You are on page 1of 6

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/232869178

Basic social influence is underestimated

Article in Psychological Inquiry · October 2005


DOI: 10.1207/s15327965pli1604_03

CITATIONS READS

58 363

1 author:

Robert B Cialdini
Arizona State University
147 PUBLICATIONS 19,640 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Robert B Cialdini on 01 July 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. All in-text references underlined in blue are added to the original document
and are linked to publications on ResearchGate, letting you access and read them immediately.
Psychological Inquiry Copyright © 2005 by
2005, Vol. 16, No. 4, 158–161 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Basic Social Influence Is Underestimated


Robert B. Cialdini
Department of Psychology
Arizona State University

Root Wisdom most noteworthy outcome of the demonstration. It was


that, when interviewed afterward, not one of the eight
As a Rule, People Grossly givers in the modeled help condition attributed his or
Underestimate the Guiding Role That her donation decision to the confederate’s donation de-
Others Play in Personal Choices cision. Instead, they claimed that some other factor had
been the cause—“I liked the song he was playing” or “I
Scholars of various kinds, including some re-
felt sorry for the guy” or “I had some extra change in
nowned social psychologists, have long documented
my pocket.” Because we know what happened in the
the powerful influence that observed social behavior
control condition, we also know that it was not any of
has on subsequent social behavior (e.g., Festinger,
these factors that made the difference. It was the simi-
1954; Le Bon, 1960; MacKay, 1841/1932; Milgram,
lar action of another. But in the absence of those con-
Bickman, & Berkowitz, 1969). What’s surprising,
trol data, when participants searched their respective
given the ubiquity and strength of the evidence, is how
phenomenologies, they never once located the true
little note people take of this potent form of influence
cause of their own behavior.
at three crucial and often-encountered decision points:
This illustrates a more specific psychological point
when, as observers, they decide how to interpret the
than the one articulated masterfully by Nisbett and
causes of their own actions; when, as tacticians, they
Wilson (1977) that, in general, people are poor at rec-
decide how to influence the actions of others; and
ognizing why they behave as they do. It asserts that
when, as experts, they decide whether to seek the input
they will be particularly clueless when identifying the
of others. Let’s take each in turn.
similar actions of others as causal antecedents. More
systematic evidence in this regard comes from a study
(Schultz, Nolan, Cialdini, Goldstein, & Griskevicius,
Branch Wisdom No. 1
2005) of perceived motivations for energy conserva-
tion that Wesley Schultz and I conducted recently
People Frequently Ignore or Severely
along with our students, Noah Goldstein, Vladas
Underestimate the Extent to Which
Griskevicius, and Jessica Nolan.
Their Actions in a Situation Are
As part of a large-scale survey of residential en-
Determined by the Similar Actions of
ergy users, we inquired into respondents’ views of
Others There
their reasons for conserving energy at home as well
A while ago, I was asked by the producers of a TV as reports of their actual residential energy saving ac-
news magazine show to assist with a segment on why tivities such as installing energy efficient appliances
people help in everyday (nonemergency) settings. I and light bulbs, adjusting thermostats, and turning off
agreed, which involved collaborating on the filming of lights. When respondents were asked to rate the im-
some opportunities for everyday aid such as helping a portance to them of several reasons for energy con-
woman who had locked herself out of her car or stop- servation—because it will help save the environment,
ping to compensate a street performer. Regarding the because it will benefit society, because it will save me
second of these helping opportunities, I had suggested money, or because other people are doing it—they
a conceptual replication of social psychological re- rated these motivations in the order just listed, with
search on modeled giving (e.g., Bryan & Test, 1967): the actions of others (the descriptive social norm;
Set up a street musician in a heavily trafficked spot and Cialdini, Reno, & Kallgren, 1990) clearly in last
count the number of passers-by who put money in his place. However, when we examined the relationship
hat after a confederate had done so versus after no such between participants’ beliefs in these reasons and
contribution had been modeled. their attempts to save energy, we found the reverse:
Although the consequent donation results can The belief that others were conserving correlated
hardly be described as unimpressive—the modeled twice as highly with reported energy saving efforts
help condition produced eight contributions compared than did any of the reasons that had been rated as
to just one in the control condition—that was not the more important personal motivators.
BASIC SOCIAL INFLUENCE

To assure that our findings were not the result of the Because they do not give sufficient weight to the
correlational nature of the survey methodology power of descriptive normative information, there is
(through a false consensus effect), a follow-up study an understandable tendency of social program devel-
employed an experimental design. Residents of a opers to try to mobilize action against a problem by
midsize California community received persuasive ap- depicting it as regrettably frequent. And, this is cer-
peals on door-hangers placed on their doorknobs once tainly not unique to environmental programs: Infor-
a week for 4 consecutive weeks. The appeals empha- mation campaigns stress that alcohol and drug use is
sized to residents that energy conservation efforts intolerably high, that adolescent suicide rates are
would (a) help the environment or (b) benefit society or alarming, and that rampant polluters are spoiling the
(c) save them money or (d) were common (normative) environment. Although these claims may be both true
in their neighborhood. Interviews with participants re- and well intentioned, the campaigns’ creators have
vealed that those who received the normative appeals missed something critically important: Within the
rated them as least likely to motivate their conservation statement “Look at all the people who are doing this
behavior. Yet, when we examined actual energy usage undesirable thing” lurks the powerful and undercut-
(by recording participants’ electricity meter readings), ting normative message “Look at all the people who
the normative appeal proved most helpful, resulting in are doing it.” It is conceivable, then, that in trying to
significantly more conservation than any of the other alert the public to the widespread nature of a prob-
appeals (Schultz et al., 2005). lem, public service communicators can make it
The upshot of these studies is plain. When it comes worse.
to estimating the causes of their conduct, people seem To explore this possibility as it applies to individu-
especially blind to the large relative role of others’ sim- als’ decisions to despoil the environment, my col-
ilar behavior. They do not just fail to get this relative leagues and I at ASU conducted an experimental test
role right; they tend to get it precisely wrong. (described in Cialdini, 2003). At the Petrified Forest,
we alternated a pair of signs in high-theft areas of the
park. The first urged visitors not to take wood, and it
Branch Wisdom No. 2 depicted a scene showing three thieves in action. Our
other sign also urged visitors not to take wood, but it
People Frequently Ignore or Severely depicted a lone thief. Visitors who passed the first type
Underestimate the Persuasive Impact of sign became significantly more likely to steal than
That Others’ Behavior Can Have on those who passed the second type of sign (7.92% vs.
the Choices of a Target Audience 1.67%).2 Thus, because they underappreciate the
power of descriptive normative information, program
Although marketers and advertisers have learned
developers can engage in tactics that are counterpro-
through experience to avoid this error, it often shows it-
ductive.
self in the persuasion choices of policy-makers and
In addition, via the same judgment error, program
program developers. Take, for instance, the case of an
developers may fail to implement tactics that can be
antivandalism program in Arizona’s Petrified Forest
highly productive. For example, instead of focusing on
National Park, which is regularly in crisis because of
evidence that many people act to harm the environ-
the estimated theft of more than a ton of wood per
ment, public service communicators would be well ad-
month by visitors. As part of the program, new arrivals
vised to focus on evidence that many people act to pre-
quickly learn of the past thievery from prominently
serve it.
placed signage: “Your heritage is being vandalized ev-
To investigate this idea, we examined resource
ery day by theft losses of petrified wood of 14 tons a
conservation choices in hotel rooms, where guests of-
year, mostly a small piece at a time.” Although it is un-
ten encounter a card asking them to reuse their tow-
derstandable that park officials would want to instigate
els. As anyone who travels frequently knows, this
corrective action by describing the dismaying size of
card may urge the action in various ways. Sometimes
the problem, such a message is far from optimal. In-
it requests compliance for the sake of the environ-
deed, by normalizing the unwanted activity, the mes-
ment, sometimes it does so for the sake of future gen-
sage stands a good chance of backfiring.1
erations, and sometimes it exhorts guests to cooperate
with the hotel to save resources. What the card never
1As a case in point, not long ago a graduate student of mine visited says, however, is that the majority of guests do reuse
the Petrified Forest National Park with his fiancée—a woman he de-
scribed as the most honest person he had ever known, someone who
had never taken a paper clip or rubber band without returning it. They
quickly encountered the park sign warning visitors against stealing
2These data are best understood in the context of previous re-
petrified wood and decrying the 14 tons of pilfered wood each year.
While still reading the sign, he was shocked to hear this otherwise search indicating that the ratio of thefts to park visitors falls just un-
wholly law-abiding woman whisper, “We’d better get ours now.” der 3%.

159
CIALDINI

their towels when given the opportunity.3 We sus- Pauling). At first, Watson ticked off a set of contribu-
pected that this omission was costing the hotels—and tory factors that, although crucial, did not surprise
the environment—plenty. me: He and Crick had identified the problem as the
Here is how we tested our suspicion. With the col- most important one to attack; they were passionate
laboration of the management of a hotel in the Phoenix about it, devoting themselves single-mindedly to the
area, we put one of four different cards in its task; and, they were willing to try approaches that
guestrooms. One of the cards stated “HELP SAVE came from outside their areas of familiarity.
THE ENVIRONMENT,” which was followed by in- But then he described a reason for their success that
formation stressing respect for nature. A different card stunned me, maintaining that he and Crick had cracked
stated “HELP SAVE RESOURCES FOR FUTURE the elusive code of DNA because they were not the
GENERATIONS,” which was followed by informa- most intelligent of the scientific competitors pursuing
tion stressing the importance of saving energy for the the answer. According to Watson, the smartest of the
future. A third type of card stated “PARTNER WITH lot was Rosalind Franklin, a brilliant British scientist
US TO HELP SAVE THE ENVIRONMENT,” which who was working in Paris at the time. “Rosalind was so
was followed by information urging guests to cooper- intelligent,” observed Watson, “that she rarely sought
ate with the hotel in preserving the environment. A fi- advice. If you’re the brightest person in the room,
nal type of card stated “JOIN YOUR FELLOW you’re in trouble.” For me, that comment was both a
CITIZENS IN HELPING TO SAVE THE revelation and a confirmation, throwing new light on a
ENVIRONMENT,” which was followed by informa- familiar error I had observed in the actions of many
tion that the majority of hotel guests do reuse their tow- well intentioned leaders.
els when asked. The outcome? Compared to the first Far too often, a leader (who, by virtue of greater ex-
three messages, the final (descriptive normative) mes- perience or wisdom or skill, is the ablest prob-
sage increased towel reuse by an average of 28.4% lem-solver in a particular arena) fails to ask for input
(Goldstein, Cialdini, & Griskevicius, 2005). from associates because he or she is, after all, the ablest
It seems worth special note that the message that problem-solver there. The consequences are
generated the most participation in the hotel’s towel re- suboptimal solutions, choices, and directions that
cycling program was the one that no hotel (to our could have been avoided via consultation. It is easy to
knowledge) has ever used. Apparently, this simple but see how Rosalind Franklin could have made a similar
effective type of appeal does not occur in the persua- mistake: As the most intellectually gifted worker in the
sive efforts of hotel conservation program developers area, why should she want to seek the advice of the less
because they never estimate that it will work as power- gifted?
fully as it does. Data from the lab of Patrick Laughlin tell us why
(Laughlin, Bonner, & Miner, 2002; Laughlin, Zander,
Knievel, & Tan, 2003). Even the best problem-solver
Branch Wisdom No. 3
operating individually will be beaten to a demonstrably
correct solution by a cooperating unit. That is so for two
When They Are Expert on a Topic,
main reasons. First, the lone problem-solver cannot
People Frequently Ignore or Severely
match the diversity of knowledge and perspectives of a
Underestimate the Extent to Which
multiperson unit that includes him or her. Other mem-
Others’ Input Can Improve Their
bers will have had experiences with similar or related
Decisions
problems that will allow the team to recognize fruitful
I came across a remarkable interview a couple of versus fruitless choices more clearly and quickly. Fur-
years ago on the 50th anniversary of the publication ther, this diversity of input can do more than merely add
of perhaps the most important scientific discovery of to the storehouse of information that the best prob-
our time—the (double helix) structure of DNA, as re- lem-solver can employ; it can also stimulate thinking
vealed in the Nobel Prize-winning work of James processes that would not have developed in wholly in-
Watson and Francis Crick. The interview, with Wat- ternal monologues. I would wager that we all can recall
son (An Interview With James Watson, 2003), was being led to a productive insight by the comment of a
designed to inquire into those aspects of the duo’s ef- colleague who did not deliver the insight itself but who
forts that had led them to solve the problem ahead of sparked an association that did the trick.
an array of highly accomplished rival investigators Second, the solution-seeker who goes it alone loses
(including two-time Nobel Prize winner Linus a significant information processing advantage—the
power of parallel processing. Whereas a cooperating
3According to data from the Project Planet Corporation, which
unit can distribute the many subtasks of a prob-
manufactures these types of cards, nearly three quarters of guests lem-solving campaign among its members, the lone
who are informed about a hotel’s reuse program comply at least once operator must perform each sequentially. This require-
during their stay. ment extends the effort considerably in time. In addi-

160
BASIC SOCIAL INFLUENCE

tion, it strains the capacities and energies of the prob- Cialdini, R. B., Reno, R. R., & Kallgren, C. A. (1990). A focus the-
lem-solver because the subtasks often include ory of normative conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to re-
duce littering in public places. Journal of Personality and So-
activities that are daunting in their difficulty (e.g., in- cial Psychology, 58, 1015–1026.
formation integration), time consuming in their execu- Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Hu-
tion (e.g., library/Internet research), and demotivating man Relations, 7, 117–140.
in their tediousness (e.g., fact checking). Goldstein, N., Cialdini, R. B., & Griskevicius, V. (2005). A room
What is the take-away wisdom here? Leaders at- with a viewpoint: Using norm-based appeals to motivate con-
servation behaviors in a hotel setting. Manuscript submitted for
tacking a knotty problem possessing an objectively publication.
correct solution should collaborate unfailingly with Interview With James Watson [An]. (2003, February 17). Time Mag-
team members toward its resolution—even when they azine, 57.
are the best informed or most experienced or ablest of Laughlin, P. R., Bonner, B. L., & Miner, A. G. (2002). Groups per-
them all. This means setting up systems that ensure form better than the best individuals on letters-to-numbers
problems. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro-
collaborative exchanges whether or not the collabora- cesses, 88, 605–620.
tion seems necessary. To do less is a fool’s gamble. Laughlin, P. R., Zander, M. L., Knievel, E. M., & Tan, T. K.
(2003). Groups perform better than the best individuals on
letters-to-numbers problems: Informative equations and ef-
Note fective strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 85, 684–694.
Correspondence should be sent to Robert B. Le Bon, G. (1960). Psychology of the crowd. New York: Viking.
Cialdini, Department of Psychology, Arizona State (Original work published 1895)
MacKay, C. (1841/1932). Popular delusions and the madness of
University, Box 871104, Tempe, AZ 85287–1104. crowds. New York: Farrar, Straus, & Giroux.
E-mail: robert.cialdini@asu.edu Milgram, S., Bickman, L., & Berkowitz, L. (1969). Note on the
drawing power of crowds of different size. Journal of Personal-
ity and Social Psychology, 13, 79–82.
References Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can
know: Verbal report on mental processes. Psychological Re-
Bryan, J. H., & Test, M. A. (1967). Models and helping: Naturalistic view, 84, 231–259.
studies in aiding behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Schultz, P. W., Nolan, J. M., Cialdini, R. B., Goldstein, N., &
Psychology, 6, 400–407. Griskevicius, V. (2005). Descriptive normative beliefs, norma-
Cialdini, R. B. (2003). Crafting normative messages to protect the environ- tive feedback and energy conservation. Manuscript submitted
ment. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 105–109. for publication.

161
View publication stats

You might also like