You are on page 1of 6

This article was downloaded by: [Eindhoven Technical University]

On: 18 November 2014, At: 01:30


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Creativity Research Journal


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hcrj20

Creativity and Brain-Functioning in Product


Development Engineers: A Canonical Correlation
Analysis
a b
Frederick Travis & Yvonne Lagrosen
a
Maharishi University of Management
b
University West , Sweden
Published online: 08 May 2014.

To cite this article: Frederick Travis & Yvonne Lagrosen (2014) Creativity and Brain-Functioning in Product Development
Engineers: A Canonical Correlation Analysis, Creativity Research Journal, 26:2, 239-243, DOI: 10.1080/10400419.2014.901096

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2014.901096

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and
are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of
the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
CREATIVITY RESEARCH JOURNAL, 26(2), 239–243, 2014
Copyright # Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 1040-0419 print=1532-6934 online
DOI: 10.1080/10400419.2014.901096

RESEARCH NOTE

Creativity and Brain-Functioning in Product Development


Engineers: A Canonical Correlation Analysis
Frederick Travis
Maharishi University of Management
Downloaded by [Eindhoven Technical University] at 01:30 18 November 2014

Yvonne Lagrosen
University West, Sweden

This study used canonical correlation analysis to explore the relation among scores on
the Torrance test of figural and verbal creativity and demographic, psychological and
physiological measures in Swedish product-development engineers. The first canonical
variate included figural and verbal flexibility and originality as dependent measures
and (a) higher scores on the brain integration scale, (b) faster speed of processing in
an event-related potential task, (c) faster conflict-resolution during the Stroop task,
(d) higher moral reasoning, and (e) higher manageability and lower comprehensibility
as independent measures. Flexibility and originality reflect the ability to see old situa-
tions in new ways leading to unique responses. Greater mental adaptability was associa-
ted with greater brain integration and speed of processing along with higher moral
reasoning and feeling of being in control. Future research could investigate effects of
interventions that optimize brain integration on creative output across professions.

Creativity is a factor in successful problem solving by gamma EEG as the details of the solution unfold
(Andreasen, 2011), and in innovation, entrepreneurship, (Kounios et al., 2006). More generally, the process of
and competitiveness (Granot, 2011). Creative problem hypothesis generation required by a divergent condition
solving is supported by specific patterns of brain activates frontal brain areas (Vartanian & Goel, 2007).
functioning. Alpha EEG during creativity tasks does not appear
A critical review of the literature observed that left- to indicate idling. Higher performance on Torrance
and right-hemisphere brain areas are involved during Tests of verbal creativity has been associated with great-
creativity tasks, and that alpha EEG is frequently seen er cerebral frontal blood flow (Chávez-Eakle, Graff-
during creative problem solving (Sawyer, 2013). During Guerrero, Garcı́a-Reyna, Vaugier, & Cruz-Fuentes,
generation of original ideas, alpha EEG is reported to 2007). In an EEG=fMRI study, higher frontal alpha EEG
increase in frontal and parietal brain areas (Dietrich & and higher frontal cerebral metabolic rate were both
Kanso, 2010; Martindale & Hasenfus, 1978). Higher found during creative problem solving (Fink et al., 2009).
frontal alpha activity is thought to indicate the internal Distributed brain functioning is a mark of creative
transformation of the current problem space into a problem solving. It is also a mark of successful indivi-
creative solution (Benedek, Bergner, Konen, Fink, & duals. Brain integration, as measured by frontal EEG
Neubauer, 2011). Just before solving a problem with coherence, relative alpha power, and brain preparatory
insight, a burst in frontal alpha is seen that is followed response during demanding tasks, was reported in top-
performers in sports, management, and classical music,
as compared to controls (Harung & Travis, 2012;
We thank University West for help to fund this research. Harung et al., 2011; Travis, Harung, & Lagrosen, 2011).
Correspondence should be sent to Frederick Travis, 1000 North
Higher levels of brain integration, suggesting more
4th MR 683, Fairfield, IA 52557. E-mail: ftravis@mum.edu
240 TRAVIS AND LAGROSEN

distributed brain functioning, may be a component of in the company in relation to expected performance.
successful problem solving. These were combined into a single measure for analysis.
This study explored the relation of creativity, as
measured by the Torrance tests of verbal and figural
Figural and verbal creativity. Two tests of creativity
creativity, with physiological and psychological mea-
were used from the Torrance Tests of Creative
sures in product development engineers. Product-
Thinking—the parallel lines test (figural creativity) and
development engineers were tested because creativity is
the alternative uses of a tin can test (verbal creativity).
a key factor in their success (Sprecher, 1959). EEG
These tests result in three subscales—fluency, which is
was measured during paired-reaction time tests to calcu-
the ability to produce a large number of responses;
late the level of brain integration, and during two
flexibility, which is the ability to give responses from
event-related potential tasks to measure speed of proces-
different categories; and originality, which is the ability
sing and vigilance (Polich, 2007). Participants also recei-
to produce uncommon or unique responses. This test
ved the Stroop Color-Word test, a standard measure of
has extensive norms from over 55,600 people, and
frontal executive functioning, and two psychological
has very strong reliable and test–retest coefficients
tests: Antonovsky’s Orientation to Life question-
(Torrance, Treffinger, & Ball, 1987). The Torrance tests
Downloaded by [Eindhoven Technical University] at 01:30 18 November 2014

naire, measuring psychological ‘‘sense of coherence’’


were scored according to guidelines in the Torrance Test
(Antonovsky, 1993, p. 725) and Gibb’s Social-Moral
of Creativity thinking manual.
Reflection that correlates highly with tests of cognitive
stage development (Gibbs et al., 1990).
BRAIN INTEGRATION SCALE

METHOD The Brain Integration Scale includes frontal alpha


coherence, frontal-central alpha relative power, and
Participants the difference in brain preparatory response during sim-
ple and choice paired-reaction-time trials (Travis, Tecce,
The study was conducted with engineers, involved in
Arenander, & Wallace, 2002). The simple reaction-time
product development, who worked at a major Swedish
trials included an asterisk as a warning stimuli followed
company that develops and manufactures aircraft
by a tone 1.5 seconds later. The choice reaction-time
engines and stationary gas turbines. Twenty-one engi-
trials included a one or two digit number as a warning
neers volunteered for the research. They ranged in age
stimuli followed by a second one or two digit number.
from 29 to 56 years (average 39.5  6.5 years). A major-
Engineers were asked to indicate which number was
ity had leading positions as project leaders, department
larger.
leaders, or unit leaders. They were highly educated: 8
had PhDs or were full professors, 11 had Master
degrees. Two engineers had no university education.
Event-related potential measures. Engineers were
presented 240 stimuli with one-sec interstimulus inter-
vals: 10% were rare, unexpected white noise bursts (novel
Procedure
tones); and 10% were rare, expected 85 dB 1000 Hz tones
A unit manager at the company sent out an invitation that they were asked to count (oddball tones). The other
to engineers he thought might be interested in a study 80% of stimuli (192) were standard 85 dB 500 Hz tones,
regarding product development, creativity, and brain which subjects were told to ignore.
functioning. After a general introductory meeting, inter- The novel tones result in a frontal component that
ested engineers set up appointments for individual reflects vigilance levels (Polich, 2007), and is called a
recording of EEG. They completed the paper-and-pencil P3a. The oddball tones result in a parietal component
test instruments at that time. that reflects timing and magnitude of memory and stim-
ulus categorization processes (Brázdil, Roman, Daniel,
& Rektor, 2003; Polich, 2007), and is called a P3b.
Test Instruments
Demographics. The engineers reported their ages Stroop Color-Word Test. The Stroop Color-Word
and levels of education. In addition, a unit manager at test is a standard measure of frontal executive function-
the company assigned an experience and achievement ing. In this test, subjects are presented color words writ-
level for each engineer. Experience included: number ten in a different color than the color word itself, for
of years education, years at the company, and other instance, the word red written in blue ink. Reading the
engineering experience. Achievement reflected their rank word is a fast, automatic process; naming the color of
CREATIVITY IN PRODUCT DEVELOPMENT ENGINEERS 241

the ink that the word is written in is a slow, controlled Canonical Correlation Analysis (CCA)
process (Stroop, 1935). The difference in reaction time
Canonical correlation analysis (CCA) was used to assess
in naming the word and naming the color of ink the
the relation between the psychological, physiological
word is written in is called the ‘‘color-word interference
and creativity measures. The CCA calculates a linear
effect’’ (Cohen, Dunbar, & McClelland, 1990, p. 334).
combination of independent and dependent measures
This indexes frontal executive circuits.
that are maximally correlated (Sherry & Henson, 2005).
Due to limited number of subjects, CCA was conduc-
Self-reported health. Subjects responded along a ted with two sets of independent variables. The first
7-point Likert scale, agree not at all (1) to agree com- included demographic measures (age, education, experi-
pletely (7), to two questions on health. These questions ence, and achievement), and self-reported health ques-
were: (1) I am feeling most often alert. (2) I think my tions (feeling alert and health is good). The second
health is very good. included psychological (socio-moral reflection and ori-
entation to life), performance (color-word interference
effects in the Stroop test) and brain-derived variables
Orientation to Life. This measure contains 29 items (latency during the novel and oddball tasks, and the
Downloaded by [Eindhoven Technical University] at 01:30 18 November 2014

that yield three factors defining one’s sense of coherence Brain Integration Scale). In both analyses, the second
(Antonovsky, 1979). The first factor is comprehensibility, set—dependent variables—included three figural
the belief that things happen in an orderly and predict- (fluency, flexibility, and originality) and three verbal
able fashion. The second factor is manageability, the creativity measures (fluency, flexibility, originality).
belief that you have the skills and the resources neces- The canonical correlation analysis module in STATA-10
sary to take care of things. The third factor is meaning- was used to conduct the analysis.
fulness, the belief that life is meaningful, interesting, and The first CCA with demographics, self-report health
a source of satisfaction. A meta-analysis of 50 studies questions, and creativity measures was not significant,
reported a high association of sense of coherence with Wilkes Lambda F(36,84) ¼ 0.96, p ¼ 0.59. The second
physical health (Eriksson & Lindström, 2006; Flensborg- CCA with psychological, performance, and brain
Madsen, Ventegodt, & Merrick, 2005). measures and creativity measures was statistically sig-
nificant, Wilkes Lambda F(42,38) ¼ 3.04, p ¼ 0.014.
Table 1 presents the first three canonical variates from
Gibbs Socio-Moral Reflection questionnaire. This this analysis, their correlation coefficients, percent vari-
instrument presents moral statements and asks subjects ance accounted for and Chi Square statistics. (The
to describe why a moral act may be important to them fourth and further variates accounted for less than 10%
(Gibbs et al., 1990). This instrument has high test–retest of the variance and so are not shown.) The first canoni-
reliability (r ¼ .88), high Cronbach alpha coefficients cal variate from this analysis was statistically significant
(r ¼ .92), and is highly correlated with scores on and so is further discussed.
Kohlberg’s Moral Judgment Interview (r ¼ .70; Gibbs, The next table presents the loadings of variables on
Basinger, & Fuller, 1992), a widely used test of moral the first canonical variate. Significant weights are bolded
development. The Gibb’s Sociomoral Reflection ques- for easy identification. As seen in Table 2, the inde-
tionnaires were sent to trained scorers who returned a pendent variables that significantly loaded on this
score between 1 and 4. variate were faster color-word interference times, higher
scores on the Brain Integration Scale, faster processing
of stimuli, higher moral reasoning, lower comprehensi-
bility and higher manageability. The creativity (depen-
RESULTS
dent) variables that significantly loaded on the first
The data were first tested for normality. Only two vari-
ables differed significantly from normality, the ‘‘color- TABLE 1
word interference effect’’ during the Stroop task The First Three Canonical Variates and Their Correlation
(skewness ¼ 1.4; Kurtosis ¼ 3.4) and P3b latency during Coefficients, Percent Variance Accounted For and
the odd ball task (skewness ¼ 1.7; Kurtosis ¼ 3.7). Chi-Square Statistics
Because canonical correlation analysis is sensitive to Canonical Correlation Percent Variance Chi
outliers, a Windsorized mean was calculated for these Coefficient Accounted Square df p-Value
two variables. This involves replacing the smallest and
largest values with the observations closest to them. 1st 0.92 38.6 84.9 36 0.008
2nd 0.83 32.1 26.2 25 0.121
After this procedure, none of the variables differed
3rd 0.58 15.5 10.1 16 0.662
significantly from normality.
242 TRAVIS AND LAGROSEN

TABLE 2 and comprehensibility negatively loaded on the first


Loading of Each Variable on the First Canonical Variate canonical variate. This was not expected. Higher man-
Standardized ageability is related to perceived internal control—one
Variable Weights F-Statistic p-Value believes that they have sufficient inner resources to
cope with life’s challenges (Antonovsky, 1993), which
First set: Independent variables
in a meta-analysis is associated with higher creative
Color-word interference effect 0.51 20.0 0.001
Brain integration scale 0.61 25.2 0.000 performance (Byron, Khazanchi, & Nazarian, 2013).
Oddball latency 0.50 15.9 0.001 Comprehensibility is the degree that you feel you can
Novel latency 0.06 0.23 0.639 ‘‘reasonably predict what will happen in the future.’’
Moral reasoning 0.68 33.7 0.000 Inverse loading of comprehensibility on the creativity
Comprehensibility 0.74 35.3 0.000
subscales suggests that creative people are not tied to
Manageability 0.47 13.8 0.001
Meaningfulness –0.08 .20 0.657 routine but expect new things will happen.
Second set: Dependent variables
Figural fluency 1.29 36.6 0.000
Figural flexibility 0.91 23.5 0.000 IMPLICATIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH
Downloaded by [Eindhoven Technical University] at 01:30 18 November 2014

Figural originality 0.41 6.25 0.021


Verbal fluency –0.32 1.5 0.238 Creativity is an important factor for individual success.
Verbal flexibility 0.95 26.9 0.000
It could be considered a core value of the employee
Verbal originality 2.02 58.1 0.000
along with honesty, being fair, being good, and open-
minded that would contribute to success of organiza-
canonical variate were higher flexibility and originality tions across time, cultures, and ethnicities (Hofstede,
in both figural and verbal creativity. Lower figural 1997; Lagrosen, Bäckström, & Lagrosen, 2010;
fluency also loaded on this factor. Thornbury, 2003). A recent paper argues that brain inte-
gration may be the driver of core values and so quality
management success (Lagrosen, Travis, & Lagrosen,
DISCUSSION 2012). Future research could address the impact of
interventions such as meditation practices to optimize
The canonical correlation analysis suggests a strong brain integration and creative output across professions
relation among flexibility and originality subscales and (Travis, 1979).
physiological and psychological measures in a group
of product-development engineers. High scores on the REFERENCES
flexibility subscale reflect the ability to generate res-
ponses from different categories. High scores on the Andreasen, N. C. (2011). A journey into chaos: Creativity and the
originality subscale include items that were more rarely unconscious. Mens Sana Monograph, 9, 42–53.
seen in the 55,000 protocols used to create the norms. Antonovsky, A. (1979). Health, stress and coping. New York, NY:
The creativity subscale, fluency, which was negatively Jossey-Bass.
loaded on the canonical variate, is a measure of number Antonovsky, A. (1993). The structure and properties of the sense of
coherence scale. Social Science & Medicine, 36, 725–733.
of items. Focusing on generating many responses may Benedek, M., Bergner, S., Konen, T., Fink, A., & Neubauer, A. C.
miss consideration of different kinds of responses and (2011). EEG alpha synchronization is related to top-down proces-
so miss original solutions. sing in convergent and divergent thinking. Neuropsychologia, 49,
Brain integration and speed of processing (oddball 3505–3511.
Brázdil, M., Roman, R., Daniel, P., & Rektor, I. (2003). Intracerebral
latency) significantly correlated with the creativity sub-
somatosensory event-related potentials: Effect of response type
scales. The Brain Integration Scale includes higher fron- (button pressing versus mental counting) on p3-like potentials
tal coherence and diffuse increase in alpha activity, within the human brain. Clinical Neurophysiology, 114, 1489–1496.
which supports the model of creativity as involving the Byron, K., Khazanchi, S., & Nazarian, D. (2013). The relationship
whole brain. Faster processing would allow one to con- between stressors and creativity: A meta-analysis examining
sider more alternatives during problem solving leading competing theoretical models. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95,
201–212.
to greater flexibility of categories and higher originality. Chávez-Eakle, R., Graff-Guerrero, A., Garcı́a-Reyna, J., Vaugier, V., &
Four psychological measures also significantly corre- Cruz-Fuentes, C. (2007). Cerebral blood flow associated with creative
lated with the creativity subscales. Faster resolution of performance: A comparative study. Neuroimage, 38, 519–528.
conflict in the Stroop task, a reliable measure of frontal Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the control
executive thinking, supports the role of frontal areas in of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of
the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97, 332–361.
creative processing. Higher moral reasoning, an index Dietrich, A., & Kanso, R. (2010). A review of EEG, ERP, and neuro-
of development, suggests that development supports imaging studies of creativity and insight. Psychology Bulletin, 136,
creative thinking. Manageability positively loaded 822–848.
CREATIVITY IN PRODUCT DEVELOPMENT ENGINEERS 243

Eriksson, M., & Lindström, B. (2006). Antonovsky’s sense of coher- Lagrosen, Y., Travis, F., & Lagrosen, S. (2012). Brain integration as a
ence scale and the relation with health: A systematic review. Journal driver for quality management success. International Journal of
of Epidemiological Community Health, 60, 376–381. Quality and Service Science, 4, 253–269.
Fink, A., Grabner, R. H., Benedek, M., Reishofer, G., Hauswirth, V., Martindale, C., & Hasenfus, N. (1978). EEG differences as a function
Fally, M., Neuper, C., Ebner, F., Aljoscha, C., and Neubauer, A. of creativity, stage of the creative process, and effort to be original.
(2009). The creative brain: Investigation of brain activity during Biological Psychology, 6, 157–167.
creative problem solving by means of EEG and fMRI. Human Brain Polich, J. (2007). Updating p300: An integrative theory of P3a and
Mapping, 30, 734–748. P3b. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118, 2128–2148.
Flensborg-Madsen, T., Ventegodt, S., & Merrick, J. (2005). Sense Sawyer, K. (2013). The cognitive neuroscience of creativity: A critical
of coherence and physical health. A review of previous findings. review. Creativity Research Journal, 23, 137–154.
Scientific World Journal, 25, 665–673. Sherry, A., & Henson, R. (2005). Conducting and interpreting
Gibbs, J. C., Arnold, K. D., Morgan, R. L., Schwartx, E. S., canonical correlation analysis in personality research: A user-
Gavaghan, M. P., & Tappan, M. B. (1990). Construction and friendly primer. Journal of Personality Assessment, 84, 37–48.
validation of a measure of moral reasoning. Child Development, Sprecher, T. (1959). A study of engineers’ criteria for creativity.
55, 527–553. Journal of Applied Psychology, 43, 141–148.
Gibbs, J. C., Basinger, K. S., & Fuller, D. (1992). Moral maturity. Stroop, J. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.
Granot, E. (2011). Creative managers and managing creativity: A Thornbury, J. (2003). Creating a living culture: The challenges for
Downloaded by [Eindhoven Technical University] at 01:30 18 November 2014

hermeneutic exploration. American Journal of Business, 26, 161–182. business leaders. Corporate Governance, 3, 68–79.
Harung, H. S., & Travis, F. (2012). Higher mind–brain development Torrance, E., Treffinger, D., & Ball, O. (1987). Guideline for adminis-
in successful leaders: Testing a unified theory of performance. tration and scoring=comments on using TTCT. Bensenville, IL:
Cognitive Processing, 6, 20–27. Scholastic Testing Service.
Harung, H. S., Travis, F., Pensgaard, A. M., Boes, R., Cook-Greuter, Travis, F. (1979). The transcendental meditation technique and crea-
S., & Daley, K. (2011). Higher psycho-physiological refinement in tivity: A longitudinal study of Cornell university undergraduates.
world-class Norwegian athletes: Brain measures of performance Journal of Creative Behavior, 13, 169–181.
capacity. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine and Science in Sports, Travis, F., Harung, H. S., & Lagrosen, Y. (2011). Moral development,
21, 32–41. executive functioning, peak experiences and brain patterns in pro-
Hofstede, G. (1997). Cultures and organisations, software of the mind. fessional and amateur classical musicians: Interpreted in light of a unified
New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. theory of performance. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 1256–1264.
Kounios, J., Frymaire, J. L., Bowden, E. M., Fleck, J., Subramaniam, K., Travis, F., Tecce, J., Arenander, A., & Wallace, R. K. (2002). Patterns
& Parrish, T. B. (2006). The prepared mind: Neural activity prior to of EEG coherence, power, and contingent negative variation
problem presentation predicts subsequent solution by sudden insight. characterize the integration of transcendental and waking states.
Psychological Science, 17, 882–290. Biological Psychology, 61, 293–319.
Lagrosen, Y., Bäckström, I., & Lagrosen, S. (2010). The relationship Vartanian, O., & Goel, V. (2007). Neural correlates of creative
between quality management and health—Exploring the underlying cognition. In C. Martindale, P. Locher & V. M. Petrov (Eds.),
dimensions. International Journal of Productivity and Quality Evolutionary and neurocognitive approaches to aesthetics, creativity,
Management, 5, 44–51. and the arts (pp. 195–207). Amityville, NJ: Baywood.

You might also like