You are on page 1of 12

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/341922033

Detouring in the family system as an antecedent of children's adjustment


problems

Article in Journal of Family Psychology · June 2020


DOI: 10.1037/fam0000727

CITATIONS READS
3 465

4 authors:

Jesse L Coe Patrick T Davies


Centers for Disease Control and Prevention University of Rochester
31 PUBLICATIONS 422 CITATIONS 214 PUBLICATIONS 16,081 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Rochelle F Hentges Melissa L. Sturge-Apple


National Institutes of Health University of Rochester
39 PUBLICATIONS 716 CITATIONS 131 PUBLICATIONS 4,329 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Patrick T Davies on 20 April 2021.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Family Psychology
© 2020 American Psychological Association 2020, Vol. 34, No. 7, 814 – 824
ISSN: 0893-3200 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/fam0000727

Detouring in the Family System as an Antecedent of Children’s


Adjustment Problems

Jesse L. Coe Patrick T. Davies


Alpert Medical School of Brown University and Bradley/Hasbro University of Rochester
Children’s Research Center, Providence, Rhode Island

Rochelle F. Hentges Melissa L. Sturge-Apple


University of Calgary University of Rochester
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Guided by conceptualizations of relational boundaries from family systems theory, this study examined
unique links between detouring (e.g., alliance between parents against child) and young children’s
psychological functioning after accounting for general family negativity and conflict. Participants in this
longitudinal (i.e., 2 annual waves of data collection), multimethod (i.e., observation, survey, semistruc-
tured interview), multiinformant (i.e., parent, teacher, observer) study included 218 young children (mean
[M] age ⫽ 5.76 years) and their families. The findings from cross-lagged panel analysis indicated that
detouring uniquely predicted increases in children’s externalizing problems, hostile attribution bias, and
peer rejection and victimization over a 1-year period (i.e., the transition from kindergarten to first grade).
Conversely, the findings failed to support a child effects model in that child adjustment problems did not
predict increases in detouring or family negativity and conflict over time.

Keywords: detouring, externalizing problems, hostile attribution bias, peer rejection and victimization

Young children who display heightened aggression in conjunc- and peer rejection and victimization. Further, studies that have
tion with rejection or victimization by their peers are at high risk focused on examining triadic family interactions as a predictor of
for persistent psychological, social, and emotional difficulties that child adjustment have primarily tested broader constructs of neg-
carry into adolescence and adulthood (e.g., Bierman, Kalvin, & ativity or conflict and warmth or support (e.g., Cummings, Koss,
Heinrichs, 2015; Coie, Terry, Lenox, Lochman, & Hyman, 1995). & Davies, 2015; Demby, Riggs, & Kaminski, 2017).
In recognizing the importance of understanding the determinants Family systems theory (FST; Cox & Paley, 1997; Minuchin,
of this high-risk pattern of behaviors in childhood, a growing body 1974) has been highly valuable in providing a more nuanced
of research has focused on elucidating family risk factors (e.g., approach to characterizing how different patterns of broader fam-
Bolger & Patterson, 2001; Runions & Keating, 2007; Yaros, ily functioning may have unique developmental implications for
Lochman, & Wells, 2016). However, in spite of researchers’ children. According to FST, the family can be broken down into
repeated calls for understanding how characteristics of the broader subsystems consisting of individual members (e.g., child, mother,
family system (e.g., both caregivers and child) affect early child partner) or dyads (e.g., parent– child, interparental), but the family
development (e.g., Cox & Paley, 1997; Kerig, 2016), very few as a whole is best understood by the functioning within and across
studies have moved beyond examining the influence of dyadic each subsystem. Metaphorical boundaries allow for differentiation
relationships (e.g., parent– child, interparental) on early aggression between these smaller subsystems within the larger family system

Editor’s Note. Barbara H. Fiese served as the action editor for this awarded to Patrick T. Davies and Melissa L. Sturge-Apple. Jesse L. Coe is
article.—BHF funded by the National Institute of Mental Health (T32 MH019927). Rochelle
F. Hentges is supported by a fellowship from the Talisman Energy Fund in
Support of Healthy Living and Injury Prevention and the Alberta Children’s
This article was published Online First June 4, 2020. Hospital Research Institute through the Owerko Centre. The findings presented in
X Jesse L. Coe, Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Alpert this article have not been previously presented or disseminated elsewhere. The
Medical School of Brown University, and Bradley/Hasbro Children’s authors are grateful to the children, parents, teachers, and community agencies
Research Center, Providence, Rhode Island; Patrick T. Davies, Department that participated in this project. We would also like to thank Mike Ripple, the
of Psychology, University of Rochester; X Rochelle F. Hentges, Depart- Mt. Hope Family Center staff, and the graduate and undergraduate students at
ment of Psychology, University of Calgary; Melissa L. Sturge-Apple, the University of Rochester who assisted in this project.
Department of Psychology, University of Rochester. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jesse L.
This research was supported by a grant from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver Coe, Bradley/Hasbro Children’s Research Center, 1 Hoppin Street, Suite
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (R01 HD065425) 204, Providence, RI 02903. E-mail: jesse_coe@brown.edu

814
DETOURING AND ADJUSTMENT PROBLEMS 815

and serve as implicit rules for defining and understanding family which contributes to the development of a hostile attribution bias
relationships according to both the amount and quality of contact that stabilizes over time (Choe, Lane, Grabell, & Olson, 2013;
and the transmission of resources across family subsystems. From Dodge, 2006). In a detouring family, the child is frequently the
this perspective, effective family functioning is defined by clear target of parental rejection or scapegoating. These experiences of
and flexible boundaries that permit each family member to func- exclusion or derision may in turn serve as blueprints for how the
tion within subsystems without the interference of other members child interprets future social situations. For example, he or she may
but access resources from the larger family unit when needed (Cox be more likely to perceive a peer as being aggressive (e.g., did it
& Paley, 1997; Minuchin, 1974). Theorists and prior empirical on purpose to harm or exclude the child) even when the situation
studies have identified other patterns of functioning in which the involves an ambiguous intent (e.g., child not being asked to join in
boundaries are not as clear or flexible. The two most commonly playing a game). Although some studies have shown links between
studied boundary disturbances are disengagement (family is emo- disruptions in the parent– child relationship (e.g., physical abuse,
tionally distant) and enmeshment (family is emotionally entan- authoritative parenting, parental aggression) and children’s height-
gled), and both have been linked with children’s internalizing and ened hostile attribution bias (Dodge, Pettit, Bates, & Valente,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

externalizing symptomatology (e.g., Jacobvitz, Hazen, Curran, & 1995; Nelson & Coyne, 2009; Runions & Keating, 2007; Yaros et
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Hitchens, 2004; Kerig, 1995; Sturge-Apple, Davies, & Cummings, al., 2016), no studies have expanded the focus to triadic family
2010). However, much less is known about how other boundary functioning to test whether detouring serves as a unique predictor.
disturbances in the form of alliances among family members are Additionally, the child’s anger and frustration resulting from
associated with children’s psychological functioning. experiencing ostracism and rejection by his or her parents may
Alliance formations are characterized by coalitions among fam- engender increases in externalizing problems characterized by
ily members where a third member is excluded or attacked in relational aggression, overt hostility, and conduct problems (Kerig,
attempts to either bring the allied members closer together or 1995; Vuchinich, Emery, & Cassidy, 1988). Although very few
deflect negativity onto the excluded member (Minuchin, 1974). As studies have tested the links between detouring and externalizing
a primary type of alliance, detouring patterns of family functioning problems, some preliminary support for this assertion exists. In
occur when parents form an alliance against the child (Kerig, one of the only published studies to empirically examine these
2016). Parents may “detour” around their own conflicts by deflect-
associations, Kerig (1995) found concurrent links between family
ing attention onto the child and his or her needs or difficulties as
members’ ratings of detouring (assessed via the Family Cohesion
a way of attempting to bring themselves closer together (Kerig,
Index) and parents’ reports of child externalizing problems in a
1995). Although this behavioral pattern is thought to often be
sample of 6- to 10-year-old children. Similarly, Yahav and Sharlin
outside of the parents’ awareness, detouring may become an es-
(2002) found that in a sample of 10- to 17-year-old children, those
tablished pattern of family relatedness through unconscious con-
with elevated levels of externalizing symptoms reported greater
ditioning processes (e.g., parents are unconsciously reinforced by
perceptions of being blamed by family members for various family
experiencing marital unity that has been difficult to achieve oth-
conflicts. However, to our knowledge, no studies have tested these
erwise). This process may create an artificial sense of closeness
links longitudinally or in early childhood.
and reduce strain in the interparental relationship, but the result for
In addition to its implications for how children process social
the child is often rejection and exclusion from the family system
(Fauber, Forehand, Thomas, & Wierson, 1990). In turn, it is information and regulate their behavior, detouring may also have
theorized that the targeted rejection experienced by children in a important implications for how children fare socially with peers.
detouring family may serve as a robust predictor of adjustment Script theory asserts that within the context of early family rela-
problems (e.g., Kerig, 1995, 2016; Minuchin, 1974). However, tionships, children develop internal schemas and expectations re-
very few studies have systematically explored these potential links garding themselves and others, which are then applied to subse-
between detouring and child problems. Therefore, our primary aim quent social interactions (Johnston, Roseby, & Kuehnle, 2009;
was to examine detouring, above and beyond a general construct of Sroufe & Fleeson, 1986). When these early relationships are
family negativity and conflict, as a unique predictor of young neglectful, volatile, or insensitive, children may develop negative
children’s psychological and social adjustment problems (i.e., ex- expectations regarding future relationships with others as well as
ternalizing problems, hostile attribution bias, peer rejection and views of themselves as incompetent or worthy of blame (Cicchetti,
victimization). Lynch, Shonk, & Manly, 1992; Cicchetti & Toth, 1998). These
internalizations may lead them to select and structure later inter-
actions in such a way that familiar relationship patterns reoccur
Detouring as a Predictor of Child
and are validated (Sroufe & Fleeson, 1986). For example, several
Adjustment Problems studies have shown that experiences of maltreatment, parental
The targeted rejection and exclusion experienced in a detouring criticism, and parent– child conflict are associated with children
family, as opposed to negativity and conflict that are generalized being the victims of bullying in middle childhood and adolescence
across the family, may be especially harmful to children. Detour- (e.g., Bolger & Patterson, 2001; Georgiou & Stavrinides, 2013;
ing may specifically give rise to a pattern of cognitions and Shields & Cicchetti, 2001). Because children in detouring families
behaviors consistent with a high-risk profile of aggression, victim- are often rejected, excluded, or ganged up on by their parents, this
ization and rejection, and dysregulation. Social information pro- may translate into them also being bullied or rejected by other
cessing (SIP) theory asserts that when children frequently experi- children when they transition into school settings. However, no
ence social interactions in which they are the targets of negativity, prior studies have explicitly tested whether detouring predicts peer
they are more likely to overattribute malevolent intent to others, rejection and victimization.
816 COE, DAVIES, HENTGES, AND STURGE-APPLE

Early Childhood as a Sensitive Period designs cannot rule out potential child-driven effects on family
functioning. For example, consistent with FST’s proposition of
We specifically examined detouring as a predictor of child reciprocity between children and the larger family system (Cox &
problems during the transition into the early school years based on Paley, 1997; Minuchin, 1974), it is possible that child misbehavior
a number of important developmental considerations. First, early may increase parental blame and hostility toward the child. There-
childhood is regarded as a period of heightened risk for children fore, concurrent links between detouring and child externalizing
experiencing adversity in their rearing environments (Shonkoff & problems (e.g., Kerig, 1995; Yahav & Sharlin, 2002) could reflect
Phillips, 2000). During this time, children are forming expectan- multiple directions of effect. In the current study, we specified a
cies about the availability and consistency of their care and the cross-lagged, autoregressive model to achieve a more definitive
salient figures in their lives, as well as broader concerns about the understanding of the nature of associations between detouring and
stability and health of relationships in the family (Cicchetti, Cum- child adjustment outcomes (i.e., also explicitly accounting for
mings, Greenberg, & Marvin, 1990). Therefore, disruptions in child-driven effects on detouring and family negativity and con-
family relationships (e.g., detouring, negativity and conflict) dur- flict).
ing this time are thought to be particularly harmful in relation to
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Furthermore, a heavy reliance on self-report measures contra-


children’s psychological and social adjustment. Second, family
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

dicts the theoretical premise that boundary patterns are implicit


systems theorists emphasize that examining family processes and, as a result, commonly outside the awareness of family mem-
within the context of major transitions provides unique opportu- bers (Minuchin, 1974). This lack of awareness causes family
nities for understanding how family functioning affects child de- members to be inaccurate sources, as evidenced by low correla-
velopment (e.g., Cowan, 1991; Sturge-Apple et al., 2010). Chil- tions in reports across family informants (e.g., Kerig, 1995). Ex-
dren’s transition into the early school years represents a key isting observational assessments of detouring also have weak-
stage-salient task that carries with it many novel challenges (e.g., nesses in that they often conflate detouring with other forms of
peer interactions, structured routines, greater time apart from fam- family alliance (e.g., parent– child coalition; Lindahl & Malik,
ily; Ladd & Burgess, 2001). Children may therefore be more likely 2000). Therefore, our study is particularly innovative in that it
during this period to draw on prior experiences with family rela- represents the first rigorous, longitudinal test of detouring as a
tionships in guiding their behavior and how they process social predictor of child behavior problems and adopts a novel observa-
information (McHale, Kuersten, & Lauretti, 1996). Third, early tional assessment of detouring. To reduce the operation of
childhood is a critical period for the development of stable patterns common-method and informant variance in the analyses, we uti-
of social information processing (Dodge, 2006). Although attribu- lized a multimethod (i.e., observation, survey, semistructured in-
tions and social processing become more complex and differenti- terview), multiinformant (i.e., observer, parent, teacher) approach
ated by situation as children get older, theorists posit that the to assessing key constructs. We also included child sex and total
development of a hostile attribution bias in early childhood pro- household income per capita as covariates.
vides the foundation for lifelong social information processing
(Dodge, 2006). It is therefore critical to examine these processes
and their precursors during this important developmental period.
Method
Finally, research has consistently shown that externalizing behav-
iors (e.g., Loeber, Green, Lahey, Christ, & Frick, 1992) and peer Participants
rejection and victimization (e.g., Buhs & Ladd, 2001) in early Participants were drawn from a larger longitudinal project de-
childhood are robust predictors of adjustment problems across the signed to examine the impact of interparental conflict and family
life span. relationships on young children. Participants in the larger project
included 243 families (mother, partner, and preschool child) re-
Present Study
siding in a moderate-sized metropolitan area in the northeastern
To our knowledge, the present study represents the first longi- United States. To obtain a demographically diverse sample, par-
tudinal test of detouring as a predictor of early childhood adjust- ticipants were recruited through preschools, Head Start agencies,
ment problems. As a more rigorous test of this novel aim, we Internet advertisements, family-friendly locations (e.g., farmers’
specifically examined the unique predictive value of detouring markets, libraries), and public and private daycares. Given the
above and beyond a general construct of family negativity and timing of the family interaction task used to assess detouring
conflict. Innovation in this study is rooted in its goal of testing (described later in the article), the current study only included data
novel theoretically guided hypotheses regarding links between from families at Waves 2 and 3 of the larger project (i.e., when the
detouring and behaviors characteristic of a high-risk pattern of child was in kindergarten and first grade). Of the 243 families that
aggression, victimization and rejection, and dysregulation. Consis- participated at Wave 1 of the study, 235 returned for Wave 2, but
tent with FST, we specifically hypothesized that detouring would 17 of the 235 mothers no longer had a partner who served as
predict increases in child externalizing problems, hostile attribu- another caregiver for the child. Because the primary predictor (i.e.,
tion bias, and peer rejection and victimization over a 1-year period. detouring) in the current study was conceptualized as the interac-
The few previous examinations of detouring as a predictor of child tional pattern between two caregivers and the child, we only
behavior problems have been somewhat limited in that they pre- included data from the 218 families that had two caregivers. For
dominantly use cross-sectional data and self-report survey mea- clarity, throughout the rest of the article, we refer to the kinder-
sures of detouring (e.g., Family Cohesion Index; Kerig, 1995). The garten assessment as Wave 1 and the first-grade assessment as
use of cross-sectional designs does not meet quantitative criteria Wave 2. Retention across the two waves of data collection was
for testing directionality (e.g., Cole & Maxwell, 2003), and such 93%. The average age of child participants was 5.76 years (stan-
DETOURING AND ADJUSTMENT PROBLEMS 817

dard deviation [SD] ⫽ .47) at Wave 1 and 6.77 years (SD ⫽ .48) making explicit comments to create distance from and exclude the
at Wave 2, and 57% were girls. The sample was racially diverse, child (e.g., “Just let Daddy and I do it; we don’t think you know
with 45% of family members identifying as Black or African how”), or using the child as a persistent distraction to focus on
American, 46% as White, 7% as multiracial, and 3% as another (e.g., ganging up on the child when he or she makes a mistake;
race. Approximately 15% of family members identified as Latinx. discussing concerns about the child’s performance or behavior as
The median household income of families was $36,100 per year if the child is not present). More mildly, this is reflected in
(range ⫽ $0 –$162,000), with most families (65%) receiving some situations where closeness between the parents occurs at the ex-
form of public assistance. The median education for parents con- pense of closeness with the child (e.g., the child withdraws from
sisted of a General Education Development (GED) or high school the task or is not engaged with by the parents). A key consideration
diploma (range ⫽ no high school diploma to master’s or PhD when coding detouring is that the parents must be actively sup-
degree). Of adult participants, 99% of mothers and 70% of their porting each other in an alliance against the child. Therefore,
partners were the biological parents of the child, 26% of partners detouring was not coded when parents were rejecting the child in
were mothers’ romantic partners but not the biological parent, 1% independent ways or if one parent was rejecting the child while the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

were the child’s grandparent, and 3% were another family mem- other was simply passive (e.g., mother but not partner engaged in
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ber; 90% lived together (mean [M] years lived together ⫽ 7.03, harsh or rejecting parenting behavior in the task). Two trained
SD ⫽ 4.64), and 49% were married. coders independently rated more than 20% of the videos at each
wave to assess interrater reliability. Intraclass correlation coeffi-
cients (ICCs) were .83 at Wave 1 and .75 at Wave 2.
Procedures and Measures
Although this coding system was used for the first time in the
Parents and children visited our research center laboratory for present study, findings from supplementary analyses support its
two annual waves of data collection. Children’s classroom teachers validity as an assessment of detouring. Specifically, we examined
also completed and mailed questionnaires about children’s behav- associations between our detouring measure and coparenting rela-
ior in kindergarten and first grade. The Research Subjects Review tionship dimensions in the tower-building task that were coded by
Board at the University of Rochester approved all scientific pro- a different team of independent raters. First, supporting convergent
cedures prior to conducting the study, and parents gave written validity and consistent with theoretical conceptualizations of de-
consent for both themselves and their child to participate. Parents touring (e.g., Kerig, 2016), higher ratings of detouring were sig-
and teachers were compensated monetarily for participation, and nificantly correlated with observational ratings of coparental col-
children received small toys at each visit. lusion (i.e., the degree to which the coparenting relationship is
Detouring. At Waves 1 and 2, families (mother, partner, and characterized by prioritizing the importance of the partner rela-
child) participated in a family interaction task (FIT) that was tionship at the expense of the parents’ relationship with the child;
adapted from previous studies (e.g., McHale, Kuersten-Hogan, & Parry & Davies, 2017; r ⫽ .58, p ⬍ .001). Second, supporting the
Lauretti, 2001). Families were instructed that they had 6 min to discriminant validity of our detouring code, detouring was unre-
work together to build a tower out of blocks. Experimenters told lated to what should be distinct coparenting relationship con-
families that a vertical marker affixed to the table represented the structs, including coparental warmth, competition, conflict, and
height of the “highest tower a family has ever built” and shared balanced participation in caregiving roles (all ps ⱖ .25).
that they would beat the “record” if they built their tower higher Family negativity and conflict. At Waves 1 and 2, the video-
than the marker. If the tower fell before time was up, they were recorded FITs were also coded using the Negativity and Conflict
instructed to rebuild it. To maximize the ability to capture families’ scale of the System for Coding Interactions and Family Function-
natural boundary patterns, additional instructions were limited to ing (SCIFF; Lindahl & Malik, 2000). The Negativity and Conflict
the request that the family work together to build the tower. Once scale assesses the overall negative tone or level of tension in the
an experimenter gave the instructions, the family was left alone to family and was coded on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (very low)
complete the activity. The task was video-recorded for subsequent to 5 (high). To be rated highly on the Negativity and Conflict scale,
coding. displays of negative affect (e.g., tension, anger, irritation) must be
Because very few studies have assessed detouring using obser- present throughout most (e.g., more than half) of the interaction
vational methods, we used prior family interaction coding systems and fairly intense in nature (e.g., raised voices; comments and
(Jacobvitz et al., 2004; Lindahl & Malik, 2000) as a guide in behaviors that convey annoyance, frustration, or hostility). More
developing a novel observational assessment of detouring during mildly, this code involves moments of tension, frustration, or anger
the FIT. Specifically, the Detouring scale assesses the degree to that are partially resolved, even if some lingering tension may
which the parents form an alliance against the child in a way that occur. The same two trained coders independently rated over 20%
functions to create distance between themselves and the child and of the videos at each wave to assess interrater reliability. ICCs
increase closeness in their own relationship. Using a 9-point scale were .88 at Waves 1 and 2.
ranging from 1 (not at all characteristic) to 9 (highly character- Child hostile attribution bias. To assess children’s hostile
istic), a family rated highly on detouring is characterized by attribution bias at Waves 1 and 2, a trained experimenter admin-
parents who engage in active and purposeful behaviors to exclude istered a structured interview with each child after the presentation
the child. Examples include purposefully ignoring the child and his of each of six peer-problem stories adapted from the Social Prob-
or her appropriate bids for attention (e.g., not responding to child’s lem Solving (SPS) vignettes (Dodge et al., 1995). Experimenters
constructive requests for help with the blocks or appeals to con- encouraged the child participants to pretend that they were the
tribute), belittling the child or the child’s contributions to the task protagonist in the stories (e.g., “Pretend that you are playing
(e.g., “Why would you put that there? You’re messing it up!”), outside at recess and some other kids are playing a game. You
818 COE, DAVIES, HENTGES, AND STURGE-APPLE

would really like to play the game with them, but they haven’t consistencies of the resulting scale were acceptable at both
asked you.”). Of the six vignettes, two depicted challenges enter- waves (internal consistency of two subscales within scale: ␣s ⫽
ing a group of peers with ambiguous intent (e.g., the child wants .70 at Wave 1 and .65 at Wave 2).
to play a game with other kids, but they have not invited him or her Covariates. Two demographic covariates, derived from a ma-
to play), two depicted provocation with an indication of negative ternal interview at Wave 1, were children’s sex (0 ⫽ females, 1 ⫽
peer intent (e.g., another child takes the child’s place in line), and males) and total household income per capita, calculated by divid-
two depicted transgressions with ambiguous peer intent (e.g., the ing the total annual household income by the number of individ-
child lets peers play with his or her favorite toy while he or she uals living in the home.
leaves the room for a few minutes, and the toy is broken when he
or she returns). Results
Children’s responses to three questions from the structured
interview were used to assess their hostile attribution bias in each
story. Specifically, children were first asked, “How come the
Descriptive Analyses
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

boy/girl [description of the specific peer event in the story]?” Table 1 provides the means, standard deviations, and intercor-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Children’s open-ended responses for each of the six stories were relations for the variables used in the primary analyses.
recorded verbatim for subsequent coding. Children also responded
with “yes” or “no” to the following two questions for each story:
Analytic Strategy for Primary Analyses
(a) “Are the kids being mean to you?” and (b) “Did the other kids
do it on purpose?” Children’s transcribed responses were coded by Prior to conducting our analyses, we examined whether rates of
two trained raters for hostile attributions using a 5-point scale missingness in the data set were associated with any of the 12
ranging from 1 (very benign) to 5 (very hostile) that incorporated study variables and covariates. Only three analyses were signifi-
the responses to all three questions. Ratings of 1 were reserved for cant: Higher rates of missingness were associated with peer rejec-
responses that uniformly and definitively conveyed that peer mo- tion and victimization at Wave 2 (r ⫽ .28, p ⬍ .001) and family
tives in the story were completely benign (e.g., [Why did they do negativity and conflict at Waves 1 (r ⫽ .17, p ⫽ .02) and 2 (r ⫽
it?] “It was an accident”; [Are they being mean to you?] “No”; .17, p ⫽ .04). Hypotheses were tested using autoregressive path
[Was it on purpose?] “No”). Conversely, ratings of 5 reflected analysis within a structural equation modeling (SEM) framework,
responses that consistently conveyed hostile or malevolent intent specified with Mplus Version 7.4 statistical software (Muthén &
on behalf of the peers (e.g., [Why did they do it?] “They hate me”; Muthén, 1998 –2012). To maximize the size of the sample, full-
[Are they being mean to you?] “Yes”; [Was it on purpose?] information maximum-likelihood estimation (FIML; Enders,
“Yes”). The two trained coders overlapped on 100% of the chil- 2001) was used to accommodate missing data (12.39% of values).
dren’s responses to assess interrater reliability. ICCs for each story The FIML method is regarded as the method of choice for esti-
were acceptable: mean ICC ⫽ .92 (range .90 –.95) at Wave 1; mating missing data because it minimizes bias in regression and
mean ICC ⫽ .90 (range ⫽ .82–.94) at Wave 2. The ratings for the standard error estimates for all types of missing data (i.e., missing
six stories were averaged into a single composite of hostile attri- completely at random [MCAR], missing at random [MAR], not
bution bias at each wave (␣s ⫽ .73 at Wave 1 and .69 at Wave 2). missing at random [NMAR]) when the amount of missing data
Child externalizing problems. Maternal report on the Ex- does not exceed 20% (Schlomer, Bauman, & Card, 2010).
ternalizing scale of the MacArthur Health and Behavior Ques- To address our primary analytic aim of examining the nature
tionnaire (HBQ; Ablow et al., 1999) was used to assess child and directionality of associations between family (i.e., detouring,
externalizing problems at Waves 1 and 2. The Externalizing negativity and conflict) and child (i.e., externalizing problems,
scale consisted of the average of three subscales on the HBQ: hostile attribution bias, peer rejection and victimization) variables
(a) Relational Aggression (mean of 6 items; e.g., “tells others over time, we specified an autoregressive path model using re-
not to play with or be a peer’s friend”), (b) Overt Hostility peated measures of all primary constructs across the two time
(mean of 4 items; e.g., “kicks, bites, or hits other children”), and points. All autoregressive paths between contiguous measurement
(c) Conduct Problems (mean of 12 items; e.g., “destroys things occasions were freely estimated, and structural paths were esti-
belonging to his/her family or other children”). Response alter- mated between (a) both family-level variables at Wave 1 and the
natives for each item were as follows: 0 (Never or not true), 1 three child variables at Wave 2 and (b) the three child variables at
(Sometimes or somewhat true), and 2 (Often or very true). The Wave 1 and the two family-level variables at Wave 2. Correlations
internal consistencies of the resulting scale were acceptable at were estimated among all exogenous (Wave 1) predictors and
both time points (internal consistency of three subscales within between residual error terms on all endogenous (Wave 2) out-
scale: ␣s ⫽ .80 at Wave 1 and .89 at Wave 2). comes. Given their potential roles as covariates, total household
Child peer rejection and victimization. Assessments of income per capita and child sex were included as covariates in the
peer rejection and victimization at Waves 1 and 2 were derived primary analyses. However, the results indicated that child sex was
from teacher reports on the Peer Relations scale of the HBQ. unrelated to any of the other variables in the model and did not
The scale consisted of the average of two subscales: Bullied by alter the pattern of significant results. Therefore, to maximize
Peers (mean of 3 items; e.g., “is picked on by other children”) parsimony, child sex was dropped from the model, and only
and Peer Acceptance/Rejection (mean of 8 items; e.g., “actively income was included as a covariate in the primary analyses. The
disliked by other children who reject him/her from their play”). resulting model had 8 degrees of freedom (i.e., 2 degrees of
Each item was rated along a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (Not freedom from a priori exclusion of cross-lagged paths between
at all like child) to 4 (Very much like child). The internal detouring and family conflict and 6 degrees of freedom from a
DETOURING AND ADJUSTMENT PROBLEMS 819

Table 1
Means, Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelations of the Main Variables

Variables M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

Covariates
1. Child sex .43 .50 —
2. Household income per capita 9.97 6.81 .09 —
Wave 1 (kindergarten)
3. Detouring 2.77 2.02 ⫺.02 ⫺.21ⴱ —
4. Negativity and conflict 2.71 1.36 .03 ⫺.26ⴱ .31ⴱ —
5. Hostile attribution bias 3.82 .86 .03 ⫺.02 ⫺.04 ⫺.06 —
6. Externalizing problems .22 .23 ⫺.00 ⫺.18ⴱ .11 .12 .02 —
7. Peer rejection and victimization 1.40 .48 ⫺.03 ⫺.23ⴱ .10 .13 ⫺.01 .30ⴱ —
Wave 2 (first grade)
8. Detouring 1.99 1.39 ⫺.06 ⫺.11 .32ⴱ .15 ⫺.05 .16ⴱ .17 —
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

9. Negativity and conflict 2.69 1.07 .09 ⫺.22ⴱ .24ⴱ .45ⴱ ⫺.12 .11 .11 .09 —
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

10. Hostile attribution bias 3.64 .76 ⫺.02 ⫺.05 .18ⴱ .11 .17ⴱ .05 .01 .02 .11 —
11. Externalizing problems .22 .27 ⫺.01 ⫺.14ⴱ .20ⴱ .17ⴱ ⫺.02 .77ⴱ .29ⴱ .29ⴱ .09 ⫺.04 —
12. Peer rejection and victimization 1.42 .47 ⫺.09 ⫺.17ⴱ .20ⴱ .02 ⫺.04 .26ⴱ .47ⴱ .28ⴱ .04 .03 .26ⴱ
Note. Child sex: 0 ⫽ females, 1 ⫽ males. Income in thousands.

p ⬍ .05.

priori exclusion of cross-lagged paths among the three child out- 1.00; SRMR ⫽ .02. For clarity, only significant correlations are
comes). Model fit was assessed using standard criteria, including depicted in Figure 1. Autoregressive paths from Wave 1 to Wave
chi-square (␹2) tests, root mean square error of approximation 2 were significant for detouring (␤ ⫽ .27, p ⫽ .001), negativity
(RMSEA), the comparative fit index (CFI), and the standardized and conflict (␤ ⫽ .43, p ⬍ .001), hostile attribution bias (␤ ⫽ .18,
root-mean-squared residual (SRMR). Acceptable fit values include p ⫽ .01), externalizing problems (␤ ⫽ .75, p ⬍ .001), and peer
a nonsignificant ␹2, RMSEA and SRMR of .05 or lower, and CFI rejection and victimization (␤ ⫽ .45, p ⬍ .001). Total household
of .90 or higher, with .95 and higher preferred (Hu & Bentler, income per capita did not predict changes in any of the child or
1999). family variables over time (all ps ⱖ .47).
The findings for the structural paths supported our hypotheses
Results of Primary Analyses
that detouring would predict increases in adjustment problems
The path model shown in Figure 1 provided an excellent fit with over time for children, over and above family negativity and
the data, ␹2 (8, N ⫽ 218) ⫽ 4.15, p ⫽ .84; RMSEA ⫽ .00; CFI ⫽ conflict. Specifically, higher ratings of detouring predicted chil-

W1 Detouring .27** W2 Detouring


.31***

.16*
W1 Family Negativity & Conflict .43*** W2 Family Negativity & Conflict .26**

W1 Child Hostile Attribution Bias .18* W2 Child Hostile Attribution Bias


-.15*

.11*

.75***
-.26***

W1 Child Externalizing Problems W2 Child Externalizing Problems


.19*

W1 Child Peer Rejection & .45*** W2 Child Peer Rejection &


Victimization Victimization

Total Household Income Per Capita

Figure 1. Autoregressive path model examining associations among detouring, family negativity and conflict,
child hostile attribution bias, externalizing problems, and peer rejection and victimization. Parameter estimates
for the structural paths are standardized path coefficients. Dashed lines indicate nonsignificant pathways. For
clarity, only significant correlations are shown. W1 ⫽ Wave 1 (kindergarten); W2 ⫽ Wave 2 (first grade).

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
820 COE, DAVIES, HENTGES, AND STURGE-APPLE

dren’s greater hostile attribution bias (␤ ⫽ .16, p ⫽ .04), exter- sequential because research has shown that these behaviors cohere
nalizing problems (␤ ⫽ .11, p ⫽ .03), and peer rejection and to a high-risk pattern of reactive aggression (Card & Little, 2006;
victimization (␤ ⫽ .19, p ⫽ .03) at Wave 2 after controlling for Dodge, 2006; Hubbard, McAuliffe, Morrow, & Romano, 2010).
Wave 1 autoregressive paths. Conversely, negativity and conflict Aggression that is reactive (e.g., defensive or “hot-blooded”; in-
did not predict increases in any of the child variables over time (all volves angry, emotionally dysregulated responses to perceived
ps ⱖ .34), and none of the child variables predicted increases in offenses or frustrations) serves as a potent predictor of psychoso-
detouring (all ps ⱖ .29) or family negativity and conflict over time cial adjustment problems across the life span (e.g., delinquency,
(all ps ⱖ .17). peer victimization, emotion dysregulation, attention-deficit/hyper-
activity disorder [ADHD] symptoms, internalizing problems, poor
social skills; Card & Little, 2006). In young children at high risk
Discussion
for reactive aggression, a strong hostile attribution bias often goes
Detouring in the family system has been theorized to predict a hand in hand with difficulties regulating behaviors and emotions
wide variety of negative adjustment outcomes for children (e.g., (e.g., externalizing problems; Choe et al., 2013; Shields & Cic-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Kerig, 1995, 2016; Minuchin, 1974), but very few studies have chetti, 1998; Teisl & Cicchetti, 2008). Because these children are
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

empirically examined these expected associations. To address this highly dysregulated, reactive, and aggressive, several studies have
significant gap in knowledge, our longitudinal, multimethod, mul- shown that they also tend to be bullied or rejected by their peers
tiinformant study tested whether detouring uniquely predicted in- (e.g., Card & Little, 2006; Schwartz et al., 1998). Over time, this
creases in children’s hostile attribution bias, externalizing prob- pattern of behaviors crystallizes into a stable profile of reactive
lems, and peer rejection and victimization during the transition aggression, and links between reactive aggression and adjustment
from kindergarten to first grade. The findings indicated that de- problems become stronger as children get older (Card & Little,
touring predicted increases in all three adjustment outcomes above 2006; Dodge, 2006; Hubbard et al., 2010). Therefore, because
and beyond a general construct of family negativity and conflict. detouring was linked with all three risk factors for reactive aggres-
Furthermore, in contrast to hypotheses proposed by child effects sion, these children may be at especially high risk for a wide array
models, none of the child variables predicted increases in either of difficulties across the life span.
detouring or negativity and conflict over time. However, it is also important to note that not all three child
Building on the few prior cross-sectional examinations of asso- outcomes in our study were related to one another. Although
ciations between detouring and child adjustment problems (e.g., externalizing problems and peer rejection and victimization were
Kerig, 1995; Yahav & Sharlin, 2002), our findings break new associated with each other at both time points (rs ⫽ .30 at Wave
ground in establishing detouring as a unique predictor of prospec- 1 and .26 at Wave 2), hostile attribution bias was not related to
tive changes in children’s hostile attribution bias, externalizing either outcome at either time point. Consistent with the concept of
problems, and peer rejection and victimization in early childhood. multifinality in developmental psychopathology (Cicchetti & Ro-
Speaking to its salience as a risk factor, detouring predicted all gosch, 1996), it is possible that detouring may lead to different
three adjustment outcomes, whereas the general construct of fam- outcomes for different children based on the context in which it
ily negativity and conflict did not. These findings support FST’s occurs and the variability in child, parent, and family characteris-
assertion that there is inherent value in identifying the relational tics. As one example, although detouring families all share the
function (i.e., purpose or motive based on the context of the function of increasing closeness in the interparental relationship at
interaction) underlying the organization and pattern of behaviors the expense of the relationship with the child, it may be expressed
within the family system (Combrinck-Graham, 1985, 1989; in somewhat different ways. In some cases, parents may actively
Minuchin, 1974). Whereas assessments of negativity and conflict exclude the child or be openly negative, hostile, and rejecting
capture physical forms of family behaviors (e.g., yelling, frustra- toward the child (e.g., scapegoating; Buchanan & Waizenhofer,
tion, anger), there could be multiple explanations or functions of 2001). However, detouring also occurs when parents achieve some
these behavioral displays, and they may not necessarily be directed degree of intimacy or closeness, albeit in a tenuous and, in many
at the child. However, detouring within the family specifically cases, artificial way, through excessive shared concerns about the
functions to increase closeness in the interparental subsystem at child’s well-being (e.g., fragilizing the child; Minuchin, 1974).
the expense of closeness with the child (Minuchin, 1974). There- Therefore, in other cases, it may be expressed as a shared concern
fore, the child is actively excluded from a key part of the family for the child and his or her distress (Kerig, 1995). Ultimately, this
system and denied resources provided by the family (Fauber et al., still creates closeness in the interparental relationship, and the
1990). This differs from parents’ engagement in coordinated co- parents tend to interact more with each other about the child’s
parenting, wherein parents display mutual support for one another behavior to the relative exclusion of the child in the interaction
as parents; respect one another’s authority; and create a structured, sequence. It could be that children’s increased hostile attribution
safe climate for the child (Feinberg, 2003). Whereas these behav- bias emerges only in cases when detouring is expressed in the
iors signify a supportive parental relationship and are generally former, rather than latter, behavioral pattern. Moreover, individual
linked with positive developmental outcomes for children (Fein- differences in child temperament (e.g., negative emotionality, in-
berg, 2003), the targeted rejection and exclusion experienced by hibitory control) and emotion regulation may also alter the ways in
the child in a detouring family may predict his or her adjustment which children are affected by detouring and how this influences
problems (Kerig, 1995, 2016; Minuchin, 1974). their adjustment outcomes. Alternatively, it is also possible that (a)
Our findings that detouring predicted increases in all three of the differences in the methods used to assess hostile attribution bias
outcomes (i.e., hostile attributions, externalizing, and peer victim- (i.e., coded responses to vignettes) versus the other two outcomes
ization and rejection) examined in this study are particularly con- (i.e., questionnaire) could explain the lack of a correlation, or (b)
DETOURING AND ADJUSTMENT PROBLEMS 821

at this relatively early developmental stage, the forms of adjust- In contrast to our primary hypothesis that detouring would
ment are still in the process of consolidating together and may predict increases in child adjustment problems over time, we did
emerge as a correlated suite of behaviors as children progress not find any empirical support for propositions of child-driven
through the elementary school years. Further research is needed to effects models that child behavior problems would serve as pre-
identify potential mechanisms and moderating factors that explain cursors of detouring. Our use of a multimethod, multiinformant
the variability in outcomes for children who develop in families measurement battery may partially explain the null results. Spe-
characterized by detouring. cifically, researchers have posited that parents’ perceptions of their
In addressing why detouring may have predicted child adjust- children’s behavior, rather than the behavior itself, predict how
ment problems in our study, it is possible that children’s appraisals they interact with their children (Webster-Stratton, 1990). There-
of themselves and their families may be key underlying mecha- fore, we may not have identified any child-driven effects on
nisms. Script theory asserts that children’s early experiences with detouring or negativity and conflict because two of the child
family relationships are reflected in their internal working models outcomes (hostile attribution bias, peer rejection and victimization)
of themselves and their families (Johnston et al., 2009). Therefore, were assessed using teacher reports and coder ratings of child
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

when these early relationships are disrupted or unsupportive, there responses to vignettes. Furthermore, it could be that bidirectional
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

may be negative repercussions for the ways children view them- relations between children’s behavior and detouring happen on a
selves (e.g., incompetent, worthy of blame, victim) and their moment-to-moment basis. Therefore, global assessments of adjust-
families (e.g., uncaring, unsupportive, volatile, threatening). Be- ment problems may not fully capture how parents and children
cause children tend to rely more heavily on prior representations as respond to one another in the moment to perpetuate a coercive
a way to simplify and comprehend challenging and novel inter- transactional process between child misbehavior and detouring in
personal contexts (Johnston et al., 2009), children from detouring the family. Microlevel coding systems designed to capture child
families who are repeated targets of parental rejection or exclusion behavior and reciprocal relations between parent and child behav-
may in turn draw on these internal representations in interpreting ior in the context of the task may reveal transactions not evident in
others’ actions as being aggressive and purposeful (i.e., hostile the data used in the current study (e.g., Elam & DiLalla, 2018).
attribution bias); struggle with regulating their anger and aggres- Alternatively, it is possible that bidirectional relations become
more pronounced when children get older. For example, many
sive behavior (i.e., externalizing problems); and seek out or struc-
studies demonstrating support for child-driven effects on family
ture interactions in other settings where they are also excluded,
functioning have been with samples of children in middle child-
rejected, or targeted (e.g., peer victimization and rejection). This
hood and adolescence (e.g., Cui, Donnellan, & Conger, 2007; Fite,
interpretation is consistent with findings from other studies that
Colder, Lochman, & Wells, 2006; Jenkins, Simpson, Dunn, Ras-
children’s internal representations of themselves or their families
bash, & O’Connor, 2005; Kerr & Stattin, 2003). However, studies
mediate associations between other indices of relationship discord
that test these types of models with younger children have not
(e.g., family instability, interparental conflict) and adjustment
consistently found such associations (e.g., Coe, Davies, Hentges,
problems (e.g., Coe, Davies, & Sturge-Apple, 2017, 2018).
& Sturge-Apple, 2020; Eisenberg et al., 2005). Further research
Research guided by the cognitive-contextual framework and
with more waves of data and alternative methods is needed to more
emotional security theory may also point to other potential under-
thoroughly examine potential bidirectional associations between
lying processes. For example, although not directly examined in detouring and child functioning.
relation to detouring, findings from studies examining adolescent The findings must also be interpreted in the context of our
triangulation into interparental conflict (e.g., cross-generational study’s limitations. First, although families in our study were from
alliances) identify children’s appraisals of self-blame for interpa- a variety of racial, ethnic, and socioeconomic backgrounds, cau-
rental conflict and emotional reactivity in the face of conflict as tion should be exercised in generalizing our results to families
mediators of associations between triangulation and psychological experiencing higher risk, families of high affluence, or families
adjustment problems (e.g., Buehler & Welsh, 2009; Fosco & with children in different developmental periods (e.g., middle
Grych, 2010). Extrapolated to our findings, it is possible that when childhood, adolescence). Second, the effect sizes of predictive
children repeatedly experience detouring in family interactions, pathways were relatively modest in magnitude, which points to
they may begin to blame themselves for family conflict and be high variability in the experiences of families and children who
much more reactive to conflict when it occurs. In turn, these experience detouring. It will be critical for future research to
cognitions and emotions may manifest in subsequent adjustment examine whether other parent (e.g., psychopathology), child (e.g.,
problems. Additionally, as a complementary interpretation of our temperament), and family (e.g., conflict) factors moderate these
results, coercive family process models posit that if aggressive associations. Third, the novel observational assessment of detour-
behavior is effective in defusing interparental conflict by deflect- ing utilized in our study may be regarded as a strength, but it will
ing attention onto the child, parents may inadvertently reinforce be important for future work to replicate and further expand on our
this behavior by engaging in detouring rather than arguing with findings. Fourth, exploring characteristics of the child (e.g., self-
one another (Davis, Hops, Alpert, & Sheeber, 1998). In turn, child blame, cognitive appraisals, internal representations, emotional
behavior problems continue and proliferate into other contexts reactivity) and parent (e.g., parenting difficulties, child-related
(e.g., at school, with peers). Because our study did not explicitly cognitions) as explanatory mechanisms for the effects of detouring
test intermediary processes in the predictive pathways, it will be on child outcomes is an important future direction for research.
important for future research to further explore what underlying Fifth, because our study was limited to only two waves, future
mechanisms account for associations between detouring and child research should more fully address how detouring in the family
adjustment problems. system may affect the potentially dynamic and reciprocal relation-
822 COE, DAVIES, HENTGES, AND STURGE-APPLE

ship between hostile attribution bias, externalizing problems, and victimization. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 44,
peer rejection and victimization over time. 367–379. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2013.873983
In addition to potential limitations of generalizability and con- Bolger, K. E., & Patterson, C. J. (2001). Developmental pathways from
ceptual considerations, it is also important to acknowledge the child maltreatment to peer rejection. Child Development, 72, 549 –568.
potential limitations of our measures and analytic approach. First, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00296
in the full analytic model, there was a negative correlation between Buchanan, C. M., & Waizenhofer, R. (2001). The impact of interparental
conflict on adolescent children: Considerations of family systems and
the residual error variance of Wave 2 hostile attribution bias and
family structure. In A. Booth, A. C. Crouter, & M. Clements (Eds.),
externalizing problems (r ⫽ ⫺.15). This could be the result of a
Couples in conflict (pp. 149 –160). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
method effect or another variable outside of the model having a Buehler, C., & Welsh, D. P. (2009). A process model of adolescents’
negative effect on one outcome and a positive effect on the other. triangulation into parents’ marital conflict: The role of emotional reac-
However, because (a) the variables are not related at a bivariate tivity. Journal of Family Psychology, 23, 167–180. http://dx.doi.org/10
level, (b) there is not a theoretically or empirically guided expla- .1037/a0014976
nation, and (c) the correlation is modest in magnitude, it is likely Buhs, E. S., & Ladd, G. W. (2001). Peer rejection as an antecedent of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

this inverse correlation is spurious. Second, although our study young children’s school adjustment: An examination of mediating pro-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

utilized measures that have been demonstrated to have strong cesses. Developmental Psychology, 37, 550 –560. http://dx.doi.org/10
psychometric properties in prior research, the internal consistency .1037/0012-1649.37.4.550
of our measures of hostile attribution bias and peer rejection and Card, N. A., & Little, T. D. (2006). Proactive and reactive aggression in
victimization in our sample was on the lower end of acceptable. It childhood and adolescence: A meta-analysis of differential relations
will therefore be critical for future research to replicate our find- with psychosocial adjustment. International Journal of Behavioral De-
ings in other samples of families and children. Similarly, because velopment, 30, 466 – 480. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025406071904
Choe, D. E., Lane, J. D., Grabell, A. S., & Olson, S. L. (2013). Develop-
all variables included in analytic models were manifest (i.e., we
mental precursors of young school-age children’s hostile attribution bias.
did not use any latent variables), our constructs still contain mea-
Developmental Psychology, 49, 2245–2256. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
surement error and can result in biased estimates. Future work with
a0032293
larger samples and more expansive measurement batteries for each Cicchetti, D., Cummings, E. M., Greenberg, M. T., & Marvin, R. S. (1990).
construct will be important to afford analyses using latent variables An organizational perspective on attachment beyond infancy: Implica-
with multiple indicators. Finally, although our use of autoregres- tions for theory, measurement, and research. In M. T. Greenberg, D.
sive cross-lagged modeling with repeated measures of all family Cicchetti, & E. M. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years:
and child constructs allowed for the examination of possible child- Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 3– 49). Chicago, IL: University
driven effects on family functioning, it will be critical for future of Chicago Press.
research to more definitively test whether bidirectional relations Cicchetti, D., Lynch, M., Shonk, S., & Manly, J. T. (1992). An organiza-
exist using multiple waves of data (i.e., three or more time points) tional perspective on peer relations in maltreated children. In R. D. Parke
and other analytic approaches (e.g., Berry & Willoughby, 2017; & G. W. Ladd (Eds.), Family-peer relationships: Modes of linkage (pp.
Hamaker, Kuiper, & Grasman, 2015). 345–383). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
In summary, the findings from our multimethod, multiinformant Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality in
longitudinal study indicated that detouring in the family system developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8,
597– 600. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400007318
uniquely predicted increases in young children’s hostile attribution
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (1998). The development of depression in
bias, externalizing problems, and peer rejection and victimization,
children and adolescents. American Psychologist, 53, 221–241. http://
over and above a general construct of family negativity and con-
dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.53.2.221
flict. The results support the developmental utility of FST bound- Coe, J. L., Davies, P. T., Hentges, R. F., & Sturge-Apple, M. L. (2020).
ary conceptualizations in understanding how characteristics of the Understanding the nature of associations between family instability,
broader family system affect child development. Applying FST unsupportive parenting, and children’s externalizing symptoms. Devel-
boundary constructs from family therapy to developmental re- opment and Psychopathology, 32, 257–269. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/
search designs has the potential to better inform interventions (e.g., S0954579418001736
Cowan & Cowan, 2006) and lead to a more in-depth understanding Coe, J. L., Davies, P. T., & Sturge-Apple, M. L. (2017). The multivariate
of the developmental implications of different family dynamics. roles of family instability and interparental conflict in predicting chil-
dren’s representations of insecurity in the family system and early school
adjustment problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45, 211–
References 224. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-016-0164-6
Ablow, J. C., Measelle, J. R., Kraemer, H. C., Harrington, R., Luby, J., Coe, J. L., Davies, P. T., & Sturge-Apple, M. L. (2018). Family instability
Smider, N., . . . Kupfer, J. D. (1999). The MacArthur Three-City and young children’s school adjustment: Callousness and negative in-
Outcomes Study: Evaluating multi-informant measures of young chil- ternal representations as mediators. Child Development, 89, 1193–1208.
dren’s symptomatology. Journal of the American Academy of Child & http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12793
Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 1580 –1590. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/ Coie, J., Terry, R., Lenox, K., Lochman, J., & Hyman, C. (1995). Child-
00004583-199912000-00020 hood peer rejection and aggression as predictors of stable patterns of
Berry, D., & Willoughby, M. T. (2017). On the practical interpretability of adolescent disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 697–713.
cross-lagged panel models: Rethinking a developmental workhorse. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400006799
Child Development, 88, 1186 –1206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev Cole, D. A., & Maxwell, S. E. (2003). Testing mediational models with
.12660 longitudinal data: Questions and tips in the use of structural equation
Bierman, K. L., Kalvin, C. B., & Heinrichs, B. S. (2015). Early childhood modeling. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 558 –577. http://dx.doi
precursors and adolescent sequelae of grade school peer rejection and .org/10.1037/0021-843X.112.4.558
DETOURING AND ADJUSTMENT PROBLEMS 823

Combrinck-Graham, L. (1985). A developmental model for family sys- Fite, P. J., Colder, C. R., Lochman, J. E., & Wells, K. C. (2006). The
tems. Family Process, 24, 139 –150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1545- mutual influence of parenting and boys’ externalizing behavior prob-
5300.1985.00139.x lems. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27, 151–164.
Combrinck-Graham, L. (1989). Family models of childhood psychopathol- http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2005.12.011
ogy. In L. Combrinck-Graham (Ed.), Children in family contexts: Per- Fosco, G. M., & Grych, J. H. (2010). Adolescent triangulation into parental
spectives on treatment (pp. 67– 89). New York, NY: Guilford Press. conflicts: Longitudinal implications for appraisals and adolescent-parent
Cowan, P. A. (1991). Individual and family life transitions: A proposal for relations. Journal of Marriage and Family, 72, 254 –266. http://dx.doi
a new definition. In P. A. Cowan & E. M. Hetherington (Eds.), Family .org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2010.00697.x
transitions (pp. 3–30). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Georgiou, S. N., & Stavrinides, P. (2013). Parenting at home and bullying
Cowan, P. A., & Cowan, C. P. (2006). Developmental psychopathology at school. Social Psychology of Education: An International Journal, 16,
from family systems and family risk factors perspectives: Implications 165–179. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11218-012-9209-z
for family research, practice, and policy. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen Hamaker, E. L., Kuiper, R. M., & Grasman, R. P. P. P. (2015). A critique
(Eds.), Developmental psychopathology (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 530 –587). of the cross-lagged panel model. Psychological Methods, 20, 102–116.
New York, NY: Wiley. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038889
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Cox, M. J., & Paley, B. (1997). Families as systems. Annual Review of Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Psychology, 48, 243–267. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1 structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struc-
.243 tural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
Cui, M., Donnellan, M. B., & Conger, R. D. (2007). Reciprocal influences 10705519909540118
between parents’ marital problems and adolescent internalizing and Hubbard, J. A., McAuliffe, M. D., Morrow, M. T., & Romano, L. J. (2010).
externalizing behavior. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1544 –1552. Reactive and proactive aggression in childhood and adolescence: Pre-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1544 cursors, outcomes, processes, experiences, and measurement. Journal of
Cummings, E. M., Koss, K. J., & Davies, P. T. (2015). Prospective Personality, 78, 95–118. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009
relations between family conflict and adolescent maladjustment: Secu- .00610.x
rity in the family system as a mediating process. Journal of Abnormal Jacobvitz, D., Hazen, N., Curran, M., & Hitchens, K. (2004). Observations
of early triadic family interactions: Boundary disturbances in the family
Child Psychology, 43, 503–515. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-014-
predict symptoms of depression, anxiety, and attention-deficit/
9926-1
hyperactivity disorder in middle childhood. Development and Psycho-
Davis, B. T., Hops, H., Alpert, A., & Sheeber, L. (1998). Child responses
pathology, 16, 577–592. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579404004675
to parental conflict and their effect on adjustment: A study of triadic
Jenkins, J., Simpson, A., Dunn, J., Rasbash, J., & O’Connor, T. G. (2005).
relations. Journal of Family Psychology, 12, 163–177. http://dx.doi.org/
Mutual influence of marital conflict and children’s behavior problems:
10.1037/0893-3200.12.2.163
Shared and nonshared family risks. Child Development, 76, 24 –39.
Demby, K. P., Riggs, S. A., & Kaminski, P. L. (2017). Attachment and
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00827.x
family processes in children’s psychological adjustment in middle child-
Johnston, J., Roseby, V., & Kuehnle, K. (2009). In the name of the child:
hood. Family Process, 56, 234 –249. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/famp
A developmental approach to understanding and helping children of
.12145
conflict and violent divorce. New York, NY: Springer.
Dodge, K. A. (2006). Translational science in action: Hostile attributional
Kerig, P. K. (1995). Triangles in the family circle: Effects of family
style and the development of aggressive behavior problems. Develop-
structure on marriage, parenting, and child adjustment. Journal of Fam-
ment and Psychopathology, 18, 791– 814. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/
ily Psychology, 9, 28 – 43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.9.1.28
S0954579406060391
Kerig, P. K. (2016). Family systems from a developmental psychopathol-
Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & Valente, E. (1995). Social ogy perspective. In D. Cicchetti (Ed.), Developmental psychopathology:
information-processing patterns partially mediate the effect of early Vol. 1. Theory and method (3rd ed., pp. 580 – 630). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
physical abuse on later conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Psy- Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2003). Parenting of adolescents: Action or reac-
chology, 104, 632– 643. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.104.4.632 tion? In A. C. Crouter & A. Booth (Eds.), Children’s influence on family
Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., Fabes, R. A., & Liew, dynamics: The neglected side of family relationships (pp. 121–151).
J. (2005). Relations among positive parenting, children’s effortful con- Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
trol, and externalizing problems: A three-wave longitudinal study. Child Ladd, G. W., & Burgess, K. B. (2001). Do relational risks and protective
Development, 76, 1055–1071. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624 factors moderate the linkages between childhood aggression and early
.2005.00897.x psychological and school adjustment? Child Development, 72, 1579 –
Elam, K. K., & DiLalla, L. F. (2018). Minute-to-minute trajectories of 1601. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00366
child unresponsiveness and parent sensitivity in parent– child interac- Lindahl, K. M., & Malik, N. M. (2000). The system for coding interactions
tions: The role of DRD4. Social Development, 27, 952–966. http://dx and family functioning. In P. Kerig & K. M. Lindahl (Eds.), Family
.doi.org/10.1111/sode.12308 observational coding systems: Resources for systemic research (pp.
Enders, C. K. (2001). A primer on maximum likelihood algorithms avail- 77–91). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
able for use with missing data. Structural Equation Modeling, 8, 128 – Loeber, R., Green, S. M., Lahey, B. B., Christ, M. A. G., & Frick, P. J.
141. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0801_7 (1992). Developmental sequences in the age of onset of disruptive child
Fauber, R., Forehand, R., Thomas, A. M., & Wierson, M. (1990). A behaviors. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 1, 21– 41. http://dx.doi
mediational model of the impact of marital conflict on adolescent .org/10.1007/BF01321340
adjustment in intact and divorced families: The role of disrupted par- McHale, J., Kuersten-Hogan, R., & Lauretti, A. (2001). Evaluating copa-
enting. Child Development, 61, 1112–1123. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/ renting and family-level dynamics during infancy and early childhood:
1130879 The coparenting and family rating system. In P. K. Keurig & K. M.
Feinberg, M. E. (2003). The internal structure and ecological context of Lindahl (Eds.), Family observational coding systems: Resources for
coparenting: A framework for research and intervention. Parenting, systemic research (pp. 147–166). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Science and Practice, 3, 95–131. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/ McHale, J. P., Kuersten, R., & Lauretti, A. (1996). New directions in the
S15327922PAR0302_01 study of family-level dynamics during infancy and early childhood. New
824 COE, DAVIES, HENTGES, AND STURGE-APPLE

Directions for Child Development, 1996, 5–26. http://dx.doi.org/10 Shonkoff, J. P., & Phillips, D. A. (Eds.). (2000). From neurons to neigh-
.1002/cd.23219967403 borhoods: The science of early childhood development. Washington,
Minuchin, S. (1974). Families and family therapy. Cambridge, MA: Har- DC: National Academy Press.
vard University Press. Sroufe, L. A., & Fleeson, J. (1986). Attachment and the construction of
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998 –2012). Mplus user’s guide (7th relationships. In W. Hartup & Z. Rubin (Eds.), Relationships and de-
ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Author. velopment (pp. 51–71). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Nelson, D. A., & Coyne, S. M. (2009). Children’s intent attributions and Sturge-Apple, M. L., Davies, P. T., & Cummings, E. M. (2010). Typolo-
feelings of distress: Associations with maternal and paternal parenting gies of family functioning and children’s adjustment during the early
practices. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 223–237. http:// school years. Child Development, 81, 1320 –1335. http://dx.doi.org/10
dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-008-9271-3 .1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01471.x
Parry, L. Q., & Davies, P. T. (2017). Coding scheme for coparenting in the Teisl, M., & Cicchetti, D. (2008). Physical abuse, cognitive and emotional
family interaction task. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester. processes, and aggressive/disruptive behavior problems. Social Devel-
Runions, K. C., & Keating, D. P. (2007). Young children’s social infor- opment, 17, 1–23.
mation processing: Family antecedents and behavioral correlates. De- Vuchinich, S., Emery, R. E., & Cassidy, J. (1988). Family members as
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

velopmental Psychology, 43, 838 – 849. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012- third parties in dyadic family conflict: Strategies, alliances, and out-
1649.43.4.838 comes. Child Development, 59, 1293–1302. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Schlomer, G. L., Bauman, S., & Card, N. A. (2010). Best practices for 1130492
missing data management in counseling psychology. Journal of Coun- Webster-Stratton, C. (1990). Stress: A potential disruptor of parent per-
seling Psychology, 57, 1–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018082 ceptions and family interactions. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology,
Schwartz, D., Dodge, K. A., Coie, J. D., Hubbard, J. A., Cillessen, A. H., 19, 302–312. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp1904_2
Lemerise, E. A., & Bateman, H. (1998). Social-cognitive and behavioral Yahav, R., & Sharlin, S. A. (2002). Blame and family conflict: Symptom-
correlates of aggression and victimization in boys’ play groups. Journal atic children as scapegoats. Child & Family Social Work, 7, 91–98.
of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 431– 440. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/ http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2206.2002.00231.x
A:1022695601088 Yaros, A., Lochman, J. E., & Wells, K. C. (2016). Parental aggression as
Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1998). Reactive aggression among maltreated a predictor of boys’ hostile attribution across the transition to middle
children: The contributions of attention and emotion dysregulation. school. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 40, 452– 458.
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27, 381–395. http://dx.doi.org/10 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025415607085
.1207/s15374424jccp2704_2
Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). Parental maltreatment and emotion
dysregulation as risk factors for bullying and victimization in middle Received July 8, 2019
childhood. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 30, 349 –363. http:// Revision received April 29, 2020
dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15374424JCCP3003_7 Accepted May 7, 2020 䡲

View publication stats

You might also like