You are on page 1of 15

Hearing Research 380 (2019) 108e122

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Hearing Research
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/heares

Review Article

Why and how music can be used to rehabilitate and develop speech
and language skills in hearing-impaired children
Ritva Torppa a, Minna Huotilainen a, b, *
a
Cognitive Brain Research Unit, Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Finland
b
CICERO Learning Network, Faculty of Educational Sciences, P.O.Box 9, 00014, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This paper presents evidence for a strong connection between the development of speech and language
Received 24 April 2018 skills and musical activities of children and adolescents with hearing impairment and/or cochlear im-
Received in revised form plants. This conclusion is partially based on findings for typically hearing children and adolescents,
11 June 2019
showing better speech and language skills in children and adolescents with musical training, and
Accepted 14 June 2019
Available online 25 June 2019
importantly, showing increases of speech and language skills in children and adolescents taking part in
musical training. Further, studies of hearing-impaired children show connections between musical skills,
involvement in musical hobbies, and speech and language skills. Even though the field is still lacking
Keywords:
Cochlear implant
large-scale randomised controlled trials on the effects of musical interventions on the speech and lan-
Hearing aid guage skills of children and adolescents with hearing impairments and cochlear implants, the current
Language evidence seems enough to urge speech therapists, music therapists, music teachers, parents, and chil-
Speech dren and adolescents with hearing impairments and/or cochlear implants to start using music for
Music enhancing speech and language skills. For this reason, we give our recommendations on how to use
Child music for language skill enhancement in this group.
Brain © 2019 The Authors. Published by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 108
1.1. The tasks of the auditory system . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 109
1.2. Music can enhance auditory processing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 109
2. Musical activities and children with hearing impairments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 111
2.1. Correlational and cross-sectional studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 111
2.2. Intervention studies without a control group . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 113
2.3. Musical interventions with a control group . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 113
3. Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 118
4. How to use music to enhance speech and language skills of hearing-impaired children . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 118
Contributions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 120
Declaration of interest . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 120
Funding . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 120
Acknowledgements . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 120
Supplementary data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 120
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 120

1. Introduction
* Corresponding author. Cognitive Brain Research Unit, Department of Psychol-
ogy and Logopedics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Finland. Music has been an integral part of the everyday care of hearing-
E-mail address: minna.huotilainen@helsinki.fi (M. Huotilainen).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2019.06.003
0378-5955/© 2019 The Authors. Published by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
R. Torppa, M. Huotilainen / Hearing Research 380 (2019) 108e122 109

impaired children for centuries (in addition to speech therapy and music consist of perceptually discrete elements that are organized
other rehabilitation of speech and language skills). This review into hierarchically structured, coherent sequences. This organisa-
examines the scientific foundations of this: we review the reasons tion of language and music is not random, but proceeds according
why music could and should be used for improving the speech and to syntactic principles (Patel, 2003).
language skills of children with hearing impairments. In addition, The similarities of the syntactic processing of language and
we propose instructions and guidance for the use of music with music, as well as the similarities at the acoustic level, have given
children of different ages, varying backgrounds, mild to severe rise to models and proposals concerning the effects of music on
hearing impairments, with cochlear implants and/or hearing aids, speech perception and language capabilities (Patel, 2003, 2011,
for speech therapists, music therapists, music educators, parents, 2014). In addition to the similarities and the theoretical work, there
and other members of the child's community. is neural evidence that musical activities can affect language skills.
The first evidence for the effects of musical training on brain
1.1. The tasks of the auditory system structure was provided by the seminal studies of Pantev, Elbert, and
their coworkers (Elbert et al., 1995; Pantev et al., 1998), showing
The human auditory system performs an abundance of tasks. enlarged motor and somatosensory cortical areas in professional
During human evolution, as hunter-gatherers, a key task of our musicians. These effects were originally interpreted as being
auditory system was to serve as a warning system of potential caused by brain plasticity due to musical practice. However, studies
danger, perhaps the sound of a predator. The warning system must of adult musicians, ranging from observations of structural changes
be very accurate and efficient in detecting danger, while at the same like enlarged and highly active auditory cortical areas and motor
time the system must allow the full use of our cognitive capabilities. and somatosensory areas, and enhanced white matter tracts, even
A hunter-gatherer can be very heavily engaged in a visual task (for though showing interesting differences between musicians and
example, searching for and collecting berries in the forest), but also non-musicians, fail to assess the causality of such differences.
safe at the same time, since the auditory system pre-consciously Theoretically it is possible that individuals with “musician-like”
monitors the safety of the environment (Posner, 1980). If any brain structures determined by genetic factors end up becoming
sound of a potential predator is heard, the auditory system calls for musicians more often than others. As a result, studies of the effects
a switch of attention towards the sound (Na €a
€t€
anen, 1992). The of music have moved towards long-term follow-up studies and
possibility of placing trust in the auditory system has allowed interventional paradigms.
humans to develop cognitive skills up to the present high level. This Importantly, Hyde et al. (2009) found improvements in auditory
is a balancing act: if the warning system is too sensitive, cognitive skills and neural structures in 5-6-year-old children after musical
activities are disturbed too easily and often by irrelevant sounds, training of 15 months. The structural neural changes were mainly in
and it is difficult to concentrate (Posner, 1980). On the other hand, if the temporal, frontal, and parieto-occipital cortical areas, i.e., the
the warning system is not sensitive enough, there is a risk of danger very same areas in which adult professional musicians show the
when an important sound is unnoticed. largest and most consistent cortical enlargements. In addition,
Another important task of the auditory system is communica- Dittinger et al. (2017) found that in a task of learning novel words,
tion. Like most mammals, humans use sounds and gestures in children with musical training were faster and had enhanced brain
communicating with others. Researchers propose that some responses to learnt words. These findings highlight the connection
communicative gestures and sounds evolved into human lan- between musical training and auditory attentional, language and
guages, while others evolved into music and dance (Mithen et al., learning functions in children.
2006). Both language and music place high requirements on the Similarly, Putkinen et al. (2014a,b) studied a group of children
auditory system. It has been proposed that by studying the acoustic starting musical hobbies at the age of 7 years and a comparable
characteristics and features of all human languages and musical control group starting other hobbies, and found that brain re-
genres we can learn about the capabilities of the auditory system - sponses related to sound perception, discrimination and attention
or, put the other way around - the capacity of the human auditory allocation were initially very similar for the two groups, but two or
system dictates the boundaries of expressive features in language more years later, the brain responses had increased in those chil-
and music (Patel, 2010). Language and music, even though dren who started musical hobbies.
constantly changing, would not evolve in directions that require the Moreno et al. (2011) used an intensive, 20-day intervention with
use of acoustic features that are too difficult to extract, detect, randomised groups of 4-6-year-old children who played a com-
compare or remember for the average human auditory system. puter game teaching either music or visual arts for 2 h per day. Only
Today, our auditory system still has these two key tasks: the musical training improved the verbal intelligence (vocabulary
monitoring the environment for safety or sounds of interest, and scores from the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelli-
communicating via language and music. Skills in these tasks are gence; Wechsler, 2010) of the children and there was a parallel
relevant for children with hearing impairment who are users of increase in neural indices of executive functions and auditory
different types of implants and hearing aids. working memory. For the visual training, there was a trend for an
improvement only in spatial skills, which could not be distin-
1.2. Music can enhance auditory processing guished from a practice effect. In two other studies (Janus et al.,
2016; Moreno et al., 2015) a similar intervention of 20 days, 2 h/
Music has very specific acoustic characteristics. Melodies day showed improvements in musical skills and music-related
include fast progressions of notes typically chosen from consistent brain responses as well as executive functions in children whose
scales that typically include uneven steps. Chords are collections of intervention taught them musical skills. These cognitive aspects are
simultaneously played notes that form harmonies. Musical timbres important for speech perception, language learning and learning in
are characterised by their spectral peaks and their changes, fast general. Thus, improvement in cognitive skills with musical activ-
friction and noise sounds, sound wave envelopes and several other ities is probably one reason for their positive effects for speech
characteristics, which are highly similar to those that characterise perception and language skills (for a review, Moreno and Bidelman,
the important basic elements of speech. 2014).
Both speech and music are human universals, found in all hu- Kraus and Strait (2015) gave children the possibility of attending
man cultures (Besson et al., 2011; Patel, 2014). Both language and musical training for two years, and observed several changes in
110 R. Torppa, M. Huotilainen / Hearing Research 380 (2019) 108e122

domains of language. First, after two years, the fidelity of the brain while not of executive functions (inhibition from NEPSY II test
stem response in replicating the acoustic content of a consonant- battery) for children whose kindergartens offered musical play
vowel syllable had significantly increased. Second, high brain school (where activities include singing and musical instrument
stem response fidelity and temporal accuracy correlated with bet- playing) as a weekly routine. They did not find similar improve-
ter reading skills. In a waiting-list design, the children served as ments for children in two types of control groups (in kindergartens
their own controls, enhancing the value of the results. offering dancing activities or no extra training). Thus, both of the
Tierney et al. (2015) investigated whether musical training studies above and the study of Moreno et al. (2011) indicate that
starting in adolescence (without prior musical training in child- musical activities offered to randomly chosen children improve
hood) and offered at school can affect the development of the verbal intelligence (vocabulary skills) while evidence for the
auditory system and language processing. They found that three improvement of non-verbal intelligence and executive functions is
years of in-school musical training accelerated the development of unclear (for a review, see Linnavalli et al., 2018).
the N1eP1-response which was used as an index of the maturation The majority of studies investigating the perception of speech in
of the auditory cortex in comparison to a group receiving leader- noise by musicians and non-musicians show improvements for
ship training. Futher, they observed improved phonological musicians (for a review, see Coffey et al., 2017). There is also evi-
awareness, but not phonological memory or rapid naming in the dence for improvement of perception of speech in noise with
group receiving musical training compared to leadership training. musical training for normally hearing children. Slater et al. (2015)
Musical training across life has been shown to be associated with conducted a study using children participating in community-
faster neural timing in response to speech in late adulthood based musical activities (age approximately 7 years at the begin-
(White-Schwoch et al., 2013). ning of the study). After a first assessment using the Hearing in
Most of the research focusing on music-induced enhancements Noise Test (HINT), participants were randomly assigned to one of
of brain responses to sounds or performance in neuropsychological two groups: one group began musical training right away and
tests has used adult-led or computerized musical training. Such completed 2 years of training (including singing and musical in-
training is straightforward to quantify in terms of minutes or hours strument playing), while the second group waited a year and then
of training per day or per week. Informal musical activities, in received 1 year of musical training. It was found that speech-in-
contrast, are difficult but not impossible to quantify. Putkinen et al. noise perception improved only for the group receiving 2 years of
(2013) formulated a large questionnaire tackling all possible types training.
of musical activities that a child can spontaneously engage herself There is also much evidence on the effects of musical training,
or himself in, with the parents, siblings and friends, or alone. A including singing and musical instrument playing, on phonological
comparison of the amounts of such informal musical activity to awareness (for a meta-analysis, see Gordon et al., 2015), including
brain responses recorded in a distractive-sound paradigm which controlled intervention studies (Flaugnacco et al., 2015; Patscheke
measures the brain responses to surprising, attention arousing et al., 2018). Interestingly, Patscheke et al. (2018) compared
sounds showed smaller distraction-related brain responses in rhythmic training with musical instruments (no singing) to pitch-
children with the largest amount of informal musical activities. This based training with singing. They found that phonological aware-
was interpreted as a more developed attention-allocation system to ness improved only with singing training.
relevant and irrelevant sounds. Auditory attentive skills are highly All in all, the studies reviewed above and several others show
relevant for language learning, since all sensory modalities (audi- that musical training, be it instrument training, singing or musical
tory, visual, tactile, even taste and smell) constantly send a myriad play school, and informal musical activities all enhance the devel-
of stimulation, only a fraction of which reaches the working opment of the auditory system in childhood and adolescence, and
memory and can thus be effectively learnt (Cowan, 1988; Gomes that such enhanced development is in many cases accompanied by
et al., 2000). improved language-perception skills. Advancing language devel-
It should be noted, however, that in many of the studies opment and communication in general is, however, not the only
described above, the musical hobbies were chosen by the children use of music. Humans use music for several other reasons, such as
and their families. Children who choose musical hobbies often relaxation, well-being, inspiration, social cohesion, and building
come from families with a higher-than-average socio-economic and communicating cultural norms and systems (MacDonald et al.,
status (Hartas, 2011). Even though this is typically compensated for 2002; Saarikallio, 2011; Saarikallio and Erkkila €, 2007). All of these
in the control group and in the statistical analysis, interventional should be taken into account when considering the rights and
studies where children are randomly allocated to musical learning needs of children to be involved in the world of music - whether
and other types of hobbies are the strongest studies when it comes they have typical hearing or are users of different types of hearing
to the causality of the effects. aids or implants.
Jaschke et al. (2018) randomised 6-year-old children (n ¼ 147) to Children with hearing impairment have deficits in auditory
two musical intervention groups, one active visual arts group, and a attention (Fagan and Pisoni, 2009; Houston et al., 2003), in auditory
no-arts control group. For the musical intervention (1e2 h/week), working memory and in perception of spectral details (for an
early lessons introduced melody, meter and rhythm as well as overview, Torppa et al., 2014a), similarly to adults with hearing
different musical instruments. The lessons started with a impairments (Moore, 2003). These evidently partially underlie
welcoming song, followed by music theoretical and historical in- their difficulties in perception of phonemes (Eisenberg, 2007),
formation in the context of the song and ended with collective speech in noise (Asp et al., 2012; Caldwell and Nittrouer, 2013;
music making, singing, playing and improvising. The children were Moore, 2003), simultaneous sounds, sound localization, and
followed for 2.5 years. Development of verbal intelligence and ex- perception of pitch and prosody (word and sentence stress, Lyxell
ecutive functions as well as academic performance were assessed et al., 2009; O’Halpin, 2010; emotional prosody, Kalathottukaren
for each group. Test scores for executive functions (inhibition and et al., 2017; question vs. statement intonation, Peng et al., 2008).
planning) and verbal intelligence increased significantly more for They often lag behind typically hearing children in language skills,
the two music groups than for the visual art and no-arts controls. such as expressive and receptive language skills (Fitzpatrick et al.,
Recently, Linnavalli et al. (2018) found similar improvement of 2011), vocabulary (Lund, 2016; Percy-Smith et al., 2013; Va €limaa
verbal intelligence (Vocabulary, Block design and Matrix reasoning et al., 2018), expressive morphological and syntactic skills (Boons
subtests from WISC IV), and additionally, phoneme processing, et al., 2013; Soleymani et al., 2016), production of narratives
R. Torppa, M. Huotilainen / Hearing Research 380 (2019) 108e122 111

(Boons et al., 2013), phonological awareness (Ambrose et al., 2012; children with CIs (Torppa et al., 2014a). Perception of stress, in turn,
Soleymani et al., 2016), and verbal intelligence (Geers and Nicholas, is an important feature in assisting speech segmentation (on the
2013; Hashemi and Monshizadeh, 2012; Wu et al., 2008). role of prosodic stress, see Jusczyk et al., 1999), speech perception
From the evidence reviewed above, showing how music-related (on the role of stress and word stress patterns, i. e., speech rhythm,
exposure and training can enhance the activity of the auditory see Bolger et al., 2014; Mattys, 1997; Scho€n and Tillmann, 2015) and
system and the brain, as well as auditory attentive, language, and language learning (on the role of word and sentence stress, see
learning performance, it is tempting to suggest that music should Cutler and Foss, 1977; Ma €nnel and Friederici, 2013; Thiessen and
be used as a rehabilitation method for children with varying types Saffran, 2007). Because hearing impairments often lead to deficits
of hearing impairments, both to help them enhance their speech in the perception of pitch (Oxenham, 2008), improving the
and language skills, and to advance their skills in hearing in general. perception of pitch and prosody with musical activities is especially
It has indeed become increasingly common to use music as part of important for hearing-impaired children. We also include studies
speech therapy and as part of the general rehabilitation of these assessing the perception of musical rhythm since, for normal-
children. For example, Auditory Verbal Therapy (AVT, Estabrooks, hearing adults, this is connected to the perception of word stress
1994), the Hanen program (Manolson, 1992), the Lindfors Foun- (Hausen et al., 2013) and speech in noise (also for children with CIs,
dation MUKULA-project (http://lindforsinsaatio.net/) in Finland, see Torppa et al., 2018). Due to this connection, an improvement in
and several materials (like STEPS Together, http://www. the perception of musical rhythm may be of importance also for
earfoundation.org.uk/shop/items/76, Baby Beats, Rocca, 2015) language perception. Furthermore, we assess the possible con-
emphasize the role of music and especially singing in auditory and nections between speech-in-noise and music perception, since
language rehabilitation. The Mary Hare School in the UK has for these connections are thought to be a prerequisite for musical
several years used music as an integral part of the education of deaf training to improve speech perception and language skills (Besson
children (Rocca, 2012). It is, however, crucial to investigate the et al., 2011; Patel, 2014).
outcome of such music-related methods, preferably using inter-
vention studies with control groups and with children randomly 2.1. Correlational and cross-sectional studies
allocated to the intervention and control groups, and preferably
with long-term follow-up of the development of speech and lan- Correlational studies related to speech perception or language
guage skills. Such studies are very demanding. In the following, we skills are presented in Table 1. Cross-sectional studies are included
describe studies that report any type of effect of using music with in Table 1 and in the current section since, even though they give
hearing-impaired children. information about links to musical activities, similarly to other
correlational studies, they cannot address causality.
2. Musical activities and children with hearing impairments Correlational and cross-sectional evidence for hearing-impaired
children shows better perception of pitch (Chen et al., 2010) and
Only a few studies have addressed the correlation of musical music (cued with changes in f0 or duration; Polonenko et al., 2017)
activities with speech perception or language skills for children with longer duration of musical training (listening, singing, or
with hearing impairments. Moreover, Gfeller (2016) concluded that musical instrument playing; Table 1).
there is no evidence from randomised, fully controlled intervention Two studies have assessed connections between the perception
studies on the impact of musical training interventions on speech of speech and of music, with different measurements and results.
perception or language skills of children with CIs. This is in strong Koşaner et al. (2012) failed to find connections between speech and
contrast to the musical intervention studies showing the impact of music perception assessed with a questionnaire (music questions:
musical training on speech perception and language skills of chil- singing skills, recognizing songs, tunes, and timbre) for hearing-
dren with typical hearing, reviewed above. impaired children (see Table 1). Torppa et al. (2018) examined
One major reason for the lack of intervention studies is the small whether the cortical processing of musical instrument sounds for
population of children with CIs and with other hearing impair- children with CIs is correlated with the perception of speech in
ments compared to typically hearing children. Another reason is noise. For the entire group of children with CIs, the perception of
that families with hearing-impaired children may live long dis- speech in noise was better for children with larger mismatch
tances from each other, and they typically have to visit clinics, also negativity (MMN) brain responses (improved neural discrimina-
far from home, for tuning of the hearing aid or CI. The families are tion) to a change of sound from piano to cymbal (musical instru-
also busy with rehabilitation, like speech and language therapy. ment timbre). For the children with CIs who sang regularly at home,
Therefore, they may not have the resources needed to take part in “CI singers” (for details, see Table 1; see also section 2.3), speech-in-
an intervention study. All in all, research with large-scale rando- noise perception was better for children with earlier P3a responses,
mised controlled trials (RCT) in CI users and hearing-impaired reflecting an attention shift towards sound changes salient for the
children and adolescents is scarce. listener, for all three changes in musical instrument (from piano to
Due to the lack of RCTs, it is necessary to assess the correlational cembalo, violin and cymbal).
and longitudinal evidence, which hopefully provides a basis for Torppa et al. (2010; Table 1) found that, for children with CIs, the
future large-scale intervention studies. Moreover, musical activities perception of sentence and word stress was better with more
at home are enjoyable and typical for children with both normal parental singing and with more musical instrument playing at
and impaired hearing (Driscoll et al., 2015). The important role of home, while the perception of sentence stress was also better with
parents and home activities in auditory rehabilitation and in speech more participation in music play school-like activities. The corre-
and language therapy for children with hearing impairments is well lation of stress perception with listening to music from television or
known (Hull, 2013; Estabrooks, 1994). Thus, it is also important to CDs was not significant.
inspect how informal musical activities at home might improve Rochette et al. (2014) conducted a cross-sectional study of
speech perception and language skills in children and adolescents prelingually deaf children who used CIs and/or hearing aids. They
with hearing impairment or CIs. compared children attending musical activities for several years
Results for the perception of pitch (the perceptual correlate of (see Table 1) to those who did not participate in musical activities.
fundamental frequency, f0) are included in this review because f0 is They found that compared to children with no musical training,
an important cue for the perception of prosodic stress, also for children with musical training performed better in tasks measuring
112 R. Torppa, M. Huotilainen / Hearing Research 380 (2019) 108e122

Table 1
Correlational and cross-sectional studies in children with hearing impairments

Study N, age, device Design of the study Measurements Results

Studies investigating relationships of musical training with pitch and music perception only
Chen et al., (2010) N ¼ 27 (13 in musical 13 CI children attended YAMAHA Music Test stimuli consisted of 2 For all children, pitch (f0) perception
training), age 5e14 years, School for 2e36 months with NH sequential piano tones, ranging was better with longer duration of
CI. children; listening, singing, score from C (256 Hz) to B (495 Hz). musical training. (Pitch (f0) perception
reading, and instrument playing. Children identified the pitch (f0) was better in children older than 6
Correlations of pitch (f0) perception relationship between the 2 tones years vs.  6 year and with larger pitch
with duration of musical training (and (same, higher, or lower). In intervals; no correlations between pitch
with current age, age at first CI, gender, statistical analyses, children divided perception and the age at first CI,
and type of CI) were studied. into older than 6 years vs.  6 years. gender, or type of CI).
Polonenko et al. N ¼ 50. 26 bilateral CI, 8 Performance of 26 CI, CI þ HA, NH MBEMA (iPad application). For first Children who had participated in
(2017) CI þ HA, children was compared. Interviews 4 subtests (ST): Discrimination musical training were faster and more
16 NH children; age 6e18 showed that 12 bilateral CI and 2 between different or similar accurate in music perception,
years. CI þ HA, and 11 NH children had melodies: The note changed to an regardless of their hearing status.
participated in musical training 1e10 out-of-key note (Scale ST), changed Reaction time on specific subtests
years: music theory classes, practicing the pitch direction (Contour ST), the decreased with years of musical
at least one non-percussive instrument, interval while maintaining the training, age, and, for CI users, better
and/or singing training. To determine same scale and contour (Interval ST) residual hearing.
the results for effect of musical training, or the duration between notes
in RM ANOVA (controlling for age), the (Rhythm ST). In the last ST
between-subject factors were hearing (incidental Memory), the children
group (normal and bilateral device indicated if they had heard the
hearing) and music group (no musical excerpt before or not. 2 practice and
training, musical training). 20 test items for each ST.
Studies investigating relationships with perception of prosody
Torppa et al., (2010) N ¼ 17, age 4e12 years, CI. Correlations of amount of parental Perception of compound vs. phrasal In the CI group, better sentence stress
(The same subjects as in the singing and other musical experience at stress (word stress) and narrow perception correlated strongly with
larger sample in Torppa home with children's perception of focus (sentence stress). more parental singing and less strongly
et al., (2014a,b), 2018, in word stress and sentence stress were Questionnaire assessing musical with playing more a musical instrument
print. studied. Duration of experience on and hearing-related background. at home. Sentence stress perception
parental singing: up to 9 years, starting was better with more participation in
from implantation. Performance was musical play school -like activities.
compared for children participating in Word stress perception was better with
musical play school -like activities and more parental singing and playing
those who did not participate in these. instruments. Correlations with listening
to music from television or CDs not
significant.
Studies investigating relationships with perception of speech in quiet and noise
Koşaner et al., 2012 Group A: N ¼ 12, age 19e37 Correlations of speech perception with Auditory speech perception from Correlations of scores on the subscales
(see also Table 2) months; Group B: N ¼ 7, music perception (and age at first CI the EARS test or from LEAQ. ‘Recognizing songs, tunes, timbre’ with
age 34e60 months; Group fitting, length of CI use, and Evaluation form, filled in by LEAQ or LiP scores (Group A)/scores on
C: N ¼ 6, age 49e91 chronological age) were investigated in teachers: Singing skills, the Bisyllabic Closed-Set speech
months. CI. children participating in musical Recognizing songs, tunes, and perception (Group B) not significant. (A
intervention, separately for subgroups timbre; Responding to music and significant correlation between Total
(A and B). rhythm. evaluation score and length of CI use for
both groups; Correlations with age at
first CI not significant).
Rochette et al., N ¼ 28, age 6e10 years, CI Performance was compared for 14 Sound identification; Auditory Musically trained children were better
(2014) and/or HA. children with HI without musical stream segregation task (changing in auditory stream segregation,
activities and 14 HI children who had or not-changing simultaneous auditory working memory and
participated 1,5 to 4 years in group animal sound streams); Auditory phonemic discrimination tasks.
music lessons (5e6 children in each), working memory task (reproducing Multiple-regressions suggested that
using voice and musical instruments, sounds using the keyboard); Same- good performance was at least partly
sensomotor activities with music, different identification of mono- or driven by music lessons.
musical exercises involving memory, bisyllabic nonsense words with
analyzing the emotional value of different phonemes.
musical pieces, playing simple self-
written pieces of music together.
Assignment to groups based on type of
hearing devices. Groups did not differ
significantly in age, age at HI correction,
duration of device use and type of
correction (HA/CI).
Torppa et al., (2018) N ¼ 21, age 4e13 years, CI. Relationships of perception of speech in SRT for 75% correct words in The perception of speech in noise in
(see also Table 3) noise with ERP responses (MMN and sentences in steady male-weighted children with CIs was better with larger
P3a) and discrimination of pitch and noise. MMN (preattentive amplitude of MMN to a change of sound
intensity in synthesized bisyllables discrimination of) and P3a from piano to cymbal, and in the CI
were analyzed with LMM (age (attentions shift towards) changes singing group only, with earlier P3a to
controlled for) where group factor in f0, intensity and timbre; changes in timbre.
“singing” was included (12 CI children discrimination of f0 and intensity in
who sang regularly and vs. 9 CI non- synthesized bisyllables.
singers) (see Table 3). Duration of
singing experience (parental/child): up
to 10 years, starting from implantation.
Studies investigating relationships with language skills
R. Torppa, M. Huotilainen / Hearing Research 380 (2019) 108e122 113

Table 1 (continued )

Study N, age, device Design of the study Measurements Results

Torppa et al. in N ¼ 21, age 5e13 years, CI; In 21 children with CIs, and 31 children Word finding test, WISC vocabulary In the CI children, all language skills
print NH reference group. with NH, the relationships of word and (VIQ), production of rhymes were better with more musical
sentence stress perception, formal (phonological awareness). activities, and word finding and VIQ
musical activities with emphasis on improved with more parental singing
singing (CIm/NHm group) vs. others and better stress perception. Similar
(CIn/NHn group) and parental activities associations of word finding and VIQ
(singing, reading, other activities) with with musical activities were found for
language skills were investigated with children with NH. For all children, all
linear regression analysis (for the language skills improved with better
musical training in the CI group, see stress perception.
Table 3, Torppa et al., 2014a).

N ¼ number of participants. CI ¼ cochlear implant, HA ¼ hearing aid, NH ¼ normal hearing. HI ¼ hearing impairment. F0 ¼ fundamental frequency; pitch is perceptual
correlate of f0. EARS ¼ the Evaluation of Auditory Responses to Speech test battery. LEAQ ¼ LittlEARS Auditory Questionnaire. LiP ¼ Listening Progress Profile; Nikolopoulos
et al., 2000. MBEMA ¼ Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Musical Abilities (Peretz et al., 2013). RM ANOVA ¼ repeated measures analysis of variance. LMM ¼ linear mixed
modeling. VIQ ¼ verbal intelligence quotient, measured with vocabulary subtest of the WISC-IV (Wechsler, 2010). ERP ¼ event-related potential. MMN ¼ mismatch negativity
brain response. P3a ¼ positive response peaking after MMN, reflects attention shift towards sound change.

auditory working memory, in auditory scene analysis, and in pho- (2013) studied development of the perception of musical pitch,
netic discrimination. Multiple-regression analyses indicated that timbre and rhythm, and the role of device/hearing in the devel-
the improved performance was partly driven by the musical opment of these, for children with CI and/or HAs and normal
training. hearing (age 9e13 years) participating in a weekly Music Club at
For normally hearing children several RCT studies have shown school. No development in test performance was found.
improvements in language skills with musical activities (see sec- Koşaner et al. (2012) designed, implemented, and evaluated an
tion 1.2). RCT studies assessing this issue in children with hearing 18-month family-centered rehabilitation program (Musical EARS®).
impairments are still lacking. However, Torppa et al. (in print) The program evaluation form included sections on singing, recog-
found that musically active children with CIs (at age 5e13 years), nizing songs, tunes, and timbre, and responding to music and
after participation in singing and playing at home and outside of rhythm. Development in these skills and speech perception (from
the home for several years, performed better than other children questionnaires) was assessed. Mean total scores in an evaluation
with CIs in word finding, verbal intelligence (WISC vocabulary; the form for musical skills increased for preschool-aged children, while
same task as in Linnavalli et al., 2018; Moreno et al., 2011, see the scores for speech perception did not improve significantly. Lo
section 1) and phonological awareness measured with production et al. (2015) conducted two melodic contour training programs
of rhymes (see Gordon et al., 2015, and section 1.2), while musically and studied their effects on speech perception for 16 adults with CIs
non-active children performed more poorly (Torppa et al., in print; and 12 adults with normal hearing. One program manipulated
Table 1). Similar results on improved language skills for musically musical interval sizes, and the other program manipulated note
active children (for word finding and verbal intelligence) were durations. Both programs lasted for 6 weeks. There was improve-
found for an enlarged normal-hearing group at a similar age. For CI ment in speech perception only for CI users in both melodic
and normal-hearing groups, all language skills improved with training programs. Specifically, consonant perception in silence and
better perception of prosodic stress, giving evidence that percep- question/statement prosody was improved. Normal-hearing lis-
tion of prosody assists language learning of both child groups teners performed at ceiling for these tasks. No improvement was
(previous evidence for normal-hearing children, see Ma €nnel and found for speech perception in noise for either of the groups.
Friederici, 2013; Cutler and Foss, 1977; Thiessen and Saffran, Several studies reported in Table 2 showed that musical activ-
2007). It was also found that parental singing was correlated ities seem to be enjoyable for children with hearing impairments
significantly with word finding and verbal intelligence, but only for and their families. Abdi et al. (2001) and Rocca (2012) reported
the children with CIs (Torppa et al., in print). enhancement of musical instrument playing and other musical
skills with musical training for children of various ages. Koşaner
et al. (2012) and Rocca (2015) studied musical programs targeted
2.2. Intervention studies without a control group for very young children with CIs and their families. Koşaner et al.
(2012) found that participation in and enjoyment of musical ac-
There are several intervention studies without a control group tivities increased for both children and parents. Rocca determined if
where performance over time of one group of children with hearing the program could be used for babies, and found that parents
impairments participating in musical intervention has been fol- observed increased vocalisation, attention, and anticipation of the
lowed (Table 2). Such studies measure the effect of two things: activities in their babies, and increased confidence in singing,
development of auditory or musical skills related to musical moving, and playing with their baby, including during daily rou-
learning, and the development of the same skills related to general tines. The reports of the teachers of children participating in the
maturation and to exposure to speech, practicing of speech and study of Innes-Brown et al. (2013) suggested that participation in
language skills, and so on. In such a study, it is impossible to the Music Club improved children's confidence with voice, psy-
disentangle these two effects. chosocial development, social engagement with peers and
Two of these studies have assessed how the development of involvement in general music classes (Table 2).
auditory perception is related to music perception (Table 2). Fu
et al. (2015) assessed whether melodic contour training using a
home computer could improve the perception of melodies for 5e10 2.3. Musical interventions with a control group
year old children with CIs. After 4 weeks of intensive training,
perception of melodic contours markedly improved. Measurements This section introduces results from musical intervention
8 weeks later showed no decline in performance. Innes-Brown et al. studies with several study and intervention designs, all including a
Table 2

114
Intervention studies without control group.

Study Device/age and N Type and size of music group/duration Specific goals/statistical analyses Outcome measures Results
of intervention

General goal: To improve music (pitch/f0) perception


Fu et al. (2015) CI 6 CI children in computer-based The improvement of musical pitch/ The participants identified changes in After 4 weeks of training, MCI
/5e10 years, melodic contour identification (MCI) melodic contour perception./ANOVA for melodic contours (5tone, 3tone, and performance sharply improved for all
N ¼ 6 in intervention, (8 CI home training. They trained 5-tone assessing development of the musical 5piano sample stimuli). They clicked on test stimuli. Performance did not
children not in musical complex or piano sample melodic intervention group. one of the nine response choices on the decline during 8 weeks after training
training; no comparisons contours with 23 root notes. screen; response boxes were labeled stopped. (For the 6 trained subjects,
between intervention and /A half hour per day, every day for a and included an illustration of the f0 there was a significant effect of contour
non-intervention groups). period of 10 weeks. contour pitch direction. length).
General goals: To study and develop a music program, and the development of musical skills and/or music perception
Abdi et al., CI All CI children participated in a music To assess the feasibility and methods of Teachers rated musical skills and All children improved in playing se-tar.
(2001) /2,5e12,5 years, program based on Orff method and se- utilizing music as a means of responsiveness to music. Musical skills and responsiveness to
N ¼ 23. tar playing. habilitation of children with CIs./Case music were reported to increase; all
/3 to 13 months, 1 lesson per week. studies; no statistical analyses. parents expressed their satisfaction
with the program.
Rocca (2012) CI and/or HA Music program of Mary Hare School. To describe the benefits of the music The UK National program of the Early Many children with CIs can play, sing,

R. Torppa, M. Huotilainen / Hearing Research 380 (2019) 108e122


/NA. Parents of babies and preschool program for social communication Years Foundation stage profile is used at and participate confidently in social
children are encouraged to integrate skills and musicality. Mary Hare. The article gives a general musical contexts. Some past student
musical activities into their daily /No statistical analyses. overview of the development of recipients have trained at universities
routine. Music theory, playing and musical skills. in the United
singing in group settings and individual Kingdom to become music teachers,
music instrument/singing lessons. performers, and composers.
/Weekly music lessons up to the age of
14.
Innes-Brown CI, CI þ HA, 2 HAs All children participated in a weekly To assess the development of Tonal, rhythmic tests from IMMA; Improvement in test performance was
et al., (2013) /9e13 years, N with CI or Music Club during lunch time at school perception of musical pitch, timbre and timbre perception test. not significant. Teachers reported that
CI þ HA ¼ 6, N with 2 45 min per week: vocal play, the rhythm to assess the role of device/ participation in the Music Club
HAs ¼ 5, N with NH ¼ 4 (in integration of aural, visual, and hearing for the development. improved children's confidence with
tests, 15e17 children/per kinesthetic modes of learning: Kodaly /LMM. voice, psychosocial development, social
time point). method. engagement with peers and
/A school year. involvement in general music classes.
General goals: To study and develop a music program; development of musical skills, speech perception or language skills
Koşaner et al., CI All children participated in Musical To increase the quality and quantity of Evaluation form, filled in by teachers: Evaluation form scores increased
2012 /Group A: N ¼ 12, age 19 EARS® program: singing, recognizing musical activities in habilitation; Singing skills, recognizing songs, tunes, significantly: increase of speech scores
e37 months; Group B: songs, tunes and timbre, and feasibility of the evaluation tool./ and timbre; responding to music and not significant. Participation in and
N ¼ 7, age 34e60 months; responding to music and rhythm. ANOVA. rhythm. Auditory speech perception enjoyment of musical activities
Group C: N ¼ 6, age 49e91 Activities introduced in group sessions from the EARS test or from LEAQ. increased for both children and parents.
months. were repeated in individual sessions
and parents were encouraged to repeat
activities at home. Recorded music was
mostly used.
/18 months.
Rocca (2015) Pre-CI group: All children participated in Baby To determine if the program could be Questionnaires for parents and The program was appropriate for babies
HAs, post-CI group, CI or BeatTM program for parents and babies. used to monitor outcomes for babies. professionals, completed at the end of and for parents' use at home. Parents
CI þ HA. Aims to elicit in deaf babies musicality Program is designed to improve parent an 8-month trial. observed increased vocalisation,
N ¼ 15. through the use of backing tracks and interaction, listening behaviour, attention, and anticipation of the
/age 5e11 months. the parent's voice. 4 key areas: music communication, social and emotional activities in their babies, increased
and movement, instrumental (timbre) development, pre-post-implantation./ confidence in singing, moving, and
& music/singing, early symbolic sounds, No statistical analyses. playing with their baby, also during
musical symbolism. Imitation of the daily routines.
vocalisations of Ling sounds and words,
their anticipation.
/8 months.

N ¼ number of participants. CI ¼ cochlear implant. HA ¼ hearing aid. LMM ¼ linear mixed modeling. ANOVA ¼ analysis of variance. F0 ¼ fundamental frequency; pitch is perceptual correlate of f0. EARS ¼ the Evaluation of
Auditory Responses to Speech test battery; LEAQ ¼ LittlEARS Auditory Questionnaire. MCI ¼ melodic contour identification. NA ¼ not applicable. IAMMA ¼ Intermediate Measures of Music Audiation (Gordon, 1979).
R. Torppa, M. Huotilainen / Hearing Research 380 (2019) 108e122 115

control group with a minimal amount of musical activities or year-old children with CIs (Torppa et al., 2014b, Table 3). It was
training (Table 3; note that Welch et al., 2015, used a normal- assessed how P3a brain responses, reflecting attention shift to-
hearing reference group which had only slightly less musical ac- wards sound changes, developed over 14e17 months in those
tivities than the hearing-impaired intervention group). This section children with CIs who sang informally and regularly at home (“CI
also describes results from adults with CIs, one study comparing singers”), and those who sang only rarely. The only factor
auditory learning and auditory-motor learning related to lyrics (not explaining CI-children's singing was that their parents had sung
included in Table 3), and one study where hearing-impaired chil- more for them from their early childhood (Torppa, 2015, p. 56;
dren served as controls for themselves (AB-BA design, Hidalgo et al., Table 3). The P3a was elicited by a change in a sound sequence with
2017, Table 3). a regular and predictable rhythm. The CI singers had consistently
Not all intervention studies with a control group showed im- larger and earlier P3a responses than the CI non-singers. The largest
provements with musical training on perception of music or and most statistically significant differences between groups were
prosody-related auditory cues, or speech perception. Petersen et al. found for the P3a responses to changes in musical instruments
(2015) conducted a musical intervention (rhythm, singing and ear (timbre) and f0. The P3a responses became larger and earlier during
training and computer-based exercises, Table 3) using adolescents the follow-up period for the CI singers only. It was also found at the
with CIs. Normal-hearing adolescents served as control subjects. second time point of the measurements that the CI singers sang the
The duration of the intervention was 6 days (20 h), distributed over rhythms of a children's song (“Twinkle twinkle little star”) better,
2 weeks. They measured mismatch negativity (MMN) brain re- indicating better perception of rhythm for them than for the CI non-
sponses for changes in f0, rhythm, intensity and timbre, behavioral singers. These results suggest that informal singing at home could
discrimination of these same features, and speech perception in lead not only to better music perception but also to better auditory
noise. No improvement with training was found (see also Innes- attention (Torppa et al., 2014b).
Brown et al., 2013, Table 2, for null results). Only one study reported improved perception of speech in noise
There are studies indicating improvements with musical with musical activities for children with CIs (for null results, for
training in music perception and musical skills but not in speech hearing-impaired children, Petersen et al., 2015, and Welch et al.,
perception. In the pioneering study of Yucel et al. (2009, Table 3), 2015, Table 3; for adults, Lo et al., 2015, see section 2.2 above;
parents trained their recently cochlear-implanted children at home Fuller et al., 2018; Petersen et al., 2012). Torppa et al. (2018; Table 3)
using a keyboard (f0 and rhythm discrimination exercises) and studied how CI singers (who sang informally and regularly at home)
encouraged their children to listen to and dance with simple, re- and CI non-singers developed in the perception of speech in noise
petitive melodies (Table 3). Compared to children without musical during 14e17 months. It was expected that compared to the CI non-
training, the musical intervention group was better in open-set singers, the CI singers would perceive speech in noise better, since
speech perception (measured with repetition of sentences) three their attention shift towards sound changes was improved, and
months after the training began. However, at the end of the 2-year attention is important for the perception of speech in noise, and
training period there were no differences between groups in also because they sang rhythms better (for more details, Torppa
speech perception. By the end of the second year, the results from a et al., 2018). As expected, the CI singers had better speech
questionnaire indicated that the music group was better than the perception in noise than the CI non-singers, which is in line with
control group in all aspects of musical skills, including singing results from normal-hearing children, showing improvement of
melodies and producing rhythms, thus indicating improvement in perception of speech in noise with long-term (2 years) musical
the perception of pitch and rhythm with musical training. Welch training, including singing and musical instrument playing (Slater
et al. (2015; Table 3) found rather similar results with singing et al., 2015; see section 1). However, in the study of Torppa et al.
training (most often in a music class for 29 children, and occa- (2018), compared to the CI non-singers, the CI singers did not
sionally in small groups) with 5-7-year-old hearing-impaired develop in this skill more during the follow up. It was concluded
children. Normally hearing children participated in class-based that even though the results could not address causality, a
singing training, but they did not have training in small groups connection was found between improved speech perception in
(see Table 3). It was found that the comfortable singing range, noise and long-term singing at an early age of the CI singers, by
general ability to sing (pitch and rhythm patterns) and pitch children themselves and/or by their parents. The study (and the
perception improved only for the children with hearing impair- study of Torppa et al., 2014b) could not differentiate the role of
ments. They found no improvement in perception of speech in parental singing from the singing of the children themselves, since
noise, which the authors interpreted as being due to the context: these two were strongly connected (Torppa, 2015). It is worth
classrooms with normal-hearing and hearing-impaired children noting that the parents often sang face to face with the child. This
learning together in quite large groups. Moreover, Petersen et al. could improve attention to spectral changes because the child
(2012) found in a controlled musical intervention study (training could see the mouth and lip shapes and movements, which are
6 months, singing, playing piano, computer-based listening), using related to the spectral content of consonant and vowel sounds. The
adults recently implanted with cochlear implants, improvements in slow rate and predictability of children's songs may be particularly
music perception (discrimination of timbre, melodic contour, and beneficial for the perception of these spectral changes. However, it
rhythm) only for the musical intervention group. However, the cannot be ruled out that better perception of the sound intensity
perception of speech in noise and emotional prosody improved envelope (important for rhythm perception, for which the CI
similarly in both musical intervention and control groups. The singers were better than the CI-non-singers; Torppa et al., 2014b)
improved perceptual and musical skills with musical intervention also plays a role, since this is known to be an important cue for
from the two studies above are in line with the correlational results perception of speech and for perception of musical instrument
(Chen et al., 2010; Polonenko et al., 2017, Table 1) and results from timbre, especially for CI users (Drennan and Rubinstein, 2008; Kong
interventions without a control group for children with CIs (Abdi et al., 2011).
et al., 2001; Fu et al., 2015; Koşaner et al., 2012; Rocca, 2012, Since the CI singers sang the rhythms of a song better than the CI
Table 2). non-singers, it is possible that the CI singers could benefit from the
The positive results above are also in line with the results from a regular rhythm of the stimuli (see above, Torppa et al., 2014b). Their
study which investigated how singing at home is related to the auditory system may have been better able to track the moments
development of cortical processing of music and attention in 4e13- when important information was expected. Triggered by the
Table 3

116
Intervention studies with a control group.

Study Size and type of control group/ Device Size and type of music group Specific goals Outcome measures Results
allocation to control and music /age /duration of intervention /statistical analyses
groups

General goals: Music perception (pitch/f0) and auditory attention


Torppa et al. 9 CI peers did not sing at home (CI CI 12 CI children (CI singers) sang Development of preattentive Event-related potentials: MMN, Only CI singers developed in
2014b non- singers). No differences from /4e13 years. regularly at home before and discrimination and attention P3a for changes in pitch (f0), attention shift (P3a), significantly
the CI singers with respect to other between tests; regular parental shift toward changes in musical timbre, duration, (intensity) towards changes in timbre;
musical background than parental singing starting from implantation tones. Accuracy of singing at T2. and gaps in every other tone; Compared to CI non-singers, CI
singing (parents of CI non-singers /first tests (T1) at age 4e13 years, /LMM. evaluation of accuracy of singers had larger and earlier P3a
sang less), age, hearing-related second tests (T2) 14e17 months singing (f0, rhythm and lyrics) for changes in pitch (f0) and earlier
aspects, CI type, age at CI, gender, later. of the CI children. P3a for changes in timbre, and they
socio-economic background, /Total duration of singing to 10 sang better rhythms of songs.
etiology. years (on average 5 times per week
/Based on a questionnaire. before T1, 4 times per week
between T1/T2).
General goals: Music perception and/or perception of prosody
Torppa et al., 13 CI peers (CIn children) with non- CI 8 CIm children in musical play Perception of prosody and Perception of word and CIm children performed similarly to

R. Torppa, M. Huotilainen / Hearing Research 380 (2019) 108e122


(2014a) musical hobbies. Compared to /4e13 years. school-like activities with parents auditory cues for prosody (f0, sentence stress; discrimination NH controls in stress and pitch (f0)
music group, parental education or alone (small groups) þ home intensity, duration). of f0, intensity and duration in perception in bisyllables (and digit
lower, which was controlled for in activities ¼ singing by parents and /LMM. synthesized bisyllables; digit span). CIn children performed more
all statistical analyses. No children, playing instruments, span forward, PIQ. poorly than CIm and NH groups.
differences from the music group dancing Only CIm children improved with
with respect to age, hearing-related /starting from implantation, first age in word stress and intensity
aspects, CI type, age at CI, gender, tests (T1) at age of 4e13, second perception in bisyllables, and
etiology. tests (T2) 14e17 months after. improved over time in digit span.
/Based on a questionnaire. /Total duration up to 10 years.
Weekly þ musical activities at
home.
Good et al., (2017) 9 CI peers in visual arts training. No CI 9 CI children in individualized, Perception of music and MBEMA (scale, contour, Musical training enhanced:
difference from the music group /6e15 years. private piano lessons: technical emotional speech prosody. interval, rhythm, and incidental discrimination of melodic contour,
with respect to age, age at CI, CI exercises with piano, music theory /Mixed-design ANOVA. memory), perception of rhythm, memory for melodies, and
experience, speech perception./ rudiments, learning songs and emotional speech prosody emotional prosody especially in
Pseudorandom or random rehearsal of the (audio-only audio-only task. Art training did not
allocation. songs vocally. and audiovisual). lead to the same improvements.
/6 months.
Hidalgo et al., The same HI children participating a CI and/or HA Rhythmic and audio-motoric Temporal adaptation in speech Picture naming in alternation Children with HI became sensitive
(2017) speech therapy session. /5e6 years. exercises: following the beat of the interaction. with a virtual partner. to the manipulation of temporal
/AB-BA design. song (clapping the hands, shaking /RM ANOVA. Alternation rate and temporal regularity of stress occurrences only
maracas, body tapping, imitating predictability (match vs. after rhythmic training.
different rhythms of songs, mismatch of stress occurrences)
presented by the speech therapist were manipulated.
on a djembe.
/30 min.
General goals: Music perception and/or perception of speech in quiet or noise
Yucel et al., (2009) 9 CI children without musical CI, CI þ HA 9 CI children. Parents trained their Pitch (f0) and rhythm Meaningful auditory By the end of 3rd month, music
intervention. /newly implanted, children at home to recognize pitch perception; speech perception. integration scale (MAIS) or group was better in open-set
Newly implanted, the age at the age at (f0) differences between notes and /Mann-Whitney U-tests. infantetoddler MAIS and the speech perception.
implantation ranged between 12 implantation to recognize rhythms using a meaningful use of speech scale Music group had more exposure to
and 36 months. ranged between 39 keyboard. Parents also played (MUSS); musical stages music at the end of the first year. By
and 96 months. simple melodies repeatedly and questionnaire, open- and the end of the second year music
encouraged their children to listen closed-set speech perception. group developed more than the
and dance with them. control group in all aspects of
/2 years. musical skills.
Petersen et al., 10 NH peers without musical CI /15e18 years. 11 CI adolescents in musical Preattentive and behavioral MMN brain responses for No improvement associated with
(2015) training. training: rhythm, singing, ear discrimination of changes in musical pitch (f0), rhythm, training.
training and computer-based music. Perception of speech in intensity and timbre changes,
R. Torppa, M. Huotilainen / Hearing Research 380 (2019) 108e122 117

rhythm of music and speech, attention shift to the expected mo-

CI ¼ cochlear implant; NH ¼ normal hearing; HI ¼ hearing impairment; HA ¼ hearing aid. AB-BA design ¼ all children participated in intervention and control training and served as controls and intervention group. Order of
intervention and control training was counterbalanced across children. LMM ¼ linear mixed modeling. ANOVA ¼ analysis of variance. RM ¼ repeated measures. MMN ¼ mismatch negativity brain response. P3a ¼ positive
response peaking after MMN, reflects attention shift towards sound change salient for the listener. F0 ¼ fundamental frequency; pitch is perceptual correlate of f0. PIQ ¼ performance intelligence quotient (block design) from
CI singers were better in perception
Comfortable singing range, general

patterns) and pitch (f0) perception ments is known to lead to better speech perception at these mo-

effects in perception of speech in


ability to sing (pitch and rhythm

improved in the HI children. No €n and Tillmann, 2015). Based on results

of speech in noise than CI non-


ments (Mattys, 1997; Scho
of Cason et al. (2015), rhythmic priming can improve phonological
production in children with hearing aids and CIs (not included in
the tables). They conducted an experiment where children had to
repeat sentences that were or were not preceded by a rhythmical
prime which either matched or mismatched the meter (i.e., the
chain of stressed syllables) of the sentence. Matching conditions

singers.
resulted in greater phonological accuracy than baseline and mis-
noise.

matching conditions. Plausibly, musical rhythm may have built


expectations which improved both the perception and production
50% correct words in sentences.
discrimination of these, SRT for

Singing Programme (NSP) Sing

specially designed chord pitch


A singing competence profile,

of speech. This process is thought to assist speech perception in


adaptively to obtain the SRT.
SRT for 75% correct words in
discrimination test; Speech

WISC (Wechsler, 2010). SRT ¼ speech reception threshold. MBEMA ¼ a children's version of the MBEA, the Montreal Battery for Evaluation of Musical Abilities (Peretz et al., 2013).
shaped noise was adjusted

€ n, 2012; see also Mattys, 1997; Scho


€n and
illustrated in 4  4 picture
evaluation of the National

sentences in steady noise.

general (Cason and Scho


Up (Welch et al., 2015); a
based on that used in the

sentences, 6 colour and 8


number options. Speech-
perception in noise: In

Sentences/words were

Tillmann, 2015).
Good et al. (2017; Table 3) investigated the effect of musical
training for children with CIs (age 6e15 years). The children were
assigned to individual musical training (including playing piano
and rehearsal of songs vocally) and individual painting lesson
matrix.

groups. Participants could choose the location of training without


knowing the type of training, or they were assigned randomly to
noise. /Mixed-effects ANOVA; t-

musical and painting training groups. The groups did not differ in
Perception of speech in noise.
pitch discrimination, speech

age at testing, age at implantation, CI experience, or speech


Singing competence, chord

perception skills. In line with the correlational and interventional


evidence above (Tables 1e3), musical training was associated with
perception in noise.

improved discrimination of melodic contour and rhythm and


incidental memory for melodies as well as emotional prosody. Art
/RM ANOVA.

training did not lead to the same improvements. Interestingly,


Fuller et al. (2018) conducted a randomised, controlled intervention
/LMM.
tests.

study using adults with CIs. The participants attended 6 weeks at


face-to-face music therapy (training of rhythm perception, musical
small-group interactions (HI pairs).

implantation, also CI children sang

speech perception, music perception, singing, vocal emotion


Emphasis on building a repertoire

/Weekly across two school terms.


of simple songs with actions and
vocal exploration. Also activities
that drew on visual imagery for
sound and that included simple
lessons in large groups (n ¼ 29)
exercises. /6 days (20 h), over 2

12 HI children in weekly music

identification, and music improvisation), computer-based melodic


with NH peers, occasionally in

notation and physical gesture.

Parental singing started from

before the follow-up started.


The same as in Torppa et al.,

contour training or non-musical activities. Improvement of


perception of emotional prosody was found only with face-to-face
music therapy. No improvement in the perception of speech in
quiet or noise was found in this study for any of the groups (Fuller
2014bsee above.

et al., 2018).
Torppa et al. (2014a; Table 3) studied children with CIs (age
weeks.

4e13 years), determining how vocal (synthesized speech) f0, in-


tensity and duration discrimination and prosodic perception
(perception of word and sentence stress) and auditory working
CI and/or HA /5e7

memory developed for 21 children exposed to music (CIm) or not


CI/4e13 years.

(CIn). Measurements were conducted twice, 14e17 months apart.


Children with CIs in the CIm group participated in musical activities
(singing, sensorimotor activities, and music instrument playing),
years.

before and between the measurements (Table 3), most of them


from immediately after implantation. Compared to the CIn chil-
2014bsee above; the same study as
Welch et al., (2015) 17 NH peers in music lessons with

dren, the CIm group took part in significantly more musical activ-
the HI group. They did not have

ities in the home. The CIn children participated in other than


Torppa et al. (2018) The same as in Torppa et al.,

musical hobbies. The musically active CIm group performed as well


as normal-hearing children in stress perception and f0 discrimi-
small-group training.

nation and in auditory working memory (digit span), while the CIn
group performed more poorly. Additionally, only the CIm group
improved with age in word-stress perception and intensity
in Table 1.

discrimination, and over time (during the follow up), in digit span
(for the link of musical training to auditory working memory of
hearing-impaired children, see also Rochette et al., 2014, Table 1).
Hidalgo et al. (2017; Table 3) studied how preceding short-term
rhythmic musical training or speech therapy affected speech
perception for children with CIs and/or hearing aids. The partici-
pating children named pictures presented on a screen, taking turns
with a virtual partner. Alternation rate (fast or slow) and temporal
predictability (match vs mismatch of word stress occurrences) were
.

manipulated. The children with hearing impairment were tested


118 R. Torppa, M. Huotilainen / Hearing Research 380 (2019) 108e122

twice, once after 30 min of traditional speech therapy and once after improvement with singing in one study only (Torppa et al., 2014b,
30 min of individual musical rhythmic training conducted by a 2018, Tables 1 and 3; see also improvement of perception of timbre
speech therapist. It was found that children with hearing loss in adults with CIs, Petersen et al., 2012, section 2.3). And finally,
became sensitive to the manipulation of the temporal regularity of signs of improvements with musical activities have been found for
speech stress occurrences only after rhythmic training, when they perception of phonemes (Rochette et al., 2014, Table 1; see also
could adapt their turns to the speech rhythm (stress patterns) of the Cason et al., 2015, section 2.3; for adult CI users, see Lo et al., 2015,
interlocutor. The authors concluded that rhythmic training may help section 2.2) and speech in noise (Torppa et al., 2018, Table 3),
children with hearing impairment to structure the temporal flow of auditory attention (Torppa et al., 2014b; Table 3), auditory working
their verbal interactions. According to the authors, this could be memory (Rochette et al., 2014, Table 1; Torppa et al., 2014a, Table 3)
indicative of meter (i.e., the regular recurrence of a beat or accents) and language skills (Torppa et al., in print), which are important for
perception and/or production being underdeveloped in children learning and educational success.
with hearing loss, since normally hearing children were able to do Even though the results are promising, more evidence is needed.
the task without rhythmic training. It is thus possible that rhythmic The number of studies and participants in the studies reviewed is
training improves the ability to perceive and understand rhythmical small, the study designs vary, and even the studies with a control
and metrical structure (Cason et al., 2015). However, musical group are not always well controlled (see Tables 1e3). There is a lot
training may also have enhanced the encoding of acoustic cues such to improve in the research paradigms, such as assessing speech
as f0, intensity and duration, that characterise stress patterns perception with a larger battery of tests, and in reporting the
(Torppa et al., 2014a), or training could have enhanced auditory content and practicalities of the intervention in more detail (for
processing in general (Hidalgo et al., 2017). factors affecting the results of musical training, from the perspec-
Vongpaisal et al. (2016; not included in the tables) studied how tive of speech-in-noise perception, see Coffey et al., 2017). Above
children with CIs learned new songs in listening only (auditory all, there is lack of RCTs assessing the effects of musical activities on
learning), or listening and dancing (auditory-motor learning) con- language skills of children with hearing impairments.
ditions. The children could listen or listen and dance to the songs Children and their families enjoy musical activities. Families
until they felt they had learned the songs. After the learning phase, who know about the benefits of music or appreciate music, most
the ability to identify song excerpts, mistuned excerpts (with the often encourage their children to take part in musical hobbies and
original instruments), and the same songs played with a piano also to make music at home (Driscoll et al., 2015).
sound was studied. Compared to listening only, the combination of According to the studies reviewed above, musical activities may
listening and dancing led to better identification of songs in ver- increase involvement in music, and all studies showing significant
sions that preserved the original instrumental beats and lyrics. results show improvements of musical skills, music or speech
perception or cognitive aspects with musical activities; none of
them report negative effects. Music may have positive conse-
2.4. Summary quences for the lives of children with hearing impairments. From an
ethical point of view, it is not wise to exclude them from musical
For children with hearing impairments, improvements in activities at home or outside of the home. Therefore, while waiting
musical skills and/or music perception (pitch or rhythm), linked to for better scientific evidence, we feel that it is important to list
perception of speech prosody, have been found with musical ac- some principles and give our recommendations for the use of music
tivities in several studies (in 2/2 correlational, 2/3 intervention for children with CIs and/or hearing aids.
studies without a control group and with statistical analyses, 4/5
intervention studies with a control group; Tables 1e3). There are
also signs of improvement of detection of temporal regularities 3. How to use music to enhance speech and language skills of
(related to perception of stress patterns and speech in general) with hearing-impaired children
musical activities (Hidalgo et al., 2017; Torppa et al., 2014b, Table 3).
Perception of prosody, which is important for speech perception It should be understood clearly that traditional speech and
and language acquisition (see section 2), improved only with language therapy is of vital importance to hearing-impaired chil-
musical activities in the 3/3 intervention studies with a control dren and adolescents. However, musical activities can be used in
group (emotional prosody: Good et al., 2017, stress perception, the context of speech and language therapy, as an additional
Torppa et al., 2014a; plausibly also in Hidalgo et al., 2017, where the therapy, as a separate hobby, and/or as an activity at home.
children served as controls for themselves, Table 3) (for adult CI We have drafted the following recommendations on the basis of
users, question-statement prosody, see Lo et al., 2015, section 2.2; the intervention studies and correlational studies described above,
emotional prosody, Fuller et al., 2018, section 2.3). Cortical pro- and on the basis of the traditional auditory rehabilitation, music
cessing of changes in timbre of musical instruments, which is therapy methodologies, and speech and language therapy
connected to better perception of speech in noise, showed methods.

1. Start using music systematically at an early age, before implantation or the application of hearing aids. Continue musical activities more than one year, to improve
speech perception.
For children with hearing impairments, the early start of musical activities, like singing or musical play school, may be beneficial for the perception of prosody and for the
perception of speech in noise, and perhaps for language skills (see the children's ages at the start of musical activities in Torppa et al., 2014a, 2018; vs. null results from
adolescents, Petersen et al., 2015; adults, Fuller et al., 2018). Furthermore, families of children with CIs feel that they and their babies benefit from and enjoy musical
activities starting before implantation (Rocca, 2015), and a hearing-impaired child can benefit from musical activities even before implantation or receiving a hearing aid
due to residual hearing or tactile perception of low frequencies in time synchrony with visual stimulation (multisensory perception, see below). It is also known that the
brain is most plastic at an early age, before 3e4 years (for a review, Sharma et al., 2015), and therefore, musical activities at this age can lead most efficiently to changes in
the brain. However, it may also be good to continue at least one year or longer with musical activities to improve speech perception (see the duration of activities in
Chobert et al., 2014; Slater et al., 2015; Torppa et al., 2018; vs. Fuller et al., 2018; Petersen et al., 2015).
2. Use bodily movements in the rhythm of the music. Use orientations.
The use of music, especially when it involves dance and other sensorimotor activities (used in musical play schools and similar activities, Torppa et al., 2014a), or dancing,
seems to be beneficial for the learning of lyrics (dancing, Vongpaisal et al., 2016) or perception of prosody (Torppa et al., 2014a) by children with CIs. Rhythmic,
R. Torppa, M. Huotilainen / Hearing Research 380 (2019) 108e122 119

(continued )

multisensory stimulation simultaneously with lyrics can improve the perception of speech (for a review, see Torppa et al., 2018). The joy of music that the child can
observe in group music situations can awake the curiosity of the child, and he/she will naturally want to join in (Abdi et al., 2001; Innes-Brown et al., 2013; Koşaner et al.,
2012; Rocca, 2015; for normally-hearing children, Krueger, 2011). For small infants with hearing impairment, holding the baby against the adult's body while singing or
humming, and carrying the baby while walking or dancing to the rhythm of the music are good methods for stimulating the audio-tactile-vestibular system (Phillips-
Silver and Trainor, 2005), and for keeping the child near enough to hear the lyrics well (the closer the distance between the singer and the child is, the louder is the sound
is for the child).
Use consistent, large body movements that are rhythmically co-occurring with the music. Let your body movements reflect the style of the music: fast or slow, happy or sad,
aggressive or melancholic, to aid learning of the rhythm and beat of music (see for example Rocca, 2015), which are both plausibly important for the perception of speech
(section 2.3). Repeat the same bodily movements with the same piece of music consistently. Repetition is highly important for the auditory learning of children with
hearing impairments (Estabrooks, 1994; Hull, 2013).
Help the child locate musical sounds in space: point with your finger and anchor your gaze to the direction of the sound. You can make this into a game: where is the sound
coming from? This gives the child the opportunity to learn to recognize sounds and to practice sound localization, and therefore these are common activities in speech and
language therapy for hearing-impaired children (Estabrooks, 1994). Multisensory integration at the neural level is vital for making sense of multisensory information like
speech, which contains both visual and auditory stimulation, and this has been extensively studied (Calvert et al., 1998; King and Calvert, 2001; Shimojo and Shams,
2001).
Be consistent with your movements with respect to the sound parameters. In all musical play with hearing-impaired children, sounds high in f0 should be connected with
high hand movements, and sounds low in f0 should be connected with low hand movements or movements of the feet. Move your hand or a toy up and down with the f0
of the music (Rocca, 2012). These exercises give an opportunity for multisensory, audio-visual learning of changes in f0. Especially early-implanted children may be good
multisensory integrators (Schorr et al., 2005), and may benefit from visual cues. Hand movements which visualize f0 are also used in musical training for normal-hearing
children (for example, Kodaly, https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Kod%C3%A1ly_method).
3. Use singing as your main instrument, especially with a young child.
All studies of hearing-impaired children showing improvement of speech perception (prosody, phonemic discrimination, speech in noise) associated with musical ac-
tivities have included singing, face-to-face with children (see Tables 1 and 3, and the review above; see also Fuller et al., 2018, for adults with CIs). There are several
reasons why singing can be more efficient for speech perception than musical instrument playing (for an overview, Torppa et al., 2018).
Make sure that the hearing-impaired children can see your lips moving when you sing. This helps their brains connect the auditory information to lip and tongue movements
(multisensory perception, Schorr et al., 2005). Especially vowels are accompanied by changes in the spectral content of speech stimulus (Stevens, 1998), and seeing lip and
tongue movements can lead to better perception of spectral details and better perception of speech in quiet or noise (for an overview, see Torppa et al., 2018). This is most
natural at an early age, with parents. It is also important to keep in mind that it is easiest for the hearing-impaired auditory system to make sense of music when one
instrument, especially singing, is presented without accompaniment (for the difficulties in perception of simultaneous musical sounds by hearing-impaired individuals,
see Galvin et al., 2009; Looi et al., 2008).
4. Engage children in musical activities in small groups; use several musical instruments and pictures/toys presenting lyrics.
Ensure that the group size is small enough (plausibly up to 6 children) to avoid excessive noise and to keep the interest of the child on the lips and voice of the group leader
or the other group members and in making music. For hearing-impaired children, large group sizes are associated with null-results on speech-in-noise perception (Welch
et al., 2015), while studies suggesting improvement of perception of prosody or other aspects of speech perception with musical activities used individual lessons (Good
et al., 2017), individual singing at home (Torppa et al., 2018) or groups of 5e6 children (Rochette et al., 2014).
Use small, colourful, interesting instruments like rhythmic instruments, xylophones, and small stringed instruments to let the children become curious about musical
sounds. Let the children be active and try out different types of musical instruments. Playing an instrument by oneself is a highly multisensory activity which can lead to
better perception of f0, timbre, or rhythm, linked to perception of prosody or speech in noise (Schorr et al., 2005; Torppa et al., 2014a, 2018), and may be more effective
than listening only (Lappe et al., 2008). Playing staccato or legato can also improve the perception of intensity envelopes (Patel, 2011), which are also important for
perception of speech (Chobert et al., 2014). Since the perception of lyrics is challenging when children or group leaders play instruments together with singing (see Galvin
et al., 2009; Looi et al., 2008, above), use pictures visualizing lyrics, especially at the first stage of learning of lyrics of songs.
5. Use plenty of repetition.
Sing and play the same songs over and over again. Repetition is very helpful for auditory memory and it allows the auditory system to form models of the song and, over
the course of the repetitions, make the model more and more precise (Estabrooks, 1994; Hull, 2013). Using the same song always at the beginning and at the end of the
music session will help the child orientate and grasp the structure of the music and the session better e a method that is traditionally used in musical play schools and in
music therapy.
6. Use plenty of turn-taking.
Use songs which include turn-taking: The child will be given an opportunity for auditory closure (to continue the song when the adult stops singing, as used in the Hanen
program and AVT, Estabrooks, 1994), or the adult will sing a very short section first and then it is the child's turn to repeat it. Using short sections is important, since
children with hearing impairments have difficulties with auditory working memory (for an overview, see Torppa et al., 2014a), and, thus, in memorizing long sections.
Moreover, turn-taking allows the child to listen, to produce speech sounds, and to compare the adult's and his/her own speech production, including phoneme quality and
prosodic features. This is important since repeating lyrics with slow tempo, which is typical for music (Patel, 2014), may facilitate the learning of phonological codes for
words (for an overview, see Torppa et al., 2018).
7. Give advice to families on how to use music with their child.
Involve families in music-making with their child. Use videos and teaching materials to help the parents understand how integral and easy music-making can be in
everyday life. Teach songs to the parents and help them engage their own child into the world of music in their own home. This can increase the amount of singing and
other musical activities at home, which can further lead to improvements in the perception of prosody, in attention functions, and further, in the perception of speech in
noise and in language skills (see the review above; Torppa et al., 2014a,b; Torppa et al., 2018; Torppa et al., in print). Help parents if they encounter problems with music
schools or academies, to help children to continue musical activities long enough to improve speech perception and language skills (see suggestion 1 above).
If the hearing-impaired child has siblings, involve them in the musical activities, as well as the parents of the child. The siblings, when engaged in musical activities, will
continue singing and playing music at home, with plenty of repetition of music and a model for music making, thus creating a good environment for their hearing-
impaired sibling (Driscoll et al., 2015; for the role of repetition, see above).
8. Use computer games and apps to help the child perceive and produce sounds.
Give the hearing-impaired child, his/her parents, and teachers lists of apps and computer games that are available for practicing sound perception and production. Advise
the child and his/her parents on how to use the software. (For the possible benefits of computer-based melodic contour training for adult CI recipients, for perception of
prosody and consonants: see Lo et al., 2015; for perception of melodic contours, see Fuller et al., 2018; for perception of melodic contours for children with CIs, see Fu
et al., 2015, Table 2).
9. Give advice to school music teachers.
The hearing-impaired child has the right to take part in daycare and school music lessons, and to obtain a positive and encouraging image of his/herself as a music maker.
To achieve this, not only inform speech and language therapists, professionals at daycares and school teachers, including music teachers, about the results and suggestions
in this review, but also give advice to them about the effects of the hearing impairment on music perception of the child. For example, the teacher should know that a child
with a CI has good possibilities for learning rhythm (for an overview, Gfeller, 2016), but should not be asked to sing alone in front of other children. Poor perception of
pitch (see section 1.2, and Gfeller, 2016) make it difficult to sing in tune. Thus, singing in front of normally hearing peers can lead to unpleasant feeling or even bullying.
However, it is also important to keep in mind that pitch-based training can improve the perception of pitch and together with this, speech perception (see section 2.4 and
Tables 1e3). To motivate adolescents, a good solution for school music lessons is a band practice room where the hearing-impaired child can be part of a pop/rock band
playing drums and percussion. Such activities are often very much appreciated by children and teenagers and provide a good basis for healthy, positive self-esteem as a
(continued on next page)
120 R. Torppa, M. Huotilainen / Hearing Research 380 (2019) 108e122

(continued )

music maker, as noted in the Resonaari music camps of the association for children with CIs and their families in Finland (LapCI ry), in Mary Hare School (Rocca, 2012), and
for normally-hearing teenagers (Saarikallio, 2011).
10. Support musical hobbies of teenagers with hearing impairments.
There are several ways that children and teenagers with hearing impairments can have musical hobbies. In music schools, an individual study plan is needed. The United
Nation's Declaration of the Rights of the Child states that all children have the right to culture, which includes music (Ruisma €ki and Ruokonen, 2009). A hearing
impairment should not prevent the child from taking part in musical hobbies. Musical schools and academies need to realize this right and make it practically possible for
all children and adolescents to take part in their teaching. Foundations and other sources that lend musical instruments to students should specifically consider hearing-
impaired students.

With this list of advice and recommendations, we believe that 1092-4388(2012/11-0338.


Calvert, G.A., Brammer, M.J., Iversen, S.D., 1998. Crossmodal identification. Trends
hearing-impaired children, their parents, siblings, other family
Cognit. Sci. 2, 247e253. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01189-9.
members, teachers, speech therapists and other people close to Cason, N., Hidalgo, C., Isoard, F., Roman, S., Scho €n, D., 2015. Rhythmic priming en-
them can enhance the environment with music, making it possible hances speech production abilities: evidence from prelingually deaf children.
for these children to benefit from musical activities in respect to Neuropsychology 29. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000115, 1021e07.
Cason, N., Scho €n, D., 2012. Rhythmic priming enhances the phonological processing
speech perception and learning, thus supporting these children and of speech. Neuropsychologia 50 (11), 26522e26658. https://doi.org/10.1016/
adolescents to reach their abilities as full members of the society j.neuropsychologia.2012.07.018.
and in actively contributing as citizens. Chen, J.K.C., Chuang, A.Y.C., McMahon, C., Hsieh, J.C., Tung, T.H., Li, L.P.H., 2010.
Music training improves pitch perception in prelingually deafened children
with cochlear implants. Pediatrics. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-3620
Contributions peds-2008.
Chobert, J., Francois, C., Velay, J.L., Besson, M., 2014. Twelve months of active musical
training in 8- to 10-year-old children enhances the preattentive processing of
The two authors have equally contributed to the initial idea of syllabic duration and voice onset time. Cerebr. Cortex 24, 956e967. https://
the review, to the drafting the first version and revision of the doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs377.
manuscript, and R. T. finalized it. Coffey, E.B.J., Mogilever, N.B., Zatorre, R.J., 2017. Speech-in-noise perception in
musicians: a review. Hear. Res. 352, 496e499. https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.heares.2017.02.006.
Declaration of interest Cowan, N., 1988. Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and
their mutual constraints within the human information-processing system.
Psychol. Bull. 104 (2), 163. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.104.2.163.
None. Cutler, A., Foss, D.J., 1977. On the role of sentence stress in sentence processing.
Lang. Speech 20, 1e10. https://doi.org/10.1177/002383097702000101.
Funding Dittinger, E., Chobert, J., Ziegler, J.C., Besson, M., 2017. Fast brain plasticity during
word learning in musically-trained children. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11, 233. ISSN
: 1662-5161.
This work was supported by the Signe and Ane Gyllenberg Drennan, W.R., Rubinstein, J.T., 2008. Music perception in cochlear implant users
Foundation (the grant for the first author, Ritva Torppa). and its relationship with psychophysical capabilities. J. Rehabil. Res. Dev. 45,
779e789. https://doi.org/10.1682/JRRD.2007.08.0118.
Driscoll, V., Gfeller, K., Tan, X., See, R.L., Cheng, H.Y., Kanemitsu, M., 2015. Family
Acknowledgements involvement in music impacts participation of children with cochlear implants
in music education and music activities. Cochlear Implants Int. 16, 137e146.
https://doi.org/10.1179/1754762814Y.0000000103.
We thank the reviewers and especially Brian Moore for helpful Eisenberg, L.S., 2007. Current state of knowledge: speech recognition and produc-
comments. tion in children with hearing impairment. Ear Hear. 28, 766e772. https://
doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0b013e318157f01f.
Elbert, T., Pantev, C., Wienbruch, C., Rockstroh, B., Taub, E., 1995. Increased cortical
Appendix A. Supplementary data representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players. Science 270
(5234), 305e307. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.270.5234.305.
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at Estabrooks, W., 1994. Auditory-Verbal Therapy for Parents and Professionals.
Alexander Graham Bell Association for the deaf, Washington DC.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2019.06.003. Fagan, M.K., Pisoni, D.B., 2009. Perspectives on multisensory experience and
Torppa et al., cognitive development in infants with cochlear implants. Scand. J. Psychol. 50,
457e462. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00743.x.
Fitzpatrick, E.M., Crawford, L., Ni, A., Durieux-Smith, A., 2011. A descriptive analysis
References of language and speech skills in 4- to 5-yr-old children with hearing loss. Ear
Hear. 32, 605e616. https://doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0b013e31821348ae.
Abdi, S., Khalessi, M.H., Khorsandi, M., Gholami, B., 2001. Introducing music as a Flaugnacco, E., Lopez, L., Terribili, C., Montico, M., Zoia, S., Scho €n, D., 2015. Music
means of habilitation for children with cochlear implants. Int. J. Pediatr. Oto- training increases phonological awareness and reading skills in developmental
rhinolaryngol. 59 (2) https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-5876(01)00460-8, 1051e13. dyslexia: a randomized control trial. PLoS One 10. https://doi.org/10.1371/
Ambrose, S.E., Fey, M.E., Eisenberg, L.S., 2012. Phonological awareness and print journal.pone.0138715.
knowledge of preschool children with cochlear implants. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Fu, Q.J., Galvin 3rd, J.J., Wang, X., Wu, J.-L., 2015. Benefits of music training in
Res. 55, 811e823. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2011/11-0086. Mandarin-speaking pediatric cochlear implant users. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res.
Asp, F., Ma€ki-Torkko, E., Karltorp, E., Harder, H., Hergils, L., Eskilsson, G., Stenfelt, S., 58, 163e169. https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_JSLHR-H-14-0127.
2012. Bilateral versus unilateral cochlear implants in children: speech recog- Fuller, C.D., Galvin 3rd, J.-J., Maat, B., Baskent, D., Free, R.H., 2018. Comparison of two
nition, sound localization, and parental reports. Int. J. Audiol. 51 https://doi.org/ music training approaches on music and speech perception in cochlear implant
10.3109/14992027.2012.705898, 8178e32. users. Trends Hear 22, 1e22. https://doi.org/10.1177/2331216518765379.
Besson, M., Chobert, J., Marie, C., 2011. Transfer of training between music and Galvin 3rd, J.J., Fu, Q.-J., Oba, S.I., 2009. Effect of a competing instrument on melodic
speech: common processing, attention, and memory. Front. Psychol. 2, 94. contour identification by cochlear implant users. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 125,
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00094. EL98eEL103. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3062148.
€ n, D., 2014. Metrical rhythm implicitly orients attention in
Bolger, D., Coull, J.T., Scho Geers, A.E., Nicholas, J.G., 2013. Enduring advantages of early cochlear implantation
time as indexed by improved target detection and left inferior parietal activa- for spoken language development. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 56, 643e653.
tion. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 593e605. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00511. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0347.
Boons, T., De Raeve, L., Langereis, M., Peeraer, L., Wouters, J., van Wieringen, A., Gfeller, K., 2016. Music-based training for pediatric CI recipients: a systematic
2013. Expressive vocabulary, morphology, syntax and narrative skills in pro- analysis of published studies. Eur. Ann. Otorhinolaryngol. Head Neck Dis. 133S,
foundly deaf children after early cochlear implantation. Res. Dev. Disabil. 34, S50eS56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anorl.2016.01.010.
2008e2022. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.03.003. Gomes, H., Molholm, S., Christodoulou, C., Ritter, W., Cowan, N., 2000. The devel-
Caldwell, A., Nittrouer, S., 2013. Speech perception in noise by children with opment of auditory attention in children. Front. Biosci. 5 (1), d108ed120.
cochlear implants. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 56, 1330. https://doi.org/10.1044/
R. Torppa, M. Huotilainen / Hearing Research 380 (2019) 108e122 121

https://doi.org/10.2741/Gomes. Manolson, A., 1992. It Takes Two to Talk. A Parent's Guide to Helping Children
Good, A., Gordon, K.A., Papsin, B.C., Nespoli, G., Hopyan, T., Peretz, I., Russo, F.A., Communicate, third ed. Hanen Centre, Toronto.
2017. Benefits of music training for perception of emotional speech prosody in Mattys, S.L., 1997. The use of time during lexical processing and segmentation: a
deaf children with cochlear implants. Ear Hear. 38, 4554e4564. https://doi.org/ review. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 4, 310e329. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210789.
10.1097/aud.0000000000000402. Mithen, S., Morley, I., Wray, A., Tallerman, M., Gamble, C., 2006. The Singing Ne-
Gordon, E.E., 1979. Primary Measures of Music Audiation and the Intermediate anderthals: the Origins of Music, Language, Mind and Body, by Steven Mithen.
Measures of Music Audiation. G.I.A. Publications, Chicago, IL. Weidenfeld & Nicholson, London, 2005.
Gordon, R.L., Fehd, H.M., McCandliss, B.D., 2015. Does music training enhance lit- Moore, B.C.J., 2003. Speech processing for the hearing-impaired: successes, failures,
eracy skills? A meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 6 https://doi.org/10.3389/ and implications for speech mechanisms. Speech Commun. 41 https://doi.org/
fpsyg.2015.01777. 10.1016/S0167-6393(02)00095-X, 819e1.
Hartas, D., 2011. Families' social backgrounds matter: socio-economic factors, home Moreno, S., Bialystok, E., Barac, R., Schellenberg, E.G., Cepeda, N.J., Chau, T., 2011.
learning and young children's language, literacy and social outcomes. Br. Educ. Short-term music training enhances verbal intelligence and executive function.
Res. J. 37 (6) https://doi.org/10.1080/01411926.2010.506945, 8939e14. Psychol. Sci. 22, 1425e1433. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611416999.
Hashemi, S.B., Monshizadeh, L., 2012. The effect of cochlear implantation in Moreno, S., Bidelman, G.M., 2014. Examining neural plasticity and cognitive benefit
development of intelligence quotient of 6-9 deaf children in comparison with through the unique lens of musical training. Hear. Res. 308 https://doi.org/
normal hearing children (Iran, 2009-2011). Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 76, 10.1016/j.heares.2013.09.012, 849e7.
802e804. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2012.02.046. Moreno, S., Lee, Y., Janus, M., Bialystok, E., 2015. Short-term second language and
Hausen, M., Torppa, R., Salmela, V.R., Vainio, M., S€ €mo
arka € , T., 2013. Music and music training induces lasting functional brain changes in early childhood.
speech prosody: a common rhythm. Front. Psychol. 4 https://doi.org/10.3389/ Child Dev. 86 (2) https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12297, 3944e06.
fpsyg.2013.00566. €€
Na at€
anen, R., 1992. Attention and Brain Function. Routledge.
Hidalgo, C., Falk, S., Scho € n, D., 2017. Speak on time! Effects of a musical rhythmic Nikolopoulos, T.P., Wells, P., Archbold, S.M., 2000. Using Listening Progress Profile
training on children with hearing loss. Hear. Res. 351, 1118. https://doi.org/ (LIP) to assess early functional auditory performance in young implanted
10.1016/j.heares.2017.05.006. children. Deaf. Educ. Int. 2 (3), 142e151.
Houston, D.M., Pisoni, D.B., Kirk, K.I., Ying, E.A., Miyamoto, R.T., 2003. Speech O'Halpin, R., 2010. The Perception and Production of Stress and Intonation by
perception skills of deaf infants following cochlear implantation: a first report. Children with Cochlear Implants [dissertation]. University College London,
Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 67, 479e495. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165- London, UK. http://eprints.ucl.ac.uk/20406/.
5876(03)00005-3. Oxenham, A.J., 2008. Pitch perception and auditory stream segregation: implica-
Hull, R. (Ed.), 2013. Introduction to Aural Rehabilitation. Plural Publishing, San tions for hearing loss and cochlear implants. Trends Amplif. 12, 316e331.
Diego, CA. https://doi.org/10.1177/1084713808325881.
Hyde, K.L., Lerch, J., Norton, A., Forgeard, M., Winner, E., Evans, A.C., Schlaug, G., Pantev, C., Oostenveld, R., Engelien, A., Ross, B., Roberts, L.E., Hoke, M., 1998.
2009. Musical training shapes structural brain development. J. Neurosci. 29, Increased auditory cortical representation in musicians. Nature 392 (6678), 811.
3019e3025. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5118-08.2009. https://doi.org/10.1038/33918.
Innes-Brown, H., Marozeau, J.P., Storey, C.M., Blamey, P.J., 2013. Tone, rhythm, and Patel, A.D., 2003. Language, music, syntax and the brain. Nat. Neurosci. 6 (7), 674.
timbre perception in school-age children using cochlear implants and hearing https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1082.
aids. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 24 (9) https://doi.org/10.3766/jaaa.24.9.4, 7898e06. Patel, A.D., 2010. Music, Language, and the Brain. Oxford university press.
Jaschke, A.C., Honing, H., Scherder, E.J.A., 2018. Longitudinal analysis of music ed- Patel, A.D., 2011. Why would musical training benefit the neural encoding of
ucation on executive functions in primary school children. Front. Neurosci. 12 speech? The OPERA hypothesis. Front. Psychol. 2, 142. https://doi.org/10.3389/
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2018.00103. fpsyg.2011.00142.
Janus, M., Lee, Y., Moreno, S., Bialystok, E., 2016. Effects of short-term music and Patel, A.D., 2014. Can nonlinguistic musical training change the way the brain
second-language training on executive control. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 144, processes speech? The expanded OPERA hypothesis. Hear. Res. 308, 98e108.
84e97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.11.009. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2013.08.011.
Jusczyk, P.W., Houston, D.M., Newsome, M., 1999. The beginnings of word seg- Patscheke, H., Dege , F., Schwarzer, G., 2018. The effects of training in rhythm and
mentation in English-learning infants. Cogn. Psychol. 39, 159e207. https:// pitch on phonological awareness in four- to six-year-old children. Psychol.
doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0716. Music 1e6. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735618756763.
Kalathottukaren, R.T., Purdy, S.C., Ballard, E., 2017. Prosody perception and pro- Peng, S.C., Tomblin, J.B., Turner, C.W., 2008. Production and perception of speech
duction in children with hearing loss and age- and gender-matched controls. intonation in pediatric cochlear implant recipients and individuals with normal
J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 28, 2832e2894. https://doi.org/10.3766/jaaa.16001. hearing. Ear Hear. 29, 336e351. https://doi.org/10.1097/
King, A.J., Calvert, G.A., 2001. Multisensory integration: perceptual grouping by eye AUD.0b013e318168d94d.
and ear. Curr. Biol. 11, r322er325. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0960-9822(01) Percy-Smith, L., Busch, G., Sandahl, M., Nissen, L., Josvassen, J.L., Lange, T., Rusch, E.,
00175-0. Caye-Thomasen, P., 2013. Language understanding and vocabulary of early
Kong, Y.-Y., Mullangi, A., Marozeau, J., Epstein, M., 2011. Temporal and spectral cues cochlear implanted children. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 77, 184e188.
for musical timbre perception in electric hearing. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 54, Peretz, I., Gosselin, N., Nan, Y., Caron-Caplette, E., Trehub, S.E., Beland, R., 2013.
981e994. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2010/10-0196. A novel tool for evaluating children's musical abilities across age and culture.
Koşaner, J., Kilinc, A., Deniz, M., 2012. Developing a music programme for preschool Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7, 30. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2013.00030.
children with cochlear implants. Cochlear Implants Int. 13 (4) https://doi.org/ Petersen, B., Mortensen, M.V., Hansen, M., Vuust, P., 2012. Singing in the key of life -
10.1179/1754762811Y.0000000023, 2372e47. a pilot study on effects of musical ear training. Psychomusicology 22, 134e151.
Kraus, N., Strait, D.L., 2015. Emergence of biological markers of musicianship with https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031140.
school-based music instruction. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1337 (1), 163e169. Petersen, B., Weed, E., Sandmann, P., Brattico, E., Hansen, M., Sorensen, S.D.,
Krueger, J.W., 2011. Doing things with music. Phenomenol. Cognitive Sci. 10 (1), Vuust, P., 2015. Brain responses to musical feature changes in adolescent
1e22. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-010-9152-4. cochlear implant users. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9 https://doi.org/10.3389/
Lappe, C., Herholz, S.C., Trainor, L.J., Pantev, C., 2008. Cortical plasticity induced by fnhum.2015.00007.
short-term unimodal and multimodal musical training. J. Neurosci. 8 https:// Phillips-Silver, J., Trainor, L.J., 2005. Feeling the beat: movement influences infant
doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2254-08.2008, 9632e, 9639. rhythm perception. Science 308 (5727), 14301e14430. https://doi.org/10.1126/
Linnavalli, T., Putkinen, V., Lipsanen, J., Huotilainen, M., Tervaniemi, M., 2018. Music science.1110922.
playschool enhances children's linguistic skills. Sci. Rep. 8 https://doi.org/ Polonenko, M.J., Giannantonio, S., Papsin, B.C., Marsella, P., Gordon, K.A., 2017. Music
10.1038/s41598-018-27126-5. perception improves in children with bilateral cochlear implants or bimodal
Lo, C.Y., McMahon, C.M., Looi, V., Thompson, W.F., 2015. Melodic contour training devices. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 141 (6), 4494e4507. https://doi.org/10.1121/
and its effect on speech in noise, consonant discrimination, and prosody 1.4985123.
perception for cochlear implant recipients. Behav. Neurol. 352869 https:// Posner, M.I., 1980. Orienting of attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 32 (1), 3e25. https://
doi.org/10.1155/2015/352869. doi.org/10.1080/00335558008248231.
Looi, V., McDermott, H., McKay, C., Hickson, L., 2008. Music perception of cochlear Putkinen, V., Tervaniemi, M., Huotilainen, M., 2013. Informal musical activities are
implant users compared with that of hearing aid users. Ear Hear. 29, 421e434. linked to auditory discrimination and attention in 2-3-year-old children: an
https://doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0b013e31816a0d0b. event-related potential study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 37, 654e661. https://doi.org/
Lund, E., 2016. Vocabulary knowledge of children with cochlear implants: a meta- 10.1111/ejn.12049.
analysis. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 21, 107e121. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/ Putkinen, V., Tervaniemi, M., Saarikivi, K., de Vent, N., Huotilainen, M., 2014a.
env060. Investigating the effects of musical training on functional brain development
Lyxell, B., Wass, M., Sahlen, B., Samuelsson, C., Asker-Arnason, L., Ibertsson, T., M€
aki- with a novel Melodic MMN paradigm. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 110, 8e15.
Torkko, E., Larsby, B., Hallgren, M., 2009. Cognitive development, reading and https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2014.01.007.
prosodic skills in children with cochlear implants. Scand. J. Psychol. 50, Putkinen, V., Tervaniemi, M., Saarikivi, K., Ojala, P., Huotilainen, M., 2014b. Enhanced
463e474. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00754.x. development of auditory change detection in musically trained school-aged
MacDonald, R.A., Hargreaves, D.J., Miell, D. (Eds.), 2002. Musical Identities. OUP, children: a longitudinal event-related potential study. Dev. Sci. 17, 282e297.
Oxford. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12109.
Ma€nnel, C., Friederici, A.D., 2013. Accentuate or repeat? Brain signatures of devel- Rocca, C., 2012. A different musical perspective: improving outcomes in music
opmental periods in infant word recognition. Cortex 49, 2788e2798. https:// through habilitation, education, and training for children with cochlear im-
doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.09.003. plants. Semin. Hear. 33 https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0032-1329229, 4254e33.
122 R. Torppa, M. Huotilainen / Hearing Research 380 (2019) 108e122

Rocca, C., 2015. Developing the musical brain to boost early pre-verbal, commu- 157046.
nication and listening skills: the implications for musicality development pre- Torppa, R., Faulkner, A., Huotilainen, M., J€ arvikivi, J., Lipsanen, J., Laasonen, M.,
and post-cochlear implantation. It is not just about Nursery Rhymes! Cochlear Vainio, M., 2014a. The perception of prosody and associated auditory cues in
Implants Int. 16, S.32eS.38. https://doi.org/10.1179/1467010015Z.0000 early-implanted children: the role of auditory working memory and musical
00000277. activities. Int. J. Audiol. 53, 182e191. https://doi.org/10.3109/
Rochette, F., Moussard, A., Bigand, E., 2014. Music lessons improve auditory 14992027.2013.872302.
perceptual and cognitive performance in deaf children. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8 Torppa, R., Faulkner, A., J€
arvikivi, J., Vainio, M., 2010. Acquisition of focus by normal
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00488. hearing and Cochlear Implanted children: the role of musical experience. In:
Ruisma €ki, H., Ruokonen, I., 2009. Children's right to good music educationdsome Speech Prosody 2010. Chicago, USA. http://hdl.handle.net/10138/23603.
practical applications and synthesis towards the ideals of the children's rights Torppa, R., Faulkner, A., Kujala, T., Huotilainen, M., Lipsanen, J., 2018. Developmental
declaration. Problems in Music Pedagogy 4, 7e17. links between speech perception in noise, singing, and cortical processing of
Saarikallio, S., Erkkila€, J., 2007. The role of music in adolescents' mood regulation. music in children with cochlear implants. Music Percept. 36, 156e174. https://
Psychol. Music 35 (1), 88e109. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735607068889. doi.org/10.1525/MP.2018.36.2.156.
Saarikallio, S., 2011. Music as emotional self-regulation throughout adulthood. Torppa, R., Faulkner, A., Laasonen, M., Lipsanen, J., Sammler, D., in print. Links of
Psychol. Music 39 (3), 307e327. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735610374894. prosodic stress perception and musical activities to language skills of children
Scho€n, D., Tillmann, B., 2015. Short- and long-term rhythmic interventions: per- with CIs and normal hearing. Ear Hear..
spectives for language rehabilitation. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1337, 32e39. https:// Torppa, R., Huotilainen, M., Leminen, M., Lipsanen, J., Tervaniemi, M., 2014b.
doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12635. Interplay between singing and cortical processing of music: a longitudinal
Schorr, E.A., Fox, N.A., van Wassenhove, V., Knudsen, E.I., 2005. Auditoryevisual study in children with cochlear implants. Front. Psychol. 5 https://doi.org/
fusion in speech perception in children with cochlear implants. Proc. Natl. Acad. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01389.
Sci. Unit. States Am. 102, 18748e18750. https://doi.org/10.1073/ €limaa, T., Kunnari, S., Laukkanen-Nevala, P., Lonka, E., the National Clinical
Va
pnas.0508862102. Research Team, 2018. Early vocabulary development in children with bilateral
Sharma, A., Campbell, J., Cardon, G., 2015. Developmental and cross-modal plas- cochlear implants. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 53, 3e15. https://doi.org/
ticity in deafness: evidence from the P1 and N1 event related potentials in 10.1111/1460-6984.12322.
cochlear implanted children. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 95 (2), 135e144. Vongpaisal, T., Caruso, D., Yuan, Z., 2016. Dance movements enhance song learning
Shimojo, S., Shams, L., 2001. Sensory modalities are not separate modalities: plas- in deaf children with cochlear implants. Front. Psychol. 7 https://doi.org/
ticity and inter- actions. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11, 505e509. 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00835.
Slater, J., Skoe, E., Strait, D.L., O'Connell, S., Thompson, E., Kraus, N., 2015. Music Wechsler, D., 2010. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, fourth ed.: Manual.
training improves speech-in-noise perception: longitudinal evidence from a Psykologien Kustannus Oy, Helsinki, Finland.
community-based music program. Behav. Brain Res. 291, 244e252. https:// Welch, G.F., Saunders, J., Edwards, S., Palmer, Z., Himonides, E., Knight, J., Mahon, M.,
doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.05.026. Griffin, S., Vickers, D.A., 2015. Using singing to nurture children's hearing? A
Soleymani, Z., Mahmoodabadi, N., Nouri, M.M., 2016. Language skills and phono- pilot study. Cochlear Implants Int. 16, S63eS70. https://doi.org/10.1179/
logical awareness in children with cochlear implants and normal hearing. Int. J. 1467010015Z.000000000276.
Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 83, 16e21. https://doi.org/10.1016/ White-Schwoch, T., Carr, K.W., Anderson, S., Strait, D.L., Kraus, N., 2013. Older adults
j.ijporl.2016.01.013. benefit from music training early in life: biological evidence for long-term
Stevens, K.N., 1998. Acoustic Phonetics. The MIT Press, London, UK. training-driven plasticity. J. Neurosci. 33 (45), 17667e17674. https://doi.org/
Thiessen, E.D., Saffran, J.R., 2007. Learning to learn: infants' acquisition of stress- 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2560-13.2013.
based strategies for word segmentation. Lang. Learn. Dev. 3, 73e100. https:// Wu, C.-M., Lee, H.-L., Hwang, J.-H., Sun, Y.-S., Liu, T.-C., 2008. Intellectual ability of
doi.org/10.1080/15475440709337001. Mandarin-speaking children using cochlear implants. Audiol. Neurotol. 13,
Tierney, A., Krizman, J., Kraus, N., 2015. Music training alters the course of 302e308. https://doi.org/10.1159/000124278.
adolescent auditory development. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 112, Yucel, E., Sennaroglu, G., Belgin, E., 2009. The family oriented musical training for
10062e10067. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1505114112. children with cochlear implants: speech and musical perception results of two
Torppa, R., 2015. Pitch-related Auditory Skills in Children with Cochlear Implants: year follow-up. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 73 (7), 1043e1052. https://
the Role of Auditory Working Memory, Attention and Music [dissertation]. doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2009.04.009.
University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland. https://helda.helsinki.fi/handle/10138/

You might also like