You are on page 1of 12

773927

research-article2018
JADXXX10.1177/1087054718773927Journal of Attention DisordersLeRoy et al.

Article
Journal of Attention Disorders

Neuropsychological Performance
1­–12
© The Author(s) 2018
Reprints and permissions:
Patterns of Adult ADHD Subtypes sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1087054718773927
https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054718773927
journals.sagepub.com/home/jad

Amy LeRoy1, Claudia Jacova1, and Caedy Young1

Abstract
Objective: Neuropsychological performance patterns associated with adult ADHD subtypes are unknown. The aim of
the current systematic review was to identify and synthesize available literature regarding neuropsychological performance
associated with adult ADHD subtypes. Method: Searches were completed using the databases PsycINFO and PubMed
for studies published before March 2017 addressing adult ADHD subtypes and neuropsychological performance. Data
characterizing the neuropsychological tests utilized in each study were obtained and sorted into eight domains. To summarize
the results of all comparisons (ADHD subtype compared with control, or to each other), we counted the proportion of
tests within each domain with significant group differences. Results/Conclusion: We deemed four domains informative
in differentiating ADHD subtypes from controls. Of these, memory was the only domain that held promise in distinguishing
ADHD-Inattentive and ADHD-Combined. Limitations of the available literature are highlighted and recommendations for
future research are provided. (J. of Att. Dis. XXXX; XX[X] XX-XX)

Keywords
adult ADHD, subtype, cognition, neuropsychology, systematic review

Introduction subtypes, with ADHD-I as the most prevalent affecting 45%


of the ADHD population. ADHD-C follows with 34%, and
ADHD was first described as a lifelong condition with three ADHD-H is the least prevalent with 21% of the adult ADHD
specific subtypes in the fifth and current version of the population (Woo & Rey, 2005). Psychiatric comorbidity also
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th varies for subtypes. In comparison to ADHD-I, ADHD-C
ed.; DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association [APA], shows increased comorbidity with anxiety and substance use
2013). Prior to this description, ADHD was known as a dis- disorders (Murphy, Barkley, & Bush, 2002). In addition,
order of childhood that was due to a motoric disinhibition subtypes also appear to experience adverse life events differ-
(2nd ed.; DSM-II; APA, 1968), due to a deficit in attention ently. In comparison to ADHD-I and ADHD-H, ADHD-C
(3rd ed.; DSM-III; APA, 1980), or due to a deficit in atten- experience significantly more suicide attempts and lifetime
tion and hyperactivity combined (3rd ed., rev.; DSM-III-R; arrests (Murphy et al., 2002).
APA, 1987). The DSM-IV was the first to describe ADHD The diagnosis of adult ADHD is difficult because current
as a disorder that was characterized by varying degrees of diagnostic criteria rely on childhood symptoms (Barkley &
inattention and hyperactivity (4th ed.; APA, 1994). Biederman, 1997). This overreliance on childhood symp-
Depending on the degree of inattention and/or hyperactiv- toms does not take into account the differences in presenta-
ity, individuals were categorized into subtypes, including tion that may occur as individuals with ADHD age. A gold
the Inattentive (ADHD-I), the Hyperactive (ADHD-H), or standard for an accurate diagnosis of ADHD comprised of
the Combined (ADHD-C) subtype. This conceptualization multiple components has been agreed upon (Davidson,
of ADHD has continued into the present through the DSM- 2008; Gualtieri & Johnson, 2005; Kooij et al., 2010; Taylor
IV-TR (4th ed., text rev.; APA, 2000) and now into the DSM- et al., 2004). The first component is a confirmation of child-
5 (APA, 2013) with the addition of the adult presentation. hood symptoms to ensure ADHD onset was prior to the age
Given how recently the fourth and fifth versions of the DSM
were published, research on the differentiation of adult
ADHD from childhood ADHD, and ADHD subtypes from 1
Pacific University, Hillsboro, OR, USA
each other is still in its early stages.
Corresponding Author:
The extant research evidence on adult ADHD subtypes is Claudia Jacova, Associate Professor, School of Graduate Psychology,
sparse but suggests that differences do exist between some, Pacific University, 222 SE 8th Ave., Hillsboro, OR 97123, USA.
if not all, subtypes. Prevalence, for instance, varies for Email: cjacova@pacificu.edu
2 Journal of Attention Disorders 00(0)

of 12 years (APA, 2013). The second component is the Barkley’s hypotheses suggest deficits in executive function-
assessment of current symptoms to determine whether ing for ADHD-H and ADHD-C, and more profound and
ADHD symptoms are still present. The third and fourth specific deficits in attention for ADHD-I.
components of the diagnostic process are ADHD symptom To date, there are 13 reviews that address the neuropsy-
rating scales and an evaluation of family history related to chological performance of adult ADHD and that were pub-
ADHD, considering that genetic factors play a primary etio- lished prior to March 2017 when the current review was
logical role in ADHD (Faraone & Mick, 2010; Gizer, Ficks, conducted. All previous reviews had common limitations.
& Waldman, 2009; Larsson et al., 2013; Larsson, Chang, The first limitation was a lack of a comprehensive assess-
D’Onofrio, & Lichtenstein, 2014; Lesch et al., 2008; Levy, ment, with evaluation of only one cognitive domain or par-
Hay, McStephen, Wood, & Waldman, 1997; Sprich, ticipants’ IQ (Bridgett & Walker, 2006; Kofler et al., 2013;
Biederman, Crawford, Mundy, & Faraone, 2000). Finally, Skodzik, Holling, & Pedersen, 2017). The second limitation
to obtain the only objective data in the diagnostic process, was a generalization of performance across subtypes that
the fifth and final component is neuropsychological testing did not allow for distinction of neuropsychological perfor-
(Gualtieri & Johnson, 2005). In this phase of diagnosis, mance patterns by subtype (Alderson, Kasper, Hudec, &
cognitive difficulties are outlined. In children with ADHD, Patros, 2013; Boonstra, Oosterlaan, Sergeant, & Buitelaar,
defining cognitive strengths and difficulties through a neu- 2005; Davidson, 2008; Gallagher & Blader, 2001; Hervey,
ropsychological evaluation is crucial to elucidate the typical Epstein, & Curry, 2004; Kebir & Joober, 2011; Schoechlin
pattern of impairment that is seen with those with ADHD & Engel, 2005; Weyandt & DuPaul, 2006; Woods, Lovejoy,
and aid in the correct diagnosis. Additional research is nec- & Ball, 2002). In general, these reviews found that adults
essary for differentiating adult and childhood ADHD, but with ADHD performed worse than controls on tests of
there may be similar clinical utility of the neuropsychologi- memory and some tests of executive functioning (Schoechlin
cal profile in adults, revealing the individual’s strengths and & Engel, 2005; Woods et al., 2002). There was some dis-
weaknesses, providing a positive and accurate diagnosis of agreement in the executive functioning domain regarding
ADHD, and aiding in treatment. the specific tests that differentiated between ADHD and
Determining the neuropsychological profile of adult controls (Schoechlin & Engel, 2005; Woods et al., 2002).
ADHD could aid in the accurate diagnosis of adult ADHD Only one review addressed adult ADHD subtypes in rela-
subtypes. Neuropsychological findings in children with tion to neuropsychological performance (Willcutt et al.,
ADHD suggest that subtypes differ in their performance, 2012). This review required comparison of at least two sub-
with children with ADHD-C and ADHD-I showing deficits types in the included articles, was primarily interested in
in processing speed, and the executive functions of inhibition validating the DSM-IV symptomatology, and assessed pre-
and planning, compared with children with ADHD-H. In dominantly executive functioning performance. This review
addition, in comparison to ADHD-I and ADHD-H, children only included literature up to 2010, 3 years prior to the pub-
with ADHD-C show deficits in perceptual reasoning (Woo & lication of the DSM-5 (APA, 2013) and a surge of published
Rey, 2005). While childhood ADHD has been well character- studies on adult ADHD. In sum, no review to date addresses
ized neuropsychologically, the evidence may not apply to a comprehensive picture of the available literature regard-
adult ADHD because of suggested differences in symptom ing subtype differences in neuropsychological performance
presentation and manifestation (Asherson et al., 2012; that is inclusive of the DSM-5 and adult presentation. It is
Gualtieri & Johnson, 2005; Matza, Paramore, & Prasad, likely that subtype differences have contributed to the lack
2005; Resnick, 2005; Sawyer et al., 2002). In sum, it is likely of clarity of the overall profile.
that there are neuropsychological subtype differences, and The objective of this review was to evaluate and synthe-
these may not be the same in adult as in childhood ADHD. size the literature covering neuropsychological performance
Various hypotheses exist regarding neuropsychological associated with adult ADHD subtypes. In line with Barkley’s
performance patterns associated with ADHD subtypes. The hypotheses, we hypothesized that those with ADHD-I per-
most widely proposed theory regarding subtype neuropsy- form worse on measures of memory, attention, and process-
chological performance differences was proposed by ing speed, whereas those with ADHD-C and ADHD-H
Barkley (1997). This theory was based on animal and perform worse on measures of executive functioning. Given
human research literature pertaining to the prefrontal cor- the lack of literature and hypotheses regarding other neuro-
tex, the primary brain region implicated in the development psychological domains, we examined subtype performance
of ADHD. Barkley’s theory suggests that the functional differences in these domains without a priori hypotheses.
deficits of ADHD-I are different from those of ADHD-H
and ADHD-C. The latter two are most impaired on mea-
Method
sures of executive functioning and sustained attention
whereas for ADHD-I, Barkley hypothesized deficits in The present systematic review was conducted in accordance
memory, selective attention, and processing speed. Overall, with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews
LeRoy et al. 3

and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) statement (Moher, Liberati, studies. Included studies were reviewed independently by
Tetzlaff, & Altman, 2009). the third author (C.Y.) with 83.3% of agreement on article
selection. Disagreements in final article selection were
resolved by discussion among the raters. The full list of
Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
included and excluded articles at each step is provided in a
We used the following criteria to determine inclusion of full PRISMA flowchart (see Figure 1; Moher et al., 2009).
texts that could be included in our review. First, the full text
of the articles had to be in the English language and include
Data Extraction and Analyses
adult human participants (aged 18 years and above). Second,
articles needed to assess individuals with ADHD, clinically For each study, sample and study characteristics were
diagnosed prior to the study assessment or meeting criteria extracted. Data characterizing the neuropsychological tests
for ADHD as determined by clinical tools (i.e., standardized utilized in each study were also obtained and sorted into
ADHD scales with normative data) used during the assess- eight commonly used domains that they are known, or
ment. Third, ADHD had to be diagnosed using the DSM-IV assumed, to measure (see Table 1, for an overview of the
(APA, 1994), DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000), or DSM-5 (APA, domains and tests allocated to each domain; Edwards,
2013) criteria. Articles that included adults diagnosed with Jacova, Sepehry, Pratt, & Benavente, 2013; Jacova et al.,
any version of ADHD using the DSM-II or DSM-III were 2012). The domains included executive functioning, work-
not included due to the fact that they conceptualize and ing memory, memory, attention, processing speed, percep-
diagnose ADHD subtypes differently than what is currently tual reasoning, language, and motor abilities. These
done in the field. Fourth, ADHD subtypes had to be clearly particular domains were chosen to address a comprehensive
identified and used in the analysis of the neuropsychologi- profile of neuropsychological performance. Many measures
cal measures. Studies that only assessed ADHD symptoms reported multiple subscores within tests (i.e., Continuous
(i.e., hyperactive, impulsive, and inattentive symptoms) in Performance Test that measured both omission and commis-
relation to neuropsychological performance were excluded. sion errors). All subscores within measures were assessed to
This was done in an effort to ensure that only individuals verify that they fit within the domain the overall measure
who met diagnostic criteria for ADHD were compared in was sorted into. Three measures were separated and sorted
relation to neuropsychological performance, rather than by their subscores to ensure domains did not capture the per-
individuals who experienced some of the symptomatology formance of a separate domain; these measures included the
but did not meet diagnostic criteria. Fifth, to ensure we were Go/No-Go task, the Stop-Signal task, and the Stop-Change
able to obtain a comprehensive profile with studies that task that were all separated by their Go and No-Go sub-
adequately addressed neuropsychological performance, scores. Subscores within each measure were sorted into
only studies containing at least two objective (i.e., not self- domains based on agreement between the first (A.L.) and
report) tests of neuropsychological performance were senior author (C.J.). After assigning measures/subscores to
included. Articles that included multiple tests but reported domains, to calculate differences for ADHD subtypes com-
them as a composite score (e.g., reporting only the Full pared with control groups or another ADHD subtype, results
Scale Intelligence Quotient [FSIQ] from the Wechsler Adult were evaluated by counting the proportion of tests within
Intelligence Scale–IV [WAIS-IV]) were excluded. Finally, each domain with significant group differences.
studies needed to be published articles that presented origi-
nal data. Narrative or nonsystematic reviews, case studies, Obtained studies.  Twenty-four studies met our inclusion cri-
letters, editorials, and commentaries were excluded. teria. However, six had to excluded because they did not
present their data in enough detail, or used uncommon tests
and/or experimental designs that were not comparable with
Literature Search other tests. Thus, the final number of studies included in our
The electronic databases PsycINFO and PubMed were review was 16. Due to the stricter inclusion criteria that we
searched for literature published before March 2017 using applied, 11 studies utilized in the previous review of ADHD
adult ADHD, or adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor- subtypes and neuropsychological performance (Willcutt
der combined with subtype, or cognitive, or cognition, or et al., 2012) had to be excluded because they included indi-
neuropsychology, or neuropsychological as keywords. viduals younger than 18 years in their analyses, they
Additional relevant articles were also included by hand- assessed only one neuropsychological task, or the data were
searching reference lists of the retrieved articles. Once all composite (i.e., only providing FSIQ data rather than indi-
articles were collected, title and author screening was per- vidual test performance). In a few studies, along with other
formed first to remove duplicate citations. Following the neuropsychological test scores, composite neuropsycho-
removal of duplicates, the first author (A.L.) screened titles, logical scores (e.g., FSIQ) were provided. In these cases,
abstracts, and full-text citations to determine inclusion of only the single tests were included in the review.
4 Journal of Attention Disorders 00(0)

Idenficaon
Records identified through Additional records identified
database searching through other sources
(n = 5566) (n = 15)

Screening

Records after duplicates removed Records excluded


(n = 1693) (n = 1369)

Articles with unclear


Full-text articles data, uncommon tests or
Eligibility

assessed for eligibility experimental designs


(n = 324) (n = 8)

Full-text articles
excluded, with reasons
(n = 300)
Studies included in
Included

qualitative synthesis
(n = 16)

Figure 1.  PRISMA flow diagram outlining included and excluded articles.
Source. Adapted from “Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses: The PRISMA Statement,” by D. Moher, A. Liberati, J.
Tetzlaff, and D. Altman, 2009,. PLoS Medicine, 6, pp. 1006-1012.

Table 1.  Neuropsychological Domains and Associated Tests.

Functional domains Tests


Executive SOA Script Generation; Simon Task; D-KEFS Colour-Word Interference Test; Stroop (different versions); Stop
functioning Task, TMT B; Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; Tower of London; Object Alternation Test; Controlled Word
Fluency Test; Divergent Thinking Task; Raven Advanced Matrices Test; Letter Fluency; Cancel Underline;
D-KEFS TMT; Stop-Signal Task (No-Go); Stop-Change Task (No-Go); Object Usage; Progressive Planning
Test; Go/No-Go Task (No-Go); WAIS-III Similarities
Attention CPT-II; CPT; Stop-Change Task (Go); Stop-Signal Task (Go); Attentional Blink Task; Posner Visual Orienting
Task; Test of Variables of Attention; Go/No-Go Task (Go); Gordon Diagnostic System
Working memory SOA Working Memory Task; WAIS-III Arithmetic; WAIS-III Digit Span Forward and Backward; WAIS-III
Letter-Number Sequence; “Brown-Peterson Paradigm” Auditory Consonant Trigram Test; Simon Game;
WMS-IV Spatial Span; Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test; University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test
Memory SOA Prospective Memory Task; SOA Retrospective Memory Task; California Verbal Learning Test-II; Benton
Visual Retention Test; Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; modified Diagnosticum fur Cerebralschädigung;
WMS-IV Logical Memory I and II; Shum Visual Learning Test; WMS-IV Visual Reproduction I and II
Processing speed TMT A; WAIS-III Digit Symbol
Perceptual Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (Copy); WAIS-III Block Design
reasoning
Language WAIS-III Vocabulary; Category Fluency
Motor Antisaccade Task

Note. Parentheses refer to the measure within the test used in analyses. SOA = Simulation of Occupational Activities; D-KEFS = Delis–Kaplan Execu-
tive Function System; TMT = Trail Making Test; WAIS-III = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–III; CPT = Continuous Performance Test; WMS =
Wechsler Memory Scale.
LeRoy et al. 5

Table 2.  Sample Characteristics.

n Mean age % male Education (years)

Study Year Subtype ADHD Ctrla ADHD Ctrl ADHD Ctrl ADHD Ctrl
Delisle 2001 C 30 30 39.03 40.00 53.0 53.0 14 14
Suarez 2015 C 15 16 22.13 21.70 73.0 87.5 NR NR
Braun 2013 C 28 30 39.39 39.70 46.0 50.0 14 14
Dinn 2001 C; I; H 13; 8; 4 11 35.60 35.40 36.0 45.5 15 16
Dige 2008 C; I; H 52; 27; 26 NR NR — 48.6 — 11 —
Dobson-Patterson 2016 C; I 16; 16 30 37.90 39.60 43.8 30.0 13 14
Miller 2012 C; I 51-83; 31-40b 71-85b 19.60 19.60 0.0 0.0 NR NR
Bekker 2005 C 24 24 34.30 34.90 50.0 50.0 NR NR
Carr 2006 C; I 27; 23 30 23.80 24.20 72.0 73.0 NR NR
Murphy 2001 C+H; I 60; 36 — 21.10 21.20 75.2 68.8 13 14
Epstein 2001 C; I; H 10; 14; 1 — 33.60 33.40 40.0 50.0 NR NR
Kakuszi 2016 C 33 29 31.60 32.90 75.8 72.4 15 15
Konrad 2012 C 37 34 32.50 30.20 56.8 47.1 13 14
Schweitzer 2006 C; I 17; 16 18 34.90 31.90 63.6 72.2 16 17
Gansler 1998 I; H 16; 14 — 28.90 35.00 NR NR 14 14
Riccio 2005 C; I 18;14 — 22.29 — 56 — 15 —

Note. Ctrl = control; C = combined; I = inattentive; H = hyperactive/impulsive; — = not applicable; NR = not reported.
a
Sample sizes for control groups are only included if they were used in the analysis of neuropsychological performance compared with ADHD subtypes.
b
Sample size varies depending on the test.

Results they allowed medication. The executive functioning domain


was assessed most often (15 studies equating to 94%), fol-
Study Details and Sample Characteristics lowed by attention in 13 (81%) studies, working memory in
As shown in Table 2, the 16 studies included a total of 718 seven (44%) studies, memory in six (38%) studies, percep-
ADHD participants and 337 controls. Within the ADHD tual reasoning in four (25%) studies, processing speed in
participants, 461 (64%) were diagnosed with ADHD-C, three (19%) studies, language in two (13%) studies, and
210 (29%) were diagnosed with ADHD-I, and 47 (7%) motor abilities in one (6%) study. The 16 studies reported
were diagnosed with ADHD-H. Out of the 16 studies, 15 154 different subscores using 52 neuropsychological tests.
(94%) assessed ADHD-C, 10 (63%) assessed ADHD-I, Studies included a range of two to 15 neuropsychological
and 5 (31%) assessed ADHD-H. Participants in both the tests to address cognitive performance. To evaluate if indi-
ADHD and control groups were young, with the average viduals met diagnostic criteria for ADHD, participants were
age ranging from 19.60 to 39.39 years in the ADHD given a range of two to eight diagnostic scales. Diagnostic
groups, and ranging from 19.60 to 40.00 years in the con- scales and participants were judged by one to three separate
trol groups. There was an equal distribution of male and raters to determine interrater reliability for meeting diag-
females within the studies, with the average percent males nostic criteria.
in the ADHD groups ranging from 0% to 75.8% and the
average percent males in the control groups ranging from ADHD Subtypes Versus Controls (Table 4)
0% to 87.5%. The average level of education for the
ADHD groups ranged from 11 to 16 years and 14 to 17 Domains adequately assessed in these comparisons included
years for the control groups; however, four studies did not executive functioning, attention, working memory, and
report average education levels for ADHD or control memory. The domains of language, perceptual reasoning,
groups. processing speed, and motor abilities were assessed by few,
As shown in Table 3, 12 (75%) of the studies compared a single, or no tests.
ADHD subtypes to controls, nine (56%) compared ADHD
subtypes to other subtypes, and one (6%) study compared ADHD-C
ADHD subtypes to normative data. A majority of studies Executive functioning. Tests of executive functioning
did not allow psychostimulant medications for at least 12 were the most frequently administered. Significant group
hours prior to testing (i.e., 69% did not allow any stimulant differences indicating poorer performance in ADHD-C
medications). Three (19%) studies did not report whether were found for 10 out of the 17 (59%) tests.
6 Journal of Attention Disorders 00(0)

Table 3.  Study Characteristics.

Neuropsychological assessment ADHD assessment

Stimulant
Study Design medications? Domainsa No. of tests No. of scales No. of raters
Delisle ADHD/ctrl No: 24-hr wo 1, 2, 3, 4 5 2 1
Suarez ADHD/ctrl No: 6-month wo 1, 2 3 3 2
Braun ADHD/ctrl No: 24-hr wo 1, 2, 3, 4 7 2 1
Dinn ADHD/ctrl Yes 1, 2 5 2 NR
Dige ADHD/ctrl No: None 1, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7 15 2 1
ADHD/ADHD
ADHD/nrm
Dobson-Patterson ADHD/ctrl No: 12-hr wo 1, 2, 4 10 2 NR
ADHD/ADHD
Miller ADHD/ctrl NR 1, 2, 3, 6 7 3 NR
ADHD/ADHD
Bekker ADHD/ctrl No: None 1, 2, 6, 7 4 5 2
Carr ADHD/ctrl No: 24-hr wo 1, 8 2 8 3
ADHD/ADHD
Murphy ADHD/ADHD No: 24-hr wo 1, 2, 3, 5 8 3 2
Epstein ADHD/ADHD No: 24-hr wo 1, 2 3 2 NR
Kakuszi ADHD/ctrl Yes 1, 2 2 2 1
Konrad ADHD/ctrl NR 1, 2 2 4 2
Schweitzer ADHD/ctrl No: 24-hr wo 3 5 4 NR
ADHD/ADHD
Gansler ADHD/ADHD NR 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6 13 2 NR
Riccio ADHD/ADHD No: Day wo 1, 2, 4 9 2 2

Note. Ctrl = normal controls; Nrm = normative data; wo = washout; NR = not reported. a1 = executive functioning; 2 = attention; 3 = working
memory; 4 = memory; 5 = processing speed; 6 = perceptual reasoning; 7 = language; 8 = motor.

Table 4.  Proportion of Tests By Domain With Significant Working Memory.  Tests of working memory ranked sec-
Group Differences (ADHD vs. Controls). ond in terms of tests administered. Of 16 tests, seven (44%)
indicated reliably poorer performance for ADHD-C.
Subtype

Hyperactive/ Memory.  In this domain, seven tests were administered,


Domain Combined Inattentive Impulsive three (43%) of which indicated significantly worse perfor-
mance by the ADHD subtype.
Executive 10/17 (59%) 7/11 (64%) 2/5 (40%)
functioning
Attention 9/11 (82%) 3/4 (75%) 0/1 (0%) Perceptual reasoning.  One of two tests in this domain (50%)
Working memory 7/16 (44%) 6/11 (55%) 1/1 (100%) indicated significantly poorer performance in ADHD-C.
Memory 3/7 (43%) 3/4 (75%) 1/1 (100%)
Processing speed 0/0 0/0 0/0 ADHD-I
Perceptual 1/2 (50%) 1/1 (100%) 0/0 Executive functioning.  Also for this subtype, the executive
reasoning function domain was the most widely addressed. Of 11 tests
Language 0/1 (0%) 0/0 0/0 administered, seven (64%) showed significantly worse per-
Motor 1/1 (100%) 0/1 (0%) 0/0 formance for ADHD-I.

Note. All proportions indicate worse performance by the subtype. p < .05 Attention.  There were only four tests of attention in the
for all significant group differences.
comparisons of ADHD-I to normal controls. Three of these
(75%) showed performance differences, with poorer scores
Attention. Of 11 tests of attention administered, nine for ADHD-I.
(82%) were found to significantly differentiate the perfor-
mance of ADHD-C from normal controls. ADHD-C per- Working Memory.  This domain was also widely assessed
formed worse on all nine tests. in comparing ADHD-I with controls. Eleven tests were given
LeRoy et al. 7

Table 5.  Proportion of Tests By Domain With Significant Group Differences (ADHD vs. ADHD).

Subtype comparison

Combined vs. Inattentive vs. Hyperactive/impulsive


Domain inattentive hyperactive/impulsive vs. combined
Executive functioning 0/16 (0%) 1/8 (13%) I; Ha 0/4 (0%)
Attention 1/7 (14%) C 1/3(33%) I 0/2 (0%)
Working memory 0/14 (0%) 2/6 (33%) I 0/4 (0%)
Memory 4/8 (50%) C 2/7 (29%) H 0/3 (0%)
Processing speed 0/2 (0%) 0/3 (0%) 0/2 (0%)
Perceptual reasoning 0/2 (0%) 1/1 (100%) H 0/1 (0%)
Language 0/2 (0%) 0/2 (0%) 0/2 (0%)
Motor 1/1 (100%) C 0/0 0/0

Note. Letters next to proportions refer to subtype with worse performance in the comparison. p < .05 for all significant group differences. C = ADHD-C;
I = ADHD-I; H = ADHD-H.
a
Each subtype showed worse performance on 13% of tests, separately.

to participants, and six (55%) differentiated the groups, all Processing speed.  No differences were found on two tests of
indicating worse performance for the ADHD subtype. processing speed.

Memory. Of the four tests were administered, three Perceptual reasoning.  No differences emerged on two tests
(75%) revealed group differences, all indicating worse per- of perceptual reasoning.
formance for ADHD-I.
Language.  Neither of the two language tests differenti-
ADHD-H ated between ADHD-C and ADHD-I.
Executive functioning. This subtype was adequately
assessed only in the executive functioning domain. Of five
ADHD-I Versus ADHD-H 
tests administered, two (40%) revealed significantly worse
performance for ADHD-H. Domains assessed by at least two tests in comparisons of
these two subtypes included executive functioning, attention,
working memory, memory, processing speed, and language.
ADHD-C Versus ADHD-I (Table 5) Perceptual reasoning and motor abilities were not assessed.
Domains assessed by at least two tests in comparisons of
these two subtypes included executive functioning, atten- Executive functioning.  The two subtypes were compared on
tion, working memory, memory, processing speed, per- eight tests but only two found significant performance dif-
ceptual reasoning, and language. The motor domain was ferences between the subtypes. One (13%) found signifi-
assessed by a single test. cantlyly worse performance for ADHD-I, the other(13%)
significantly worse performance for ADHD-H.
Executive functioning. ADHD-C and ADHD-I were com-
pared on 16 tests but none revealed significant performance Attention.  The two subtypes were compared on three tests. Only
differences. one (33%) of these tests showed significantly worse performance
for ADHD-I. No other significant differences were found.
Attention.  The two subtypes were compared on seven mea-
sures of attention but only one (14%) found significantly Working memory.  Of the six tests that compared ADHD-I
poorer performance for ADHD-C. with ADHD-H, two (33%) revealed subtype differences,
with significantly worse performance by ADHD-I.
Working memory. In this domain, the two subtypes were
compared on 14 tests, with no significant differences on any Memory. ADHD-H performed significantly worse on two
measure. out of seven (29%) memory tests administered. There were
no other subtype differences.
Memory.  Eight tests of memory were used to compare sub-
type performances. Four (50%) tests showed significant dif- Processing speed.  Three tests of processing speed were uti-
ferences between subtypes. All group differences indicated lized in the comparison of ADHD-I and ADHD-H. No sig-
poorer performance for ADHD-C. nificant differences were found between subtypes.
8 Journal of Attention Disorders 00(0)

Language.  No significant differences were found between adequate coverage of the memory domain, these results can
ADHD-I and ADHD-H on the two tests of language admin- be considered suggestive of genuinely worse performance in
istered to participants. ADHD-C compared to ADHD-I. This finding is not consis-
tent with Barkley’s hypotheses regarding ADHD-I, which
proposed worse memory for this subtype. Barkley’s hypoth-
ADHD-H versus ADHD-C
eses also do not appear supported in the domain of attention
Domains assessed by at least two tests in comparisons of where ADHD-I and ADHD-C appear to perform similarly.
these two subtypes included executive functioning, attention, Because Barkley’s hypotheses were originally conceptual-
working memory, memory, processing speed, and language. ized with childhood ADHD in mind, our lack of support may
Perceptual reasoning and motor abilities were not assessed. suggest that these hypotheses are accurate for childhood but
not for adult subtypes.
Executive functioning. Four tests of executive functioning Few studies evaluated ADHD-H in comparison to con-
were used in comparisons of ADHD-H and ADHD-C. trols. Lower performance was detected in the domains of
There were no significant findings on any of the tests. executive functioning, memory, and working memory, but
this evidence rests on too limited a number of studies to
Attention.  Two tests evaluated differences between ADHD- draw any firm conclusion. ADHD-H comparisons with the
H and ADHD-C. Neither found significant differences other two subtypes revealed a mix of differences (lower as
between subtypes. well as higher performance) compared with ADHD-I, and
no differences compared with ADHD-C despite multiple
Working memory.  Four tests were administered to look at comparisons. However, investigations of ADHD-H are lim-
working memory differences. None were found significant ited and do not allow for any evaluations of Barkley’s
in differentiating performance between ADHD-H and hypotheses regarding this subtype.
ADHD-C. We noted several methodological limitations in the stud-
ies included in this review. A primary limitation was unequal
Memory.  Two tests were used in the memory domain, and representation of subtypes. ADHD-H only made up 7% of
neither test significantly distinguished performance between the ADHD participants in the studies, while ADHD-C made
ADHD-H and ADHD-C. up 64%. This inequality does not accurately reflect the
prevalence of subtypes that is seen in the ADHD population
Processing speed.  Of the two tests that assessed attention dif- as a whole (i.e., ADHD-I is the most prevalent subtype;
ferences between ADHD-H and ADHD-C, neither found a Woo & Rey, 2005), plausibly biasing the findings in this
significant difference between subtypes. review. A second limitation is inadequate coverage of neu-
ropsychological domains. The tests and measures used to
Language.  Only two tests evaluated language and ADHD-H assess neuropsychological performance varied drastically
and ADHD-C differences. No significant differences were across domains and studies. We had to exclude perceptual
found. reasoning, language, and motor measures from further anal-
ysis of all comparisons because few to no tests were admin-
istered in these domains. Neuropsychological studies of
Discussion childhood ADHD have suggested deficits on measures of
In this systematic review, we summarized the existing evi- perceptual reasoning and language (Woo & Rey, 2005).
dence regarding neuropsychological performance differ- Motor abilities have been implicated in both childhood and
ences between ADHD subtypes and attempted to identify adult ADHD research (Rosa Neto, Goulardins, Rigoli, Piek,
performance patterns associated with each subtype. Below & Oliveira, 2015; Stray et al., 2013). Future research should
we discuss our main findings. examine these domains more comprehensively.
ADHD-C and ADHD-I were the two subtypes most ade- A third limitation was missing or inconsistent method-
quately studied. Compared with controls, both appear to per- ological information. Multiple studies did not report
form worse on measures of executive functioning, attention, whether or not they allowed stimulant medication. Studies
working memory, and memory. Direct comparisons of the that did report this information varied on whether or not
two subtypes showed that those with ADHD-C performed they allowed medication prior to testing. Medication is
worse in the memory domain. There were few to no differ- known to normalize neuropsychological performance,
ences in other domains. We inspected the specific memory suggesting performance of individuals with ADHD on
measures that the subtypes were compared on to ascertain psychostimulants, in particular, does not accurately repre-
whether memory was comprehensively assessed in the sub- sent true cognition of ADHD (Kempton et al., 1999).
type comparisons. Measures included immediate and delayed Information on participants’ education level, known to
recall or recognition of visual and verbal materials. Given the affect performance (Finlayson, Johnson, & Reitan, 1977),
LeRoy et al. 9

was also missing in multiple studies. Finally, the number Conclusion


of raters performing the diagnostic assessment for ADHD
was often not reported, thus calling into question the accu- Adult ADHD is a complex disorder with three distinct sub-
rate diagnosis of ADHD and its subtypes. The gaps and types. Neuropsychological evaluations may aid in the diag-
inconsistencies in the information provided make the find- nosis of overall ADHD and possibly of its subtypes. Our
ings very difficult to interpret. review has highlighted that there are four domains that are
Based on these limitations, we present recommendations particularly informative when assessing adult ADHD, inde-
for future neuropsychological research on adult ADHD and pendent of subtype. These domains include executive func-
its subtypes. Studies should use a common, comprehensive, tioning, attention, working memory, and memory. We have
and standardized battery that covers the key domains of found that they reliably differentiate between those with
executive functioning, attention, working memory, and adult ADHD and controls. However, we have not been able
memory. Efforts should be made to characterize specific to identify domains that would aid in the differentiation of
processes and functions within each domain. Performance subtypes, with a possible exception of memory measures.
of ADHD and its subtypes should be evaluated beyond Our hope is for future research to address the current gaps
comparisons with control groups, by referencing scores to in the empirical evidence and elucidate subtype differences
normative data. We found only one study that utilized nor- on neuropsychological measures. Those differences can
mative data to characterize performance in those with plausibly streamline adult ADHD diagnosis, substantially
ADHD. Finally, most of the studies reviewed here exam- reduce misdiagnoses, and allow for treatment approaches to
ined adult samples in the young- to middle-age range. be created that are catered to not only ADHD but also the
Future studies should make an effort to incorporate older specific subtype and associated deficits.
adults into their samples to track performance of ADHD
along the life span (Fischer, Gunter-Hunt, Steinhafel, & Declaration of Conflicting Interests
Howell, 2012). The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
Systematic literature reviews are also subject to limita- to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
tions. One such limitation is that the current review is not a
meta-analysis, with estimation of effect sizes. Given the Funding
considerable variability in assessment methodology, we The authors received no financial support for the research, author-
chose not to undertake a meta-analysis. Future research ship, and/or publication of this article.
should use meta-analytic techniques to provide more inclu-
sive and quantitative information. References
We also recognize that we used very broadly defined References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in
neuropsychological domains to capture the heterogeneous, the systematic review.
comprehensive performance of ADHD. Previous research Alderson, R., Kasper, L. J., Hudec, K. L., & Patros, C. H. (2013).
has suggested more specific performance characteristics in Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and working
ADHD, for example, verbal long-term memory (Schoechlin memory in adults: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology,
& Engel, 2005), memory acquisition processes (Skodzik 27, 287-302. doi:10.1037/a0032371
American Psychiatric Association. (1968). Diagnostic and statis-
et al., 2017), and divided and sustained attention (Salomone,
tical manual of mental disorders (2nd ed.). Washington, DC:
Fleming, Bramham, O’Connell, & Robertson, 2016).
American Psychiatric Publishing.
Therefore, our results may be indicative of the domains we American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statis-
chose and different results could have been obtained by tical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC:
evaluating subdomains. Similarly, the allocation of tests Author.
into our chosen domains was somewhat arbitrary and sub- American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statis-
jective and may have biased our findings. Despite our use of tical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., Rev.). Washington,
multiple raters in this process, it was not possible to obtain DC: Author.
complete agreement. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statis-
Finally, we need to acknowledge the potential error tical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC:
introduced by the publication bias, where studies finding no Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and sta-
significant differences between clinical and control groups
tistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.).
may remain unpublished. It is possible that the studies sum-
Washington, DC: Author.
marized here offer a biased portrayal of the neuropsycho- American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statis-
logical pattern of ADHD and its subtypes. Future studies tical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC:
will use a meta-analytic technique, calculating fail-safe N to American Psychiatric Publishing.
compensate for a lack of included studies with nonsignifi- Asherson, P., Akehurst, A., Kooji, J., Huss, M., Beusterien,
cant findings. K., Sasané, R., . . . Hodgkins, P. (2012). Under diagnosis
10 Journal of Attention Disorders 00(0)

of adult ADHD: cultural influences and societal burden. inhibition in adults with ADHD. Journal of Clinical and
Journal of Attention Disoders, 16(5 Suppl), 20S-38S. Experimental Neuropsychology, 23, 362-371. doi:10.1076/
doi:10.1177/1087054711435360. jcen.23.3.362.1186
Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, Faraone, S. V., & Mick, E. (2010). Molecular genetics of attention
and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatric Clinics of North
ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65-94. America, 33, 159-180. doi:10.1016/j.psc.2009.12.004
Barkley, R. A., & Biederman, J. (1997). Toward a broader defi- Finlayson, M. A., Johnson, K. A., & Reitan, R. M. (1977).
nition of the age-of-onset criterion for attention-deficit Relationship of level of education to neuropsychological
hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy measures in brain-damaged and non-brain-damaged adults.
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 1204-1210. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 45, 536-542.
doi:10.1097/00004583-199709000-00012 Fischer, B. L., Gunter-Hunt, G., Steinhafel, C. H., & Howell, T.
*Bekker, E. M., Overtoom, C. C., Kenemans, J. L., Kooij, J. J., (2012). The identification and assessment of late-life ADHD
De Noord, I., Buitelaar, J. K., & Verbaten, M. N. (2005). in memory clinics. Journal of Attention Disorders, 16, 333-
Stopping and changing in adults with ADHD. Psychological 338. doi:10.1177/1087054711398886
Medicine, 35, 807-816. Gallagher, R., & Blader, J. (2001). The diagnosis and neuropsy-
Boonstra, A. M., Oosterlaan, J., Sergeant, J. A., & Buitelaar, J. chological assessment of adult attention deficit/hyperactivity
K. (2005). Executive functioning in adult ADHD: A meta- disorder. Scientific study and practical guidelines. Annals of
analytic review. Psychological Medicine, 35, 1097-1108. the New York Academy of Sciences, 931, 148-171.
*Braun, C. M., Delisle, J., Suffren, S., & Bolduc, M. (2013). *Gansler, D. A., Fucetola, R., Krengel, M., Stetson, S.,
Atypical left-right balance of visuomotor awareness in adult Zimering, R., & Makary, C. (1998). Are there cognitive
ADHD (combined type) on a test of executive function. subtypes in adult attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder?
Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 18, The Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 186, 776-781.
385-406. doi:10.1080/1357650X.2012.695796 doi:10.1097/00005053-199812000-00006
Bridgett, D. J., & Walker, M. E. (2006). Intellectual functioning Gizer, I. R., Ficks, C., & Waldman, I. D. (2009). Candidate gene
in adults with ADHD: A meta-analytic examination of full studies of ADHD: A meta-analytic review. Human Genetics,
scale IQ differences between adults with and without ADHD. 126, 51-90. doi:10.1007/s00439-009-0694-x
Psychological Assessment, 18, 1-14. doi:10.1037/1040- Gualtieri, C. T., & Johnson, L. G. (2005). ADHD: Is objective
3590.18.1.1 diagnosis possible? Psychiatry (Edgmont), 2(11), 44-53.
*Carr, L. A., Nigg, J. T., & Henderson, J. M. (2006). Attentional Hervey, A. S., Epstein, J. N., & Curry, J. F. (2004).
versus motor inhibition in adults with attention-deficit/ Neuropsychology of adults with attention-deficit/hyperactiv-
hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology, 20, 430-441. ity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology, 18,
doi:10.1037/0894-4105.20.4.430 485-503. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.18.3.485
Davidson, M. A. (2008). ADHD in adults: A review of the lit- Jacova, C., Pearce, L. A., Costello, R., McClure, L. A., Holliday,
erature. Journal of Attention Disorders, 11, 628-641. S. L., Hart, R. G., & Benavente, O. R. (2012). Cognitive
doi:10.1177/1087054707310878 impairment in lacunar strokes: The SPS3 trial. Annals of
*Delisle, J., & Braun, C. M. (2011). A context for normaliz- Neurology, 72, 351-362. doi:10.1002/ana.23733
ing impulsiveness at work for adults with attention deficit/ *Kakuszi, B., Tombor, L., Papp, S., Bitter, I., & Czobor, P. (2016).
hyperactivity disorder (combined type). Archives of Clinical Altered response-preparation in patients with adult ADHD:
Neuropsychology, 26, 602-613. doi:10.1093/arclin/acr043 A high-density ERP study. Psychiatry Research, 249, 57-66.
*Dige, N., Maahr, E., & Backenroth-Ohsako, G. (2008). doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2016.02.008
Memory tests in subgroups of adult attention deficit hyper- Kebir, O., & Joober, R. (2011). Neuropsychological endopheno-
activity disorder reveals simultaneous capacity deficit. types in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A review of
International Journal of Neuroscience, 118, 569-591. genetic association studies. European Archives of Psychiatry
doi:10.1080/00207450701239384 and Clinical Neuroscience, 261, 583-594. doi:10.1007/
*Dinn, W. M., Robbins, N. C., & Harris, C. L. (2001). Adult s00406-011-0207-5
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Neuropsychological Kempton, S., Vance, A., Maruff, P., Luk, E., Costin, J., & Pantelis,
correlates and clinical presentation. Brain and Cognition, 46, C. (1999). Executive function and attention deficit hyperac-
114-121. doi:10.1016/S0278-2626(01)80046-4 tivity disorder: Stimulant medication and better executive
*Dobson-Patterson, R., O’Gorman, J. G., Chan, R. C., & Shum, function performance in children. Psychological Medicine,
D. H. (2016). ADHD subtypes and neuropsychological per- 29, 527-538.
formance in an adult sample. Research in Developmental Kofler, M. J., Rapport, M. D., Sarver, D. E., Raiker, J. S., Orban,
Disabilities, 55, 55-63. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2016.03.013 S. A., Friedman, L. M., & Kolomeyer, E. G. (2013). Reaction
Edwards, J. D., Jacova, C., Sepehry, A. A., Pratt, B., & Benavente, time variability in ADHD: A meta-analytic review of 319 stud-
O. R. (2013). A quantitative systematic review of domain- ies. Clinical Psychology Review, 33, 795-811. doi:10.1016/j.
specific cognitive impairment in lacunar stroke. Neurology, cpr.2013.06.001
80, 315-322. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e31827deb85 *Konrad, A., Dielentheis, T. F., El Masri, D., Dellani, P. R.,
*Epstein, J. N., Johnson, D. E., Varia, I. M., & Conners, C. Stoeter, P., Vucurevic, G., & Winterer, G. (2012). White mat-
K. (2001). Neuropsychological assessment of response ter abnormalities and their impact on attentional performance
LeRoy et al. 11

in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. European de Psiquiatria, 37, 228-234. doi:10.1590/1516-4446-2014-


Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 262, 351- 1533
360. doi:10.1007/s00406-011-0251-1 Salomone, S., Fleming, G. R., Bramham, J., O’Connell, R. G., &
Kooji, S., Bejerot, S., Blackwell, A., Caci, H., Casas-Brugué, M., Robertson, I. H. (2016). Neuropsychological deficits in adult
Carpentier, P., . . . Asherson, P. (2010). European consensus ADHD: Evidence for differential attentional impairments,
statement on diagnosis and treatment of adult ADHD: The deficient executive functions, and high self-reported func-
European Network Adult ADHD. BMC Psychiatry, 10, 67. tional impairments. Journal of Attention Disorders. First pub-
doi:10.1186/1471-244X-10-67 lished online 14 January. doi:10.1177/1087054715623045
Larsson, H., Asherson, P., Chang, Z., Ljung, T., Friedrichs, B., Sawyer, M. G., Whaites, L., Rey, J. M., Hazell, P. L., Graetz, B.
Larsson, J. O., & Lichtenstein, P. (2013). Genetic and envi- W., & Baghurst, P. (2002). Health-related quality of life of
ronmental influences on adult attention deficit hyperactivity children and adolescents with mental disorders. Journal of the
disorder symptoms: A large Swedish population-based study American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41,
of twins. Psychological Medicine, 43, 197-207. doi:10.1017/ 530-537. doi:10.1097/00004583-200205000-00010
s0033291712001067 Schoechlin, C., & Engel, R. R. (2005). Neuropsychological
Larsson, H., Chang, Z., D’Onofrio, B. M., & Lichtenstein, P. performance in adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disor-
(2014). The heritability of clinically diagnosed attention defi- der: Meta-analysis of empirical data. Archives of Clinical
cit hyperactivity disorder across the lifespan. Psychological Neuropsychology, 20, 727-744. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2005.
Medicine, 44, 2223-2229. doi:10.1017/s0033291713002493 04.005
Lesch, K. P., Timmesfeld, N., Renner, T. J., Halperin, R., Roser, *Schweitzer, J. B., Hanford, R. B., & Medoff, D. R. (2006).
C., Nguyen, T. T., & Jacob, C. (2008). Molecular genetics Working memory deficits in adults with ADHD: Is there
of adult ADHD: Converging evidence from genome-wide evidence for subtype differences? Behavioral and Brain
association and extended pedigree linkage studies. Journal of Functions, 2, Article 43. doi:10.1186/1744-9081-2-43
Neural Transmission, 115, 1573-1585. doi:10.1007/s00702- Skodzik, T., Holling, H., & Pedersen, A. (2017). Long-term
008-0119-3 memory performance in adult ADHD. Journal of Attention
Levy, F., Hay, D. A., McStephen, M., Wood, C., & Waldman, I. Disorders, 21, 267-283. doi:10.1177/1087054713510561
(1997). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A category Sprich, S., Biederman, J., Crawford, M. H., Mundy, E., & Faraone,
or a continuum? Genetic analysis of a large-scale twin study. S. V. (2000). Adoptive and biological families of chil-
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, dren and adolescents with ADHD. Journal of the American
737-744. doi:10.1097/00004583-199706000-00009 Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 1432-1437.
Matza, L. S., Paramore, C., & Prasad, M. (2005). A review of the doi:10.1097/00004583-200011000-00018
economic burden of ADHD. Cost Effectiveness and Resource Stray, L. L., Kristensen, O., Lomeland, M., Skorstad, M., Stray,
Allocation, 3, Article 5. doi:10.1186/1478-7547-3-5 T., & Tonnessen, F. E. (2013). Motor regulation problems and
*Miller, M., Ho, J., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2012). Executive functions pain in adults diagnosed with ADHD. Behavioral and Brain
in girls with ADHD followed prospectively into young adult- Functions, 9, Article 9. doi:10.1186/1744-9081-9-18
hood. Neuropsychology, 26, 278-287. doi:10.1037/a0027792 *Suarez, I., Burle, B., Tobon, C., Pineda, D., Lopera, F.,
Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., & Altman, D. G. (2009). Hasbroucq, T., & Casini, L. (2015). Deciphering interference
Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta- control in adults with ADHD by using distribution analyses
analyses: The PRISMA statement. PLoS Medicine, 6(7), and electromyographic activity. Acta Psychologica, 159, 85-
e1000097. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097 92. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.05.010
*Murphy, K. R., Barkley, R. A., & Bush, T. (2001). Executive Taylor, E., Döpfner, M., Sergeant, J., Asherson, P., Banaschewski,
functioning and olfactory identification in young adults with T., Buitelaar, J., . . . Zuddas, A. (2004). European guidelines
attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology, 15, for hyperkinetic disorder - first upgrade. European Child
211-220. and Adolescent Psychiatry, 13, Suppl 1, 17-30. doi:10.1007/
Murphy, K. R., Barkley, R. A., & Bush, T. (2002). Young adults s00787-004-1002-x
with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Subtype differ- Weyandt, L. L., & DuPaul, G. (2006). ADHD in college stu-
ences in comorbidity, educational, and clinical history. The dents. Journal of Attention Disorders, 10, 9-19. doi:10.1177/
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 190, 147-157. 1087054705286061
Resnick, R. J. (2005). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in Willcutt, E. G., Nigg, J. T., Pennington, B. F., Solanto, M. V.,
teens and adults: They don’t all outgrow it. Journal of Clinical Rohde, L. A., Tannock, R., & Lahey, B. B. (2012). Validity
Psychology, 61, 529-533. doi:10.1002/jclp.20117 of DSM-IV attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom
*Riccio, C. A., Wolfe, M., Davis, B., Romine, C., George, dimensions and subtypes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
C., & Lee, D. (2005). Attention deficit hyperactivity dis- 121, 991-1010. doi:10.1037/a0027347
order: Manifestation in adulthood. Archives of Clinical Woo, B. S., & Rey, J. M. (2005). The validity of the DSM-IV sub-
Neuropsychology, 20, 249-269. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2004. types of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Australian &
07.005 New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 39, 344-353. doi:10.1080/
Rosa Neto, F., Goulardins, J. B., Rigoli, D., Piek, J. P., & j.1440-1614.2005.01580.x
Oliveira, J. A. (2015). Motor development of children with Woods, S. P., Lovejoy, D. W., & Ball, J. D. (2002).
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Revista Brasileira Neuropsychological characteristics of adults with ADHD: A
12 Journal of Attention Disorders 00(0)

comprehensive review of initial studies. Journal The Clinical Claudia Jacova, PhD, is an associate professor at the Pacific
Neuropsychologist, 16, 12-34. doi:10.1076/clin.16.1.12.8336 University School of Graduate Psychology. She is interested in
aging, cognitive impairment, and dementia.
Author Biographies Caedy Young, MS, is a PhD student at the Pacific University
Amy LeRoy, MS, is a PhD student at the Pacific University School of Graduate Psychology. Her research is focused on
School of Graduate Psychology. She is interested in neuropsy- neurodegenerative disease, memory function, and cognitive
chology and issues related to diagnostic assessment of psychiatric disorders. She specializes in gerontology and neuropsychologi-
disorders, including ADHD. cal assessment.

You might also like