You are on page 1of 42

Article

Organizational Psychology Review


2023, Vol. 13(3) 237–278
The hot and the cold in © The Author(s) 2023
Article reuse guidelines:
destructive leadership: Modeling sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/20413866231153098

the role of arousal in explaining journals.sagepub.com/home/opr

leader antecedents and follower


consequences of abusive
supervision versus exploitative
leadership

Franziska Emmerling
Technical University of Munich, Germany

Claudia Peus
Technical University of Munich, Germany

Jill Lobbestael
Maastricht University, the Netherlands

Abstract
Due to its devastating consequences, research needs to theoretically and empirically disentangle
different sub-types of destructive leadership. Based on concepts derived from aggression research
distinguishing re- and proactive aggression, we provide a process model differentiating abusive
supervision and exploitative leadership. High versus low arousal negative affect is installed as
the central mediating factor determining (1) whether perceived goal-blockage (leadership antece-
dents) leads to abusive supervision versus exploitative leadership and (2) whether a specific lead-
ership behavior leads to active versus passive follower behavior (leadership consequence).

Paper received 13 December 2019. Received revised December 13, 2019

Corresponding author:
Franziska Emmerling, Chair for Research and Science Management, Technical University of Munich, Arcisstrasse 21,
Munchen, 80333, Bayern, Germany.
Email: franziska.emmerling@tum.de

Creative Commons Non Commercial CC BY-NC: This article is distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/
by-nc/4.0/) which permits non-commercial use, reproduction and distribution of the work without further permission
provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access page (https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/
nam/open-access-at-sage).
238 Organizational Psychology Review 13(3)

Further, theoretical anchoring of individual and contextual moderators onto the model’s process
paths is provided and exemplary hypotheses for concrete moderation effects are deduced. Based
on the provided process model, we highlight four recommendations to facilitate process-based
construct differentiation in future research on destructive leadership. To precisely understand
the differences and commonalities in different forms of destructive leadership will ultimately
enable custom-tailored inter- and prevention.

Plain Language Summary


Negative leadership—also named “destructive” leadership—has very bad effects on followers
and organizations. There are not just one, but many forms of destructive leadership and it is
important to understand where different sub-types come from (i.e., to understand their ante-
cedents) and which specific effect they have (i.e., to understand their consequences). In this
paper, we focus on better understanding two forms of destructive leadership, namely abusive
supervision and exploitative leadership. These two forms are similar to the two main forms
of aggression. Abusive supervision is similar to reactive aggression, an impulsive “hot blooded”
form of aggression. Exploitative leadership is similar to proactive aggression, a premeditated
“cold blooded” form of aggression. We explain the parallels between the two forms of aggres-
sion and the two forms of leadership and provide a model which allows to predict when one
versus the other form of leadership occurs and to which follower behavior they lead. An
important factor in this model is the physiological characteristic of the emotional reaction to
an event (i.e., arousal). An emotional reaction can be high in arousal; for instance, anger is a
high arousal negative emotional reaction. On the contrary, boredom, for instance, is a low arou-
sal negative emotional reaction. Dependent on whether both a leader and a follower react to a
negative event (e.g., not getting what they want, being treated badly by others) with high or low
arousal, their behavior will be different. We explain how this mechanism works and how it can
help us to better predict leaders’ and followers’ behavior. We also outline how individual char-
acteristics of the leader and follower and characteristics of their environment and context inter-
act with arousal and their behavior.

Keywords
destructive leadership, abusive supervision, consequences, antecedents, negative leadership,
exploitative leadership, arousal, affect

Most—if not all—organizations rely on skillful et al., 2008). Furthermore, negative behavior
leadership to optimize their potential, unite is generally more harmful than an equivalent
followers, reduce conflict, and distill various positive behavior is helpful (Baumeister et al.,
centrifugal forces into success (Peus et al., 2001). Also, destructive leadership—which pre-
2016). While positive forms of leadership (e.g., sents itself with many faces—not only negatively
Antonakis et al., 2003) enable individuals, orga- affects the direct target but also indirectly impacts
nizations, and societies to flourish, destructive the target’s environment (Hoobler & Brass, 2006).
forms of leadership can have devastating conse- The immense and widespread negative influence
quences (Schyns & Schilling, 2013). This nega- of destructive leadership therefore necessitates
tive impact is magnified psychologically as nuanced empirical investigations into its antece-
humans perceive and process negative input dents as well as its consequences in order to
more intensely than positive input (Unkelbach obtain a better understanding of how individuals
Emmerling et al. 239

and organizations could prevent or react to destruc- et al. (2007) as a continuous display of influen-
tive leadership. tial behavior by a leader causing harm. Over
In this paper, we will review the evolution of time, the term “destructive leadership” has
constructs in research on destructive leadership arisen to mean more than one specific construct
and evidence its antecedents and consequences. and this has become an umbrella term. In add-
Against this background, we will draw on ition, recently, its initially somewhat basic def-
aggression research by suggesting a process inition was rendered more precise by
model which outlines how arousal mediates highlighting its deliberate foci. For instance,
leader antecedents and follower consequences Schyns and Schilling (2013) specified in their
of two exemplary forms of destructive leader- seminal meta-analysis, that the dark side of
ship characterized by opposite arousal states, leadership takes on many forms, depending on
i.e., abusive supervision (Tepper, 2000) versus whether the perception of or actual behavior is
exploitative leadership (Schmid et al., 2019). discussed, whether action is intentional, which
Positive forms of leadership and its effects on types of behavior (i.e., physical, verbal, non-
followers and organizations have been studied verbal) are included in the conceptualization,
extensively in the past (Antonakis et al., 2003; and to what extent consequences are considered.
Antonakis & Day, 2017). Traditionally, the Krasikova et al. (2013) defined destructive leader-
focus of leadership research was on unraveling ship in their comprehensive overview as volitional
and explaining those forms of leadership that behavior while also stressing that the leader does
facilitate a desired consequence among followers not have to be consciously aware of their intent to
or in organizations, most notably transformational harm (James & LeBreton, 2010, 2012). These
and charismatic leadership (Bass, 1985a, 1985b). authors thereby pioneered the demarcation of
Over the course of the last two decades, particular clear destructive forms of leadership from adjacent
moral forms of leadership (Lemoine et al., 2019), phenomena, such as non-volitional harmful leader-
such as ethical leadership (Brown & Treviño, ship behavior, ineffective leadership (e.g., due to
2006a, 2006b), authentic leadership (Peus et al., incompetence), and simply non-existent or laissez-
2013; Walumbwa et al., 2008), and servant lead- faire leadership. They outlined two further manifes-
ership (Liden et al., 2008; van Dierendonck, tations of destructive leadership: the leading of
2011) emerged (for an overview, see Zhu et al., followers toward harmful goals and employing
2019). harmful methods of leading. These can—but do
An acknowledgement that the dark side of not necessarily have to—co-occur, i.e., either
leadership is equally in need of scientific atten- type is sufficient to define destructive leadership.
tion has emerged more hesitantly. Initially, it Focusing on the history of research on
was suggested that leadership entailing negative destructive leadership, several developmental
behavior or consequences contradicts the very landmarks stand out. The first study on antece-
definition of “what leadership is” (Yukl & dents and consequences of destructive forms of
van Fleet, 1992) and could not therefore be leadership was published in 1997 and the number
defined as leadership. And yet, the last two of publications has increased steadily ever since.
decades have seen an increase in research on Studied constructs diversified across time. Early
destructive forms of leadership, which has gen- conceptualizations included petty tyranny
erated ample theoretical and empirical insights (Ashforth, 1997) and abusive supervision (Tepper,
on various forms of destructive leadership (for 2000), followed by—for instance—supervisor
an overview, see Krasikova et al., 2013; undermining (Duffy et al., 2006), aversive leader-
Schyns & Schilling, 2013; Tepper et al., 2017). ship (Bligh et al., 2007), despotic leadership (De
Various definitions of destructive leadership Hoogh & Den Hartog, 2008), destructive leader-
have been introduced, mostly originating from a ship (Aasland et al., 2010), narcissistic leadership
ground-breaking conceptualization by Einarsen (Hochwarter & Thompson, 2012), toxic leadership
240 Organizational Psychology Review 13(3)

(Aubrey, 2012), and exploitative leadership emergent model. He focused on the time continuum,
(Schmid et al., 2018). including supervisor perception of injustice and con-
The high and ever-increasing number of con- tract violation (antecedents), abusive supervision
structs used in past research on destructive lead- (leadership), and various effects (consequences;
ership warrants integration. It necessitates there subordinates’ attitudes, resistance, aggression
being a clear distinction between overlap and and deviance, performance contributions, psy-
differentiation within this construct space (as chological distress, and family well-being).
demanded by Krasikova et al., 2013; Le et al., Furthermore, his work considered differentiable
2010; Shaffer et al., 2016; Thoroughgood entities (i.e., the character and behavior of super-
et al., 2018), as well as a specification of basic visors and subordinates, as well as the work
distinctive processes underlying different leader- context). Similarly, by proposing the toxic triangle
ship behaviors (DeRue et al., 2011; Piccolo et al., model of destructive leadership, i.e., describing
2012; van Knippenberg & Sitkin, 2013). This characteristics of leaders, followers, and environ-
work follows those suggestions and develops a mental contexts in reference to destructive leader-
process model on the differentiation between ship, Padilla et al. (2007) underlined the
abusive supervision and exploitative leadership importance of considering all levels of analysis
based on their antecedents and consequences. involved in order to fully grasp destructive lead-
One way of illuminating the basic distinctive ership. Building on the toxic triangle model,
processes underlying different forms of destruc- Thoroughgood et al. (2018) integrated further
tive leadership which shows promise is to focus theories and focused on the role of followers in
on criterion validity (Crocker & Algina, 1986; the destructive leadership process. They pro-
McDonald, 2013; Novick, 1966). Criterion val- posed a cohesive taxonomy of vulnerable fol-
idity refers to the function of a construct when lowers, i.e., the type of follower associated with
predicting independent consequences and cri- destructive leadership. Krasikova et al. (2013)
teria. Constructs can be differentiated based on went even further and described destructive lead-
their consequences (e.g., physical aggression ership “as a product of dispositional and context-
leads to physical injury while psychological vio- ual factors” (p. 1316). Thoroughgood et al. (2018)
lence does not) or, even if consequences are demand an inclusive definition that acknowledges
similar, based on their antecedents. Criterion val- “destructive leadership as (i) a group process
idity, therefore, predicts the fact that constructs involving flawed, toxic, or ineffective leaders,
require differentiation in the case of either dis- susceptible followers, and conducive environ-
tinct antecedents or consequences, since this ments; consisting of (ii) destructive group or
implies differing underlying processes and/or organizational outcomes, as well as (iii) a
potential intervention strategies. dynamic time frame” (p. 2). Throughout the last
The general idea of conceptualizing destruc- two decades therefore, the field has clearly moved
tive forms of leadership as a process (antece- toward including process elements (i.e., antecedents
dents—leadership—consequence) has emerged and consequences) in its theorizing of destructive
gradually. In early research on destructive leadership. The dyadic process—particularly as
forms of leadership—when petty tyranny was regards leaders and followers—constitutes the
proposed as a construct—Ashforth (1994) was core of previous theorizing.
already recommending investigating individual
predispositions and situational facilitators (i.e., Leader antecedents and follower
antecedents) of petty tyranny as well as their consequences of destructive
effects on followers (i.e., consequences). In 2007,
Tepper provided the first review on antecedents leadership
and consequences of negative leadership (mainly When it comes to leader characteristics as antece-
on abusive supervision) and set the stage for an dents of destructive forms of leadership, ample
Emmerling et al. 241

evidence is provided. Factors such as compromised blockage honors both the complexity and multi-
mental health (e.g., depression: Byrne et al., 2014; factorial nature of the destructive leadership
Dionisi & Barling, 2019; Tepper et al., 2006/ emo- process. Note that the focus lies on “perceived”
tional exhaustion: M. M. Fan et al., 2020; Walsh & goal-blockage: whether there is an actual object-
Arnold, 2018), problematic personality traits (e.g., ive goal-blockage is of secondary interest, more
high Machiavellianism: Kiazad et al., 2010; pertinent is whether a leader subjectively experi-
Lyons et al., 2019; Wisse & Sleebos, 2016/ high ences it. Thereby, goals are understood as the
narcissism: Lyons et al., 2019; Nevicka et al., engagement in satisfactory activities. These can
2018), or low self-control (Mawritz et al., 2017; include concrete performance or career goals
Yam et al., 2016) have been associated with (e.g., promotion, monetary success), psychological
higher levels of destructive leadership. needs (e.g., acknowledgement, respect, status, self-
While leaders and their characteristics efficacy), or positive affect (e.g., pleasure, fun,
undoubtedly play a crucial role in the emergence excitement). Rooted in this broad understanding
of destructive forms of leadership, they do not of goals, a leader’s perceived goal-blockage has
operate in a vacuum. In other words, there is no evolved as the central anchor point of the destruc-
such concept as “absolute evil” in leadership— tive leadership process (Krasikova et al., 2013).
leader characteristics interact with followers- Emphasizing the dyadic process underlying
related and organizational variables to create a leadership, it is negative consequences at the fol-
multifactorial bed on which destructive forms of lower level that constitutes the most prevalent
leadership grow. The antecedents of various outcome in research on destructive leadership by
forms of destructive leadership can, however, be far. A wide range of active (i.e., outwards directed
analyzed not only at the leadership level, but / high energy) versus passive (i.e., more inwards
also at that of the follower (e.g., deviant behavior), directed / low energy) behaviors have been
and the organization/environment (e.g., downsiz- studied as being consequences of destructive
ing of a company). To conceptualize this point forms of leadership. When it comes to active beha-
of view, Krasikova et al. (2013) tailored their the- viors, followers have been shown to respond with
oretical model toward destructive leadership as aggression (Burton et al., 2011; Burton &
being a result of a leader’s perceived goal- Hoobler, 2011; Brees et al., 2014; Carleton et al.,
blockage and thereby, they substantially advanced 2016; Dupré et al., 2006; Lian et al., 2014;
theorizing in the field. Without neglecting the Pradhan et al., 2019a; Richard et al., 2018), counter-
many interactive and recursive elements within productive work behavior (Akram et al., 2019;
the leadership process, the field acknowledges Brender-Ilan & Sheaffer, 2019; Chu, 2014; Duffy
perceived goal-blockage as the central construct et al., 2006; Goswami et al., 2019; Ju et al., 2019;
which anchors the leadership process. Perceived Low et al., 2019; Ogunfowora, 2013; Shoss et al.,
goal-blockage leads to destructive forms of lead- 2013; Simon et al., 2015; Watkins et al., 2019;
ership. Perceived goal-blockage is described as a Yang et al., 2019), deviance (Avey et al., 2015;
complex phenomenon comprising two central Javed et al., 2019; Mackey et al., 2015; Mawritz
aspects. On the one hand, leaders experience per- et al., 2017; Rice et al., 2020a; Schmid et al.,
ceived goal-blockage if their goals are misaligned 2019; Thoroughgood et al., 2018; Valle et al.,
with organizational goals (Krasikova et al., 2013). 2019; Vogel et al., 2016; Wang & Jiang, 2014),
On the other hand, leaders also perceive goals as or reactance (Bligh et al., 2007; Goswami et al.,
blocked if their own and/or organizational goals 2015; Haggard & Park, 2018; Liu et al., 2010;
are thwarted by followers (Krasikova et al., Tepper et al., 2001). Destructive forms of leadership
2013). Krasikova et al. (2013) further outline have also been linked with passive follower beha-
how the interplay between leaders’ characteristics viors such as withdrawal (Allen et al., 2016; Chi
and organizational context can enhance perceived & Liang, 2013; Huang et al., 2020; Kim et al., 2019;
goal-blockage. To assign a central role to goal- Mawritz et al., 2014; Wei & Si, 2013), turnover /
242 Organizational Psychology Review 13(3)

turnover intention (Agarwal, 2019; Ogunfowora Abusive supervision. Abusive supervision is


et al., 2019; Palanski et al., 2014; Pradhan et al., defined as the “subordinates’” perceptions of
2018; Ramdeo & Singh, 2019; Rice et al., the extent to which supervisors engage in the
2020a; Schmid et al., 2018; Pradhan et al., sustained display of hostile verbal and non-
2019a; Wang & Chan, 2019), and silence verbal behaviors, excluding physical contact”
(Al-Hawari et al., 2020a; Burris et al., 2008; (Tepper, 2000, p.178). It was introduced as
Carnevale et al., 2018; Huang et al., 2019a; one of the first constructs in research on destruc-
Huang et al., 2019b; Jung & Yoon, 2019; tive forms of leadership based on theoretical
Kiewitz et al., 2016; Ouyang et al., 2015; Park conceptual considerations. Tepper’s (2000)
et al., 2018; Wang et al., 2020a; Wang & Jiang, 15-item Abusive Supervision Scale as well as
2015; Wu et al., 2018b; Xu et al., 2020; Yao its frequently used shortened 5-item version
et al., 2019; Xu et al., 2015). (Mitchell & Ambrose, 2007) includes items
In summary, extensive and robust theorizing as such as “[the leader] ridicules me,” “puts me
well as evidence suggests that (1) leader’s per- down in front of others,” or “tells me I am
ceived goal-blockage is a crucial precursor and incompetent.” The definition and measurement
(2) negative active/passive follower behavior is of abusive supervision focuses on overtly dis-
a crucial result of destructive leadership. In the fol- played, often impulsive negative behavior.
lowing, we will outline this dyadic process further The construct points at leaders’ anger and its
by focusing on specific forms of destructive lead- immediate expression in front of and toward
ership and introducing concepts of research ori- employees. Note that Mitchell and Ambrose’s
ginating in the field of aggression. (2007) work magnifies this perspective on the
construct, having introduced a 5-item short
The hot and cold in destructive measure which explicitly and deliberately
focuses on items assessing active impulsive
leadership behavior (e.g., shouting, putting an employee
In the context of this work, we will focus on two down in front of others); this short measure
forms of destructive leadership, i.e., abusive has become the dominant tool with which to
supervision (Tepper, 2000) and exploitative lead- assess abusive supervision.
ership (Schmid et al., 2019)1. These two con- Exploitative leadership. Rooted in the
structs lend themselves to being differentiable motivation to provide a more nuanced picture
focuses for three reasons. Firstly, they frame the of destructive leadership, specifically taking
historic development of research on destructive leaders’ self-interest into account, Schmid et al.
leadership—with abusive supervision being the (2019) developed the construct of exploitative
first construct concretely measurable to emerge leadership. They define exploitative leadership
and exploitative leadership being the most as “leadership with the primary intention to
recent. Secondly, these two constructs are charac- further the leader’s self-interest. Such leaders
terized by clearly differentiable yet parallel defini- exploit others by (1) acting egoistically, (2) exert-
tions—with abusive supervision involving ing pressure and manipulating followers, (3) over-
impulsive and exploitative leadership signified burdening followers, or, on the other hand, (4)
by more deliberate courses of destructive action. consistently underchallenging followers, allowing
Thirdly, abusive supervision and exploitative no development” (Schmid et al., 2019, p.1404).
leadership are—to date—the only two constructs The construct of exploitative leadership and how
in research on destructive forms of leadership to measure it has been developed based on test-
which have been directly compared within an theoretical principles (exploratory followed by
empirical framework, if not with respect to their confirmatory factor analysis, Schmid et al.,
antecedents, then at least with respect to their con- 2019). The establishment of its differential value
sequences (Schmid et al., 2019). within a nomological network has further
Emmerling et al. 243

confirmed that exploitative leadership is best specifically, proactive aggression “refers to


described as falling into one (or more) of five sep- acts committed in negative affective states
arate but related sub-dimensions, i.e., genuine such as anger or frustration, or in response to
egoistic behavior, exerting pressure, undermining provocation” (Miller & Lynam, 2006, p.1470).
development, taking credit, and manipulative Proactive aggression has been “characterized as
leader behavior (Schmid et al., 2019). The instrumental, organized, and “cold-blooded,” with
15-item Exploitative Leadership Scale includes little evidence of autonomic arousal” (Raine
items such as “[the leader] passes the teams et al., 2006, p. 160). Abusive supervision—based
work off as his or her own,” “manipulates on its definition and measurement that focuses on
others to reach his or her goals,” or “does not impulsive and overtly harmful action toward subor-
give me opportunities to further develop myself dinates—is, therefore, close to the idea of reactive
professionally because his or her own goals aggression.2 In contrast, exploitative leadership
have priority” (Schmid et al., 2019). centers explicitly around premeditated, delibera-
Due to there being striking parallels with tive, and proactively destructive behavior.
respect to definitions and prevalent constructs, Leaders consciously exploit an employee’s work
research on destructive forms of leadership also and hinder personal development to serve their
benefits from research into aggression. Aggression self-interest. As such, the construct resembles the
research has been developing constantly over the idea of proactive as opposed to reactive aggression.
course of many decades. In their seminal work on Although far more recent, the introduction of
human aggression which merges historic and exploitative leadership as a construct in research
updated research streams, Anderson and Bushman on destructive forms of leadership mirrors
(2002) define aggression as “any behavior directed developments in aggression research. Just as
toward another individual that is carried out with the with exploitative leadership in the leadership
proximate (immediate) intent to cause harm” field, evidence on proactive as opposed to react-
(p. 28). They add that, furthermore, the conditions ive aggression has also accumulated more grad-
of (1) the perpetrator’s belief that their behavior ually in the aggression field. One reason for this
will harm and (2) the victim’s motivation to avoid might be that proactively aggressive behavior—
said harm need to be met in order to classify a as with exploitative leadership—is more subtle
behavior as aggressive. The parallels between this in that the actor’s high and deliberate intrinsic
conceptual approach to negative behavior and the motivation to harm is often difficult to observe
development of definitions in research on destruc- (Raine et al., 2006). This can divert societal
tive leadership are self-evident with destructive and scientific attention and makes a phenom-
leadership literally focusing on the intent to harm enon hard to identify, measure, and—therefore
in a leadership-specific context (Einarsen et al., —study. In contrast, reactive impulsive aggres-
2007; Krasikova et al., 2013). sion—as with abusive supervision—is out in
When it comes to different types of aggres- the open and unable to be overlooked; circum-
sion, the distinction between reactive and pro- stances making those constructs primary and
active aggression has proven to be a fruitful early foci of aggression and leadership research
and widely used concept (Crick & Dodge, respectively. Even though more premeditated
1996; Miller & Lynam, 2006; Poulin & forms of destructive behavior might only be
Boivin, 2000; Raine et al., 2006). In general obvious at second glance, their immensely nega-
terms, reactive and proactive aggression are tive impact—often rooted in the planful deliber-
similar in that they both intend to harm and fun- ation of how to maximize the intended harm and
damentally different in that the former is charac- gain—should not be underestimated. Proactive
terized by impulsiveness, high arousal, and aggression, for instance, has been linked with par-
immediacy, while the latter by deliberation, ticularly severe and violent crimes (Cima et al.,
low arousal, and planful action. More 2013; Miller & Lynam, 2006; Woodworth &
244 Organizational Psychology Review 13(3)

Porter, 2002). Importantly, it is not our intention in information encoding, mental representations,
the slightest to make the case that one of the two response accessing, and response evaluation.
sides (proactive versus reactive / abusive versus Reactive aggression results from problems in
exploitative) is more important, harmful, or rele- the early stages of information processing; for
vant, but we simply wish to emphasize that—in instance, information encodings and representa-
order to capture destructive behavior and its tions rooted in a hostile attribution bias (“this
impact—any scientific perspective must incorpor- employee bumped into me because he/she
ate both. The mutual exclusivity of reactive and wanted to provoke me” instead of “…because
proactive aggression is debated in aggression he/she lost balance”) have been uniquely
research (Bushman & Anderson, 2001). Even so, linked with high arousal (e.g., anger) and react-
the field also emphasizes that both forms of ive aggression (Crick & Dodge, 1996; Gagnon
destructive behavior can co-occur (Fite et al., & Rochat, 2017; Tuente et al., 2019). In con-
2006); in other words, while both behaviors are trast, proactive aggression and the preceding
conceptually different and can be attributed to dis- low arousal (e.g., boredom) is fueled by pro-
tinct constructs, they can—of course—be dis- blems in the later stages of information process-
played simultaneously or consecutively by one ing, namely during selection and evaluation of
individual depending on the context. For instance, potential behavioral responses; for instance,
an individual can impulsively shout at a provoca- expecting rewards such as personal gain or
teur (reactive aggression) and then premeditate pleasure from displaying aggressive behavior
revenge (proactive aggression). Not only can (“I will get praised if I pass others’ work off
both forms of aggression co-occur within one indi- as my own” or “I enjoy / find it exciting to see
vidual, but also within one aggressive act others being exploited”) increases proactive
(Bushman & Anderson, 2001). A bank robbery, aggression (Crick & Dodge, 1996; Hubbard
for instance, might be deliberately planned to et al., 2001; Smithmyer et al., 2000). Note that
obtain financial resources and power (proactive this seminal model on the antecedents of re-
aggression), but on the scene, a robber can be trig- versus proactive aggression (Crick & Dodge,
gered by a costumer’s behavior and impulsively 1996) can be applied organically to the notion
lash out (reactive aggression). What it comes of perceived goal-blockage being the central
down to is that reactive and proactive aggression anchor point to the destructive leadership
are distinct but parallel dimensions (Poulin & process (Krasikova et al., 2013). Following
Boivin, 2000; Stanford et al., 2003). Similar the model’s logic, leaders’ perceived goal-
assumptions can be made about abusive supervi- blockage leads to different levels of arousal
sion versus exploitative leadership. A leader can and, thereby, leadership behaviors depending
put an employee down in front of others (abusive on whether it occurs in early versus late stages
supervision) and subsequently hinder the same of social information processing. While per-
employee’s personal development to maximally ceived goal-blockage rooted in the early
exploit labor (exploitative leadership). Likewise, stages of social information processing (e.g.,
even the most consistently exploitative leader “this employee’s behavior is a personal
might lash out impulsively when triggered by an attack”) will lead to high arousal negative
employee’s seemingly sub-optimal performance. affect (e.g., anger) and high levels of abusive
When it comes to the antecedents of reactive supervision, perceived goal-blockage rooted in
versus proactive aggression, Crick and Dodge the later stages (e.g., “I cannot advance my per-
(1996) provided a popular framework, i.e., the sonal advancement enough; if only I could sell
Social Information Processing Model (SIPM). my employees’ work as my own”) will be
The model states that the emergence of reactive accompanied by low arousal (e.g., boredom)
versus proactive aggression (enactment) is and favor high levels of exploitative leadership.
fueled by differential pattern in preceding Paralleling the central theoretical perspectives
Emmerling et al. 245

on aggression and destructive leadership abusive supervision, not exploitative leadership


research (social information processing & per- and vice versa, (2) which follower consequences
ceived goal-blockage) and applying them to result specifically from abusive supervision not
the concrete example of differentiating reactive exploitative leadership and vice versa, and (3)
from proactive aggression and abusive supervi- which variables moderate these processes.
sion from exploitative leadership, highlights the Drawing from concepts outlined in aggression
theoretical potential which lies in fusing research, we have built a respective process model.
together these two streams of thought.
The fact that abusive supervision and Introducing arousal into the
exploitative leadership cater to a more impul-
sive and “reactive” versus a more premeditated destructive leadership process
“proactive” dimension suggests that they differ As outlined, the definitions of reactive and pro-
with respect to antecedents and consequences. active aggression serve to differentiate their
While aggression research is rooted in a widespread underlying states of arousal explicitly. While
theoretical and empirical tradition of comparing reactive aggression is characterized by high
constructs directly (i.e., theoretically and empirically arousal or negative affective states, proactive
differentiating re- from proactive aggression: Card aggression is, in contrast, characterized by low
& Little, 2006; Hubbard et al., 2010; Kempes arousal (Miller & Lynam, 2006; Raine et al.,
et al., 2005), research on destructive forms of lead- 2006). So, when applying the reactive/proactive
ership has so far allocated relatively little attention parallel to that of destructive leadership, one
to direct construct differentiation. Schmid et al. central assumption is that autonomic arousal
(2019) have provided initial evidence with respect also forms a crucial variable in differentiating
to selected follower consequences of abusive super- these two perspectives on destructive behavior.
vision versus exploitative leadership. In two inde- Arousal describes the universal physiological reac-
pendent studies, they found that the inclusion of tion to any kind of stimulus which is relevant for
exploitative leadership over abusive supervision an organism (Reisenzein, 1994). As such,
into regression models increased the predictive val- arousal is defined by two central characteristics;
idity for both leadership behaviors’ negative rela- arousal is always a physiological reaction to a con-
tionship with job satisfaction and affective crete and specific experience (e.g., stimulus, indi-
commitment as well as their positive relationship vidual, event) and this reaction is immediate,
with burnout and workplace deviance (Schmid direct, and instantaneous (i.e., in its first instance
et al., 2019). This finding points toward differences not modifiable). Arousal is—next to valence—
in predictive validity between the two constructs one of the two definitory elements of emotion
and, thereby, their conceptual distinction. The spe- (Barrett, 1998; Kensinger, 2004); any emotion
cific process (antecedents—leadership—conse- can be located in a two-dimensional space span-
quence) differentiating both leadership behaviors ning along the axes of low versus high arousal
—i.e., which antecedents or affective reaction to and positive versus negative valence. For instance,
perceived goal-blockage lead to abusive supervision anger, fear, and frustration are prime examples of
versus exploitative leadership and which conse- high arousal/negatively valenced emotions, while
quences result from abusive supervision versus sadness or boredom is described as low. As the
exploitative leadership—remains to be examined. current work centers around destructive forms of
Enabling empirical research to specify this process leadership, we will focus exclusively on negative
necessitates a theoretical model that allows for the valence in the following and omit theoretical con-
deduction of differential hypotheses on the pro- siderations on positively valenced emotions.
cesses underlying abusive supervision versus When aiming to understand the emergence
exploitative leadership. Such a model should of both leader and follower behavior, assigning
describe (1) which antecedents lead to specifically a central role to arousal seems intuitive if
246 Organizational Psychology Review 13(3)

drawing inspiration from aggression research Most evidence focuses on anger (Kant et al.,
based on the following basic mechanism. 2013; Mawritz et al., 2014), anxiety (Byrne
Behaviors are usually preceded by high versus et al., 2014; Mawritz et al., 2014), hostility
low arousal affective states and this link is mod- (Garcia et al., 2014; Liang et al., 2016), and
erated by individual characteristics and context. other unspecified negative affectivity in
For instance, anger is the archetypical—almost general (Eissa et al., 2019; Gabler et al., 2014;
formative—high arousal emotion preceding Naeem et al., 2019; Pan & Lin, 2018; Walsh
reactive aggression (Wilkowski & Robinson, & Arnold, 2018). Single studies have, further,
2010). This does not, however, mean that linked irritation (Pundt & Schwarzbeck, 2018)
anger necessarily leads to reactive aggression. and frustration (Eissa & Lester, 2017) on the
On the one hand, their link is moderated by side of the leader to destructive leadership beha-
the personal characteristics of the actor; for viors. Notably, the affective states studied have
instance, personality traits such as high neuroti- been—as far as empirical papers have provided
cism, low agreeableness, and low emotional sufficient information to allow for categorization
intelligence intensify the effect of anger on —high arousal states and were mostly, while
aggression (García-Sancho et al., 2017). On not exclusively, investigated in the context of
the other hand, contextual variables moderate abusive supervision. However, empirical evi-
the process of anger inducing aggression; for dence needs to be extended toward other forms
instance, a long time period between the anger- of destructive leadership, such as exploitative
inducing event and the opportunity to act leadership. Furthermore, future research should
enables a high arousal affect to calm and, include arousal explicitly—in addition to very
thus, weakens the anger aggression link general affectivity—in order to discover its medi-
(Bushman et al., 2001; Joireman et al., 2003; ating role in the effect of perceived goal-blockage
Wang et al., 2020b). In a different example, a on the emergence of destructive leadership.
very supportive environment or peer-group Arousal and follower consequences. On the
can equally weaken the anger aggression link other hand, affective states within followers
(Levendosky et al., 2002; O’Leary et al., have been studied as consequences of destruc-
2007; Swartout, 2013). tive forms of leadership. For instance, elevated
Based on the described mechanisms, arousal levels of high arousal states in followers such
is an interesting variable to introduce into the as anger (Ferris et al., 2016; Khan, 2020;
conceptualization of why a leader displays a Mitchell et al., 2015; Peng et al., 2019;
certain behavior and why a follower reacts to Priesemuth & Schminke, 2019; Simon et al.,
a leader behavior in one way rather than 2015), anxiety (Huang et al., 2019b; Pyc et al.,
another. So far, arousal has not played a funda- 2017; Tepper, 2000; Tepper et al., 2007), and
mental role in research on destructive forms of frustration (Avey et al., 2015; Harris et al.,
leadership as usually, it is not directly assessed. 2013a; Harris et al., 2013b) have been linked
However, indirectly the concept has already with destructive forms of leadership. Elevated
permeated the field. Although not in the levels of low arousal affective states including
context of differentiating abusive supervision insecurity (Li et al., 2020a; Otto et al., 2018;
versus exploitative leadership nor active van Prooijen & de Vries, 2016; Wang et al.,
versus passive follower behavior, it is the case 2019), shame (Fatima et al., 2020; Peng et al.,
that emotions characterized by different states 2019), and depression (Bortolon et al., 2019)
of autonomic arousal have been studied in have been associated with destructive leadership
research on destructive leadership before. equally. The field’s task is now to establish links
Arousal and leader antecedents. On the one of these affective states empirically with specific
hand, leader affective states have been investi- forms of destructive leadership and active versus
gated as antecedents of destructive leadership. passive follower behaviors.
Emmerling et al. 247

As outlined, the field can present initial evi- Rooted in the arousal theory of behavior—
dence for the theoretically central role arousal stating that arousal level can predict behavior
and affective states play in the emergence of differentially (Andrew, 1974; Herman et al.,
leader and follower behavior. The emerging 2018)—this model specifies as a first step how
picture describes the complex dyadic process of high versus low arousal leads to abusive super-
(1) leader arousal mediating the effect of leader vision versus exploitative leadership and subse-
antecedents on abusive supervision versus quently, active versus passive follower behavior
exploitative leadership and (2) follower arousal in reaction to those leadership behaviors. While
mediating the effect of destructive leadership focusing on the dyad of leader-follower behav-
on active versus passive follower behavior. ior, the model provides theoretical anchoring of
We have outlined the development of a individual and contextual levels of analysis.
process perspective on destructive forms of The proposed process model is then outlined
leadership. Leader antecedents, in particular in detail and concrete propositions regarding
perceived goal-blockage, are triggering the exemplary moderations are deduced.
emergence of destructive forms of leadership, With respect to antecedents, the model
which in turn lead to negative active versus assumes that abusive supervision versus exploit-
passive follower behavior as consequences. ative leadership is caused by perceived goal-
We have highlighted how focusing on two spe- blockage on the side of the leader and that this
cific forms of leadership (i.e., abusive supervi- effect is mediated by leader arousal. So far, no
sion versus exploitative leadership) enables framework has aimed to explain how perceived
the utilization of concepts from aggression goal-blockage triggers one specific type of
research (i.e., reactive versus proactive aggres- destructive leadership behavior versus another.
sion) and their underlying processes. We have The development of our process perspective is
introduced arousal as a central mediating con- rooted in the theoretical parallels between, on
struct and have reviewed evidence on leader the one hand, abusive and reactive supervision
affective states having been studied as antece- and, on the other hand, exploitative leadership
dents and follower affective states as conse- and proactive aggression. Thus, we propose
quences of destructive leadership behaviors. leaders’ immediate affective reaction to per-
The evidence outlined suggests that high and low ceived goal-blockage as the central mediating
arousal negative affect plays a crucial role in both factor predicting the hot versus cold in destruc-
the emergence of abusive supervision versus tive leadership. A range of emotions and
destructive leadership, as well as active versus arousal states have been studied in the field of
passive follower behavior. In the following, we destructive leadership including anger, frustra-
present a model which specifies the processes dis- tion, and boredom. Fundamentally, these affect-
tinguishing between abusive supervision and ive states can be classified into high arousal
exploitative leadership based on leader antecedents negative affect (e.g., anger, frustration) and low
and follower consequences, focusing on the medi- arousal negative affect (e.g., sadness, boredom).
ating role of arousal. If a leader’s immediate affective reaction to
perceived goal-blockage is high in arousal
A process model on arousal and (e.g., he/she is angry), it is very likely that this
will lead to a “hot” or impulsive type of destruc-
destructive leadership tive leadership behavior, such as, for instance,
Based on the theoretical considerations outlined abusive supervision; an angry leader might
and evidence subsequently reviewed, we react with overtly aggressive behavior toward
present a model on the process underlying the employees. The link between high arousal
antecedents and consequences of abusive super- negative affect, for instance anger, and reactive
vision versus exploitative leadership (Figure 1). aggression is intuitive and backed up by ample
248 Organizational Psychology Review 13(3)

Figure 1. Process model differentiating antecedents and consequences of abusive supervision and
exploitative leadership.

evidence. The conceptualization of the anger- (hot emotions leading to proactive aggression),
aggression link dates way back (Averill, 1983) ample evidence points at the association of, for
and numerous theories have summarized the instance, boredom as a specific example of a
vast amount of empirical evidence supporting low arousal affective reaction and destructive
the causal link between anger and reactive behavior. Self-report studies have also found
aggression (Berkowitz, 2012; Lochman et al., positive associations between trait boredom
2010; Roberton et al., 2012; Wyckoff, 2016), and aggression in college (Dahlen et al., 2004;
including biophysiological models (Achterberg Rupp & Vodanovich, 1997; Wilson & Scarpa,
et al., 2016; Hortensius et al., 2012). Thereby, 2013), community (Pfattheicher et al., 2020;
anger has been linked specifically with react- Stephens & Sullman, 2015; van Tilburg et al.,
ive not proactive aggression (White & Turner, 2019), and clinical samples (Cao & An, 2020;
2014). The phenomenon that high arousal Isacescu & Danckert, 2018). Interviews with
negative affect increases the likelihood of reac- “internet trolls” identified boredom as the
tively destructive behavior is widely studied prime motivator for harmful online behavior
and respective findings organically translate (Shachaf & Hara, 2010); this is especially inter-
into the assumption that, in the context of lead- esting in this context as trolling is defined as a
ership, high arousal reactions to perceived proactive rather than reactive destructive behavior.
goal-blockage also facilitate abusive supervi- Pfattheicher et al. (2020) demonstrated a positive
sion specifically. association between self-reported boredom prone-
If, on the other hand, a leader’s immediate ness and aggression tendencies in online, military,
affective reaction to perceived goal-blockage and parenting contexts. They, furthermore, demon-
is low in arousal (e.g., boredom), it is very strated that boredom proneness correlates with sad-
likely that this will lead to a “cold” or controlled istic fantasies. Boredom is a low arousal and
destructive leadership behavior, such as, for aversive emotion, resulting from wanting but
instance, exploitative leadership. While the being unable to engage in satisfying activities;
link between low arousal and exploitation feeling bored makes people seek out activities
might seem less intuitive than its counterpart that will end their boredom, irrespective of
Emmerling et al. 249

associated costs (Westgate & Wilson, 2018). destructive leadership—mediates, or in other


Boredom motivates people to seek out new words, influences which follower behavior will
experiences, even when those experiences are be displayed. Thereby, high arousal negative
negative (Bench & Lench, 2019). These experi- affect will lead to reactively destructive and there-
ences are very often identified as the proactive, fore active or impulsive behaviors. This link can
deliberate choice to harm others. Following this be deduced from models in the context of aggres-
evidence and argument, our model assumes sion research derived from the anger-aggression
that a bored leader might react by deliberately link widely studied (Achterberg et al., 2016;
planning the exploitation of employees, and Averill, 1983; Berkowitz, 2012; Hortensius et al.,
therefore, that a low arousal affective reaction 2012; Lochman et al., 2010; Roberton et al.,
leads to exploitative leadership. 2012; White & Turner, 2014; Wyckoff, 2016).
If, on the other hand, a follower experiences
Proposition I: The leader’s affective reaction a controlled destructive leadership behavior (e.g.,
to perceived goal-blockage impacts the result- exploitative leadership) the follower’s immediate
ing destructive leadership behavior; high affective reaction might be, for instance, of low
arousal affective reactions lead to abusive arousal (e.g., sadness) and therefore trigger less
supervision, while low arousal affective reac- impulsive or passive follower behavior (e.g., turn-
tions lead to exploitative leadership. over or reduced work engagement). The link
between low arousal negative affect and passive
With respect to consequences, the model or avoidance-oriented behaviors is well established
assumes that the effect of abusive supervision (Leonidou & Panayiotou, 2022; Leventhal, 2008)
and exploitative leadership on follower behaviors and has been found in respect to real life (Rispens
is mediated by follower arousal. Past research has & Demerouti, 2016) and experimentally induced
clearly demonstrated that manifold forms of low arousal negative affect (Alexopoulos & Ric,
destructive leadership lead to an overwhelming 2007; Lecours et al., 2013). The arousal character-
array of negative consequences. Previous models izing a negative affective reaction can even predict
have, however, not specified which leadership experiential avoidance, with low arousal predicting
behaviors lead to which consequences. As an over- high levels of avoidance (Leonidou & Panayiotou,
view of the empirical status quo of the field demon- 2022). Generally, low arousal negative affect is a
strates, the affective reactions of not only the precursor of non-clinical depression on the level
leaders but also the followers (including, e.g., of cognition (Finucane et al., 2010), behavior
anger, sadness, boredom) are of utmost importance. (Woods & White, 2005), physiology (Osotsi
We propose that the immediate affective reaction et al., 2020; Shirai & Suzuki, 2017), and of course
of followers to a specific type of destructive leader- clinical depression (Csukly et al., 2009). This phe-
ship behavior mediates their behavioral reaction. nomenon translates to the work context and moti-
If, on the one hand, a follower experiences vates our model’s assumption that low arousal
an impulsive destructive leadership behavior negative affective reaction to any form of destructive
(e.g., abusive supervision) the follower’s imme- leadership leads to passive and avoidance-oriented
diate affective reaction might be, for instance, behaviors on the side of the followers.
of high arousal (e.g., anger) and, thus, trigger
“hot” or impulsive follower behavior (e.g., Proposition II: The follower’s affective reac-
aggression toward co-workers). In certain tion to destructive leadership behaviors
ways, the follower consequences develop simi- impacts resulting follower behaviors; high
larly to how reactively destructive leadership arousal affective reactions lead to active
emerges. The type of arousal characterizing an behaviors such as aggression, while low
affective reaction to an aversive event—in the arousal affective reactions lead to passive
case of the follower being subjected to behaviors such as withdrawal.
250 Organizational Psychology Review 13(3)

The proposed model emphasizes that leader- if—for instance—high self-control on the side
ship is an inherently recursive process. Active of the actor or a resourceful environment
and passive follower behavior recursively come into play. Consequently, understanding
impact a leader’s perceived goal-blockage. the moderation effects is one key to ultimately
Follower behaviors such as, for instance, devi- reducing destructive behaviors, including
ance or withdrawal, directly affect if and to destructive leadership.
what degree leaders might perceive their goal Over the course of the following, we will
as blocked. If a follower reacts to destructive briefly present an overview on the type of indi-
leadership with aggression against co-workers, vidual leader-/follower-related and contextual
he/she will harm the team coherence and variables that have been studied in the field.
thereby, performance; the resulting low per- We will then deduce exemplarily concrete pro-
formance will impact the leader’s future goal positions focusing on single individual and con-
of delivering an outstanding team performance textual moderators on both the side of the
for, e.g., a product launch to his name. Similar antecedents, as well as the consequences of
scenarios would also arise when more passive destructive leadership.
follower behaviors, such as withdrawal or turn- When it comes to leaders, manifold leader
over, are also at play. The leadership process is, characteristics have been studied. For instance,
a variety of personality variables and traits such
therefore, a loop, where outcomes of one
as low agreeableness (Breevaart & de Vries,
destructive leadership behavior can trigger or
2017), low competence (Meglich et al., 2019),
be the antecedents of the next.
high entitlement (Whitman et al., 2013), high
The process described does not emerge in
machiavellianism (Kiazad et al., 2010; Lyons
isolation but is embedded in a complex
et al., 2019; Wisse & Sleebos, 2016), high nar-
system of interactions across all levels of ana-
cissism (Lyons et al., 2019; Nevicka et al.,
lysis involved in leadership (i.e., leaders, fol-
2018), low neural executive control (Waldman
lowers, organization/environment). More
et al., 2018), high perfectionism (Guo et al.,
precisely, the arousal-behavior link is not abso-
2020), and high psychopathy (Lyons et al.,
lute, but subject to moderation effects. While 2019; Mathieu & Babiak, 2016; Wisse &
arousal itself is an automatic immediate reaction Sleebos, 2016) have been found to relate to
to an experience (Reisenzein, 1994), its behav- destructive leadership. When it comes to com-
ioral outcome is not. A high/low arousal nega- promised mental health, manifold aspects includ-
tive affect does neither immediately, nor ing alcohol abuse (Byrne et al., 2014),
automatically, nor under every circumstance depression (Byrne et al., 2014; Dionisi &
lead to a proactive/reactive behavior such as Barling, 2019; Tepper et al., 2006), emotional
abusive supervision versus exploitative leader- exhaustion (Fan et al., 2020; Walsh & Arnold,
ship, or active versus passive negative follower 2018), psychological distress (Y. Li et al.,
behavior. Instead, how affect impacts behavior 2016), low sleep quality (Barnes et al., 2015;
is moderated by individual and contextual vari- Tariq et al., 2020), and elevated stress levels
ables. On the one hand, this is bad news when it (Burton et al., 2012; Li et al., 2020b;
comes to reinforcing variables; negative behav- Shillamkwese et al., 2020) have been empirically
ior resulting from negative affect can be linked to increased destructive leadership.
enhanced, if—for instance—an actor’s prob- Furthermore, low self-control (Mawritz et al.,
lematic personality traits or contextual stressors 2017; Yam et al., 2016) and a history of abuse
add up. On the other hand, such moderation (Garcia et al., 2014; Kiewitz et al., 2012) have
effects can also be good news when it comes been associated with destructive leadership beha-
to mitigating variables; negative behavior viors. It is crucial to integrate these individual
resulting from negative affect can be reduced, characteristics into the outlined process model.
Emmerling et al. 251

Focusing, for instance, on a leader’s personality, Proposition III (leader characteristic as


we can assume that narcissism plays a central moderator): High arousal negative affect
role in shaping the impact of leader affect on leads to more abusive supervision when
leadership behavior. Both grandiose and vulner- leader narcissism is high.
able narcissism have been linked with specific
sub-dimensions of negative behavior and aggres- Exploitative leadership, in our process
sion (Lobbestael et al., 2014). model, is preceded by low arousal negative
Our process model states that high arousal affective reaction to perceived goal-blockage.
negative affect in response to perceived goal- This path is equally affected by individual mod-
blockage leads to elevated levels of abusive erators including personality traits. Psychopathy,
supervision. This path is not unalterable. A for instance, is the most prominent personality
leader high in narcissistic traits will exhibit trait (in extreme expression: personality disorder)
much higher level of abusive supervision com- that has been connected with proactive aggres-
pared to a leader with few or no narcissistic sion exclusively, and therefore with strategic
traits. Several mechanisms theoretically fuel and premeditated negative behavior (Cima &
this moderation effect. If, on the one hand, per- Raine, 2009). Low arousal affective reaction to
ceived goal-blockage and the subsequent high aversive events predicts both psychopathy and
arousal affective reaction meet authority, super- the likelihood of proactive aggression (Raine
iority, self-admiration, and entitlement, the et al., 2014; Woodworth & Porter, 2002).
escalation toward negative behavior will be When accompanied by high intelligence—pre-
accelerated. On the one hand, a leader high in sumably prevalent in individuals reaching high
narcissistic traits might interpret the fact that a rank positions—, psychopathy can favor highly
goal was not achieved as being down to the mis- functional behavior in a competitive context
conduct of the employees and view it as a personal and is widely discussed in leadership research
attack paralleling the well-established concept of (see “snakes in suits” phenomenon; Babiak et
threatened egotism in narcissism literature al., 2007 ). A leader might perceive the goal of
(Baumeister et al., 2000). The leader might, then, career advancement as blocked and react with a
completely overlook the complex root causes low arousal affective reaction (e.g., boredom
which might have caused the misconduct (e.g., due to too little excitement). When accompanied
refusal to work due to cognitive overload) and by psychopathic traits, a likely response to this
attempt to solve the problem by lashing out at scenario could be the premeditated exploitation
employees (i.e., abusive supervision). On the of employees without giving credit which
other hand, when perceived goal-blockage and enhances the whole department’s performance,
the associated negative affect develop in an indi- thereby, solely fostering the leader’s excitement
vidual who is self-critical, has a grounded sense and career goals (i.e., exploitative leadership).
of self-esteem, and a healthy notion of being a fal- In contrast, when accompanied by low psycho-
lible human being (e.g., exposes few or no charac- pathic traits the level of exploitative leadership
teristics of grandiose narcissism), dynamics might as an outcome of this scenario will likely
be shifted toward finding more constructive solu- decrease, because the leader will be less caught
tions for affective tension. Specifically, a leader up in self-referential, sensation-seeking thought
might ask employees for 360-degree feedback on cycles and be more able to empathically and con-
why a goal was not achieved and consequently structively react to employees.
develop constructive ways to move forward and
avoid similar scenarios in the future (e.g., under- Proposition IV (leader characteristic as
stand that the task exceeded the employees’ cogni- moderator): Low arousal negative affect
tive capacity and either change it or provide further leads to more exploitative leadership when
resources). leader psychopathic traits are high.
252 Organizational Psychology Review 13(3)

When it comes to followers, characteristics ideation (Liu et al., 2019b) have been associated
such as less commitment (Aryee et al., 2007; with destructive leadership. Similarly, attribution
Burris et al., 2008; Caesens et al., 2019; ; styles in favor of the leader or the environment
Luu, 2018, 2019; Tepper, 2000; Tillman et al., (Chen & Liu, 2019; Yagil, 2005), a tendency
2018; Schmid et al., 2019; Yan et al., 2020; toward cognitive avoidance strategies (Bortolon
Yu et al., 2016; Zhao et al., 2018) and low self- et al., 2019; Simon et al., 2015), and surface
esteem (Akhtar & Shaukat, 2016; Ashforth, acting (Adams & Webster, 2013; Bortolon et al.,
1997; Bernardo et al., 2018; Farh & Chen, 2019; Carlson et al., 2012; Chi et al., 2018; Liang
2014; Fatima et al., 2020; Haggard & Park, et al., 2018; Perko et al., 2017; Wu & Hu, 2013)
2018; Hobman et al., 2009; Jian et al., 2012; have also been linked to destructive leadership.
Rafferty & Restubog, 2011; Shen et al., 2019; Referring to our process model, individual
Vogel & Mitchell, 2017; Wongleedee, 2020) characteristics on the side of the follower crucially
have been found to result from destructive lead- shape the impact that the affective reaction to a
ership behaviors. A variety of personality vari- leadership behavior has on that follower’s behav-
ables and traits including follower narcissism ior. Consider, for instance, the attribution style of
(Wang & Jiang, 2014), low need for auton- a follower as a crucial moderating factor of high
omy/competence/relatedness (Liu et al., arousal negative affect triggering active follower
2019a), low vigor (Kirrane et al., 2019; Yan behavior (e.g., aggression). Ample research has
shown that ambiguous or even negative behavior
et al., 2020), and low vitality (Nyberg et al.,
has very variable effects in its recipient based on
2011) have been linked empirically to destruc-
attribution processes (Lobbestael et al., 2013). If
tive leadership both as antecedents and conse-
a leader behaves impulsively and shouts at an
quences. Manifold aspects of compromised
employee in front of colleagues (i.e., abusive
mental health including increased substance
supervision), this can trigger a high arousal nega-
abuse (Bamberger & Bacharach, 2006), depres-
tive affect (e.g., anger) within that employee.
sion (Duffy et al., 2006; Pyc et al., 2017;
Based on the employee’s attribution style,
Tepper, 2000; Tepper et al., 2007), emotional
various outcomes are conceivable. While a
exhaustion (Akram et al., 2019; Al-Hawari hostile actor-based attribution style (e.g., “the
et al., 2020b; Allen et al., 2016; Aryee et al., leader did this on purpose to put me down and
2008; Chi & Liang, 2013; Frieder et al., 2015; because he/she is mean”) will enhance the likeli-
Frone & Blais, 2019; Han et al., 2017; Harvey hood that the employee will act aggressively, a
et al., 2007; Huang et al., 2020; Khan, 2015; more situational attribution style (e.g., “today
Khan et al., 2010; Laila et al., 2019; Lee et al., the leader is really stressed because of the shifting
2018; Mackey et al., 2013; Molino et al., 2019; market conditions and missing resources, he/she
Nauman et al., 2018; Nevicka et al., 2018; lost their temper because it is his/her utmost
Pradhan & Jena, 2018a; Peltokorpi, 2019; goal to make us succeed as a company”) will
Tepper, 2000; Tepper et al., 2007; Sârbescu temper the employeès likelihood toward acting
et al., 2017; Scheuer et al., 2016; Stempel & with aggression.
Rigotti, 2018; Sulea et al., 2012; Wang &
Chan, 2019; Wang et al., 2016; Wu & Hu, Proposition V (follower characteristic as
2009; Wu et al., 2013; Xu et al., 2015; Yuan moderator): High arousal negative affect in
et al., 2020), elevated stress levels (Agarwal, followers leads to more active behavior
2019; Ashforth, 1997; Booth et al., 2020; (e.g., aggression) when the follower is char-
Matos et al., 2018; Nyberg et al., 2011; acterized by a hostile attribution bias.
Schmid et al., 2019; Uysal, 2019; Woestman &
Wasonga, 2015; Wu et al., 2018a; Yao et al., Contextual variables in research on destruc-
2019; Zhu & Zhang, 2019), and even suicidal tive leadership often—albeit not exclusively—
Emmerling et al. 253

refer to the organization. Variables related to processes or emotional recalibration to take


organizational norms and climate that have place. On the one hand, a leader’s high
been associated with destructive leadership arousal after seeing a product launch fail due
behaviors include aggressive norms within an to flawed engineering will likely trigger their
organization (Restubog et al., 2011), justice lashing out at subordinate engineers. Low
related constructs (Gabler & Hill, 2015; Lian arousal, in contrast, is not likely to lead to
et al., 2012; Low et al., 2019; Ramdeo & Singh, this, even if the situation were the same. The
2019; Tepper, 2000; Velez & Neves, 2017; reaction will be stronger if, for example, man-
Zellars et al., 2002), low granted autonomy agement higher up sets an ultimatum for the
(Brender-Ilan & Sheaffer, 2019), and low organiza- respective product to be introduced by a
tional ethicality (Ogunfowora, 2013). Furthermore, certain date else the entire department will be
work and task characteristics such as work over- cut. On the other hand, if a leader faces this situ-
load (Harris et al., 2013a; Molino et al., 2019), ation but knows that management will be open
missing resources (Bregenzer et al., 2019; to re-evaluate the launch date and provide any
Hochwarter & Thompson, 2012), job strain or further resources needed (e.g., increase the
tension (Harvey et al., 2007; Khan, 2015; Khan engineering work force), dynamics will differ.
et al., 2010; Mackey et al., 2013; McAllister This leader will now have the opportunity for
et al., 2018; Nahum-Shani et al., 2014), and organ- emotion regulation and be able to take a pro-
izational obstruction (Mackey et al., 2018) have verbial “deep breath” before further instructing
been found to relate to destructive leadership. the subordinate engineers, therefore decreasing
When focusing on communication and social inter- the likelihood of abusive supervision.
action within a work context, team conflict (Farh &
Chen, 2014; Gardner & Rasmussen, 2018), ostra- Proposition VI (contextual moderator):
cism, low team cohesion / cooperation (Al Zaabi High arousal negative affect leads to more
et al., 2018; Decoster et al., 2013; Priesemuth abusive supervision when time pressure
et al., 2014), and low team performance/productiv- (time until opportunity to act) is high.
ity (Al Zaabi et al., 2018; Frazier & Bowler, 2015;
Priesemuth et al., 2014) have been associated with On the side of follower behavior, contextual
destructive leadership behaviors. Adopting a variables including organizational norms and
process perspective, contextual variables moderate climate play an equally vital role. Research
both the effect of the affective state of leaders on has clearly shown that negative behavior, espe-
leadership as well as of followers on their reaction. cially aggression, is never a one-way-street;
For instance, contextual variables such as contextual norms intensify or temper the nega-
time and performance pressure can determine tive behavior (cf. to alcohol intoxication
leadership triggered by high arousal negative leading to more aggression in the presence of
affect. Previous research has shown that time aggressive norms, Parrott & Eckhardt, 2018).
to act is a central determining factor in the emer- If facing exploitation by a leader, a follower
gence of aggression (Bushman et al., 2001; might be frustrated (e.g., high arousal negative
Joireman et al., 2003; Wang et al., 2020b). If affect). According to our process model, such
a leader perceives a goal as blocked and experi- frustration can lead to active negative behavior,
ences the resulting high arousal negative affect for example, as aggression toward leaders and/
(e.g., anger), context-specific time and perform- or co-workers. This process is affected by con-
ance pressure on the side of the organization or textual moderators including aggressive norms.
the market will increase the likelihood that Within an organization characterized by aggres-
anger will lead to overt verbal aggression sive norms (e.g., a law firm with an attack-style
(e.g., abusive supervision). This effect occurs work ethic), a follower’s frustration is much
as there will be no chance for any deliberation more likely to result in high levels of aggression
254 Organizational Psychology Review 13(3)

than it would in an organization characterized intertwining theoretical aspects from both fields
by cooperativeness. In a context of aggressive seems intuitive. While the introduction of mani-
norms, a frustrated follower will gear his/her fold constructs is of merit in representing a phe-
behavior to the extent that leaders and nomenon in its complexity, it can lead to
co-workers display aggressive behavior when fragmentation of empirical evidence. Research
facing perceived goal-blockage and the result- on destructive forms of leadership has generated
ing negative affect. In a context of cooperative numerous different constructs, but has provided
norms, a follower will moderate negative next to no evidence on comparing these with
behavioral reactions to frustration by explicitly respect to antecedents and consequences (excep-
observing and implicitly sensing his leaders tion Schmid et al., 2019). While disentangling
and co-workers reverting to cooperation and the precursors and effects of the many different
communication when problems arise. constructs in the field by directly comparing
them within single empirical studies and respect-
Proposition VII (contextual moderator): ive replications is absolutely crucial, this type of
High arousal negative affect leads to more fol- work is demanding, time-consuming, and highly
lower aggression when the organizational iterative. An additional way forward could be by
climate is characterized by aggressive norms. way of theoretical considerations emphasizing
underlying integrative tendencies (Anderson &
Bushman, 2002). Aggression research, for
Discussion instance, has profited from mapping the many
expressions of aggressive behavior onto the
Theoretical and practical implications hot-versus-cold plain field, distinguishing behav-
The model and propositions outlined emphasize ior based on high versus low arousal level (i.e.,
the importance of deliberately including arousal reactive versus proactive aggression; Card &
as well as individual and contextual moderators Little, 2006; Hubbard et al., 2010; Kempes
into explanatory frameworks on the emergence et al., 2005). Thereby, these dimensions are
of abusive supervision versus exploitative lead- meant neither to be exclusive nor exhaustive;
ership. Adopting this suggested perspective on for instance, reactive and proactive forms of
destructive forms of leadership has various aggression can co-occur consecutively as well
implications on theory and practice. as simultaneously (Bushman & Anderson,
When it comes to theoretical implications, 2001; Fite et al., 2006; Poulin & Boivin, 2000;
the line of thought presented highlights how— Stanford et al., 2003). To introduce such basic
so far—detached streams of literature can be dimensions into theorizing and thereby hypothe-
combined fruitfully to develop more nuanced sizing preceding empirical work, can, however,
process descriptions. It is surprising that research bring about an overwhelmingly complex con-
on destructive leadership rarely reverts to aggres- struct space, deflect from repetitive and isolated
sion research as a source of theoretical and empirical work on single specific constructs,
empirical inspiration, although both fields share and lead to applicable insights. Focusing on the
an interest in aiming to better understand nega- hot versus cold in destructive leadership can
tive, intentionally harmful human behavior have similar benefits. Differentiating between
(Anderson & Bushman, 2002; Krasikova et al., high versus low arousal or impulsive versus
2013). Aggression in its manifold forms is fun- deliberative action when it comes to destructive
damental to the human experience (Crick & forms of leadership can help integrate the over-
Dodge, 1996) and—rooted in that perspective whelming number of constructs, extend the
—it can be argued to a certain extent, that focus beyond the striking dominance of
destructive leadership is nothing but aggressive abusive supervision as one specific form of
behavior in a leadership context. Therefore, destructive leadership in empirical work and
Emmerling et al. 255

thereby map the reality of negative leadership affect as a means of unraveling the complexity
taking the manifold involved parties and contexts of destructive leadership, its antecedents, and
based on higher ecological validity into consider- consequences.
ation. We therefore hope that spotlighting the hot When it comes to practical implications,
versus cold in destructive leadership will inspire considering the mediating role of arousal
further theorizing, opening up ways to integrate states with respect to antecedents and conse-
and streamline the diverse and valuable, thought- quences of destructive leadership opens up
fragmented evidence with respect to antecedents opportunities for both leader- and follower-
and consequences in the field. oriented interventions. Arousal is an automatic
It is important to note that the model pre- response to an event of the human autonomous
sented focuses on clear-cut high- versus low- system and as such cannot be consciously con-
arousal negative affective states. However, trolled in the acute situation. This, however,
there might be affective states involved which does not mean that arousal and thereby affective
are not that clearly defined on the arousal inten- reactions cannot be altered. Arousal is, for
sity spectrum. A prime example for this type of instance, one of the central target points of
affective state is guilt. While clearly aversive, mindfulness-based stress reduction (Bishop,
guilt cannot be defined as a high- versus low- 2002; Kabat-Zinn, 2003) and similar forms of
arousal state, but rather incorporates aspects breathing meditation (Holmes, 1984; Jones
from both sides (Weierich et al., 2010). With et al., 2018). Such techniques instruct the prac-
respect to follower consequences, Tröster & titioner to proactively learn to slow down and
Van Quaquebeke (2021) outline that victims deepen their breathing and this is often accom-
of abusive supervision might blame themselves panied by a reduction in heart rate (Lumma
and, thus, feel guilt; to resolve that guilt they et al., 2015; Wu & Lo, 2008). These strategies
invest in helping their abusive supervisor. This not only lead to a direct reduction of arousal
is in line with findings that when people feel in the acute training situation, but they also
guilty about something they have done, they are mediate the reduction of affective reaction
more likely to perform prosocial actions to eradi- (Sears & Kraus, 2009), and have been shown
cate their guilt (Xu et al., 2011). Tröster & to have long-lasting effects on arousal and
Quaquebeke’s perspective on destructive leader- affectivity beyond training (Gamaiunova et al.,
ship is an outstanding example of what we hope 2019). Meditation affects autonomic reactions
to inspire with the model presented, i.e., the devel- (i.e., arousal) to negative stimuli far more than
opment of process models enabling the explan- to positive (Pavlov et al., 2015). What it
ation of seemingly counterintuitive phenomena comes down to is that any intervention targeted
around destructive leadership (e.g., why would at reducing negative affective reactions to
an abused employee help an abusive supervisor? adverse events and increasing stress resilience
why would a bored leader revert to exploitative works via modifying affective reactions to stres-
leadership? why would followers subjected to sors. Considering the mediating role of arousal
the bad example of destructive leadership be states with respect to antecedents and conse-
aggressive themselves?). We believe that it is quences of destructive leadership therefore, is
crucial to consider the mediating role of negative a way to put the brakes hard onto the destructive
affective states as antecedents of destructive forms leadership process. Destructive leadership has
of leadership and their follower consequences. devastating consequences (Schyns & Schilling,
We therefore provide a process model on high- 2013) and, thus, its reduction is a pragmatic
versus low-arousal negative affective states and goal. One strategy for how to do so is to
hope to inspire conjointly with other similar reduce stressors which introduce negative affec-
work (Tröster & Van Quaquebeke, 2021) more tivity. Following this logic, in order to reduce
out-of-the-box thinking which utilizes negative destructive leadership, its precursor—namely
256 Organizational Psychology Review 13(3)

perceived goal-blockage—must be reduced; to et al., 2019; Gabler et al., 2014; Glasø et al.,
reduce negative follower consequences the stres- 2018; Henle & Gross, 2014; Hoobler & Hu,
sor inducing those consequences - namely the 2013; Naeem et al., 2019; Pan & Lin, 2018;
destructive leadership itself - must be reduced. Schmid et al., 2018; Walsh & Arnold, 2018;
While this approach is intuitive and should Yagil et al., 2011). We advise studying specified
always be promoted, in practice it is often unreal- affective states which vary in arousal, because
istic. Some leaders face continuous perceived variance in arousal and affect is theoretically rele-
goal-blockage just as some followers simply vant when it comes to explaining different pro-
deal with destructive leaders. A low-threshold, cesses underlying leadership. As specified in the
pragmatic way of intervening—especially when model presented, different states of arousal in
removing the stressor is not (currently/yet) a con- leaders lead to different leadership behaviors
crete option—is to train both leaders and fol- (e.g., abusive supervision versus exploitative lead-
lowers to increase their resilience to adverse ership), and different states of arousal in followers
events by reducing their negative affective reac- lead to different follower consequences in response
tion. Mindfulness training and other forms of to those leadership behaviors (e.g., active versus
meditation for stress reduction have successfully passive behaviors). Affect is defined not only
been applied in work contexts (Hafenbrack, across one but two axes (Barrett, 1998;
2017; Heckenberg et al., 2018). While this Kensinger, 2004) and research on destructive lead-
approach might not be entirely novel, its applica- ership therefore needs to account not only for
tion in the context of destructive leadership finds valence (positive / negative), but also for arousal
theoretical fuel in the proposed process model level. Without doing so, investigated processes
focusing on the mediating role of arousal and, remain unspecific.
furthermore, enables a much more optimistic Secondly, research on destructive forms of
and practicable vision of how to reduce the wide- leadership needs to advance the measurement
spread adverse effects of destructive leadership. of affective states and arousal by including
evidence-based self-report tools and physi-
ology. Once the theoretical relevance of affect-
Directions for future research ive states and arousal for the emergence and
The arousal-focused model presented aims at impact of different destructive leadership beha-
inspiring future research to include arousal viors has been recognized, it is worth carefully
and affect more frequently and deliberately assessing how to measure them. Ample work
and moreover, to tie it in theoretically to inves- has been done defining crucial issues with
tigations on the emergence and impact of differ- emotion measurement and evidence-based
ent destructive leadership behaviors. The task is assessment strategies (Larsen & Fredrickson,
to provide empirical evidence not only with 1999; Rogers, & Robinson, 2014). Arousal is
respect to the propositions outlined above, but —per definition (Reisenzein, 1994)—a physio-
also to inspire newly generated hypotheses logical state. We can assess physiological states
rooted in the process perspective provided. To indirectly based on self-reporting (Walden
succeed, future work can profit from following et al., 2003). For instance, the self-assessment
some guidelines. manikin (Bradely & Lang, 1994; Nabizadeh
Firstly, affective states in research on antece- Chianeh et al., 2012) has been proven a valid,
dents and consequences of destructive forms of reliable, and practical tool with which to two-
leadership should be specified with respect to dimensionally assess acute affective states based
the underlying state of arousal. When it comes on valence and arousal. Such a measure could be
to studying affective states, research on destruc- included effortlessly into classic study designs in
tive forms of leadership often examines unspeci- leadership research. While the value of self-report
fied negative affectivity (Brees et al., 2016; Eissa measures and the practicability of their application
Emmerling et al. 257

should never be underestimated, physiological mea- consequences in order to manage and counter-
sures are self-evidently most suited to measure auto- act devastating outcomes. It is for this reason
nomic arousal. Heart rate (Wascher, 2021), heart that research on negative and deviant behavior,
rate variability (Nardelli et al., 2015), and electro- including delinquency and crime (Katsiyannis
dermal activity/skin-conductance/galvanic skin et al., 2013; Leschied et al., 2008; Murray &
response (Keil et al., 2008) are direct indicators of Farrington, 2010), traditionally calls for longi-
autonomic arousal which should be utilized in tudinal and experimental approaches. With our
research on destructive leadership. Admittedly, model, we hope to encourage more time-lagged
assessing physiological measures requires more and longitudinal studies investigating the time-
effort than recording self-reporting. Nevertheless, line of the leadership process to be undertaken.
these methods have slowly but surely found their Such approaches need to be intensified, as longi-
way into research on (constructive) leadership tudinal research is crucial to better understanding
(Hoogeboom et al., 2021; Waller et al., 2017) a complex and dynamic social phenomenon such
and evidence accumulated up till now has con- as destructive leadership. Future research should
tributed to elaborate and practical guidelines on focus on the dyadic process underlying destruc-
their application in the context of organizational tive forms of leadership (leader antecedents—
research (Christopoulos et al., 2019; Massaro & leadership—follower consequences), while not
Pecchia, 2019). neglecting the recursivity of this process (fol-
Thirdly, research on destructive forms of lower behavior recursively affects leader’s per-
leadership needs to enable predictive and ceived goal-blockage) and the context in which
causal claims. To date, the overwhelming dom- the process evolves (e.g., organizational modera-
inance of correlational study designs in research tors). Destructive leadership—similar to aggres-
on destructive leadership implies that the bulk sion—is never about one single individual
of currently available evidence—although showing one single behavior in one single situ-
very insightful and increasingly based on ation. Different leaders interact with different fol-
complex analysis techniques—neither enables lowers, showing various destructive behaviors
predictive nor causal claims. Longitudinal within and across situations. Furthermore, the
studies (enabling predictive claims; Arjas & multilevel perspective should not be overlooked
Eerola, 1993) and even more so experimental as within an organization several units (e.g.,
studies (enabling causal claims; Podsakoff & teams) under various hierarchically organized
Podsakoff, 2019) are still scarce. The lack of leaders interact and both followers and leaders
evidence enabling predictive and causal claims might be members of more than one unit.
not only highlights a generic gap often faced While the identification of correlational rela-
by social science (for leadership research in tionships between constructs (e.g., leadership con-
general see Avey et al., 2008), but creates a structs, antecedents and/or consequences) is a
problem specific to the research of destructive major empirical task, a growing field must eventu-
forms of leadership. When it comes to destruc- ally translate this evidence into research designs
tive behavior, the predictive value of evidence enabling the establishment of causal links
(i.e., causal inferences on what triggers a (Antonakis et al., 2010; Fischer et al., 2017;
certain destructive behavior and on what it Martin et al., 2020). Experimental studies are a
will lead to) is of the utmost pragmatic rele- prime strategy when unraveling causal relation-
vance (Glueck & Glueck, 2013) due to the ships (Podsakoff & Podsakoff, 2019). Field experi-
immense negative impact of destructive leader- ments especially (Gonzalez-Morales et al., 2018;
ship. We need predictive evidence on the ante- Lange & Rowold, 2019) lend themselves to
cedents of destructive leadership behaviors in extending the research design portfolio toward
order to avoid those behaviors in the first that goal, although they are still rare in research
place and we need evidence on their on destructive forms of leadership. In order to
258 Organizational Psychology Review 13(3)

design experiments which can enable causal evidence in the field draws from questionnaire-
claims, specific methodological challenges must based correlational designs, we have little caus-
be addressed. While the experimental studies in ally interpretable evidence at hand. While
the field are innovative in widening the methodo- authors mostly provide sound theoretical consid-
logical portfolio of studying destructive forms of erations on where to place a construct in an
leadership, it has to be noted that they still do not empirical model, the sheer variety of theoretical
approach destructive leadership as actual behavior. ideas and model-placements for one and the
It might seem impossible to assess destructive same constructs hints toward the fact that over-
leadership behaviorally, i.e., to observe actual arching theoretical models, which enable the
destructive leadership behavior in a controlled deduction of concrete hypotheses with respect
setting. While such attempts are certainly chal- to specific leadership behaviors, are still
lenging, it needs to be stressed that other fields missing. While the critique of the field described
investigating negative social interaction (e.g., might pose problems for the comparability of
aggression research) rose to that challenge more single studies, it does not detract from the
than two decades ago (Giancola & Parrott, 2008; broader picture we aim at painting here. We
Lobbestael et al., 2020; Taylor, 1967) and could have plentiful evidence that personal and con-
serve as sources of inspiration. Field as well as textual characteristics play an important role for
laboratory experiments (Podsakoff & Podsakoff, antecedents and consequences of various forms
2019; Sieweke & Santoni, 2020) facilitate the of destructive leadership. To elevate this rather
usage of tools which not only enable a more generic statement to actionable insights, we
direct measurement of actual behavior when com- need to insert these moderators into a model
pared to questionnaires (which can only focus on which specifies concrete process paths with
perceptions of behavior), but their specific respect to specific leadership behaviors.
methods also provide direct access to cognition In this paper, we provide propositions on two
and affect. Importantly, as a single experiment individual and contextual moderators, each
usually focuses on building evidence for single or deduced from the developed process model.
a few causal links, more than with any other Future research will hit a creative streak when
approach experimental work needs to be grounded extending the logic behind our suggestions to
in a process framework. Our model enables the further moderating variables. Importantly, we
deduction of specific, directed hypotheses which aim at identifying specific categories of modera-
explicate concrete levels of measurement with tors—i.e., leader characteristics, follower charac-
respect to involved constructs. teristics, context / organizational characteristics.
Fourthly, research on destructive forms of While in this article we focus on specific exam-
leadership will profit from a theoretically ples drawn for each of these categories, there
guided implementation of individual versus are plenty more variables which can be dis-
contextual moderators into a process perspec- cussed. When it comes to leader or follower
tive. Note that—so far—knowledge of individ- characteristics, further personality traits (e.g.,
ual and contextual moderators is based on Machiavellianism, Big Five, competence), a
diverse evidence. Umbrella terms (e.g., person- broad range of mental health issues (e.g.,
ality traits or mental health) comprise manifold anxiety, borderline, addiction), or other con-
sub-constructs and similar constructs are often straints (e.g., work-family situation, previous
measured inconsistently. More inconsistency experience with destructive leadership) can
arises from the nomological characterization of play a role. When it comes to contextual / organ-
variables; the same concepts are often studied izational characteristics manifold aspects of
interchangeably as antecedents of destructive organizational climate (e.g., voice, implicit and
leadership, consequences, mediators, or modera- explicit norms and values, justice, culture), task
tors. As the overwhelming majority of empirical characteristics (e.g., demands, time pressure,
Emmerling et al. 259

importance, self-/leader-/organization-directed rele- when the construct was introduced (Tepper,


vance), or external forces (e.g., crises, market 2000); likewise “exploitative leadership” is the
developments, scarcity of resources) are relevant. construct name chosen by the original authors
Inspiration on which individual and contextual (Schmid et al., 2019). In this context, “supervi-
sion” and “leadership” refer to the same idea.
characteristics to focus on can be drawn from
This text conforms with the original terminology
both previous research on such variables in the
at the cost of linguistic consistency.
context of destructive leadership as well as promin-
2. Note: Tepper’s (2000) definition of abusive super-
ent moderators from aggression research. A focus vision deliberately excludes physically harmful
on moderators is one promising avenue which behavior which is, however, included in the defin-
could develop interventions modulating behavior ition of reactive aggression (Miller & Lynam,
—whether this might be destructive leadership or 2006). The explicit exclusion of physical aggression
negative follower behavior. from the definition of abusive leadership might
reflect the specificity of the construct to a work/lead-
ership context. In such context, the prevalence of
Conclusion physical aggression might be relatively low due to
the relatively high education of actors, the largely
Following the arguments outlined, the field can
public stage on which they act, and the highly preva-
succeed in adopting a process perspective on lent social and status norms to display “controlled”
how arousal mediates the emergence of specific behavior. These factors highly stigmatize physical
leadership behaviors (i.e., abusive supervision attacks toward others. Note that aggression research
versus exploitative leadership) and follower reac- (Little et al., 2003) emphasizes that the form in
tions to those leader behaviors (i.e., active versus which aggression manifests (i.e., verbal, physical,
follower behavior). As destructive leadership has against objects) is conceptually much less relevant
immense and widespread negative impacts, than the underlying mechanisms (e.g., reactive
research needs to continue down the path of versus proactive aggression).
better understanding its antecedents and conse-
quences. Future empirical investigations should References
extend their current focus by deliberately including Aasland, M. S., Skogstad, A., Notelaers, G., Nielsen,
arousal and affective state and by providing pre- M. B. & Einarsen, S. (2010). The prevalence of
dictive and causal evidence on the differentiation destructive leadership behaviour. British Journal
of specific leadership behaviors. Generated of Management, 21(2), 438–452. https://doi.org/
insights will open up new vistas on how to deal 10.1111/j.1467-8551.2009.00672.x
with and prevent destructive leadership in practice. Achterberg, M., van Duijvenvoorde, A. C.,
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. & Crone, E. A.
Funding (2016). Control your anger! The neural basis of
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following aggression regulation in response to negative
financial support for the research, authorship, and/ social feedback. Social Cognitive and Affective
or publication of this article: This work was sup- Neuroscience, 11(5), 712–720. https://doi.org/
ported by the European Commission, (grant number 10.1093/scan/nsv154
H2020-MSCA-IF-2017 No.798638). Adams, G. A. & Webster, J. R. (2013). Emotional regu-
lation as a mediator between interpersonal mistreat-
ORCID iD ment and distress. European Journal of Work and
Franziska Emmerling https://orcid.org/0000-0002- Organizational Psychology, 22(6), 697–710. https://
7350-7473 doi.org/10.1080/1359432X.2012.698057
Agarwal, U. A. (2019). Examining links between
Notes abusive supervision, PsyCap, LMX and outcomes.
1. Note that “abusive supervision”—as opposed to Management Decision, 57(5), 1304–1334. https://
“abusive leadership”—is the term proper coined doi.org/10.1108/MD-02-2017-0103
260 Organizational Psychology Review 13(3)

Akhtar, S. & Shaukat, K. (2016). Impact of petty Antonakis, J., Avolio, B. J. & Sivasubramaniam, N.
tyranny on alienation from work: Role of self- (2003). Context and leadership: An examination
esteem and power-distance. Global Journal of of the nine-factor full-range leadership theory
Flexible Systems Management, 17(3), 275–285. using the multifactor leadership questionnaire.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40171-016-0126-4 The Leadership Quarterly, 14(3), 261–295.
Akram, Z., Li, Y. & Akram, U. (2019). When employ- https://doi.org/10.1016/S1048-9843(03)00030-4
ees are emotionally exhausted due to abusive Antonakis, J., Bendahan, S., Jacquart, P. & Lalive, R.
supervision. A conservation-of-resources perspec- (2010). On making causal claims: A review and
tive. International Journal of Environmental recommendations. The Leadership Quarterly,
Research and Public Health, 16(18), 3300. 21(6), 1086–1120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.
https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16183300 2010.10.010
Alexopoulos, T. & Ric, F. (2007). The evaluation- Antonakis, J. & Day, D. V. (eds.). (2017). The nature
behavior link: Direct and beyond valence. of leadership. Sage Publications. https://doi.org/
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 10.4135/9781506395029.
43(6), 1010–1016. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp. Arjas, E. & Eerola, M. (1993). On predictive causal-
2006.10.017 ity in longitudinal studies. Journal of Statistical
Al-Hawari, M. A., Bani-Melhem, S. & Quratulain, S. Planning and Inference, 34(3), 361–386. https://
(2020a). Abusive supervision and frontline employ- doi.org/10.1023/A:1009620223494
ees’ attitudinal outcomes: The multilevel effects of Aryee, S., Chen, Z. X., Sun, L.-Y. & Debrah, Y. A.
customer orientation. International Journal of (2007). Antecedents and outcomes of abusive
Contemporary Hospitality Management, 32(3), supervision: Test of a trickle-down model.
1109–1129. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJCHM-06-2019- Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(1), 191–201.
0510 https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.92.1.191
Al-Hawari, M. A., Bani-Melhem, S. & Quratulain, S. Ashforth, B. E. (1994). Petty tyranny in organiza-
(2020b). Do frontline employees cope effectively tions. Human Relations, 47(7), 755–778. https://
with abusive supervision and customer incivility? doi.org/10.1177/001872679404700701
Testing the effect of employee resilience. Journal Ashforth, B. E. (1997). Petty tyranny in organizations:
of Business and Psychology, 35(2), 223–240. A preliminary examination of antecedents and con-
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-019-09621-2 sequences. Canadian Journal of Administrative
Allen, D. G., Peltokorpi, V. & Rubenstein, A. L. (2016). Sciences / Revue Canadienne Des Sciences de
When “embedded” means “stuck”: Moderating L’Administration, 14(2), 126–140. https://doi.org/
effects of job embeddedness in adverse work envir- 10.1111/j.1936-4490.1997.tb00124.x
onments. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101(12), Aubrey, D. W. (2012). The effect of toxic leadership.
1670–1686. https://doi.org/10.1037/apl0000134 ARMY WAR COLL CARLISLE BARRACKS
Al Zaabi, H. H., Elanain, H. M. A. & Ajmal, M. M. PA. https://doi.org/10.3390/soc11020054.
(2018). Impact of toxic leadership on work out- Averill, J. R. (1983). Studies on anger and aggres-
comes: An empirical study of public banks in sion: Implications for theories of emotion.
the UAE. International Journal of Public Sector American Psychologist, 38(11), 1145–1160.
Performance Management, 4(3), 373. https://doi. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.38.11.1145
org/10.1504/IJPSPM.2018.093470 Avey, J. B., Luthans, F. & Mhatre, K. H. (2008). A call
Anderson, C. A. & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human for longitudinal research in positive organizational
aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, behavior. Journal of Organizational Behavior: The
53(1), 27–51. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev. International Journal of Industrial, Occupational
psych.53.100901.135231 and Organizational Psychology and Behavior,
Andrew, R. J. (1974). Arousal and the causation of 29(5), 705–711. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.517
behaviour. Behaviour, 51(3-4), 135–164. https:// Avey, J. B., Wu, K. & Holley, E. (2015). The influ-
doi.org/10.1163/156853974(00174 ence of abusive supervision and job
Emmerling et al. 261

embeddedness on citizenship and deviance. culture detachment. Social Indicators Research,


Journal of Business Ethics, 129(3), 721–731. 139(1), 277–292. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-014-2192-x 016-1446-7
Bamberger, P. A. & Bacharach, S. B. (2006). Abusive Bishop, S. R. (2002). What do we really know about
supervision and subordinate problem drinking: mindfulness-based stress reduction? Psychosomatic
Taking resistance, stress and subordinate personal- Medicine, 64(1), 71–83. https://doi.org/10.1097/
ity into account. Human Relations, 59(6), 723– 00006842-200201000-00010
752. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018726706066852 Bligh, M. C., Kohles, J. C., Pearce, C. L. & Justin, J.
Barnes, C. M., Lucianetti, L., Bhave, D. P. & E., (Gene) & Stovall, J. F. (2007). When the
Christian, M. S. (2015). “You wouldn’t like me romance is over: Follower perspectives of aversive
when I’m sleepy”: Leaders’ sleep, daily abusive leadership. Applied Psychology, 56(4), 528–557.
supervision, and work unit engagement. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2007.00303.x
Academy of Management Journal, 58(5), 1419– Booth, J. E., Shantz, A., Glomb, T. M., Duffy, M. K.
1437. https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2013.1063 & Stillwell, E. E. (2020). Bad bosses and self-
Barrett, L. F. (1998). Discrete emotions or dimen- verification: The moderating role of core self-
sions? The role of valence focus and arousal evaluations with trust in workplace management.
focus. Cognition & Emotion, 12(4), 579–599. Human Resource Management, 59(2), 135–152.
https://doi.org/10.1080/026999398379574 https://doi.org/10.1002/hrm.21982
Bass, B. M. (1985a). Leadership and performance Bortolon, C., Lopes, B., Capdevielle, D., Macioce, V. &
beyond expectations. Free Press. https://doi.org/ Raffard, S. (2019). The roles of cognitive avoidance,
10.1002/hrm.3930250310. rumination and negative affect in the association
Bass, B. M. (1985b). Leadership: Good, better, best. between abusive supervision in the workplace and
Organizational Dynamics, 13(3), 26–40. https:// non-clinical paranoia in a sample of workers
doi.org/10.1016/0090-2616(85)90028-2 working in France. Psychiatry Research, 271(1),
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C. & 581–589. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.
Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. 11.065
Review of General Psychology, 5(4), 323–370. Bradley, M. M. & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring
https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323 emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the
Baumeister, R. F., Bushman, B. J. & Campbell, semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy
W. K. (2000). Self-esteem, narcissism, and and Experimental Psychiatry, 25(1), 49–59. https://
aggression: Does violence result from low self- doi.org/10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9
esteem or from threatened egotism? Current Brees, J. R., Mackey, J., Martinko, M. J. & Harvey, P.
Directions in Psychological Science, 9(1), 26– (2014). The mediating role of perceptions of abusive
29. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00053 supervision in the relationship between personality
Bench, S. W. & Lench, H. C. (2019). Boredom as a and aggression. Journal of Leadership &
seeking state: Boredom prompts the pursuit of Organizational Studies, 21(4), 403–413. https://doi.
novel (even negative) experiences. Emotion org/10.1177/1548051813505859
(Washington, D.C.), 19(2), 242–254. https://doi. Brees, J. R., Martinko, M. J. & Harvey, P. (2016).
org/10.1037/emo0000433 Abusive supervision: Subordinate personality or
Berkowitz, L. (2012). A different view of anger: The supervisor behavior? Journal of Managerial
cognitive-neoassociation conception of the relation Psychology, 31(2), 405–419. https://doi.org/10.
of anger to aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 38(4), 1108/JMP-04-2014-0129
322–333. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21432 Breevaart, K. & de Vries, R. E. (2017). Supervisor’s
Bernardo, A. B. I., Daganzo, M. A. A. & Ocampo, HEXACO personality traits and subordinate per-
A. C. G. (2018). Abusive supervision and well- ceptions of abusive supervision. The Leadership
being of Filipino migrant workers in Macau: Quarterly, 28(5), 691–700. https://doi.org/10.
Consequences for self-esteem and heritage 1016/j.leaqua.2017.02.001
262 Organizational Psychology Review 13(3)

Bregenzer, A., Felfe, J., Bergner, S. & Jiménez, P. 108(1), 273–279. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-
(2019). How followers’ emotional stability and 295X.108.1.273
cultural value orientations moderate the impact Bushman, B. J., Baumeister, R. F. & Phillips, C. M.
of health-promoting leadership and abusive (2001). Do people aggress to improve their
supervision on health-related resources. German mood? Catharsis beliefs, affect regulation, oppor-
Journal of Human Resource Management: tunity, and aggressive responding. Journal of
Zeitschrift Für Personalforschung, 33(4), 307– Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 17–33.
336. https://doi.org/10.1177/2397002218823300 https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.1.17
Brender-Ilan, Y. & Sheaffer, Z. (2019). How do self- Byrne, A., Dionisi, A. M., Barling, J., Akers, A.,
efficacy, narcissism and autonomy mediate the Robertson, J., Lys, R., Wylie, J. & Dupré, K.
link between destructive leadership and counter- (2014). The depleted leader: The influence of
productive work behaviour. Asia Pacific leaders’ diminished psychological resources on leader-
Management Review, 24(3), 212–222. https:// ship behaviors. The Leadership Quarterly, 25(2), 344–
doi.org/10.1016/j.apmrv.2018.05.003 357. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2013.09.003
Brown, M. E. & Treviño, L. K. (2006a). Ethical lead- Cao, Q. & An, J. (2020). Boredom proneness and
ership: A review and future directions. The aggression among people with substance use dis-
Leadership Quarterly, 17(6), 595–616. https:// order: The mediating role of trait anger and impul-
doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2006.10.004 sivity. Journal of Drug Issues, 50(1), 77–88.
Brown, M. E. & Treviño, L. K. (2006b). Socialized https://doi.org/10.1177/0022042619886822
charismatic leadership, values congruence, and Card, N. A. & Little, T. D. (2006). Proactive and
deviance in work groups. Journal of Applied reactive aggression in childhood and adolescence:
Psychology, 91(4), 954–962. https://doi.org/10. A meta-analysis of differential relations with psy-
1037/0021-9010.91.4.954 chosocial adjustment. International Journal of
Burris, E. R., Detert, J. R. & Chiaburu, D. S. (2008). Behavioral Development, 30(5), 466–480.
Quitting before leaving: The mediating effects of https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025406071904
psychological attachment and detachment on Carleton, E. L., Barling, J., Christie, A. M.,
voice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(4), 912– Trivisonno, M., Tulloch, K. & Beauchamp,
922. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.93.4.912 M. R. (2016). Scarred for the rest of my career?
Burton, J. P. & Hoobler, J. M. (2011). Aggressive Career-long effects of abusive leadership on pro-
reactions to abusive supervision: The role of inter- fessional athlete aggression and task performance.
actional justice and narcissism. Scandinavian Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 38(4),
Journal of Psychology, 52(4), 389–398. https:// 409–422. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.2015-0333
doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2011.00886.x Carlson, D. S., Ferguson, M., Hunter, E. & Whitten,
Burton, J. P., Hoobler, J. M. & Kernan, M. C. (2011). D. (2012). Abusive supervision and work-family
When research setting is important: The influence conflict: The path through emotional labor and
of subordinate self-esteem on reactions to burnout. The Leadership Quarterly, 23(5), 849–
abusive supervision. Organization Management 859. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2012.05.003
Journal, 8(3), 139–150. https://doi.org/10.1057/ Carnevale, J., Huang, L. & Harms, P. (2018).
omj.2011.24 Speaking up to the “emotional vampire”: A con-
Burton, J. P., Hoobler, J. M. & Scheuer, M. L. (2012). servation of resources perspective. Journal of
Supervisor workplace stress and abusive supervi- Business Research, 91, 48–59. https://doi.org/10.
sion: The buffering effect of exercise. Journal of 1016/j.jbusres.2018.05.041
Business and Psychology, 27(3), 271–279. https:// Chen, S.-C. & Liu, N.-T. (2019). When and how vicari-
doi.org/10.1007/s10869-011-9255-0 ous abusive supervision leads to bystanders’
Bushman, B. J. & Anderson, C. A. (2001). Is it time supervisor-directed deviance: A moderated-
to pull the plug on hostile versus instrumental mediation model. Personnel Review, 48(7), 1734–
aggression dichotomy? Psychological Review, 1755. https://doi.org/10.1108/PR-09-2018-0368
Emmerling et al. 263

Chi, N.-W., Chen, Y.-C., Huang, T.-C. & Chen, S.-F. aggression: Effects of impulsiveness and sensation
(2018). Trickle-down effects of positive and negative seeking. Personality and Individual Differences,
supervisor behaviors on service performance: The 37(8), 1615–1627. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.
roles of employee emotional labor and perceived 2004.02.016
supervisor power. Human Performance, 31(1), 55– Decoster, S., Camps, J., Stouten, J., Vandevyvere, L.
75. https://doi.org/10.1080/08959285.2018.1442470 & Tripp, T. M. (2013). Standing by your organ-
Chi, S.-C. S. & Liang, S.-G. (2013). When do subor- ization: The impact of organizational identifica-
dinates’ emotion-regulation strategies matter? tion and abusive supervision on followers’
Abusive supervision, subordinates’ emotional perceived cohesion and tendency to gossip.
exhaustion, and work withdrawal. The Journal of Business Ethics, 118(3), 623–634.
Leadership Quarterly, 24(1), 125–137. https:// https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-012-1612-z
doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2012.08.006 De Hoogh, A. H. B. & Den Hartog, D. N. (2008).
Christopoulos, G. I., Uy, M. A. & Yap, W. J. (2019). The Ethical and despotic leadership, relationships
body and the brain: Measuring skin conductance with leader’s social responsibility, top manage-
responses to understand the emotional experience. ment team effectiveness and subordinates’ opti-
Organizational Research Methods, 22(1), 394–420. mism: A multi-method study. The Leadership
https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428116681073 Quarterly, 19(3), 297–311. https://doi.org/10.
Chu, L.-C. (2014). Mediating toxic emotions in the 1016/j.leaqua.2008.03.002
workplace - the impact of abusive supervision. DeRue, D. S., Nahrgang, J. D., Wellman, N. E. D. &
Journal of Nursing Management, 22(8), 953– Humphrey, S. E. (2011). Trait and behavioral the-
963. https://doi.org/10.1111/jonm.12071 ories of leadership: An integration and
Cima, M. & Raine, A. (2009). Distinct characteristics of meta-analytic test of their relative validity.
psychopathy relate to different subtypes of aggres- Personnel Psychology, 64(1), 7–52. https://doi.
sion. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(8), org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.2010.01201.x
835–840. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.031 Dionisi, A. M. & Barling, J. (2019). What happens at
Cima, M., Raine, A., Meesters, C. & Popma, A. home does not stay at home: The role of family
(2013). Validation of the Dutch Reactive and romantic partner conflict in destructive lead-
Proactive Questionnaire (RPQ): Differential cor- ership. Stress and Health, 35(3), 304–317.
relates of reactive and proactive aggression from https://doi.org/10.1002/smi.2858
childhood to adulthood. Aggressive Behavior, Duffy, M. K., Ganster, D. C., Shaw, J. D., Johnson, J. L.
39(2), 99–113. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21458 & Pagon, M. (2006). The social context of under-
Crick, N. R. & Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social mining behavior at work. Organizational Behavior
information-processing mechanisms in reactive and Human Decision Processes, 101(1), 105–126.
and proactive aggression. Child Development, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2006.04.005
67(3), 993–1002. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467- Dupré, K. E., Inness, M., Connelly, C. E., Barling, J.
8624.1996.tb01778.x & Hoption, C. (2006). Workplace aggression in
Crocker, L. & Algina, J. (1986). Introduction to teenage part-time employees. Journal of Applied
Classical and Modern Test Theory. Holt, Psychology, 91(5), 987–997. https://doi.org/10.
Rinehart and Winston. 1037/0021-9010.91.5.987
Csukly, G., Czobor, P., Szily, E., Takács, B. & Einarsen, S., Aasland, M. S. & Skogstad, A. (2007).
Simon, L. (2009). Facial expression recognition Destructive leadership behaviour: A definition
in depressed subjects: The impact of intensity and conceptual model. The Leadership Quarterly,
level and arousal dimension. The Journal of 18(3), 207–216. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.
Nervous and Mental Disease, 197(2), 98–103. 2007.03.002
https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181923f82 Eissa, G. & Lester, S. W. (2017). Supervisor role over-
Dahlen, E. R., Martin, R. C., Ragan, K. & Kuhlman, load and frustration as antecedents of abusive
M. M. (2004). Boredom proneness in anger and supervision: The moderating role of supervisor
264 Organizational Psychology Review 13(3)

personality. Journal of Organizational Behavior, Management, 41(3), 841–863. https://doi.org/10.


38(3), 307–326. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.2123 1177/0149206311434533
Eissa, G., Lester, S. W. & Gupta, R. (2019). Frieder, R. E., Hochwarter, W. A. & DeOrtentiis,
Interpersonal deviance and abusive supervision: P. S. (2015). Attenuating the negative effects of
The mediating role of supervisor negative emo- abusive supervision: The role of proactive voice
tions and the moderating role of subordinate behavior and resource management ability. The
organizational citizenship behavior. Journal of Leadership Quarterly, 26(5), 821–837. https://
Business Ethics, 166(3), 577–594. https://doi. doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2015.06.001
org/10.1007/s10551-019-04130-x Frone, M. R. & Blais, A.-R. (2019). Work fatigue in a
Fan, X., Wang, Q., Liu, J., Liu, C. & Cai, T. (2020). non-deployed military setting: Assessment, preva-
Why do supervisors abuse subordinates? Effects lence, predictors, and outcomes. International
of team performance, regulatory focus, and emo- Journal of Environmental Research and Public
tional exhaustion. Journal of Occupational and Health, 16(16), 2892. https://doi.org/10.3390/
Organizational Psychology, 59(4), 1385– ijerph16162892
1406. https://doi.org/10.1111/joop.12307 Gabler, C. B. & Hill, R. P. (2015). Abusive supervision,
Farh, C. I. C. & Chen, Z. (2014). Beyond the individual distributive justice, and work-life balance:
victim: Multilevel consequences of abusive supervi- Perspectives from salespeople and managers.
sion in teams. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99(6), Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management,
1074–1095. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037636 35(3), 247–261. https://doi.org/10.1080/08853134.
Fatima, T., Majeed, M. & Jahanzeb, S. (2020). 2015.1058167
Supervisor undermining and submissive behavior: Gabler, C. B., Nagy, K. R. & Hill, R. P. (2014).
Shame resilience theory perspective. European Causes and consequences of abusive supervision
Management Journal, 38(1), 191–203. https://doi. in sales management: A tale of two perspectives.
org/10.1016/j.emj.2019.07.003 Psychology & Marketing, 31(4), 278–293. https://
Ferris, D. L., Yan, M., Lim, V. K. G., Chen, Y. & doi.org/10.1002/mar.20694
Fatimah, S. (2016). An approach-avoidance Gagnon, J. & Rochat, L. (2017). Relationships
framework of workplace aggression. Academy of between hostile attribution bias, negative
Management Journal, 59(5), 1777–1800. https:// urgency, and reactive aggression. Journal of
doi.org/10.5465/amj.2014.0221 Individual Differences, 38(4), 211–219. https://
Finucane, A. M., Whiteman, M. C. & Power, M. J. doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000238
(2010). The effect of happiness and sadness on Gamaiunova, L., Brandt, P. Y., Bondolfi, G. & Kliegel,
alerting, orienting, and executive attention. M. (2019). Exploration of psychological mechanisms
Journal of Attention Disorders, 13(6), 629–639. of the reduced stress response in long-term meditation
https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054709334514 practitioners. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 104, 143–
Fischer, T., Dietz, J. & Antonakis, J. (2017). 151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.02.026
Leadership process models: A review and synthe- García-Sancho, E., Dhont, K., Salguero, J. M. &
sis. Journal of Management, 43(6), 1726–1753. Fernández-Berrocal, P. (2017). The personality
https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206316682830 basis of aggression: The mediating role of anger
Fite, P. J., Colder, C. R. & Pelham, W. E. Jr. (2006). and the moderating role of emotional intelligence.
A factor analytic approach to distinguish pure and Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 58(4), 333–
co-occurring dimensions of proactive and reactive 340. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12367
aggression. Journal of Clinical Child and Garcia, P. R. J. M., Restubog, S. L. D., Kiewitz, C.,
Adolescent Psychology, 35(4), 578–582. https:// Scott, K. L. & Tang, R. L. (2014). Roots run deep:
doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp3504_9 Investigating psychological mechanisms between
Frazier, M. L. & Bowler, W. M. (2015). Voice history of family aggression and abusive supervi-
climate, supervisor undermining, and work out- sion. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99(5), 883–
comes: A group-level examination. Journal of 897. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036463
Emmerling et al. 265

Gardner, D. H. & Rasmussen, W. (2018). Workplace Haggard, D. L. & Park, H. M. (2018). Perceived super-
bullying and relationships with health and perform- visor remorse, abusive supervision, and LMX.
ance among a sample of New Zealand veterinarians. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 39(10),
New Zealand Veterinary Journal, 66(2), 57–63. 1252–1267. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.2285
https://doi.org/10.1080/00480169.2017.1395715 Han, G. H., Harms, P. D. & Bai, Y. (2017).
Giancola, P. R. & Parrott, D. J. (2008). Further evi- Nightmare bosses: The impact of abusive supervi-
dence for the validity of the Taylor aggression sion on employees’ sleep, emotions, and creativ-
paradigm. Aggressive Behavior, 34(2), 214–229. ity. Journal of Business Ethics, 145(1), 21–31.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20235 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-015-2859-y
Glasø, L., Skogstad, A., Notelaers, G. & Einarsen, S. Harris, K. J., Harvey, P., Harris, R. B. & Cast, M.
(2018). Leadership, affect and outcomes: (2013a). An investigation of abusive supervision,
Symmetrical and asymmetrical relationships. vicarious abusive supervision, and their joint
Leadership & Organization Development impacts. The Journal of Social Psychology,
Journal, 39(1), 51–65. https://doi.org/10.1108/ 153(1), 38–50. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.
LODJ-08-2016-0194 2012.703709
Glueck, S. & Glueck, E. (2013). Delinquents and Harris, K. J., Lambert, A. & Harris, R. B. (2013b).
nondelinquents in perspective. Harvard HRM Effectiveness as a moderator of the relation-
University Press. https://doi.org/10.4159/harvard. ships between abusive supervision and technol-
9780674188754. ogy work overload and job outcomes for
Gonzalez-Morales, M. G., Kernan, M. C., Becker, technology end users. Journal of Applied Social
T. E. & Eisenberger, R. (2018). Defeating Psychology, 43(8), 1686–1695. https://doi.org/
abusive supervision: Training supervisors to 10.1111/jasp.12122
support subordinates. Journal of Occupational Harvey, P., Stoner, J., Hochwarter, W. & Kacmar, C.
Health Psychology, 23(2), 151–162. https://doi. (2007). Coping with abusive supervision: The
org/10.1037/ocp0000061 neutralizing effects of ingratiation and positive
Goswami, A., Nair, P. K. & Grossenbacher, M. A. affect on negative employee outcomes. The
(2015). Impact of aggressive humor on dysfunc- Leadership Quarterly, 18(3), 264–280. https://
tional resistance. Personality and Individual doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2007.03.008
Differences, 74, 265–269. https://doi.org/10. Heckenberg, R. A., Eddy, P., Kent, S. & Wright, B. J.
1016/j.paid.2014.10.037 (2018). Do workplace-based mindfulness medita-
Goswami, A., Park, H. I. & Beehr, T. A. (2019). tion programs improve physiological indices of
Does the congruence between leaders’ implicit stress? A systematic review and meta-analysis.
followership theories and their perceptions of Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 114, 62–71.
actual followers matter? Journal of Business and https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2018.09.010
Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-019- Henle, C. A. & Gross, M. A. (2014). What have I
09638-7 done to deserve this? Effects of employee person-
Guo, L., Chiang, J. T., Mao, J. & Chien, C. (2020). ality and emotion on abusive supervision. Journal
Abuse as a reaction of perfectionistic leaders: A of Business Ethics, 122(3), 461–474. https://doi.
moderated mediation model of leader perfection- org/10.1007/s10551-013-1771-6
ism, perceived control, and subordinate feedback Herman, A. M., Critchley, H. D. & Duka, T. (2018).
seeking on abusive supervision. Journal of The role of emotions and physiological arousal in
Occupational and Organizational Psychology, modulating impulsive behaviour. Biological
5, https://doi.org/10.1111/joop.12308 Psychology, 133, 30–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Hafenbrack, A. C. (2017). Mindfulness meditation as j.biopsycho.2018.01.014
an on-the-spot workplace intervention. Journal of Hobman, E. V., Restubog, S. L. D., Bordia, P. &
Business Research, 75, 118–129. https://doi.org/ Tang, R. L. (2009). Abusive supervision in advis-
10.1016/j.jbusres.2017.01.017 ing relationships: Investigating the role of social
266 Organizational Psychology Review 13(3)

support. Applied Psychology, 58(2), 233–256. https:// 2035–2054. https://doi.org/10.1108/PR-01-2019-


doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2008.00330.x 0002
Hochwarter, W. A. & Thompson, K. W. (2012). Huang, L.-C., Su, C.-H., Lin, C.-C. & Lu, S.-C.
Mirror, mirror on my boss’s wall: Engaged enact- (2019b). The influence of abusive supervision
ment’s moderating role on the relationship on employees’ motivation and extra-role beha-
between perceived narcissistic supervision and viors: The daily-basis investigation. Chinese
work outcomes. Human Relations, 65(3), 335– Management Studies, 13(3), 514–530. https://
366. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018726711430003 doi.org/10.1108/CMS-04-2018-0495
Holmes, D. S. (1984). Meditation and somatic Hubbard, J. A., Dodge, K. A., Cillessen, A. H., Coie,
arousal reduction: A review of the experimental J. D. & Schwartz, D. (2001). The dyadic nature of
evidence. American Psychologist, 39(1), 1–10. social information processing in boys’ reactive
https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.39.1.1 and proactive aggression. Journal of Personality
Hoobler, J. M. & Brass, D. J. (2006). Abusive super- and Social Psychology, 80(2), 268–280. https://
vision and family undermining as displaced doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.268
aggression. Journal of Applied Psychology, Hubbard, J. A., McAuliffe, M. D., Morrow, M. T. &
91(5), 1125–1133. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021- Romano, L. J. (2010). Reactive and proactive aggres-
9010.91.5.1125 sion in childhood and adolescence: Precursors, out-
Hoobler, J. M. & Hu, J. (2013). A model of injustice, comes, processes, experiences, and measurement.
abusive supervision, and negative affect. The Journal of Personality, 78(1), 95–118. https://doi.
Leadership Quarterly, 24(1), 256–269. https:// org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00610.x
doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2012.11.005 Isacescu, J. & Danckert, J. (2018). Exploring the rela-
Hoogeboom, M. A., Saeed, A., Noordzij, M. L. & tionship between boredom proneness and self-
Wilderom, C. P. (2021). Physiological arousal vari- control in traumatic brain injury (TBI).
ability accompanying relations-oriented behaviors Experimental Brain Research, 236(9), 2493–
of effective leaders: Triangulating skin conductance, 2505. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4674-9
video-based behavior coding and perceived effect- James, L. R. & LeBreton, J. M. (2010). Assessing
iveness. The Leadership Quarterly, 32(6), 101493. aggression using conditional reasoning. Current
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2020.101493 Directions in Psychological Science, 19(1), 30–
Hortensius, R., Schutter, D. J. & Harmon-Jones, E. 35. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721409359279
(2012). When anger leads to aggression: James, L. R. & LeBreton, J. M. (2012). Assessing the
Induction of relative left frontal cortical activity implicit personality through conditional reason-
with transcranial direct current stimulation ing. American Psychological Association.
increases the anger–aggression relationship. Javed, B., Fatima, T., Yasin, R. M., Jahanzeb, S. &
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, Rawwas, M. Y. A. (2019). Impact of abusive
7(3), 342–347. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr012 supervision on deviant work behavior: The role
Huang, J., Guo, G., Tang, D., Liu, T. & Tan, L. of Islamic work ethic. Business Ethics: A
(2019a). An eye for an eye? Third parties’ European Review, 28(2), 221–233. https://doi.
silence reactions to peer abusive supervision: org/10.1111/beer.12212
The mediating role of workplace anxiety, and Jian, Z., Kwan, H. K., Qiu, Q., Liu, Z. Q. & Yim,
the moderating role of core self-evaluation. F. H. (2012). Abusive supervision and frontline
International Journal of Environmental employees’ service performance. The Service
Research and Public Health, 16(24), 5027. Industries Journal, 32(5), 683–698. https://doi.
https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16245027 org/10.1080/02642069.2011.614338
Huang, L.-C., Lin, C.-C. & Lu, S.-C. (2020). The Joireman, J., Anderson, J. & Strathman, A. (2003). The
relationship between abusive supervision and aggression paradox: Understanding links among
employee’s reaction: The job demands-resources aggression, sensation seeking, and the consideration
model perspective. Personnel Review, 49(9), of future consequences. Journal of Personality and
Emmerling et al. 267

Social Psychology, 84(6), 1287–1302. https://doi. Psychiatry, 14(1), 11–19. https://doi.org/10.


org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.6.1287 1007/s00787-005-0432-4
Jones, D. R., Graham-Engeland, J. E., Smyth, J. M. Kensinger, E. A. (2004). Remembering emotional
& Lehman, B. J. (2018). Clarifying the associa- experiences: The contribution of valence and
tions between mindfulness meditation and arousal. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 15(4), 241–
emotion: Daily high-and low-arousal emotions 252. https://doi.org/10.1515/revneuro.2004.15.4.241
and emotional variability. Applied Psychology: Khan, S. (2020). Do workplace contextual factors
Health and Well-Being, 10(3), 504–523. https:// engender abusive supervision? Australian
doi.org/10.1111/aphw.12135 Journal of Management. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Ju, D., Huang, M., Liu, D., Qin, X., Hu, Q. & Chen, 0312896219899435
C. (2019). Supervisory consequences of abusive Khan, S. N. (2015). Abusive supervision and nega-
supervision: An investigation of sense of power, tive employee outcomes: The moderating effects
managerial self-efficacy, and task-oriented leader- of intimidation and recognition. Journal of
ship behavior. Organizational Behavior and General Management, 41(1), 61–81. https://doi.
Human Decision Processes, 154, 80–95. https:// org/10.1177/030630701504100105
doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2019.09.003 Khan, S. N., Qureshi, I. M. & Ahmad, H. I. (2010).
Jung, H. S. & Yoon, H. H. (2019). The effects of Abusive supervision & negative employee out-
social undermining on employee voice and comes. In Article in European Journal of Social
silence and on organizational deviant behaviors Sciences, 15(4).
in the hotel industry. Journal of Service Theory Kiazad, K., Restubog, S. L. D., Zagenczyk, T. J.,
and Practice, 29(2), 213–231. https://doi.org/10. Kiewitz, C. & Tang, R. L. (2010). In pursuit of
1108/JSTP-06-2018-0131 power: The role of authoritarian leadership in
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based stress the relationship between supervisors’
reduction (MBSR). Constructivism in the Machiavellianism and subordinates’ perceptions
Human Sciences, 8(2), 73. of abusive supervisory behavior. Journal of
Kant, L., Skogstad, A., Torsheim, T. & Einarsen, S. Research in Personality, 44(4), 512–519. https://
(2013). Beware the angry leader: Trait anger doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2010.06.004
and trait anxiety as predictors of petty tyranny. Kiewitz, C., Restubog, S. L. D., Shoss, M. K.,
The Leadership Quarterly, 24(1), 106–124. Garcia, P. R. J. M. & Tang, R. L. (2016).
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2012.08.005 Suffering in silence: Investigating the role of
Katsiyannis, A., Thompson, M. P., Barrett, D. E. & fear in the relationship between abusive supervi-
Kingree, J. B. (2013). School predictors of sion and defensive silence. Journal of Applied
violent criminality in adulthood: Findings from Psychology, 101(5), 731–742. https://doi.org/10.
a nationally representative longitudinal study. 1037/apl0000074
Remedial and Special Education, 34(4), 205– Kiewitz, C., Restubog, S. L. D., Zagenczyk, T. J.,
214. https://doi.org/10.1177/0741932512448255 Scott, K. D., Garcia, P. R. J. M. & Tang, R. L.
Keil, A., Smith, J. C., Wangelin, B. C., Sabatinelli, D., (2012). Sins of the parents: Self-control as a
Bradley, M. M. & Lang, P. J. (2008). Electrocortical buffer between supervisors’ previous experience
and electrodermal responses covary as a function of of family undermining and subordinates’ percep-
emotional arousal: A single-trial analysis. tions of abusive supervision. The Leadership
Psychophysiology, 45(4), 516–523. https://doi.org/ Quarterly, 23(5), 869–882. https://doi.org/10.
10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00667.x 1016/j.leaqua.2012.05.005
Kempes, M., Matthys, W., De Vries, H. & Van Kim, K. Y., Atwater, L., Latheef, Z. & Zheng, D.
Engeland, H. (2005). Reactive and proactive (2019). Three motives for abusive supervision:
aggression in children A review of theory, find- The mitigating effect of subordinates attributed
ings and the relevance for child and adolescent motives on abusive supervision’s negative out-
psychiatry. European Child & Adolescent comes. Journal of Leadership & Organizational
268 Organizational Psychology Review 13(3)

Studies, 26(4), 476–494. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Servant Leadership. Academy of Management


1548051818781816 Annals, 13(1), 148–187. https://doi.org/10.5465/
Kirrane, M., Kilroy, S. & O’Connor, C. (2019). The annals.2016.0121
moderating effect of team psychological empower- Leonidou, C. & Panayiotou, G. (2022). Can we
ment on the relationship between abusive supervi- predict experiential avoidance by measuring sub-
sion and engagement. Leadership & Organization jective and physiological emotional arousal?
Development Journal, 40(1), 31–44. https://doi. Current Psychology, 41(10), 7215–7227. https://
org/10.1108/LODJ-07-2018-0252 doi.org/10.1007/s10608-017-9856-7
Krasikova, D. V., Green, S. G. & LeBreton, J. M. Leschied, A., Chiodo, D., Nowicki, E. & Rodger, S.
(2013). Destructive leadership: A theoretical (2008). Childhood predictors of adult criminality:
review, integration, and future research agenda. A meta-analysis drawn from the prospective lon-
Journal of Management, 39(5), 1308–1338. gitudinal literature. Canadian Journal of
https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206312471388 Criminology and Criminal Justice, 50(4), 435–
Laila, U., Iqbal, S. & Rasheed, M. (2019). Abusive 467. https://doi.org/10.3138/cjccj.50.4.435
supervision and workplace deviance: The moder- Levendosky, A. A., Huth-Bocks, A. & Semel, M. A.
ating role of power distance. In Pakistan Journal (2002). Adolescent peer relationships and mental
of Commerce and Social Sciences, 13(2). health functioning in families with domestic vio-
Lange, S. & Rowold, J. (2019). Mindful leadership: lence. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent
Evaluation of a mindfulness-based leader interven- Psychology, 31, 206–218. https://doi.org/10.
tion. Gruppe. Interaktion. Organisation. Zeitschrift 1207/153744202753604485
Für Angewandte Organisationspsychologie (GIO), Leventhal, A. M. (2008). Sadness, depression, and avoid-
50(3), 319–335. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11612- ance behavior. Behavior Modification, 32(6), 759–
019-00482-0 779. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445508317167
Larsen, R. J. & Fredrickson, B. L. (1999). Measurement Li, V., Jiang, L. & Xu, X. (Violet) (2020a). From
issues in emotion research. Well-being: The workplace mistreatment to job insecurity: The
Foundations of Hedonic Psychology, 40, 60. moderating effect of work centrality. Stress and
Le, H., Schmidt, F. L., Harter, J. K. & Lauver, K. J. Health, 1050. https://doi.org/10.1002/smi.2915
(2010). The problem of empirical redundancy of Li, Y., Wang, Z., Yang, L.-Q. & Liu, S. (2016). The
constructs in organizational research: An empir- crossover of psychological distress from leaders
ical investigation. Organizational Behavior and to subordinates in teams: The role of abusive
Human Decision Processes, 112(2), 112–125. supervision, psychological capital, and team per-
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2010.02.003 formance. Journal of Occupational Health
Lecours, S., Philippe, F., Arseneault, S., Boucher, MÈ & Psychology, 21(2), 142–153. https://doi.org/10.
Ahoundova, L. (2013). Alexithymia, cognitive com- 1037/a0039960
plexity, and defensive avoidance of emotion in a situ- Li, Z., He, B., Sun, X. & Zhang, Y. (2020b).
ation of experimentally induced sadness. Journal of Demonstrating the psychological aspects of stressors
the American Psychoanalytic Association, 61(3), and abusive supervision behavior: Attainment of
573–578. https://doi.org/10.1177/0003065113490024 sustainability under the rubric of resources theory.
Lee, J. (Jessie), Wang, G. & Piccolo, R. F. (2018). Frontiers in Psychology, 11(March), 1–13. https://
Jekyll and hyde leadership: A multilevel, multi- doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00293
sample examination of charisma and abuse on fol- Lian, H., Brown, D. J., Ferris, D. L., Liang, L. H.,
lower and team outcomes. Journal of Leadership Keeping, L. M. & Morrison, R. (2014). Abusive
& Organizational Studies, 25(4), 399–415. supervision and retaliation: A self-control frame-
https://doi.org/10.1177/1548051818757692 work. Academy of Management Journal, 57(1),
Lemoine, G. J., Hartnell, C. A. & Leroy, H. (2019). 116–139. https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2011.0977
Taking stock of moral approaches to leadership: Lian, H., Ferris, D. L. & Brown, D. J. (2012). Does
An integrative review of ethical. Authentic, and taking the good with the bad make things worse?
Emmerling et al. 269

How abusive supervision and leader–member vulnerable narcissism in self-reported and labora-
exchange interact to impact need satisfaction and tory aggression and testosterone reactivity.
organizational deviance. Organizational Behavior Personality and Individual Differences, 69, 22–
and Human Decision Processes, 117(1), 41–52. 27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.05.007
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2011.10.003 Lobbestael, J., Cima, M. & Arntz, A. (2013). The
Liang, L. H., Brown, D. J., Lian, H., Hanig, S., Ferris, relationship between adult reactive and proactive
D. L. & Keeping, L. M. (2018). Righting a wrong: aggression, hostile interpretation bias, and anti-
Retaliation on a voodoo doll symbolizing an social personality disorder. Journal of
abusive supervisor restores justice. The Leadership Personality Disorders, 27(1), 53–66. https://doi.
Quarterly, 29(4), 443–456. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. org/10.1521/pedi.2013.27.1.53
leaqua.2018.01.004 Lobbestael, J., Emmerling, F., Brugman, S., Broers,
Liang, L. H., Lian, H., Brown, D. J., Ferris, D. L., N., Sack, A. T., Schuhmann, T., Bonnemayer, C.,
Hanig, S. & Keeping, L. M. (2016). Why are Benning, R. & Arntz, A. (2020). Towards a more
abusive supervisors abusive? A dual-system self- valid assessment of behavioral aggression: An
control model. Academy of Management Journal, open source platform and an empirically derived
59(4), 1385–1406. https://doi.org/10.5465/amj. scoring method for using the Competitive
2014.0651 Reaction Time Task (CRTT). Assessment, in
Liden, R. C., Wayne, S. J., Zhao, H. & Henderson, D. press. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191120959757
(2008). Servant leadership: Development of a Lochman, J. E., Barry, T., Powell, N. & Young, L.
multidimensional measure and multi-level assess- (2010). Anger and aggression. In Practitioner’s
ment. The Leadership Quarterly, 19(2), 161–177. guide to empirically based measures of social
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2008.01.006 skills (pp. 155–166). Springer. https://doi.org/10.
Little, T. D., Henrich, C. C., Jones, S. M. & Hawley, P. 1016/j.comppsych.2015.04.012.
H. (2003). Disentangling the “whys” from the Low, Y. M., Sambasivan, M. & Ho, J. A. (2019).
“whats” of aggressive behaviour. International Impact of abusive supervision on counterproduct-
Journal of Behavioral Development, 27(2), 122– ive work behaviors of nurses. Asia Pacific
133. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250244000128 Journal of Human Resources, 1744–7941.12234.
Liu, J., Kwan, H. K., Wu, L. & Wu, W. (2010). https://doi.org/10.1111/1744-7941.12234
Abusive supervision and subordinate supervisor- Lumma, A. L., Kok, B. E. & Singer, T. (2015). Is
directed deviance: The moderating role of trad- meditation always relaxing? Investigating heart
itional values and the mediating role of revenge rate, heart rate variability, experienced effort and
cognitions. Journal of Occupational and likeability during training of three types of medita-
Organizational Psychology, 83(4), 835–856. tion. International Journal of Psychophysiology,
https://doi.org/10.1348/096317909X485216 97(1), 38–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.
Liu, S., Zhu, Q. & Wei, F. (2019a). How abusive super- 2015.04.017
vision affects employees’ unethical behaviors: A Luu, T. (2019). Discretionary HR practices and
moderated mediation examination of turnover inten- employee well-being: The roles of job crafting and
tions and caring climate. International Journal of abusive supervision. Personnel Review, 49(1), 43–
Environmental Research and Public Health, 16(21), 66. https://doi.org/10.1108/PR-05-2018-0162
4187. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16214187 Lyons, B. D., Moorman, R. H. & Mercado, B. K.
Liu, Y., Gul, H., Zhang, J. & Usman, M. (2019b). (2019). Normalizing mistreatment? Investigating
Abusive supervision and suicidal ideation: The Dark Triad, LMX, and abuse. Leadership &
mediating role of basic psychological need satis- Organization Development Journal, 40(3), 369–
faction. Death Studies, 1–8. https://doi.org/10. 380. https://doi.org/10.1108/LODJ-11-2018-0408
1080/07481187.2019.1609134 Mackey, J. D., Brees, J. R., McAllister, C. P., Zorn,
Lobbestael, J., Baumeister, R. F., Fiebig, T. & Eckel, M. L., Martinko, M. J. & Harvey, P. (2018).
L. A. (2014). The role of grandiose and Victim and culprit? The effects of entitlement
270 Organizational Psychology Review 13(3)

and felt accountability on perceptions of abusive McAllister, C. P., Mackey, J. D. & Perrewé, P. L.
supervision and perpetration of workplace bully- (2018). The role of self-regulation in the relation-
ing. Journal of Business Ethics, 153(3), 659– ship between abusive supervision and job tension.
673. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-016-3348-7 Journal of Organizational Behavior, 39(4), 416–
Mackey, J. D., Ellen Ill, B. P., Hochwarter, W. A. & 428. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.2240
Ferris, G. R. (2013). Subordinate social adaptabil- McDonald, R. P. (2013). Test theory: A unified treat-
ity and the consequences of abusive supervision ment. Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/
perceptions in two samples. The Leadership 9781410601087.
Quarterly, 24(5), 732–746. https://doi.org/10. Meglich, P., Valentine, S. & Eesley, D. (2019).
1016/j.leaqua.2013.07.003 Perceptions of supervisor competence, perceived
Mackey, J. D., Frieder, R. E., Perrewé, P. L., employee mobility, and abusive supervision:
Gallagher, V. C. & Brymer, R. A. (2015). Human capital and personnel investments as
Empowered employees as social deviants: The means for reducing maltreatment in the workplace.
role of abusive supervision. Journal of Business Personnel Review, 48(3), 691–706. https://doi.org/
and Psychology, 30(1), 149–162. https://doi.org/ 10.1108/PR-08-2017-0239
10.1007/s10869-014-9345-x Miller, J. D. & Lynam, D. R. (2006). Reactive and pro-
Martin, R., Hughes, D. J., Epitropaki, O. & Thomas, active aggression: Similarities and differences.
G. (2020). In pursuit of causality in leadership Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 1469–
training research: A review and pragmatic recom- 1480. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.06.004
mendations. The Leadership Quarterly, 101375. Mitchell, M. S. & Ambrose, M. L. (2007). Abusive
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2019.101375 supervision and workplace deviance and the moder-
Massaro, S. & Pecchia, L. (2019). Heart rate variabil- ating effects of negative reciprocity beliefs. Journal
ity (HRV) analysis: A methodology for organiza- of Applied Psychology, 92(4), 1159–1168. https://
tional neuroscience. Organizational Research doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.92.4.1159
Methods, 22(1), 354–393. https://doi.org/10. Mitchell, M. S., Vogel, R. M. & Folger, R. (2015). Third
1177/1094428116681072 parties’ reactions to the abusive supervision of cow-
Mathieu, C. & Babiak, P. (2016). Corporate psychop- orkers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100(4),
athy and abusive supervision: Their influence on 1040–1055. https://doi.org/10.1037/apl0000002
employees’ job satisfaction and turnover intentions. Molino, M., Cortese, C. & Ghislieri, C. (2019).
Personality and Individual Differences, 91, 102– Unsustainable working conditions: The associ-
106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.12.002 ation of destructive leadership, use of technology,
Matos, K. & O’Neill, O., (Mandy) & Lei, X. (2018). and workload with workaholism and exhaustion.
Toxic leadership and the masculinity contest Sustainability, 11(2), 446. https://doi.org/10.
culture: How “win or die” cultures breed abusive 3390/su11020446
leadership. Journal of Social Issues, 74(3), 500– Murray, J. & Farrington, D. P. (2010). Risk factors
528. https://doi.org/10.1111/josi.12284 for conduct disorder and delinquency: Key find-
Mawritz, M. B., Folger, R. & Latham, G. P. (2014). ings from longitudinal studies. The Canadian
Supervisors’ exceedingly difficult goals and Journal of Psychiatry, 55(10), 633–642. https://
abusive supervision: The mediating effects of hin- doi.org/10.1177/070674371005501003
drance stress, anger, and anxiety. Journal of Nabizadeh Chianeh, G., Vahedi, S., Rostami, M. &
Organizational Behavior, 35(3), 358–372. Nazari, M. A. (2012). Validity and reliability of
https://doi.org/10.1002/job.1879 self-assessment manikin.
Mawritz, M. B., Greenbaum, R. L., Butts, M. M. & Naeem, M. & Weng, Q. (Derek), Ali, A. & Hameed,
Graham, K. A. (2017). I just can’t control myself: A Z. (2019). An eye for an eye: Does subordinates’
self-regulation perspective on the abuse of deviant negative workplace gossip lead to supervisor
employees. Academy of Management Journal, 60(4), abuse? Personnel Review, 49(1), 284–302.
1482–1503. https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2014.0409 https://doi.org/10.1108/PR-05-2018-0174
Emmerling et al. 271

Nahum-Shani, I., Henderson, M. M., Lim, S. & O’Leary, K., Smith Slep, A. M. & O’Leary, S. G.
Vinokur, A. D. (2014). Supervisor support: (2007). Multivariate models of men’s and
Does supervisor support buffer or exacerbate the women’s partner aggression. Journal of
adverse effects of supervisor undermining? Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 752–
Journal of Applied Psychology, 99(3), 484–503. 764. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.75.5.752
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035313 Osotsi, A., Oravecz, Z., Li, Q., Smyth, J. & Brick,
Nardelli, M., Valenza, G., Greco, A., Lanata, A. & T. R. (2020). Individualized modeling to distin-
Scilingo, E. P. (2015). Recognizing emotions guish between high and low arousal states using
induced by affective sounds through heart rate vari- physiological data. Journal of Healthcare
ability. IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing, Informatics Research, 4(1), 91–109. https://doi.
6(4), 385–394. https://doi.org/10.1109/TAFFC.2015. org/10.1007/s41666-019-00064-1
2432810 Otto, K., Thomson, B. & Rigotti, T. (2018). When dark
Nauman, S., Fatima, T. & Haq, I. U. (2018). Does leadership exacerbates the effects of restructuring.
despotic leadership harm employee family life: Journal of Change Management, 18(2), 96–115.
Exploring the effects of emotional exhaustion https://doi.org/10.1080/14697017.2018.1446691
and anxiety. Frontiers in Psychology, Ouyang, K., Lam, W. & Wang, W. (2015). Roles of
9(MAY). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018. gender and identification on abusive supervision
00601 and proactive behavior. Asia Pacific Journal of
Nevicka, B., De Hoogh, A. H. B., Den Hartog, D. N. Management, 32(3), 671–691. https://doi.org/10.
& Belschak, F. D. (2018). Narcissistic leaders and 1007/s10490-015-9410-7
their victims: Followers low on self-esteem and Padilla, A., Hogan, R. & Kaiser, R. B. (2007). The
low on core self-evaluations suffer most. toxic triangle: Destructive leaders, susceptible fol-
Frontiers in Psychology, 9(MAR). https://doi. lowers, and conducive environments. The
org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00422 Leadership Quarterly, 18(3), 176–194. https://
Novick, M. R. (1966). The axioms and principal doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2007.03.001
results of classical test theory. Journal of Palanski, M., Avey, J. B. & Jiraporn, N. (2014). The
Mathematical Psychology, 3(1), 1–18. https:// effects of ethical leadership and abusive supervi-
doi.org/10.1016/0022-2496(66)90002-2 sion on job search behaviors in the turnover
Nyberg, A., Holmberg, I., Bernin, P., Alderling, M., process. Journal of Business Ethics, 121(1), 135–
Åkerblom, S., Widerszal-Bazyl, M., Magrin, M. E., 146. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-013-1690-6
Hasselhorn, H.-M., Milczarek, M., D’Angelo, G., Pan, S.-Y. & Lin, K. J. (2018). Who suffers when
Denk, M., Westerlund, H. & Theorell, T. (2011). supervisors are unhappy? The roles of leader–
Destructive managerial leadership and psychological member exchange and abusive supervision.
well-being among employees in Swedish, Polish, Journal of Business Ethics, 151(3), 799–811.
and Italian hotels. Work, 39(3), 267–281. https:// https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-016-3247-y
doi.org/10.3233/WOR-2011-1175 Park, J. H., Carter, M. Z., DeFrank, R. S. & Deng, Q.
Ogunfowora, B. (2013). When the abuse is unevenly (2018). Abusive supervision, psychological dis-
distributed: The effects of abusive supervision tress, and silence: The effects of gender dissimi-
variability on work attitudes and behaviors. larity between supervisors and subordinates.
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 34(8), Journal of Business Ethics, 153(3), 775–792.
1105–1123. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.1841 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-016-3384-3
Ogunfowora, B., Weinhardt, J. M. & Hwang, C. Parrott, D. J. & Eckhardt, C. I. (2018). Effects of
C. (2019). Abusive supervision differenti- alcohol on human aggression. Current Opinion
ation and employee outcomes: The roles of in Psychology, 19, 1–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
envy, resentment, and insecure group attach- copsyc.2017.03.023
ment. Journal of Management. https://doi. Pavlov, S. V., Reva, N. V., Loktev, K. V., Korenyok, V.
org/10.1177/0149206319862024 V. & Aftanas, L. I. (2015). Impact of long-term
272 Organizational Psychology Review 13(3)

meditation practice on cardiovascular reactivity during Poulin, F. & Boivin, M. (2000). Reactive and pro-
perception and reappraisal of affective images. active aggression: Evidence of a two-factor
International Journal of Psychophysiology, 95(3), model. Psychological Assessment, 12(2), 115–
363–371. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.01. 122. https://doi.org/10.1037//1040-3590.12.2.115
002 Pradhan, S. & Jena, L. K. (2018a). Abusive supervi-
Peltokorpi, V. (2019). Abusive supervision and emo- sion and job outcomes: A moderated mediation
tional exhaustion: The moderating role of power study. Evidence-Based HRM: A Global Forum
distance orientation and the mediating role of for Empirical Scholarship, 6(2), 137–152.
interaction avoidance. Asia Pacific Journal of https://doi.org/10.1108/EBHRM-06-2017-0030
Human Resources, 57(3), 251–275. https://doi. Pradhan, S., Jena, L. K. & Mohapatra, M. (2018).
org/10.1111/1744-7941.12188 Role of gender on the relationship between
Peng, A. C., Schaubroeck, J. M., Chong, S. & Li, Y. abusive supervision and employee’s intention to
(2019). Discrete emotions linking abusive super- quit in Indian electricity distribution companies.
vision to employee intention and behavior. Gender in Management: An International
Personnel Psychology, 72(3), 393–419. https:// Journal, 33(4), 282–295. https://doi.org/10.1108/
doi.org/10.1111/peps.12310 GM-01-2017-0008
Perko, K., Kinnunen, U. & Feldt, T. (2017). Long-term Pradhan, S., Srivastava, A. & Jena, L. K. (2019a).
profiles of work-related rumination associated with Abusive supervision and intention to quit:
leadership, job demands, and exhaustion: A three- Exploring multi-mediational approaches. Personnel
wave study. Work & Stress, 31(4), 395–420. Review, 49(6), 1269–1286. https://doi.org/10.1108/
https://doi.org/10.1080/02678373.2017.1330835 PR-12-2018-0496
Peus, C., Braun, S. & Frey, D. (2013). Pradhan, S., Srivastava, A. & Mishra, D. K. (2019b).
Situation-based measurement of the full range of Abusive supervision and knowledge hiding: The
leadership model—development and validation mediating role of psychological contract violation
of a situational judgment test. The Leadership and supervisor directed aggression. Journal of
Quarterly, 24(5), 777–795. https://doi.org/10. Knowledge Management, 24(2), 216–234.
1016/j.leaqua.2013.07.006 https://doi.org/10.1108/JKM-05-2019-0248
Peus, C., Braun, S. & Schyns, B. (2016). Leadership Priesemuth, M. & Schminke, M. (2019). Helping thy
lessons from compelling contexts (Monographs in neighbor? Prosocial reactions to observed abusive
Leadership and Management, Volume 8). supervision in the workplace. Journal of
Emerald Group Publishing Limited. Management, 45(3), 1225–1251. https://doi.org/
Pfattheicher, S., Lazarević , L. B., Westgate, E. C. & 10.1177/0149206317702219
Schindler, S. (2020). On the relation of boredom Priesemuth, M., Schminke, M., Ambrose, M. L. &
and sadistic aggression. Journal of Personality Folger, R. (2014). Abusive supervision climate: A
and Social Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1037/ multiple-mediation model of its impact on group out-
pspi0000335 comes. Academy of Management Journal, 57(5),
Piccolo, R. F., Bono, J. E., Heinitz, K., Rowold, J., Duehr, 1513–1534. https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2011.0237
E. & Judge, T. A. (2012). The relative impact of com- Pundt, A. & Schwarzbeck, K. (2018). Abusive super-
plementary leader behaviors: Which matter most? The vision from an integrated self-control perspective.
Leadership Quarterly, 23(3), 567–581. https://doi.org/ Applied Psychology, 67(3), 473–497. https://doi.
10.1016/j.leaqua.2011.12.008 org/10.1111/apps.12125
Podsakoff, P. M. & Podsakoff, N. P. (2019). Pyc, L. S., Meltzer, D. P. & Liu, C. (2017).
Experimental designs in management and leader- Ineffective leadership and employees’ negative
ship research: Strengths, limitations, and recom- outcomes: The mediating effect of anxiety and
mendations for improving publishability. The depression. International Journal of Stress
Leadership Quarterly, 30(1), 11–33. https://doi. Management, 24(2), 196–215. https://doi.org/10.
org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2018.11.002 1037/str0000030
Emmerling et al. 273

Rafferty, A. E. & Restubog, S. L. D. (2011). The Richard, O. C., Boncoeur, O. D., Chen, H. & Ford,
influence of abusive supervisors on followers’ D. L. Jr. (2018). Supervisor abuse effects on sub-
organizational citizenship behaviours: The ordinate turnover intentions and subsequent inter-
hidden costs of abusive supervision. British personal aggression: The role of power-distance
Journal of Management, 22(2), 270–285. https:// orientation and perceived human resource
doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8551.2010.00732.x support climate. Journal of Business Ethics.
Raine, A., Dodge, K., Loeber, R., Gatzke-Kopp, L., https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-018-4019-7
Lynam, D., Reynolds, C., Stouthamer-Loeber, Rispens, S. & Demerouti, E. (2016). Conflict at work,
M. & Liu, J. (2006). The reactive–proactive negative emotions, and performance: A diary study.
aggression questionnaire: Differential correlates Negotiation and Conflict Management Research,
of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescent 9(2), 103–119. https://doi.org/10.1111/ncmr.12069
boys. Aggressive Behavior, 32(2), 159–171. Roberton, T., Daffern, M. & Bucks, R. S. (2012).
https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20115 Emotion regulation and aggression. Aggression
Raine, A., Fung, A. L. C., Portnoy, J., Choy, O. & and Violent Behavior, 17(1), 72–82. https://doi.
Spring, V. L. (2014). Low heart rate as a risk org/10.1016/j.avb.2011.09.006
factor for child and adolescent proactive aggres- Rogers, K. B. & Robinson, D. T. (2014). Measuring
sive and impulsive psychopathic behavior. affect and emotions. Handbook of the Sociology
Aggressive Behavior, 40(4), 290–299. https:// of Emotions, II, 283–303.
doi.org/10.1002/ab.21523 Rupp, D. E. & Vodanovich, S. J. (1997). The role of
Ramdeo, S. & Singh, R. (2019). Abusive supervision, boredom proneness in self-reported anger and
co-worker abuse and work outcomes: Procedural aggression. Journal of Social Behavior and
justice as a mediator. Evidence-Based HRM: A Personality, 12(4), 925–936.
Global Forum for Empirical Scholarship, 7(3), Sârbescu, P., Sulea, C. & Moza, D. (2017).
325–341. https://doi.org/10.1108/EBHRM-09-2018- Supervisor undermining and driving errors in
0060 truck drivers: A moderated mediation model.
Reisenzein, R. (1994). Pleasure-arousal theory and Transportation Research Part F: Traffic
the intensity of emotions. Journal of Personality Psychology and Behaviour, 45, 122–130. https://
and Social Psychology, 67(3), 525–539. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2016.12.006
doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.3.525 Scheuer, M. L., Burton, J. P., Barber, L. K.,
Restubog, S. L. D., Scott, K. L. & Zagenczyk, T. J. Finkelstein, L. M. & Parker, C. P. (2016).
(2011). When distress hits home: The role of con- Linking abusive supervision to employee engage-
textual factors and psychological distress in pre- ment and exhaustion. Organization Management
dicting employees’ responses to abusive Journal, 13(3), 138–147. https://doi.org/10.1080/
supervision. Journal of Applied Psychology, 15416518.2016.1214063
96(4), 713–729. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021593 Schmid, E. A., Pircher Verdorfer, A. & Peus, C.
Rice, D. B., Letwin, C., Taylor, R. & Wo, X. (2020a). (2019). Shedding light on leaders’ self-interest:
Extending the trickle-down model of abusive Theory and measurement of exploitative leader-
supervision. The role of moral disengagement. ship. Journal of Management, 45(4), 1401–1433.
The Journal of Social Psychology, 1–7. https:// https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206317707810
doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2020.1752133 Schmid, E. A., Pircher Verdorfer, A. & Peus, C. V.
Rice, D. B., Taylor, R. & Forrester, J. K. (2020b). (2018). Different shades - different effects?
The unwelcoming experience of abusive supervi- Consequences of different types of destructive
sion and the impact of leader characteristics: leadership. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(JUL).
Turning employees into poor organizational citi- https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01289
zens and future quitters. European Journal of Schyns, B. & Schilling, J. (2013). How bad are the
Work and Organizational Psychology, 1–18. effects of bad leaders? A meta-analysis of destruc-
https://doi.org/10.1080/1359432X.2020.1737521 tive leadership and its outcomes. The Leadership
274 Organizational Psychology Review 13(3)

Quarterly, 24(1), 138–158. https://doi.org/10. multimotive approach. Journal of Applied


1016/j.leaqua.2012.09.001 Psychology, 100(6), 1798–1810. https://doi.org/
Sears, S. & Kraus, S. (2009). I think therefore I om: 10.1037/apl0000031
Cognitive distortions and coping style as media- Smithmyer, C. M., Hubbard, J. A. & Simons, R. F.
tors for the effects of mindfulness meditation on (2000). Proactive and reactive aggression in delinquent
anxiety, positive and negative affect, and hope. adolescents: Relations to aggression outcome expect-
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65(6), 561–573. ancies. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 29(1),
https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20543 86–93. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15374424jccp2901_9
Shachaf, P. & Hara, N. (2010). Beyond vandalism: Stanford, M. S., Houston, R. J., Mathias, C. W.,
Wikipedia trolls. Journal of Information Villemarette-Pittman, N. R., Helfritz, L. E. &
Science, 36(3), 357–370. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Conklin, S. M. (2003). Characterizing aggressive
0165551510365390 behavior. Assessment, 10(2), 183–190. https://doi.
Shaffer, J. A., DeGeest, D. & Li, A. (2016). Tackling the org/10.1177/1073191103010002009
problem of construct proliferation: A guide to asses- Stempel, C. R. & Rigotti, T. (2018). Leaders’ gender,
sing the discriminant validity of conceptually related perceived abusive supervision and health.
constructs. Organizational Research Methods, 19(1), Frontiers in Psychology, 9(DEC). https://doi.org/
80–110. https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428115598239 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02427
Shen, C., Yang, J., He, P. & Wu, Y. J. (2019). How Stephens, A. N. & Sullman, M. J. (2015). Trait pre-
does abusive supervision restrict employees’ dictors of aggression and crash-related behaviors
feedback-seeking behavior? Journal of across drivers from the United Kingdom and the
Managerial Psychology, 34(8), 546–559. https:// Irish Republic. Risk Analysis, 35(9), 1730–1745.
doi.org/10.1108/JMP-10-2018-0480 https://doi.org/10.1111/risa.12379
Shillamkwese, S. S., Tariq, H., Obaid, A., Weng, Q. Sulea, C., Filipescu, R., Horga, A., Orţ an, C. &
& Garavan, T. N. (2020). It’s not me, it’s you: Fischmann, G. (2012). Interpersonal mistreatment
Testing a moderated mediation model of subor- at work and burnout among teachers. Cognition,
dinate deviance and abusive supervision through Brain, Behavior: An Interdisciplinary Journal,
the self-regulatory perspective. Business Ethics: 16(4), 553–570.
A European Review, 29(1), 227–243. https://doi. Swartout, K. M. (2013). The company they keep:
org/10.1111/beer.12245 How peer networks influence male sexual aggres-
Shirai, M. & Suzuki, N. (2017). Is sadness only one sion. Psychology of Violence, 3, 157–171. https://
emotion? Psychological and physiological doi.org/10.1037/a0029997
responses to sadness induced by two different Tariq, H. & Weng, Q. (Derek), Garavan, T. N.,
situations:“loss of someone” and “failure to Obaid, A. & Hassan, W. (2020). Another sleep-
achieve a goal”. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, less night: Does a leader’s poor sleep lead to sub-
288. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00288 ordinate’s poor sleep? A spillover/crossover
Shoss, M. K., Eisenberger, R., Restubog, S. L. D. & perspective. Journal of Sleep Research, 29(1),
Zagenczyk, T. J. (2013). Blaming the organization 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12904
for abusive supervision: The roles of perceived organ- Taylor, S. P. (1967). Aggressive behavior and
izational support and supervisor’s organizational physiological arousal as a function of provocation
embodiment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98(1), and the tendency to inhibit aggression. Journal of
158–168. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030687 Personality, 35(2), 297–310. https://doi.org/10.
Sieweke, J. & Santoni, S. (2020). Natural experiments in 1111/j.1467-6494.1967.tb01430.x
leadership research: An introduction, review, and Tepper, B. J. (2000). Consequences of abusive super-
guidelines. The Leadership Quarterly, 31(1), vision. Academy of Management Journal, 43(2),
101338. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2019.101338 178–190. https://doi.org/10.2307/1556375
Simon, L. S., Hurst, C., Kelley, K. & Judge, T. A. Tepper, B. J., Duffy, M. K., Henle, C. A. & Schurer
(2015). Understanding cycles of abuse: A Lambert, L. (2006). Procedural injustice, victim
Emmerling et al. 275

precipitation, and abusive supervision. Personnel deviance. The Journal of Social Psychology,
Psychology, 59(1), 101–123. https://doi.org/10. 159(3), 299–312. https://doi.org/10.1080/
1111/j.1744-6570.2006.00725.x 00224545.2018.
Tepper, B. J., Duffy, M. K. & Shaw, J. D. (2001). 1466776
Personality moderators of the relationship between Van Dierendonck, D. (2011). Servant leadership: A
abusive supervision and subordinates’ resistance. review and synthesis. Journal of Management,
Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(5), 974–983. 37(4), 1228–1261. https://doi.org/10.1177/
https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.86.5.974 0149206310380462
Tepper, B. J., Moss, S. E., Lockhart, D. E. & Carr, J. Van Knippenberg, D. & Sitkin, S. B. (2013). A critical
C. (2007). Abusive supervision, upward mainten- assessment of charismatic-transformational leader-
ance communication, and subordinates’ psycho- ship research: Back to the drawing board? The
logical distress. Academy of Management Academy of Management Annals, 7(1), 1–60.
Journal, 50(5), 1169–1180. https://doi.org/10. https://doi.org/10.5465/19416520.2013.759433
5465/amj.2007.20159918 Van Prooijen, J.-W. & de Vries, R. E. (2016).
Tepper, B. J., Simon, L. & Park, H. M. (2017). Abusive Organizational conspiracy beliefs: Implications
supervision. Annual Review of Organizational for leadership behaviors and employee outcomes.
Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 4(1), Journal of Business and Psychology, 31(4), 479–
123–152. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-orgpsych- 491. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-015-9428-3
041015-062539 Van Tilburg, W. A., Igou, E. R., Maher, P. J. & Lennon,
Thoroughgood, C. N., Sawyer, K. B., Padilla, A. & J. (2019). Various forms of existential distress are
Lunsford, L. (2018). Destructive leadership: A associated with aggressive tendencies. Personality
critique of leader-centric perspectives and and Individual Differences, 144, 111–119. https://
toward a more holistic definition. Journal of doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.02.032
Business Ethics, 151(3), 627–649. https://doi. Velez, M. J. & Neves, P. (2017). The relationship
org/10.1007/s10551-016-3257-9 between abusive supervision, distributive justice
Tröster, C. & Van Quaquebeke, N. (2021). When and job satisfaction: A substitutes for leadership
victims help their abusive supervisors: The role approach. European Review of Applied
of LMX, self-blame, and guilt. Academy of Psychology, 67(4), 187–198. https://doi.org/10.
Management Journal, 64(6), 1793–1815. https:// 1016/j.erap.2017.05.005
doi.org/10.5465/amj.2019.0559 Vogel, R., Homberg, F. & Gericke, A. (2016).
Tuente, S. K., Bogaerts, S. & Veling, W. (2019). Abusive supervision, public service motivation, and
Hostile attribution bias and aggression in employee deviance: The moderating role of employ-
adults-a systematic review. Aggression and ment sector. Evidence-Based HRM: A Global Forum
Violent Behavior, 46, 66–81. https://doi.org/10. for Empirical Scholarship, 4(3), 214–231. https://doi.
1016/j.avb.2019.01.009 org/10.1108/EBHRM-08-2015-0034
Unkelbach, C., Fiedler, K., Bayer, M., Stegmüller, M. Vogel, R. M. & Mitchell, M. S. (2017). The motiv-
& Danner, D. (2008). Why positive information is ational effects of diminished self-esteem for
processed faster: The density hypothesis. Journal employees who experience abusive supervision.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(1), 36– Journal of Management, 43(7), 2218–2251.
49. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.36 https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206314566462
Uysal, H. T. (2019). The mediation role of toxic lead- Walden, T. A., Harris, V. S. & Catron, T. F. (2003).
ership in the effect of job stress on job satisfaction. How I feel: A self-report measure of emotional
International Journal of Business, 24(1), 55–73. arousal and regulation for children.
Valle, M., Kacmar, K. M., Zivnuska, S. & Harting, T. Psychological Assessment, 15(3), 399–412.
(2019). Abusive supervision, leader-member https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.15.3.399
exchange and moral disengagement: A Waldman, D. A., Wang, D., Hannah, S. T., Owens,
moderated-mediation model of organizational B. P. & Balthazard, P. A. (2018). Psychological
276 Organizational Psychology Review 13(3)

and neurological predictors of abusive supervision. Reports, 118(3), 810–828. https://doi.org/10.


Personnel Psychology, 71(3), 399–421. https://doi. 1177/0033294116644369
org/10.1111/peps.12262 Wang, Y., Cao, S., Zhang, Q. & Xia, L. X. (2020b).
Waller, L., Reitz, M., Poole, E., Riddell, P. M. & The longitudinal relationship between angry
Muir, A. (2017). Experiential learning as rumination and reactive–proactive aggression and
preparation for leadership: An exploration of the the moderation effect of consideration of future
cognitive and physiological processes. consequences-immediate. Aggressive Behavior,
Leadership & Organization Development 46(6), 476–488. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21913
Journal, 38(4), 513–529. https://doi.org/10.1108/ Wascher, C. A. (2021). Heart rate as a measure of
LODJ-03-2015-0057 emotional arousal in evolutionary biology.
Walsh, M. M. & Arnold, K. A. (2018). Mindfulness Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society
as a buffer of leaders’ self-rated behavioral B, 376(1831), 20200479. https://doi.org/10.1098/
responses to emotional exhaustion: A dual rstb.2020.0479
process model of self-regulation. Frontiers in Watkins, T., Fehr, R. & He, W. (2019). Whatever it
Psychology, 9(DEC). https://doi.org/10.3389/ takes: Leaders’ perceptions of abusive supervi-
fpsyg.2018.02498 sion instrumentality. The Leadership Quarterly,
Walumbwa, F. O., Avolio, B. J., Gardner, W. L., 30(2), 260–272. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.
Wernsing, T. S. & Peterson, S. J. (2008). 2018.09.002
Authentic leadership: Development and valid- Wei, F. & Si, S. (2013). Tit for tat? Abusive supervi-
ation of a theory-based measure. Journal of sion and counterproductive work behaviors: The
Management, 34(1), 89–126. https://doi.org/10. moderating effects of locus of control and per-
1177/0149206307308913 ceived mobility. Asia Pacific Journal of
Wang, C.-C., Hsieh, H.-H. & Wang, Y.-D. (2020a). Management, 30(1), 281–296. https://doi.org/10.
Abusive supervision and employee engagement 1007/s10490-011-9251-y
and satisfaction: The mediating role of employee Weierich, M. R., Wright, C. I., Negreira, A.,
silence. Personnel Review, 49(9), 1845–1858. Dickerson, B. C. & Barrett, L. F. (2010).
https://doi.org/10.1108/PR-04-2019-0147 Novelty as a dimension in the affective brain.
Wang, R. & Chan, D. K. S. (2019). Subordinate reac- Neuroimage, 49(3), 2871–2878. https://doi.org/
tions to ethical leaders’ abusive behavior: a mul- 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.047
tiple-wave study. Asia Pacific Journal of Westgate, E. C. & Wilson, T. D. (2018). Boring thoughts
Human Resources. https://doi.org/10.1111/1744- and bored minds: The MAC model of boredom and
7941.12222 cognitive engagement. Psychological Review,
Wang, R. & Jiang, J. (2014). How narcissistic 125(5), 689–713. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000097
employees respond to abusive supervision: Two White, B. A. & Turner, K. A. (2014). Anger rumin-
roles of narcissism in decreasing perception and ation and effortful control: Mediation effects on
increasing deviance. Psychological Reports, reactive but not proactive aggression. Personality
115(2), 372–380. https://doi.org/10.2466/01.21. and Individual Differences, 56, 186–189. https://
PR0.115c22z2 doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.08.012
Wang, R. & Jiang, J. (2015). How abusive supervi- Whitman, M. V., Halbesleben, J. R. B. & Shanine, K.
sors influence employees’ voice and silence: K. (2013). Psychological entitlement and abusive
The effects of interactional justice and organiza- supervision: Political skill as a self-regulatory
tional attribution. The Journal of Social mechanism. Health Care Management Review,
Psychology, 155(3), 204–220. https://doi.org/10. 38(3), 248–257. https://doi.org/10.1097/HMR.
1080/00224545.2014.990410 0b013e3182678fe7
Wang, R., Jiang, J., Yang, L. & Chan, D. K. S. Wilkowski, B. M. & Robinson, M. D. (2010). The
(2016). Chinese Employees’ psychological anatomy of anger: An integrative cognitive
responses to abusive supervisors. Psychological model of trait anger and reactive aggression.
Emmerling et al. 277

Journal of Personality, 78(1), 9–38. https://doi. Wu, T.-Y. & Hu, C. (2009). Abusive supervision and
org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00607.x employee emotional exhaustion: Dispositional
Wilson, L. C. & Scarpa, A. (2013). Baseline heart antecedents and boundaries. Group &
rate, sensation seeking, and aggression in young Organization Management, 34(2), 143–169.
adult women: A two-sample examination. https://doi.org/10.1177/1059601108331217
Aggressive Behavior, 39(4), 280–289. https:// Wu, T.-Y., Hu, C. & Yang, C.-C. (2013). Abusive
doi.org/10.1002/ab.21477 supervision and workload demands from supervi-
Wisse, B. & Sleebos, E. (2016). When the dark ones sors: Exploring two types of supervisor-related
gain power: Perceived position power strengthens stressors and their association with strain. Stress
the effect of supervisor Machiavellianism on and Health, 29(3), 190–198. https://doi.org/10.
abusive supervision in work teams. Personality 1002/smi.2440
and Individual Differences, 99, 122–126. https:// Wyckoff, J. P. (2016). Aggression and emotion: Anger,
doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.05.019 not general negative affect, predicts desire to
Woestman, D. S. & Wasonga, T. A. (2015). aggress. Personality and Individual Differences,
Destructive leadership behaviors and workplace 101, 220–226. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.
attitudes in schools. NASSP Bulletin, 99(2), 147– 06.001
163. https://doi.org/10.1177/0192636515581922 Xu, A. J., Loi, R. & Lam, L. W. (2015). The bad boss
Wongleedee, K. (2020). Turnover intention and takes it all: How abusive supervision and leader-
abusive supervision and management: member exchange interact to influence employee
Investigating the role of self-identity and future silence. The Leadership Quarterly, 26(5), 763–
work self-salience. Systematic Reviews in 774. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2015.03.002
Pharmacy, 11(1), 462–471. https://doi.org/10. Xu, H., Bègue, L. & Shankland, R. (2011). Guilt and
5530/srp.2020.1.58 guiltlessness: An integrative review. Social and
Woods, S. & White, E. (2005). The association Personality Psychology Compass, 5(7), 440–457.
between bullying behaviour, arousal levels and https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00364.x
behaviour problems. Journal of Adolescence, Xu, Q., Zhao, Y., Xi, M. & Li, F. (2020). Abusive super-
28(3), 381–395. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. vision, high-performance work systems, and subor-
adolescence.2004.09.002 dinate silence. Personnel Review, 49(8), 1637–
Woodworth, M. & Porter, S. (2002). In cold blood: 1653. https://doi.org/10.1108/PR-01-2019-0029
Characteristics of criminal homicides as a func- Yagil, D. (2005). Employees’ attribution of abusive
tion of psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal supervisory behaviors. International Journal of
Psychology, 111(3), 436–445. https://doi.org/10. Organizational Analysis, 13(4), 307–326. https://
1037/0021-843X.111.3.436 doi.org/10.1108/eb029009
Wu, M., Peng, Z. & Estay, C. (2018a). How destruc- Yagil, D., Ben-Zur, H. & Tamir, I. (2011). Do
tive leadership influences compulsory organiza- employees cope effectively with abusive supervi-
tional citizenship behavior. Chinese Management sion at work? An exploratory study. International
Studies, 12(2), 453–468. https://doi.org/10.1108/ Journal of Stress Management, 18(1), 5–23.
CMS-10-2017-0298 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020548
Wu, M., Peng, Z. & Estay, C. (2018b). How role stress Yam, K. C., Fehr, R., Keng-Highberger, F. T., Klotz,
mediates the relationship between destructive leader- A. C. & Reynolds, S. J. (2016). Out of control: A
ship and employee silence: The moderating role of self-control perspective on the link between
job complexity. Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology, surface acting and abusive supervision. Journal
12, e19. https://doi.org/10.1017/prp.2018.7 of Applied Psychology, 101(2), 292–301. https://
Wu, S. D. & Lo, P. C. (2008). Inward-attention medita- doi.org/10.1037/apl0000043
tion increases parasympathetic activity: A study based Yan, X., Wang, Z., Su, J. & Luo, Z. (2020).
on heart rate variability. Biomedical Research, 29(5), Relationship between core self-evaluations and
245–250. https://doi.org/10.2220/biomedres.29.245 team identification: The perception of abusive
278 Organizational Psychology Review 13(3)

supervision and work engagement. Current supervision and safety behaviors. Journal of
Psychology, 39(1), 121–127. https://doi.org/10. Business Ethics, 162(1), 213–228. https://doi.
1007/s12144-017-9749-7 org/10.1007/s10551-018-3983-2
Yang, J.-H., Lin, C.-C., Fang, S.-C. & Huang, C.-Y. Yukl, G. & Van Fleet, D. D. (1992). Theory and
(2019). An uncertainty management theory on the research on leadership in organizations. In
effects of abusive supervision: The moderating Dunnette, M. D. & Hough, L. M. (Eds.),
role of future orientation. Management Handbook of industrial and organizational psych-
Decision, 57(11), 3079–3095. https://doi.org/10. ology (pp. 147–197). Consulting Psychologists
1108/MD-06-2017-0604 Press.
Yao, Z., Zhang, X., Liu, Z., Zhang, L. & Luo, J. Zellars, K. L., Tepper, B. J. & Duffy, M. K. (2002).
(2019). Narcissistic leadership and voice behav- Abusive supervision and subordinates’ organiza-
ior: The role of job stress, traditionality, and tional citizenship behavior. Journal of Applied
trust in leaders. Chinese Management Studies, Psychology, 87(6), 1068–1076. https://doi.org/
14(3)543–563. https://doi.org/10.1108/CMS-11- 10.1037/0021-9010.87.6.1068
2018-0747 Zhu, J., Song, L. J., Zhu, L. & Johnson, R. E. (2019).
Yu, K., Lin, W., Wang, L., Ma, J., Wei, W., Wang, H., Visualizing the landscape and evolution of leader-
Guo, W. & Shi, J. (2016). The role of affective com- ship research. The Leadership Quarterly, 30(2),
mitment and future work self salience in the abusive 215–232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2018.
supervision-job performance relationship. Journal 06.003
of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, Zhu, J. & Zhang, B. (2019). The double-edged sword
89(1), 28–45. https://doi.org/10.1111/joop.12101 effect of abusive supervision on subordinates’
Yuan, X., Xu, Y. & Li, Y. (2020). Resource depletion innovative behavior. Frontiers in Psychology,
perspective on the link between abusive 10(JAN). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00066

You might also like