You are on page 1of 8

Morphophonology

Hans Basbøll, University of Southern Denmark, Odense M, Denmark


Ó 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Abstract

Morphophonology is the interaction between morphology and phonology, and these two linguistic disciplines are defined in
Section Linguistic Disciplines and the Sign within a structuralist tradition of linguistics. Section Interaction between
Phonology and Morphology: Three Examples presents three case studies of morphophonological interaction, departing from
phonotactics (regular inflectional morphology of English), prosody (the Danish laryngeal prosody stød in its relation to word
structure), and phonological variation (German and Danish stem change in plural formation). In section Morphophonemic
Analysis, finally, the stages in a morphophonemic analysis are outlined, predominantly as part of a discovery procedure and
within a structuralist tradition.

Introduction: Terminology acoustically), the expression is an acoustical image (French image


acoustique).
The term morphophonology is here taken in a broad sense, in The relation between content and expression in a linguistic
agreement with its composite parts morpho- and -phonology, as sign is, according to Saussure, arbitrary (French arbitraire:
covering the interaction between the linguistic components, or ‘le signe linguistique est arbitraire’); i.e., there is no natural
modules, morphology (cf Spencer and Zwicky, 1998) and connection between the two: the relation is due to a social
phonology (cf Goldsmith, 1995). Accordingly, it is not to be convention (normally not an explicit and conscious one,
understood as a separate linguistic component (such as however). A sign like ‘horse’ cannot be decomposed into smaller
morphology, phonology or syntax, for example). There are signs: it is one linguistic building block. Its content may be
two further terms which are sometimes used in the same or decomposed into smaller semantic elements (such as ‘animal,’
in a closely related sense, viz. morphonology and morphophone- ‘four-legged,’ and so on); and its expression can be decomposed
mics. Morphonology (in German Morphonologie, French morphono- into smaller phonological elements (like initial /h/ and final /s/,
logie) was the term which the leading founding father of Prague etc.). But there is no general correspondence between the
phonology used (Trubetzkoy, 1929, 1931). More recently, it content elements and the expression elements (as there would
has been the title of Dressler’s (1985) book Morphonology: the be in the counterfactual state of affairs where /h/ would corre-
dynamics of derivation. Morphophonemics has been a favorite spond to ‘animal,’ and so on). Such minimal signs (as ‘horse’ in
term in American structuralism since Menomini morphophone- our example) are called morphemes. The arbitrarity of the sign
mics by its leading founding father (Bloomfield, 1939). The is a characteristic of morphemes only, not of linguistic signs
term morphophonemics suggests more emphasis on the in general. When smaller signs are combined into larger and
segmental building blocks in linguistic structure than the other more complex signs, the relation between content and expres-
terms which are neutral with respect to the distinction between sion is no longer arbitrary, but motivated, viz. by the individual
segments and prosody. signs and by the construction(s) they enter into; e.g., when black
and bird are compounded into blackbird, the meaning (of
a particular ornithological species) is motivated, but not deter-
Linguistic Disciplines and the Sign mined, by the meaning of black and bird.
The basic linguistic disciplines – ‘language-internally,’
The Linguistic Sign and Basic Linguistic Disciplines
excluding ‘hyphen-disciplines’ like socio(-)linguistics, psycho(-
The sign (French signe) is a key notion in structuralist linguistics, )linguistics and text(-)linguistics, for example – can be defined
in the tradition following the Swiss linguist Saussure (1916). departing from the linguistic sign: Semantics is concerned with
The sign is a unit – psychic and social – with two sides, viz. the content-side of language; and phonology with the expression-
content (French signifié ‘what is being signified’), or meaning, side of language (cf Section Phonology and Its Subparts). Sign
and expression (French signifiant ‘what is signifying’), or sound. combinatorics represents the traditional linguistic discipline
(Content and expression is the terminology of Hjelmslev grammar, divided into syntax and morphology (cf Section
(1943), and there is a comprehensive and complicated history Morphology and the Word). When we depart from semantics
of the linguistic sign, with linguistically central names like and transcend the border between language-internal and
Peirce (e.g., 1965), and up to today’s cognitive linguistics, language-external phenomena, we meet pragmatics. And when
e.g., in works by Langacker (1987).) One of Saussure’s exam- we transcend the border between language-internal and
ples is the sign ‘horse’ (French ‘cheval’). Its content is, according language-external phenomena from phonology, we meet
to Saussure, not the animal in blood and flesh – the animal is phonetics (cf Section Phonology and Its Subparts). Thus both
just being referred to – but the concept of a horse; nor is the pragmatics and phonetics contain language-internal as well as
expression the concrete sounds (neither articulatorily nor language-external aspects. Lexicon, finally, as a linguistic

826 International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edition, Volume 15 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.53036-0
Morphophonology 827

discipline is concerned with the arbitrary aspects of language, subdiscipline plays a pertinent role in morphophonology
viz. of the relation between content and expression of (see Dressler and Dziubalska-Ko1aczyk, 2006 on ‘morphono-
morphemes (and partly words). tactics’), cf Section Phonotactics and Morphology: English
Regular Inflections.
Phonetics – the discipline concerned with language sounds (cf
Morphology and the Word
Section The Linguistic Sign and Basic Linguistic Disciplines) –
Grammar can thus, from a strictly structuralist point of view, is a crucial discipline for phonology, although it is not –
be characterized as the linguistic discipline which treats sign within the structuralist linguistic tradition – considered
combinatorics, i.e., it is concerned with the syntagmatic a subdiscipline of phonology; but phonetics is a necessary
(concerned with the linear order of elements in the speech condition for phonological analyses and descriptions that aim
chain) – as opposed to paradigmatic (concerned with choices to be psychologically and physically realistic as well as testable,
on a particular position in the speech chain) – axis; grammar cf Section Nonchange of Phonetic Content between Levels:
is subdivided into two (sub)disciplines, viz. syntax and A Principle of Naturalness.
morphology. The notion word is crucial to define the difference
between those two: in syntax, the building blocks are words
Nonchange of Phonetic Content between Levels: A Principle
and the frame is (primarily) the sentence; and in morphology,
of Naturalness
the building blocks are morphemes and the frame the
word. Therefore, the definition of a word is essential for Lemmas are lexical units. They can be divided into those
morphophonology – the interaction between phonology which do not have several different expressions phonologi-
and morphology. cally, but only one (‘nonalternating lemmas’) and ‘alter-
The term word designates a central linguistic unit, and this nating lemmas,’ with more than one phonological
term is, in distinction to most other units used in linguistic expression (cf Section Phonological Variation and
descriptions, by linguists, also a word used frequently by Morphology: Stem Change in German and Danish Plurals).
linguistically naïve speakers in general. This is related to the For nonalternating lemmas, the phonological representation
fact that the word is a unit with some psychological reality in the lexicon is, I shall propose, the (phonetic) surface form,
(and also to the fact that words are separated by space in disregarding only rather superficial phonetic details. This
orthography). The most appropriate linguistic definition of means, for example, that lemmas which do not enter into
a word is by means of distributional characteristics: a word can a particular morphological alternation – for example
occur in isolation, it can be moved around in an utterance, involving the Danish syllabic prosody stød, cf Section
and its morphemes occur in a fixed internal order, all these Prosody and Morphology: Danish Stød in Word Structure –
criteria being interrelated and connected to its psychological are lexically specified accordingly (as having stød or not
reality. On the other hand, the word cannot be defined exclu- having stød). This is the null-hypothesis within the present
sively by semantic criteria – as suggested by the existence of lex- framework. Thus, a lexical specification of a certain phono-
icalized phrases, idioms (e.g., kick the bucket), complex lexical logical property implies constancy of this property, irrespec-
items, etc. – nor can it be defined cross-linguistically by phono- tive of the morphological context, except for surface phonetic
logical criteria. It is language-dependent to which degree word adjustments. A number of predictions with respect to alterna-
boundaries have a phonetic manifestation of any sort, i.e., tions, in different paradigms and types of words, follow from
languages differ typologically in this respect. this approach to the lexicon (see Basbøll, 2005, 2006). The
fact that the symbols in the lexical representation are iden-
tical to symbols in the phonetic representation is an expres-
Phonology and Its Subparts
sion of the naturalness principle and holds not just for the
Phonology is the linguistic discipline concerned with the expres- relation between phonetics and phonology, but also in the
sion side of linguistic signs. (I disregard the analysis of written morphology and the lexicon.
language in this article, and thus the issue of graphemes, letters,
etc., cf Section Before Morphophonemic Analysis Can Start.)
Whereas morphemes are the building blocks of morphology, Interaction between Phonology and Morphology:
phonemes are the primary building blocks of phonology. Three Examples
Phonemes are established by means of the commutation test
Phonotactics and Morphology: English Regular Inflections
using minimal pairs, cf Section Input Level to Morphophonemic
Analysis: Phonemes. Phonemes are not linguistic signs, and The typical pattern for regular inflectional morphology in
they have no semantic content as opposed to morphemes, English can be illustrated by the plural formation of nouns
but they build up the expression of signs, together with (similar principles apply to 3d person pres. tense, and
prosodic elements. The basic subdivision of phonological units (with -ed) to past tense). The ending is either [z], [s] or ,
falls between segmental units, viz. phonemes – vowels and depending on the final segment of the stem, e.g., hens, dogs,
consonants – and suprasegmental units, i.e., prosodies. The latter cats, busses . Rather than listing all
category encompasses syllable prosodies and word accents possible final segments of stems, the phonetic/phonological
(such as word stress, tonal word accents and the Danish stød, category of this segment can be represented through charac-
cf Section Prosody and Morphology: Danish Stød in Word terizations like ‘voiced nonsibilant,’ ‘voiceless nonsibilant’
Structure) and intonation. A particular subpart of phonology and ‘sibilant’ – for the final segments combining with [z],
is phonotactics, i.e., combinatorics of phonemes, and this [s] and , respectively – typically using distinctive features
828 Morphophonology

(for voicedness, sibilance, etc., as suggested above, but their Change in German and Danish Plurals. A morphophonolog-
exact inventory is controversial). Furthermore, the rules for ically interesting subpattern of stem change þ suffix can be
the use of [z], [s] and can be simplified by being applied illustrated by lives, loaves, houses (sg. life, loaf, house – the latter
in a specified sequence. pronounced with [s] whereas the plural form has intervocalic
What are the phonemes that are realized as [z], [s] and [z]). The stem change only concerns the final obstruent, it is
(as morphological endings, e.g., for plural)? According to the a simple phonological change from voiceless (in the sg.
standard Bloomfieldian principle of ‘once a phoneme, always stem) to voiced (in the pl. stem), and the principles of regular
a phoneme’ (see Section Input Level to Morphophonemic plural formation reviewed above apply to the pl. stem: [z] is
Analysis: Phonemes), the phonemic analysis is straightfor- selected as the plural suffix.
ward: [z] is a realization of /z/, [s] of /s/ and of / / (or
/əz/, a difference which is insignificant here). According to a
Prosody and Morphology: Danish Stød in Word Structure
Praguian analysis, [z] and [s] would be analyzed differently:
plural [z] – in examples like hens, viz. where the stem ends The Danish stød is a laryngeal syllable rhyme prosody (some-
in a sonorant, e.g., a vowel or a nasal – would be a realization what resembling creaky voice). It only occurs in heavy
of the phoneme /z/ since in this position there is a contrast (bimoraic) syllables which can be defined as those having
between [s] and [z], cf buzz, bus . In examples like a long sonority rhyme – a long vowel, or a short vowel fol-
dogs, on the other hand (where the stem ends in a voiced lowed by a sonorant consonant (e.g., a nasal or ) – and
obstruent), plural [z] would not be a realization of the primary or secondary stress (in a 3 degrees of stress-system),
phoneme /z/, however, since there is no contrast to [s] in see Basbøll (2005: 82–87, 265ff). Stød is contrastive, and
this position (e.g., * is not a possible phonetic form in there are many minimal pairs (i.e., pairs of words whose
English). Similarly, plural [s] would not be a realization of pronunciation is identical except for the presence or absence
the phoneme /s/ since there is no contrast to [z] in this posi- of stød, cf Sections Phonology and Its Subparts and Input Level
tion (e.g., * is not a possible phonetic form in English). to Morphophonemic Analysis: Phonemes), e.g., musen ‘the
Plural [s], as well as plural [z] after obstruents, therefore real- muse,’ definite form of the indefinite muse
izes a neutralization product – an ‘archiphoneme’ – according versus musen ‘the mouse,’ definite of the indefinite mus
to Praguian principles, i.e., a phonological unit which repre- . The grammatical distribution of stød is
sents what is common to [s] and [z], without being phonolog- extremely complex, but it can be accounted for in Basbøll’s
ically specified for voicedness or voicelessness (even though Non-Stød Model. The relation between the syllable prosody
[z] is phonetically voiced and [s] phonetically voiceless), cf stød, and word structure in Danish, is revealing for the inter-
Section Input Level to Morphophonemic Analysis: Phonemes. action between phonology and morphology, viz. for
Since the English plural ending [s] only occurs where [z] is morphophonology.
phonotactically impossible, the following procedure (Basbøll, The Danish word has a three-layered structure as follows:
1972, 2006) determines the choice of plural endings without {[(stem P1)P2]P3} where P1, P2 and P3 are positions for
any reference to phonetic or phonological content (such as suffixes and where the position of the suffixes follows general
segments, distinctive features, etc.): principles of integration of suffixes into stems (in the word
structure), see Basbøll (2005: 351ff). The minimal word, in
1. Is [z] (phonotactically) OK? If yes: choose [z], if no: go to
‘(.),’ the basic word, in ‘[.],’ and the maximal word, in
(2)
‘{.},’ each define a phonological domain, i.e., a sequence
2. Is [s] (phonotactically) OK? If yes: choose [s], if no:
within which certain phonological rules apply. The unmarked
choose
situation in the native vocabulary is that heavy (bimoraic, see
Illustrated with hens, dogs, cats, busses as examples: above in Section Prosody and Morphology: Danish Stød in
Word Structure) syllables have stød. There is, however, a prin-
1. are (phonotactically) OK; but * ,*
ciple of non-stød which applies to penultimate syllables in
2. is (phonotactically) OK; but * not, so:
a minimal word, and to monosyllabic stems before a syllable
in the basic word. Examples are the pl. form huse ‘houses,’ sg.
Since we know that speaker-hearers, including small chil- hus where huse has non-stød just like the mono-
dren acquiring the language, are sensitive to phonotactical morphemic word muse above (penultimate syllable of the
patterns in the language around them – even though we do minimal word); the past form talte ‘spoke’ versus the com-
not know the details of this mechanism – this is an interesting pounded verb with the same monosyllabic second part udtalte
proposal not only because it is simple, but also from a psycho- ‘pronounced’ (basic words [ (tal) te ] and
linguistic perspective. But where does the order [z] before [s] [ (udtal) te ], respectively), i.e., a monosyllabic versus a polysyl-
before come from? An obvious answer is frequency – as labic stem before a syllable in the basic word. In all these
plural endings – in the language (and we know that frequency cases, the position of the suffix in word structure is predicted
plays a role in language acquisition). by the model, P1 being the position for lexicalized (inte-
In addition to this completely regular pattern, English grated) nonverbal suffixes (such as the plural suffix -e, the
exhibits pl. forms identical to the sg. (as sheep), and a limited non-lexicalized pl. suffix being -er), and P2 for lexicalized
number of forms with the suffix -en (e.g., oxen, sg. ox), forms verbal suffixes (such as the past tense suffix -te, the nonlexical-
with stem change (as men, sg. man), and forms with a combi- ized past tense suffix being -ede). The definite suffix -en (in
nation of stem change and suffix (as children, sg. child), cf musen ‘the mouse’ above) always occurs on P3, thus stød is
Section Phonological Variation and Morphology: Stem retained in the definite form of mus above since the following
Morphophonology 829

syllable occurs outside the basic word. Accordingly, the distri- by morphology. Umlaut – also ‘metaphony,’ a change in
bution of stød and non-stød (in heavy syllables), a prosodic stem vowel as in, e.g., English mouse, mice – in both
phonological phenomenon, cannot be predicted by the German and Danish is a case in point. From a synchronic
phonetic or phonological context alone, but crucially depends point of view, the relations between the singular and the
on grammatical structure, wherefore its description belongs in plural stem in words with Umlaut are phonologically
the realm of morphophonology. unmotivated (in German ja u o awj in sg. (e.g., Fass, Fluss,
Knopf, Haus ) correspond to j j
in pl. (Fässer, Flüsse, Knöpfe, Häuser
Phonological Variation and Morphology: Stem Change in ), and in Danish j j in sg.
German and Danish Plurals (e.g., and, barn, fod, hånd ) correspond
to j j in pl. (ænder, børn, fødder, hænder
In both German and Danish, noun plurals are formed with
). There is nothing in the
either a suffix alone (incl. zero), or stem change alone, or
phonological context – as against the morphological one –
a combination (incl. pl. ¼ sg.), cf Section Phonotactics and
in today’s languages that dictate the strong stem change.
Morphology: English Regular Inflections. The following
Furthermore, these stem changes apply to a limited (fewer
three-way gradation of stem change, in the dimension of
in Danish than in German) subset only of morphemes, but
transparency versus opacity, based upon general phonological
very far from all, and there are obvious ambiguities in
principles, has been proposed as a hypothesis applicable to
either direction (e.g., in Danish, sg. jaj can correspond to
languages exhibiting stem change as a morphophonological
pl. jεj or jøj, and pl. jøj to sg. jaj or joj). All this shows that
mechanism (Basbøll et al., 2011; Laaha et al., 2011):
we are dealing, for strong stem change, with morpho-
1. ‘No change’: no phonological change of the plural stem phonology, not phonology (cf Section From Phonemes to
when compared to the singular stem (e.g., bil, biler ‘car,’ Morphophonemes: Introducing Morphological
‘cars’ ). The category ‘no change’ does not Relatedness).
preclude purely phonetic changes, however, cf Section
Since stem change produces opacity which supposedly makes
Before Morphophonemic Analysis Can Start.
acquisition more difficult, no change, weak change and strong
2. ‘Weak change’: a phonological change of the plural stem
change, defined cross-linguistically as above, are predicted to
which is either prosodic or due to an automatic segmental
delay acquisition in this order. Furthermore, from these hypoth-
process. The category weak change, as far as segmental
eses it is predicted that strong change in German will be less diffi-
phonological changes are concerned (cf Section
cult for children than strong change in Danish since it is more
Phonology and Its Subparts), involves changes in the string
homogeneous and has a higher type frequency (there are far
of concrete phonemes – in a Bloomfieldian sense (cf Section
more lemmas in German than in Danish representing each
Input Level to Morphophonemic Analysis: Phonemes) –
strong change). These predictions have been successfully tested
but not in morphophonemes. For German this can be
in a large experiment with (Austrian) German and Danish chil-
illustrated with the pl. Bilder ‘pictures,’ pronounced with
dren by Laaha et al. (2011).
[d], sg. Bild pronounced with [t]; these two sounds are in
contrast initially (/t/ vs /d/), but finally there is no [d] due
to the automatic segmental process of devoicing, and thus
no final phoneme */d/ (cf Section Input Level to Morphophonemic Analysis
Morphophonemic Analysis: Phonemes); morphophono-
Before Morphophonemic Analysis Can Start
logically, on the other hand, there would be jdj in both sg.
and pl. Whereas an automatic segmental change does not The procedures surveyed below presuppose that the language
qualify as a phonological change, obviously, the reversal of is known to the phonologist undertaking the phonemic anal-
such an automatic segmental process does, viz. German ysis. To segment the sound chain into words and to identify
revoicing of the stem-final obstruent in the case of Bild- phonetic realizations as referring to the same word is no trivial
Bilder since it is not automatic, cf e.g., Welt ‘world,’ pl. task, due to inherent characteristics of the speech signal. The
Welten, both pronounced with [t]. acoustic signal (in contradiction to the written language
with spaces between words) consists of a continuous sound
Prosodic changes can be illustrated by Danish stød changes
string with unclear or nonexisting markings of boundaries
(stød drop and stød addition, cf Section Prosody and
between syllables and such higher level units as words.
Morphology: Danish Stød in Word Structure). The distinc-
Furthermore, the variability in the sound shape of individual
tion between prosodic and segmental phonological
words – both due to speaker characteristics and to the
changes is relevant, also psycholinguistically, as illustrated
communication situation – makes it difficult to identify
by the fact that Danes, when asked to reverse the syllables of
different acoustic realizations of the same unit as referring
the girl’s name Mona produce and not
to the same linguistic entity, e.g., a word. Speakers can draw
* , thereby suggesting that the prosodic frame
on prosodic cues such as stress, fundamental frequency (the
including stress and vowel length is stored separately
main phonetic correlate of tone) and sonority (German
from the string of segmental phonemes (Grønnum, 2007:
Schallfülle, e.g., so that vowels, nasals and obstruents in that
42–44).
order have decreasing inherent sonority) in order to segment
3. ‘Strong change’: a segmental change of the plural stem words in the sound chain, as well as on distributional –
which is phonologically nonautomatic and primarily motivated including phonotactical – patterns in the input.
830 Morphophonology

The linguist’s phonemic analysis is not undertaken directly from contrastive segments to phonemes is constituted by
on the basis of the sound chain itself – on concrete unana- the following steps, observing the biuniqueness condition,
lyzed utterances – but departs from a phonetic transcription, viz. the principle that it is possible to infer both the phonemes
i.e., a rendering of the continuous speech chain in terms of from the contrastive segments, and also the contrastive
a string of phonetic symbols (for vowels and consonants), segments from the phonemes (according to the phonological
with or without diacritics (small marks added to phonetic rules for the norm in question):
symbols) – e.g. for aspiration, lowering, syllabicity, etc. –
1. Phonetically closely related sounds which are not in
and with or without symbols for prosodic properties like
contrast – in the sense that they form minimal word pairs,
stress, word tones, etc. Such a notation is discrete in the math-
or potential minimal word pairs – are identified phone-
ematical sense, with a finite and well defined number of
mically. Typically, two such sound symbols will be in
symbols, e.g., taken from the International Phonetic Alphabet
complementary distribution – viz. each sound symbol
(IPA-symbols). But note that a huge amount of analysis,
only occurs in contexts of sounds where the other is
explicit and/or implicit, underlies any competent use of
excluded – but they might be free variants too (i.e., occur
such a phonetic transcription system.
in contexts which are wholly or partly identical, but with
There is a fundamental distinction between a transcription
no difference in meaning). The identification is based
of a concrete token (one individual instance, by a particular
upon phonetic similarity not shared by any other sound
individual on a particular occasion) of speech – whether
segment in the phonetic notation. With reference to a well
‘on-line,’ e.g., in field work, or from tape etc., these two cases
defined analysis of distinctive features, the sound symbols
being clearly different – and a transcription of a pronunciation
to be identified phonemically should possess a cluster of
type (a single abstract pronunciation, representing an infinite
distinctive features not found together in any other sound.
number of possible tokens). The latter task demands a system-
Examples of such identifications in Danish are [j] and to
atic transcription within explicit conventions and presupposes
/j/, and and to /r/.
knowledge of the particular speech form. Only phonetic
2. Further identifications of phonetically closely related
distinctions that either convey differences in meaning within
sounds which do not contrast in any specific context, can
the speech norm in question, or differences with sociolin-
be made, still respecting the principle of biuniqueness, i.e.,
guistic significance (e.g., sexual, sociological, regional or chro-
one and the same sound can be identified in two different
nological), are typically rendered in a systematic phonetic
ways phonemically, presupposing that the phonemic
transcription. As far as individual differences are concerned,
context is different. It is a condition for this kind of
level of distinctness (‘register’), is a relevant parameter. For
phonemic identification that the realization is motivated
the procedures of phonemic analysis surveyed in this article,
by a systematic phonological principle (such as, for
the phonetic transcriptions concern a high level of distinct-
Danish, vowel coloring by an adjacent /r/, pronounced as
ness within a well defined speech norm. Furthermore, the
or dependent on position, in the direction of this /r/
stream of speech contains segmental and prosodic informa-
). An example of such an identification in Danish is the
tion intertwined, and the extraction of prosody from the
assignment of [ε] after a belonging to the same syllable
segmental chain is anything but straightforward.
(‘tautosyllabic’) and of [e] not after a tautosyllabic to
the phoneme /e/ (e.g., bredt, lidt ‘broad (neuter), little
Input Level to Morphophonemic Analysis: Phonemes (adv.)’ ), and the assignment of [a] after a tau-
tosyllabic and of [ε] not after a tautosyllabic to the
In the phonemic analysis, in order to establish contrastive
phoneme /ε/ (e.g., ret, let ‘straight, easy’ ). On this
segments, the commutation test using minimal pairs is applied
procedure, including phonological fusion, i.e., bi(mor-
(cf Section Phonology and Its Subparts). Thereby both contras-
pho)phonemic interpretation of a single phonetic, or
tive segments and positions in the sound chain are established as
phonemic, segment, see Basbøll (2006).
phonemically relevant entities. For example, taking the Danish
word sat ‘sat (ptc.)’ [sad] as point of departure, the minimal The relation between contrastive segments and positions
pairs sat, hat ‘hat’ [had], nat ‘night’ [nad] establish [s], [h] and (in the sound chain) are looked upon in very different ways
[n] as three different contrastive segments before [ad]; the by different schools or traditions of phonology (cf Fischer-
minimal pairs sat, sut ‘comforter (for babies)’ [sud], sit Jørgensen, 1975). In my view there is still genuine insight
‘his/her/its (refl. poss.)’ [sid] establish [a], [u] and [i] as three to be gained from structuralist phonology, and from
different contrastive segments between [s] and [d]; and the combining an eclectic structuralist approach with more
minimal pairs sat, SAS ‘(the common Danish pronunciation modern ways of considering phonology-phonetics and
of the Scandinavian Airlines System)’ [sas], sand ‘sand’ phonology-morphology-lexicon interrelations.
establish [d], [s], and [n] (with stød) as three different contras- At one end of the scale we find the strict (post-) Bloomfiel-
tive segments after [sa]. By the very same procedure, three dians (Bloomfield, 1933; cf Bloch and Trager, 1942; Joos,
different phonologically relevant positions are being established, 1957) with the tenet ‘once a phoneme, always a phoneme,’
i.e., there are exactly three positions (‘places’) in the sound i.e., when a contrastive segment has been established in one
chain [sad] where the segments can be contrasted with other position, it is phonemic in all positions. When /p/ and /b/
segments, and this is established through the minimal pair or are established as contrasting allophones (word-)initially in
commutation test. German, for example (cf the minimal pair Pein ‘torment’
The analysis into phonemes is based upon a distinct pronounced with initial [p] and Bein ‘leg’ with initial [b]),
pronunciation of single words in the norm chosen. The path the sound [p] (word-)finally must be an allophone of /p/,
Morphophonology 831

even though the sound [b] does not occur (word-)finally In Danish, the position just before a stressed (nonnative)
wherefore there can be no contrast in this position either; suffix, like -i, -ik, -at, -ere is revelatory for the morphopho-
the phoneme /b/ is then said to be defectively distributed. A nemic identity of consonants undergoing consonant grada-
consequence of this analysis is that Lob ‘praise,’ Lobe (dat.) tion. Thereby the morphophonemic analysis of the syllable-
exhibits a morphological alternation between final phonemes /b/, /d/, / /, / / leads to the following alterna-
/p/ and /b/ (syllable-initially). For example, the stop in Rad tions and identifications 1 through 5. The generalizations only
‘wheel’ and Rat ‘council’ are pronounced identi- pertain to a segment in word-final position alternating with
cally word-finally (and hence syllable-finally), but enter a segment in morpheme-final position just before a stressed
different morphological alternations, cf Rade ‘wheel (dat.)’ (nonnative) suffix beginning with a vowel.
and Rate ‘council (dat.)’ . According to the
1. Syllable-final /d/ alternates with syllable-initial /t/, both
Bloomfield-school, this can be analyzed so that the phoneme
representing the morphophoneme jtj (e.g., in vat ‘cotton
/t/ word-finally can alternate with either of the phonemes /t/
wool’ [vad], vattere ‘quilt’ and demokrat ‘demo-
or /d/ syllable-initially. Both of the morphophonemes jtj and
crat’ , demokrati ‘democracy’
jdj are thus realized by the phoneme /t/ in word-final
);
position.
2. syllable-final / / alternates with syllable-initial /k/, both
In Prague phonology (cf Trubetzkoy, 1939), there would
representing the morphophoneme jkj (e.g., in lak ‘lacquer,
be neutralization word-finally in German, i.e., a particular
n.’ , lakere ‘lacquer, v.’ and patriark ‘patriarch’
phonemic entity, an archiphoneme, would occur in the posi-
, patriarkat ‘patriarchate’ );
tion of no contrast, viz. a segment which is phonetically voice-
3. syllable-final /b/ alternates with syllable-initial /p/ in some
less but has no specification for voicing phonologically and is
(nonnative) words, thus representing the morphopho-
thus neither a /p/ nor a /b/, but something which is common
neme jpj (e.g., in mikroskop ‘microscope’ ,
to the two (cf Section Phonotactics and Morphology: English
mikroskopi ‘microscopy’ );
Regular Inflections). Such an incompletely specified segment is
4. syllable-final /b/ alternates with syllable-initial /b/ in some
given a separate phonological notation, e.g., /P/ (as in Lob
(nonnative) words, thus representing the morphopho-
) for a segment which is phonetically a voiceless plosive
neme jbj (e.g., in hydrofob ‘hydrophobe’ ,
like initial /p/, but is not in contrast (opposition) to a /b/
hydrofobi ‘hydrophobia’ );
(which is in contrast to /p/ initially). A consequence of this
5. syllable-final / / alternates with syllable-initial /d/ in
analysis is that Lob, Lobe (dat.) exhibits a mor-
(nonnative) words, thus representing the morphopho-
phophonological alternation between /P/ (word-finally and
neme jdj (e.g., in abbed ‘abbot’ , abbedisse ‘abbess’
hence syllable-finally too) and /b/ (syllable-initially).
).

From Phonemes to Morphophonemes: Introducing


Morphological Relatedness Morphophonemes as Candidate Lexical Segments
Phonetic and phonemic transcriptions rely exclusively on Section Morphophonemic Analysis until now was an attempt
input from sound. By contrast morphophonemic transcriptions to characterize morphophonemic transcriptions from below
(between vertical strokes) involve also morphological infor- (‘bottom-up’), i.e., from sound via phonemes to morphopho-
mation. Heuristically, two phonemes can be identified mor- nemes, as part of a discovery procedure (cf Harris, 1951). But
phophonemically if they occur in parallel positions in morphophonemes can also be considered candidates for
different forms of the same morpheme – e.g. in different underlying segments in the lexicon.
inflectional forms belonging to the same paradigm – if it Whereas the morphophonemes are derivable from the
applies to a significant set of morphemes in a systematical surface structure, by means of a bottom-up procedure, lexical
way. A Danish example would be /j/ in spøge ‘make jokes’ segments are the units posited as underlying. There is not
, and / / in its past participle spøgt , a unique string of morphophonemes corresponding to every
which may be considered as representing the same morpho- string of phonemes or sounds, but rather there may be
phoneme . There is no claim of biuniqueness (cf Section different solutions, so to speak, to this discovery procedure,
Input Level to Morphophonemic Analysis: Phonemes) depending on which sets of morphological alternations are
between morphophonemes and sound chain, viz. the same to be accounted for.
phoneme in the same phonemic context can represent The relation between phonemes and morphophonemes is
different morphophonemes – as /v/ in sagfører ‘barrister’ not biunique: whereas it is in general possible in Danish – for
represents the morphophoneme , cf føre sag ‘liti- the main speech norm chosen here – to derive the phonemes
gate’ , contra jvj in savklinge ‘saw blade’ of a given word form from the morphophonemes, by means
, cf sav ‘saw’ . of phonological principles and mechanisms, the opposite is
The morphophonemic analysis should make phonological not true: Such a biuniqueness presupposes an explicit exhaus-
patterns simpler and more general, e.g., with simpler phono- tive enumeration of the word forms that are considered
tactics and more general principles of realization. An example formally related and nonsuppletive (i.e., not phonologically
from Danish could be: jpj, jtj, jkj are always realized as unrelated forms, such as English be, is, was). In many cases,
plosives, whereas jbj, jdj, have variable realizations as either in many specific phonemic contexts, several morphopho-
plosive or approximant, depending on their position in the nemes correspond to the same phoneme, and vice versa: the
phonological syllable. same morphophoneme corresponds to several phonemes.
832 Morphophonology

From the point of view of the addressee, and not least of the synthesis in this sense was taken, viz. when morphophonemes
person acquiring the language, there will be several candidates were considered as candidate lexical segments. From that
for lexical segments in many cases, and an ongoing change in perspective, a phonological description – to avoid the term
the lexical representations can be expected, according to ‘analysis’ in order not to confuse the narrow and the broad
(increased) awareness of morphophonological relations, sense of this term – can be given from lexical segments until
i.e., morphological relations paired to their phonological phonemes, and further on via contrastive segments to
realizations. phonetic segments. Such a description involves realization
A decision on such issues depends on whether we are talk- principles or rules of some sort, e.g., as those used in different
ing about versions of generative phonology in the broad sense, and
somehow recalling basic descriptive principles behind
1. hypotheses about speakers, i.e., about psycholinguistics
Bloomfield’s (1939) Menomini morphophonemics.
and psychological reality; or
2. methodological principles for the analysis, regardless of
any relation to the behavior of real speaker-hearers.
Note
In the case of (1), speakers’ knowledge and awareness of
orthography must be expected to play a substantial role in Sections Phonotactics and Morphology: English Regular
alphabetized cultures, and this issue should be subjected to Inflections and Morphophonemic Analysis are based upon
empirical investigation. If we are to speculate about the Basbøll (2006). The IPA-transcription of Danish follows
psychological reality for speakers of Danish of two equally Basbøll (2005), however without diacritics for voicelessness
possible morphophonemic transcriptions and (Danish [b d ] are voiceless). I am indebted to Nina
for Danish , which may mean either ‘brave’ or ‘crash’ Grønnum for numerous valuable stylistic and other
(neither word enters into morphological alternations which suggestions.
might reveal the underlying identity of the stem-final conso-
nant), it may be relevant, for example, that brav ‘brave’ is
See also: First Language Acquisition, Linguistic Theory of;
written with the letter v, finally, that occurs initially in words
Linguistic Typology; Linguistics: Overview; Morphology in
pronounced with [v] (/v/), and the homophonous brag ‘crash’
Linguistics; Phonology; Suprasegmentals.
with the letter g that occurs initially in words pronounced with
initial [ ] (/ /), cf vi, gå ‘we,’ ‘go’ [vi], . Furthermore,
different speakers’ ideas of relatedness between word forms
– whether historically founded or not – may be relevant for Bibliography
their lexical representations, and also this matter should be
investigated empirically. These issues represent the strong Basbøll, H., 1972. Remarks on the regular plural formation of English nouns.
sense of morphophonemes as candidate lexical segments. Language and Literature 1 (3). Copenhagen, 39–43.
In the case of (2), naturalness can be chosen as a methodo- Basbøll, H., 2005. The Phonology of Danish. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Basbøll, H., 2006. Phonemic analysis. In: Brown, K. (Ed.), The Encyclopedia of
logical guideline for the relation between different levels (see
Language and Linguistics, second ed. Elsevier, Oxford, pp. 351–358.
Section Nonchange of Phonetic Content between Levels: A Basbøll, H., Kjærbæk, L., Lambertsen, C., 2011. The Danish noun plural landscape.
Principle of Naturalness): the correspondence between the Acta Linguistica Hafniensia 43 (2), 81–105.
contrastive segment [X] and the phoneme /X/ is more natural Bloch, B., Trager, G.L., 1942. Outline of Linguistic Analysis. Linguistic Society of
than between [X] and /Y/, and the correspondence between America, Baltimore, MD.
Bloomfield, L., 1933. Language. Henry Holt, New York.
the phoneme /X/ and the morphophoneme jXj is more Bloomfield, L., 1939. Menomini morphophonemics. Travaux du Cercle Linguistique
natural than between /X/ and jZj. This is not just a question de Prague 8, 105–115.
of arbitrary labels: both contrastive segments and phonemes Dressler, W.U., 1985. Morphonology: The Dynamics of Derivation. Linguistica
have phonetic content, even though the degree of abstraction Extranea, Studia, vol. 12. Karoma Publishers, Ann Arbor.
Dressler, W.U., Dziubalska-Ko1aczyk, K., 2006. Proposing morphonotactics. Italian
is not the same; and morphophonemes as well have phonetic
Journal of Linguistics 18, 249–266.
content – and can be analyzed in terms of distinctive features, Fischer-Jørgensen, E., 1975. Trends in phonological theory. A Historical Introduction
and so on – even though they also encode information about (Akademisk Forlag, København). Travaux du cercle linguistique de Copenhague,
morphological alternations which may have consequences for second ed. (1995), vol. 27. Linguistic Circle of Copenhagen.
the phonetic content. Goldsmith, J.A. (Ed.), 1995. The Handbook of Phonological Theory. Blackwell, Oxford.
Grønnum, N., 2007. Rødgrød med Fløde. En lille bog om dansk fonetik. Akademisk
Forlag, Copenhagen.
Hamp, E.P., Householder, F.W., Austerlitz, R. (Eds.), 1966. Readings in Linguistics,
Concluding Remarks II. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Harris, Z.S., 1951. Methods in Structural Linguistics. University of Chicago Press,
Chicago (Reprinted as Structural Linguistics (1963)).
Morphophonemic analysis in the narrow sense is a ‘bottom-
Hjelmslev, L., 1943. Omkring Sprogteoriens Grundlæggelse. Festskrift udgivet af
up’-procedure from phonemes, e.g., established from sound Københavns Universitet. (Reprinted in (1966) Akademisk, Copenhagen; and in
by means of the commutation test, to morphophonemes. English translation (1966) Prolegomena to a theory of language. The University of
But the morphophonemic analysis in the broad sense – Wisconsin Press, Madison).
inversely – can also proceed the other way round, viz. as Joos, M., 1957. Readings in Linguistics. ACLS, Washington.
Laaha, S., Kjærbæk, L., Basbøll, H., Dressler, W.U., 2011. The impact of sound
a ‘top-down’-procedure, called synthesis in the narrow sense structure on morphology: an experimental study on children’s acquisition of German
(e.g., in Hjelmslev’s (1943) terminology). In Section Morpho- and Danish noun plurals focusing on stem change. Acta Linguistica Hafniensia 43
phonemes as Candidate Lexical Segments, a perspective of (2), 106–126.
Morphophonology 833

Langacker, R.W., 1987. Foundations of Cognitive Grammar. Stanford University Trubetzkoy [Trubeckoj], N.S., 1929. Sur la morphonologie. Travaux du Cercle Lin-
Press, Stanford. guistique de Prague 1, 85–88.
Peirce, C.S., 1965. In: Hartshorne, C., Weiss, P. (Eds.), Collected Papers. Harvard Trubetzkoy [Trubeckoj], N.S., 1931. Gedanken über Morphonologie. Travaux du
University Press, Cambridge, MA. Cercle Linguistique de Prague 4, 160–163.
Saussure, F. de, 1916. Cours de linguistique générale. Payot, Paris. Trubetzkoy [Trubeckoj], N.S., 1939. Grundzüge der Phonologie. Travaux du Cercle
Spencer, A., Zwicky, A.M. (Eds.), 1998. The Handbook of Morphology. Blackwell, Oxford. Linguistique de Prague 7.

You might also like