You are on page 1of 16

Psychology of Men & Masculinities © 2021 American Psychological Association

2021, Vol. 22, No. 4, 611–626 ISSN: 1524-9220 https://doi.org/10.1037/men0000365

The Military Hypermasculine Mystique: Sex, Status,


and Emotional Control at the United States Military Academy
Hillary S. Schaefer1, Dave I. Cotting2, Eliot S. Proctor3, Diane M. Ryan1, and Richard M. Lerner1
1
Eliot-Pearson Department of Child Study and Human Development, Tufts University
2
Institute for Defense Analyses, Alexandria, Virginia, United States
3
United States Army, United States

Although hostile expressions of masculinity have robust negative impacts in multiple domains among
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

civilian populations, in the military, masculinity is a rarer target of inquiry and remains a polarizing subject.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

This article examines hostile and hypermasculine attitudes that prior work suggests may be associated with
negative consequences for the social environment and men themselves in a U.S. military educational
environment (N = 1,560 freshmen and 499 graduating senior men). Multivariate moderated multiple
regressions (MMMR) linked military hypermasculinity facets (Machiavellian desire for status and control;
conformity to masculine norms including emotional control, violence tolerance, needing to win, and
impersonal sex preference; hostile and benevolent sexism) to performance outcomes including academics,
leadership, and fitness; perceptions of peers, leadership, and antibias programming; and behavior. Desire for
status, impersonal sex preference, violence tolerance, and benevolent sexism predicted negative perfor-
mance outcomes. Violence tolerance, emotional control, and hostile sexism were associated with poorer
perception of character education and/or anti-bias programming. Needing to win and control were
associated with positive performance outcomes. Healthy messaging surrounding emotions, sex, and
perception of women may help assuage toxic expressions of masculinity in the military. Contextualized
demonstration of the counterproductive aspects of military hypermasculinity might sidestep opposition to
altering hegemonic norms.

Public Significance Statement


Our results and the work of others show that hostile masculinities result in poor outcomes for men
holding these beliefs and for the social environment generally. In our sample, poor outcomes for
hypermasculine military attitudes included lower connection to peers, poorer grades and leadership, and
rejection of diversity training efforts. Our findings suggest that healthy messaging surrounding sex and
display of emotions are especially important to counteract problems associated with hypermasculine
military culture.

Keywords: hypermasculinity, sexism, military, diversity training, higher education

Supplemental materials: https://doi.org/10.1037/men0000365.supp

As the military has historically drawn on the masculinization of women and an association of manly with being violent and powerful
men to inculcate warrior attributes (Arkin & Dobrofsky, 1978; Do & (Mosher & Sirkin, 1984), and other hostile and toxic expressions
Samuels, 2020), it may provide an environment to explore conse- of masculinity that place high value on stereotypically male char-
quences of certain expressions of masculinity. These include hyper- acteristics, such as aggression, dominance, toughness, power, and
masculinity, which is characterized by negative sexual attitudes about heterosexual prowess, and devalue stereotypically female-associated
characteristics, such as emotionality (Corprew et al., 2014; Turchik &
Wilson, 2010). These expressions of masculinity have consistently
been associated with severe negative consequences for men them-
selves, such as suicidality, and for others, including sexual assault
Hillary S. Schaefer https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1124-5479 (Burns & Mahalik, 2011; Murnen et al., 2002; Stander et al., 2018).
Dave I. Cotting https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1798-5013 In the military, qualitative investigations, examinations of doctrine,
Diane M. Ryan https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2102-8988 and surveys of servicemembers describe a range of hostile and
We sincerely thank the rest of the Project Arete team for their help and
hypermasculine attitudes as integral to, and adaptive in, military
support. This article was supported by a grant from the Templeton Religion
Trust (to Richard M. Lerner).
identity and socialization along with other values, such as service
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Hillary and sacrifice (Green et al., 2010; Hinojosa, 2010; Shields et al., 2017;
S. Schaefer, Eliot-Pearson Department of Child Study and Human Devel- Woodward, 2000). However, it remains unclear whether those with
opment, Tufts University, 26 Winthrop Street, Medford, MA 02155, hostile expressions of masculinity are ultimately more or less suc-
United States. Email: hillary.schaefer@tufts.edu cessful in this environment (Harway & Steel, 2015). It is vital to

611
612 SCHAEFER, COTTING, PROCTOR, RYAN, AND LERNER

understand the professional outcomes of those with expressions of studies have tended to utilize broad conceptualizations of hyper-
masculinity that have strong connections to sexual assault and masculinity (Stander et al., 2018). Understanding the components
harassment in particular, as these remain pervasive problems and of military masculinity independently, as has been done in civilian
barriers to greater representation of women in the armed forces (Lane, populations (Levant & Wimer, 2014), is yet understudied, obscuring
2020; Wilson, 2018). whether some aspects are in fact protective or beneficial, and for
The United States Military Academy (USMA) focuses on a what outcomes.
“culture of character growth” across 47 months with experiences Whatever the course of military hypermasculinity for those who
and training aiming to, “transform the identity of a cadet to that of an express it, the impact on others is clear and these attitudes degrade
Officer” (United States Military Academy [USMA], 2018, p. 25). the military social environment. Military hypermasculinity has been
Notwithstanding a devotion to character education efforts, multiple, linked to antipathy toward women and gay men and to sexual assault
independent investigations have found worse gender climates and and harassment perpetration (Harris et al., 2018; Morris, 1996). It
hostile masculine attitudes in military academies as compared to also increases risk of hazing, bullying, sexual assault, and sexual
civilian educational institutions, even when comparing military acad- harassment risk for male victims and decreases reporting (Koeszegi
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

emy cadets to cadets in civilian universities (Carroll & Clark, 2006; et al., 2014; O’Brien et al., 2015). The U.S. military continually
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Drake, 2006; Matthews et al., 2009; Robinson Kurpius & Lucart, engages in educational attempts to improve climate and reduce
2000). Further delineation of military hypermasculinity, particularly bullying/hazing, harassment, and sexual assault; some military
at USMA as an educational and military training environment staffed antibias programs have demonstrated efficacy (Rau et al., 2010),
predominately by servicemembers, may offer avenues for interven- but the overall evidence base for antisexism and antiharassment
tion to foster healthy organizational climate in other environments programs is lacking (Orchowski et al., 2018). However, the effec-
(Harway & Steel, 2015; Rosenstein et al., 2018). The primary aim of tiveness of many antibias programs is questionable across profes-
this study was thus to understand how hostile, hypermasculine sional contexts (Dobbin & Kalev, 2018; Lai et al., 2016), suggesting
attributes that have strong connections to gender-based mistreatment that greater knowledge of interfering factors may have cross-context
and social dysfunction related to military outcomes, including per- relevance.
formance success and perceptions of others. Several aspects of military hypermasculine culture suggest inter-
fering factors to the efficacy of such programs, although this has not
yet been addressed empirically; the belief that a negative climate is a
Military Hypermasculinity
“natural” result of mixed-gender environments is alive and well
Qualitative and theoretical works have used a social construc- (Arbeit, 2017; Trobaugh, 2018) and military hypermasculinity can
tionist approach to define a military hypermasculinity that includes be used to justify sexist beliefs (Do & Samuels, 2020). Attention to
toughness, power, stoicism, aggression, and superiority over others, individual-level beliefs systems is often unpopular; an initiative to
even other groups within the military (e.g., Fox & Pease, 2012; measure rape myths and hostile/benevolent sexism in National
Shields et al., 2017). Conforming to masculine ideals and rejecting Guard servicemembers was met with outcry (Kime, 2019). This
what is considered feminine is considered key to military training suggests hesitance to address individual and cultural factors that
and socialization (Burns & Mahalik, 2011; McAllister et al., 2019); civilian literature highlights (e.g., Abbey et al., 2011). Understand-
such internalization of military hypermasculinity may be achieved ing how militarily-relevant hypermasculine attributes interface with
via crucibles aimed at “turning boys to men” (Arkin & Dobrofsky, gender climate education may help improve efficacy or provide
1978; Barrett, 1996). The tension between the military hypermas- novel intervention targets (Becker et al., 2014; Dobbin & Kalev,
culine identity described by servicemembers, hostile masculinites as 2018); any facets that interfere with anti-bias education and are also
an empirically supported risk factor for sexual assault/harassment, in any way rewarded or selected for deserve particular attention
and the problem of sexual assault/harassment in the military has (Richard & Molloy, 2020). Due to the limited literature connecting
been noted (Richard & Molloy, 2020). However, defenders of the hostile expressions of masculinity to professional success in this or
benefits of military hypermasculinity tend to argue in favor of a related settings, below we review available work of such facets
(hypermasculine) status quo, lest military effectiveness be compro- hypothesized to be militarily relevant and influential on gender and
mised (Titunik, 2008). social climate, the key criteria for the present study.
Although military hypermasculinity is more commonly qualita-
tively explored than its downstream effects investigated, available
Status, Control, and Power
studies show similar negative impacts as civilian settings. Toxic and
hypermasculine attributes are related to adverse health outcomes, Power is a fundamental and overarching expression of hostile
relationship problems, and suicide, although much of this work is in masculinities, which includes one’s relative status, superiority, and
Veterans and concerns difficulties in the military-to-civilian transi- control over others; these qualities may be particularly salient during
tion rather than during service (Braswell & Kushner, 2012; Cox & military training (Arkin & Dobrofsky, 1978). Power, status, and
O’Loughlin, 2017). However, other work has suggested that control themes are integral to conceptions of military masculinity
although hypermasculinity does increase during military training, and leadership competence, but conflicting themes of belonging and
attitudes toward intimate partner violence actually improve (institutionally mandated) egalitarianism are also present, obscuring
(Hendrix, 2006). Whereas qualitative work encourages context how individual intrapersonal levels of these attributes manifest in
specificity and nuance in the definition of military masculinity military outcomes (Do & Samuels, 2020; Hale, 2012). Social or
(Braswell & Kushner, 2012; Richard & Molloy, 2020), and quanti- interpersonal dominance is a well-studied manifestation of desire for
tative civilian work has also discouraged treating masculinity as a power and status; it overlaps with but is discernable from sexist
monolith (e.g., Hammer et al., 2018), military-focused quantitative beliefs (Pratto et al., 2013) and increases with military training as a
MILITARY HYPERMASCULINITY AT USMA 613

result of socialization (Nicol et al., 2007). Preoccupation with status aggression could be more problematic in a context already focused
and control of others has also been associated with poorer reactions on violence, or, as is sometimes purported in military doctrine and
to interventions that generally reduce prejudice, such as intergroup expressed by servicemembers, the military may provide an envi-
contact (Kteily et al., 2019). Status and control are often studied ronment where individuals with tolerance for dangerousness and
under larger umbrellas as part of social dominance or hostile (instrumental) violence can be shaped by rules (Barrett, 1996;
masculinity, but when investigated separately can show divergent Hale, 2012).
effects; desire for status, but not control, may be particularly
counterproductive to performance outcomes (Hutter et al., 2015).
Emotional Control
Interpersonal control, conversely, appears more connected to gender
violence (Whitaker, 2013), including in a military academy context Military culture provides mixed messages about emotional dis-
(Callahan, 2009). This tension between control, power, and status as plays that may ultimately hinder men’s ability to seek support and
associated with negative outcomes but also championed as a part of express distress, despite some acceptance of certain emotions in
military masculinity is key: Understanding how these facets operate some contexts (McAllister et al., 2019). Emotions are generally
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

is important to contextualize who is ultimately successful (Klein & described as feminine, threatening to military effectiveness and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Gallus, 2018). display rules sometimes resemble efficient emotion regulation (e.g.,
resilience; Ashley et al., 2017), and at other times are more akin to
suppression or overcontrol (e.g., stoicism; Green et al., 2010). This
Impersonal Sex Preference
is a nontrivial difference given the divergence of concomitants of
Impersonal sex behavior or self-reported preference for imper- efficient emotion regulation versus suppression; emotion suppres-
sonal sex has been linked to misperceptions of sexual intent and sion is linked to strict gender norm beliefs and a slew of negative
sexual objectification of women (Mikorski & Szymanski, 2017; outcomes including aggression, social impairment, disinterest in
Wegner & Abbey, 2016). Importantly, it has also been linked to seeking help when needed (Humphreys et al., 2009; Liaqat et al.,
increased aggression toward and hostile perception of women after 2020). Men with high affiliation with masculine gender roles self-
being rejected (Woerner et al., 2018), and, as part of the confluence report lower affect intensity, but this appears driven by intolerance
model, is related to sexual assault perpetration, especially in con- of internal experiences rather than genuinely attenuated intensity
junction with hostility toward women (Abbey et al., 2011; (Berke et al., 2019; Cohn et al., 2010; Jakupcak et al., 2003).
Malamuth et al., 1991). Civilian work suggests that impersonal Ultimately, emotional (over)control is physiologically counterpro-
sex behavior is related to an erratic lifestyle and criminal behavior, ductive, as attempts to ignore or suppress reactions inhibit engage-
especially in youth and younger adults, and is indicative of negative ment of effective coping mechanisms and resolution of stressors
outcomes generally (LeBreton et al., 2013). This construct is less (Roberton et al., 2012, 2014), and although emotional control is
commonly studied in a military population, and its relation to consistently described as integral to military masculinity, whether or
professional outcomes and gender-climate-focused instruction is not it is connected to success here is unclear.
unclear. Impersonal sex preference was also of particular interest
to the present study USMA’s character education included discus-
Competitiveness/Needing to Win
sions of “hookup culture” and healthy romantic relationships (Board
of Visitors, 2017). Theoretical models of gender roles have highlighted competitive-
ness as a masculine attribute, especially in relation to military
masculinity (e.g., Archer, 2013), and may be exacerbated in
Violence Tolerance
majority-male settings (Bird, 2003). Competitiveness may be par-
Most definitions of hostile expressions of masculinity include ticularly salient during military training (Swain, 2016); USMA in
some facet of tolerance of violence, and behaviorally hypermascu- particular utilizes a ranking system of cadets that ultimately deter-
linity has been connected to interpersonal violence and aggressive mines outcomes such as job selection that is continuously calculated
acts in a range of observational and empirical work (Parrott & over time (USMA, 2016a). Competitiveness has been linked to
Zeichner, 2003; Scharrer, 2005; Whitley, 2001), including targeted control over partners and ultimately sexual assault risk in young men
violence against women and gay men, but also more general (Caron et al., 1997). However, other studies have shown that,
tendencies to expect positive outcomes from aggression and to among masculine norms, competitiveness can be protective in
interpret neutral situations as hostile (instrumental aggression and some contexts (Iwamoto et al., 2010; Kaya et al., 2019). The utility
hostile attribution bias, respectively; e.g., Walters, 2007; Wharton of competitiveness as a masculine attribute in either predicting
et al., 2014). However, the military context complicates the rela- success or interfering with gender climate in a military setting is
tionship somewhat. Military training has an overarching goal to unknown.
shape recruits into “professionals in violence” (Barrett, 1996) and
glorification of violence is characteristic of military identity and
Hostility Toward Women
training (Johnson, 2010); this is an undeniable background that may
alter the conceptualization, outcomes, and/or expression of mascu- Some conceptualizations of hypermasculinities have included
line aggression. Like many military hypermasculine components, hostile attitudes toward women as integral, whereas others have
violence and aggression have been studied with respect to negative not, somewhat complicating the overall literature on the influence of
impact on women (Koeszegi et al., 2014), but the role of violence sexism specifically versus hostile and toxic expressions of mascu-
tolerance in military professional outcomes is understudied. This linity that include sexism (Peters et al., 2007). When investigated
could conceivably go in either direction: Either hypermasculine singularly, hostility toward women has strong empirical connections
614 SCHAEFER, COTTING, PROCTOR, RYAN, AND LERNER

to negative outcomes for women and mixed-gender groups, includ- Like other military training environments, at USMA the trans-
ing intimate partner violence (Lisco et al., 2012; Renzetti et al., formation of young adults over time through crucible experiences is
2018) and increased risk of sexual assault perpetration (Abrams a key supposition but also may increase hypermasculinity and bring
et al., 2003; Forbes et al., 2004). Even sexist humor, which may be negative concomitants (Braswell & Kushner, 2012; Hendrix, 2006).
particularly common in a military setting (Cornelius & Monk- To evaluate the effect of training we were also interested in
Turner, 2019), serves to increase victim-blaming and self-reported differences between freshmen and graduating seniors. By evaluating
proclivity toward sexual assault (Thomae & Pina, 2015). In a military hypermasculinity facets for their influence on key outcomes
military setting, hostile sexism is associated with maltreatment of as moderated by length of training, we were able to build on existing
female trainees (Barron & Ogle, 2014), and with negative attitudes literature to disentangle productive versus counterproductive as-
toward women in combat roles (Young & Nauta, 2013); ultimately, pects of military hypermasculinity by what men do (de Boise, 2019;
hostile sexism erodes cohesion and have a negative impact on Harway & Steel, 2015). In line with work suggesting increases in
retention of women (Moore, 2010). However clear the negative hypermasculinity over the course of training (e.g., Callahan, 2009;
impact of sexism on women, the effect of hostile sexism on those Hendrix, 2006), we generally expected significant moderation
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

with the belief is an understudied aspect of traditionally masculine effects of year, such that relationships between hypermasculinity
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

professional settings, and its risk for interference with antisexism facets and outcomes would be stronger for seniors than freshmen.
training has been suggested but not yet explored.
Method
Benevolent Sexism Participants, Procedure, and Data Sources
Hostile sexism is often considered in tandem with benevolent Participants were USMA freshman men from the graduating
sexism, or beliefs that women are inherently nurturing but weak, and classes of 2021 and 2022 (N = 1,560; 66% White, 13% Black,
ergo should be limited to specific roles in society (Glick & Fiske, 10% Hispanic, 9% Asian, 2% other; 20% intercollegiate athlete) and
2018). Hostility toward women is more directly connected to graduating senior men from the classes of 2017 and 2018 (N = 499
violence against and derogation of women (Lisak & Roth, 1988), men; 71% White, 10% Black, 8% Hispanic, 8% Asian, 3% Other;
whereas benevolent sexism is related to forces that limit women’s 12% intercollegiate athlete). Demographic identifiers used were
roles in society (Barreto & Ellemers, 2005; Glick & Fiske, 2011). those collected by USMA, which use male or female to identify
Benevolent sexism is less commonly conceptualized as a hostile gender, and a single item for race and ethnicity.
expression of masculinity; however, it may be particularly relevant For self-report measures, a battery of character-related question-
in a military setting as traditional values of family to include the role naires was given at two timepoints: Once to incoming freshman in
of the military wife are imbedded in military culture (e.g., Rosen July, prior to summer training and the start of the academic year; in
et al., 2003). Benevolent sexism relates to support of a majority- the spring, a similar battery plus additional questions about experi-
male military and disapproval of female servicemembers (Uğurlu & ences at USMA was administered to all students. In the spring, the
Özdemir, 2017; Young & Nauta, 2013), so it may be especially questionnaire battery was administered during one class session of a
important to military gender climate. character education module (cadet character development program
[CCDP], explained below) that features small-group discussions
about topics related to character and leadership. The cadet discus-
The Present Study sion facilitator read a statement about the voluntary and confidential
nature of the survey, and volunteers were then provided with time to
This study aimed to bridge military and civilian literature with a complete it online. For incoming freshman, the battery was admin-
nuanced coverage of both hostile facets of military hypermasculinity istered as part of a larger assessment day on a paper survey, in which
and outcomes it might affect, under the premise that hypermascu- students could volunteer to complete our questionnaire battery. For
linity can be counterproductive to men (and thus high endorsement freshman analyses, self-report data were used for incoming students
of hypermasculinity facets related to negative outcomes), even in a (prior to start of the academic year); for seniors, the spring adminis-
military context (e.g., Green et al., 2010). Our selected expressions tration was used. Nonquestionnaire outcome data for the respective
of military hypermasculinity that have been connected to poor social time periods (freshman or senior year) were obtained from USMA’s
climate and are hypothesized to impact men included: Desire for data warehouses and were connected by an anonymous identification
status and control, hostile and benevolent sexism, and conformity to number in accordance with Institutional Review Board procedures.
masculine norms of emotional control, violence tolerance, imper-
sonal sex preference, and competitiveness/needing to win. Out-
comes included key performance metrics from the three graded Self-Reported Measures of Masculine Attributes
programs at USMA (2018), including academic grade point average Measures were selected from a battery of questionnaires given as
(GPA), leadership grade, and physical fitness tests; behavioral part of a larger data collection effort. These attributes are detailed
indicators; and perceptions of Officers and peers, and of antibias below; for items see Appendix 1.
education. The limited literature specific to our context, facets, and
outcomes precludes formulation of exhaustive expectations, but we
Conformity to Masculine Norms
hypothesized that masculinity facets would generally relate to lower
performance, maladaptive behavior, and poorer social/institutional Four subscales were selected from the Conformity to Masculine
perceptions, with a positive relationship between competitiveness Norms Inventory that related to our context (CMNI; Mahalik et al.,
(winning) and performance expected (Kaya et al., 2019). 2003). Playboy (impersonal sex preference), violence (violence
MILITARY HYPERMASCULINITY AT USMA 615

tolerance), winning (needing to win), and emotional control have Two of these programs included sexual harassment assault response
been connected to negative outcomes, especially with respect to prevention (SHARP) and CCDP; others not selected included, for
gender and gender climate as reviewed above. For the present example, a challenging physical fitness experience. CCDP was a
sample, Cronbach’s α = .77, .75., .75, and .73 respectively. Other small-group discussion series on character-related topics. It included
subscales from this measure, such as primacy of work, were less content directly related to gender–climate issues, including sessions
directly relevant to our focus on masculinity expressions that on “hookup culture” and toxic masculinity, as well as other themes,
influence gender and social climate. Heterosexual self-presentation such as racial diversity and the honor code. SHARP is an Army-wide
was also omitted as sexuality-related questions are not typically initiative to reduce instances of sexual assault, harassment, report
assessed in our context. While conceptually relevant, power over retaliation, and related problems (Arbeit, 2017); USMA’s program
women was not used as the larger project necessitated surveying included a day-long symposium on relationships and consent.
women with identical questions; therefore, desire for status and To assess perception of peers and leadership, we adapted three
control from Machiavellianism, below, were used to assess power items each from the Inventory for Parents and Peer Attachment
and dominance-related attributes. (Armsden & Greenberg, 1987) and the Perceptions of Leadership
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

scale (Snyder et al., 2012), respectively. Cronbach’s α = .93 and


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Status and Control .82. For peer connection, we asked for agreement with the follow-
ing: “I trust fellow cadets,” “Other cadets care about me,” and
Three items from two subscales were taken from Machiavellian- “Other cadets are there when I need them.” For perception of
ism (Dahling et al., 2009): Control (e.g., “I enjoy having control leadership, we asked whether students’ commanding Officers:
over other people”) and Status (e.g., “Status is a good sign of “Demonstrate good examples of sound moral character?,” “hold
success in life”). For the present sample, Cronbach’s α = .71 and others accountable for their conduct,” and “promote and safeguard
.77, respectively. the welfare of subordinates.”

Hostile and Benevolent Sexism Behavioral Reports


Three items each were used from the Hostile and Benevolent USMA staff, faculty, or student leaders may submit a behavioral
Sexism subscales of the Ambivalent Sexism Inventory (Glick & report in order to provide feedback to the student’s supervising
Fiske, 2018). This subscale forms the hostile sexism factor Officer. Reports are tagged as positive or negative and include
(Hayduk & Littvay, 2012), and is one of the most commonly information about the event. Positive reports were often submitted
used metrics of negative beliefs about women. For the present for volunteer work or exceptional academic performance, whereas
sample, Cronbach’s α = .75 and .65 for hostile and benevolent negative reports minor rule-breaking conduct. The majority of
sexism, respectively. reports (approximately 63% in the total sample) are submitted for
students in their freshman year, when students are most closely
Performance Outcomes supervised. Most students do not receive any behavioral reports their
senior year, so this outcome was only used for the freshman group.
Program Grades Numbers of negative and positive reports for each student were
USMA performance outcomes included GPA from the academic input in models.
program and a military leadership grade given by a supervising
officer, which comprises about half of the total grade in the military Analytic Approach
program for each semester (USMA, 2016b).
Our primary hypotheses concerned the relationships between
masculinity facets in relating to key USMA outcomes. Two categories
Army Physical Fitness Test
of outcomes were available for all students: Performance measures
The Army Physical Fitness Test (APFT) consists of a 2-min effort and self-reported perceptions. We also considered behavioral reports,
of push-ups, 2-min of sit-ups, and 2-mile run. All Soldiers, including which were only tabulated for freshmen. Two multivariate moderated
USMA cadets, complete this test twice a year. Raw scores were multiple regressions (MMMR) assessed whether military hypermas-
converted to points according to U.S. Army Field Manual 7-22 (U.S. culinity attributes were related to (a) performance outcomes (GPA,
Department of the Army, 2013). To limit multicollinearity, only leadership grade, AFPT run, and pushup scores) and (b) perceptions
push-up and run scores were used; sit-up score was more highly (SHARP and character education, supervising Officers, peers), and
related to either push-ups or run (r = .56 and .55, respectively), than tested whether student year (freshman or senior) significantly
push-ups and run related to each other (r = .46). impacted the relationships between facets and outcomes (moderation
effect). For the year by masculinity facet moderation effects, includ-
Perception of Peers, Leadership, Institutional Character ing all eight interactions was undesirable, as this full model had
Variance Inflation Factors (VIF) > 30, suggesting poor statistical
Values, and Programs
power and inflated standard errors (Thompson et al., 2017). To
As part of the spring administration of questionnaires, we as- combat this, nested models tested the sequential addition of modera-
sessed perception of USMA’s character development efforts and tion effects for two variables at a time (status and control, playboy and
internalization of USMA character values. To address the former, emotional control, violence and winning, hostile and benevolent
students rated how influential various programs and experiences sexism). Pillai F-statistics determined whether the set of interactions
were on a 1–5 scale to assess perception of specific experiences. were kept, and the final models were interpreted. For freshmen only,
616 SCHAEFER, COTTING, PROCTOR, RYAN, AND LERNER

Note. GPA = grade point average; APFT = Army Physical Fitness Test; SHARP = sexual harassment assault response prevention; Char ed = character education. In the correlation matrix, grey color
0.11**

0.07**

−0.37**
−0.41**
−0.19**
−0.11**

−0.20**
0.09*
−0.09*

0.05*

0.10*
two multiple regressions determined relationships between masculin-

0.01

−0.01
0.04

0.03

0.09
−0.04
18
ity attributes and positive and negative behavioral reports. Based on
previous work demonstrating differences in several of our key out-

−0.21**

−0.09**
−0.09**
0.22**
0.33**
−0.05*

0.06*
comes by race and athlete status (Schaefer et al., 2021), even when

0.02

0.03

−0.03

0.04
0.05
0.04
0.04
0.06
0.08
17
SAT is accounted for, race, recruited athlete status, and SAT were
included as predictors for all models. This step helped assure that

0.17**

0.17**

0.18**
0.12*
0.10*
results were not simply due to third-variable effects of demographics

−0.05

−0.09
−0.06
−0.02
−0.04
−0.05
0.00
0.09

0.05
0.01
16
or educational background. MMMR models tested for omnibus
effects of masculinity facets on groups of outcomes and examined

0.14*
−0.10
0.04
−0.02
−0.08
−0.08
−0.09
0.03
−0.02
0.03
0.03
0.05
0.02

0.03
whether seniors and freshmen differed in how these attributes influ-

15
enced outcomes; however, intercorrelated predictors in regression
models complicate interpretation of the relative impacts of significant

−0.27**

−0.25**
−0.3**

0.4**
−0.11*
terms, and correlations among our military hypermasculinity facets

−0.09
−0.06
−0.02

−0.08
0.08
−0.02
−0.07
−0.03
14
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

were expected (Thompson et al., 2017; Tonidandel & LeBreton,


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

2011). To address this, relative importance, a measure of partitioned

−0.18**

−0.15**
−0.14*

−0.11*

−0.12*
R2 of predictors that accounts for multicollinearity, provided supple-

−0.11

−0.06

0.08
0.01
−0.05
0.04
0.08
13
mental effect size information. Relative importance has been used to
address related masculinity factors in particular (LeBreton et al.,

0.11**

0.26** 0.17**
0.35** 0.28**
0.46**
0.08*

0.07*
2013), and assesses the unique role, and thus more stable estimate,

0.01

−0.07* −0.03
0.03

−0.08** −0.01
−0.09** 0.01
12
that related predictors have in a regression (Tonidandel & LeBreton,
2011). The lmg metric was used as it is the most commonly
recommended metric of importance and calculates the R2 of each

−0.05
0.05

−0.03
−0.02
0.01
11
predictor by averaging over all possible orders of predictors
(Groemping & Matthias, 2018). Relative importance models were

−0.17**

−0.10**

−0.08**

−0.14**
0.47**
−0.06*
run individually by the outcome but included the same predictors as

0.06

−0.02
−0.02
10

the MMMRs. R statistical software, Version 4.0.2 was used for all
analyses (R Core Team, 2013). 0.14**
−0.12**

−0.12**
−0.18**
−0.08*

−0.03
−0.02
−0.02
9

Results
0.21** 0.11**

0.10** −0.08**

0.20**
0.06*
0.20** −0.01

0.02
−0.02
8

Descriptive statistics and correlations among military hypermas-


culinity attributes and key outcomes described are given in Table 1
for freshmen and Table 2 for seniors. Nested MMMRs for perfor-
0.01
0.04
0.04
7

mance and perception outcomes first determined which moderation


effects for year were significant, then the two final models inter-
0.32**
0.22**
0.08**
0.66**
0.75**

preted. For the performance outcomes (GPA, leadership grade,


6

APFT run, and pushup scores), nested MMMRs demonstrated


Descriptive Statistics and Correlations for the Freshman Group

0.09** 0.01 0.17**

0.71**

that only the student year by hostile and benevolent sexism mod-
.93 0.28** 0.28** 0.00 0.07*

0.03 0.05
5

erators (entered together) contributed significantly to the model ( p =


.016, other ps > .5, see Supplemental Table 1 for details). They
4

indicated that the effects of other masculinity attributes were not


significantly different between freshmen and seniors. Results of this
3

model are presented in Table 3. Omnibus multivariate effects were


significant for all predictors except emotional control and the
benevolent sexism by year interaction. For the univariate effects,
2

status was negatively associated with leadership grade, and benev-


olent sexism and playboy were both negatively related to both GPA
.71
.76

.95
.66
.68

.85
.82
1.0
3.12 1.1
2.96 1.0
1.1

2.57 1.4
2.59 1.4

4.76 3.6
1.79 2.3
SD

and leadership grade. Violence and hostile sexism were negatively


90.4 15
APFT pushup score 90.3 13

related to GPA and run score. When examining relative weights,


indicates the outcome variables.
3.14
3.26
2.24

3.57
2.97
2.63

3.92
Mean

3.5
2.9

3.7

playboy and benevolent sexism contributed the most partitioned


variance among masculinity attributes negatively predicting GPA
* p < .05. ** p < .01.

and leadership grades. Winning was positively related to both fitness


Negative behaviors
SHARP perception
Char ed perception
Benevolent sexism

Positive behaviors
Officer perception
Emotional control
Desire for control

Leadership grade
Desire for status

APFT run score

Peer connection

scores, indicating that this masculine attribute was beneficial to


Hostile sexism

physical fitness performance, and control was positively related to


Variable

Violence
Winning

all outcomes except run. The hostile sexism by student year


Playboy
Table 1

GPA

interaction indicated that seniors, as compared to freshmen, had a


more positive relationship between hostile sexism and push-ups
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
10.
11.
12.
13.
14.
15.
16.
17.
18.

(Figure 1, left).
MILITARY HYPERMASCULINITY AT USMA 617

−0.18**

0.13**

0.18**
0.34**

Note. GPA = grade point average; APFT = Army Physical Fitness Test; SHARP = sexual harassment assault response prevention; Char ed = character education. In the correlation matrix, grey color
−0.15**
−0.14**

0.18**
0.11*

0.10*

0.10*
0.11*
For perception measures, results of the nested models demonstrated

0.07
0.09

−0.01

0.03
16 that there were significant moderators among the status and control
interactions, and for hostile and benevolent sexism ( p = .027 and

−0.14**

0.14**
−0.14**
0.11*
.0031, respectively; Supplemental Table 2). Omnibus effects of the
0.10*

0.10*
0.10*
0.09*

0.10*
0.12*
−0.04
0.04

0.02
−0.05
15

final MMMR were significant for emotional control, control, vio-


lence, benevolent sexism, and the hostile by year and status by year
interactions (Table 4). In terms of univariate effects, emotional
−0.14**
−0.18**

0.69**
−0.11*
0.05
0.04
−0.02

−0.06
−0.08
0.05

−0.05
0.06
−0.02
control and violence were negatively related to impressions of
14

SHARP (antisexual harassment education) and USMA’s character


education program. Benevolent sexism was positively related to
−0.13**
−0.23**

−0.14**
−0.10*

−0.11*
−0.10*

−0.10*
perception of character education. For perception of supervising
0.05
0.07
0.06

0.04

−0.03
13

officers and peer connection, there were positive student year moder-
ation effects (interactions) for control and negative interactions with
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

0.14**
0.13**

0.23**
0.25**
0.52**

hostile sexism (Figure 1, middle and right). Seniors had a stronger


0.10*
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

−0.02
0.05
−0.01
0.02
0.07
12

negative relationship between hostile sexism and these perceptions,


indicating that seniors high on hostile sexism had poorer perceptions
of their supervisors and less connection to peers. Status had the
−0.14**

0.13**

0.26**
0.31**
0.00
−0.03
0.03
0.04

−0.04
0.03

opposite effect, in the seniors with a strong desire for status perceived
11

supervising Officers and their peers more positively.


For behavioral reports, which were only tabulated for freshmen,
−0.16**

0.33**

only status, control, emotional control, and playboy had significant


−0.01
−0.08
0.02
0.05
0.06
−0.06
−0.08
10

effects (Table 5). Playboy was associated with more negative and
fewer positive behavioral reports; Machiavellian desire for control
−0.17**

demonstrated the opposite pattern. High desire for status was related
−0.05
0.02

−0.03
−0.01
0.04

−0.03
−0.1*
9

to fewer positive reports, and emotional control was significantly


related to more negative behavioral reports.
−0.23**

0.12**
0.39**
0.10*
0.11*

0.00
0.02
8

Discussion
Understanding how certain expressions of masculinity manifest in
0.27**
0.21**
0.15**
0.17**

0.10*

regard to professional outcomes of USMA cadets helps illuminate


0.07
7

how these beliefs operate naturalistically, and has implications for


academic, military, and traditionally masculine occupational settings.
0.13**
0.18**

0.37**
−0.10*
0.02

The military context represents a specific environment and results


6

must be interpreted with that specificity in mind, but it is also an ideal


setting to evaluate effects of military hypermasculinity on men, given
−0.10*
−0.04
0.03

0.05

the military’s position culturally and historically as a bastion of


5

masculine development (Barrett, 1996; Do & Samuels, 2020). The


Descriptive Statistics and Correlations for Graduating Seniors

results of the present study argue for a nuanced consideration of


0.11*
0.00

0.06

military hypermasculinity via its facets and context-specific out-


4

comes, and, in line with hypotheses, demonstrate how hostile facets


0.20**

of masculinity can be counterproductive even to those holding these


0.06
3

beliefs. In most cases, agreement with hostile masculine attributes was


associated with poorer performance, worse conduct, lower impres-
0.45**

sions of others, and rejection of antibias and character education


2

efforts.
Impersonal sex preference (CMNI-playboy) was related to poorer
.84

.96
.88
.89

.50
.61

.94
.83
1.1

1.1

1.1
1.2

1.1
1.2

grades and behaviors, which agreed with prior work in college


SD

14
11

students and young men generally (Abbey et al., 2011; Malamuth


et al., 1991; Waterman et al., 2020). Most work on impersonal sex
Mean

3.03
3.25
2.36

2.27
3.22
3.93
3.61
2.94
3.11

3.87
3.84

indicates the outcome variables.


3.2
3.1

2.2
89.3
91.4

uses it as an outcome to uncover belief systems underlying the


behavior. The current results demonstrate that impersonal sex as a
* p < .05. ** p < .01.
APFT pushup score
SHARP perception
Char ed perception
Benevolent sexism

stated preference is associated with poorer functioning and was


Officer perception
Emotional control
Desire for control

Leadership grade
Desire for status

APFT run score

Peer connection

similar to work that has connected it to lifestyle instability (LeBreton


Hostile sexism
Variable

et al., 2013). Healthy messaging surrounding sex and relationships


Violence
Winning

might be particularly important for young servicemembers in par-


Playboy
Table 2

GPA

ticular given different narratives surrounding impersonal sex, which


can focus on sex despite “undesirable” characteristics of a partner
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
10.
11.
12.
13.
14.
15.
16.

or fear of negative consequences of a continuing relationship


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

618

Table 3
Multivariate Moderated Multiple Regression for Performance Outcomes

Univariate tests
GPA Leadership grade Run Push-ups
MV
Variable F B t p RW B t p RW B t p RW B t p RW

SAT 138*** .0027 21 <.001 .29 .0002 1.2 .21 .026 .005 1.3 .20 .0049 −.0052 −1.5 .13 .0012
Recr. athlete 17*** −.052 −1.3 .19 .028 −.38 −7.5 <.001 .07 .86 .71 .48 <.001 1.1 1.1 .29 .0022
Race 7.7*** −.24 −4.9 <.001 .061 −.48 −7.7 <.001 .051 −9.2 −6.2 <.001 .045 −3.5 −2.7 .0074 .013
Year 28.2*** −.023 −.21 .83 .02 .11 .78 .43 .073 −11 −3.4 <.001 .0029 −6.7 −2.3 .024 <.001
Status 3.3* −.013 −.83 .41 .0022 −.07 −3.4 <.001 .017 −.93 −1.9 .059 .0042 −.61 −1.4 .16 <.001
Control 4.3** .076 3.8 <.001 .0091 .066 2.6 .0095 .0027 .79 1.3 .19 .0018 1.1 2.1 .034 .0041
Playboy 2.8* −.042 −2.7 .0062 .007 −.051 −2.5 .011 .0052 −.31 −.65 .52 .0023 −.021 −.05 .96 <.001
Emot control .77 −.0021 −.14 .89 <.001 .012 .61 .54 <.001 −.051 −.11 .92 <.001 −.56 −1.3 .18 .0016
Violence 5.3*** −.064 −3.6 <.001 .0029 .0048 .21 .83 .0011 −1.1 −2.1 .036 .0016 −.07 −.15 .88 <.001
Winning 3.3* .033 1.8 .068 .0020 .00087 .038 .97 .0012 1.5 2.8 .0053 .0057 1.4 2.9 .0039 .0072
Hostile sexism 2.4* −.040 −1.96 .050 .0078 −.016 −.71 .48 .004 −1.5 −2.8 .005 .0031 −.76 −1.6 .11 <.001
Benev sexism 3.2* −.052 −2.7 .0063 .010 −.074 −3.0 .0024 .018 −1.0 −1.8 .079 .0014 .028 .055 .96 <.001
Hostile × Year 2.6* −.029 −1.0 .29 <.001 .043 1.2 .22 <.001 1.2 1.4 .15 .0021 1.7 2.3 .021 .0051
Benev × Year 2.1 .068 2.4 .016 .003 .031 .86 .39 .0015 1.5 1.8 .072 .0038 .28 .38 .71 <.001
Adjusted R2 = .43 Adjusted R2 = .24 Adjusted R2 = .067 Adjusted R2 = .024
Note. Multivariate (MV) tests were run on (4,1163) df for all continuous predictors, gender, and recruited athlete covariates. Race had (16, 4664) df for the MV test, and for univariate effects the White–Black
SCHAEFER, COTTING, PROCTOR, RYAN, AND LERNER

student contrasts are shown for brevity, as this has the most power. Univariate effects are gray if nonsignificant at the multivariate level and bolded if significant. Positive betas for the year interaction indicate
that the slope is more positive for seniors as compared to freshmen. SAT = Scholastic Aptitude Test; GPA = grade point average; RW = relative weight, or partitioned R2 for that variable controlling for
multicollinearity.
* p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001.
MILITARY HYPERMASCULINITY AT USMA 619

(Berry-Cabán et al., 2020). We did not assess whether impersonal sex emotional suppression in a military context (Evans, 2011;
preference was more detrimental due to objectification of partners, Sherman, 2010) and elevated to the status of a character attribute
avoidance of close relationships, desire to affirm masculinity, the (Sherman, 2007; Stricker et al., 2017). However, a barrage of
behavior itself, or any other aspect. Dismantling this effect in future literature links restricted emotionality to negative consequences
work may help refine interventions in social skill-focused and sex- for self and others (e.g., Cohn et al., 2010). Arguably, stoicism
positive (vs. abstinence) perspective (Carter, 2018). Attention toward as currently practiced better resembles a rebranding of alexithymia,
impersonal sex and its driving factors might not only reduce risk of specifically Normative Male Alexithymia, which has clear connec-
sexual assault (Logan-Greene & Cue Davis, 2011), but also reduce tion to poor outcomes (Karakis & Levant, 2012). Attempts to
general negative consequences for those with this inclination. Anti- counteract stoicism messages and encourage emotional flexibility
objectification education, which has shown efficacy in a general might care to discuss aspects and skills individually, which can
population, might be useful here too (Guizzo & Cadinu, 2020). sidestep some of the gendered association with emotionality
Results concerning emotional control and violence tolerance (Petrides et al., 2004), and present a flexible and nuanced view
(CMNI-violence), themselves linked via emotional intelligence of emotions that allows men to express previously taboo emotions
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

and regulation in prior work (García-Sancho et al., 2014; such as distress and fear.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Roberton et al., 2012), are also important. Both attributes were Not all associations between military hypermasculinity and out-
associated with rejection of both character and antisexism educa- comes were counterproductive. Needing to win (CMNI-winning)
tion; emotional control was further associated with poorer social was positively related to improved physical performance and no
connection and violence tolerance with poorer performance out- negative outcomes, which is supported by other work highlighting
comes. Emotional overcontrol is one aspect of poor emotional the protective effects of competitiveness and how it relates to a
functioning marked by to reduced ability to regulate emotions masculine identity of top physical fitness (Kaya et al., 2019;
and low tolerance of the experience of emotions (Berke et al., Messner, 1992). Control was positively related to grades and
2018); prior work has connected it to intimate partner violence, peer perception, and for seniors, status was positively related to
as well as suicide risk and alcohol use in military populations (Berke perceptions of leaders. These constructs were taken from Machia-
et al., 2019; Burns & Mahalik, 2011). The messaging surround vellianism; work on Machiavellianism and narcissism suggest these
emotions in the military is complex (McAllister et al., 2019), but at constructs are associated with higher impression management but
minimum military culture paints emotions as something “touchy- poorer insight, suggesting that status and control might be somewhat
feely” to be managed (Sewell, 2009; Shields et al., 2017). Even motivating in this context (Park & Colvin, 2014; Smith & Webster,
measures of self-control in a military setting can actually capture the 2017). The negative relationship between status and leadership
negative, rigid aspects of emotional overcontrol, underscoring how performance suggested that there are limits to the utility of such
imbedded emotional control is in the military concept of discipline motivation, however. In the same way that antisocial and Machia-
and self-control (Bergmann et al., 2019). vellian attitudes can be addressed (Whitehead et al., 2007), under-
Stoicism is a popular movement in the military that traditionally scoring the negative personal consequences of a preoccupation with
focused on efficient emotion regulation but has turned toward status on important outcomes might loosen adherence to this value

Figure 1
Relationships Between Pushup Score and Hostile Sexism, and Between Officer Perception and Sexism and Status, That Were Significantly
Different Between Freshmen and Graduation Seniors

Seniors Freshmen Seniors


Officer impression
Pushup score

Freshmen
Freshmen
Seniors

Hostile sexism Hostile sexism Status


Note. As compared to freshmen, seniors had a more positive relationship between push-ups and sexism, and between their perception of supervising Officers
and their preoccupation with status. Seniors had a more negative relationship between their perception of supervising Officers and their level of hostile sexism.
See the online article for the color version of this figure.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

620

Table 4
Multivariate Moderated Multiple Regression for Perception Questionnaires

Univariate tests
SHARP perception Char education perception Officer perception Peer connection
MV
Variable F B t p RW B t p RW B t p RW B t p RW

SAT 2.4* −.0011 −2.6 .011 .014 −.0011 −2.7 .0062 .011 .0000074 .023 .98 <.001 −.00032 −1.1 .26 <.001
Recr athlete 2.4* .15 .95 .34 .0077 −.092 −.67 .50 <.001 −.061 −.53 .60 <.001 −.27 −2.6 .0085 .009
Race 2.0* .28 1.5 .15 .019 −.12 −.73 .47 .0097 .32 2.3 .021 .009 −.11 −.88 .38 .011
Year 4.0** −1.5 −2.4 .015 .018 −.24 −.47 .64 .0027 −.48 −1.1 .28 .0014 .052 .14 .89 .0021
Status .91 −.079 −.84 .40 <.001 −.14 −1.7 .086 <.001 −.13 −1.9 .061 .0024 −.074 −1.2 .22 .0023
Control 3.3* −.071 −.59 .56 <.001 .13 1.3 .21 .0014 .075 .85 .40 .0042 .24 3 .0025 .012
Playboy .76 .037 .7 .48 <.001 .012 .27 .78 <.001 −.069 −1.8 .073 .0095 −.074 −2.2 .031 .013
Emot control 3.9** −.14 −2.5 .012 .0066 −.17 −3.7 <.001 .017 −.03 −.76 .45 <.001 −.068 −1.9 .050 .010
Violence 3.1* −.21 −3.4 <.001 .026 −.14 −2.6 .01 .010 .0027 .059 .95 <.001 −.027 −.68 .50 <.001
Winning .80 −.0096 −.16 .88 .0038 −.037 −.7 .49 .0038 .055 1.2 .22 .0049 .046 1.2 .25 .0033
Hostile sexism 8.2*** −.2 −2.5 .012 .022 −.06 −.89 .37 .0084 .066 1.2 .25 .014 −.026 −.52 .61 .020
Benev sexism 2.5* .12 1.4 .17 .0075 .17 2.2 .028 .013 −.055 −.84 .40 .0028 −.047 −.81 .42 <.001
Status × Year 3.8** .078 .69 .49 .0053 .15 1.5 .13 .0031 .27 3.3 .0011 .011 .18 2.4 .017 .0042
Control × Year 1.7 .25 1.7 .081 <.001 −.0001 −.001 1.0 <.001 .000019 0 1.0 <.001 −.15 −1.6 .12 <.001
Hostile × Year 5.1*** .015 .16 .87 <.001 −.031 −.36 .72 <.001 −.3 −4.2 <.001 .016 −.15 −2.4 .017 .0054
Benev × Year 1.8 −.021 −.2 .84 <.001 −.08 −.89 .38 .0042 .17 2.2 .028 .0086 .10 1.5 .14 0.0011
Adjusted R2 = .11 Adjusted R2 = .070 Adjusted R2 = .065 Adjusted R2 = .072
SCHAEFER, COTTING, PROCTOR, RYAN, AND LERNER

Note. Multivariate (MV) tests were run on (4,632) df for all continuous predictors, gender, and recruited athlete covariates. Race had (16, 2540) df for the MV test, and for univariate effects the White–Black
student contrasts are shown for brevity, as this has the most power. Univariate effects are gray if nonsignificant at the multivariate level and bolded if significant. Positive betas for the year interaction indicate
that the slope is more positive for seniors as compared to freshmen. SAT = Scholastic Aptitude Test; SHARP = sexual harassment assault response prevention; RW= relative weight, or partitioned R2 for that
variable controlling for multicollinearity.
* p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001.
MILITARY HYPERMASCULINITY AT USMA 621

Table 5
Multiple Regression Models for Behavioral Reports Among Freshmen

Positive behavior reports Negative behavior reports


Variable B t p RW B t p RW

SAT .0023 2.6 .011 .020 −.0023 −3.8 <.001 .023


Recruited athlete −2.8 −10 <.001 .089 .16 .88 .38 .0039
Race .21 .62 .54 .0033 1.3 5.8 <.001 .039
Status −.31 −2.7 .0064 .0092 .11 1.5 .13 .0033
Control .35 2.5 .012 .0035 −.26 −2.8 .0050 .0057
Playboy −.29 −2.7 .0081 .0070 .13 1.8 .075 .0040
Emotional control .04 .36 .72 <.001 .15 2.0 .042 .0030
Violence .23 1.8 .072 .0033 .13 1.5 .12 <.001
Winning −.061 −.48 .63 .0024 .10 1.3 .21 .0014
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Hostile sexism −.14 −1.4 .16 .0035 .0078 .12 .90 <.001
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Benevolent sexism .0062 .06 .95 .0012 .11 1.6 .10 .0043
Adjusted R2 = .13 Adjusted R2 = .074
Note. Tests were run on 1257 df for continuous predictors and for race the White-Black student statistics are shown for brevity, as this has the most power.
Significant masculinity attributes are bolded for clarity. SAT = Scholastic Aptitude Test; RW= relative weight, or partitioned R2 for that variable controlling for
multicollinearity.

system by demonstrating how it is counterproductive. Alternatively, albeit of two class cohorts each; whether effects reflect development,
aspects of a preference for control could be related to self-regulation or an artifact of other cohort differences, cannot be determined. In
and a desire to achieve in a ranked, highly competitive environment addition, our focus was on professional performance, institutional
(Tangney et al., 2004). As noted above the assessment and messag- perception, and other more generalized effects. We did not address
ing surrounding “control” is complicated in a military environment, how sexism impacts others, or whether individuals concerningly
but it appears that some aspects of interpersonal control operate in a high on any factor were at increased risk for perpetuating harass-
positive manner for younger military trainees. ment/bullying, assault, or bias (Inesi & Cable, 2015). We also
Poor perception of character education and antisexism training identified masculine attributes hypothesized to be important in
was an important outcome to this study; addressing low interest in, this setting, which does not preclude the importance of other
or acceptance of, prejudice reduction education is vital, as this possible manifestations, such as fragile masculinity (DiMuccio &
motivation may be key to program efficacy (e.g., Devine et al., Knowles, 2020).
2012). Like many military and professional environments, USMA Additionally, USMA students reporting high levels of hostile
exerts considerable time and resources toward antibias and character sexism, violence tolerance, or emotional control believed that
education efforts (USMA, 2018), and, like many other professional antibias and character education programming were ineffective;
environments, sexism and other biased attitudes persist. Hostile however, this finding does not address whether these programs
sexism was related to negative perceptions of antisexism training, were, in fact, effective in reducing sexist beliefs in these students or
and with previous research suggested that these beliefs are detri- others. The nature and content of character development and anti-
mental to antibias efforts (Cundiff et al., 2009). For freshmen, sexism education at USMA varies by year, even across the cohorts
attitudes were measured at entry to USMA and the perception sampled, which is common to military interventions (Orchowski
measures collected at year-end; hostility toward women appeared et al., 2018). Others have stressed the need to standardize and
to influence perception of organizational values and education over critically evaluate these educational efforts (Gidycz et al., 2018).
a relatively long period of time. Emotional control and violence The results of this study suggested that facets of military hyper-
tolerance were also associated with rejection of both types of masculinity undermine the character development efforts of USMA
education efforts. Benevolent sexism was linked to better perception and hostility toward women undercuts SHARP programming;
of character education, suggesting the messages were well perceived however, whether these results would generalize broadly to other
by those individuals; it is encouraging that these individuals might interventions is an important direction for future work.
be particularly receptive to USMA’s character education program, Finally, these data concern U.S. young adult military trainees in a
given the link between benevolent sexism and objections to women specific educational environment, and how results generalize to the
in service (e.g., Uğurlu & Özdemir, 2017; Young & Nauta, 2013). general military, or other traditionally masculine environments, is
These findings may help further contextualize the limited efficacy of unknown. The question of why military hypermasculinity persists
antibias programming and possibly identify intervention targets despite negative consequences to character, social, and performance
when programs are less effective than desired (Applebaum, outcomes, cannot be answered by these data. Our results underscore
2019; Dobbin & Kalev, 2018). Whether tailoring antibias education that, even in a traditionally masculine environment, military hy-
efforts to the needs of those most likely to reject it increases permasculinity is often counterproductive; therefore, how indivi-
antiprejudice motivation and program efficacy is an important future duals succeed and ultimately gain power despite poorer abilities and
direction. behavior is a crucial question for future work. In line with hypothe-
There are several important limitations to the current research. ses, most moderation effects demonstrated that the strength of
Importantly, our freshman and senior groups were separate samples, relationship between hypermasculinity facets and outcomes was
622 SCHAEFER, COTTING, PROCTOR, RYAN, AND LERNER

stronger for seniors as compared to freshmen; in conjunction other Military Academy at West Point. Journal of Sex Research, 54(8), 949–
work suggesting hypermasculinity may increase with training (e.g., 961. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2016.1207055
Callahan, 2009), this may indicate that hypermasculinity is more Archer, E. M. (2013). The power of gendered stereotypes in the US Marine
salient over time in this environment. Notably, for seniors, hostile Corps. Armed Forces and Society, 39(2), 359–391. https://doi.org/10.1177/
sexism was associated with higher physical performance (push- 0095327X12446924
Arkin, W., & Dobrofsky, L. R. (1978). Military socialization and masculin-
ups); in the larger Army, applications to tracks for high profile
ity. Journal of Social Issues, 34(1), 151–168. https://doi.org/10.1111/j
positions and “star” opportunities highlight physical fitness scores
.1540-4560.1978.tb02546.x
as one of few performance metrics. Therefore, the positive connec- Armsden, G. C., & Greenberg, M. T. (1987). The inventory of parent and
tion between hostile sexism and occupational success demonstrated peer attachment: Individual differences and their relationship to psycho-
by Cheng et al. (2020) might be most salient postgraduation, logical well-being in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
especially for selection into “star” tracks that afford cumulative 16(5), 427–454. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02202939
advantages over the course of the career, and tend to elevate the Ashley, W., Tapia, J., Constantine Brown, J. L., & Block, O. (2017). Don’t
values and priorities of previous cohorts (Corona, 2011; Moore & fight like a girl: Veteran preferences based on combat exposure and gender.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Trout, 1978). As the Army transitions to a new fitness test, which is Affilia, 32(2), 230–242. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886109916685800
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

more holistic and better translates to relevant physical tasks versus Barreto, M., & Ellemers, N. (2005). The burden of benevolent sexism: How
the APFT, but also places greater emphasis on muscular strength/ it contributes to the maintenance of gender inequalities. European Journal
of Social Psychology, 35(5), 633–642. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.270
endurance (United States Army, 2019), further evaluation of mili-
Barrett, F. J. (1996). The organizational construction of hegemonic mascu-
tary promotion and retention is crucial to demonstrate whether
linity: The case of the US Navy. Gender, Work and Organization, 3(3),
hostile expressions of masculinity are as counterproductive in the 129–142. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0432.1996.tb00054.x
greater military as at USMA, or if this finding reverses. Barron, L. G., & Ogle, A. D. (2014). Individual differences in instructor
attitudes underlying maltreatment and effective mentoring in basic mili-
Conclusion tary training. Military Psychology, 26(5–6), 386–396. https://doi.org/10
.1037/mil0000053
In this study, impersonal sex preference, emotional control, vio- Becker, J. C., Zawadzki, M. J., & Shields, S. A. (2014). Confronting and
lence tolerance, and hostile sexism were associated with negative reducing sexism: A call for research on intervention. Journal of Social
consequences. Targeted education surrounding healthy relationships, Issues, 70(4), 603–614. https://doi.org/10.1111/josi.12081
anti-objectification of women and sexual partners generally, and more Bergmann, J. S., Renshaw, K. D., & Paige, L. (2019). Psychological well-
nuanced views of emotion and aggression might help subvert the being in Iraq and Afghanistan veterans: Risk and protective factors.
Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 11(4),
negative consequences to self and others of these hypermasculinity
434–441. https://doi.org/10.1037/tra0000416
facets. Vitally, hostility toward women remains an imperative focus Berke, D. S., Reidy, D., & Zeichner, A. (2018). Masculinity, emotion
for intervention in a military context and may relate to rejection of regulation, and psychopathology: A critical review and integrated model.
educational efforts, implying that those who reject antisexism mes- Clinical Psychology Review, 66, 106–116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr
saging providing by USMA are those who need it most. As the .2018.01.004
military moves to an increasingly interpersonal leadership develop- Berke, D. S., Reidy, D. E., Gentile, B., & Zeichner, A. (2019). Masculine
ment strategy (Fallesen et al., 2011), and in other contexts where discrepancy stress, emotion-regulation difficulties, and intimate partner
gender remains salient and antibias efforts have been less than violence. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 34(6), 1163–1182. https://
effective, full consideration of the factors that contribute to gendered doi.org/10.1177/0886260516650967
maltreatment and poor social relationships is crucially important. Berry-Cabán, C. S., Orchowski, L. M., Winstead, T. L., Metzger, E. C., &
Military hypermasculinity has been connected to sexual assault, Kazemi, D. M. (2020). Conceptualizations of hooking up among male
soldiers: A qualitative analysis. Military Medicine, 185(Suppl. 1), 355–
aggression, and degradation of social climate in decades of work,
361. https://doi.org/10.1093/milmed/usz230
yet remains a crucial omission and apparent elephant in the room in
Bird, S. R. (2003). Sex composition, masculinity stereotype dissimilarity and
military efforts to address toxic workplace climates. Perhaps demon- the quality of men’s workplace social relations. Gender, Work and
stration of the counterproductive aspects of military hypermasculinity Organization, 10(5), 579–604. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0432.00212
in terms of leadership, behavior, and character can help organizations Board of Visitors. (2017). Annual report. United States Military Academy.
more confidently identify and eradicate their negative concomitants. https://s3.amazonaws.com/usma-media/inline-images/about/board_of_
visitors/board_of_visitors_2017_annual_report.pdf
Braswell, H., & Kushner, H. I. (2012). Suicide, social integration, and
References
masculinity in the U.S. military. Social Science & Medicine, 74(4),
Abbey, A., Jacques-Tiura, A. J., & LeBreton, J. M. (2011). Risk factors for 530–536. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.07.031
sexual aggression in young men: An expansion of the confluence model. Burns, S. M., & Mahalik, J. R. (2011). Suicide and dominant masculinity
Aggressive Behavior, 37(5), 450–464. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20399 norms among current and former United States military servicemen.
Abrams, D., Viki, G. T., Masser, B., & Bohner, G. (2003). Perceptions of Professional Psychology, Research and Practice, 42(5), 347–353. https://
stranger and acquaintance rape: The role of benevolent and hostile sexism doi.org/10.1037/a0025163
in victim blame and rape proclivity. Journal of Personality and Social Callahan, J. L. (2009). Manifestations of power and control: Training as the
Psychology, 84(1), 111–125. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.111 catalyst for scandal at the United States Air Force Academy. Violence
Applebaum, B. (2019). Remediating campus climate: Implicit bias training is Against Women, 15(10), 1149–1168. https://doi.org/10.1177/107780
not enough. Studies in Philosophy and Education, 38(2), 129–141. https:// 1209344341
doi.org/10.1007/s11217-018-9644-1 Caron, S. L., Halteman, W. A., & Stacy, C. (1997). Athletes and rape: Is there
Arbeit, M. R. (2017). “Make sure you’re not getting yourself in trouble”: a connection? Perceptual and Motor Skills, 85(Suppl. 3), 1379–1393.
Building sexual relationships and preventing sexual violence at the U.S. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1997.85.3f.1379
MILITARY HYPERMASCULINITY AT USMA 623

Carroll, M. H., & Clark, M. D. (2006). Men’s acquaintance rape scripts: A college students’ experiences with dating aggression and sexual coercion.
comparison between a regional University and a Military Academy. Sex Violence Against Women, 10(3), 236–261. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077
Roles, 55(7), 469–480. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-006-9102-3 801203256002
Carter, A. (2018). Reducing sexual victimization on college campuses: The Fox, J., & Pease, B. (2012). Military deployment, masculinity and trauma:
impact of a collegiate-level human sexual behavior course on students’ Reviewing the connections. Journal of Men’s Studies, 20(1), 16–31.
level of sexual assertiveness. Seton Hall University Dissertations and https://doi.org/10.3149/jms.2001.16
Theses (ETDs). https://scholarship.shu.edu/dissertations/2607 García-Sancho, E., Salguero, J. M., & Fernández-Berrocal, P. (2014).
Cheng, P., Shen, W., & Kim, K. Y. (2020). Personal endorsement of Relationship between emotional intelligence and aggression: A systematic
ambivalent sexism and career success: An investigation of differential review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 19(5), 584–591. https://doi.org/
mechanisms. Journal of Business and Psychology, 35(6), 783–798. 10.1016/j.avb.2014.07.007
Cohn, A. M., Jakupcak, M., Seibert, L. A., Hildebrandt, T. B., & Zeichner, A. Gidycz, C. A., Wyatt, J., Galbreath, N. W., Axelrad, S. H., & McCone, D. R.
(2010). The role of emotion dysregulation in the association between (2018). Sexual assault prevention in the military: Key issues and recom-
men’s restrictive emotionality and use of physical aggression—ProQuest. mendations. Military Psychology, 30(3), 240–251. https://doi.org/10
Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 11(1), 53–64. https://doi.org/10.1037/ .1080/08995605.2018.1489663
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

a0018090 Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (2011). Ambivalent sexism revisited. Psychology of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Cornelius, C., & Monk-Turner, E. (2019). I’ll trade you skittles for a Women Quarterly, 35(3), 530–535. https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684311414832
blowjob: Assessing the role of anti-female memes in military sexual Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (2018). The Ambivalent Sexism Inventory:
harassment and assault. Journal of Political and Military Sociology, Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism. In S. T. Fiske (Ed.),
46(1), 221–260. https://doi.org/10.5744/jpms.1008 Social cognition (pp. 116–160). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/
Corona, V. P. (2011). Career patterns in the U.S. Army Officer Corps. Public 9781315187280-6
Organization Review, 11(2), 109–134. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11115- Green, G., Emslie, C., O’Neill, D., Hunt, K., & Walker, S. (2010). Exploring
010-0114-7 the ambiguities of masculinity in accounts of emotional distress in the
Corprew, C. S., Matthews, J. S., & Mitchell, A. D. (2014). Men at the military among young ex-servicemen. Social Science & Medicine, 71(8),
crossroads: A profile analysis of hypermasculinity in emerging adulthood. 1480–1488. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.07.015
Journal of Men’s Studies, 22(2), 105–121. https://doi.org/10.3149/jms Groemping, U., & Matthias, L. (2018). Package ‘relaimpo’. Relative
.2202.105 importance of regressors in linear models (R package version).
Cox, D. W., & O’Loughlin, J. (2017). Posttraumatic stress mediates traditional Guizzo, F., & Cadinu, M. (2020). Women, not objects: Testing a sensitizing
masculinity ideology and romantic relationship satisfaction in veteran men. web campaign against female sexual objectification to temper sexual
Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 18(4), 382–389. https://doi.org/10.1037/ harassment and hostile sexism. Media Psychology, 0(0), 1–29. https://
men0000067 doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2020.1756338
Cundiff, N. L., Nadler, J. T., & Swan, A. (2009). The influence of cultural Hale, H. C. (2012). The Role of Practice in the Development of Military
empathy and gender on perceptions of diversity programs. Journal of Masculinities. Gender, Work and Organization, 19(6), 699–722. https://
Leadership & Organizational Studies, 16(1), 97–110. https://doi.org/10 doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0432.2010.00542.x
.1177/1548051809334193 Hammer, J. H., Heath, P. J., & Vogel, D. L. (2018). Fate of the total score:
Dahling, J. J., Whitaker, B. G., & Levy, P. E. (2009). The development and Dimensionality of the Conformity to Masculine Norms Inventory-46
validation of a new Machiavellianism Scale. Journal of Management, (CMNI-46). Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 19(4), 645–651.
35(2), 219–257. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206308318618 https://doi.org/10.1037/men0000147
de Boise, S. (2019). Editorial: Is masculinity toxic? NORMA, 14(3), 147– Harris, R. J., McDonald, D. P., & Sparks, C. S. (2018). Sexual Harassment in
151. https://doi.org/10.1080/18902138.2019.1654742 the Military: Individual Experiences, Demographics, and Organizational
Devine, P. G., Forscher, P. S., Austin, A. J., & Cox, W. T. L. (2012). Long- Contexts. Armed Forces and Society, 44(1), 25–43. https://doi.org/10
term reduction in implicit race bias: A prejudice habit-breaking interven- .1177/0095327X16687069
tion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(6), 1267–1278. Harway, M., & Steel, J. H. (2015). Studying masculinity and sexual assault
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2012.06.003 across organizational culture groups: Understanding perpetrators. Psy-
DiMuccio, S. H., & Knowles, E. D. (2020). The political significance of chology of Men & Masculinity, 16(4), 374–378. https://doi.org/10.1037/
fragile masculinity. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 34, 25–28. a0039694
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2019.11.010 Hayduk, L. A., & Littvay, L. (2012). Should researchers use single in-
Do, J. J., & Samuels, S. M. (2020). I am a warrior: An analysis of the military dicators, best indicators, or multiple indicators in structural equation
masculine-warrior narrative among U.S. Air Force Officer Candidates. Armed models? BMC Medical Research Methodology, 12(1), Article 159.
Forces & Society, 47(1), 25–47. https://doi.org/10.1177/0095327X20931561 https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2288-12-159
Dobbin, F., & Kalev, A. (2018). Why doesn’t diversity training work? The Hendrix, T. (2006). The effects of military training to men’s attitudes toward
challenge for industry and academia. Anthropology Now, 10(2), 48–55. intimate partner violence. [Doctoral dissertation]. The Ohio State
https://doi.org/10.1080/19428200.2018.1493182 University. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1164236965
Drake, M. J. (2006). Ambivalence at the academies: Attitudes toward women Hinojosa, R. (2010). Doing hegemony: Military, men, and constructing a
in the military at the federal service academies. Social Thought & hegemonic masculinity. Journal of Men’s Studies, 18(2), 179–194. https://
Research, 27, 43–68. https://www.jstor.org/stable/23250020 doi.org/10.3149/jms.1802.179
Evans, J. (2011). Why is stoicism so popular in the US Army? Philosophy for Humphreys, T. P., Wood, L. M., & Parker, J. D. A. (2009). Alexithymia and
Life. https://www.philosophyforlife.org/blog/why-is-stoicism-so-popular- satisfaction in intimate relationships. Personality and Individual Differ-
in-the-us-army ences, 46(1), 43–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.09.002
Fallesen, J. J., Keller-Glaze, H., & Curnow, C. K. (2011). A selective review Hutter, K., Füller, J., Hautz, J., Bilgram, V., & Matzler, K. (2015). Machia-
of leadership studies in the US Army. Military Psychology, 23(5), vellianism or morality: Which behavior pays off in online innovation
462–478. https://doi.org/10.1080/08995605.2011.600181 contests? Journal of Management Information Systems, 32(3), 197–228.
Forbes, G. B., Adams-Curtis, L. E., & White, K. B. (2004). First- and https://doi.org/10.1080/07421222.2015.1099181
Second-generation measures of sexism, rape myths and related beliefs, and Inesi, M. E., & Cable, D. M. (2015). When accomplishments come back to
hostility toward women: Their interrelationships and association with haunt you: The negative effect of competence signals on women’s
624 SCHAEFER, COTTING, PROCTOR, RYAN, AND LERNER

performance evaluations. Personnel Psychology, 68(3), 615–657. https:// Lisco, C. G., Parrott, D. J., & Tharp, A. T. (2012). The role of heavy episodic
doi.org/10.1111/peps.12083 drinking and hostile sexism in men’s sexual aggression toward female
Iwamoto, D. K., Liao, L., & Liu, W. M. (2010). Masculine norms, avoidant intimate partners. Addictive Behaviors, 37(11), 1264–1270. https://
coping, asian values and depression among Asian American Men. Psy- doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.06.010
chology of Men & Masculinity, 11(1), 15–24. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Logan-Greene, P., & Cue Davis, K. (2011). Latent profiles of risk among a
a0017874 community sample of men: Implications for sexual aggression. Journal of
Jakupcak, M., Salters, K., Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2003). Masculinity Interpersonal Violence, 26(7), 1463–1477. https://doi.org/10.1177/
and Emotionality: An investigation of men’s primary and secondary 0886260510369138
emotional responding. Sex Roles, 49(3), 111–120. https://doi.org/10 Mahalik, J. R., Locke, B. D., Ludlow, L. H., Diemer, M. A., Scott, R. P. J.,
.1023/A:1024452728902 Gottfried, M., & Freitas, G. (2003). Development of the Conformity to
Johnson, B. (2010). A few good boys: Masculinity at a military-style charter Masculine Norms Inventory. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 4(1), 3–
school. Men and Masculinities, 12(5), 575–596. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 25. https://doi.org/10.1037/1524-9220.4.1.3
1097184X09342228 Malamuth, N. M., Sockloskie, R. J., Koss, M. P., & Tanaka, J. S. (1991).
Karakis, E. N., & Levant, R. F. (2012). Is normative male alexithymia Characteristics of aggressors against women: Testing a model using a
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

associated with relationship satisfaction, fear of intimacy and communi- national sample of college students. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

cation quality among men in relationships? Journal of Men’s Studies, Psychology, 59(5), 670–681. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.59.5.670
20(3), 179–186. https://doi.org/10.3149/jms.2003.179 Matthews, M. D., Ender, M. G., Laurence, J. H., & Rohall, D. E. (2009). Role
Kaya, A., Iwamoto, D., Brady, J., Clinton, L., & Grivel, M. (2019). The role of group affiliation and gender on attitudes toward women in the military.
of masculine norms and gender role conflict on prospective well-being Military Psychology, 21(2), 241–251. https://doi.org/10.1080/08995600
among men. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 20(1), 142–147. https:// 902768750
doi.org/10.1037/men0000155 McAllister, L., Callaghan, J. E. M., & Fellin, L. C. (2019). Masculinities and
Kime, P. (2019, October 14). The Pentagon is asking if women have a emotional expression in UK servicemen: ‘Big boys don’t cry’? Journal of
‘quality of purity few men possess.’ Military Times. https://www.milita Gender Studies, 28(3), 257–270. https://doi.org/10.1080/09589236.2018
rytimes.com/pay-benefits/2019/10/11/the-pentagon-is-asking-if-women- .1429898
have-a-quality-of-purity-few-men-possess/ Messner, M. A. (1992). Power at play: Sports and the problem of mascu-
Klein, M., & Gallus, J. A. (2018). The readiness imperative for reducing linity. Beacon Press.
sexual violence in the US armed forces: Respect and professionalism as the Mikorski, R., & Szymanski, D. M. (2017). Masculine norms, peer group,
foundation for change. Military Psychology, 30(3), 264–269. https:// pornography, Facebook, and men’s sexual objectification of women.
doi.org/10.1080/08995605.2017.1422949 Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 18(4), 257–267. https://doi.org/10
Koeszegi, S. T., Zedlacher, E., & Hudribusch, R. (2014). The war against the .1037/men0000058
female soldier? The effects of masculine culture on workplace aggression. Moore, B. L. (2010). Effects of sexual harassment on job satisfaction,
Armed Forces and Society, 40(2), 226–251. https://doi.org/10.1177/009 retention, cohesion, commitment and unit effectiveness: The case of the
5327X12460019 air force. State University of New York at Buffalo United States. https://
Kteily, N. S., Hodson, G., Dhont, K., & Ho, A. K. (2019). Predisposed to apps.dtic.mil/docs/citations/AD1044614
prejudice but responsive to intergroup contact? Testing the unique benefits of Moore, D. W., & Trout, B. T. (1978). Military advancement: The visibility
intergroup contact across different types of individual differences. Group theory of promotion. The American Political Science Review, 72(2), 452–
Processes & Intergroup Relations, 22(1), 3–25. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 468. https://doi.org/10.2307/1954104
1368430217716750 Morris, M. (1996). By force of arms: Rape, war, and military culture. Duke
Lai, C. K., Skinner, A. L., Cooley, E., Murrar, S., Brauer, M., Devos, T., Law Journal, 45(4), 651–782. https://doi.org/10.2307/1372997
Calanchini, J., Xiao, Y. J., Pedram, C., Marshburn, C. K., Simon, S., Mosher, D. L., & Sirkin, M. (1984). Measuring a macho personality
Blanchar, J. C., Joy-Gaba, J. A., Conway, J., Redford, L., Klein, R. A., constellation. Journal of Research in Personality, 18(2), 150–163.
Roussos, G., Schellhaas, F. M. H., Burns, M., : : : Nosek, B. A. (2016). https://doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(84)90026-6
Reducing implicit racial preferences: II. Intervention effectiveness across Murnen, S. K., Wright, C., & Kaluzny, G. (2002). If ” boys will be boys,” Then
time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(8), 1001–1016. girls will be victims? A meta-analytic review of the research that relates
https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000179 masculine ideology to sexual aggression. Sex Roles, 46(11), 359–375. https://
Lane, A. (2020). Women in the Canadian armed forces. In T. Juneau, P. doi.org/10.1023/A:1020488928736
Lagassé, & S. Vucetic (Eds.), Canadian defence policy in theory and Nicol, A. A. M., Charbonneau, D., & Boies, K. (2007). Right-wing
practice (pp. 351–364). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030- authoritarianism and social dominance orientation in a Canadian military
26403-1_20 sample. Military Psychology, 19(4), 239–257. https://doi.org/10.1080/
LeBreton, J. M., Baysinger, M., Abbey, A., & Jacques-Tiura, A. J. (2013). 08995600701548072
The relative importance of psychopathy-related traits in predicting imper- O’Brien, C., Keith, J., & Shoemaker, L. (2015). Don’t tell: Military culture
sonal sex and hostile masculinity. Personality and Individual Differences, and male rape. Psychological Services, 12(4), 357–365. https://doi.org/10
55(7), 817–822. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.07.009 .1037/ser0000049
Levant, R. F., & Wimer, D. J. (2014). Masculinity constructs as protective Orchowski, L. M., Berry-Cabán, C. S., Prisock, K., Borsari, B., & Kazemi,
buffers and risk factors for men’s health. American Journal of Men’s D. M. (2018). Evaluations of sexual assault prevention programs in
Health, 8(2), 110–120. https://doi.org/10.1177/1557988313494408 military settings: A synthesis of the research literature. Military Medicine,
Liaqat, H., Malik, T. A., & Bilal, A. (2020). Impact of masculinity and 183(Suppl. 1), 421–428. https://doi.org/10.1093/milmed/usx212
normative male alexithymia on interpersonal difficulties in young adult Park, S. W., & Colvin, C. R. (2014). Narcissism and discrepancy between
males. Mediterranean Journal of Clinical Psychology, 8(2), 2. https:// self and friends’ perceptions of personality. Journal of Personality, 82(4),
doi.org/10.6092/2282-1619/mjcp-2488 278–286. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12053
Lisak, D., & Roth, S. (1988). Motivational factors in nonincarcerated Parrott, D. J., & Zeichner, A. (2003). Effects of hypermasculinity oh
sexually aggressive men. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, physical aggression against women. Psychology of Men & Masculinity,
55(5), 795–802. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.55.5.795 4(1), 70–78. https://doi.org/10.1037/1524-9220.4.1.70
MILITARY HYPERMASCULINITY AT USMA 625

Peters, J., Nason, C., & Turner, W. M. (2007). Development and testing of a job performance. Personality and Individual Differences, 104, 453–459.
new version of the hypermasculinity index. Social Work Research, 31(3), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.09.010
171–182. https://doi.org/10.1093/swr/31.3.171 Snyder, J. A., Fisher, B. S., Scherer, H. L., & Daigle, L. E. (2012). Unsafe in
Petrides, K. V., Furnham, A., & Martin, G. N. (2004). Estimates of emotional the camouflage tower: Sexual victimization and perceptions of Military
and psychometric intelligence: Evidence for gender-based stereotypes. Academy leadership. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 27(16), 3171–
The Journal of Social Psychology, 144(2), 149–162. https://doi.org/10 3194. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260512441252
.3200/SOCP.144.2.149-162 Stander, V. A., Thomsen, C. J., Merrill, L. L., & Milner, J. S. (2018).
Pratto, F., Çidam, A., Stewart, A. L., Zeineddine, F. B., Aranda, M., Aiello, Longitudinal prediction of sexual harassment and sexual assault by male
A., Morselli, D.,Chryssochoou, X., Cichocka, A., Cohrs, J. C., Durrheim, enlisted Navy personnel. Military Psychology, 30(3), 229–239. https://
K., Eicher, V., Foels, R., Górska, P., Lee, I.-C., Licata, L., Liu, J. H., Li, L., doi.org/10.1037/mil0000171
Meyer, I., : : : Henkel, K. E. (2013). Social dominance in context and in Stricker, A. G., Arenas, F. J., Westhauser, T. C., & Hawkins-Scribner, T.
individuals. Social Psychological & Personality Science, 4(5), 587–599. (2017). Positive education of stoic warriors as reflective practitioners in the
https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550612473663 profession of arms. Reflective Practice, 18(1), 133–146. https://doi.org/10
Rau, T. J., Merrill, L. L., McWhorter, S. K., Stander, V. A., Thomsen, C. J., .1080/14623943.2016.1251415
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Dyslin, C. W., Crouch, J. L., Rabenhorst, M. M., & Milner, J. S. (2010). Swain, J. (2016). Changing the identities of young army recruits and new
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Evaluation of a sexual assault education/prevention program for male U.S. ways of looking at hegemonic forms of military masculinity. Culture,
Navy personnel. Military Medicine, 175(6), 429–434. https://doi.org/10 Society and Masculinities, 8(1), Article 1. https://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/id/
.7205/MILMED-D-09-00218 eprint/1536147/
R Core Team. (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control
computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. http://www predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal
.R-project.org/ success. Journal of Personality, 72(2), 271–324. https://doi.org/10.1111/j
Renzetti, C. M., Lynch, K. R., & DeWall, C. N. (2018). Ambivalent sexism, .0022-3506.2004.00263.x
alcohol use, and intimate partner violence perpetration. Journal of Inter- Thomae, M., & Pina, A. (2015). Sexist humor and social identity: The role
personal Violence, 33(2), 183–210. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260 of sexist humor in men’s in-group cohesion, sexual harassment, rape
515604412 proclivity, and victim blame. Humor: International Journal of Humor
Richard, K., & Molloy, S. (2020). An examination of emerging adult military Research, 28(2), 187–204. https://doi.org/10.1515/humor-2015-0023
men: Masculinity and U.S. military climate. Psychology of Men & Thompson, C. G., Kim, R. S., Aloe, A. M., & Becker, B. J. (2017). Extracting
Masculinity, 21(4), 686–698. https://doi.org/10.1037/men0000303 the variance inflation factor and other multicollinearity diagnostics from
Roberton, T., Daffern, M., & Bucks, R. S. (2012). Emotion regulation and typical regression results. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 39(2),
aggression. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 17(1), 72–82. https:// 81–90. https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2016.1277529
doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2011.09.006 Titunik, R. F. (2008). The myth of the macho military. Polity, 40(2),
Roberton, T., Daffern, M., & Bucks, R. S. (2014). Maladaptive emotion 137–163. https://doi.org/10.1057/palgrave.polity.2300090
regulation and aggression in adult offenders. Psychology, Crime & Law, Tonidandel, S., & LeBreton, J. M. (2011). Relative importance analysis: A
20(10), 933–954. https://doi.org/10.1080/1068316X.2014.893333 useful supplement to regression analysis. Journal of Business and Psy-
Robinson Kurpius, S. E., & Lucart, A. L. (2000). Military and civilian chology, 26(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-010-9204-3
undergraduates: attitudes toward women, masculinity, and authoritarian- Trobaugh, E. (2018). Women, regardless: Understanding gender bias in U.S.
ism. Sex Roles, 43(3), 255–265. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:10070850 Military Integration. Joint Force Quarterly, 88(1), 46–53. https://ndupress
15637 .ndu.edu/Publications/Article/1411860/women-regardless-understanding-
Rosen, L. N., Knudson, K. H., & Fancher, P. (2003). Cohesion and the gender-bias-in-us-military-integration/
culture of hypermasculinity in U.S. Army units. Armed Forces and Turchik, J. A., & Wilson, S. M. (2010). Sexual assault in the U.S. military: A
Society, 29(3), 325–351. https://doi.org/10.1177/0095327X0302900302 review of the literature and recommendations for the future. Aggression and
Rosenstein, J. E., Angelis, K. D., McCone, D. R., & Carroll, M. H. (2018). Violent Behavior, 15(4), 267–277. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2010.01.005
Sexual assault and sexual harassment at the US military service academies. Uğurlu, N. S., & Özdemir, F. (2017). Predicting attitudes toward the
Military Psychology, 30(3), 206–218. https://doi.org/10.1080/08995605 masculine structure of the military with Turkish identification and ambiv-
.2017.1422950 alent sexism. Sex Roles, 76(7–8), 511–519. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Schaefer, H. S., Callina, K. S., Powers, J., Kobylski, G., Ryan, D., & Lerner, s11199-016-0676-0
R. M. (2021). Examining diversity in developmental trajectories of cadets’ United States Army. (2019). Army Combat Fitness Test: Initial operation
performance and character at the United States military academy. Journal capability. Center for Initial Military Training. https://usacimt.tradoc.army
of Character Education, 17(1), 59–80. .mil/assets/Army%20Combat%20Fitness%20Test%20-%20IOC%20Guide%
Scharrer, E. (2005). Hypermasculinity, Aggression, and Television Vio- 20-%20OCT%202019.pdf
lence: An Experiment. Media Psychology, 7(4), 353–376. https://doi.org/ United States Military Academy. (2016a). Academic program (Redbook).
10.1207/S1532785XMEP0704_3 https://www.westpoint.edu/sites/default/files/pdfs/RedBook_GY2020_
Sewell, G. F. (2009). Emotional Intelligence and the Army Leadership 20170803.pdf
Requirements Model. Military Review, 89(6), 93–98. United States Military Academy. (2016b). Military program (Greenbook).
Sherman, N. (2007). Stoic warriors: The ancient philosophy behind the https://www.westpoint.edu/sites/default/files/pdfs/ABOUT/Student%
military mind. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof: 20Consumer%20Info/Greenbook%20(Military).pdf
oso/9780195315912.001.0001 United States Military Academy. (2018). Developing leaders of character.
Sherman, N. (2010, May 30). A crack in the Stoic’s Armor. Opinionator. https:// https://www.westpoint.edu/sites/default/files/pdfs/ABOUT/Superintendent/
opinionator.blogs.nytimes.com/2010/05/30/a-crack-in-the-stoic-armor/ Developing%20Leaders%20of%20Character%202018.pdf
Shields, D. M., Kuhl, D., & Westwood, M. J. (2017). Abject masculinity and U.S. Department of the Army. (2013). The Army profession, Army doctrine
the military: Articulating a fulcrum of struggle and change. Psychology of reference publication no. 1. Department of the Army. https://irp.fas.org/
Men & Masculinity, 18(3), 215–225. https://doi.org/10.1037/men0000114 doddir/army/adrp1.pdf
Smith, M. B., & Webster, B. D. (2017). A moderated mediation model of Walters, G. D. (2007). Measuring proactive and reactive criminal thinking
Machiavellianism, social undermining, political skill, and supervisor-rated with the PICTS: Correlations with outcome expectancies and hostile
626 SCHAEFER, COTTING, PROCTOR, RYAN, AND LERNER

attribution biases. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 22(4), 371–385. Whitley, B. E., Jr. (2001). Gender-role variables and attitudes toward
https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260506296988 homosexuality. Sex Roles, 45(11), 691–721. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:
Waterman, E. A., Wesche, R., Leavitt, C. E., & Lefkowitz, E. S. 1015640318045
(2020). Fraternity membership, traditional masculinity ideologies, Wilson, L. C. (2018). The prevalence of military sexual trauma: A meta-
and impersonal sex: Selection and socialization effects. Psychology analysis. Trauma, Violence & Abuse, 19(5), 584–597. https://doi.org/10
of Men & Masculinity, 21(1), 58–68. https://doi.org/10.1037/me .1177/1524838016683459
n0000209 Woerner, J., Abbey, A., Helmers, B. R., Pegram, S. E., & Jilani, Z. (2018).
Wegner, R., & Abbey, A. (2016). Individual differences in men’s misper- Predicting men’s immediate reactions to a simulated date’s sexual rejec-
ception of women’s sexual intent: Application and extension of the tion: The effects of hostile masculinity, impersonal sex, and hostile
confluence model. Personality and Individual Differences, 94, 16–20. perceptions of the woman. Psychology of Violence, 8(3), 349–357.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.12.027 https://doi.org/10.1037/vio0000172
Wharton, M., Day, A., Mohr, P., Gerace, A., & Howells, K. (2014). The Woodward, R. (2000). Warrior heroes and little green men: Soldiers, military
impact of masculinity on anger arousal in ambiguous situations. Journal of training, and the construction of rural masculinities. Rural Sociology,
Relationships Research, 5, Article e1. https://doi.org/10.1017/jrr.2014.1 65(4), 640–657. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1549-0831.2000.tb00048.x
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Whitaker, M. P. (2013). Centrality of control-seeking in men’s intimate Young, L. M., & Nauta, M. M. (2013). Sexism as a predictor of attitudes
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

partner violence perpetration. Prevention Science, 14(5), 513–523. https:// toward women in the military and in combat. Military Psychology, 25(2),
doi.org/10.1007/s11121-012-0332-z 166–171. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0094958
Whitehead, P. R., Ward, T., & Collie, R. M. (2007). Time for a
change: Applying the good lives model of rehabilitation to a high-
risk violent offender. International Journal of Offender Therapy Received March 29, 2021
and Comparative Criminology, 51(5), 578–598. https://doi.org/10 Revision received July 27, 2021
.1177/0306624X06296236 Accepted July 27, 2021 ▪

You might also like