You are on page 1of 11

Journal of Environmental Psychology 90 (2023) 102086

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Environmental Psychology


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jep

Connectedness to nature promotes downplaying others’ humanity among


anxiously attached people
Gewei Chen, Jianning Dang *
Beijing Key Laboratory of Applied Experimental Psychology, National Demonstration Center for Experimental Psychology Education (Beijing Normal University), Faculty
of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Handling Editor: L. McCunn Global advocates have argued in favor of increasing connectedness to nature, the benefits of which have been
substantially supported for both the environment and humans. However, we extend mainstream opinions by
Keywords: identifying the potential negative impact of connectedness to nature on human relationships under specific
Connectedness to nature conditions. Specifically, we propose that connectedness to nature promotes dehumanization among anxiously
Dehumanization
attached people. We tested this assumption with three studies (N = 1276). Studies 1 and 2 revealed that, for
Anxious attachment
people with high levels of anxious attachment, connectedness to nature positively predicted dehumanization of
Preference for nature
thieves (Study 1) and people who casually litter (Study 2). By manipulating connectedness to nature, Study 3
found that dehumanization elicited by connectedness to nature among people with high attachment anxiety
predicted greater preference for nature over prisoners. These trends were eliminated or even reversed for those
with low levels of anxious attachment. The present research enriches our understanding of people’s social re­
lationships with nature and humans.

1. Introduction their dehumanization (Brandt & Reyna, 2011; Rottman et al., 2021).
Thus, an intriguing yet underexplored question arises: Will connected­
To combat environmental crises, the United Nations Environment ness to nature promote dehumanization of distant others?
Programme (UNEP) has advocated for countries worldwide to make To the best of our knowledge, no empirical studies have systemati­
peace with nature (UNEP, 2021). Echoing environmental advocates, cally explored this question. Substantial evidence has suggested that
ecologists and psychologists have demonstrated that increasing connectedness to nature or animals negatively predicts prejudice and
connectedness to nature (i.e., feelings of affiliation and oneness with discrimination (Amiot & Bastian, 2015; Bastian et al., 2012; Costello &
nature; Mayer & Frantz, 2004) is beneficial for environment (Leopold, Hodson, 2010, 2014a, 2014b; Dhont et al., 2016; Ng & Leung, 2022;
1949; Liu et al., 2019; Mayer et al., 2009; Mayer & Frantz, 2004; Nisbet Roszak et al., 1995; Starzyk et al., 2021). Despite the conceptual dis­
et al., 2009; Tam et al., 2013). However, this strategy may not always be tinctions between prejudice and dehumanization (e.g., Haslam &
beneficial for, and sometimes may even be harmful to, human re­ Stratemeyer, 2016), this line of research indicates that connectedness to
lationships. For example, Swedish climate activist Greta Thunberg ad­ nature may diminish social distance from others, which reduces dehu­
vocates mitigating global climate change but opposes the use of manization (Haslam, 2022). However, echoing Greta Thunberg’s opin­
chopsticks in China (Global Times, 2021). She demonstrates strong ions and ELF’s violence, empirical cues also indicate that connectedness
concern about nature while ignoring Chinese people’s fundamental to nature may promote dehumanization (e.g., Brandt & Reyna, 2011;
rights. The radical environmentalist organization Earth Liberation Front Mourey et al., 2017; Rottman et al., 2021; Shin & Kim, 2020; Waytz
(ELF) aims to protect the environment using more extreme tactics. et al., 2010). Thus, connectedness to nature may not be beneficial to
However, its members treat people who harm the environment violently human relationships under specific conditions or among specific kinds
and even commit crimes (e.g., sabotage and arson), thereby threatening of people. Inspired by the preoccupied characteristics of anxiously
these targets’ welfare (Spadaro, 2020). In these examples, the environ­ attached people (Brennan et al., 1998; Lavigne et al., 2011), the present
ment is considered far more valuable than distant others, suggesting research innovatively proposes and examines that connectedness to

* Corresponding author. Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, 19 Xinjiekouwai Street, Beijing, 100875, China.
E-mail addresses: 202121061071@mail.bnu.edu.cn (G. Chen), jndang@bnu.edu.cn (J. Dang).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2023.102086
Received 14 December 2022; Received in revised form 18 July 2023; Accepted 19 July 2023
Available online 20 July 2023
0272-4944/© 2023 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
G. Chen and J. Dang Journal of Environmental Psychology 90 (2023) 102086

nature may promote dehumanization of distant others among anxiously others. For instance, people who have experienced unsatisfying social
attached people. relationships may seek to establish social relationships with nonhuman
agents (e.g., pets) by attributing more humanlike characteristics to them
1.1. Connectedness to nature and dehumanization (Brown et al., 2016; Mourey et al., 2017). After feeling connected to
these agents, they reduce striving to maintain connections with (Mourey
Humans have an intrinsic need to be affiliated with nature because et al., 2017) and ascribe less humanness to other people (Shin & Kim,
they have lived for a far longer time in nature than in cities (Kellert & 2020). More relevant to our research, people who endorse a religious
Wilson, 1993). Drawing on this biophilia hypothesis, Mayer and Frantz worldview that emphasizes connectedness to the natural world have
(2004) proposed the concept of connectedness to nature, which refers to been found to perceive further social distance from other humans
feelings of affiliation and oneness with nature. As a hallmark of psy­ (Kramp, 2015), which in turn may result in greater dehumanization of
chological closeness with nature, connectedness to nature is associated others (Haslam, 2022). Likewise, people who ascribe greater moral
with pro-environmental behaviors, such as conserving energy and using worth to nature than to marginalized outgroups attribute more mental
green products (Liu et al., 2019; Mayer & Frantz, 2004; Nisbet et al., capacity to nature and less to outgroups (Rottman et al., 2021).
2009; Tam et al., 2013). Furthermore, connectedness to nature appears A common rationale underlying this avenue of research is that, once
beneficial to human health and subjective well-being. For example, feeling connected with nonhuman agents (e.g., nature), some people
connectedness to nature positively predicts outcomes such as life satis­ may be preoccupied with those agents and stop seeking new social ex­
faction, positive affect, and quality of life (Cervinka et al., 2012; Mayer periences. Drawing on adult attachment research, this specific response
et al., 2009; Mayer & Frantz, 2004). pattern is characterized by anxious attachment (Brennan et al., 1998;
Dehumanization refers to attributing less humanness to others, Lavigne et al., 2011). Therefore, we argue that anxious attachment may
especially with respect to the human mind (Haslam, 2006; Waytz et al., play a vital role in determining how connectedness to nature is associ­
2010). “Mind” is an umbrella term for mental capacities and includes ated with or influences dehumanization.
two aspects: agency (i.e., capacity to act and plan) and experience (i.e.,
capacity to feel; Gray et al., 2007; Waytz et al., 2010). Thus, denial of 1.2. Anxious attachment as a moderator
different aspects of the mind reveals two forms of dehumanization:
dehumanization on agency (i.e., attributing less agency to others) and Attachment is an inborn system that motivates people to obtain
dehumanization on experience (i.e., attributing less experience to safety and security by seeking proximity to attachment figures in times
others). Given that dehumanization involves seeing others as distant of need (Bowlby, 1982). Researchers have typically found two di­
(Haslam, 2022), people tend to view close others as being fully human mensions when exploring attachment patterns in adults: anxiety and
but perceive distant others as being less than human (Kteily et al., 2015; avoidance (Brennan et al., 1998). Specifically, attachment anxiety re­
Leyens et al., 2001). For example, people tend to view immoral in­ flects the extent to which a person is excessively worried regarding the
dividuals (e.g., thieves and prisoners; Bastian et al., 2013; Deska et al., love and availability they can receive from their partners in times of
2020; Zhang et al., 2015) and marginalized or stigmatized groups (e.g., need, while attachment avoidance reflects the extent to which people
immigrants and obese people; Esses et al., 2013; Sim et al., 2022) as show distrust and defensively maintain behavioral and emotional in­
having less agency than others. Moreover, dehumanization has been dependence from their partners (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2013). Previous
shown to predict aggression and hostility against dehumanized groups research has demonstrated that people with high levels of attachment
in both historical conflicts and contemporary society (Bandura et al., anxiety (rather than attachment avoidance) yearn to develop more
1975; Bruneau et al., 2020; Costello & Hodson, 2010; Kteily et al., 2015; secure social relationships, but typically fail to do so because their
Kteily & Landry, 2022; Opotow, 1990; Smith, 2011), thereby exerting excessive pursuit of love and fear of rejection may drive away their
destructive effects on human relationships and increasing the social partners (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2013). Therefore, they are more eager to
distance between people (Haslam, 2006, 2022). seek social connections to counteract loneliness. These characteristics
Although connectedness to nature and dehumanization tap people’s may lead people with high attachment anxiety to be more likely to
social interactions with different targets (i.e., nature and distant others dehumanize distant others when they experience connectedness to
or marginalized groups), previous research has provided a path for nature.
linking human-nature relationships to the quality of social relationships Anxiously attached people may utilize connectedness to nature as a
among humans. Notably, most previous research has examined how compensatory strategy to cope with their unsatisfied need for connec­
connectedness to nature or animals influences prejudice and discrimi­ tion. Their endless desire for intimacy and fear of potential rejection
nation against marginalized groups (e.g., ethnic minorities and immi­ make anxiously attached people less socially accepted by others
grants; Amiot & Bastian, 2015; Dhont et al., 2016; Ng & Leung, 2022; (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2013). As a compensatory strategy to regain a
Starzyk et al., 2021). However, dehumanization is conceptually sense of security and connection, they may turn to nonhumans (e.g.,
different from prejudice and discrimination. Dehumanization may strip objects or gods) for intimacy and thus regard nonhumans as attachment
away not only desirable human qualities (e.g., ambition) but also un­ figures (Granqvist et al., 2010; Keefer et al., 2012; Neave et al., 2016;
desirable ones (e.g., irresponsibility; Haslam, 2006; Haslam et al., Norris et al., 2012). Beyond fully controllable objects or omniscient and
2005). In contrast, prejudice and discrimination generally manifest as omnipotent gods, nature is also a quality candidate for a substitute
unfavorable evaluations and harmful behaviors toward others (Dovidio attachment figure (Jordan, 2009). It is important to note that we are not
et al., 2010). In other words, someone may be viewed or treated unfa­ arguing that a sense of social deficits or anxious attachment is directly
vorably yet not be dehumanized (Haslam & Stratemeyer, 2016). More­ associated with greater connectedness to nature as existing evidence on
over, dehumanization and prejudice activate inferior frontal cortex (a this relationship is inconclusive (Moreton et al., 2019; Poon et al., 2015;
region related to animalistic behaviors) and posterior cingulate cortex (a Zelenski & Nisbet, 2014). Rather, we assume that, for anxiously attached
region related to liking), respectively (Bruneau et al., 2018). Dehu­ people, nature could be considered a special social entity with which
manization predicts intergroup conflicts beyond prejudice (Bruneau they can connect, given that recent research has revealed that nature
et al., 2020; Kteily et al., 2015). It is necessary to extend previous satisfies the human need for relatedness (Yang et al., 2022).
research by examining how connectedness to nature is associated with However, anxiously attached people tend to stop seeking social
or influences dehumanization. connections once their need for connectedness is temporarily met.
Regarding dehumanization, although direct evidence is lacking, People with high attachment anxiety experience unsatisfying social re­
some researchers have suggested that connectedness to nature may, at lationships and constantly seek social connections to ease feelings of
least under specific conditions, facilitate downplaying the humanity of rejection and loneliness. They are more likely to strive for social

2
G. Chen and J. Dang Journal of Environmental Psychology 90 (2023) 102086

relationships out of their desire to reduce social deficits rather than their more vulnerable to being dehumanized. In this study, we expected a
genuine interest in others (Lavigne et al., 2011). Therefore, once they positive association between connectedness to nature and dehuman­
have attained social connections with other humans or nonhuman ization to be found primarily among those with high levels of anxious
agents, they may temporarily experience emotional relief and withdraw attachment.
their striving to seek further affiliations (Dang & Liu, 2023; Lavigne
et al., 2011). Accordingly, anxiously attached individuals are prone to 2.1. Method
be preoccupied with their attachment figures and thus show less concern
for others (Brennan et al., 1998; Maranges et al., 2022) and greater Two bilingual researchers translated all the research materials from
social distance from outgroups (Bao et al., 2015; Boag & Carnelley, English to Chinese and conducted a back-translation to check the
2012, 2016; Carnelley & Boag, 2019). Therefore, when experiencing equivalence.
connectedness to nature, they may be preoccupied with nature and stop
seeking to extend their social networks, resulting in greater social dis­ 2.1.1. Participants
tance from and dehumanization of others. Relying on the smallest effect size of interest (Lakens, 2014) of f2 =
In contrast, the trend of connectedness to nature being associated 0.02 and α = 0.05, a total sample size of N = 395 would provide 80%
with or leading to dehumanization may be eliminated or even reversed power to detect the moderating role of anxious attachment in the as­
among people with low levels of anxious attachment. Those with lower sociation between connectedness to nature and dehumanization. We
levels of anxious attachment engage in social interactions in a relatively recruited a total of 440 Chinese participants via Aishiyan1 (https://aish
non-defensive way and are less likely to be preoccupied with one specific iyan.bnu.edu.cn), paying 5 CNY (0.69 USD) to each participant. We
target (Lavigne et al., 2011; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2013). Once they feel excluded 38 participants who failed the attention check; thus, the final
connected with a target, they may continue striving for other connec­ sample included 402 participants2 (292 women and 110 men, Mage =
tions. Previous research has revealed that connectedness to nature is 23.05 years, SDage = 5.23).
positively associated with connectedness to humanity as a whole and
connectedness to distant others (Moreton et al., 2019; Saroglou et al., 2.1.2. Procedure and materials
2008). A perception of connectedness to other people is essential in After providing informed consent, participants completed measures
attributing humanness to them (Haslam, 2022). In this regard, a greater of connectedness to nature and anxious attachment in a random
sense of connectedness to nature may elicit the attribution of greater sequence. Then, participants rated the extent to which they dehumanize
humanness to and thus less dehumanization of distant others. Therefore, thieves. Moreover, we used a measure developed by Haddock et al.
we predict that for people with low levels of anxious attachment, (1993) to assess prejudice against thieves as a covariate. For exploratory
connectedness to nature and dehumanization will demonstrate a less purposes, we measured variables relevant to connectedness to nature (i.
positive or, perhaps, a negative relationship. e., nature relatedness and inclusion of nature in self; Nisbet et al., 2009;
Schultz, 2002) and examined their roles in predicting dehumanization.
1.3. Overview We also measured individual differences in the tendency toward
anthropomorphizing (Neave et al., 2015) to examine whether it mod­
Based on the theoretical and empirical evidence discussed in the erates the association between connectedness to nature and dehuman­
previous section, we hypothesized that attachment anxiety would ization. The descriptions of these variables and their respective analyses
moderate the relationship between connectedness to nature and dehu­ are included in the Supplementary Material because they were not the
manization. Specifically, we hypothesized that connectedness to nature focal interest of this study.
would be positively associated with or promote dehumanization of
distant others (e.g., thieves, litterers, or prisoners) primarily among 2.1.2.1. Connectedness to nature. We assessed connectedness to nature
people with high levels of attachment anxiety. We conducted three using the 14-item Connectedness to Nature Scale (CNS; Mayer & Frantz,
studies to test this hypothesis. In Study 1, we used a correlational design 2004). Each item (e.g., “I often feel a sense of oneness with the natural
to provide preliminary evidence for our hypothesis. In Study 2, to test world around me”) was rated on a seven-point scale (1 = totally disagree,
the generalizability of our findings, we used a different target of dehu­ 7 = totally agree). After reverse-coding the negatively worded items, we
manization that was more relevant in a nature-related context. In Study averaged the ratings to create an anxious attachment index (α = 0.79).
3, we induced connectedness to nature and examined its effect on Higher scores indicated higher levels of connectedness to nature.
dehumanization. We further extended our hypothesis by examining
whether increased dehumanization would predict a preference for na­
2.1.2.2. Anxious attachment. We assessed anxious attachment using the
ture over marginalized groups (i.e., prisoners).
18-item anxious attachment subscale of the Experience in Close Re­
All the data and materials are available on the Open Science
lationships scale (Brennan et al., 1998). Each item (e.g., “I worry about
Framework (OSF; https://osf.io/9vfu2/?view_only=8945658ff7bb4
being abandoned”) was rated on a seven-point scale (1 = totally disagree,
bdeaf9a88643804810d). In addition, Study 1 was preregistered (htt
7 = totally agree). After reverse-coding the negatively worded items, we
ps://osf.io/7dhe3/?view_only=7bbf5439ceee4d7785424486d4
averaged the ratings to create an anxious attachment index (α = 0.92).
96a1d6). We provide information about the stimulus materials,
Higher scores indicated higher levels of anxious attachment.
exploratory factor analyses of the dehumanization measure in Study 3,
and supplemental analyses for Studies 1–3 in the Supplementary Ma­
2.1.2.3. Dehumanization. We assessed dehumanization using an
terial. For each study, we reported all measures, conditions, data ex­
adapted 18-item mind attribution scale (Gray et al., 2007). Each item (e.
clusions, and sample size calculations.
g., “Compared to average people, to what extent do you think thieves are
2. Study 1
1
Aishiyan used in Study 1 and Credamo used in Studies 2 and 3 are online
Study 1 was designed to obtain preliminary evidence of the moder­
survey platforms similar to Amazon Mechanical Turk. These platform advertise
ating role of anxious attachment in the association between connect­ surveys by selectively presenting them to eligible participants across the
edness to nature and dehumanization. Specifically, we assessed country.
participants’ dehumanization of thieves. We chose this target because 2
There were nine outliers (i.e., ±3 standard deviations from the mean). The
thieves are a distant social group for most people, and their criminal significance levels of the results were the same with or without the inclusion of
behavior may reflect a lack of self-control, potentially making them these outliers; therefore, we reported the results without excluding them.

3
G. Chen and J. Dang Journal of Environmental Psychology 90 (2023) 102086

capable of memorizing?“) was rated on a seven-point scale (1 = much participants with anxious attachment scores of 2.54 (B = − 0.14, 95%CI
less than average people, 4 = equal to average people, 7 = much more than [− 0.28, 0.00], SE = 0.07) or below, connectedness to nature negatively
average people). We reverse-coded and averaged all ratings and to create predicted dehumanization on experience. For participants with anxious
an overall dehumanization index (α = 0.78). Higher scores indicated attachment scores of 6.68 (B = 0.24, 95%CI [0.00, 0.49], SE = 0.12) or
higher levels of overall dehumanization. above, connectedness to nature positively predicted dehumanization on
People may strip away different aspects of humanness for different experience. However, for those with anxious attachment scores ranging
groups (Gray et al., 2011; Haque & Waytz, 2012; Sim et al., 2022); from 2.54 to 6.68, the association was non-significant (Fig. 1c).
therefore, we further distinguished between two aspects of dehuman­ We re-conducted these analyses after controlling for prejudice
ization: dehumanization on agency (e.g., the capacity for self-control) against thieves; supplemental analyses yielded similar results (see Sup­
and experience (e.g., the capacity to experience fear). We plementary Material), suggesting the robustness of the results.
reverse-coded and averaged relevant ratings to create dehumanization Study 1 provided preliminary evidence for the moderating role of
on agency (α = .73) and experience (α = 0.61) indices, respectively. anxious attachment in the association between connectedness to nature
Higher scores indicated higher levels of dehumanization on agency or and dehumanization. Specifically, connectedness to nature positively
experience. predicted dehumanization only among participants with high levels of
Finally, participants reported their gender and age. After completing anxious attachment but not among those with moderate levels. In
the online survey, participants were debriefed and remunerated. addition, connectedness to nature negatively predicted dehumanization
among participants with low levels of anxious attachment. Our findings
2.2. Results and discussion suggest that, depending on whether attachment anxiety is at a high or
low level, connectedness to nature can predict greater or less dehu­
Table 1 shows the correlations between variables. manization, respectively.
To test the moderating role of anxious attachment in the association
between connectedness to nature and overall dehumanization, we used 3. Study 2
the PROCESS macro (Hayes, 2017; Model 1). As shown in Table 2, the
results indicated that neither connectedness to nature nor anxious Study 2 aimed to replicate and extend Study 1. To examine the
attachment significantly predicted overall dehumanization; however, generalizability of our findings, Study 2 used a new target of dehu­
the two-way interaction was significant. To further explore the moder­ manization that is more relevant in a nature-related context. Study 1
ating role of anxious attachment, we used the Johnson-Neyman tech­ used a target (i.e., thieves) that is ostensibly irrelevant to nature.
nique (Johnson & Neyman, 1936) to identify significant regions in the However, when anxiously attached people are preoccupied with nature,
association between connectedness to nature and overall dehumaniza­ they may be more likely to dehumanize those who harm nature directly
tion across different levels of anxious attachment. The results showed rather than thieves. Therefore, Study 2 introduced people who casually
that, for those with anxious attachment scores of 3.00 (B = − 0.12, 95% litter as dehumanization targets. Moreover, we measured dehumaniza­
CI [− 0.23, 0.00], SE = 0.06) or below, connectedness to nature nega­ tion with a different rating scale. Dehumanization can be measured in
tively predicted overall dehumanization. For participants with anxious two forms: absolute and relative (Haslam & Loughnan, 2014). Study 1
attachment scores of 4.68 (B = 0.11, 95%CI [0.00, 0.22], SE = 0.06) or assessed relative dehumanization, which was operationalized as the
above, connectedness to nature positively predicted overall dehuman­ level of mind that participants perceived thieves as having relative to
ization. However, for those with anxious attachment scores ranging average people. Study 2 measured absolute dehumanization by
from 3.00 to 4.68, the association between connectedness to nature and instructing participants to make absolute judgments about the mental
overall dehumanization was non-significant (Fig. 1a). capabilities of litterers. We predicted that connectedness to nature
We then tested a similar model with dehumanization on agency as would positively predict a tendency toward dehumanization for those
the dependent variable. Neither connectedness to nature nor anxious with high attachment anxiety.
attachment significantly predicted dehumanization on agency; however,
the two-way interaction was significant (Table 2). Further analyses 3.1. Method
revealed that, for participants with anxious attachment scores of 2.93 (B
= − 0.16, 95%CI [− 0.31, 0.00], SE = 0.08) or below, connectedness to 3.1.1. Participants
nature negatively predicted dehumanization on agency. For participants Relying on the effect size from Study 1 (f 2 = 0.031) and α = 0.05, a
with anxious attachment scores of 4.34 (B = 0.13, 95%CI [0.00, 0.26], sample of N = 256 would provide 80% power to detect the moderating
SE = 0.07) or above, connectedness to nature positively predicted role of anxious attachment. However, for exploratory purposes, we
dehumanization on agency. However, for those with anxious attachment aimed to examine whether connectedness to nature predicts dehuman­
scores ranging from 2.93 to 4.34, the association was non-significant ization when controlling for nature anthropomorphism and avoidant
(Fig. 1b). attachment. Considering the complexity of the model, we recruited a
We conducted a similar regression on dehumanization on experi­ total of 600 Chinese participants via Credamo (https://www.credamo.
ence. Neither connectedness to nature nor anxious attachment signifi­ com), paying 5 CNY (0.69 USD) to each participant. We excluded five
cantly predicted dehumanization on experience; however, the two-way participants who failed the attention check; therefore, the final sample
interaction was significant (Table 2). Further analyses revealed that, for included 595 participants3 (393 women and 202 men; Mage = 31.34
years, SDage = 11.72).
Table 1
Descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations between variables in study 1. 3.1.2. Procedures and materials
Variables M SD 1 2 3 4
After providing informed consent, participants completed in a fixed
order measures of anxious attachment, connectedness to nature, and
1. Connectedness to nature 5.03 0.73
dehumanization of people who casually litter. For exploratory purposes,

2. Overall dehumanization 4.09 0.69 .01 –
3. Dehumanization on 4.51 0.90 .03 .87*** – we measured nature anthropomorphism (Tam et al., 2013) and avoidant
agency
4. Dehumanization on 3.82 0.68 − .02 .91*** .59*** –
experience 3
There were 17 outliers (i.e., ±3 standard deviations from the mean). The
5. Anxious attachment 3.92 1.09 − .04 − .03 − .04 − .02
significance levels of the results were the same with or without the inclusion of
Note. ***p < .001. these outliers; therefore, we reported the results without excluding them.

4
G. Chen and J. Dang Journal of Environmental Psychology 90 (2023) 102086

Table 2
Regression analyses of moderated models in study 1.
Predictor variable Outcome variable: Overall dehumanization Outcome variable: Dehumanization on Outcome variable: Dehumanization on
agency experience

B [95% CI] SE t p B [95% CI] SE t p B [95% CI] SE t p

Connectedness to nature 0.01 [-0.08, 0.05 0.20 .845 0.04 [-0.08, 0.06 0.72 .471 − 0.01 [-0.10, 0.05 − 0.28 .778
0.10] 0.16] 0.08]
Anxious attachment − 0.03 [-0.09, 0.03 − 0.83 .405 − 0.04 [-0.12, 0.04 − 1.00 .316 − 0.02 [-0.08, 0.03 − 0.53 .599
0.04] 0.04] 0.05]
Connectedness to nature × Anxious 0.14 [0.05, 0.04 3.30 .001 0.20 [0.10, 0.05 3.75 <.001 0.09 [0.01, 0.04 2.27 .024
attachment 0.22] 0.31] 0.17]

R2 .03 .04 .01

using the CNS (α = 0.81), as in Study 1.

3.1.2.3. Dehumanization. We assessed dehumanization using an


adapted six-item mind attribution scale (Gray et al., 2007), similar to
that applied in Study 1. However, the target was replaced with people
who casually litter. In contrast to Study 1, the participants rated their
mind attributions to the target directly with no reference. Specifically,
three items measured dehumanization on agency (e.g., “To what extent
do you think that people who casually litter are capable of planning?“)
and three items measured dehumanization on experience (e.g., “To what
extent do you think that people who casually litter are capable of feeling
rage?“). Items were rated on a seven-point scale (1 = totally disagree, 7 =
totally agree). We reverse-coded and averaged the ratings to create
overall dehumanization (α = 0.81), dehumanization on agency (α =
0.76), and dehumanization on experience (α = 0.86) indices. Higher
scores indicated higher levels of dehumanization.
Finally, participants reported their gender and age. After completing
the online survey, participants were debriefed and remunerated.

3.2. Results and discussion

Table 3 shows the correlations between variables.


We used the PROCESS macro (Hayes, 2017; Model 1) with
connectedness to nature as the independent variable, overall dehu­
manization as the dependent variable, anxious attachment as the
moderator. As shown in Table 4, the results indicated that both
connectedness to nature and anxious attachment significantly predicted
overall dehumanization. However, the two-way interaction was not
significant. When considering dehumanization on experience as the
dependent variable, the results yielded only a main effect of connect­
edness to nature and not of attachment anxiety or the two-way
interaction.
We also tested a moderation model with dehumanization on agency
Fig. 1. The moderating role of anxious attachment in the association between as the dependent variable. The results showed that both connectedness
connectedness to nature and (a) overall dehumanization, (b) dehumanization to nature and anxious attachment significantly predicted dehumaniza­
on agency, and (c) dehumanization on experience in study 1. tion on agency. Moreover, the two-way interaction was significant
Note. CNS = connectedness to nature scale. Deh = overall dehumanization; (Table 4). To further explore the moderating role of anxious attachment,
Deh_A = dehumanization on agency; Deh_E = dehumanization on experience. we used the Johnson-Neyman technique. The results showed that, for

attachment (Fraley et al., 2011; Zhang et al., 2022) as covariates. The Table 3
descriptions of these variables and their analyses are included in the Descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations between variables in study 2.
Supplementary Material.
Variable M SD 1 2 3 4

1. Connectedness to 5.37 0.72


3.1.2.1. Anxious attachment. We assessed anxious attachment with the

nature
anxious attachment subscale of the Experience in Close Relationships- 2. Overall 5.21 0.99 .21*** –
Relationship Structure scale (Fraley et al., 2011; Zhang et al., 2022). dehumanization
Participants completed three items (e.g., “I’m afraid that they may 3. Dehumanization 5.85 0.93 .26*** .75*** –
on agency
abandon me”) on a seven-point scale (1 = totally disagree, 7 = totally
4. Dehumanization 4.57 1.41 .12** .90*** .40*** –
agree). We averaged the ratings to create an anxious attachment index on experience
(α = 0.85). Higher scores indicated higher levels of anxious attachment. 5. Anxious 3.06 1.61 − .23*** − .16*** − .20*** − .09*
attachment
3.1.2.2. Connectedness to nature. We assessed connectedness to nature Note. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

5
G. Chen and J. Dang Journal of Environmental Psychology 90 (2023) 102086

Table 4
Regression analyses of moderated models in study 2.
Predictor variable Outcome variable: Overall dehumanization Outcome variable: Dehumanization on Outcome variable: Dehumanization on
agency experience

B [95% CI] SE t p B [95% CI] SE t p B [95% CI] SE t p

Connectedness to nature 0.24 [0.13, 0.06 4.30 <.001 0.29 [0.19, 0.05 5.56 <.001 0.20 [0.04, 0.08 2.40 .017
0.35] 0.39] 0.36]
Anxious attachment − 0.07 [-0.12, 0.03 − 2.88 .004 − 0.09 [-0.13, 0.02 − 3.73 <.001 − 0.06 [-0.13, 0.04 − 1.60 .110
− 0.02] − 0.04] 0.01]
Connectedness to nature × Anxious 0.03 [-0.03, 0.03 0.95 .341 0.08 [0.03, 0.03 2.89 .004 − 0.02 [-0.11, 0.04 − 0.51 .611
attachment 0.09] 0.14] 0.07]

R2 .06 .10 .02

participants with anxious attachment scores of 1.30 (B = 0.15, 95%CI further examine the generalizability of our findings. Third, we examined
[0.00, 0.29], SE = 0.07) or above, connectedness to nature positively whether increased dehumanization would further promote a preference
predicted dehumanization on agency. However, for those with anxious for nature over prisoners, similar to the discrimination found in inter­
attachment scores below 1.30, the association was non-significant group contexts (e.g., Bruneau et al., 2020; Kersbergen & Robinson,
(Fig. 2). 2019; Kteily et al., 2015). We define a preference for nature as the
We re-conducted these analyses after controlling for nature anthro­ tendency to prioritize the welfare of nature over that of distant others.
pomorphism and avoidant attachment; supplemental analyses yielded Dehumanization involves seeing others as entities not worthy of moral
similar results (see Supplementary Material), suggesting the robustness considerations, leading to reduced consideration of their welfare (Waytz
of the results. et al., 2010). As such, people who feel connected to nature but dehu­
In summary, Study 2 partially replicated the results of Study 1, manize prisoners are expected to value nature over prisoners. In this
revealing the moderating role of anxious attachment in the association study, we operationalized a preference for nature over prisoners as the
between connectedness to nature and dehumanization on agency. Spe­ difference between the funding amount allocated to programs aimed at
cifically, connectedness to nature positively predicted dehumanization protecting the environment and that allocated to programs aimed at
on agency for people with higher levels of anxious attachment, while protecting prisoners’ rights. We predicted that a promoting effect of
this pattern was not observed for those with very low levels of anxious connectedness to nature on dehumanization and subsequent preferences
attachment. However, regarding overall dehumanization or dehuman­ would be found primarily among people with high levels of anxious
ization on experience, the positive association between connectedness to attachment.
nature and dehumanization existed irrespective of the level of anxious
attachment. This could be because people who feel connected to nature
4.1. Method
are averse to anti-environment others, such as those who casually litter.
Perceptions of others as disgusting provoke dehumanization (Buckels &
4.1.1. Participants
Trapnell, 2013). With the findings of Study 1, we assume that the target
The dependent variable in Study 3 was similar to that in Study 1;
may play an essential role in the association between connectedness to
therefore, we used the effect size obtained in Study 1 (f 2 = 0.031), and α
nature and dehumanization. We return to this point in the General
= 0.05 and an N = 256 would provide 80% power to detect the
Discussion section.
moderating role of anxious attachment on the effect of connectedness to
nature on dehumanization. We recruited a total of 303 Chinese partic­
4. Study 3
ipants via Credamo, paying 3 CNY (0.41 USD) to each participant. We
excluded 24 participants who failed the attention check; thus, the final
Study 3 extended Studies 1 and 2 in three ways. First, we examined
sample included 279 participants4 (194 women and 85 men, Mage =
the effect of connectedness to nature on dehumanization by manipu­
27.22 years, SDage = 7.42). Participants were randomly assigned to
lating connectedness to nature with a bogus-feedback paradigm. Sec­
either the low (n = 133) or the high (n = 146) connectedness to nature
ond, we used a different target of dehumanization (i.e., prisoners) to
condition.

4.1.2. Procedures and materials


After providing informed consent, participants completed measure
of anxious attachment and connectedness to nature manipulation.
Subsequently, participants rated the extent to which they dehumanized
prisoners and preferred nature over prisoners. For exploratory purposes,
we measured avoidant attachment (Fraley et al., 2011; Zhang et al.,
2022) as a covariate. A description of the measurement instrument for
avoidant attachment and the relevant analyses that control for avoidant
attachment are included in the Supplementary Material.

4.1.2.1. Anxious attachment. We assessed anxious attachment (α =


0.84) as in Study 2.

4.1.2.2. Connectedness to nature manipulation. We manipulated

4
There were eight outliers (i.e., ±3 standard deviations from the mean). The
Fig. 2. The moderating role of anxious attachment in the association between significance levels of the results were the same with or without the inclusion of
connectedness to nature and dehumanization on agency in study 2. these outliers; therefore, we reported the results without excluding them.

6
G. Chen and J. Dang Journal of Environmental Psychology 90 (2023) 102086

connectedness to nature using a method similar to that in Twenge et al. 4.2.1. Manipulation check
(2007). Participants completed a 21-item scale that ostensibly assessed We conducted a t-test to compare levels of connectedness to nature
their relationship with nature. The scale consists of six items extracted between both conditions. The data violated the homoscedasticity hy­
from the Big Five Inventory (Rammstedt & John, 2007; e.g., “I see pothesis; therefore, we used Welch’s test to correct the results. The re­
myself as someone who is outgoing and sociable; ” 1 = totally disagree, 7 sults showed that participants in the high connectedness to nature
= totally agree), and 15 items measuring self-transcendence and benev­ condition (M = 5.69, SD = 1.02) scored significantly higher than their
olence value (Schwartz, 1992; e.g., mature love; 1 = not at all, 7 = very counterparts in the low connectedness to nature condition (M = 3.42,
important).5 We chose these items because previous studies have SD = 1.30), t(250.38) = 16.09, p < .001, d = 1.94, 95%CI [1.70, 2.18].
demonstrated their associations with connectedness to nature (Lengieza Therefore, the manipulation of connectedness to nature was successful.
& Swim, 2021). Subsequently, participants received bogus feedback that
their connectedness to nature score was either in the 68th percentile 4.2.2. Moderating role of anxious attachment
(high connectedness to nature condition) or the 32nd percentile (low We used the PROCESS macro (Hayes, 2017; Model 1) with
connectedness condition) among 12,000 peers. Then, participants were dummy-coded connectedness to nature conditions (high = 1, low = 0) as
provided at least 2 min to think about and write down the reasons why the independent variable, overall dehumanization as the dependent
they had such high (low) connectedness to nature. As a manipulation variable, anxious attachment as the moderator. As Table 6 shows, the
check, participants completed three items (e.g., “Right now, I feel that I results indicated that neither connectedness to nature condition nor
am interconnected with and have a reciprocal influence on nature; ” 1 = anxious attachment significantly predicted overall dehumanization.
totally disagree, 7 = totally agree). We averaged the ratings to create a However, the two-way interaction was significant. Further analyses
connectedness to nature index (α = 0.93). Higher scores indicated using the Johnson-Neyman technique revealed that, for participants
higher levels of connectedness to nature. with anxious attachment scores of 6.27 (B = 0.50, 95%CI [0.00, 1.01],
SE = 0.26) or above, connectedness to nature increased overall dehu­
4.1.2.3. Dehumanization. We assessed dehumanization using an manization. However, for those with anxious attachment scores below
adapted six-item mind-attribution scale (Gray et al., 2007). However, 6.27, the effect was non-significant (Fig. 3a).
two items (i.e., “Compared to average people, to what extent do you We then tested a moderation model with dehumanization on agency
think prisoners are capable of planning/feeling rage?“) were removed as the dependent variable, and the results only indicated a significant
because of their low factor loadings for relevant factors (see the Sup­ two-way interaction (Table 6). Further analyses revealed that, for those
plementary Materials). Among the remaining four items, two measured with anxious attachment scores of 1.40 (B = − 0.37, 95%CI [− 0.74,
dehumanization on agency (e.g., “Compared to average people, to what 0.00], SE = 0.19) or below, connectedness to nature reduced dehu­
extent do you think prisoners are capable of self-control?“) and two manization on agency. However, for those with anxious attachment
measured dehumanization on experience (e.g., “Compared to average scores above 1.40, the effect was non-significant (Fig. 3b).
people, to what extent do you think prisoners are capable of feeling A regression on dehumanization on experience showed that neither
fear?“). Each item was rated on a seven-point scale (1 = much less than the main effects nor two-way interaction was significant (Table 6).
average people, 4 = equal as average people, 7 = much more than average Therefore, we only considered dehumanization and dehumanization on
people). We reverse-coded and averaged ratings to create overall dehu­ agency in the subsequent analysis.
manization (α = 0.58), dehumanization on agency (α = 0.60), and Most importantly, supplemental analyses that controlled for avoi­
dehumanization on experience (α = 0.63) indices. Higher scores indi­ dant attachment yielded similar results (see the Supplementary Mate­
cated higher levels of dehumanization. rial), suggesting the robustness of the results.

4.1.2.4. Preference for nature over prisoners. We used a hypothetical 4.2.3. Effects on a preference for nature over prisoners
scenario to assess participants’ potential preference for nature over As shown in Table 5, indicators of dehumanization were positively
prisoners. Participants read a scenario in which they were government associated with a preference for nature over prisoners. To examine
officials and needed to allocate funds (1 million) to the budgets of two whether this preference was the downstream consequence of increased
programs. The first program aimed to increase protections of prisoners’ overall dehumanization using the PROCESS macro (Hayes, 2017; Model
basic human rights, while the second focused on improving environ­ 7, 5000 resamples), we tested a moderated mediation model with
mental protections. Given the fixed funding amount, allocating more to connectedness to nature conditions as the independent variable, pref­
protect the environment resulted in less funds being allocated to protect erence for nature as the dependent variable, overall dehumanization as
prisoners’ rights. After reading the scenario, participants wrote down the mediator, anxious attachment as the moderator.
the amount of funding they would allocate to each program. The budget The results showed that, the main effect of connectedness to nature
was in units of ten thousand, and participants provided their answers in was non-significant (B = − 3.22, 95%CI [− 10.57, 4.13], SE = 3.74, t =
a range of 0–100 (the allocated funds must total 100). We created a − 0.86, p = .389), while the main effect of overall dehumanization was
preference for nature index by subtracting the amount of money allo­ significant (B = 6.94, 95%CI [3.32, 10.57], SE = 1.84, t = 3.77, p <
cated to the first program from that allocated to the second one. Higher .001). Moreover, the moderated mediation model was effective
scores indicated a greater preference for nature over prisoners. (moderated mediation index = 1.10, bootstrap 95%CI [0.12, 2.28], SE
Finally, participants reported their gender and age. After completing = 0.56). Further analyses using the Johnson-Neyman technique
the online survey, participants were debriefed and remunerated. revealed that, for participants with anxious attachment scores of 5.57
(ab = 2.72, 95%CI [0.00, 6.11], SE = 1.54) or above, connectedness to
4.2. Results and discussion nature promoted preference for nature via increased overall dehuman­
ization. However, for those with anxious attachment scores below 5.57,
Table 5 shows the correlations between variables. the indirect effect was non-significant (Fig. 4a).
Next, we used dehumanization on agency as the mediator and con­
ducted a similar moderated mediation effect analysis. The results
showed that, the main effect of connectedness to nature was non-
significant (B = − 2.85, 95%CI [− 10.29, 4.60], SE = 3.78, t = − 0.75,
5
We adapted the scale by deleting items that explicitly mentioned nature (e. p = .452), while the main effect of dehumanization was significant (B =
g., protecting the environment). We thought that the deletion was necessary to 4.64, 95%CI [1.23, 8.05], SE = 1.73, t = 2.68, p = .008). Moreover, the
avoid potential conflicts between participants’ choices and our bogus feedback. moderated mediation model was effective (moderated mediation index

7
G. Chen and J. Dang Journal of Environmental Psychology 90 (2023) 102086

Table 5
Descriptive statistics and zero-order correlation between variables in study 3.
Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6

1. CN conditions – – –
2. CN manipulation check 4.61 1.62 .70*** –
3. Overall dehumanization 5.06 1.01 .004 .01 –
4. Dehumanization on agency 5.84 1.09 − .03 − .06 .71*** –
5. Dehumanization on experience 4.28 1.47 .03 .06 .85*** .24*** –
6. Preference for nature over prisoners 43.40 31.88 − .05 − .01 .22*** .16** .19** –
7. Anxious attachment 3.16 1.68 − .05 − .03 .004 .07 − .05 .02

Note. CN conditions = connectedness to nature conditions, which were coded: high connectedness to nature condition = 1, low connectedness to nature condition = 0.
**p < .01. ***p < .001.

Table 6
Regression analyses of moderated models in study 3.
Predictor variable Outcome variable: Overall Outcome variable: Dehumanization on agency Outcome variable:Dehumanization on
dehumanization experience

B [95% CI] SE t p B [95% CI] SE t p B [95% CI] SE t p

CN conditions 0.01 [-0.23, 0.12 0.07 .944 − 0.06 [-0.32, 0.13 − 0.47 .642 0.08 [-0.27, 0.42] 0.18 0.44 .660
0.25] 0.20]
Anxious attachment 0.01 [-0.06, 0.04 0.21 .836 0.05 [-0.02, 0.13] 0.04 1.33 .183 − 0.04 [-0.14, 0.05 − 0.70 .484
0.08] 0.07]
CN conditions × Anxious 0.16 [0.02, 0.30] 0.07 2.20 .029 0.18 [0.02, 0.33] 0.08 2.28 .023 0.14 [-0.07, 0.35] 0.10 1.34 .182
attachment

R2 .02 .02 .01

Note. CN conditions = connectedness to nature conditions, which were coded: high connectedness to nature condition = 1, low connectedness to nature condition = 0.

Fig. 3. The moderating role of anxious attachment in the effects of connect­ Fig. 4. The moderating role of anxious attachment in the conditional indirect
edness to nature on (a) overall dehumanization and (b) dehumanization on effects via (a) overall dehumanization and (b) dehumanization on agency in
agency in study 3. study 3.
Note. CN = connectedness to nature.
indirect effect was non-significant (Fig. 4b).
= 0.82, bootstrap 95%CI [0.06, 1.92], SE = 0.47). Further analyses In summary, Study 3 provided partial support for the hypothesis,
revealed that, for participants with anxious attachment scores of 1.35 revealing that connectedness to nature promoted overall dehumaniza­
(ab = − 1.77, 95%CI [− 4.36, 0.00], SE = 1.16) or below, connectedness tion only among people with higher levels of anxious attachment.
to nature reduced preference for nature via decreased dehumanization. Similar to the findings of Study 1, for people with low levels of anxious
However, for those with anxious attachment scores above 1.35, the attachment, connectedness to nature reduced dehumanization on

8
G. Chen and J. Dang Journal of Environmental Psychology 90 (2023) 102086

agency of prisoners. Dehumanization bolstered by connectedness to return to human relationships, but increasingly prevents them from
nature predicted greater preference for nature. The results suggest that engaging with human society. Thus, anxiously attached people may
whether connectedness to nature is beneficial or harmful to human re­ rarely share close relationships with other humans again if they are
lationships depends on the level of anxious attachment. preoccupied with particular nonhuman agents.
On a practical level, the present research paves the way for pro­
5. General Discussion moting people’s harmonious relationships with the social world. Our
findings suggest that anxiously attached people who feel socially con­
Human-nature relationships have become a scholarly focus world­ nected to nature may be preoccupied with it. Thus, they disregard the
wide, with advocates broadly arguing that close human-nature re­ welfare and humanity of distant others. Therefore, interventions tar­
lationships can substantially benefit the environment and people’s geting the quality of interpersonal relationships effectively establish a
subjective well-being (e.g., Cervinka et al., 2012; Mayer & Frantz, 2004; secure social climate and reduce people’s undesirable emotional bonds
UNEP, 2021). In the present research, we focused on how connectedness with nature. Stated differently, a social connection with nonhumans, in
to nature is associated with or influences dehumanization and examined this case, nature, can provide resources to address people’s connected­
the moderating role of anxious attachment in this association. Through ness needs (Wan & Chen, 2021). However, establishing satisfying social
three studies, we found that connectedness to nature was associated relationships with humans is vital for promoting the development of
with or led to greater dehumanization primarily among people with positive representations of others (Lavigne et al., 2011).
high levels of anxious attachment.
Notably, although the results of these studies mostly supported our 5.2. Limitations and future directions
hypothesis, Studies 2 and 3 did not yield a significant moderation
pattern when using dehumanization on experience as the dependent Some limitations should be addressed in future studies. First, the
variable. Likewise, the Johnson-Neyman plot for Study 1 also showed a present research mainly adopts the mind attribution perspective of
smaller significant region (i.e., weaker moderating effect) with dehu­ dehumanization (Waytz et al., 2010), although some researchers have
manization on experience as the dependent variable. This may be demonstrated that dehumanization is more than mind attribution
because of the specific targets of dehumanization. Previous research has (Fincher et al., 2018; Kteily & Landry, 2022), including differential at­
found that people downplay different aspects of mind for different tar­ tributions of humanness or secondary emotions and demeaning psy­
gets (Gray et al., 2007, 2011; Sim et al., 2022). Specifically, thieves chological needs (Haslam et al., 2005; Leyens et al., 2001; Schroeder &
(Study 1), people who casually litter (Study 2), and prisoners (Study 3) Epley, 2020). Therefore, future researchers could examine whether our
may be perceived as lacking self-control and morality, which reflects hypothesis is still supported by measuring other types of dehumaniza­
denials of agency (Gray et al., 2007). However, these target groups may tion. Further, consistent with previous research (Gray et al., 2011), the
still be capable of feeling anger, fear, and joy, which reflects their measures of dehumanization used in Studies 1 and 3 did not show high
normal experience capacity. Therefore, people may engage in dehu­ reliability. Future research should replicate our study with more refined
manization on agency rather than experience of these targets. To vali­ measures of dehumanization.
date this potential explanation, future research can use targets generally Second, the present research selected individuals who transgress
perceived as having a lack of experience to further explore the related norms as the targets of dehumanization because they are more vulner­
effects. For example, physicians usually deny patients’ experience of able to being dehumanized (e.g., Bastian et al., 2013). This leaves the
feeling pain (Haque & Waytz, 2012). In addition, organizations may question of whether our findings can be generalized to liked or neutral
view employees in the workplace as means to their ends, thereby lacking people who are distant in space or social similarity (e.g., people living in
the capacity to experience emotions (Christoff, 2014). distant places with very different cultures). In addition, the results
revealed that for people with low levels of anxious attachment,
5.1. Implications connectedness to nature was associated with or lead to less dehuman­
ization of thieves and prisoners, but not of litterers. Connectedness to
On a theoretical level, the present research extends mainstream nature involves seeing nature as part of one’s self-concept (e.g., Schultz,
research that emphasizes the benefits of connectedness to nature and 2002); therefore, harm to nature may be regarded as harm to the self
reveals its negative consequences for anxiously attached people. Previ­ and, thus, elicit stronger aversion regardless of the level of anxious
ous research has found that connectedness to nature may not only attachment. The targets of dehumanization may determine the point at
promote environmental conservation but also benefit psychological which the sign reverses for the relationship between connectedness to
health and intergroup relationships (Cervinka et al., 2012; Mayer et al., nature and dehumanization, and thus influence the portion of popula­
2009; Mayer & Frantz, 2004; Ng & Leung, 2022; Nisbet et al., 2009; tion among whom connectedness to nature promotes dehumanization.
Roszak et al., 1995; Starzyk et al., 2021). In line with this, the results of Another possible reason might be that we measured the absolute
Study 1 indicate the potential benefits of connectedness to nature for dehumanization of litterers but the relative dehumanization of thieves
enhancing the quality of social relationships with distant others. How­ and prisoners. Future research should explore these possibilities.
ever, this research consistently demonstrates that connectedness to na­ Lastly, Study 3 induced connectedness to nature by presenting par­
ture may promote dehumanization among people with high levels of ticipants with bogus feedback about their relationship with nature,
anxious attachment. By elucidating the negative aspects of connected­ ostensibly on the basis of a test. This paradigm may help make partici­
ness to nature under specific conditions, the present research not only pants believe that the feedback they received reflect their long-term
helps to further the current understanding of the effects of connected­ relationships and acted as a strong manipulation of connectedness
ness to nature but also inspires future researchers to reconsider how (Twenge et al., 2001, 2007). However, this manipulation is arguably
other human-non-human (e.g., human-animal) relationships may in­ interfered with the demand effects. Therefore, future research should
fluence human relationships. test our findings using other manipulations of connectedness to nature.
The present research also suggests the gloomy future anxiously
attached people face in their social relationships with humans. Owing to 5.3. Conclusion
excessive pursuit of loves, they usually cannot establish stable inter­
personal relationships (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2013), and often regard As global advocates continue to argue in support of increasing
nonhumans as the last option for gaining connection and security (e.g., connectedness to nature, exploring whether it potentially elicits nega­
Granqvist et al., 2010; Keefer et al., 2012). Ironically, feeling connected tive influences under specific conditions is vital. In the present research,
to nonhumans does not eventually help anxiously attached people we uncovered that connectedness to nature plays a specific role in

9
G. Chen and J. Dang Journal of Environmental Psychology 90 (2023) 102086

promoting dehumanization and a subsequent preference for nature over Bruneau, E., Szekeres, H., Kteily, N., Tropp, L. R., & Kende, A. (2020). Beyond dislike:
Blatant dehumanization predicts teacher discrimination. Group Processes & Intergroup
distant humans for anxiously attached people, although it may enhance
Relations, 23(4), 560–577. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430219845462
the quality of human relationships for people with low levels of anxious Buckels, E. E., & Trapnell, P. D. (2013). Disgust facilitates outgroup dehumanization.
attachment. As such, feeling connected to nature is not always benefi­ Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 16(6), 771–780. https://doi.org/10.1177/
cial, at least for those with high levels of anxious attachment. The im­ 1368430212471738
Carnelley, K. B., & Boag, E. M. (2019). Attachment and prejudice. Current Opinion in
plications of connectedness to nature on human relationships are worthy Psychology, 25, 110–114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.04.003
of further theoretical and practical consideration. Cervinka, R., Röderer, K., & Hefler, E. (2012). Are nature lovers happy? On various
indicators of well-being and connectedness with nature. Journal of Health Psychology,
17(3), 379–388. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105311416873
Author contributions Christoff, K. (2014). Dehumanization in organizational settings: Some scientific and
ethical considerations. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 1–5. https://doi.org/
10.3389/fnhum.2014.00748
G. Chen and J. Dang contributed to Conceptualization, Writing-
Costello, K., & Hodson, G. (2010). Exploring the roots of dehumanization: The role of
original draft and Writing-review & editing. G. Chen performed Data animal—human similarity in promoting immigrant humanization. Group Processes &
curation and Formal analysis under the Supervision of J. Dang. Intergroup Relations, 13(1), 3–22. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430209347725
Costello, K., & Hodson, G. (2014a). Explaining dehumanization among children: The
interspecies model of prejudice. British Journal of Social Psychology, 53(1), 175–197.
https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12016
Declaration of competing interest
Costello, K., & Hodson, G. (2014b). Lay beliefs about the causes of and solutions to
dehumanization and prejudice: Do nonexperts recognize the role of human-animal
We have no known conflict of interest to disclose. relations? Lay beliefs. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 44(4), 278–288. https://
doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12221
Dang, J., & Liu, L. (2023). Does connectedness need satisfaction diminish or promote
Acknowledgments social goal striving? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 49(6), 891–909.
https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672221084539
Deska, J. C., Almaraz, S. M., & Hugenberg, K. (2020). Dehumanizing prisoners:
The authors acknowledge the financial support provided by National
Remaining sentence duration predicts the ascription of mind to prisoners. Personality
Natural Science Foundation of China (32100869; 32271124), the Proj­ and Social Psychology Bulletin, 46(11), 1614–1627. https://doi.org/10.1177/
ect of Humanities and Social Science Research of the Ministry of Edu­ 0146167220911496
Dhont, K., Hodson, G., & Leite, A. C. (2016). Common ideological roots of speciesism and
cation of China (21YJC190003), the Major Project of the National Social
generalized ethnic prejudice: The social dominance human–animal relations model
Science Foundation of China (18ZDA332), and the Fundamental (SD–HARM). European Journal of Personality, 30(6), 507–522. https://doi.org/
Research Funds for the Central University (2019NTSS30). The authors 10.1002/per.2069
would like to thank Editage (www.editage.cn) for English language Dovidio, J. F., Hewstone, M., Glick, P., & Esses, V. M. (2010). Prejudice, stereotyping,
and discrimination: Theoretical and empirical overview. In J. F. Dovidio,
editing. M. Hewstone, P. Glick, & V. M. Esses (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of prejudice,
stereotyping, and discrimination (pp. 3–28). SAGE. https://doi.org/10.4135/
9781446200919.n1.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
Esses, V. M., Medianu, S., & Lawson, A. S. (2013). Uncertainty, threat, and the role of the
media in promoting the dehumanization of immigrants and refugees:
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi. Dehumanization of immigrants and refugees. Journal of Social Issues, 69(3), 518–536.
https://doi.org/10.1111/josi.12027
org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2023.102086.
Fincher, K. M., Kteily, N. S., & Bruneau, E. G. (2018). Our humanity contains multitudes:
Dehumanization is more than overlooking mental capacities. Proceedings of the
References National Academy of Sciences, 115(15). https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1800359115
Fraley, R. C., Heffernan, M. E., Vicary, A. M., & Brumbaugh, C. C. (2011). The
experiences in close relationships—relationship structures questionnaire: A method
Amiot, C. E., & Bastian, B. (2015). Toward a psychology of human–animal relations.
for assessing attachment orientations across relationships. Psychological Assessment,
Psychological Bulletin, 141(1), 6–47. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038147
23(3), 615–625. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022898
Bandura, A., Underwood, B., & Fromson, M. E. (1975). Disinhibition of aggression
Global Times. (2021). Thunberg mocked as ’double-standard environmentalist,’ ’puppet of
through diffusion of responsibility and dehumanization of victims. Journal of
Western politicians’ for targeting China on annual emissions. May 9. Global Times https
Research in Personality, 9(4), 253–269. https://doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(75)
://www.globaltimes.cn/page/202105/1222988.shtml.
90001-X
Granqvist, P., Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2010). Religion as attachment: Normative
Bao, Z., Zhang, W., Lai, X., Sun, W., & Wang, Y. (2015). Parental attachment and Chinese
processes and individual differences. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14(1),
adolescents’ delinquency: The mediating role of moral disengagement. Journal of
49–59. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868309348618
Adolescence, 44(1), 37–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2015.06.002
Gray, H. M., Gray, K., & Wegner, D. M. (2007). Dimensions of mind perception. Science,
Bastian, B., Costello, K., Loughnan, S., & Hodson, G. (2012). When closing the
315(5812). https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1134475, 619–619.
human–animal divide expands moral concern: The importance of framing. Social
Gray, K., Knobe, J., Sheskin, M., Bloom, P., & Barrett, L. F. (2011). More than a body:
Psychological and Personality Science, 3(4), 421–429. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Mind perception and the nature of objectification. Journal of Personality and Social
1948550611425106
Psychology, 101(6), 1207–1220. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025883
Bastian, B., Denson, T. F., & Haslam, N. (2013). The roles of dehumanization and moral
Haddock, G., Zanna, M. P., & Esses, V. M. (1993). Assessing the structure of prejudicial
outrage in retributive justice. PLoS One, 8(4), Article e61842. https://doi.org/
attitudes: The case of attitudes toward homosexuals. Journal of Personality and Social
10.1371/journal.pone.0061842
Psychology, 65(6), 1105–1118. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.6.1105
Boag, E. M., & Carnelley, K. B. (2012). Self-reported discrimination and discriminatory
Haque, O. S., & Waytz, A. (2012). Dehumanization in medicine: Causes, solutions, and
behaviour: The role of attachment security: Discrimination and attachment security.
functions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(2), 176–186. https://doi.org/
British Journal of Social Psychology, 51(2), 393–403. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-
10.1177/1745691611429706
8309.2011.02065.x
Haslam, N. (2006). Dehumanization: An integrative review. Personality and Social
Boag, E. M., & Carnelley, K. B. (2016). Attachment and prejudice: The mediating role of
Psychology Review, 10(3), 252–264. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1003_4
empathy. British Journal of Social Psychology, 55(2), 337–356. https://doi.org/
Haslam, N. (2022). Dehumanization and the lack of social connection. Current Opinion in
10.1111/bjso.12132
Psychology, 43, 312–316. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.08.013
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Retrospect and prospect. American Journal of
Haslam, N., Bain, P., Douge, L., Lee, M., & Bastian, B. (2005). More human than you:
Orthopsychiatry, 52(4), 664–678. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.1982.
Attributing humanness to self and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
tb01456.x
89(6), 937–950. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.6.937
Brandt, M. J., & Reyna, C. (2011). The chain of being: A hierarchy of morality.
Haslam, N., & Loughnan, S. (2014). Dehumanization and infrahumanization. Annual
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(5), 428–446. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Review of Psychology, 65, 399–423. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-
1745691611414587
115045
Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult
Haslam, N., & Stratemeyer, M. (2016). Recent research on dehumanization. Current
attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson, & W. S. Rholes (Eds.),
Opinion in Psychology, 11, 25–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.03.009
Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46–76). The Guilford Press.
Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis:
Brown, C. M., Hengy, S. M., & McConnell, A. R. (2016). Thinking about cats or dogs
A regression-based approach. Guilford Press.
provides relief from social rejection. Anthrozoös, 29(1), 47–58. https://doi.org/
Johnson, P. O., & Neyman, J. (1936). Tests of certain linear hypotheses and their
10.1080/20414005.2015.1067958
application to some educational problems. Statistical Research Memoirs, 1, 57–93. htt
Bruneau, E., Kteily, N., Jacoby, N., & Saxe, R. (2018). Denying humanity: The distinct
ps://www.proquest.com/scholarly-journals/tests-certain-linear-hypotheses-thei
neural correlates of blatant dehumanization. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
r-application/docview/615048518/se-2.
General, 147(7), 1078–1093. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000417

10
G. Chen and J. Dang Journal of Environmental Psychology 90 (2023) 102086

Jordan, M. (2009). Nature and self—an ambivalent attachment? Ecopsychology, 1(1), Opotow, S. (1990). Moral exclusion and injustice: An introduction. Journal of Social
26–31. https://doi.org/10.1089/eco.2008.0003 Issues, 46(1), 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1990.tb00268.x
Keefer, L. A., Landau, M. J., Rothschild, Z. K., & Sullivan, D. (2012). Attachment to Poon, K.-T., Teng, F., Chow, J. T., & Chen, Z. (2015). Desiring to connect to nature: The
objects as compensation for close others’ perceived unreliability. Journal of effect of ostracism on ecological behavior. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 42,
Experimental Social Psychology, 48(4), 912–917. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. 116–122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.03.003
jesp.2012.02.007 Rammstedt, B., & John, O. P. (2007). Measuring personality in one minute or less: A 10-
Kellert, S. R., & Wilson, E. O. (1993). The biophilia hypothesis. Island Press. item short version of the Big five inventory in English and German. Journal of
Kersbergen, I., & Robinson, E. (2019). Blatant dehumanization of people with obesity. Research in Personality, 41(1), 203–212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2006.02.001
Obesity, 27(6), 1005–1012. https://doi.org/10.1002/oby.22460 Roszak, T. E., Gomes, M. E., & Kanner, A. D. (1995). Ecopsychology: Restoring the earth,
Kramp, J. M. (2015). Call of the wild: The negative tendency in the nature religions of healing the mind. Sierra Club Books.
American youth. Journal of Religion and Health, 54(1), 61–75. https://doi.org/ Rottman, J., Crimston, C. R., & Syropoulos, S. (2021). Tree-huggers versus human-lovers:
10.1007/s10943-013-9732-z Anthropomorphism and dehumanization predict valuing nature over outgroups.
Kteily, N., Bruneau, E., Waytz, A., & Cotterill, S. (2015). The ascent of man: Theoretical Cognitive Science, 45(4), Article e12967. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12967
and empirical evidence for blatant dehumanization. Journal of Personality and Social Saroglou, V., Buxant, C., & Tilquin, J. (2008). Positive emotions as leading to religion
Psychology, 109(5), 901–931. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000048 and spirituality. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 3(3), 165–173. https://doi.org/
Kteily, N. S., & Landry, A. P. (2022). Dehumanization: Trends, insights, and challenges. 10.1080/17439760801998737
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26(3), 222–240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Schroeder, J., & Epley, N. (2020). Demeaning: Dehumanizing others by minimizing the
tics.2021.12.003 importance of their psychological needs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Lakens, D. (2014). Performing high-powered studies efficiently with sequential analyses: 119(4), 765–791. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000199
Sequential analyses. European Journal of Social Psychology, 44(7), 701–710. https:// Schultz, P. W. (2002). Inclusion with nature: The psychology of human-nature relations.
doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2023 In P. Schmuck, & W. P. Schultz (Eds.), Psychology of sustainable development (pp.
Lavigne, G. L., Vallerand, R. J., & Crevier-Braud, L. (2011). The fundamental need to 61–78). Springer US. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-0995-0_4.
belong: On the distinction between growth and deficit-reduction orientations. Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(9), 1185–1201. https://doi.org/ advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. Advances in Experimental Social
10.1177/0146167211405995 Psychology, 25, 1–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60281-6. Elsevier.
Lengieza, M. L., & Swim, J. K. (2021). The paths to connectedness: A review of the Shin, H. I., & Kim, J. (2020). My computer is more thoughtful than you: Loneliness,
antecedents of connectedness to nature. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, Article 763231. anthropomorphism and dehumanization. Current Psychology, 39(2), 445–453.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.763231 https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-9975-7
Leopold, A. (1949). A sand county almanac: With essays on conservation from round river. Sim, M., Almaraz, S. M., & Hugenberg, K. (2022). Bodies and minds: Heavier weight
New York: Ballantine Books. targets are de-mentalized as lacking in mental agency. Personality and Social
Leyens, J.-P., Rodriguez-Perez, A., Rodriguez-Torres, R., Gaunt, R., Paladino, M.-P., Psychology Bulletin, 48(9), 1367–1381. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Vaes, J., & Demoulin, S. (2001). Psychological essentialism and the differential 01461672211039981
attribution of uniquely human emotions to ingroups and outgroups. European Journal Smith, D. L. (2011). Less than human: Why we demean, enslave, and exterminate others. St.
of Social Psychology, 31(4), 395–411. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.50 Martin: s Press.
Liu, T., Geng, L., Ye, L., & Zhou, K. (2019). “Mother Nature” enhances connectedness to Spadaro, P. A. (2020). Climate change, environmental terrorism, eco-terrorism and
nature and pro-environmental behavior. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 61, emerging threats. Journal of Strategic Security, 13(4), 58–80. https://doi.org/
37–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.12.003 10.5038/1944-0472.13.4.1863
Maranges, H. M., Chen, S. K., & Conway, P. (2022). Insecure and insensitive: Avoidant Starzyk, K. B., Fontaine, A. S. M., Strand, L. K., & Neufeld, K. H. S. (2021). Attitudes
and anxious attachment predict less concern for others in sacrificial moral dilemmas. toward reconciliation in Canada: Relationships with connectedness to nature,
Personality and Individual Differences, 185, Article 111274. https://doi.org/10.1016/ animal–human continuity, and moral expansiveness. Canadian Journal of Behavioural
j.paid.2021.111274 Science/Revue Canadienne des Sciences du Comportement, 53(4), 381–390. https://doi.
Mayer, F. S., & Frantz, C. M. (2004). The connectedness to nature scale: A measure of org/10.1037/cbs0000248
individuals’ feeling in community with nature. Journal of Environmental Psychology, Tam, K.-P., Lee, S.-L., & Chao, M. M. (2013). Saving Mr. Nature: Anthropomorphism
24(4), 503–515. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.10.001 enhances connectedness to and protectiveness toward nature. Journal of Experimental
Mayer, F. S., Frantz, C. M., Bruehlman-Senecal, E., & Dolliver, K. (2009). Why is nature Social Psychology, 49(3), 514–521. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.02.001
beneficial? The role of connectedness to nature. Environment and Behavior, 41(5), Twenge, J. M., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Ciarocco, N. J., & Bartels, J. M. (2007).
607–643. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916508319745 Social exclusion decreases prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2013). An attachment perspective on loneliness. In Psychology, 92(1), 56–66. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.1.56
R. J. Coplan, & J. C. Bowker (Eds.), The handbook of solitude (1st ed., pp. 34–50). Twenge, J. M., Baumeister, R. F., Tice, D. M., & Stucke, T. S. (2001). If you can’t join
Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118427378.ch3. them, beat them: Effects of social exclusion on aggressive behavior. Journal of
Moreton, S., Arena, A., & Tiliopoulos, N. (2019). Connectedness to nature is more Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 1058–1069. https://doi.org/10.1037//
strongly related to connection to distant, rather than close, others. Ecopsychology, 11 0022-3514.81.6.1058
(1), 59–65. https://doi.org/10.1089/eco.2018.0063 UNEP. (2021). Making peace with nature: A scientific blueprint to tackle the climate,
Mourey, J. A., Olson, J. G., & Yoon, C. (2017). Products as pals: Engaging with biodiversity and pollution emergencies.
anthropomorphic products mitigates the effects of social exclusion. Journal of Wan, E. W., & Chen, R. P. (2021). Anthropomorphism and object attachment. Current
Consumer Research, 44(2), 414–431. https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucx038 Opinion in Psychology, 39, 88–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.08.009
Neave, N., Jackson, R., Saxton, T., & Hönekopp, J. (2015). The influence of Waytz, A., Epley, N., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Social cognition unbound: Insights into
anthropomorphic tendencies on human hoarding behaviours. Personality and anthropomorphism and dehumanization. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
Individual Differences, 72, 214–219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.08.041 19(1), 58–62. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721409359302
Neave, N., Tyson, H., McInnes, L., & Hamilton, C. (2016). The role of attachment style Yang, Y., Cai, H., Yang, Z., Zhao, X., Li, M., Han, R., & Chen, S. X. (2022). Why does
and anthropomorphism in predicting hoarding behaviours in a non-clinical sample. nature enhance psychological well-being? A self-determination account. Journal of
Personality and Individual Differences, 99, 33–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Environmental Psychology, 83, Article 101872. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
paid.2016.04.067 jenvp.2022.101872
Ng, H. K. S., & Leung, A. N. M. (2022). Nature connectedness and nature exposure Zelenski, J. M., & Nisbet, E. K. (2014). Happiness and feeling connected: The distinct role
interactively influence social dominance orientation and policy support for of nature relatedness. Environment and Behavior, 46(1), 3–23. https://doi.org/
marginalized groups during the COVID-19 pandemic. Environment and Behavior, 54 10.1177/0013916512451901
(2), 412–449. https://doi.org/10.1177/00139165211031198 Zhang, H., Chan, D. K.-S., Teng, F., & Zhang, D. (2015). Sense of interpersonal security
Nisbet, E. K., Zelenski, J. M., & Murphy, S. A. (2009). The nature relatedness scale: and preference for harsh actions against others: The role of dehumanization. Journal
Linking individuals’ connection with nature to environmental concern and behavior. of Experimental Social Psychology, 56, 165–171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Environment and Behavior, 41(5), 715–740. https://doi.org/10.1177/ jesp.2014.09.014
0013916508318748 Zhang, Q., Hou, Z.-J., Fraley, R. C., Hu, Y., Zhang, X., Zhang, J., & Guo, X. (2022).
Norris, J. I., Lambert, N. M., Nathan DeWall, C., & Fincham, F. D. (2012). Can’t buy me Validating the experiences in close relationships–relationship structures scale among
love? Anxious attachment and materialistic values. Personality and Individual Chinese children and adolescents. Journal of Personality Assessment, 104(3), 347–358.
Differences, 53(5), 666–669. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.05.009 https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2021.1947844

11

You might also like