Sciences, 94, 149–150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbs​
Burns, M. S. (1997). Burns brief inventory of communication pro.2013.09.073
and cognition: Right hemisphere inventory. Pearson. Cameron, R. M., Wambaugh, J. L., & Mauszycki, S. C.
Burns, M. S. (2004). Clinical management of agnosia. Top- (2010). Individual variability on discourse measures
ics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 11(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/​ over repeated sampling times in persons with apha-
10.1310/N13K-YKYQ-3XX1-NFAV sia. Aphasiology, 24(6–8), 671–684. https://doi.org/​
Butler, K. M., & Zacks, R. T. (2006). Age deficits in the con- 10.1080/02687030903443813
trol of prepotent responses: Evidence for an inhibitory Camp, C. J. (2001). From efficacy to effectiveness to dif-
decline. Psychology and Aging, 21(3), 638–643. https:// fusion: Making the transitions in dementia interven-
psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0882-7974.21.3.638 tion research. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 11(3/4),
Byczewska-Konieczny, K., & Kielar-Turska, M. (2017). 495–517. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602010042000079
Syntactic abilities in old age and their relation to Camp, C. J. (2010). Origins of Montessori programming
working memory and cognitive flexibility. Psychologia for dementia. Non-Pharmacological Therapies in Demen-
Rozwojowa, 22(2), 45–53. https://doi.org/10.4467/2084 tia, 1(2), 163–174.
3879pr.17.009.7041 Camp, C. J., Foss, J. W., O’Hanlon, A. M., & Stevens, A. B.
Byng, S., Kay, J., Edmundson, A., & Scott, C. (1990). Apha- (1996). Memory interventions for persons with demen-
sia tests reconsidered. Aphasiology, 4(1), 67–91. https:// tia. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10(3), 193–210. https://
doi.org/10.1080/02687039008249055 doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199606)10:3​<193:​:​
Byng, S., Nickels, L., & Black, M. (1994). Replicating ther- AID-ACP374>3.0.CO;2-4
apy for mapping deficits in agrammatism: Remap- Camp, C. J., Judge, K. S., Bye, C. A., Fox, K. M., Bowden, J.,
ping the deficit? Aphasiology, 8(4), 315–341. https://doi​ Bell, M., . . . Mattern, J. M. (1997). An intergenerational
.org/10.1080/02687039408248663 program for persons with dementia using Montessori
Byng, S., Pound, C., & Parr, S. (2000). Living with aphasia: methods. The Gerontologist, 37(5), 688–692. https://doi​
A framework for therapy interventions. In I. Papatha- .org/10.1093/geront/37.5.688
nasiou (Ed.), Acquired neurogenic communication disor- Capilouto, G., Wright, H. H., & Wagovich, S. A. (2005).
ders: A clinical perspective (pp. 49–75). Whurr. CIU and main event analyses of the structured dis-
Byrne, K., & Orange, J. (2005). Conceptualizing commu- course of older and younger adults. Journal of Communi-
nication enhancement in dementia for family care- cation Disorders, 38, 431–444. https://doi.org/10.1016/j​
givers using the WHO-ICF framework. Advances in .jcomdis.2005.03.005
Speech-Language Pathology, 7(4), 187–202. https://doi​ Capilouto, G. J., Wright, H. H., & Wagovich, S. A. (2006).
.org/10.1080/14417040500337062 Reliability of main event measurement in the discourse
Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction of individuals with aphasia. Aphasiology, 20(2–4), 205–
in older adults: The HAROLD model. Psychology and 216. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030500473122
Aging, 17(1), 85–100. https://doi.org/10.1037/​0882-​ Caplan, D., DeDe, G., Waters, G., Michaud, J., & Tripo-
7974.17.1.85 dis, Y. (2011). Effects of age, speed of processing, and
Cadório, I., Lousada, M., Martins, P., & Figueiredo, D. working memory on comprehension of sentences with
(2017). Generalization and maintenance of treatment relative clauses. Psychology and Aging, 26(2), 439–450.
gains in primary progressive aphasia (PPA): A sys- https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021837
tematic review. International Journal of Language & Caplan, D., Waters, G., & Alpert, N. (2003). Effects of age
Communication Disorders, 52(5), 543–560. https://doi​ and speed of processing on rCBF correlates of syntac-
.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12310 tic processing in sentence comprehension. HBM Human
Cahana-Amitay, D., Albert, M. L., & Oveis, A. (2014). Psy- Brain Mapping, 19(2), 112–131. https://doi.org/10.1002/
cholinguistics of aphasia pharmacotherapy: Asking the hbm.10107
right questions. Aphasiology, 28(2), 133–154. https://doi​ Caporael, L. R. (1981). The paralanguage of caregiving:
.org/10.1080/02687038.2013.818099 Baby talk to the institutionalized aged. Journal of Per-
Cahana-Amitay, D., Albert, M. L., Pyun, S.-B., Westwood, sonality and Social Psychology, 40(5), 876–884. https://
A., Jenkins, T., Wolford, S., & Finley, M. (2011). Lan- doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.40.5.876
guage as a stressor in aphasia. Aphasiology, 25(2), 593– Carlomagno, S., Losanno, N., Emanuelli, S., & Casadio,
614. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2010.541469 P. (1991). Expressive language recovery or improved
519
References  

communicative skills: Effects of P.A.C.E. therapy on ative care in Australia, International Journal of Speech-
aphasics’ referential communication and story retell- Language Pathology, 23:1,57-69. https://doi.org/10.1080/​
ing. Aphasiology, 5(4–5), 419–424. https://doi.org/​10​ 17549507.2020.1730966
.1080/02687039108248544 Chahda, L., Mathisen, B. A., & Carey, L. B. (2017). The role
Carragher, M., Sage, K., & Conroy, P. (2013). The effects of of speech-language pathologists in adult palliative care.
verb retrieval therapy for people with non-fluent apha- International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19(1),
sia: Evidence from assessment tasks and conversation. 58–68. https://doi.org/10.1080/17549507.2016.1241301
Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 23(6), 846–887. https:// Chan, R. C. K. (2000). Attentional deficits in patients with
doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2013.832335 closed head injury: A further study to the discrimina-
Carrera, E., & Tononi, G. (2014). Diaschisis: Past, present, tive validity of the test of everyday attention. Brain
future. Brain, 137(9), 2408–2422. https://doi.org/10​.10​ Injury, 14(3), 227–236. https://doi.org/10.1080/​0269​90​
93/​brain/awu101 500120709
Carvalho, I. P., Pais, V. G., Almeida, S. S., Ribeiro-Silva, Chang, W. D., & Bourgeois, M. (2019). Effects of visual
R., Figueiredo-Braga, M., Teles, A., & Mota-Cardoso, stimuli on decision-making capacity of people with
R. (2011). Learning clinical communication skills: Out- dementia for end-of-life care. American Journal of
comes of a program for professional practitioners. Speech-Language Pathology 229(3), 1–16. https://doi.org
Patient Education and Counseling, 84(1), 84–89. https:// /​10.1044/2019_AJSLP-19-0028
doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2010.05.010 Chapey, R. (2008). Cognitive stimulation: Stimulation of
Cason, J., & Brannon, J. A. (2011). Telehealth regulatory recognition/comprehension, memory, and convergent,
and legal considerations: Frequently asked questions. divergent, and evaluative thinking. In R. Chapey (Ed.),
International Journal of Telerehabilitation, 3(2), 15–18. Language intervention strategies in aphasia and related neu-
https://doi.org/10.5195/ijt.2011.6077 rogenic communication disorders (4th ed., pp. 469–506).
Centeno, J. G. (2017, November). Editor’s column: Clin- Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
ical management of communicatively impaired adult Chapey, R., Duchan, J. F., Elman, R. J., Garcia, L. J., Kagan,
neurorehabilitation caseloads in a diverse aging world. A., Lyon, J., & Simmons Mackie, N. (2000). Life partic-
Perspectives of the ASHA Special Interest Groups (SIG 2), ipation approach to aphasia: A statement of values for
2(Pt. 3), 88–90. https://doi.org/10.1044/persp2.SIG2.88 the future. gives the source as The ASHA Leader, 5, 3.
Centeno, J. G., Kiran, S., & Armstrong, E (2020). Aphasia https://doi.org/10.1044/leader.FTR.05032000.4
management in growing multiethnic populations. Apha- Chapman, L. R., & Hallowell, B. (2015). A novel pupil-
siology, 34(11), 1314–1318, https://doi.org/10.1080/ lometric method for indexing word difficulty in indi-
02687038.2020.1781420 viduals with and without aphasia. Journal of Speech,
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). Language, and Hearing Research, 58, 1508–1520. https://
(2014). About HIV. https://www.cdc.gov/hiv/basics/ doi.org/10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-14-0287
whatishiv.html Chapman, L. R., & Hallowell, B. (2021a). Expecting ques-
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). (2020). tions modulates cognitive effort in a syntactic process-
Disability and health promotion. https://www.cdc.gov/ ing task: Evidence from pupillometry. Journal of Speech,
ncbddd/disabilityandhealth/disability-strategies.html Language, and Hearing Research, 63(12), 1–13. https://
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). (2021). doi.org/10.1044/2020_JSLHR-20-00071
HIV. https://www.cdc.gov/hiv Chapman, L. R., & Hallowell, B. (2021b). The unfolding
Cernak, I., & Noble-Haeusslein, L. J. (2010). Traumatic of cognitive effort during sentence processing: Pupillo-
brain injury: An overview of pathobiology with metric evidence from people with and without aphasia.
emphasis on military populations. Journal of Cerebral Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research.
Blood Flow & Metabolism, 30(2), 255–266. https://doi​ Chaumet, G., Quera-Salva, M.-A., MacLeod, A., Hartley,
.org/10.1038/jcbfm.2009.203 S., Taillard, J., Sagaspe, P., & Philip, P. (2008). Is there
Chabon, S. S., & Cohn, E. R. (2011). The communication dis- a link between alertness and fatigue in patients with
orders casebook: Learning by example. Pearson. traumatic brain injury? Neurology, 71(20), 1609–1613.
Chabon, S., Morris, J., & Lemoncello, R. (2011). Ethical https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000334753.49193.48
deliberation: A foundation for evidence-based practice. Chen, C., Parsons, M. W., Clapham, M., Oldmeadow, C.,
Seminars in Speech and Language, 32(4), 298–308. https:// Levi, C. R., Lin, L., . . . Bivard, A. (2017). Influence of
doi.org/10.1055/s-0031-1292755 penumbral reperfusion on clinical outcome depends on
Chahda, L., Carey, L.B., Mathisen, B.A., & Threats, T. baseline ischemic core volume. Stroke, 48(10), 2739–2745.
(2021). Speech-language pathologists and adult palli- https://doi.org/10.1161/STROKEAHA.117.018587
520  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Cherney, L., Oehring, A., Whipple, K., & Rubenstein, T. Cherney, L. R., Lee, J. B., Kim, K.-Y. A., & van Vuuren,
(2011). “Waiting on the words”: Procedures and out- S. (2021). Web-based Oral Reading for Language in
comes of a drama class for individuals with aphasia. Aphasia (Web ORLA®): A pilot randomized control
Seminars in Speech and Language, 32(3), 229–242. https:// trial. Clinical Rehabilitation, 35(7), 976–987. https://doi​
doi.org/10.1055/s-0031-1286177 .org/10.1177/0269215520988475
Cherney, L. R. (1995). Efficacy of oral reading in the treat- Cherney, L. R., Merbitz, C. T., & Grip, J. C. (1986). Efficacy
ment of two patients with chronic Broca’s aphasia. Top- of oral reading in aphasia treatment outcome. Rehabili-
ics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 2(1), 57–67. https://doi.org/ tation Literature, 47(5–6), 112–118.
10.1080/10749357.1995.11754055 Cherney, L. R., Patterson, J. P., Raymer, A., Frymark, T., &
Cherney, L. R. (1998). Pragmatics and discourse: An intro- Schooling, T. (2008). Evidence-based systematic review:
duction. In L. R. Cherney, C. A. Coelho, & B. B. Shadden Effects of intensity of treatment and constraint-​induced
(Eds.), Analyzing discourse in communicatively impaired language therapy for individuals with stroke-​induced
adults (pp. 1–8). Aspen. aphasia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Re-
Cherney, L. R. (2004). Aphasia, alexia, and oral reading. search, 51(5), 1282–1299. https://doi.org/​10.1044/​1092-
Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 11(1), 22–36. https://doi. 43​88​(2008/07-0206)
org/10.1310/VUPX-WDX7-J1EU-00TB Cherney, L. R., & van Vuuren, S. (2012). Telerehabilitation,
Cherney, L. R. (2010a). Oral reading for language in apha- virtual therapists, and acquired neurologic speech and
sia: Impact of aphasia severity on cross-modal out- language disorders. Seminars in Speech and Language,
comes in chronic nonfluent aphasia. Seminars in Speech 33(3), 243–257. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0032-1320044
and Language, 31(1), 42–51. https://doi.org/​10.1055/​ Cherry, K. E., & Simmons-D’Gerolamo, S. S. (2005). Long-
s-0029-1244952 term effectiveness of spaced retrieval memory training
Cherney, L. R. (2010b). Oral reading for language in for older adults with probable Alzheimer’s disease.
aphasia (ORLA): Evaluating the efficacy of computer-​ Experimental Aging Research, 31(3), 261–289. https://doi​
delivered therapy in chronic nonfluent aphasia. Top- .org/10.1080/03610730590948186
ics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 17(6), 423–431. https://doi​ Chiou, H. S., & Allison, A. H. (2020). Semantic Dementia.
.org/10.1310/tsr1706-423 In R. L. Utianski (Ed.), Primary progressive aphasia and
Cherney, L. R. (2012). Aphasia treatment: Intensity, dose other frontotemporal dementias: Diagnosis and treatment
parameters, and script training. International Journal of of associated communication disorders (pp. 45–75). Plural
Speech-Language Pathology, 14(5), 424–431. https://doi​ Publishing.
.org/10.3109/17549507.2012.686629 Choe, M. C., Gregory, A. J., & Haegerich, T. M. (2018).
Cherney, L. R., Coelho, C. A., & Shadden, B. B. (1998). What pediatricians need to know about the CDC
Analyzing discourse in communicatively impaired adults. guideline on the diagnosis and management of mTBI.
Aspen. Frontiers in Pediatrics, 6, 249. https://doi.org/10.3389/
Cherney, L. R., Gardner, P., Logemann, J. A., Newman, L. fped.2018.00249
A., O’Neil-Pirozzi, T., Roth, C. R., . . . Disorders Clin- Chomsky, N. (1986). Knowledge of language: Its nature, ori-
ical Trails Research Group. (2010). The role of speech-​ gins, and use. Praeger.
language pathology and audiology in the optimal Cho-Reyes, S., & Thompson, C. K. (2012). Verb and sen-
management of the service member returning from tence production and comprehension in aphasia: North-
Iraq or Afghanistan with a blast-related head injury: western Assessment of Verbs and Sentences (NAVS).
Position of the Communication Sciences and Disorders Aphasiology, 26(10), 1250–1277. https://doi.org/10.1080/​
Clinical Trials Research Group. Journal of Head Trauma 02687038.2012.693584
Rehabilitation, 25(3), 219–224. https://doi.org/10.1097/ Christensen, H., Anstey, K. J., Leach, L. S., & Mackin-
HTR.0b013e3181dc82c1 non, A. J. (2008). Intelligence, education, and the brain
Cherney, L. R., Halper, A. S., Holland, A. L., & Cole, R. reserve hypothesis. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse
(2008). Computerized script training for aphasia: Pre- (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (pp. 133–188).
liminary results. American Journal of Speech-Language Psychology Press.
Pathology, 17(1), 19–34. https://doi.org/10.1044/​1058-​ Chung, J., & Lai, C. (2009). Snoezelen for dementia. Cochrane
0360(2008/003) Database of Systematic Reviews. https://doi​.org/​10​.1002/​
Cherney, L. R., Kaye, R. C., & van Vuuren, S. (2014). Acqui- 14651858.CD003152
sition and maintenance of scripts in aphasia: A com- Cicerone, K. D., Dahlberg, C., Malec, J. F., Langenbahn, D.
parison of two cuing conditions. American Journal of M., Felicetti, T., Kneipp, S., . . . Catanese, J. (2005). Evi-
Speech-Language Pathology, 23(2), S343-360. https://doi​ dence-based cognitive rehabilitation: Updated review
.org/10.1044/2014_AJSLP-13-0097 of the literature from 1998 through 2002. Archives of
521
References  

Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 86(8), 1681–1692. Coelho, C. A., Liles, B. Z., & Duffy, R. J. (1995). Impair-
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2005.03.024 ments of discourse abilities and executive functions in
Cifu, D. X., Cohen, S. I., Lew, H. L., Jaffee, M., & Sigford, traumatically brain-injured adults. Brain Injury, 9(5),
B. (2010). The history and evolution of traumatic brain 471–477. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699059509008206
injury rehabilitation in military service members and Coelho, C. A., McHugh, R. E., & Boyle, M. (2000). Seman-
veterans. American Journal of Physical Medicine & Reha- tic feature analysis as a treatment for aphasic dysno-
bilitation, 89(8), 688–694. https://doi.org/10.1097/PHM​ mia: A replication. Aphasiology, 14(2), 133–142. https://
.0b013e3181e722ad doi.org/10.1080/026870300401513
Clark, W., Mortensen, L., & Christie, J. (1986). Mount Coelho, C., Ylvisaker, M., & Turkstra, L. S. (2005). Nonstan-
Wilga High Level Language Test. Mt. Wilga Rehabilita- dardized assessment approaches for individuals with
tion Centre. traumatic brain injuries. Seminars in Speech and Language,
Clausen, N. S., & Beeson, P. M. (2003). Conversational 26(4), 223–241. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2005-922102
use of writing in severe aphasia: A group treatment Coelho, C., Youse, K., & Le, K. (2002). Conversational dis-
approach. Aphasiology, 17(6/7), 625. https://doi.org/​ course in closed-head-injured and non-brain-injured
10.1080/02687030344000003 adults. Aphasiology, 16(4–6), 659–672. https://doi.org/​
Cobley, C. S., Thomas, S. A., Lincoln, N. B., & Walker, M. F. 10.1080/02687030244000275
(2012). The assessment of low mood in stroke patients Cohen, L., Remy, P., Leroy, A., Geny, C., & Degos, J. D.
with aphasia: Reliability and validity of the 10-item (1991). Minor hemisphere syndrome following a left
hospital version of the stroke aphasic depression ques- hemispheric lesion in a right handed patient. Journal of
tionnaire (SADQH-10). Clinical Rehabilitation, 26(4), Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 54(9), 842–843.
372–381. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269215511422388 https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.54.9.842
Code, C. (2012). Apportioning time for aphasia rehabilita- Cohn, E. R. (2012). Tele-ethics in telepractice for commu-
tion. Aphasiology, 26(5), 729–735. https://doi.org/10.10 nication disorders. Perspectives on Telepractice, 2, 3–15.
80/02687038.2012.676892 https://doi.org/10.1044/tele2.1.3
Code, C., Mackie, N., Armstrong, E., Stiegler, L., Arm- Cohn, E. R., Brannon, J. A., & Cason, J. (2011). Resolving
strong, J., Bushby, E., & Webber, A. (2001). The public barriers to licensure portability for telerehabilitation
awareness of aphasia: An international survey. Interna- professionals. International Journal of Telerehabilitation,
tional Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 36, 3(2), 31–34. https://doi.org/10.5195/ijt.2011.6078
1–6. https://doi.org/10.3109/13682820109177849 Cohn, E. R., & Cason, J. (2016). Telepractice, telehealth,
Code, C., Müller, D. J., & Herrmann, M. (1999). Percep- and telemedicine: Acquiring knowledge from other
tions of psychosocial adjustment to aphasia: Applica- disciplines. Perspectives of the ASHA Special Interest
tions of the Code-Müller protocols. Seminars in Speech Groups, 1(18), 19–29. https://doi.org/10.1044/persp1​
and Language, 20(1), 51–62; quiz 63. https://doi.org/​ .SIG​18.19
10.1055/s-2008-1064008 Cohn, E. R., & Watzlaf, V. J. M. (2011). Privacy and Inter-
Code, C., Papathanasiou, I., Rubio-Bruno, S., Cabana, net-based telepractice. Perspectives on Telepractice, 1,
M., Villanueva, M., Haaland-Johansen, L., . . . Robert, 26–37. https://doi.org/10.1044/tele1.1.26
A. (2016). International patterns of the public aware- Coker, L. H., & Shumaker, S. A. (2003). Type 2 diabetes
ness of aphasia. International Journal of Language & mellitus and cognition: An understudied issue in wom-
Communication Disorders, 51(3), 276–284. https://doi​ en’s health. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 54(2), 129–
.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12204 139. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(02)00523-8
Code, C., & Petheram, B. (2011). Delivering for aphasia. Colaço, D., Mineiro, A., Leal, G., & Castro-Caldas, A.
International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 13(1), (2010). Revisiting “The influence of literacy in parapha-
3–10. https://doi.org/10.3109/17549507.2010.520090 sias of aphasic speakers.” Clinical Linguistics & Phonet-
Coelho, C. A., Duffy, J. R., & Sinotte, M. P. (2008). Schuell’s ics, 24(11), 890–905. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206​
stimulation approach to rehabilitation. In R. Chapey .2010.511406
(Ed.), Language intervention strategies in aphasia and Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-​
related neurogenic communication disorders (5th ed., pp. activation theory of semantic processing. Psycholog-
403–​449). Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. ical Review, 82(6), 407–428. https://doi.org/10.1037/​
Coelho, C. A., Grela, B., Corso, M., Gamble, A., & Feinn, 0033-295X.82.6.407
R. (2005). Microlinguistic deficits in the narrative dis- Collins, M. (1986). Diagnosis and treatment of global aphasia.
course of adults with traumatic brain injury. Brain College-Hill Press.
Injury, 19(13), 1139–1145. https://doi.org/10.1080/​0269​ Comité Permanent de Liaison des Orthophonistes-​
9050500110678 Logopèdes de L’Union Européenne. (2007). Revision
522  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

of the minimum standards for education. https://cplol​.eu/​ Cornis-Pop, M., Mashima, P., Roth, C., Maclennan, D.,
images/Documents/education/Revised_Min_Stan​ Picon, L., Hammond, C., . . . Frank, E. (2012). Guest
dards_2007_la.pdf editorial: Cognitive-communication rehabilitation for
Commission on Social Determinants of Health (2008). combat-related mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of
Closing the gap in a generation: Health equity through action Rehabilitation Research and Development, 49, xi–xxxii.
on the social determinants of health. WHO. https://www​ https://doi.org/10.1682/JRRD.2012.03.0048
.who.int/publications/i/item/WHO-IER-CSDH-08.1 Corwin, A. I. (2018). Overcoming elderspeak: A qualita-
Conklyn, D., Novak, E., Boissy, A., Bethoux, F., & Chemali, tive study of three alternatives. The Gerontologist, 58(4),
K. (2012). The effects of modified melodic intonation 724–729. https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnx009
therapy on nonfluent aphasia: A pilot study. Journal of Côté, H., Payer, M., Giroux, F., & Joanette, Y. (2007).
Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 55(5), 1463–1471. Towards a description of clinical communication
https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0105) impairment profiles following right-hemisphere dam-
Conley, A., & Coelho, C. A. (2003). Treatment of word age. Aphasiology, 21(6–8), 739–749. https://doi.org/​
retrieval impairment in chronic Broca’s aphasia. Apha- 10.1080/02687030701192331
siology, 17(3), 407–428. https://doi.org/10.1080/​729​ Côté, I., & Lafance, R. (2018). Le Théâtre Aphasique: Dossier
255460 de Presse. https://theatreaphasique.org/medias/
Conlon, C. P., & McNeil, M. K. (1989). The efficacy of treat- Coufal, K., Parham, D., Jakubowitz, M., Howell, C., &
ment for two globally aphasic adults using visual action Reyes, J. (2018). Comparing traditional service deliv-
therapy (Vol. 19, pp. 185–195). Pro-Ed. https://aphasi​ ery and telepractice for speech sound production
ology.pitt.edu/archive/00000114/ using a functional outcome measure. American Journal
Connolly, G. K. (1998). Legibility and readability of small of Speech-Language Pathology, 27(1), 82–90. https://doi​
print: Effects of font, observer age and spatial vision. Univer- .org/10.1044/2017_AJSLP-16-0070
sity of Calgary. https://prism.ucalgary.ca/bitstream/ Courtney, A. C., & Courtney, M. W. (2009). Athoracic
handle/1880/26040/31338Connolly.pdf​ ? sequence​ mechanism of mild traumatic brain injury due to
=1&isAllowed=y blast pressure waves. Medical Hypotheses, 72(1), 76–83.
Connor, L. T., Obler, L. K., Tocco, M., Fitzpatrick, P. M., https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2008.08.015
& Albert, M. L. (2001). Effect of socioeconomic status Covey, S. R. (1989). The 7 habits of highly effective people:
on aphasia severity and recovery. Brain and Language, Restoring the character ethic. Simon & Schuster.
78(2), 254–257. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.2001.2459 Covey, S. R. (2013). The 7 habits of highly effective people:
Connor, L. T., Spiro, A., Obler, L. K., & Albert, M. L. Powerful lessons in personal change. Simon & Schuster.
(2004). Change in object naming ability during adult- Cowl, A. L., & Gaugler, J. E. (2014). Efficacy of creative
hood. Journal of Gerontology, 59(5), 203–209. https://doi​ arts therapy in treatment of Alzheimer’s disease and
.org/10.1093/geronb/59.5.P203 dementia: A systematic literature review. Activities,
Conroy, P. J., Snell, C., Sage, K. E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. Adaptation & Aging, 38(4), 281–331. https://doi.org/10​
(2012). Using phonemic cueing of spontaneous naming .1080/01924788.2014.966547
to predict item responsiveness to therapy for anomia Crary, M. A., Wertz, R. T., & Deal, J. L. (1992). Classifying
in aphasia. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilita- aphasias: Cluster analysis of Western Aphasia Battery
tion, 93(1, Suppl.), S53–S60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j​ and Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination results.
.apmr.2011.07.205 Aphasiology, 6(1), 29–36. https://doi.org/10.1080/​
Cooke, S. F., & Bliss, T. V. P. (2005). Long-term potentiation 02687039208248575
and cognitive drug discovery. Current Opinion in Inves- Craver, C. F., & Small, S. L. (1997). Subcortical aphasia and
tigational Drugs (London, England: 2000), 6(1), 25–34. the problem of attributing functional responsibility to
Coppens, P., Parente, M., & Lecours, A. (1998). Aphasia parts of distributed brain processes. Brain and Language,
in illiterate individuals. In P. Coppens, Y. Lebrun, & A. 58(3), 427–435. https://doi.org/10.1006/ brln.1997.1809
Basso (Eds.), Aphasia in atypical populations (pp. 175– Crenshaw, K., Harris, L. C., HoSang, D., & Lipsitz, G.
202). Psychology Press. (Eds.). (2019). Seeing race again: Countering colorblindness
Corless, I. B., Michel, T. H., Nicholas, M., Jameson, D., across the disciplines. University of California Press.
Purtilo, R., & Dirkes, A. M. A. (2009). Educating health Crielesi, M., Roche, L., Monopoli, G., Yeates, G. N., &
professions students about the issues involved in Monte, S. (2019). Mindfulness interventions for peo-
communicating effectively: A novel approach. Journal ple with aphasia — case evidence from individual and
of Nursing Education, 48(7), 367–373. https://doi.org/​ group therapy formats. In Psychotherapy and Aphasia.
10.3928/01484834-20090615-03 Routledge.
523
References  

Croot, K. (2002). Diagnosis of AOS: Definition and criteria. 28(1 Suppl.), 293–320. https://doi.org/10.1044/​2018​
Seminars in Speech and Language, 23(4), 267–280. https:// _AJSLP-​17-0166
doi.org/10.1055/s-2002-35800 Dalton, S. G. H., Shultz, C., Henry, M. L., Hillis, A. E., &
Cross, K. P. (1981). Adults as learners. Jossey-Bass. Richardson, J. D. (2018). Describing phonological para-
Crosson, B., McGregor, K., Gopinath, K. S., Conway, T. phasias in three variants of primary progressive apha-
W., Benjamin, M., Chang, Y.-L., & White, K. D. (2007). sia. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 27(1S),
Functional MRI of language in aphasia: A review of the 336–349. https://doi.org/10.1044/2017_AJSLP-16-0210
literature and the methodological challenges. Neuropsy- Damasio, H. (2008). Neural basis of language disorders. In
chology Review, 17(2), 157–177. https://doi.org/10.1007/ R. Chapey (Ed.), Language intervention strategies in adult
s11065-007-9024-z and related neurogenic communication disorders (5th ed.,
Crottaz-Herbette, S., Fornari, E., Notter, M. P., Bind- pp. 20–41). Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
schaedler, C., Manzoni, L., & Clarke, S. (2017). Reshap- Darley, F. L. (1982). Aphasia. W. B. Saunders.
ing the brain after stroke: The effect of prismatic Daumüller, M., & Goldenberg, G. (2010). Therapy to
adaptation in patients with right brain damage. Neuro​ improve gestural expression in aphasia: A controlled
psychologia, 104, 54–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro​ clinical trial. Clinical Rehabilitation, 24(1), 55–65. https://
psychologia.2017.08.005 doi.org/10.1177/0269215509343327
Cruice, M., Isaksen, J., Randrup-Jensen, L., Eggers Viberg, Davidson, B., Worrall, L., & Hickson, L. (2003). Identifying
M., & ten Kate, O. (2015). Practitioners’ perspectives the communication activities of older people with apha-
on quality of life in aphasia rehabilitation in Denmark. sia: Evidence from naturalistic observation. Aphasiol-
Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 67(3), 131–144. https:// ogy, 17(3), 243–264. https://doi.org/10.1080/729255457
doi.org/10.1159/000437384 Davie, G. L., Hutcheson, K. A., Barringer, D. A., Weinbers,
Cruice, M., Worrall, L., & Hickson, L. (2005). Personal J. S., & Lewin, J. S. (2009). Aphasia in patients after
factors, communication and vision predict social par- brain tumor resection. Aphasiology, 23(9), 1196–1206.
ticipation in older adults. Advances in Speech Language https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030802436900
Pathology, 7(4), 220–232. https://doi.org/10.1080/​1441​ Davis, G. (1986). Pragmatics and treatment. In R. Chapey
7040500337088 (Ed.), Language intervention strategies in adult aphasia
Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., Kwan, V. S. Y., Glick, P., (2nd ed., pp. 251–265). Williams & Wilkins.
Demoulin, S., Leyens, J.-P., . . . Ziegler, R. (2009). Ste- Davis, G. A. (1980). A critical look at PACE therapy [Clini-
reotype content model across cultures: Towards uni- cal aphasiology paper]. https://aphasiology.pitt.edu/
versal similarities and some differences. BJSO British archive/00000567/
Journal of Social Psychology, 48(1), 1–33. https://doi​ Davis, G. A. (2005). PACE revisited. Aphasiology, 19(1),
.org/10.1348/014466608X314935 21–38. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030444000598
Cummings, L. (2019). On making a sandwich: Procedural Davis, G. A., & Wilcox, M. J. (1985). Adult aphasia rehabili-
discourse in adults with right-hemisphere damage. In tation: Applied pragmatics. College-Hill Press.
A. Capone, M. Carapezza, & F. Lo Piparo (Eds.), Fur- Davis, L. A., & Stanton, S. T. (2005). Semantic feature anal-
ther advances in pragmatics and philosophy: Part 2, Theories ysis as a functional therapy tool. Contemporary Issues in
and applications (Vol. 20, pp. 331–355). Springer Interna- Communication Sciences and Disorders, 32, 85–92. https://
tional. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-00973-1_19 www.asha.org/uploadedFiles/asha/publications/
Cutter, M., & Polovoy, C. (2014). Under pressure. The cicsd/2005FSemantic\FeatureAnalysis.pdf
ASHA Leader, 19(6), 36–44. https://doi.org/10.1044/ de Aguiar, V., Paolazzi, C. L., & Miceli, G. (2015). tDCS
leader.FTR1.19062014.36 in post-stroke aphasia: The role of stimulation param-
Dabul, B. (2000). Apraxia Battery for Adults (ABA-2). Pro-Ed. eters, behavioral treatment and patient characteris-
Dahmen, N. S., & Cozma, R. (2009). Media takes: On aging. tics. Cortex, 63, 296–316. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
International Longevity Center-USA. cortex.2014.08.015
Dalton, S., Kim, H., Richardson, J., & Wright, H. (2020). Deal, M. (2003). Disabled people’s attitudes toward
A compendium of core lexicon checklists. Seminars other impairment groups: A hierarchy of impair-
in Speech and Language, 41, 45–60. https://doi.org/​ ments. Disability & Society, 18(7), 897–910. https://doi.
10.1055/s-0039-3400972 org/10.1080/0968759032000127317
Dalton, S. G. H., & Richardson, J. D. (2019). A large-scale de Boissezon, X., Démonet, J.-F., Puel, M., Marie, N., Rabo-
comparison of main concept production between yeau, G., Albucher, J.-F., . . . Cardebat, D. (2005). Subcortical
persons with aphasia and persons without brain aphasia: A longitudinal PET study. Stroke, 36(7), 1467–1473.
injury. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.0000169947.08972.4f
524  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

de Boissezon, X., Peran, P., de Boysson, C., & Démonet, de Riesthal, M., & Diehl, S. (2017). Conceptual, method-
J. (2007). Pharmacotherapy of aphasia: Myth or real- ological, and clinical considerations for a core outcome
ity? Brain and Language, 102(1), 114–125. https://doi​ set for discourse. Aphasiology, 32, 1–3. https://doi.org/
.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2006.07.004 10.1080/02687038.2017.1398805
Decker, S. E., & Martino, S. (2013). Unintended effects of Department of Veterans Affairs. (2009). VA/DoD clinical
training on clinicians’ interest, confidence, and com- practice guideline for management of concussion/
mitment in using motivational interviewing. Drug and mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of Rehabilitation
Alcohol Dependence, 132(3), 681–687. https://doi​.org/​ Research and Development, 46(6), CP1–68. https://doi​
10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.04.022 .org/10.1682/JRRD.2009.06.0076
DeDe, G. (2013). Effects of verb bias and syntactic ambi- Depp, C. A., & Jeste, D. V. (2006). Definitions and predic-
guity on reading in people with aphasia. Aphasiology, tors of successful aging: A comprehensive review of
27(10–12), 1408–1425. https://doi.org/10.1080/026870 larger quantitative studies. American Journal of Geriat-
38.2013.843151 ric Psychiatry, 14(1), 6–20. https://doi.org/10.1097/01​
DeKosky, S. T., Williamson, J. D., Fitzpatrick, A. L., Kro- .JGP.0000192501.03069.bc
nmal, R. A., Ives, D. G., Saxton, J. A., & Furberg, C. Dial, H. R., Hinshelwood, H. A., Grasso, S. M., Hubbard,
D. (2008). Ginkgo biloba for prevention of dementia: H. I., Gorno-Tempini, M.-L., & Henry, M. L. (2019).
A randomized controlled trial. JAMA, 300(19), 2253– Investigating the utility of teletherapy in individuals
2262. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2008.683 with primary progressive aphasia. Clinical Interventions
D’Elia, L. F., Satz, P., Uchiyama, C. L., & White, T. (1996). in Aging, 14, 453–471. https://doi.org/10.2147/CIA
Color Trails Test: Professional manual. Psychological Assess- .S178878
ment Resources. Dickey, M. W., Choy, J. J., & Thompson, C. K. (2007).
Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2001). Delis-Ka- Real-time comprehension of wh-movement in apha-
plan executive function system. Psychological Corp. sia: Evidence from eyetracking while listening. Brain
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A. (2000). and Language, 100(1), 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
California Verbal Learning Test (2nd ed.). Psychological bandl.2006.06.004
Corporation. Dickey, M. W., & Thompson, C. K. (2007). The relation
Dell, G. S., & O’Seaghdha, P. G. (1992). Stages of lexical between syntactic and morphological recovery in
access in language production. Cognition, 42(1–3), 1–3. agrammatic aphasia: A case study. Aphasiology, 21(6–8),
https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(92)90046-K 604–616. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030701192059
Dell, G. S., Schwartz, M. F., Nozari, N., Faseyitan, O., & Dickinson, J., Friary, P., & McCann, C. M. (2017). The influ-
Branch Coslett, H. (2013). Voxel-based lesion-param- ence of mindfulness meditation on communication and
eter mapping: Identifying the neural correlates of a anxiety: A case study of a person with aphasia. Aphasi-
computational model of word production. COGNIT ology, 31(9), 1044–1058. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687
Cognition, 128(3), 380–396. https://doi.org/10.1016/j​ 038.2016.1234582
.cognition.2013.05.007 Dietz, A., & Boyle, M. (2018). Discourse measurement in
Demorest, M. E., & Erdman, S. A. (1986). Scale compo- aphasia research: Have we reached the tipping point?
sition and item analysis of the communication profile Aphasiology, 32(4), 459–464. https://doi.org/10.1080/02
for the hearing impaired. Journal of Speech and Hear- 687038.2017.1398803
ing Research, 29(4), 515–535. https://doi.org/10.1044/ Dietz, A., Duncan, E. S., Bislick, L., Stegman, S., Collins, J.,
jshr.2904.535 Mamlekar, C., . . . McCarthy, M. J. (2020). Yoga as ther-
Dennis, N. A., & Cabeza, R. (2008). Neuroimaging of apy for people with aphasia. Perspectives of the ASHA
healthy cognitive aging. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salt- Special Interest Groups, 5(4), 853–860. https://doi.org/​
house (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (3rd 10.1044/2020_PERSP-20-00028
ed., pp. 1–54). Psychology Press. Dietz, A., Thiessen, A., Griffith, J., Peterson, A., Sawyer,
DePalma, R. G., Burris, D. G., Champion, H. R., & Hodg- E., & Mckelvey, M. (2013). The renegotiation of social
son, M. J. (2005). Blast injuries. New England Journal of roles in chronic aphasia: Finding a voice through AAC.
Medicine, 352(13), 1335–1342. https://doi.org/10.1056/ Aphasiology, 27(3), 309–325. https://doi.org/10.1080/02​
NEJMra042083 687038.2012.725241
DePalma, R. G., & Hoffman, S. W. (2018). Combat blast Dijkstra, K., Bourgeois, M. S., Allen, R. S., & Burgio, L. D.
related traumatic brain injury (TBI): Decade of recog- (2004). Conversational coherence: Discourse analysis of
nition; promise of progress. Behavioural Brain Research, older adults with and without dementia. Journal of Neu-
340, 102–105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2016.08 rolinguistics, 17(4), 263–283. https://doi.org/10.1016/
.036 S0911-6044(03)00048-4
525
References  

DiLollo, A., & Favreau, C. (2010). Personcentered care Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52(2), 143–155. https://doi.
and speech and language therapy. Seminars in Speech org/10.1044/jshd.5202.143
and Language, 31(2), 90–97. https://doi.org/10.10​55/​ Dronkers, N. F. (1996). A new brain region for coordinat-
s-0030-1252110 ing speech articulation. Nature, 384(6605), 159–161.
Dilworth-Anderson, P., Pierre, G., & Hilliard, T. S. (2012). https://doi.org/10.1038/384159a0
Social justice, health disparities, and culture in the care of Dronkers, N. F., Plaisant, O., Iba-Zizen, M., & Cabanis,
the elderly. Journal of Law, Medicine & Ethics, 40(1), 26–32. E. (2007). Paul Broca’s historic cases: High resolution
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1748-720X.2012.00642.x MR imaging of the brains of Leborgne and Lelong.
Dlouhy, S., & Mitchell, P. (2015). Upcycling sheltered work- Brain, 130, 1432–1441. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/
shops: A revolutionary approach to transforming workshops awm042
into creative spaces. Swallow Press. Dronkers, N. F., Wilkins, D. P., Van Valin, R. D., Jr., Red-
Doherty, M., & Lay, C. (2019). Development of a train- fern, B. B., & Jaeger, J. J. (2004). Lesion analysis of the
ing program to increase student clinician competency brain areas involved in language comprehension.
when communicating with people with aphasia. Jour- Cognition, 92(1–2), 145–177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j​
nal of Occupational Therapy Education, 3. https://doi​ .cognition.2003.11.002
.org/​10.26681/jote.2019.030204 Drummond, S. S. (1993). Dysarthria Examination Battery.
Doolittle, G. C., Yaezel, A., Otto, F., & Clemens, C. (1998). Communication Skill Builders.
Hospice care using home-based telemedicine systems. Drummond, S. S. (2006). Neurogenic communication disor-
Journal of Telemedicine and Telecare, 4(Suppl. 1), 58–59. ders: Aphasia in cognitive-communication disorders (pp.
https://doi.org/10.1258/1357633981931470 200–213). Charles C. Thomas.
Douglas, J. M., O’Flaherty, C. A., & Snow, P. C. (2000). Mea- Duff, M. C., Mutlu, B., Byom, L., & Turkstra, L. S. (2012).
suring perception of communicative ability: The devel- Beyond utterances: Distributed cognition as a frame-
opment and evaluation of the La Trobe communication work for studying discourse in adults with acquired
questionnaire. Aphasiology, 14(3), 251–268. https://doi​ brain injury. Seminars in Speech and Langauge, 33(1),
.org/10.1080/026870300401469 44–54. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0031-1301162
Douglas, N., & Burshnic, V. (2019). Implementation sci- Duffau, H. (2005). The anatomo-functional connectiv-
ence: tackling the research to practice gap in com- ity of language revisited: New insights provided by
munication sciences and disorders. Perspectives of the electrostimulation and tractography. Neuropyschologia,
ASHA Special Interest Groups, 4, 1–5. https://doi​.org/​ 46, 927–934. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro​psycho​
10.1044/2018_PERS-ST-2018-0000 logia.2007.10.025
Douglas, N. F., & Smyth, D. (2021). Life participation for Duffy, J. R. (1974). Comparison of brain injured and non-
people with dementia. In A. L. Holland & R. J. Elman brain injured subjects on an objective test of manual apraxia
(Eds.), Neurogenic communication disorders and the life [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. University of
participation approach (pp. 159–180). Plural Publishing. Connecticut.
Dow, D., Dow-Richards, C., & Dow, D. M. (2013). Brain Duffy, J. R. (2013). Motor speech disorders: Substrates, dif-
attack: My journey of recovery from stroke and apha- ferential diagnosis, and management (3rd ed.). Elsevier
sia by David Dow. Speechless Publishing Group. Mosby.
Dow, M., Dow, D., & Sutton, M. (2017). Healing the broken Duffy, J. R. (2014). The values of board certification. Acad-
brain: Leading experts answer 100 questions about stroke emy of Neurologic Communication Disorders & Sciences
recovery. Hay House. Newsletter, 12(1), 2. https://www.ancds.org/assets/
Doyle, P., McNeil, M., & Hula, W. (2003). The burden of docs/Newsletter/ancds_spring_2014.pdf
stroke scale (BOSS): Validating patient-reported com- Duffy, J. R., Fossett, T. R. D., & Thomas, J. E. (2011). Clinical
munication difficulty and associated psychological practice in acute care hospital settings. In L. L. Lapointe
distress in stroke survivors. Aphasiology, 17(3), 291–304. (Ed.), Aphasia and related neurogenic language disorders
https://doi.org/10.1080/729255459 (4th ed.). Thieme.
Doyle, P., McNeil, M., Park, G., Goda, A., Rubenstein, Duffy, J. R., Werven, G. W., & Aronson, A. E. (1997). Tele-
E., Spencer, K., & Szwarc, L. (2000). Linguistic vali- medicine and the diagnosis of speech and language
dation of four parallel forms of a story retelling pro- disorders. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 72(12), 1116–1122.
cedure. Aphasiology, 14(5–6), 537–549. https://doi. https://doi.org/10.4065/72.12.1116
org/10.1080/026870300401306 Dunham, M. J., & Newhoff, M. (1979). Melodic intona-
Doyle, P. J., Goldstein, H., & Bourgeois, M. S. (1987). Exper- tion therapy: Rewriting the song. Clinical Aphasiology,
imental analysis of syntax training in Broca’s aphasia: 9, 286–294. https://aphasiology.pitt.edu/archive/​0000​
A generalization and social validation study. Journal of 0402/01/09-33.pdf
526  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Dunkle, R. E., & Hooper, C. R. (1983). Using language to Educational Testing Service. (2014). The Praxis series for test
help depressed elderly aphasic persons. Social Case- takers: Speech language pathology. https://www.ets.org/
work, 64, 539–545. praxis/prepare/materials/5330
Dunn, K. E. (2011). Cognition and aging: Primary and ter- Edwards-Gaither, L. (2018). Cultural considerations for
tiary aging factors. In M. A. Toner, B. B. Shadden, & M. telepractice: An introduction for speech-language
B. Gluth (Eds.), Aging and communication (2nd ed., pp. pathologists. Perspectives of the ASHA Special Interest
145–168). Pro-Ed. Groups, 3(18), 13–20. https://doi.org/10.1044/persp3.
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). Peabody Picture Vocabu- SIG18.13
lary Test-4 (PPVT-4) (4th ed.). Pearson Assessments. Edwards, S., & Bastiaanse, R. (2007). Assessment of apha-
Dyer, S. M., Harrison, S. L., Laver, K., Whitehead, C., sia in a multi-lingual world. In M. J. Ball & J. S. Damico
& Crotty, M. (2018). An overview of systematic (Eds.), Clinical aphasiology: Future directions (pp. 245–
reviews of pharmacological and non-pharmacological 258). Psychology Press.
interventions for the treatment of behavioral and psy- Effects of hypnosis and imagery training.pdf (n.d.).
chological symptoms of dementia. International Psy- Efstratiadou, E. A., Papathanasiou, I., Holland, R., Archonti,
chogeriatrics, 30(3), 295–309. https://doi.org/10.1017/ A., & Hilari, K. (2018). A systematic review of semantic
S1041610217002344 feature analysis therapy studies for aphasia. Journal of
Eadie, T., Yorkston, K. M., Klasner, E. R., Dudgeon, B. Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61(5), 1261–1278.
J., Deitz, J. C., Baylor, C. R., & Amtmann, D. (2006). https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-16-0330
Measuring communicative participation: A review of Eftedal, M., Kvaal, A. M., Ree, E., Øyeflaten, I., & Maeland,
self-report instruments in speech-language pathology. S. (2017). How do occupational rehabilitation clinicians
American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 15(4), approach participants on long-term sick leave in order
307–320. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2006/030) to facilitate return to work? A focus group study. BMC
Ebert, K. D., & Kohnert, K. (2010). Common factors in Health Services Research, 17(1), 744. https://doi.org/​
speech-language treatment: An exploratory study of 10.1186/s12913-017-2709-y
effective clinicians. Journal of Communication Disor- Ehlhardt, L. A., Sohlberg, M. M., Kennedy, M., Coelho,
ders, 43(2), 133–147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcom​dis​ C., Ylvisaker, M., Turkstra, L. S., & Yorkston, K. (2008).
.2009.12.002 Evidence-based practice guidelines for instructing indi-
Eccles, M. P., & Mittman, B. S. (2006). Welcome to Imple- viduals with neurogenic memory impairments: What
mentation Science. Implementation Science, 1(1), 1. have we learned in the past 20 years? Neuropsycholog-
https://doi.org/10.1186/1748-5908-1-1 ical Rehabilitation, 18(3), 300–342. https://doi.org/10​
Edgeworth, J. A., Robertson, I. H., & McMillan, T. M. .1080/09602010701733190
(1998). The Balloons Test. Thames Valley Test Co. Eikelenboom, P., Bate, C., Van Gool, W. A., Hoozemans,
Edmonds, L. A. (2016). A review of verb network strength- J. J. M., Rozemuller, J. M., Veerhuis, R., & Williams,
ening treatment: Theory, methods, results, and clinical A. (2002). Neuroinflammation in Alzheimer’s disease
implications. Topics in Language Disorders, 36(2), 123– and prion disease. Glial Physiology and Pathophysiology,
135. https://doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0000000000000088 40(2), 232–239. https://doi.org/10.1002/glia.10146
Edmonds, L. A., & Babb, M. (2011). Effect of verb network Elbourn, E., Kenny, B., Power, E., Honan, C., McDon-
strengthening treatment in moderate-to-severe aphasia. ald, S., Tate, R., . . . Togher, L. (2018). Discourse recov-
American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 20(2), 131– ery after severe traumatic brain injury: Exploring the first
145. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2011/10-0036) year. Brain Injury, 33(2), 143–159. https://www.tandfon​
Edmonds, L. A., Mammino, K., & Ojeda, J. (2014). Effect line.com/doi/epub/10.1080/02699052.2018.1539246
of verb network strengthening treatment (VNeST) in ?needAccess=true
persons with aphasia: Extension and replication of Elder, G. A., & Cristian, A. (2009). Blast-related mild trau-
previous findings. American Journal of Speech-Language matic brain injury: Mechanisms of injury and impact
Pathology, 23(2), S312–S329. https://doi.org/10.1044/​ on clinical care. Mount Sinai Journal of Medicine, 76(2),
2014_AJSLP-13-0098 111–118. https://doi.org/10.1002/msj.20098
Edmonds, L. A., Nadeau, S. E., & Kiran, S. (2009). Effect of Ellis, J. M., Ben, M. R., & Teshuva, K. (2017). Laughter yoga
verb network strengthening treatment (VNeST) on lex- activities for older people living in residential aged care
ical retrieval of content words in sentences in persons homes: A feasibility study. Australasian Journal on Age-
with aphasia. Aphasiology, 23(3), 402–424. https://doi ing, 36(3), E28–E31. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajag.12447
.org/10.1080/02687030802291339 Ellmo, W., Graser, J., Krchnavek, B., Hauck, K., & Cal-
Edmonds, L. J. (2005). Disabled people and development. abrese, D. (1995). Measure of cognitive-linguistic abilities
Asian Development Bank. (MCLA). Speech Bin.
527
References  

Elman, R. J. (2007a). Group treatment of neurogenic commu- Farias, D., Davis, C., & Harrington, G. (2006). Draw-
nication disorders: The expert clinician’s approach. Plural ing: Its contribution to naming in aphasia. Brain and
Publishing. Language, 97(1), 53–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j​
Elman, R. J. (2007b). The importance of aphasia group treat- .bandl.2005.07.074
ment for rebuilding community and health. Topics in Faroqi-Shah, Y., & Virion, C. R. (2009). Constraint-induced
Language Disorders, 4, 300. https://doi.org/10.1097/01​ language therapy for agrammatism: Role of gram-
.TLD​.0000299884.31864.99 maticality constraints. Aphasiology, 23(7/8), 977–988.
Elman, R. J. (2016). Aphasia centers and the life partici- https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030802642036
pation approach to aphasia: A paradigm shift. Topics Federmeier, K. D., Van Petten, C., Schwartz, T. J., & Kutas,
in Language Disorders, 36(2), 154–167. https://doi.org/​ M. (2003). Sounds, words, sentences: Age-related
10.1097/TLD.0000000000000087 changes across levels of language processing. Psychol-
Elman, R. J., & Bernstein-Ellis, E. (1999). The efficacy of ogy and Aging, 18(4), 858–872. https://doi.org/​10.1037/​
group communication treatment in adults with chronic 0882-7974​.18.4.858
aphasia. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, Fedorenko, E., Duncan, J., & Kanwisher, N. (2012). Lan-
42(2), 411–419. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4202.411 guage-selective and domain-general regions lie side by
Elman, R. J., Ogar, J., & Elman, S. H. (2000). Aphasia: Aware- side within Broca’s area. Current Biology, 22(21), 2059–
ness, advocacy, and activism. Aphasiology, 14(5/6), 455– 2062. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.011
459. https://doi.org/10.1080/026870300401234 Fedorenko, E., Fillmore, P., Smith, K., Bonilha, L., & Frid-
Enderby, P. (2012). How much therapy is enough? The riksson, J. (2015). The superior precentral gyrus of the
impossible question! International Journal of Speech-​ insula does not appear to be functionally specialized
Language Pathology, 14(5), 432–437. https://doi.org/10 for articulation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 113(7), 2376–
.3109/17549507.2012.686118 2382. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00214.2014
Enderby, P., & Petheram, B. (2002). Has aphasia therapy Feeney, M. P., & Hallowell, B. (2000). Practice and list
been swallowed up? Clinical Rehabilitation, 16(6), 604– effects on the synthetic sentence identification test
608. https://doi.org/10.1191/0269215502cr505oa in young and elderly listeners. Journal of Speech, Lan-
Enderby, P. M. (1983). Frenchay dysarthria assessment. College-​ guage,and Hearing Research, 43(5), 1160–1167. https://
Hill Press. doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4305.1160
Enderby, P. M., & John, A. (2015). Therapy outcome measures Fein, M., Bayley, C., Rising, K., & Beeson, P. M. (2020).
for rehabilitation professionals. J&R Press. A structured approach to train text messaging in an
Enderby, P. M., & Palmer, R. (2008). Frenchay dysarthria individual with aphasia. Aphasiology, 34(1), 102–118.
assessment (2nd ed.) (FDA2). Pro-Ed. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2018.1562150
Erdodi, L., & Roth, R. (2017). Low scores on BDAE Com- Fergadiotis, G., & Wright, H. H. (2011). Lexical diversity
plex Ideational Material are associated with invalid for adults with and without aphasia across discourse
performance in adults without aphasia. Applied Neu- elicitation tasks. Aphasiology, 25(11), 1414–1430. https://
ropsychology: Adult, 24(3), 264–274. https://doi.org/​ doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2011.603898
10.1080/​23279095.2016.1154856 Fergadiotis, G., Wright, H. H., & Green, S. B. (2015).
Escher, A. A., Amlani, A. M., Viani, A. M., & Berger, S. Psychometric evaluation of lexical diversity indices:
(2018). Occupational therapy in an intensive compre- Assessing length effects. Journal of Speech, Language,
hensive aphasia program: Performance and satisfac- and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 58(3), 840–852. https://
tion outcomes. American Journal of Occupational Ther- doi.org/10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-14-0280
apy, 72, 7203205110. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.2018 Ferguson, A., Duffield, G., & Worrall, L. (2010). Legal deci-
.026187 sion-making by people with aphasia: Critical incidents
EURES. (2021). Finland labor market. https://www.europe- for speech pathologists. International Journal of Language
anjobdays.eu/en/content/finland & Communication Disorders, 45(2), 244–258. https://doi.
Falchook, A. D., Heilman, K. M., Finney, G. R., Gonza- org/10.3109/13682820902936714
lez-Rothi, L. J., & Nadeau, S. E. (2014). Neuroplasticity, Ferguson, A., Worrall, L., McPhee, J., Buskell, R., Arm-
neurotransmitters and new directions for treatment of strong, E., & Togher, L. (2003). Testamentary capacity
anomia in Alzheimer’s disease. Aphasiology, 28(2), 219– and aphasia: A descriptive case report with implica-
235. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2013.793283 tions for clinical practice. Aphasiology, 17(10), 965–980.
Falconer, C., & Antonucci, S. M. (2012). Use of semantic https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030344000337
feature analysis in group discourse treatment for apha- Fernandes, F. D. M., de Andrade, C. R., BefiLopes, D.
sia: Extension and expansion. Aphasiology, 26(1), 64–82. M., Wertzner, H. F., & Limongi, S. C. (2010). Emerg-
https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2011.602390 ing issues concerning the education of speech and
528  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

language pathologists and audiologists in Brazil and (MMSE-2) standard kit. PAR, Psychological Assessment
South America. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 62(5), Resources.
223–227. https://doi.org/10.1159/000314784 Foster, A., O’Halloran, R., Rose, M., & Worrall, L. (2014).
Finestone, H., & Blackmer, J. (2007). Refusal to eat, capac- “Communication is taking a back seat”: Speech pathol-
ity, and ethics in stroke patients: A report of 3 cases. ogists’ perceptions of aphasia management in acute
Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 88(11), hospital settings. Aphasiology, 30(5), 585–608. https://
1474–1477. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2007.07.018 doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2014.985185
Finger, S., Tyler, K. L., & Boller, F. (2010). History of neurol- Foster, A., Worrall, L., Rose, M., & O’Halloran, R. (2015).
ogy (Vol. 95). Elsevier. “That doesn’t translate”: The role of evidence-based
Fink, L. D. (2003). A self-directed guide to designing courses practice in disempowering speech pathologists in acute
for significant learning. Jossey-Bass. aphasia management. International Journal of Language
Fink, R. B., Martin, N., Schwartz, M. F., Saffron, E. M., & and Communication Disorders, 50(4), 547–563. https://
Myers, J. L. (1992). Facilitation of verb retrieval skills doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12155
in aphasia: A comparison of two approaches. In M. L. Foundas, A. L. (2013). Limb apraxia: A disorder of goal-di-
Lemme (Ed.), Clinical aphasiology (Vol. 21, pp. 263–275). rected actions. In A. Chatterjee & B. Coslett (Eds.), The
Pro-Ed. roots of cognitive neuroscience: Behavioral neurology and
Fink, R. B., Schwartz, M. F., & Myers, J. L. (1998). Inves- neuropsychology (pp. 187–220). Oxford University Press.
tigations of the sentence query approach to mapping Fourie, R. J. (2009). Qualitative study of the therapeutic
therapy. Brain and Language, 65(1), 203–207. https://doi​ relationship in speech and language therapy: Per-
.org/10.1006/brln.1998.2011 spectives of adults with acquired communication and
Fink, R. B., Schwartz, M. F., Rochon, E., Myers, J. L., Soco- swallowing disorders. International Journal of Language
lof, G. S., & Bluestone, R. (1995). Syntax stimulation & Communication Disorders, 44(6), 979–999. https://doi​
revisited: An analysis of generalization of treatment .org/10.1080/13682820802535285
effects. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, Fox, L., Poulsen, S., Clark Bawden, K., & Packard, D.
4, 99–104. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360.0404.99 (2004). Critical elements and outcomes of a residential
Fischer, R. S., Alexander, M. P., Gabriel, C., Gould, E., & family-based intervention for aphasia caregivers. Apha-
Milione, J. (1991). Reversed lateralization of cognitive siology, 18(12), 1177–1199. https://doi.org/10.1080/​02​
functions in right handers. Brain, 114(Pt. 1A), 245–261. 687030444000525
Fiske, S. T. (2008). Social cognition and the normality of Franzen, M. D. (2003). Reliability and validity in neuropsycho-
prejudgment. In J. F. Dovidio, P. Glick, & L. A. Rudman logical assessment (3rd ed.). Kluwer Academic/Plenum.
(Eds.), On the nature of prejudice: Fifty years after Allport Frattali, C. M., Thompson, C. K., Holland, A. L., Wohl, C.,
(pp. 36–53). Blackwell. Wenck, C. J., Slater, S. C., & Paul, D. (2017). Functional
Flinker, A., & Knight, R. T. (2018). Broca’s area in com- assessment of communication skills for adults (ASHA FACS).
prehension and production, insights from intracra- American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.
nial studies in humans. Current Opinion in Behavioral Freckmann, A., Hines, M., & Lincoln, M. (2017). Clinicians’
Sciences, 21, 170–175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. perspectives of therapeutic alliance in face-to-face and
cobeha.2018.04.012 telepractice speech-language pathology sessions. Inter-
Flinker, A., Korzeniewska, A., Shestyuk, A. Y., Fra- national Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19(3), 287–
naszczuk, P. J., Dronkers, N. F., Knight, R. T., & Crone, N. 296. https://doi.org/10.1080/17549507.2017.1292547
E. (2015). Redefining the role of Broca’s area in speech. Freed, D., Celery, K., & Marshall, R. (2004). Effectiveness
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(9), of personalised and phonological cueing on long-term
2871–2875. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1414491112 naming performance by aphasic subjects: A clinical
Flynn, L., Cumberland, A., & Marshall, J. (2009). Public investigation. Aphasiology, 18(8), 743–757. https://doi​
knowledge about aphasia: A survey with compara- .org/​10.1080/02687030444000246
tive data. Aphasiology, 23(3), 393–401. https://doi.org/​ Freed, D. B., & Marshall, R. C. (1995). The effect of person-
10.1080/02687030701828942 alized cueing on long-term naming of realistic visual
Foerch, C., Hessen, A. S., Misselwitz, B., Sitzer, M., stimuli. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology,
Berger, K., Neumann-Haefelin, T., & Steinmetz, H. 4(4), 105. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360.0404.105
(2005). Difference in recognition of right and left hemi- Freed, D. B., Marshall, R. C., & Nippold, M. A. (1995). Com-
spheric stroke. Lancet, 366(9483), 392–393. https://do​ parison of personalized cueing and provided cueing on
i.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(05)67024-9 the facilitation of verbal labeling by aphasic subjects.
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., White, T., & Messer, M. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38(5), 1081–1090.
A. (2010). Mini-Mental State Examination (2nd ed.) https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3805.1081
529
References  

French, J., & Gronseth, G. (2008). Lost in a jungle of evi- Furnas, D. W., & Edmonds, L. A. (2014). The effect of com-
dence: We need a compass. Neurology, 71(20), 1634– puterised Verb Network Strengthening Treatment on
1638. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000336533. lexical retrieval in aphasia. Aphasiology, 28(4), 401-420.
Fridriksson, J., Bonilha, L., Baker, J. M., Moser, D., & Ror- https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2013.869304
den, C. (2010). Activity in preserved left hemisphere Gaddie, A., Kearns, K. P., & Yedor, K. (1991). A qualitative
regions predicts anomia severity in aphasia. Cerebral analysis of response elaboration training effects. Clin-
Cortex, 20(5), 1013–1019. https://doi.org/10.1093/cer​ ical Aphasiology, 19, 171–183. http://aphasiology.pitt
cor/​bhp160 .edu/113/
Fridriksson, J., den Ouden, D.-B., Hillis, A. E., Hickok, G., Gainotti, G. (2015). Contrasting opinions on the role of the
Rorden, C., Basilakos, A., . . . Bonilha, L. (2018). Anat- right hemisphere in the recovery of language: A critical
omy of aphasia revisited. Brain, 141(3), 848–862. https:// survey. Aphasiology, 29(9), 1020–1037. https://doi.org/
doi.org/10.1093/brain/awx363 10.1080/02687038.2015.1027170
Fridriksson, J., Fillmore, P., Guo, D., & Rorden, C. (2015). Galarneau, M. R., Woodruff, S. I., Dye, J. L., Mohrle, C.
Chronic Broca’s aphasia is caused by damage to Broca’s R., & Wade, A. L. (2008). Traumatic brain injury during
and Wernicke’s areas. Cerebral Cortex, 25(12), 4689–4696. operation Iraqi freedom: Findings from the United
https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu152 States Navy–Marine Corps Combat Trauma Registry.
Fridriksson, J., Holland, A. L., Beeson, P. M., & Morrow, L. Journal of Neurosurgery, 108(5), 950–957. https://doi​
A. (2005). Spaced retrieval treatment of anomia. Apha- .org/10.3171/JNS/2008/108/5/0950
siology, 19(2), 99–109. https://doi.org/10.1080/026870​ Galling, M. A., Goorah, N., Berthier, M. L., & Sage, K.
30444000660 (2014). A clinical study of the combined use of bro-
Fridriksson, J., Hubbard, H. I., & Hudspeth, S. G. (2012). mocriptine and speech and language therapy in the
Transcranial brain stimulation to treat aphasia: A clini- treatment of a person with aphasia. Aphasiology, 28(2),
cal perspective. Seminars in Speech and Language, 33(3), 171–187. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2013.838616
188–202. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0032-1320039 Ganti, L., Bodhit, A. N., Daneshvar, Y., Patel, P. S., Pul-
Fridriksson, J., Rorden, C., Elm, J., Sen, S., George, M. S., vino, C., Hatchitt, K., . . . Tyndall, J. A. (2013). Impact of
& Bonilha, L. (2018). Transcranial direct current stimu- helmet use in traumatic brain injuries associated with
lation vs sham stimulation to treat aphasia after stroke: recreational vehicles. Advances in Preventive Medicine,
A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Neurology, 75(12), 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/450195
1470. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaneurol.2018.2287 Ganzfried, E. S. (2018). Living with aphasia: Realities,
Friedman, E. M., & Ryff, C. D. (2012). Theoretical perspec- challenges, and opportunities. Perspectives of the ASHA
tives: A biopsychosocial approach to positive aging. In Special Interest Groups, 3(2), 68–79. https://doi.org/10.10​
S. K. Whitbourne & M. J. Sliwinski (Eds.), The Wiley-​ 44/persp3.SIG2.68
Blackwell handbook of adulthood and aging. Wiley. Gao, B., Jiang, S., Wang, X., & Chen, J. (2000). The role
Friedman, S. M., Mulhausen, P., Cleveland, M. L., Coll, of pre-injury IQ in the determination of intellectual
P. P., Daniel, K. M., Hayward, A. D., . . . White, H. K. impairment from traumatic head injury. Journal of Neu-
(2019). Healthy aging: American Geriatrics Society ropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 12(3), 385–388.
white paper executive summary. Journal of the American https://doi.org/10.1176/jnp.12.3.385
Geriatrics Society, 67(1), 17–20. Gardner, H., Zurif, E. B., Berry, T., & Backman, E. (1976).
Friedmann, N. (1994). Morphology in agrammatism: A disso- Visual communication in aphasia. Neuropsychologia,
ciation between tense and agreement [Unpublished mas- 14(3), 275–292. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932​(76)​
ter’s thesis]. Department of Cognitive Psychology, Tel 90023-3
Aviv University, Israel. Garrett, K. L., & Huth, C. (2002). The impact of graphic
Friedmann, N. (2001). Agrammatism and the psychologi- contextual information and instruction on the con-
cal reality of the syntactic tree. Journal of Psycholinguis- versational behaviours of a person with severe apha-
tic Research, 30, 71–90. https://doi.org/10.1023/​A:1005​ sia. Aphasiology, 16(4–6), 523–536. https://doi.org/​
256224207 10.1080/02687030244000149
Friedmann, N., & Grodzinsky, Y. (1997). Tense and agree- Garrett, K. L., & Kimelman, M. D. Z. (2000). AAC & apha-
ment in agrammatic production: Pruning the syntactic sia: Cognitive-linguistic considerations. In D. R. Beukel-
tree. Brain and Language, 56, 397–425. https://doi.org/​ man, K. M. Yorkston, & J. Reichle (Eds.), Augmentative
10.1006/brln.1997.1795 and alternative communication for adults with acquired neu-
Fritsch, G. T., & Hitzig, E. (1870). On the electrical excit- rologic disorders (pp. 339–374). Paul H. Brookes.
ability of the cerebrum. In G. Von Bonin (Trans. & Ed.), Garrett, K. L., & Lasker, J. P. (2013). Adults with severe
Some papers on the cerebral cortex. Charles C Thomas. aphasia and apraxia of speech. In D. Beukelman & P.
530  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Mirenda (Eds.), Augmentative and alternative communica- Anthrozoös, 28(2), 181–197. https://doi.org/10.1080/08
tion: Supporting children and adults with complex commu- 927936.2015.11435396
nication needs (pp. 404–446). Paul H. Brookes. Gill, S. K., & Leff, A. P. (2014). Dopaminergic therapy in
Gasparova, Z., Stara, V., & Stolc, S. (2014). Effect of anti- aphasia. Aphasiology, 28(2), 155–170. https://doi.org/10​
oxidants on functional recovery after in vitro-induced .1080/02687038.2013.802286
ischemia and long-term potentiation recorded in the Giza, C. C., Kutcher, J. S., Ashwal, S., Barth, J., Getchius, T.
pyramidal layer of the CA1 area of rat hippocampus. S. D., Gioia, G. A., . . . Zafonte, R. (2013). Summary of
General Physiology and Biophysics, 33(1), 43–52. evidence-based guideline update: Evaluation and man-
Gatehouse, S., & Noble, W. (2004). The speech, spatial agement of concussion in sports. Neurology, 80(24), 2250–
and qualities of hearing scale (SSQ). International Jour- 2257. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e318​28​d57dd
nal of Audiology, 43(2), 85–99. https://doi.org/​10.1080/​ GLADD. (2012). Talking about: Overall approaches for LGBT
14992020400050014 equality. https://www.lgbtmap.org/file/talking-about-
Gawande, A. (2014). Being mortal: Medicine and what mat- overall-approaches-for-lgbt-issues.pdf
ters in the end. Metropolitan Books. GLADD. (2014). An ally’s guide to terminology: Talking about
Gearing, R. E., El-Bassel, N., Ghesquiere, A., Baldwin, LGBT people and equality. https://www.glaad.org/sites/
S., Gillies, J., & Ngeow, E. (2011). Major ingredients of default/files/allys-guide-to-terminology_1.pdf
fidelity: A review and scientific guide to improving GLADD. (2016). GLADD media reference guide. https://
quality of intervention research implementation. Clin- www.glaad.org/sites/default/files/GLAAD-Media-
ical Psychology Review, 31(1), 79–88. https://doi.org/​ Reference-Guide-Tenth-Edition.pdf
10.1016/j.cpr.2010.09.007 Glass, A. V., Gazzaniga, M. S., & Premack, D. (1973). Arti-
George, M. S., Mercer, J. S., Walker, R., & Manly, T. ficial language training in global aphasics. Neuropsycho-
(2008). A demonstration of endogenous modulation logia, 11(1), 95–103. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932​
of unilateral spatial neglect: The impact of apparent (73)90069-9
time-pressure on spatial bias. Journal of the International Gleason, J. B., Goodglass, H., Green, E., Ackerman, N., &
Neuropsychological Society, 14(1), 33–41. https://doi​ Hyde, M. R. (1975). The retrieval of syntax in Broca’s
.org/10.1017/S135561770808003X aphasia. Brain and Language, 2, 451–471. https://doi​
Georgeadis, A. C., Brennan, D. M., Barker, L. M., & Baron, .org/10.1016/S0093-934X(75)80083-6
C. R. (2004). Telerehabilitation and its effect on story Glisky, E. L. (2007). Changes in cognitive function in
retelling by adults with neurogenic communication dis- human aging. In D. R. Riddle (Ed.), Brain aging: Models,
orders. Aphasiology, 18(5–7), 639–652. https://pubmed​ methods, and mechanisms (pp. 3–20). CRC Press.
.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/27303484/ Global Ministerial Forum on Research for Health. (2008,
Georgiadis, A. L., Al-Kawi, A., Janjua, N., Kirmani, J. F., November 17–19). The Bamako call to action on
Ezzeddine, M. A., & Qureshi, A. (2015). Cerebral angi- research for health: Straightening research for health,
ography can demonstrate changes in collateral flow development, and equity. The Lancet, 372(9653), 1855.
during induced hypertension. Radiology Case Reports, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(08)61789-4
2(4), 37. https://doi.org/10.2484/rcr.2007.v2i4.37 Goda, S. (1962). Spontaneous speech, a primary source of
Gerber, S., & Gurland, G. B. (1989). Applied pragmatics therapy material. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders,
in the assessment of aphasia. Seminars in Speech and 27(2), 190–192. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.2702.190
Language, 10(4), 263–281. https://doi.org/10.1055/​s-20​ Gold, M., VanDam, D., & Silliman, E. R. (2000). An open-​
08-1064268 label trial of bromocriptine in nonfluent aphasia: A qual​
Gerdner, L. A., & Schoenfelder, D. P. (2010). Evidence-based itative analysis of word storage and retrieval. Brain and
guideline: Individualized music for elders with demen- Language, 74(2), 141–156. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln​
tia. Journal of Gerontological Nursing, 36(6), 7–15. https:// .2000.2332
doi.org/10.3928/00989134-20100504-01 Goldberg, E., Podell, K., Bilder, R., & Jaeger, J. (2000). The
German, D. (2016). TAWF-2: Test of Adolescent/Adult Word Executive Control Battery. Psych Press.
Finding (2nd ed.). Pro-Ed. Goldberg, S., Haley, K. L., & Jacks, A. (2012). Script train-
Geschwind, N., & Levitsky, W. (1968). Human brain: Left- ing and generalization for people with aphasia. Ameri-
right asymmetries in temporal speech region. Science, can Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 21(3), 222–238.
161(3837), 186–187. https://doi.org/10.1126/science​ https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2012/11-0056)
.161​.3837.186 Golden, C. J., & Freshwater, S. M. (2002). Stroop Color and
Gilbey, A., & Tani, K. (2015). Companion animals and Word Test: A manual for clinical and experimental uses.
loneliness: A systematic review of quantitative studies. Stoelting.
531
References  

Goldfarb, R., & Bader, E. (1979). Espousing melodic intona- Goodglass, H. (1968). Studies on the grammar of aphasics.
tion therapy in aphasia rehabilitation: A case study. Inter- In N. S. Rosenberg & K. Joplin (Eds.), Development in
national Journal of Rehabilitation Research, 2(3), 333–342. applied psycholinguistic research. Macmillan.
https://doi.org/10.1097/00004356-197909000-00002 Goodglass, H. (1993). Understanding aphasia. Academic
Goldstein, H., & Olswang, L. (2018). Is there a science to Press.
facilitateimplementation of evidence-based practices Goodglass, H., Gleason, J. B., Bernholtz, N. A., & Hyde, M.
and programs? Evidence-Based Communication Assess- R. (1972). Some linguistic structures in the speech of a
ment and Intervention, 11(3–4), 55–60. https://doi.org/ Broca’s aphasic. Cortex, 8(2), 191–212. https://doi.org/​
10.1080/17489539.2017.1416768 10.1016/S0010-9452(72)80018-2
Goldstein, K. (1948). Language and language disturbances. Goodglass, H., Kaplan, E., Barresi, B., Weintraub, S.,
Grune & Stratton. & Segal, O. (2001). The Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Ex-
Goldstein, K., & Scheerer, M. (1941). Abstract and concrete amination (BDAE) (3rd ed.). Lippincott Williams &
behavior an experimental study with special tests. Psy- Wilkins.
chological Monograph, 53(2), 1–151. https://doi.org/​ Goodman, R. A., & Caramazza, A. (1985). The Johns Hopkins
10.1037/h0093487 University Dysgraphia Battery. Johns Hopkins University.
Golper, L. A. C., Wertz, R. T., Frattali, C., Yorkston, K., Gosnell, J., Costello, J., & Shane, H. (2011). Using a clin-
Myers, P., Katz, R., . . . Wambaugh, J. (2001). Evi- ical approach to answer, “What communication apps
dence-based practice guidelines for the management of com- should we use?” Perspectives on Augmentative and Alter-
munication disorders in neurologically impaired individuals: native Communication, 20(3), 87. https://doi.org/10​.10​
Project introduction. https://www.ancds.org/assets/ 44/aac20.3.87
docs/EBP/practiceguidelines.pdf Gosseries, O., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Bruno, M.-A.,
Gómez-Romero, M., Jiménez-Palomares, M., Rodríguez-​ Demertzi, A., Schnakers, C., Boly, M. M., . . . Laureys,
Mansilla, J., Flores-Nieto, A., Garrido-Ardila, E. M., & S. (2011). Disorders of consciousness: Coma, vegeta-
González-López-Arza, M. V. (2017). Benefits of music tive and minimally conscious states. In D. Cvetkovic
therapy on behaviour disorders in subjects diagnosed & I. Cosic (Eds.), States of consciousness (pp. 29–55).
with dementia: A systematic review. Neurología (English Springer. https://link.springer.com/chapter/​10.1007/​
Edition), 32(4), 253–263. https://doi.org/​10.1016/j.nr​ 978-3-642-18047-7_2
leng.2014.11.003 Government of Canada. (2019). Speech therapist in Canada.
Gómez Taibo, M. L., Parga Amado, P., Canosa Domínguez, https://www.jobbank.gc.ca/marketreport/outlook-​
N., Vieiro Iglesias, P., & García Real, T. (2014). Conversa- occupation/22734/ca
tions about self-identity in Alzheimer disease: Augmen- Granachar, R. P. (2003). Behavioral assessment following
tative and alternative communication memory books as traumatic brain injury. In R. P. Granachar (Ed.), Trau-
an aid. Revista de Logopedia, Foniatría Y Audiología, 34(2), matic brain injury: Methods for clinical and forensic neuro-
60–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j​.rlfa.2013.04.008 psychiatric assessment (pp. 233–234). CRC Press.
Gondusky, J. S., & Reiter, M. P. (2005). Protecting mili- Grandpierre, V., Milloy, V., Sikora, L., Fitzpatrick, E.,
tary convoys in Iraq: An examination of battle injuries Thomas, R., & Potter, B. (2018). Barriers and facilita-
sustained by a mechanized battalion during Opera- tors to cultural competence in rehabilitation services:
tion Iraqi Freedom Military Medicine, 170(6), 546–549. A scoping review. BMC Health Services Research, 18(1),
https://doi.org/10.7205/MILMED.170.6.546 23. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12913-017-2811-1
González-Fernández, M., Davis, C., Molitoris, J. J., Graves, R. E. (2009). The legacy of the Wernicke‐Lichtheim
Newhart, M., Leigh, R., & Hillis, A. E. (2011). Formal model. Journal of the History of the Neurosciences, 6(1),
education, socioeconomic status, and the severity of 3–20. https://doi.org/10.1080/09647049709525682
aphasia after stroke. Archives of Physical Medicine and Gravier, M. L., Dickey, M. W., Hula, W. D., Evans, W.
Rehabilitation, 92(11), 1809–1813. https://doi.org/10​ S., Owens, R. L., Winans-Mitrik, R. L., & Doyle, P. J.
.1016/​j.apmr.2011.05.026 (2018). What matters in semantic feature analysis: Prac-
Gonzalez Rothi, L. J., & Heilman, K. M. (2014). Apraxia: tice-related predictors of treatment response in aphasia.
The neuropsychology of action. Taylor and Francis. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 27(1S),
Goodglass, H. (1962). Redefining the concept of agram- 438–453. https://doi.org/10.1044/2017_AJSLP-16-0196
matism in aphasia. In C. L. Croatto & C. Croatto (Eds.), Greenwald, M. (2004). “Blocking” lexical competitors in
Proceedings of the Twelfth International Speech and Voice severe global agraphia: A treatment of reading and
Therapy Conference (pp. 108–115). International Associ- spelling. Neurocase, 10(2), 156–174. https://doi.org/​
ation of Logopedics and Phoniatrics, Padua, Italy. 10.1080/13554790409609946
532  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Greenwood, C. E. (2003). Dietary carbohydrate, glucose nology and semantics. Cortex, 86, 275–289. https://doi​
regulation, and cognitive performance in elderly per- .org/​10.1016/j.cortex.2016.04.016
sons. Nutrition Reviews, 61(5), S68–S74. https://doi.org/​ Haley, K. L., Cunningham, K. T., Barry, J., & de Riesthal,
10.1301/nr.2003.may.S68-S74 M.. (2019). Collaborative goals for communicative life
Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & participation in aphasia: The FOURC model. Ameri-
J. L. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics: Vol. 3. Speech can Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 28(1), 1–13.
acts (pp. 41–58). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_AJSLP-18-0163
Griffith, R., & Tengnah, C. (2007). Mental capacity act of Haley, K. L., Womach, J. L., Helm-Estabrooks, N., Caignon,
2005. British Journal of Community Nursing, 13(6), 284– D., & McCulloch, K. L. (2010). The life interests and values
288. https://doi.org/10.12968/bjcn.2008.13.6.29463 cards. University of North Carolina School of Medicine.
Gronseth, G., & French, J. (2008). Practice parameters and Hall, N., Boisvert, M., & Steele, R. (2013). Telepractice in
technology assessments: What they are, what they the assessment and treatment of individuals with apha-
are not, and why you should care. Neurology, 71(20), sia: A systematic review. International Journal of Telere-
1639–1643. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.00003​365​ habilitation, 5(1). https://doi.org/10.5195/ijt.2013.6119
35.27773.c0 Hallowell, B. (1999, October). Students learn while serv-
Gronwall, D. M. (1977). Paced auditory serial addition ing: Respite for caregivers of persons with dementing
test. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44(2), 367–373. https:// illnesses. NSSLHA News & Notes, 4.
doi.org/10.2466/pms.1977.44.2.367 Hallowell, B. (2000). A student-run respite network for
Gruber-Baldini, A., Ye, J., Anderson, K., & Shulman, L. caregivers of persons with dementing illness. Commu-
(2009). Effects of optimism/pessimism and locus of nication Connection, 14(1), 10.
control on disability and quality of life in Parkinson’s Hallowell, B. (2008). Strategic design of protocols to evalu-
disease. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders, 15(9), 665– ate vision in research on aphasia and related disorders.
669. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.park reldis.2009.03.005 Aphasiology, 22(6), 600–617. https://doi.org/​10.1080/​
Guiraud, V., Amor, M. B., Mas, J.-L., & Touzé, E. (2010). 02687030701429113
Triggers of ischemic stroke. Stroke, 41(11), 2669–2677. Hallowell, B. (2012a). Exploiting eye-mind connections for
https://doi.org/10.1161/STROKEAHA.110.597443 clinical applications in language disorders. In R. Gold-
Guskiewicz, K. M., Marshall, S. W., Bailes, J., McCrea, farb (Ed.), Translational speech-language pathology and
M., Cantu, R. C., Randolph, C., & Jordan, B. D. (2005). audiology (pp. 335–341). Plural Publishing.
Association between recurrent concussion and late- Hallowell, B. (2012b, April). First do no harm: Asking tough
life cognitive impairment in retired professional foot- ethical questions of students and faculty members engaged
ball players. Neurosurgery, 719–726. https://doi.org/​ in global outreach, education, and research. [Paper presen-
10.1227/01.NEU.0000175725.75780.DD tation]. Global Summit on Higher Education in Com-
Haarbauer-Krupa, J., Moser, L., Smith, G., Sullivan, D. M., munication Sciences and Disorders, Newport Beach,
& Szekeres, S. F. (1985). Cognitive rehabilitation ther- CA. https://new.capcsd.org/proceedings/2012/Post​
apy: Middle stages of recovery. In M. Ylvisaker (Ed.), ers/8.%20Hallowell%202012%20-%20 First%20do%20
Head injury rehabilitation: Children and adolescents (pp. no%20harm.pdf
287–310). College-Hill Press. Hallowell, B. (2012c). Using NSF-sponsored projects to
Hacker, V. L., Thomas, S. A., & Stark, D. (2009). Validation enrich students’ written communication skills. In 2007
of the stroke aphasic depression questionnaire using Annual review of engineering design projects to aid persons
the brief assessment schedule depression cards in an with disabilities (pp. 25–31). Creative Learning Press/
acute stroke sample. British Journal of Clinical Psychol- National Science Foundation.
ogy, 49(Pt. 1), 123–127. https://doi.org/10.1348/​01​4​ Hallowell, B. (2014). A magnificent new era for global col-
466509X467440 laborations in higher education in communication sciences
Hagen, C., Malkraus, D., Durham, P., & Bowman, K. and disorders. https://academy.pubs.asha.org/2012/​
(1979). Levels of cognitive functioning. In Rehabilitation 04/​a-​magnificent-new-era-for-global-collaborations-
of the head injured adult: Comprehensive physical manage- in-higher-education-in-communication-sciences-and-
ment (pp. 87–89). Professional Staff Association of Ran- disorders/
cho Los Amigos Hospital. Hallowell, B. (2021). Strategically promoting access to our
Halai, A. D., Woollams, A. M., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. services. In M. W. Hudson & M. DeRuiter (Eds.), Pro-
(2017). Using principal component analysis to capture fessional issues in speech-language pathology and audiology
individual differences within a unified neuropsycho- (5th ed., pp. 381–397). Plural Publishing.
logical model of chronic post-stroke aphasia: Revealing Hallowell, B., & Chapey, R. (2008a). Delivering language
the unique neural correlates of speech fluency, pho- intervention services to adults with neurogenic com-
533
References  

munication disorders. In R. Chapey (Ed.), Language communication sciences and disorders (CSD) professionals.
intervention strategies in adult aphasia (5th ed., pp. 203– ASHA Press.
227). Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Hammill, D. D., & Bryant, B. R. (1991). Detroit Tests of Learn-
Hallowell, B., & Chapey, R. (2008b). Introduction to lan- ing Aptitude–Adult (DTLA-A). Pro-Ed.
guage intervention strategies in adult aphasia. In R. Hammill, D. D., Pearson, N., & Wiederholt, J. L. (2009).
Chapey (Ed.), Language intervention strategies in aphasia CTONI-2 Comprehensive Test of Nonverbal Intelligence.
and related communication disorders (5th ed., pp. 3–19). Pro-Ed.
Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Hancock, A., & Haskin, G. (2015). Speech-language pathol-
Hallowell, B., Combiths, P., Flynn, T. Hyter, Y. D., Lansing, ogists’ knowledge and attitudes regarding lesbian,
C.R., Ramikissoon, I., & Watson, J. B. (2021). Final report gay, bisexual, transgender, and queer (LGBTQ) popu-
of the ad hoc committee to develop guidance for members and lations. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology,
students engaging globally in clinical, scholarly, and other pro- 24(2), 206–221. https://doi.org/10.1044/2015_AJSLP
fessional activities. American Speech-Language-Hearing -14-0095
Association. Hannemann, B. T. (2006). Creativity with dementia patients:
Hallowell, B., Douglas, N., Wertz, R. T., & Kim, S. (2004). Can creativity and art stimulate dementia patients pos-
Control and description of visual function in research itively? Gerontology, 52(1), 59–65. https://doi.org/​10​
on aphasia and related disorders. Aphasiology, 18(5–7), .1159/000089827
611–623. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030444000084 Harmon, K. G., Drezner, J. A., Gammons, M., Guskiewicz,
Hallowell, B., Enderby, P., Mills, J. A., DeGroote, W., Skel- K. M., Halstead, M., Herring, S. A., . . . Roberts, W. O.
ton, P., Diaz, J., & Relan, P. (2021). Rehabilitation for (2013). American Medical Society for Sports Medicine
patients with communication impairment after COVID- position statement: Concussion in sport. British Jour-
19 illness [OpenWHO]. World Health Organization. nal of Sports Medicine, 47(1), 15–26. https://doi.org/​
https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.23594.75209 10.1136/bjsports-2012-091941
Hallowell, B., & Hickey, E. (2014). Engaging in ethical and Haro-Martínez, A. M., García-Concejero, V. E., López-​
sustainable international experiences. https://leader.pubs​ Ramos, A., Maté-Arribas, E., López-Táppero, J., Lubrini,
.asha.org/doi/10.1044/leader.OV1.20052015.24 G., . . . Fuentes, B. (2017). Adaptation of melodic intona-
Hallowell, B., & Hickey, E. (2015). How to help — not tion therapy to Spanish: A feasibility pilot study. Apha-
harm — underserved populations abroad. The ASHA siology, 31(11), 1333–1343. https://doi.org/10.1080/026
Leader, 20(5), 24–25. https://doi.org/10.1044/leader​ 87038.2017.1279731
.OV1.20052015.24 Haro-Martínez, A. M., Lubrini, G., Madero-Jarabo, R.,
Hallowell, B., & Ivanova, M. V. (2009). Development and Díez-Tejedor, E., & Fuentes, B. (2019). Melodic intona-
standardization of a multiple-choice test of auditory tion therapy in post-stroke nonfluent aphasia: A ran-
comprehension for aphasia in Russian. Journal of Medi- domized pilot trial. Clinical Rehabilitation, 33(1), 44–53.
cal Speech-Language Pathology, 17(2), 83–98. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269215518791004
Hallowell, B., & Lansing, C. R. (2004). Tracking eye move- Harris, G. M. (2014). An interpretive phenomenological anal-
ments to study cognition and communication. The ysis of religious coping and relationship with God among
ASHA Leader, 9(21), 22–25. older adults with functional impairments. Acumen Univer-
Hallowell, B., Shaw, V., Heuer, S., & Schwartz, F. (2015). sity of Alabama Libraries’ Digital Archives.
Relationships of real-time glucose levels on cognitive-lin- Harry, B. (1992). Cultural diversity, families, and the spe-
guistic performance in adults with and without diabetes. cial education system: Communication and empowerment.
Unpublished manuscript. Teachers College Press.
Hallowell, B., Wertz, R. T., & Kruse, H. (2002). Using eye Hartley, A. (2006). Changing role of the speed of process-
movement responses to index auditory comprehension: ing construct in the cognitive psychology of human
An adaptation of the revised token test. Aphasiology, aging. In J. E. Birren & K. Schaire (Eds.), Handbook of the
16(4–6), 587–594. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030​24​ psychology of aging (6th ed., pp. 183–207). Elsevier.
4000121 Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, com-
Halsband, U., Mueller, S., Hinterberger, T., & Strickner, prehension, and aging: A review and a new view. Psy-
S. (2009). Plasticity changes in the brain in hypnosis chology of Learning & Motivation, 22, 193–225. https://
and meditation. Contemporary Hypnosis, 26(4), 194–215. doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60041-9
https://doi.org/10.1002/ch.386 Hashimoto, N., & Frome, A. (2011). The use of a modified
Hamilton, A. F., Ramos-Pizarro, C. A., & Rivera Pérez, J. semantic features analysis approach in aphasia. Journal
F., Gonzalez, W., & Bevery-Drucker, K. L. (Eds.) (2020). of Communication Disorders, 44(4), 459–469. https://doi​
Exploring cultural responsiveness: Guided scenarios for .org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2011.02.004
534  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Haskins, E. C., Cicerone, K., Eberle, R., Dams-O’Connor, Helm-Estabrooks, N. (2001). Cognitive Linguistic Quick
K., Langenbahn, D., & Shapiro-Rosenbaum, A. (2012). Test. The Psychological Corporation.
Cognitive rehabilitation manual: Translating evidence-based Helm-Estabrooks, N., Albert, M. L., & Nicholas, M. (2014).
recommendations into practice. American Congress of Manual of aphasia and aphasia therapy (3rd ed.). Pro-Ed.
Rehabilitation Medicine Publishing. Helm-Estabrooks, N., Emery, P., & Albert, M. L. (1987).
Hassan, F. H., & Hallowell, B. (in press-a). Development Treatment of aphasic perseveration (TAP) program:
and evaluation of the Life Participation Approach to Apha- A new approach to aphasia therapy. Archives of Neu-
sia Knowledge Scale. rology, 44(12), 1253–1255. https://doi.org/10.1001/arch​
Hassan, F. H., & Hallowell, B. (in press-b). Impacts of neur​.1987.00520240035008
resource limitation and strategies for improving aphasia Helm-Estabrooks, N., Fitzpatrick, P. M., & Barresi, B.
rehabilitation in Malaysia: Perceptions of speech-language (1982). Visual action therapy for global aphasia. Journal
pathologists. of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 47(4), 385–389. https://
Hassing, L. B., Grant, M. D., Hofer, S. M., Pedersen, N. L., doi.org/10.1044/jshd.4704.385
Nilsson, S. E., Berg, S., & Johansson, B. (2004). Type 2 Helm-Estabrooks, N., & Hotz, G. (1991). Brief Test of Head
diabetes mellitus contributes to cognitive decline in Injury (BTHI). Riverside.
old age: A longitudinal population-based study. Jour- Helm-Estabrooks, N., & Nicholas, M. (2000). Sentence pro-
nal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10(4), duction program for aphasia. Pro-Ed.
599–607. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617704104165 Helm-Estabrooks, N., Nicholas, M. L., & Morgan, A.
Hays, D. G., & Erford, B. T. (2014). Developing multicultural (1989). Melodic intonation therapy. Pro-Ed.
counseling competence: A systems approach. Pearson. Helm-Estabrooks, N., Ramage, A. E., Bayles, K. A., &
Healthcare Interactive, Inc. (2008). The savvy caregiver. Cruz, R. (1998). Perseverative behavior in fluent and
https://www.hcinteractive.com/SavvyCaregiver nonfluent aphasia adults. Aphasiology, 12(8), 689–698.
Heaton, S. K., Thompson, L. L., Psychological Assess- https://doi.org/10.1080/02687039808249566
ment Resources, & Business Video Productions. (1995). Helm-Estabrooks, N., & Ramsberger, G. (1986). Treatment
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Psychological Assessment of agrammatism in long-term Broca’s aphasia. Brit-
Resources. ish Journal of Disorders of Communication, 21(1), 39–45.
Hecht, S. W. (2008). Herbal contributions to the manage- https://doi.org/10.3109/13682828609018542
ment of the multi-factorial cognitive disorders — Alz- Helm-Estabrooks, N., Ramsberger, G., Morgan, A. R., &
heimer’s disease and vascular dementia. Perspectives Nicholas, M. (1989). Boston assessment of severe aphasia.
on Neurophysiology and Neurogenic Speech and Language Riverside Press.
Disorders, 18, 114–123. https://doi.org/10.1044/nnsld​ Helm-Estabrooks, N., & Whiteside, J. (2012). Use of life
18​.3.114 interests and values (LIV) cards for self-determination
Heckhausen, J., Wrosch, C., & Schulz, R. (2010). A moti- of aphasia rehabilitation goals. Perspectives on Neuro-
vational theory of life-span development. Psycholog- physiology and Neurogenic Speech and Language Disorders,
ical Review, 117(1), 32–60. https://doi.org/10.1037/ 22(1), 6–11. https://doi.org/10.1044/nnsld22.1.6
a0017668 HelpAge International and Handicap International.
Heidari, M., Borujeni, M.G., Rezaei, P., Abyaneh, S.K., & (2012). A study of humanitarian financing for older people
Heidari, K. (2020). Effect of laughter therapy on depres- and people with disabilities, 2010–2011. https://reliefweb​
sion and quality of life of the elderly living in nursing .int/report/world/study-humanitarian-financing-​older-​
homes. Malaysian Journal of Medical Sciences, 27(4), 119– people-and-people-disabilities-2010–2011
129. https://doi.org/10.21315/mjms2020.27.4.11 Hengst, J. A. (2020). Understanding Everyday Communica-
Heilman, K. M., & Gonzalez Rothi, L. J. (2003). Apraxia. In tive Interactions: Introduction to Situated Discourse Analy-
K. M. Heilman & E. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical neuropsy- sis for Communication Sciences and Disorders. Routledge.
chology (4th ed., pp. 215–235). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003034537
Helm, N. A., & Barresi, B. (1980). Voluntary control of Henry, M. L., Hubbard, H. I., Grasso, S. M., Mandelli,
involuntary utterances: A treatment approach for M. L., Wilson, S. M., Sathishkumar, M. T., . . . Gorno-​
severe aphasia. In Proceedings of the Conference on Clinical Tempini, M. L. (2018). Retraining speech production
Aphasiology [June 1–5, Bar Harbor, ME]. BRK Publish- and fluency in non-fluent/agrammatic primary pro-
ers. https://aphasiology.pitt.edu/archive/00000587/ gressive aphasia. Brain, 141(6), 1799–1814. https://doi​
Helm-Estabrooks, N. (1992a). ADP: Aphasia Diagnostic Pro- .org/​10.1093/brain/awy101
files. Riverside. Henseler, I., Regenbrecht, F., & Obrig, H. (2014). Lesion
Helm-Estabrooks, N. (1992b). Test of Oral and Limb Apraxia. correlates of patholinguistic profiles in chronic apha-
Riverside. sia: Comparisons of syndrome-, modality- and symp-
535
References  

tom-level assessment. Brain, 137, 918–930. https://doi​ Hickman, C. S., & Dyer, W. M. (1998). Improving telemed-
.org/10.1093/brain/awt374 icine consultation with TeleDoc and the emergent tech-
Hepburn, K. W., Lewis, M., Sherman, C. W., & Tornatore, nologies. In M. L. Armstrong (Ed.), Telecommunications
J. (2003). The savvy caregiver program: Developing and for health professionals (pp. 204–214). Springer.
testing a transportable dementia family caregiver train- Hicks, R. R., Fertig, S. J., Desrocher, R. E., Koroshetz, W.
ing program. The Gerontologist, 43(6), 908–915. https:// J., & Pancrazio, J. J. (2010). Neurological effects of blast
doi.org/10.1093/geront/43.6.908 injury. Journal of Trauma, 68(5), 1257–1263. https://doi​
Hersh, D. (2009). How do people with aphasia view their .org/10.1097/TA.0b013e3181d8956d
discharge from therapy? Aphasiology, 23(3), 331–350. Higgins, C., Kearns, Á., & Franklin, S. (2012). The devel-
https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030701764220 opment of a semantic feature analysis based mobile
Hersh, D., Worrall, L., O’Halloran, R., Brown, K., Grohn, B., application for individuals with aphasia [Poster]. In
& Rodriguez, A. D. (2013). Assess for success: Evidence Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Mobile
for therapeutic assessment. In N. Simmons-Mackie, J. Systems, Applications, and Services (pp. 513–514). ACM.
M. King, & D. R. Beukelman (Eds.), Supporting com- https://doi.org/10.1145/2307636.2307710
munication for adults with acute and chronic aphasia (pp. Hilari, K., Byng, S., Lamping, D., & Smith, S. (2003). Stroke
145–164). Paul H. Brookes. and aphasia quality of life scale-39: Evaluation of accept-
Heuer, S., & Hallowell, B. (2007). An evaluation of multiple-​ ability, reliability and validity. Stroke, 34, 1944–1950.
choice test images for comprehension assessment in https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.0000081987.46660.ED
aphasia. Aphasiology, 21(9), 883–900. https://doi.org/10​ Hilari, K., Klippi, A., Constantinidou, F., Horton, S., Penn,
.1080/02687030600695194 C., Raymer, A., . . . Worrall, L. (2015). An international
Heuer, S., & Hallowell, B. (2009). Visual attention in a perspective on quality of life in aphasia: A survey of
multiple-choice task: Influences of image characteris- clinician views and practices from sixteen countries.
tics with and without presentation of a verbal stimulus. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 67(3), 119–130. https://
Aphasiology, 23(3), 351–363. https://doi.org/10.1080/​ doi.org/10.1159/000434748
02687030701770474 Hill, A., Blevins, R., & Code, C. (2019). Revisiting the
Heuer, S., & Hallowell, B. (2015). A novel eyetracking public awareness of aphasia in Exeter: 16 years on.
method to assess attention allocation in individuals International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 21(5),
with and without aphasia using a dual-task paradigm. 504–512. https://doi.org/10.1080/17549507.2018.1485​
Journal of Communication Disorders, 55, 15–30. https:// 742
doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2015.01.005 Hill, A. J., Theodoros, D. G., Russell, T. G., Cahill, L. M.,
Heuer, S., Ivanova, M., & Hallowell, B. (2017). More Ward, E. C., & Clark, K. M. (2006). An Internet-based
than the verbal stimulus matters: Visual attention in telerehabilitation system for the assessment of motor
language assessment for people with aphasia using speech disorders: A pilot study. American Journal of
multiple-choice image displays. Journal of Speech, Lan- Speech-Language Pathology, 15, 45–56. https://doi.org/​
guage, and Hearing Research, 60(5), 1348–1361. https:// 10.1044/1058-0360(2006/006)
doi.org/10.1044/2017_JSLHR-L-16-0087 Hillis, A. E. (1989). Efficacy and generalization of treat-
Heuer, S., & Willer, R. (2020). How is quality of life ment for aphasic naming errors. Archives of Physical
assessed in people with dementia? A systematic litera- Medicine and Rehabilitation, 70(80), 632–636.
ture review and a primer for speech-language patholo- Hillis, A. E., Barker, P. B., Beauchamp, N. J., Gordon, B.,
gists. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 29, & Wityk, R. J. (2000). MR perfusion imaging reveals
1–14. https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_AJSLP-19-00169 regions of hypoperfusion associated with aphasia and
Hickey, E. M., Bourgeois, M. S., & Brush, J. (2018). Inter- neglect. Neurology, 55(6), 782–788. https://doi.org/​
professional interventions for participation and quality 10.1212/WNL.55.6.782
of life. In E. M. Hickey & M. S. Bourgeois (Eds.), Demen- Hillis, A. E., Barker, P. B., Wityk, R. J., Aldrich, E. M.,
tia: Person-centered assessment and intervention (pp. 214– Restrepo, L., Breese, E. L., & Work, M. (2004). Variabil-
259). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. ity in subcortical aphasia is due to variable sites of cor-
Hickey, E. M., & Douglas, N. F. (2021). Person-centered mem- tical hypoperfusion. Brain and Language, 89(3), 524–530.
ory and communication interventions for dementia: A case https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2004.01.007
study approach (pp. xiii, 178). Plural Publishing. Hillis, A. E., & Caramazza, A. (1992). The reading process
Hickin, J., Best, W., Herbert, R., Howard, D., & Osborne, F. and its disorders. In D. I. Margolin (Ed.), Cognitive neu-
(2002). Phonological therapy for word-finding difficul- ropsychology in clinical practice (pp. 229–253). Oxford
ties: A re-evaluation. Aphasiology, 16(10–11), 981–999. University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030244000509
536  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Hillis, A. E., Kleinman, J. T., Newhart, M., Heidler-Gary, J., tants. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 11(3–4), 399–427.
Gottesman, R., Barker, P. B., & Chaudhry, P. (2006). https://doi.org/10.1080/09602010042000051
Restoring cerebral blood flow reveals neural regions Hoffman, S. W., Shesko, K., & Harrison, C. R. (2010).
critical for naming. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(31), 8069– Enhanced neurorehabilitation techniques in the DVBIC
8073. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20​88-​06​ assisted living pilot project. NeuroRehabilitation, 26(3),
.2006 257–269. https://doi.org/10.3233/NRE-2010-0561
Hinckley, J. J. (2002). Vocational and social outcomes of Hoge, C. W., McGurk, D., Thomas, J. L., Cox, A. L., Engel,
adults with chronic aphasia. Journal of Communication C. C., & Castro, C. A. (2008). Mild traumatic brain
Disorders, 35(6), 543–560. https://doi.org/10.1016/S00​ injury in U.S. soldiers returning from Iraq. New England
21-9924(02)00119-3 Journal of Medicine, 358(5), 453–463. https://doi.org/​10​
Hinckley, J., Boyle, E., Lombard, D., & Bartels Tobin, L. .1056/NEJMoa072972
(2014). Towards a consumer-informed research agenda Holland, A. L. (1982). Observing functional communi-
for aphasia: Preliminary work. Disability and Rehabilita- cation of aphasic adults. Journal of Speech and Hearing
tion, 36(12), 1042–1050. https://doi.org/10.3109/09638 Disorders, 47, 50–56. https://doi.org/10.1044/​ 1058-​
288.2013.829528 0360(2010/09-0095)
Hinckley, J. J., & Douglas, N. F. (2013). Treatment fidel- Holland, A. L. (1991). Pragmatic aspects of intervention
ity: Its importance and reported frequency in aphasia in aphasia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 6(2), 197–211.
treatment studies. American Journal of Speech-Language https://doi.org/10.1016/0911-6044(91)90007-6
Pathology, 22(2), S279–S284. https://doi.org/​10.1044/​ Holland, A. L. (2021). The social imperative for aphasia
1058-0360(2012/12-0092) rehabilitation: A personal history. In A. L. Holland
Hinckley, J. J., Douglas, N. M., Goff, R. A., & Nakano, & R. J. Elman (Eds.), Neurogenic communication disor-
E. V. (2013). Supporting communication with partner ders and the life participation approach (pp. 1–19). Plural
training. In N. Simmons-Mackie, J. M. King, & D. R. Publishing.
Beukelman (Eds.), Supporting communication for adults Holland, A. L., & Elman, R. J. (2021). Neurogenic communi-
with acute and chronic aphasia (pp. 245–274). Paul H. cation disorders and the life participation approach. Plural
Brookes. Publishing.
Hirode, G., & Wong, R. J. (2020). Trends in the prevalence Holland, A. L., Frattali, C., & Fromm, D. (2018). CADL-
of metabolic syndrome in the United States, 2011–2016. 3: Communication Activities of Daily Living (3rd ed.).
JAMA, 323(24), 2526. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama​ Pro-Ed.
.2020.4501 Holland, A. L., & Fridriksson, J. (2001). Aphasia manage-
Hitzig, S. L., & Sheppard, C. L. (2017). Implementing Mon- ment during early phases of recovery following stroke.
tessori methods for dementia: A scoping review. The American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10(1),
Gerontologist, 57(5), e94–e114. https://doi.org/10.1093/ 19–28. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2001/004)
geront/gnw147 Holland, A. L., Fromm, D., & Swindell, C. S. (1986). The
Ho, K. M., Weiss, S. J., Garrett, K. L., & Lloyd, L. L. (2005). The labeling problem in aphasia: An illustrative case.
effect of remnant and pictographic books on the com- Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 51(2), 176–180.
municative interaction of individuals with global apha- https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.5102.176
sia. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 21(3), Holland, A. L., Halper, A. S., & Cherney, L. R. (2010).
218–232. https://doi.org/10.1080/07434610400016694 Tell me your story: Analysis of script topics selected
Hoepner, J., & Hemmerich, A. (2018). A cross-sectional, by persons with aphasia. American Journal of Speech-
mixed methods examination of a modified “Flipped Language Pathology, 19(3), 198. https://doi.org/10.1044/​
Classroom” pedagogy: The “Sandwich Approach.” 1058-0360(2010/09-0095)
Journal of Interactional Research in Communication Dis- Holland, A. L., & Nelson, R. L. (2014). Counseling in
orders, 9(1), 5–43. https://doi.org/10.1558/jircd.34318 communication disorders: A wellness perspective. Plural
Hoepner, J. K., Sather, T. W., Homolka, T., & Clark, M. B. Publishing.
(2021). Immersion learning at an aphasia camp: Ana- Hoover, E. L., & Carney, A. (2014). Integrating the iPad
lysing student video reflections. International Journal of into an intensive, comprehensive aphasia program.
Speech-Language Pathology, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.10​ Seminars in Speech and Language, 35(1), 25–37. https://
80/17549507.2020.1844294 doi.org/10.1055/s-0033-1362990
Hoerster, L., Hickey, E. M., & Bourgeois, M. S. (2001). Hopper, T. L. (2003). “They’re just going to get worse
Effects of memory aids on conversations between nurs- anyway”: Perspectives on rehabilitation for nursing
ing home residents with dementia and nursing assis- home residents with dementia. Journal of Communica-
537
References  

tion Disorders, 36(5), 345–359. https://doi.org/10.1016/ tive Care, 14(4), 274–290. https://doi.org/10.1080/1552​
S0021-9924(03)00050-9 4256.2018.1508538
Hopper, T. L. (2007). The ICF and dementia. Seminars in Howe, L. L. (2009). Giving context to postdeployment
Speech and Language, 28(4), 273–282. post-concussive-like symptoms: Blast-related poten-
Hopper, T., Bayles, K., Harris, F., & Holland, A. (2001). tial mild traumatic brain injury and comorbidities. The
The relationship between minimum data set ratings Clinical Neuropsychologist, 23(8), 1315–1337. https://doi.
and scores on measures of communication and hearing org/10.1080/13854040903266928
among nursing home residents with dementia. Ameri- Howe, T. (2008). The ICF contextual factors related to
can Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10(4), 370–381. speech-language pathology. International Journal of
https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2001/031) Speech-Language Pathology, 10(1–2), 27–37. https://doi​
Hopper, T., Bourgeois, M., Pimentel, J., Qualls, C. D., .org/10.1080/14417040701774824
Hickey, E., Frymark, T., & Schooling, T. (2013). An Howe, T. J., Worrall, L. E., & Hickson, L. M. H. (2004). What is
evidence-based systematic review on cognitive inter- an aphasia-friendly environment? Aphasiology, 18, 1015–
ventions for individuals with dementia. American Journal 1038. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030444000499
of Speech-Language Pathology/American Speech-Language- Howells, S., Barton, G., & Westerveld, M. (2016). Explor-
Hearing Association, 22(1), 126–145. https://doi.org/​ ing the development of cultural awareness amongst
10.1044/1058-0360(2012/11-0137) post-graduate speech-language pathology students.
Hopper, T., Holland, A., & Rewega, M. (2002). Conversa- International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 18(3),
tional coaching: Treatment outcomes and future direc- 259–271. https://doi.org/10.3109/17549507.2016.1154982
tions. Aphasiology, 16(7), 745–761. https://doi.org/​ Hoyau, E., Boudiaf, N., Cousin, E., Pichat, C., Fournet, N.,
10.1080/02687030244000059 Krainik, A., . . . Baciu, M. (2017). Aging modulates the
Hopper, T. L., Mahendra, N., Kim, E., Azuma, T., Bayles, hemispheric specialization during word production.
K. A., Cleary, S. J., & Tomoeda, C. K. (2005). Evidence- Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 9, 125. https://doi.org/​
based practice recommendations for working with 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00125
individuals with dementia: Spaced-retrieval training. Hsieh, S., Schubert, S., Hoon, C., Mioshi, E., & Hodges, J.
Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology, 13(4), R. (2013). Validation of the Addenbrooke’s Cognitive
xxvii–xxxiv. https://doi.org/10.1044/leader.FTR3​.10​ Examination III in frontotemporal dementia and Alz-
152005.10 heimer’s disease. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Dis-
Horner, J. (2003). Morality, ethics, and law: Introductory orders, 36(3–4), 242–250.
concepts. Seminars in Speech and Language, 24(4), 263– Huang, J., Qin, X., Shen, M., & Huang, Y. (2020). An over-
274. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2004-815580 view of systematic reviews and meta-analyses on
Horner, J. (2013). Communication access, rights, and pol- acupuncture for post-stroke aphasia. European Journal
icies. In N. Simmons-Mackie, M. King, & D. R. Beu- of Integrative Medicine, 37, 101133. https://doi.org/10​
kelman (Eds.), Supporting communication for adults with .1016/j.eujim.2020.101133
acute and chronic aphasia (pp. 303–324). Paul H. Brookes. Huckans, M., Pavawalla, S., Demadura, T., Kolessar, M.,
Horowitz, S. (2013). The healing power of music and Seelye, A., Roost, N., & Storzbach, D. (2010). A pilot
dance. Alternative and Complementary Therapies, 19(5), study examining effects of group-based Cognitive
265. https://doi.org/10.1089/act.2013.19502. Strategy Training treatment on self-reported cognitive
Horton-Deutsch, S., Twigg, P., & Evans, R. (2007). Health problems, psychiatric symptoms, functioning, and
care decision-making of persons with dementia. compensatory strategy use in OIF/OEF combat vet-
Dementia, 6(1), 105–120. https://doi.org/10.1177/​1471​ erans with persistent mild cognitive disorder and his-
301207075643 tory of traumatic brain injury. Journal of Rehabilitation
Hough, M. S. (1990). Narrative comprehension in adults Research and Development, 47(1), 43–60. https://doi.org/​
with right and left hemisphere brain-damage: Theme 10.1682/JRRD.2009.02.0019
organization. Brain and Language, 38(2), 253–277. Huisingh, R., Bowers, L., Zachman, L., Blagden, C., &
https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(90)90114-V Orman, J. (1990). The Word Test–Elementary–Revised.
Hough, M. S. (2010). Melodic intonation therapy and apha- Lingui​Systems.
sia: Another variation on a theme. Aphasiology, 24(6–8), Hula, W. D., Doyle, P. J., Stone, C. A., Austermann Hula,
775–786. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030903501941 S. N., Kellough, S., Wambaugh, J. L., . . . St. Jacque, A.
Hovland, C. (2018). Welcoming death: Exploring pre-death (2015). The aphasia communication outcome measure
grief experiences of caregivers of older adults with (ACOM): Dimensionality, item bank calibration, and
dementia. Journal of Social Work in End-of-Life & Pallia- initial validation. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing
538  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Research, 58(3), 906. https://doi.org/10.1044/2015_JSL International Communication Project (2014). International


HR-L-14-0235 Communication Project. https://internationalcommuni​
Human Rights Campaign. (n.d.). Glossary of terms. https:// cationproject.com
www.hrc.org/resources/glossary-of-terms Interprofessional Education Collaborative (IPEC). (2016).
Hund-Georgiadis, M., Zysset, S., Weih, K., Guthke, T., IPEC core competencies. https://www.ipecollaborative​.org
& von Cramon, D. Y. (2001). Crossed nonaphasia in Interprofessional Education Collaborative Expert Panel.
a dextral with left hemispheric lesions: A functional (2011). Core competencies for interprofessional collaborative
magnetic resonance imaging study of mirrored brain practice: Report of an expert panel. Washington, DC: Inter-
organization. Stroke, 32, 2703–2707. https://stroke.aha​ professional Education Collaborative. https://www​
journals.org/content/32/11/2703 .aacom.org/docs/default-source/insideome/ccrpt05-
Hurkmans, J., de Bruijn, M., Boonstra, A. M., Jonkers, R., 10-11.pdf?sfvrsn=77937f97_2
Bastiaanse, R., Arendzen, H., & Reinders-Messelink, Isquith, P. K., Roth, R. M., & Gioia, G. A. (2010). Tasks
H. A. (2012). Music in the treatment of neurological of executive control (TEC). Psychological Assessment
language and speech disorders: A systematic review. Resources.
Aphasiology, 26(1), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1080/0268 Ivanova, M. V., & Hallowell, B. (2012). Validity of an
7038.2011.602514 eye-tracking method to index working memory in
Huttner-Koros, A. (2015, August 21). The hidden bias people with and without aphasia. Aphasiology, 26(3–4),
of science’s universal language. The Atlantic. https:// 556–578. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2011.618219
www.theatlantic.com/science/archive/2015/08/ Ivanova, M. V., & Hallowell, B. (2013). A tutorial on aphasia
english-​universal-language-science-research/400919/ test development in any language: Key substantive and
Hux, K., Buechter, M., Wallace, S., & Weissling, (2010). Using psychometric considerations. Aphasiology, 27(8), 891–
visual scene displays to create a shared communica- 920. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2013.805​728
tion space for a person with aphasia. Aphasiology, 24(5), Ivanova, M. V., & Hallowell, B. (2014). A new modified lis-
643–660. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030902869299 tening span task to enhance validity of working mem-
Hyter, Y. D. (2014). Conceptual framework for responsive ory assessment for people with and without aphasia.
global engagement in Communication Sciences and Journal of Communication Disorders, 52, 78–98. https://
Disorders. Topics in Language Disorders, 34(2), 105–120. doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2014.06.001
https://doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0000000000000015 Jackson, H. H. (1878). On affectations of speech from dis-
Hyter, Y. D. (2021). Hyter’s pathway for responsive and ease of the brain. Brain, 1, 304–330.
sustainable practices. In Y. D. Hyter & M. B. Salas-​ Jackson, J. L., & Kuriyama, A. (2019). How often do system-
Provance. Culturally responsive practices in speech, lan- atic reviews exclude articles not published in English?
guage and hearing sciences (2nd ed.). Plural Publishing. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 34(8), 1388–1389.
Hyter, Y. D., & Salas-Provance, M. B. (2019). Culturally https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-019-04976-x
responsive practices in speech, language, and hearing sci- Jacobs, B. J., & Thompson, C. K. (2000). Cross-modal gen-
ences. Plural Publishing. eralization effects of training noncanonical sentence
India Today. (2014). Speech therapists. https://indiatoday.​ in comprehension and production in agrammatic aphasia.
today.in/education/story/speech-therapists/1/363 Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 43(1),
861.htmltoday.in/education/story/speech-therapists 5–20. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4301.05
Ingersoll-Dayton, B., Spencer, B., Kwak, M., Scherrer, K., Jaffee, M. S., Helmick, K. M., Girard, P. D., Meyer, K. S.,
Allen, R. S., & Campbell, R. (2013). The couples life Dinegar, K., & George, K. (2009). Acute clinical care
story approach: A dyadic intervention for dementia. and care coordination for traumatic brain injury within
Journal of Gerontological Social Work, 56(3), 237–254. Department of Defense. Journal of Rehabilitation Research
https://doi.org/10.1080/01634372.2012.758214 and Development, 46(6), 655–666. https://doi.org/10​
Ingstad, B., & Reynolds Whyte, S. (1995). Disability and cul- .1682/​JRRD.2008.09.0114
ture. University of California Press. Jakubowitz, M., & Schill, M. J. (2008). Ethical implications
Institute for Healthcare Improvement. (n.d.). IHI triple aim of using outdated standardized tests. SIG 16 Perspec-
initiative. https://www.ihi.org/Engage/Initiatives/ tives on School-Based Issues, 9(2), 79–83. https://doi.org/​
TripleAim/pages/default.aspx 10.1044/sbi9.2.79
Institute of Safe Medicine Practices (2021). ISMP Targeted Jamieson, M., Cullen, B., McGee-Lennon, M., Brewster, S.,
Medication Safety Best Practices for Hospitals. https:// & Evans, J. (2017). Technological memory aid use by
www.ismp.org people with acquired brain injury. Neuropsychological
International Coach Federation. (2015). What is professional Rehabilitation, 27(6), 919–936. https://doi.org/10.1080/
coaching? https://coachfederation.org/ 09602011.2015.1103760
539
References  

Jehkonen, M., Laihosalo, M., & Kettunen, J. (2006). Ano- Jurica, S. J., Leitten, C. L., & Mattis, S. (2001). Dementia
sognosia after stroke: Assessment, occurrence, sub- rating scale: Professional manual. Psychological Corp.
types and impact on functional outcome reviewed. Kaderavek, J. N., & Justice, L. M. (2010). Fidelity: An
Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 114(5), 293–306. https:// essential component of evidence-based practice in
doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0404.2006.00723.x speech-language pathology. American Journal of Speech-​
Jin, J., & Bridges, S. M. (2014). Educational technologies in Language Pathology, 19(4), 369. https://doi.org/1​ 0.104​ 4/​
problem-based learning in health sciences education: 1058-0360(2010/09-0097)
A systematic review. Journal of Medical Internet Research, Kagan, A. (2011). A-FROM in action at the Aphasia Insti-
16(12), e251. https://doi.org/10.2196/jmir.3240 tute. Seminars in Speech and Language, 32(3), 216–228.
Johnson, A. F., & Jacobson, B. H. (2017). Medical speech-lan- https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0031-1286176
guage pathology: A practitioner’s guide. Thieme Publishers. Kagan, A., Black, S. E., Duchan, J. F., Simmons-Mackie, N.,
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, F. (2009). Joining together: Group & Square, P. (2001). Training volunteers as conversation
theory and group skills (10th ed.). Allyn & Bacon. partners using “supported conversation for adults with
Johnson, M. L., Taub, E., Harper, L. H., Wade, J. T., aphasia” (SCA): A controlled trial. Journal of Speech,
Bowman, M. H., Bishop-McKay, S., . . . Uswatte, G. Language, and Hearing Research, 44(3), 624–638. https://
(2014). An enhanced protocol for constraint-induced doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2001/051)
aphasia therapy II: A case series. American Journal of Kagan, A., & Kimelman, M. D. (1995). Informed consent in
Speech-Language Pathology, 23(1), 60–72. https://doi​ aphasia research: Myth or reality. Clinical Aphasiology,
.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2013/12-0168) 23, 65–75. http://aphasiology.pitt.edu/1304
Johnson, P., Bradd, P., Hallowell, B., & Hyter, Y. (2021, Kagan, A., & Simmons-Mackie, N. (2007). Beginning with
June). Global ethical challenges in speech-language pathol- the end: Outcome-driven assessment and interven-
ogy. National Conference of Speech Pathology Austra- tion with life participation in mind. Topics in Language
lia. Online Disorders, 27(4), 309–317. https://doi.org/10.1097/01​
John-Steiner, V. (2006). Creative collaboration. Oxford Uni- .TLD.0000299885.39488.bf
versity Press. Kagan, A., Simmons-Mackie, N., Rowland, A., Huijbregts,
Joint Commission. (2010). Advancing effective communica- M., Shumway, E., McEwen, S., . . . Sharp, S. (2008). Count-
tion, cultural competence, and patient- and family-centered ing what counts: A framework for capturing real-life
care. https://www.jointcommission.org/-/media/tjc/ outcomes of aphasia intervention. Aphasiology, 22(3),
doc​uments/resources/patient-safety-topics/health- 258–280. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030701282595
equity/aroadmapforhospitalsfinalversion727pdf Kagan, A., Simmons-Mackie, N., Victor, J. C., Carling-​
.pdf?db=web&hash=AC3AC4BED1D973713C2CA6​B2​ Rowland, A., Hoch, J., Huijbregts, M., . . . Mok, A.
E5ACD01B (2011). Assessment for living with aphasia (ALA). Aphasia
Joltin, A., Camp, C. J., & McMahon, C. M. (2003). Institute.
Spaced-retrieval over the telephone: An intervention Kahn-Denis, K. B. (1997). Art therapy with geriatric
for persons with dementia. Clinical Psychologist, 7(1), dementia clients. Art Therapy, 14(3), 194–199. https://
50–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/13284200410001707483 doi.org/10.1080/07421656.1987.10759281
Jubeh, K., & Abdalla, A. (2020). A participation revolution: Kalaria, R. N. (2016). Neuropathological diagnosis of
Creating genuine dialogue and partnerships between vascular cognitive impairment and vascular dementia
humanitarian actors and the disability movement. with implications for Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neuro-
Humanitarian Exchange, 78, 9–13. pathologica, 131(5), 659–685. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Judd, T. (1989). Crossed “right hemisphere syndrome” s00401-016-1571-z
with limb apraxia: A case study. Neuropsychology, 3(3), Kanaya, A. M., Barrett-Connor, E., Gildengorin, G., &
159–173. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0091765 Yaffe, K. (2004). Change in cognitive function by glu-
Jung, W., Kwon, S., Park, S., & Moon, S. (2012). Can cose tolerance status in older adults: A 4-year pro-
combination therapy of conventional and oriental spective study of the Rancho Bernardo study cohort.
medicine improve poststroke aphasia? Comparative, Archives of Internal Medicine, 164(12), 1327–1333. https://
observational, pragmatic study. Evidence-Based Com- doi.org/10.1001/archinte.164.12.1327
plementary and Alternative Medicine, 2012. https://doi​ Kang, E. K., Sohn, H. M., Han, M.-K., & Paik, N.-J. (2017).
.org/10.1155/2012/654604 Subcortical aphasia after stroke. Annals of Rehabili-
Junqué, C., Litvan, I., & Vendrell, P. (1986). Does reversed tation Medicine, 41(5), 725. https://doi.org/10.5535/
laterality really exist in dextrals? A case study. Neuro- arm.2017.41.5.725
psychologia, 24(2), 241–254. https://doi.org/10.1016/​ Kaplan, D. M., Tarvydas, V. M., & Gladding, S. T. (2014).
0028-3932(86)90056-4 20/20: A vision for the future of counseling: The new
540  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

consensus definition of counseling. Journal of Coun- and related neurogenic communication disorders (5th ed.,
seling & Development, 92(3), 366–372. https://doi.org/​ pp. 376–400). Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
10.1002/j.1556-6676.2014.00164.x Kearns, K. P. (1985). Response elaboration training for
Kapoor, A. (2017). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stim- patient initiated utterances. In R. N. Brookshire (Ed.),
ulation therapy for post-stroke non-fluent aphasia: Clinical aphasiology (pp. 196–204). BRK.
A critical review. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 24(7), Kearns, K. P., & Scher, G. P. (1989). The generalization of
547–553. https://doi.org/10.1080/10749357.2017.1331 response elaboration training effects. Clinical Aphasiol-
417 ogy, 18, 223–245. https://aphasiology.pitt.edu/archive/​
Karanth, P. (2000). Multilingual/multiliterate/multicul- 00000076/01/18-17.pdf
tural studies of aphasia: The Rosetta Stone of neuro- Kearns, K. P., & Yedor, K. (1991). An alternating treatments
linguistics in the new millennium. Brain and Language, comparison of loose training and a convergent treat-
71(1), 113–115. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1999.2227 ment strategy. Clinical Aphasiology, 20, 223–238.
Karlawish, J. (2008). Measuring decision-making capacity Keengwe, J., Onchwari, G., & Oigara, J. N. (2014). Promot-
in cognitively impaired individuals. Neurosignals, 16(1), ing active learning through the flipped classroom model.
91–98. https://doi.org/10.1159/000109763 Information Science Reference.
Karnath, H.-O., Milner, D., & Vallar, G. (2002). The cogni- Keith, R. W. (2009). SCAN-3 for Adolescents and Adults: Tests
tive and neural bases of spatial neglect. Oxford University for auditory processing disorders. Pearson.
Press. Kelley, J. M., Kraft-Todd, G., Schapira, L., Kossowsky, J.,
Karnath, H.-O., & Rorden, C. (2012). The anatomy of spa- & Riess, H. (2014). The influence of the patient-clini-
tial neglect. Neuropsychologia, 50(6), 1010–1017. https:// cian relationship on healthcare outcomes: A systematic
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.06.027 review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled
Kataria, D. M. (2011). Laugh for no reason. Madhuri trials. PLOS ONE, 9(4), 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1371/
International. journal.pone.0094207
Katz, R. C. (2001). Computer applications in aphasia treat- Kelly, H., Brady, M. C., & Enderby, P. (2010). Speech
ment. In R. Chapey (Ed.), Language intervention strategies and language therapy for aphasia following stroke.
in aphasia and related neurogenic communication disorders Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. https://doi.
(4th ed., pp. 718–738). Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. org/​10.1002/14651858​.CD000425.pub4
Katz, R. C., Hallowell, B., Code, C., Armstrong, E., Roberts, Keltner, N. L., & Cooke, B. B. (2007). Biological perspec-
P., Pound, C., & Katz, L. (2000). A multinational com- tives: Traumatic brain injury — War related. Perspectives
parison of aphasia management practices. International in Psychiatric Care, 43(4), 223–226. https://doi.org/​
Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 35(2), 10.1111/j.1744-6163.2007.00138.x
303–314. https://doi.org/10.1080/136828200247205 Kember, D., Ho, A., & Hong, C. (2008). The importance
Kaufmann-Meyer, M. (2018). Happy anniversary! Comite of establishing relevance in motivating student learn-
Permanent de Liaison des Orthophonistes/Logopedes ing. Active Learning in Higher Education, 9(3), 249–263.
de l’EU. https://doi.org/10.1177/1469787408095849
Kauhanen, M. L., Korpelaninen, J., Hiltunen, P., Maatta, Kemper, S., & Harden, T. (1999). Experimentally disentan-
R., Mononen, H., Brusin, E., & Myllyla, V. V. (2000). gling what’s beneficial about elderspeak from what’s
Aphasia, depression, and non-verbal cognitive impair- not. Psychology and Aging, 14(4), 656–670. https://doi​
ment in ischemic stroke. Cerebrovascular Diseases, 10(6), .org/10.1037/0882-7974.14.4.656
455–461. https://doi.org/10.1159/000016107 Kemper, S., & Kemtes, K. (2000). Aging and message
Kay, J., Lesser, R., & Coltheart, M. (1992). Psycholinguistic production and comprehension. In D. C. Park & N.
Assessment of Language Processing in Aphasia (PALPA). Schwartz (Eds.), Cognitive aging: A primer (pp. 197–213).
Lawrence Erlbaum. Psychology Press.
Kay, J., Lesser, R., & Coltheart, M. (1997). Psycholinguistic Kemper, S., Schmalzried, R., Herman, R., & Mohankumar,
Assessments of Language Processing in Aphasia (PALPA). D. (2011). The effects of varying task priorities on lan-
Psychology Press. guage production by young and older adults. Experi-
Kean, M. L. (1977). The linguistic interpretation of apha- mental Aging Research, 37(2), 198–219. https://doi.org/
sic syndromes: Agrammatism in Broca’s aphasia, an 10.1080/0361073X.2011.554513
example. Cognition, 5(1), 9–46. https://doi.org/10.10​ Kemper, S., & Sumner, A. (2001). The structure of verbal
16/0010-0277(77)90015-4 abilities in young and older adults. Psychology and
Kean, M. L. (1985). Agrammatism. Academic Press. Aging, 16(2), 312–322. https://doi.org/10.1037/​0882-​
Kearns, K., & Elman, R. J. (2008). Group therapy for apha- 7974.16.2.312
sia: Theoretical and practical considerations. In R. Kempler, D., Teng, E. L., Taussig, M., & Dick, M. B. (2010).
Chapey (Ed.), Language intervention strategies in aphasia The common objects memory test (COMT): A simple
541
References  

test with cross-cultural applicability. Journal of the Inter- related concussion: Systematic review. Sports Medicine,
national Neuropsychological Society, 16(3), 537. https:// 44(4), 449–471. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-013-0134-x
doi.org/10.1017/S1355617710000160 King, J. M. (2013a). Communication supports. In N. Sim-
Kempler, D., Van Lancker, D., Marchman, V., & Bates, E. mons-Mackie, J. M. King, & D. R. Beukelman (Eds.),
(1999). Idiom comprehension in children and adults Supporting communication for adults with acute and
with unilateral brain damage. Developmental Neuro- chronic aphasia (pp. 51–72). Paul H. Brookes.
psychology, 15, 327–349. https://doi.org/10.1080/​875​6​ King, J. M. (2013b). Supporting communication with tech-
5649909540753 nology. In N. Simmons-Mackie, J. M. King, & D. R. Beu-
Kendall, D., Conway, T., Rosenbek, J., & Gonzalez-Rothi, kelman (Eds.), Supporting communication for adults with
L. (2003). Case study: Phonological rehabilitation of acute and chronic aphasia (pp. 73–98). Paul H. Brookes.
acquired phonologic alexia. Aphasiology, 17(11), 1073– King, J. M., Simmons-Mackie, N., & Beukelman, D. R.
1095. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030344000355 (2013). Supporting communication: Improving the expe-
Kennedy, M. R. T., Coelho, C., Turkstra, L., Ylvisaker, M., rience of living with aphasia. In N. Simmons-Mackie, J.
Moore Sohlberg, M., Yorkston, K.,. . . Kan, P.-F. (2008). M. King, & D. R. Beukelman (Eds.), Supporting commu-
Intervention for executive functions after traumatic nication for adults with acute and chronic aphasia (pp. 1–10).
brain injury: A systematic review, meta-analysis and Paul H. Brookes.
clinical recommendations. Neuropsychological Rehabil- Kinney, J. M., & Rentz, C. A. (2005). Observed well-be-
itation, 18(3), 257–299. https://doi.org/10.1080/​0960​2​ ing among individuals with dementia: Memories
010701748644 in the Making©, an art program, versus other struc-
Keren, G., & Willemsen, M. C. (2009). Decision anomalies, tured activity. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease
experimenter assumptions, and participants’ compre- and Other Dementias, 20(4), 220–227. https://doi.org/​
hension: Reevaluating the uncertainty effect. Journal of 10.1177/153331750502000406
Behavioral Decision Making, 22(3), 301–317. https://doi​ Kiran, S. (2008). Typicality of inanimate category exem-
.org/10.1002/bdm.628 plars in aphasia treatment: Further evidence for seman-
Kertesz, A. (2006). Western Aphasia Battery — Enhanced. tic complexity. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
Psychological Corporation. Research, 51(6), 1550–1568. https://doi.org/10.1044/​
Kertesz, A. (2007). Western Aphasia Battery — Revised. Har- 1092-4388(2008/07-0038)
court Assessment. Kiran, S., Caplan, D., Sandberg, C., Levy, J., Berardino,
Kessels, R. P. C., Bucks, R. S., Willison, J. R., & Byrne, L. A., Ascenso, E., . . . Tripodis, Y. (2012). Development
M. T. (2011). Location Learning Test — Revised. https://hdl​ of a theoretically based treatment for sentence com-
.handle.net/2066/99466 prehension deficits in individuals with aphasia.
Khayum, R., & Mooney, A. R. (2021). Primary progres- American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 21(2),
sive aphasia: A practical roadmap for navigating S88–S102. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2012/11
person-centered evaluation and treatment. In A. L. -0106)
Holland & R. J. Elman (Eds.), Neurogenic communication Kiran, S., & Johnson, L. (2008). Semantic complexity in
disorders and the life participation approach (pp. 131–158). treatment of naming deficits in aphasia: Evidence from
Plural Publishing. well-defined categories. American Journal of Speech-
Khazei, A., Jarvis-Selinger, S., Ho, K., & Lee, A. (2005). An Language Pathology, 17(4), 389–400. https://doi.org/​
assessment of the telehealth needs and health-care priori- 10.1044/1058-0360(2008/06-0085)
ties of Tanna Island: A remote, underserved and vulnera- Kiran, S., Sandberg, C., & Sebastian, R. (2011). Treatment
ble population. Journal of Telemedicine and Telecare, 11(1), of category generation and retrieval in aphasia: Effect
35–40. https://doi.org/10.1177/​1357​63​3X0501100108 of typicality of category items. Journal of Speech, Lan-
Kilov, A., Togher, L., & Grant, S. (2009). Problem solv- guage, and Hearing Research, 54(4), 1101–1117. https://
ing with friends: Discourse participation and per- doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2010/10-0117)
formance of individuals with and without traumatic Kiran, S., & Thompson, C. K. (2003). The role semantic
brain injury. Aphasiology, 23(5), 584–605. https://doi​ complexity in treatment of naming deficits: Training
.org/10.1080/02687030701855382 semantic categories in fluent aphasia by controlling
Kim, H., & Wright, H. H. (2020). Concurrent validity and exemplar typicality. Journal of Speech, Language, and
reliability of the core lexicon measure as a measure of Hearing Research, 46(4), 773–787.
word retrieval ability in aphasia narratives. American Kiresuk, T. J., & Sherman, R. E. (1968). Goal attainment
Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 29(1), 101–110. scaling: A general method for evaluating comprehen-
https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_AJSLP-19-0063 sive community mental health programs. Community
King, D., Brughelli, M., Hume, P., & Gissane, C. (2014). Mental Health Journal, 4(6), 443–453. https://doi.org/​
Assessment, management and knowledge of sport-​ 10.1007/BF01530764
542  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Kiresuk, T. J., Smith, A., & Cardillo, J. E. (2014). Goal attain- Analog Mood Scales. Clinical Rehabilitation, 26(12),
ment scaling: Applications, theory, and measurement. Psy- 1133–1140. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269215512442670
chology Press. Kopelman, M. D., Thomson, A. D., Guerrini, I., & Mar-
Kirmess, M., & Maher, L. M. (2010). Constraint induced shall, E. J. (2009). The Korsakoff syndrome: Clinical
language therapy in early aphasia rehabilitation. aspects, psychology and treatment. Alcohol and Alco-
Aphasiology, 24(6–8), 725–736. https://doi.org/10.1080/​ holism, 44(2), 148–154. https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/
02687030903437682 agn118
Kitwood, T. (1997). Dementia reconsidered: The person comes Kormanik, M., & Rocco, T. (2009). Internal versus external
first. Open University Press. control of reinforcement: A review of the locus of con-
Kitwood, T., & Bredin, K. (1994). Charting the course of trol construct. Human Resource Development Review, 8(4),
quality care. Journal of Dementia Care, 2(3), 22–23. 463–483. https://doi.org/10.1177/1534484309342080
Kleim, J. A., & Jones, T. A. (2008). Principles of experience-​ Kosky, C., & Schlisselberg, G. (2013). Oral communication
dependent neural plasticity: Implications for rehabili- skills in senior citizens: A community service model.
tation after brain damage. Journal of Speech, Language, SIG 10 Perspectives on Issues in Higher Education, 16,
and Hearing Research, 51(1), S225–239. https://doi.org/​ 28–38. https://doi.org/10.1044/ihe16.1.28
10.1044/1092-4388(2008/018) Koton, S., Tanne, D., Bornstein, N. M., & Green, M. S.
Klein, R., McNamara, P., & Albert, M. L. (2006). Neuro- (2004). Triggering risk factors for ischemic stroke: A
pharmacologic approaches to cognitive rehabilitation. case-crossover study. Neurology, 63(11), 2006–2010.
Behavioural Neurology, 17(1), 1–3. https://doi.org/​10​ https://doi.org/10.1212/01.WNL.0000145842.25520.A2
.1155/2006/298756 Kramer, A. F., Fabiani, M., & Colcombe, S. (2006). Contri-
Klein, R. B., & Albert, M. L. (2004). Can drug therapies butions of cognitive neuroscience to the understanding
improve language functions of individuals with apha- of behavior and aging. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie
sia? A review of the evidence. Seminars in Speech and (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (6th ed., pp. 57–
Language, 25(2), 193–204. https://doi.org/10.1055/​ 83). Academic Press.
s-2004-825655 Krasny-Pacini, A., Evans, J., Sohlberg, M. M., & Chevig-
Knollman-Porter, K. (2008). Acquired apraxia of speech: nard, M. (2016). Proposed criteria for appraising goal
A review. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 15(5), 484–493. attainment scales used as outcome measures in reha-
https://doi.org/10.1310/tsr1505-484 bilitation research. Archives of Physical Medicine and
Knowles, M. (1984). The adult learner: A neglected species Rehabilitation, 97(1), 157–170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j​
(3rd ed.). Gulf. .apmr.2015.08.424
Kolk, H. (1995). A time-based approach to agrammatic Krestel, H., Annoni, J.-M., & Jagella, C. (2013). White
production. Brain and Language, 50(3), 282–303. https:// matter in aphasia: A historical review of the Dejerines’
doi.org/10.1006/brln.1995.1049 studies. Brain and Language, 127(3), 526–532. https://
Kolk, H. H., & Heeschen, C. (1990). Adaptation symp- doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2013.05.019
toms and impairment symptoms in Broca’s aphasia. Krishnan, G., Tiwari, S., Pai, A. R., & Rao, S. N. (2012).
Aphasiology, 4, 221–231. https://doi.org/10.10​80/​0268​ Variability in aphasia following subcortical hemor-
7039008249075 rhagic lesion. Annals of Neurosciences, 19(4), 158–160.
Kolk, H. H., Van Grunsven, M. J. F., & Keyser, A. (1985). https://doi.org/10.5214/ans.0972.7531.190404
On parallelism between production and comprehen- Kristensson, J., & Saldert, C. (2018). Naming of objects and
sion in agrammatism. In M.-L. Kean (Ed.), Agramma- actions after treatment with phonological components
tism (pp. 165–206). Academic Press. analysis in aphasia. Clinical Archives of Communication
Kong, A. P.-H., Law, S.-P., & Chak, G. W.-C. (2017). A com- Disorders, 3(2), 137–150. https://doi.org/10.21849/cacd​
parison of coverbal gesture use in oral discourse among .2018.00367
speakers with fluent and nonfluent aphasia. Journal of Kucheria, P., Moore Sohlberg, M., Machalicek, W., Seeley,
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(7), 2031–2046. J., & DeGarmo, D. (2020). A single-case experimental
https://doi.org/10.1044/2017_JSLHR-L-16-0093 design investigation of collaborative goal setting prac-
Kontos, A. P., & Collins, M. W. (2018). Concussion moving tices in hospital-based speech-language pathologists
forward: What’s next? In A. P. Kontos & M. W. Collins, when provided supports to use motivational inter-
Concussion: A clinical profile approach to assessment and viewing and goal attainment scaling. Neuropsychological
treatment (pp. 175–188). American Psychological Asso- Rehabilitation, 1–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011​
ciation. https://doi.org/10.1037/0000087-009 .2020.1838301
Kontou, E., Thomas, S. A., & Lincoln, N. B. (2012). Psy- Kuljic-Obradovic, D. C. (2003). Subcortical aphasia: Three
chometric properties of a revised version of the Visual different language disorder syndromes? European
543
References  

Journal of Neurology, 10(4), 445–448. https://doi.org/​ for revised Bloom’s taxonomy by using deep learning.
10.1046/j.1468-1331.2003.00604.x International Journal of Engineering Trends and Technology,
Kuller, L. H., Shemanski, L., Psaty, B. M., Borhani, N. O., 69(3), 211–218. https://doi.org/10.14445/22315381/
Gardin, J., Haan, M. N., . . . Tracy, R. (1995). Subclinical IJETT-V69I3P232
disease as an independent risk factor for cardiovascu- LaFrance, C., Garcia, L. J., & Labreche, J. (2007). The effect
lar disease. Circulation, 92(4), 720–726. https://doi.org/​ of a therapy dog on the communication skills of an adult
10.1161/01.CIR.92.4.720 with aphasia. Journal of Communication Disorders, 40(3),
Kumari, J. (2020, November 24). India needs trained audiol- 215–224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2006.06.010
ogists and speech-language pathologists. The Times of Langlois, J. A., Rutland-Brown, W., & Wald, M. M. (2006).
India. https://timesofindia.indiatimes.com/home/ed The epidemiology and impact of traumatic brain injury:
ucation/news/india-needs-trained-audio ​ logy-and-spe A brief overview. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation,
ech-language-pathologists/articleshow/​79384098.cms 21(5), 357–378. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001199-2006​
Kurczek, J., & Duff, M. (2011). Cohesion, coherence, and 09000-00001
declarative memory: Discourse patterns in individuals Lanyon, L., & Rose, M. L. (2009). Do the hands have it?
with hippocampal amnesia. Aphasiology, 25(6–7), 6–7. The facilitation effects of arm and hand gesture on
https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2010.537345 word retrieval in aphasia. Aphasiology, 23(7–8), 809–822.
Kurland, J., Pulvermüller, F., Silva, N., Burke, K., & Andri- https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030802642044
anopoulos, M. (2012). Constrained versus uncon- LaPointe, L. L., & Eisenson, J. (2008). Examining for apha-
strained intensive language therapy in two individuals sia: Assessment of aphasia and related impairments–Fourth
with chronic, moderate-to-severe aphasia and apraxia edition (EFA-4). Pro-Ed.
of speech: Behavioral and fMRI outcomes. American LaPointe, L. L., & Horner, J. (2017). Reading Comprehension
Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 21(2), S65–S87. Battery for Aphasia-2. Pro-Ed.
https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2012/11-0113) Laska, A. C., Mårtensson, B., Kahan, T., von Arbin, M., &
Kurland, J., Stanek, E. J., Stokes, P., Li, M., & Andrianopou- Murray, V. (2007). Recognition of depression in aphasic
los, M. (2016). Intensive language action therapy in stroke patients. Cerebrovascular Diseases, 24(1), 74–79.
chronic aphasia: A randomized clinical trial examin- https://doi.org/10.1159/000103119
ing guidance by constraint. American Journal of Speech-​ Lasker, J. P., & Garrett, K. L. (2005). Multimodal communi-
Language Pathology, 25(4S). https://doi.org/10.1044/​ cation screening task for persons with aphasia: Booklet and
2016_AJSLP-15-0135 score sheet–revised https://cehs.unl.edu/documents/
Kurland, J., & Stokes, P. (2018). Let’s talk real talk: An secd/​aac/assessment/score.pdf
argument to include conversation in a D-COS for apha- Lasker, J. P., Stierwalt, A. G., Spence, M., & Calvin-Root, C.
sia research with an acknowledgment of the challenges (2010). Using webcam interactive technology to imple-
ahead. Aphasiology, 32(4), 475–478. https://doi.org/10​ ment treatment for severe apraxia: A case example.
.1080/02687038.2017.1398808 Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology, 18(4), 4–10.
Labouvie-Vief, G. (1984). Logic and self-regulation from Laughlin, S. A., Naeser, M. A., & Gordon, W. P. (1979).
youth to maturity: A model. In M. L. Commons & F. A. Effects of three syllable durations using the melodic
Richards (Eds.), Beyond formal operations. Praeger. intonation therapy technique. Journal of Speech and Hear-
Lacey, E. H., Lott, S. N., Snider, S. F., Sperling, A., & Fried- ing Research, 22(2), 311–320. https://doi.org/10.1044/
man, R. B. (2010). Multiple oral re-reading treatment jshr.2202.311
for alexia: The parts may be greater than the whole. Laures, J. S., & Shisler, R. J. (2004). Complementary and
Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 20(4), 601–623. https:// alternative medical approaches to treating adult neuro-
doi.org/10.1080/09602011003710993 genic communication disorders: A review. Disability and
Lacey, E. H., Lott, S. N., Sperling, A. J., Snider, S. F., & Fried- Rehabilitation, 26(6), 315–325. https://doi.org/10.1080/​
man, R. B. (2007). Multiple oral re-reading treatment for 0963828032000174106
alexia: It works, but why? Brain and Language, 103(1–2), Laures-Gore, J. S., Farina, M., Moore, E., & Russell, S. (2017).
115–116. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011003710993 Stress and depression scales in aphasia: Relation between
Lacombe, J., Jolicoeur, P., Grimault, S., Pineault, J., & Jou- the Aphasia Depression Rating Scale, Stroke Aphasia
bert, S. (2015). Neural changes associated with seman- Depression Questionnaire-10, and the Perceived Stress
tic processing in healthy aging despite intact behavioral Scale. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 24(2), 114–118.
performance. Brain and Language, 149, 118–127. https:// https://doi.org/10.1080/10749357.2016.1198528
doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2015.07.003 Laures-Gore, J., & Marshall, R. S. (2008). Acupuncture as a
Laddha, M. D., Lokare, V. T., Kiwelekar, A. W., & Netak, L. treatment technique for aphasia and cognitive impair-
D. (2021). Classifications of the summative assessment ments. Perspectives on Neurophysiology and Neurogenic
544  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Speech and Language Disorders, 18, 107–113. https://doi. Lee, R., Wong, J., Lit Shoon, W., Gandhi, M., Lei, F., Eh,
org/10.1044/nnsld18.3.107 K., Rawtaer, I., & Mahendran, R. (2019). Art therapy
Laureys, S., Owen, A. M., & Schiff, N. D. (2004). Brain func- for the prevention of cognitive decline. The Arts in
tion in coma, vegetative state, and related disorders. Psychotherapy, 64, 20–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j​
The Lancet Neurology, 3(9), 537–546. https://doi.org/​ .aip.2018.12.003
10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00852-X Lehman-Blake, M. T., & Tompkins, C. A. (2001). Pre-
Lavis, J. N., Guindon, G. E., Cameron, D., Boupha, B., dictive inferencing in adults with right hemisphere
Dejman, M., Osei, E. J. A., & Sadana, R. (2010). Bridging brain damage. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hear-
the gaps between research, policy and practice in low- ing Research, 44(3), 639–654. https://doi.org/10.1044/​
and middle-income countries: A survey of researchers. 1092-4388(2001/052)
Canadian Medical Association Journal, 182(9), E350–E361. Lemoncello, R., & Ness, B. (2013). Evidence-based practice
https://doi.org/10.1503/cmaj.081164 and practice-based evidence applied to adult, medical
Law, B. M. (2021). Like TBI, COVID may affect the brain speech-language pathology. Perspectives on Gerontology,
long-term. But why? The ASHA LeaderLive. https:// 18, 14. https://doi.org/10.1044/gero18.1.14
leader.pubs.asha.org/do/10.1044/leader.FTR2.2603​ Lemoncello, R., & Hoepner, J. (2020). Something sweet
2021.50/full/?utm_source=asha&utm_medium=enews​ . . . LPA one cupcake at a time. Aphasia Access Podcasts.
letter&utm_term=picks&utm_content=032321&utm_ https://www.aphasiaaccess.org/podcasts/
campaign=ashanow Lenneberg, E. (1967). Biological foundations of language.
Lawlor, E. F., Kreuter, M. W., Sebert-Kuhlmann, A. K., & Wiley.
McBride, T. D. (2015). Methodological innovations in Lenneberg, E. (1973). The neurology of language. Daedalus,
public health education: Transdisciplinary problem 102, 115–133. https://www.jstor.org/stable/20024149
solving. American Journal of Public Health, 105(Suppl. 1), Leonard, C., Laird, L., Burianová, H., Graham, S., Grady,
S99–S103. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2014.302462 C., Simic, T., & Rochon, E. (2015). Behavioural and neu-
Lawson, R., & Fawcus, M. (1999). Increasing effective com- ral changes after a “choice” therapy for naming defi-
munication using a total communication approach. In cits in aphasia: Preliminary findings. Aphasiology, 29(4),
S. Byng, K. Swinburn, & C. Pound (Eds.), The aphasia 506–525. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2014.971099
therapy file (pp. 61–71). Psychology Press. Leonard, C., Rochon, E., & Laird, L. (2008). Treating nam-
Laxe, S., Zasler, N., Tschiesner, U., López-Blazquez, R., ing impairments in aphasia: Findings from a phonolog-
Tormos, J. M., & Bernabeu, M. (2011). ICF use to iden- ical components analysis treatment. Aphasiology, 22(9),
tify common problems on a TBI neurorehabilitation 923–947. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030701831474
unit in Spain. NeuroRehabilitation, 29(1), 99–110. https:// Leppävuori, A., Pohjasvaara, T., Vataja, R., Kaste, M.,
doi.org/10.3233/NRE-2011-0683 & Erkinjuntti, T. (2003). Generalized anxiety disor-
Lê, K., Coelho, C., Mozeiko, J., Krueger, F., & Grafman, J. ders three to four months after ischemic stroke. Cere-
(2012). Predicting story goodness performance from brovascular Diseases, 16(3), 257–264. https://doi.org/​
cognitive measures following traumatic brain injury. 10.1159/000071125
American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 21(2), 115– Leritz, E. C., McGlinchey, R. E., Lundgren, K., Grande,
125. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2012/11-0114) L. J., & Milberg, W. P. (2008). Using lexical familiarity
Leach, E., Cornwell, P., Fleming, J., & Haines, T. (2010). judgments to assess verbally mediated intelligence in
Patient centered goal-setting in a subacute rehabilita- aphasia. Neuropsychology, 22(6), 687–696. https://doi​
tion setting. Disability and Rehabilitation, 32(2), 159–172. .org/10.1037/a0013319
https://doi.org/10.3109/09638280903036605 Levin, H. S., O’Donnell, V. M., & Grossman, R. G. (1979).
Leahy, M. M., McTiernan, K., Smith, M. M., Sloane, P., The Galveston orientation and amnesia test. Journal of
Walsh, I. P., Walshe, M., & Ni Cholmain, C. (2010). Nervous and Mental Disease, 167(11), 675–684.
Foundation studies in education for therapy practice: Lewin, S. A., Skea, Z. C., Entwistle, V., Zwarenstein, M., &
Curriculum updating. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, Dick, J. (2001). Interventions for providers to promote
62(5), 255–259. https://doi.org/10.1159/000314789 a patient-centred approach in clinical consultations.
Lee, J. E., Kaye, R. C., & Cherney, L. R. (2009). Conver- Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. https://doi​
sational script performance in adults with non-fluent .org/​10.1002/14651858.CD003267
aphasia: Treatment intensity and aphasia severity. Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Loring, D. W., Hannay, H.
Aphasiology, 23(7– 8), 885–897. https://doi.org/10.1080/​ J., & Fischer, J. S. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment
02687030802669534 (4th ed.). Oxford University Press.
Lee, L. L. (1971). Northwestern Syntax Screening Test. North- Li, E. C., Kitselman, K., Dusatko, D., & Spinelli, C. (1988).
western University Press. The efficacy of PACE in the remediation of naming defi-
545
References  

cits. Journal of Communication Disorders, 21(6), 491–503. aphasia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54(1),
https://doi.org/10.1016/0021-9924(88)90019-6 113–124. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.5401.113
Li, Y., Qu, Y., Yuan, M., & Du, T. (2015). Low-frequency Lorenzen, B., & Murray, L. L. (2008). Bilingual aphasia:
repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for patients A theoretical and clinical review. American Journal of
with aphasia after stoke: A meta-analysis. Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 17(3), 299–317. https://doi​
Rehabilitation Medicine, 47(8), 675–681. https://doi.org/​ .org/10.1044/1058-0360(2008/026)
10.2340/16501977-1988 Love, T., & Oster, E. (2002). On the categorization of apha-
Liang, J., Li, F., Wei, C., Song, H., Wu, L., Tang, Y., & sic typologies: The SOAP (a test of syntactic complex-
Jia, J. (2014). Rationale and design of a multicenter, ity). Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31(5), 503–529.
Phase 2 clinical trial to investigate the efficacy of tra- https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021208903394
ditional Chinese medicine SaiLuoTong in vascular Loverso, F. L., Prescott, T. E., & Selinger, M. (1988). Cueing
dementia. Journal of Stroke and Cerebrovascular Diseases, verbs: A treatment strategy for aphasic adults (CVT).
23(10), 2626–2634. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jstroke​ Journal of Rehabilitation Research and Development,
cerebrovasdis.2014.06.005 25(2), 47–60. https://www.rehab.research.va.gov/
Lin, Q., Lu, J., Chen, Z., Yan, J., Wang, H., Ouyang, H., . . . jour/88/25/2/pdf/loverso.pdf
O’Young, B. (2016). A survey of speech-language-hear- Loverso, F. L., Selinger, M., & Prescott, T. E. (1979). Appli-
ing therapists’ career situation and challenges in main- cation of verbing strategies to aphasia treatment. Clin-
land China. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 68(1), 10–15. ical Aphasiology, 9, 229–238. https://aphasiology.pitt​
https://doi.org/10.1159/000442284 .edu/archive/00000395/01/09-27.pdf
Lincoln, N. B., Sutcliffe, L. M., & Unsworth, G. (2000). Vali- Lowell, S., Beeson, P. M., & Holland, A. L. (1995). The effi-
dation of the Stroke Aphasic Depression Questionnaire cacy of a semantic cueing procedure on naming per-
(SADQ) for use with patients in hospital. Clinical Neu- formance of adults with aphasia. American Journal of
ropsychological Assessment, 1, 88–96. Speech-Language Pathology, 4, 109–114. https://doi.org/​
Lindsey, A., Hurley, E., Mozeiko, J., & Coelho, C. (2019). 10.1044/1058-0360.0404.109
Follow-up on the story goodness index for characteriz- Lowit, A., & Kent, R. D. (2011). Assessment of motor speech
ing discourse deficits following traumatic brain injury. disorders. Plural Publishing.
American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 28(1S), LPAA Project Group. (2000). Life participation approach to
330–340. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_AJSLP-17- aphasia: A statement of values for the future. https://www​
0151 .asha.org/Publications/leader/2000/000215/Life-
Linebarger, M. C., McCall, D., & Berndt, R. S. (2004). The Participation-Approach-to-Aphasia-A-Statement-of-
role of processing support in the remediation of apha- Values-for-the-Future.htm
sic language production disorders. Cognitive Neuro- Lubinski, R., Moscato, B. S., & Willer, B. S. (1997). Preva-
psychology, 21(2–4), 267–282. https://doi.org/​10.10​80/​ lence of speaking and hearing disabilities among adults
02643290342000537 with traumatic brain injury from a national house-
Linebaugh, C. (1983). Treatment of anomic aphasia. In C. hold survey. Brain Injury, 11(2), 103–114. https://doi​
Perkins (Ed.), Current therapies for communication disor- .org/10.1080/026990597123692
ders: Language handicaps in adults (pp. 181–189). Thieme- Luck, A. M., & Rose, M. L. (2007). Interviewing people
Stratton. with aphasia: Insights into method adjustments from
Linebaugh, C., & Lehner, L. (1977). Cueing hierarchies and a pilot study. Aphasiology, 21(2), 208–224. https://doi​
word retrieval: A therapy program. Clinical Aphasiology, .org/​10.1080/02687030601065470
7, 248–260. http://aphasiology.pitt.edu/622/ Luckowski, A. (2014). Patients with dementia: Caring for
Linebaugh, C. W., Shisler, R. J., & Lehner, L. (2005). Cue- horses lifts the spirits. Nursing, 44(7), 23–25. https://
ing hierarchies and word retrieval: A therapy program. doi.org/10.1097/01.NURSE.0000450789.10849.c5
Aphasiology, 19(1), 77–92. https://doi.org/10.1080/​0268​ Lucks Mendel, L., Mendel, M. I., & Battle, D. E. (2004).
7030444000363 Climbing the academic ladder. The ASHA Leader, 9,
Liu, Z., Zhang, Y., Yan, X., & Liu, J. (2015). Acupuncture for 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1044/leader.FTR1.09142004.1
stroke: An overview of systematic reviews. Integrative Lund, M. L., Tamm, M., & Branholm, I. (2001). Patients’
Medicine Research, 4(1), 10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j​ perceptions of their participation in rehabilitation
.imr.2015.04.311 planning and professionals’ view of their strategies to
Lomas, J., Pickard, L., Bester, S., Elbard, H., Finlayson, A., encourage it. Occupational Therapy International, 8(3),
& Zoghaib, C. (1989). The Communicative Effective- 151–167. https://doi.org/10.1002/oti.143
ness Index: Development and psychometric evalua- Lundgren, K. (2004). Complementary and alternative
tion of a functional communication measure for adult approaches to treating communication disorders. Sem-
546  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

inars in Speech and Language, 25(2), 119–120. https://doi​ Maguire, A. M., & Ogden, J. A. (2002). MRI brain scan
.org/10.1055/s-2004-825649 analyses and neuropsychological profiles of nine
Luria, A. R., & Hutton, J. T. (1977). A modern assessment of patients with persisting unilateral neglect. Neuropsycho-
the basic forms of aphasia. Brain and Language, 4(2), 129– logia, 40(7), 879–887. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-
151. https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(77)90012-8 3932(01)00169-5
Luterman, D. (2008). Counseling persons with communica- Mahadevan, S., & Park, Y. (2008). Multifaceted therapeu-
tion disorders and their families. Pro-Ed. tic benefits of Ginkgo biloba L.: Chemistry, efficacy,
Lyon, J. G. (1995). Drawing: Its value as a communication safety, and uses. Journal of Food Science, 73(1), R14–R19.
aid for adults with aphasia. Aphasiology, 9(1), 33–50. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-3841.2007.00597.x
https://doi.org/10.1080/02687039508248687 Mahendra, N., & Arkin, S. (2003). Effects of four years of
Lyon, J. G. (1996). Optimizing communication and partici- exercise, language, and social interventions on Alz-
pation in life for aphasic adults and their primary care- heimer discourse. Journal of Communication Disorders,
givers in natural settings: A model for treatment. In G. 36(5), 395–422. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0021-9924​(03)​
Wallace (Ed.), Adult aphasia rehabilitation (pp. 137–160). 00048-0
Butterworth-Heinemann. Mahendra, N., & Arkin, S. M. (2004). Exercise and vol-
Lyon, J. G. (1998). Coping with aphasia. Singular Publishing. unteer work: Contexts for AD language and memory
Lyon, J. G. (1999). A commentary on qualitative research interventions. Seminars in Speech and Language, 25(2),
in aphasia. Aphasiology, 13(9–11), 689–690. https://doi​ 151–167. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2004-825652
.org/10.1080/026870399401795 Mahendra, N., Bayles, K. A., & Harris, F. P. (2005). Effect of
Lyon, J. G., Cariski, D., Keisler, L., Rosenbek, J., Levine, presentation modality on immediate and delayed recall
R., Kumpula, J., & Blanc, M. (1997). Communication in individuals with Alzheimer’s disease. American Jour-
partners: Enhancing participation in life and communi- nal of Speech-Language Pathology, 14(2), 144–155. https://
cation for adults with aphasia in natural settings. Apha- doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2005/015)
siology, 11(7), 693–708. https://doi.org/10.1080/​02687​ Mahendra, N., Hopper, T., Bayles, K. A., Azuma, T.,
039708249416 Clearly, S., & Kim, E. (2006). Evidence-based practice
Lyon, J. G., & Helm-Estabrooks, N. (1987). Drawing: Its recommendations for working with individuals with
communicative significance for expressively restricted dementia: Montessori-based interventions. Journal of
aphasic adults. Topics in Language Disorders, 8(1), 61–71. Medical Speech-Language Pathology, 14(1), xv–xxvx.
https://doi.org/10.1097/00011363-198712000-00008 Maher, L. M., Kendall, D., Rodriguez, A., Pingel, K.,
Maas, E., Robin, D. A., Austermann Hula, S. N., Freedman, Swearengin, J. A., Leon, S. A., . . . Rothi, L. J. G. (2006).
S. E., Wulf, G., Ballard, K. J., & Schmidt, R. A. (2008). A pilot study of usedependent learning in the context
Principles of motor learning in treatment of motor of constraint induced language therapy. Journal of the
speech disorders. American Journal of Speech-Language International Neuropsychological Society, 12(6), 843–852.
Pathology, 17(3), 277–298. https://doi.org/10.1044/​ https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617706061029
1058-0360(2008/025) Makin, S. D., Doubal, F. N., Shuler, K., Chappell, F. M.,
Macauley, B. (2006). Animal-assisted therapy for persons Staals, J., Dennis, M. S., & Wardlaw, J. M. (2018). The
with aphasia: A pilot study. Journal of Rehabilitation impact of early-life intelligence quotient on post stroke
Research & Development, 43(3), 357–365. https://doi​ cognitive impairment. European Stroke Journal, 3(2),
.org/​10.1682/JRRD.2005.01.0027 145–156. https://doi.org/10.1177/2396987317750517
MacDonald, S. (2005). Functional Assessment of Verbal Rea- Malay Mail. (2016). UKM study shows Malaysia needs more
soning and Executive Strategies (FAVRES). CCD. speech therapists. https://www.malaymail.com/news/
Mack, J. E., & Thompson, C. K. (2017). Recovery of online malaysia/2016/07/12/ukm-study-shows-malaysia-
sentence processing in aphasia: Eye movement changes needs-more-speech-therapists/1160307
resulting from treatment of underlying forms. Journal of Malkmus, D., & Stenderup, K. (1974). Ranchos Los Amigos
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(5), 1299–1315. Cognitive Scale–Revised. Ranchos Los Amigos Hospital.
https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-16-0108 Manheim, L. M., Halper, A. S., & Cherney, L. (2009).
Maddy, K. M., Capilouto, G. J., & McComas, K. L. (2014). Patient-reported changes in communication after com-
The effectiveness of semantic feature analysis: An evi- puter-based script training for aphasia. Archives of Phys-
dence-based systematic review. Annals of Physical and ical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 90(4), 623–627. https://
Rehabilitation Medicine, 57(4), 254–267. https://doi.org/​ doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2008.10.022
10.1016/j.rehab.2014.03.002 Mansbach, W. E., MacDougall, E. E., & Rosenzweig, A. S.
Maggio, R. (1997). Talking about people: A guide to fair and (2012). The Brief Cognitive Assessment Tool (BCAT):
accurate language. Oryx Press. A new test emphasizing contextual memory, executive
547
References  

functions, attentional capacity, and the prediction of Marshall, R., & Mohapatra, B. (2017). Integrative interven-
instrumental activities of daily living. Journal of Clin- tion: A new perspective and brief review in aphasia.
ical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 34(2), 183–194. Disability & Rehabilitation, 39(19).
https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2011.630649 Marshall, R. C. (1983). Communication styles of fluent
Marcotte, K., Adrover-Roig, D., Damien, B., de Préaumont, aphasic clients. In H. Winitz (Ed.), Treatment of language
M., Généreux, S., Hubert, M., & Ansaldo, A. I. (2012). disorders (pp. 163–180). University Park Press.
Therapy-induced neuroplasticity in chronic aphasia. Marshall, R. C., & Freed, D. B. (2006). The personalized
Neuropsychologia, 50(8), 1776–1786. https://doi.org/​ cueing method: From the laboratory to the clinic. Amer-
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.04.001 ican Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 15(2), 103–111.
Marcotte, K., & Ansaldo, A. I. (2010). The neural correlates https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2006/011)
of semantic feature analysis in chronic aphasia: Dis- Marshall, R. C., Karow, C. M., Freed, D. B., & Babcock, P.
cordant patterns according to the etiology. Seminars in (2002). Effects of personalised cue form on the learning
Speech and Language, 31(1), 52–63. https://doi.org/​10​ of subordinate category names by aphasic and non-
.1055/s-0029-1244953 brain-damaged subjects. Aphasiology, 16(7), 763–771.
Marcotte, K., Laird, L., Bitan, T., Meltzer, J. A., Graham, S. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030244000040
J., Leonard, C., & Rochon, E. (2018). Therapy-induced Marshall, R. S., & Basilakos, A. (2014). Hot or not? A sur-
neuroplasticity in chronic aphasia after phonological vey regarding knowledge and use of complementary
component analysis: A matter of intensity. Frontiers in and alternative practices in speech-language pathol-
Neurology, 9, 225. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2018​ ogy. Contemporary Issues in Communication Sciences and
.00225 Disorders, 41, 235–251.
Marien, P., Engelborghs, S., Pickut, B. A., & De Deyn, P. Marshall, R. S., & Laures-Gore, J. (2008). What is com-
P. (2000). Aphasia following cerebellar damage: Fact plementary and alternative medicine? Perspectives on
or fallacy? Journal of Neurolinguistics, 13(2–3), 145–171. Neurophysiology and Neurogenic Speech and Language
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0911-6044(00)00009-9 Disorders, 18(3), 86. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984
Marquardt, K. (2015). Best social services jobs: Speech-language .12325
pathologist. jobs/speech-language-pathologist Marshall, R. S., Laures-Gore, J., DuBay, M., Williams, T.,
Marshall, C. R., Hardy, C. J. D., Volkmer, A., Russell, L. & Bryant, D. (2015). Unilateral forced nostril breath-
L., Bond, R. L., Fletcher, P. D. . . . Warren, J. D. (2018). ing and aphasia — Exploring unilateral forced nostril
Primary progressive aphasia: A clinical approach. Jour- breathing as an adjunct to aphasia treatment: A case
nal of Neurology, 265(6), 1474–1490. https://doi.org/​10​ series. Journal of Alternative and Complementary
.1007/s00415-018-8762-6 Medicine, 21(2), 91–99. https://doi.org/10.1089/acm.20
Marshall, J., Best, W., Cocks, N., Cruice, M., Pring, T., 13.0285
Bulcock, G., & Caute, A. (2012). Gesture and naming Marshall, R. S., Laures-Gore, J., & Love, K. (2018). Brief
therapy for people with severe aphasia: A group study. mindfulness meditation group training in aphasia:
Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 55(3), Exploring attention, language and psychophysiological
726–738. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2011/11- outcomes. International Journal of Language & Communi-
0219) cation Disorders, 53(1), 40–54. https://doi.org/10.1111/​
Marshall, J., Booth, T., Devane, N., Galliers, J., Green- 1460-6984.12325
wood, H., Hilari, K., . . . Woolf, C. (2016). Evaluating Marshall, R. S., & Mohapatra, B. (2017). Integrative inter-
the benefits of aphasia intervention delivered in vir- vention: A new perspective and brief review in aphasia.
tual reality: Results of a quasi-randomised study. PLOS Disability and Rehabilitation, 39(19), 1999–2009. https://
ONE, 11(8), e0160381. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal​ doi.org/10.1080/09638288.2016.1212283
.pone.0160381 Martin, A. D. (1977). Aphasia testing: A second look at
Marshall, J., Devane, N., Edmonds, L., Talbot, R., Wil- the Porch Index of Communicative Ability. Journal of
son, S., Woolf, C., & Zwart, N. (2018). Delivering word Speech and Hearing Disorders, 42(4), 547–562. https://doi​
retrieval therapies for people with aphasia in a vir- .org/10.1044/jshd.4204.547
tual communication environment. Aphasiology, 32(9), Martin, E. M., Lu, W. C., Helmick, K., French, L., & War-
1054–1074. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2018.148 den, D. L. (2008). Traumatic brain injuries sustained
8237 in the Afghanistan and Iraq wars. American Journal of
Marshall, J. F. (1984). Brain function: Neural adaptations Nursing, 108(4), 40–47. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.NAJ​
and recovery from injury. Annual Review of Psychol- .0000315260.92070.3f
ogy, 35, 277–308. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps​ Martin, N., Fink, R., & Laine, M. (2004). Treatment of word
.35.020184.001425 retrieval deficits with contextual priming. Aphasiology,
548  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

18(5–7), 457–471. https://doi.org/10.1080/026870304​ McAllister, S., Lincoln, M., Ferguson, A., & McAllister,
44000129 L. (2006). COMPASS: Competency assessment in speech
Martin, N., Fink, R. B., Renvall, K., & Laine, M. (2006). Effec- pathology. Speech Pathology Association of Australia.
tiveness of contextual repetition priming treatments for McAllister, S., Lincoln, M., Ferguson, A., & McAllister, L.
anomia depends on intact access to semantics. Journal (2011). A systematic program of research regarding the
of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12(6), 853– assessment of speech-language pathology competen-
866. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617706061030 cies. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology,
Martínez, A., Villarroel, V., Seoane, J., & del Pozo, F. (2004). 13(6), 469–479. https://doi.org/10.3109/17549507.2011
A study of a rural telemedicine system in the Amazon .580782
region of Peru. Journal of Telemedicine and Telecare, 10(4), McCann, C., Tunnicliffe, K., & Anderson, R. (2013). Pub-
219–225. https://doi.org/10.1258/1357633041424412 lic awareness of aphasia in New Zealand. Aphasiology,
Mashima, P., Waldron-Perrine, B., Seagly, K., Milman, 27(5), 568–580. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2012
L., Ashman, T., Mudar, R., & Paul, D. (2019). Looking .740553
beyond test results: Interprofessional collaborative McCarty, J., & Warren, S. (2017). A winning appeal: When
management of persistent mild traumatic brain in- insurers deny a claim, filing an appeal can result in pay-
jury symptoms. Topics in Language Disorders, 39(3), 293- ment for the clinician and improved coverage policies
312. in the future. The ASHA Leader, 22(6), 34–37. https://
Massaad, E., Shin, J. H., & Gibbs, W. N. (2021). The prog- doi.org/10.1044/leader.BML.22062017.34
nostic role of magnetic resonance imaging biomarkers McCauley, R. J., & Swisher, L. (1984). Psychometric review
in mild traumatic injury. JAMA Network Open, 4(3), of language and articulation tests for preschool chil-
e211824. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanet​work​open​ dren. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 49(1),
.2021.1824 34–42. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.4901.34
Massaro, M. E., & Tompkins, C. A. (1994). Feature analysis McCooey-O’Halloran, R., Worrall, L., Toffolo, D., Code, C.,
for treatment of communication disorders in traumat- & Hickson, L. (2004). The inpatient functional communica-
ically brain injured patients: An efficacy study. Clinical tion interview (IFCI). Speechmark.
Aphasiology, 22, 245–256. https://aphasiology.pitt.edu/ McCrory, P., Meeuwisse, W. H., Aubry, M., Cantu, B.,
archive/00000174/01/22-19.pdf Dvořák, J., Echemendia, & R. J., McCrea, M. (2013). Con-
Mathias, J. L., Bowden, S. C., Bigler, E. D., & Rosenfeld, J. sensus statement on concussion in sport: The 4th interna-
V. (2007). Is performance on the Wechsler test of adult tional conference on concussion in sport held in Zurich,
reading affected by traumatic brain injury? British Jour- November 2012. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 47(5),
nal of Clinical Psychology, 46(4), 457–466. https://doi​ 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2013-092313
.org/10.1348/014466507X190197 McGill, M., & Fiddler, K. (2021). A users guide for under-
Mathisen, B., Carey, L. B., Carey-Sargeant, C. L., Webb, standing and addressing telepractice technology chal-
G., Millar, C., & Krikheli, L. (2015). Religion, spiritu- lenges via ZOOM. Perspectives of the ASHA Special
ality, and speech-language pathology: A viewpoint Interest Groups, 6, 494-499.
for ensuring patient-centered holistic care. Journal of McIntosh, R. D., Brodie, E. E., Beschin, N., & Robertson, I.
Religion and Health, 54(6), 2309–2323. https://doi.org/​ H. (2000). Improving the clinical diagnosis of personal
10.1007/s10943-015-0001-1 neglect: A reformulated comb and razor test. Cortex,
Mathisen, B., & Threats, T. (2018). Speech pathology and 36(2), 289–292. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452​
spiritual care. In L. Carey & B. Mathisen (Eds.) Spir- (08)​70530-6
itual Care and Allied Health Practice: A Person-centered McKelvey, M., & Weissling, K. (2013). There is a continued
Approach, 22–54. Jessica Kingsley Publishers. need for empirical data supporting the treatment of peo-
Matuszek, S. (2010). Animal-facilitated therapy in vari- ple with aphasia in the acute care setting including the
ous patient populations: Systematic literature review. use of modified melodic intonation therapy. Evidence-​
Holistic Nursing Practice, 24(4), 187–203. https://doi​ Based Communication Assessment and Intervention, 7(2),
.org/10.1097/HNP.0b013e3181e90197 79–83. https://doi.org/10.1080/17489539.2013​.849934
Mavi, İ. (2007). Perspectives on public awareness of McNeil, M. R., & Kimelman, M. D. (2001). Darley and the
stroke and aphasia among Turkish patients in a neu- nature of aphasia: The defining and classifying con-
rology unit. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 21(1), 55–70. troversies. Aphasiology, 15(3), 221–229. https://doi.org/​
https://doi.org/10.1080/02699200600903254 10.1080/02687040042000223
Mayer, J. F., & Murray, L. L. (2002). Approaches to the treat- McNeil, M. R., & Pratt, S. R. (2001). Defining aphasia: Some
ment of alexia in chronic aphasia. Aphasiology, 16(7), theoretical and clinical implications of operating from a
727–743. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030143000870 formal definition. Aphasiology, 15(10–11), 901–911.
549
References  

McNeil, M. R., & Prescott, T. E. (1978). Revised Token Test. Mesulam, M.-M., Thompson, C. K., Weintraub, S., &
Pro-Ed. Rogalski, E. J. (2015). The Wernicke conundrum and
McNeil, M. R., Prescott, T. E., & Chang, E. (1975). A mea- the anatomy of language comprehension in primary
sure of PICA ordinality. Clinical Aphasiology, 3. http:// progressive aphasia. Brain, 138(8). https://doi.org/​10​
aphasiology.pitt.edu/491/ .1093/brain/awv154
McPherson, A., Furniss, F. G., Sdogati, C., Cesaroni, F., Mesulam, M.-M., & Weintraub, S. (2014). Is it time to
Tartaglini, B., & Lindesay, J. (2001). Effects of individ- revisit the classification guidelines for primary progres-
ualized memory aids on the conversation of persons sive aphasia? Neurology, 82(13), 1108–1109. https://doi​
with severe dementia: A pilot study. Aging & Mental .org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000000272
Health, 5(3), 289–294. https://doi.org/10.1080/​ 1360​ Meulenbroek, P., & Cherney, L. R. (2021). Computer-based
7860120064970 workplace communication training in persons with
McRae, K., Hare, M., Elman, J. L., & Ferretti, T. (2005). traumatic brain injury: The work-related communica-
A basis for generating expectancies for verbs from tion program. Journal of Communication Disorders, 91,
nouns. Memory & Cognition, 33(7), 1174–1184. https:// 106104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2021.106104
doi.org/10.3758/BF03193221 Meulenbroek, P., & Keegan, L. (2021). The life participa-
Meinzer, M., Djundja, D., Barthel, G., Elbert, T., & Rochstroh, tion approach and social reintegration after traumatic
B. (2005). Long-term stability of improved language brain injury. In A. L. Holland & R. J. Elman (Eds.), Neu-
functions in chronic aphasia after constraint-induced rogenic communication disorders and the life participation
aphasia therapy. Stroke, 36(7), 1462–1466. https://doi​ approach (pp. 181–207). Plural Publishing.
.org/10.1161/01.STR.0000169941.29831.2a Meyer, K., Kaplan, J. T., Essex, R., Webber, C., Damasio, H.,
Mella, N., Fagot, D., Lecerf, T., & Ribaupierre, A. (2015). & Damasio, A. (2010). Predicting visual stimuli on the
Working memory and intraindividual variability in basis of activity in auditory cortices. Nature Neurosci­
processing speed: A lifespan developmental and indi- ence, 13(6), 667–668. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2533
vidual differences study. Memory & Cognition, 43(3), Meyers, J., & Meyers, K. R. (1995). Rey Complex Figure Test
340–356. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0491-1 and Recognition Trial: Professional manual. Psychological
Meller, W., Sheehan, W., & Thurber, S. (2008). Phenom- Assessment Resources.
enology of coarse brain disease. In S. H. Fatemi & P. Meyerson, D. E., & Zuckerman, D. (2019). Identity theft:
J. Clayton (Eds.), The medical basis of psychiatry (pp. Rediscovering ourselves after stroke. Andrews McMeel
445–454). Humana Press. https://doi.org/10.1007/​ Publishing.
978-​1-59745-252-6_26 Michaelsen, L. K., Sweet, M., & Parmelee, D. (2008).
Meneilly, G. S., Cheung, E., Tessier, D., Yakura, C., & Tuo- Team-based learning: Small group learning’s next big step.
kko, H. (1993). The effect of improved glycemic control Jossey-Bass.
on cognitive functions in the elderly patient with dia- Michaelson, S., Rose, J. T., & May, A. E. (1967). Con-
betes. Journal of Gerontology, 48(4), M117–121. https:// trolling for “experimenter effect” in the psychometric
doi.org/10.1093/geronj/48.4.M117 assessment of brain damage. British Journal of Medical
Mentis, M., & Prutting, C. A. (1987). Cohesion in the dis- Psychology, 40(4), 371–374. https://doi.org/10.1111/​j​
course of normal and head injured adults. Journal of .2044-8341.1967.tb00586.x
Speech and Hearing Research, 30(1), 88–98. https://doi​ Mielczarek, E. V., & Engler, B. D. (2012). Measuring
.org/10.1044/jshr.3001.88 mythology: Startling concepts in NCCAM grants. Skep-
Meredith, H.H., & Yeates, G. N. (Eds), (2020). Psychother- tical Inquirer, 1, 34.
apy and Aphasia: Interventions for Emotional Wellbeing and Mihailidis, A., Blunsden, S., Boger, J., Richards, B., Zutis,
Relationships. Routledge & CRC Press K., Young, L., & Hoey, J. (2010). Towards the devel-
Mernoff, S. T., & Correia, S. (2010). Military blast injury in opment of a technology for art therapy and demen-
Iraq and Afghanistan: The Veterans Health Administra- tia: Definition of needs and design constraints. The
tion’s polytrauma system of care. Medicine and Health, Arts in Psychotherapy, 37(4), 293–300. https://doi.org/​
Rhode Island, 93(1), 16–18, 21. http://rimed.org/med​ 10.1016/j.aip.2010.05.004
health​ri/2010-01/2010-01-16.pdf Millis, B. J., & Cottell, P. G. (1998). Cooperative learning for
Mesulam, M.M., Coventry, C., Bigio, E. H., Geula, C., higher education faculty. The Oryx Press.
Thompson, C., Bonakdarpour, B., Gefen, T., Rogalski, Milman, L., & Holland, A. (2012). The Scales of Cognitive and
E. J., & Weintraub, S. (2021). Nosology of primary pro- Communicative Ability for Neurorehabilitation. Pro-Ed.
gressive aphasia and the neuropathology of language. Mirman, D., Yee, E., Blumstein, S. E., & Magnuson, J. S.
Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology, 1281, (2011). Theories of spoken word recognition deficits
33–49. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-51140-1_3 in aphasia: Evidence from eye-tracking and compu-
550  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

tational modeling. Brain and Language, 117(2), 53–68. tive function following TBI. Aphasiology, 25(6–7), 826–
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2011.01.004 835. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2010.543983
Mitchell, P. H. (2016). Nursing assessment of depression in Mueller, J., Kiernan, R., & Langston, J. W. (2014). Cognistat.
stroke survivors. Stroke, 47(1). https://doi.org/10.1161/ Psychology Press.
STROKEAHA.115.008362 Mulcair, G., Pietranton, A.A. & Williams, C. (2018) The
Mitrushina, M., Boone, K. B., Razani, J., & D’Elia, L. F. International Communication Project: Raising global
(2005). Handbook of normative data for neuropsychological awareness of communication as a human right. Interna-
assessment (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press. tional Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 20(1), 34-38,
Miyake, A., Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A. (1994). A capac- https://doi.org/10.1080/17549507.2018.1422023
ity approach to syntactic comprehension disorders: Mumby, K., Bowen, A., & Hesketh, A. (2007). Apraxia of
Making normal adults perform like aphasic patients. speech: How reliable are speech and language thera-
Cognitive Neuropsychology, 11(6), 671–717. https://doi​ pists’ diagnoses? Clinical Rehabilitation, 21(8), 760–767.
.org/10.1080/02643299408251989 https://doi.org/10.1177/0269215507077285
Moberg, P. J., & Rick, J. H. (2008). Decision-making capac- Munoz-Sandoval, A. F., Cummins, J., Alvarado, C. G., &
ity and competency in the elderly: A clinical and neu- Ruef, M. L. (1998). Bilingual Verbal Ability Tests. Riverside.
ropsychological perspective. NeuroRehabilitation, 23(5), Munoz-Sandoval, A. F., Cummins, J., Alvarado, C. G., &
403–413. Ruef, M. L. (2005). Bilingual Verbal Ability Tests (BVAT).
Monetta, L., Tremblay, T., & Joanette, Y. (2003). Semantic Nelson.
processing of words, cognitive resources and N400: Munyi, C. W. (2012). Past and present perceptions
An event-related potentials study. Brain and Cognition, towards disability: A historical perspective. Disability
53(2), 327–330. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626​(03)​ Studies Quarterly, 32(2). https://dsq-sds.org/article/
00136-2 view/3197/3068
Monte, S., Crielesi, M.A., di Peietra, M., M. Matera, K.H. Murphy, C. (2021). Murphys don’t quit: 5 Keys to unlocking
Meredith, & Muo, R. (2020). A preliminary study of hope when life seems hopeless. Morgan James Publishing.
“Laboratorio di Conversazione Narrativa”: Group Murphy, D., Lyons, R., Carroll, C., Caulfield, M., & de
psychotherapy supporting communication for people Paor, G. (2018). Communication as a human right: Cit-
with aphasia. In K.H. Meredith & G. N. Yeates (Eds), izenship, politics and the role of the speech-language
Psychotherapy and aphasia: Interventions for emotional pathologist. International Journal of Speech-Language
wellbeing and relationships (pp. 80–94). Routledge & CRC Pathology, 20, 16–20. https://doi.org/10.1080/1754950
Press 7.2018.1404129
Morais, L. H., Schreiber, H. L., & Mazmanian, S. K. (2020). Murphy, J., Alexander,J., McLinton,A. (2016) Talking
The gut microbiota-brain axis in behaviour and brain Mats: A model of communication training The Journal
disorders. Nature Reviews Microbiology, 19, 241–255. of Dementia Care 24(5) 22-25
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41579-020-00460-0 Murray, C. K., Reynolds, J. C., Schroeder, J. M., Harrison, M.
Morgan, A. L., & Helm-Estabrooks, N. (1987). Back to the B., Evans, O. M., & Hospenthal, D. R. (2005). Spectrum
drawing board: A treatment program for nonverbal apha- of care provided at an echelon II medical unit during
sic patients [Clinical aphasiology paper]. In Proceedings operation Iraqi freedom. Military Medicine, 170(6), 516–
of the Conference on Clinical Aphasiology [May 31–June 520. https://doi.org/10.7205/MILMED.170.6.516
4, Lake of the Ozarks, MO]. BRK Publishers. https:// Murray, J., Schneider, J., Banerjee, S., & Mann, A. (1999).
aphasiology.pitt.edu/archive/00000921/ Eurocare: A cross-national study of co-resident spouse
Morrow, K. L., & Fridriksson, J. (2006). Comparing fixed- carers for people with Alzheimer’s disease: II. A qual-
and randomized-interval spaced retrieval in anomia itative analysis of the experience of caregiving. Inter-
treatment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 39(1), national Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14(8), 662–667.
2–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2005.05.001 https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-1166(199908)​14:8​
Morrow-Odom, K. L., & Barnes, C. K. (2019). Mental <662::AID-GPS993>3.0.CO;2-4
health professionals’ experiences with aphasia. Journal Murray, L. L. (2004). Cognitive treatments for aphasia:
of Rehabilitation, 85(1), 15–21. Should we and can we help attention and working
Mortley, J., Enderby, P., & Petheram, B. (2001). Using a memory problems? Journal of Medical Speech-Language
computer to improve functional writing in a patient Pathology, 12(3), xxv–xi.
with severe dysgraphia. Aphasiology, 15(5), 443–461. Murray, L. L. (2012). Direct and indirect treatment ap-
https://doi.org/10.1080/02687040042000188 proaches for addressing short-term or working mem-
Mozeiko, J., Le, K., Coelho, C., Krueger, F., & Grafman, J. ory deficits in aphasia. Aphasiology, 26(3–4), 317–337.
(2011). The relationship of story grammar and execu- https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2011.589894
551
References  

Murray, L. L., & Clark, H. M. (2006). Neurogenic disorders ogy and Neuroscience, 28(4), 511–529. https://doi.org/​
of language: Theory driven clinical practice. Thomson Del- 10.3233/RNN-2010-0559
mar Learning. Nagre, A. (2018). Perioperative stroke — Prediction, pre-
Murray, L. L., & Ray, A. H. (2001). A comparison of relax- vention, and protection. Indian Journal of Anaesthesia,
ation training and syntax stimulation for chronic non- 62(10), 738. https://doi.org/10.4103/ija.IJA_292_18
fluent aphasia. Journal of Communication Disorders, 34(1), Nakase-Thompson, R. (2004). The Mississippi Aphasia
87–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0021-9924(00)00043-5 Screening Test. The Center for Outcome Measurement in
Murray, L. L., Timberlake, A., & Eberle, R. (2007). Treat- Brain Injury (COMBI). https://www.tbims.org/combi/
ment of underlying forms in a discourse context. Apha- mast/index.html
siology, 21(2), 139–163. https://doi.org/10.1080/​0268​7​ Nasreddine, Z. S. (2021). Montreal Cognitive Assessment
030601026530 (MoCA). https://www.mocatest.org/
Music and Memory. (2015). Music and memory. https:// National Center for Complementary and Integrative
musicandmemory.org/ Health. (2020). Ginkgo. https://www.nccih.nih.gov/
Musiek, F. E., Baran, J. A., & Shinn, J. (2004). Assessment health/​ginkgo
and remediation of an auditory processing disorder National Center for Complementary and Integrative
associated with head trauma. Journal of the American Health. (2021). Complementary, alternative, or integrative
Academy of Audiology, 15(2), 117–132. https://doi.org/​ health: What’s in a name? https://nccih.nih.gov/health/
10.3766/jaaa.15.2.3 integrative-health
Myburgh, J. A. (2009). Severe and multiple trauma. In A. National Center for Dissemination of Disability Research.
D. Bersten & N. Soni (Eds.), Oh’s intensive care manual (1999). Disability, diversity, and dissemination: A re-
(p. 771). Elsevier B.V. view of the literature on topics related to increasing the
Myers, P. J., Wilmington, D. J., Gallun, F. J., Henry, J. A., & utilization of rehabilitation research outcomes among
Fausti, S. A. (2009). Hearing impairment and traumatic diverse consumer groups. Research Exchange, 4(1), 1–74.
brain injury among soldiers: Special considerations National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke.
for the audiologist. Seminars in Hearing, 30(1), 5–27. (2015a). Frontotemporal dementia information page: National
https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0028-1111103 Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke. https://
Myers, P. S. (1999). Right hemisphere damage: Disorders of www.ninds.nih.gov/Disorders/All-Disorders/Fronto
cognition and communication. Singular Publishing. temporal-Dementia-Information-Page
Myers, P. S., & Blake, M. L. (2008). Communication disor- National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke.
ders associated with right hemisphere damage. In R. (2015b). Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease fact sheet. https://www​
Chapey (Ed.), Language intervention strategies in aphasia .ninds.nih.gov/disorders/cjd/detail_cjd.htm
and related neurogenic communication disorders (5th ed., National Institute on Aging, National Institute of Health,
pp. 963–987). Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. & U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
Myers, P. S., & Linebaugh, C. W. (1981). Comprehension (2004). 2003 progress report on Alzheimer’s disease:
of idiomatic expressions by right-hemisphere-damaged Research advances at NIH. https://www.alzheimers.org
adults. In R. H. Brookshire (Ed.), Clinical aphasiology: National Stroke Association. (2006). Recovery after stroke:
Conference proceedings. BRK. Coping with emotions. https://www.stroke.org/site/
Nadeau, S. E., & Crosson, B. (1997). Subcortical aphasia. DocServer/NSAFactSheet_Emotions.pdf?docID=990
Brain and Language, 58(3), 355–402; discussion 418–423. NBCC International. (2015). Professional counseling. https://
https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1997.1707 www.nbccinternational.org/Who_we_are/Profes​
Naeser, M. A., & Helm-Estabrooks, N. (1985). CT scan sional_Counseling
lesion localization and response to melodic intonation Ndi, A. (2012). Setting the stage of “Ab/normality” in
therapy with nonfluent aphasia cases. Cortex, 21(2), 203– rehabilitative narratives: Rethinking medicalization of
223. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(85)80027-7 the disabled African body. Disability Studies Quarterly,
Naeser, M. A., Martin, P. I., Nicholas, M., Baker, E. H., 32(2). https://dsq-sds.org/article/view/3195
Seekins, H., Kobayashi, M., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Neiman, M. R., Duffy, R. J., Belanger, S. A., & Coelho, C. A.
Improved picture naming in chronic aphasia after TMS (1994). Concurrent validity of the Kaufman hand move-
to part of right Broca’s area: An open protocol study. ment test as a measure of limb apraxia. Perceptual and
Brain and Language, 93(1), 95–105. https://doi.org/​10​ Motor Skills, 79(3), 1279–1282. https://doi.org/10.2466/
.1016/j.bandl.2004.08.004 pms.1994.79.3.1279
Naeser, M. A., Martin, P. I., Treglia, E., Ho, M., Kaplan, E., Neiman, M. R., Duffy, R. J., Belanger, S. A., & Coelho, C.
Bashir, S., . . . Pascual-Leone, A. (2010). Research with A. (2000). The assessment of limb apraxia: Relationship
rTMS in the treatment of aphasia. Restorative Neurol- between performances on single-and multiple-object
552  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

tasks by left hemisphere damaged aphasic subjects. Nickels, L., & Best, W. (1996). Therapy for naming disor-
Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 10(4), 429–448. https:// ders (Part I): Principles, puzzles and progress. Apha-
doi.org/10.1080/096020100412005 siology, 10(1), 21–47. https://doi.org/10.1080/​0268​703​
Nelson, T. D. (2008). The young science of prejudice against 9608248397
older adults: Established answers and open questions Nickels, L., Byng, S., & Black, M. (1991). Sentence process-
about ageism. In E. Borgida & S. T. Fiske (Eds.), Beyond ing deficits: A replication of therapy. The British Journal
common sense (pp. 45–61). Blackwell. https://online​ of Disorders of Communication, 26(2), 175–199. https://
library.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/9780470696422.ch3/ doi.org/10.3109/13682829109012002
summary Nicolo, P., Rizk, S., Magnin, C., Pietro, M. D., Schnider,
NetQues Project Management Team. (2014). NetQues. A., & Guggisberg, A. G. (2015). Coherent neural oscilla-
https://www.netques.eu/ tions predict future motor and language improvement
Netral News. (2018). Indonesia needs more speech therapists. after stroke. Brain, 138(10), 3048–3060. https://doi.org/​
https://www.en.netralnews.com/news/health/read/​ 10.1093/brain/awv200
20699/indonesia.needs.more.speech.therapists Niemann, H., Ruff, R. M., & Kramer, J. H. (1996). An
Neuberger, J., & Tallis, R. (1999). We do need a new attempt towards differentiating attentional deficits in
word for patients? British Medical Journal, 318(7200), traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology Review, 6(1),
1756–1756. 11–46. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01875418
Neumann Y. (2018). A case series comparison of semanti- Njemanze, P. C. (2003). Crossed aphasia in a dextral with
cally focused vs. phonologically focused cued naming right hemispheric lesion: A functional transcranial
treatment in aphasia. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, Doppler study. Stroke, 34(11), 213–214. https://doi.org/​
32(1), 1–27. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699206.2017.13 10.1161/01.STR.0000099064.02408.D9
26166 Nobleza, S. O. C., Smeer, S., Sansing, L., Silaghi, D. A.,
Newhoff, M., Bugbee, J. K., & Ferreira, A. (1981). A change Tarasaria, K., & Jasne, A. S. (2021). COVID-19 and
of PACE: Spouses as treatment targets. Clinical Apha- stroke. Practical Neurology, 743, 48–52. https://practical​
siology, 11, 234–243. https://aphasiology.pitt.edu/ neurology.com/articles
archive/00000647/ Northcott, S., & Hilari, K. (2011). Why do people lose their
Newman, C. W., & Weinstein, B. E. (1986). Judgments friends after a stroke? International Journal of Language
of perceived hearing handicap by hearing-impaired & Communication Disorders, 46(5), 524–534. https://doi​
elderly men and their spouses. Journal of Academic Reha- .org/10.1111/j.1460-6984.2011.00079.x
bilitative Audiology, 19, 109–115. Northcott, S., Simpson, A., Moss, B., Ahmed, N., & Hilari,
Nicholas, L. E., & Brookshire, R. H. (1993). A system for K. (2017). How do speech-and-language therapists
quantifying the informativeness and efficiency of the address the psychosocial well-being of people with
connected speech of adults with aphasia. Journal of aphasia? Results of a UK online survey. International
Speech and Hearing Research, 36(2), 338–350. https://doi​ Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 52(3),
.org/10.1044/jshr.3602.338 356–373. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12278
Nicholas, L. E., & Brookshire, R. H. (1995). Performance Northcott, S., Simpson, A., Moss, B., Ahmed, N., & Hilari,
deviations in the connected speech of non-brain- K. (2018). Supporting people with aphasia to “settle
damaged and aphasic adults. American Journal of Speech- into a new way to be”: Speech and language therapists’
Language Pathology, 4(4), 118–123. https://aphasiology​ views on providing psychosocial support. International
.pitt.edu/archive/00000287/ Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 53(1),
Nicholas, L. E., MacLennan, D. L., & Brookshire, R. H. 16–29. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12323
(1986). Validity of multiplesentence reading com- Norton, A., Zipse, L., Marchina, S., & Schlaug, G. (2009).
prehension tests for aphasic adults. Journal of Speech Melodic intonation therapy: Shared insights on how it
and Hearing Disorders, 51(1), 82–87. https://doi.org/​ is done and why it might help. Annals of the New York
10.1044/jshd.5101.82 Academy of Sciences, 1169, 431–436. https://doi.org/​
Nicholas, M., Sinotte, M. P., & Helm-Estabrooks, N. (2011). 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04859.x
C-Speak Aphasia alternative communication program Novack, T. A., Caldwell, S. G., Duke, L. W., Bergquist, T.
for people with severe aphasia: Importance of execu- F., & Gage, R. J. (1996). Focused versus unstructured
tive functioning and semantic knowledge. Neuropsycho- intervention for attention deficits after traumatic brain
logical Rehabilitation, 21(3), 322–366. injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 11(3).
Nickels, L. (2002). Therapy for naming disorders: Revis- https://doi.org/10.1097/00001199-199606000-00008
iting, revising, and reviewing. Aphasiology, 16(10/11), Nozari, N., & Dell, G. (2013). How damaged brains
935–979. repeat words: A computational approach. Brain and
553
References  

Language, 126(3), 327–337. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. internal strategies as a memory compensation tech-


bandl.2013.07.005 nique after brain injury: A systematic review. Journal
Obler, L. K., & Albert, M. L. (1979). Action Naming Test of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 31(4), E1–E11. https://doi​
(experimental edition). Boston VA Medical Center. .org/10.1097/HTR.0000000000000181
Odekar, A., & Hallowell, B. (2005). Comparison of alterna- Onslow, M. (2008). Eternity and clinical translation of
tives to multidimensional scoring in the assessment of speech-language pathology research. International Jour-
language comprehension in aphasia. American Journal nal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10(3), 118–126. https://
of Speech-Language Pathology, 14(4), 337–345. https:// doi.org/10.1080/17549500801891632
doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2005/032) Orange, J. B., & Colton-Hudson, A. (1998). Enhancing
Odekar, A., Hallowell, B., Kruse, H., Moates, D., & Lee, communication in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type.
C.-Y. (2009). Validity of eye movement methods and Topics in Geriatric Rehabilitation, 14(2), 56–75. https://
indices for capturing semantic (associative) priming doi.org/10.1097/00013614-199812000-00007
effects. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, Orange, J. B., Ryan, E. B., Meredith, S. D., & MacLean, M.
52(1), 31–48. https://doi.org/10.1044/​1092-4388​(2008/​ J. (1995). Application of the communication enhance-
07-0100) ment model for long-term care residents with Alzhei-
O’Donnell, M. J., Chin, S. L., Rangarajan, S., Xavier, D., Liu, mer’s disease. Topics in Language Disorders, 15(2), 20–35.
L., Zhang, H., . . . Yusuf, S. (2016). Global and regional https://doi.org/10.1097/00011363-199502000-00004
effects of potentially modifiable risk factors associated Oren, S., Willerton, C., & Small, J. (2014). Effects of spaced
with acute stroke in 32 countries (INTERSTROKE): retrieval training on semantic memory in Alzheimer’s
A case-control study. The Lancet, 388(10046), 761–775. disease: A systematic review. Journal of Speech, Language
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(16)30506-2 and Hearing Research, 57(1), 247–270. https://doi​.org/​
O’Donnell, M. J., Xavier, D., Liu, L., Zhang, H., Chin, S. 10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0352)
L., Rao-Melacini, P., . . . INTERSTROKE investigators. Orenstein, E., Basilakos, A., & Marshall, R. S. (2012).
(2010). Risk factors for ischaemic and intracerebral hae- Effects of mindfulness meditation on three individuals
morrhagic stroke in 22 countries (the INTERSTROKE with aphasia. International Journal of Language & Com-
study): A case-control study. Lancet (London, England), munication Disorders, 6, 673. https://doi.org/10.1111/​
376(9735), 112–123. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140- j.1460-6984.2012.00173.x
6736​(10)60834-3 Orjada, S. A., & Beeson, P. M. (2005). Concurrent treatment
Oelschlaeger, M. L., & Thorne, J. C. (1999). Application for reading and spelling in aphasia. Aphasiology, 19(3–5),
of the correct information unit analysis to the natu- 341–351. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030444000796
rally occurring conversation of a person with aphasia. Orozco, G., Lee, W. M. L., Blando, J., & Shooshani, B.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42(3), (2014). Introduction to multicultural counseling for helping
636–648. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4203.636 professionals (3rd ed.). Routledge.
Office of the New York Attorney General. (2015). A.G. Orsulic-Jeras, S., Schneider, N., & Camp, C. (2000). Special
Schneiderman asks major retailers to halt sales of certain feature: Montessori-based activities for long-term care
herbal supplements as DNA tests fail to detect plant mate- residents with dementia. Topics in Geriatric Rehabilita-
rials listed on majority of products tested. https://ag.ny tion, 16(1), 78–91. https://doi.org/10.1097/​0001​3614-
.gov/press-release/2015/ag-schneiderman-asks-major- 200009000-00009
retailers-halt-sales-certain-herbal-supplements-dna Orsulic-Jeras, S., Schneider, N. M., Camp, C. J., Nicholson,
O’Halloran, R., Carragher, M., & Foster, A. (2017). The P., & Helbig, M. (2001). Montessori-based dementia
consequences of the consequences: The impact of the activities in long-term care: Training and implemen-
environment on people with aphasia over time. Topics tation. Activities, Adaptation & Aging, 25(3–4), 107–120.
in Language Disorders, 1. https://doi.org/10.1097/TLD​ https://doi.org/10.1300/J016v25n03_08
.0000000000000109 O’Sullivan, P., Chao, S., Russell, M., Levine, S., & Fabiny,
O’Halloran, R., & Larkins, B. (2008). The ICF activities and A. (2008). Development and implementation of an
participation related to speech-language pathology. objective structured clinical examination to provide
International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10(1– formative feedback on communication and interper-
2), 18–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/14417040701772620 sonal skills in geriatric training. Journal of the American
Okie, S. (2005). Traumatic brain injury in the war zone. Geriatrics Society, 56(9), 1730–1735. https://doi.org/​
New England Journal of Medicine, 352(20), 2043–2047. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01860.x
https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMp058102 Otal, B., Olma, M. C., Flöel, A., & Wellwood, I. (2015).
O’Neil-Pirozzi, T. M., Kennedy, M. R. T., & Sohlberg, Inhibitory non-invasive brain stimulation to homol-
M. M. (2016). Evidence-based practice for the use of ogous language regions as an adjunct to speech and
554  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

language therapy in post-stroke aphasia: A meta-anal- practice. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 38(3), 460–471.
ysis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 236. https://doi​ https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-015-9618-5
.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00236 Park, C. L., Lechner, S. C., Antoni, M. H., & Stanton, A. L.
Overton, W. F. (2010). Life-span development: Concepts (Eds.). (2008). Medical illness and positive life change: Can
and issues. In W. F. Overton (Ed.), The handbook of life- crisis lead to personal transformation? American Psycho-
span development, cognition, biology, and methods (Vol. 1, logical Association.
pp. 1–29). John Wiley & Sons. Parker Oliver, D., Patil, S., Benson, J. J., Gage, A., Wash-
Owolabi, L. F., & Yakasai, M. M. (2012). Stroke-related ington, K., Kruse, R. L., & Demiris, G. (2017). The effect
Wernicke’s aphasia mistaken for psychosis: A case of internet group support for caregivers on social sup-
report. Journal of Medicine in the Tropics, 14(1), 83–85. port, self-efficacy, and caregiver burden: A meta-analy-
Owsley, C., & Sloane, M. E. (1990). Vision and aging. In R. sis. Telemedicine and E-Health, 23(8), 621–629. https://doi
D. Nebes & S. Corkin (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychol- .org/​10.1089/tmj.2016.0183
ogy (Vol. 4, pp. 229–249). Elsevier Science. Parr, S. (2007). Living with severe aphasia: Tracking social
Pachet, A., Aster, K., & Brown, L. (2010). Clinical utility exclusion. Aphasiology, 21(1), 98–123. https://doi.org/​
of the Mini-Mental Status examination when assessing 10.1080/02687030600798337
decision-making capacity. Journal of Geriatric Psychia- Parr, S., Byng, S., Gilpin, S., & Ireland, C. (1997). Talking
try and Neurology, 23(1), 3–8. https://doi.org/​10.1177/​ about aphasia: Living with loss of language after stroke.
0891988709342727 Open University Press.
Paciaroni, M., & Bogousslavsky, J. (2011). Jules Joseph Pascoe, M., Klop, D., Mdlalo, T., & Ndhambi, M. (2018).
Déjerine versus Pierre Marie. Frontiers of Neurology and Beyond lip service: Towards human rights-driven
Neuroscience, 29, 162–169. https://doi.org/10.1159/​0003​ guidelines for South African speech-language patholo-
21784 gists. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology,
Palmer, A. D., Newsom, J. T., & Rook, K. S. (2016). How 20(1), 67–74. https://doi.org/10.1080/17549507.2018.13​
does difficulty communicating affect the social relation- 97745
ships of older adults? An exploration using data from a Pataraia, E., Simos, P. G., Castillo, E. M., Billingsley-Marshall,
national survey. Journal of Communication Disorders, 62, R. L., McGregor, A. L., Breier, J. I., . . . Papanicolaou, A.
131–146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2016.06.002 C. (2004). Reorganization of language-specific cortex in
Palmer, R., & Patterson, G. (2011). One size does not fit all: patients with lesions or mesial temporal epilepsy. Neu-
Obtaining informed consent from people with apha- rology, 63(10), 1825–1832. https://doi.org/​10.1212/01​
sia. Advances in Clinical Neuroscience and Rehabilitation, .WNL.0000144180.85779.9A
11(2), 30–31. https://www.acnr.co.uk/MJ11/30_ACN​ Patterson, A., Hansson, K., Lowit, A., Stansfield, J, & Trin-
RMJ11_rehab.pdf ite, B. (2015). EU collaboration in speech and language
Pang, Y., Wu, L.-B., & Liu, D.-H. (2010). Acupuncture ther- therapy education: The NetQues project. Perspectives
apy for apoplectic aphasia: A systematic review. Chinese on Global Issues in Communication Sciences and Related
Acupuncture & Moxibustion, 30(7), 612–616. Disorders, 5(1), 21–32. https://doi.org/10.1044/gics5​
Papadopoulos, K., Paralikas, T., Barouti, M., & Chro- .1.21
nopoulou, E. (2014). Self-esteem, locus of control and Patterson, J. P. (2002). Post-stroke depression in persons
various aspects of psychopathology of adults with with chronic aphasia. Perspectives on Gerontology, 7(2),
visual impairments. International Journal of Disability, 5–9. https://doi.org/10.1044/gero7.2.5
Development and Education, 61(4), 403–415. https://doi​ Patterson, K., & Howard, D. (1992). Pyramids and Palm
.org/10.1080/1034912X.2014.955785 Trees Test. Pearson.
Pape, T. L.-B., Jaffe, N. O., Savage, T., Collins, E., & War- Patterson, R., Robert, A., Berry, R., Cain, M., Rochon, E.,
den, D. (2004). Unresolved legal and ethical issues in Iqbal, M., & Leonard, C. (2012). Public awareness of
research of adults with severe traumatic brain injury: aphasia in southern Ontario: A survey. Stroke, 43(11),
Analysis of an ongoing protocol. Journal of Rehabilita- E151.
tion Research and Development, 41, 155–174. https://doi​ Paul, D., Frattali, C., Holland, A., Thompson, C., Caper-
.org/10.1682/JRRD.2004.02.0155 ton, C., & Slater, S. (2004). Quality of Communication Life
Paratz, E. D. (2011). The significance of aphasia in neuro- Scale. ASHA.
logical cancers. Australian Medical Student Journal, 2(1), Pauranik, A., George, A., Sahu, A., Nehra, A., Paplikar,
15–18. https://www.amsj.org/archives/868 A., Bhat, C., . . . Faroqi-Shah, Y. (2019). Expert group
Park, C. L., Braun, T., & Siegel, T. (2015). Who practices meeting on aphasia: A report. Annals of Indian Academy
yoga? A systematic review of demographic, health- of Neurology, 22(2), 137. https://doi.org/10.4103/aian​
related, and psychosocial factors associated with yoga .AIAN_330_18
555
References  

Paxton, J. L., Barch, D., Storandt, M., & Braver, T. S. Perlmuter, L. C., Tun, P., Sizer, N., McGlinchey, R. E., &
(2006). Effects of environmental support and strategy Nathan, D. M. (1987). Age and diabetes related changes
training on older adults’ use of context. Psychology and in verbal fluency. Experimental Aging Research, 13(1–2),
Aging, 21(3), 499–509. https://doi.org/​10.1037/​0882- 9–14. https://doi.org/10.1080/03610738708259294
7974.21.3.499 Perry, A., Morris, M., Unsworth, C., Duckett, S., Skeat, J.,
Payne, J. C. (2014). Adult neurogenic language disorders: Dodd, K., . . . Reilly, K. (2004). Therapy outcome mea-
Assessment and treatment. A comprehensive ethnobiological sures for allied health practitioners in Australia: The
approach. Plural Publishing. AusTOMs. International Journal for Quality in Health
Payne, J. C. (2015). Supporting family caregivers of adults Care, 16(4), 285–291.
with communication disorders: A resource guide for speech- Piaget, J. (1936). La naissance de l’intelligence chez l’enfant.
language pathologists and audiologists. Plural Publishing. Delachaux et Nieslé.
Pazzaglia, M., Smania, N., Corato, E., & Aglioti, S. M. Pick, A. (1931). In the handbuch der normalen und patholo-
(2008). Neural underpinnings of gesture discrimina- gischen physiologie (Vol. 15). Springer-Verlag.
tion in patients with limb apraxia. Journal of Neuro- Pierce, J. E., Menahemi-Falkov, M., O’Halloran, R., Togher,
science, 28(12), 3030–3041. https://doi.org/10.1523/J​ L., & Rose, M. L. (2019). Constraint and multimodal
NEUROSCI.5748-07.2008 approaches to therapy for chronic aphasia: A system-
Peach, R. K. (2008). Global aphasia: Identification and atic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychological Reha-
management. In R. Chapey (Ed.), Language intervention bilitation, 29(7), 1005–1041. https://www.tandfonline​
strategies in adult aphasia and related neurogenic commu- .com/doi/abs/10.1080/09602011.2017.1365730?journal​
nication disorders (5th ed., pp. 565–594). Lippincott Wil- Code=pnrh20
liams & Wilkins. Pillay, M., Tiwari, R., Kathard, H., & Chikte, U. (2020).
Peach, R. K. (2013). Cognitive basis for sentence planning Sustainable workforce: South African audiologists and
difficulties in discourse after traumatic brain injury. speech therapists. Human Resources for Health, 18(1), 47.
American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 22(2), https://doi.org/10.1186/s12960-020-00488-6
S285– S297. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360​(2013/​ Pimental, P., & Knight, J. (2000). MIRBI-2: Mini inventory of
12-0081) right brain injury–Second edition. Pro-Ed.
Peach, R. K., & Reuter, K. A. (2010). A discourse-based Pitt, R., Theodoros, D., Hill, A. J., & Russell, T. (2019). The
approach to semantic feature analysis for the treatment impact of the telerehabilitation group aphasia interven-
of aphasic word retrieval failures. Aphasiology, 24(9), tion and networking programme on communication,
971–990. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030903058629 participation, and quality of life in people with aphasia.
Pedersen, P. M., Jorgensen, H. S., Nakayama, H., Raaschou, International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 21(5),
H. O., & Olsen, T. S. (1997). Comprehensive assessment 513–523. https://doi.org/10.1080/17549507.2018.1488​
of activities of daily living in stroke. Archives of Physical 990
Medicine and Rehabilitation, 78(2), 161–165. https://doi​ Pitts, L. L., Hurwitz, R., Lee, J. B., Carpenter, J., & Cher-
.org/10.1016/S0003-9993(97)90258-6 ney, L. R. (2018). Validity, reliability and sensitivity of
Peluso, S., De Rosa, A., De Lucia, N., Antenora, A., Illario, the NORLA-6: Naming and oral reading for language
M., Esposito, M., & De Michele, G. (2018). Animal-​ in aphasia 6-point scale. International Journal of Speech-
assisted therapy in elderly patients: Evidence and con- Language Pathology, 20(2), 274–283. https://doi.org/10​
troversies in dementia and psychiatric disorders and .1080/17549507.2016.1276962
future perspectives in other neurological diseases. Jour- Plassman, B. L., Havlik, R. J., Steffens, D. C., Helms, M. J.,
nal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 31(3), 149–157. Newman, T. N., Drosdick, D., . . . Breitner, J. C. (2000).
https://doi.org/10.1177/0891988718774634 Documented head injury in early adulthood and risk
Penfield, W., & Roberts, L. (1959). Speech and brain mecha- of Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias. Neurology,
nisms. Princeton University Press. 55(8), 1158–1166.
Penn, C. (1988). The profiling of syntax and pragmatics Poeck, K., & Pietron, H.-P. (1981). The influence of stretched
in aphasia. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 2(3), 179. speech presentation on token test performance of apha-
https://doi.org/10.1080/02699208808985255 sic and right brain damaged patients. Neuropsychologia,
Penn, C., Frankel, T., Watermeyer, J., & Müller, M. (2009). 19(1), 133–136. https://doi.org/10.1016/​0028-3932​(81)​
Informed consent and aphasia: Evidence of pitfalls in 90052-X
the process. Aphasiology, 23(1), 3–32. https://doi.org/​ Politis, A. (2014). Breaking with tradition: A paradigm
10.1080/02687030701521786 shift in cognitive rehabilitation. Perspectives on Neuro-
Peri, K., Kerse, N., & Halliwell, J. (2004). Goalsetting for physiology and Neurogenic Speech and Language Disorders,
older people: A literature review and synthesis. UniServices. 24(1), 4–9. https://doi.org/10.1044/nnsld24.1.4
556  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Pollens, R. (2004). Role of the speech-language pathologist Power, G. A. (2010). Dementia beyond drugs: Changing the
in palliative hospice care. Journal of Palliative Medicine, culture of care. Health Professions Press.
7(5), 694–702. https://doi.org/10.1089/jpm.2004.7.694 Power, G. A. (2014). Dementia beyond disease: Enhancing
Polovoy, C. (2014). From silence to a “din of interaction.” well-being. Health Professions Press.
The ASHA Leader, 19, 20–21. https://doi.org/10.1044/ Prescott, T. E., Selinger, M., & Loverso, F. L. (1982). An
leader.LML.19102014.20 analysis of learning generalization and maintenance of
Ponsford, J. L., & Kinsella, G. (1988). Evaluation of a verbs by an aphasic patient. Clinical Aphasiology, 12, 178–
remedial programme for attentional deficits following 182. https://aphasiology.pitt.edu/archive/00000724/
closed-head injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimen- Prins, M., Veerbeek, M., Willemse, B. M., & Pot, A. M.
tal Neuropsychology, 10(6), 693–708. https://doi.org/​ (2019). Use and impact of the Alzheimer Experience:
10.1080/01688638808402808 A free online media production to raise public aware-
Poole, A. (2012). Which are more legible: Serif or sans serif ness and enhance knowledge and understanding of
typefaces? https://alexpoole.info/blog/which-are- dementia. Aging & Mental Health, 1–8. https://doi.org/​
more-legible-serif-or-sans-serif-typefaces/ 10.1080/13607863.2019.1579781
Popovici, M., & Mihăilescu, L. (1992). Melodic intonation Prosser, M., & Sze, D. (2014). Problem-based learning: Stu-
in the rehabilitation of Romanian aphasics with bucco-​ dent learning experiences and outcomes. Clinical Lin-
lingual apraxia. Romanian Journal of Neurology and Psy- guistics & Phonetics, 28(1/2), 112–123. https://doi.org/
chiatry, 30(2), 99–113. 10.3109/02699206.2013.820351
Porch, B. E. (1967). Porch Index of Communicative Ability: Pulvermüller, F., Neininger, B., Elbert, T., Mohr, B., Rock-
Vol. 1: Theory and development. Consulting Psychologists stroh, B., Koebbel, P., & Taub, E. (2001). Constraint-­
Press. induced therapy for chronic aphasia after stroke. Stroke,
Porch, B. E. (2001). Porch Index of Communicative Ability– 32(7), 1621–1626. https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR​.32​
Revised (4th ed.). Consulting Psychologists Press. .7.1621
Porch, B. E. (2008). Treatment of aphasia subsequent to Pulvermüller, F., & Roth, V. M. (1991). Communicative
the Porch Index of Communicative Ability (PICA). aphasia treatment as a further development of pace
In R. Chapey (Ed.), Language intervention strategies in therapy. Aphasiology, 5(1), 39–50. https://doi.org/​10​
aphasia and related communication disorders (4th ed., pp. .1080/02687039108248518
800–813). Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Purdy, M. (2002). Executive function ability in persons
Postman-Caucheteux, W. A., Birn, R. M., Pursley, R. H., with aphasia. Aphasiology, 16(4–6), 549–557. https://doi.
Butman, J. A., Solomon, J. M., Picchioni, D., McAr- org/10.1080/02687030244000176
dle, J., & Braun, A. R. (2010). Single-trial fMRI shows Purdy, M., & Hindenlang, J. (2005). Educating and training
contra­lesional activity linked to overt naming errors caregivers of persons with aphasia. Aphasiology, 19(3–5),
in chronic aphasic patients. Journal of Cognitive Neuro- 377–388. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030444000822
science, 22(6), 1299–1318. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn​ Purves, B. A., Petersen, J., & Puurveen, G. (2013). An
.2009.21261 aphasia mentoring program: Perspectives of speech-​
Potter, R. E., & Goodman, N. J. (1983). The implementation language pathology students and of mentors with
of laughter as a therapy facilitator with adult aphasics. aphasia. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology,
Journal of Communication Disorders, 16(1), 41–48. https:// 22(2), S370–S379. https://doi.org/10.1044/​1058-0360​
doi.org/10.1016/0021-9924(83)90025-4 (2013/12-0071)
Pound, C., Duchan, J., Penman, T., Hewitt, A., & Parr, S. Quinlan, J. D., Guaron, M. R., Deschere, B. R., & Stephens,
(2007). Communication access to organizations: Inclu- M. B. (2010). Care of the returning veteran. American
sionary practices for people with aphasia. Aphasiology, Family Physician, 82(1), 43–49. https://www.aafp.org/
21, 23–28. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030600798212 afp/2010/0701/p43.html
Powell, J. A., Hale, M. A., & Bayer, A. J. (1995). Symptoms Quintas, R., Cerniauskaite, M., Ajovalasit, D., Sattin, D.,
of communication breakdown in dementia: Carers’ per- Boncoraglio, G., Parati, E. A., & Leonardi, M. (2012).
ceptions. International Journal of Language & Communi- Describing functioning, disability, and health with the
cation Disorders, 30(1), 65–75. https://doi.org/10.3109/​ International Classification of Functioning, Disability,
1368​2829509031323 and Health brief core set for stroke. American Journal
Power, E., Anderson, A., & Togher, L. (2011). Applying the of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 91(2), S14–S21.
WHO ICF framework to communication assessment https://doi.org/10.1097/PHM.0b013e31823d4ba9
and goal setting in Huntington’s disease: A case discus- Quique, Y. M., Evans, W. S., & Dickey, M. W. (2019).
sion. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44(3), 261–275. Acquisition and generalization responses in aphasia
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2010.12.004 naming treatment: A meta-analysis of semantic feature
557
References  

analysis outcomes. American Journal of Speech-Language gesture+verbal treatment for noun and verb retrieval
Pathology, 28(1S), 230–246. https://doi.org/10.1044/​ in aphasia. Journal of the International Neuropsycholog-
2018_AJSLP-17-0155 ical Society, 12(6), 867–882. https://doi.org/10.1017/
Ramsberger, G., & Helm-Estabrooks, N. (1988). Visual S1355617706061042
action therapy for bucco-facial apraxia. https://aphasi- Raymer, A. M., Strobel, J., Thomason, B. J., & Reff, K. L.
ology.pitt.edu/archive/00000087/01/18-28.pdf (2010). Errorless versus errorful training of spelling in
Randolph, C., Tierney, M. C., Mohr, E., & Chase, T. N. individuals with acquired dysgraphia. Neuropsycholog-
(1998). The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of ical Rehabilitation, 20(1), 1–15. https://doi.org/​10.1080/​
Neuropsychological Status (RBANS): Preliminary clin- 09602010902879834
ical validity. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neu- Reimer, T. J., Hagen, C., Malkmus, D., Durham, P.,
ropsychology, 20(3), 310–319. https://doi.org/10.1076/ Stenderup, K., Peterson, C., . . . Education Institute.
jcen.20.3.310.823 (1995). The Rancho Levels of Cognitive Functioning. Los
Rao, P. R. (1995). Drawing conclusions on the efficacy Amigos Research & Education Institute.
of “drawing” as a treatment option for persons with Reisberg, B., Ferris, S. H., de Leon, M. J., & Crook, T.
severe aphasia. Aphasiology, 9, 59–62. https://doi.org/​ (1982). The global deterioration scale for assessment
10.1080/02687039508248690 of primary degenerative dementia. American Journal of
Rao, S., Leo, G. J., Haughton, V. M., St. Aubin-Faubert, P., Psychiatry, 139(9), 1136–1139.
& Bernardin, L. (1989). Correlation of magnetic reso- Reitan, R. M. (1981). Reitan-Indiana Aphasia Screening Test.
nance imaging with neuropsychological testing in mul- Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory.
tiple sclerosis. Neurology, 39, 161–166. https://doi.org/​ Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1997). The influence of age
10.1212/WNL.39.2.161 and education on neuropsychological performances
Rapp, B., & Kane, A. (2002). Remediation of deficits of persons with mild head injuries. Applied Neuropsy-
affecting different components of the spelling process. chology, 4(1), 16–33. https://doi.org/10.1207/s153248​
Aphasiology, 16(4–6), 439–454. https://doi.org/10​.1080/​ 26​an0401_3
02687030244000301 Rentz, C. A. (2002). Memories in the making: Outcome-​
Raskin, S. A., Buckheit, C., & Sherrod, C. (2010). Memory based evaluation of an art program for individuals with
for Intentions Test. Psychological Assessment Resources. dementing illnesses. American Journal of Alzheimer’s
Rautakoski, P. (2011). Training total communication. Apha- Disease and Other Dementias, 17(3), 175–181. https://doi​
siology, 25(3), 344–365. https://doi.org/10.1080/026870 .org/10.1177/153331750201700310
38.2010.530671 Reynolds, C. R. (2002). Comprehensive Trail Making Test
Raven, J. C. (2007). Advanced progressive matrices: APM. (CTMT). Pro-Ed.
Pearson. Richardson, J., Jacks, A., Dalton, S., Schultz, C. & Adams, J.
Raven, J. C., Raven, J., & Court, J. H. (2003). Manual for (2021). Aphasia at the margins: A focus on the underserved.
Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Section 1: Clinical Aphasiology conference [online].
General overview. Harcourt Assessment. Richardson, J. D., Fillmore, P., Rorden, C., LaPointe, L. L.,
Ravona-Springer, R., Luo, X., Schmeidler, J., Wysocki, M., & Fridriksson, J. (2012). Re-establishing Broca’s initial
Lesser, G., Rapp, M., . . . Schnaider Beeri, M. (2010). findings. Brain and Language, 123(2), 125–130. https://
Diabetes is associated with increased rate of cognitive doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2012.08.007
decline in questionably demented elderly. Dementia and Richmond, T., Peterson, C., Cason, J., Billings, M., Terrell,
Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 29(1), 68–74. https://doi E. A., Lee, A. C. W., . . . Brennan, D. (2017). American
.org/10.1159/000265552 Telemedicine Association’s principles for delivering
Raymer, A. M., Beeson, P., Holland, A., Kendall, D., telerehabilitation services. International Journal of Tel-
Maher, L. M., Martin, N., . . . Rothi, L. J. G. (2008). erehabilitation, 9(2), 63–68. https://doi.org/10.5195/
Translational research in aphasia: From neuroscience IJT.2017.6232
to neurorehabilitation. Journal of Speech, Language and Rider, J. D., Wright, H. H., Marshall, R. C., & Page, J. L.
Hearing Research, 51(1), S259–S275. https://doi.org/​ (2008). Using semantic feature analysis to improve con-
10.1044/1092-4388(2008/020) textual discourse in adults with aphasia. American Jour-
Raymer, A. M., Cudworth, C., & Haley, M. (2003). Spelling nal of Speech-Language Pathology, 17, 161–172. https://
treatment for an individual with dysgraphia: Analysis of doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2008/016)
generalisation to untrained words. Aphasiology, 17(6/7), Ridgeway, V., Robertson, I. H., Ward, T., & Nimmo-Smith, I.
607. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030344000058 (1994). Test of Everyday Attention. Thames Valley Test Co.
Raymer, A. M., Singletary, F., Rodriguez, A., Ciampitti, Ripich, D. N., & Horner, J. (2004). The neurodegener-
M., Heilman, K. M., & Rothi, L. J. G. (2006). Effects of ative dementias: Diagnoses and interventions. The
558  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

ASHA Leader, 9, 4–15. https://doi.org/10.1044/leader​ Robey, R. R. (2004). A five-phase model for clinical-out-
.FTR1.09082004.4 come research. Journal of Communication Disorders, 37(5),
Ripich, D. N., & Wykle, M. (1996). Communicating with 401–411. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.04.003
persons with Alzheimer’s disease: The FOCUSED pro- Robey, R. R., & Schultz, M. C. (1998). A model for conduct-
gram for caregivers [Training manual]. Psychological ing clinical-outcome research: An adaptation of the stan-
Corporation. dard protocol for use in aphasiology. Aphasiology, 12(9),
Ripich, D. N., Wykle, M., & Niles, S. (1995). Alzheimer’s 787–810. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687039808249573
disease caregivers: The FOCUSED program. Geriatric Roche, N. L., Fleming, J. M., & Shum, D. H. K. (2002).
Nursing, 16(1), 15–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0197-​ Self-awareness of prospective memory failure in adults
4572(05)80073-4 with traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 16(11), 931–
Ripich, D. N., Ziol, E., Fritsch, T., & Durand, E. J. (2000). 945. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699050210138581
Training Alzheimer’s disease caregivers for success- Rochon, E., Laird, L., Bose, A., & Scofield, J. (2005). Mapping
ful communication. Clinical Gerontologist, 21(1), 37–56. therapy for sentence production impairments in non-
https://doi.org/10.1300/J018v21n01_05 fluent aphasia. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 15(1),
Ripich, D. N., Ziol, E., & Lee, M. M. (1998). Longitudinal 1–36. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602010343000327
effects of communication training on caregivers of per- Rogers, M. A., King, J. M., & Alcorn, N. B. (2000). Proac-
sons with Alzheimer’s disease. Clinical Gerontologist, tive management of primary progressive aphasia. In
19(2), 37–55. https://doi.org/10.1300/J018v19n02_04 D. R. Beukelman, K. M. Yorkston, & J. Reichle (Eds.),
Rising, K., & Beeson, P. M. (2020) The logopenic vari- Augmentative and alternative communication for adults
ant of primary progressive aphasia. In R. L. Utianski with acquired neurologic disorders (pp. 305–337). Paul H.
(Ed.), Primary progressive aphasia and other frontotemporal Brookes.
dementias: Diagnosis and treatment of associated communi- Rohde, A., Doi, S. A., Worrall, L., Godecke, E., Farrell, A.,
cation disorders (pp. 19–43). Plural Publishing. O’Halloran, R., . . . Wong, A. (2020). Development and
Roach, A., Schwartz, M. F., Martin, N., Grewal, R. S., & diagnostic validation of the Brisbane Evidence-Based
Brecher, A. (1996). The Philadelphia Naming Test: Language Test. Disability and Rehabilitation. https://doi​
Scoring and rationale. Clinical Aphasiology, 24, 121– .org/10.1080/09638288.2020.1773547
133. https://aphasiology.pitt.edu/archive/0000​0215/​ Rohde, A., Townley-O’Neill, K., Trendall, K., Worrall, L.,
01/24-09.pdf & Cornwell, P. (2012). A comparison of client and thera-
Roberts, D., & Gaspard, G. (2013). A palliative approach pist goals for people with aphasia: A qualitative explor-
to care of residents with dementia. Nursing Older Peo- atory study. Aphasiology, 26(10), 1298–1315. https://doi​
ple, 25(2), 32–36. https://doi.org/10.7748/nop​2013​.03​ .org/10.1080/02687038.2012.706799
.25.2.32.e703 Rohde, A., Worrall, L., Godecke, E., O’Halloran, R., Farrell,
Roberts, P., Code, C., & McNeil, M. (2003). Describing par- A., & Massey, M. (2018). Diagnosis of aphasia in stroke
ticipants in aphasia research: Part 1. Aphasiology, 17(10), populations: A systematic review of language tests.
911–932. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030344000328 PLOS ONE, 13(3), e0194143. https://doi.org/10.1371/
Roberts, P. M. (2001). Aphasia assessment and treat- journal.pone.0194143
ment in bilingual and multicultural populations. In R. Román, G. C., Tatemichi, T. K., Erkinjuntti, T., Cummings,
Chapey (Ed.), Language intervention strategies in aphasia J. L., Masdeu, J. C., Garcia, J. H., . . . Scheinberg, P. (1993).
and related neurogenic communication disorders (4th ed., Vascular dementia: Diagnostic criteria for research stud-
pp. 208–232). Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. ies. Report of the NINDS-AIREN International Workshop,
Roberts, P. M. (2008). Issues in assessment and treatment 43(2), 250–260. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.43.2.250
for bilingual and culturally diverse patients. In R. Rose, M. L. (2013). Releasing the constraints on aphasia
Chapey (Ed.), Language intervention strategies in aphasia therapy: The positive impact of gesture and multimo-
and related neurogenic communication disorders (5th ed., dality treatments. American Journal of Speech-Language
pp. 245–275). Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Pathology, 22(2), S227–S239. https://doi.org/​10.1044/​
Roberts, P. M., & Doucet, N. (2011). Performance of 1058-0360(2012/12-0091)
French-speaking Quebec adults on the Boston Nam- Rose, M. L., Cherney, L. R., & Worrall, L. E. (2013). Inten-
ing Test. Canadian Journal of Speech-Language Pathology sive comprehensive aphasia programs: An interna-
and Audiology, 35(3), 254–267. https://cjslpa.ca/detail. tional survey of practice. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation,
php?ID=1078 20(5), 379–387. https://doi.org/10.1310/tsr2005-379
Robey, R. R. (1998). A meta-analysis of clinical outcomes Rose, M. L., & Douglas, J. (2003). Limb apraxia, pantom-
in the treatment of aphasia. Journal of Speech, Language ine, and lexical gesture in aphasic speakers: Prelimi-
and Hearing Research, 41(1), 172–187. https://doi.org/​ nary findings. Aphasiology, 17(5), 453–464. https://doi​
10.1044/jslhr.4101.172 .org/​10.1080/02687030344000157
559
References  

Rose, M. L., Ferguson, A., Power, E., Togher, L., & Worrall, lman (Eds.), Apraxia: The neuropsychology of action (pp.
L. (2014). Aphasia rehabilitation in Australia: Current 61–74). Psychology Press.
practices, challenges and future directions. International Rowland, A., & McDonald, L. (2009). Evaluation of social
Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 16(2), 169–180. work communication skills to allow people with apha-
https://doi.org/10.3109/17549507.2013.794474 sia to be part of the decision-making process in health-
Rose, T., Worrall, L., Hickson, L., & Hoffmann, T. (2010). care. Social Work Education, 28(2), 128–144. https://doi​
Do people with aphasia want written stroke and apha- .org/​10.1080/02615470802029965
sia information? A verbal survey exploring prefer- Royal College of Physicians. (2014). Speech and language
ences for when and how to provide stroke and aphasia therapy provision for people with dementia: RCSLT posi-
information. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 17(2), 79–98. tion paper 2014. https://www.rcslt.org/wp-content/
https://doi.org/10.1310/tsr1702-79 uploads/media/Project/RCSLT/dementia-position-​
Rose, T. A., Worrall, L. E., McKenna, K. T., Hickson, L. paper-2014.pdf
M., & Hoffmann, T. C. (2009). Do people with apha- Royall, D. (2005). The emperor has no clothes: Demen-
sia receive written stroke and aphasia information? tia treatment on the eve of the aging era. Journal of the
Aphasiology, 23(3), 364–392. https://doi.org/​10.1080/​ American Geriatrics Society, 53, 163–164. https://doi​
02687030802568108 .org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53029.x
Rosenbek, J. C., LaPointe, L. L., & Wertz, R. T. (1989). Apha- Royall, D., Palmer, R., Chiodo, L. K., & Polk, M. J. (2005).
sia: A clinical approach. Little, Brown & Co. Executive control mediates memory’s association
Rosenblatt, A. S., Li, R., Fortier, C., Liu, X., Fonda, J. R., with change in instrumental. Journal of the American
Villalon, A., McGlinchey, R. E., & Jorge, R. E. (2019). Geriatrics Society, 53, 11–17. https://doi.org/10.1111/​
Latent factor structure of PTSD symptoms in veter- j.1532-5415.2005.53004.x
ans with a history of mild traumatic brain injury and Royall, D. R., Mahurin, R. K., & Gray, K. F. (1992). Bedside
close-range blast exposure. Psychological Trauma: The- assessment of executive cognitive impairment: The
ory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 11(4), 442–450. https:// executive interview. Journal of the American Geriatrics
doi.org/10.1037/tra0000399 Society, 40(12), 1221–1226. https://doi.org/​10.1111/
Rosenfeld, J. V., & Ford, N. L. (2010). Bomb blast, mild trau- j.1532-5415.1992.tb03646.x
matic brain injury and psychiatric morbidity: A review. Ruff, R. M. (1996). Ruff Figural Fluency Test: Professional
Injury, 41(5), 437–443. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.injury​ manual. Psychological Assessment Resources.
.2009.11.018 Ruml, W., Caramazza, A., Shelton, J. R., & Chialant, D.
Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (2009). Artifacts in behavioral (2000). Testing assumptions in computational theories
research: Robert Rosenthal and Ralph L. Rosnow’s classic of aphasia. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 217–248.
books. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2730
Ross, J. D., & Ross, C. M. (1976). Ross Test of Higher Cognitive Rush, B. K., Barch, D., & Braver, T. S. (2006). Accounting for
Processes: Administration manual. Academic Therapy. cognitive aging: Context processing, inhibition or process-
Ross, K. B., & Wertz, R. T. (2003). Discriminative valid- ing speed? Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition.
ity of selected measures for differentiating nor- Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 13(3–4),
mal from aphasic performance. American Journal of 588–610. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825580600680703
Speech-Language Pathology, 12(3), 312–319. https://doi. Rusted, J., Sheppard, L., & Waller, D. (2006). A multi-centre
org/10.1044/1058-0360(2003/077) randomized control group trial on the use of art therapy
Ross, K. B., & Wertz, R. T. (2004). Accuracy of formal for older people with dementia. Group Analysis, 39(4),
tests for diagnosing mild aphasia: An application of 517–536. https://doi.org/10.1177/0533316406071447
evidence-based medicine. Aphasiology, 18(4), 337–355. Ryan, C. M. (1988). Neurobehavioral complications of
https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030444000002 type I diabetes: Examination of possible risk factors.
Ross-Swain, D., & Fogle, P. (1996). Ross Information Process- Diabetes Care, 11(1), 86–93. https://doi.org/10.2337/
ing Assessment–Geriatric. Pro-Ed. diacare.11.1.86
Ross-Swain, D., & Fogle, P. (2012). Ross Information Process- Ryan, C. M., Geckle, M. O., & Orchard, T. J. (2003). Cogni-
ing Assessment–Geriatric (2nd ed.). Pro-Ed. tive efficiency declines over time in adults with type 1
Roth, R. M., Isquith, P. K., & Gioia, G. A. (2005). Behav- diabetes: Effects of micro and macrovascular compli-
ior Rating Inventory of Executive Function–Adult version cations. Diabetologia, 46(7), 940–948. https://doi.org/​
(BRIEF-A). Psychological Assessment Resources. 10.1007/s00125-003-1128-2
Rothi, L. J. G., & Heilman, K. M. (2014). Apraxia: The neu- Ryan, E. B., Bourhis, R. Y., & Knops, U. (1991). Evaluative
ropsychology of action. Psychology Press. perceptions of patronizing speech addressed to elders.
Rothi, L. J. G., Raymer, A. M., & Heilman, K. M. (1997). Psychology and Aging, 6(3), 442–450. https://doi.org/​
Limb praxis assessment. In L. J. G. Rothi & K. M. Hei- 10.1037/0882-7974.6.3.442
560  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Sabat, S. R. (2005). Capacity for decision-making in Alz- Sansone, J. (2014). Bioplasticity: Hypnosis Mind Body Heal-
heimer’s disease: Selfhood, positioning and semiotic ing. High Energy Publishing.
people. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychi- Santo Pietro, M. J., & Boczko, F. (1998). The Breakfast
atry, 39(11–12), 1030–1035. https://doi.org/​10.1080/​ Club: Results of a study examining the effectiveness
j.1440-1614.2005.01722.x of a multi-modality group communication treatment.
Sabo, S., de Zapien, J., Teufel-Shone, N., Rosales, C., Berg- American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Demen-
sma, L., & Taren, D. (2015). Service learning: A vehicle tias, 13(3), 146–158. https://doi.org/10.1177/15333​175​
for building health equity and eliminating health dis- 9801300307
parities. American Journal of Public Health, 105(Suppl. 1), Santo Pietro, M. J., & Otsuni, E. (2003). Successful communi-
S38–S43. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2014.302364 cation with persons with Alzheimer’s disease: An in-service
Sacchett, C. (2002). Drawing in aphasia: Moving towards the training manual (2nd ed.). Butterworth-Heinemann.
interactive. International Journal of Human-Computer Stud- Saper, R. B., Phillips, R. S., Sehgal, A., Khouri, N., Davis,
ies, 57(4), 263–277. https://doi.org/10.1006/ijhc​.2002​.1018 R. B., Paquin, J., . . . Kales, S. N. (2008). Lead, mercury,
Sacchett, C., Byng, S., Marshall, J., & Pound, C. (1999). and arsenic in U.S.- and Indian-manufactured Ayurve-
Drawing together: Evaluation of a therapy programme dic medicines sold via the Internet. JAMA, 8, 915.
for severe aphasia. International Journal of Language & Sarkaki, A., Rafieirad, M., Hossini, S. E., Farbood, Y.,
Communication Disorders, 34(3), 265–289. https://doi​ Motamedi, F., Mansouri, S. M. T., & Naghizadeh, B.
.org/10.1080/136828299247414 (2013). Improvement in memory and brain long-term
Saffran, E. M., Berndt, R. S., & Schwartz, M. F. (1989). The potentiation deficits due to permanent hypoperfusion/
quantitative analysis of agrammatic production: Pro- ischemia by grape seed extract in rats. Iranian Journal of
cedure and data. Brain and Language, 37(3), 440–479. Basic Medical Sciences, 16(9), 1004–1010.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(89)90030-8 Sarno, J. E., Rusk, H. A., Diller, L., & Sarno, M. (1972). The
Saha, S., Mamun, K. A., Ahmed, K., Mostafa, R., Naik, effect of hyperbaric oxygen on the mental and verbal
G. R., Darvishi, S., . . . Baumert, M. (2021). Progress in ability of stroke patients. Stroke, 3(1), 10–15. https://doi​
brain computer interface: Challenges and opportuni- .org/10.1161/01.STR.3.1.10
ties. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 15. https://doi​ Sather, T. W., & Howe, T. J. (2021). The role of the enciron-
.org/​10.3389/fnsys.2021.578875 ment: Supporting language, communication, and
Salima, S., & Tammy, H. (2016). Decision-making capacity participation. In A. L. Holland & R. J. Elman (Eds.),
and aphasia: Speech-language pathologists’ perspec- Neurogenic communication disorders and the life participa-
tives. Aphasiology, 30(4), 381–395. https://doi.org/10.10​ tion approach (pp. 81–104). Plural Publishing.
80/02687038.2015.1065468 Sather, T., Howe, T., Nelson, N., & Lagerwey, M. (2017).
Salter, K., McClure, J. A., Foley, N. C., & Teasell, R. (2011). Optimizing the experience of flow for adults with apha-
Community integration following TBI: An examination sia a focus on environmental factors. Topics in Language
of community integration measures within the ICF Disorders, 37, 25–37. https://doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0000​
framework. Brain Injury, 25(12), 1147–1154. https://doi​ 000000000111
.org/10.3109/02699052.2011.613088 Saur, D. (2006). Dynamics of language reorganization after
Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult stroke. Brain, 129(6), 1371–1384. https://doi.org/10​.10​
age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103(3), 93/​brain/awl090
403–428. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.403 Saur, D., Lange, R., Baumgaertner, A., Schraknepper, V.,
Salthouse, T. A. (2000). Aging and measures of processing Willmes, K., Rijntjes, M., & Weiller, C. (2006). Dynamics
speed. Biological Psychology, 54(1–3), 35–54. https://doi​ of language reorganization after stroke. Brain, 129(Pt.
.org/​10.1016/S0301-0511(00)00052-1 6), 1371–1384. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl090
Sambunaris, A., & Hyde, T. M. (1994). Stroke-related apha- Saver, J. L., Fonarow, G. C., Smith, E. E., Reeves, M. J.,
sias mistaken for psychotic speech: Two case reports. Grau-Sepulveda, M. V., Pan, W., & Schwamm, L. H.
Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 7(3), 144– (2013). Time to treatment with intravenous tissue plas-
147. https://doi.org/10.1177/089198879400700303 minogen activator and outcome from acute ischemic
Sampson, M., Johnson, G., & Brown, J. (2013). On the stroke. JAMA, 309(23), 2480–2488. https://doi.org/​
pulse: Audit-proof your documentation. The ASHA 10.1001/​jama.2013.6959
Leader, 18(8), 30. https://doi.org/10.1044/leader. Saxton, J. (2004). The Severe Impairment Battery. Harcourt
OTP.18082013.30 Assessment.
Sandson, J., & Albert, M. L. (1984). Varieties of persever- Schaffer, K. & Henry, M. (2020). Nonfluent/agrammatic
ation. Neuropsychologia, 22(6), 715–732. https://doi. primary progressive aphasia. In R.L. Utianski (Ed.), Pri-
org/10.1016/0028-3932(84)90098-8 mary progressive aphasia and other frontotemporal demen-
561
References  

tias: diagnosis and treatment of associated communication Schnakers, C., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Giacino, J., Ventura,
disorders (pp. 77–100). Plural Publishing. M., Boly, M., Majerus, S., . . . Laureys, S. (2009). Diag-
Schaie, K. W. (2005). Developmental influences on adult intel- nostic accuracy of the vegetative and minimally con-
ligence: The Seattle Longitudinal Study. Oxford Univer- scious state: Clinical consensus versus standardized
sity Press. neurobehavioral assessment. BMC Neurology, 9(1), 35.
Schaie, K. W., & Willis, S. L. (2002). Adult development and https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2377-9-35
aging. Prentice Hall. Schneider, B. A., Daneman, M., & Murphy, D. R. (2005).
Scheibel, R. S. (2017). Functional magnetic resonance Speech comprehension difficulties in older adults:
imaging of cognitive control following traumatic brain Cognitive slowing or age-related changes in hearing?
injury. Frontiers in Neurology, 8. https://doi.org/​10.33​ Psychology and Aging, 20(2), 261–271. https://doi.org/​
89/​fneur.2017.00352 10.1037/0882-7974.20.2.261
Schenkenberg, T., Bradford, D. C., & Ajax, E. T. (1980). Line Schneider, N. M., & Camp, C. J. (2003). Use of Montes-
bisection and unilateral visual neglect in patients with sori-based activities by visitors of nursing home res-
neurologic impairment. Neurology, 30(5), 509. https:// idents with dementia. Clinical Gerontologist, 26(1–2),
doi.org/10.1212/WNL.30.5.509 71–84. https://doi.org/10.1300/J018v26n01_07
Schensul, J. J., Torres, M., & Wetle, T. T. (1992). Educational Schneiderman, A. I., Braver, E. R., & Kang, H. K. (2008).
materials and innovative dissemination strategies: Alzhei- Understanding sequelae of injury mechanisms and
mer’s disease among Puerto Rican elderly. Institute for mild traumatic brain injury incurred during the con-
Community Research. flicts in Iraq and Afghanistan: Persistent postconcus-
Schiller, N., Ferreira, V., & Alario, F.-X. (2007). Words, sive symptoms and posttraumatic stress disorder.
pauses, and gestures: New directions in language pro- American Journal of Epidemiology, 167(12), 1446–1452.
duction research. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22(8), https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwn068
1145–1150. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960701491415 Schneiderman, E. I., Murasugi, K. G., & Saddy, J. D. (1992).
Schlaug, G., Altenmüüller, E., & Thaut, M. (2010). Music Story arrangement ability in right brain-damaged
listening and music making in the treatment of neu- patients. Brain and Language, 43(1), 107–120. https://
rological disorders and impairments. Music Perception: doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(92)90024-9
An Interdisciplinary Journal, 27(4), 249–250. https://doi​ Scholl, D. I., McCabe, P., Nickels, L., & Ballard, K. J. (2021).
.org/10.1525/mp.2010.27.4.249 Outcomes of semantic feature analysis treatment for
Schlaug, G., Marchina, S., & Norton, A. (2008). From sing- aphasia with and without apraxia of speech. International
ing to speaking: Why singing may lead to recovery of Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 56(3),
expressive language function in patients with Broca’s 485–500. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12597
aphasia. Music Perception, 25(4), 315–323. https://doi​ Schönberger, M., Humle, F., & Teasdale, T. W. (2006).
.org/​10.1525/MP.2008.25.4.315 The development of the therapeutic working alli-
Schlaug, G., Norton, A., Marchina, S., Zipse, L., & Wan, C. ance, patients’ awareness and their compliance during
Y. (2010). From singing to speaking: Facilitating recov- the process of brain injury rehabilitation. Brain Injury,
ery from nonfluent aphasia. Future Neurology, 5(5), 20(4), 445–454. https://doi.org/10.1080/0269905060066​
657–665. https://doi.org/10.2217/fnl.10.44 4772
Schlosser, R. (2002). On the importance of being earnest Schretlen, D. (1997). The Brief Test of Attention: Professional
about treatment integrity. Augmentative and Alternative manual. Psychological Assessment Resources.
Communication, 18(1), 36–44. https://doi.org/10.1080/ Schretlen, D. J. (2010). Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
aac.18.1.36.44 (M-WCST). Psychological Assessment Resources.
Schlosser, R. W. (2004). Goal attainment scaling as a clinical Schroyen, S., Adam, S., Marquet, M., Jerusalem, G., Thiel,
measurement technique in communication disorders: S., Giraudet, A.-L., & Missotten, P. (2018). Communica-
A critical review. Journal of Communication Disorders, tion of healthcare professionals: Is there ageism? Euro-
37(3), 217–239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2003​ pean Journal of Cancer Care, 27(1), e12780. https://doi
.09.003 .org/​10.1111/ecc.12780
Schlund, M. W. (1999). Self-awareness: Effects of feedback Schuell, H. (1953). Aphasic difficulties understanding
and review on verbal self reports and remembering spoken language. Neurology, 3(3), 176–184. https://doi​
following brain injury. Brain Injury, 13(5), 375–380. .org/10.1212/WNL.3.3.176
https://doi.org/10.1080/026990599121566 Schuell, H. (1954). Clinical observations on aphasia. Neu-
Schmahmann, J. D., & Sherman, J. C. (1998). The cerebel- rology, 4(3), 179–189.
lar cognitive affective syndrome. Brain, 121, 561–579. Schuell, H. (1965). The Minnesota Test of Differential Diagno-
https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/121.4.561 sis of Aphasia. University of Minnesota Press.
562  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Schuell, H. (1973). Differential diagnosis of aphasia with the Shadden, B. B. (1998). Obtaining the discourse sample. In
Minnesota Test (2nd ed.). University of Minnesota Press. L. R. Cherney, B. B. Shadden, & C. A. Coelho (Eds.),
Schuell, H., & Jenkins, J. J. (1959). The nature of language Analyzing discourse in communicatively impaired adults
deficit in aphasia. Psychological Review, 66(1), 45–67. (pp. 9–34). Aspen.
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0045014 Shadden, B. B. (2011). Language and aging: Primary and
Schuell, H., Jenkins, J. J., & Jimenez-Pabon, E. (1964). Apha- tertiary factors. In M. A. Toner, B. B. Shadden, & M.
sia in adults: Diagnosis, prognosis, and treatment. Harper B. Gluth (Eds.), Aging and communication (pp. 205–234).
& Row. Pro-Ed.
Schwartz, M. F. (1984). What the classical aphasia catego- Shah, S. H., Engelhardt, R., & Ovbiagele, B. (2008). Patterns
ries can’t do for us, and why. Brain and Language, 21(1), of complementary and alternative medicine use among
3–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(84)90031-2 United States stroke survivors. Journal of the Neurologi-
Schwartz, M. F., Saffran, E. M., Fink, R. B., Myers, J. L., cal Sciences, 271(1–2), 1–2. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns​
& Martin, N. (1994). Mapping therapy: A treatment .2008.04.014
programme for agrammatism. Aphasiology, 8(1), 19–54. Shapiro, L. P. (1997). Tutorial: An introduction to syntax.
https://doi.org/10.1080/02687039408248639 Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 40(2),
Schwartz, M. F., Saffran, E. M., & Marin, O. M. (1980). 254–272. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4002.254
Fractionating the reading process in dementia: Evi- Sherratt, S., & Simmons-Mackie, N. (2016). Shared
dence from word specific print-to-sound associations. humour in aphasia groups: “They should be called
In M. Coltheart, K. Patterson, & J. C. Marshall (Eds.), cheer groups.” Aphasiology, 30(9), 1039–1057. https://
Deep dyslexia (pp. 259–269). Routledge & Kegan Paul. doi​.org/10.1080/02687038.2015.1092495
Searle, J. R. (1969). Speech acts: An essay in the philosophy of Sherrington, C., Herbert, R. D., Maher, C. G., & Moseley, A.
language. Cambridge University Press. M. (2000). PEDro. A database of randomized trials and
Sebastian, S., Stein, L. K., & Dhamoon, M. S. (2019). Infec- systematic reviews in physiotherapy. Manual Therapy,
tion as a stroke trigger. Stroke, 50(8), 2216–2218. https:// 5(4), 223–226. https://doi.org/10.1054/math.2000.0372
doi.org/10.1161/STROKEAHA.119.025872 Shiani, A., Joghataei, M. T., Ashayeri, H., Kamali, M.,
Seifert, L. S. (2001). Customized art activities for individ- Razavi, M. R., & Yadegari, F. (2018). Comprehension
uals with Alzheimer-type dementia. Activities, Adap- of complex sentences in the Persian-speaking patients
tation & Aging, 24(4), 65–74. https://doi.org/10.1300/ with aphasia. Basic and Clinical Neuroscience, 10(3), 199–
J016v24n04_06 208. https://doi.org/10.32598/bcn.9.10.185
Sekhon, J., Douglas, J., & Rose, M. (2015). Current Austra- Shin, S. S., Bales, J. W., Edward Dixon, C., & Hwang, M.
lian speech-language pathology practice in addressing (2017). Structural imaging of mild traumatic brain
psychological wellbeing in people with aphasia after injury may not be enough: Overview of functional
stroke. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathol- and metabolic imaging of mild traumatic brain injury.
ogy, 17(3), 252–262. https://doi.org/10.3109/17549507 Brain Imaging and Behavior, 11(2), 591–610. https://doi​
.2015.1024170 .org/10.1007/s11682-017-9684-0
Sekhon, J. K., Oates, J., Kneebone, I., & Rose, M. (2019). Shogren, K. A. (2011). Culture and self-determination:
Counselling training for speech-language therapists A synthesis of the literature and directions for future
working with people affected by post‐stroke aphasia: research and practice. Career Development for Exceptional
A systematic review. International Journal of Language Individuals, 34(2), 115–127. https://doi.org/10.11​77/​
& Communication Disorders, 54(3), 321–346. https://doi. 0885728811398271
org/10.1111/1460-6984.12455 Sidiropoulos, K., Bormann, T., & Ackermann, H. (2014).
Seki, K., & Sugishita, M. (1983). Japanese-applied melodic Cortical and fibre tract interrelations in conduction
intonation therapy for Broca aphasia. Brain and Nerve, aphasia. Aphasiology, 28(10), 1151–1167. https://doi.org/​
35(10), 1031–1037. 10.1080/02687038.2014.907864
Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Authentic happiness: Using the Silkes, J., & Anjum, J. (2021). The role and use of event-re-
new positive psychology to realize your potential for lasting lated potentials in aphasia: A scoping review. Brain and
fulfillment. Free Press. Language, 219, 104966. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl​
Shadden, B. (2001). Psychosocial Aspects of Aphasia: Whose Per- .2021.104966
spectives? 25th World Congress of the International Asso- Simmons-Mackie, N. (2004). Just kidding! Humour and
ciation of Logopedics and Phoniatrics, Montreal, CA. therapy for aphasia. In J. F. Duchan & S. Byng (Eds.),
Shadden, B. (2005). Aphasia as identity theft: Theory and Challenging aphasia therapies: Broadening the discourse
practice. Aphasiology, 19(3–5), 211–223. https://doi.org/​ and extending the boundaries (pp. 101–117). Psychology
10.1080/02687930444000697 Press.
563
References  

Simmons-Mackie, N. (2013a). Frameworks for managing updated systematic review. Archives of Physical Medi-
communication support for people with aphasia. In cine and Rehabilitation, 97(12), 2202–2221.e8. https://doi​
N. Simmons-Mackie, J. M. King, & D. R. Beukelman .org/10.1016/j.apmr.2016.03.023
(Eds.), Supporting communication for adults with acute and Simmons-Mackie, N., & Schultz, M. (2003). The role of
chronic aphasia (pp. 11–50). Paul H. Brookes. humour in therapy for aphasia. Aphasiology, 17(8), 751–
Simmons-Mackie, N. (2013b). Staging communication 766. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030344000229
supports across the health care continuum. In N. Sim- Simmons-Mackie, N., Worrall, L., Murray, L., Enderby, P.,
mons-Mackie, J. M. King, & D. R. Beukelman (Eds.), Rose, M.L., Jin Paek, E., & Klippi, A. (2017). The top
Supporting communication for adults with acute and ten: Best practice recommendations for aphasia. Apha-
chronic aphasia (pp. 99–144). Paul H. Brookes. siology, 31, 131–151. doi:10.1080/02687038.2016.1180662
Simmons-Mackie, N. (2018). Aphasia in North America. Simpson, F. (2006). Mount Wilga High Level Language
Aphasia Access. Test: Administration & scoring manual plus test form
Simmons-Mackie, N. (2021). The life participation approach with UK adaptations and large print additions. https://
to aphasia: Looking back and moving forward. In A. L. www.docstoc.com/docs/29634047/MOUNT​WILGA-​
Holland & R. J. Elman (Eds.), Neurogenic communication HIGH-LEVEL-LANGUAGE-TEST
disorders and the life participation approach (pp. 209–225). Singh, N. N., Lancioni, G. E., Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M. F., &
Plural Publishing. Winton, A. S. W. (2014). Assistive technology for people
Simmons-Mackie, N., Code, C., Armstrong, E., Stiegler, with Alzheimer’s disease. In G. E. Lancioni & N. N.
L., & Elman, R. J. (2002). What is aphasia? Results of Singh (Eds.), Assistive technologies for people with diverse
an international survey. Aphasiology, 16(8), 837–848. abilities (pp. 219–250). Springer. https://link.springer.
https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030244000185 com/chapter/10.1007/978-1-4899-8029-8_8
Simmons-Mackie, N., & Damico, J. S. (2011). Counselling Skenazy, J., A., & Bigler, E. D. (1984). Neuropsychological
and aphasia treatment: Missed opportunities. Topics in findings in diabetes mellitus. Journal of Clinical Psychol-
Language Disorders, 4, 336. ogy, 40(1), 246–258. https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-
Simmons-Mackie, N., & Elman, R. J. (2011). Negotiation 4679(198401)40:1<246::AID-JCLP22704​ 0 0148>​ 3 .0​ .
of identity in group therapy for aphasia: The aphasia CO;2-P
café. International Journal of Language & Communication Skenes, L. L., & McCauley, R. J. (1985). Psychometric
Disorders, 46(3), 312–323. https://doi.org/10.3109/136 review of nine aphasia tests. Journal of Communication
82822.2010.507616 Disorders, 18(6), 461–474. https://doi.org/10.1016/​00​
Simmons-Mackie, N., Elman, R. J., Holland, A. L., & Dam- 21-9924(85)90033-4
ico, J. S. (2007). Management of discourse in group ther- Sklar, M. (1983). Sklar aphasia scale. Western Psychological
apy for aphasia. Topics in Language Disorders, 1, 5. Services.
Simmons-Mackie, N., Kagan, A., Victor, J. C., Carling-Row- Small, B. (2018). New humanitarian guidelines launched for
land, A., Mok, A., Hoch, J. S., . . . Streiner, D. L. (2014). ageing and disability inclusion. HelpAge International.
The assessment for living with aphasia: Reliability and https://www.helpage.org/newsroom/latest-news/
construct validity. International Journal of Speech-Lan- new-humanitarian-guidelines-launched-for-ageing-​
guage Pathology, 16(1), 82–94. https://doi.org/10.3109/ and-disability-inclusion/
17549507.2013.831484 Small, J. A., Gutman, G., Makela, S., & Hillhouse, B. (2003).
Simmons-Mackie, N., & King, J. M. (2013). Communication Effectiveness of communication strategies used by
support for everyday life situations. In N. Simmons-​ caregivers of persons with Alzheimer’s disease during
Mackie, J. M. King, & D. R. Beukelman (Eds.), Support- activities of daily living. Journal of Speech, Language,
ing communication for adults with acute and chronic aphasia and Hearing Research, 46(2), 353–367. https://doi.org/​
(pp. 221–244). Paul H. Brookes. 10.1044/1092-4388(2003/028)
Simmons-Mackie, N., King, J. M., & Beukelman, D. R. Small, S. L. (2004). A biological model of aphasia rehabilita-
(2013). Supporting communication for adults with acute tion: Pharmacological perspectives. Aphasiology, 18(5–7),
and chronic aphasia. Paul H. Brookes. 473–492. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030444000156
Simmons-Mackie, N., Raymer, A., Armstrong, E., Holland, Smart, J. F., & Smart, D. W. (1997). The racial/ethnic
A., & Cherney, L. R. (2010). Communication partner demography of disability. Journal of Rehabilitation, 63(4),
training in aphasia: A systematic review. Archives of 9–15.
Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 91(12), 1814–1837. Smith, A. (1973). Symbol Digit Modality Test (SDMT). West-
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2010.08.026 ern Psychological Services.
Simmons-Mackie, N., Raymer, A., & Cherney, L. R. (2016). Smith, S. C., Lamping, D. L., Banerjee, S., Harwood, R.,
Communication partner training in aphasia: An Foley, B., Smith, P., . . . Knapp, M. (2005). Measurement
564  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

of health-related quality of life for people with demen- Sparks, R. W., & Holland, A. L. (1976). Method: Melodic
tia: Development of a new instrument (DEMQOL) and intonation therapy for aphasia. Journal of Speech and
an evaluation of current methodology. Health Technology Hearing Disorders, 41, 287–297. https://doi.org/10.1044/
Assessment, 9(10), 1–iv. https://doi.org/10.3310/hta9100 jshd.4103.287
Snyder, C. R., & Lopez, S. J. (2002). Handbook of positive Speech Pathology Australia. (2020). Professional standards
psychology. Oxford University Press. for speech pathologists in Australia. https://www.speech​
Sohlberg, M. M. (2000). Assessing and managing unaware- pathologyaustralia.org.au
ness of self. Seminars in Speech and Language, 21(2), 135. Spell, L. A., Richardson, J. D., Basilakos, A., Stark, B. C.,
https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2000-7561 Teklehaimanot, A., Hillis, A. E., & Fridriksson, J. (2020).
Sohlberg, M. M., Avery, J., Kennedy, K. M., Coelho, C. A., Developing, Implementing, and Improving Assessment
Ylvisaker, M., Turkstra, L. S., & Yorkston, K. M. (2003). and Treatment Fidelity in Clinical Aphasia Research.
Practice guidelines for direct attention training. Journal American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 29(1), 286–
of Medical Speech-Language Pathology, 11(3), xix–xxxix. 298. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_AJSLP-19-00126
Sohlberg, M. M., Kennedy, M., Avery, J., Coehlo, C., Turk- Spencer, K. A., Tompkins, C. A., Schultz, R., & Rau,
stra, L. S., Ylvisaker, M., & Yorkston, K. M. (2007). Evi- M. T. (1995). The psychosocial outcomes of stroke:
dence based practice for the use of external aids as a A longitudinal study of depression risk. Clinical
memory rehabilitation technique. Journal of Medical Aphasiology, 23, 9–23. https://aphasiology.pitt.edu/
Speech-Language Pathology, 15, xv–li. archive/00000185/01/23-02.pdf
Sohlberg, M. M., & Mateer, C. A. (1987). Effectiveness of Spieler, D. H., & Balota, D. A. (2000). Factors influencing
an attention training program. Journal of Clinical and word naming in younger and older adults. Psychology
Experimental Neuropsychology, 9, 117–130. https://doi​ and Aging, 15(2), 225–231. https://doi.org/​10.10​37/​
.org/​10.1080/01688638708405352 0882-7974.15.2.225
Sohlberg, M. M., & Mateer, C. A. (2001). Attention Process Spreen, O., & Benton, A. L. (1977). Neurosensory center com-
Training Test. Lash & Associates. prehensive examination for aphasia: Manual of directions
Sohlberg, M. M., & Mateer, C. A. (2010). APT-III: Attention (Rev. ed.). Neuropsychology Laboratory, University of
process training: A direct attention training program for per- Victoria.
sons with acquired brain injury. Lash & Associates. Spreen, O., & Risser, A. H. (2003). Assessment of aphasia.
Sohlberg, M. M., McLaughlin, K. A., Todis, B., Larsen, J., & Oxford University Press.
Glang, A. (2001). What does it take to collaborate with Springer, L., Glindemann, R., Huber, W., & Willmes, K.
families affected by brain injury? A preliminary model. (1991). How efficacious is pace-therapy when “lan-
Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 16(5), 498–511. guage systematic training” is incorporated? Aphasiol-
https://doi.org/10.1097/00001199-200110000-00008 ogy, 5(4–5), 391–399. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687039​
Sohlberg, M. M., & Turkstra, L. S. (2011). Optimizing cogni- 108248541
tive rehabilitation: Effective instructional methods. Guilford Stahl, B., Henseler, I., Turner, R., Geyer, S., & Kotz, S.
Press. (2013). How to engage the right brain hemisphere in
Sonies, B. C. (1997). Scales of adult independence, language aphasics without even singing: Evidence for two paths
and recall. Pro-Ed. https://www.library.ohiou.edu/ezp- of speech recovery. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience,
auth/redir/athens.php?https://search.ebscohost.com/ 7(35), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00035
login.aspx?direct=true&db=mmt&AN=test.1434&site= Stallings, J. W. (2010). Collage as a therapeutic modality
eds-live&scope=site for reminiscence in patients with dementia. Art Therapy:
Sosa, R. M. C., Martínez, F. M. I., Gómez, O. J. L., & Jáure- Journal of the American Art Therapy Association, 27(3), 136–
gui-Renaud, K. (2009). Early auditory middle latency 140. https://doi.org/10.1080/07421656.2010.10129667
evoked potentials correlates with recovery from apha- Stanley, M. L., Simpson, S. L., Dagenbach, D., Lyday, R.
sia after stroke. Clinical Neurophysiology, 120(1), 136– G., Burdette, J. H., & Laurienti, P. J. (2015). Changes in
139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2008.10.011 brain network efficiency and working memory perfor-
Sparks, R. W. (2008). Melodic intonation therapy. In R. mance in aging. PLOS ONE, 10(4), e0123950. https://
Chapey (Ed.), Language intervention strategies in aphasia doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0123950
and related neurogenic communication disorders (5th ed., Stark, B. C. (2019). A comparison of three discourse elicita-
pp. 837– 851). Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. tion methods in aphasia and age-matched adults: Impli-
Sparks, R. W., Helm, N. A., & Albert, M. (1974). Aphasia cation for language assessment and outcome. American
rehabilitation resulting from Melodic Intonation Ther- Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 28(3), 1067–1083.
apy. Cortex, 10(4), 303–316. https://doi.org/10.1016/ https://doi.org/10.1044/​2019_​AJSLP-18-0265
S0010-9452(74)80024-9
565
References  

Stark, B. C., Dutta, M., Murray, L. L., Bryant, L., Fromm, Strijbos, J.-W., & Fischer, F. (2007). Methodological chal-
D., MacWhinney, B., . . . Sharma, S. (2021). Standardizing lenges for collaborative learning research. Learning and
assessment of spoken discourse in aphasia: A working Instruction, 17(4), 389–393. https://doi.org/10.1016/j​
group with deliverables. American Journal of Speech- .learninstruc.2007.03.004
Language Pathology, 30(1S), 491–502. https://doi.org/​ Strong, K. A., & Shadden, B. B. (2021). Stories at the heart
10.1044/2020_AJSLP-19-00093 of life participation: Both the telling and the listening
Stark, B., Dutta, M., Murray, L., Fromm, D., Bryant, L., matter. In A. L. Holland & R. J. Elman (Eds.), Neurogenic
Harmon, T., Ramage, A., & Roberts, A. (2020). Spoken communication disorders and the life participation approach
discourse assessment and analysis in aphasia: An inter- (pp. 105–130). Plural Publishing.
national survey of current practices. https://doi.org/​ Sturrock, A., & Leavitt, B. R. (2010). The clinical and
10.31234/osf.io/v3xga genetic features of Huntington’s disease. Journal of Geri-
Starrfelt, R., Ólafsdóttir, R. R., & Arendt, I.-M. (2013). atric Psychiatry and Neurology, 23(4), 243–259. https://
Rehabilitation of pure alexia: A review. Neuropsycholog- doi.org/10.1177/0891988710383573
ical Rehabilitation, 23(5), 755–779. https://doi.org/10.10 Sugarman Education (2021). Jobs Outlook — speech and
80/09602011.2013.809661 language therapists. https://www.sugarman.co.uk/ca
Steger, M. (2013). Globalization: A very short introduction. ndidate-support/jobs-outlook-speech-and-language-​
Oxford University Press. therapists
Stein, J., & Brady Wagner, L. C. (2006). Is informed consent Sulheim, S., Holme, I., Ekeland, A., & Bahr, R. (2006).
a “yes or no” response? Enhancing the shared deci- Helmet use and risk of head injuries in alpine skiers
sion-making process for persons with aphasia. Topics and snowboarders. JAMA, 295(8), 919–924. https://doi​
in Stroke Rehabilitation, 13(4), 42–46. https://doi.org/​ .org/10.1001/jama.295.8.919
10.1310/tsr1304-42 Sung, J. E., McNeil, M. R., Pratt, S. R., Dickey, M. W., Hula,
Steinberg, B. A., Bieliauskas, L. A., Smith, G. E., Langel- W. D., Szuminsky, N. J., & Doyle, P. J. (2009). Verbal
lotti, C., & Ivnik, R. J. (2005). Mayo’s older Americans working memory and its relationship to sentence-level
normative studies: Age and IQ-adjusted norms for reading and listening comprehension in persons with
the Boston naming test, the MAE token test, and the aphasia. Aphasiology, 23(7–8), 1040–1052. https://doi​
judgment of line orientation test. The Clinical Neuropsy- .org/10.1080/02687030802592884
chologist, 19(3–4), 280–328. https://doi.org/10.1080/​ Sutcliffe, L. M., & Lincoln, N. B. (1998). The assessment
13854040590945229 of depression in aphasic stroke patients: The develop-
Stern, R. A. (1997). Visual analog mood scales professional ment of the stroke aphasic depression questionnaire.
manual. Psychological Assessment Resources. Clinical Rehabilitation, 12(6), 506–513. https://doi.org/​
Stevens, L. C. (2009). Understanding how students learn: 10.1191/026921598672167702
Preparing students to become professionals. SIG, 10 Swan, K., Hopper, M., Wenke, R., Jackson, C., Till, T.,
Perspectives on Issues in Higher Education, 12, 16–23. & Conway, E. (2018). Speech-language pathologist
https://doi.org/10.1044/ihe12.1.16 interventions for communication in moderate-severe
Stierwalt, J.A. (2020). Introduction. In R. L. Utianski (Ed.), dementia: A systematic review. American Journal of
Primary progressive aphasia and other frontotemporal Speech-Language Pathology, 27(2), 836–852. https://doi​
dementias: Diagnosis and treatment of associated communi- .org/​10.1044/2017_AJSLP-17-0043
cation disorders. (pp. 1–18). Plural Publishing. Swinburn, K., & Byng, S. (2006). The communication disabil-
Storey, J. E., Rowland, J. T. J., Conforti, D. A., & Dick- ity profile. Connect Press.
son, H. G. (2004). The Rowland Universal Dementia Swinburn, K., Porter, G., & Howard, D. (2004). Comprehen-
Assessment Scale (RUDAS): A multicultural cog- sive Aphasia Test. Psychology Press.
nitive assessment scale. International Psychogeriatrics, Sydenham, E., Dangour, A. D., & Lim, W. S. (2012). Omega-3
16(1), 13–31. https://doi.org/10.1017/S104161020400 fatty acid for the prevention of cognitive decline and
0043 dementia. Sao Paulo Medical Journal, 130(6), 419. https://
Stöttinger, E., Guay, C. L., Danckert, J., & Anderson, doi.org/10.1590/S1516-31802012000600013
B. (2018). Updating impairments and the failure to Syder, D., Body, R., Parker, M., & Boddy, M. (1993). Shef-
explore new hypotheses following right brain damage. field Screening Test for Acquired Language Disorders. NFER
Experimental Brain Research, 236(6), 1749–1765. https:// Nelson.
doi.org/10.1007/s00221-018-5259-6 Szaflarski, J. P., Ball, A. L., Grether, S., Alfwaress, F.,
Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (2006). A com- Griffith, N. M., Neils-Strunjas, J., . . . Reichhardt, R.
pendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, (2008). Constraint-induced aphasia therapy stimulates
and commentary (3rd ed.). Oxford University Press. language recovery in patients with chronic aphasia
566  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

after ischemic stroke. Medical Science Monitor, 14(5), injury screening: Preliminary findings in a U.S. Army
CR243–CR250. brigade combat team. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabil-
Szelies, B., Mielke, R., Kessler, J., & Heiss, W.-D. (2002). itation, 24(1), 14–23. https://doi.org/10.1097/HTR​.0b​
Prognostic relevance of quantitative topographical 013e31819581d8
EEG in patients with poststroke aphasia. Brain and Lan- Tesak, J., & Code, C. (2008). Milestones in the history of apha-
guage, 82(1), 87–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/S00​93-​ sia: Theories and protagonists. Psychology Press.
934X(02)00004-4 Theodoros, D., Hill, A., Russell, T., Ward, E., & Wootton, R.
Taber, K. H., Warden, D. L., & Hurley, R. A. (2006). Blast-​ (2008). Assessing acquired language disorders in adults
related traumatic brain injury: What is known? Jour- via the Internet. Telemedicine Journal and E-Health, 14(6),
nal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 18(2), 552–559. https://doi.org/10.1089/tmj.2007.0091
141–142. https://doi.org/10.1176/jnp.2006.18.2.141 Thomas, W., Fox, N., Norton, L., Rashap, A. W., Angelelli,
Tactus Therapy. (2015). Naming therapy: The essential app J., Tellis-Nyak, V., . . . Brostoski, D. (2005). The Eden
for word finding [Mobile application software]. https:// Alternative Domains of Well-Being. https://www.edenalt​
tactustherapy.com/app/naming/ .org/about-the-eden-alternative/the-eden-alternative-
Tanaka, Y., Albert, M., Cahana-Amitay, D., Midori, H., domains-of-well-being/
Fujita, K., Miyazaki, M., . . . Tanaka, M. (2013). Com- Thompson, C. K. (2000a). Neuroplasticity: Evidence from
bined therapy with propranolol and bromocriptine aphasia. Journal of Communication Disorders, 33, 357–366.
for treatment of aphasia. Procedia Social and Behavioral https://doi.org/10.1016/S0021-9924(00)00031-9
Sciences, 94, 251–252. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbs​ Thompson, C. K. (2000b). The neurobiology of language
pro.2013.09.125 recovery in aphasia. Brain and Language, 71(1), 245–248.
Tanaka, Y., Miyazaki, M., & Albert, M. L. (1997). Effects https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1999.2260
of increased cholinergic activity on naming in aphasia. Thompson, C. K. (2002). Northwestern Verb Production Bat-
Lancet, 350(9071), 116–117. https://doi.org/10.1016/ tery [Unpublished manuscript].
S0140-6736(05)61820-X Thompson, C. K. (2008). Treatment of syntactic and mor-
Tanner, D., & Culbertson, W. (1999). Quick assessment for phologic deficits in agrammatic aphasia: Treatment of
aphasia. Academic Communication Associates. underlying forms. In R. Chapey (Ed.), Language inter-
Tariq, R. A., & Sharma, S. (2021). Inappropriate medical vention strategies in aphasia and related neurogenic com-
abbreviations. In StatPearls. StatPearls Publishing. munication disorders (pp. 735–756). Lippincott Williams
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK519006/ & Wilkins.
Tavalaro, J. (1997). Look up for yes. Kodansha Internation Thompson, C. K., Ballard, K. J., & Shapiro, L. P. (1998).
Taylor, H. G., & Solomon, J. R. (1979). Reversed laterality: The role of syntactic complexity in training wh-move-
A case study. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 1(4), ment structures in agrammatic aphasia: Optimal order
311–322. https://doi.org/10.1080/01688637908401105 for promoting generalization. Journal of the International
Taylor, J. B. (2006). My stroke of insight: A brain scientist’s Neuropsychological Society, 4(6), 661–674.
personal journey. Viking. Thompson, C. K., Choy, J., Holland, A., & Cole, R. (2010).
Teasdale, G., & Jennett, B. (1974). Assessment of coma and Sentactics: Computer-automated treatment of underly-
impaired consciousness. Lancet, 2(7872), 81–84. https:// ing forms. Aphasiology, 24(10), 1242–1266. https://doi​
doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(74)91639-0 .org/10.1080/02687030903474255
Teng, E. L., & Chui, H. C. (1987). The Modified Mini-Men- Thompson, C. K., & den Ouden, D. B. (2008). Neuroimag-
tal State (3MS) examination. Journal of Clinical Psychia- ing and recovery of language in aphasia. Current Neu-
try, 48(8), 314–318. rology and Neuroscience Reports, 8(6), 475–483. https://
Teoh, W. Q., Brebner, C., & McAllister, S. (2018). Bilingual doi.org/10.1007/s11910-008-0076-0
assessment practices: Challenges faced by speech-​ Thompson, C. K., Hall, H. R., & Sison, C. E. (1986). Effects
language pathologists working with a predominantly of hypnosis and imagery training on naming behavior
bilingual population. Speech, Language and Hearing, in aphasia. Brain and Language, 28(1), 141–153. https://
21(1), 10–21. https://doi.org/10.1080/2050571X.2017​ doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(86)90097-0
.1309788 Thompson, C. K., Riley, E. A., Ouden, D. den, Meltzer-As-
Terrell, B. Y., & Ripich, D. N. (1989). Discourse compe- scher, A., & Lukic, S. (2013). Training verb argument
tence as a variable in intervention. Seminars in Speech structure production in agrammatic aphasia: Behav-
and Language, 10(4), 282–297. https://doi.org/10.1055/​ ioral and neural recovery patterns. Cortex, 49(9), 2358–
s-2008-1064269 2376. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.02.003
Terrio, H., Brenner, L. A., Ivins, B. J., Cho, J. M., Helmick, Thompson, C. K., & Shapiro, L. P. (2005). Treating agram-
K., Schwab, K., . . . Warden, D. (2009). Traumatic brain matic aphasia within a linguistic framework: Treatment
567
References  

of underlying forms. Aphasiology, 19(10–11), 1021–1036. Tompkins, C. A. (2012). Rehabilitation for cognitive-com-
https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030544000227 munication disorders in right hemisphere brain dam-
Thompson, C. K., Shapiro, L. P., Kiran, S., & Sobecks, J. age. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 93(1),
(2003). The role of syntactic complexity in treatment S61–S69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2011.10.015
of sentence deficits in agrammatic aphasia: The Com- Tompkins, C. A., Bloise, C. G., Timko, M. L., & Baumgaert-
plexity Account of Treatment Efficacy (CATE). Journal ner, A. (1994). Working memory and inference revision
of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46(3), 591–607. in brain-damaged and normally aging adults. Journal of
https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2003/047) Speech and Hearing Research, 37(4), 896–912. https://doi​
Thornton, R., & Light, L. (2006). Language comprehension .org/10.1044/jshr.3704.896
and production in normal aging. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Tompkins, C. A., Fassbinder, W., Lehman-Blake, M. T.,
Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (6th & Baumgaertner, A. (2002). The nature and implica-
ed., pp. 261–287). Elsevier. tions of right hemisphere language disorders: Issues in
Threats, T. T. (2005). Cultural sensitivity in health-care set- search of answers. In A. E. Hillis (Ed.), Handbook of adult
tings. Perspectives on Communication Disorders and Sci- language disorders: Integrating cognitive neuropsychology,
ences in Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Populations, neurology, and rehabilitation (pp. 429–448). Psychology
12, 3–6. https://doi.org/10.1044/cds12.3.3 Press.
Threats, T. T. (2010a). The complexity of social/cultural Tompkins, C. A., & Lehman, M. T. (1998). Interpreting
dimension in communication disorders. Folia Phoniat- intended meanings after right hemisphere brain dam-
rica et Logopaedica, 62(4), 158–165. https://doi.org/​10.11​ age: An analysis of evidence, potential accounts, and clin-
59/​000314031 ical implications. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 5, 29–47.
Threats, T. T. (2010b). The ICF and speech-language pathol- https://doi.org/10.1310/2NTF-GTQU-MXN0-L3U7
ogy: Aspiring to a fuller realization of ethical and moral Toro, P., Schönknecht, P., & Schröder, J. (2009). Type II dia-
issues. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, betes in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s
12(2), 87–93. https://doi.org/10.3109/​1754​95​ 00903568476 disease: Results from a prospective population-based
Threats, T. T., & Worrall, L. (2004). Classifying communi- study in Germany. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 16(4),
cation disability using the ICF. Advances in Speech Lan- 687–691. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-2009-0981
guage Pathology, 6(1), 53–62. https://doi.org/10.1080/​ Townend, E., Brady, M., & McLaughlan, K. (2007a). A sys-
14417040410001669426 tematic evaluation of the adaptation of depression diag-
Ting, D. S. J., Pollock, A., Dutton, G. N., Doubal, F. N., nostic methods for stroke survivors who have aphasia.
Ting, D. S. W., Thompson, M., & Dhillon, B. (2011). Stroke, 38(11), 3076–3083. https://doi.org/10.1161/
Visual neglect following stroke: Current concepts and STROKEAHA.107.484238
future focus. Survey of Ophthalmology, 56(2), 114–134. Townend, E., Brady, M., & McLaughlan, K. (2007b). Exclu-
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.survophthal.2010.08.001 sion and inclusion criteria for people with aphasia in
Tocco, M., Bayles, K., Lopez, O., Hofbauer, R., Pejovic, V., studies of depression after stroke: A systematic review
Miller, M., & Saxton, J. (2014). Effects of memantine and future recommendations. Neuroepidemiology, 29(1–
treatment on language abilities and functional commu- 2), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1159/000108913
nication: A review of data. Aphasiology, 28(2), 236–257. Travis, R. (2019). Forever and Ever, Amen. Thomas Nelson.
https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2013.838617 https://www.thomasnelson.com/9781400207985/
Togher, L., Balandin, S., Young, K., Given, F., & Canty, M. forever-and-ever-amen/
(2006). Development of a communication training pro- Trudeau, D. L., Anderson, J., Hansen, L. M., Shagalov, D.
gram to improve access to legal services for people with N., Schmoller, J., Nugent, S., & Barton, S. (1998). Find-
complex communication needs. Topics in Language Dis- ings of mild traumatic brain injury in combat veterans
orders, 26(3), 199–209. https://hdl.handle.net/10536/ with PTSD and a history of blast concussion. Journal
DRO/DU:30067075 of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 10(3), 308–
Tolbert, J., Orgera, K., & Damico, A. (2020). Key facts about 313. https://doi.org/10.1176/jnp.10.3.308
the uninsured population. KFF. https://www.kff.org/ Truscott, M. (2004). Person to person: Adapting leisure
uninsured/issue-brief/key-facts-about-the-uninsured-​ and creative activities for people with early stage
population/ dementias. Alzheimer’s Care Quarterly, 5(2), 92–102.
Tompkins, C. A. (2008). Theoretical considerations for Tsegaye, M. T., De Bleser, R., & Iribarren, C. (2011). The
understanding “understanding” by adults with right effect of literacy on oral language processing: Implica-
hemisphere brain damage. Perspectives on Neurophysiol- tions for aphasia tests. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics,
ogy and Neurogenic Speech and Language Disorders, 18(2), 25(6–7), 628–639. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.20
45–54. https://doi.org/10.1044/nnsld18.2.45 11.567348
568  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Tucker-Drob, E. M., & Salthouse, T. A. (2013). Individual in unconscious patients with traumatic brain injury.
differences in cognitive aging. Wiley-Blackwell. Journal of Neuroscience Nursing, 41(3), E10–E16. https://
Tuomainen, J., & Laine, M. (1991). Multiple oral reread- doi.org/10.1097/JNN.0b013e3181a23e94
ing technique in rehabilitation of pure alexia. Aphasi- U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics (2017). Projections of Occu-
ology, 5(4–5), 401–409. https://doi.org/10.1080/​0268​ pational Employment, 2016–26. https://www.bls.gov/
7039108248542 careeroutlook/2017/article/occupational-projections-​
Turken, U., & Dronkers, N. F. (2011). The neural archi- charts.htm
tecture of the language comprehension network: U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2021). Speech-language
Converging evidence from lesion and connectivity pathologists. U.S. Department of Labor, occupational out-
analyses. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 5. https:// look handbook. https://www.bls.gov/ooh/healthcare/
doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2011.00001 speech-language-pathologists.htm
Turkstra, L. S. (2010). The positive behavioral momentum U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2010).
of Mark Ylvisaker. Seminars in Speech and Language, National standards for culturally and linguistically appro-
31(3), 162–167. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0030-1257532 priate services (CLAS) in health care. https://www.think​
Turkstra, L. S. (2013). Inpatient cognitive rehabilitation: culturalhealth.hhs.gov/Content/clas.asp
Is it time for a change? Journal of Head Trauma Reha- U.S. Food and Drug Administration. (2013). Hyperbaric
bilitation, 28(4), 332–336. https://doi.org/10.1097/ oxygen therapy: Don’t be misled. https://www.rxlist.com/​
HTR.0b013e31828b4f3f script/main/art.asp?articlekey=173012#:~:text=Hyper​
Turkstra, L. S., Coelho, C., & Ylvisaker, M. (2005). The baric%20Oxygen%20Therapy%3A%20Don%27t%20
use of standardized tests for individuals with cogni- Be%20Misled%20No%2C%20hyperbaric,has%20
tive-communication disorders. Seminars in Speech and not%20been%20cleared%20or%20approved%20by%
Language, 26(4), 215–222. 20FDA.
Turkstra, L. S., Ylvisaker, M., Coelho, C., Kennedy, M. R. U.S. News and World Report. (2021a). U.S. news best jobs
T., Sohlberg, M. M., Avery, J., & Yorkston, K. (2005). rankings. https://money.usnews.com/careers/best-​
Practice guidelines for standardized assessment for jobs/​rankings
persons with traumatic brain injury. Journal of Medical U.S. News and World Report. (2021b). Occupational out-
Speech-Language Pathology, 13, ix–xxviii. look occupation finder. https://www.bls.gov/ooh/occu​
Turner-Stokes, L., Williams, H. & Johnson, J. (2009). Goal pation-finder.htm
Attainment Scaling; does it provide added value as a Vallat-Azouvi, C., Azouvi, P., Le-Bornec, G., & Brunet-​
person-centred measure for evaluation of outcome in Gouet, E. (2019). Treatment of social cognition impair-
neurorehabilitation following acquired brain injury? ments in patients with traumatic brain injury: A critical
Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 41, 528-535. review. Brain Injury, 33(1), 87–93. https://doi.org/10.10
Ueda, S., & Okawa, Y. (2003). The subjective dimension 80/02699052.2018.1531309
of functioning and disability: What is it and what is van Alphen, M. K. H. (2019). Setting communication goals
it for? Disability and Rehabilitation, 25, 596–601. https:// with people with aphasia and informal caregivers using Goal
doi.org/10.1080/0963828031000137108 Attainment Scaling [Master’s thesis]. Utrecht University.
Ufer, K., & Wilson, B. A. (2000). BADS: Behavioral assessment van der Gaag, A., Smith, L., Davis, S., Moss, B., Corne-
of the dysexecutive syndrome. Thames Valley Test Co. lius, V., Laing, S., & Mowles, C. (2005). Therapy and
United Nations (n.d.). United Nations careers: What we support services for people with long-term stroke and
do. https://careers.un.org/lbw/home.aspx?view​type​ aphasia and their relatives: A six-month follow-up
=WWD&lang=en-US study. Clinical Rehabilitation, 19(4), 372–380. https://doi​
United Nations. (2006). Convention on the rights of persons .org/10.1191/0269215505cr785oa
with disabilities. https://www.un.org/disabilities/con- Van der Meulen, I., van de Sandt-Koenderman, M. W. M.
vention/conventionfull.shtml E., Heijenbrok, M. H., Visch-Brink, E., & Ribbers, G. M.
United Nations, Department of Economic and Social (2016). Melodic intonation therapy in chronic aphasia:
Affairs, Population Division (2017). World population Evidence from a pilot randomized controlled trial.
prospects: The 2017 revision, key findings and advance Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10. https://doi.org/​
tables. Working Paper No. ESA/P/WP/248. 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00533
United Nations General Assembly. (1948). “Universal dec- Van der Meulen, I., van de Sandt-Koenderman, W. M.,
laration of human rights” (217 [III] A). https://www. Heijenbrok-Kal, M. H., Visch-Brink, E. G., & Ribbers,
un.org/en/universal-declaration-human-rights G. M. (2014). The efficacy and timing of melodic into-
Urbenjaphol, P., Jitpanya, C., & Khaoropthum, S. (2009). nation therapy in subacute aphasia. Neurorehabilita-
Effects of the sensory stimulation program on recovery
569
References  

tion & Neural Repair, 28(6), 536–544. https://doi.org/​ roimaging characteristics. Neuroscience Letters, 743,
10.1177/1545968313517753 135564. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2020.135564
Van der Ploeg, E. S., Eppingstall, B., Camp, C. J., Runci, von Steinbüchel, N., Wilson, L., Gibbons, H., Hawthorne,
S. J., Taffe, J., & O’Connor, D. W. (2013). A random- G., Höfer, S., Schmidt, S., . . . Truelle, J.-L. (2010). Quality
ized crossover trial to study the effect of personalized, of Life after Brain Injury (QOLIBRI): Scale development
one-to-one interaction using Montessori-based activ- and metric properties. Journal of Neurotrauma, 27(7),
ities on agitation, affect, and engagement in nursing 1167–1185. https://doi.org/10.1089/neu.2009.1076
home residents with dementia. International Psycho- Voss, P., Bodner, E., & Rothermund, K. (2018). Ageism:
geriatrics, 25(4), 565–575. https://doi.org/10.1017/S104​ The relationship between age stereotypes and age
1610212002128 discrimination. In L. Ayalon & C. Tesch-Römer (Eds.),
Van Eetvelt, S., Marella, M., Logam, L., & Robinson, A. Contemporary perspectives on ageism: International per-
(2020). What does the evidence say? A literature review spectives on aging (Vol. 19, pp. 11–31). Springer. https://
of the evidence on including people with disabilities doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-73820-8_2
and older people in humanitarian response. Humanitar- Wada, J., & Rasmussen, T. (1960). Intracarotid injection of
ian Exchange, 78, 24–27. https://odihpn.org/magazine/ sodium amytal for the lateralization of cerebral speech
what-does-the-evidence-say-a-literature-review-of- dominance experimental and clinical observations.
the-evidence-on-including-people-with-disabilities-​ Journal of Neurosurgery, 17(2), 266–282. https://doi​
and-older-people-in-humanitarian-response/ .org/10.3171/jns.1960.17.2.0266
van Vuuren, S., & Cherney, L. R. (2014). A virtual thera- Wade, D. T. (2018). How often is the diagnosis of the
pist for speech and language therapy. In T. Bickmore, permanent vegetative state incorrect? A review of the
S. Marsella, & C. Sidner (Eds.), Intelligent virtual agents evidence. European Journal of Neurology, 25(4), 619–625.
(pp. 438–448). Springer International. https://link. https://doi.org/10.1111/ene.13572
springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-3-319-09767-1_55 Waggoner, T. L. (1994). Color vision testing made easy. Home
Vance, D. E., & Johns, R. N. (2003). Montessori improved Care Vision.
cognitive domains in adults with Alzheimer’s disease. Wagner, P. J., Lentz, L., & Heslop, S. D. M. (2002). Teach-
Physical & Occupational Therapy in Geriatrics, 20(3–4), ing communication skills: A skills-based approach.
19–33. https://doi.org/10.1080/J148v20n03_02 Academic Medicine: Journal of the Association of American
Vasterling, J. J., Verfaellie, M., & Sullivan, K. D. (2009). Medical Colleges, 77(11), 1164.
Mild traumatic brain injury and posttraumatic stress Währborg, P. (1991). Assessment and management of emo-
disorder in returning veterans: Perspectives from cog- tional and psychosocial reactions to brain damage and apha-
nitive neuroscience. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(8), sia. Singular Publishing.
674–684. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.08.004 Walker, G. M., & Schwartz, M. F. (2012). Short Form
Ventry, I. M., & Weinstein, B. E. (1982). The hearing handi- Philadelphia Naming Test: Rationale and empiri-
cap inventory for the elderly: A new tool. Ear and Hear- cal evaluation. American Journal of Speech-Language
ing, 3(3), 128–134. Pathology, 21(2), S140– S153. https://doi.org/10.1044/​
Verhaeghen, P. (2003). Aging and vocabulary scores: 1058-​0360(2012/11-0089)
A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 18(2), 332–339. Walker-Batson, D. (2000). Use of pharmacotherapy in the
https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.18.2.332 treatment of aphasia. Brain and Language, 71(1), 252–
Verna, A., Davidson, B., & Rose, T. (2009). Speech-language 254. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1999.2262
pathology services for people with aphasia: A survey Walker-Batson, D., Curtis, S., Natarajan, R., Ford, J.,
of current practice in Australia. International Journal of Dronkers, N., Salmeron, E., . . . Unwin, D. H. (2001).
Speech-Language Pathology, 11(3), 191–205. https://doi A double-blind, placebo-controlled study of the use of
.org/10.1080/17549500902726059 amphetamine in the treatment of aphasia. Stroke, 32(9),
Vignolo, L. A. (1964). Evolution of aphasia and language 2093–2098. https://doi.org/10.1161/hs0901.095720
rehabilitation: A retrospective exploratory study. Cor- Walker-Batson, D., Mehta, J., Smith, P., & Johnson, M. (2016).
tex, 1(3), 344–367. https://doi.org/10.1016/S00​10-​9452​ Amphetamine and other pharmacological agents in
(64)80008-3 human and animal studies of recovery from stroke. Prog-
Vines, B. W., Norton, A. C., & Schlaug, G. (2011). Non-​ ress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry,
invasive brain stimulation enhances the effects of 64, 225–230. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2015.04.002
melodic intonation therapy. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. Wallace, G. L. (2006). Blast injury basics: A primer for
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00230 the medical speech-language pathologist. The ASHA
Vogrig, A., Gigli, G. L., Bnà, C., & Morassi, M. (2021). Leader, 11(9), 26–28. https://doi.org/10.1044/leader​
Stroke in patients with COVID-19: Clinical and neu- .FTR7.11092006.26
570  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Wallace, S. E., Purdy, M., & Skidmore, E. (2014). A mul- Wambaugh, J. L., Wright, S., & Nessler, C. (2012). Modi-
timodal communication program for aphasia during fied response elaboration training: A systematic exten-
inpatient rehabilitation: A case study. NeuroReha- sion with replications. Aphasiology, 26(12), 1407–1439.
bilitation, 35(3), 615–625. https://doi.org/10.3233/ https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2012.702887
NRE-141136 Wambaugh, J. L., Wright, S., Nessler, C., & Mauszycki, S.
Wallace, S. J., Worrall, L., Rose, T., & Le Dorze, G. (2014). C. (2014). Combined Aphasia and Apraxia of Speech
A good outcome for aphasia. Aphasiology, 28(11), 1400– Treatment (CAAST): Effects of a novel therapy. Journal
1404. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2014.935119 of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 57(6), 2191–
Wallace, S. J., Worrall, L., Rose, T., Le Dorze, G., Breiten- 2207. https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_JSLHR-L-14-0004
stein, C., Hilari, K., . . . Webster, J. (2019). A core outcome Wang, Q.-Y., & Li, D.-M. (2016). Advances in art therapy for
set for aphasia treatment research: The ROMA consen- patients with dementia. Chinese Nursing Research, 3(3),
sus statement. International Journal of Stroke, 14(2), 180– 105–108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cnre.2016.06.011
185. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747493018806200 Wang, S.-C., Yu, C.-L., & Chang, S.-H. (2017). Effect of
Waller, G. (2009). Evidence-based treatment and therapist music care on depression and behavioral problems in
drift. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(2), 119–127. elderly people with dementia in Taiwan: A quasi-ex-
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2008.10.018 perimental, longitudinal study. Aging & Mental Health,
Wallesch, W., Joanette, Y., & Lecours, A. R. (Eds.) (1996). 21(2), 156–162. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2015
Classic cases in neuropsychology (pp. 45–52). Psychology .1093602
Press. Warden, D. (2006). Military TBI during the Iraq and
Wallston, K., Malcarne, A., Flores, V. L., Hansdottir, I., Afghanistan wars. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabili-
Smith, C. A., Stein, M. J., . . . Clements, P. J. (1999). Does tation, 21(5), 398–402. https://doi.org/10.1097/​0000​
God determine your health? The God Locus of Health 1199-​200609000-00004
control scale. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 23(2), 131– Wardlaw, J. M., Murray, V., Berge, E., del Zoppo, G.,
142. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018723010685 Sandercock, P., Lindley, R. L., & Cohen, G. (2012).
Wallston, K. A., Wallston, B. S., & DeVellis, R. (1978). Recombinant tissue plasminogen activator for acute
Development of the multidimensional health locus of ischaemic stroke: An updated systematic review and
control (MHLC) scales. Health, Education & Behavior, meta-analysis. Lancet, 379(9834), 2364–2372. https://
6(1), 160–170. doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60738-7
Wambaugh, J. L., Doyle, P. J., Martinez, A. L., & Kalin- Wechsler, D. (2001). Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR).
yak-Fliszar, M. (2002). Effects of two lexical retrieval Psychological Corp.
cueing treatments on action naming in aphasia. Journal Wechsler, D. (2009). WMS-IV Wechsler Memory Scale (4th
of Rehabilitation Research and Development, 39(4), 455– ed.). Pearson.
466. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687040143000302 Weiduschat, N., Thiel, A., Rubi-Fessen, I., Hartmann, A.,
Wambaugh, J. L., & Ferguson, M. (2007). Application of Kessler, J., Merl, P., . . . Heiss, W. D. (2011). Effects of
semantic feature analysis to retrieval action names in repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in apha-
aphasia. Journal of Rehabilitation Research & Develop- sic stroke: A randomized controlled pilot study. Stroke,
ment, 44(3), 381–394. https://doi.org/10.1682/JRRD​ 42(2), 409–415. https://doi.org/10.1161/STROKE​AHA​
.2006.05.0038 .110.597864
Wambaugh, J. L., Mauszycki, S., Cameron, R., Wright, S., Weimer, M. (2014). The art of asking questions. Faculty
& Nessler, C. (2013). Semantic feature analysis: Incor- Focus. https://www.facultyfocus.com/articles/
porating typicality treatment and mediating strategy effective-teaching-strategies/art-asking-questions/
training to promote generalization. American Journal of Weinger, K., & Jacobson, A. M. (1998). Cognitive impair-
Speech-Language Pathology, 22(2), S334– S369. https:// ment in patients with type 1 (insulin-dependent) dia-
doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2013/12-0070) betes mellitus: Incidence, mechanisms and therapeutic
Wambaugh, J. L., Mauszycki, S., & Wright, S. (2014). implications. CNS Drugs, 9(3), 233. https://doi.org/​
Semantic feature analysis: Application to confrontation 10.2165/00023210-199809030-00006
naming of actions in aphasia. Aphasiology, 28(1), 1–24. Weiss, H., Agimi, Y., & Steiner, C. (2010). Youth motor-
https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2013.845739 cycle-related brain injury by state helmet law type:
Wambaugh, J. L., Nessler, C., & Wright, S. (2013). Mod- United States, 2005–2007. Pediatrics, 126(6), 1149–1155.
ified response elaboration training: Application to https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2010-0902
procedural discourse and personal recounts. American Welland, R., Lubinski, R., & Higginbotham, D. (2002). Dis-
Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 22(2), S409–S425. course comprehension test performance of elders with
https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2013/12-0063) dementia of the Alzheimer type. Journal of Speech, Lan-
571
References  

guage & Hearing Research, 45(6), 1175–1187. https://doi​ Wiig, E. H., & Semel, E. M. (1974). Development of com-
.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002/095) prehension of logico-grammatical sentences by grade
Wepman, J. M. (1972). Aphasia therapy: A new look. Jour- school children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 16, 627–636.
nal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 37, 203–214. https:// https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1974.38.1.171
doi.org/10.1044/jshd.3702.203 Wilcox, M. J., Davis, G. A., & Leonard, L. B. (1978). Apha-
Wertz, R., & Irwin, W. H. (2001). The efficacy of language sics’ comprehension of contextually conveyed mean-
rehabilitation in aphasia. Aphasiology, 15(3), 231–247. ing. Brain and Language, 6(3), 362–377. https://doi​
https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.3701.03 .org/10.1016/0093-934X(78)90069-X
Wertz, R. T., Collins, M. J., Weiss, D., Kurtzke, J. F., Friden, Williams, K., Kemper, S., & Hummert, M. L. (2003).
T., Brookshire, R. H., . . . Resurreccion, E. (1981). Vet- Improving nursing home communication: An inter-
erans Administration cooperative study on aphasia: vention to reduce elderspeak. The Gerontologist, 43(2),
A comparison of individual and group treatment. 242–247. https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/43.2.242
Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 24(4), 580–594. Williams, K., Kemper, S., & Hummert, M. L. (2005).
https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.24 04.580 Enhancing communication with older adults: Over-
Wertz, R. T., Dronkers, N. F., Bernstein-Ellis, E., Sterling, coming elderspeak. Journal of Psychosocial Nursing and
L. K., Shubitowski, Y., Elman, R., & Deal, J. L. (1992). Mental Health Services, 43(5), 12–16. https://doi.org/​
Potential of telephonic and television technology for 10.3928/0098-9134-20041001-08
appraising and diagnosing neurogenic communication Williams, K., Shaw, C., Lee, A., Kim, S., Dinneen, E., Turk,
disorders in remote settings. Aphasiology, 6(2), 195–202. M., . . . Liu, W. (2017). Voicing ageism in nursing home
https://doi.org/10.1080/02687039208248591 dementia care. Journal of Gerontological Nursing, 43(9),
Wertz, R. T., Weiss, D. G., Aten, J. L., Brookshire, R. H., 16–20. https://doi.org/10.3928/00989134-20170523-02
García-Buñuel, L., Holland, A. L., . . . Brannegan, R. Williams, K. T. (1997). Expressive Vocabulary Test–Second
(1986). Comparison of clinic, home, and deferred lan- edition (EVT 2). Journal of the American Academy of Child
guage treatment for aphasia: A Veterans Administra- & Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 864–872.
tion cooperative study. Archives of Neurology, 43(7), Wilshire, C. E., Lukkien, C. C., & Burmester, B. R. (2014).
653–658. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.1986​.0052​ The sentence production test for aphasia. Aphasiology,
0070011008 28(6), 658–691. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2014
Westby, C. (2009). Considerations in working success- .893555
fully with culturally/linguistically diverse families in Wilson, B., Cockburn, J., & Halligan, P. (1987a). Behavioral
assessment and intervention of communication dis- Inattention Test. Thames Valley Test Co.
orders. Seminars in Speech & Language, 30(4), 279–289. Wilson, B., Cockburn, J., & Halligan, P. (1987b). Develop-
https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0029-1241725 ment of a behavioral test of visuospatial neglect. Archives
Whitaker, H. A. (1984). Editorial note: Two views on apha- of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 68(2), 98–102.
sia classification. Brain and Language, 21, 1–2. Wilson, B., Evans, J. J., Emslie, H., Foley, J., Shiel, A., Wat-
White, T., & Stern, R. A. (2003). NAB, Neuropsychological son, P., . . . Groot, Y. (2005). Cambridge Prospective Mem-
Assessment Battery: Psychometric and technical manual. ory Test. Pearson.
Psychological Assessment Resources. Wilson, B. A., Alderman, N., Burgess, P. W., Emslie, H., &
Whitten, P. (2006). Telemedicine: Communication tech- Evans, J. J. (1996). The behavioural assessment of the dysex-
nologies that revolutionize healthcare services. Gener- ecutive syndrome. Thames Valley Test Co.
ations, 30(2), 20–24. Wilson, B. A., Baddeley, A. D., & Cockburn, J. (2008). River-
Whurr, R. (1996). The Aphasia Screening Test AST (2nd ed.). mead Behavioural Memory Test– Third Edition (RBMT-3).
Singular Publishing. Pearson.
Wickenden, M. (2013). Widening the SLP lens: How can Wilson, S. M., & Schneck, S. M. (2021). Neuroplasticity
we improve the well-being of people with commu- in post-stroke aphasia: A systematic review and meta-
nication disabilities globally. International Journal of analysis of functional imaging studies of reorganization
Speech-Language Pathology, 15(1), 14–20. https://doi.org/​ of language processing. Neurobiology of Language, 2(1),
10.3109/17549507.2012.726276 22–82. https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00025
Wiig, E. H., Nielson, N. P., Minthon, L., & Warkentin, S. Winans-Mitrik, R. L., Hula, W. D., Dickey, M. W., Schum-
(2003). Alzheimer’s Quick Test: Assessment of parietal func- acher, J. G., Swoyer, B., & Doyle, P. J. (2014). Description
tion. Svensk Version & Norsk Versjon. Psykologifør- of an intensive residential aphasia treatment program:
laget. Rationale, clinical processes, and outcomes. American
Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. (1989). Test of Language Compe- Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 23(2), S330–S342.
tence — Expanded edition. Pearson Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_AJSLP-13-0102
572  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Winblad, B., Palmer, K., Kivipelto, M., Jelic, V., Frati- World Health Organization (WHO). (2004). The Mexico
glioni, L., Wahlund, L.-O., . . . Petersen, R. C. (2004). statement on health research. Knowledge for better health:
Mild cognitive impairment — Beyond controversies, Strengthening health systems.
towards a consensus: Report of the International World Health Organization (WHO). (2006). Constitution of
Working Group on mild cognitive impairment. Journal the World Health Organization. https://www.who.int/
of Internal Medicine, 256(3), 240–246. https://doi.org/​ governance/eb/who_constitution_en.pdf
10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01380.x World Health Organization (WHO). (2010). Framework
Wingfield, A., Tun, P. A., & McCoy, S. L. (2005). Hear- for action on interprofessional education and collaborative
ing loss in older adulthood. Current Directions in Psy- practice. https://whqlibdoc.who.int/hq/2010/WHO_
chological Science, 14(3), 144–148. https://doi.org/​ HRH_HPN_ 10.3_eng.pdf
10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00356.x World Health Organization (WHO). (2020). The top 10
Winhuisen, L., Thiel, A., Schumacher, B., Kessler, J., Rudolf, causes of death. https://www.who.int/news-room/
J., Haupt, W. F., & Heiss, W. D. (2005). Role of the con- fact-sheets/detail/the-top-10-causes-of-death
tralateral inferior frontal gyrus in recovery of language World Health Organization (WHO) & World Bank. (2011).
function in poststroke aphasia: A combined repeti- World report on disability.
tive transcranial magnetic stimulation and positron Worrall, L. (2006). Professionalism and functional out-
emission tomography study. Stroke, 36(8), 1759–1763. comes. Journal of Communication Disorders, 39(4), 320–
https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.0000174487.81126.ef 327. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2006.02.007
Wiseman-Hakes, C., MacDonald, S., & Keightley, M. Worrall, L., Brown, K., Cruice, M., Davidson, B., Hersh,
(2010). Perspectives on evidence based practice in ABI D., Howe, T., & Sherratt, S. (2010). The evidence for a
rehabilitation. “Relevant research”: Who decides? Neu- life-coaching approach to aphasia. Aphasiology, 24(4),
roRehabilitation, 26(4), 355–368. 497–514. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030802698152
Wolf, P. A., D’Agostino, R. B., Belanger, A. J., & Kannel, W. Worrall, L. E., Hudson, K., Khan, A., Ryan, B., & Simmons-​
B. (1991). Probability of stroke: A risk profile from the Mackie, N. (2017). Determinants of living well with
Framingham Study. Stroke, 22(3), 312–318. https://doi​ aphasia in the first year poststroke: A prospective
.org/10.1161/01.str.22.3.312 cohort study. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabil-
Wolfe, E. W., & Smith, E. V. (2007). Instrument develop- itation, 98(2), 235–240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr​
ment tools and activities for measure validation using .2016.06.020
Rasch models: Part I. Instrument development tools. Worrall, L., Rose, T., Howe, T., McKenna, K., & Hickson,
Journal of Applied Measurement, 8(1), 97–123. L. (2007). Developing an evidence-base for accessibil-
Woodcock, R. W., McGregor, K., & Mather, N. (2007). ity for people with aphasia. Aphasiology, 21(1), 124–136.
Woodcock Johnson III Normative Update (NU) Tests of Cog- https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030600798352
nitive Abilities (WJIII NU). Riverside. Worrall, L., Sherratt, S., Rogers, P., Howe, T., Hersh, D.,
Woolgar, A., Hampshire, A., Thompson, R., & Duncan, Ferguson, A., & Davidson, B. (2011). What people with
J. (2011). Adaptive coding of task-relevant informa- aphasia want: Their goals according to the ICF. Apha-
tion in human frontoparietal cortex. Journal of Neuro- siology, 25(3), 309–322. https://doi.org/10.1080/026870
science, 31(41), 14592–14599. https://doi.org/10.1523/ 38.2010.508530
JNEUROSCI.2616-11.2011 Worrall, L., Wallace, S., Rose, T., Brady, M., Kong, A., Hal-
World Health Organization (WHO). (n.d.b). WHO lowell, B., & Murray, L. L.(2016). Let’s call it “apha-
community-based rehabilitation (CBR). https://www sia”: Rationales for eliminating the term “dysphasia.”
.who.int/disabilities/cbr/en/ International Journal of Stroke, 11(8), 848–851. https://doi​
World Health Organization (WHO). (n.d.-c). Rehabilit- .org/10.1177/1747493016654487
sation 2030: A call for action. https://www.who.int/ Wright, H. H., & Capilouto, G. J. (2009). Manipulating
initiatives/rehabilitation-2030 task instructions to change narrative discourse perfor-
World Health Organization (WHO). (1980). International clas- mance. Aphasiology, 23(10), 1295–1308. https://doi.org/​
sification of impairments, disabilities, and handicaps: A man- 10.1080/02687030902826844
ual of classification relating to the consequences of disease. Wright, H. H., Capilouto, G. J., Srinivasan, C., & Fergadi-
World Health Organization (WHO). (1999). ICIDH-2 otis, G. (2011). Story processing ability in cognitively
International Classification of Functioning and Disability: healthy younger and older adults. Journal of Speech,
Beta-2 draft, full version, July 1999. https://www.sustain- Language, and Hearing Research, 54(3), 900–917. https://
able-design.ie/arch/Beta2full.pdf doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0253)
World Health Organization (WHO). (2001). International Wright, H. H., Capilouto, G., Wagovich, S., Cranfill, T., &
Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF). Davis, J. (2005). Development and reliability of a quanti-
573
References  

tative measure of adults’ narratives. Aphasiology, 19(3–5), Ylvisaker, M. (1998). Traumatic brain injury rehabili-
263–273. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030444000732 tation: Children and adolescents (Vol. 14, 2nd ed.).
Wright, H. H., Marshall, R. C., Wilson, K. B., & Page, J. Butterworth-Heinemann.
L. (2008). Using a written cueing hierarchy to improve Ylvisaker, M. (2006). Self-coaching: A context-sensitive,
verbal naming in aphasia. Aphasiology, 22(5), 522–536. person-centred approach to social communication after
https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030701487905 traumatic brain injury. Brain Impairment, 7(3), 246–258.
Wright, H. H., & Shisler, R. J. (2005). Working memory in https://doi.org/10.1375/brim.7.3.246
aphasia: Theory, measures, and clinical implications. Yivisaker, M., Coelho, C., Kennedy, M., Sohlberg, Turk­
American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 14(2), stra, L., Avery, & Yorkston. (2002). Reflections on evi-
107–118. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2005/ dence-based practice and rational clinical decision
012) making. Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology,
Wylie, K., McAllister, L., Davidson, B., & Marshall, J. 10, xxvi–xxxiii.
(2013). Changing practice: Implications of the World Ylvisaker, M., & Feeney, T. (2009). Apprenticeship in
Report on Disability for responding to communica- self-regulation: Supports and interventions for indi-
tion disability in underserved populations. Interna- viduals with self-regulatory impairments. Develop-
tional Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 15(1), 1–13. mental Neurorehabilitation, 12(5), 370–379. https://doi​
https://doi.org/10.3109/17549507.2012.745164 .org/10.3109/17518420903087533
Wylie, K., McAllister, L., Davidson, B., & Marshall, J. Ylvisaker, M., Shaughnessy, M. F., & Greathouse, D. (2002).
(2016). Communication rehabilitation in sub-Saharan An interview with Mark Ylvisaker about students with
Africa: A workforce profile of speech and language traumatic brain injury. North American Journal of Psy-
therapists. African Journal of Disability, 5(1). https://doi​ chology, 4(4), 291.
.org/​10.4102/ajod.v5i1.227 Ylvisaker, M., Turkstra, L. S., & Coelho, C. (2005). Behav-
Xiao, Y., Wang, J., Jiang, S., & Luo, H. (2012). Hyperbaric ioral and social interventions for individuals with trau-
oxygen therapy for vascular dementia. Cochrane Data- matic brain injury: A summary of the research with
base of Systematic Reviews, (7), 1–21. https://doi.org/​ clinical implications. Seminars in Speech and Language,
10.1002/14651858.CD009425.pub2 26(4), 256–267. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2005-922104
Xing, S., Zhu, C., Zhang, R., & An, L. (2014). Huperzine Ylvisaker, M., Turkstra, L. S., Coehlo, C., Yorkston, K.,
A in the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease and vascu- Kennedy, M., Sohlberg, M. M., & Avery, J. (2007).
lar dementia: A meta-analysis. Evidence-Based Comple- Behavioural interventions for children and adults with
mentary and Alternative Medicine, 2014. https://doi.org/​ behaviour disorders after TBI: A systematic review of
10.1155/2014/363985 the evidence. Brain Injury, 21(8), 769–805. https://doi​
Xu, S. S., Gao, Z. X., Weng, Z., Du, Z. M., & Xu, W. A. .org/10.1080/02699050701482470
(1995). Efficacy of tablet Huperzine A on memory, cog- Yorkston, K. M., & Beukelman, D. R. (1980). An analysis
nition, and behavior in Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Phar- of connected speech samples of aphasic and normal
macologica Sinica, 16(5), 391–395. speakers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 45(1),
Xydakis, M. S., Fravell, M. D., Nasser, K. E., & Casler, J. D. 27–36. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.4501.27
(2005). Analysis of battlefield head and neck injuries Yorkston, K. M., & Beukelman, D. R. (1984). Assessment of
in Iraq and Afghanistan. Otolaryngology–Head and Neck intelligibility of dysarthric speech. Pro-Ed.
Surgery, 133(4), 497–504. https://doi.org/10.1016/j​ Yoshikawa, Y., Ohmaki, E., Kawahata, H., Maekawa, Y.,
.otohns.2005.07.003 Ogihara, T., Morishita, R., & Aoki, M. (2019). Beneficial
Yamamoto, S., DeWitt, D., & Prough, D. (2018). Impact effect of laughter therapy on physiological and psycho-
and blast traumatic brain injury: Implications for ther- logical function in elders. Nursing Open, 6(1), 93–99.
apy. Molecules, 23(2), 245. https://doi.org/10.3390/ https://doi.org/10.1002/nop2.190
molecules23020245 Youmans, G., Holland, A., Muñoz, M., & Bourgeois, M.
Yeates, G. N. (2020). The potential contributions of mind- (2005). Script training and automaticity in two indi-
body interventions within psychological support fol- viduals with aphasia. Aphasiology, 19(3–5), 435–450.
lowing aphasia: A conceptual review and case study. In https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030444000877
K.H. Meredith & G. N. Yeates (Eds), Psychotherapy and Ystad, M. A., Wehling, E., Rootwelt, H., Espeseth, T.,
chg to: aphasia: Interventions for emotional wellbeing and Westlye, L. T., Andersson, M., . . . Lundervold, A.
relationships (pp. 136–164). (2009). Hippocampal volumes are important predic-
Ylvisaker, M. (1992). Communication outcome following tors for memory function in elderly women. BMC
traumatic brain injury. Seminars in Speech and Language, Medical Imaging, 9, 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471
13, 239–251. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2008-1064200 -2342-9-17
574  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Yue, J., Dong, B. R., Lin, X., Yang, M., Wu, H. M., & Wu, Zhu, Z., Hou, X., & Yang, Y. (2018). Reduced syntactic
T. (2012). Huperzine A for mild cognitive impairment. processing efficiency in older adults during sentence
Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. https://online­ comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. https://doi​
library.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/14651858.CD008827​ .org/​10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00243
.pub2/abstract Zientz, J., Rackley, A., Chapman, S. B., Hopper, T., Mahen-
Zacks, R. T., & Hasher, L. (1993). Capacity theory and the dra, N., Kim, E. S., & Cleary, S. (2007). Evidence-based
processing of inferences. In L. L. Light & D. M. Burke practice recommendations for dementia: Educating
(Eds.), Language, memory, and aging (pp. 154–170). Cam- caregivers on Alzheimer’s disease and training commu-
bridge University Press. nication strategies. Journal of Medical Speech-Language
Zeng, Y., Zhao, Y., Zhang, T., Zhao, D., Zhao, F., & Lu, Pathology, 15(1), liii–lxiv.
E. (2020). A brain-inspired model of theory of mind. Zigmond, A. S., & Snaith, R. P. (1983). Hospital anxiety
Frontiers in Neurorobotics, 14. https://doi.org/10.3389/ and depression scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavia, 6,
fnbot.2020.00060 361–370.
Zhang, H.-F., Huang, L.-B., Zhong, Y.-B., Zhou, Q.-H., Zraick, R. I., Allen, R. M., & Johnson, S. B. (2003). The
Wang, H.-L., Zheng, G.-Q., & Lin, Y. (2016). An over- use of standardized patients to teach and test inter-
view of systematic reviews of Ginkgo biloba extracts personal and communication skills with students in
for mild cognitive impairment and dementia. Fron- speech-language pathology. Advances in Health Sciences
tiers in Aging Neuroscience, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/ Education: Theory and Practice, 8(3), 237–248. https://doi​
fnagi.2016.00276 .org/10.1023/A:1026015430376
Zhang, T., Wu, X., Cao, S., & Park, S. (2018). Efficacy of Zraick, R. I., Harten, A. C., & Hagstrom, F. (2014). Interpro-
the Oriental herbal medicine, Jie Yu Dan, for allevi- fessional education and practice: A primer for training
ating post-stroke aphasia: A systematic review and future clinicians. SIG 10 Perspectives on Issues in Higher
meta-analysis of randomized clinical trials. European Education, 17, 39–46. https://doi.org/10.1044/aihe​17.2.39
Journal of Integrative Medicine, 24, 35–48. https://doi​ Zuscak, S. J., Peisah, C., & Ferguson, A. (2016). A collab-
.org/​10.1016/j.eujim.2018.10.012 orative approach to supporting communication in the
Zhang, Z., Wang, X., Chen, Q., Shu, L., Wang, J., & Shan, G. assessment of decision-making capacity. Disability and
(2002). Clinical efficacy and safety of huperzine Alpha Rehabilitation, 38(11), 1107–1114. https://doi.org/10​
in treatment of mild to moderate Alzheimer disease, .3109/09638288.2015.1092176
a placebo-controlled, double-blind, randomized trial.
Zhonghua Yi Xue Za Zhi, 82(14), 941–944.
Index

Note: Page numbers in bold reference images and tables.

A neurodegenerative disease, 72
stroke as cause of. See Stroke
A-FROM. See Living with Aphasia: Framework for thiamine deficiency, 72
Outcome Measurement thyroid disorders, 72
A-GOAT. See Galveston Orientation and Amnesia Test traumatic brain injury. See Traumatic brain injury
for Aphasia vitamin B12 deficiency, 72
A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed, 288 eye-movement problems with, 93
AAC. See Alternative and augmentative neurophysiological principles pertinent to, 78–80
communications traumatic brain injury-related, 4
AAN. See American Academy of Neurology Acquired immunodeficiency syndrome. See HIV/
AAPPSPA. See American Academy of Private Practice AIDS
in Speech Pathology and Audiology Acquired neurogenic communication disorders
AASP. See Acute Aphasia Screening Protocol depression in, 411–412
ABA-2. See Apraxia Battery for Adults, Second Edition global capacity building for, 210–212
Abbreviations, 330, 331–339 global engagement in, 212–214
ABCD. See Arizona Battery for Communication group therapy for, 413
Disorders of Dementia life improvements after, 413
AbilityNet, 385 Acquired neurogenic language disorders. See also
AbleNet, 385 Acquired cognitive-linguistic disorders
“Abstract attitude,” 46–47 advocacy for, 203–204
Academics, PhD-level staff members in, 9 anxiety associated with, 269
Academy of Aphasia, 25 assessment of, 221–233
Academy of Neurologic Communication Disorders bacterial infections as cause of, 68–69
and Sciences, 21, 25, 285, 353, 392, 399 challenges in identifying etiology of, 73
Acalculia, 242 congenital neurogenic language disorders versus,
Acceleration-deceleration injuries, 64, 65, 67 4–5
Accountability, 191 definition of, 3–4
ACE-III. See Addenbrooke’s Cognitive depression associated with, 268
Examination-III description of, 3–5
ACESA. See Assessment of Communicative diabetes mellitus as cause of, 70–71, 71
Effectiveness in Severe Aphasia diabetic encephalopathy as cause of, 70–71
Achromatopsia, 84 humor and, 376
Ackerman, Diane, 196 metabolic syndrome as cause of, 71
ACOM. See Aphasia Communication Outcome neoplasm as cause of, 69–70
Measure opportunistic infections as cause of, 69
Acquired cognitive-linguistic disorders screening for, 242–245, 243–244
definition of, 3–4 severity of, factors that may affect, 84
description of, 3–4 stroke as cause of. See Stroke
etiologies of toxemia as cause of, 70
alcohol abuse, 72 traumatic brain injury. See Traumatic brain injury
challenging in identifying, 73 Acquired neurogenic motor speech disorders, 5
diabetes mellitus, 71, 71 ACRT. See Anagram, Copy, and Recall Treatment

575
576  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

ACS. See Activity Sort Cards brain-related changes associated with, 117–118
ACT. See Anagram and Copy Treatment; Aphasia cognitive-linguistic changes associated with,
couples therapy 121–124, 122
Action Naming Test, 278, 290 cultural aspects of, 211–212
Action potentials, 85 definition of, 115–116
Active patient, 28 demographics of, 117
Activity, 52 discourse affected by, 120–121
Activity limitations, 51 healthy, 116–117
Activity Sort Cards, 290 language comprehension affected by, 123
Acute Aphasia Screening Protocol, 286 memory affected by, 118–119, 121, 122
Acute care medical model, 389 mild cognitive impairment versus, 316
Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination-III, 301 neuroplasticity losses during, 79
Additive conjunctives, 322 pragmatics affected by, 121
ADEAR Center. See Alzheimer’s Disease Education primary, 121
and Referral Center reading affected by, 120
Adjunct, 419 secondary, 121
Adjuvant, 419 self-regulatory skills gained during, 116
ADP. See Aphasia Diagnostic Profiles sentence comprehension affected by, 120
Adult day care centers, 179 vocabulary affected by, 119
Adult learning word-finding problems associated with, 119–120,
cycling approach to, xxvii 122
theory of, xxvi writing affected by, 120
Advance directives, 205 Aging population, 117, 209–210
Advocacy Aging well, 116–117
for aphasia, 39, 39 Agnosia
ethics in, 203–204 apperceptive, 93
for health care reform, 184 associative, 93
human rights in, 203–204 auditory, 94, 159
knowledge translation, 203, 355 visual, 93, 259
morality principles in, 203–204 visual object, 259
proactive approaches to, 193–206 Agrammatic aphasia, 466
speech-language pathologist’s role in, 176 Agrammatism, 138, 473–474
AES. See Apathy Evaluation Scale AIDS. See Assessment of the Intelligibility of
Affect, 156 Dysarthric Speech; HIV/AIDS
Age ALA. See Assessment for Living with Aphasia
biological, 116 Alarcon, Nancy, 346
chronological, 115–116, 252 Albert test, 257, 257
cognitive, 116 Alcohol abuse, 72, 168
as potentially confounding factor, 252 Alertness, 158
psychological, 116 Alexia, 4, 32
social, 116 Alexia with or without agraphia, 481
stroke recovery prognosis affected by, 83 ALFA. See Assessment of Language-Related
traumatic brain injury recovery prognosis affected Functional Activities
by, 83 Allen chart, 254–255
Age-associated cognitive decline, 72 Allocentric neglect, 158, 255, 257
Age-related identity threat, 119 Allopathic, 419
Age-related macular degeneration, 253 ALPS. See Aphasia Language Performance Scales
Aged, 29 Alternative and augmentative communications, 371,
Ageism, 126 382, 384–385
Agent, 463–464, 470 Alzheimer’s Association, 166, 415
Aging. See also Older adults Alzheimer’s disease. See also Dementia
biopsychosocial models of, 116 age of onset, 166
brain health in, 124, 125 dementia associated with, 166–167
577
Index  

in developing countries, 210 computed tomography, 110


diagnosis of, 166–167 magnetic resonance, 110
herbs for, 425 Angioplasty, 62
ischemic cerebrovascular disease and, 167 Angular gyrus, 141
mild cognitive impairment caused by, 166 Angular injuries, 65
neurofibrillary tangles associated with, 117, 166 Annual deductible, 184
positron emission tomography images of, 108 Anomia, 32, 69. See also Cueing hierarchies to anomia
risk factors for, 166 Anomic aphasia, 136, 141–142
strengths of persons with, 222 Anosognosia, 158, 266
sundowner’s syndrome associated with, 166 ANT. See Action Naming Test
Alzheimer’s Disease Education and Referral Center, Antecedent-based behavior management, 350
415 Anterior cerebral artery, 80–81, 81
Alzheimer’s Disease International, 415 Anterior choroidal artery, 81
American Academy of Neurology, 351 Anterior communicating artery, 81
American Academy of Private Practice in Speech Anterior inferior cerebellar artery, 81
Pathology and Audiology, 178 Anterograde amnesia, 146
American Brain Tumor Association, 415 Antioxidants, 361
American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine’s Brain Antiplatelets, 62
Injury Interdisciplinary Special Interest Group, Anxiety, 269
380 AoS. See Apraxia, of speech
American Psychiatric Association criteria for Apathy Evaluation Scale, 303
depression, 268, 268, 411 Aphasia
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association as acquired, 37–38
certification requirements of, 19 advocacy for, 39, 39
evidence-based practice guidelines, 353 agrammatic, 466
Functional Assessment of Communication Skills for anomic, 136, 141–142
Adults, 290 anterior types of, 132
Functional Communication Measure, 277 assessment of, 290–296
National Outcomes Measurement System, 277 awareness about, 195–197, 197–199, 409
payment model updates from, 183 biopsychosocial framework for, 45
Special Interest Group 2, 25 brain tumors associated with, 69
Special Interest Group 15, 25 Broca’s. See Broca’s aphasia
American Stroke Association, 63, 415 characteristics of, 37–40, 134
Amnesia, 146 in children, 37–38
Amphetamines, 360 classification of, 131, 134, 135–136, 142–143
Amsterdam Nijmegen Everyday Language Test, 279, cognitive neuropsychological framework for, 43–45
290, 317 cognitive rehabilitation applications to, 379–381
Amusia, 159 conceptualizing of, 40, 42–48
Amyloid plaques, 117, 166 concrete-abstract framework for, 46–47
Anagram(s), 480 conduction, 132, 135, 140
Anagram, Copy, and Recall Treatment, 481 crossed, 136, 141
Anagram and Copy Treatment, 479–481 definition of, 3–5, 32, 37–40, 41
Anastomosis, 80 description of, 37
ANCDS. See Academy of Neurologic Communication disciplines relevant to, 7, 7
Disorders and Sciences as dissociation syndrome, 134
ANELT. See Amsterdam Nijmegen Everyday dysgraphia as symptom of, 142
Language Test dyslexia as symptom of, 142
Anemia, sickle cell, 59 expressive, 38, 132
Aneurysm fluent, 132–134, 133, 316
definition of, 58 frameworks for, 40, 42–48
hemorrhagic stroke caused by, 58 global, 83, 135
Angiography group treatment for, 381–382, 383–384
cerebral, 109, 109–110 herbs for, 425
578  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Aphasia (continued) Aphasia Alliance, 25, 415


incidence of, 195 Aphasia and Stroke Association of India, 415
intensive programs for, 385–387 Aphasia Center of California, 415
jargon, 134 Aphasia centers, 179, 181, 184
as language disorder, 38 Aphasia Communication Outcome Measure, 277, 290
language disorders associated with, 164 Aphasia Corner, 415
language modalities affected by, 38 Aphasia couples therapy, 373
life-affecting impacts of, 371 Aphasia Diagnostic Profiles, 276, 281, 290
lifelong consequences of, 46 Aphasia Education and Advocacy Team, Lansing
linguistic errors associated with, 6 (Michigan) Area Aphasia Support Group, 198
manifestations of, 131 Aphasia-friendly communication, 349–350
medical framework for, 42 Aphasia Games for Health, 377
microgenetic framework for, 47 Aphasia Institute, 415
mixed transcortical, 136, 140 Aphasia Language Performance Scales, 286
mood assessments in, 269 Aphasia mentoring programs, 375
motor speech disorders and, 38 Aphasia Now, 415
multidimensional framework for, 42 Aphasia Recovery Connection, 180, 197, 377, 386, 386,
neuroanatomical models of, 54 415
neurolinguistic definition of, 41 Aphasia Screening Test, Second Edition, 286
neurological cause of, 38 Aphasia Software Finder, 385
nonfluent, 132–134, 133, 316 Aphasia Tones, 39
optic, 93 Aphasia United, 25
paraphasias associated with, 134, 137, 139 Aphasia Bank, 318
person-first language for, 28 Aphasia Scripts, 384, 446
posterior types of, 132 Aphasiologists. See also Clinical aphasiologists;
pragmatic abilities in, 315 Clinician
prevalence of, 195 academic credentials of, 20–21
primary progressive. See Primary progressive career opportunities for, 7
aphasia certification requirements for, 22
prognosis for recovery from, 83 in colleges, 8
propositional language framework for, 47 credentials of, 20–21
psycholinguistic framework for, 43, 44 definition of, 5
quality of life affected by, 41 educational requirements of, 22
receptive, 38, 132 ethical considerations for, 213–215
residential programs for, 385–387 interprofessional collaborative competencies of, 15,
rights of individuals with, 204–205 15–16
severity of, 46, 83 as leaders, 6, 9, 176
social determinants of health framework for, 46 learning by, 6–7, 12
social framework for, 45–46 licensure of, 22
specific language impairment versus, 5 in management positions, 176
subcortical, 136, 141 mentoring by, 176
sudden onset of, 408 as multicultural and multilingual, 6
syndromes of, 131–143, 133, 135–137 patients and, 5–6
thought process framework for, 47 professional work settings for, 8
transcortical motor, 136 research, 21
transcortical sensory, 135 in research settings, 8
traumatic brain injury as cause of, 146 in residential settings, 126
unidimensional framework for, 40, 42 scientific knowledge of, 7
well-being effects of, 46 specialization of, 21–22
Wernicke-Lichtheim model of, 134 suggestions for, 18, 19
Wernicke’s, 79, 83, 105, 134, 135, 137, 140, 315 teaching by, 176
Aphasia Access, 25, 347, 386, 415 undesirable traits of, 18
579
Index  

in universities, 8 language, 252–272, 317


work settings for, 8, 126 life participation focus of, 222
Aphasiology, xxvi, 5, 31 linguistic differences, 227–233
Apperceptive agnosia, 93 location for, 224
Apps, 382, 384–385 measurement error in, 229
Apraxia memory, 241, 267
constructional, 259 mild cognitive impairment, 301–302
ideational, 261 multicultural aspects of, 227–229
limb, 261–262 nonstandardized, 232
of speech, 5, 169, 260–261, 452 as ongoing intervention process, 343–344
Apraxia Battery for Adults, Second Edition, 261 outcomes, 276–277
APT. See Attention Process Training person-centered, 221–222, 223, 276
APT-Test. See Attention Process Training-Test personal factors, 224
AQT. See A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed primary progressive aphasia, 301–302
ARC. See Aphasia Recovery Connection problem-solving challenges for, 226–227
Argument structure, 470 as process, 219
Aricept. See Donepezil process analysis approach, 229
Arizona Battery for Communication Disorders of psychometric properties addressed in, 235–237
Dementia, 301 purposes of, 219–220, 224
Arousal problems, as potentially confounding factor, reasons for, 275–277
263–265 as research process, 229, 230
Arteriosclerosis, 59 results of. See Assessment results
Arteriovenous malformation, 58–59 right brain injury, 300
Artificial intelligence, 382 role in, 221
Asanas, 422 scheduling of, 224
ASF. See Aphasia Software Finder speaking directly to person during, 225–226
ASHA. See American Speech-Language-Hearing standardized, 231–232
Association standardized testing and, 228
Assessment(s) strengths-based, 222, 245
acquired neurogenic cognitive-linguistic disorders, test design factors, 237
303–310 timing of, 224
aphasia, 290–296 traumatic brain injury, 149, 277–278, 297–300
best practices in, 219–234, 233, 316 treatment and, 219
brain tumor, 70 Assessment for Living with Aphasia, 276–277, 291
clinical practice relevancy of, 220 Assessment instruments. See also Assessment tools
clinician’s role in, 318 administration of, 281–282
cognition, 288–289 alternative response modes, 278–279
cognitive-linguistic abilities, 238–242 appropriateness for content, 279
collaborative approach to, 226–227 availability of, 281
communication disability effects, 224–225 constructs, 278
comprehensive, 245–246 data collection from, 276–277
conditions for, 224 difficulty level of items, 278
confounding factors in, 237–242, 238–241 evaluation of, 282, 283–284
context factors, 237 framework for conceptualizing language and
criterion-referenced measures in, 232–233 communication, 280
cultural differences, 227–233 instructions for, 281
dementia, 301–302 norming of, 278
dynamic, 231–232, 313 online resources for, 285
environmental factors, 224 potentially confounding factors, 278
experts versus tests in diagnosis, 227 practicality of, 281
eye contact during, 226 psychometric properties of, 279–280
interpersonal factors, 237 quality of, 279–280
580  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Assessment instruments (continued) Association Internationale Aphasie, 25


response modes, 278–279 Associative agnosia, 93
results of, 282 AST. See Aphasia Screening Test, Second Edition
scoring of, 281 Astrocytoma, 69
selection of, 275–282, 285 Ataxia, 262
standardization of, 278 Atherosclerosis, 59
theoretical model of, 280–281 Atherosclerotic plaque, 58, 59
updating of, 279 Athetosis, 263
Assessment of Communicative Effectiveness in Severe Attention
Aphasia, 290 assessment of, 241
Assessment of Language-Related Functional deficits in
Activities, 291 as potentially confounding factor, 265–266
Assessment of the Intelligibility of Dysarthric Speech, in right hemisphere syndrome, 154, 155, 158
261, 303 in traumatic brain injury survivors, 146
Assessment process divided, 266
comprehensive, 245–246 focused, 266
person-centered focus of, 221–222 in language processing, 380
persons involved in, 224 selective, 266
psychometric properties addressed in, 235–237 Attention Process Training, 297
as research process, 354 Attention Process Training-Test, 379
Assessment Protocol of Pragmatic Skills, 317 Attention switching, 266
Assessment reports Audiologist, 259
abbreviations used in, 330, 331–339 Auditory agnosia, 94, 159
information included in, 330 Auditory association areas, 95
Assessment results Auditory comprehension, 238, 271, 272, 278, 347, 380
best practices for sharing of, 327–330 Auditory discrimination, 259
clinician’s influence on, 221 Auditory distractions, 123
communicative strengths in, 330 Auditory evoked potentials, 111
counseling related to, 409 Auditory evoked response testing, 259
cultural impacts on, 228 Auditory language/linguistic processing, 94, 95
documenting of, 327–340 Auditory lexical perception, 43
reporting of, 329–330 Auditory-perceptual impairments, in right
sharing of, 327–328 hemisphere syndrome, 155, 159
writing used to share, 327, 329–330 Auditory phonological analysis, 43
Assessment tools. See also Assessment instruments Auditory processing, 238
acquired neurogenic cognitive-linguistic disorders, Auditory sound agnosia, 94
303–310 Auditory stimulation, 381
administration of, 281–282 Auditory system. See also Hearing
aphasia, 290–296 neurophysiology of, 94, 95
availability of, 281 supplemental review of, 98–99
dementia, 301–302 Auditory verbal agnosia, 94
evaluation of, 282, 283–284 AusTOM. See Australian Therapy Outcome Measure
instructions for, 281 Australia
language screening, 286–287 career outlook in, 9
mild cognitive impairment, 301–302 certification requirements in, 19, 20
online resources for, 285 cultural competence in, 14
practicality of, 281 health care system in, 183
primary progressive aphasia, 301–302 Australian Aphasia Association, 25, 415
results of, 281–282 Australian Therapy Outcome Measure, 277
right brain injury, 300 Autobiographical memory, 118, 122
scoring of, 281–282 Automatic speech, 239
traumatic brain injury, 297–300 Autotopagnosia, 259
Association for Frontotemporal Degeneration, 416 AVM. See Arteriovenous malformation
581
Index  

Awareness problems, as potentially confounding in intervention, 343–355, 350


factor, 263–265 in responding to misguided statements, 410–411
Ayurvedic medicine, 420, 424 in sharing of assessment results, 327–330
Better Conversations with Aphasia: A Learning
Resource, 416
B
Beukelman, David, 347
B/F VAT. See Bucco-facial visual action therapy BI TBI. See Blast-induced traumatic brain injury
Back to the Drawing Board, 438–439 Biden, Joe, 196
Bacopa monniera, 425 Bilateral quadrantopsia, 89, 92
Bacterial infections, 68–69 Bilingual Verbal Ability Tests, 304
BADS. See Behavioral Assessment of Dysexecutive Binocular field of view, 87, 87
Syndrome Biological age, 116
Ballismus, 263 Biopsy, for brain tumor diagnosis, 70
Balloons Test, 303 Biopsychosocial framework, 45
Barkley Deficits in Executive Functioning Scale, 303 Biopsychosocial models of aging, 116
BAS-DEC. See Brief Assessment Schedule Depression Birmingham Cognitive Screen, 288
Cards BIT. See Behavioral Inattention Test
Basilar artery, 80, 81 Bitemporal (heteronymous) hemianopsia, 89, 91
BBICC-RHI. See Burns Brief Inventory of Blast-induced traumatic brain injury, 66–67, 148
Communication and Cognition: Right Blindness, 89, 90
Hemisphere Inventory Blood-brain barrier, 68
BC-ANCDS. See Board certification in adult Blood glucose, 70
neurogenic communication disorders and Blood oxygen-level dependent effect, 106
sciences BNT. See Boston Naming Test
BCS. See Birmingham Cognitive Screen BNVR. See Butt Nonverbal Reasoning Test
BDAE-3. See Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Board certification in adult neurogenic
Examination-3 communication disorders and sciences, 21
BDB. See Back to the Drawing Board Body functions, 52
BDEFS. See Barkley Deficits in Executive Functioning Body structures, 52, 231
Scale BOLD effect. See Blood oxygen-level dependent effect
Bedside Evaluation Screening Test, Second Edition, BOSS. See Burden of Stroke Scale, The
286 Boston Assessment of Severe Aphasia, 279, 291
BEFAST mnemonic, 63, 64 Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination-3, 276, 281,
Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function- 291
Adult Version, 303 Boston Group Classification, 134
Behavioral Assessment of Dysexecutive Syndrome, Boston Naming Test, 278, 291, 464
267, 297 Boston Naming Test-2, 280
Behavioral Inattention Test, 259, 303 Bourgeois, Michelle, 347
Behavioral intervention, 79 Bradykinesia, 262
Behavioral management, 380 Brain
Behavioral treatment, 359 age-related changes in, 117–118, 124, 125
Behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia, 168 anatomy of, 76–78, 76–78
Belmont Report, 213 atrophy of, 117
Below-average intelligence, 252 blood supply to
Beneficence, 203 anatomy of, 77, 80–83, 81–82
Benign senescent memory loss, 72 circle of Willis, 80–82, 81, 109
Benton Visual Retention Test, Fifth Edition, 303 functions of, 80
BEST-2. See Bedside Evaluation Screening Test, imaging of, 82
Second Edition interruptions to, 60–61
Best practices supplemental review of, 96–98
in assessment, 219–234, 233, 316 Brodmann’s areas of, 54, 76
clinical aphasiologist’s use of, 14 cortical landmarks and structures of, 77
in discourse analysis, 323 edema of, 60, 358
582  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Brain (continued) circumlocutions in, 139


functional areas of, 76, 77 Copy and Recall Treatment for, 479, 480
health-promoting behaviors for, 124, 125 depression and, 139
hemispheric specialization of, 78–79 description of, 83, 132
interconnectivity in, 79 discourse in, 315
intrahemispheric specialization of, 79 discovery of, 137–138
left hemisphere of, 78–79 dysnomia in, 139
lobes of, 77 emotional lability in, 139
meninges of, 78 Helm Elicited Language Program for Syntax
necrosis of, 60 Stimulation for, 475
neuroanatomy of, 76–78, 76–78, 96 language production in, 138
plasticity of, 79–80, 358 motor speech deficits in, 139
right hemisphere of, 78–79 paraphasias in, 139
structure-function relationships in, 78–79 paresis associated with, 139
traumatic injury to. See Traumatic brain injury Response Elaboration Training for, 451–452
tumors of, 69–70 reversible passives and, 138
ventricles of, 78 Broca’s area, 76
views of, 77 Brodmann’s areas, 54, 76, 89, 134, 140
white matter of, 77, 117–118 Broussard, Tom, 195, 197, 199, 328
Brain attack, 57 BT. See Balloons Test
Brain-behavior interfaces, 385 BTA. See Brief Test of Attention
Brain damage, 32 BTHI. See Brief Test of Head Injury
Brain injuries. See also Traumatic brain injury Bucco-facial visual action therapy, 440
blast-induced, 66–67 Burden of Stroke Scale, The, 304, 446
recovery from Burns Brief Inventory of Cognition and
behavioral treatment in, 359 Communication, 304
mechanisms of, 358–359 Burns Brief Inventory of Communication and
neurobiology of, 365 Cognition: Right Hemisphere Inventory, 300
spontaneous, 79, 358 Butt Nonverbal Reasoning Test, 292
Brain Injury Association, 25, 416 BVAT. See Bilingual Verbal Ability Tests
Brain Injury Interdisciplinary Special Interest Group, bvFTD. See Behavioral variant frontotemporal
380 dementia
Brain stimulation, 360–361 BVMT-R. See Brief Visuospatial Memory
Brain Trauma Foundation, 416 Test-Revised
Brainstem BVRT-5. See Benton Visual Retention Test, Fifth
cochlear nuclei of, 95 Edition
structures of, 77–78 Byng, Sally, 346
Breakfast Club, The, 399, 399 Byrne, Kerry, 346
BRIEF-A. See Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive
Function-Adult Version
C
Brief Assessment Schedule Depression Cards, 269
Brief Cognitive Assessment Tool, 288 CADL-3. See Communicative Activities of Daily
Brief Test of Attention, 304 Living, Third Edition
Brief Test of Head Injury, 297 CAIAC. See Cognitive approach to improving
Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised, 304 auditory comprehension
Brisbane Evidence-Based Language Test, 281, 291 Calcarine fissure, 87, 89
British Aphasiology Society, 25, 416 Calculation, 242
Broca, Pierre Paul, 137 California State University East Bay Aphasia
Broca’s aphasia Treatment Program, 197
agrammatism associated with, 138 California Verbal Learning Test, Second Edition, 304
Anagram and Copy Treatment for, 480 Cambridge Prospective Memory Test, 304
catastrophic reaction in, 139 CAMPROMPT. See Cambridge Prospective Memory
characteristics of, 135 Test
583
Index  

Canada in Europe, 19–20


aphasia centers in, 386 of speech-language pathologists, 19
career outlook in, 9 in United States, 19, 22
certification requirements in, 20 CETI. See Communicative Effectiveness Index
Canadian Partnership for Stroke Recovery, 285 Change in the Weather: Life After Stroke, 196
Canonical sentences, 470, 473 Chapey, Roberta, 346
Capacity, 52 Chart method, of Semantic Feature Analysis, 458,
Capitation, 185 458–459
CAPM. See Comprehensive Assessment of CHAT, 318
Prospective Memory CHI. See Closed-head injury
Carbon dioxide, 80 Child aphasia, 5
Care partner, 30, 392–393 Children
Career aphasia in, 37–38
credentials necessary for, 20–21 motherese with, 124–125
opportunities and outlook, 7–9 China, 9
strategies for development of, 22, 23 Chinese medicine, 420, 424
Caregiver Cholinergic drugs, 360
coaching of, 373, 392–393 Cholinesterase inhibitors, 365
description of, 30 Chorea, 263
support for, 392–393 Chronological age, 115–116, 252
training of, 362, 373–374, 392–393 CIAT. See Constraint-induced aphasia therapy
Carotid arteries, 80, 81 CILT. See Constraint-induced language therapy
CART. See Copy and Recall Treatment CIMT. See Constraint-induced movement therapy
Case-based decision-making, 344 Circle of Willis
Case-based learning, xxvi anatomy of, 80–82, 81
Case history, 246, 246–247 cerebral angiography of, 109
Case managers, 188, 190 Circumlocution, 165
Case rate, 185 Circumlocutions, 139
CAT. See Coding Analysis Toolkit; Comprehensive CIT. See Constraint-induced therapy
Aphasia Test; Computed axial tomography CITE. See Boston Assessment of Severe Aphasia
Cataracts, 89 CIU. See Correct information unit
Catastrophic reaction, 139 CLAN, 318
CATE. See Complexity account of treatment efficacy CLAS. See National Standards for Culturally and
Causal conjunctives, 322 Linguistically Appropriate Services in Health
CBR. See Community-based rehabilitation Care
CCRCs. See Continuing care retirement communities Clinical aphasiologists. See also Aphasiologists;
CCRSA. See Communication Confidence Rating Scale Clinician
for Aphasia best practices use by, 14
CDP. See Communication Disability Profile; career opportunities and outlook for, 7–9
Communicative Drawing Program conflicts of interest by, 206
Ceiling rule, 231 credentials of, 20–21
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 68 cultural humility of, 14–15, 28
Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services, 183 cultural responsiveness by, 14–15, 28
Central auditory processing disorders, 259 educational requirements for, 20
Central nervous system, 77. See also Brain interprofessional collaborative competencies of, 15,
Cerebral angiography, 109, 109–110 15–16
Cerebral arteries, 80–81, 81–82 as leaders, 6, 9
Cerebral blood flow licensure of, 22
age-related reductions in, 117 lifelong learning by, 12, 175
regional, 106 patients and, 5–6
Cerebrovascular accident, 57. See also Stroke role of, xxiv
Certification scientific knowledge of, 7
in Australia, 19 suggestions for, 18, 19
584  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Clinical aphasiologists (continued) Cochrane Libraries, 352, 428


terminology used to describe, 31 Code-Müller Protocols, 292
undesirable traits of, 18 Codeswitching, 157
Clinical aphasiology, 5, 21 Coding Analysis Toolkit, 319
Clinical Aphasiology Conference, 25 Coglink, 384
Clinical doctoral programs, 21 Cognistat, 288
Clinical Guidelines for Stroke Management, 351 Cognition
Clinical practice assessments of, 245, 278
assessment areas relevant to, 220 nonverbal aspects of, 276
business aspects of, 176 screening of, 288–289
discourse analysis application to, 324–325 Cognitive age, 116
settings for. See Service settings Cognitive approach to improving auditory
strength of recommendations for, 351 comprehension, 380
Clinical services Cognitive-communicative impairments
accessibility to, 211 in dementia, 164–165
cost-control systems’ effect on, 189 in mild cognitive impairment, 164
health care finance systems’ effect on, 189 in right hemisphere syndrome, 154, 154–155
infrastructure factors that affect access to, 211 Cognitive development, 116
“medically necessary,” 187, 191 Cognitive effort, 112
payment for. See Payment Cognitive-linguistic abilities
preauthorization for, 186 aging-related changes in, 121–124, 122
settings for. See Service settings assessment of, 238–242
Clinician. See also Aphasiologists; Clinical decline in, neurodegenerative intervention’s effect
aphasiologists; Speech-language pathologists on, 365–366
assessment results influenced by, 221 neurodegenerative conditions that affect, 163–164
characteristics of, 12–18, 13 in older adults, 121, 122
credentials of, 20–21 Cognitive-linguistic disorders. See Acquired
cultural humility of, 14–15, 28 cognitive-linguistic disorders
cultural responsiveness by, 14–15, 28 Cognitive linguistic profile, 315
educational requirements of, 20, 22, 186 Cognitive Linguistic Quick Test, 281, 288
excellence of, 12–18, 13, 15–17 Cognitive neuropsychological approaches, 378–379
interprofessional collaborative competencies of, 15, Cognitive neuropsychological framework, 43–45
15–16 Cognitive Neuroscience Society, 25
lifelong learning by, 12, 175 Cognitive processing
requirements to be, xxiii age-related changes in, 124
skilled services, 186–187 model, of reading, 263
suggestions for, 18, 19 Cognitive rehabilitation, 379–381
undesirable traits of, 18 Cognitive resources hypothesis, 156
as vehicle, 15–18, 221 Cognitive stimulation approach, 380–381
Clock drawing test, 257, 258 Cognitive supports, 380
Closed-class words, 138 Cohesion analysis, 320–321
Closed-head injury, 64–65, 67, 316 Collaboration of Aphasia Trialists, 25
CLQT. See Cognitive Linguistic Quick Test Collaborative learning, xxvi
Co-survivors, 30 Collateral circulation, 80
Coach, 403–406 Collateral sprouting, 358, 360
Coaching Color Trails Test, 305
caregiver, 373, 392–393 Color vision deficits, 255
information sharing in, 413–415 Color Vision Testing Made Easy, 255
life, 404–405 Coma, 263, 264–265, 265
multicultural differences’ effect on, 407 Comb and Razor Test, 305
wellness, 404 Comité Permanent de Liaison des Orthophonistes–
Cochlea, 95 Logopèdes de L’Union Européenne, 20
Cochrane Collaboration, 352 Common carotid artery, 80
585
Index  

Common Objects Memory Test, 305 learning about, 420–421


Communication mind-body practices, 421–422
access training in, 196, 197 multicultural differences, 427
aphasia-friendly, 349–350 natural products, 424–425, 425
community-based approaches, 202–203 potential for harm, 429
in dementia reasons for popularity of, 425–426
challenges associated with, 164–166 religion, 424
FOCUSED program for enhancing, 397, 398 research studies on, 426, 426–427
support for, 400 speech-language pathologist’s support for people
functional, 346–347 considering use of, 427
as human right, 194–195, 343 spirituality, 424
supported, 371–372, 397 visualization, 423–424
total, 372–373, 397 wellness as focus of, 419–420
words used in, 27 yoga, 422, 423
Communication Confidence Rating Scale for Aphasia, Complexity account of treatment efficacy, 364, 435,
292 460, 472, 474
Communication counselors, 405 Comprehension
Communication disabilities auditory, 238, 271, 272, 278, 347, 380
medicalization of, 202 discourse, 322
real-life impacts of, 224–225 language, 123
underestimation of, 8 reading, 238, 347, 483
Communication Disability Profile, 277, 292 sentence, 120
Communication Forum Scotland, 416 syntactic, 122
Communication partner training, 373–374, 374 Comprehensive Aphasia Test, 281, 292
Communication problems Comprehensive assessment, 245–246
in dementia, 164–166 Comprehensive Assessment of Prospective Memory,
in mild cognitive impairment, 164–166 297
in right hemisphere syndrome, 154, 154–155 Comprehensive Test of Nonverbal Intelligence,
in traumatic brain injury survivors, 146, 146–147, Second Edition, 305
150 Comprehensive Trail-Making Test, 305
Communication Profile for the Hearing Impaired, 305 Computational models, 43
Communication support, 371–372 Computed axial tomography, 101–102, 102–103
Communication training Computed axial tomography perfusion studies, 102
for partners, 373–374, 374 Computed tomography angiography, 110
for traumatic brain injury survivors, 374 Computer-based script training, 446
Communicative Activities of Daily Living, Third COMT. See Common Objects Memory Test
Edition, 292, 317 Concrete-abstract framework, 46–47
Communicative Drawing Program, 436–438 Concurrent validity, 236
Communicative Effectiveness Index, 277, 292, 317 Concussion, 67–68
Community-based rehabilitation, 202–203 Conduction aphasia, 132, 135, 140
COMPASS, 19 Conduit d’approche, 140
Compassion, 406 Cones, 85, 86
Competencies Confabulation, 168
cultural, 14 Confidence, 229
decision-making, determination of, 205 Conflicts of interest, 206
interprofessional collaborative, 15, 15–16 Confounding factors, potentially
Complementary and integrative approaches age as, 252
counseling of people considering, 428–429 anxiety as, 269
definition of, 419–420 apraxia of speech as, 260–261
evidence regarding, 426, 426–427 arousal problems as, 263–265
herbs, 424–425, 425, 429 in assessment, 237–242, 238–241
hyperbaric oxygen therapy, 428–429 attention problems as, 265–266
hypnosis, 423–424 awareness problems as, 263–265
586  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Confounding factors, potentially (continued) Construct validity, 236


color vision deficits as, 255 Constructional apraxia, 259
concomitant cognitive and linguistic deficits as, Content validity, 236
267–268 Content words, 138–139
controlling for, 278 Context-processing deficiency theories, 123
definition of, 237, 252 Contextual factors, 52–53
depression as, 268, 268–269 Contingency management, 380
in differential diagnosis, 251–252 Continuing care retirement communities, 177–178
dysarthria as, 261 Continuous perseveration, 137
dysgraphia as, 263 Contrecoup injuries, 64, 65–66
emotional lability as, 270, 411 Control participant, 30
executive function deficits as, 266–267 Controlled trials, 352–353
hearing problems as, 259–260 Convergent validity, 236
higher-level visual integration problems as, 259 Conversation, discourse versus, 314
lack of awareness of deficits as, 266 Conversational coaching, 373
in language assessment, 252–272 Coordination of care, 191
limb apraxia as, 261–262 Copay, 184
memory problems as, 267 Copy and Recall Treatment
motor challenges as, 260–263 definition of, 478
motor deficits as, 262–263 evidence-based practice and, 479–480
ocular motor problems as, 255 implementation of, 478–479
paralysis as, 262 principles of, 478
paresis as, 262 Cornea, 86
pragmatic deficits as, 267 Correct information unit, 321
process analysis approach, 270–271, 271–272 Cortical mapping, 111. See also Electrocorticography
reading problems as, 263 Cortical reorganization, 358–359
visual acuity deficits as, 253–255, 254 Cost-control systems, 189–191
visual attention deficits as, 256 Council on Academic Accreditation in Speech-
visual field deficits as, 255 Language Pathology, 186
visual neglect as, 255–259, 256–258 Counseling
writing deficits as, 263 assessment results-related, 409
Confrontation naming ability, 453 on complementary and integrative approaches,
Confrontation naming tasks, 456, 461 428–429
Conjunctives, 322 definition of, 404
Connect: The Communication Disability Network, 416 at discharge, 409
Consciousness end-of-life care enhanced by, 412
differential diagnosis of, 264–265 information sharing in, 413–415
loss of, 60 missed opportunities for, 412–413
signs of, 264 multicultural differences’ effect on, 407
states of, 263–264, 264 rehabilitation, 405
Conservatorship of a person’s property, 205 self-advocacy, 409–410
Constant Therapy, 382, 384 at start of intervention, 409
Constraint-induced aphasia therapy, 436, 443 after traumatic change, 408, 408–409
Constraint-induced language therapy in treatment, 409
definition of, 443 Counseling mindset, 406
description of, 363 Counseling moments, 407–409
evidence-based practice and, 444–445 Counselors
implementation of, 444 communication, 405
principles of, 443 speech-language pathologist as, 403–406
Promoting Aphasics’ Communicative Effectiveness Coup injuries, 64, 65–66
and, 444 COVID-19, 180–181
Constraint-induced movement therapy, 443 CPHI. See Communication Profile for the Hearing
Constraint-induced therapy, 363 Impaired
587
Index  

Creative Abundance model, 375 Deep dyslexia, 4, 93


Credentials, 20–21 Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System, 267, 297
Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease, 69, 168 Dementia
Criterion-referenced measures, 232–233 AIDS dementia complex, 69
Criterion validity, 236 of Alzheimer’s type, 166. See also Alzheimer’s
Cross-cultural learning, 6 disease
Crossed aphasia, 136, 141 assessment tools for, 301–302
CRT. See Comb and Razor Test cognitive-communicative impairments in, 164–165
CT. See Computed axial tomography cognitive symptoms of, 165, 165
CTA. See Computed tomography angiography communication in
CTMT. See Comprehensive Trail-Making Test challenges associated with, 164–166
CTONI-2. See Comprehensive Test of Nonverbal FOCUSED program for enhancing, 397, 398
Intelligence, Second Edition support for, 400
CTT. See Color Trails Test definition of, 164
Cue-behavior associations, 395 in developing countries, 210
Cueing hierarchies to anomia diagnostic criteria for, 72
in Copy and Recall Treatment, 479 direct intervention/treatment for, 391–392
definition of, 455 etiologic-based classification of, 166
evidence-based practice and, 456–457 executive function deficits in, 164–165, 165
principles of, 455–456 frontotemporal, 167–168
Cues, 455–457 group treatment for, 399
Cultural competence, 14 herbs for, 425
Cultural differences, assessment awareness of, 227–233 incorrect diagnosis of, 171
Cultural humility, 14–15, 28, 228 with Lewy bodies, 167
Cultural responsiveness, 14–15, 28, 228 in long-term care residents, 399
Culturally and Linguistically Appropriate Services in memory problems in, 165, 165
Health Care, 14 Montessori approach to, 397–399
Culturally responsive practice, xxiv multi-infarct, 167
Culture Parkinson’s-associated, 167
assessment results affected by, 228 pragmatic impairments in, 165
discourse influenced by, 323 progressive nature of, 171
intervention affected by, 345 pseudodementia, 171
CVA. See Cerebrovascular accident quality of life in, 390
CVLT-II. See California Verbal Learning Test, Second reminiscence-based programs for, 400
Edition reversible, 169, 171
Cycling approach, to adult learning, xxvii speech-language pathologist services in
challenges associated with delivery of, 389–390
quality of life benefits of, 390
D
reimbursement for, 390–391
D-KEFS. See Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System role of, 171–172
d2 Test of Attention, 305 scope of practice, 390
Damico, Jack, 346–347 strengths of persons with, 222
DCT-2. See Discourse Comprehension Test, Second symptoms of, 72, 166–168
Edition third-party payors’ response to, 171
DEB. See Dysarthria Examination Battery vascular, 167, 425
Decision making word-finding problems in, 165
case-based, 344 Dementia Advocacy and Support Network, 416
in competencies, 205–206 Dementia Beyond Disease: Enhancing Well-Being, 350
errors in, 229 Dementia Beyond Drugs: Changing the Culture of Care,
Declaration of Helsinki, 213 350
Decubitus ulcer, 270 Dementia Rating Scale-2, 301
Dedoose, 319 Dementia Reconsidered, Revisited: The Person Still Comes
Deductible, 184 First, 392
588  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Dementia Reconsidered: The Person Comes First, 350, 392 assessment of, 240
DementiaBank, 318 in Broca’s aphasia, 315
DEMQOL. See Quality of Life in Dementia competence measures for, 318–322, 320–321
Dendritic branching, 358 context for, 317
Denial of reimbursement, 188–189 conversation versus, 314
Deoxyhemoglobin, 106 cultural influences on, 323
Department of Veterans Affairs, 184 definition of, 231, 313
Depression extralinguistic, 314
American Psychiatric Association criteria for, 268, genres of, 314
268, 411 importance of, 315–316
Broca’s aphasia and, 139 linguistic, 314
mental health services for, 405 paralinguistic, 314
in neurogenic communication disorders, 411–412 right hemisphere syndrome-related impairments in,
as potentially confounding factor, 268, 268–269 154, 154–155, 160
symptoms of, 268, 268 Semantic Feature Analysis application to, 460
Derivational linguistic theory, 470 subsystems of, 313
Detroit Test of Learning Aptitude — Adult, 306 traumatic brain injury-related impairments in, 147
Diabetes mellitus, 70–71, 71 in traumatic brain injury survivors, 315
Diabetic encephalopathy, 70–71 types of, 314–315
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Discourse Abilities Profile, 317
164, 411 Discourse analysis
Diagnostic labeling, 329 best practices in, 323
Diagnostic process challenges in, 324–325
assessment in, 220 clinical practice application of, 324–325
tests used in, 227 coding of sample, 318, 323
Diaschisis, 60, 358 definition of, 315
Dietary modifications, 61 description of, 220
Different Strokes: Support for Younger Stroke differential diagnosis information from, 316
Survivors, 416 equipment needed for, 324
Differential diagnosis importance of, 315–316
confounding factors in, 251–252 intervention based on, 323–324
consciousness, 264–265 intraindividual variability in, 323, 324
definition of, 251 measures used in, 318, 320–321, 321
discourse analysis for information about, 316 programs for, 319
importance of, 163 quantitative and qualitative methods in, 324
reasons for difficulty in, 251–252 relevance of, 325
in traumatic brain injury survivors, 149 research and, 316, 324–325
“Differently abled,” 29 results of, 317, 323–324
Diffuse axonal injury, 65–66 sampling strategies for, 316–318
Diffuse injury, 65 standardized tests for, 317
Diffusion magnetic resonance imaging, 103, 106 strengths and weaknesses identified with, 315–316
Diffusion tensor imaging, 103, 106 time requirements for, 324–325
Diffusion-weighted imaging, 104 transcribing, 319
Direct injury, 64 treatment planning use of, 316
Disability variability in, 323, 323
cultural aspects of, 211–212 Discourse coherence
definition of, 51–52 age-related declines in, 120
people with, 29 definition of, 322
variations in perception of, 8 in right hemisphere syndrome, 157
World Health Organization models of, 116 Discourse cohesion, 322
Discharge, counseling at, 409 Discourse comprehension indices, 320, 322
Discourse Discourse Comprehension Test, Second Edition, 293,
age-related changes in, 120–121 317
589
Index  

Discovery phase, of outcomes research, 352 Dysphasia, 4, 32


Discriminant validity, 236 Dysprosodia, 157
Disfluencies, 120, 139 Dysprosody, 157
Dissociation syndrome, 134 Dysthymic disorder, 411
Distal limb visual action therapy, 440
Distance-based service, 180–182
Distractibility, 122
E
Distractions, 123–124 EBP. See Evidence-based practice
Divided attention, 266 ECB. See Executive Control Battery
DL VAT. See Distal limb visual action therapy ECNA. See Everyday Communication Needs
DLB. See Dementia, with Lewy bodies Assessment
DME. See Durable medical equipment Ecological validity, 237
Doctoral programs, 21 Economy-of-effort hypothesis, 138
Domain-referenced measures, 232 Edema, brain, 60, 358
Donepezil, 360, 365 Education
Doors and People, 305 as potentially confounding factor, 252–253
Douglas, Natalie, 347 socioeconomic status and, 253
Dow, David, 197, 199 speech-language pathologist requirements, 20
Dow-Richards, Carol, 199 EEG. See Electroencephalography
DP. See Doors and People EFA-4. See Examining for Aphasia, Fourth Edition
Drawing-focused approaches, 438 Effect size, 352
DRS-2. See Dementia Rating Scale-2 Effectiveness, 351
DSM-5. See Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Effectiveness and efficiency test phase, of outcomes
Disorders research, 352
d2TA. See d2 Test of Attention Effectiveness test phase, of outcomes research, 352
DTI. See Diffusion tensor imaging Efficacy, 351
DTLA — A. See Detroit Test of Learning Efficacy test phase, of outcomes research, 352
Aptitude — Adult Efficiency, 351
Dual-component model of limb apraxia, 262 Egocentric neglect, 158, 255
Duchan, Judith, 346 Elderly, 29. See also Aging; Older adults
Durable medical equipment, 151 Elderspeak, 124–126, 125
Durable power of attorney for health care, 205 Electrocorticography, 111–112, 112
DWI. See Diffusion-weighted imaging Electroencephalography, 110–112, 111
Dynamic assessments, 231–232, 313 Ellipsis, 322
Dysarthria, 5, 139, 261 Elman, Roberta, 346
Dysarthria Examination Battery, 261, 306 Embolic stroke, 58
Dyschromatopsia, 84 Embolism, 58, 58
Dysgraphia Emotional lability, 139, 270, 411
as aphasia symptom, 142 Empathy, 406–407
definition of, 4, 32 Employer-sponsored health insurance, 184, 189
as potentially confounding factor, 263 Empowerment, 6, 404, 407, 413, 435
problem-solving approach for, 481 Encephalopathy
process analysis approach for, 477 definition of, 68
Dyslexia diabetic, 70–71
as aphasia symptom, 142 HIV/AIDS-associated, 168
deep, 4, 93 metabolic, 110
definition of, 4, 32, 93 toxic, 70, 110
Oral Reading for Language in Aphasia, 483 Wernicke’s, 72
process analysis approach for, 477 End-of-life care, 412
surface, 93 Endarterectomy, 62
Dyslipidemia, 71 Environmental factors, 52, 224
Dysnomia, 32, 139, 169, 457, 464, 466 Environmental systems approaches, 371
Dysphagia, 190 Epilepsy, 110
590  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Episodic memory, 119, 122 Verb as Core and, 467


Equal protection of the laws, 203–204 Verb Network Strengthening Treatment and,
ERPs. See Event-related potentials 465–466
Errorless learning methods, 391, 395–397 Visual Action Therapy and, 440–441
Ethics Evoked potentials, 110–111
in advocacy, 203–204 EVT. See Expressive Vocabulary Test
in global engagement, 213–214 Examining for Aphasia, Fourth Edition, 293
International Classification of Functioning, Executive Control Battery, 306
Disability, and Health applicability to, 53 Executive function
in interprofessional collaboration, 16 assessment of, 241
productivity standards and, 206 constructs of, 267
in research, 29 deficits in
Europe, 19–20 in dementia, 164–165
European Brain Injury Society, 25 in frontal lobe syndrome, 148
EVA Park, 377, 461 in older adults, 122
Event-related potentials, 110–111 as potentially confounding factor, 266–267
Everyday Communication Needs Assessment, 293 in right hemisphere syndrome, 148, 155
Evidence in traumatic brain injury survivors, 146–147
levels of, 351–352 types of, 81, 266
practice-based, 176, 353–354, 354 pragmatic abilities and, 267
Evidence-based pedagogy, xxvi in traumatic brain injury survivors, 146–147, 379
Evidence-based practice Executive Interview, The, 267, 301
Anagram and Copy Treatment and, 480–481 EXIT25. See Executive Interview, The
Back to the Drawing Board and, 439 Expressive aphasia, 38, 132
clinical aphasiologist’s use of, 14, 350–354 Expressive language
Communicative Drawing Program and, 437–438 approaches for enhancing, 443–454
constraint-induced language therapy and, 444–445 right hemisphere syndrome-related challenges in,
constructs used in, 351 154, 154–155, 156–158
Copy and Recall Treatment and, 479–480 Expressive Vocabulary Test, 306
cueing hierarchies to anomia and, 456–457 External locus of control, 407
health care services and, 187 Extralinguistic discourse, 314
Helm Elicited Language Program for Syntax Eyetracking, 112, 255, 279
Stimulation and, 475–476
information to support, 353
F
levels of evidence, 351–352
Mapping Therapy and, 473–474 FAB-R. See Florida Affect Battery, Revised
Melodic Intonation Therapy and, 449–451 Face validity, 236–237
Multiple Oral Rereading and, 482–483 Falls, 68
phonological components analysis and, 463 Family Caregiver Alliance, 416
practice-based evidence and, 176, 353–354, 354 Far vision, 253–254
problem-solving approach and, 481 FAST-2. See Frenchay Aphasia Screening Test, Second
Promoting Aphasics’ Communicative Effectiveness Edition
and, 436 FAVRES. See Functional Assessment of Verbal
research initiatives secondary to, 191 Reasoning and Executive Strategies
Response Elaboration Training and, 452 FCTP. See Functional Communication Therapy
scientifically based theory’s role in, 366 Planner
script training and, 445–446 FDA. See Frenchay Dysarthria Assessment
Semantic Feature Analysis and, 460–461 FDA-2. See Frenchay Dysarthria Assessment, Second
Sentence Production Program for Aphasia and, Edition
474–476, 475 FDG. See Fluorodeoxyglucose
Treatment of Aphasic Perseveration and, 454 Federal health care plans, 183
Treatment of Underlying Forms and, 472 Federal regulations, 191
591
Index  

Fee-for-service, 185 Frontotemporal temporal lobe degeneration, 167


Feedback, 434 FTD. See Frontotemporal dementia
Fiber tracking, 103, 106 FTD Support Forum, 416
Figurative language deficits, in right hemisphere FTLD. See Frontotemporal temporal lobe degeneration
syndrome, 156–158 Function words, 138
Financial conflicts of interest, 206 Functional Assessment of Communication Skills for
Finland, 9 Adults, 290
First and Second World Assemblies on Ageing, 194 Functional Assessment of Verbal Reasoning and
First-in/last-out model of cognitive loss, 398 Executive Strategies, 297
First-responder training programs, 196, 198 Functional communication, 346–347
Five-phase outcome research model, 352 Functional Communication Measure, 277
Fixed-interval/uniform approach, to spaced retrieval Functional Communication Therapy Planner, 293
training, 395 Functional Linguistic Communication Inventory, 301
FLCI. See Functional Linguistic Communication Functional magnetic resonance imaging, 106, 110, 112,
Inventory 444, 449, 472
Flip the Rehab Model, 222 Functional maintenance program, 391
Floor rule, 231 Functioning, 52
Florida Affect Battery, Revised, 306 Fundraising, 185
Flow psychology, 422
Fluent aphasia, 132–134, 133, 316
Fluorodeoxyglucose, 107
G
fMRI. See Functional magnetic resonance imaging Gabby: A Story of Courage and Hope, 196
Focal injury, 65 Galveston Orientation and Amnesia Test, 293
Focused attention, 266 Galveston Orientation and Amnesia Test for Aphasia,
FOCUSED program, 397, 398 297
FOQUS Aphasia, 324 Garcia, Linda, 346
For-profit clinics/agencies, 178, 185 GAS. See Goal attainment scaling
Forever and Ever, Amen, 196 GATE. See General Architecture for Text Engineering
Forgetfulness, 72 GCS. See Glasgow Coma Scale
Formal operational stage, 116 GDS. See Global Deterioration Scale
Forward-chaining technique, 452 Gender
Frameworks stroke and, 60
biopsychosocial, 45 traumatic brain injury and, 64, 145
cognitive neuropsychological, 43–45 Gender expression, 228–229
concrete-abstract, 46–47 Gender identity, 228–229
medical, 42 General Architecture for Text Engineering, 319
microgenetic, 47 General Practitioner Assessment of Cognition, 288
multidimensional, 42 General slowing hypothesis, 124
propositional language, 47 Generative naming, 236
psycholinguistic, 43, 44 Gestational diabetes, 71
social, 45–46 Gestures during speech, 372
social determinants of health, 46 Gifford, Gabrielle, 196
thought process, 47 Gilpion, Sue, 346
unidimensional, 40, 42 Ginkgo biloba, 425
Framingham Stroke Risk Profile, 62 Glasgow Coma Scale, 265, 265, 298
Frenchay Aphasia Screening Test, Second Edition, 286 Glioblastoma multiforme, 69
Frenchay Dysarthria Assessment, 261, 306 Gliomas, 69
Frenchay Dysarthria Assessment, Second Edition, 306 Global aphasia
Friendship and Aphasia, 416 characteristics of, 135, 139–140
Frontal lobe syndrome, 148 prognosis for, 83
Frontal operculum, 137, 139 stereotypy associated with, 139–140
Frontotemporal dementia, 167–168 Global capacity building, 210–212
592  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Global Deterioration Scale, 301 customers in, 190


Global engagement, 212–214 disparities in, 193
Global humanitarian programs, 210 right hemisphere syndrome survivors in, 160–161
Global Initiative for Inclusive Information and speech-language pathologists in, 9
Communication Technologies, 385 Health care agencies, 185
Global paraphasias, 134 Health care finance systems, 189
Global perspectives and trends, xxiv–xxv, 209–210 Health care plans
Glucose, 70, 106–107 employer-sponsored, 184, 189
Goal attainment scaling, 347–348 nationalized, 183
GOAT. See Galveston Orientation and Amnesia Test services covered by, 186
Golden, Avi, 200, 201 Health condition
Government-sponsored programs. See also Medicaid; “core sets” of categories for, 53
Medicare domains of, 52
cost-savings tactics used by, 190 Health insurance, 184, 191
payment by, 183–184 Health literacy, 210
GPCOG. See General Practitioner Assessment of Health maintenance organizations, 177
Cognition Health-related scholarship, 203
GRADE Working Group. See Grading of Health survey sampling, 8
Recommendations Assessment, Development, Healthy adults, 29–30
and Evaluation Working Group Healthy aging, 116–117
Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Healthy lifestyle education, 191
Development, and Evaluation Working Group, Hearing. See also Auditory system
351 loss of, 259–260
Grammatical processing deficits, 347 neurophysiology of, 94, 95
Grammaticality judgments, 138 as potentially confounding factor, 259–260
Graphemes, 4, 159 supplemental review of, 98–99
Graphic organizer method, of Semantic Feature in traumatic brain injury survivors, 147
Analysis, 459, 459–460 Hearing aids, 260
Graphomotor abilities, 478 Hearing Handicap Inventory for the Elderly, 306
Group treatment, 381–382, 383–384, 399, 413 Hearing Handicap Inventory for the Elderly — Spouse,
Guardianship, 205 306
Guided imagery, 423 Helm Elicited Language Program for Syntax
Stimulation, 474–476
HelpAge International, 194
H
HELPSS. See Helm Elicited Language Program for
HADS. See Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale Syntax Stimulation
Hair cells, 95 Hematoma
Handicap International, 194 definition of, 59
Handicaps, 51 intracerebral, 65
“Hardening of the arteries.” See Atherosclerosis subdural, 59, 65
HBOT. See Hyperbaric oxygen therapy Hemi-inattention, 158
Head injury. See also Traumatic brain injury Hemianopsia, 89, 91
mild cognitive impairment secondary to, 72 Hemineglect, 255
prevention of, 68 Hemispatial neglect, 158, 255–256, 257, 259
screening for, 150 Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults, 118
Health Hemispheric specialization, 78–79. See also Left
brain, 124, 125 hemisphere; Right hemisphere
factors that promote, 124, 125 Hemoglobin, 106
physical activity benefits for, 61 Hemophilia, 59
Health care Hemorrhage
coordination of, 191 intracerebral, 59
cost controls in, 189–191 intracranial, 103
cultural aspects of, 211–212 intraparenchymal, 59
593
Index  

subarachnoid, 59 Hypoperfusion, 60–61


Hemorrhagic stroke, 57–59 Hyporesponsiveness, 159
Herbs, 424–425, 425, 429 Hyter’s model, 214
Heschl’s gyri, 94, 95
Heterosexist language, 229, 246
HHIE. See Hearing Handicap Inventory for the
I
Elderly ICD-10-CM. See International Classification of
HHIE — SP. See Hearing Handicap Inventory for the Diseases-Clinical Modification, 10th Revision
Elderly — Spouse ICDH-2, 51
Hickey, Ellen, 347 ICF. See International Classification of Functioning,
High-density lipoprotein, 59, 71 Disability, and Health
Higher-level visual integration problems, 259 ICIDH. See International Classification of
Hippocampus, 123 Impairments, Disabilities, and Handicaps
HIV/AIDS ICVD. See Ischemic cerebrovascular disease
AIDS dementia complex, 69, 168 Ideational apraxia, 261
antiviral medications for, 68 Identity Theft: Rediscovering Ourselves after Stroke, 196
dementia associated with, 168 Ideograms, 159
encephalopathy associated with, 168 Ideographic scripts, 159
mild neurocognitive disorder associated with, 168 Ideomotor apraxia, 261
neurogenic language disorders caused by, 68–69 IFCI. See Inpatient Functional Communication
HMOs. See Health maintenance organizations Interview
Holistic health, 419 Illiteracy, 253
Holland, Audrey, 346 Imageable, 479
Home health agencies, 178 Impairment(s)
Homonymous hemianopsia, 89, 92 cognitive-communicative. See Cognitive-
Hospice, 179–180 communicative impairments
Hospital(s) definition of, 51
accreditation of, 205 mild cognitive. See Mild cognitive impairment
service delivery in, 176–177 specific language, 5
speech-language pathologists in, 21 visual-perceptual, in right hemisphere syndrome,
Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale, 269 155, 158–159
Human immunodeficiency virus/acquired Implementation science, 355
immunodeficiency syndrome. See HIV/AIDS In an Instant, 196
Human rights In-home care, 178
in advocacy, 203–204 Incidence, 7–8, 195, 209–210
communication as, 194–195, 343 Inclusive language, 31–32
International Classification of Functioning, India, 9
Disability, and Health applicability to, 53 Infarct/infarction, 60
Humanitarian Standards Partnership, 194 Infection
Humility, 14–15, 28, 54 bacterial, 68–69
Humor, 6, 156, 165, 376 cortical function affected by, 69
Huntington’s disease, 168 opportunistic, 68–69
Huntington’s Disease Society of America, 416 as stroke trigger, 63, 63
Huperzine alpha, 425 Inferences, 322
Hyperaffectivity, 156 Inferencing, 157
Hyperbaric oxygen therapy, 428–429 Inferior colliculus, 95
Hyperglycemia, 70–71 Information exchange, 320, 322
Hyperlipidemia, 71 Information processing, 43
Hypermetropia, 89 Information sharing, 362–363, 413–415
Hyperresponsiveness, 159 Informativeness indices, 320, 321
Hypertension, 61, 62 Informed consent, 29
Hypnosis, 423–424 Inhibition theories, 124
Hypoaffectivity, 156 Inhibitory deficit theories, 124
594  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Inpatient Functional Communication Interview, 293 Interstimulus intervals, 365


Institute for Healthcare Improvement, 190 Intervention. See also Treatment
Insula, 139 active engagement in, 347
Insurance intermediary, 183–184 antioxidants, 361
Integrative approaches. See Complementary and assessment and, 343–344
integrative approaches attention-based focus of, 363
Intelligence behavioral challenges that affect, 350
below-average, 252 best practices in, 343–355, 350
as potentially confounding factor, 252–253 caregivers included in, 344
pre-onset, 252 communication partners outside of client’s friends
Inter-examiner reliability, 235 and family in, 345
Interdisciplinary learning, 175–176 conditions for, 345
Interdisciplinary team, 182 counseling moments affected by time course of,
Internal carotid artery, 80, 81 407–409
Internal consistency, 236 cultural differences, 345
Internal locus of control, 407 discourse-based, 323–324
Internal reliability, 236 family members included in, 344
International Association of Logopedics and functional communication, 346–347
Phoniatrics, 25 goal attainment scaling, 347–348
International Behavioral Neuroscience Society, 25 goals of, 344–345
International Classification of Diseases-Clinical International Classification of Functioning,
Modification, 10th Revision, 330 Disability, and Health applicability to, 53–54
International Classification of Functioning, Disability, linguistic differences, 345
and Health locations for, 345
activity and participation, 231 medical necessity of, 187
body structure and function, 231 neuropsychological approaches to, 348
definition of, 45–46, 51–52 nutritional supplements, 361
ethics and, 53 pharmacological, 359–360
goals of, 52 progress monitoring during, 344
health condition domains, 52 purpose of, 344–345
human rights and, 53 relevant material as focus of, 347
intervention and, 53–54 strengths-based focus of, 348
life participation approaches, 371 team-based approach, 348–349
neurogenic language disorders and, 54–55 therapy versus, 30
rehabilitation and, 53–54 timing of, 345
religion in, 424 Intoning, 447
spirituality in, 424 Intra-examiner reliability, 235–236
traumatic brain injury survivor application of, 146, Intracerebral hematoma, 65
148 Intracerebral hemorrhage, 59
treatment applications of, 346 Intracranial hemorrhage, 103
International Classification of Impairments, Intrahemispheric specialization, 79
Disabilities, and Handicaps, 51 Intraparenchymal hemorrhage, 59
International Clinical Phonetics and Linguistics Ireland, 9
Association, 25 Ischemic cerebrovascular disease, 167
International Cognitive Linguistics Association, 25 Ischemic penumbra, 60, 104, 358, 360
International Communication Project, 195, 213 Ischemic stroke, 57, 59
International Neuropsychological Society, 25 Ishihara plates, 255
Interpreter, 227–228
Interprofessional collaboration
J
competencies in, 15, 15–16, 182
description of, 175–176 Jargon aphasia, 134
Interprofessional communication, 16 JHUDB. See Johns Hopkins University Dysgraphia
Interprofessional learning, xxvi Battery
595
Index  

Jie Yu Dan, 425 psycholinguistic model of, 44, 477


Johns Hopkins University Dysgraphia Battery, 278, 307 theories of, 43
Justice, 203 Language production
in Broca’s aphasia, 138
telegraphic, 138
K Language proficiency, 227–228
Kagan, Aura, 346–347 Language tasks, positron emission tomography
KDT. See Kissing and Dancing Test imaging during, 108
Kelly, Mark, 196 Lateral geniculate body of thalamus, 87
Kissing and Dancing Test, 293 Lateral lemniscus, 95
Kitwood, Thomas, 350, 392 LaTrobe Communication Questionnaire, 298
Knowledge Laughter yoga, 422, 423
conveyance of, 17 LCQ. See LaTrobe Communication Questionnaire
metalinguistic, 472 Lea chart, 254, 254
of performance, 365 Leaders, 6, 9, 176
of response, 365 Learning
Knowledge translation, 203, 355 by aphasiologists, 6–7, 12
Korean herbs, 425 case-based, xxvi
Korsakoff’s syndrome, 168 collaborative, xxvi
Kwalitan, 319 cross-cultural, 6
engaged approach to, xxvi
interdisciplinary, 175–176
L interprofessional, xxvi
Lack of awareness of deficits, 266 lifelong, 12, 54, 175, 227
Language problem-based, xxvi
aphasia effects on modalities of, 38 procedural, 122
assessment of, 252–272, 278, 317 team-based, xxvi
comprehension of, 123 treatment parameters that affect, 365
education effects on, 253 Learning Health Systems Rehabilitation Research
expressive use of, 39 Network, 355
impairment of, in older adults, 121, 122 Left anterior cerebral artery, 80–81
inclusive, 31–32 Left hemisphere
left hemisphere dominance in, 78 description of, 78–79
literacy effects on, 253 hemispatial neglect in patients with lesions of, 256,
person-first, 28 257
production of, 38 Left internal carotid artery, 80
reception of, 38 Left middle cerebral artery, 82
receptive use of, 39 Left posterior cerebral artery, 82
screening tool for, 286–287 Left vertebral artery, 80, 109
spontaneous spoken, 240 Left visual neglect, 158
tonal, 157 Legal procedures, for self-determination, 205
unidimensional framework of, 40, 42 Lemoncello, Rik, 375
welcoming, 31–32 Lens, 85, 86, 89
Language disorders Levels of evidence, 351–352
aphasia as, 38 Lewy Body Dementia Association, 416
coping with, 358 Lexical decision tasks, 253
metrics for, 322 Lexical diversity, 321
relationships affected by, 224, 225–226 Lexical perseveration, 137
self-ratings of, indexing of, 269 Lexical processing, 455
Language of generalized intellectual impairment, 4, Life coaching, 404–405
164 Life Interests and Value Cards, 245, 276, 293
Language processing Life participation
attention in, 380 assessment of, 222, 276
596  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Life participation diffusion, 103, 106


contextual factors for, 224 diffusion-weighted imaging, 104
global focus on, 211 functional, 106, 110, 112, 444, 449, 472
neurological disorders’ effect on, 420 perfusion-weighted imaging, 104
treatment applicability of, 370–372 principles of, 103
Life Participation Approach to Aphasia, xxiii, xxvii, scanner for, 104
46, 179, 346–347, 370–371 T1-weighted images, 103–104, 105
Life-span model of postformal cognitive T2-weighted images, 103–104, 105
development, 116 Magnetic resonance venography, 110
Limb apraxia, 261–262 Magnetoencephalography, 111
Line bisection task, 256, 256 Main event index, 321
Line cancellation task, 257, 257 Majority world, 9
Lingraphica, 180, 384 Malaysia, 9
Linguistic differences Malingering, 151
assessment awareness of, 227–233 Managed care, 189–190
intervention affected by, 345 Management, 176
Linguistic discourse, 314 Mapping hypothesis, 138
Literacy Mapping Therapy, 472–474
as potentially confounding factor, 252–253 MAST. See Mississippi Aphasia Screening Test
socioeconomic status and, 253 MAXQDA, 319
Literal paraphasias, 134, 139 McEwen, Mark, 196
Living will, 205 MCI. See Mild cognitive impairment
Living with Aphasia: Framework for Outcome MCLA. See Measure of Cognitive-Linguistic Abilities
Measurement, 222, 371 MCSTPA. See Multimodal Communication Screening
LLT-R. See Location Learning Test, Revised Task for Persons with Aphasia
Location Learning Test, Revised, 301, 307 Measure of Cognitive-Linguistic Abilities, 298
Locked-in syndrome, 264 Measurement error, 229
Locus of control, 84, 407 Medial geniculate body, 95
Logopedists, 19 Medicaid, 183–184, 186
Logopenic variant primary progressive aphasia, 169, 170 Medical framework, 42
Logorrhea, 137 Medical schools, 20
Long-term care facilities, 177, 211 Medicalization, 202, 412
Long-term potentiation, 358–360, 365 “Medically necessary” services, 187, 191
Loose training programs, 452 Medicare
Loss of consciousness, 60 description of, 183–184
Low vision, 255 Part A, 183
LPAA. See Life Participation Approach to Aphasia Part B, 183
LPAA Project Group, 346 Part D, 183
LTP. See Long-term potentiation reimbursable services, 186, 391
lvPPA. See Logopenic variant primary progressive MEG. See Magnetoencephalography
aphasia Melodic Intonation Therapy
Lyon, Jon, 346 definition of, 446
duration of, 449
evidence-based practice and, 449–451
M
implementation of, 447–449
M-WCST. See Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Level I, 447
Mackie-Simmons, Nina, 346–347 Level II, 447–448
MAE. See Multilingual Aphasia Examination, Third Level III, 448
Edition Level IV, 448
Magnetic resonance angiography, 110 principles of, 446
Magnetic resonance imaging Memory
Broca’s aphasia on, 138 age-related challenges in, 118–119, 121, 122
challenges associated with, 103 assessment of, 241, 267
computed axial tomography versus, 102–103 autobiographical, 118, 122
597
Index  

episodic, 119, 122 Mini-stroke, 61


herbs for, 425 Minimal terminal unit, 322
loss of, 72 Minnesota Test for Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia,
procedural, 118, 122 42
semantic, 118 Minority-world countries, 69, 212
short-term, 119, 122 MIRBI-2. See Mini Inventory of Right Brain Injury,
source, 119, 122 Second Edition
treatment parameters that affect, 365 Misguided statements, 410–411
working, 119, 122, 123, 380 Mississippi Aphasia Screening Test, 286
Memory books, 393–395 MIST. See Memory for Intentions Test
Memory for Intentions Test, 307 MIT. See Melodic Intonation Therapy
Memory problems Mixed transcortical aphasia, 136, 140
in dementia, 165, 165 MMSE-2. See Mini-Mental State Examination, Second
as potentially confounding factor, 267 Edition
in right hemisphere syndrome, 155, 158 MoCA. See Montreal Cognitive Assessment
in traumatic brain injury survivors, 146 Modified Mini-Mental State Examination, 289
Memory wallets, 393–395 Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, 307
Meninges, 78 Montessori approach, 397–399
Meningioma, 69 Montreal Cognitive Assessment, 282, 289
Meningoencephalitis, 68 MOR. See Multiple Oral Rereading
Mental health services, 405 Moral principles, 203, 204
Mental well-being, 62 Morality, 203
Mentoring, 176, 375 Morbid, 32
Metabolic encephalopathy, 110 Motherese, 124–125
Metalinguistic awareness, 470–471 Motion artifacts, 102
Metalinguistic knowledge, 472 Motivational interviewing, 347
Metastatic tumors, 70 Motivational theory of life-span development, 116
Meyerson, Debra, 196, 199 Motor deficits, 262–263
Microgenetic framework, 47 Motor speech disorders
Middle cerebral artery, 81, 82 aphasia concomitantly presenting with, 38
Middle ear, 95 in Broca’s aphasia, 139
Mild cognitive impairment Motor vehicle-related traumatic brain injuries, 68, 145
aging versus, 316 Mount Wilga High Level Language Test, 298
Alzheimer’s disease as cause of, 166 Mount Wilga High Level Language Test, Revised, 298
assessment tools for, 301–302 Moxibustion, 425
attention problems in, 165 MRA. See Magnetic resonance angiography; Mutual
cognitive-communicative impairments in, 164 recognition agreement
cognitive symptoms of, 165, 165 MRI. See Magnetic resonance imaging
communication challenges associated with, MRV. See Magnetic resonance venography
164–166, 267 mTBI. See Mild traumatic brain injury
definition of, 163 MTDDA. See Minnesota Test for Differential
executive function deficits in, 165 Diagnosis of Aphasia
head injury as cause of, 72 Multi-infarct dementia, 167
memory problems in, 165, 165 Multicultural differences, 407, 427
perceptual problems in, 165 Multicultural work, 6
positron emission tomography images of, 108 Multidimensional framework, 42
pragmatic deficits in, 267, 315–316 Multidisciplinary team, 182
Mild traumatic brain injury, 67–68, 164 Multilingual Aphasia Examination, Third Edition, 286
Mind-body practices, 421–422 Multilingual work, 6
Mindfulness, 422 Multimodal Communication Screening Task for
Mini Inventory of Right Brain Injury, Second Edition, Persons with Aphasia, 287
300 Multiple Oral Rereading, 318, 481–483
Mini-Mental State Examination, Second Edition, 279, Murphy, Lauren, 200, 200
288 Music and Memory, 416
598  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Mutual recognition agreement, 20 NCCIH. See National Center for Complementary and
MWHLLT. See Mount Wilga High Level Language Integrative Health
Test Necrosis, brain, 60
MWHLLT-R. See Mount Wilga High Level Language Negative connotations, 32
Test, Revised Neocolonialism, 202
My Stroke of Insight, 58, 409 Neologisms, 134
Myopia, 89 Neologistic paraphasia, 137
Neoplasm, 69–70. See also Tumors
Neural networks, 464
N
Neural recovery, 360
N-CEP. See National Center for Evidence-Based Neuritic plaques, 166–167
Practice in Communication Disorders NeuroAbilities, 385
NAA. See National Aphasia Association Neuroanatomy, 76–78, 76–78, 96
NAB. See Neuropsychological Assessment Battery Neurocognitive disorders
Naming, 239, 278, 456–457 dementia. See Dementia
Naming and Oral Reading for Language in Aphasia Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
6-point scale, 287, 294 classification of, 164
Nasal half, of retina, 87 mild cognitive impairment. See Mild cognitive
NASHC. See National Association of Speech and impairment
Hearing Centers Neurodegenerative disease
National Aphasia Association, 25, 39, 195, 409, 416 cognitive-linguistic abilities affected by, 72, 163–164
National Association of Speech and Hearing Centers, definition of, 72
178 interventions in, effect on cognitive-linguistic
National Brain Tumor Society, 416 decline, 365–366
National Center for Adaptive Neurotechnologies, Neurofibrillary tangles, 117, 166–167
385 Neurogenic cognitive-linguistic disorders
National Center for Complementary and Alternative acquired
Medicine, 420 assessment tools for, 303–310
National Center for Complementary and Integrative definition of, 3–5
Health, 420 disciplinary areas relevant to, 7, 7
National Center for Evidence-Based Practice in discourse relevancy to, 315
Communication Disorders, 353 incidence of, 7–8
National Center for Interprofessional Practice and prevalence of, 7–8
Education, 182 challenges associated with, xxiii–xxiv
National Health Service, 183, 416 classification of, 4
National Institute for Health Research, 416 congenital, 4–5
National Institute on Aging, 166 people/persons with
National Institutes of Health diversity of, 5
Neuroscience @ NIH, 25 preferred use of terminology, 29
Office of Alternative Medicine, 420 writing and talking about, 27–30
National Outcomes Measurement System, 277 websites for, 415–416
National Parkinson Foundation, 416 Neurogenic communication disorders
National Standards for Culturally and Linguistically acquired
Appropriate Services in Health Care, 14 depression in, 411–412
National Stroke Association, 415 global capacity building for, 210–212
Nationalized health care plans, 183 global engagement in, 212–214
Natural Language Toolkit, 319 group therapy for, 413
Natural products, 424–425, 425 life improvements after, 413
Natural supplements, 429 awareness about, 195–197, 197–199, 409
NAVS. See Northwestern Assessment of Verbs and board certification in, 21
Sentences celebrities with, 196
NCCEA. See Neurosensory Center Comprehensive challenges associated with, 18–19
Examination for Aphasia depression in, 411–412
599
Index  

etiology of magnetic resonance imaging. See Magnetic


bacterial infections, 68–69 resonance imaging
diabetes mellitus, 70–71, 71 positron emission tomography, 106–107, 108
metabolic syndrome, 71 single photon emission computed tomography, 109
neoplasm, 69–70 Neuronal regeneration, 358
opportunistic infections, 69 Neuropharmacology, 359
stroke. See Stroke Neurophysiology
toxemia, 70 of hearing, 94, 95
traumatic brain injury. See Traumatic brain injury principles of, 78–80
fantastic elements of, 5–7 Neuroplasticity, 79–80, 358
global trends in, 209–210 Neuropsychological Assessment Battery, 307
incidence of, 209–210 Neuropsychology, 20
prefixes used to symptoms of, 32 Neurosensory Center Comprehensive Examination
prevalence of, 209–210 for Aphasia, 294
speech-language pathologist specialization in, Neurotransmitters, 117
175–176 Neurotypical people, 30
Neurogenic language disorders New Zealand, 20
acquired New Zealand Speech-Language Therapists’
advocacy for, 203–204 Association, 20
anxiety associated with, 269 nfvPPA. See Nonfluent agrammatic primary
assessment of, 221–233 progressive aphasia
bacterial infections as cause of, 68–69 NHS. See National Health Service
challenges in identifying etiology of, 73 NLTK. See Natural Language Toolkit
depression associated with, 268 Noise buildup, 260
description of, 3–5 NOMS. See National Outcomes Measurement System
diabetes mellitus as cause of, 70–71, 71 Noncanonical sentences, 470–471, 473
diabetic encephalopathy as cause of, 70–71 Nonfluent agrammatic primary progressive aphasia,
humor and, 376 169, 170
metabolic syndrome as cause of, 71 Nonfluent aphasia, 132–134, 133, 316
neoplasm as cause of, 69–70 Nonmaleficence, 203
opportunistic infections as cause of, 69 Nonstandardized testing, 232
screening for, 242–245, 243–244 Nonverbal BriefScreen, 289
severity of, factors that may affect, 84 Nonverbal memory, 241
stroke as cause of. See Stroke NORLA-6. See Naming and Oral Reading for
toxemia as cause of, 70 Language in Aphasia 6-point scale
traumatic brain injury. See Traumatic brain injury Norm-referenced measures, 232
aphasia frameworks and, 48 Normative data, 231
congenital, 4–5 Northwestern Assessment of Verbs and Sentences,
healthy adults and, 29–30 278, 307
International Classification of Functioning, Northwestern Syntax Screening Test, 294
Disability, and Health relevancy for, 54–55 Northwestern Verb Production Battery, 464
reciprocal scaffolding using persons with, 374 Not-for-profit clinics, 178
telepractice in, 180 NSST. See Northwestern Syntax Screening Test
telerehabilitation for, 181 Nutritional supplements, 361
treatment of, best practices in, 343–350 NZSLA. See New Zealand Speech-Language
Neurogenic memory impairments, 315 Therapists’ Association
Neurogenic motor speech disorders, 5
Neuroimaging
cerebral angiography, 109, 109–110
O
computed axial tomography, 101–102, 102–103 Objects, 43–44
description of, 101 Occipital lobe, 105
electrocorticography, 111–112, 112 Occlusive stroke, 57–58
electroencephalography, 110–112, 111 Ocular motor deficits, 85, 93
600  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Ocular motor problems, 255 Outcome measures, 373


“Odd-man-out” tasks, 380 Outcomes assessment, 276–277
Office of Alternative Medicine, 420 Outcomes research, 352
OHI. See Open-head injury Outer ear, 95
Old age homes. See Skilled nursing facilities Outreach programs, 196
Older adults. See also Aging
cognitive-linguistic characteristics of, 121, 122
P
devaluation of, 221
disfluencies in, 120 PACE. See Promoting Aphasics’ Communicative
elderly versus, 29 Effectiveness
elderspeak with, 124–126, 125 Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test, 298, 307
falls in, 68 PACST. See Putney Auditory Comprehension
global population of, 209 Screening Test
hemispheric asymmetry reduction in, 118 PALPA. See Psycholinguistic Assessments of
language impairment in, 121, 122 Language Processing in Aphasia
life expectancy of, 117 Paralinguistic discourse, 314
memory in, 118–119, 121, 122 Paralysis, 262
perceptual processing difficulties in, 119 Paramnesia, 146
population increases in, 117, 126 Paraphasias, 134, 137, 139
reserve capacity of, 118 Paresis, 262
stereotypes of, 125–126 Parkinson’s-associated dementia, 167
syntactic processing in, 119–120 Parr, Susie, 346
word-finding problems in, 119–120, 122 Participants, research, 29–30
Omega-3 fatty acids, 424 Participation, 51–52
One Hundred Names for Love, 196 Participation Model of AAC, 347, 382
Online games, 376–378, 377 Partner training, 373–374, 374
Open-class words, 138–139 PASAT. See Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test
Open-head injury, 65 PASS. See Progressive Aphasia Severity Scale
Ophthalmic artery, 81 Passion Works Studio, 375
Opportunistic infections, 68–69 Passive patient, 28
Optic aphasia, 93 Pathologic lability, 270
Optic chiasm, 85, 86, 87 Pathology, 31
Optic disc, 86 Patient-driven payment model, 183
Optic nerve “Patient” (object), 463–464
anatomy of, 85, 86 “Patient” (person), 28–29
fibers of, 85, 87 Patient-specific hypothesis testing, 344
lesion of, 89, 90 Payment. See also Reimbursement
Optic radiations, 86, 87 government-sponsored programs, 183–184
Optic tract, 87 health insurance, 184
Optic tract fiber lesions, 89, 92 Medicaid, 183–184, 186
Optimizing phase, of outcomes research, 352 Medicare, 183–184, 186
Oral Reading for Language in Aphasia, 483–484 methods of, 182
Oral Reading for Language in Aphasia Virtual mixed funding options, 185
Therapist, 483–484, 484 out of pocket, 182
Oral rereading method, 479 per diem, 182
Orange, J.B., 346 philanthropic donations, 185
Organizations, 23, 25 private pay, 184–185
ORLA. See Oral Reading for Language in Aphasia reimbursable services for, 185–188
ORLA VT. See Oral Reading for Language in Aphasia salary, 183
Virtual Therapist sources of, 183–185
Out of pocket payment, 182 PBA. See Pseudobulbar affect
Outcome, 351 PCA. See Phonological components analysis
601
Index  

PDPM. See Patient-driven payment model Pound, Carole, 346


Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Fourth Edition, 308 Powerful Tools for Caregivers, 416
Pedagogy, xxvi PPA. See Primary progressive aphasia
PEDro scale, 353 PPAOS. See Primary progressive aphasia of speech
Penetrating traumatic brain injury, 66 PPT. See Pyramids and Palm Trees Test
Penumbra, 60, 104, 358, 360 PPVT-4. See Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Fourth
Per diem funding scheme, 185 Edition
Per diem payment, 182 Practice-based evidence, 176, 353–354
Perceived stress, 269 Pragmatic abilities
Performance, 52 in aphasia, 315
Perfusion-weighted imaging, 104 assessment of, 240
Perseveration, 137, 137, 453 category constructs related to, 320
Persistent depressive disorder, 411 deficits and impairments
Person-centered approaches, 347, 392 in mild cognitive impairment, 267, 315–316
Person-centered assessment, 221–222, 223, 276 as potentially confounding factor, 267
Person-centered care, 344 in right hemisphere syndrome, 154, 156
Person-first language, 28 in traumatic brain injury, 147, 148, 316
Personal factors, 52, 224 definition of, 313
PET. See Positron emission tomography dementia-related impairments in, 165
Pharmacological treatment, 359–360 executive function abilities and, 267
Pharmacotherapy, 359 in older adults, 121
Philadelphia Naming Test, 278, 294 Pranayama, 422
Philanthropic donations, 185 Praxis examination, 20
Phonemic paraphasias, 134 Pre-onset intelligence, 252
Phonological components analysis, 461–463, 462 Preauthorization, 186
Phonological output buffer, 43–44 Predicting Language Outcome and Recovery After
Phonological processing, 482 Stroke, 416
Phonological spelling, 478 Predictive validity, 236
Photoreceptors, 85 Preexisting pathways, unmasking of, 358
Phrase movement, 470 Prefixes, 32
Physical activity Premorbid, 32
health benefits of, 61 Press of speech, 137
metabolic syndrome and, 71 Prevalence, 7–8, 195, 209–210
Physician’s order, 186 Primary aging, 121
Piaget, Jean, 116 Primary auditory area, 76
PICA. See Porch Index of Communicative Ability Primary auditory cortex, 95
PICA-R. See Porch Index of Communicative Primary blast injuries, 66
Ability — Revised Primary motor area, 76
Pick’s disease, 167 Primary progressive aphasia
PL VAT. See Proximal limb visual action therapy age of onset, 141
Planum temporale, 78–79 assessment tools for, 301–302
Plasticity, 79–80, 358 characteristics of, 136, 140, 164
PLORAS. See Predicting Language Outcome and definition of, 72
Recovery After Stroke diagnosis of, 169, 170
Pontine arteries, 81 direct treatment for, 391–392
Porch Index of Communicative Ability, 441 evaluative algorithm for, 169, 170
Porch Index of Communicative Ability — Revised, functional magnetic resonance imaging of, 107, 107
280–281, 294 logopenic variant, 169, 170
Positive psychology, 404, 407 in long-term care residents, 399
Positron emission tomography, 106–107, 108 nonfluent agrammatic, 169, 170, 391
Posterior cerebral artery, 81, 82 onset of, 72
Posterior communicating artery, 81 progression of, 409
602  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Primary progressive aphasia (continued) Propositional language framework, 47


quality of life in, 390 Propositions, 47
semantic variant, 169, 170, 391 Prosody deficits
speech-language pathologist services in in right hemisphere syndrome, 157–158
challenges associated with delivery of, 389–390 in traumatic brain injury survivors, 148
quality of life benefits of, 390 Prosopagnosia, 93, 159, 259
role of, 171–172 Proximal limb visual action therapy, 440
scope of practice, 390 Pseudobulbar affect, 270, 411
symptoms of, 168–169 Pseudodementia, 171
treatment for, 391–392 Psycholinguistic Assessments of Language Processing
types of, 141 in Aphasia, 43, 44, 263, 278, 295
word-finding problems in, 141 Psycholinguistic framework, 43, 44
Primary progressive aphasia of speech, 392 Psycholinguistic models of language processing, 44,
Primary sensory area, 76 477
Primary tumors, 69 Psychological age, 116
Primary visual area, 76 Psychology, 20
Primary visual cortex, 86, 87 Psychometric indices, 280
Prion disorders, 69 Psychometric properties, 235–237
Private pay, 184–185 Psychosocial support, 406
Private practice, 178 Pupillometry, 112
Problem-based learning, xxvi Pure-tone thresholds, 259
Problem-solving approach, 481 Pursuit movements, 93
Procedural learning, 122 Putney Auditory Comprehension Screening Test, 279,
Procedural memory, 118, 122 287
Process analysis approach, 229, 270–271, 271–272, 378, PWI. See Perfusion-weighted imaging
477 Pyramids and Palm Trees Test, 295
Productivity standards, 206
Professional networks, 6
Professional organizations, 23, 25
Q
Professionals, 200–201 QAA. See Quick Assessment for Aphasia
Profile of Communication Appropriateness, 317 QAD. See Quick Assessment for Dysarthria
Prognosis QCLS. See Quality of Communication Life Scale
global aphasia, 83 QDA Miner, 319
judgments about, 328–329 Qol-AD. See Quality of Life in Alzheimer’s Disease
sharing of, 409 QOLIBRI. See Quality of Life After Brain Injury
stroke, 83, 84 Quadrantopsia, bilateral, 89, 92
traumatic brain injury, 83, 84 Quality of Communication Life Scale, 295
Progress monitoring, 344 Quality of life, 41, 390
Progressive Aphasia Severity Scale, 294 Quality of Life After Brain Injury, 298
Proloquo2Go, 384 Quality of Life in Alzheimer’s Disease, 302
Promise Me, Dad: A Year of Hope, Hardship and Purpose, Quality of Life in Dementia, 302
196 Quaternary blast injuries, 67
Promoting Aphasics’ Communicative Effectiveness Query-based approach, xxvi
constraint-induced language therapy and, 444 Quick Assessment for Aphasia, 287
definition of, 433 Quick Assessment for Dysarthria, 261, 308
description of, 358
empowerment benefits of, 435
R
evidence-based practice and, 436
feedback in, 434 Radioisotopes, 106
implementation of, 434–435 Rancho Los Amigos Scale of Cognitive Functioning,
principles of, 433–434 282, 298
scoring scale for, 435, 435 Randomized, controlled clinical trials, 351–353
Pronouns, 32 Randomized, double-blind controlled clinical trial, 352
603
Index  

Randomized-interval/adjusted approach, to spaced “Rehabilitation 2030: A Call for Action,” 194


retrieval training, 395, 396 Rehabilitation centers, 177
RAPM. See Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices Rehabilitation counseling, 405
Rapport building, 243, 327 Rehabilitation gym, 177
Rare Dementia Support, 416 Reimbursement. See also Payment
Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices, 308 cost-control systems’ effect on, 189–191
Raven’s Progressive Matrices, 308 denial of, 188–189
RBANS. See Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of documentation for, 185–186
Neuropsychological Status for dysphagia services, 190
RBI. See Right brain injury evidence-based practice, 187
RBMT-3. See Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test, evidence of treatment progress required for,
Third Edition 187–188, 190
RCBA. See Reading Comprehension Battery for factors that promote, 186
Aphasia for “medically necessary services, 187, 191
rCBF. See Regional cerebral blood flow physician’s order for, 186
RCFT. See Rey Complex Figure Test requirements for, 185–188, 186
RCSLT. See Royal College of Speech and Language skilled services, 186–187
Therapists Relationships, 224, 225–226
Reading Reliability, 235–236
age-related changes in, 120 Religion, 424
approaches for improving, 477–485 Reltan-Indiana Aphasia Screening Test, 287
cognitive processing model of, 263 Reminiscence-based programs, 400
comprehension of, 238, 347, 483 Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of
letter-by-letter approach to, 481 Neuropsychological Status, 302
Multiple Oral Rereading for, 318, 481–483 Reperfusion, 60, 358, 360
Oral Reading for Language in Aphasia, 483–484 Repetition, 239
positron emission tomography imaging during, 108 Repetitive spelling, 479
Reading Comprehension Battery for Aphasia, 263, Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation, 361, 391
278, 295 Research
Reading deficits and disorders assessment as, 229, 230
dyslexia, 4 complementary and integrative approaches, 426,
post-onset, 232 426–427
in visual neglect, 257, 258 discourse analysis in, 316, 324–325
Reading problems need for, 9
as potentially confounding factor, 263 participants in, 29–30
right hemisphere syndrome-related, 155, 159 promotion of, 203
Reasoning, 241 steps involved in, 229, 230
Receptive aphasia, 38, 132 transnational approaches to, 212
Receptive language, in right hemisphere syndrome, Research aphasiologist, 21–22
154, 154–155, 156–157 Research-practice relationships, 354
Reciprocal scaffolding, 374 Research process, assessment process as, 354
Recurrent perseveration, 137, 137, 453 Reserve capacity, 118
Referrals, 201, 405 Residential programs, 385–387
Refraction, 85 Residential settings, 126
Regional cerebral blood flow, 106 Resource capacity theories, 123
Register, 314 Respect for people, 203
Rehabilitation Response Elaboration Training, 451–452
cognitive, 379–381 Responsiveness, cultural, 14–15, 28
community-based, 202–203 Rest, 363
International Classification of Functioning, Rest homes. See Skilled nursing facilities
Disability, and Health applicability to, 53–54 Restitutive approaches, 358, 363, 457
outcomes measures for, 276–277 Restorative approach, 358
service settings for, 177 RET. See Response Elaboration Training
604  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Retina, 85, 86 memory impairments in, 155, 158


Retrograde amnesia, 146 normal versus disordered abilities in, 160
Reversible dementia, 169, 171 pragmatics in, 154, 156
Reversible passives, 138 prosody deficits in, 157–158
Revised Token Test, 233, 278, 281–282, 295 reading impairments in, 155, 159
Rey Complex Figure Test, 308 receptive language challenges in, 154, 154–155,
RFFT. See Ruff Figural Fluency Test 156–157
RHDBank, 318 screening for, 161
RHLB-2. See Right Hemisphere Language Battery, social cognition deficit hypothesis of, 156
Second Edition suppression deficit hypothesis in, 157
RHS. See Right hemisphere syndrome symptom classification in, 159–160
RIC Evaluation in Right Hemisphere Dysfunction-3, theory of mind deficits in, 156
The, 300 tonal languages and, 157
RICE-3. See RIC Evaluation in Right Hemisphere underdiagnosis of, 159
Dysfunction-3, The visual-perceptual impairments in, 155, 158–159
Right anterior cerebral artery, 80–81 writing impairments in, 155, 159
Right brain injury, 153, 256, 300. See also Right Right internal carotid artery, 80
hemisphere syndrome Right middle cerebral artery, 82
Right ear advantage, 94 Right posterior cerebral artery, 82
Right hemisphere Right vertebral artery, 80
description of, 78–79 RIPA-2. See Ross Information Processing Assessment,
hemispatial neglect in patients with lesions of, 256 Second Edition
Right Hemisphere Language Battery, Second Edition, RIPA — G:2. See Ross Information Processing
300 Assessment — Geriatric, Second Edition
Right hemisphere syndrome. See also Right brain Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test, Third Edition,
injury 299
affect in, 156 Rods, 85, 86
anosognosia in, 158 Rogers, Margaret, 346
attention deficits in, 154, 155, 158 Roles and responsibilities
auditory-perceptual impairments in, 155, 159 in interprofessional collaboration, 16
challenges associated with, 159–161 leadership, 176
codeswitching challenges in, 157 management, 176
cognitive-communicative challenges associated mentoring, 176
with, 154, 154–155 research, 176
cognitive resources hypothesis of, 156 in rights of individuals with aphasia and related
communication affected by, 154, 154–155 disorders, 204–205
definition of, 153 of speech-language pathologists, 175–176
discourse coherence challenges in, 157, 160 teaching, 176
discourse in, 154, 154–155 Rosenbaum Pocket Vision Screener, 254
executive function deficits in, 148 Ross Information Processing Assessment, Second
expressive language challenges in, 154, 154–155, Edition, 299
156–158 Ross Information Processing Assessment — Geriatric,
figurative language deficits in, 156–158 Second Edition, 302
in health care contexts, 160–161 Ross Test of Higher Cognitive Processes, 308
hemispatial neglect in, 256 Rotational injuries, 65, 66
humor and appreciation in, 156 Rowland Universal Dementia Assessment Scale, 302
hyperaffectivity in, 156 Royal College of Speech and Language Therapists, 20
hyperresponsiveness in, 159 RPM. See Raven’s Progressive Matrices
hypoaffectivity in, 156 RTHCP. See Ross Test of Higher Cognitive Processes
hyporesponsiveness in, 159 rTMS. See Repetitive transcranial magnetic
inferencing difficulties in, 157 stimulation
lack of awareness of, 159 RTT. See Revised Token Test
605
Index  

RUDAS. See Rowland Universal Dementia open-ended questions in, 242–243


Assessment Scale questions in, 242–243, 243–244
Ruff Figural Fluency Test, 299 right hemisphere syndrome, 161
timing of, 245
traumatic brain injury, 150
S
vision, 254
Saccadic eye movements, 93 visual neglect, 256–259, 258
SADQ-H. See Stroke Aphasic Depression Script training, 445–446
Questionnaire SCWT-A. See Stroop Color and Word Test-Adult
SAILR. See Scales of Adult Independence, Language, SDMT. See Symbol Digit Modality Test
and Recall Secondary aging, 121
Sailuotiong, 425 Secondary blast injuries, 66–67
Saint Louis University Mental Status Examination, Secondary tumors, 70
289 Selective attention, 266
Salary, 183 Self-advocacy, 409–410
SALT. See Systematic Analysis of Language Self-awareness deficits, 266
Transcripts Self-coaching, 345
Salt intake, 61 Self-determination, 205, 205
SAQOL-39. See Stroke and Aphasia Quality of Life Self-identity, 11
Scale Self-reflection, 14
Sarah Bellum’s Bakery and Workshop, 375, 375 Self-regulatory skills, 116
SAS. See Sklar Aphasia Scale Semantic-conceptual knowledge, 437
Savvy Caregiver Program, 392–393 Semantic Feature Analysis
Scaffolding, reciprocal, 374 baseline phase and target selection for, 457–458
Scales of Adult Independence, Language, and Recall, chart method of, 458, 458–459
302 definition of, 457
Scales of Cognitive Ability for Traumatic Brain Injury, description of, 358
299 discourse-level tasks application of, 460
Scales of Cognitive and Communicative Ability for evidence-based practice and, 460–461
Neurorehabilitation, 289 graphic organizer method of, 459, 459–460
SCAN-3: A Test for Auditory Processing Disorders in implementation of, 457–460
Children and Adults, 308 principles of, 457
SCATBI. See Scales of Cognitive Ability for Traumatic Semantic judgments, 465
Brain Injury Semantic memory, 118
SCCAN. See Scales of Cognitive and Communicative Semantic paraphasias, 134, 139
Ability for Neurorehabilitation Semantic perseveration, 137, 137
Schuell’s stimulation approach, 381 Semantic roles, 463
Scientific knowledge, 349 Semantic variant primary progressive aphasia, 169,
Sclera, 85, 86 170, 391
SCOLP. See Speed and Capacity of Language Senior citizen, 29. See also Aging; Older adults
Processing Test Sensitivity, 236, 279
Scope of practice, 148–149, 390, 405–406, 427 Sensorimotor tracking, 113
Scotoma, 89, 90 Sensory stimulation, 359
SCP. See Savvy Caregiver Program Sentence(s)
Screenings canonical, 470, 473
acquired neurogenic language disorders, 242–245, Mapping Therapy, 472–474
243–244 noncanonical, 470–473
cognition, 288–289 Sentence comprehension, 120
definition of, 242 Sentence Production Program for Aphasia, 474–476,
head injury, 150 475
hearing problems, 259–260 Sentence Production Test for Aphasia, 295
language, 286–287 SentenceShaper, 384
606  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Service settings Sodium amytal infusion, 110


adult day care centers, 179 Software packages, 384
aphasia centers, 179, 181, 184 Source memory, 119, 122
continuing care retirement communities, 177–178 South Africa, 9, 14
distance-based service, 180–182 South America, 9
health maintenance organizations, 177 SPA. See Speech Pathology Australia
home health agencies, 178 Spaced retrieval training, 391, 395–397, 396
hospice, 179–180 Specialization, 21–22
hospitals, 176–177 Specific language impairment, 5
long-term care facilities, 177 Specificity, 236, 279
not-for-profit clinics, 178 SPECT. See Single photon emission computed
private practice, 178 tomography
rehabilitation centers, 177 Speech
skilled nursing facilities, 177 apraxia of, 5, 169, 260–261, 452
university-based clinics, 178–179 automatic, 239
Severe Impairment Battery, The, 302 disfluent, 139
Sexual orientation, 228–229 gestures during, 372
SFA. See Semantic Feature Analysis left hemisphere dominance in, 78
Shadden, Barbara, 346 positron emission tomography imaging during, 108
Shear waves, 66 press of, 137
Sheffield Screening Test for Acquired Language telegraphic, 138
Disorders, 287 in traumatic brain injury survivors, 147
Shirley Ryan Ability Lab, 285 Speech acts, 314, 321, 322
Short-term memory, 119, 122 Speech-language pathologists
SIB. See Severe Impairment Battery, The Bureau of Labor Statistics data on, 9
Sickle cell anemia, 59 career outlook for, 9
Signal degradation theories, 123 certification of, 19, 22
Simulations, 377–378 as coach, 403–406
Single photon emission computed tomography, 109 in competence determinations, 205
Skilled nursing facilities conflicts of interest by, 206
description of, 177 as counselor, 403–406
Medicare reimbursement for services in, 183 credentials of, 20
physician’s order for services, 186 decision making involvement by, 205–206
Skilled services, 186–187 dementia care by. See Dementia, speech-language
Sklar Aphasia Scale, 287 pathologist services in
Sleep, 363 education requirements for, 20
Slow Road to Better, 198 generalized practice of, 21–22
SLPs. See Speech-language pathologists in health care settings, 9
SLTs. See Speech-language therapists hospital-based, 21
SLUMS. See Saint Louis University Mental Status neurogenic communication disorders specialization
Examination of, 175–176
Snellen charts, 254 payment for. See Payment
SNFs. See Skilled nursing facilities primary progressive aphasia care by, 171–172
SOAP Test of Syntactic Complexity, 278, 295 productivity standards, 206
Social age, 116 roles and responsibilities of, 204–205. See also Roles
Social circles, 225–226 and responsibilities
Social cognition deficit hypothesis, 156 salary for, 183
Social determinants of health framework, 46 scope of practice for, 148–149, 390, 405–406, 427
Social framework, 45–46 specialization of, 21–22, 175–176
Social media, 415 swallowing knowledge of, 191
Social Security Income, 151 terminology used to describe, 31
Socially focused programs, 378 traumatic brain injury survivors, 148–149
Socioeconomic status, 253 Speech-language therapists, 19, 31
607
Index  

Speech Pathology Australia, 19–20, 203, 204 consciousness conditions after, 264
Speech therapists, 31 definition of, 57
SpeechBite, 353, 399 in developing countries, 210
Speed, Spatial, and Qualities of Hearing Scale, The, dysgraphia caused by, 478
309 embolic, 58
Speed and Capacity of Language Processing Test, 309 family support after, 60
Speed-of-processing theories, 124 Framingham Stroke Risk Profile for, 62
Spelling, repetitive, 479 gender differences in, 60
Spiral ganglia, 95 hemorrhagic, 57–59
Spirituality, 424 hyperbaric oxygen therapy for, 428–429
Split-half reliability, 236 ischemic, 57, 59
Spongiform encephalopathies, 69 language recovery after, 110
Spontaneous recovery, 79, 361–362 loss of consciousness associated with, 60
Spontaneous spoken language, 240 modifiable risk factors for, 62–63
Sports-related traumatic brain injuries, 68 occlusive, 57–58
SPPA. See Sentence Production Program for Aphasia patient support after, 60
Spreading activation theory of semantic processing, physiological effects of, 60
457 prevention of, 61–63, 62, 191
SPTA. See Sentence Production Test for Aphasia recovery from
SRT. See Spaced retrieval training mechanisms of, 358–359
SSI. See Social Security Income neurobiology of, 365
SSQ. See Speed, Spatial, and Qualities of Hearing pharmacological agents for, 359–360
Scale, The prognosis for, 83, 84
SSTALD. See Sheffield Screening Test for Acquired risk factors for, 59, 61–63, 62, 71, 191
Language Disorders risk scoring systems for, 62
ST. See Speech therapists sudden onset of, 60
Stakeholders, 354 support groups for, 378
Standardized assessments, 231–232 survivors of
Standardized testing depression in, 412
for discourse analysis, 317 life improvements in, 413
nonstandardized testing versus, 232 thrombolytics for, 60
Statutory surrogacy, 205 thrombotic, 58
Stereotypes, 125–126, 195 tissue plasminogen activator for, 60
Stereotypy, 139–140 transient ischemic attack versus, 61
Stimulation-facilitation approach to language treatment for
treatment, 381 focus of, 362–363
Stimulus power, 456 hyperbaric oxygen therapy, 428–429
Story completeness, 321 pharmacological, 359–360
Story goodness index, 321 timing of, 60–61, 362
Story grammar, 321 triggers for, 63, 63
Storytelling, 120, 314, 321 vascular dementia caused by, 167
Strengths-based assessments, 222, 245 warning signs of, 63, 63–64
Stress Stroke and Aphasia Quality of Life Scale, 276, 296
perceived, 269 Stroke Aphasic Depression Questionnaire, 269, 279,
reduction of, 62 296
Stress-saliency hypothesis, 138 Stroke Association, 416
Stress waves, 66 Stroke clubs, 378
Stroke Stroke Comeback Center, 198
anxiety after, 269 Stroke Diary: A Primer for Aphasia Therapy, 328, 328
aphasia caused by, 5, 38 Stroke Educator, 197
BEFAST mnemonic for, 63, 64 Stroke Foundation of Australia, 351
brain edema associated with, 60 Stroke Onward, 196–197
causes of, 59–60 Stroke Survivor, 416
608  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Stroop Color and Word Test-Adult, 309 Tasks of Executive Control, 309
Stuck-in-set perseveration, 137 Tavistock Trust for Aphasia, 416
Subarachnoid hemorrhage, 59 TAWF-2. See Test of Adolescent/Adult Word
Subcortical aphasia, 136, 141 Finding-2
Subdural hematoma, 59, 65 Taylor, Jill Bolte, 58, 200
Subpropositional language, 47 TBIBank, 318
Suffer, 32 tDCS. See Transcranial direct current stimulation
Sundowner’s syndrome, 166 TEA. See Test of Everyday Attention
Sundowning, 166 Teaching, 176
Superior cerebellar artery, 81 Team-based approach, 348–349
Superior olivary nuclei, 95 Team-based learning, xxvi
Superior temporal convolution, 95 Teamwork, 16, 182
Supported communication, 371–372, 397 TEC. See Tasks of Executive Control
Supported work environments, 375 Tectal pathway, 89
Supporting Family Caregivers of Adults With Telegraphic speech, 138
Communication Disorders, 404 Telehealth, 180
Suppression deficit hypothesis, 157 Telepractice, 180
Surface dyslexia, 93 Telerehabilitation, 180, 181
Surface-level linguistic measures, 320, 321 Temporal conjunctives, 322
svPPA. See Semantic variant primary progressive Temporal half, of retina, 87
aphasia Temporal lobe, 105
Symbol Digit Modality Test, 309 Terrorism-related traumatic brain injuries, 148
Symbols trails task, 380 Tertiary blast injuries, 67
Syndromes. See also specific syndrome Test(s)
of aphasia, 131–143, 133, 135–137 design factors for, 237
definition of, 142 multicultural considerations, 228
Syntactic comprehension, 122 translated, 228
Syntactic deficits Test of Adolescent/Adult Word Finding-2, 309
description of, 469 Test of Everyday Attention, 233, 265–266, 280, 309
Helm Elicited Language Program for Syntax Test of Language Competence, Expanded Edition, 310
Stimulation, 474–476 Test of Nonverbal Intelligence-4, 310
Mapping Therapy for, 472–474 Test of Oral and Limb Apraxia, 261
Sentence Production Program for Aphasia, 474–476, Test-retest reliability, 235
475 Text-to-speech technology, 384
Treatment of Underlying Forms for. See Treatment Thalamus
of Underlying Forms anatomy of, 86
Syntactic processing, 119–120 in auditory linguistic processing, 95
Syntactic production, 122 lateral geniculate body of, 87
Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts, 318, 319 The Joint Commission, 195, 205
Systematic reviews, 352, 380 Thematic role training, 470–472
Systems science, 355 Theory of mind, 156
Therapist drift, 369–370
Therapy, treatment versus, 30. See also Intervention;
T
Treatment
T1-weighted images, 103–104, 105 Therapy Outcome Measure, 277
T2-weighted images, 103–104, 105 Thiamine deficiency, 72, 168
Tactus Therapy Solutions, 382, 384, 386, 465 Third-party payer
Tai Ji, 422 decision makers, 188
TalkBank, 318 definition of, 185
Talking Mats, 245 as health care customer, 190
TalkPath, 384 reimbursement of services by, 185–188, 390
TalkRocket Go, 384 Thought process framework, 47
TAP. See Treatment of Aphasic Perseveration Threats, Travis, 346
609
Index  

3MS. See Modified Mini-Mental State Examination hearing loss associated with, 259
Thrombolytics, for stroke, 60 imaging of, 109
Thrombotic stroke, 58 mild, 67–68, 164
Thrombus, 58, 58 motor vehicle-related, 68, 145
Thyroid disorders, 72 open-head injury as cause of, 65
TIA. See Transient ischemic attack penetrating, 66
Tip-of-the-tongue experiences, 119 positron emission tomography of, 109
Tissue plasminogen activator, 60, 360 prevention of, 68
TLC-3. See Test of Language Competence, Expanded recovery from
Edition mechanisms of, 358–359
Toastmaster programs, 375–376 prognosis for, 83, 84
Toastmasters International, 375–376 rotational injuries, 65, 66
TOLA. See Test of Oral and Limb Apraxia screening for, 150
TOM. See Therapy Outcome Measure single photon emission computed tomography of,
Tonal languages, 157 109
TONI-4. See Test of Nonverbal Intelligence-4 spaced retrieval training for, 391, 395–397, 396
Total communication approaches, 372–373, 397 sports-related, 68
TouchChat, 384 survivors of. See Traumatic brain injury survivors
Toxemia, 70 translational injury, 64
Toxic encephalopathy, 70, 110 treatment focus after, 362–363
tPA. See Tissue plasminogen activator in warfare settings, 66–67, 148
Tractography, 103, 106 Traumatic brain injury survivors
Training antecedent-based behavior management in, 350
caregiver, 362, 373–374, 392–393 assessments in, 149, 277–278, 297–299
communication. See Communication training attention process training for, 379–380
partner, 373–374, 374 behavioral management for, 380
script, 445–446 behavioral sequelae in, 146–147
spaced retrieval, 391, 395–397, 396 challenges faced by, 146, 149–150
thematic role, 470 cognitive rehabilitation for, 380
Transcortical motor aphasia, 136, 140 cognitive sequelae in, 146–147
Transcortical sensory aphasia, 135, 140 communication in
Transcranial direct current stimulation, 361, 450 support for, 318
Transcranial magnetic stimulation, 361, 391 symptoms of, 146, 146–147, 150
Transdisciplinary team, 182 training program for, 374
Transient ischemic attack, 60, 167 concomitant conditions in, 150
Translational injury, 64 conversational coaching for, 373
Transmission deficit theories, 123 demographics of, 145–146
Traumatic brain injury depression in, 412
acceleration-deceleration injuries, 64, 65, 67 differential diagnosis in, 149
angular injuries, 65 discourse in, 315
aphasia caused by, 5, 38, 146 durable medical equipment for, 151
blast injury-induced, 66–67, 148 economic considerations for, 150–151
closed-head injury as cause of, 64–65, 67, 316 employability of, 150–151
cognitive-linguistic disorders associated with, 4 executive function deficits in, 146–147, 148
consciousness conditions after, 264 financial benefits to, 150–151
contrecoup injuries, 64, 65–66 frontal lobe syndrome in, 148
coup injuries, 64, 65–66 interdisciplinary collaborations for, 149
definition of, 64 International Classification of Functioning,
diffuse injury, 65 Disability, and Health applicability to, 146, 148
dysgraphia caused by, 478 language sequelae in, 147
falls as cause of, 68 life improvements in, 413, 414
focal injury, 65 malingering by, 151
gender differences in, 64, 145 memory aids in, 380
610  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Traumatic brain injury survivors (continued) restitutive approaches, 358, 363


pragmatic deficits in, 147, 148, 316 restorative approach, 358
prosody in, 148 social approaches to, 370–372
reassurance for, 409 socially focused programs, 378
screening of, 150 software packages, 384
self-awareness by, lack of, 150 spontaneous recovery versus progress made from,
self-regulation approaches for, 380 362
sequelae experienced by, 146–147 stimulation-facilitation approach, 381
service-seeking initiative by, lack of, 150 for stroke, 362–363
speech-language pathologist’s work with, 148–149, supported communication, 371–372
151 therapy versus, 30
terrorism-related, 148 timing of, 60–61, 362
verbal abilities in, 316 Toastmaster programs, 375–376
war-related, 148 total communication approaches, 372–373
working with, 148–149, 151 transcranial direct current stimulation, 361
workplace immersion programs for, 374–375 transcranial magnetic stimulation, 361
Travis, Randy, 196 workplace immersion programs, 374–375
Treatment. See also Intervention; specific modality Treatment fidelity, 369–370, 465, 472
alternative and augmentative communications, 371, Treatment groups, 381–382, 383–384
382, 384–385 Treatment of Aphasic Perseveration, 452–454
antioxidants, 361 Treatment of Underlying Forms
aphasia mentoring programs, 375 definition of, 469
apps for, 382, 384–385 evidence-based practice and, 472
at assessment, 219 implementation of, 470–472
behavioral, 359 metalinguistic awareness, 470–471
best practices for, 343–355, 350 noncanonical sentences, 470–471
brain stimulation, 360–361 principles of, 470
caregiver training, 373–374, 374 thematic role training, 470–472
cognitive neuropsychological approaches Treatment planning, 316
applicable to, 378–379 “Tree pruning hypothesis,” 138
counseling during, 409 “Triple Aim Initiative,” 190
dosage of, 363–364 Tropes, 319
environmental systems approaches to, 371 Trousseau, Armand, 3
evidence of progress in, as reimbursement criterion, TUF. See Treatment of Underlying Forms
187–188, 190 Tumbling E chart, 254
focus of, 362–363 Tumors
group-based, 381–382, 383–384, 399 metastatic, 70
humor as, 376 neurogenic language disorders caused by, 69–70
initiation of, 366 primary, 69
intensity of, 363–364 secondary, 70
intensive programs, 385–387 TWT-R. See Word Test-Revised
level of complexity for, 364–365 Tympanic membrane, 95
life participation approaches to, 370–372 Type 1 diabetes mellitus, 70–71
nutritional supplements, 361 Type 2 diabetes mellitus, 70–71
online games, 376–378, 377 Type-token ratio, 321
parameters of, 365
partner training, 373–374, 374
U
pharmacological, 359–360
purposes of, 357–358 UAM Corpus Tool, 319
reciprocal scaffolding, 374 UCL CSLIR Aphasia Research Group Blog, 416
repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation, 361 UK Stroke Forum, 25
residential programs, 385–387 Unidimensional framework, 40, 42
611
Index  

United Kingdom stimulus selection and creation for, 464


career outlook in, 9 Verbal cueing, 466
certification requirements in, 20 Verbal fluency, 119, 236
National Health Service in, 183 Verbal memory, 241
United Kingdom Acquired Brain Injury Forum, 416 Verbal paraphasias, 134
United Nations Convention on the Rights of Persons Verbal perseveration, 137
with Disabilities, 204 Vertebral artery, 80, 81, 109
United Nations Principles for Older Persons, 194 Vestibulocochlear nerve, 95
United Nations Universal Declaration of Human Victim, 32
Rights, 213 Video-assisted speech technology, 384
University-based clinics, 178–179 Video captioning, 385
University of Sydney Brain and Mind Centre, 285 Video-implemented script training for aphasia, 391,
Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome, 263 446
Unskilled services, 187 Vinpocetine, 425
Utilization reviewers, 188, 190 Virtual Connections for Aphasia, 180, 376, 377
Virtual reality, 385
Viruses, 68–69
V VISTA. See Video-implemented script training for
VA. See Department of Veterans Affairs aphasia
VA Pittsburgh Healthcare System, 386 Visual Action Therapy, 439–441
VAC. See Verb as Core Visual acuity
Validity deficits in, as potentially confounding factor,
below-average intelligence effects on, 252 253–255, 254
types of, 236–237 description of, 85, 123
Values tests of, 254–255
global health priorities and, 210 Visual agnosia, 93, 259
in interprofessional collaboration, 16 Visual Analog Mood Scales, 269, 310
moral principles and, 203, 204 Visual Analog Mood Scales-Revised, 269, 310
VAMS. See Visual Analog Mood Scales Visual analog scales, 269, 270
VAMS-R. See Visual Analog Mood Scales-Revised Visual Analogue Self-Esteem Scale, 269, 310
Vascular dementia, 167, 425 Visual association areas, 89
VASES. See Visual Analogue Self-Esteem Scale Visual attention deficits, 85, 93, 256
Vasodilators, 62 Visual distractions, 123
VAST. See Verb and Sentence Test; Video-assisted Visual field
speech technology binocular, 87
VAT. See Visual Action Therapy definition of, 87
VCIU. See Voluntary Control of Involuntary for each eye, 87, 88
Utterances nasal half of, 87, 88
Vegetative state, 263, 264 neglect of, 93
Vehicle, clinician as, 15–18, 221 temporal half of, 87, 88
Ventricular system, of brain, 78 testing of, 255
Verb and Sentence Test, 296 Visual field deficits
Verb as Core, 466–467 bitemporal (heteronymous) hemianopsia, 89, 91
Verb naming, 467 blindness, 89, 90
Verb Network Strengthening Treatment definition of, 255
agent-patient pairs, 464–465 higher-level, 93–94
baseline for, 464 homonymous hemianopsia, 89, 92
definition of, 463 as potentially confounding factor, 255
evidence-based practice and, 465–466 scotoma, 89, 90
implementation of, 464–465 types of, 89, 90–92
principles of, 463–464 Visual integration deficits, 85
semantic judgments, 465 Visual neglect, 93, 255–259, 256–258
612  Aphasia and Other Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders

Visual object agnosia, 259 function, 138


Visual-perceptual impairments, in right hemisphere open-class, 138–139
syndrome, 155, 158–159 Word cards, 471
Visual perceptual processing, 271 Word-finding problems
Visual scene displays, 382 in anomic aphasia, 141–142
Visual screening chart, 254 approaches for improving, 455–467
Visual sensory deficits, 84–85 assessment of, 236, 278
Visual system in dementia, 165
anatomy of, 85–89, 86–88 description of, 119–120, 122
deficits in, 84–85, 253–259 in primary progressive aphasia, 141
physiology of, 85–89 treatment for, 358
structures of, 77 Word-processing programs, 385
supplemental review of, 98 Word Test-Revised, 311
Visualization, 423–424 WordStat, 319
Visuoconstructive deficits, 93, 259 Working memory, 119, 122, 123, 380
Vitamin B12 deficiency, 72 Workplace immersion programs, 374–375
VMS-R. See Visual Analog Mood Scales-Revised World Bank Office of Disability and Development,
VNeST. See Verb Network Strengthening Treatment 194
Vocabulary, 119 World Health Organization
Voluntary Control of Involuntary Utterances, 451 community-based rehabilitation as defined by, 202
Volunteers, 400 constitution of, 53
definition of, 53
disability models of, 116
W ICDH-2, 51
W-STLC. See Wilg-Semel Test of Linguistic Concepts International Classification of Functioning,
WAB — R. See Western Aphasia Batter — Revised Disability, and Health. See International
Wada test, 110 Classification of Functioning, Disability, and
WCST. See Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Health
Websites, 415–416 International Classification of Impairments,
Wechsler Memory Scale — III, 280, 310 Disabilities, and Handicaps, 51
Wechsler Test of Adult Reading, 253, 310 “Rehabilitation 2030: A Call for Action,” 194
Welcoming language, 31–32 World Report on Disability, 53, 194
Well-being, 237 World Report on Disability, 53, 194
Wellness coaching, 404 World Stroke Association, 25
Wernicke-Lichtheim model, 134 Writing
Wernicke’s aphasia, 79, 83, 105, 134, 135, 137, 140, 315 age-related changes in, 120
Wernicke’s area, 76, 79, 140 Anagram and Copy Treatment for, 479–481
Wernicke’s encephalopathy, 72 approaches for improving, 477–485
Western Aphasia Batter — Revised, 232, 276, 296 assessment of, 240, 278
White matter, 77, 117–118 assessment results shared through, 327, 329–330
WHO. See World Health Organization Copy and Recall Treatment for. See Copy and Recall
Wilg-Semel Test of Linguistic Concepts, 299 Treatment
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, 310 deficits and disorders of
WJIII NU. See Woodcock Johnson III Normative dysgraphia, 4
Update (NU) Tests of Cognitive Abilities as potentially confounding factor, 263
WMS — III. See Wechsler Memory Scale — III negative connotations in, 32
Woodcock Johnson III Normative Update (NU) Tests “patient,” alternatives to term, 28–29
of Cognitive Abilities, 311 person-first language in, 28
Woodruff, Bob, 196 phonological spelling for, 478
Woodruff, Lee, 196 right hemisphere syndrome-related problems with,
Word(s) 155, 159
closed-class, 138 terminology considerations in, 27–33
content, 138–139 WTAR. See Wechsler Test of Adult Reading
613
Index  

X Z
X-rays, 101 Zoom, 376–377, 377
Zuckerman, Danny, 199
Zuckerman, Steve, 199
Y
Ylvisaker, Mark, 344–346, 350
Yoga, 422, 423

You might also like