You are on page 1of 11

International Journal of Behavioral Development # 2002 T he International Society for the

2002, 26 (6), 481–491 Study of Behavioural Development


http://www.tandf.co.uk/journals/pp/01650254.html DOI: 10.1080/01650250143000436

Similarities and differences in mothers’ parenting of


preschoolers in China and the United States*

Peixia Wua, Clyde C. Robinsona, Chongming Yanga, Craig H. Harta, Susanne F. Olsena,
Christin L. Portera, Shenghua Jin b, Jianzhong Wob , and Xinzi Wuc

This investigation was designed to extend the work of Chao (1994) by examining parenting
constructs emphasised in the Chinese culture in conjunction with parenting constructs derived and
emphasised in North America. Mothers of preschool-age children from mainland China (N ˆ 284)
and the United States (N ˆ 237) completed two self-report parenting questionnaires. One assessed
dimensions of parenting practices emphasised in China (encouragement of modesty, protection,
directiveness, shaming/love withdrawal, and maternal involvement). The second measured speciŽc
stylistic dimensions within Baumrind’s global conceptualisations of authoritative (warmth/
acceptance, reasoning/induction, democratic participation) and authoritarian (physical coercion,
verbal hostility, nonreasoning/punitive) parenting. Mostly invariant factor structures were obtained
across cultures for both measures. Results showed that the Žve parenting constructs emphasised in
China were mostly nonoverlapping and independent in both cultures. In addition, the parenting
constructs emphasised in China were relatively independent from the constructs emphasised in
North America. As anticipated, Chinese mothers scored higher than US mothers on all parenting
constructs emphasised in China except maternal involvement. For parenting constructs emphasised
in North America, Chinese mothers scored lower than US mothers on warmth/acceptance and
democratic participation, but scored higher on physical coercion.

Authoritative and authoritarian parenting style typologies have certain aspects of parenting and speciŽc child outcomes may be
a long tradition of inquiry in the North American literature valued more in one context than another (Chao, 2001; Chen,
(Baumrind, 1971 ; Maccoby & Martin, 1983). However, recent Liu, & Li, 2000a)
studies have raised questions concerning the ethnocentricity of T his investigation extends this line of work by examining
North American parenting constructs, particularly as pertain- whether parenting constructs, regardless of the cultural
ing to diverse groups (see Baumrind, 1996; Chao, 1994; emphasis and meaning attached to them, are evident in
Darling & Steinberg, 1993; Nucci, 1994). SpeciŽcally, it has Chinese and North American cultures. Most studies have
been argued that Western constructs of authoritative and used conceptually derived measures of Chinese and North
authoritarian parenting may not capture important features of American parenting with little attention given to psychometric
Chinese child-rearing (Chao, 1994 ; Steinberg, Dornbusch, & developmen t in ways that would allow for direct comparisons
Brown, 1992). For example, based on research with 50 US of constructs across cultues (e.g. Chao, 1994 ; Chen et al.,
immigrant Chinese mothers of preschool-age children, Chao 1997). Thus, an important next step would be to create
(1994) proposed that the indigenous Chinese concept of invariant measures of what are typically thought of as Chinese
‘‘training’’, which denotes concern, care, and involvement, in and Western parenting constructs that can be used for
the context of Žrm and controlling parenting, has important assessing their relevance to each cultural context. Moreover,
socialisation meanings that extend beyond North American- little empirical research has investigated if typical parenting
based parenting styles. Alternatively, Chen and colleagues practices emphasised in China might be evident in North
recently suggested that authoritarian and authoritative patterns American settings. Although parenting constructs may be
are present in mainland China, and similarly to Western evident across cultural settings, some patterns may receive
societies, have signiŽcance for children’s developmen t (Chen, more emphasis because of differing cultural norms and the
Dong, & Zhou, 1997 ; Chen et al., 2000b). Despite these goals toward which children are socialised (cf. Chao, 2001 ;
disparate views, both perspectives accommodate the notion Chen et al., 2000a). Therefore, a second aim of this study was
that parenting patterns may be emphasised differently and to assess whether cultural emphases would be reected in
retain different meanings in the two cultural contexts because latent mean differences between Chinese and US mothers of

a
Brigham Young University, Provo, USA; b Beijing Normal University, China; c University of Virginia, Charlottesville, USA.

Correspondence should be addressed to Clyde C. Robinson, PhD, Beijing, China, July, 2000 in a paper symposium entitled ‘‘Under-
1403 SFLC, School of Family Life, Brigham Young University, standing parenting behaviour and its correlates in the context of
Provo, UT 84602, USA, Tel: (801) 378-4065 ; Fax: (801 ) 378-2820 ; Chinese culture’’. The authors express gratitude to the College of
E-mail: clyde_robinson@ byu.edu. Family, Home, and Social Sciences, the Camilla Eyring Kimball
Portions of this study were presented at the XVIth Biennial meetings Endowment, and the Family Studies Center at Brigham Young
of the International Society for the Study of Behavioural Development, University for providing funding for this work.
482 WU ET AL. / PARENT ING IN CHINA AND THE US

preschool-age children. This is important for cross-cultural personal interaction style rather than individual qualities in the
research because it helps establish measurement equivalenc e developmen t of one’s personality (Hsu, 1971). To Žt in, people
for parenting constructs across Asian and Western cultures in collectivist cultures ideally strive to inhibit behavioural and
(Berry, Poortinga, Segall, & Dasen, 1992). emotional expression of individual needs and desires to keep
from ‘‘sticking out’’ (Ho, 1986).
Parenting styles and practices Humble, modest behaviour, emphasising group over in-
dividual accomplishments and interests, is a central Confucian
Recently, reserchers have made a distinction between speciŽc principle, positively valued and encouraged in contemporary
parenting styles and parenting practices (Stevenson-Hinde,
Chinese society (T riandis, 1993). The ability to cooperate with
1998). Parenting practices are designed to meet speciŽc goals others and develop positive relationships is considered an index
such as helping children develop socially, achieve academically, of individual social maturity. Children who are sensitive,
or conform to societal expectations (see Hart, Robinson, modest, and cooperative are called ‘‘Guai Hai Zi’’ in
Olsen, & McNeilly-Choque, 1998a; Mize, Russell, & Pettit, Mandarin, or ‘‘well-behaved ’’ (Chen, 1998). Consistent with
1998). Rather than being domain-speciŽc, parenting styles these concepts, Chao (1995) found that immigrant Chinese
have been deŽned as ‘‘aggregates or constellations of behaviors mothers in the US continued to emphasise their child’s
that describe parent-child interactions over a wide range of relationships with others over individual psychological attri-
situations and that are presumed to create a pervasive butes. Chao’s Žndings are noteworthy since the Chinese
interactional climate’’ (Mize & Pettit, 1997 , p. 291). This concept of self-interdependenc e (T riandis, 1993 ) or collective
climate, as reected in global patterns of style (e.g., Baum- self (Wu, 1996 ) is quite different from views held in cultures
rind’s authoritative and authoritarian styles), is thought to help where individualism is more highly valued, such as the US
children be more open to the parental input and direction that (Gorman, 1998 )
are reected in speciŽc practices (e.g., Grusec & Goodnow,
1994). Protection. Parental protection in the Chinese cultural context
Even though parenting styles can encompass parenting reects the parental intention of ensuring a safe environment
practices, styles may transcend cultural boundaries in ways that and fostering dependenc y on adults, which is seen as a primary
practices may not (Darling & Steinberg, 1993). Although responsibility of parents of young children. From a young age
distinguishing between styles and practices can be useful (Hart, children are required to pledge reverence to their parents. At
Newell, & Olsen, in press; Mize et al., 1998), some practices the same time, parents have the major responsibility to govern,
may also transcend cultural boundaries. The main goal of this teach, and discipline their child (Chao, 1994). T hese expecta-
investigation was to assess whether certain parenting practices tions are consistent with Chinese cultural values emphasising
and styles can be measured in both Chinese and North the importance of family, the responsibility of parenthood, and
American cultural settings. If so, this would suggest that both the duty to raise well-adjusted children (Žlial obligation). From
style and practice variables are culture-general in nature. this perspective, young children are generally viewed as being
However, they may have varied meanings for different cultural incapable of understanding (Ho & Kang, 1984). Thus,
groups, reected in how much emphasis individuals in each Chinese parents typically view their primary role as a protector,
culture place upon them (cf. Chao, 2001; Chen et al., 2000a). with the intent of ensuring a safe and appropriate milieu for
We anticipated that variations in cultural emphasis would be their young child and fostering dependenc y on parents for
reected in cultural latent mean differences in parenting meeting the child’s needs.
practice constructs. Because ‘‘global parenting style is ex- Compared with North American parents of young children,
pressed partly through parenting practices’’ as parents carry Chinese parents are more protective (Chen et al., 1998; Lin &
out socialisation practices in authoritative or authoritarian ways Fu, 1990). For example, Chinese parents often encourage their
(Darling & Steinberg, 1993 , p. 493), there may be mean level young child to stay physically close to and be dependen t on
differences in stylistic patterns of parenting as well. We Žrst them (Ho, 1986). Other research (Hart et al., 1998b) suggests
consider how parenting practices emphasised in China are that Chinese mothers are prone to mediate peer contacts of
currently conceptualised. their preschool-age children in ways that foster less indepen-
dence from home than their North American counterparts.
Parenting practices emphasised in China Maternal restrictions of their child’s activities to the home are
meant to protect and foster dependency, not dominate or
Elements that help a cultural group adjust to its environmen t inhibit. Such practices are referred to as protective restraint
are transmitted through socialisation to each generation (Wu, 1996). In this sense, perceiving children as incapable of
(Triandis, 1993). Thus, valued social behaviours are reected understanding could lead to high parental protection, or what
in parenting practices and in the degree to which parents agree Western scholars refer to as overprotection (cf. Barber, Bean,
with them (e.g., Chao, 1994). Socialisation practices empha- & Erickson, 2002 ; Rubin, Nelson, Hastings, & Asendorpf,
sised in China include encouragement of modesty, protection, 1999).
directiveness, shaming/love withdrawal, and maternal involve-
ment. Shaming/love withdrawal. Chinese parents also use shaming
and love withdrawal to foster adherence to societal norms and
Encouragement of modest behaviour. In the Chinese culture, to promote sensitivity towards the perceptions and feelings of
modesty is reected in moderate, humble, and social conform- others. Young children ‘‘are strongly socialised to be aware of
ing behaviour when interacting with others. Historically, what others think of them, and are encouraged to act so as to
achieving and maintaining social order and interpersonal maximise the positive esteem they are granted from others
harmony have been primary concerns in the traditional while trying to avoid incurring their disapproval’’ (Schoenhals,
collectivist Chinese society (Chen et al., 1998). For example, 1993 , p. 192). The intended outcome of this socialisation is
the Chinese concept of jen emphasises an individual’s inter- that children are expected to be sensitive to shame and to other
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT , 2002, 26 (6), 481–491 483

people’s opinions, judgements, and evaluations (Fung, 1999). thus far have been somewhat peripheral to mainstream
In addition to shaming, Ho (1986) indicated that compared to parenting style conceptualisations in North America. However,
North Americn mothers, Chinese mothers used more ‘‘love- protection, directiveness, shaming/love withdrawal, and ma-
oriented’’ methods of child-rearing, such as threat of with- ternal involvement are parenting patterns that have been
drawal or actual withdrawal of love (cf. T seng & Wu, 1985). identiŽed and studied in previous Western research (e.g.
Barber, Bean, & Erikson, 2002 ; Chen et al., 1998 ; Eisenberg &
Directiveness. Paental directiveness refers to taking a major Murphy, 1997 ; Rubin et al., 1999). Likewise, conforming to
responsibility in regulating children’s behaviou r and academic group norms reected in the modesty construct while
performance. Although somewhat akin to training ideologies simultaneously valuing individual expression is deemed im-
found among immigrant Chinese families (Chao, 2001), the portant in North America (Gorman, 1998 ; T riandis, 1995).
focus of this construct is on ways that mainland Chinese Thus, we expected to be able to measure these constructs in
mothers correct young children’s behaviour. Wu (1996) noted our North American sample.
that Chinese parents are prone to scold and criticise in Even though measurable in both Chinese and North
attempts to control and correct young children’s behaviour, American samples, the degree to which these socialisation
especially pushing them in academic pursuits (Kriger & Kross, practices are emphasised may vary by culture according to
1972 ; Lin & Fu, 1990). As with parental protectiveness, meanings that are attached to them (Chao, 2001; Chen et al.,
directiveness may reect Chinese cultural beliefs that young 2000a). Furthermore, due to differing cultural norms and
children are incapable of understanding and making decisions expectations, Darling and Steinberg (1993) suggested that
that are in their best interest. These beliefs and practices enactment of certain parenting practices may vary across
contrast with Western democratic socialisation ideology that different ecologies. Given Chinese cultural values and socia-
promotes more autonomy granting in young children (Russell, lisation goals, we anticipated that Chinese mothers would
Mize, & Bissaker, in press). score higher than North American mothers on self-perception
Although recently challenged by Chen and colleagues indicators of their enactment of these parenting practices.
(1997, 2000b), the prevailing belief is that Chinese child-
rearing practices that reect intrusive control (or directiveness)
should not be characterised as authoritarian and, thus, Parenting styles in Chinese and North American
promoting negative child outcomes in the North American contexts
sense of the term (e.g., Baumrind, 1996 ; Chao, 1994 ; Baumrind’s (1971) parenting styles (e.g., authoritative,
Steinberg et al., 1992). When enacted in the context of a authoritarian, and permissive) have been incorporated in much
supportive and physically close parent-child relationship, Chao of the parenting research conducted in Western societies.
(1994) has argued that Chinese children typically identify with Although Baumrind and others (Maccoby & Martin, 1983)
parental training ideals that promote achievemen t and con- have identiŽed multiple parenting typologies, for our cross-
formity to societal expectations. cultural comparison we focused only on the authoritative and
authoritarian patterns. Permissiveness was not included in our
Maternal involvement. Historically, it is anticipated that study because it has been shown to be an unreliable construct
Chinese mothers will be immensely involved and devoted to with Chinese samples and may not be appropriate in the
their children, especially during the early years. Chao (1994) Chinese cultural context (Chen et al., 1997; McBride-Chang
noted that immigrant Chinese maternal involvement and & Chang, 1998).
sacriŽce may pave the road for Asian children’s high academic
achievemen t in the United States. Moreover, in mainland Authoritative parenting style. Authoritative parents (Baum-
China, Chen (1998) found three factors, which he labelled rind, 1971, 1989, 1996 ) attempt to direct children’s activities
authoritative, authoritarian, and protective parenting practices. in a rational, issue-oriented manner. T hey reason with their
Common to all three factors, however, were items reecting children, facilitate verbal give and take (Baumrind, 1989), and
maternal care and involvement . T hese Žndings are consistent encourage children to think independentl y and to respond in
with traditional Chinese cultural notions of the ideal mother prosocial ways (Baumrind, 1996). Also, authoritative parents
Žgure. Model Chinese mothers are often described as loving are ‘‘affectively responsive’’ by being loving, supportive, and
and kind (Ci Ai ), especially when the child is very young. Ideas encouraging children’s individual interests (Baumrind, 1989).
involving the child being the sole interest and concern of the Thus, authoritative parenting is comprised of three distinct
mother, being taken everywhere with the mother, and being in stylistic dimensions relevant to preschool-age children (Hart et
the constant care of the mother can be readily identiŽed in the al., in press). These are reected in (1) a high degree of
Chinese literature (Chao, 1994). warmth, nurturance, and acceptance (i.e., positive emotional
Maternal involvement may overlap somewhat with parental connection with the child), (2) a high degree of psychological
protection since both reect Chinese notions of parental autonomy granting or democratic participation, and (3) a high
responsibility. However, maternal involvement denotes more degree of regulation or behavioural control that places fair and
salient features of a highly involved, close mother-child consistent limits on child behaviour, primarily through reason-
relationship intended to help children succeed academically, ing about rules and establishing consequences for misbeha-
whereas parental protection is more focused on child safely and viour (e.g., Barber & Olsen, 1997; Baumrind, 1996 ; Hart et al.,
the fostering of child dependenc e on parental governance. 1992).
T his study builds upon research exploring parenting styles
with older children in China (Chen et al., 1997 , 2000a) by
Differences in parenting practices between China and focusing on younger children. Moreover, the three stylistic
North America dimensions of connection, regulation, and autonomy granting
Most parenting practices emphasised in China and discussed were included for cross-cultural comparisons (see Darling &
484 WU ET AL. / PARENT ING IN CHINA AND THE US

Steinberg, 1993; Gray & Steinberg, 1999 ; Smetana, 1995). We of two early childhood programmes. Maternal participation
anticipated that Chinese mothers would perceive themselves as rates from each classroom involved in the research were above
engaging their children in less autonomy granting (i.e., 70% in both cultural settings.
democratic participation) and more limit setting compared Mothers in the two samples were relatively well educated,
with their North American counterparts. Although Chinese having completed an average of 2 to 3 years of education
parenting practices reect high protection and involvement, it beyond high school (Chinese: M ˆ 13.21, SD ˆ 2.44; US: M
has been hypothesised that Chinese parents are less openly ˆ 14.00, SD ˆ 2.39). Chinese mothers had a mean age of 32.9
emotionally expressive of their affection than North American years (SD ˆ 3.7) and US mothers’ average age was 30.2 years
parents (Lin & Fu, 1990). Accordingly, our measure was (SD ˆ 5.4). T he mean age for the children in the Chinese
designed to capture overt displays of affection and acceptance. sample was 62.6 months (SD ˆ 9.3) and the average age for
We also expected Chinese mothers to be less demonstrative in US children was 57.2 months (SD ˆ 5.9).
their displays of connectedness.

Authoritarian parenting style. Authoritarian parents value


Measures
obedience, favour punitive and forceful measures, believe Items comprising our parenting styles and parenting practice
children should accept their parents’ word for what is right, measures were designed to reect features of Chinese and
and do not encourage verbal give and take (Baumrind, 1989). North American child-rearing as reviewed in the literature.
Authoritarian parenting style, as measured in this study, T he items emphasised in China were also derived, in part,
included items representing three harsh disciplinary dimen- from information gleaned in four informal semistructured
sions of (1) physical coercion (e.g., physical punishment), (2) focus group interviews in both Beijing and in the US. Each
verbal hostility (e.g., yelling, arguing), and (3) nonreasoning/ group was comprised of six to seven mothers of preschoolers.
punitive (e.g. punishes without explanation, threatens with no Mothers described their most important child-rearing respon-
justiŽcation) that went beyond the construct of verbal scolding sibilities, what were appropriate ways to handle various
and criticism reected in the Chinese directiveness construct. hypothetical child misbehaviour scenarios, and speciŽc con-
Directive practices may or may not occur outside a climate of cerns about helping their children conform to societal
harshness and hostility that are denoted in authoritarian expectations. The majority of responses mapped onto parent-
parenting styles. Past research has consistently indicated that ing themes emphasised in China (as well as onto authoritative
Chinese parents are more likely than North American parents and authoritarian dimensions) in each cultural context. After
to use physical coercion and verbal hostility in parent-child these general themes were identiŽed, mothers were asked to
interactions (Leung, Lau, & Lam, 1998 ; Steinberg et al., consider sample items derived from an item bank we had
1992). We anticipated similar Žndings in this study. As with developed , borrowed, or modiŽed from other parenting
authoritativeness, this investigation was designed to extend our measures (e.g., Barber, 1996 ; Block, 1965 ; Chao, 1994). T hey
knowledge of ways that dimensions of authoritarian parenting then provided feedback as to each item’s relevance to the
styles are relevant in each culture (cf. Hart et al., 2000a). themes identiŽed and helped eliminate items that were unclear
or confusing. This process resulted in eliminating approxi-
mately 20% of the initial items. New items were developed
Contributions of this research when deemed necessary. Interviewing mothers helped assure
A major contribution of this investigation is that, to our that Chinese and North American parenting views mapped
knowledge, it is the Žrst attempt to make direct comparisons onto the constructs discussed in our literature review and that
between mainland Chinese and North American cultures using they were relevant to everyday parenting in both cultures.
invariant (comparable) measures of parenting constructs Parents involved in focus group interviews were different from
derived from multi-sample structural equation modelling those who participated in this study.
(SEM) techniques. SEM procedures model measurement Prior to pilot testing in focus groups, all items were
error and the intercorrelations among the indicators of a successfully forward- and back-translated by Chinese linguists
construct. Using SEM techniques, we sought to determine who were uent in both Chinese and English. Translators
whether statistical models derived from our parent measures received assistance from the investigators for clariŽcations
were invariant across cultures. T his is necessary for direct regarding difŽcult-to-translate items. T hese procedures as-
comparisons across latent constructs derived from each sured that the items were conceptually equivalent, meaning that
cultural setting (Byrne, Shavelson, & Muthén, 1989 ; Little, they would be similarly understood by parents in both settings,
1997 ; Widaman & Reise, 1997). even though they may carry somewhat different psychological
meanings (see Berry, 1989 ; Hart et al., 1998a, 2000b). New
items developed from focus group interviews were subjected to
the same translation procedures. For all measures, mothers
Method
were asked to evaluate each item on the questionnaires, based
on their perceptions of how they interact with their preschool-
Participants age target child.
Two samples comprised of mothers of 553 preschool-age
children from two diverse cultural contexts participated in this Parenting dimensions emphasised in North America. A version of
study. Sample one included mothers of 284 children (154 boys the Parenting Styles and Dimensions Questionnaire (PSDQ;
and 130 girls) attending one of two nursery schools in Beijing, Robinson, Mandleco, Olsen, & Hart, 2001) was completed
China. Sample two was from an urban moderate-sized by mothers from both samples. A modiŽed PSDQ was
community in the Western United States. It consisted of contrived using procedures described later and included 26
mothers of 237 children (121 boys and 116 girls) attending one items forming two stylistic patterns of parenting: authoritative
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT , 2002, 26 (6), 481–491 485

and authoritarian. The authoritative pattern consisted of three Table 2


stylistic dimensions: (1) connection—warmth/acceptance (e.g., Confirmatory factor pattern of parenting practices emphasised in
expresses affection by hugging, kissing, etc.), seven items; (2) China
regulation—reasoning/induction (e.g. gives child reasons why
rules should be obeyed), four items; and (c) autonomy Factor loadings
granting—democratic participation (e.g., allows child to give Item content China US
input into family rules), four items. The authoritarian pattern
consisted of three stylistic dimensions: (1) verbal hostility (e.g., Encouragement of modesty
yells and shouts when child misbehaves), three items; (2) Discourage child from strongly expressing his/her .57 .41
physical coercion (e.g., spanks when child is disobedient), Žve point of view
items; and (3) nonreasoning/punitive (takes away privileges Discourage child from proudly acknowledging .46 .40
compliments
with little if any explanations), three items. Mothers rated
Discourage child from appearing overconŽdent .42 .72
themselves on each item by assessing ‘‘how often they Discourage child from showing off his/her skills .61 .59
perceived themselves exhibiting parenting behaviours reected
in each item’’ using a 5-point scale anchored by 1 (never) and 5 Protection
It is important to supervise all of child’s activities .52 .62
(always).
Expect child to be close by when playing .58 .66
Overly worry about child getting hurt .69 .51
Parenting practices emphasised in China. Parenting practices
emphasised in the Chinese culture were assessed using an item Maternal involvement
bank that included 5 to 7 items representing each of the Mothers express love by helping children to .47 .59
parenting constructs. Using procedures described next, this succeed in school
was reduced to an 18-item measure including the following A mother’s sole interest is in taking care of her .55 .75
children
constructs: encouragemen t of modesty (e.g., discourages child
Children should be in the constant care of .40 .53
from showing off his/her skills and knowledge to get attention), their mothers
four items; protection (e.g., expects child to be close by when Mothers should do everything for their .45 .75
playing), three items; directiveness (e.g., tells child what to do), children’s education
three items; shaming/love withdrawal (e.g., tells child that he/
Shaming/Love withdrawal
she should be ashamed when he/she misbehaves), four items;
Tell child we get embarrassed when doesn’t .60 .62
and beliefs about maternal involvement (e.g., a mother’s sole meet expectations
interest is in taking care of her children), four items. Items were Make child feel guilty when doesn’t meet our .67 .64
rated on a 5-point scale anchored by 1 (never) and 5 (always). expectations
Items that were deemed to reect mother-child relationship Tell child that he/she should be ashamed when .52 .48
ideologies concerning maternal involvement were selected misbehaving
from Chao’s ‘‘training’’ questionnaire (1994) administered to Less friendly with child if he/she doesn’t see .41 .54
an immigrant Chinese sample (N ˆ 50) in the United States. things our way
Correlations between mothers’ education and the parenting Directiveness
constructs emphasised in the Chinese and North American Scolds or criticises when child’s behaviour does .50 .46
cultures are shown in T able 1. The correlation pattern for the not meet our expectations
Chinese sample is that the more education the mothers have Demands child do things .53 .41
the less they report engaging in speciŽc traditional cultural Tells child what to do .41 .57
parenting constructs (i.e., maternal involvement, protection,
directiveness) emphasised in China and the more they report acceptance, democratic participation) emphasised in North
engaging in certain parenting constructs (i.e., warmth/ America. For the US sample, the more education the mothers
have the less they report engaging in maternal involvement ,
Table 1 encouragemen t of modesty, nonreasoning, and warmth/invol-
Correlations between mothers’ education and parenting constructs vement. These relationships should be interpreted with some
Mothers’ education
caution since the education variation of mothers within each
sample is somewhat restricted.
Parenting constructs Chinese US

Emphasised in China
Shaming/Love withdrawal ¡.07 .16 Results
Maternal involvement ¡.42** ¡.31*
Protection ¡.22* .02
Measurement model of parenting practices emphasised
Encouragement of modesty .03 ¡.23*
Directiveness ¡.34* .12
in China
Emphasised in North America Initially, we used a multi-sample conŽrmatory factor analysis
Warmth/Acceptance .27* ¡.23* (MCFA) procedure to test the measurement model of the Žve
Reasoning/Induction ¡.02 ¡.10 latent constructs derived from the Chinese parenting literature
Democratic participation .52** ¡.09 (e.g., modesty encouragement, protection, directiveness,
Physical coercion ¡.10 ¡.01
shaming/love withdrawal, and beliefs about maternal involve-
Verbal hostility ¡.15* ¡.03
Nonreasoning/Punitive ¡.02 ¡.17*
ment). We wanted to determine whether factor loadings could
be identiŽed that were invariant across both the Chinese and
* p 5 .05; ** p 5 .001. US samples. T ypically, invariance of factor loadings (Little,
486 WU ET AL. / PARENT ING IN CHINA AND THE US

Table 3
a
Latent correlations of parenting practices emphasised in China for the Chinese and US samples

Shaming/Love Maternal Encourage


withdrawal involvement Protection modesty Directiveness

Shaming/Love withdrawal 1.00 .21* .13 .20* .54**


(.02) (.07) (.14) (.63)**
Maternal involvement 1.00 .35* .30* .34*
(.49)** (.14) (.04)
Protection 1.00 .03 .23*
(.05) (.39)*
Encourage modesty 1.00 .06
(.32)*
Directiveness 1.00

* p 5 .01; ** p 5 .001.
a
Correlations of the constructs for the US sample are in parentheses.

1997), or at least partial invariance (Byrne et al., 1989), is Žt decreased slightly, w2 (252) ˆ 410.59, p 5 .005, w2 /df ˆ
essential for identifying similar constructs in order to compare 1.62, GFI ˆ .92, TLI ˆ .84, CFI ˆ .87, and differed
latent means cross-culturally (Widaman & Reise, 1997). In signiŽcantly from the Žve-factor model, w2 (8)diff ˆ 45.82, p 5
order to provide the best model Žt and to eliminate items that .001. Thus, the Žve-factor baseline solution provided a better
did not help in meeting at least partial invariance criteria, a estimate of the parenting constructs emphasised in China for
series of MCFA was performed (cf. Byrne, 1989; T omada & both cultures.
Schneider, 1997). A conŽgurably invariant measurement
model emerged that estimated the same factor structure
without metric equality constraints (unconstrained) across Measurement model of authoritative and
the two samples, and served as a baseline model, which authoritarian styles emphasised in North America
provided a fairly good Žt to the data: w2 (244) ˆ 364.77, p ˆ As with the parenting model emphasised in China, a series of
.00, w2 /df ˆ 1.45, GFI ˆ .93, T LI ˆ .88, CFI ˆ .90. T he MCFA was performed (cf. Byrne, 1989 ; Tomada & Schneider,
standardised factor loadings for the baseline model are 1997), with an eye towards dropping items that did not help
presented in T able 2. 1 A model constraining the factor meet at least partial invariance criteria. Items representing an
loadings (except two of the modesty encouragement items) authoritative parenting style, which includes three latent
to be equal across the two samples produced a chi square that constructs (warmth/acceptance, reasoning/induction, and de-
did not differ signiŽcantly from the baseline model, w2 (11)diff mocratic participation), were subjected to an MCFA. A similar
ˆ 13.78, p ˆ .25. These results suggest that most of the factor procedure was followed for identifying the authoritarian
loadings for the parenting constructs emphasised in China are construct dimensions (verbal hostility, physical coercion,
invariant (comparable) across the two cultures. Items compris- nonreasoning/punitive). A 15-item, unconstrained three-factor
ing this solution were deemed to represent the constructs well. model of the authoritative constructs emerged that Žt both the
For both the Chinese and US mothers (see Table 3), the US and Chinese samples well: w2 (168) ˆ 288.40, p 5 .001,
2
patterns of correlations among the Žve latent parenting w /df ˆ 1.72, GFI ˆ .93, T LI ˆ .91, and CFI ˆ .93. T he
constructs emphasised in China were relatively low (Chinese unconstrained model of the three authoritarian constructs also
f ˆ .03 to .35; US: f ˆ .02 to .49). For both samples there provided a fairly good Žt: w2 (76) ˆ 186.90, p 5 .001, w2 /df ˆ
was one exception, a moderately strong correlation between 2.46, GFI ˆ .94, T LI ˆ .87, and CFI ˆ .91, and was
shaming/love withdrawal and directiveness (Chinese: f ˆ .54; comprised of 11 items.
US: f ˆ .66). These Žndings indicate that most of these T he cross-cultural comparability of the authoritative and
constructs are relatively independen t in both cultures. T o test authoritarian constructs was tested using the chi-square
whether shaming/love withdrawal and directiveness constructs differences between the unconstrained and constrained mea-
are well distinguished, a four-factor model combining the items surement models. Factor loadings were found to be invariant
from both of these dimensions onto a single factor was across the two samples for the authoritative constructs,
2
compared to the Žve-factor baseline model. The goodness of w (12)diff ˆ 14.94, p ˆ .25, and the authoritarian constructs,
2
w (8)diff ˆ 14.48, p ˆ .07. T hus, similar constructs of
authoritative and authoritarian were identiŽed in both the
1
In Structural Equation Modelling (SEM), only three or four indicators are North American and Chinese cultures. Standardised factor
needed in order for the measurement model to be over identiŽed. Any more than loadings for the authoritative parenting constucts are presented
that may be superuous (Kline, 1998). Factor loadings in this study were
acceptable based on prior studies indicating that they are typically under-
in T able 4, and for the authoritarian constructs in Table 5.
estimated in SEM when using Likert-type scaling. This is due to the treatment of For the authoritative stylistic pattern, the intercorrelations
the ordinal data (Bollen, 1989; Coenders, Satorra, & Saris, 1997; Rigdon & among the warmth/acceptance, reasoning/induction, and
Ferguson, 1991). Factor loadings of .40 and above are deemed reliable when
democratic participation latent constructs were .56, .72, and
sample sizes are greater than 150 (Stevens, 1996). CoefŽcient alpha is a
questionable indicator of reliability and is not typically reported in conŽrmatory .76, respectively, for the US sample and .80, .82, and .85,
factor analysis (Komaroff, 1997; Schmitt, 1996). respectively, for the Chinese sample. T o test whether these
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT , 2002, 26 (6), 481–491 487

Table 4 Table 5
Within-group completely standardised solution from multi-sample Within-group completely standardised solution from multi-sample
confirmatory factor analysis of authoritative parenting styles and confirmatory factor analysis of authoritative parenting styles and
dimensions emphasised in North America dimensions emphasised in North America

Factor loadings Factor loadings

Item content: Authoritative style China US Item content: Authoritative style China US

Warmth/Acceptance (connection) Physical coercion


Gives praise when child is good .61 .57 Uses physical punishment as a way of disciplining .55 .47
Expresses affection by hugging, kissing, and .53 .60 child
holding child Spanks when child is disobedient .71 .64
T ells child that we appreciate what he/she tries to .64 .57 Slaps when child misbehaves .71 .58
accomplish Grabs child when he/she is being disobedient .67 .59
Gives comfort and understanding when child is .60 .60 Guides child by punishment more than by reason .59 .52
upset
Verbal hostility
Show sympathy when child is hurt or frustrated .44 .47
Explodes in anger towards child .70 .69
Aware of problems or concerns about child in .53 .47
Yells or shouts when child misbehaves .62 .67
school
Argues with child .65 .51
Encourages child to talk about his/her troubles .62 .57
Non-reasoning/Punitive
Reasoning/Induction (regulation)
Punishes by taking privileges away with little .49 .57
T alks it over and reasons with child when .60 .65
explanation
misbehaving
Punishes by putting child off with little or no .55 .46
Encourages child to talk about consequences of .60 .70
justiŽcation
behaviour
When child asks why he/she has to conform, says: .40 .46
Explains the consequences of the child’s behaviour .40 .57
because I said so
Gives child reasons why rules should be obeyed .59 .62

Democratic participation (autonomy granting)


Apologises to child when making a mistake in .42 .46 dimensions revealed generally nonoverlappin g constructs (f ˆ
parenting ¡.39 to .15). T he strongest correlations were between warmth/
Allows child to give input into family rules .44 .44 acceptance and shaming/love withdrawal (f ˆ ¡.52), and
T akes child’s desire into account before asking .40 .46 between warmth/acceptance and maternal involvemen t (f ˆ
to do something .41). In the Chinese sample, the constructs were also generally
Encourages child to express him/herself even .40 .50
nonoverlapping (f ˆ ¡.12 to .34). T he strongest correlations
when disagreeing with parents
were between directiveness and reasoning/induction (f ˆ .56),
directiveness and warmth/acceptance (f ˆ .29), and between
constructs were well distinguished, a single-factor model protection and democratic participation (f ˆ .34).
combining the items from all three dimensions onto a single Latent correlations between parenting practices emphasised
factor was compared to a three-factor baseline model. T he in China and the three authoritarian stylistic parenting
goodness of Žt decreased substantially, w2 (175) ˆ 414.95, dimensions presented in Table 6 also revealed that most of
2
p 5 .001, w /df ˆ 2.37, GFI ˆ .90, T LI ˆ .84, CFI ˆ .86, and the constructs were fairly independen t in the US sample (f ˆ
differed signiŽcantly from the three-factor model, w2 (7)d iff ˆ ¡.06 to .31), with the exception of four moderately strong
106.55, p 5 .001, indicating that the three-factor model was a correlations (directiveness and physical coercion, .64; direc-
better Žt of the data. tiveness and verbal hostility, .63; directiveness and nonreason-
For the authoritarian stylistic construct, the intercorrela- ing, .46; shaming/love withdrawal and verbal hostility, .61).
tions among the physical coercion, verbal hostility, and There was no relationship between directiveness and verbal
nonreasoning/punitive latent constructs were .80, .72, and hostility or nonreasoning in the Chinese sample. T he generally
.60, respectively, for the US sample and .67, .65, and .69 nonoverlapping pattern with the US sample was also similar in
respectively for the Chinese sample. As with the authoritative the Chinese sample (f ˆ ¡.10 to .44). T he two moderate
pattern, a model combining the items from all three correlations were between shaming/love withdrawal and non-
authoritarian dimensions onto a single factor was compared reasoning/punitive (f ˆ .44) and between directiveness and
to a three-factor baseline model. The goodness of Žt was physical coercion (f ˆ .42). All of the latent correlations in
substantially lower, w2 (84) ˆ 297.65, p 5 .001, w2 /df ˆ 3.54, Table 6 were estimated by combining the measurement
GFI ˆ .91, T LI ˆ .77, CFI ˆ .86, and differed signiŽcantly models (parenting practices emphasised in China with
from the three-factor model, w2 (8)diff ˆ 110.75, p 5 .001, authoritative parenting; parenting practices emphasised in
again indicating that the three-factor model was a better Žt of China with authoritarian parenting) from the two conŽrmatory
the data. factor analyses.

Relations between parenting constructs emphasised in Latent mean comparisons


China and North America Ideally, cross-cultural differences in the means of the latent
As seen in Table 6, in the US sample, correlations between constructs should be compared when all factor loadings are
parenting practice constructs emphasised in China and the invariant across all the cultural samples (Little, 1997 ; Wida-
latent constructs of the three authoritative stylistic parenting man & Reise, 1997). As noted earlier, factor loadings for the
488 WU ET AL. / PARENT ING IN CHINA AND THE US

Table 6
a
Latent correlations between parenting practices emphasised in China and parenting dimension constructs emphasised in North America

Parenting styles and dimensions emphasised in North America


Authoritative style Authoritarian style

Warmth/ Reasoning Democratic Physical Verbal Nonreasoning


Practices emphasised in China Acceptance Induction participation coercion hostility punitive

Encouragement of modesty ¡.05 .08 ¡.06 ¡.07 .01 .15


(¡.30)* (.06) (¡.33)* (.27)* (.31)* (.21)
Protection .23* .24* .34* .07 .21* .05
(.14) (.08) (.13) (.05) (.05) (.30)*
Directiveness .29* .56** ¡.08 .42** ¡.10 .01
(¡.36)* (¡.16) (¡.39)* (.64)** (.63)** (.46)**
Shaming/Love withdrawal .01 .24* .08 .20* .14 .44**
(¡.52)** (¡.23)* (¡.36)* (.41)** (.61)** (.30)*
Maternal involvement ¡.12 ¡.02 .00 .18 .16 .19
(.41)** (.14) (.15) (.07) (¡.06) (.19)
a
*p 5 .01; ** p 5 .001. Correlations of the constructs for the US sample are in parentheses.

items comprising most of the parenting constructs met this only measurable but generally nonoverlappin g and indepen-
requirement, with the exception of the modesty construct. dent in both cultures. In addition, the parenting practice
However, latent means can still be compared across samples constructs emphasised in China were relatively independen t
when partial invariance of factor loadings are identiŽed (Byrne from the stylistic constructs emphasised in North America.
et al., 1989). For the measurement model to be identiŽed, the T hese Žndings reinforce arguments made in the literature
means of the observed variables were constrained to be equal addressing the importance of using ecologically valid frame-
across groups. works and functionally equivalent measures in cross-cultural
Table 7 presents the latent means of the Chinese mothers studies (e.g., Little, 1997).
for the parenting practices emphasised in China and author- Parenting is considered to be heavily inuenced by cultural
itative-authoritarian stylistic dimensions with the latent means background . T hus, parents within speciŽc cultures may have
of the US mothers constrained. As can be seen from Table 7, varying beliefs about and use different practices in child-
statistically signiŽcant differences were found for four of the rearing which provide a developmental niche for children living
parenting practice constructs emphasised in China. Chinese in diverse contexts (Chen et al., 1998 ; Super & Harkness,
mothers’ latent means were higher than those of their US 1986). Consistent with this concept, we found that even
counterparts on encouragemen t of modesty, protection, though several parenting practice constructs derived from
shaming/love withdrawal, and directiveness. For the author-
itative parenting stylistic dimensions, Chinese mothers’ latent
Table 7
means were lower on warmth/acceptance and lower on
Latent mean comparisons of parenting practices emphasised in
democratic participation. For the authoritarian parenting
China and authoritative and authoritarian parenting dimensions
stylistic dimension Chinese mothers’ latent means were higher a
emphasised in North America
than those of US mothers only on the use of physical coercion.
Chinese mothers’ latent means
with US means constrained
Discussion Mean t p5

A major contribution of this research is that a model of Parenting practices emphasised in China
parenting constructs emphasised and derived empirically from Encouragement of modesty .10 2.92 .01
indigenous Chinese cultural notions was found to be applicable Protection .19 2.66 .01
Directiveness 1.03 11.86 .001
for mothers in the US. This indicates that parenting practice
Shaming/Love withdrawal 1.80 20.62 .001
constructs that are highly valued in China are measurable in Maternal involvement .03 0.48 —
the US, even though their role may have been emphasised less
in the North American literature. For reasons noted in the Authoritative dimensions emphasised
Introduction, these practices, although measurable in our in North America
Warmth/Acceptance ¡.32 ¡6.38 .001
North American sample, are not likely to stem from the same
Reasoning/Induction .02 0.29 —
underlying socialisation goals that are prevalent in Chinese Democratic participation ¡.15 ¡2.24 .05
society. Our research also suggested that a model of parenting
constructs derived from global parenting styles emphasised in Authoritarian dimensions emphasised
North America was also relevant for mainland Chinese in North America
Physical coercion .19 4.77 .01
mothers. In addition, results of latent mean comparisons
Verbal hostility .04 0.85 —
suggest that Chinese and US mothers score signiŽcantly Nonreasoning/Punitive .05 ¡0.47 —
differently from each other on most of the parenting style
a
and practice constructs. Findings also showed that the Žve SigniŽcant differences between latent means were determined by a
parenting practice constructs emphasised in China were not critical ratio.
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT , 2002, 26 (6), 481–491 489

Chinese cultural notions could also be empirically identiŽed in as fathers have been employed outside the home. Thus, both
the US sample, signiŽcant cultural differences in parenting the ofŽcial ideology and the high rate of maternal employment
existed between these two cultures. For example, one might encourage more equal parenting (Berndt, Cheung, Lau,
distinctive feature unique to parenting practices emphasised Hau, & Lew, 1993). As a result, Chinese mothers may be less
in China involves the strong focus on modesty and cooperation likely to endorse sole maternal responsibility for child-rearing.
among young children. Our results indicate that Chinese Our Žndings suggest that even though social changes have
mothers perceived themselves as engaging in greater modesty taken place in mainland China during the last two decades,
encouragement than US mothers. These Žndings support current parenting beliefs and practices may still be inuenced
those who argue that modesty is more valued in collectivist by some Confucian traditions. For example, the Confucian
societies (Triandis, 1995), where individuals are encouraged to tradition of social order emphasises developing harmonious
restrain their personal desires for the beneŽt and interests of interpersonal relationships. Likewise, in contemporary Chinese
the society at large (Chen et al., 1998). society, children are encouraged to be sensitive, modest, and
In addition to modesty, shaming/love withdrawal is viewed cooperative (‘‘Guai Hai Zi’’) or ‘‘well-behaved’’ (Chen, 1998).
as a dominant moral training technique for achieving speciŽc Our Žndings that Chinese mothers score higher on modesty
socialisation goals requiring members in the Chinese society to encouragemen t support the continued inuence of Confucian
acquire sensitivity to shame and to be aware of other person’s ideals even though they may not be directly connected by
opinions, judgements, and evaluations. Thus, the goal of parents to Confucianism in modern China (cf. Lau & Yeung,
shaming/love withdrawal may be to motivate the child to take 1996).
responsibility for his/her own actions (Fung, 1999). Since Our results also support Wolf ’s (1970) assertion that
shaming/love withdrawal may be more positively valued in the cultural child-rearing practices evolve slowly and ‘‘practices
Chinese culture, it is not difŽcult to understand why Chinese among Chinese are not particularly open to Western inuences
mothers in this study report using this pattern of parenting and are more likely to accurately reect traditional goals and
more often than US mothers. values’’ (p. 39). T hus, one implication for future researchers is
The current study also indicates that Chinese mothers view the need to explore further the impact of Confucian principles
themselves as being more protective with their young children on family interactions and relationships to better understand
than US mothers. T his result is largely consistent with previous socialisation processes in the Chinese family.
reports and speculations in the literature (e.g., Chen et al., As far as cultural notions of parenting constructs empha-
1998 ; Kriger & Kross, 1972; Lin & Fu, 1990). Chen (in press) sised in North America are concerned, this study also
suggested that high parental protectiveness appears to Žt the contributes to the literature by examining empirically derived
Chinese culture since it is associated with children’s beha- stylistic dimensions of authoritative and authoritarian parent-
vioural inhibition, which is considered an adaptive outcome in ing rather than just overall parenting styles. Even though our
Chinese society. In contrast, shy and inhibited behaviours are results from the multi-sample conŽrmatory factor analysis
regarded as socially immature and maladaptive in North indicate that there are broad commonalities between the US
American individualistic cultures (Rubin & Asendorpf, and Chinese samples in the basic overall structures of
1993), and are often responded to with overprotective authoritative and authoritarian parenting, as anticipated,
parenting (Rubin et al., 1999). Thus, protective parenting, Chinese mothers scored signiŽcantly higher on physical
which may lead to maladaptive outcomes for Western children, coercion and lower on warmth/acceptance and democratic
may be negatively valued and discouraged in an individualistic participation than did US mothers. Our Žndings of higher
culture where independen t exploratory behaviour is more reported levels of physical coercion in the Chinese sample are
highly encouraged. consistent with many previous studies examining overall
Similar to protectiveness, it was anticipated that Chinese authoritarian parenting (e.g. Chao, 1994 ; Chen et al., 1997).
mothers would report exhibiting more directiveness with their Also, our results indicating that Chinese mothers scored lower
children than US mothers. Our Žndings are consistent with on warmth/acceptance are consistent with those of Chen et al.
those of others who found Chinese mothers placing high (1998), who reported that Chinese mothers scored signiŽ-
demands on their children’s after-school activities (Wu, 1996 ) cantly lower on acceptance than their Caucasian counterparts
and using scolding and criticisms to foster self-control (Lin & in Canada (cf. Lin & Fu, 1990). Our Žndings, however, are less
Fu, 1990). In the US, directive parenting is linked to consistent with those of Chao (1994), who found that
Baumrind’s conceptualisation of an authoritarianism style immigrant Chinese mothers in the US did not score lower on
(Robinson, Mandleco, Olsen, & Hart, 1995 ) and, thus, may overall authoritativeness than Caucasion mothers. Since
be viewed negatively by more educated parents who value parenting does not take place in a cultural vacuum, one could
autonomy granting with their children (Rodgers, 1998 ; Russell speculate that immigrant Chinese mothers may be inuenced
et al., in press). by some parenting notions emphasised in the North American
Surprisingly, we found no signiŽcant cultural differences on culture.
the belief construct of maternal involvement using four items One beneŽt of studying speciŽc parenting practices and
from Chao’s (1994) original Chinese Child-Rearing Ideology stylistic dimensions is that cultural speciŽc patterns may
Items . Chao did Žnd a cultural difference in studying emerge. For example, our Žndings indicated that for US
immigrant Chinese mothers in the US on two of the four mothers, a positive relationship was found between shaming/
items making up this construct. Our noncultural difference on love withdrawal and verbal hostility. This relationship was not
this construct may be due to a greater equality in parental roles apparent for Chinese mothers. Thus suggests that the practice
for parents in mainland China. Since 1950, the laws of the of shaming children towards particular socialisation goals in
People’s Republic of China have emphasised the joint China may be viewed quite differently from verbally hostile
responsibility of fathers and mothers for child-rearing (Chen parenting styles that are more reective of a pervasive
et al., 2000a). In addition, as many mainland Chinese mothers interactional climate. US mothers, however, may view both
490 WU ET AL. / PARENT ING IN CHINA AND THE US

verbal hostility and shaming/love withdrawal as being similarly In summary, the results of this study suggest that both US
associated as part of the interactional climate of the home (cf. and Chinese parenting may include parenting practices
Barber, 1996). emphasised in the Chinese cultural tradition as well as
Likewise, directive practices were associated with verbal parenting style dimensions emphasised in North America.
hostility and nonreasoning/punitive authoritarian parenting in Even though these parenting constructs are measurable in both
our North American sample but not in the Chinese sample. cultures, future research should continue to include both an
Yet, directiveness was associated with reasoning/induction and emic (arising from the culture) and etic (similar across
warmth/acceptance in the Chinese but not in the US sample. cultures) approach to further investigate the underlying
This reinforces the view that directiveness may serve a different philosophies that may differentiate ways that these parenting
function in Chinese society in ways noted in our Introduction constructs are enacted in diverse cultural settings (Berry,
(i.e. used for teaching self-control rather than for punishing). 1989).
Also supporting conceptualisations in the Introduction, direc- Manuscript received July 2000
Revised manuscript received October 2001
tiveness was related to maternal involvement in the Chinese
but not in the US sample. Yet, directiveness was associated
with less autonomy granting (democratic participation) in the References
US but not in the Chinese sample. This suggests that maternal
Barber, B.K. (1996). Parental psychological control: Revisiting a neglected
involvemen t and directiveness may go hand in hand with construct. Child Development, 67, 3296–3319.
Chinese mothering to foster dependenc e in young children. Barber, B.K., Bean, R.L., & Erickson, L.D. (2002). Violating the self: Parental
However, US mothers appear to emphasise less directive psychological control of children and adolescents. In B. Barber (Ed.), Intrusive
parenting: How psychological control affects children and adolescents. Washington,
approaches in an attempt to foster more autonom y than DC: American Psychological Association Press.
dependenc y in their involvement with preschoolers. Barber, B.K., & Olsen, J.A. (1997). Socialisation in context: Connection,
Similar to other studies of this nature (e.g., Chao, 1994), regulation, and autonomy in the family, school, and neighborhood, and with
peers. Journal of Adolescent References, 12, 287–315.
this study is limited by the sole use of self-report data gathered
Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Developmental
from parents. Although some studies have indicted moderate Psychology Monograph , 4, 1–103.
agreement between observational and self-report measures of Baumrind, D. (1989). Rearing competent children. In W. Damon (Ed.), Child
parenting (e.g. Kochanska, Kuczynski, & Radke-Yarrow, development today and tomorrow (pp. 349–378). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Baumrind, D. (1996). The discipline controversy revisited. Family Relations, 45,
1989 ; Pettit, Clawson, Dodge, & Bates, 1996), future research 405–414.
could beneŽt by using multi-method assessments that incor- Berry, J.W. (1989). Imposed etics-emics-derived etics: The operationalisation of
porate observational methodologies as well (e.g., Chen et al., a compelling idea. International Journal of Psychology , 24, 721–735.
Berry, J.W., Poortinga, Y.H., Segall, M.H., & Dasen, P.R. (1992). Cross-cultural
1998). As a further note of caution, one should keep in mind
psychology: Research and applications. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
that mean differences in self-ratings could be due to response Berndt, T.J., Cheung, P.C., Lau, S., Hau, K. & Lew, W.J.F. (1993). Perception
sets reecting different societal norms. T his may result in self- of parenting in mainland China, Taiwan, and Hong Kong: Sex differences
ratings that do not reect a common metric across cultures and society differences. Developmental Psychology, 29, 156–164.
Block, J.H. (1965). The child-rearing practices report: A technique for evaluating
(Triandis, 1995). For example, in Confucian societies, there is parental socialization orientations . Berkeley, CA: University of California,
the doctrine of the mean (Zong Yong Ahi Dao in Mandarin), Institute of Human Development.
which encourages the avoidanc e of extremes. Thus, Chinese Bollen, K.A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York: John
Wiley.
mothers may have rated their own parenting more in
Byrne, B.M. (1989). A primer of LISREL: Basic applications and programming for
accordance with culturally inuenced norms than in accor- conŽ rmatory factor-analytic models. New York: Springer-Verlag.
dance with their actual behaviour. Notwithstanding this Byrne, B.M., Shavelson, R.J., & Muthén, B. (1989). Testing for the equivalence
possibility, Žndings were consistent with prior research in this of factor covariance and mean structures: The issue of partial measurement
invariance. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 456–466.
area. Because it is unclear to what extent response bias may Chao, R.K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authoritarian parenting style:
have inuenced the results of this investigation, observational Understanding Chinese parenting through the cultural notion of training.
studies would be helpful for validating our Žndings. Child Development, 65, 1111–1119.
Chao, R.K. (1995). Chinese and European American cultural models of the self
Another limitation is that the samples represented more reected in mother‘s child-rearing beliefs. Ethos, 23, 328–354.
highly educated mothers from urban areas in both cultural Chao, R.K. (2001). Extending research on the consequences of parenting style
contexts. In addition, it cannot be assumed that Žndings from for Chinese Americans and European Americans. Child Development, 72,
samples from the speciŽc cities/areas within the cultures 1832–1843.
Chen, X. (1998). The changing Chinese family: Resources, parenting practices,
(Intermountain US West and Beijing) can be generalised to and children’s social-emotional problems. In U.P. Gielen & A.L. Comunian
the larger North American and Chinese culture (see Lai, (Eds.), Family and family therapy in international perspective. Trieste, Italy:
Zhang, & Wang, 2000). This does not allow our conclusions to Edizioni LINT.
Chen, X. (in press). Growing up in a collectivistic culture: Socialisation and
be generalised to families representing more diverse socio- socio-emotional development in Chinese children. In A.L. Comunian & U.P.
economic and ethnic background s in both cultures. Despite Gielen (Eds.), Human development in cross-cultural perspective. Padua, Italy:
these limitations, our Žndings build upon prior work by further Cedam.
reŽning conceptual frameworks for understanding parenting Chen, X., Dong, Q., & Zhou, H. (1997). Authoritative and authoritarian
parenting practices and social and school performance in Chinese children.
constructs emphasised in China that pertain to parents of International Journal of Behavioral Development, 21, 855–873.
young children. The next step would be to investigate further Chen, X., Hastings, P.D., Rubin, K.H., Chen, H., Cen, G., & Stewart, S.L.
how these parenting patterns are associated with speciŽc child (1998). Child-rearing attitudes and behavioural inhibition in Chinese and
Canadian toddlers: A cross-cultural study. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1–10.
outcomes in each cultural setting and build upon the work that Chen, X., Liu, M., & Li, D. (2000a). Parental warmth, control, and indulgence
has already been conducted in this area regarding some of and their relations to adjustment in Chinese children: A longitudinal study.
these constructs (e.g., Chen et al., 1997 ; 1998; 2000b; Olsen Journal of Family Psychology, 14, 401–419.
Chen, X., Liu, M., Li, D., Cen, G., Chen, H., & Wang, L. (2000b). Maternal
et al., 2002). T his would compliment recent research exploring
authoritative and authoritarian attitudes and mother-child interactions and
invariant measures of child outcomes in Chinese and North relationships in urban China. International Journal of Behavioral Development,
American settings (Hart et al., 2000b). 22, 119–126.
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT , 2002, 26 (6), 481–491 491
Coenders, G., Satorra, A., & Saris, W.E. (1997). Alternative approaches to McBride-Chang, C., & Chang, L. (1998). Adolescent-parent relations in Hong
structural modeling of ordinal data: A Monte Carlo study. Structural Equation Kong: Parenting styles, autonomy, and school achievement. The Journal of
Modeling, 4, 261–282. Genetic Psychology, 159, 421–436.
Darling, N., & Steinberg, L. (1993). Parenting style as context: An integrative Mize, J., & Pettit, G.S. (1997). Mothers’ social coaching, mother-child
model. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 487–496. relationship style, and children’s peer competence: Is the medium the
Eisenberg, N., & Murphy, B. (1997). Parenting and children’s moral message? Child Development, 28, 139–147.
development. In M.H. Bornstein (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 4: Mize, J., Russell, A., & Pettit, G.S. (1998). Further explorations of family-peer
Applied and practical parenting (pp. 227–254). Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence connections: The role of parenting practices and parenting style in children’s
Erlbaum. development of social competence. In P.T. Slee & K. Rigby (Eds.), Children’s
Fung, H. (1999). Becoming a moral child: The socialisation of shame among peer relations (pp. 31–44). New York: Routledge.
young Chinese children. Ethos, 27, 180–209. Nucci, L. (1994). Mothers’ beliefs regarding the personal domain of children.
Gorman, J.C. (1998). Parenting attitudes and practices of immigrant Chinese New Directions for Child Development, 66, 81–97.
mothers of adolescents. Family Relations, 47, 73–80. Olsen, S.F., Yang, C.M., Hart, C.H., Robinson, C.C., Wu, P., Nelson, D.A.,
Gray, M.R., & Steinberg, L. (1999). Unpacking authoritative parenting: Nelson, L.J., Jin, S., & Wo, J. (2002). Maternal psychological control and
Reassessing a multidimensional construct. Journal of Marriage and the Family, preschool children’s behavioural outcomes in China, Russia, and the United
61, 574–586. States. In B. Barber (Ed.), Intrusive parenting: How psychological control affects
Grusec, J.E., & Goodnow, J.J. (1994). Impact of parental discipline methods on children and adolescents. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association
the child’s internalisation of values: A reconceptualisation. Developmental Press.
Psychology , 30, 4–19. Pettit, G.S., Clawson, M.A., Dodge, K.A., & Bates, J.E. (1996). Stability and
Hart, C.H., DeWolf, M.D., Wozniak, P., & Burts, D.C. (1992). Maternal and change in peer-rejected status: The role of child behaviour, parenting, and
paternal disciplinary styles: Relations with preschoolers’ playground beha- family ecology. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42, 267–294.
vioral orientations and peer status. Child Development, 63, 879–892. Rigdon, E.E., & Ferguson, C.E. Jr (1991). The performance of the polychoric
Hart, C.H., Nelson, D.A., Robinson, C.C., Olsen, S.F., & McNeilly-Choque, correlation coefŽcient and selected Žtting functions in conŽrmatory factor
M.K. (1998a). Overt and relational aggression in Russian nursery-school-age analysis with ordinal data. Journal of Marketing Research, 28, 491–497.
children: Parenting style and marital linkages. Developmental Psychology, 34, Robinson, C.C., Mandleco, B., Olsen, S.F., & Hart, C.H. (1995). Authoritative,
687–697. authoritarian, and permissive parenting practices: Development of a new
Hart, C.H., Nelson, D.A., Robinson, C.C., Olsen, S.F., McNeilly-Choque, measure. Psychological Reports, 77, 818–830.
M.K., Porter, C.L., & McKee, T.R. (2000a). Russian parenting styles and Robinson, C.C., Mandleco, B., Olsen, S.F., & Hart, C.H. (2001). The parenting
family processes: Linkages with subtypes of victimisation and aggression. In styles and dimensions questionnaire. In B.F. Perlmutter, J. Touliatos, &
K.A. Kerns, J.M. Contreras, & A.M. Neal-Barnet t (Eds.), Family and peers: G.W., Holden (Eds.). Handbook of family measurement techniques: Vol. 3.
Linking two social worlds (pp. 47–84). Westport, CT: Praeger. Instruments and Index (pp. 319–321). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Hart, C.H., Newell, L.D., & Olsen, S.F. (in press). Parenting skills and social/ Rodgers, D.B. (1998). Supporting autonomy in young children. Young Children,
communicative competence in childhood. In J.O. Greene & B.R. Burleson 53, 75–80.
(Eds.), Handbook of communication and social interaction skill . Mahwah, NJ: Rubin, K.H., & Asendorpf, J.B. (Eds). (1993). Social withdrawal, inhibition, and
Lawrence Erlbaum. shyness in childhood. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Rubin, K.H., Nelson, L.J., Hastings, P., & Asendorpf, J. (1999). The transaction
Hart, C.H., Yang, C., Nelson, D.A., Jin, S., Bazarskaya, N., & Nelson, L.
between parents’ perceptions of their children’s shyness and their parenting
(1998b). Peer contact patterns, parenting practices, and preschoolers’ social
styles. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 23, 937–957.
competence in China, Russia, and the United States. In P. Slee & K. Rigby
Russell, A., Mize, J., & Bissaker, K. (in press). Parent-child relationships. In P.K.
(Eds), Children’s peer relations (pp. 3–30). London: Routledge.
Smith & C.H. Hart (Eds), Handbook of childhood social development. Oxford:
Hart, C.H., Yang, C., Nelson, L.J., Robinson, C.C., Olsen, J.A., Nelson, D.A.,
Blackwell.
Porter, C.L., Jin, S., Olsen, S.F. & Wu, P. (2000b). Peer acceptance in early
Schmitt, N. (1996). Uses and abuses of coefŽcient alpha. Psychological
childhood and subtypes of socially withdrawn behaviour in China, Russia, and
the United States. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 73–81. Assessment, 8, 350–353.
Schoenhals, M. (1993). The paradox of power in the People’s Republic of China
Ho, D.Y.F. (1986). Chinese pattern of socialisation: A critical review. In M.H.
middle school, Armonk, NY: M.E. Shape.
Bond (Ed.), The psychology of the Chinese people (pp. 1–37). Oxford: Oxford
Smetana, J.G. (1995). Parenting styles and conceptions of parental authority
University Press.
during adolescence. Child Development, 66, 299–316.
Ho, D.Y.F., & Kang, T.K. (1984). Intergenerational comparisons of child-
Steinberg, L. (1990). Autonomy, conict, and harmony in the family relation-
rearing attitudes and practice in Hong Kong. Developmental Psychology, 20,
ship. In S.S. Feldman & G.R. Elliott (Eds), At the threshold: The developing
1004–1016.
adolescent (pp. 255–276). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Hsu, F.L.K. (1971). Psychological homeostasis and jen: Conceptual tools for Steinberg, L., Dornbusch, S., & Brown, B.B. (1992). Ethnic differences in
advancing psychological anthropology. American Anthropology, 73, 23–44. adolescents’ achievement: An ecological perspective. American Psychologist,
Kline, R.B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling, New
47, 723–729.
York: Guilford Press. Stevens, J. (1996). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences. Mahwah, NJ:
Kochanska, G., Kuczynski, L., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1989). Correspondence Lawrence Erlbaum.
between mothers’ self-reported and observed child-rearing practices. Child Stevenson-Hinde, J. (1998). Parenting in different cultures: Time to focus.
Development, 60, 56–63. Developmental Psychology, 34, 698–700.
Komaroff, E. (1997). Effects of simultaneous violations of essential Tau Super, C.M., & Harkness, S. (1986). The developmental niche: A conceptua-
equivalenc e and uncorrelated error on coefŽcient alpha. Applied Psychological lisation at the interface of child and culture. International Journal of Behavioral
Measurement, 21, 337–348. Development , 9, 545–569.
Kriger, S.F., & Kross, W.H. (1972). Child-rearing attitudes of Chinese, Jewish, Tomada, G., & Schneider, B.H. (1997). Relational aggression, gender, and peer
and Protestant mothers. The Journal of Social Psychology, 86, 205–210. acceptance: Invariance across culture, stability over time, and concordance
Lai, A.C, Zhang, Z., & Wang, W. (2000). Maternal child-rearing practices in among informants. Developmental Psychology , 33, 601–609.
Hong Kong and Beijing Chinese families: A comparative study. International Triandis, H.C. (1993). Collectivism and individualism as cultural syndromes.
Journal of Psychology, 35, 60–66. Cross-Cultural Research, 27, 155–180.
Lau, S., & Yeung, P.P.W. (1996). Understanding Chinese child development: Triandis, H.C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview
The role of culture in socialisation. In S. Lau (Ed.), Growing up the Chinese Press.
way: Chinese child and adolescent development (pp. 29–44). Hong Kong: The Tseng, W.S., & Wu, D.Y.H. (1985). Introduction: The characteristics of
Chinese University Press. Chinese culture. In W.S. Tseng and D.Y.H. Wu (Eds.), Chinese cultural and
Leung, K., Lau, S., & Lam, W. (1998). Parenting styles and academic mental health (pp. 3–13). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
achievement: A cross-cultural study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 44, 157–172. Widaman, K.F., & Reise, S.P. (1997). Exploring the measurement invariance of
Lin, C.C., & Fu, V.R. (1990). A comparison of child-rearing practices among psychological instruments: Applications in the substance use domain. In K.J.
Chinese, immigrant Chinese, and Caucasian-American parents. Child Bryant, M. Windle, & S.G. West (Eds), The science of prevention (pp. 281–
Development, 61, 429–433. 324). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press.
Little, T.D. (1997). Mean and covariance structures (MACS) analysis of cross- Wolf, M. (1970). Child training and the Chinese family. In M. Freedman (Ed.),
cultural data: Practical and theoretical issues. Multivariate Behavioral Research , Family and kinship in Chinese society (pp. 37–62). Stanford, CA: Stanford
32, 53–76. University Press.
Maccoby, E.E., & Martin, J.A. (1983). Socialisation in the context of family: Wu, D.Y.H. (1996). Parental control: Psycho-cultural interpretation of Chinese
Parent-child interaction. In E.M. Hetherington (Ed.), Handbook of child patterns of socialization. In S. Lau (Ed.), Growing up in the Chinese way:
psychology: Vol 4. Socialisation, personality and social development (4th ed., pp. Chinese child and adolescent development. Hong Kong: The Chinese University
1–101). New York: John Wiley. Press.

You might also like