You are on page 1of 15

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/51090228

Leaving the Parental Nest: Adjustment Problems,


Attachment Representations, and Social Support
During the Transition from High School to Military
Service

Article in Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology · May 2011


Impact Factor: 3.31 · DOI: 10.1080/15374416.2011.563464 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

8 34

3 authors, including:

Miri Scharf Ofra Mayseless


University of Haifa University of Haifa
54 PUBLICATIONS 1,038 CITATIONS 68 PUBLICATIONS 2,060 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All in-text references underlined in blue are linked to publications on ResearchGate, Available from: Ofra Mayseless
letting you access and read them immediately. Retrieved on: 13 April 2016
Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology

ISSN: 1537-4416 (Print) 1537-4424 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hcap20

Leaving the Parental Nest: Adjustment Problems,


Attachment Representations, and Social Support
During the Transition from High School to Military
Service

Miri Scharf , Ofra Mayseless & Inbal Kivenson-Baron

To cite this article: Miri Scharf , Ofra Mayseless & Inbal Kivenson-Baron (2011) Leaving the
Parental Nest: Adjustment Problems, Attachment Representations, and Social Support During
the Transition from High School to Military Service, Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent
Psychology, 40:3, 411-423, DOI: 10.1080/15374416.2011.563464

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2011.563464

Published online: 29 Apr 2011.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 349

View related articles

Citing articles: 5 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hcap20

Download by: [University of Haifa Library] Date: 18 November 2015, At: 10:31
Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 40(3), 411–423, 2011
Copyright # Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 1537-4416 print=1537-4424 online
DOI: 10.1080/15374416.2011.563464

Leaving the Parental Nest: Adjustment Problems, Attachment


Representations, and Social Support During the Transition
from High School to Military Service
Miri Scharf, Ofra Mayseless, and Inbal Kivenson-Baron
Counseling and Human Development, University of Haifa
Downloaded by [University of Haifa Library] at 10:31 18 November 2015

Adjustment to the transition from high school to military service in Israel was examined
in a longitudinal study with a sample of 120 late-adolescent girls. During their senior
year in high school (Time 1) the young women were administered the Adult Attachment
Interview. Their coping and adjustment to the new environment were assessed (at two
further points in time after the transition) by reports of the young women, their
mothers, their fathers, and their friends. Young women with preoccupied attachment
representations demonstrated the lowest levels of adjustment at both the second and
third time points. Young women with dismissing attachment representations did not dif-
fer from those with autonomous attachment representations. Satisfaction with social
support from parents mediated the association between attachment representations
and adjustment (assessed by the young women’s reports).

In most Western cultures late adolescence involves the or military service, which entail separation and the need
transition from high school to a new environment that to adjust to a new and different environment (Kenny &
requires more autonomous functioning (Seiffge-Krenke, Rice, 1995; Larose et al., 2005; Mikulincer & Florian,
2006). For many youngsters in most Western cultures it 1995; Scharf, Mayseless, & Kivenson-Baron, 2004).
means a move into the college environment (Bernier, Such transitions challenge the coping skills of young-
Larose, Boivin, & Soucy, 2004; Kenny & Rice, 1995; sters and might engender clinical adjustment problems
Larose, Bernier, & Tarabulsy, 2005). In the Israeli con- for some of them (Bernier, Larose, & Whipple, 2005).
text this normative transition is into the military milieu, In the present study we longitudinally investigated the
as 18-year-olds are expected to serve a mandatory 2- to potential contributions of state of mind with respect to
3-year period of military service. attachment to adolescent girls’ adjustment to the tran-
Individual differences in attachment representations sition from high school to military service.
are expected to be implicated in the way adolescents
cope with this important and central transition. Attach-
ment representations reflect the individuals’ internalized
INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN STATE OF
conceptions and affect regulation regarding their attach-
MIND WITH RESPECT TO ATTACHMENT
ment relations, in particular those with their parents.
These internalizations are assumed to guide the indivi-
Individual differences in attachment representations in
dual’s expectations, perceptions, emotion regulation,
adolescence and adulthood have been identified through
and functioning in situations that are attachment related
the Adult Attachment Interview (Hesse, 2008). Indivi-
(e.g., separation from attachment figures) and in stress-
duals with a secure or autonomous state of mind freely
inducing circumstances. Such are transitions to college
and coherently describe their relationships with their
parents; they value closeness and intimacy and find these
Correspondence should be addressed to Miri Scharf, Counseling
relationships influential in their lives (Hesse, 2008).
and Human Development, University of Haifa, Mount Carmel, Haifa, When coping with new challenges they are able to adapt
Israel. E-mail: scharfm@edu.haifa.ac.il more easily to the various stressors (Kobak & Sceery,
412 SCHARF, MAYSELESS, KIVENSON-BARON

1988; Zimmermann, 1999) because they are less over- et al., 2005). Individuals with preoccupied attachment
whelmed by the stress than others (Adam, Gunnar, & representations reported more fear of failure during
Tanaka, 2004; Seiffge-Krenke, 2006) and because they the first semester in college, felt less comfortable seeking
have better coping skills such as turning to others for help from their teachers, and gave lower priority to their
support (Allen et al., 2002; Seiffge-Krenke, 2006). studies. In another sample of academically at-risk
Three other categories are considered insecure: dis- students, a preoccupied tendency was related to a drop
missing, preoccupied, and unresolved. Individuals with in grades between high school and college, to a relative
dismissing attachment representations strive to limit decrease in adjustment from the beginning to the end of
the place of closeness in their life. They also idealize, the 1st year in college, and to lower levels of adjustment
derogate, or recall very few of their experiences with at the end of the year (Bernier et al., 2004). The profile
their parents (Hesse, 2008). When coping, they tend to of results for individuals with dismissing attachment
underactivate their attachment system (Dozier & representations was less consistent. In one study,
Kobak, 1992), which involves minimizing the perceived students with dismissing attachment representations
effect and relevance of the stressors while trying to rely reported less preparation for examinations and lower
on themselves more than on others when in need (Hesse, quality of attention and scored lower grades than their
2008). Individuals with preoccupied attachment repre- secure counterparts during the first three semesters in
Downloaded by [University of Haifa Library] at 10:31 18 November 2015

sentations describe their experiences with their parents college (Larose et al., 2005). However, in the second
incoherently in anger, passivity, or confusion. When sample, dismissing tendencies were not associated with
coping, they tend to hyperactivate the attachment sys- college adjustment (Bernier et al., 2004).
tem, hence they focus on the stressors (Creasey, 2002), Larose and Bernier (2001) further explored the role of
feel more anxious and depressed (Allen, Moore, social support during that transition. They found that a
Kuperminc, & Bell, 1998; Kobak, Sudler, & Gamble, preoccupied tendency was associated with higher stress
1991), and have a higher need for assistance, yet feel less and loneliness in college, distrust of potential suppor-
relieved and less satisfied than others with the support ters, and difficulty seeking help from teachers. Stress
they receive (Larose & Bernier, 2001). A fourth group and seeking help mediated the association between a
includes individuals who are unresolved with respect preoccupied tendency and loneliness. A dismissing tend-
to loss or trauma, and their functioning is expected to ency was associated with lower trust in potential suppor-
be worse than that of the other two insecure groups. ters at high school, seeking less help from friends and
teachers in college and peer-rated withdrawal. In sum,
both preoccupation and dismissing tendencies or states
ATTACHMENT AND ADAPTING TO of mind were associated with adjustment difficulties
THE COLLEGE TRANSITION (e.g., loneliness, withdrawal, anxiety, and lower grades)
and lower mobilization of social support.
The implications of individual differences in attachment
representations to the college transition have been exam-
ined in several studies. College students with secure TRANSITION FROM HIGH SCHOOL
attachment representations were rated by peers as more TO MILITARY SERVICE
ego-resilient, less anxious, and less hostile, and they
reported lower distress and higher levels of social support The high school and college contexts emphasize academ-
than others (Kobak & Sceery 1988). College students ic skills, and both are moderately open to autonomous
with dismissing attachment representations were not exploration and personal pursuits, the college environ-
different from their autonomous peers in their reported ment being even more inviting of such freedom than
distress, but their friends perceived them as more high school (Bowen, 1977). The transition into military
anxious, more hostile, and less ego-resilient than auton- service is quite different. The military context is a highly
omous peers. Nevertheless, students with dismissing demanding and rigid ecology, where the young recruits
attachment representations reported lower levels of lose some of their personal freedom and identity as they
social support from family than others. Students with must wear uniforms, obey orders, and perform duties
preoccupied attachment representations reported higher that often conflict with their personal desires. Young
levels of distress than others, and their peers also viewed soldiers are further exposed to new and challenging
them as more anxious and less ego-resilient than their physical and psychological demands (Scharf et al.,
autonomous counterparts. 2004). Military life may also restrict the opportunity to
Two studies targeted the academic domain in college. form and maintain relationships with close others such
Individuals with an autonomous (secure) attachment as parents and friends, as furloughs are infrequent and
representation showed better learning dispositions short. Yet military service also provides new challenges,
throughout the college transition than others (Larose such as meeting new people from diverse socioeconomic
LEAVING THE PARENTAL NEST 413

backgrounds and assuming responsibilities rarely higher levels of difficulties than other people, especially
assigned to adolescents at that age. The stressors inherent lower levels of social connectedness, this is not always
in the military environment might challenge individuals’ apparent in their own self-reporting (Kobak & Sceery,
flexibility, ego resilience, and social capacities and expose 1988). As for individuals with a preoccupied attachment
personal vulnerabilities as well as instigate difficulties representation, it has been suggested that their tendency
and clinical problems (Monson, Taft, & Fredman, 2009). to hyperactivation of the attachment system may impart
The transition to military service has been previously to them a biased view of themselves, which is not shared
examined in several studies (Israelashvili & Wegman- by others (Dozier & Lee, 1995; Larose & Bernier, 2001).
Rozi, 2007; Mayseless & Hai, 1998; Scharf et al., For example, early adolescents with a preoccupied
2004), showing in general that positive and autonomous attachment representations perceived themselves as
relations with parents are associated with better coping more socially rejected than was revealed in a sociometric
and adaptation. However, the ramifications of attach- procedure (Granot & Mayseless, 2001). Similarly,
ment representations for coping in this environment adolescent preoccupation was associated with levels of
have been examined in only one study with adolescent self-reported internalized and externalized symptoms
boys (Scharf et al., 2004). In that study individuals with higher than those reported by their parents (Berger
dismissing attachment representations reported using et al., 2005). Thus, our study engaged four distinct
Downloaded by [University of Haifa Library] at 10:31 18 November 2015

less problem-focused coping than did individuals with sources: the young women, peers, mothers, and fathers.
autonomous attachment representations, and their peers The inclusion of fathers as reporters is especially
in basic training perceived them as coping worse noteworthy and provides a viewpoint rarely seen in such
instrumentally and socially than their autonomous research.
counterparts. During this stressful transition they also
perceived their parents as less sensitively responsive to
them than did individuals with autonomous attachment ADJUSTMENT AND THE ROLE OF
representations (Scharf et al., 2004). SOCIAL SUPPORT
The adaptation and adjustment to military service
may be somewhat different for adolescent girls. First, The difficulties of individuals with insecure attachment
basic training for young women, as well as their service, representations persist and are salient even after the
is usually less arduous and demanding than that of initial adaptation and coping period in college (Larose
young men. Second, whereas for young men, socializa- et al., 2005). To examine adjustment problems over time
tion has been described as focusing on the development we assessed the young women’s initial adjustment 6 to
of individuation and separation, female socialization has 10 weeks after conscription, and then 6 to 9 months later
been described as focusing on connectedness (Josselson, when they had been in the new environment for some
1996). Young men and women differ in their vulner- time and already resided in their permanent bases. We
ability to stress, the social support that they receive, further examined the role of social support as experi-
and the coping strategies they use when confronting enced at the first period following the transition in
stress (Cummings, Davies, & Simpson, 1994; Scharf, mediating the association between attachment represen-
2001; Stroebe, 2001). Hence, leaving home may affect tations and problems in adaptation at the second
young women differently than men in terms of adjust- assessment in the military context. One of the main dis-
ment problems (Berman & Sperling, 1991). Examining advantages of individuals with insecure attachment
young women’s adaptation at this important turning representations is their difficulty to draw on their social
point could further our understanding of strengths and network for support or to be satisfied with this support,
vulnerabilities that might promote or hinder healthy and this interferes with their capacity to adjust to the
adaptations to developmental transitions. new environment (Larose & Bernier, 2001). In this study
we wanted to examine the mediating role of these
processes. In the college context, seeking help from
THE IMPORTANCE OF SEVERAL teachers regarding learning situations, but not from
SOURCES OF REPORT friends, mediated the link between preoccupation and
loneliness (Larose & Bernier, 2001). Because at this
How individuals with dismissing or preoccupied attach- developmental stage parents continue to serve as pri-
ment representations perceive their functioning may mary attachment figures for most emerging adults
differ somewhat from how others perceive it (Berger, (Scharf & Mayseless, 2007), we assessed support seeking
Jodl, Allen, McElhaney, & Kuperminc, 2005; Dozier & from parents in addition to support seeking from
Tyrrell, 1998). Individuals with dismissing attachment friends, and both were expected to be part of the young
representations are expected to downplay their difficult- women’s coping efforts, in such stressful situation as the
ies. As a result, although others perceive them as having transition to military service.
414 SCHARF, MAYSELESS, KIVENSON-BARON

CURRENT STUDY Procedure


Permission to conduct this investigation was provided
Based on this background literature, the following
by the university Institutional Review Board. Parti-
hypotheses were tested in the current study. First, young
cipants for the study were recruited over 2 consecutive
women with preoccupied attachment representations
years in two waves of data collection, with 60 families
were predicted to show the greatest emotional difficult-
participating in each wave. Prospective participants
ies and the highest levels of adjustment problems at
were identified and selected through published lists of
the two assessments following the transition as well as
high school graduates. Families were contacted by mail
reporting lowest satisfaction with social support.
and then by phone, informed about the research, and
Second, young women with dismissing attachment
asked for their cooperation. In the phone conversation
representations, who were expected to downplay their
we screened families to make sure that they met our
adjustment difficulties, were predicted to be similar to
research requirements (i.e., intact families, young
young women with autonomous attachment representa-
women indicated that they wanted to serve in the
tions in their self-reported emotional difficulties and
military). This constraint did not result in a highly
levels of adjustment but were predicted to employ less
skewed sample, as divorce rates are much lower in Israel
support seeking than others. Their lower level of
Downloaded by [University of Haifa Library] at 10:31 18 November 2015

than in the United States (12.1% according to the Sta-


adjustment was predicted to be revealed in reports by
tistical Abstract of Israel, 2008) and because more than
others (parents and friends). Third, the higher levels of
80% of the young women in these neighborhoods serve
adjustment problems in young women with insecure
in the military. Written parental consent and written
attachment representations at the second assessment of
adolescent assent to participate were obtained from all
adjustment to the military context (T3) were predicted
participants. Monetary compensation of $40 was offered
to be mediated through lower social support seeking
to each family and was paid at the end of the first assess-
(in individuals with dismissing attachment representa-
ment. The young women and the family also received
tions) and lower satisfaction with this support (in indivi-
small gifts. As the study required the investment of
duals with preoccupied attachment representations)
several hours by each family member, most families
during the initial period of military service (T2).
who declined did so because of time pressures. The final
sample reflects consent by 55% of eligible families.
At T1 the young women were administered the Adult
METHOD
Attachment Interview (AAI; George, Kaplan, & Main,
1985) and filled out questionnaires. The next assessment
Participants
(T2) took place 6 to 10 weeks after conscription,
One hundred twenty late-adolescent girls in Israel who approximately 3 to 8 months following T1. Timing of
were planning to start compulsory military service away conscription across the year is determined based on
from home were recruited during the spring semester of birth dates and the military needs. This point in time
their senior year in high school. The families were (several weeks following the transition) was chosen to
recruited from middle-class neighborhoods and were assess the young women’s initial adaptation to the tran-
largely well educated (74% of the fathers and 73% of sition. At this stage most young women have not yet
the mothers had at least a college education); 58% of been stationed in their permanent bases. Of the original
the families were of Western origin, and their living 120 adolescent girls, 110 participated and filled out ques-
quarters were of moderately low density (1.17 persons tionnaires regarding their adjustment and social
per room). support. We also collected information on the young
At the time of the first assessment (T1), during the women’s adjustment from their mothers (n ¼ 83), their
second semester of the young women’s senior year in fathers (n ¼ 82), and their friends from home (n ¼ 74).
high school, their ages were 17 to 18 years. Mean age The young women were requested to provide the name
of mothers was 46.40 years (SD ¼ 4.40) and of fathers and phone number of their best friend from home who
was 48.99 years (SD ¼ 5.33). The number of children in knew them well and would agree to participate. The
these families varied between one and five, with a mean mean length of the friendship was 319.88 weeks
of 2.87 (SD ¼ .70). About one third of the adolescents (SD ¼ 22.36). Reasons for nonparticipation in this
(32%) were firstborn children. Approximately 72% of assessment included difficulty locating the young
the families described themselves as secular, and the women, who came home for very brief furloughs, mostly
remainder as upholding the Jewish religious tradition, with only short notice to the family, and the limited time
but not in the orthodox way. None of these back- for contacting the participants, as the study design
ground variables was associated with the research required that this be done within the first several weeks
variables. following conscription.
LEAVING THE PARENTAL NEST 415

The third assessment (T3) took place 6 to 9 months to assess adolescents’ emotional difficulties on two
following T2. This point in time was chosen because scales: Distress (10 items, e.g., feeling depressed, lonely,
by this time the young women were already residing in nervous, anxious; a ¼ .88) and Well-Being (10 items,
their permanent placements and had had the time to e.g., ‘‘happy, satisfied, or pleased with personal life’’;
adapt to this. One hundred fifteen adolescents partici- a ¼ .90). The young women were asked to answer each
pated by filling out questionnaires regarding their item using a 1 (never) to 6 (all the time) scale, in reference
adjustment. Of these women, 77 resided at the military to their feelings over the previous 2 weeks. The measure
base and went home for short furloughs, and 38 slept has shown high internal reliability and good test–retest
at home and commuted daily to their base. At T3, 113 reliability, as well as construct and discriminant validity
mothers and 115 fathers participated, and 108 adoles- (Florian & Drory, 1990). Due to the high intercorrela-
cents had their friends (mean length of friendship ¼ tion between the scales (r ¼ .84), we constructed one
360.41 weeks, SD ¼ 21.20) participate in the study. The general emotional difficulties scale by averaging across
subsamples of women, mothers, fathers, and friends the two scales, after reversing the scores of the Well-
who participated in all assessments and of those who Being scale.
participated only in some as well as the samples from
the two waves of data collection showed no differences
Downloaded by [University of Haifa Library] at 10:31 18 November 2015

Adjustment to military service. The Student Adap-


on any of the research variables. Similarly, differences
tation to College Questionnaire (SACQ; Baker & Siryk,
in timing of the T2 and T3 assessments were not associa-
1984), which was adapted to the military context
ted with any of the study variables.
(Wintre & Ben-Knaz, 2000) was filled out by the young
women to assess their adjustment to the specific context
Measures: Time 1 of military service. The questionnaire contains 67 state-
ments pertaining to various aspects of adjustment:
Attachment. The AAI (George et al., 1985) is a
general adjustment to military environment (‘‘I know
semistructured interview that asks interviewees to pro-
why I’m in the military and what I want out of it’’;
vide semantic and episodic descriptions of their early
a ¼ .94), social adjustment (‘‘I have several close social
childhood relationships with their primary caregivers.
ties in the military’’; a ¼ .92), personal-emotional adjust-
During the course of the interview, interviewees are also
ment (‘‘Being on my own, taking responsibility for
asked about separations, losses, and their current rela-
myself, has not been easy’’; a ¼ .86), and institutional
tionships with their caregivers. The scoring procedures
commitment (‘‘On balance, I would rather be home than
for the AAI included ratings of probable childhood
here’’; a ¼ .90). Its validity has been established in many
experiences and current state of mind, and assignment
studies (e.g., Gerdes & Mallinckrodt, 1994), including in
of a participant to one of four classifications reflecting
the military context (Wintre & Ben-Knaz, 2000). Due to
their state of mind with respect to attachment:
the high intercorrelations among the scales (ranging.
autonomous, dismissing, preoccupied, and unresolved.
64–.87), we constructed one global adjustment to the
Participants in the unresolved group are also placed in
military scale by averaging across the four scales.
one of the foregoing categories as alternatives.
The AAI has proven to be a highly reliable measure
that shows strong concurrent, predictive, and discrimi- Social support. The Social Support Questionnaire
nant validity (see a review by Hesse, 2008). Interviews (Barrera, 2000) was filled out by the young women. It
were tape-recorded and transcribed verbatim and then contains 10 items referring to tangible, emotional, and
coded by the first and the third authors, who are reliable informational support (e.g., an item referring to the
and certified coders trained by Main and Hesse (first emotional domain: ‘‘Sometimes we want encouragement
author), and Jacobvits and Pederson (third author). and reassurance to help us manage or deal with a specific
The coders neither interviewed the participants nor situation’’). For each item we first asked the young
had any information regarding the family. The AAI women how often during the foregoing month they
transcripts were identified by special code numbers only. wanted to get such support (a ¼ .78). In addition, for each
Twenty-two interviews were dual coded, and coders item they were requested to report how often they turned
agreed on 91.1% of the major classifications (j ¼ .70, to mother, father, and best friend (a ¼ .89, .89, and .85,
p < .001). Disagreements were negotiated through joint respectively) and how satisfied they were with the sup-
discussion. port provided (a ¼ .90, .91, and .89, respectively). The
questionnaire has been used extensively in past research,
which has supported its reliability and validity (Barrera,
Measures: Time 2
1986; Finch et al., 1997). Although the correlations
Self-report of emotional difficulties. The Mental between turning to support and satisfaction with the
Health Inventory (MHI; Veit & Ware, 1983) was used support provided by each figure were high (.65–.69),
416 SCHARF, MAYSELESS, KIVENSON-BARON

we retained these as separate variables because of distinct (a ¼ .79, .83, .73, respectively), and social problems
expected differences for the different attachment groups. (a ¼ .77, .81, .78, respectively). The intercorrelations
However, as the correlations between scores related to within each informant were high (.63–.76 for mothers,
mother’s and father’s support were high (.73–.85), we .72–.81 for fathers, and .50–.76 for peers), therefore
computed one score for turning to parental support for data reduction purposes we constructed one global
and another for satisfaction from parental support, adjustment problems scale for each informant, aver-
averaging across mothers’ and fathers’ scales. aging across the three scales. Intercorrelations among
reporters were .69 for mother–father, .50 for mother–
friend, and .60 for father–friend. The associations
Parents’ and peers’ report of adjustment. Mothers,
between young women’s reports on their adjustment to
fathers, and peers answered 20 questions drawn from the
the military and reports by others were also significant
adjustment measure (SACQ) and the MHI with regard
ranging between .51 and .64.
to the previous month: young women’s depression=
distress (nine items, e.g., ‘‘My daughter=friend has been
feeling tense or nervous lately’’; a ¼ .92, .94, .90, respec- Data Analyses
tively, for mothers, fathers, and peers), malfunctioning
We conducted several repeated measures multivariatae
Downloaded by [University of Haifa Library] at 10:31 18 November 2015

(five items, e.g., ‘‘She is finding her role in the army


difficult’’; a ¼ .68, .72, .78, respectively), and social pro- analyses of variance (MANOVAs) across Time 2 and
blems (six items, e.g., ‘‘She has been having difficulty Time 3 assessments to examine differences between state-
feeling at ease with other people in the military’’; of-mind groups. When significant, these MANOVAs
a ¼ .72, .74, .72, respectively). Due to the high intercorre- were followed by a priori contrasts reflecting our hypoth-
lations among the scales of each reporter (.52–.77 for eses. We further examined the effects of commuter=
mothers, .62–.82 for fathers, and .63–.66 for peers), we residential status using two-way MANOVAs. Finally,
constructed one global adjustment problems scale for we examined social support at Time 2 as a mediator of
each reporter by averaging across the three scales. Inter- the association between attachment representations and
correlations among reporters were .67 for mother– adjustment at Time 3 using hierarchical regression
father, .44 for mother–friend, and .26 for father–friend. analyses. When time was significant in the repeated
The associations between the young women’s reports measures MANOVA, we conducted these hierarchical
on their adjustment to the military and reports by others regression analyses controlling for the respected measure
were also significant, ranging from .41 to .50. of adjustment at Time 2.

Measures: Time 3 RESULTS


Self-report of adjustment. The young women filled
the same adjustment questionnaires that they completed Descriptive Analyses: Time 1 Attachment
at T2: The MHI (a ¼ .93 for well-being and a ¼ .93 for Seventy (58.3%) of the adolescents were classified as
distress) and the SACQ to assess military adjustment: autonomous, 38 (32.5%) as dismissing, and 12 (10%) as
general adjustment to military environment (a ¼ .94), preoccupied with respect to attachment. Due to the small
social adjustment (a ¼ .92), personal-emotional adjust- size of the unresolved group (five adolescents were cate-
ment (a ¼ .86), and institutional commitment (a ¼ .90). gorized as unresolved, with three having preoccupied
As in the previous assessment, due to the high intercorre- subclassification and two an autonomous subclassifica-
lation between the MHI scales (r ¼ .84), we constructed tion), we did not analyze this group separately. Instead
one general emotional difficulties scale by averaging we used their alternative subclassification, resulting in
across the two scales, after reversing the scores on the three major state-of-mind groups (see earlier). This
Well-Being scale. The intercorrelations among the distribution is similar to those in other nonclinical
SACQ scales ranged between .56 and .86 and, as in samples across different cultures (van IJzendoorn &
the previous assessment, we constructed one adjustment Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2010).
to the military scale for Time 3, averaging across the
four scales.
Adjustment Over Time
Parents’ and peers’ reports of adjustment. Mothers, The 2 (time)  3 (attachment groups) repeated measures
fathers, and peers answered the same adjustment ques- MANOVA for young women’s reports of general
tionnaires that they answered at T2: young women’s emotional difficulties (MHI) and adjustment to the
depression=distress (a ¼ .93, .94, .92, for mothers, military (SACQ) revealed a significant main effect of
fathers, and friends, respectively), malfunctioning time, F(2, 103) ¼ 7.80, p < .001, g2 ¼ .13, with higher
LEAVING THE PARENTAL NEST 417

TABLE 1
Young Women’s State of Mind With Respect to Attachment and Their Adaptation to Military Service—Time 2 and Time 3

Autonomous (1)a Preoccupied (2)b Dismissing (3)c

Self-Report M SD M SD M SD F Contrasts

Adjustment to Military—Time 2 6.83 1.36 6.22 1.81 6.87 1.12


Adjustment to Military—Time 3 6.72 1.19 5.46 1.65 6.52 1.42
Main Effect Adjustment 2.75y g2 ¼ .05 2 < 1,3
General Emotional Difficulties (MHI)—Time 2 2.46 .83 2.81 1.24 2.33 .64
General Emotional Difficulties (MHI)—Time 3 2.43 .81 3.03 .87 2.28 .82
Main Effect General Emotional Difficulties (MHI) 2.87y g2 ¼ .05 1,3 < 2
Mothers’ Reportd
Adjustment Problems—Time 2 2.45 1.13 3.83 1.50 2.30 1.46
Adjustment Problems—Time 3 2.36 1.23 4.29 1.96 2.43 1.46
Main Effect Adjustment Problems 6.68 g2 ¼ .14 2 > 1,3
Fathers’ Reporte
Adjustment Problems—Time 2 2.67 1.44 3.85 1.31 2.69 1.37
Adjustment Problems—Time 3 2.56 1.32 3.85 1.58 2.56 1.62
Downloaded by [University of Haifa Library] at 10:31 18 November 2015

Main Effect Adjustment Problems 3.45 g2 ¼ .04 2 > 1,3


Peers’ Report f
Adjustment Problems—Time 2 2.23 1.01 3.45 1.53 2.62 1.14
Adjustment Problems—Time 3 2.46 1.08 4.35 1.88 2.73 1.19
Main Effect Adjustment Problems 7.50 g2 ¼ .18 2 > 1,3

Note: MHI ¼ Mental Health Inventory.


a
n ¼ 67.
b
n ¼ 10.
c
n ¼ 30.
d
n ¼ 51, 7, 25.
e
n ¼ 50, 7, 25.
f
n ¼ 48, 7, 19.
y
p < .10.  p < .05.  p < .01.  p < .001.

difficulties in Time 3 than in Time 2. Follow-up analyses reports revealed a significant main effect of state of mind
of variance revealed that the time effect was significant with respect to attachment, F(2, 79) ¼ 3.45, p < .05,
only for the adjustment to the military variable. In g2 ¼ .08. Planned contrasts revealed that young women
addition, the effect of the young women’s state of mind with preoccupied state of mind were perceived by their
with respect to attachment approached significant, fathers as having more adjustment problems than their
F(2, 104) ¼ 2.88, p < .06, g2 ¼ .05. Follow-up analyses counterparts (p < .05). Time and Time  Attachment
of variance for general emotional difficulties and adjust- Group interaction were not significant. Finally, the
ment to the military also approached significance, with MANOVA for reports by the young women’s friends
planned contrasts revealing that the preoccupied group was also significant, F(2, 67) ¼ 7.50, p < .001, g2 ¼ .18.
was significantly different from the autonomous and Planned contrasts showed that young women with pre-
the dismissing groups (p < .05), which did not differ from occupied representations were perceived by their friends
one another. The interaction between time and attach- as having more adjustment problems than young
ment groups was not significant (see Table 1). women with autonomous representations (p < .05; see
The 2 (time)  3 (attachment groups) repeated Table 1). Again, time and Time  Attachment Group
measures MANOVA conducted to examine mothers’ interaction were not significant.1
reports regarding the young women’s adjustment to
the military revealed a significant main effect of the 1
The repeated measures analyses included only those participants
young women’s state of mind with respect to attach- who answered both Time 2 and Time 3 questionnaires. Because of
ment, F(2, 80) ¼ 6.68, p < .01, g2 ¼ .14. Time and the relatively reduced number of participants in Time 2, these analyses
included a smaller sample than that for Time 3. Therefore we also
Time  Attachment Group interaction were not signifi- conducted separate one-way analyses with attachment groups as the
cant. Follow-up planned contrasts demonstrated that independent variable to examine Time 3 adjustment by the various
young women with preoccupied state of mind were per- reporters. In all these analyses, except for fathers’ report, which
ceived by their mothers as exhibiting more problems approached significance, state of mind with respect to attachment
than their counterparts (p < .05). Similarly, the repeated was significant and planned contrasts showed that young women with
preoccupied representations reported more adjustment problems than
measures MANOVA conducted to examine fathers’ young women with autonomous and dismissing representations.
418 SCHARF, MAYSELESS, KIVENSON-BARON

TABLE 2
Young Women’s State of Mind With Respect to Attachment and Social Support—Time 2

Autonomous (1)a Preoccupied (2)b Dismissing (3)c

M SD M SD M SD F Contrasts

Wanting Support 3.83 .59 3.14 1.08 3.68 .60 5.04 g2 ¼ .09 2<1
Turning for Support to Parents 3.61 .85 2.82 1.24 3.57 .76 3.83 g2 ¼ .07 2 < 1,3
Satisfaction With Support—From Parents 4.19 .85 3.32 1.33 4.26 .72 4.89 g2 ¼ .08 2 < 1,3
Turning for Support—to Friend 3.62 .81 2.94 1.02 3.63 .81 3.08 g2 ¼ .06 2 < 1,3
Satisfaction With Support—From Friend 4.11 .84 3.49 1.35 4.21 .89 2.48y g2 ¼ .05 2 < 1,3
a
n ¼ 68.
b
n ¼ 10.
c
n ¼ 31.
y
p < .10.  p < .05. 
p < .01.

The MANOVA of social support with regard to par- support (the mediator) was associated with the predictor
Downloaded by [University of Haifa Library] at 10:31 18 November 2015

ents and friends was significant, F(5, 102) ¼ 3.20, p < .01, (adjustment). Satisfaction with parents’ support at Time
g2 ¼ .14. Differences were evident with regard to the 2 was significantly associated with all the Time 3 indica-
amount of support the young women wanted from tors of adjustment that were significantly predicted by
others, how much they turned to parents and friend for the AAI: the young women’s own reports on general
support, and how satisfied they were with the support emotional difficulties(r ¼ .46) and adjustment to the
provided by parents. Satisfaction with friend’s support military (r ¼ .46); mothers’ reports regarding adjustment
only approached significance (see Table 2). Planned con- problems (r ¼ .33); fathers’ reports regarding adjust-
trasts revealed that young women with preoccupied ment problems (r ¼ –.30); and peers’ reports regarding
attachment representations reported wanting less sup- adjustment problems (r ¼ .29). Satisfaction with peers’
port than their autonomous counterparts and reported support was also significantly associated with most Time
turning less to their parents and friends for support 3 adjustment scores: the young women’s own reports on
and being less satisfied with the support received from general emotional difficulties (r ¼ .38) and adjustment
parents and from friends than were their autonomous to the military (r ¼ .36), fathers’ reports regarding
and dismissing counterparts (all comparisons, p < .05). adjustment problems (r ¼ .22), and mothers’ reports
Because of the possibility that commuter versus resi- regarding adjustment problems (r ¼ .25). However,
dential status (C-R) at Time 3 would affect the young the analyses with friends’ reports regarding adjustment
women’s adjustment, and the additional possibility that problems was not significant. Consequently, following
their adjustment at Time 2 could affect their C-R status, our hypothesis for the preoccupied group regarding
several analyses were run where AAI groups and C-R the mediating role of satisfaction from support, five
status served as independent variables and adjustment hierarchical regression analyses were conducted for
and social support indicators as dependent variables. predicting Time 3 adjustment. All of these analyses
The C-R status was not associated with any of the included attachment representations in the first step
variables we examined, nor was there any significant and the two mediators, satisfaction from parents’ sup-
interaction with AAI state-of-mind groups. port and satisfaction from peers’ support in the second
step. This did not include friends’ reports regarding
adjustment problems, where only satisfaction from
Examining the Social Support as a Mediator
parents’ support at T2 could serve as a mediator and
in Predicting Adjustment
was therefore the only one included in Step 2.
The third hypothesis, related to the role of social The hierarchical regression analyses predicting the
support, was tested by hierarchical regression analyses. young women’s reports showed support for mediation.
Following Baron and Kenny (1986), we first regressed Specifically, with the inclusion of the mediators (satisfac-
the criterion variable (adjustment Time 3) on the predic- tion of support from parents and from peers) in the
tor variable (preoccupied vs. others) and then regressed regression models, the b coefficients of the AAI signifi-
the criterion variable on both the predictor and the cantly decreased or became nonsignificant, and the b
mediator (i.e., social support). This was done for all coefficient of at least one of the mediators was significant
adjustment variables at Time 3. (see Table 3). None of the other regression analyses
Following Baron and Kenny (1986) suggestions showed a marked decrease in b coefficients of the AAI
regarding the assessment of mediation, we examined coupled with significant b coefficient of at least one of
whether satisfaction with parents’ and with peers’ the mediators. Application of the Sobel test (MacKinnon,
LEAVING THE PARENTAL NEST 419

TABLE 3 seen in Table 3, when adjustment to the military at


Hierarchical Regression Analyses—Prediction of Time 3 Adjustment Time 2 was entered in the first step in the hierarchical
With Satisfaction With Social Support as Mediator
regression, AAI was still a significant predictor. This
Step and Predictors – Time 3 b DF df DR2 finding demonstrates that even after accounting for
adjustment problems at Time 2, preoccupied state of
Adjustment to Military (Young
mind still contributed to the deterioration in adjustment
Women’s Report)
First Step 7.50 1,104 .07 of the young women at Time 3. Satisfaction with parents’
Attachment Representations .26 support (z ¼ 2.14, p < .05) also mediated this effect.
Second Step 9.75 2,102 .15
Attachment Representations .13
Satisfaction With Support by .29
DISCUSSION
Parents – Time 2
Satisfaction With Support by .17
Friends – Time 2 The results of our study revealed a fairly consistent
Final Model 9.42 3,102 .22 picture. In general, young women with preoccupied
Adjustment to Military (Young representations demonstrated the lowest levels of adjust-
Women’s Report) Controlling
ment to the military context. Following the young
Downloaded by [University of Haifa Library] at 10:31 18 November 2015

for Time 2 Adjustment to


Military women’s transition to the military service (Time 2), as
First step 55.13 1,104 .35 well as in their permanent placements during their
Adjustment to Military – Time 2 .59 military service (Time 3), fathers, mothers, and peers
Second Step 5.54 1,103 .03 of preoccupied young women identified problems in
Adjustment to Military – Time 2 .56
their adjustment. The young women themselves also
Attachment Representations .18
Third Step 4.10 2,101 0.5 reported more adjustment problems and more emotion-
Adjustment to Military – Time 2 .50 al difficulties, although this self-report effect approached
Attachment Representations .12 significance only at Time 2. The fact that these difficult-
Satisfaction With Support by .22 ies were identified by people close to the young women
Parents – Time 2
suggests that it was not only a subjective evaluation by
Satisfaction With Support by .03
Friends – Time 2 the young women but a difficulty discernable and
Final Model 18.77 4,101 .43 observable by others as well. This attests to the consist-
General Emotional Difficulties ency, and perhaps the salience, of the difficulty. The
(Young Women’s Report) fathers’ perspective is especially noteworthy, as fathers
First Step 5.57 1,104 .05
have rarely been included as reporters of the functioning
Attachment Representations .23
Second Step 9.31 2,102 .15 of their adolescent or emerging adult offspring.
Attachment Representations .11 In line with studies that examined the transition to
Satisfaction With Support .23 college (Larose et al., 2005), we too found long-term dif-
From Parents – Time 2 ficulties. The lower functioning and poorer emotional
Satisfaction With Support by .23
adjustment was evident during the first stressful weeks
Friends – Time 2
Final Model 8.36 3,102 .21 of the transition but also several months after the young
women were stationed at their permanent base camps.
Note: Attachment representations were coded 0 ¼ nonpreoccupied, Those with preoccupied representations seem not to
1 ¼ preoccupied.

have adapted well to the new environment even in the
p < .05.  p < .01.  p < .001.
long run. In fact, using their self-reports regarding their
adjustment to the military, the young women’s adjust-
Warsi, & Dwyer, 1995) to examine whether satisfaction ment and adaptation compared with others was poorer
with support at Time 2 extended the influence of the at Time 3 than at Time 2. It might be the case that in the
AAI to the adjustment variables (indirect pathway), context of the general difficulties experienced by others
showed that the Sobel test was significant for satisfaction in the stressful situation at Time 2, the neediness and
with parents’ support in young women’s own reports anxiety of the preoccupied young women might not
regarding adjustment: general emotional difficulties have been so salient, at least for them. However in the
(z ¼ 2.44, p < .01) and adjustment to the military (z ¼ longer run, when the anxiety of others probably sub-
2.54, p < .02). However, the Sobel test was not signifi- sided, that of the preoccupied young women became
cant for satisfaction of support from peers. clearer and more upsetting to them.
Because the effect of time was significant for the Another possibility is related to the quality of
young women’s reports regarding their adjustment to relationships of preoccupied young women with their
the military, we also ran the hierarchical regression parents. Preoccupied adolescents often evince higher
analysis controlling for Time 2 adjustment. As can be levels of conflict with their parents than others do
420 SCHARF, MAYSELESS, KIVENSON-BARON

(Bernier et al., 2005). Leaving such a home to a new not the reports by others. For example, lower capacities
environment with its challenges and stimulation may for affect regulation, such as being easily frustrated,
have been a fairly relieving experience for these young discouraged, saddened, or angry, may result in lower
women. With time, however, and as the excitement of levels of instrumental and social functioning. A tendency
the new experiences declined, their problematic emotion to ruminate on negative experiences and discount positive
regulation strategies surfaced and their lower function- experiences may also lead to a lower capacity to socialize
ing compared with others became clearer to them. or to concentrate on the required tasks. Cassidy (2000)
In line with this latter interpretation young women suggested that a history of hyperactivation of
with preoccupied attachment representations reported attachment-related stimuli could lead to major difficulty
wanting less support at Time 2 and turning less to their in focusing on other aspects of the environment that
parents and their friends for support at Time 2. They require considerable attention (Dykas & Cassidy, 2007).
further reported lower levels of satisfaction with parents’ These possible mediating variables should be examined
support at Time 2. Despite being perceived by close in future research.
others as more stressed, young women with preoccupied These results correspond well with findings on
representations opted to turn for support less than the transition from high school to college, where
others, and in particular they were less satisfied with preoccupation was associated with lower levels of
Downloaded by [University of Haifa Library] at 10:31 18 November 2015

the support they received from their parents. Hence they adjustment—emotionally, socially, and scholastically.
were less able to use this support to help them cope with The demonstration of lower levels of adjustment by
the challenges of the new environment. It seems that individuals with preoccupied attachment representations
although these young women need help, they are less in such very different environments exposes their general
inclined to ask for it. This could be due to their mistrust vulnerability, which emerges in highly diverse contexts.
of close others and their repeated past disappointments. By contrast, and contrary to our assumptions, young
Such reluctance might also reflect their attempt to deny women with dismissing attachment representations did
their dependency needs (Larose & Bernier, 2001). This not differ in their adjustment and adaptation from
mismatch between their inner turmoil (Creasey & Ladd, individuals with secure=autonomous representations or
2004) and their incompetence in regulating it may make show lower levels of functioning. Several reasons may
these young women especially vulnerable, particularly account for this finding.
over time. Dismissing attachment representations may actually
In our study we investigated the possible mediating be somewhat adaptive in the context of military service.
role of social support. We hypothesized that because Specifically, the deactivating affect regulation character-
individuals with preoccupied representations are less istic of individuals with dismissing attachment represen-
able to use the support provided to them by close others tations is moderately suited to the functioning required
in times of stress in a way that satisfies their needs, they in the military context, with its ‘‘distant’’ authoritarian
are less able to adjust well to the transition also in the climate and its focus on instrumental functioning.
long run. Our hypothesis received partial support. Consequently this transition might be smoother for dis-
Specifically, satisfaction from parents’ support but not missing individuals than challenges that require intimacy
from that of friends mediated the effect of the young and closeness (Scharf et al., 2004). However, in a pre-
women’s preoccupied state of mind on their reports of vious study, with adolescent boys in military service,
adjustment to military service at Time 3. This was boys with dismissing attachment representations were
apparent even when levels of adjustment to military found to function worse than those with autonomous
service at Time 2 were taken into account. This finding attachment representations (Scharf et al., 2004). If any-
suggests that satisfaction with social support from par- thing, we would expect that in young women, who are
ents may be a central factor in the young women’s socialized to close relationships (Josselson, 1996), a dis-
adjustment, when assessed from their own perspective, missing affect regulation might be less acceptable and
and that this lower satisfaction from parents’ support adaptive than in young men. The results, which did not
may be at the core of the deterioration in their function- support this expectation, may be attributable to the rela-
ing and their lower adjustment at Time 3 compared to tively less stressful military service of young women than
Time 2. This result attests to the significant role of rela- young men. This less challenging service, in a context
tionships with parents even during emerging adulthood, that is compatible with dismissing affect regulation,
at least in the Israeli culture (Scharf & Mayseless, 2004). may not have been a strong enough stressor for young
It is also possible that other factors may account for women with dismissing representations to expose their
the inferior adjustment of young women with preoccupied vulnerabilities. Other challenges, such as those requiring
representations, especially when we take into account that closeness and intimacy, or more persistent stressors, may
satisfaction from parents’ support only mediated the expose the predictable covert vulnerabilities of indivi-
young women’s own reports on their functioning but duals with dismissing attachment representations.
LEAVING THE PARENTAL NEST 421

Because individuals with dismissing attachment variance in individual differences in adaptation to the
representations tend to paint a neutral or a positive transition from high school to a new context is not
picture, parents and friends could not have known accounted for by individual differences in state of mind
about their inner difficulties. In contrast, in previous with respect to attachment. Other aspects, such as the
studies that examined the college transition, peers’ young women’s temperament, sociability, or intellectual
reports were obtained from friends at college who might capacities, may be relevant. These possibilities need to
see the individuals’ functioning firsthand (Kobak & be addressed in future studies along with assessment of
Sceery, 1998; Larose & Bernier, 2001). Had we used attachment representations.
reports by others residing in the military context, such The present study was conducted in a specific cultural
as military commanders, we might also have been able milieu (i.e., Israeli culture) and a specific environment
to uncover difficulties for individuals with dismissing (i.e., the military context). Several specific aspects of
attachment representations. these contexts may restrict the ability to generalize the
Other researchers had similar difficulties identifying results. The Israeli collectivist culture, which promotes
lower functioning of individuals with dismissing attach- involvement, support, and networking among its
ment representations, even in the college environment members (Salomon & Mayseless, 2003), may also affect
(Bernier et al., 2004). This may also have to do with some of the results. In such a context, the neediness and
Downloaded by [University of Haifa Library] at 10:31 18 November 2015

the type of their affect regulation. Deactivation of enmeshment of preoccupied individuals may be less sali-
attachment-related aspects may be less hazardous and ent and less disturbing than in individualistic cultures.
problematic to daily functioning, as well as functioning This kind of cultural context also invites and promotes
in different challenging circumstances, than hyperactiva- high levels of support from close others as well as from
tion. For example, distraction (a deactivating approach) peers regardless of individual differences. This may
was associated with an easing of dysphoric mood when minimize the role of social support as a factor that
dealing with stress (Broderick, 2005), whereas rumi- discriminates among the state-of-mind groups. In parti-
nation (especially the brooding subtype)—a hyperacti- cular, the less significant role of support from friends
vating approach—proved problematic and presaged may be related to the general high level of support that
the development of depressive symptoms (Burwell & may mask individual differences. Future studies may
Shirk, 2007). Dismissing strategies may be less taxing need to examine more closely the distinctive role of
in terms of conscious executive functions than the active friends’ support in such transitions.
preoccupation with attachment-related issues in general, This study focused only on women’s adaptation to
and particularly in contexts that conform to their affect the developmental transition from high school to
regulation tendencies. Future research may need to military service. To address the possibility of gender
investigate such possibilities and test for possible differ- differences in adaptation to such transition, future stu-
ences in conditions that challenge the affect regulation dies would need to include large samples of participants
strategies of the two insecure groups. representing the two genders.
The results of our study demonstrate, by and large,
that in very diverse contexts (college and military
Implications for Research, Policy, and Practice
service) and different cultural milieus (Israel and the
Although this sample included 120 participants, only a United States), individuals with preoccupied attachment
small number of preoccupied young women were iden- representations demonstrate higher levels of adjustment
tified. This number was sufficient to observe significant problems than others, and in both contexts these
differences, and the discrepancy in number of cases for differences are also observed several months following
the different attachment categories did not lead to the transition. Despite showing only moderate effect
violation of the assumption of equal variances. Being a sizes, this result strengthens the general applicability of
nonrisk sample with intact two-parent families might attachment predictions to stressful transitions during
have contributed to the relatively low rates of insecure emerging adulthood.
states of mind, which are similar to those in other Success in coping with these transitions was concep-
nonrisk samples. To extend our understanding of this tualized as a developmental task and an important
group, future research should examine larger groups of marker of maturity in Western cultures (Arnett &
nonrisk samples. With this group of preoccupied young Galambos, 2003). Our results highlight the importance
women the effects were quite clear, yet their size was of identifying at-risk individuals (even among a
small to moderate (between 5% to 18% explained high-functioning population) who might have difficulties
variance). This might reflect the expected effect for this in coping with this normative developmental transition.
high-functioning sample, and is similar to effect sizes The findings from this nonrisk sample underscore the
previously reported for the transition to college. Never- adjustment problems that might characterize adoles-
theless, it reflects the fact that a large part of the cents from less advantageous home environments, in
422 SCHARF, MAYSELESS, KIVENSON-BARON

particular those where enmeshed and conflictual Burwell, R. A., & Shirk, S. R. (2007). Subtypes of rumination in
relationships with parents prevail. These adolescents adolescence: Associations between brooding, reflection, depressive
symptoms, and coping. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent
could experience the transition as more taxing than Psychology, 36, 56–65. doi:10.1207=s15374424jccp3601_6
others and even develop severe adjustment problems Cassidy, J. (2000). Adult romantic attachments: A developmental
that persist over time. This possibility should be recog- perspective on individual differences. Review of General Psychology,
nized by psychologists, counselors, and social workers 4, 111–131. doi:10.1037=1089-2680.4.2.111
and addressed both in identifying vulnerable adolescents Creasey, G. (2002). Psychological distress in college-aged women:
Links with unresolved=preoccupied attachment status and the
and providing support to them before and during this mediating role of negative mood regulation expectancies.
significant transition. Attachment and Human Development, 4, 261–277. doi:10.1080=
14616730210167249
Creasey, G., & Ladd, A. (2004). Negative mood regulation expectan-
REFERENCES cies and conflict behaviors in late adolescent college student roman-
tic relationships: The moderating role of generalized attachment
Adam, E. K., Gunnar, M. R., & Tanaka, A. (2004). Adult attachment, representations. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 14, 235–255.
parent emotion, and observed parenting behavior: Mediator and doi:10.1111=j.1532-7795.2004.01402005.x
moderator models. Child Development, 75, 110–122. doi:10.1111= Cummings, M. E., Davies, P. T., & Simpson, K. S. (1994). Marital
j.1467-8624.2004.00657.x conflict, gender, and children’s appraisals and coping efficacy as
Downloaded by [University of Haifa Library] at 10:31 18 November 2015

Allen, J. P., Marsh, P., McFarland, C., McElhaney, K. B., Land, D. J., mediators of child adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology, 8,
Jodl, K. M., et al. (2002). Attachment and autonomy as predictors 141–149. doi:10.1037=0893-3200.8.2.141
of the development of social skills and delinquency during Dozier, M., & Kobak, R. R. (1992). Psychophysiology in attachment
mid-adolescence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, interviews: Converging evidence for deactivating strategies. Child
70, 56–66. doi:10.1037=0022-006X.70.1.56 Development, 63, 1473–1480. doi:10.2307=1131569
Allen, J. P., Moore, C., Kuperminc, G., & Bell, K. (1998). Attachment Dozier, M., & Lee, S. (1995). Discrepancies between self- and other-
and adolescent psychosocial functioning. Child Development, 69, report of psychiatric symptomatology: Effects of dismissing attach-
1406–1419. doi:10.2307=1132274 ment strategies. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 217–226.
Arnett, J. J., & Galambos, N. L. (Eds.). (2003). New directions for child doi:10.1017=S095457940000643X
and adolescent development: Exploring cultural conceptions of the Dozier, M., & Tyrrell, C. (1998). The role of attachment in therapeutic
transition to adulthood. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. relationships. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment
Baker, R. W., & Siryk, B. (1984). Measuring adjustment to college. theory and close relationships (pp. 221–248). New York: Guilford.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 31, 179–189. doi:10.1037=0022- Dykas, M. J., & Cassidy, J. (2007). Attachment and the processing of
0167.31.2.179 social information in adolescence. New Directions for Child and
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator vari- Adolescent Development, 117, 42–55.
able distinction: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Finch, J. F., Barrera, M. Jr., Okun, M. A., Bryant, W. H., Pool, G. J.,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. & Snow-Turek, A. L. (1997). The factor structure of received social
doi:10.1037=0022-3514.51.6.1173 support: Dimensionality and the prediction of depression and life
Barrera, M. (1986). Distinctions between social support concepts, satisfaction. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 16, 323–342.
measures, and models. American Journal of Community Psychology, Florian, V., & Drory, Y. (1990). Mental Health Inventory (MHI):
14, 413–445. doi:10.1007=BF00922627 Psychometric properties and normative data in the Israeli
Barrera, M. J. (2000). Social support research in community population. Psychologia: Israel Journal of Psychology, 2, 26–35.
psychology. In J. Rappaport & E. Seidman (Eds.), Handbook of George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1985). An adult attachment inter-
community psychology (pp. 215–245). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: view. Unpublished manuscript, University of California at Berkeley.
Kluwer Academic. Gerdes, H., & Mallinckrodt, B. (1994). Emotional, social, and
Berger, L. E., Jodl, K. M., Allen, J. P., McElhaney, K. B., & academic adjustment of college students: A longitudinal study of
Kuperminc, G. P. (2005). When adolescents disagree with others retention. Journal of Counseling and Development, 72, 281–288.
about their symptoms: Differences in attachment organization as Granot, D., & Mayseless, O. (2001). Attachment security and adjustment
an explanation of discrepancies between adolescent, parent, and peer to school in middle childhood. International Journal of Behavioral
reports of behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 17, Development, 25, 530–541. doi:10.1080=01650250042000366
509–528. doi:10.1017=S0954579405050248 Hesse, E. (2008). The Adult Attachment Interview: Historical and
Berman, W. H., & Sperling, M. B. (1991). Parental attachment and current perspectives. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook
emotional distress in the transition to college. Journal of Youth of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp.
and Adolescence, 20, 427–440. doi:10.1007=BF01537184 552–598). New York: Guilford.
Bernier, A., Larose, S., Boivin, M., & Soucy, N. (2004). Attachment Israelashvili, M., & Wegman-Rozi, O. (2007). Longitudinal evaluation
state of mind: Implications for adjustment to college. Journal of of a preparation program for mandatory military service. Military
Adolescent Research, 19, 783–806. doi:10.1177=0743558403260096 Psychology, 19, 175–196.
Bernier, A., Larose, S., & Whipple, N. (2005). Leaving home for Josselson, R. (1996). Revising herself: The story of women’s identity
college: A potentially stressful event for adolescents with preoccu- from college to midlife. New York: Oxford University Press.
pied attachment patterns. Attachment and Human Development, 7, Kenny, M. E., & Rice, K. G. (1995). Attachment to parents and
171–185. doi:10.1080=14616730500147565 adjustment in late adolescent college students: Current status, appli-
Bowen, H. R. (1977). Investment in learning: The individual and social cations, and future considerations. The Counseling Psychologist, 23,
value of American higher education. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 433–456. doi:10.1177=0011000095233003
Broderick, P. C. (2005). Mindfulness and coping with dysphoric mood: Kobak, R. R., & Sceery, A. (1988). Attachment in late adolescence:
Contrasts with rumination and distraction. Cognitive Therapy and Working models, affect regulation and representations of self and
Research, 29, 501–510. doi:10.1007=s10608-005-3888-0 others. Child Development, 59, 135–146. doi:10.2307=1130395
LEAVING THE PARENTAL NEST 423

Kobak, R., Sudler, N., & Gamble, W. (1991). Attachment and Scharf, M., & Mayseless, O. (2004, July). Relationships between parents
depressive symptoms during adolescence: A developmental path- and their emerging adults’ children: Do these hearts grow fonder? Paper
ways analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 461–474. presented at the biennial meeting of the International Society for the
doi:10.1017=S095457940000763X Study of Behavioral Development, Ghent, Belgium.
Larose, S., & Bernier, A. (2001). Social support processes: Mediators Scharf, M., & Mayseless, O. (2007). Putting eggs in more than one bas-
of attachment state of mind and adjustment in late adolescence. ket: A new look at developmental processes of attachment in
Attachment and Human Development, 3, 96–120. doi:10.1080= adolescence. In M. Scharf & O. Mayseless (Eds.), Attachment in
14616730010024762 Adolescence: Reflections and New Angles: New Directions for Child
Larose, S., Bernier, A., & Tarabulsy, G. M. (2005). Attachment state and Adolescent Development, 117, 1–22.
of mind, learning dispositions, and academic performance during Scharf, M., Mayseless, O., & Kivenson-Baron, I. (2004). Adolescents’
the college transition. Developmental Psychology, 41, 281–289. attachment representations and developmental tasks in emerging
doi:10.1037=0012-1649.41.1.281 adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 40, 430–444. doi:10.1037=
MacKinnon, D. P., Warsi, G., & Dwyer, J. H. (1995). A simulation 0012-1649.40.3.430
study of mediated effect measures. Multivariate Behavioral Research, Seiffe-Krenke, I. (2006). Leaving home or still in the nest? Parent—
30, 41–62. doi:10.1207=s15327906mbr3001_3 child relationships and psychological health as predictors of
Mayseless, O., & Hai, I. (1998). Leaving home transition in Israel: different leaving home patterns. Developmental Psychology, 42,
Changes in parent–adolescent relationships and adolescents’ 864–876. doi:10.1037=0012-1649.42.5.864
adaptation to military service. International Journal of Behavioral Statistical Abstract of Israel. (2008). Jerusalem: The Government of
Development, 22, 589–609. doi:10.1080=016502598384289 Israel, Central Bureau of Statistics.
Downloaded by [University of Haifa Library] at 10:31 18 November 2015

Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (1995). Appraisal of and coping with Stroebe, M. (2001). Gender differences in adjustment to bereavement:
a real-life stressful situation: The contribution of attachment An empirical and theoretical review. Review of General Psychology,
styles. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 406–414. 5, 62–83. doi:10.1037=1089-2680.5.1.62
doi:10.1177=0146167295214011 van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2010).
Monson, C. M., Taft, C. T., & Fredman, S. J. (2009). Military-related Invariance of adult attachment across gender, age, culture, and
PTSD and intimate relationships: From description to theory-driven socioeconomic status?. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships,
research and intervention development. Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 200–208. doi:10.1177=0265407509360908
29, 707–714. doi:10.1016=j.cpr.2009.09.002 Veit, C. T., & Ware, J. E. (1983). The structure of psychological stress
Salomon, G., & Mayseless, O. (2003). Dialectic contradictions in the and well-being in general populations. Journal of Consulting and
experiences of Israeli Jewish adolescents: Efficacy and stress, Clinical Psychology, 51, 730–742.
closeness and friction, and conformity and non-compliance. In Wintre, M. G., & Ben-Knaz, R. (2000). It’s not academic, you’re in the
F. Pajares & T. Urdan (Eds.), Adolescence and education, Volume army now: Adjustment to the army as a comparative context for adjust-
III: International perspectives on adolescence (pp. 149–171). Greenwich, ment to the university. Journal of Adolescent Research, 15, 145–172.
CT: Information Age. Zimmermann, P. (1999). Structure and functions of internal working
Scharf, M. (2001). A ‘‘natural experiment’’ in childrearing ecologies models of attachment and their role for emotion regulation.
and adolescents’ attachment and separation representations. Child Attachment and Human Development, 1, 291–306. doi:10.1080=
Development, 72, 236–251. doi:10.1111=1467-8624.00276 14616739900134161

You might also like