You are on page 1of 9

Accident Analysis and Prevention 171 (2022) 106664

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Accident Analysis and Prevention


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/aap

The effect of anger on pedestrian avoidance in a simulated driving task


Qian Zhang a, b, c, Weina Qu a, b, Yan Ge a, b, *
a
CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Beijing, China
b
Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
c
School of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi’an, China

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Studies of the influence of emotions on driving behaviour have produced contradictory conclusions. This
Anger confusion is related to two factors: emotional arousal and driving tasks. The purpose of this study is to explore the
Pedestrian-crossing event effects of anger and happiness on the driving behaviour of drivers who encounter a pedestrian-crossing event on
Risky behaviour
an unmarked road, which requires strategic and behavioural choices. Thirty-nine drivers completed a simulated
Simulated driving
driving task to avoid pedestrians under the influence of state emotion. The results showed that anger increased
the average driving speed, the minimum speed when encountering a pedestrian, the probability of passing in
front of a pedestrian, and the lateral distance to the pedestrian from the right. However, there was no difference
between the impacts of happy and neutral moods on driving behaviour. These results suggest that general risky
driving behaviour (e.g., speeding) is mainly affected by anger state. Meanwhile avoidance behaviour patterns in
pedestrian-crossing tasks, as a driving behaviour related to prosocial attitudes, are also affected by emotional
valence. Recommendations and implications for further research on driving anger are discussed.

1. Introduction the one hand, a high arousal state decreases individuals’ response ca­
pacity. For example, drivers’ brake reaction times are marginally slower
Emotion is an important factor in the processes of thinking and when a lead-vehicle event occurs 500 ms after an individual is shown
performing an action, and driving performance is no exception (Nass high-arousal images (Trick et al., 2012). Furthermore, drivers’ reaction
et al., 2005). Whether emotion positively or negatively influences times in detecting peripheral visual targets increase with higher levels of
driving behaviour is unknown. Some studies have suggested that emo­ anxiety (Janelle et al., 1999). On the other hand, potential hazards are
tions have protective effects, such as decreasing driving speed or perceived better during neutral states than during emotional arousal
reducing traffic violations, by increasing perceived risk and decreasing (Jones et al., 2014). Anger and happiness, both categorized as high-
the willingness to engage in reckless driving (Barnard and Chapman, arousal emotions, have been assumed to have a similar effect on
2016; Lu et al., 2013; Schmidt-Daffy, 2013; Taubman-Ben-Ari, 2012). driving performance. However, little empirical evidence has shed light
However, other studies have shown negative effects of emotions. For on whether these two emotions influence driving performance in the
example, anger is typically thought to be a negative emotion that evokes same way.
approach motivation (Adams et al., 2006; Harmon-Jones, 2003) and Anger has been recognized as a contributor to dangerous driving
leads to aggressive driving behaviour (Bogdan et al., 2016). The behaviour; it is a direct factor in motor vehicles’ involvement in colli­
different findings may be due to various attributes of the emotion, such sions and the severity of consequences (Deffenbacher et al., 2003;
as arousal and categorization. Mesken et al., 2007; Roidl et al., 2013; Underwood et al., 1999). Re­
searchers have presented the effect of anger on driving behaviour from
various perspectives and using various measures. Some studies using
1.1. Why focus on anger and happiness? self-report methods have measured the habits of drivers in angry states
(Dahlen and White, 2006; Deffenbacher et al., 2003; Deffenbacher et al.,
Excessive arousal is an emotional characteristic that negatively af­ 2000; Deffenbacher et al., 2001; Sullman and Stephens, 2013; Sullman,
fects driving performance and the willingness to engage in reckless 2006; Sullman et al., 2007). Other studies have focused on drivers’
driving (Easterbrook, 1959; Taubman-Ben-Ari, 2012; Yerkes, 2004). On

* Corresponding author at: 16 Lincui Road, Chaoyang District, Beijing 100101, China.
E-mail address: gey@psych.ac.cn (Y. Ge).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2022.106664
Received 27 September 2021; Received in revised form 3 February 2022; Accepted 1 April 2022
Available online 9 April 2022
0001-4575/© 2022 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Q. Zhang et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 171 (2022) 106664

performance using driving simulators (Stephens and Groeger, 2011; Taking into account the impact of task differences on emotional ef­
Stephens et al., 2013) or during actual driving (Mesken et al., 2007). fects, it is worth summarizing and considering the driving tasks and
Anger has been found to lead to higher speeds, greater acceleration, and indicators in previous studies. First, self-reported driving history (e.g.,
poorer lateral position vehicle control (Mesken et al., 2007; Roidl et al., accidents and violations) and the frequency of various types of driving
2014; Techer et al., 2017). Moreover, anger affects drivers’ overall behaviour (e.g., aggressive driving behaviour, risky driving behaviour,
driving performance and outcomes during specific events, such as drunk driving, errors) are widely used as dependent variables in survey
resulting in harder braking during lane merging events, shorter waiting studies (Dula and Ballard, 2003; Harris et al., 2014; Nesbit et al., 2007;
time during left-turn events, closer following distance during car- Parker et al., 1995; Qu et al., 2014). Second, longitudinal and lateral
following events and more traffic violations (running red lights, vehicle control, such as speed, acceleration, lateral deviation and reac­
crossing lines) (Abdu et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2016). tion time, are often recorded in driving simulator studies and considered
Whether happiness impairs driving performance remains unclear. to be related to attention and basic cognitive ability (Mesken et al.,
Some studies have shown that happy arousal can lead to more driving 2007; Roidl et al., 2014; Steinhauser et al., 2018; Techer et al., 2017;
errors (e.g., worse lane keeping and more aggressive driving and traffic Ünal et al., 2013). Finally, and perhaps most importantly, strategies and
violations) (Jeon et al., 2014b) and higher speeds (Rhodes et al., 2015). patterns in specific driving events have received increasing attention.
A reduction in the perceived risk of general accidents resulting from For example, the relationship between emotions and the number of
humorous emotions has also been found (Hu et al., 2013; Lu et al., traffic violation types (e.g., yellow or red light crossings, collisions with
2013). However, evidence that happiness and neutral conditions do not weaving cars, pedestrians hit) has been explored in simulator studies via
differ in their effects on driving performance is even more noteworthy. objective measures (Abdu et al., 2012; Jeon et al., 2014b). Zhang et al.
Drivers who listen to happy music or notice pleasant words and images (2016) focused on the pattern of driving performance during lane
show more distraction but a decrease in speed (Chan and Singhal, 2013; merging events, including later and harder braking and narrower and
Pêcher et al., 2009) and better lane keeping (Hancock et al., 2012), more heuristic processing-style eye movement under angry conditions.
which is considered a beneficial compensation behaviour. Furthermore, Moreover, individual preference (Zhang et al., 2019) and response ca­
many researchers agree that pleasure has a positive effect on human pacity (Zhang et al., 2020) have been studied in car-following driving
attention and cognition (Carver, 2003; Fredrickson, 2004; Isen, 2001). A tasks. However, pedestrian-crossing events, which are among the most
traffic study showed that inducing a happy mood with music did not common accident scenarios, have received insufficient attention.
increase response times or fixation durations beyond those associated Pedestrian crossing scenarios lead to traffic accidents for drivers who
with a sad mood in hazard perception tasks (Zimasa et al., 2017). The show little willingness to give way to pedestrians (Bella and Silvestri,
above review suggests that the impact of happiness on driving behaviour 2015; Várhelyi, 1998) and even greater aggression when pedestrians
and the impact of anger are not consistent across studies. cross at an unmarked crosswalk (Katz et al., 1975; Ragland and Mitman,
The difference in the impact of happiness and anger on driving 2007). More importantly, aggressive priming has been found to increase
behaviour may be caused by valence. This view has been supported by the failure to yield the right of way and to drive closer to pedestrians
the research of Trick et al. (2012), in which image valence had an effect than no priming (Chai and Zhao, 2016). Recent studies have focused on
on steering performance: drivers showed improvement after viewing the crossing strategies of pedestrians (Yang et al., 2006; Zhao and Wu,
positive images but worse performance after viewing negative images. 2012; Zhou et al., 2009; Zhuang and Wu, 2011); however, the subtle
Another study focusing on emotional valence and risk perception in the changes in driving patterns under different emotional states with respect
transportation field also found that negative affect showed opposite and to pedestrian crossings need to be further studied.
stronger effects on drivers’ risk perception than positive affect (Hu et al., Given that the number of pedestrian deaths in 2016 was more than
2013). Specifically, negative emotion significantly elevated drivers’ risk 16,000 in China, accounting for 25% of the national road traffic fatal­
perception compared with drivers who experienced positive emotion ities, more attention to driving patterns when approaching pedestrian
and those in a control condition (Hu et al., 2013). In contrast, other crossings is needed. This study has two main purposes. First, this
studies have shown a consistent effect of positive and negative emotions simulator study primarily focuses on the effects of anger and happiness
on driving performance. These studies found that emotional images, on driving patterns in a pedestrian-crossing task. Second, by comparing
whether positive or negative, reduced sensitivity to potential hazards, as the effects of anger and happiness on driving performance, the effect of
indicated by lower subjective ratings of risk, smaller physiological re­ valence on driving performance can be further revealed. In summary,
sponses and shorter fixation durations when viewing a hazard after the hypotheses of this study are as follows:
viewing positive or negative images than after viewing neutral images
(Jones et al., 2014). a. Compared to neutral conditions, anger not only leads to risky driving
behaviour (e.g., speeding) but also encourages drivers to be more
1.2. Why focus on pedestrian avoidance situations? impulsive in avoidance patterns (e.g., the minimum speed when
encountering a pedestrian, the probability of passing in front of a
The lack of consistent results has been thought to be related to two pedestrian, and the lateral distance to the pedestrian).
factors: emotional arousal and driving tasks. On the one hand, Eheren­ b. Compared to neutral conditions, happiness leads to risky driving
freund-Hager et al. (2017) found that arousing positive affect and behaviour (e.g., speeding) but does not encourage drivers to be more
negative affect led to increased risky driving and that relaxing positive impulsive in avoidance patterns.
affect moderated risk-taking. In addition, the effects of emotion on
driving were found to be highly task-specific and dependent on the 2. Method
attentional demands of multiple tasks while driving (Steinhauser et al.,
2018). In a simulator study with two different driving tasks, brake re­ 2.1. Participants
actions were unaffected by positive and negative emotions in a modified
continuous tracking and reaction task but selectively influenced by Thirty-nine male drivers with more than three years of driving
anger in a modified version of the three vehicle platooning task (Stein­ experience were recruited through an online advertisement at a driving
hauser et al., 2018). It was explained that the distance to the lead car was service company in Beijing. Their ages ranged from 27 years to 43 years
flexible and personalized only in the three vehicle platooning task, and (M = 32.54; SD = 3.59), with an average driving experience of 10.15
anger shortened the distance to the lead car; thus, more salient brake years (SD = 3.93). They drove an average of 30,835 km/year (SD =
lights in this condition decreased the braking reaction time (Steinhauser 19,162) and had a total mileage of 337,179 km (SD = 310,373). See
et al., 2018). Table 1 for more details.

2
Q. Zhang et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 171 (2022) 106664

Table 1
Participant demographics (n = 39).
N Percent (%)

Age
20–30 years 14 35.9
31–40 years 24 61.5
41–56 years 1 2.6
Driving years
≤ 5 years 2 5.1
6–10 years 21 53.8
> 10 years 16 41.0
Annual mileage
≤ 20000 km 16 41.0
20001–40000 km 13 33.3
40001–60000 km 8 20.5
> 60001 km 2 5.1

Fig. 1. Simulated traffic scenario and events of target pedestrians from the left
2.2. Emotion induction and check
and right sides of the road.

For the present study, three emotion priming videos were chosen
subjective emotional scale. Next, the participants were asked to drive
from the Standardized Database of Chinese Emotional Film Clips (Ge
according to their daily driving habits in a simulator with pedestrian
et al., 2018). The participants in the anger condition watched a segment
crossings. They were also informed of the speed limit and asked to arrive
from the movie “City of Life and Death” that lasted 73 s. The clip that
at the destination as soon as possible. After driving, they again rated
was used to induce happiness was a segment from the movie “Better and
their emotions using the same scale. At the end of each session, the
Better” that lasted approximately 85 s. The neutral video was a 41-sec­
participants took a one-minute break. Following completion of all three
ond clip from a popular science documentary called “Space Millen­
sessions, they were asked to complete a demographic questionnaire that
nium”. More descriptions of the reasons for and details of the use of
included age, driving experience, annual mileage and three-year colli­
video clips to induce emotions have been presented in a published
sion history. This process lasted approximately 45 min, and the partic­
article (Q. Zhang et al., 2019). The participants’ emotions were checked
ipants were rewarded with RMB 200 yuan (approximately $29.08 US)
using a self-report method with a self-assessment manikin (SAM)
for participation. The study was approved by the Institutional Review
(Bradley and Lang, 1994). The participants were asked to rate their
Board of the Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences.
anger, happiness, arousal and valence levels on a 9-point scale from 1
(not at all) to 9 (very much) (Abdu et al., 2012).
3. Results
2.3. Apparatus and scenarios
3.1. Manipulation check
The driving task was completed using a STISIM® driving simulator
The t test was used to compare the emotion scores in the various
(STISIM DRIVE M100K). The driving scenario was presented on a 27-
conditions after priming and after driving. For the anger condition, the
inch LCD with 1920 × 1200-pixel resolution. The driving simulator
anger score was significantly higher than in the neutral condition after
also included a Logitech Momo® steering wheel with force feedback, a
priming (t = -9.16, p < 0.001), and the effect was maintained until the
gas pedal, and a brake pedal.
drivers finished driving (t = -3.65, p = 0.001). For the happiness con­
There was a practice phase before the formal experiment to ensure
dition, the happiness score was significantly higher than in the neutral
that the participants had no questions about the driving simulator. The
condition after priming (t = -7.82, p < 0.001), but this effect was not
driving scenarios included free driving without incident, following a car
maintained until the drivers finished driving (t = -7.82, p = 0.164).
and events similar to the formal experiment. This phase lasted 20 min.
One-way ANOVA was used to compare emotional valence and
The scenario in the formal experiment involved a one-way lane with
arousal in the three conditions after priming and after driving. The main
a speed limit of 20 mph and right-hand traffic. The driver’s seat was on
effect of emotion on valence was significant after emotion priming
the left side of the vehicle. Twenty-four pedestrians separately appeared
(F(2,76) = 88.25**, η2 = 0.699) and after driving (F(2,76) = 9.32**, η2 =
2 ft from the roadway edge line, with 12 on the left and 12 on the right.
0.197). The post hoc analysis confirmed that the valence in the anger
Eight pedestrians crossed the road (target stimulus), and the other 16
condition was significantly lower than in the neutral and happiness
pedestrians did not move (nontarget stimulus). For target pedestrians on
conditions after priming and after driving (all ps < 0.010). The valence
the left side, when the vehicle was within 200 ft of them, the pedestrian
in the happiness condition was significantly higher in the neutral and
began to cross the road from the left side to the right side at a constant
anger condition (ps < 0.010); however, after driving, the valence in the
speed of 2 mph. For target pedestrians on the right side, when the
happiness condition was significantly higher than in the anger condition
vehicle was within 120 ft of them, the pedestrian began to cross the road
(p = 0.004) but not significantly higher than the neutral condition (p =
at a constant speed of 2 mph (see Fig. 1).
1.000).
For the arousal scores, the main effect of emotion was also significant
2.4. Procedure
after priming (F(2,76) = 35.87**, η2 = 0.486) as well as after driving
(F(2,76) = 7.56**, η2 = 0.166). The post hoc analysis confirmed that after
After signing the informed consent form, the participants began a
priming, arousal in the anger and happiness conditions was significantly
two-stage practice session to familiarize themselves with the driving
higher than in the neutral condition after priming. Moreover, arousal
simulator and task for 20 min. Then, they began the formal experiment
scores in the anger condition were significantly higher than those in the
in three sessions. The first session involved driving under a neutral
happy condition (all ps < 0.010). After driving, arousal scores were
emotion condition, while the order of the other two aroused-emotion
significantly higher in the angry condition than in the neutral condition
sessions involved driving under happiness and anger conditions and
(p = 0.005), while arousal in the happy condition was not significantly
was counterbalanced. Each session included two phases. First, the par­
different from the other two conditions (pneutral-happiness = 0.183, panger-
ticipants watched a film clip and then evaluated their emotions on a

3
Q. Zhang et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 171 (2022) 106664

= 0.076).
happiness
These results indicate that emotional arousal was effective. The
emotion of anger was maintained throughout the driving task, while the
emotion of happiness showed a marked decline. The means and standard
deviations of the emotion scores are shown in Table 2.

3.2. Effect of emotion on average speed

To determine the effect of emotion on average driving speed, the


main effect of affective state (neutral, happiness, anger) was calculated
with ANOVA and was found to be significant (F(2,76) = 7.39, η2 = 0.156,
p = 0.002). A Bonferroni post hoc corrected analysis was then con­
ducted. The participants in the anger condition (M = 18.40, SD = 1.61)
exhibited significantly higher speed than those in the neutral (M =
17.94, SD = 1.27) (p = 0.022) and happiness (M = 17.82, SD = 1.43) (p
= 0.007) conditions, while the speed difference between the neutral and
happiness conditions was not significant (p = 1.00) (see Fig. 2).

3.3. Effect of emotion on pedestrian avoidance behaviour


Fig. 2. Average speed in the angry, happy and neutral conditions. Error bars
3.3.1. Avoidance patterns indicate the 95% confidence intervals.
Three different driving patterns of avoiding collisions with pedes­
trians were observed: passing in front of pedestrians, passing behind 3.3.3. Lateral distance from a simulated pedestrian
pedestrians and stopping to wait for pedestrians to pass (see Fig. 3). The The lateral distance from a simulated pedestrian indicated the lateral
descriptive statistics of the three avoidance patterns in the neutral, lane position of the driver with respect to the simulated pedestrian when
happiness and anger conditions are shown in Table 3. To determine the the value of the longitudinal distance was zero and positive to the right
effect of emotion on the percentage of the passing in front pattern, the (in ft).
main effect of affective state (neutral, happiness, anger) was calculated A 3 (affective state: neutral, happiness, anger) × 2 (pedestrian po­
with ANOVA and was found to be significant (F(2,76) = 16.14, η2 = sition: left and right) repeated-measures ANOVA was calculated. The
0.298, p < 0.001). The Bonferroni post hoc corrected analysis indicated main effects of affective state (F(2,76) = 1.20, η2 = 0.031, p = 0.308) and
that the percentage of passing in front of the target pedestrians in the pedestrian position (F(1,38) = 0.21, η2 = 0.006, p = 0.649) were not
anger condition (M = 0.43, SD = 0.37) was significantly higher than in significant. However, the interaction effect was significant (F(2,76) =
the neutral (M = 0.26, SD = 0.30) (p = 0.000) and happiness conditions 4.34, η2 = 0.102, p = 0.016), and a simple effect analysis with Bonferroni
(M = 0.21, SD = 0.30) (p = 0.001), while there was no significant dif­ correction was conducted. The drivers’ lateral distances from left-target
ference between the happiness and neutral conditions (p = 0.581) (see pedestrians were not significantly different among the three affective
Fig. 4). state conditions (all ps > 0.100). The lateral distance from a right-target
pedestrian in the anger condition (M = 13.97) was significantly less than
3.3.2. Minimum driving speed that in the neutral condition (M = 15.23) (p = 0.005), while the distance
The minimum driving speed reflected the minimum longitudinal in the happiness condition (M = 14.68) was not significantly different
velocity (in mph) of the vehicle when it was within 200 ft of a left target from the other two conditions (p happiness vs anger = 0.263, p happiness vs
pedestrian or 120 ft of a right target pedestrian (Chai and Zhao, 2016). neutral = 0.475). The results are shown in Fig. 6.
To determine the effect of emotion on minimum driving speed, a 3
(affective state: neutral, happiness, anger) × 2 (pedestrian position: left 4. Discussion
and right) repeated-measures ANOVA was calculated. The main effect of
affective state was significant (F(2,76) = 12.11, η2 = 0.242, p = 0.00003), Researchers commonly explore the rules and mechanisms of the in­
and a Bonferroni post hoc corrected analysis was conducted. The par­ fluence of emotion on driving based on the perspective of emotional
ticipants significantly increased their minimum driving speed in the properties, mainly valence and arousal. Previous studies have drawn
anger condition (M = 9.49) compared with the neutral (M = 7.01) (p = inconsistent conclusions. The sources of these differences may be that
0.0004) and happiness (M = 7.52) (p = 0.002) conditions, while the driving tasks involve different cognitive processes and that there are
difference between the neutral and happiness conditions was not sig­ subtle differences between discrete emotion categories. Hence, this
nificant (p = 0.866). The main effect of pedestrian position was signif­ study explored the effects of anger and happiness on the driving be­
icant (F(1,38) = 11.50, η2 = 0.232, p = 0.002), and the minimum driving haviours of drivers when they encountered a pedestrian-crossing event
speed was significantly higher when pedestrians were encountered on on an unmarked road, which involved strategy and behaviour choices.
the left side (M = 9.18) than when they were encountered on the right The results showed that anger made drivers adopt risky avoidance
side (M = 6.82) (p = 0.002). The interaction effect was not significant strategies more often than neutral and happy states did, as indicated by a
(F(2,76) = 2.78, η2 = 0.068, p = 0.068). The results are shown in Fig. 5. higher percentage of drivers passing in front of pedestrians, a larger

Table 2
Means (SD) of self-reported scores of emotion scales in the anger, happiness and neutral conditions.
Anger condition Happiness condition Neutral condition

After induction After driving After induction After driving After induction After driving

Anger scores 5.49 (3.06) 2.18 (1.88) 1.00 (0.00) 1.44 (1.31) 1.00(0.00) 1.15(0.49)
Happiness scores 1.21(0.73) 1.82 (1.60) 4.38 (2.27) 2.51(1.86) 1.74(1.82) 2.15(2.03)
Arousal scores 5.00 (2.73) 2.92 (2.06) 3.18 (1.96) 2.41(1.93) 1.51(1.07) 1.97(1.53)
Valence scores 2.36 (1.68) 3.87 (1.72) 6.10 (1.25) 4.79 (0.89) 5.05(0.76) 4.87(1.15)

4
Q. Zhang et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 171 (2022) 106664

Fig. 3. Vehicle track for 120 feet before and after the position of target pedestrians. The top shows left-side events, and the bottom shows right-side events.

Table 3
Descriptive statistics of three avoidance patterns in the neutral, happiness and anger conditions.
Neutral condition Happiness condition Anger condition

Left Right Left Right Left Right

Pass front 59 (37.8%) 22 (14.1%) 41 (26.3%) 25 (16.0%) 94(60.3%) 39(25%)


Pass behind 73 (46.8%) 68 (43.6%) 61 (39.1%) 48 (30.8%) 11(7.1%) 30(19.2%)
Stop 24 (15.4%) 66 (42.3%) 54 (34.6%) 83 (19.2%) 51(32.7%) 87(55.8%)

average passing speed and closer distance to pedestrians. last.


The method of inducing anger and happiness by watching film Consistent with expectations, anger increases drivers’ average speed,
fragments is feasible under the current conditions, as indicated by sig­ their minimum speed when encountering a pedestrian, the probability
nificant changes in self-reported emotion scores, but the effect of that they will pass in front of pedestrians, and the lateral distance to a
maintaining emotions needs to be improved. Previous studies have pedestrian from the right. On the one hand, anger increased the average
shown that emotions affect driving behaviour directly as well as indi­ driving speed, which is consistent with the findings of previous studies.
rectly by affecting visual attention (Steinhauser et al., 2018). For Driving simulator studies show that anger promotes risky driving
example, emotional sounds, pictures and words can lead to more dis­ behaviour, which is frequently reflected by increased driving speed
tractions, thus affecting driving performance (Chan and Singhal, 2013; (Abdu et al., 2012; Jeon et al., 2014a; Mesken et al., 2007; Rhodes et al.,
Di Stasi et al., 2010; Pêcher et al., 2009; Walker and Trick, 2019). 2015). On the other hand, the minimum speed when encountering a
Therefore, if driving tasks are accompanied by emotional stimuli, pedestrian under the angry condition was greater than that under the
drivers’ performance will be affected by both emotion and distraction. neutral condition. There are two possible explanations. One explanation
This study attempted to avoid confusion caused by emotion and to avoid is that the data in this study show a higher proportion of passing in front
distraction by inducing emotions in the participants before they of pedestrians rather than stopping to give way to pedestrians under the
completed driving tasks. This approach works better in conditions of anger condition, which involves more driving acceleration to maintain
anger than in conditions of happiness. The results show that drivers priority. The other possible reason is that drivers’ brake response was
became angrier across the happiness and neutral conditions. This could impaired in the anger condition. The effect of anger on delaying emer­
be due to compounding frustration about the task, which may have gency responses has been supported by another study on pedestrian-
diluted the state of the happy condition, especially when this was done crossing tasks (Chai and Zhao, 2016). More importantly, this study

5
Q. Zhang et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 171 (2022) 106664

current findings do not support this hypothesis, possibly because the


arousal of happy emotions was not high enough. According to the
inverted U-shaped curve relationship between task performance and
arousal, performance is predicted to be inhibited by arousal levels that
are too high or too low (Teigen, 1994; Yerkes, 2004). In the current
study, self-reported emotional arousal after anger induction was
significantly higher than that after happiness induction, although
arousal was higher in both conditions than in the neutral condition. That
is, happiness did not have the same negative impact on driving behav­
iour as anger because its arousal was not high enough. Second, happi­
ness did not lead to more risky pedestrian avoidance patterns than
anger, which supports the hypothesis. The avoidance pattern is a strat­
egy of driving that depends on the driver’s prosocial attitudes and cur­
rent situation, such as time pressure. Previous studies have indicated
that persons in negative emotional states are more likely to take risks in
gambling, strategic decisions and lottery tasks (Arkes et al., 1988; Isen
and Patrick, 1983; Kuvaas and Kaufmann, 2010; Mittal and Ross, 1998),
are less likely to compromise in consumer decision making (Chuang and
Lin, 2007), and are less likely to have difficulties with impulse control in
Fig. 4. Percentage of passing in front of the target pedestrian in the angry, compulsive eating and procrastination (Tice et al., 2001) than those in
happy and neutral conditions. Error bars indicate the 95% confidence intervals. positive emotional states. Driving with extra care around pedestrians
and driving more cautiously to accommodate people or vehicles on the
found that anger affects drivers’ strategy and approach when they side of the road (e.g., slowing down, moving over) are considered pro­
attempt to avoid pedestrians; drivers showed a higher frequency of social driving behaviours (Harris et al., 2014). Although there is
passing in front of pedestrians and a closer lateral distance to pedes­ currently no direct evidence that emotions affect prosocial driving
trians. However, it is worth noting that the effect of anger on the lateral behaviour, a large body of research in the field of social psychology
distance to pedestrians and vehicles occurred only when the pedestrians suggests that arousing positive emotions can promote altruistic behav­
appeared on the right side. It seems that pedestrians passing from the left iour (Cialdini et al., 1982). That is, driving performance related to
side pose less threat to drivers than those passing from the right side, and prosocial attitudes is primarily affected by emotional valence rather
drivers do not need to stop or change lanes to avoid hitting these pe­ than arousal. It seems that the impact of emotional valence on driving
destrians. Future research should pay more attention to the influence of tasks reflects the impact on the cognitive processes involved.
pedestrian-vehicle distance and the pedestrian-driver position relation­ The present study has some limitations that should be addressed in
ship (i.e., near-driver side/off-driver side) on drivers’ hazard perception. future studies. First, the materials used for emotion induction were film
We compared the effects of anger and happiness and found that the clips unrelated to driving, so the induced emotions faded as the drivers
impact of the two emotions on driving performance in avoiding pedes­ transferred their attention to the driving tasks. Although it has been
trians differed, which may highlight the effect of emotional valence found that positive emotions induced by driving-related and driving-
rather than arousal. First, based on previous studies showing that unrelated video clips are both related to higher self-reported willing­
arousing affect, including positive and negative affect, led to increased ness to drive recklessly (Taubman-Ben-Ari, 2012), using driving-related
risky driving (Eherenfreund-Hager et al., 2017), we expected both materials to induce emotions can better promote this state, making it
happiness and anger to lead to greater average driving velocity. The more likely to continue during the driving task. In addition, emotions

Fig. 5. Means of minimum driving speed in the angry, happy and neutral conditions. Error bars indicate the 95% confidence intervals.

6
Q. Zhang et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 171 (2022) 106664

Fig. 6. Mean lateral distance to target pedestrians in the angry, happy and neutral conditions. Error bars indicate the 95% confidence intervals.

can be measured using more objective metrics rather than self-reported 7. Authors’ statement
methods, which are susceptible to experimental manipulation. Second,
we manipulated three types of emotions in a within-participant design, Qian Zhang: Conceived and designed the experiments, Data-
which avoided the effects of individual differences in material under­ Analysis, Manuscript Writing.
standing and emotional experience. However, this approach might have Weina Qu: Conceived and designed the experiments, Supported
led to carry-over effects due to the relatively short break between the funding.
induction of different emotions. Third, the sample in this study was Yan Ge: Conceived and designed the experiments , Manuscript
small and consisted of males with rich driving experience, which limits Writing, Supported funding.
the generalization of the research conclusions.

5. Conclusion Declaration of Competing Interest

The present study investigated the effect of anger and happiness on The authors declare that they have no known competing financial
driving performance in a pedestrian-crossing task by comparing the two interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence
emotions, which were found to have different impacts. The study suc­ the work reported in this paper.
cessfully demonstrated that anger not only leads to risky driving
behaviour (e.g., speeding) but also encourages drivers to be more Acknowledgements
impulsive in their avoidance patterns and strategy choices in pedestrian-
crossing tasks. However, there was no difference between the impact of This study was partially supported by the National Key R&D Program
happy and neutral moods on driving behaviour. High arousal is thought of China (Grant No. 2021YFC3001500, the National Natural Science
to be associated with risky driving in general (e.g., speeding), while Foundation of China (Grant Nos. 32071066, 32071064), the National
avoidance behaviour patterns in pedestrian-crossing tasks, as a driving Key R&D Program of China (Grant No. 2021YFC3001500) and the Key
behaviour related to prosocial attitudes, are presumed to be related to Projects of the PLA’s 13th Five-Year Logistic Research Plan
emotional valence. (BKJ19J012).

6. Research implications References

Driving anger poses many threats to road safety, and researchers Abdu, R., Shinar, D., Meiran, N., 2012. Situational (state) anger and driving.
have tried to intervene in a number of ways, such as through driving Transportation research part F: traffic psychology and behaviour, 15(5), 575-580.
doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2012.05.007.
safety education and intervention for high-risk drivers. The results of the Adams, R.B., Ambady, N., Macrae, C.N., Kleck, R.E., 2006. Emotional expressions
current study suggest that interventions for anger during driving can forecast approach-avoidance behavior. Motivation & Emotion 30 (2), 177–186.
reduce arousal, but they should also consider valence. In addition, more Arkes, H.R., Herren, L.T., Isen, A.M., 1988. The role of potential loss in the influence of
affect on risk-taking behavior ☆. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 42 (2),
targeted emotional interventions are needed for driving tasks involving 181–193.
different cognitive processes. In vehicle-human interaction traffic sce­ Barnard, M.P., Chapman, P., 2016. Are anxiety and fear separable emotions in driving? A
narios, the choice of driving behaviour is often related to prosocial at­ laboratory study of behavioural and physiological responses to different driving
environments. Accid. Anal. Prev. 86, 99–107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
titudes. Anger interventions that focus on shifting valence may be more aap.2015.10.021.
effective in improving driving performance. Bella, F., Silvestri, M., 2015. Effects of safety measures on driver’s speed behavior at
pedestrian crossings. Accid. Anal. Prev. 83, 111–124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
aap.2015.07.016.
Bogdan, S.R., Măirean, C., Havârneanu, C.-E., 2016. A meta-analysis of the association
between anger and aggressive driving. Transp. Res. Part F: Traffic Psycholo. Behav.
42, 350–364. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2016.05.009.

7
Q. Zhang et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 171 (2022) 106664

Bradley, M.M., Lang, P.J., 1994. Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the Nesbit, S.M., Conger, J.C., Conger, A.J., 2007. A quantitative review of the relationship
semantic differential. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 25 (1), 49–59. between anger and aggressive driving. Aggression Violent Behav. 12 (2), 156–176.
Carver, C., 2003. Pleasure as a sign you can attend to something else: placing positive Parker, D., West, R., Stradling, S., Manstead, A.S.R., 1995. Behavioural characteristics
feelings within a general model of affect. Cogn. Emot. 17 (2), 241–261. and involvement in different types of traffic accident. Accid. Anal. Prev. 27 (4),
Chai, J., Zhao, G., 2016. Effect of exposure to aggressive stimuli on aggressive driving 571–581. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-4575(95)00005-K.
behavior at pedestrian crossings at unmarked roadways. Accid. Anal. Prev. 88, Pêcher, C., Lemercier, C., Cellier, J.-M., 2009. Emotions drive attention: effects on
159–168. driver’s behaviour. Saf. Sci. 47 (9), 1254–1259. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Chan, M., Singhal, A., 2013. The emotional side of cognitive distraction: implications for ssci.2009.03.011.
road safety. Accid. Anal. Prev. 50, 147–154. Qu, W., Ge, Y., Jiang, C., Du, F., Zhang, K., 2014. The Dula Dangerous Driving Index in
Chuang, S.-C., Lin, H.-M., 2007. The effect of induced positive and negative emotion and China: an investigation of reliability and validity. Accid. Anal. Prev. 64, 62–68.
openness-to-feeling in student’s consumer decision making. J. Business Psychol. 22 Ragland, D. R., Mitman, M. F., 2007. Driver/pedestrian understanding and behavior at
(1), 65–78. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-007-9049-6. marked and unmarked crosswalks.
Cialdini, R.B., Kenrick, D.T., Baumann, D.J., 1982. Effects of mood on prosocial behavior Rhodes, N., Pivik, K., Sutton, M., 2015. Risky driving among young male drivers: The
in children and adults. Development of Prosocial Behavior 13. effects of mood and passengers. Transp. Res. Part F: Traffic Psychol. Behav. 28,
Dahlen, E.R., White, R.P., 2006. The Big Five factors, sensation seeking, and driving 65–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2014.11.005.
anger in the prediction of unsafe driving. Personality Individ. Differ. 41 (5), Roidl, E., Siebert, F.W., Oehl, M., Höger, R., 2013. Introducing a multivariate model for
903–915. predicting driving performance: the role of driving anger and personal
Deffenbacher, J.L., Huff, M.E., Lynch, R.S., Oetting, E.R., Salvatore, N.F., 2000. characteristics. J. Saf. Res. 47, 47–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsr.2013.08.002.
Characteristics and treatment of high-anger drivers. J. Counseling Psychol. 47 (1), Roidl, E., Frehse, B., Höger, R., 2014. Emotional states of drivers and the impact on
5–17. speed, acceleration and traffic violations—a simulator study. Accid. Anal. Prev. 70,
Deffenbacher, J.L., Lynch, R.S., Oetting, E.R., Yingling, D.A., 2001. Driving anger: 282–292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2014.04.010.
correlates and a test of state-trait theory. Personality Individ. Differ. 31 (8), Schmidt-Daffy, M., 2013. Fear and anxiety while driving: Differential impact of task
1321–1331. demands, speed and motivation. Transp. Res. Part F: Traffic Psychol. Behav. 16,
Deffenbacher, J.L., Deffenbacher, D.M., Lynch, R.S., Richards, T.L., 2003. Anger, 14–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2012.07.002.
aggression, and risky behavior: a comparison of high and low anger drivers. Behav. Steinhauser, K., Leist, F., Maier, K., Michel, V., Pärsch, N., Rigley, P., Wurm, F.,
Res. Ther. 41 (6), 701–718. Steinhauser, M., 2018. Effects of emotions on driving behavior. Transp. Res. Part F:
Di Stasi, L.L., Contreras, D., Cañas, J.J., Cándido, A., Maldonado, A., Catena, A., 2010. Traffic Psychol. Behav. 59, 150–163.
The consequences of unexpected emotional sounds on driving behaviour in risky Stephens, A.N., Groeger, J.A., 2011. Anger-congruent behaviour transfers across driving
situations. Saf. Sci. 48 (10), 1463–1468. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. situations. Cogn. Emot. 25 (8), 1423–1438.
ssci.2010.07.006. Stephens, A.N., Trawley, S.L., Madigan, R., Groeger, J.A., 2013. Drivers display anger-
Dula, C.S., Ballard, M.E., 2003. Development and evaluation of a measure of dangerous, congruent attention to potential traffic hazards. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 27 (2),
aggressive, negative emotional, and risky driving1. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 33 (2), 178–189.
263–282. Sullman, M.J.M., 2006. Anger amongst New Zealand drivers. Transp. Res. Part F: Traffic
Easterbrook, J.A., 1959. The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of Psychol. Behav. 9 (3), 173–184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2005.10.003.
behavior (Vol. 66). Sullman, M.J.M., Stephens, A.N., 2013. A comparison of the driving anger scale and the
Eherenfreund-Hager, A., Taubman – Ben-Ari, O., Toledo, T., Farah, H., 2017. The effect propensity for angry driving scale. Accid. Anal. Prev. 58, 88–96. https://doi.org/
of positive and negative emotions on young drivers: a simulator study. Transp. Res. 10.1016/j.aap.2013.05.002.
Part F: Traffic Psychol. Behav. 49, 236–243. Sullman, M.J.M., Gras, M.E., Cunill, M., Planes, M., Font-Mayolas, S., 2007. Driving
Fredrickson, B.L., 2004. The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Am. anger in Spain. Personality Individ. Differ. 42 (4), 701–713. https://doi.org/
Psychol. 359 (1449), 1367–1377. 10.1016/j.paid.2006.08.014.
Ge, Y., Zhao, G., Zhang, Y., Houston, R.J., Song, J., 2019. A standardised database of Taubman-Ben-Ari, O., 2012. The effects of positive emotion priming on self-reported
Chinese emotional film clips. Cogn. Emot. 33 (5), 976–990. reckless driving. Accid. Anal. Prev. 45, 718–725.
Hancock, G.M., Hancock, P.A., Janelle, C.M., 2012. The impact of emotions and Techer, F., Jallais, C., Corson, Y., Moreau, F., Ndiaye, D., Piechnick, B., Fort, A., 2017.
predominant emotion regulation technique on driving performance. Work 41 Suppl Attention and driving performance modulations due to anger state: Contribution of
1(6), 5882. electroencephalographic data. Neurosci. Lett. 636, 134–139. https://doi.org/
Harmon-Jones, E., 2003. Anger and the behavioral approach system. Personality Individ. 10.1016/j.neulet.2016.11.011.
Differ. 35 (5), 995–1005. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00313-6. Teigen, K.H., 1994. Yerkes-Dodson: a law for all seasons. Theory & Psychology 4 (4),
Harris, P.B., Houston, J.M., Vazquez, J.A., Smither, J.A., Harms, A., Dahlke, J.A., 525–547. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354394044004.
Sachau, D.A., 2014. The Prosocial and Aggressive Driving Inventory (PADI): a self- Tice, D.M., Bratslavsky, E., Baumeister, R.F., 2001. Emotional distress regulation takes
report measure of safe and unsafe driving behaviors. Accid. Anal. Prev. 72, 1–8. precedence over impulse control: if you feel bad, do it! J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 80 (1),
Hu, T.-Y., Xie, X., Li, J., 2013. Negative or positive? The effect of emotion and mood on 53–67.
risky driving. Transp. Res. Part F: Traffic Psychol. Behav. 16, 29–40. Trick, L.M., Brandigampola, S., Enns, J.T., 2012. How fleeting emotions affect hazard
Isen, A.M., 2001. An influence of positive affect on decision making in complex perception and steering while driving: The impact of image arousal and valence.
situations: theoretical issues with practical implications. J. Consumer Psychol. 11 Accid. Anal. Prev. 45, 222–229.
(2), 75–85. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327663JCP1102_01. Ünal, A.B., Waard, D.D., Kai, E., Steg, L., 2013. Driving with music: effects on arousal and
Isen, A.M., Patrick, R., 1983. The effect of positive feelings on risk taking: when the chips performance. Transp. Res. Part F: Psychol. Behav. 21, 52–65.
are down. Organizational Behavior & Human Performance 31 (2), 194–202. Underwood, G., Chapman, P., Wright, S., Crundall, D., 1999. Anger while driving.
Janelle, C.M., Singer, R.N., Williams, A.M., 1999. External distraction and attentional Transp. Res. Part F: Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2 (1), 55–68.
narrowing: visual search evidence. J. Sport Exercise Psychol. 21 (1), 70–91. Várhelyi, A., 1998. Drivers’ speed behaviour at a zebra crossing: a case study. Accid.
Jeon, M., Walker, B.N., Gable, T.M., 2014a. Anger effects on driver situation awareness Anal. Prev. 30 (6), 731–743. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0001-4575(98)00026-8.
and driving performance. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments 23 (1), Walker, H.E.K., Trick, L.M., 2019. How the emotional content of roadside images affect
71–89. driver attention and performance. Saf. Sci. 115, 121–130. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Jeon, M., Walker, B.N., Yim, J.-B., 2014b. Effects of specific emotions on subjective j.ssci.2019.02.004.
judgment, driving performance, and perceived workload. Transp. Res. Part F: Traffic Yang, J., Deng, W., Wang, J., Li, Q., Wang, Z., 2006. Modeling pedestrians’ road crossing
Psychol. Behav. 24, 197–209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2014.04.003. behavior in traffic system micro-simulation in China. Transp. Res. Part A: Policy
Jones, M., Chapman, P., Bailey, K., 2014. The influence of image valence on visual Practice 40 (3), 280–290.
attention and perception of risk in drivers. Accid. Anal. Prev. 73, 296–304. M. Yerkes, R., & D. Dodson, J. (2004). The Relation of Strength of Stimulus to Rapidity of
Katz, A., Zaidel, D., Elgrishi, A., 1975. An experimental study of driver and pedestrian Habit Formation (Vol. 18).
interaction during the crossing conflict. Hum. Factors 17 (5), 514–527. https://doi. Zhang, T., Chan, A.H.S., Ba, Y., Zhang, W., 2016. Situational driving anger, driving
org/10.1177/001872087501700510. performance and allocation of visual attention. Transp. Res. Part F: Traffic Psychol.
Kuvaas, B., Kaufmann, G., 2010. Impact of mood, framing, and need for cognition on Behav. 42, 376–388.
decision makers’ recall and confidence. J. Behav. Decision Making 17 (1), 59–74. Zhang, Q., Ge, Y., Qu, W., Hennessy, D.A., Zhang, K., 2019. Effects of anger and collision
Lu, J., Xie, X., Zhang, R., 2013. Focusing on appraisals: How and why anger and fear history on driver space preference. Transp. Res. Part F: Traffic Psychol. Behav. 63,
influence driving risk perception. J. Saf. Res. 45, 65–73. 108–117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2019.04.002.
Mesken, J., Hagenzieker, M.P., Rothengatter, T., de Waard, D., 2007. Frequency, Zhang, Q., Qu, W., Ge, Y., Sun, X., Zhang, K., 2020. The effect of the emotional state on
determinants, and consequences of different drivers’ emotions: An on-the-road study driving performance in a simulated car-following task. Transp. Res. Part F: Traffic
using self-reports,(observed) behaviour, and physiology. Transp. Res. Part F: Traffic Psychol. Behav. 69, 349–361. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2020.02.004.
Psychol. Behav. 10 (6), 458–475. Zhao, G., Wu, C., 2012. The effects of driver identity on driving safety in a retrospective
Mittal, V., Ross, W.T., 1998. The impact of positive and negative affect and issue framing feedback system. Accid. Anal. Prev. 45, 354–365. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
on issue interpretation and risk taking ☆ ☆☆. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 76 aap.2011.08.002.
(3), 298–324.
Nass, C., Jonsson, I.M., Harris, H., Reaves, B., Endo, J., Brave, S., Takayama, L., 2005.
Improving automotive safety by pairing driver emotion and car voice emotion. Paper
presented at the Chi 05 Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing
Systems.

8
Q. Zhang et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 171 (2022) 106664

Zhou, R., Horrey, W.J., Yu, R., 2009. The effect of conformity tendency on pedestrians’ Weina Qu received the Ph.D. degree from Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of
road-crossing intentions in China: an application of the theory of planned behavior. Sciences in 2011. She is an Associate Professor at Institute of Psychology, Chinese Acad­
Accid. Anal. Prev. 41 (3), 491–497. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2009.01.007. emy of Sciences. Her main research interest is driving safety, and human-computer
Zhuang, X., Wu, C., 2011. Pedestrians’ crossing behaviors and safety at unmarked interaction.
roadway in China. Accid. Anal. Prev. 43 (6), 1927–1936. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
aap.2011.05.005.
Yan Ge received the Ph.D. degree from Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of
Zimasa, T., Jamson, S., Henson, B., 2017. Are happy drivers safer drivers? Evidence from
Sciences in 2008. She is an Associate Professor at Institute of Psychology, Chinese Acad­
hazard response times and eye tracking data. Transp. Res. Part F: Traffic Psychol.
emy of Sciences. Her main research interest is emotion, driving safety, and human-
Behav. 46, 14–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2016.12.005.
computer interaction.

Qian Zhang received the Ph.D. degree from Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of
Sciences in 2019. She is an Assistant Professor at School of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal
University, Xi’an, China. Her main research interest is emotion and driving safety.

You might also like