You are on page 1of 12

Instruction and Exposure:

How Do They Contribute


To Second Language Acquisition?
Ej B. Shresta
Bilkent University
ABSTRACT Adults have access to two distinct approaches to develop their second language
competence. These are instruction and exposure (Krashen 1982). Both approaches contribute
to second language acquisition in their unique ways. This article describes how they con-
tributed to the development of oral proficiency in English as a second language in mutually ex-
clusive learning situations in Nepal.
There were 58 randomly selected subjects in the study, who came from two distinct groups.
The first group was composed o fpeople who had learned English mainly through formal class-
room instruction with grammar-based approaches. The second group was composed ofpeople
who had learned English mainly through informal contact with English-speaking people. Eng-
lish speech samples were collected through personal interviews and presentations based on vi-
sual materials. These oral responses were judged holistically by five independentjudges. They
were also analyzed by means o fgrammar and fluency-relatederrors. Data were analyzed using
t-tests and correlation procedures.
The main finding was that both instruction and exposure contributed to second language ac-
quisition in their own unique ways. The former seemed to promote accuracy and the latter flu-
ency. For communicationpurposes, however, fluencyseemed to be more critical than accuracy.

Introduction resent two different approaches to second lan-


Second language acquisition specialists all guage acquisition. They are formal classroom
over the world are familiar with generations of instruction and informal natural exposure. In
students who, despite having spent ten or formal classroom instruction, students are
more years in classes learning a second lan- generally exposed to selectively graded cur-
guage, emerge as nonfunctioning adults in riculum materials that are characterized by
second language performance, especially some form of rule isolation, error detection,
with respect to oral performance. On the and/or error correction (Krashen 1981). In in-
other hand, countless numbers of cases can formal natural exposure, however, there is no
be cited in which second language learners formal articulation of rules, nor are the mate
have learned to use the second language with rials selectively graded; the basic focus is on
ease and facility without ever going to school. the communication of meaning (Pica 1983).
They seemed to have acquired this skill virtu-
ally in the street, far from the classroom. Formal VersustnformalExposure
These apparently contradictory cases have There seems to be no clearcut superiority
baffled second language acquisition special- of one approach over the other as far as E L
ists, especially English as a second language development is concerned. One group of
(FSL) specialists,for quite some time, and rep studies indicates that formal classroom in-
struction does indeed help in developing E L
Tej 6. Shresfa (Ph.D., The University of Connecticut) is proficiency. A major portion of the work in
Instructor in the M.A. TEFL Program a t Bilkent University, this direction came from Krashen and his a s
Ankara, Turkey. sociates (Krashen, Seliger, and Hartnett 1974;

Foreign Language Annals, 31,No. 2, 1998


FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANNALSSVMMER 1998

Krashen and Seliger 1975; Krashen 1976; dent when ESL proficiency is measured not
Krashen and Seliger 1976; Krashen, Zelinski, only in terms of linguistic competence but
Jones, and Usprich 1978). Similar findings r e also in terms of sociolinguistic competence.
garding the beneficial effects of formal class- Billmyer (1990) reports that formal instruction
room instruction have been reported by in social rules helped ESL learners produce
others (Briere 1978; Chihara and Oller 1978; culturerelevant appropriate native-like pro-
Bialystok 1979; Pica 1983;Long 1983; Billmyer ductions, whereas similar attempts in another
1990; and Ellis 1991). study by King and Silver (1993) reveal no ef-
Another group of studies points out that fect of instruction.
formal classroom instruction does not make
any significant contribution to ESL develop Rationale Tor the fiesent Study
ment, especially when opportunities to prac- This brief review of literature on the relative
tice English exist outside the classroom effectiveness of formal instruction and infor-
(Upshur 1968; Hale and Budar 1970; Mason mal exposure has been a mixed bag. The
1971; Schumann 1976; Martin 1980; Kadia main problem is that the findings of these
1989; and Tang 1991). studies could not be attributed to one specific
The main argument of the proponents of in- condition of learning. Krashen and his associ-
formal exposure to the natural environment is ates’ subjects who learned English through
based on their belief that language acquisi- formal instruction, for example, lived in the
tion is a process of creative construction. They United States and had the benefit of informal
believe that second language learners sub- exposure. Similarly,all claims made on behalf
consciously internalize various rules and in- of informal exposure were based on studies
ductively create new constructions when they that were conducted in a so-called informal
attend to primary linguistic data in the natural natural environment where access to formal
environment (Dulay and Burt 1976). instruction or its characteristics was easily
On the other hand, proponents of formal available. One could not say for sure that the
classroom instruction argue that whereas chil- observations made on the basis of such data
dren might be able to benefit from informal were strictly products of formal instruction or
exposure to natural environment, adults must informal exposure.
be exposed to formal classroom instruction to In order to really understand the unique con-
acquire a second language (Krashen 1981). tributions of formal instruction and informal ex-
They claim that formal classroom instruction posure to second language development, one
filled with comprehensible input designed to should study them in mutually exclusive con-
convey meaning in a low-anxiety situation is ditions of learning. Such conditions exist in
especially beneficial for beginning adult ESL Nepal. It is one of the few countries in the world
learners for whom the outside world is not where possibilities exist for people to acquire
prepared to provide such input for necessary ESL through mutually exclusive formal or infor-
intake. In fact, some other proponents of for- mal language learning environments. One can
mal classroom instruction even go to the ex- easily identify people in Nepal whose ESL p r o
tent of stating that it is beneficial not only to ficiency can be attributed beyond doubt to ei-
beginning adult ESL learners but also to inter- ther formal language learning experience or
mediate and advanced ESL learners, both informal natural exposure.
adults and children alike (Long 1983). Still
others state that second language learners The Study
who receive formal instruction outperform Therefore, this study explored the following
those who do not, both in terms of rate of research question:
achievement and ultimate level of achieve- How do formal classroom instruction and
ment (Ellis 1991). informal natural exposure contribute to the
The differential effect of instruction is evi- development of oral proficiency in English as

232
FOREIGN LANGUAGE A N N W U M M E R 1998

a second language under mutually exclusive purposes, only in the formal English classes of
conditions of second language learning? their local schools. Instructional practices at
In order to determine E L oral proficiency these schools and the curriculum materials in
of the subjects, a holistic scoring method, use would convince an independent observer
analysis of errors in grammar and structure, of an overriding influence of grammar-based
analysis of problems related to fluency in approaches in the teaching of English in
speech, and the extent of vocabulary devel- Nepal (Ministry of Education and Culture
opment were used as criterion measures. 1981).

Methods Formal Exposure Period


Since formal context subjects included
Subjects tenth-grade students through college gradu-
Potential subjects were divided into two ates, the period of exposure to formal c1a.s
groups: those who had been exposed to E n g room instruction in English ranged from seven
lish mainly through formal classroom instruc- years to twelve years. Let us briefly estimate
tion and those who came into contact with what this meant in terms of hours per year.
English either through their experience as The average number of working days at
trekking guides or through their exposure to general public schools in Nepal is about 190
Englishspeaking tourists. Some 300 potential days a year from Sunday through Friday, with
subjects were identified for each group; of Friday as a half working day. English is a r e
these, ten percent were chosen at random quired subject in the Nepalese schools, and it
from each group as the sample population to is taught for three hours a week in grades four
be studied in detail. and five, and five hours a week in grades six
through ten. In other words, a n average stu-
Formal Context Subjects dent in a general public school in Nepal r e
The first group of subjects consisted of 29 ceives about 115 hours a year of English
people who learned English mainly through instruction in grades four and five and about
formal classroom instruction. They repre- 150 hours a year in grades six through ten. By
sented the formal context subjects in the the end of grade ten, therefore, roughly 1,OOO
study. Most of them were from various outly- hours of English instruction is given to an av-
ing districts of Nepal and were in the capital erage student who graduates from a general
city for a brief period of about a year as a part public school in Nepal; this instruction contin-
of preservice and inservice teacher training ues throughout the postsecondary undergrack
programs. Besides this, some tenthgrade high uate program in varying amounts, depending
school students from outside the city of Kath- upon the field of study. The minimum is five
mandu were also subjects. hours a week.
These formal context subjects learned their This period of exposure is a very rough esti-
English in classes taught through grammar- mate indeed because it takes into account
based methods. They had mainly Nepalese only instruction hours at schools and colleges
teachers for their models of English speech. in Nepal. It does not include the hours stu-
Other than an occasional use of modem cul- dents invest in the study of English at home.
tural blessings such as movies and videos, Obviously, several more hours are spent in
these formal context subjects did not have ex- homework and test preparation on a regular
posure to an Englishspeaking environment by basis.
way of visiting Englishspeaking countries, nor
did they have any chance to come in contact Informal Context Subjects
on a sustained basis with native Englishspeak- The second group, also consisting of 29
ing people within their own country. Their ex- people, represented the informal group.
posure to English was thus, for all intents and These informal subjects virtually picked up

233
FOREIGN LANGUAGE A N N U - S U M M E R 1998

their English in the streets of Kathmandu or Nepal. An average trekking trip lasts about a
on trekking routes within the hills and moun- week to ten days, which roughly adds up to 60
tains of Nepal. The majority of subjects came working days a year. Given six hours of con-
from the legendary Sherpa community, tact a day with Englishspeaking tourists on a
whose members work a s mountaineering trekking trip, this exposure amounts to 360
and trekking guides for foreign tourists and hours a year. In about three years, therefore,
expedition teams. they have about 1,000 hours of exposure to
Most of these informal subjects had no for- Englishspeaking people.
mal schooling at all. A few who had somehow As with the formal subjects, it is hard to be
managed to go to school early on in their life precise about the exact exposure period.
were either pushed out or pushed themselves Moreover, it is always difficult to establish a
out of the formal school system before long o n e t w n e correspondence between the for-
because of their need to work. Even those mal and informal settings with regard to the
who had a few-years of formal schooling had quality of a given period of exposure. One
no formal instruction in English because it is year of informal exposure is not exactly the
taught only from grade four onward in public same as one year of formal exposure both in
schools of Nepal. As working adolescents and terms of quality and intensity. Even within the
adults, these informal subjects could ill afford same exposure setting, the quality of exposure
to go back to school to learn English. There- fluctuates from year to year, depending on in-
fore, it is fair to state that the E L proficiency teraction or classroom variables.
of the informal group was a product of untu-
tored, natural exposure to English. Data Collection
The informal context subjects were compa-
rable to the formal context subjects in terms of hlot Testing
their age level and gender mix. Among the Before the actual data collection was
subjects were four females who were working started, a pilot testing was conducted on a ran-
as domestic help in the households of Ameri- d o m sample of a small group representing
can residents in Kathmandu. The majority of mutually exclusive formal and informal s u b
subjects, however, were the Sherpas who jects. It was designed to determine the effec-
work as trekking and mountaineering guides tiveness of the materials and methods used in
for foreign trekkers and mountain climbers in eliciting the ESL speech. During this field-test-
Nepal. ing phase of the study, it was found that very
few students below grade ten from the general
Informal fiposure Period public schools in Nepal were able to speak
The period of exposure to an informal lan- English intelligibly even at the basic interper-
guage learning environment for these subjects sonal communication level involving elemen-
ranged from three years to fifteen years. They tary vocabulary. Therefore, in the present
were especially active during the trekking sea- study, the formal group of subjects included
son that starts sometime in October and goes only students from grade ten through gradu-
through May, with a break in winter from mid- ate level who had learned English mainly
December to mid-January. The remaining pe- through formal classroom instruction. In other
riod from mid-June through September is the words, the period of ESL exposure for the for-
monsoon season in Nepal. During this period mal group ranged from seven years to twelve
the informal context subjects have little years or so.
chance to be in contact with foreign trekkers Similarly, during the field testing, it was
and tourists. found that very few people with fewer than
These informal subjects make an average of three years of informal ESL exposure could use
about six to eight trips a year as trekking the language intelligibly for communication
guides for foreign tourists and trekkers in purposes at or beyond the sentence level.

234
FOREIGN LANGUAGE A"ALS--SUiUiUER 1998

Therefore, the criterion of a minimum 01 three cal know-how or other extralinguistic abilities
years of informal exposure was established. on the part of the subjects to respond to the
The majority of those in the informal group, pictorial presentation. Since the writer himself
however, had had five to ten years of exposure, was the interviewer, there was no problem in
with one man having had 15 years. This man, explaining the purpose of the study in Nepali
though, was not five times more proficient than and preparing the subjects for the interview.
those with three years of exposure, nor were All the interviewer was interested in was the
those with ten years of exposure twice as profi- collection of adequate samples of ESL speech
cient as those with five years of exposure. In from the subjects, and he would continue to
fact, there were quite a few cases in which in- reinforce positively whenever they made an
formal subjects with less exposure scored attempt to speak freely without hesitations. As
higher in the holistic judgment than those with a matter of fact, in order to lower such nega-
longer exposure. This finding seemed to con- tive feelings as apprehension, anxiety, ner-
firm what some believed to be the lack of lin- vousness, and tension, most of the interviews
ear positive correlation between informal were conducted in a relaxed atmosphere at a
exposure period and second language profi- corner or cubicle of a bar or restaurant in
ciency (Krashen 1982). Kathmandu.
The pilot testing provided necessary feed- In this way, speech samples were collected
back to improve upon the tools and tech- and preserved on cassette tapes. They were
niques of ESL speech elicitation. Appropriate later analyzed to investigate if there were sig-
changes were made accordingly. This im- nificant differences between the formal and
proved their effectiveness by way of generat- informal subjects in their ESL proficiency.
ing free and unrehearsed samples of ESL
speech that were appropriate for tapping ESL Data Analysis
competence (Littlewood 1984). The speech samples were first analyzed
holistically by a team of five independent
Speech Sample Elicitation Techniques judges who were educated Americans. Four
After necessary adjustments were made, ac- of them had Ph.D. degrees and the remaining
tual data collection was undertaken. Both the one had a master's degree, teaching ESL. They
formal and informal subjects were interviewed based their judgments on their overall impres-
individually with the help of questionnaires sion of the individual speech samples, each
that were designed to gather background infor- about five minutes long, which were random-
mation as to the type and period of exposure to ized between subject group representatives in
S L and other socioeconomic variables. They order of presentation for scoring.
were also presented with a series of pictures Prior to scoring the speech samples, all the
from The Ramayana and The Mahabharata, judges went through a practice session that
two popular sources of the Nepalese culture lasted until they were fully confident with, and
and tradition. The subjects were asked to tell in unanimous in the use of, the scoring criteria
English about the important episodes and that was somewhat similar to the Foreign Ser-
events presented in the pictures and describe vice Institute scale. The criterion scale em-
the main characters presented there. phasized the use of language as a tool of
Every effort was made to collect an ade- communication. The criteria were developed
quate sample of ESL speech from both groups on a fivepoint scale, with one being poor and
of subjects. For example, the subjects were five being excellent (see Appendix A).
constantly reminded of the fact that there was After the holistic scoring of each speech
no truth value attached to their description of sample by the five independent judges, a
the events presented in the pictures so that holistic mean was computed for each subject
they would feel free to express whatever they by way of averaging the five holistic scores. In
liked. Moreover, it did not require any techni- order to address the issue of reliability of the

235
FOREIGN LANGUAGE A N N - U m R 1998

holistic scoring method, a correlation matrix TABLE 2


among the five raters was computed, which is
prese'nted below: T-Test of the Holistic Mean Scores

TABLE 1 Variable: Holistic Mean


Group N Mean S.D. T DF P
CorrelationsAcross Five Raters for l.(Fomal) 29 2.675 0.890 -3.005 56 0.004
Estimating the Interrater Reliability 2.(lnformal) 29 3.282 0.624
Score (N = 58)
R1 R2 R3 R4 The t-test on the holistic mean scores indi-
R2 0.6689 cated that there was a significant difference
(0.0001) between the two groups @< .Ol). The infor-
mal group secured a significantly higher holis-
R3 0.6144 0.7017 tic mean score than the formal group. In the
(0.0001) (0.Oool) opinion of the five educated American judges
who spoke standard English, the informal sub
R4 0.5280 0.6360 0.7256 jects were better than the formal subjects in
(0.0001) (0.000l) (0.0001) their ESL oral proficiency.
This result was further analyzed in specific
R5 0.4808 0.5542 0.6579 0.8970 details. What made five judges judge the way
(0.0001) (0.0001) (0.0001) (0.Oool) they did? It was decided to supplement the
Note: Probabilities are shown in parentheses. judges' subjective scoring with some sort of
objective measures. First, an error analysis on
The correlation coefficients among the grammar and structural problems was under-
raters ranged between .48 and .90, which taken. It consisted of calculating the number
were all significant (pc.01). The average inter- of subject-verb disagreements, e.g., "He go;"
correlation coefficient among the five raters adjective-noun disagreements, e.g., "Two
was .67, calculated by the Z-transformation book;" omissions of copulas, e.g., "He going;"
method. The standardized item alpha analog and w o r d a d e r problems, e.g., "English speak
for interrater reliability of the average score many people." Based on a combined score on
across five raters using the Spearman-Brown these four problem areas, a composite score
method approached .91. on grammar and structure problems was ob
tained for each subject in both the formal and
Results informal groups. The mean difference be-
After the interrater reliability was estab- tween the two groups was analyzed using a t-
lished, speech samples were analyzed to de- test and the results are presented below:
termine if there was any significant difference
in ESL proficiency between the two groups of TABLE 3
subjects who learned English in two different
ways. A t-test based on their holistic mean T-Test of the Composite Mean Scores
scores, as measures of ESL oral proficiency, On Grammar and Structure Problems
was computed. The results are presented in Variable: Grammar and Structure Problems
Table 2: Group N Mean S.D. T DF P
l.(Formal) 29 3.134 2.263-3.0918 56.0 0.0031
P.(Informal) 29 5.172 2.726

The t-test indicated a statistically significant


mean difference between the two groups on
grammar and structure problems (p< .Ol).

236
FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANNALSSUMMER 1998

The composite mean score of the formal grammatical and structural problems than the
group was significantly less than that of the in- former.
formal group, which indicated that the formal How d o these problems affect oral profi-
subjects made significantly fewer errors than ciency in E L ? As stated earlier, in the opinion
the informal subjects did on grammar and of the five judges, the informal group was sig
structure. nificantly better than the formal group in its
Despite this apparent superiority of the for- E L oral proficiency. This indicates that for ef-
mal group of subjects in terms of their ability fective communication purposes, the ability
to use grammar and structure correctly, what to speak English fluently is more critical than
could be the reason for their receiving lower the ability to speak grammatically correct but
scores on overall oral proficiency in E!jL from halting English sentences. In the opinion of
the judges? The speech samples of both for- the five judges, fluency is more important than
mal and informal subjects were further ana- accuracy in communicating an oral message.
lyzed. This time it was decided to analyze the This is illustrated by the following correlation
samples in terms of fluency-related problems, table:
which consisted of audible and inaudible
pauses, hesitations, repetitions, and fragmen- TABLE 5
tations in speech. A composite score on flu- Correlation Between the Holistic Mean
ency-related problems was obtained for each Score and Composite Mean Scores
subject in both the formal and informal On Grammar and Fluency-Related
groups by combining scores from each of the Problems (N=58)
three fluency-related problems. A t-test was
used to determine if any significant mean dif- Holistic Mean Grammar Problems
ference existed between the two groups with Grammar 4.0246
respect to this issue. The results are presented problems (0.8545 )
below: Fluency 4.6339 4.1060
problems (0.0001) (0.4282)
TABLE 4 Note: Probabilities are shown in parentheses.

T-Test of the Composite Mean Scores As the table indicates, there was no signifi-
On Fluency-Related Problems cant correlation between the holistic mean
Variable: Fluency-RelatedProblems score and the composite mean score on gram-
Group N Mean S.D. T DF P matical and structural errors (subject-verb
l.(Formal) 29 19.172 12.464 2.975 56.0 0.004 disagreements, adjective-noun disagree-
2.(lnformal) 29 11.034 7.844 ments, problems with “tebe”verbs, and word
order problems). In other words, errors in
The t-test on problems related to fluency in grammar and structure did not significantly
speech also yielded a significant mean differ- disrupt or break down the communication b e
ence @< .Ol). The mean score of the formal tween the speaker and listeners. What seemed
subjects on the fluency-related problems, to disrupt the communication was the lack of
which consisted of unwanted pauses, repeti- fluency in ESL speech because there was a
tions, and fragmentations, was significantly significant negative correlation between the
higher than that of the informal group. On the scores on the fluency-related problems and
other hand, the formal group had significantly the holistic mean score (r = -.63, p< .Ol).
lower mean scores than the informal group on The data analysis also indicated that there
grammatical and structural errors (p< .01). was a significant correlation between vocabu-
This suggests that the formal group suffered lary development and the holistic mean score
more from fluency-related problems than the (r = .42,p < .Ol). In order to measure the ex-
informal group, and that the latter had more tent of vocabulary development in English, a

237
FOREIGN L&VGUAGE ANN-UMMER 1998

word count of different content words in a classroom instruction seemed to promote ac-
100-word speech sample was undertaken. curacy and the informal natural exposure
Those subjects who had higher scores in the based on personal contact and meaningful
word count were also found to have received interaction with English-speaking people
higher holistic mean scores from the judges. seemed to promote fluency. An analysis of the
Earlier, it was found that there was n o signifi- opinion of the five independent judges indi-
cant correlation between the score on gram- cated that, for basic communication pur-
mar and structure problems and the holistic poses, fluency in ESL speech was considered
mean score. to be more critical than accuracy in grammar.

Discussion ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
In the final analysis, the study seems to I would like to thank Theodore S. Rodgers, Patri-
throw some additional light of support o n cia N. Sullivan, and two anonymous reviewers for
Krashen's input hypothesis, which states that their careful reading of the manuscript and insight-
second language acquisition is a function of ful comments on it; however, all the weaknesses
comprehensible input designed to convey and omissions are mine alone.
messages in low-anxiety situations (Krashen
1985). The formal subjects' preoccupation REFERENCES
with the form of the language at the expense Bialystok, Ellen. 1978. "A Theoretical Model of Lan-
of the meaning conveyed not only hindered guage Learning." Language Learning 28: 6983.
the smooth flow of their ESL speech but also Billmyer, Kristine. 1990. "I Really Like Your Lifestyle:
disrupted the line of their communication. ESL Learners Learning How to Compliment."
When the focus was on the form of the lan- Penn Working-Papersin Educational Linguistics 6:
guage, their anxiety level heightened because 3148.
of their concern for grammatical accuracy. As Briere, Eugene J. 1978. "Variables Affecting Na-
a result, ESL speech suffered from unwanted tive Mexican Children's Learning Spanish as a
pauses, hesitations, repetitions, false starts, Second Language." Language Learning 28: 159-
and fragmentations that inevitably annoyed 174.
the judges and might have thus contributed to Chihara, Tetsuro, and John W. Oller, Jr. 1978. "Atti-
their lower scores. tudes and Proficiency in EFL: A Sociolinguistic
On the other hand, the informal subjects' Study of Japanese Speakers." Language Learn-
concern for the meaning of the language con- ing 28: 55-68.
veyed rather than the form it carried not only Dulay, Heidi C., and Marina K. Burt. 1976. "Cre-
relieved them of the anxiety and tension that ative Construction in Second Language Learn-
caused the fluency-related problems for the for- ing and Teaching." Language Learning, Special
mal subjects but also facilitated their flow of Issue Number 4: 65-79.
speech and thereby improved their communi- Ellis, Rod. 1991. Instructed Second Language Ac-
cation. This probably was the reason for their quisition. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
scoring higher grades from the judges. Sec- Hale, Thomas, and Eva Budar. 1970. "Are TESOL
ondly, a good stock of the ESL vocabulary was Classes the Only Answer?" Modern Language
more useful than having a good command Journal 54: 487-92 .
over grammar and structure with poor vocabu- Kadia, Kayiba. 1989. "The Effect of Formal Instruc-
lary for basic communication purposes. tion o n Monitored and o n Spontaneous Natu-
ralistic Interlanguage Performance: A Case
Conclusion Study." 773OL Quarterly 22: 50915.
This study suggests that formal and informal King, Kendall A,, a n d Rita-Elaine Silver. 1993.
language learning environments seem to con- "'Sticking Points:' Effects of Instruction on NNS
tribute to second language acquisition in their Refusal Strategies." Penn Working-Papersin Ed-
own unique ways. The formal grammar-based ucational Linguistics 9: 47-82.

238
FOREIGN LANGUAGE A"ALS--SUMMER 1998

Krashen, Stephen D. 1976. "Formal and Informal Long, Michael H. 1983. "Does Second Language In-
Linguistic Environments in Language Acquisi- struction Make a Difference? A Review of Re-
tion and Language Learning." TESOL Quarterly search." TESOL Quarterly 17: 35982.
10: 157-68. Mason, Charles. 1971. "The Relevance of Intensive
-. 1981. Second Language Acquisition and Training in English as a Foreign Language for
Second Language Learning. Oxford: Pergamon University Students." Language Learning 21: 197-
Press. 204.
-. 1982. Principles and Practice in Second Ministry of Education and Culture, Curriculum and
Language Acquisition. Oxford: Pergamon Press. Textbook Development Center. 1981. Reuised
-. 1985. The Input Hypothesis:Issues and Im English Curriculum forLower Secondary and Sec-
plications. New York: Longman, Inc. ondary Leuel. Sano Thimi, Nepal: Janak Educa-
Krashen, Stephen D., Herbert W. Seliger, and Dayle tional Materials Center, Ltd.
Hartnett. 1974. "Two Studies in Second Lan- Pica, Teresa. 1983. "Adult Acquisition of English as
guage Learning." Kritikon Litterarum 3: 22B28. a Second Language Under Different Conditions
Krashen, Stephen D., and Herbert W. Seliger. 1975. of Exposure." Language Learning 33: 465-95.
"The Essential Contributions of Formal Instruc- St. Martin, Gail M. 1980. "English Language Acqui-
tion in Adult Second Language Learning." sition: The Effects of Living with an American
TESOL Quarterly 9: 17383. Family." 7ESOL Quarterly 14: 38890.
.- 1976. "The Role of Formal and Informal Schumann, John. 1976. "Second Language Acqui-
Linguistic Environments in Adult Second Lan- sition: The Pidginization Hypothesis." Language
guage Learning." International Journal o f Psy- Learning 26: 391408.
cholinguistics 3: 15-2 1. Tang, Gladys. 1991. "Do Learning Environments
Krashen, Stephen D., Carl M. Jones, Stanley J. Make a Difference? A Study on the Acquisition
Zelinski 111, and Celia Usprich. 1978. "How Im- of the English Interrogatives by Three Types of
portant Is Instruction?" English Language Teach- Cantonese Classroom Learners." CUHK-Papers-
ing Journal 32: 257-61. in-Linguistics 3: 4982.
Littlewood, William. 1984. Foreign andSecond Lan- Upshur, John A. 1968. "Four Experiments of the R e
guage Learning: Language Acquisition Research lation Between Foreign Language Teaching and
and its Implications for the Classroom. Cam- Learning." Language Learning 18: 111-24.
bridge: Cambridge University Press.

APPENDIX A
lnstructions for Holistic Scoring

Holistic scoring is a method designed to assess the "overall" effectiveness of a written or oral
performance. In the present study, we are concerned with judging the oral performance of a
group of people who have learned English as a second language in Nepal. The oral perfor-
mance is judged on the basis of speech samples that were collected through a picture descrip
tion task. The pictures were related to The Rarnayana and The Mahabharata, the two most
popular religious scriptures in the country.
As a judge, you are kindly requested to listen to each speech sample attentively and score
each of those samples holistically. The main assumption of the holistic scoring is that each of
the factors involved in the oral skill is related to all the other factors and n o one factor can be
separated from the others.
As you listen to the tape, you will notice that the investigator is frequently expressing some
kind of favorable remark such as "that's good," "that's interesting," "that's very true," or a sim-
ple hum of positive expression. They were simply designed to keep the speaker talking and in
no way implied that the message was successfully conveyed. As a judge, it is up to you to d e
termine whether a message was conveyed or not.

239
FOREIGNLANGUAGE A N N ~ U M M E 1998
R

A set of scoring criteria is available for your reference. Read the criteria carefully and refer to
it whenever necessary. Score each sample immediately after you finish listening to it. Unless
there is some technical problem, please avoid the temptation of listening to the samples r e
peatedly to justify the scoring in terms of specific errors. The overall concern should b e on the
communication of the message. A detailed description of the scoring criteria along with the
points they carry is given below:

Criteria Point

A sample that clearly communicates a message and 5


compares favorably with the oral proficiency of an
educated native English speaker in terms of pronunciation,
intonation, fluency, vocabulary, grammar, and structure.

A sample that contains only minor inaccuracies in 4


pronunciation, intonation, fluency, vocabulary, grammar,
and structure and these inaccuracies in no way interfere
with the communication of the message.

A sample in which communicability is affected due to 3


inaccuracies in o n e or more of the following areas:
pronunciation, intonation, fluency, vocabulary, grammar,
and structure. These deficiencies may cause an occasional
recourse to the use of the native language, audible and
inaudible pauses between words and phrases, and some
hesitation in speech.

A sample in which communication breaks down d u e to 2


apparent deficiency in pronunciation, intonation, fluency,
vocabulary, grammar, and structure. It also contains one
or more of the following deficiencies: hesitations,
repetitions, long pauses between words and phrases, and
frequent use of the native language.

A sample in which the message is barely communicated 1


d u e to serious gaps and deficiencies in pronunciation,
intonation, fluency, vocabulary, grammar, and structure.
It also contains more of the deficiencies listed earlier for
the sample carrying a score of 2, which include
hesitations, repetitions, long audible and inaudible
pauses between words and phrases, and frequent and
prolonged use of the native language.

240
1999 Subscription Rates - Foreign Language Annals
Foreign Language Annals is published four times each year, quarterly. The 1999
subscription rate for Domestic institutions is $70.00. The rate for International
subscriptions (including subscribers in Canada and Mexico) is $80.00.
Subscriptions are on a calendar year only (January 1st-December 31st).

Your subscription to Foreign Language Annals entitles you to subscribe to the


A C r n Newsletter at the reduced member rate of $25.00 (Domestic) or $30.00
(International) per year.

ACTFL also offers multipleyear subscriptions to Foreign Language Annals. The


rates are listed below. You may elect to have a oneyear subscription or to sub-
scribe for either two or three years; please indicate your preference.

In order to initiate your subscription to Foreign Language Annals, please return


this form with your payment as soon as possible.

Please indicate amount and form of payment below:

Domestic Subscription: International Subscription:

1 year $70.00 0 1 year $80.00


0 2 years $130.00 0 2 years $150.00
0 3 years $195.00 3 years $225.00

0ACTEL Newsletter - $25.00 0A C T n Newsletter - $30.00


(with Domestic Subscription) (with International Subscription)

0 Payment enclosed.
0 Purchase Order # is coming.
0 Renewed through agency.
(Please Type or Print Clearly)
Subscriber:

Attention:

Address:

City: State Zip

Telephone:

241
h e special focus in this issue involves two often-neglected areas of language
teaching: (1) the use and abuse of true and false cognates; and (2) the teaching of
idioms and other forms of figurative language. The article by Frantzen explores the ,

problems and promises of teaching cognates within the typical foreign language pro-
gram. Cooper stresses the importance of including idioms in the foreign language cur-
riculum, and he presents specific examples of how to teach them.

Frantzen notes that false cognates are not reliably false. This fact can prove demoraliz-
ing to students as when, in the Spanish class, they are taught that pregunta should be
used instead of cuestidn-mly to find later on that the latter noun is indeed used in
contexts in which the English word "question" appears (e.g., "It is a question of Val-
ues"). Frantzen presents a wide variety of examples of how to deal with truly false and
unreliably false cognates in the classroom situation.

Cooper emphasizes the importance of teaching idioms and other forms of figurative
language. He cites research to document that a remarkably high percentage of adult
discourse involves the use of idiomatic expressions. He identifies a hierarchy of types
of idiomatic expressions, ranging from those that are easy to learn in a foreign language
to those that are extremely difficult. He includes a wide variety of suggested techniques
for teaching idiomatic expressions and figurative language.

As in past issues of Foreign Language Annals, readers are urged to consider submitting
articles that are oriented to classroom application for possible publication in this section.

-Frank M.Crittner, Editor

242

You might also like