You are on page 1of 7

EXAMINER’S MANUAL

The only nationally-standardized Nonverbal Stroop Test for


cognitive process and neuropsychological assessment.

Chris Koch, Ph. D. and


Gale Roid, Ph. D.

NSCST Manual
CAT. NO. 30152M

STOELTING CO • 620 WHEAT LANE • WOOD DALE, IL 60191


PHONE: 800-860-9775 • WEB: WWW.STOELTINGCO.COM
EMAIL: TESTS@STOELTINGCO.COM
Table of Contents
Acknowledgments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4

Chapters
1. Introduction, Theory, and Research on the Stroop Effect . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5
2. Administration, Scoring, and Case Studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9
3. Development and Standardization . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13
4. Reliability, Validity, and other Research Studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19

Appendix A . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 26
Appendix B . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 30
Appendix C . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 34
Appendix D . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38
Appendix E . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 42

References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 46

www.StoeltingCo.com • Email: PsychTests@StoeltingCo.com • Phone: 630-860-9700 • Fax: 630-860-9775 | NSCST MANUAL 3


Acknowledgments

The authors especially thank Dr. Jamie Martin of Stoelting Company for his tireless supervision of the production of
materials, monitoring of the standardization, communication with examiners, and entry of data. Several educators in
the Newberg, Oregon area assisted Dr. Koch with his various pilot and validity studies, including Michelle McWilliams,
Kevin Milner, Jennifer Salamé, Mark Ross, Brandon Porter, and Amy Jackson.

The authors thank several examiners who tested many multiple cases with the NSCST, especially Loraine Alderman
and Nan Throneberry, but also Angie Blanchard, Julie Cogar, Dee Hasselhuhn, Cynthia Hutmacher, Michele Marcera,
Eva Markham, Helen Stayna, and Lucy Torres. Dr. Roid specifically thanks his wife and daughter for many hours of
assistance during the project.

Note to Clinicians and Researchers


Clinicians and researchers who use the NSCST are encouraged to send copies of their work to the authors in care
of Stoelting Company, 620 Wheat Lane, Wool Dale, Illinois 60191. The authors can also be contacted at nonverbal-
stroop@gmail.com. We will be pleased to cite appropriate and well-designed independent research in future revisions
of this test and its manual. Additional information about the NSCST is available at https://sites.google.com/site/non-
verbalstroop/.

4 NSCST MANUAL | www.StoeltingCo.com • Email: PsychTests@StoeltingCo.com • Phone: 630-860-9700 • Fax: 630-860-9775


CHAPTER 1 Baddeley, 1996). Researchers and theorists since William
INTRODUCTION James (1890) have postulated that attention is essential to the
selection of important information for further processing from a
DESCRIPTION myriad of other competing information. Selective attention in-
The Nonverbal Stroop Card Sorting Test (NSCST) assesses volves two complementary processes. First, as the term implies,
cognitive interference in children and adults, ages 3 to 75+. The there is the selection of a particular stimulus, or signal, for ad-
approximate testing time is 5 to 10 minutes, including directions. ditional processing. Once selection has taken place, competing
Nationally standardized on 1,450 individuals and co-normed signals must be ignored or inhibited. Thus, selective attention is
with the Leiter-3 (Stoelting Cat. #34100), the NSCST uses involved with both the selection of relevant information and inhibi-
pantomime, non-vocal test administration to identify deficits in tion of irrelevant information. Similarly, attention is also important
executive functioning and attention processes. The test uses pro- for selecting a response. By implication, response selection and
cedures proven effective in many years of computerized Stroop response inhibition are related functions (Mostofsky & Sim-
research, adapted for individual assessment. monds, 2008). Consequently, tests of inhibition have become
important measures in cognitive assessment.
The NSCST can be used alone or in combination with other
Stroop measures (e.g., the Nonverbal Stroop Subtest of Leiter-3 STROOP’S ORIGINAL TASK
or Golden’s Color-Word Stroop Test, (Stoelting Cat. #30150)) or The Stroop task is one of the most commonly-used experimen-
within a larger battery of cognitive or neuropsychological tests. tal and clinical measures of inhibition. Three experiments were
Also, an Examiner’s Rating Scale, covering behaviors such as reported in Stroop’s (1935) seminal paper. In the first experiment,
attention, impulse control, activity level, and anxiety, provides participants read color-words that were presented in incongru-
an optional measure of test-taking behavior. The Scale was ent color ink. The time to complete the task was compared to a
developed as part of the Leiter-3 battery (Roid, Miller, Koch, & control condition in which the same color-words were presented
Pomplun, 2013, in press) and, originally, the Leiter-R (Roid & in black ink. No difference was found between the two conditions.
Miller, 1997). The rating scale adds observational information on The same color-word stimuli were used in Experiment 2 but
individuals’ test-taking behaviors and aids in the identification of participants named the color of the ink. Color blocks were used
attention or learning difficulties. in the control condition. Naming the ink color of incongruent
color words took significantly longer than naming the color
Two sets of cards—one with matching (congruent) color bars of blocks. This difference in naming time has been referred
and one with non-matching (incongruent) color bars—are used to as Stroop Interference; although, the term Stroop Effect is
to contrast the speed (cards per second ratios) with which the often used synonymously. Finally, in Experiment 3, participants
individual sorts the cards onto a large, laminated panel showing practiced the color naming task for eight days. Color naming
locations for each color. The difference between the congru- times in the incongruent condition decreased with practice.
ent and incongruent trials reveals the degree of interference Participants also read the words after the eight days of practice.
commonly referred to as the “Stroop effect.” The sorting times, Their word naming (i.e., reading) times were longer the first time
ratios, and interference score can be converted to normalized they named the color words. This finding has been referred to as
standard T-scores (with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation the reverse Stroop Effect.
of 10) for profile “strengths and weaknesses” assessment. The
profile scores allow the examiner to assess the magnitude of Stroop Interference is robust and highly replicable (cf.,
any unusually large difference in sorting speed, particularly for MacLeod, 1991). In fact, it has been called the gold standard
the incongruent trial, showing evidence of processing deficits and of attentional measures (MacLeod, 1992). The Color-Word
cognitive interference. stimuli presented in the Stroop task are obviously composed of
two components: color and word. Therefore, it is not surprising
The kit includes two sets of Cards, Laminate Sorting-Sheet, that theoretical explanations of Stroop Interference have focused
Stopwatch, and Manual. The Manual summarizes the theory and on how these two dimensions are processed. One explanation
research on Stroop effect, administration and scoring methods, is the relative speed of processing hypothesis (Morton & Cham-
interpretation and case studies, development and standardiza- bers, 1973; Posner & Snyder, 1975). This account assumes that
tion, and reliability and validity of the instrument. both the color and the word are processed at the same time but
at different rates. It also assumes that there is a limited capacity
BACKGROUND response channel which the color and word information must
Attention is an important aspect of executive functioning (cf., travel. Since words are processed faster than colors (Cattell,

www.StoeltingCo.com • Email: PsychTests@StoeltingCo.com • Phone: 630-860-9700 • Fax: 630-860-9775 | NSCST MANUAL 5


1886), the word information passes along the limited capacity re- ed using the Stroop task as a screening tool for assessing LD.
sponse channel first thereby causing interference when the color Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Impaired executive function-
and the word do not match. A related explanation is based on ing is characteristic of a number of developmental disorders in-
the amount of attentional resources required to process the two cluding the spectrum of autistic conditions (Hill, 2004a, b). These
dimensions instead of the speed at which they are processed. impairments can be seen among children (Robinson, Goddard,
The automaticity explanation assumes that more automatic Dritschel, Wisely, & Howlin, 2009) and adults (Hill & Bird, 2006).
processes can interfere with less automatic processes (LaBerge Level of impairment may be related to level of functioning. For
& Samuels, 1974; Shiffrin & Schneider, 1977). Since reading is example, inhibitory deficits have been found among high-func-
more automatic than color naming, word information interferes tioning autistic children (Rinehart et al., 2002; also Crane et al.,
with color naming when the word and color are incongruent. 2011). However, Bryson (1983), among others, found no differ-
The parallel distributed processing model proposed by Cohen, ence in Stroop Interference between children with ASD and typi-
Dunbar, and McClelland (1990) suggests that the color and word cally developing children. Adams and Jarrold (2009) concluded
are processed along two pathways that differ in strength. Despite that the lower reading comprehension of children with autism
differences between these three explanations of Stroop Interfer- affects Stroop Interference which could lead to misleading Stroop
ence, they primarily focus on the speed, attentional demand, or results despite the failure to find impairment on the task (Hill &
strength of word processing compared to color naming. Bird, 2006; Ozonoff & Jensen, 1999). This issue is also revisited
in the Criterion Validity section of the manual.
DISORDERS AND THE COLOR-WORD STROOP TASK
The Color-Word Stroop task has been used to examine attention- DISORDERS AND MODIFIED COLOR-WORD STROOP TASKS
al processing in a number of disorders. Three disorders, Attention There have been a number of variations to the original Stroop
Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), Learning Disability, and task. Several researchers, for instance, presented a picture with
Autism, are summarized in this section as examples. an embedded word (e.g., the word “chair” embedded in a picture
of a table). Incongruent picture-word pairings produced longer
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Research picture naming times than congruent pairings (Rosinski, 1977;
consistently shows that children with ADHD have greater dif- Rosinski, Golinkoff, & Kukish, 1975; Posnansky & Rayner, 1977;
ficulty naming the color print of incongruent color-word pairs Rayner & Posnansky, 1978; Underwood, 1976). While these
than control children (Barkley, Grodzinsky, & Paul, 1992; Frazier, studies changed the type of object paired with a word, Klein
Demaree, & Youngstrom, 2004; Nigg, Willcutt, Doyle, & Sonuga- (1964) changed the word meaning. He found that color associ-
Barke, 2005; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996). This finding has ated words, such as grass and sky, produce interference when
frequently been interpreted as a deficit in interference control for presented in incongruent color print. These task variations have
ADHD children (e.g., Barkley, 1997). However, other researchers led others to modify the Stroop task for clinical purposes. For
have suggested that differences between ADHD and control chil- example, a number of researchers have replaced color-words
dren are due to a slowing in word and color naming for the ADHD with threat words and asked participants to name the color of
children rather than a deficit in interference control (Homack & the print. Controls were found to complete the task faster than
Riccio, 2004; Nigg, 2001). This issue is revisited in the Criterion patients with generalized anxiety disorder (Bradley, Mogg, Millar,
Validity section of the Manual. & White, 1995; Mathews & MacLeaod, 1985; Mathews, Mogg,
Kentish, & Eysenck, 1995), phobias (Kindt & Brosschot, 1997;
Learning Disability (LD) Deficits in executive functioning have Martin, Horder, & Jones, 1992; Becker, Rinck, Margraf, & Roth,
been found for both boys (Seidman, Biederman, Monuteaux, 2001), obsessive compulsive disorder (Lavy, van Oppen, & van
Doyle, & Faraone, 2001) and girls (Seidman, Biederman, Valera, den Hout, 1994; McNeil, Tucker, Miranda, Lewin, & Nordgren,
Monuteaux, Doyle, & Faraone, 2006) with learning disabilities. In 1999), panic disorder (Buckley, Blanchard, & Hickling, 2002;
particular, attention deficits can accompany learning disabilities Ehlers, Margrat, Davies, & Roth, 1988), and posttraumatic stress
(Benezra & Douglas, 1988; Willcut et al., 2001; Whyte, 1994). disorder (Beck, Freeman, Shipherd, Hamblen, & Lackner, 2001;
The Stroop Color and Word Test (Cat. No. 30150) (SCWT, McNally, Amir, & Lipke, 1996). Food- and body-related words
Golden, 1978) has been used to distinguish children with an LD have also been shown to produce interference among individu-
from children without an LD. Golden and Golden (2002), for in- als with eating disorders (Ben-Tovim & Walker, 1991; Fairburn,
stance, were able to differentiate a learning disability group from Cooper, Cooper, McKenna, & Anastasiades, 1991; Johansson,
a non-LD group using the scores obtained from the SCWT with Ghaderi, & Andersson, 2005). Thus, modifying the original
89 percent accuracy. Lazarus, Ludwig, and Aberson (1984) were color-word Stroop task to include words associated with
also able to use the Stroop task to distinguish between children a particular disorder has proven to be an effective tool for
with learning disabilities and controls. Further, they recommend- examining attentional bias associated with those disorders.

6 NSCST MANUAL | www.StoeltingCo.com • Email: PsychTests@StoeltingCo.com • Phone: 630-860-9700 • Fax: 630-860-9775


DEVELOPMENT, LANGUAGE, AND STROOP INTERFERENCE young children. It is important to note that the young children ex-
Research has clearly shown that the Color-Word Stroop task is amined by Comalli, Wapner, and Werner (1962) were the same
a reliable measure with clinical utility. However, obtaining scores age as the second graders tested by Schiller (1966). Therefore,
that reflect the true ability of individuals who do not communi- the findings from these two studies are consistent. However,
cate effectively in the published language of a test has been a Schiller’s oldest subjects were college freshmen while Comalli et
long-standing concern in testing. According to the U.S. Census, al. examined individuals as old as 80 years of age. This wider
a language other than English is the primary language spoken in age range contributed to an additional finding. Stroop Inter-
approximately 20 percent of the homes across the United States. ference decreased slightly throughout adulthood but then
Although Spanish is spoken in the majority (62.3%) of these increased in old age. Other researchers have found similar
homes, there are a significant number of homes in which other patterns. For instance, Wise, Sutton, and Gibbons (1975) found
Indo-European languages (18.6%) and Asian and Pacific Island less interference among college students than elementary school
languages (15.0%) are spoken (Shin & Kominski, 2010). The students. Additionally, Cohn, Dustman, and Bradford (1984), us-
number of different languages spoken can be staggering. For ing a sample between the ages of 21 and 90, found that individu-
instance, Pasko (1994) estimated that 200 different languages als older than 60 years of age were slower, and exhibited greater
were spoken in Chicago Public Schools. However, the number interference than younger adults.
of different languages spoken within a school district is
not a problem limited to the largest school districts. For One factor that changes with age is reading ability. Consequently,
example, there are 92 different languages spoken in the Los the developmental findings are frequently explained in terms of
Angeles Unified School District (Master Plan Institute, 2010), the reading ability. For instance, age seven is generally when chil-
second largest school district in the United States with nearly dren develop reading fluency (Chall, 1983). This is also the
700,000 students (Snyder, Dillow, & Hoffman, 2009), and 94 dif- age at which Schiller (1966) found that Stroop Interference
ferent languages spoken in the Portland School District, the 94th peaks and begins to decline. Similarly, reading ability tends to
largest school district with more than 43,000 students (Portland decline in the elderly (Kirsch et al., 1993) which is when Camalli,
School District, 2012). These statistics highlight the diversity of Wapner, and Werner (1962) found that Stroop Interference began
the population and the potential for language-related difficulties to increase. Fournier et al. (1975) directly examined reading lev-
regardless of location. el and found the better readers exhibited more interference than
poor readers (also Protopapas, Archonti, & Skaloumbaka, 2007).
Concern about language is especially important for tests that are In a longitudinal study of first graders, Stanovich, Cunningham,
language-based. Attempts to develop nonverbal assessments and West (1981) also found that greater interference was linked
can be traced back to the Army Beta test (Thorndike & Lohman, with greater reading ability. In addition, they argued that the
1990). More recently, the Joint Committee on the Standards automaticity associated with reading began to level off at the end
for Educational and Psychological Testing developed several of first grade. As mentioned previously, the speed with which
standards (e.g., Standard 7.3 and Standard 9.3) addressing reading occurs is an important theoretical concept often
language-related issues (American Educational Research As- used in several explanations of Stroop Interference.
sociation, 1999). Since the Color-Word Stroop task uses words,
reading ability is critical to the task (e.g., Cox et al., 1997). Two Another area of research important to the Stroop task and
areas of research are particularly relevant to this issue. Develop- language ability is bilingualism. If the Stroop task is related
mental research regarding the Stroop task has shown that to reading ability and the speed with which words are pro-
interference increases, as children learn, from seven to nine cessed, then individuals who process one language faster
years of age. Interference begins to taper off through adult- than another should also show more interference with that
hood until approximately 60 years of age when it increases language than with the other. This is precisely what happens.
once again (cf., MacLeod, 1991). Schiller (1966), for example, Preston and Wallace (1969) were the first to demonstrate this
examined 40 subjects each in six grades ranging from first finding. They had participants who were bilingual in English
grade to the freshman year in college. He found minimal Stroop and French, English and Hungarian, and German and English.
Interference in first grade. However, interference was maximal Participants engaged in the Color-Word Stroop task in both lan-
by second and third grade and consistently declined thereafter. guages. Preston and Wallace found that the differences across
Subjects also responded faster with age. languages were about the same as the interference found within
a language with the primary language producing more interfer-
Comalli, Wapner, and Werner (1962) tested a wider age range. ence. English speaking monolinguals and Spanish-English bilin-
They administered the Stroop task to 235 subjects between the guals were tested by Dyer (1971b) using the Color-Word Stroop
ages of seven and 80 years of age. Interference was greatest in task in five different languages. Interference was maximal when

www.StoeltingCo.com • Email: PsychTests@StoeltingCo.com • Phone: 630-860-9700 • Fax: 630-860-9775 | NSCST MANUAL 7


the words appeared in the participant’s primary language and
decreased as the languages became less familiar (also Rosselli
et al., 2002). Mägiste (1984, 1985) obtained a complementary
finding by examining trends in language development. Initially,
more interference was found in participants’ primary language.
However, as they became more proficient in the second lan-
guage (i.e., balanced bilinguals) the amount of interference found
for each language was equivalent (cf., Vega & Fernandez, 2011;
Gerhand, Deregowski, & McAllister, 1995). Chen and Ho (1986)
attempted to extend this finding to Chinese-English bilinguals.
Although interference with English color-words increased as
participants became more proficient with English, Chinese stimuli
continued to produce greater interference. This particular finding
may suggest that certain languages are more susceptible to
Stroop Interference (Biederman & Tsao, 1979). Together, these
findings indicate that language development and proficiency
influence Stroop processing. More recent research suggests that
Stroop Interference among bilinguals may also be mediated by
age (Kousaie & Phillips, 2012; Gathercole et al., 2010).

PURPOSE
Despite the robustness of the Stroop Effect, research shows
that the task is tied to reading and language ability. The
purpose of the NSCST is to provide a nonverbal assessment
of inhibition processing using a modified version of the
Stroop task. In particular, by removing words from the task and
their associated developmental and language effects, the NSCST
is designed to provide an unbiased measure of basic attentional
processing (cf., Taylor & Clive, 1983).

8 NSCST MANUAL | www.StoeltingCo.com • Email: PsychTests@StoeltingCo.com • Phone: 630-860-9700 • Fax: 630-860-9775

You might also like