You are on page 1of 12

Appetite 188 (2023) 106975

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Appetite
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/appet

Research report

The relationship between childhood trauma, eating behaviours, and the


mediating role of metacognitive beliefs
Sarah Martin, Esben Strodl *
School of Psychology and Counselling, Queensland University of Technology, Australia

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Disordered eating poses a significant risk to psychological and physical health. The experience of childhood
Childhood trauma trauma has been linked to the development of disordered eating behaviours, but the causal psychological
Disordered eating mechanisms remain unclear. The metacognitive model holds promise as a potential framework for understanding
Metacognitive beliefs
the mediating psychological processes that explain how childhood trauma may lead to disordered eating. The
Metacognitions
purpose of this study was to examine the role of metacognitive beliefs mediating the relationship between
Thoughts are uncontrollable and dangerous
childhood trauma and disordered eating behaviours. Adults from the Australian community (N = 461) completed
an online self-report survey measuring childhood maltreatment (Childhood Trauma Questionnaire – Short Form),
disordered eating behaviour (Three Factor Eating Questionnaire – Revised 21), and metacognitive beliefs
(Metacognitive Questionnaire 30). Hierarchical multiple regression analyses revealed no independent associa­
tions between any forms of childhood maltreatment and cognitive restraint, while childhood emotional abuse
was uniquely associated with uncontrolled eating and emotional eating. Through bootstrapping tests, the
mediating effect between childhood trauma and uncontrolled and emotional eating consistently involved the
metacognitive beliefs that thoughts are uncontrollable and dangerous. Future longitudinal research is required to
confirm causal relationships.

1. Introduction and detrimental impacts of disordered eating, there is surprisingly


limited understanding of the psychological mechanisms that precipitate
Disordered eating is becoming increasingly recognised as a common and perpetuate such eating behaviours. Experiences of childhood
worldwide public health concern that has been linked to significant trauma have been identified as one important predisposing factor to the
impairments in physical and psychological functioning (Santomauro development of disordered eating behaviours (Emery et al., 2021; Kong
et al., 2021; Sheehan & Herman, 2015). The prevalence of disordered & Bernstein, 2009; Zelkowitz et al., 2021). While a range of possible
eating behaviours such as overconsumption, binge-eating, and rigid mediators of the association between experiences of childhood
dieting, have significantly increased in Western countries such as maltreatment and disordered eating behaviours have been considered
Australia across the last two decades (da Luz et al., 2017). Many in­ (e.g. pathological dissociation, difficulty with emotion self-regulation,
dividuals do not seek help for these behaviours due to the social body dissatisfaction, negative affect/depression, anxiety, general
acceptability of dieting/fasting, eating for emotional coping purposes, distress, self-criticism, and alexithymia; Rabito-Alcón et al., 2021), un­
and skipping meals (Neumark-Sztainer et al., 2011). However, disor­ fortunately the psychological mechanisms linking childhood trauma to
dered eating behaviours have been associated with a plethora of co­ disordered eating behaviours are still poorly understood.
morbid physical and mental health conditions such as higher BMI lower Research by Strodl and Wylie (2020) explored the relation between
self-esteem and body dissatisfaction (Markey et al., 2022), poorer distinct forms of childhood trauma (emotional abuse, emotional neglect,
self-rated health and psychological distress (Kärkkäinen et al., 2018) physical abuse, physical neglect and sexual abuse) and disordered eating
and the presence of a greater number of metabolic cardiovascular dis­ behaviours (cognitive restraint, uncontrolled eating and emotional
ease risk factors (Lopez-Cepero et al., 2018). eating) and investigated whether the diminished ability to monitor and
Despite the growing body of literature that recognises the prevalence describe emotional states (alexithymia) or beliefs about emotions

* Corresponding author. School of Psychology and Counselling, Queensland University of Technology, Victoria Park Rd, Kelvin Grove, 4059, Queensland,
Australia.
E-mail address: e.strodl@qut.edu.au (E. Strodl).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2023.106975
Received 19 December 2022; Received in revised form 4 July 2023; Accepted 10 July 2023
Available online 14 July 2023
0195-6663/© 2023 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
S. Martin and E. Strodl Appetite 188 (2023) 106975

mediated these associations. While alexithymia did not mediate these will protect me from putting on weight”. While the presence of these two
relationships, various indirect mediation effects were found for beliefs metacognitive beliefs may be deemed functional to the individual, in
about emotions. In particular, the beliefs that emotions are over­ terms of protecting the individual from gaining weight, they also in­
whelming and uncontrollable, shameful, and irrational and damaging crease the probability of becoming distressed through increasing the
were consistently implicated in the indirect associations. This finding probability of true or false positive signals of weight gain and intensi­
provided preliminary support for the metacognitive model mediating fying the perseverative negative evaluations of body size. This distress
the association between childhood maltreatment and disordered eating may then be strengthened by the activation of negative metacognitive
but raised the question of whether different forms of metacognitive beliefs such as “my worry about my body shame/weight is over­
knowledge are implicated in these associations. whelming and uncontrollable”. As distress increases, metacognitive
The metacognitive model was first introduced to the cognitive beliefs about appropriate coping strategies become activated. If such a
developmental literature by John Flavell (Flavell, 1979). Flavell con­ metacognitive belief includes “eating helps me to calm down”, then
ceptualised metacognition in terms of metacognitive knowledge, meta­ emotional eating may follow.
cognitive experiences and metacognitive strategies. Metacognitive The association between metacognitive beliefs and eating behav­
knowledge refers to an individual’s knowledge/beliefs about one’s own iours has been well supported in populations with eating disorder di­
and others’ cognitive abilities, strengths, limitations, other internal and agnoses (Cooper et al., 2004; Georgantopolous et al., 2020; Olstad et al.,
external factors that may affect cognition, as well as knowledge about 2015; Sapuppo et al., 2018; Spada et al., 2016; Vann et al., 2013, 2014).
strategies and goals of thinking (Flavell, 1979). Metacognitive experi­ Collectively, these studies highlight that metacognitive beliefs are
ences refer to “any conscious cognitive or affective experiences that important psychological mechanisms in eating disorder pathology.
accompany and pertain to any intellectual enterprise” (Flavell, 1979, p. However, less is known about the association between disordered eating
906). These include feelings, judgements/estimates, as well as aware­ and metacognitions, although emerging evidence suggests promising
ness of thoughts and ideas experienced during a task (Efklides, 2006). associations within nonclinical samples. For example, a small (N = 44)
Metacognitive knowledge and metacognitive experiences guide the se­ cross-sectional study by Quattropani et al. (2016) found significant as­
lection and implementation of metacognitive strategies aimed to control sociations between several eating disorder symptoms and metacognitive
cognition to achieve one’s goals. beliefs. Building on this research, Laghi et al. (2018) reported a positive
In addition to beliefs about emotions, another common form of correlation between metacognitive beliefs and the frequency of binge
metacognitive knowledge are beliefs about cognitions. The role of eating. While this sample was larger (n = 804) it was limited to an
metacognitive beliefs, or beliefs about emotions, in the development and adolescent sample. Similarly, Limbers et al. (2021) reported higher
maintenance of psychopathology has been promoted particularly by levels of emotional eating behaviours to be associated with negative
Adrian Wells (2002). Indeed, there is evidence that metacognitive be­ metacognitive beliefs in a nonclinical sample of adolescents. These
liefs are important in understanding the experiences of depression and studies were conducted among adolescents making the results difficult
anxiety (Russell et al., 2021; Strodl et al., 2015), alcohol misuse (Spada to generalise to adult populations.
et al., 2013), post-traumatic stress disorder (Capobianco et al., 2020), The plausibility of metacognitive beliefs mediating the association
addictive behaviours (Hamonniere & Varescon, 2018) and eating dis­ between childhood trauma and adult disordered eating is supported by a
orders (Vann et al., 2013). study by Hosseini-Ramaghani et al. (2019) that found a direct correla­
Wells (2002) suggests a core construct in understanding psychopa­ tion between childhood trauma and the strength of maladaptive meta­
thology is the activation of a dysfunctional thinking response referred to cognitive beliefs. Moreover, a review by Mansueto et al. (2019) found
as the cognitive-attentional syndrome (CAS). The CAS is characterised that exposure to traumatic childhood experiences such as abuse or
by perseverative thinking patterns, attentional bias to threat, and the use neglect is related to the development of maladaptive metacognitive
of ineffective coping strategies, such as thought avoidance and sup­ beliefs in later adulthood. The authors also reported that metacognitive
pression (Wells, 2002). The CAS is believed to be triggered, controlled, beliefs mediated the relationship between childhood adversity and re­
and maintained by positive and negative beliefs about thinking (Wells, petitive thinking/negative affect among both clinical and healthy adult
2002). Positive metacognitive beliefs include beliefs that cognitive samples (Mansueto et al., 2019). Similarly, Østefjells et al. (2017)
strategies such as worry, rumination, and threat monitoring are helpful showed that metacognitive beliefs about thoughts being uncontrollable
in achieving one’s goals (Wells, 2002). Negative metacognitive beliefs and dangerous mediated the experiences of childhood emotional abuse
include the beliefs about the negative effects of thinking patterns or and adult symptoms of depression and anxiety in individuals with psy­
strategies (e.g., ‘worrying is dangerous for me’; Wells, 2002). The Met­ chosis or bipolar disorder. While the sample limits the generalisability of
acognitions Questionnaire (MCQ-30; Wells & Cartwright-Hatton, 2004) these findings to a nonclinical general population, the results are
measures five metacognitive beliefs and processes considered relevant consistent with metacognitive theory which should also be applicable to
to the vulnerability and maintenance of psychopathology. This measure non-clinical populations. To date, no study has examined whether
assesses negative metacognitive beliefs (worry is uncontrollable and metacognition beliefs mediate the relationship between childhood
dangerous), positive beliefs about worry (benefits of worry to prevent trauma and disordered eating in adults. Identifying such a mediating
distress and improve mood), belief in the need to control thoughts, lack relationship may guide the development of novel interventions for
of cognitive confidence, and cognitive self-consciousness which char­ disordered eating based upon a metacognitive model.
acterises the tendency to monitor thoughts. These beliefs perpetuate the
CAS (Wells, 2002) which is implicated in the development and main­ 1.1. The present study
tenance of psychopathology and maladaptive coping strategies (Wells &
Carter, 2009). The above evidence suggests that childhood trauma is a risk factor
Applied within the context of eating, the activation of the CAS may for the development of disordered eating behaviours with maladaptive
be identified by repetitive thoughts about food/eating/body image, metacognitive beliefs being a potential psychological mechanism or
focused attention on food/eating/body image, and maladaptive eating mediator linking the two. Thus, the present study will examine the as­
behaviours (Vann et al., 2013, 2014). For example, negative evaluations sociation of five forms of childhood trauma (emotional abuse, physical
about one’s body shape/size might be perpetuated by positive meta­ abuse, sexual abuse, emotional neglect, and physical neglect) with three
cognitive beliefs such as “thinking about my weight will protect me from disordered eating behaviours (cognitive restraint, uncontrolled eating,
putting on weight”. Attentional bias towards cues indicating weight gain and emotional eating). This study has two hypotheses.
(e.g. the tightness of clothes) might be perpetuated by a metacognitive
belief that “identifying early signs of weight gain, through tight clothes,

2
S. Martin and E. Strodl Appetite 188 (2023) 106975

1. It is hypothesised that each form of childhood trauma (emotional 2.2. Measures


abuse, physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional neglect, physical
neglect) will independently be associated with each type of disor­ 2.2.1. Demographic questions asked about age, gender, education,
dered eating (cognitive restraint, uncontrolled eating, emotional employment status, annual income, marital status, and whether they
eating), after controlling for covariates and other forms of childhood came from an English-speaking background.
trauma. 2.2.2. COVID-19 Questions. Data collection occurred in the context
2. It is hypothesised that the relationship between childhood trauma of the COVID-19 pandemic from July 2021 to September 2021. In
(emotional abuse, physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional neglect, Australia, the pandemic disrupted aspects of daily life and restrictions
physical neglect) and each type of disordered eating (cognitive re­ were imposed such as physical distancing and lockdown measures,
straint, uncontrolled eating, and emotional eating) will be mediated which may have impacted an individual’s eating habits (O’Sullivan
by metacognitive beliefs (uncontrollable and dangerous, positive et al., 2020). Therefore, questions were included to gain insight into
beliefs, need to control thoughts, cognitive self-consciousness, whether the pandemic altered participants eating behaviours. Approx­
cognitive confidence). imately 65 percent (n = 298) of participants reported taking extra pre­
cautions to socially distance due to COVID-19 at the time of completing
2. Method the questionnaire. Individuals were asked if their eating behaviour
(cognitive restraint, uncontrolled eating, and emotional eating) was
2.1. Participants impacted by COVID-19, for frequency of behaviour and volume of food
(see Supplementary Table A).
The inclusion criteria for this study were being 18 years or older and 2.2.3. Childhood Trauma Questionnaire-Short Form (CTQ-SF). The
residing in Australia. The exclusion criteria involved self-reporting CTQ-SF is a widely used 28-item self-report measure of traumatic
having a current or previous eating disorder diagnosis (e.g. anorexia childhood experiences for adults (Bernstein et al., 2003). The scale is a
nervosa, bulimia nervosa or binge eating disorder), and self-selecting short-form version of the initial CTQ scale developed by Bernstein et al.
that they would become distressed thinking about their experience of (1994). Responses were measured on a five-point Likert scale ranging
childhood trauma, beliefs, or personal eating behaviour. Six hundred from never true (1) to very often true (5). Higher scores represent higher
and thirteen individuals began the study. Among those who commenced levels of childhood maltreatment (Bernstein & Fink, 1998). The scale
the questionnaire, 19 did not meet the inclusion criteria and 133 exited assesses emotional abuse (CEA) e.g., ‘I felt that someone in my family
before completion (the majority before completing demographics). Of hated me’; physical abuse (CPA) e.g., ‘I was punished with a belt, a
those remaining, 9 responses had over 10% missing data and were board, a cord, or some other hard object’; sexual abuse (CSA) e.g.,
subsequently excluded from the analyses. The final sample comprised of ‘Someone tried to touch me in a sexual way, or tried to make me touch
461 participants with 65.7 percent females (n = 303). Participants them’; emotional neglect (CEN); e.g., ‘My family was a source of
ranged from 18 to 82 years of age (M = 39.6; SD = 19.3). The sample strength and support’; and physical neglect (CPA) e.g., ‘I didn’t have
demographics are detailed in Table 1. enough to eat’. The CTQ-SF has indicated good to excellent internal
reliability across the five subscales among large samples (Hernandez
et al., 2013; Kongerslev et al., 2019; Mizuki & Fujiwara, 2020). The scale
has also demonstrated good construct validity and good retest reliability
(Kim et al., 2013; Spinhoven et al., 2014). Similarly, the internal reli­
Table 1 ability for the present study ranged from good to excellent for the three
Demographic characteristics of sample. eating behaviours (CEA α = 0.91, CPA α = .85, CSA α = 0.92, CEN α =
Characteristic n (%) 0.94, CPN α = .73).
Marital status 2.2.4. Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ-R21). The TFEQ-
Single 154 (33.4) R21 is a 21-item instrument that measures three types of disordered
Married 159 (34.5) eating behaviours: cognitive restraint (CR), uncontrolled eating (UE)
Divorced 29 (6.3)
and emotional eating (EE; Cappelleri et al., 2009). The TFEQ was orig­
Co-habiting 80 (17.4)
Other 39 (8.5)
inally developed by Stunkard and Messick (1985) and was later refined
Highest level of education by Karlsson et al., (2000) to reflect the TFEQ-R21. Items 1–20 were
High school 127 (27.5) measured on a four-point Likert scale ranging from definitely true (1) to
Certificate or diploma 109 (23.6) definitely false (4) and item 21 was measured on an eight-point numerical
Bachelor’s degree 138 (29.9)
scale. Higher scores represent higher levels of disordered eating. The
Post-graduate degree/masters/doctoral level 82 (17.8)
Other 5 (1.1) cognitive restraint subscale assesses individual control over food con­
Employment status sumption to influence body weight and body shape e.g., ‘I don’t eat some
Full-time 126 (27.3) foods because they make me fat’. The uncontrolled eating subscale as­
Part-time/casual 169 (36.7) sesses the likelihood to lose control overeating when feeling hungry or
Self-employed 34 (7.4)
Unemployed 88 (19.1)
exposed to external stimuli e.g., ‘Sometimes when I start eating, I just
Homemaker 27 (5.9) can’t seem to stop.’ The emotional eating subscale measures tendency to
Prefer not to answer 17 (3.7) overeat with reference to negative mood states e.g., ‘I start to eat when I
Annual income feel anxious’. The TFEQ-R21 supports measurement invariance across
$0 - $14,999 78 (16.9)
genders, has good internal consistency, and has previously demon­
$15,000 - $34,999 105 (22.8)
$35,000 - $49,000 63 (13.7) strated appropriate discriminative and convergent validity (de Medeiros
$50,000 - $64,999 51 (11.1) et al., 2017; Duarte et al., 2020). Lin et al. (2021) reported good to
$65,000 - $74,999 33 (7.2) excellent internal reliability. Similarly, the internal reliability for the
$75,000 - $99,999 48 (10.4) present study ranged from good to excellent for the three eating be­
$100,000 - $159,999 35 (7.6)
$160,000+ 17 (3.7)
haviours (CR α = 0.79, UE α = 0.88, EE α = 0.94).
Prefer not to answer 31 (6.7) 2.2.5. Metacognitions Questionnaire (MCQ-30). The MCQ-30 is a
English speaking background measure of individual differences in metacognitive beliefs, judgements,
Yes 407 (88.3) and monitoring tendencies across five distinct metacognitive factors
No 54 (11.7)
(Wells & Cartwright-Hatton, 2004). Responses to the 30-item,

3
S. Martin and E. Strodl Appetite 188 (2023) 106975

self-assessment questionnaire are measured on a four-point Likert scale emotional eating). Age, gender, level of education and income were
ranging from do not agree (1) to agree very much (4). The scale assesses covariates and socioeconomic status was operationalised using annual
five thought patterns which are negative beliefs about uncontrollability income and level of education. Gender was dummy coded into males
and danger (UD) e.g., ‘when I start worrying, I cannot stop’, positive be­ (reference category) and females (entered into the model). Alpha was
liefs about worry (PB) e.g., ‘worrying helps me cope’, negative beliefs kept at p < .05 to allow easy comparison with our two previous similar
about the need to control thoughts (NCT) e.g. ‘It is bad to think certain studies (e.g. Dawson et al., 2022; Strodl & Wylie, 2020).
thoughts’, cognitive self-consciousness (CS) e.g., ‘I am constantly aware of For the mediation hypotheses, each form of childhood trauma was
my thinking’, cognitive confidence (CC) e.g., ‘I have a poor memory’. independently assessed with the five metacognition variables, with one
Higher subscale scores indicate a higher maladaptive metacognitive eating behaviour, while controlling for covariates. To ensure a robust
style (Wells & Cartwright-Hatton, 2004). The MCQ-30 has previously interpretation, indirect effects were analyses in the present study
demonstrated good to excellent internal consistency and good test-retest through Andrew Hayes PROCESS Macro in SPSS through bias-corrected
reliability (Spada et al., 2016; Wells & Cartwright-Hatton, 2004). The and accelerated (BCa) bootstrapped confidence intervals (based on 5000
internal reliability for the present study ranged from good to excellent samples). Historically, it is argued that a causal relationship between the
(UD α = 0.91, PB α = .91, NCT α = 0.80, CS α = 0.83, CC α = 0.90). independent and dependent variable is necessary for mediation effects
to be investigated (Baron & Kenny, 1986). Contrary to traditional
2.3. Procedure methods, growing research has suggested that mediation can occur
despite a non-significant causal relationship as this method is often
The design was a cross-sectional survey. The project was approved by associated with reduced power to detect significant effects (MacKinnon,
the Queensland University Technology (QUT) Human Research Ethics 2008; Mackinnon et al., 2007; Rucker et al., 2011). Hayes (2018) sug­
Committee (Approval Number: 2021100236). Participants were gests that significant total effects are not a precondition for investigating
recruited through an Australia-wide social media campaign conducted indirect effects. This methodology for testing mediation relationships
by QUT social media department, the survey link was posted on the has been highlighted by similar studies examining the relationship be­
associate researcher’s Facebook page and the flyer was uploaded to tween childhood trauma and disordered eating (e.g., Dawson et al.,
various online community groups. A snowballing and convenience 2022; Strodl & Wylie, 2020). Thus, in the present study, the indirect
sampling technique was employed by the associate researcher for effects were analysed irrespective of whether significant total effects
further dissemination (Emerson, 2015). Additionally, psychology stu­ were detected in the hierarchical regression. Beta weights of .10 were
dents at QUT were invited to join the study through university email lists categorised as small, 0.30 was considered medium, and >0.50 classed as
and first-year psychology students were able to participate through the large (Cohen, 1988).
SONA course credit system. Community participants had the opportu­
nity enter the random prize draw to win one of two $50 vouchers while 3. Results
QUT students were offered 0.5 course credit. The survey was adminis­
tered online using Qualtrics (2021) platform. 3.1. Bivariate analyses

2.4. Statistical analyses A negative association was found between age and uncontrolled
eating (r = -0.29, p < .001) and emotional eating (r = − 0.15, p < .01). A
All analyses were conducted using IBM Statistical Package for the small positive association was found between gender and cognitive re­
Social Sciences (SPSS) version 27. Data was screened, responses with straint (r = 0.14, p < .05) and emotional eating (r = 0.05, p < .05), with
over 10% missing data were excluded, items were reverse coded where females having larger scores on these variables than males. Income was
appropriate, and subscales were calculated using mean values. A missing significantly negatively correlated with uncontrolled eating (r = − 0.12,
values analysis revealed less than 1% missing data per item and Little’s p < .05) and emotional eating (r = − 0.16, p < .001). No significant
(1988) MCAR test indicated that data was missing completely at random associations were found between education and the three disordered
(χ 2 (172) = 129.58, p = .99). Missing data was imputed using the eating behaviours. Given many of the demographics had significant
expectation maximisation procedure in SPSS. Normality statistics and associations with the dependent variables and have been known to ac­
visual inspection of histograms revealed no departure from linearity and count for influences on the investigated eating behaviour in previous
normality. However, minor clustering was observed on all scatterplots studies (e.g., Emery et al., 2021; Minnich et al., 2017; Strodl & Wylie,
containing physical neglect and sexual abuse, suggesting potential 2020), they were retained as covariates in the present study.
breaches to homoscedasticity. A transform (square root, log10) was Table 2 illustrates the means and standard deviations as well as the
conducted, and transformations did not impact the overall significance bivariate correlation matrix for the measured variables. Emotional
of the model, therefore the untransformed data was retained for sub­ abuse, physical abuse, emotional neglect, and physical neglect displayed
sequent analyses. No influential outliers were present as Cook’s distance significant, small to large associations with all MCQ-30 subscales. Sexual
scores were less than 1, indicating that the outliers would not impact the abuse evidenced small significant associations with all subscales across
overall solution. No breaches to multicollinearity or independence of the MCQ-30 scales excluding cognitive self-consciousness.
errors were present. Small to medium, positive, and significant correlations were found
The independent variable was childhood trauma which consisted of between metacognition scales uncontrollability and danger, positive
the scores for each of the five subscales in the CTQ-SF. The proposed beliefs, need to control thoughts, and cognitive self-consciousness and
mediators were the five subscales of the MCQ-30. The dependent vari­ all three eating behaviours. However, cognitive confidence yielded only
ables were the cognitive restraint, uncontrolled eating and emotional small, positive significant associations with uncontrolled and emotional
eating subscales derived from the TFEQ-R21. eating behaviours. Additionally small, positive, and significant associ­
Bivariate correlations between the independent variables, the pro­ ations were identified between emotional abuse, physical abuse and
posed mediators and the three dependent variables were calculated to physical neglect and uncontrolled eating. All forms of childhood trauma
assist the interpretation of the multivariate analyses. Cohen’s (1988) produced significant bivariate correlations with the emotional eating
standard was used to evaluate effect sizes with correlation coefficients of subscale with associations ranging from small to moderate.
0.10 classified as small, 0.30 as medium, and >0.50 as large. Three hi­
erarchical regression analyses were conducted to determine the effect of
the independent variables (all forms of childhood trauma) on each
dependent variable (cognitive restraint, uncontrolled eating, and

4
S. Martin and E. Strodl Appetite 188 (2023) 106975

Table 2
Bivariate correlation matrix for psychometric research variables.
Measure 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13

1. Emotional –
abuse
2. Physical .62*** –
abuse
3. Sexual abuse .31*** .29*** –
4. Emotional .72*** .48*** .27*** –
neglect
5. Physical .65*** .57*** .32*** .65*** –
neglect
6. MCQ (UD) .40*** .17** .16*** .21*** .21*** –
7. MCQ (PB) .18*** .11* .15** .10* .07** .45*** –
8. MCQ (NCT) .28*** .17*** .11* .15*** .14** .58*** .44*** –
9. MCQ (CS) .22*** .12* .05 .13** .13** .43*** .34*** .41*** –
10. MCQ (CC) .28*** .13** .14** .25*** .18*** .40*** .30*** .32*** .15*** –
11.TFEQ – CR .11* .03 .03 .04 .06 .16*** .15** .10* .15** .07 –
12 TFEQ – UE .24*** .14** .06 .09 .15** .35*** .23*** .30*** .15** .25*** .03 –
13. TFEQ – EE .31*** .15*** .10* .16*** .17*** .35*** .23*** .23*** .15** .22*** .07 .73*** –
Means (SD) 2.26 1.61 1.35 2.40 1.50 2.24 1.84 2.10 2.72 2.00 2.42 2.25 2.60
(1.10) (0.78) (0.73) (1.07) (0.59) (0.86) (0.70) (0.71) (0.67) (0.79) (0.62) (0.67) (0.95)

Note. N =461. MCQ = Metacognitions questionnaire. MCQ (UD) = uncontrollable and dangerous; MCQ (PB) = positive beliefs; MCQ (NCT) = need to control thoughts;
MCQ (CS) = cognitive self-consciousness; MCQ (CC) = cognitive confidence.
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

3.2. Main analyses 3.2.2.2. Uncontrolled eating. Supplementary Table C presents all tests of
indirect effects with Uncontrolled Eating. Fig. 1 illustrates the signifi­
3.2.1. Hierarchical multiple regression cant direct and indirect pathways. The total indirect effect of the com­
Table 3 summarises the hierarchical multiple regression analysis bination of mediator variables was positive and significant but small
conducted to explore the relationship between the five forms of child­ from all five forms of childhood trauma to Uncontrolled Eating. Through
hood trauma and the three forms of disordered eating behaviour, after independent analysis of the mediator variables, a significant, positive
controlling for age, gender, income, and level of education. In step one, pathway through the negative belief that thoughts are uncontrollable
gender (female) was significantly related to Cognitive Restraint, age was and dangerous to Uncontrolled Eating was evident from childhood
significantly and negatively related to Uncontrolled Eating, while age, emotional abuse, physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional neglect, and
gender and income were significantly related to Emotional Eating. physical neglect.
The addition of the five childhood trauma subscales in step 2
significantly contributed to the model explaining Uncontrolled Eating, 3.2.2.3. Supplementary Table D Illustrates tests of indirect effects tests with
ΔR2 = 0.06, ΔF(5, 451) = 6.18, p < .001, and Emotional Eating ΔR2 = the dependent variable as emotional eating. Fig. 2 displays the significant
0.08, ΔF(5, 451) = 8.55, p < .001. However, the additive effect of these pathways. The total indirect effect of the combination of mediator var­
subscales did not significantly contribute to the model predicting iables was positive and significant yet small from all five forms of
Cognitive Restraint ΔR2 = 0.01, ΔF(5, 451) = 1.27, p = .227. childhood trauma to Emotional Eating. Independent analysis of the
mediator variables revealed a significant pathway through the negative
3.2.2. Test of indirect effects belief that thoughts are uncontrollable and dangerous to uncontrolled
eating was evident from childhood emotional abuse, physical abuse,
3.2.2.1. Cognitive restraint. Supplementary Table B presents all tests of sexual abuse, emotional neglect, and physical neglect.
indirect effects with Cognitive Restraint. No significant indirect effects
were evidenced for cognitive restraint.

Table 3
Hierarchical multiple regression analysis of childhood trauma for cognitive restraint, uncontrolled eating and emotional eating.
Outcome

Cognitive restraint Uncontrolled eating Emotional eating

b [95% CI] β sr2 b [95% CI] β sr2 b [95% CI] β sr2

Step 1
Age − 0.00 [-0.01, 0.00] − .06 .00 − 0.01 [-0.01, –0.01] − .30*** .07 − 0.01 [-0.01, 0.00] − .11 .01
Female 0.20 [0.07, 0.32] .15* .02 − 0.07 [-0.20, 0.06] − .05 .00 0.20 [-0.09, 0.40] .10 .01
Income 0.02 [-0.01, 0.05] .08 .01 − 0.02 [-0.05, 0.01] − .07 .00 − 0.05 [-0.10, 0.00] − .12 .01
Education level 0.01 [-0.04, 0.07] .02 .00 0.02 [-0.04, 0.08] .04 .00 0.02 [-0.09, 0.13] .02 .00
R2 = .03* R2 = .09* R2 = .05*
Step 2
Emotional abuse 0.08 [-0.05, 0.17] .13 .01 0.16 [0.07, 0.25] .26*** .02 0.31[0.13, 0.48] .35* .04
Physical abuse − 0.02 [-0.12, 0.09] − .02 .00 0.01 [-0.10, 0.11] .01 .00 − 0.01 [-0.20, 0.18] − .01 .00
Sexual abuse − 0.00 [-0.09, 0.08] − .00 .00 0.02 [-0.07, 0.10] .02 .00 0.02 [-0.14, 0.18] .01 .00
Emotional neglect − 0.02 [-0.11, 0.06] − .04 .00 − 0.07 [-0.16, 0.01] − .12 .01 − 0.08 [-0.24, 0.08] − .09 .00
Physical neglect 0.04 [-0.10, 0.18] .036 .00 0.07 [-0.07, 0.25] .06 .00 0.01 [-0.27, 0.28] .00 .00
ΔR2 = .01 ΔR2 = .06* ΔR2 = .08*

Note. N =461. Gender reference category = Male. *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.

5
S. Martin and E. Strodl Appetite 188 (2023) 106975

Fig. 1. Significant Indirect Pathways from Childhood Trauma to Uncontrolled Eating.x


Note. Unstandardised coefficients reported. *p < .01, **p < .001.

6
S. Martin and E. Strodl Appetite 188 (2023) 106975

Fig. 2. Significant Indirect Pathways from Childhood Trauma to Emotional Eating.


Note. Unstandardised coefficients reported. *p < .01, **p < .001.

7
S. Martin and E. Strodl Appetite 188 (2023) 106975

4. Discussion 4.2. Hypothesis 2

The present study aimed to characterise the association between The findings partially support the hypothesis that the relationship
childhood trauma (i.e., childhood emotional abuse, physical abuse, between all forms of childhood trauma and each type of disordered
sexual abuse, emotional neglect, physical neglect) and three disordered eating would be mediated by all forms of metacognitive beliefs. The
eating behaviours (cognitive restraint, uncontrolled eating, and findings indicate that all forms of childhood trauma were related to
emotional eating). Additionally, it sought to examine the indirect role uncontrolled eating and emotional eating behaviours through one
that metacognitive beliefs (Uncontrollable and Dangerous, Positive Be­ metacognitive belief that thoughts are uncontrollable and dangerous.
liefs, Need to Control Thoughts, Cognitive Self-Consciousness, Cognitive These findings are in line with existing research that has found negative
Confidence) play in the associations between the five measures of metacognitive beliefs to play a role in the association between childhood
childhood maltreatment and three measures of disordered eating. trauma and psychological disorders (Hosseini-Ramaghani et al., 2019;
Myers & Wells, 2015; Østefjells et al., 2017). The findings did not detect
4.1. Hypothesis 1 any support for the other metacognitive beliefs. To the authors’
knowledge, no existing research has explored these three factors in a
The results partially support the hypotheses that each form of mediation model so direct comparison with other studies is not possible.
childhood trauma (emotional abuse, physical abuse, sexual abuse, The findings provide some level of support for the original cognitive
emotional neglect, physical neglect) would be independently associated developmental metacognitive model (Flavell, 1979) and Well’s (2002)
with each type of disordered eating (cognitive restraint, uncontrolled metacognitive model of psychopathology within a disordered eating
eating, emotional eating), after controlling for covariates and other context. That is, metacognitive knowledge, in the form of beliefs about
forms of childhood trauma. Examination of total effects revealed that cognitions, appear to be important in guiding the selection of meta­
only childhood emotional abuse significantly independently contributed cognitive strategies, in the form of maladaptive coping strategies such as
to uncontrolled eating and emotional eating behaviours. This study did uncontrolled and emotional eating behaviours. However, the findings
not find any support for direct and independent associations between differ from Well’s (2002) metacognitive model, in that only negative
childhood trauma experiences and cognitive restraint. beliefs that thoughts are uncontrollable and dangerous, and not positive
The significant association detected between childhood emotional beliefs about thinking strategies (such as worry or rumination), medi­
abuse and uncontrolled and emotional eating aligns with existing ated the association between childhood trauma and uncontrolled or
research. The literature suggests that this form of childhood abuse is the emotional eating. Comparatively, research examining the association
most prevalent (Stoltenborgh et al., 2015) and may better predict between metacognitive beliefs and disordered eating report both posi­
disordered eating in adulthood above and beyond that of childhood tive and negative metacognitive beliefs to predict various disordered
physical or sexual abuse in non-clinical community samples (Burns eating behaviours in both clinical (Aloi et al., 2021; Spada et al., 2016)
et al., 2012; Kent et al., 1999). Research examining childhood emotional and non-clinical samples (Laghi et al., 2018; Limbers et al., 2021).
abuse and constructs similar to uncontrolled eating (i.e. binge eating and Further research is therefore needed to clarify whether positive beliefs
overeating) have similarly reported significant associations in clinical about thinking strategies is an important component of a metacognitive
samples (Amianto et al., 2018; Caslini et al., 2016) and community model explaining the link between childhood trauma and disordered
samples (Feinson & Hornik-Lurie, 2016). The findings in this study eating. In addition, it is also important to highlight that the same met­
support the results presented by Michopoulos et al. (2015) and Hymo­ acognitive belief (thoughts are uncontrollable and dangerous) inde­
witz et al. (2017) who found childhood emotional abuse to be the form pendently mediated the associations between each of the five forms of
of abuse that was most associated with emotional eating behaviours in childhood trauma and two of the types of disordered eating behaviours
non-clinical community samples. Furthermore, the association between measured in this study. It therefore appears that the metacognitive
emotional abuse and emotional eating as a coping mechanism has also model identified in this study may be generic across different forms of
been characterised in previous research with non-clinical community childhood abuse and different forms of reactive disordered eating be­
samples (Burns et al., 2012; Kennedy et al., 2007). haviours. That is, any form of childhood trauma may facilitate the
While our findings are in line with other research, a couple of notable development of a belief that thoughts are uncontrollable and dangerous
differences were identified compared with the findings from similar and that, in turn, this metacognitive belief facilitates the selection of
previous research. Strodl and Wylie (2020) and Dawson et al. (2022) uncontrolled or emotional eating behaviours as maladaptive coping
both identified an independent association between childhood sexual strategies. This finding is consistent with a previous review of the
abuse and emotional eating. This is puzzling given that the present study literature indicating that multiple forms of childhood adversity are risk
adopted a similar methodology and questionnaires to that of Strodl and factors for the metacognitive belief that thoughts are uncontrollable and
Wylie (2020) and Dawson et al. (2022). This difference may be in part dangerous (Mansueto et al., 2019). These authors propose that while
due to the current sample consisting of a more even gender split, as childhood positive metacognitive beliefs might initially activate the CAS
studies have reported gender differences in the association between (according to Wells’ (2002) metacognitive model), continued poor
childhood trauma and disordered eating behaviours (Ackard et al., emotion regulation and heightened distress over time might result in the
2008; Fuemmeler et al., 2009). Other studies have found childhood strengthening of the metacognitive belief that thoughts are uncontrol­
sexual abuse in addition to emotional neglect, and physical neglect to be lable and dangerous. This is a plausible explanation and in further
associated with disordered eating behaviours (Demirci, 2018; Emery support of this explanation, there is also evidence that both positive and
et al., 2021). However, these samples consisted of adolescents and negative metacognitive beliefs are present in children (Ellis & Hudson,
young adults rather than adults with a broad age range and did not 2010) and that parenting factors may be related to genesis and perpet­
control for sociodemographic factors as in the present study. Similarly, uation (Chow & Lo, 2017; Gallagher & Cartwright-Hatton, 2008). As
while the findings of no direct independent associations between any of such, further longitudinal research is required to clarify the mechanisms
the five forms of childhood trauma and cognitive restraint is similar to linking childhood maltreatment with changes in positive metacognitive
the findings by Dawson et al. (2022), it differs from the finding by Strodl beliefs and the strengthening of the belief that thoughts are uncontrol­
and Wylie (2020) who found a direct association between childhood lable and dangerous over time.
emotional abuse and cognitive restraint. The inconsistencies in the
literature highlight that the relationship between different forms of 4.3. Implications
childhood trauma and disordered eating is complex and requires further
investigation to identify potential moderators. These findings have important practical and theoretical implications.

8
S. Martin and E. Strodl Appetite 188 (2023) 106975

In terms of a practical implication, the study strengthens the literature as the pandemic disrupted individuals’ daily life (Brown et al., 2021;
showing the role of childhood traumatic experiences in disordered Simone et al., 2021). In response to uncertainty, individuals may be
eating behaviours within a community sample. Such evidence empha­ more likely to engage in disordered eating behaviours as an effort to
sises the need to further develop effective preventative programs to cope with emotional states and/or gain control (Schlegl et al., 2020).
reduce the incidence of childhood traumatic experiences to reduce the The results from this study found that most participants’ eating behav­
long-term detrimental consequences of such experiences. In terms of a iours were approximately the same as prior to the pandemic, however, a
theoretical implication, the present study also adds to the existing body small proportion of individuals reported engaging in an increase or
of research by identifying an important metacognitive belief that ap­ decrease in the frequency and volume of eating behaviours (see Sup­
pears to be an influential psychological mechanism in uncontrolled and plementary Table A). The present study attempted to capture how
emotional eating. Conversely, the findings of this study indicate that COVID-19 altered eating behaviours but did not include this assessment
positive beliefs about thinking strategies, such as worry and rumination, in the analyses due to the absence of reliable measures. To examine this
may not be relevant to a metacognitive model of reactive disordered further, it is suggested future research replicating the design of this study
eating behaviours. It is important to note that the positive beliefs about are required at a time when social distancing and no stay-at-home
thinking subscale of the Metacognitions Questionnaire 30 was devel­ measures are imposed. In addition, given the exploratory nature of
oped based upon a metacognitive model of depression and anxiety. As this study, replication of the relationships found between independent
such, it is possible that a questionnaire that measures more precisely and dependent variables is required.
positive beliefs about perseverative thinking about body image, eating The present study utilised a community-based sample, thus findings
behaviours or food may identify a mediating role of a metacognitive may not be applicable to individuals with diagnosed eating disorders.
beliefs about the positive role of thinking processes more relevant to Given the logistical challenges of screening the participants using a
disordered eating. There is, therefore, a need to develop a questionnaire structured clinical interview with such a large sample size, we were
that may more precisely, and more comprehensively, assess the meta­ reliant on the participants self-report to exclude participants with a
cognitive model in relation to eating pathology. diagnosed eating disorder. This may have resulted in some misclassifi­
The consistent finding within this study of the important role of the cation. In addition, given the nature of this sample, further research is
belief that thoughts are uncontrollable and dangerous supports the need needed to investigate whether similar associations are found in a clinical
to further test the potency of this metacognitive belief in longitudinal sample of adults diagnosed with an eating disorder. The over-
studies. In addition, the causal role of the belief that thoughts are un­ representation of female participants and the absence of gender
controllable and dangerous in uncontrolled and emotional eating, diverse participants in the current sample is another notable limitation.
should be tested by interventional studies targeting this metacognitive While controlled for as a covariate, this sample resulted in an inability to
belief, particularly in those who have experienced childhood trauma. make gender-difference inferences. Therefore, the impact of gender on
Metacognitive therapy was developed by Wells (2002) to modify mal­ the found effects remains unknown due to the non-representative sam­
adaptive metacognitive beliefs and has been shown to be a successful ple. Future research should seek to recruit a more representative sample,
treatment measure among various psychological disorders (Normann & as it would be interesting to assess the gender effects. In addition, given
Morina, 2018; Philipp et al., 2019; Wells & Colbear, 2012). Meta­ the presence of minor cross-country variations in correlates with
cognitive therapy has also been found to be a promising treatment for disordered eating behaviours such as restrained eating (Strodl et al.,
individuals with binge eating disorder (Robertson & Strodl, 2020). 2020), there is a need to test cultural variations in the associations
Future interventional studies may consider utilising metacognitive identified in this study.
therapy to challenge the belief that thoughts are uncontrollable and This study was also limited to measuring metacognitive constructs as
dangerous community samples with experiencing high levels of un­ assessed by the MCQ-30. There is evidence that other important meta­
controlled and emotional eating and a history of childhood trauma. This cognitive processes, such as attentional bias as proposed in Wells’
is particularly important to test given that the highest probability of poor (2002) metacognitive model, are important in understanding the rela­
treatment outcomes with eating pathology has been observed in in­ tionship between traits such as impulsivity and disordered eating be­
dividuals who have experienced childhood trauma (Kong & Bernstein, haviours (Hou et al., 2011). Future research should incorporate further
2009). measures of metacognitive knowledge, metacognitive strategies and
metacognitive experiences to more fully test the applicability of the
4.4. Limitations and future directions metacognitive model to disordered eating. Similarly, the CTQ-SF was
limited in its measure of childhood maltreatment resulting in malnu­
Despite the contribution of the current study to the broader literature trition as it is possible that there may be a unique link between this form
and practice, various limitations may affect the accuracy of the findings of childhood maltreatment and disordered eating. While the CTQ-SF did
reported. Firstly, the CTQ-SF does not assess experiences of non- include one item measuring this issue “Did not have enough to eat”, the
interpersonal trauma (e.g., natural disasters, car accidents, bullying, correlations with the three measures of adult disordered eating were
witnessing violence, sudden loss, crime, or related events). These ad­ weak and statistically non-significant (see Supplementary Table E). It is
versities have also been identified as risk factors for disordered eating possible though that a more rigorous assessment of childhood malnu­
(Hecht & Hansen, 2001; Johnson et al., 2002). Thus, while the current trition might be useful in future studies. Finally, the cross-sectional
study is valuable in investigating the impacts of multiple domains of design of this study precludes confident interpretations about the di­
trauma, it may not capture the influence of wider traumatic experiences, rection of causality. Rather the findings of this study represent pre­
including experiences beyond childhood. Reporting bias and social liminary evidence that may provide direction and confidence to
desirability may be a limitation when interpreting the results of this researchers to explore the direction of causality in more resource
study. While the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire has shown good demanding longitudinal studies.
temporal stability in adults (Goltermann et al., 2023), there is also ev­
idence of respondents commonly minimising their past history of 5. Conclusion
childhood maltreatment (MacDonald et al., 2016). Furthermore, there is
good evidence of a low level of agreement between prospective and Disordered eating is common in society and the consequences of
retrospective measures of childhood maltreatment (Baldwin et al., these behaviours threaten peoples physical and mental wellbeing. The
2019). present study contributes to the limited literature exploring the role of
Additionally, the data collection occurred in the context of the metacognitive beliefs in the association between childhood trauma and
COVID-19 pandemic which may impact the generalisability of findings disordered eating behaviours. It was found childhood emotional abuse

9
S. Martin and E. Strodl Appetite 188 (2023) 106975

was associated with uncontrolled eating and emotional eating behav­ review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(6), 584–593. https://doi.org/
10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.0097
iours. Secondly, it was found that the metacognitive belief that thoughts
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in
are uncontrollable and dangerous mediated the association between all social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations.
domains of childhood trauma and uncontrolled and emotional eating Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173–1182. https://doi.org/
behaviours. None of the metacognitive beliefs measured in this study 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173
Bernstein, D., & Fink, L. (1998). Childhood trauma questionnaire. A retrospective self-report
mediated the association between childhood trauma and cognitive questionnaire and manual. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.
restraint. Bernstein, D. P., Fink, L., Handelsman, L., Foote, J., Lovejoy, M., Wenzel, K., Sapareto, E.,
These findings provide preliminary support for the role of meta­ & Ruggiero, J. (1994). Initial reliability and validity of a new retrospective measure
of child abuse and neglect. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151(8), 1132–1136.
cognitions as an underlying mechanism in the relationship between https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.151.8.1132
childhood trauma and uncontrolled eating and emotional eating, but not Bernstein, D., Stein, J., Newcomb, M., Walker, E., Pogge, D., Ahluvalia, T., Stokes, J.,
cognitive restraint. Together, the results help to inform future treatment Handelsman, L., Medrano, M., Desmond, D., & Zule, W. (2003). Development and
validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child
planning protocols when working with both clinical and subclinical Abuse & Neglect, 27(2), 169–190. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0145-2134(02)00541-0
disordered eating behaviours. The early identification and treatment of Brown, S., Opitz, M.-C., Peebles, A. I., Sharpe, H., Duffy, F., & Newman, E. (2021).
these beliefs could also prevent the onset of more severe psychopa­ A qualitative exploration of the impact of COVID-19 on individuals with eating
disorders in the UK. Appetite, 156. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2020.104977,
thology during adulthood among clients who have experienced child­ 104977–104977.
hood trauma or engage in disordered eating behaviours. Burns, E., Fischer, S., Jackson, J., & Harding, H. (2012). Deficits in emotion regulation
mediate the relationship between childhood abuse and later eating disorder
symptoms. Child Abuse & Neglect, 36(1), 32–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Author contributions chiabu.2011.08.005
Capobianco, L., Faija, C., Husain, Z., & Wells, A. (2020). Metacognitive beliefs and their
SM and ES designed the study. SM oversaw the data collection. SM relationship with anxiety and depression in physical illnesses: A systematic review.
PLoS One, 15(9), Article e0238457–e0238457. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.
and ES performed the data analysis. SM was the primary author of the pone.0238457
manuscript with ES writing sections of the manuscript as well as revising Cappelleri, J., Bushmakin, A., Gerber, R., Leidy, N., Sexton, C., Lowe, M., & Karlsson, J.
the manuscript. All authors have approved the final article. (2009). Psychometric analysis of the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire-R21: Results
from a large diverse sample of obese and non-obese participants. International
Journal of Obesity, 33(6), 611–620. https://doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2009.74
Funding Caslini, M., Bartoli, F., Crocamo, C., Dakanalis, A., Clerici, M., & Carrà, G. (2016).
Disentangling the association between child abuse and eating disorders: A systematic
review and meta-analysis. Psychosomatic Medicine, 78(1), 79–90. https://doi.org/
This research did not receive any specific grant from funding
10.1097/PSY.0000000000000233
agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors. Chow, K. W., & Lo, B. C. (2017). Parental factors associated with rumination related
metacognitive beliefs in adolescence. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 536. https://doi.org/
10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00536
Data code and availability Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203771587
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the Cooper, M. J., Wells, A., & Todd, G. (2004). A cognitive model of bulimia nervosa. British
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43(1), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1348/
corresponding author upon reasonable request.
014466504772812931
Dawson, D., Strodl, E., & Kitamura, H. (2022). Childhood maltreatment and disordered
Ethical statement eating: The mediating role of emotion regulation. Appetite, 172, Article 105952.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2022.105952
Demirci, E. (2018). Non suicidal self-injury, emotional eating, and insomnia after child
This study involves human data and has been performed in accor­ sexual abuse: Are those symptoms related to emotion regulation? Journal of Forensic
dance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The project was approved by the and Legal Medicine, 53, 17–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jflm.2017.10.012
Duarte, P., Palmeira, L., & Pinto-Gouveia, J. (2020). The three-factor eating
Queensland University Technology (QUT) Human Research Ethics
questionnaire-R21: A confirmatory factor analysis in a Portuguese sample. Eating and
Committee (Approval Number: 2021100236). Weight Disorders, 25(1), 247–256. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40519-018-0561-7
Ellis, D. M., & Hudson, J. L. (2010). The metacognitive model of generalized anxiety
disorder in children and adolescents. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 13,
Declaration of competing interest 151–163. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-010-0065-0
Emerson, R. W. (2015). Convenience sampling, random sampling, and snowball
sampling: How does sampling affect the validity of research? Journal of Visual
The authors declare no competsing interests. Impairment & Blindness, 109(2), 164–168. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0145482x1510900215
Data availability Emery, R., Yoon, C., Mason, S., & Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2021). Childhood maltreatment
and disordered eating attitudes and behaviors in adult men and women: Findings
from project EAT. Appetite, 163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2021.105224
Data will be made available on request. Feinson, M. C., & Hornik-Lurie, T. (2016). Binge eating & childhood emotional abuse:
The mediating role of anger. Appetite, 105, 487–493. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
appet.2016.05.018
Appendix A. Supplementary data Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-
developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34(10), 906–911. https://doi.org/
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi. 10.1037//0003-066X.34.10.906
Fuemmeler, B. F., Dedert, E., McClernon, F. J., & Beckham, J. C. (2009). Adverse
org/10.1016/j.appet.2023.106975.
childhood events are associated with obesity and disordered eating: Results from a
U.S. population-based survey of young adults. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 22(4),
References 329–333. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.20421
Gallagher, B., & Cartwright-Hatton, S. (2008). The relationship between parenting
factors and trait anxiety: Mediating role of cognitive errors and metacognition.
Aloi, M., Rania, M., Carbone, E. A., Caroleo, M., Calabrò, G., Zaffino, P., Nicolò, G.,
Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22(4), 722–733. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Carcione, A., Coco, G. L., Cosentino, C., & Segura-Garcia, C. (2021). Metacognition
janxdis.2007.07.006
and emotion regulation as treatment targets in binge eating disorder: A network
Georgantopolous, G., Konstantakopoulos, G., Michopoulos, I., Dikeos, D., & Gonidakis, F.
analysis study. Journal of Eating Disorders, 9(1). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-
(2020). The relationship between metacognitive beliefs and symptoms in eating
021-00376-x, 22–22.
disorders. Psychiatrikē, 31(3), 225–235. https://doi.org/10.22365/
Amianto, F., Spalatro, A. V., Rainis, M., Andriulli, C., Lavagnino, L., Abbate-Daga, G., &
jpsych.2020.313.225
Fassino, S. (2018). Childhood emotional abuse and neglect in obese patients with
Goltermann, J., Meinert, S., Hülsmann, C., Dohm, K., Grotegerd, D., Redlich, R., …
and without binge eating disorder: Personality and psychopathology correlates in
Dannlowski, U. (2023). Temporal stability and state-dependence of retrospective
adulthood. Psychiatry Research, 269, 692–699. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
self-reports of childhood maltreatment in healthy and depressed adults. Psychological
psychres.2018.08.089
Assessment, 35(1), 12. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0001175
Baldwin, J. R., Reuben, A., Newbury, J. B., & Danese, A. (2019). Agreement between
prospective and retrospective measures of childhood maltreatment: A systematic

10
S. Martin and E. Strodl Appetite 188 (2023) 106975

Hamonniere, T., & Varescon, I. (2018). Metacognitive beliefs in addictive behaviours: A Mansueto, G., Caselli, G., Ruggiero, G., & Sassaroli, S. (2019). Metacognitive beliefs and
systematic review. Addictive Behaviors, 85, 51–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. childhood adversities: An overview of the literature. Psychology Health & Medicine,
addbeh.2018.05.018 24(5), 542–550. https://doi.org/10.1080/13548506.2018.1550258
Hayes, A. F. (2018). Partial, conditional, and moderated moderated mediation: Markey, C. H., Strodl, E., Aimé, A., McCabe, M., Rodgers, R., Sicilia, A., … Fuller-
Quantification, inference, and interpretation. Communication Monographs, 85(1), Tyszkiewicz, M. (2022). A survey of eating styles in eight countries: Examining
4–40. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637751.2017.1352100 restrained, emotional, intuitive eating and their correlates. British Journal of Health
Hecht, D. B., & Hansen, D. J. (2001). The environment of child maltreatment: Contextual Psychology, 28(1), 136–155. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjhp.12616
factors and the development of psychopathology. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 6 de Medeiros, A., Yamamoto, M., Pedrosa, L., & Hutz, C. (2017). The Brazilian version of
(5), 433–457. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1359-1789(00)00015-X the three-factor eating questionnaire-R21: Psychometric evaluation and scoring
Hernandez, A., Gallardo-Pujol, D., Pereda, N., Arntz, A., Bernstein, D., Gaviria, A., pattern. Eating and Weight Disorders, 22(1), 169–175. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Labad, A., Valero, J., & Gutierrez-Zotes, J. (2013). Initial validation of the Spanish s40519-016-0256-x
childhood trauma questionnaire-short form: Factor structure, reliability and Michopoulos, V., Powers, A., Moore, C., Villarreal, S., Ressler, K. J., & Bradley, B. (2015).
association with parenting. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 28(7), 1498–1518. The mediating role of emotion dysregulation and depression on the relationship
https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260512468240 between childhood trauma exposure and emotional eating. Appetite, 91, 129–136.
Hosseini-Ramaghani, N. A., Rezaei, F., Sepahvandi, M. A., Gholamrezaei, S., & https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.03.036
Mirderikvand, F. (2019). The mediating role of the metacognition, time perspectives Minnich, A. M., Gordon, K. H., Kwan, M. Y., & Troop-Gordon, W. (2017). Examining the
and experiential avoidance on the relationship between childhood trauma and post- mediating role of alexithymia in the association between childhood neglect and
traumatic stress disorder symptoms. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 10(1). disordered eating behaviors in men and women. Psychology of Men and Masculinity,
https://doi.org/10.1080/20008198.2019.1648173, 1648173–1648173. 18(4), 414–421. https://doi.org/10.1037/men0000060
Hou, R., Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Moss-Morris, R., Peveler, R., & Roefs, A. (2011). Mizuki, R., & Fujiwara, T. (2020). Validation of the Japanese version of the childhood
External eating, impulsivity and attentional bias to food cues. Appetite, 56(2), trauma questionnaire—short form (CTQ–J). In Psychological trauma: Theory, research,
424–427. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2011.01.019 practice, and policy. https://doi-org.ezp01.library.qut.edu.au/10.1037/tra0000972.
Hymowitz, G., Salwen, J., & Salis, K. (2017). A mediational model of obesity related supp.
disordered eating: The roles of childhood emotional abuse and self-perception. Eating Myers, S. G., & Wells, A. (2015). Early trauma, negative affect, and anxious attachment:
Behaviors, 26, 27–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2016.12.010 The role of metacognition. Anxiety, Stress & Coping, 28(6), 634–649. https://doi.org/
Johnson, J. G., Cohen, P., Kasen, S., & Brook, J. S. (2002). Childhood adversities 10.1080/10615806.2015.1009832
associated with risk for eating disorders or weight problems during adolescence or Neumark-Sztainer, D., Wall, M., Larson, N., Eisenberg, M., & Loth, K. (2011). Dieting and
early adulthood. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(3), 394–400. https://doi.org/ disordered eating behaviors from adolescence to young adulthood: Findings from a
10.1176/appi.ajp.159.3.394 10-year longitudinal study. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 111(7),
Kärkkäinen, U., Mustelin, L., Raevuori, A., Kaprio, J., & Keski-Rahkonen, A. (2018). Do 1004–1011. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2011.04.012
disordered eating behaviours have long-term health-related consequences? European Normann, N., & Morina, N. (2018). The efficacy of metacognitive therapy: A systematic
Eating Disorders Review, 26(1), 22–28, 10.1002/erv.2568. review and meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/
Karlsson, J., Persson, O., Sjöström, L., & Sullivan, M. (2000). Psychometric properties fpsyg.2018.02211, 2211–2211.
and factor structure of the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ) in obese men Olstad, S., Solem, S., Hjemdal, O., & Hagen, R. (2015). Metacognition in eating disorders:
and women. Results from the Swedish Obese Subjects (SOS) study. International Comparison of women with eating disorders, self-reported history of eating disorders
Journal of Obesity, 24(12), 1715–1725. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0801442 or psychiatric problems, and healthy controls. Eating Behaviors: International Journal,
Kennedy, M. A., Ip, K., Samra, J., & Gorzalka, B. B. (2007). The role of childhood 16, 17–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2014.10.019
emotional abuse in disordered eating. Journal of Emotional Abuse, 7(1), 17–36. Østefjells, T., Lystad, J. U., Berg, A. O., Hagen, R., Loewy, R., Sandvik, L., Melle, I., &
https://doi.org/10.1300/J135v07n01_02 Røssberg, J. I. (2017). Metacognitive beliefs mediate the effect of emotional abuse on
Kent, A., Waller, G., & Dagnan, D. (1999). A greater role of emotional than physical or depressive and psychotic symptoms in severe mental disorders. Psychological
sexual abuse in predicting disordered eating attitudes: The role of mediating Medicine, 47(13), 2323–2333. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717000848
variables. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 25(2), 159–167. https://doi.org/ O’Sullivan, D., Rahamathulla, M., & Pawar, M. (2020). The impact and implications of
10.1002/(SICI)1098–108X(199903)25:2<159::AID-EAT5>3.0.CO;2-F covid-19: An Australian perspective. The International Journal of Community and
Kim, D., Bae, H., Han, C., Oh, H., & MacDonald, K. (2013). Psychometric properties of Social Development, 2(2), 134–151. https://doi.org/10.1177/2516602620937922
the childhood trauma questionnaire-short form (CTQ-SF) in Korean patients with Philipp, R., Kriston, L., Lanio, J., Kühne, F., Härter, M., Moritz, S., & Meister, R. (2019).
schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 144(1), 93–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Effectiveness of metacognitive interventions for mental disorders in adults—a
schres.2012.12.020 systematic review and meta-analysis (METACOG). Clinical Psychology &
Kong, S., & Bernstein, K. (2009). Childhood trauma as a predictor of eating Psychotherapy, 26(2), 227–240. https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.2345
psychopathology and its mediating variables in patients with eating disorders. Quattropani, M., Lenzo, V., Faraone, C., Pistorino, G., Di Bella, I., & Mucciardi, M.
Journal of Clinical Nursing, 18(13), 1897–1907. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365- (2016). The role of metacognition in eating behavior: An exploratory study.
2702.2008.02740.x Mediterranean Journal of Clinical Psychology, 4(3). https://doi.org/10.6092/2282-
Kongerslev, M., Bach, B., Rossi, G., Trauelsen, A., Ladegaard, N., Løkkegaard, S., & Bo, S. 1619/2016.4.1213
(2019). Psychometric validation of the childhood trauma questionnaire-short form Rabito-Alcón, M. F., Baile, J. I., & Vanderlinden, J. (2021). Mediating factors between
(CTQ-SF) in a Danish clinical sample. Child Abuse & Neglect, 94. https://doi.org/ childhood traumatic experiences and eating disorders development: A systematic
10.1016/j.chiabu.2019.104026, 104026–104026. review. Children, 8(2), 114. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8020114
Laghi, F., Bianchi, D., Pompili, S., Lonigro, A., & Baiocco, R. (2018). Metacognition, Robertson, S., & Strodl, E. (2020). Metacognitive therapy for binge eating disorder: A
emotional functioning and binge eating in adolescence: The moderation role of need case series study. Clinical Psychologist, 24(2), 143–154. https://doi.org/10.1111/
to control thoughts. Eating and Weight Disorders, 23(6), 861–869. https://doi.org/ cp.12213
10.1007/s40519-018-0603-1 Rucker, D. D., Preacher, K. J., Tormala, Z. L., & Petty, R. E. (2011). Mediation analysis in
Limbers, C. A., Greenwood, E., Shea, K., & Fergus, T. A. (2021). Metacognitive beliefs and social psychology: Current practices and new recommendations: Mediation analysis
emotional eating in adolescents. Eating and Weight Disorders. https://doi.org/10.1007/ in social psychology. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 5(6), 359–371.
s40519-020-01078-0 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00355.x
Little, R. J. A. (1988). A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with Russell, J. K., Strodl, E., & Kavanagh, D. J. (2021). Correlates of distress in young people
missing values. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 83(404), 1198–1202. with cystic fibrosis: The role of self-efficacy and metacognitive beliefs. Psychology
https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.1988.10478722 and Health, 36(12), 1497–1513. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2020.1861280
Lopez-Cepero, A., Frisard, C. F., Lemon, S. C., & Rosal, M. C. (2018). Association of Santomauro, D. F., Melen, S., Mitchison, D., Vos, T., Whiteford, H., & Ferrari, A. J.
dysfunctional eating patterns and metabolic risk factors for cardiovascular disease (2021). The hidden burden of eating disorders: An extension of estimates from the
among Latinos. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics, 118(5), 849–856, global burden of disease study 2019. The Lancet Psychiatry, 8(4), 320–328. https://
10.1016/j.jand.2017.06.007. doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(21)00040-7
da Luz, F., Sainsbury, A., Mannan, H., Touyz, S., Mitchison, D., & Hay, P. (2017). Sapuppo, W., Ruggiero, G. M., Caselli, G., & Sassaroli, S. (2018). The body of cognitive
Prevalence of obesity and comorbid eating disorder behaviors in South Australia and metacognitive variables in eating disorders: Need of control, negative beliefs
from 1995 to 2015. International Journal of Obesity, 41(7), 1148–1153. https://doi. about worry uncontrollability and danger, perfectionism, self-esteem and worry.
org/10.1038/ijo.2017.79 Israel Journal of Psychiatry, 55(1), 55–65.
MacDonald, K., Thomas, M. L., Sciolla, A. F., Schneider, B., Pappas, K., Bleijenberg, G., Schlegl, S., Maier, J., Meule, A., & Voderholzer, U. (2020). Eating disorders in times of
Bohus, M., Bekh, B., Carpenter, L., Carr, A., Dannlowski, U., Dorahy, M., Fahlke, C., the COVID-19 pandemic—results from an online survey of patients with anorexia
Finzi-Dottan, R., Karu, T., Gerdner, A., Glaesmer, H., Grabe, H. J., Heins, M., … nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 53(11), 1791–1800. https://doi.
Wingenfeld, K. (2016). Minimization of childhood maltreatment is common and org/10.1002/eat.23374
consequential: Results from a large, multinational sample using the childhood Sheehan, D. V., & Herman, B. K. (2015). The psychological and medical factors
trauma questionnaire. PLoS One, 11(1), Article e0146058–e0146058. https://doi. associated with untreated binge eating disorder. Primary Care Companion for CNS
org/10.1371/journal.pone.0146058 Disorders, 17(2). https://doi.org/10.4088/PCC.14r01732
MacKinnon, D. P. (2008). Introduction to statistical mediation analysis. Lawrence Erlbaum Simone, M., Emery, R. L., Hazzard, V. M., Eisenberg, M. E., Larson, N., & Neumark-
Associates. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203809556 Sztainer, D. (2021). Disordered eating in a population-based sample of young adults
MacKinnon, D. P., Fairchild, A. J., & Fritz, M. S. (2007). Mediation analysis. Annual during the COVID-19 outbreak. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 54(7),
Review of Psychology, 58, 593–614. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev. 1189–1201. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.23505
psych.58.110405.085542 Spada, M. M., Caselli, G., Fernie, B. A., Nikčević, A. V., Ruggiero, G. M., Boccaletti, F.,
Dallari, G., & Sassaroli, S. (2016). Metacognitions about desire thinking predict the

11
S. Martin and E. Strodl Appetite 188 (2023) 106975

severity of binge eating in a sample of Italian women. Eating and Weight Disorders, 21 Strodl, E., & Wylie, L. (2020). Childhood trauma and disordered eating: Exploring the
(2), 297–304. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40519-015-0205-0 role of alexithymia and beliefs about emotions. Appetite, 154. https://doi.org/
Spada, M. M., Proctor, D., Caselli, G., & Strodl, E. (2013). Metacognition in substance 10.1016/j.appet.2020.104802, 104802–104802.
misuse. Principles of addiction: Comprehensive Addictive Behaviors and Disorders, 1, Stunkard, A. J., & Messick, S. (1985). The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure
355–362. https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-398336-7.00037-1 dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 29(1),
Spinhoven, P., Penninx, B. W., Hickendorff, M., van Hemert, A. M., Bernstein, D. P., & 71–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3999(85)90010-8
Elzinga, B. M. (2014). Childhood trauma questionnaire: Factor structure, Vann, A., Strodl, E., & Anderson, E. (2013). Thinking about internal states, a qualitative
measurement invariance, and validity across emotional disorders. Psychological investigation into metacognitions in women with eating disorders. Journal of Eating
Assessment, 26(3), 717–729. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000002 Disorders, 1(1). https://doi.org/10.1186/2050-2974-1-22, 22–22.
Stoltenborgh, M., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Alink, L. R., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. Vann, A., Strodl, E., & Anderson, E. (2014). The transdiagnostic nature of metacognitions
(2015). The prevalence of child maltreatment across the globe: Review of a series of in women with eating disorders. Eating Disorders, 22(4), 306–320. https://doi.org/
meta-analyses. Child Abuse Review, 24(1), 37–50. https://doi.org/10.1002/car.2353 10.1080/10640266.2014.890447
Strodl, E., Markey, C., Aimé, A., Rodgers, R. F., Dion, J., Coco, G. L., Gullo, S., Wells, A. (2002). Emotional disorders and metacognition: Innovative cognitive therapy. John
McCabe, M., Mellor, D., Granero-Gallegos, A., Sicilia, A., Castelnuovo, G., Probst, M., Wiley & Sons. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470713662
Maïano, C., Manzoni, G. M., Begin, C., Blackburn, M.-E., Pietrabissa, G., Alcaraz- Wells, A., & Carter, K. E. P. (2009). Maladaptive thought control strategies in generalized
Ibánez, M., … Fuller-Tyszkiewicz, M. (2020). A cross-country examination of anxiety disorder, major depressive disorder, and nonpatient groups and relationships
emotional eating, restrained eating and intuitive eating: Measurement Invariance with trait anxiety. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 2(3), 224–234. https://
across eight countries. Body Image, 35, 245–254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. doi.org/10.1521/ijct.2009.2.3.224
bodyim.2020.09.013 Wells, A., & Cartwright-Hatton, S. (2004). A short form of the metacognitions
Strodl, E., Stewart, L., Mullens, A. B., & Deb, S. (2015). Metacognitions mediate HIV questionnaire: Properties of the MCQ-30. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42(4),
stigma and depression/anxiety in men who have sex with men living with HIV. 385–396. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00147-5
Health Psychology Open, 2(1), Article 2055102915581562. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Wells, A., & Colbear, J. S. (2012). Treating posttraumatic stress disorder with
2055102915581562 metacognitive therapy: A preliminary controlled trial. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
68(4), 373–381. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20871

12

You might also like