You are on page 1of 11

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1982 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

Animal Behavior Processes 0097-7403/82/0802-0131S00.75


1982, Vol. 8, No. 2, 131-141

Effect of a Stimulus Intervening Between CS


and US in Autoshaping
Robert A. Rescorla
Yale University

Five experiments explored the effect of placing a stimulus between the termi-
nation of a key light CS and the onset of a food US in an autoshaping preparation
with pigeon subjects. Whether that stimulus was itself a key light evoking pecking
or an auditory event failing to evoke pecking, it facilitated performance to the
key light CS. The use of a within-subjects design made differential conditioning
of background cues and stimulus generalization unlikely accounts. Experiment
3 found that extinction of the intervening stimulus did not remove the facilitative
effect. Experiment 4 found that pairing the CS with food and the intervening
stimulus did not produce the effect unless both occurred on the same trial.
Together, these experiments rule out an account in terms of second-order con-
ditioning of the CS by the intervening stimulus. Experiment 5 used an intervening
stimulus in a second-order conditioning design to demonstrate that the stimulus
acted to improve the association between the CS and the reinforcer. That ex-
periment found the intervening stimulus to make responding to the CS more
vulnerable to subsequent extinction of its reinforcer. Together, these experiments
document a kind of catalytic effect of the intervening stimulus on the CS-re-
inforcer association.

It is well known that Pavlovian condition- may bring the CS and US together percep-
ing is best when the conditioned stimulus tually (e.g., Wertheimer, 1955). However,
(CS) is temporally proximal to the uncon- the manner in which this stimulus has its
ditioned stimulus (US; e.g., Gormezano & effect is not well understood.
Moore, 1969; Mackintosh, 1974; Newlin The purpose of the present series of ex-
& LoLordo, 1976). When a temporal gap periments was to provide both additional
intervenes between the CS and the US, con- documentation and a partial analysis of this
ditioning is depressed. However, there have effect in an autoshaping preparation. One
been a number of recent reports that this may consider two broad classes of explana-
deficit can be attenuated if a second stimulus tion. One possibility is that the intervening
occupies the time period between the CS and stimulus acts to improve the first-order as-
the US (e.g., Bolles, Collier, Bouton, & sociation between the CS and the US. Fa-
Marlin, 1978; Kehoe, Gibbs, Garcia, & Gor- cilitation of that association might be ac-
mezano, 1979; Wasserman, Carr, & Deich, complished, for instance, if the IS reduced
1978; Balsam & Gibbon, Note 1; Kaplan, the amount of background conditioning that
Bottjer, & Hearst, Note 2). The effect of an otherwise poorly signaled US accom-
this intervening stimulus (IS) agrees both plished. This might reduce the degree to
with an older literature on delay of instru- which a well-conditioned background stim-
mental reinforcement (e.g., Grice, 1942) and ulus was present concurrently with the CS
with the intuition that a filled time period and thus permit more conditioning of that
CS (Balsam & Gibbon, Note 1). Another
This research was supported by National Science possibility is that the CS becomes directly
Foundation Grant BNS 78-02752. Thanks are due associated with the IS which in turn bears
Paula Durlach, Janis Marcus, and Allan Wagner for a favorable relation to the US (Rashotte,
many helpful comments. 1981). This second-order conditioning ac-
Requests for reprints should be sent to Robert A.
Rescorla, who is now at the Department of Psychology, count is analogous to the most popular ac-
University of Pennsylvania, 3815 Walnut Street, Phil- count for the action of intervening stimuli
adelphia, Pennsylvania 19104. in instrumental learning (e.g., Spence, 1947).
131
132 ROBERT A. RESCORLA

Experiments 1 and 2 provided an initial The apparatus consisted of four identical operant
documentation of the effect of various types chambers, each measuring 27 X 27 X 35 cm. The metal,
front panel of each chamber had a 5 X 5 cm food mag-
of intervening stimulus in an autoshaping azine in its center, located 5 cm above the wire mesh
preparation. Experiments 3 and 4 evaluated floor. Three' response keys, 2.5 cm in diameter, were '
the contribution of second-order condition- located one directly above the hopper and one on either
ing to the superiority produced by the in- side of the center of the front wall, 20 cm above the
floor. Located behind the right-hand key was an IEE
tervening stimulus. Experiment 5 provided inline projector that permitted the transillumination of
evidence that the effect results, in part, from the key with color and orientation stimuli. The left-hand
a potentiated CS-reinforcer association. half of that key could be illuminated with blue (B),
green (G), red (R), or yellow (Y) light, at which time
the other half of the key remained black. In addition,
Experiment 1 the entire key could be illuminated by a uniform white
This experiment used a within-subjects (W) stimulus. These stimuli were generated by Ekta-
chrome slides of drawings composed of "Color-aid" ar-
design to evaluate the effect of placing a tists paper. The remaining walls and ceiling of the cham-
stimulus at various points between a key bers were composed of clear Plexiglas.
light CS and a food US in autoshaping. Each These chambers were placed in sound- and light-at-
bird received four different colored key tenuating shells, with ventilation fans providing back-
ground noise of 62 dB (re 20 MN/m 2 ), On the rear wall
lights, each terminating 10 sec before the of those shells was mounted a 6-W bulb that was con-
delivery of food. The key lights differed in tinuously illuminated during the session except during
that either the entire period, the first half, the operation of the food hopper. The hopper was il-
the second half, or none of the period con- luminated when it made available Purina Pigeon Grain.
Procedure. Because these birds had previously re-
tained another stimulus. ceived autoshaping in another experiment, no magazine
In addition to providing increased sensi- training was necessary. Each conditioning session con-
tivity, a within-subjects design allows an tained twelve 5-sec presentations of each color, each
evaluation of two initial interpretations that presentation followed after a time interval of 10 sec by
can be applied to previous between-subjects 5-sec availability of grain. The W stimulus filled the 10
sec between one color and food, occupied the initial 5
experiments. The first is that the condition- sec of that time for another color, occupied the terminal
ing of IS per se facilitates responding to the 5 sec for another color, and was not presented following
CS, whatever its relation to the CS. This the fourth color. The birds were run in squads of four,
might come about because of stimulus gen- with the color assignments counterbalanced according
eralization or because responding to the IS to a Latin square. The mean intertrial interval was 1
min. Conditioning continued for 28 sessions.
generally elevates responding in the situa- "/
tion. The second possibility is that the effect
of the intervening stimulus is mediated by Results
differential conditioning of background cues. Figure 1 displays the mean response rates
In a within-subjects design, however, the IS during the various stimuli over the course of
signals the same proportion of USs in all conditioning. Although the levels of respond-
animals, but the CSs bear different temporal ing were low to all CSs, it is clear that re-
relations to the IS. Hence differences in re- sponding developed most rapidly and to the
sponding to those CSs cannot be attributed highest level in the CS for which the entire
to differences in the value of the background time gap was filled. From the second 4-day
against which they are trained or tested or block onward, that CS evoked reliably
to differences in the potential for stimulus greater responding than did the CS without
generalization from the IS. any intervening stimulus (Wilcoxon Ts < 2,
p$ < .02). The two CSs with only part of the
Method interval filled gave intermediate results. Fill-
Subjects and apparatus. The subjects were 16 fe- ing the first portion of the interval had the
male Carneaux pigeons about 1 yr old, maintained at greater effect early in Conditioning; only af-
75% of their free-feeding weights. They had previously ter the fourth block of days was that CS
participated in a second-order autoshaping experiment inferior to the CS with the full interval filled
that used a response key and stimuli that differed from
those employed here. Treatments in the present exper-
(Ts<4,ps< .05). The CS for which W oc-
iment were arranged to be orthogonal to those experi- cupied the last portion of the interval only
mental histories. gradually developed more responding than
INTERVENING STIMULUS 133

the unfilled CS. On the final block of trials, Ul 20


the two CSs with partially filled intervals z>
were not different but were both superior to I
the CS with its interval unfilled and inferior 15

to the CS with its full interval filled (Ts <


4, ps < .05).
,0
Throughout the experiment, the most re- I0

sponding was observed to W, the filling stim- 2


ulus. This responding was not differential </)
UJ
depending upon its placement in the interval
and was at an overall mean of 68 responses
per minute.
I 2 3 4 5 6
BLOCKS OF 4 DAYS
Discussion
Figure 1. Mean responses per minute during four stimuli
This experiment documents the facilita- all receiving a 10-sec trace relation to the food US. (All
tive effect of an intervening stimulus on re- [•], none [O], the first 5 sec [0], or the second 5 sec
sponding to a trape CS. It suggests that per- [O] of the trace was filled with another stimulus.)
formance is greater the more of the trace
interval is occupied by the intervening stim-
ulus. It further suggests that the initial por- tervening key light might then be to prevent
tion of the interval is the most important to the occurrence of such incompatible re-
fill, especially early in the course of condi- sponses and/to protect the CS from becoming
tioning. associated with them. In order to evaluate
These results appear to rule out differ- this possibility, Experiment 2 replicated the
ential conditioning of background stimuli basic result of Experiment 1 with the use of
and simple stimulus generalization as inter- an intervening stimulus that does not evoke
pretations of the facilitative effect of the in- key pecking.
tervening stimulus. However, they leave
open a number of alternative accounts. It Experiment 2
remains possible, for instance, that the in- When a pigeon receives an auditory CS
tervening stimulus has its effect primarily signaling food, it learns that relation, but it
by producing second-order conditioning of does not exhibit the learning in the form of
the target CS. This interpretation is en- key pecking (e.g., Nairne & Rescorla, 1981).
couraged by the observation that more fa- Informal observations in this laboratory sug-
cilitation was produced by presenting the gest that auditory signals may engender a
intervening stimulus immediately after the variety of behaviors, such as magazine ap-
CS than by an equivalent duration presen- proach, head bobbing, and pecking at the
tation later in the interval. Experiments 3 floor. Consequently, in this experiment a
and 4 provide evidence on various aspects of tone was used as the stimulus intervening
this second-order conditioning account. between a key light and food, so as to fill the
The particular nature of the intervening interval with a detectable stimulus that does
stimulus used here suggests, however, an- not evoke the same response as the key light.
other account. During an empty trace inter- In this and all subsequent experiments, the
val, the animals presumably engage in var- trace interval was shortened to 5 sec to fa-
ious behaviors other than key pecking; for cilitate acquisition.
instance, it seems likely that the animals
show approach to the food magazine during Method v
this time. Many of these behaviors may be
incompatible with key pecking. Moreover, Subjects and apparatus. The subjects were 16 fe-
male Carneaux pigeons which had previously partici-
since they immediately follow presentation pated in another autoshaping experiment with different
of the trace CS, these behaviors may become stimuli. Their assignment to conditions in the present
conditioned to that CS. The role of the in*- experiment was orthogonal to their previous experimen-
134 ROBERT A. RESCORLA

UJ 40 •- not to prevent the development of anticipa-


tory responses that are incompatible with
key pecking. In fact, informal observations
30 suggested that the tone regularly evoked
such behaviors during the trace interval.
The rapid acquisition observed in this ex-
periment compared with the acquisition in
20 Experiment 1 is likely attributable to the
I
O
Q.
simpler procedure and the shorter trace in-
tn terval. This experiment was not designed, of
UJ
cr 10 course, to evaluate the relative magnitude
of the effects produced by an intervening
u auditory stimulus and an intervening visual
stimulus, but it does indicate that a sub-
1 2 3 4 5 stantial effect can be obtained even when an
DAYS auditory stimulus is used. These results are
Figure 2. Mean responses per minute during a key light in agreement with those reported by Balsam
with a 5-sec trace relation to the food US. (The 5-sec and Gibbon (Note 1) and by Kaplan, Bottjer,
period was filled with a tone [•] or not [O].) and Hearst (Note 2).

Experiment 3
tal histories. They were maintained at 75% of their ad
lib body weights throughout the experiment. The ap- A highly plausible interpretation of the
paratus was a set of eight Skinner boxes identical in all
essentials to those of Experiment 1. The apparatus per- effects observed in Experiments 1 and 2 is
mitted the presentation of the R and G stimuli used in that the intervening stimulus second-order
Experiment 1 as well as an 1800-Hz tone of 76 dB. conditions the target CS. Previous experi-
Procedure, In order to ensure low levels of initial ments (e.g., Nairne & Rescorla, 1981; Res-
responding, all birds were first given four sessions in corla, 1979) provide ample evidence that
which the R and G key lights and the tone (T) were
each presented 12 times for 5 sec without consequence. both auditory and visual stimuli that have
On each of the next 5 days, all animals received 12 been paired with a food US can produce sub-
presentations each of R and G, followed after a S-sec stantial second-order conditioning of key
gap by 5 sec of food. For half of the animals, T inter- lights that they themselves follow. There are
vened between R and food, and for half it intervened
between G and food. The intertrial interval was 1 min. several different forms that such second-or-
der conditioning could take. One possibility
Results and Discussion is that a stimulus-stimulus (S-S) association
is formed between the CS and the IS. An-
The mean rates of responding to the key other is that a stimulus-response (S-R) as-
light CSs on the last pretest day were less sociation is formed between, the CS and some
than one per minute. Figure 2 shows the emotional response evoked by the IS. The
mean rates of responding over the course of available evidence suggests that in auto-
conditioning. Although the rates of respond- shaping in which both stimuli are key lights,
ing were similar on the first conditioning second-order conditioning is likely to be of
day, the key light followed by the tone rap- the former character. One relevant piece of
idly came to be pecked at a higher rate. By evidence is that after second-order auto-
the fifth day of conditioning, that difference shaping, extinction of the first-order stimu-
was highly reliable, T(\ 1) = 4,p < .02. Key lus dramatically attenuates responding to
pecking during the tone occurred at a rate the second-order stimulus with which it had
of less than one response per minute. been paired (e.g., Rescorla, 1979). Conse-
These results indicate that the stimulus quently, when the stimulus intervening be-
intervening between a key light and food tween a key light and food is another key
need not evoke pecking in order to facilitate light, it is reasonable to anticipate that any
responding to that key light. This suggests of its facilitative effect that is attributable
that the role of the intervening stimulus is to second-order conditioning could be re-
INTERVENING STIMULUS 135

duced by extinction of the intervening stim- l 80

ulus.
Experiment 3 used that logic to evaluate 60
the contribution of second-order condition-
ing. Two target CSs were each presented in
a trace relation to food. A different inter-
vening stimulus was used for each CS. Then
one of those intervening stimuli was extin-
£ 20
guished, and the target CSs were retested.
To the degree that the facilitative effect is
due to second-order conditioning, one would PRE 1 2 3 4 + - C
expect it to be reduced in the CS for which TEST CONDITIONING TEST
the intervening stimulus had been extin- (DAYS)
guished. For comparison purposes, each an- Figure 3. Mean responses per minute during two key
imal also received a third CS without any lights with a 5-sec trace relation to the food US. (For
event intervening before reinforcement. one stimulus [•], that trace was filled with another key
light [A]; for another [O], it was not. To the right is
responding to the CS with a filled interval after the filler
Method had been extinguished [-] or not [+] as well as re-
Subjects and apparatus. The subjects were 16 fe- sponding to the CS conditioned without the filler [O].
male Carneaux pigeons with histories similar to those
in the previous experiments, maintained in the same color discrimination for 3 reinforced and 12 nonrein-
fashion. The apparatus was that of Experiment 2 except forced trials; they then received 12 nonreinforced test
that the right-hand response key was used. A projector presentations each of the white background and the
permitted the presentation of two colors, blue (B) and grids. The issue was whether extinction of a color would
yellow (Y), each of which was projected on the bottom reduce responding to the stimulus for which it had
half of the key, and of two grid patterns each of which served as the intervenor before food.
could be projected on the top half of the key. One grid
consisted of 1-mm black lines spaced 2 mm apart on a
white background-and oriented 45° from the vertical; Results and Discussion
the other grid consisted of 1-mm black lines spaced 1
mm apart on a white background and oriented -45° Figure 3 shows the mean responding dur-
from the vertical. In addition, the top half of the key ing the various CSs and to the IS over the
could be illuminated by the white background alone. course of conditioning and during the final
When either half of the key was illuminated, the other test. The data were collapsed over stimuli to
half remained black. These stimuli are all readily dis-
criminable from one another and are regularly used in show responding during the patterns that
this laboratory as CSs and reinforcers in second-order had been followed or not by a color as well
conditioning experiments. as during the colors. To the far left are the
Procedure. In order to ensure low levels of initial rates of responding on the final preexposure
responding, all animals first received four sessions dur- day. These were, of course, quite low. The
ing each of which they received twelve 5-sec nonrein-
forced presentations each of the five stimuli to be used middle panel shows the course of condition-
in the study, blue, yellow, the two grids, and white. On ing, which was similar to that of the previous
each of the next 4 days, all birds received 12 reinforced experiment. Responding was greater to those
presentations each of the two grids and white. The stim- stimuli (two per animal) that had colors fol-
uli were 5 sec long and were followed by food 5 sec after
their termination. Blue intervened between one of the lowing them than to the stimulus that did
stimuli and food, and yellow intervened between a sec- not, 7(16) = 0, p < .01. As in Experiment
ond and food; the third stimulus had no intervening 1, responding was greatest of all to the filling
event. Animals were assigned to1 groups so as to balance color, indicated in the figure by triangles.
across the white and the two grid stimuli whether or not The subsequent discrimination between
the stimulus would be followed by a color and if so which
color. colors was rapidly learned. By the final day
Over the next 4 days, all animals received a treatment of discrimination, the mean response rate
designed to extinguish responding to one of the colors during the reinforced and nonreinforced col-
but not to the other. Half of the animals received on ors were 99 and 5 responses per minute, re-
each day 6 reinforced blue and 24 nonreinforced yellow
presentations. The other half of the animals received the spectively. The data of most interest, from
same treatment with blue and yellow interchanged. the final test of the patterns, are shown in
On the next day, all animals first received continued the final panel of Figure 3. The data are
136 ROBERT A. RE&CORLA

Table 1 was no contribution of any form of second-


Design of Experiment 4 order conditioning to the present effect. Sec-
ond-order conditioning is known sometimes
A X US
A to take the form of an association between
B US one stimulus and the response evoked by a
B X second. We do not have a full understanding
of when second-order conditioning will take
Note. All animals received all four trial types. A, B, and
X are key lights; the US is food.
an S-S or an S-R form, but we do know
that even in autoshaping it is sometimes S-
R in nature (Nairne & Rescorla, 1981).
separated according to whether the color Under those circumstances, one would not
that followed the pattern had been rein- expect reduction in the value of the color to
forced (+) or extinguished (-) or whether adversely affect responding to the pattern
no color had followed the pattern during despite the fact that the color acted to pro-
training (C). Both pattern stimuli that had duce second-order conditioning of the pat-
previously been followed by colors continued tern. Consequently, Experiment 4 provides
to show greater responding than did the un- an alternative evaluation of the second-order
filled pattern, whether or not the color had possibility.
been extinguished (Ts < 17, p < .02). But v

there was no reliable difference in respond- Experiment 4


ing between the two patterns that previously
had been followed by colors. This experiment attempted to generate
These data suggest that there was little the effect of an intervening stimulus simply
learning of an association between the ori- by combining the effects on a target CS of
entations and colors which was reflected in second-order conditioning from an IS with
responding to those orientations. Although those of first-order conditioning from the
that sort of learning has been documented ultimate reinforcer. A schematic of the de-
to occur in second-order conditioning exper- sign is shown in Table 1. The logic was to
iments with these stimuli (e.g., Rescorla, use two target CSs (A and B), each followed
1979; Rescorla & Durlach, in press), it ap- after a time period by the US on half of their
parently is not substantial in sequential pro- occurrences. In addition, each CS was fol-
cedures that terminate in a US. This finding lowed by the same intervening stimulus (X)
is in agreement with results recently re- on half of its occurrences. The CSs differed
ported by Holland (1980) for an activity in that for Stimulus A, X and the US oc-
conditioning preparation in rat subjects. In curred on the same trial (on the other trials
any case, associations between the orienta- the A was presented alone), whereas for
tions and colors were clearly not substantial Stimulus B, X and the US occurred on dif-
enough to account for the facilitative effect ferent trials. The result was that both CSs
of the intervening stimulus. were followed the same number of times by
It may be noted that this and the previous the same IS and by the same US but that
experiments used experimentally experi- they differed in the degree to which the IS
enced birds. Previous experience may change filled the gap between the CS and the US.
the general level of performance, but ar- Consequently, the IS had the same oppor-
ranging for the current treatments to be or- tunity to condition both CSs but different
thogonal to the earlier experiences makes opportunities to fill the time gap. Similarly,
unlikely any simple interaction with the ef- the US had the same opportunity to condi-
fects observed here. It is of particular rele- tion both CSs but had the preceding gap
vance to note that in this laboratory we filled only for one. Consequently, if only the
regularly observe substantial second-order addition of first- or second-order condition-
conditioning of an S-S variety in birds, with ing was generating greater responding in the
various experimental histories. / filled condition, then the two CSs should
However, the absence of S-S learning in show similar performance. If they differ, this
this experiment need not mean that there would suggest that there is something im-
INTERVENING STIMULUS 137

portant about the IS and US occurring on


the same trial which goes beyond the action
of independent processes.
Method
Subjects and apparatus. The subjects were 16 fe-
male Carneaux pigeons with histories similar to those
of the previous experiments. The apparatus was that of
Experiment 1, modified so that the right-hand key could I
ui
be illuminated by a green (G), yellow (Y),< or white (W) 2 3
light. DAYS
Procedure. On the first day of the experiment, all
animals were preexposed to the stimuli, receiving 12 Figure 4. Mean responses per minute to a key light
nonreinforced presentations each of G and Y and 24 followed on the same trial by a filler and a food US
nonreinforced presentations of W. All stimuli were 5 sec (•) or followed by those events on separate trials (O).
long and were delivered at a mean intertrial interval of
1 min.
directly as a reinforcer for it but rather by
For the next 4 days, the animals were divided into
somehow facilitating its association with the
two groups both of which received autoshaping. One
group of eight birds received 12 trials on which G was
ultimate US. Such a facilitation would pre-
followed immediately by W and then food, 12 trials on
sumably best take place when both the IS
which G was presented without consequence, 12 trials
and the US occur on the same trial.
on which Y was followed by W but no food was given,
These results also speak against an ac-
and 12 trials on which Y was followed by a S-sec gap
count in which the gap filler slows the de-
that terminated in food. The result was that G and Y
were both followed by food on half of the trials and by
velopment of latent inhibition to the CS. It
W on half of the trials; but for G, the W and food were
is possible that presenting the CS in a trace
positively correlated, and for Y they were negatively
relation to the US allows a reduction in CS
correlated. The other group of eight birds received the
salience and hence in its conditionability.
same treatments with the roles of G and Y interchanged,
Following the CS by another might, how-
Results and Discussion ever, retard that process (Lubow, Schnur,
& Rifkin, 1976) and result in the present
Prior to conditioning, the mean rates of effect. However, in this experiment both CSs
responding to G and Y were one and two were followed with equal frequency by the
responses per minute, respectively. Figure 4 same stimulus and hence should not differ
shows the results of principal interest, the
mean rates of responding to the colors during in Itthisshould
regard.
be noted that certain current
conditioning. The results were collapsed notions of conditioning would have antici-
across the particular stimulus identities and pated a quite different outcome for this ex-
separated according to whether W filled the periment. To the degree that the adminis-
gap before food or occurred on other trials. tration of the US interferes with the ability
It is clear from the figure that only the stim- of the IS to produce second-order condition-
ulus for which W actually filled the gap ing, the CS followed by W and food on the
showed a high level of responding. By the
final day of conditioning, the difference be- same trial should have been inferior (e.g.,
Cheatle & Rudy, 1978; Holland, 1980).
tween the two stimuli was highly reliable, Apparently, the facilitatiye impact of the IS
7X16) = 2, p< .02.
These results do not support a second-or- on the CS-US association is substantial
enough to overcome the loss of second-order
der conditioning interpretation. Whether the
form of that second-order conditioning is S- conditioning elsewhere observed when the
US occurs orrthe same trial as an effective
S or S-R, we would have expected substan- first-order CS.
tially more responding than was actually
observed to the stimulus for which W and Experiment 5
food occurred on separate trials. Thus these
results suggest the possibility that the IS fa- Experiments 3 and 4 present negative ev-
cilitates responding to the CS not by serving idence on a second-order conditioning inter-
138 ROBERT A. RESCORLA

Table 2 of responding to G, R, and H. On the first 3 days of


Design of Experiment 5 this stage, all birds received 30 reinforced presentations
each of X and B. On the next 2 days, they received 12
n
TT
A.
V reinforced presentations each of B and X and 24 non-
nJ3 reinforced presentations of H. Finally, all birds received
G X 2 days on which they received 16 presentations each of
n
¥J
D|J
X and B, 12 presentations of H, and 6 presentations
each of R and G. On those days, H, G, and R were
Note. All animals received all four trial types. R, G, B, never reinforced, but 4 of the 16 presentations of X and
and Y are key colors; H is a horizontal grid. B were followed by food. This partial reinforcement of
X and B was designed to match the frequencies of re-
inforcement they would be given during their subsequent
pretation and by inference support the pos- use as reinforcers in the second-order phase of the ex-
sibility that the IS serves as a kind of catalyst periment.
to promote the action of the reinforcer. Ex- On each of the next 4 days, B and X were used as
reinforcers to second-order condition R and G; H was
periment 5 attempted to provide more direct employed as the intervening stimulus. All animals re-
evidence on this possibility. It used the find- ceived three second-order trials with each of four pair-
ing, referred to above, that in second-order ings: R-X, R-B, G-X, and G-B. In each case, stimulus
autoshaping one can identify an association presentations were for 5 sec, and 5 sec intervened be-
tween stimuli. For half of the animals, H filled the time
between an S2 and the SI with which it has interval between R and X and the time interval between
been paired by measuring the change in re- G and B; for the other half of the animals, H filled the
sponding induced to S2 when SI is extin- gap between R and B and that between G and X. In
guished. In order to exploit that fact, an in- addition, all animals received two separate food-rein-
tervening stimulus was used to fill the time forced presentations each of B and X, to maintain their
level of first-order conditioning. During this phase of the
interval between an S2 and one SI but not experiment, the intertrial interval was 2 min.
between that S2 and another SI. Then one The consequence of these treatments was that if H
of the Sis was extinguished, but the other facilitates the formation of an association between S2
was not. The expectation was that if S2 is and SI, then for half of the animals R should be es-
pecially well associated with X, and G with B. Notice,
better associated with the SI for which its however, that the frequencies of presentation of the in-
gap had been filled, then extinction of that dividual events and the pairings of R and G with B, X,
SI should especially depress responding and H were all matched, so as to make possible the
toS2. inference that any observed effects are due to the cor-
relation of H with the occurrence of a particular pairing.
On each of the next 7 days, the animals were given
Method discriminative first-order conditioning with B and X. In
Subjects and apparatus. The subjects were 16 -naive each of the two groups, half of the animals received 6
female Carneaux pigeons about 6 mo old at the start reinforced presentations of B and 24 nonreinforced pre-
of the experiment. They were maintained at 75% of their sentations of X on each day; the other half had the roles
free-feeding weight throughout the experiment. The of B and X interchanged. Finally, two test sessions were
apparatus was that of Experiment 2- In this experiment, given; The first half of each contained continued first-
the left-hand key was activated. A projector located order discrimination for 3 reinforced and 12 nonrein-
behind that key permitted the presentation of red (R) forced trials; the second half contained 6 nonreinforced
or green (G) on the left-hand side, a grid of 1-mm black presentations each of R and of G. The expectation was
horizontal lines spaced 1 mm apart on a white back- that if, say, R is especially well associated with X and
ground (H) on the right-hand side, or the full illumi- G is well associated with B, then R should be more
nation of the key with either blue (B) light or an X sensitive than is G to an extinction treatment of X.
composed of 1-mm black lines on a white background.
Procedure. The animals were initially trained to eat Results and Discussion
food from the magazine. On the first day, they were
placed in the chambers, and the food hoppers were ac- First-order conditioning of B and X pro-
tivated until the birds had eaten for about 15 sec. Then
the hopper presentations were progressively shortened
ceeded rapidly, as did the discrimination of
and given less frequently until the birds consistently ate G, R, and H. By the last day preceding sec-
during a 5-sec presentation. On each of the next 3 days, ond-order conditioning, the mean responses
the animals were given 44 hopper deliveries spaced at per minute for X and B were 83 and 80,
a mean intertrial interval of 1 min. whereas those for G, R, and H were 6, 7,
Table 2 illustrates the remainder of the experiment
for half of the color counterbalancing. The birds were and 1, respectively. The course of second-
given first-order conditioning designed to produce au- order conditioning of R and G is shown on
toshaped responding to X and B while yielding low rates the left-hand side of Figure 5, which presents
INTERVENING STIMULUS 139

the mean number of trials with a response, MOO

a measure that I find more stable in describ-


ing the results of second-order conditioning BO
experiments. By the end of second-order con-
ditioning, H had attained a percentage of
trials with a response of 80. Throughout sec-
F+
ond-order conditioning, responding to B and
X remained high and similar on the various
trial types. F-
The subsequent discrimination between B
and X was reasonably rapid. By the final day
20
of this phase, the mean responses per minute
for the reinforced and nonreinforced first-
order stimuli were 100 and 7, respectively.
Responses occurred on 85% and 3% of the 2
DAYS TEST
trials, respectively. The results of most in-
terest, from the final test sessions with G and Figure 5, Mean percentage of trials with a response to
R, are shown on the right-hand side of Fig- a second-order CS paired in a trace relation with two
different first-order stimuli. (For one of those pairings,
ure 5. These results are separated according another stimulus filled the trace interval. To the right
to whether the first-order stimulus with is responding to the second-order CS after the first-order
which the color had previously had a filled stimulus with which it had a filled interval received ei-
relation was subsequently reinforced (+) or ther extinction [F-] or additional reinforcement [F+].)
extinguished (-). Since both R and G had
been paired with each first-order stimulus to the target CS seems to be a potential ac-
but only one with a filled interval, each an- count. Moreover, it is not fully due to the
imal contributed data to each type of stim- filler's serving as a reinforcer for the CS in
ulqs. These results show a greater level of the manner of second-order conditioning.
responding to the S2 whose filled SI had Apparently, the filler serves a kind of cata-
been reinforced and whose nonfilled SI had lytic function actually promoting the asso-
been extinguished than to the S2 with the ciation between the CS and the reinforcer.
converse treatment, T(10) = 3,/j < .02. This This conclusion need not mean that sec-
suggests that filling the interval actually im- ond-order conditioning never contributes to
proved the strength of the association be- the effect of an intervening stimulus in trace
tween an S2 and an SI. conditioning. The results of Experiments 3
It may especially be noted that since H and 4 suggest that second-order conditioning
had been equally paired with R and (fJ as is not a complete account. But the finding
well as with X and B, it would not be possible of Experiment 1, that filling the early portion
for changes in the value of H to mediate of the interval produces the best effect, sug-
differential responding to R and G. gests that there may be some contribution
of second-order conditioning, especially early
General Discussion in conditioning.
It is of interest to compare the effects of
This series of experiments makes it clear filling a trace interval not only with the re-
that the deficit produced by a time interval sults of simple trace conditioning but also
between a CS and a US can be attenuated with those of a delayed conditioning proce-
if another stimulus fills that interval. More- dure. Indeed, one way to think of delayed
over, that attenuation does not appear to be conditioning is as a variation of the present
attributable to a variety of plausible inter- procedures in which the intervening stimulus
pretations. It can be demonstrated under is maximally similar to the target stimulus.
circumstances in which neither differential Although the present experiments have not
background conditioning, differential oppor- explored this case, other evidence (e.g.,
tunity for competing responses, nor differ- Newlin & LoLordo, 1976) suggests that a
ential stimulus generalization from the filler delayed procedure generates responding dur-
140 ROBERT A. RESCORLA

ing the interval which is superior to that ob- ditional stimuli occur when the stimuli are
served with a filled trace procedure. A nat- present concurrently with the CS whereas
ural explanation can be given in terms of the present effects were demonstrated with
stimulus generalization, although it is pos- sequential presentation might encourage that
sible that an intervening stimulus that is speculation. Most theories of memory seem
highly similar to the target CS would serve to anticipate that the filler would harm
as a superior catalyst. rather than help the short-term memory for
The present findings extend the list of the CS (e.g., Wagner, in press). However,
ways in which one stimulus can influence it is possible that the filler somehow protects
conditioning of another in compound con- the memory of the CS from the interfering
ditioning situations. There is a good deal of effects of other stimuli that the organism
evidence that under some conditions a sec- might receive during the trace interval. Al-
ond stimulus can reduce conditioning of a ternatively, the filler might enhance the
CS paired with a reinforcer. This may occur power of the US. A US that is well signaled
either when the second stimulus is originally by the filler might be more effective than a
neutral (overshadowing) or when it has a US that is unsignaled. This interpretation
history of pretraining with the same US seems less plausible in the light of the small
(blocking). One interpretation of such find- facilitative effect in Experiment 1 of filling
ings is that two stimuli must compete for the the last portion of the interval. Moreover,
association with the US (see Dickinsqn & current empirical and theoretical work on
Mackintosh, 1978; Rescorla & Holland, the effectiveness of USs anticipates the op-
1982). There is also evidence that two stim- posite conclusion (e.g., Rescorla & Holland,
uli in a compound become associated with 1982).
each other. Such associations may be either Consequently, at present the evidence al-
S-R (e.g., Rizley & Rescorla, 1972) or S- lows little more than the acknowledgment
S (e.g., Rescorla, 1979) in character.' At that these data agree with a kind of percep-
least one case of such within-compound as- tual intuition, that events which are bridged
sociations is responsible for a potentiation in time by a third event appear to go to-
of conditioning like that observed here. Dur- gether. It may be that this is analogous to
lach and Rescorla (1980) found that asso- another perceptual influence, that of simi-
ciations between odors and tastes were partly larity, which has received some additional
responsible for the enhanced aversion that analysis (Rescorla & Gillan,;1980).
a poison was capable of producing to an odor
when they were paired in the presence of a Reference Notes
taste. Finally, there is evidence that associ- 1. Balsam, P. D., & Gibbon, J. Associative factors un-
ations between the elements of a compound derlying trace decrements in autoshaping. Paper pre-
interact with those between the elements and sented at the meeting of the Eastern Psychological
the reinforcer. Under some circumstances Association, Washington, April 1978.
that interaction is such that it attenuates 2. Kaplan, P. S,, Bottjer, S. W., & Hearst, E. Filling
a gap in our understanding of trace conditioning:
both associations (e.g.,-Cheatle & Rudy, Discrimination of the intertrial interval from the
1978; Holland, 1980; Rescorla, 1981). "empty" period before VS. Paper presented at the
But the present data suggest that there meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association,
are also cases in which the presence of a Hartford, Connecticut, April 1980.
second stimulus can truly facilitate the as-
sociation between a stimulus and the US. References
There are several possible ways in which Bolles, R. C., Collier, A. C., Bouton, M. E., & Marlin,
such a facilitation could be accomplished. N. A. Some tricks for ameliorating the trace-condi-
The presence of the filling stimulus may tioning deficit. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society,
somehow enhance the salience of the target 1978, ;/, 403-406.
CS. It might, for instance, better preserve Cheatle, M. D., & Rudy, J. W. Analysis of second-order
odor-aversion conditioning in neonatal rats: Impli-
it in a short-term memory so as to make it cations for Kamin's blocking effect. Journal of Ex-
more available for association with the US. perimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,
The fact that most interfering effects of ad- 1978, 4, 237-249.
INTERVENING STIMULUS 141

Dickinson, A., & Mackintosh, N. J. Classical condi- H. S. Terrace, & J. Gibbon (Eds.), Autoshaping and
tioning in animals. Annual Review of Psychology, conditioning theory. New York: Academic Press,
1978, 29, 587-612. 1981.
Durlach, P. J,, & Rescorla, R. A. Potentiation rather Rescorla, R. A. Aspects of the reinforcer learned in
than overshadowing in flavor-aversion learning: Ah second-order Pavlovian conditioning. Journal of Ex-
analysis in terms of within-compound associations. perimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Be- 1979,5,79-95.
havior Processes, 1980, 6, 175-187. Rescorla, R. A. Within-signal learning in autoshaping.
Gormezano, I., & Moore, J.-W. Classical conditioning: Animal Learning & Behavior, 1981, 9, 245-252.
Beyond the basic parameters. In M, H. Marx (Ed.), Rescorla, R. A., & Durlach, P. J. Within-event learning
Learning: Processes. New York; Macmillan, 1969, in Pavlovian conditioning. In N. S, Spear & R. Miller
Grice, G. R, An experimental study of the gradient of (Eds.), Information processing in animals: Memory
reinforcement in maze learning. Journal of Experi- mechanisms, Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, in press.
mental Psychology, 1942, 30, 475-489. Rescorla, R. A., & Gillan, D. J. An analysis of the
Holland, P. C. Second-order conditioning with and with- facilitative effect of similarity on second-order con-
out unconditioned stimulus presentation. Journal of ditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: An-
Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Pro- imal Behavior Processes, 1980, 6, 339-351.
cesses, 1980, 6, 238-250. Rescorla, R. A., & Holland, P. C. Behavioral studies
Kehoe, E. J., Gibbs, C. M., Garcia, E., & Gormezano, of animal learning and memory. Annual Review of
I. Associative transfer and stimulus selection in clas- Psychology, 1982, 33, 265-308.
sical conditioning of the rabbit's nictitating mem- Rizley, R. C., & Rescorla, R. A. Associations in second-
brane response to serial compound CSs. Journal of order conditioning and sensory preconditioning. Jour-
Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Pro- nal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology,
cesses, 1979, 5, 1-18. 1972,*;, 1-11.
Lubow, R. E., Schnur, P., & Rifkin, B. Latent inhibition Spence, K. W. The role of secondary reinforcement in
and conditioned attention theory. Journal of Exper- delayed reward learning. Psychological Review, 1947,
imental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 54, 1-8.
1976, 2, 163-174. Wagner, A. R, SOP: A model/ of automatic memory
processing in animal behavior. In N. S. Spear & R.
Mackintosh, N, J. The psychology of animal learning,
Miller (Eds.), Information processing in animalsf
New York: Academic Press, 1974.
Memory mechanisms. Hillsdale, NJ.: Erlbaum, in
Nairne, J. S., & Rescorla, R. A. Second-order condi- press.
tioning with diffuse auditory reinforcers in the pigeon. Wasserman, E. A., Carr, D. L., & Deich, J. D. Asso-
Learning and Motivation, 1981, / 2, 65-91. ciation of conditioned stimuli during serial condition-
Newlin, R. J., & LoLordo, V. M. A comparison of peck- ing by pigeons. Animal Learning & Behavior, 1978,
ing generated by serial, delay, and trace autoshaping 6, 52-56.
procedures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Wertheimer, M. Laws of organization in perceptual
Behavior, 1976, 25, 227-241. forms. In W. D. Ellis (Ed.), A source book ofgestalt
Rashotte, M. E. Second-order autoshaping: Contribu- psychology. New York: Humanities Press, 1955.
tions to the research and theory of Pavlovian rein-
forcement by conditioned stimuli. In C. M. Locurto, Received May 12, 1981 •

You might also like