You are on page 1of 10

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/369947641

Assessing Adult ADHD Through Objective Neuropsychological Measures: A


Critical Overview

Article in Journal of Attention Disorders · April 2023


DOI: 10.1177/10870547231167564

CITATIONS READS

0 554

5 authors, including:

Gianluca Rosso Elena Teobaldi


Università degli Studi di Torino Università degli Studi di Torino
95 PUBLICATIONS 1,421 CITATIONS 11 PUBLICATIONS 14 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Gabriele Di Salvo
Azienda Ospedaliero Universitaria San Luigi Gonzaga
32 PUBLICATIONS 227 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Gabriele Di Salvo on 16 April 2023.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


1167564
research-article2023
JADXXX10.1177/10870547231167564Journal of Attention DisordersRosso et al.

Article
Journal of Attention Disorders

Assessing Adult ADHD Through Objective


1­–9
© The Author(s) 2023
Article reuse guidelines:
Neuropsychological Measures: A Critical sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/10870547231167564
https://doi.org/10.1177/10870547231167564

Overview journals.sagepub.com/home/jad

Gianluca Rosso1,2, Caterina Portaluppi1 , Elena Teobaldi1 ,


Gabriele Di Salvo1,2,3, and Giuseppe Maina1,2,3

Abstract
Objective: This review examines the role of neuropsychological tests in the diagnostic assessment of adult ADHD,
focusing on their ability to discriminate individuals with ADHD from those with other psychiatric conditions. Method:
PubMed, Embase, and PsycINFO were searched for eligible peer-reviewed studies from inception to September 2022.
Results: Ten studies were included. Among the objective measures analyzed, Continuous Performance Tests were the
only capable to reliably distinguish individuals with ADHD from other psychiatric patients, in a combined approach with
clinical interview instruments. The other objective tests showed mixed and inconsistent results. Conclusion: This finding
suggest that further studies are needed to develop objective measures more tailored to the core symptoms of ADHD, in
order to improve the discriminatory ability of the tests and help the clinicians in the complex differential diagnosis between
ADHD and other psychiatric disorders.

Keywords
ADHD, adult, assessment, neuropsychological testing, clinical sample

Introduction coexists with other psychiatric diseases such as major


depression, anxiety disorders, bipolar disorder, personality
Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a clini- disorder, and substance or alcohol abuse (Grazioli et al.,
cally heterogeneous neurodevelopmental disorder that 2019; Katzman et al., 2017; Mucci et al., 2019; Xenaki &
includes age-inappropriate levels of inattention, hyperactiv- Pehlivanidis, 2015). Therefore, differentiating ADHD from
ity, and impulsivity. ADHD has been associated with high other clinical disorders is actually the most difficult part of
risk of comorbid psychiatric disorders and severe functional making an ADHD diagnosis in adults (Luo et al., 2019;
impairment (Biederman et al., 2006; Gjervan et al., 2012; McGough et al., 2005; Whlstedt et al., 2009). The current
Kooij et al., 2012). Despite it has long been thought to occur diagnostic clinical assessment of ADHD according to the
only in childhood, more recent studies suggest that it can criteria set in the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and
persist into adulthood in a high proportion of cases, with a Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; American
prevalence of approximately 2.5% in adult population Psychiatric Association, 2013) is behaviorally based and
(Simon et al., 2009; Song et al., 2021). does not require psychometric testing to make the diagno-
The number of adults seeking evaluation for ADHD has sis. However, objective measures are frequently used in the
risen dramatically as knowledge of the nature of the disor- routine examination of individuals in the ADHD diagnostic
der and the benefits of being diagnosed with ADHD has assessment. Certain neuropsychological batteries or test
increased (Paris et al., 2015; Song et al., 2021); thus, great (e.g., Color Word interference, California Verbal Learning
attention needs to be paid to the assessment phase, in order
to correctly identify those who suffer from the neurodevel- 1
University of Turin, Turin, Italy
opmental disorder. From a neuropsychological perspective, 2
San Luigi Gonzaga University Hospital, Orbassano, Turin, Italy
this means that more emphasis should be placed on stan- 3
Regional Coordination Centre for adult ADHD, Orbassano, Turin, Italy
dardized instruments that improve the diagnostic accuracy
Corresponding Author:
of adult ADHD assessments, particularly in case of sus- Elena Teobaldi, Department of Neurosciences “Rita Levi Montalcini,”
pected diagnosis or psychiatric disorder with overlapping University of Turin, Via Cherasco 15, Turin 10126, Italy.
symptoms. In more than 80% of patients adult ADHD Email: elena.teobaldi@unito.it
2 Journal of Attention Disorders 00(0)

Test, Continuous Performance Test, and Wisconsin Card


Sorting Test) have been recommended as being particularly
appropriate measures in ADHD assessment (Fuermaier et
al., 2019; Marshall et al., 2021), even if is still debated
whatever or not cognitive assessment should be considered
in the diagnostic algorithm (Barkley, 2019; Lange et al.,
2014; Mapou, 2019).
Comparing with healthy controls, it is established that
adult ADHD is associated with impairments in various cog-
nitive domains, with attention and executive functions defi-
cits being far more prevalent than others (Boonstra et al.,
2010; Fuermaier et al., 2015, 2022; Torgalsbøen et al.,
2021; Tucha et al., 2017), as confirmed in a recent and
extended systematic review (Onandia-Hinchado et al.,
2021).
However, despite the extensive literature on comparison
with healthy controls, only little is known about the neuro-
psychological functions of ADHD patients when compared
to psychiatric conditions and whether a specific neuropsy-
chological profile for adult ADHD can be identified.
This article brings together recent evidence about the
critical contribution of neuropsychological tests in the diag-
nostic assessment of adult ADHD, focusing on whether or
not objective measures can differentiate individuals with
ADHD from individuals with other psychiatric disorders.
Figure 1. Flow diagram.

Method
reviewers (CP, ET, and GDS) independently decided
We conducted a narrative review of published articles on which articles to include according to clinical importance
the neuropsychological test used in the assessment of adult and eligibility criteria. In case of disagreement, the senior
ADHD in a clinical sample. The electronic databases authors (GR and GM) were consulted to mediate consen-
PubMed, Embase, and PsycINFO were used for literature sual decision. Given the wide variety of outcomes reported
published in the last decade, from 2012 to September and the lack of diagnostic accuracy statistics in extant
2022, using the following keywords adapted to the search- studies reviewed a meta-analysis was not performed.
ing requirements in each database: “neuropsycholog*”
AND “assessment” AND “adult*” AND “attention hyper-
Results
activity disorder” AND “clinical controls.” Handsearching
the reference lists of the retrieved articles yielded addi- The literature search revealed nine qualifying studies, as
tional relevant articles. The inclusion criteria for this shown from the flow diagram provided in Figure 1. From
review were as follows: (a) adult participants (aged all databases combined, the initial search yielded 1,382
18 years and above); (b) participants diagnosed with results. After duplicates had been removed, 962 articles
ADHD according to DSM-5 criteria; (c) the occurrence of remained. At title and abstract screening, 923 articles were
a comparison clinical control group (participants with removed as they were not relevant or did not meet the inclu-
other psychiatric disorders); (e) the use at least of one sion criteria. A total of 41 articles were found to be suitable
objective cognitive test at the assessment; (f) peer- for full-text screening. The full-text screening of these arti-
reviewed English-language articles. The presence of cles eliminated 28 articles that still did not meet the inclu-
comorbid medical and psychiatric disorders (either current sion criteria. The studies included in the synthesis are listed
or lifetime) in participants with ADHD did not constitute in Table 1.
an exclusion criterion provided that ADHD was the pri- The results of selected articles looked into the ability of
mary diagnosis. The following studies were excluded: sys- objective neuropsychological measures to distinguish indi-
tematic or narrative review articles, meta-analysis articles, viduals receiving or confirming a diagnosis of adult ADHD
commentaries or editorials, case report/series. Articles from those with other psychiatric disorders or with ADHD
were assessed for inclusion at three stages: title screening, symptoms who do not meet the diagnostic criteria at the
abstract screening, and full text screening. Three time of assessment.
Table 1. Neuropsychological Assessment Using Single Objective Measures.
Authors (publication
year) Participants (n) Age = mean (SD) ADHD group features Cognitive domains Objective measures (test) Relevant outcomes

Edebol et al. (2012) ADHD (53) ADHD = 35.89 (12.25) Psychiatric Attention/hyperactivity/ QBTest Plus Sensitivity of the QBTest was higher for the
CC: BD II, BPD (45) CC = 42.33 (11.63) comorbidities: yesb impulsivity ADHD group. Analyses yielded 87% sensitivity
CC-ND (29) CC-ND = 35.21 (10.31) Stimulants treatment: for the ADHD group, 85% specificity for the
HC (179) HC = 31.45 (10.33) off healthy controls, 36% sensitivity for the clinical
controls, and 41% sensitivity for the group with
a disconfirmed diagnosis of ADHD.
Söderström et al. (2014) ADHD (41) ADHD = 32.46 (8.99) Psychiatric Attention/hyperactivity/ QBTest Plus QBTest Plus shows high degree of specificity
CC: mixed diagnosis CC = 30.00 (9.76) comorbidities: yesc impulsivity for the cardinal symptoms of inattention and
(23) Stimulants treatment: impulsivity (100.0 and 80.0%) but low sensitivity
off (36.6 and 58.5%) when the cut-off score of
1.5 was applied. Inattention classified 87.8% of
ADHD patients correctly but identified only
40% of the Non-ADHD group as not having
the diagnosis.
Fasmer et al. (2016) ADHD (50) ADHD = 37.7 (10.3) Psychiatric Attention CPT II The CPT II omission and commission errors,
CC: mixed diagnosis CC = 37.8 (11.8) comorbidities: yesd skewness, and variability measures were
(49) Stimulants treatment: effective in discriminating between participants
off with ADHD and those with other psychiatric
diagnoses.

Neuropsychological assessment using multiple objective measures

Authors (publication
year) Participants (n) ADHD group features Cognitive domains Objective measures (test) Relevant outcomes

Holst and Thorell ADHD (57) ADHD = 26.8 (5.9) Psychiatric Verbal working memory/ D-KEFS (Color word interference; Verbal Significant group differences were found for
(2013) CC: mixed diagnosis CC = 25.5 (5) comorbidities: yese spatial working memory/ fluency; Sorting; Tower)/WAIS-IV inhibition assessed with the Color Word task
(53) Stimulants treatment: interference control/ (Letter-Number Sequencing; Digit Span and for Letter fluency. Sensitivity of these tests
ona inhibition/set shifting/ backward; Digit Span Forward; Block ranged between 64 and 75% and specificity
planning/fluency/reaction- Design; Vocabulary)/Computerized EF between 66 and 81%.
time variability task (Find-the-Phone task and Navon-
like task)
Marshall et al. (2016) ADHD (102) Overall = 26.38 (7.79) Psychiatric Verbal working memory/ WAIS-IV (Vocabulary; Block Design; The performances obtained at the TOVA test
CC: mood disorders comorbidities: yesg spatial working memory/ Digit span; Letter Number Sequencing; and NAB Number and Letters Part A errors
and GAD (326) Stimulants treatment: interference control/long Symbol search)/NAB (Numbers and led a significant difference between the ADHD
off term memory/inhibition/ Letters test)/Word memory test/ patients and those in the control group.
set shifting/planning/ TOVA/Word memory test/Sentence
fluency/reaction-time repetition test/PASAT/CVLT/DCT/
variability WMS-IV (spatial addition)/B Test/D-
KEFS (Color word interference; Tower;
Design Fluency; Verbal fluency)

(continued)

3
4
Table 1. (continued)
Neuropsychological assessment using multiple objective measures

Authors (publication year) Participants (n) ADHD group features Cognitive domains Objective measures (test) Relevant outcomes

Pettersson et al. (2018) ADHD (60) ADHD = 28.18 (9.09) Psychiatric Attention/working memory/ WAIS-IV (Digit Span Backward and Significant group differences were found for
CC: mixed diagnosis CC = 32.75 (10.61) comorbidities: yesf verbal long-term memory/ Digit Symbol-Coding)/RAVLT/D-KEFS QBTest Plus (inattention, action, omission
(48) Stimulants treatment: verbal fluency/executive (Verbal Fluency)/TMT B/PASAT/ errors, RT variability), CPT II (commission
off functions QBTest Plus/CPT II errors, variability and reaction time), and
RAVLT VI.
Nikolas et al. (2019) ADHD (109) ADHD = 24.8 (6.2) Psychiatric Attention/verbal working WAIS-IV (Vocabulary; Block Design; Worse performance on the CVLT short-delay
CC: unipolar mood CC = 22.9 (4.5) comorbidities: NS memory/spatial working Digit span; Letter Number Sequencing; free recall (OR = 1.4) and TOVA Reaction Time
disorder (52) HC = 23.6 (5.4) Stimulants treatment: memory/interference Symbol search)/NAB (Numbers and (OR = 1.6) were associated with the ADHD
HC (85) off control/long-term Letters test)/Word memory test/ group, with a classification accuracy of 70.4%. A
memory/inhibition/set TOVA/Sentence repetition test/PASAT/ combined approach correctly classified 87% of
shifting/planning ability/ CVLT/DCT/WMS-IV spatial addition the ADHD cases.
fluency/reaction-time subtest/B Test/D-KEFS (Color word
variability interference; Tower; D e V Fluency)
Guo et al. (2021) ADHD (78) ADHD = 31.9 (10.3) Psychiatric Selective attention/vigilance/ CFADHD of VTS (Perceptual and Profiles of neuropsychological impairments were
CC: mixed diagnosis CC = 38.8 (11.2) comorbidities: yesh working memory/figural Attention Functions-Selective; similar between groups.
(71) CC-ND = 35.4 (12.1) Stimulants treatment: fluency/interference/ Attention Perceptual and Attention
CC-ND (50) NS processing speed/ Functions-Vigilance; N-back verbal task;
flexibility/planning ability/ 5-point test Figural Fluency; Stroop
inhibition/task switching Interference Test; TMT-L; Tower of
London; Go/No-Go test paradigm;
SWITCH)
Guo et al. (2022) ADHD (173) ADHD = 33.2 (9.6) Psychiatric Selective attention/vigilance/ CFADHD of VTS (Perceptual and No difference in neurocognitive performance
CC: mixed diagnosis CC = 35.3 (11.2) comorbidities: yesi working memory/figural Attention Functions-Selective; between ADHD and non-ADHD group, except
(54) CC-ND = 35.3 (11.2) Stimulants treatment: fluency/interference/ Attention Perceptual and Attention in the number of omissions of the vigilance test
CC-ND (92) NS processing speed/ Functions-Vigilance; N-back verbal task; (median: 2 vs. 1; p:0.003)
flexibility/planning ability/ 5-point test Figural Fluency; Stroop
inhibition/task switching Interference Test; TMT-L; Tower of
London; Go/No-Go test paradigm;
SWITCH)

Note. CC = Clinical Comparison group; CC-ND = Clinical Comparison group Not Diagnosed or disconfirmed; HC = Healthy Control; NS = Not Specified; BD II = Bipolar Disorder type II; BPD = Borderline Personality Disorder;
GAD = General Anxiety Disorder; CFADHD = Test battery Cognitive Functions ADHD; VTS = Vienna Test System; RAVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; BVMT-R = Brief Visuospatial Memory Test Revised; WAIS-
IV = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Fourth Edition; NAB = Neuropsychological Assessment Battery; TOVA = Test of Variables Attention; PASAT = Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task; CVLT = California Verbal Learning Test;
WMS-IV = Wechsler Memory Scale; D-KEFS = Delis Kaplan Executive System; TMT type B = Trail Making Test Part B; CVLT = California Verbal Learning Test; DCT = Dot Counting Test; QBTest Plus = Quantitative Behaviour
Test Plus; CPT II = Conner’s continuous performance test second edition; SCIP = Screen for Cognitive Impairments in Psychiatry
a
18/57.
b
Social phobia (3/53); dyslexia (3/53); GAD (1/53); depression (2/53); stress reaction (1/53); personality disorder (1/53).
c
Mood disorders (16/41); anxiety disorders (13/41); cluster B disorders (2/41); substance-dependence disorders (3/41).
d
Bipolar disorder (15/50); unipolar depression (16/50); anxiety disorder (19/50); alcohol or drug abuse (13/50); other diagnoses (14/50).
e
Major depression (9/57); bipolar disorder (3/57); unspecified anxiety disorder (3/57); panic disorder (2/57); DOC (1/57); social phobia (1/57); personality disorder (3/57).
f
Mood disorder (15/60); anxiety disorder (26/60); other axis I disorder (10/60); axis II cluster A disorder (3/60); axis II cluster B disorder (5/60); axis II cluster C disorder (6/60).
g
Prevalence in ADHD group not specified. Of the total of the participants: major depression (51/428); depression not otherwise specified (94/428); dysthymia (8/428); mood disorder not otherwise specified (21/428); GAD
(51/428).
h
Mood disorders (19/78); addiction disorders (5/78); adjustment disorders (5/78); anxiety disorders (3/78); DOC (2/78); personality disorders (1/78); oppositional defiant disorders (1/78); intellectual developmental disorders
(1/78); autistic disorders (1/78).
i
Mood disorders (27/173); addiction disorders (11/173); adjustment disorders (5/173); anxiety disorders (6/173); personality disorders (8/173); oppositional defiant disorders (1/173); autistic disorders (2/173); post-traumatic
stress disorders (1/173).
Rosso et al. 5

Neuropsychological Assessment Using After controlling for sex and the respective control tests,
Single Objective Measures significant group differences were found for inhibition
assessed with the Color Word task and for Letter fluency
Three studies attempt to compare neuropsychological func- task with low sensitivity and specificity. However, exclud-
tioning between ADHD patients and other psychiatric ing the patients on psychostimulants (n = 18), the result at
patients using the Quantitative Behaviour Test Plus (QBT the Letter fluency task changed from significant to nonsig-
Test Plus) as objective measures. It is a cognitive computer- nificant, whereas inhibitory errors on the Navon Task and
based test that combines a continuous performance test with the effect of spatial working memory, when controlling for
the measurement of motor activity and evaluates the three IQ, turned out significant. Nevertheless, when only drug-
ADHD core symptoms: hyperactivity, inattention, and free participants were included in the logistic regression
impulsivity. analysis, the overall classification rate increased only
Edebol et al. (2012) included 53 patients, 9 of whom had slightly.
mixed psychiatric comorbidities, assessed by clinical Marshall et al. (2016) conducted a retrospective study in
records, and were free from/not receiving psychostimulants a sample with mixed psychiatric diagnoses to examine how
treatment. They showed that the QBTest cardinal variables many patients would have plausibly receive a primary diag-
had a high sensitivity (87%) in distinguishing individuals nosis of ADHD if performance and symptom validity mea-
diagnosed with ADHD from patients with bipolar II disor- sures were not administered during neuropsychological
der, borderline personality disorder, or a disconfirmed diag- evaluations. Analyses were conducted on patients treated
nosis of ADHD. Similarly, Söderström et al. (2014), with psychotropic drugs, but participants with ADHD were
analyzing a medication free ADHD sample with psychiatric washed out from stimulants to perform the cognitive assess-
comorbidities (23/41), found that the QBImpulsivity and ment. Results revealed that the Test of Variable Attention—
QBInattention variables had high specificity but low sensi- TOVA (omission errors and RT variability scores) and the
tivity in their analysis in distinguishing from a mixed clini- Neuropsychological Assessment Battery—NAB (Number
cal group. To explore the diagnostic utility of the instrument, and Letters Part A errors) were able to discriminate between
a stepwise discriminant function analysis was conducted. ADHD patients and suspect efforts even though with low
The variable with the highest discriminant power was specificity; thus, the study concluded that impairment in
QBInattention, with a discriminant load of 0.826. neurocognitive performance is not sufficient to distinguish
Fasmer et al. (2016), instead, objectively measured cog- ADHD patients from those who exaggerate or mimic
nitive performance in ADHD and mood or anxiety patients symptoms.
using the Conner’s continuous performance test second edi- A more recent study (Pettersson et al., 2018) examined a
tion (CPT-II). The ADHD group, which suffered from a mix range of cognitive domains (see Table 1) in a naturalistic
of comorbidities specifically assessed by MINI Interview psychiatric sample of stimulant-free ADHD and non-
and multiple psychometric scales, was treated by psycho- ADHD patients, both characterized by Axis I and Axis II
tropic drug treatment but no stimulants. Compared to the diagnoses made through semi-structured interviews SCID-I
clinical control group, they had more omission and com- and SCID-II. The authors showed significant differences
mission errors and a higher reaction time variability. In between groups in the RAVLT memory and learning test, in
addition, the authors analyzed the differences between the attentional processes assessed by CPT II (Commission
subtypes of ADHD (combined type and inattentive type), errors, Hit reaction time SE, and Variability of SE) and in
observing that the only difference was a higher reaction some QBTest measures (Inattention, Activity, Omission
time in the combined subtype. errors, and Reaction time variance). However, the study
concluded that only variables from CPT II allow to discrim-
Neuropsychological Assessment Using inate between ADHD and non-ADHD group with a sensi-
tivity of 90% and a specificity of 83.3% if used in
Multiple Objective Measures
combination with the Diagnostic Interview for ADHD in
The other studies included in the analysis used a combined adults (DIVA).
approach of different objective neuropsychological mea- Consistent with the results just mentioned about the use-
sures to distinguish ADHD patients from those with other fulness of combined approach (e.g., a CPT with a clinical
psychiatric disorders. diagnostic instrument), Nikolas et al. (2019) used a large
First of all, Holst and Thorell (2013) explored numerous subset of neuropsychological tests at clinical evaluation of
cognitive domains using a variety of objective measures to ADHD, comparing the performance of the ADHD group,
see if the objective measures chosen (see Table 1) could not under stimulants during the cognitive testing, to a group
better differentiate between real-word ADHD patients with with unipolar depression and a healthy control group. All
mixed comorbidities (DSM-IV) and those with other psy- patients underwent a clinical evaluation for depressive and
chiatric disorders (mostly mood and/or anxiety disorders). anxious symptoms through the BDI and BAI scales,
6 Journal of Attention Disorders 00(0)

showing comparable levels of anxiety between the ADHD To date, there is no current support for any single test or
and clinical groups but significantly different levels of battery of neuropsychological tests that can satisfactorily
depressive symptoms, significantly major for the latter, even distinguish between individuals with ADHD and those with
when no codified ADHD comorbidities were specified. other psychiatric diagnoses.
Results of the stepwise binary logistic regression analysis of Two large studies of Guo et al. (2021, 2022) showed no
ADHD performance versus depressed patients, revealed that differences in neuropsychological impairments in ADHD
worse performance on the episodic verbal learning CVLT sample compared to other psychiatric patients, displaying
short-delay free recall and TOVA reaction time were associ- how neuropsychological impairments are prominent in psy-
ated with membership in the ADHD group, with a classifica- chiatric outpatients seeking a clinical evaluation of adult
tion accuracy of 70.4%. Moreover, the authors calculated cut ADHD but are not specific for ADHD itself. Indeed, the
scores for six measures (CVLT short delay free recall, difference recorded in the two studies (Guo et al., 2021,
Salthouse Listening Span trials, DKEFS inhibition/switch- 2022) in the omission errors of the CFADHD of VTS high-
ing, TOVA reaction time variability, reaction time, and omis- lights how the neurocognitive measures are inconsistent
sion errors) that maximized sensitivity and specificity for and conflicting in the diagnostic assessment. However,
each of it in predicting ADHD group membership and next given the potential influence of psychostimulant treatment,
evaluated the utility of using these cut scores in combination the lack of difference between groups should be discussed.
with rating scale information’s. Of the six neuropsychologi- In fact, the authors make no mention of whether or not the
cal measures, TOVA allowed the combined approach to ADHD patients were receiving any kind of treatment.
reach the 87% of classification accuracy. Previous studies on the effects of psychostimulants on cog-
Exploring neuropsychological functioning of individu- nitive functions have yielded conflicting results (Bron et al.,
als at clinical evaluation of adult ADHD with the Test bat- 2014; Tucha et al., 2011). For example, methylphenidate
tery Cognitive Functions ADHD (CFADHD) of the Vienna has been shown in ADHD patients to be effective in improv-
Test System (VTS), Guo et al. (2021) recorded no meaning- ing attention (Hadar et al., 2021) and other higher-order
ful differences between patients with ADHD and the clini- cognitive functions (Butzbach et al., 2019; Fuermaier et al.,
cal comparison group (CCG). At the evaluation, ADHD 2017; Rubio Morrell & Hernández Expósito, 2019), poten-
patients showed psychiatric comorbidities (see Table 1), but tially reducing the cognitive gap between ADHD and non-
no information was provided about the diagnostic criteria or ADHD patients. Nevertheless, even if in a small sample,
the ADHD treatment status. In line with this, 1 year later Holst and colleagues highlighted how including partici-
and on an independent sample with similar features (comor- pants on medication did not appears to significantly affect
bidities/treatment), the same authors found no difference in the results; indeed, the authors conducted some comple-
neurocognitive measures between the study groups, except mentary analyses excluding patients on medications and
for the number of omissions in the vigilance test (Guo et al., found only small differences between the two groups (Holst
2022). et al., 2013). Furthermore, the observation of similar pat-
terns of neuropsychological functions between ADHD and
other psychiatric patients could be supported by the view of
Discussion
ADHD as dimensional construct, with ADHD-like symp-
The purpose of this paper was to conduct a literature review toms and impairments occurring in large proportion of the
on the discriminative ability of neuropsychological objec- general psychiatric population (McLennan, 2016).
tive measures in the diagnosis of adult ADHD when com- Even studies on free-stimulant ADHD samples that
pared to a clinical sample. found differences in objective neuropsychological mea-
Guidelines vary on the importance of including a neuro- sures, highlighted a general lack of specificity in distin-
psychological evaluation to establish the diagnosis (Canadian guishing ADHD patients from other psychiatric patients
ADHD Resource Alliance [CADDRA], 2018; Fuermaier et (Edebol et al., 2012; Fasmer et al., 2016; Holst & Thorell,
al., 2019; National Collaborating Centre for Mental Health, 2013; Pettersson et al., 2018). Thus, these studies concluded
2009) and the role of objective neuropsychological tests in that the performance on neuropsychological tests indepen-
the adult ADHD diagnostic algorithm is still debated dently from treatment status may have limited incremental
(Barkley & Murphy, 2010; Barkley, 2019; Mapou, 2019). value in supporting the psychiatric differential diagnosis.
According to the literature, neuropsychological tasks A possible exception might be the Continuous Per­
have high sensitivity and specificity in distinguishing formance Tests (e.g., the CPTII or TOVA test), a widely used
ADHD patients from healthy controls (Barkley & neuropsychological approach to assess attentional deficits
Grodzinsky, 1994; Doyle et al., 2000; Lovejoy et al., 1999); troughs the evaluation of omission or commission errors, the
however, the role of neuropsychological measurements in reaction time and its variability. Indeed, in a combined
distinguishing ADHD patients from other psychiatric approach with clinical interview instruments, a continuous
patients is inconsistent. performance test appears to be capable of distinguishing
Rosso et al. 7

individuals with adult ADHD from mixed clinical control In conclusion, the role of neurocognitive assessment in
groups (Fasmer et al., 2016; Marshall et al., 2016; Nikolas et the diagnosis of adult ADHD should be deepened.
al., 2019; Pettersson et al., 2018). In particular, parameters The results of this review highlights that up to date no
belonging to the Inattention item (i.e., Omission Errors and specific test or structured neurocognitive batteries are use-
RT Variability) of the QBTest Plus (Qb+), a CPT combining ful to discriminate between patients with adult ADHD and
also the evaluation of the motor activity, has shown high patients with other psychiatric disorders. However, despite
specificity in differentiating the two cohort of patients the stated limitations, some continuous performance test
(Edebol et al., 2012; Söderström et al., 2014). This makes items have shown promising sensitivity and specificity in
CPTs, in combination with clinical interviews, potentially helping to identify adult patients with ADHD.
promising tools for assessing the core symptoms of ADHD Even in light of the high rate of psychiatric comorbidi-
in psychiatric patients. ties acting as possible confounders, additional efforts are
The findings of this review should be considered under needed to develop objective measures targeted to the core
certain limitations. and specific domains of ADHD, perhaps by combining dif-
First of all, ADHD patients had comorbid psychiatric ferent CPTs items with evidence supporting their discrimi-
disorders in almost all of the studies examined (Edebol et native power. Further prospective, controlled studies on
al., 2012; Fasmer et al., 2016; Guo et al., 2021, 2022; Holst larger samples, conducted with a more rigorous methodol-
& Thorell, 2013; Marshall et al., 2016; Pettersson et al., ogy in defining the ADHD group and the clinical control
2018; Söderström et al., 2014). These may have affected the group, are needed to give a definitive answer on whether or
cognitive performance of the patients, making it difficult to not cognitive assessment is useful in the differential diagno-
isolate the contribution of other conditions from the effect sis of adult ADHD.
of ADHD itself. However, in clinical practice the comorbid-
ity between ADHD and other psychiatric disorders is usual, Declaration of Conflicting Interests
as confirmed by epidemiological studies (Haavik et al., The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with
2010; Katzman et al., 2017). Patients seeking evaluation for respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this
ADHD frequently exhibit overlapping symptoms with other article.
disorders, such as anxiety or depression, making differen-
tial diagnosis one of the major challenges in the diagnostic Funding
assessment (Luo et al., 2019; McGough et al., 2005; The author(s) received no financial support for the research,
Whlstedt et al., 2009). Therefore, the samples of the studies authorship, and/or publication of this article.
included in this review are representative of real-world
adult patients with ADHD. ORCID iDs
Moreover, in the majority of these studies, the comorbid- Caterina Portaluppi https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1984-7346
ity rates between the ADHD group and the control group
Elena Teobaldi https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0806-057X
are similar (Fasmer et al., 2016; Guo et al., 2022; Pettersson
et al., 2018; Söderström et al., 2014), supporting the hypoth- References
esis that any potential group differences that occurred in
cognitive performances are attributable to ADHD. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and sta-
tistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). American
Second, the studies described here were not always com-
Psychiatric Press.
parable because of differences in methodologies (e.g., neuro- Aron, A. R., Dowson, J. H., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W.
cognitive batteries versus single objective cognitive measures). (2003). Methylphenidate improves response inhibition in
Third, most studies have been performed in small samples and adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological
the clinical control group were often heterogeneous or not Psychiatry, 54(12), 1465–1468. https://doi.org/10.1016/
well identified by defined diagnostic criteria. Moreover, as s0006-3223(03)00609-7
previously discussed, ADHD patients on medication with Barkley, R. A. (2019). Neuropsychological testing is not useful in
central stimulants were sometimes mixed together with drug- the diagnosis of ADHD: Stop it (or prove it)! ADHD Report,
naive ones (Guo et al., 2021, 2022), making the results less 27(2), 1–8.
solid as previous studies have found that pharmacological Barkley, R. A., & Grodzinsky, G. M. (1994). Are tests of frontal
treatments improve cognitive performance (e.g., Aron et al., lobe functions useful in the diagnosis of attention deficit dis-
orders? Clinical Neuropsychologist, 8(2), 121–139.
2003; Boonstra et al., 2005). Lastly, ADHD has been col-
Barkley, R.A., & Murphy, K.R. (2010). Impairment in occupa-
lapsed into one diagnostic group without analyzing cognitive tional functioning and adult ADHD: the predictive utility of
function in different subtypes (inattentive, hyperactive and executive function (EF) ratings versus EF tests. Archives of
combined). In fact, different subtypes of ADHD may correlate Clinical Neuropsycholog, 25(3):157–173.
differently with neurocognitive measures, affecting the degree Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Spencer, T. J., Mick, E., Monuteaux,
of sensitivity and specificity of the instruments (Salvi et al., M. C., & Aleardi, M. (2006). Functional impairments in adults
2019). with self-reports of diagnosed ADHD: A controlled study
8 Journal of Attention Disorders 00(0)

of 1001 adults in the community. The Journal of Clinical Gjervan, B., Torgersen, T., Nordahl, H. M., & Rasmussen, K.
Psychiatry, 67, 524–540. (2012). Functional impairment and occupational outcome in
Boonstra, A. M., Kooij, J. J., Oosterlaan, J., Sergeant, J. A., adults with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 16(7),
& Buitelaar, J. K. (2010). To act or not to act, that’s the 544–552 https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054711413074
problem: Primarily inhibition difficulties in adult ADHD. Grazioli, V. S., Gmel, G., Rougemont-Bücking, A., Baggio, S.,
Neuropsychology, 24(2), 209–221. Daeppen, J. B., & Studer, J. (2019). Attention deficit hyper-
Boonstra, A. M., Oosterlaan, J., Sergeant, J. A., & Buitelaar, J. activity disorder and future alcohol out-comes: Examining
K. (2005). Executive functioning in adult ADHD: A meta- the roles of coping and enhancement drinking motives among
analytic review. Psychological Medicine, 35(8), 1097–1108. young men. PLoS One, 14, e0218469. https://doi.org/10.1371/
Bron, T. I., Bijlenga, D., Boonstra, A. M., Breuk, M., Pardoen, W. F., journal.pone.0218469
Beekman, A. T., & Kooij, J. J. (2014). OROS-methylphenidate Guo, N., Fuermaier, A. B. M., Koerts, J., Mueller, B. W., Diers,
efficacy on specific executive functioning deficits in adults K., Mroß, A., Mette, C., Tucha, L., & Tucha, O. (2021).
with ADHD: A randomized, placebo-controlled cross-over Neuropsychological functioning of individuals at clinical
study. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 24(4), 519– evaluation of adult ADHD. Journal of Neural Transmission,
528. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.01.007 128(7), 877–891. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00702-020-02281-0
Butzbach, M., Fuermaier, A. B. M., Aschenbrenner, S., Weisbrod, Guo, N., Fuermaier, A. B. M., Koerts, J., Tucha, O., Scherbaum,
M., Tucha, L., & Tucha, O. (2019). Basic processes as foun- N., & Müller, B. W. (2022). Networks of neuro-psycho-
dations of cognitive impairment in adult ADHD. Journal logical functions in the clinical evaluation of adult ADHD.
of Neural Transmission, 126(10), 1347–1362. https://doi. Assessment. Advance online publication. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s00702-019-02049-1 org/10.1177/10731911221118673
Canadian ADHD Resource Alliance. (2018). Canadian ADHD Haavik, J., Halmoy, A., Lundervold, A. J., & Fasmer, O. B.
practice guidelines (4th ed.). (2010). Clinical assessment and diagnosis of adults with
Doyle, A. E., Biederman, J., Seidman, L. J., Weber, W., & Faraone, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Expert Review of
S. V. (2000). Diagnostic efficiency of neuropsychological test Neurotherapeutics, 10, 1569–1580. https://doi.org/10.1586/
scores for discriminating boys with and without attention defi- ern.10.149
cit-hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Hadar, Y., Hocherman, S., Lamm, O., & Tirosh, E. (2021). The
Psychology, 68(3), 477–488. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022- visuo-motor attention test in boys with attention deficit hyper-
006X.68.3.477 activity disorder (ADHD): Methylphenidate-placebo random-
Edebol, H., Helldin, L., & Norlander, T. (2012). Objective mea- ized controlled trial. Child Psychiatry & Human Development,
sures of behavior manifestations in adult ADHD and differen- 52(1), 96–103. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-020-00993-8
tiation from participants with bipolar II disorder, borderline Holst, Y., & Thorell, L. B. (2013). Neuropsychological function-
personality disorder, participants with disconfirmed ADHD ing in adults with ADHD and adults with other psychiat-
as well as normative participants. Clinical Practice and ric disorders: The issue of specificity. Journal of Attention
Epidemiology in Mental Health, 8, 134–143. https://doi. Disorders, 21(2), 137–148.
org/10.2174/1745017901208010134 Katzman, M. A., Bilkey, T. S., Chokka, P. R., Fallu, A., & Klassen,
Fasmer, O. B., Mjeldheim, K., Førland, W., Hansen, A. L., L. J. (2017). Adult ADHD and comorbid disorders: Clinical
Syrstad, V. E., Oedegaard, K. J., & Berle, J. Ø. (2016). Linear implications of a dimensional approach. BMC Psychiatry,
and non-linear analyses of Conner’s continuous performance 17(1), 302. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-017-1463-3
test-II discriminate adult patients with attention deficit hyper- Kooij, J. J. S., Huss, M., Asherson, P., Akehurst, R., Beusterien, K.,
activity disorder from patients with mood and anxiety disor- French, A., Sasané, R., & Hodgkins, P. (2012). Distinguishing
ders. BMC Psychiatry, 16(1), 284. https://doi.org/10.1186/ comorbidity and successful management of adult ADHD.
s12888-016-0993-4 Journal of Attention Disorders, 16(5_suppl), 3S–19S. https://
Fuermaier, A. B. M., Fricke, J. A., de Vries, S. M., Tucha, L., doi.org/10.1177/1087054711435361
& Tucha, O. (2019). Neuropsychological assessment of Lange, K. W., Hauser, J., Lange, K. M., Makulska-Gertruda,
adults with ADHD: A Delphi consensus study. Applied E., Takano, T., Takeuchi, Y., Tucha, L., & Tucha, O.
Neuropsychology Adult, 26(4), 340–354. (2014). Utility of cognitive neuropsychological assess-
Fuermaier, A. B. M., Tucha, L., Guo, N., Mette, C., Müller, B. W., ment in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Attention
Scherbaum, N., & Tucha, O. (2022). It takes time: Vigilance Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, 6(4), 241–248. https://doi.
and sustained attention assessment in adults with ADHD. org/10.1007/s12402-014-0132-3
International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Lovejoy, D. W., Ball, J. D., Keats, M., Stutts, M. L., Spain, E.
Health, 19(9), 5216. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19095216 H., Janda, L., & Janusz, J. (1999). Neuropsychological perfor-
Fuermaier, A. B. M., Tucha, L., Koerts, J., Aschenbrenner, S., mance of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
Kaunzinger, I., Hauser, J., & Tucha, O. (2015). Cognitive (ADHD): Diagnostic classification estimates for measures of
impairment in adult ADHD– perspective matters! frontal lobe/executive functioning. Journal of International
Neuropsychology, 29(1), 45–58. Neuropsychological Society, 5(3), 222–233. https://doi.
Fuermaier, A. B. M., Tucha, L., Koerts, J., Weisbrod, M., Lange, org/10.1017/s1355617799533055
K. W., Aschenbrenner, S., & Tucha, O. (2017). Effects of Luo, Y., Weibman, D., Halperin, J. M., & Li, X. (2019). A review
methylphenidate on memory functions of adults with ADHD. of heterogeneity in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder
Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 24(3), 199–211. (ADHD). Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 13, 42.
Rosso et al. 9

Mapou, R. L. (2019). Counterpoint: Neuropsychological testing is hyperactivity disorder: Meta-analysis. British Journal of
not useful in the diagnosis of ADHD, but. . .. ADHD Report, Psychiatry,0 194, 204–211. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.
27(2), 8–12. bp.107.048827
Marshall, P. S., Hoelzle, J. B., Heyerdahl, D., & Nelson, N. W. Söderström, S., Pettersson, R., & Nilsson, K. W. (2014).
(2016). The impact of failing to identify suspect effort in Quantitative and subjective behavioural aspects in the assess-
patients undergoing adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity ment of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in
disorder (ADHD) assessment. Psychological Assessment, adults. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 68(1), 30–37. https://
28(10), 1290–1302. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000247 doi.org/10.3109/08039488.2012.762940
Marshall, P. S., Hoelzle, J. B., & Nikolas, M. (2021). Diagnosing Song, P., Zha, M., Yang, Q., Zhang, Y., Li, X., & Rudan, I.
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) in young (2021). The prevalence of adult attention-deficit hyperactiv-
adults: A qualitative review of the utility of assessment mea- ity disorder: A global systematic review and meta-analysis.
sures and recommendations for improving the diagnostic pro- Journal of Global Health, 11, 04009. https://doi.org/10.7189/
cess. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 35(1), 165–198. https:// jogh.11.04009
doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2019.1696409 Torgalsbøen, B. R., Zeiner, P., & Øie, M. G. (2021). Pre-attention
McGough, J. J., Smalley, S. L., McCracken, J. T., Yang, M., and working memory in ADHD: A 25-year follow-up study.
Del’Homme, M., Lynn, D. E., & Loo, S. (2005). Psychiatric Journal of Attention Disorders, 25(7), 895–905. https://doi.
comorbidity in adult attention deficit hyperactivity disor- org/10.1177/1087054719879491
der: Findings from multiplex families. American Journal of Tucha, L., Fuermaier, A. B. M., Koerts, J., Buggenthin, R.,
Psychiatry, 162(9), 1621–1627. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi. Aschenbrenner, S., Weisbrod, M., & Tucha, O. (2017).
ajp.162.9.1621 Sustained attention in adult ADHD: Time-on-task effects of
McLennan, J. D. (2016). Understanding attention deficit hyperac- various measures of attention. Journal of Neural Transmission,
tivity disorder as a continuum. Canadian Family Physician, 124(1), 39–53. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00702-015-1426-0
62(12), 979–982. Tucha, L., Tucha, O., Sontag, T. A., Stasik, D., Laufkötter,
Mucci, F., Avella, M. T., & Marazziti, D. (2019). ADHD with R., & Lange, K. W. (2011). Differential effects of meth-
comorbid bipolar disorders: A systematic review of neuro- ylphenidate on problem solving in adults with ADHD.
biological, clinical and pharmacological aspects across the Journal of Attention Disorders, 15(2), 161–173. https://doi.
lifespan. Current Medicinal Chemistry, 26(38), 6942–6969. org/10.1177/1087054709356391
https://doi.org/10.2174/0929867326666190805153610 Whlstedt, C., Thorell, L. B., & Bohlin, G. (2009). Heterogeneity in
National Collaborating Centre for Mental Health. (2009). Attention ADHD: Neuropsychological pathways, comorbidity and symp-
deficit hyperactivity disorder: Diagnosis and management of tom domains. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37(4),
ADHD in children, young people and adults. 551–564.
Nikolas, M. A., Marshall, P., & Hoelzle, J. B. (2019). The role of Xenaki, L. A., & Pehlivanidis, A. (2015). Clinical, neuropsycho-
neurocognitive tests in the assessment of adult attention-def- logical and structural convergences and divergences between
icit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Assessment, 31(5), Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Borderline
685–698. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000688 Personality Disorder: A systematic review. Personality and
Onandia-Hinchado, I., Pardo-Palenzuela, N., & Diaz-Orueta, U. Individual Differences, 86, 438–449. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
(2021). Cognitive characterization of adult at-tention defi- paid.2015.06.049
cit hyperactivity disorder by domains: A systematic review.
Journal of Neural Transmission, 128(7), 893–937. https://doi. Author Biographies
org/10.1007/s00702-021-02302-6 Gianluca Rosso, MD, PhD, psychiatrist, is Associate Professor of
Paris, J., Bhat, V., & Thombs, B. (2015). Is adult attention- psychiatry at Department of Neurosciences, University of Turin,
deficit hyperactivity disorder being overdiagnosed? Italy. He is currently working at Psychiatric Unit of San Luigi
Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 60(7), 324–328. https://doi. Gonzaga University Hospital of Orbassano (Turin), Italy.
org/10.1177/070674371506000705
Pettersson, R., Soderstrom, S., & Nilsson, K. W. (2018). Caterina Portaluppi clinical psychologist, is research scholar-
Diagnosing ADHD in adults: An examination of the dis- ship recipient at Department of Neurosciences, University of
criminative validity of neuropsychological tests and diagnos- Turin, Italy. She is currently working at Psychiatric Unit of San
tic assessment instruments. Journal of Attention Disorders, Luigi Gonzaga University Hospital of Orbassano (Turin), Italy.
22(11), 1019–1031. Elena Teobaldi, MD, psychiatrist, is PhD student in Neuroscience
Rubio Morell, B., & Hernández Expósito, S. (2019). Differential at Department of Neurosciences, University of Turin, Italy.
long-term medication impact on executive function and delay
Gabriele Di Salvo, MD, PhD, psychiatrist, is Researcher in psy-
aversion in ADHD. Applied Neuropsychology Child, 8(2),
chiatry at Department of Neurosciences, University of Turin,
140–157. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2017.1407653
Italy. He is currently working at Psychiatric Unit of San Luigi
Salvi, V., Migliarese, G., Venturi, V., Rossi, F., Torriero, S., Viganò,
Gonzaga University Hospital of Orbassano (Turin), Italy.
V., Cerveri, G., & Mencacci, C. (2019). ADHD in adults:
Clinical subtypes and associated characteristics. Rivista di Giuseppe Maina, MD, psychiatrist, is Full Professor of psychia-
Psichiatria, 54(2), 84–89. https://doi.org/10.1708/3142.31249 try at Department of Neurosciences, University of Turin, Italy. He
Simon, V., Czobor, P., Bálint, S., Mészáros, A., & Bitter, I. is the head of the Psychiatric Unit of San Luigi Gonzaga University
(2009). Prevalence and correlates of adult attention-deficit Hospital of Orbassano (Turin), Italy.

View publication stats

You might also like